Halliday's Categories in Grammar
Halliday's Categories in Grammar
Halliday's Categories in Grammar
HISTORY
openlibrary.org/.../Categories_of_the_theory_of_gr...
Introduction
Systemics grew out of the work of J. R. Firth, but it was through his student, M. A. K.
Halliday, that it was extensively developed and refined to its present form. Halliday's
contributions in this respect have generally been hailed as "the most important
development of the ideas within the so-called 'London School' of linguistics" (Butler
1985: 1). Systemics began with Firth's attempt to develop a model to relate language
function and context. To achieve this, he proposed a framework based on the concept
of the system, defined as an "enumerated set of choices in a specific context" (Kress
1976: xiii). This gave rise to two sets of contexts for any item: the context of the
possible choices in the system under consideration, and the surrounding context in
which the system itself occurred. In a clausal structure such as S (subject) P
(predicator), for example, there is a system which operates at P. This system opens a
set of choices which are possible alternatives in that position. Clearly, the structural
configuration restricts the choices that can be made since only intransitive verbs are
permitted in the P position. If, however, the structural configuration were to be
changed to S P C (complement), the system comprising only intransitive verbal
elements at P will no longer apply. A changed context, therefore, gives rise to a
system applicable in that context, and the description of such a system depends on the
statement of the context in which it occurs (Kress 1976: xiii). Formulated this way,
the concept of the system gradually took on an increasingly important status in
Halliday's successive revisions of the Firthian model.
The unit category, as introduced in an earlier paper by Halliday (1956: 36), is "that
category to which corresponds a segment of the linguistic material about which
statements are to be made." The units initially suggested by Halliday were those of the
sentence, clause, group, word, and morpheme (Halliday 1961: 58). The sentence, as a
unit of orthography, later came to be replaced by the clause complex, thereby
separating the units of grammar from those of orthography. Units are arranged
hierarchically on a rank scale such that a unit of a particular rank consists of one or
more units next below. The rank-based theory underscores an important principle
firmly held by Halliday -- total accountability -- which requires every item to be
2
accounted for at all ranks. Total accountability, therefore, resists an analysis of a reply
such as (2) as a sentence consisting of a single morpheme. Rather, it would be treated
as a sentence consisting of a single clause which itself comprises a single group
containing a single word and which, in turn, consists of a single morpheme (Butler
1985: 17):
As noted, the relation between structure and rank can be seen in how each element of
clause structure permits only a certain group of items to operate in that position. The P
element, for example, is that element which operates only in the verb group. The scale
of delicacy, in comparison, refers to "the degree of detail in which a structure is
specified" (Butler 1985: 19), and where this is concerned, Halliday is careful to
separate primary from secondary structures. The former contains the minimum
number of elements necessary to account for the operation of a given unit. The
elements of S, P, C, and A for the clause structure and their various combinations
exemplify primary structures. Secondary structures are more detailed (or delicate) in
differentiating units of the same rank. The S element, for example, can be treated at
secondary delicacy as being either a full or empty subject.
The category of class is "that grouping of members of a given unit which is defined
by operation in the structure of the unit next above" (Halliday 1961: 64). How this
relates to Halliday's model can be seen in the P element of the clause structure -- for
the group unit, there is a verbal class which has the potential for occurring in the P
position. Class takes into account the "paradigmatic possibilities associated with
particular elements of structure" (Butler 1985: 24). The paradigmatic possibilities
themselves illustrate the scale of exponence. Using clause structure again, the items
which represent (or expound) the S element are members of the nominal class of the
group unit, such as The old man and Everyone in the room in (5) and (6) below.
Exponence relations therefore "relate terms in systems, units, classes and structures
and allow the analysis to achieve maximum generalization" (Butler 1985: 28).
5. The old man has spent fifty pounds during the last fortnight.
6. Everyone in the room would have made their excuses immediately.
The last grammatical category, system, is set up to account for "the occurrence of one
rather than another from among a number of like events" (Halliday 1961: 67).
Specifically, a system is a set of terms with these characteristics (Halliday 1961: 54):
3
The number of terms is finite: they can be listed as ABCD, and all other items
E ... are outside the system;
Each term is exclusive of all the others: a given term A cannot be identical
with B or C or D;
If a new term is added to the system this changes the meaning of all the other
terms.
Scale and category linguistics received several criticisms shortly after it was
introduced. Linguists such as Matthews noted a dilemma for the model where rank
and total accountability were concerned (in Butler 1985: 29-33). Since "and" can be
used to link two clauses together, and since, according to Halliday's rank scale,
sentences consist of clauses, then by the principle of total accountability, "and" should
also be regarded as a clause. This, however, is complicated by the fact that "and" not
only joins clauses but other stretches of language as well, from single nouns to parts
of clauses. Matthews' criticism forced Halliday to concede that words such as "and",
"or", "but", etc, are able to float between ranks and so lie "outside the scope of the
total accountability requirement" (Butler 1985: 30).
Other criticisms against Halliday's model involved the categories of class and
structure as well as the taxonomic character of the model. The last objection is a
particularly severe one, suggesting that the model merely provides taxonomic labels
in analysing structures "rather than predicting in an explicit manner exactly what
strings of elements constitute the possible grammatical sentences of a language"
(Butler 1985: 38). It further indicates the urgent need for the model to be revised.
Towards systemics
Halliday's early scale and category linguistics gradually led to a more focused
approach in his theory of language description. One paper which could be taken to
mark the emergence of systemic linguistics is the article by Halliday (1966), in which
the concept of the system is taken much further. In contrast with Halliday (1961),
where all the grammatical categories are assigned equal theoretical status, Halliday
now sees system as a category which has definitive importance in his theory. He
argues that systems represent paradigmatic relations in the same way as structures
represent syntagmatic relations. It captures the network of options which are available
for a particular environment. An illustration of the network of choices for the mood
system in English is shown in the figure below (from Berry 1975: 181):
In the figure, the term finite from the system of finiteness is an entry condition to the
mood system. The mood system is therefore dependent on the term finite even though
they have little in common by way of meaning. The square brackets in Figure 1
indicate that one or the other alternative represented within them must be chosen. For
the entry condition finite, for example, either indicative or imperative must be
selected, and if the latter, a further choice must be made between exclusive imperative
or inclusive imperative.
Most crucial in the development of the theory is Halliday's claim that paradigmatic
relations are primary, constituting the basic "underlying form of representation"
(Halliday 1966: 93-94). Syntagmatic relations, in contrast, are seen to be fully
predictable since structural descriptions can be derived from the paradigmatic choices
already made. The link, therefore, is between the deep systemic choices that are made
and the surface manifestations realised in sequential structures. One implication of
this approach to language description is the careful attitude towards structure since
structure itself and its elements are representative of an underlying systemic choice.
As Kress (1976: xix) notes, "the correspondence between all items of structure and
their features in systems must be established and is demanded by the theory."
Systemics
The emphasis on language as a system explains the name of Halliday's revised theory
(systemics). Halliday (1969: 3), however, clarifies that "the name 'systemic' is not the
same thing as 'systematic'; the term is used because the fundamental concept in the
grammar is that of the 'system'. A system is a set of options with an entry condition:
that is to say, a set of things of which one must be chosen, together with a statement
of the conditions under which the choice is available."
Useful links
1997). It's very, very, very long (the text file itself is 209kb in size), but it
provides a wonderful overview of the framework.
Information on systemic functional linguistics.
A page packed with information and resources. Check it out!
Literary stylistics homepage.
This is the course homepage for literary stylistics, taught by Dr Ismail Talib.
You'll be able to find plenty of stuff here to supplement your readings.
References