Introduction To Linguistics
Introduction To Linguistics
Palatals
Formed by touching the sides of the tongue to the roof of the mouth
/ʃ/ – ship
/ʒ/ – pleasure
/tʃ/ – cheap
/dʒ/ – jump
/j/ – yogurt
Velars
Formed by putting the back part of the tongue against the soft
palate
/k/ – curl
/g/ – girl
/ŋ/ – song
Glottal
• labials – lips The glottis is actually two vocal folds (i.e. vocal cords). It acts as a
• dentals – teeth sort of bottle cap to your windpipe.
• tongue
• palate – roof of the mouth; the hard palate is the harder Inhale and then hold your breath for a few seconds while keeping
(firmer) part of the roof of the mouth, in the front half of your mouth open. What you are actually doing to keep the air from
the mouth; the soft palate is farther back, near the rear of expelling out of your lungs by closing your glottis.
the mouth. If you read
Glottal consonants aren’t actually consonants; they just play
• alveolar ridge – the gums behind your top teeth
consonant roles in the language. In English, the following things
happen at the glottis:
/h/ as in “hi” and “Bahamas.” Say these words and notice how 2) Fricative
you’re not actually constricting or blocking airflow for this /h/ sound. We stop part of the air so it can’t come through everywhere. It is a stream
You’re just exhaling a little bit harder than you would for a normal of air. Imagine a river of water flowing. Then imagine putting a large
rock in the center of the river. The flow of the water would change
vowel sound in transition to the following vowel sound.
because of the large rock, but the water would still flow around it. This
/?/ – This is actually the culprit behind many of the “silent syllables” is a strong description of what a fricative sound is formed. There is a
we discussed in the first lesson. For example, in the phrase “wha(t) flowing river of air, and you partially block it with your tongue, teeth, or
time is it?” the /t/ in “what” is dropped and the vowel sound before lips.
it is closed at the glottis.
Sounds include: /f/ /v/ /s/ /z/ /h/ /ʃ/ /ʒ/ /ð/ /θ/
Initial Sounds
/f/ – four
/v/ – van
/s/ – sun
/z/ – zip
/h/ – hello
/ʃ/ – ship
/ʒ/ – This sound does not occur at the beginning of words in English. It
usually occurs in the middle of English words.
/ð/ – these
/θ/ – thin
Final Sounds
/f/ – half
/v/ – have
/s/ – pass
/z/ – has
/h/ – This sound does not occur at the end of words in English. It occurs
in the beginning or middle of English words.
/ʃ/ – wash
/ʒ/ – rouge – This sound at the final position is rare. It usually occurs in
the middle of English words.
/ð/ – bathe
/θ/ – bath
3) Affricate
We combine stops and fricatives to form affricates. Examples include:
/tʃ/ and /dʒ/. Affricates start with a stop and then transitions into a
fricative.
Initial Sounds
/tʃ/ – chip
/dʒ/ – just
Final Sounds
/tʃ/ – catch
/dʒ/ – badge
Vowels In many American dialects, words like caught and cot, or dawn and Don,
While the consonant sounds are mostly articulated via closure or are pronounced differently, with an [5] and [a], respectively. In other
obstruction in the vocal tract, vowel sounds are produced with a American dialects, these words are pronounced the same. If we
pronounce these pairs the same, we probably use the unrounded vowel
relatively free flow of air. To describe vowel sounds, we consider [a] in these words. For most speakers of English, however, the vowel [3]
the way in which the tongue influences the ‘shape’ through which appears in words such as hall, ball, and tall.
the airflow must pass. Be- cause these sounds are not so easily
defined In terms of place and manner of articulation, we use labels Tongue Advancement
which serve to indicate how each vowel sounds in relation to the Beside being held high or mid or low, the tongue can also be pushed for-
others. ward or pulled back within the oral cavity. For example, in the high front
vowel [i] as in beat, the body of the tongue is raised and pushed forward
so it is just under the hard palate. The high back vowel [u] of boot, on the
other hand, is made by raising the body of the tongue in the back of the
mouth, toward the velum. The tongue is advanced or pushed forward for
all the front vowels, [I, I, e, å, æ] as in seek, sick, sake, sec, sack, and
retracted or pulled back for the back vowels, [u, U, o, 3, a], as in ooze,
look, road, paw, dot. The central vowels, [A] as in luck or [E] as the first
vowel in the word another, require neither fronting nor retraction of the
tongue.
Lip Rounding
Vowel quality also depends on lip position. When we say the [u] in two,
our lips are rounded. For the [i] in tea, they are unrounded. English has
four rounded vowels: [u, U, o, ], as in loop, foot, soap, caught. All other
vowels in English are unrounded. In the vowel chart, the rounded vowels
are enclosed in a dotted line forming a rectangle.
Vocal fold vibration is the sound source for vowels. The vocal tract above Tenseness
the glottis acts as a resonator affecting the sound made by the vocal folds. Vowels that are called tenses have more extreme positions of the tongue
The shape of this resonator determines the quality of the vowel – [i] vs or the lips than vowels that are lax. The production of tense vowels
[u] vs [a], for example. involves
There are several ways in which speakers can change the shape of the Bigger changes from a mid central position in the mouth. On the vowel
vocal tract and thus change vowel quality. They do this by chart we can clearly see that the distance between the tense vowels [i]
and [u] is bigger than the distance between the lax vowels [I] and [U].
1. Raising or lowering the body of the tongue For ex- ample, tense vowels are made with a more extreme tongue
2. Advancing or retracting the body of the tongue gesture to reach the outer peripherals of the vowel space. What this means
3. Rounding or not rounding the lips is that the tongue position for the tense high front vowel [i] is higher and
4. Making these movements with a tense or a lax gesture fronter than for the lax high front vowel [I]. Lax vowels are not
peripheral, on the outer edge of the possible vowel space. Compare tense
Therefore, when describing a vowel, it is necessary to provide [i] in meet with lax [I] in mitt, or tense [u] in boot with lax [U] in put. In
information about these four aspects of the articulation of the vowel. the latter case we will find that the tense round vowel [u] is also produced
Refer to the chart in figure 4 as each aspect is discussed. with more and tighter lip round- ing than the lax counterpart [U].
Front - the tongue is lowered We can consider some sample descriptions of English vowels:
Central - the tongue is centered
Back - the tongue is lifted [i], as in beat, is high, front, unrounded, and tense vowel [5], as in caught,
is mid, back, rounded, and lax vowel
Low -
Mid - the tongue is either in forward or backward position [a], as in cot, is low, back, unrounded, and lax vowel
Back -
[A], as in cut, is mid, central, unrounded, and lax vowel (Note that ALLOPHONES
“central” and “mid” refer to the same position in the vocal tract but on An allophone is a term used to describe the group of phones
different dimensions) represented by a single phoneme in a particular language.
[e], as in cake, is mid, front, unrounded, and tense vowel
For example, In English, the phones [tʰ] (aspirated), [t]
Diphthongs (unaspirated), and [tʃ] (affricated) are all represented by the single
a sound formed by the combination of two vowels in a single syllable, in phoneme /t/ - this makes them all allophones for the phoneme /t/.
which the sound begins as one vowel and moves toward another (as in
coin, loud, and side ). Look at the following words: Trick, Tack, Stack. Each word
contains one of the previous allophones for phoneme /t/. Can you
TENSE VOWELS are enunciated with more prominent strong exertion,
figure out which word has which allophone?
marginally higher tongue positions, and longer terms than careless
vowels. … specialists use terms, for example, tense and careless to depict
the level of pressure in the tongue muscles, especially those muscles Trick = [tʃ] (africated - T makes a ‘CH’ sound)
liable for the grouping up of the tongue length-ways. Tack = [tʰ] (aspirated - an exhalation of air after the T)
Stack = [t] (unaspirated)
LAX VOWELS are likewise called short vowels: as a rule, they are more
limited than tense (long) vowels. … Another quality of remiss vowels is This makes the examples above the ALLOPHONES OF THE
that they are constantly checked: that is, they don’t happen alone at the
SAME PHONEME which is non-contrastive sounds and are
closures of words, however consistently need an after a consonant.
complimentary distribution. Means that even if we change how
SEGMENTALS are concerned with the individual speech sounds the /t/ sound the meaning will still be the same, it compliments.
(consonants and vowels) and their distinctive features, while
SUPRASEGMENTALS focus on the broader aspects of speech While on the other hand, we have ALLOPHONES OF THE
patterns, including rhythm, melody, and intonation, which convey DIFFERENT PHONEME which is contrastive sounds and are
expressive, pragmatic, and interpretive information in spoken language. overlapping distribution that is why they are called minimal
pairs.
PHONOLOGY - sound organization Example.
Phonology, on the other hand, is essentially the
In Hindi language
description of the systems and patterns of speech sounds in a
language. It is, in effect, based on the theory of what every speaker
[kʰal] - wicked person (/k/ is aspirated)
of a language unconsciously knows about the sound patterns of
[kal] - tomorrow/yesterday
that language. This study regards the speech sounds as having
functions to differentiate meanings.
[mæd] - Made
[mæt] - Mate
PHONE vs PHONEME
A phoneme is the smallest unit of meaningful sound in a
[sꞮp] - Sip
specific language. In the English language, there are 44 different
[lꞮp] - Lip
phonemes (20 distinct vowel sounds and 24 consonant sounds).
If we interchange the sound the meaning will be different which is
Example.
why it’s not a complimentary distribution because it does not
The phonemic transcription of the word book is /bʊk/. As you can
compliment each other.
see, the word book has three distinct phonemes (b, ʊ, and k).
Minimal Pairs and Sets
Whereas phones are not necessarily critical to understanding the
When two words such as pat and bat are identical in form
meaning of words, phonemes are! If one phoneme is exchanged
except for a contrast in one phoneme occurring in the same
for another, it could change the word's meaning completely. Take
position. The two words are described as a minimal pair. More
the words sheep and sheet, for example. The ending phonemes
accurately, they would be classified as a minimal pair in the
are different (/p/ and /t/), resulting in two completely different
phonology of English since Arabic, for example, does not have this
words!
contrast between the two sounds. Other examples of English
minimal pairs are fan – van, bet – bat, site – side. Such pairs have
Phone - sound
been used frequently in tests of English as a second language to
Phoneme - meaningful sounds
determine non-native speakers’ ability to understand the contrast
in meaning resulting from the minimal sound contrast.
You can tell whether transcription is phonetic or phonemic by
looking at the brackets it's encased in. Phonetic transcriptions
When a group of words are differentiated each one from
go within square brackets ( [ ] ) and phonemic transcriptions
the others by changing one phoneme (always in the same
go within slashes ( / / ).
position), then we have a minimal set. Thus, a minimal set based
on the vowel phonemes of English would include feat, fit, fat, fate,
fought, foot, and one based on consonants could have big, pig, rig,
fig, dig, wig.
One insight provided by this type of exercise with Derivational Morphemes - carries grammatical meaning in it
phonemes is that we can see that there are indeed definite content
patterns to the types of sound combinations permitted in a
language. In English, the minimal set we have just listed does not NATION+[AL] - [al] as adjective forming derivational suffix
include forms such as lig or vig. As far as we know, these are not NATIONAL+[IZE] - [ize] as verb forming derivational suffix
English words, but they can be viewed as possible English words. NA+[TION] - [tion] as noun forming derivation suffix
That is, our phonological knowledge of the pattern of sounds in [INTER]+NATIONAL - [inter] as a noun forming derivation prefix
English words would allow us to treat these forms as acceptable NATION+[S] - [s] as pluralizer
if, at some future time, they came into use. They represent
‘accidental’ gaps in the vocabulary of English. It is, however, no In French: Beau+x =Beaux - [x] as pluralizer
accident that forms such as [fsig] or [hig] do not exist or are unlikely
ever to exist, since they break what must be phonological rules Inflectional Morphemes - Function (All suffixes)
about the sequence or position of English phonemes.
Tall[er] - Tense marking morpheme
Phonological Rules Cook[ing]
Cook[ed] - Possessive forming morpheme
Phonemic Form allows us to translate or rather transcribe the
phonemes into actual sound speech “Frech language is a Gender language”
Phonetic Form represents abstractness, these are the Ell est sportive Female
meaningful sound distinctions in a language using a simplified set Il est sportif Male
of symbols. Generally, word with “e” in its end are usually for female
Stem word
Allomorphs
A morpheme can have one or more allomorphs, and these
allomorphs might be represented by the same or different
spellings.
Given a morpheme,
Can it stand alone as a word?
Yes - it’s a free morpheme (e.g., bubble, orange)
No - it’s a bound morpheme (e.g., -er in beater, -s in oranges)