Manuscript Blinded V3
Manuscript Blinded V3
Manuscript Blinded V3
Abstract
Currently, the validity of the proposals of embodied cognition and its relationship with the language
of older adults is unknown. To answer this, we carried out a neurocognitive experiment using EEG,
using linguistic variables. 27 older adults (M = 70.34 years, SD = 4.82, 15 women and 12 men)
The results indicate that the linguistic, factual and counterfactual contexts, as well as the corporeal
parameter “mental effort” modulate the understanding of language and participate with greater or
lesser preponderance in different time windows. Furthermore, the counterfactual language seems
to induce emotions that facilitate the processing of high effort while the interaction under mental
effort - counterfactuality represents a negativity that is associated with the N400 component. The
Resumen
mediante EEG, utilizando variables lingüísticas. 27 adultos mayores (M= 70.34 años, DE= 4.82,
prueba de activación.
Los resultados indican que los contextos lingüísticos, factual y contrafactual, así como el parámetro
corpóreo “esfuerzo mental” modulan la comprensión del lenguaje y participan con mayor o menor
inducir emociones que facilitan el procesamiento del alto esfuerzo mientras que la interacción bajo
esfuerzo mental – contrafactualidad representa una negatividad que se asocia con el componente
1. Introduction
Embodied cognition is comprised within the corpus of theories of the cognitive sciences which
propose that many aspects of our cognition are heavily influenced by our body and its interactions
with the environment. This perspective challenges the traditional cognitive science view, which
considers the mind to function mainly on the basis of symbolic manipulation and has yielded the
2020). Instead, accounts of embodied cognition (alongside embedded, extended, and enactive ones)
argue that it is grounded in multiple ways throughout the body's interactions with the world. While
some prefer to consider it still, due to its recent and brief development, less a well-defined theory
than a research agenda (Shapiro, 2011), due to the fact that even to this day there is no consensus
throughout which their central assumptions are accounted for; hence, amounting to a paradigmatic
these investigations do bear the commonality of providing the field with new descriptive
assumes the world to be constrained by (Milkowski, 2013; Cowart, 2004; Wilson et al., 2002). The
articulation of said common ground throughout diverse schools and research programs such as
linguistics, psychology, artificial intelligence, robotis, and neurobiology (Cappuccio, 2019; Cox,
2016; Kording et al., 2018; Ionescu & Vasc, 2014) seem to augur a fruitful convergence of visions.
3
Indeed, it has been widely adopted as a model in linguistic research; as it suggests a linkage
between body and environment to language and our understanding of it -as opposed to viewing
language as an abstract entity that is detached from physical reality- a number of researchers have
physical effort and counterfactual sentences (Urrutia & de Vega, 2012), or the ones between
balance and posture, and their spatial and temporal representations (Miles et al., 2010).
Furthermore, many metaphors and idiomatic expressions have also been shown to reflect physical
and sensory experiences, like "grasping a concept" or "shouldering a burden" (Barsalou et al., 2008;
de Vega, 2021; Fischer & Zwaan, 2008; García & Ibáñez, 2018; Glenberg & Kaschack, 2002).
This approach suggests that language comprehension isn't just an abstract process that occurs in
the brain, but also involves sensorimotor simulations. For example, while reading or hearing the
sentence "John kicked the ball", these words aren’t processed only at a linguistic level, but brain
areas related to physical movement are triggered and engaged, too (as if visualizing or imagining
the action of to kick a ball). Thus, the embodiment perspective posits that meaning is intimately
embedded in the brain's perceptual, motor, and emotional systems (Barsalou et al., 2008; de Vega,
2021; Fischer & Zwaan, 2008; García & Ibáñez, 2018; Glenberg & Kaschack, 2002). From this
perspective, the body and the specific cerebral cortices related to sensation, motor skills, and
emotions have varying degrees of participation in a dimension that ranges from the total
embodiment of semantic processing and representation to full disembodiment. The idea that
sensory and motor experiences form the basis of conceptual knowledge has a long history in
philosophy, psychology, and neuroscience (Allport & Funnell, 1981; Freud, 1891; Locke, 1847;
Wernicke, 1874). In recent years, this proposal has gained new steam under the rubric of
Although embodied theories allow a novel approach to the relationship between cognition, body
and context by proposing direct interactions between sensory-motor systems and the representation
(simulation) of meaning, the empirical evidence from behavioral and neurocognitive studies has
tended to concentrate on young adults, leaving aside stages such as older adulthood. In order for
these theories to be considered theories of cognition, and of language in particular, they require
exploration and validation throughout the life cycle (Salas-Herrera, 2015). In this sense, older
population represents a favorable sample to carry out a contrast of possible embodied proposals,
since the declines in sensory-motor systems could directly affect the processing of sentences that
contain challenging semantic content from the point of view of the neural parameters involved
Psycholinguistics, oriented towards the study of aging, has been strengthened by research that has
attempted to elucidate the effects of cognitive aging on language comprehension and production
(Véliz, Riffo & Arancibia, 2010). Models emphasizing the functional and computational
architecture of the linguistic system have been successfully tested, particularly at the level of
language comprehension (De Vega, 2008; Fisher & Zwaan, 2008; Glenberg & Gallese, 2012;
Glenberg & Kaschak, 2002). Notions such as working memory, attentional resources, and
metacognition have gained importance (Cheung & Kemper, 1992; Just & Carpenter, 1992; Kemper,
1987; Kemper et al., 2001). It has been determined that there are dimensions of language that are
able to resist the advance of age, such as semantic processing, linguistic comprehension, and
vocabulary (Burke & Shafto, 2008; Glisky, 2007). Psycholinguistics has expanded towards
experimental behavioral and neurocognitive paradigms (Barsalou, 1999; Gallese & Lakoff, 2005;
Pulvermüller, 1999, 2013; Zwaan, 2004). The discipline is currently being significantly influenced
by theories of embodiment, which propose a different way of looking at language and its
5
relationship with the body (De Vega, 2008; Fisher & Zwaan, 2008; Glenberg & Gallese, 2012;
Studies on cognitive aging and language processes have observed declines in processing speed,
divided attention, executive functions, and phonological retrieval in language production in older
adults (Salthouse, 2000; Greenwood, 2000; Danckert & Craik, 2013; Burke & Shafto, 2008).
However, semantic memory, sustained attention, and recall are generally preserved (Glisky, 2007).
Behavioral research has shown that linguistic comprehension, particularly semantic processing and
The prevailing view in psycholinguistics suggests that cognitive decline in aging, specifically in
the relationship between language and verbal working memory, is characterized by a decrease in
verbal working memory capacity, which impairs the quality of language processing (Cheung &
Kemper, 1992; Just & Carpenter, 1992; Kemper, 1987; Kemper et al., 2001). An alternative view
challenges this perspective and proposes an integration of working memory models with
consideration of cognitive architecture and linguistic experience (Caplan & Waters, 1999;
MacDonald Christiansen, 2002; Ericsson & Kintsch, 1995). This alternative approach suggests that
individual differences in language comprehension in older adults arise from variations in working
memory efficiency influenced by linguistic experience, rather than total memory capacity (Caplan
Brain imaging studies have identified brain regions such as the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex,
parietal lobe, premotor cortex, and inferior frontal gyrus as part of the verbal working memory
circuitry (Goldman-Rakic, 1987; D'Esposito & Postle, 2015; Postle, 2015; Fegen, Buchsbaum &
D'Esposito, 2015). The processing speed in cognitive aging is related to various brain structures,
functions, and regions associated with different cognitive domains (Salthouse, 2017; Haász et al.,
2013; Lockhart et al., 2014; Ferreira et al., 2014; Eckert et al., 2010; Papp et al., 2014).
6
The activation of motor representations would be directly related to action verb processing
(Beauprez & Bidet-Ildei, 2019). Since the motor plans caused by the verbs must be integrated with
the properties of the associated objects, we suppose that aging could affect the estimation of the
direct object, and thus, sentence reading when this implies variable levels of effort, since the
abilities to perform physical effort in older people are impaired compared to young people
(Portegijs et al., 2019). In fact, several studies have shown that older people have difficulties using
Simulations utilize corporeal or neural parameters, such as direction for reaching and
force/physical effort for grasping an object, imposing a hierarchical structure on the brain (Gallese
& Lakoff, 2005). The term "simulation" refers to mentally and unconsciously simulating the action
or perceptual content of a sentence using the same neural system activated during actual perception
or action (Glenberg & Robertson, 2000; Pulvermüller, 2013). Just et al. (2004) found that low-
effort imagery activates the left temporal cortex and primary visual cortex, while high-effort
imagery requires generating a multidimensional internal structure of the event. Urrutia, Gennari &
De Vega (2012) identified brain areas sensitive to effort in action sentences, overlapping with
regions activated during physical effort tasks. These areas include the left inferior parietal lobe,
supramarginal and postcentral areas, associated with action planning. Prefrontal areas were
exclusively activated for counterfactual sentences. Action language involves events performed by
animate beings and entails physical effort (Trevisan et al., 2017). Research predominantly focuses
on physical content involving hand and arm movements (Moody & Gennari, 2010), but studies
have also examined mouth and leg movements (Tettamanti et al., 2006; De Vega, Urrutia & Riffo,
The purpose of this article being to explore the degree to which embodiment is present during
processing of abstract language at varying levels of mental effort and in an indicative (real) and
counterfactual (unreal) linguistic context in older adults, and having described embodiment, as well
as mental effort, it’ll be necessary to further consider counterfactuality and representation theories
Counterfactual thinking involves contemplating alternative outcomes that did not occur but can be
imagined (Epstude & Roese, 2008). It is characterized by thoughts like "what if?" or "if only there
had been..." that explore different possibilities (Westby, 2016). Counterfactual reasoning entails
imagining alternatives to an event and shifting between real and counterfactual situations (Epstude
& Roese, 2008). The term "counterfactual reasoning" is used when individuals mentally bracket
what is known to be true and speculate about false possibilities (Urrutia & de Vega, 2012a).
Counterfactual language may interfere with action planning (De Vega & Urrutia, 2011) and
involves pre-motor regions that overlap with action execution networks (Boulenger, Hauk &
Pulvermüller, 2008). Counterfactuals elicit stronger activations and compete with each other,
requiring prefrontal inhibition or control processes (Beck, Riggs, & Gorniak, 2009; McNamara,
Durso, Brown & Lynch, 2003). Counterfactual reasoning involves prefrontal control processes that
decouple alternatives from actual states and engage competition between alternative interpretations
(Krueger, Barbey, & Grafman, 2009). Counterfactual comprehension activates medial prefrontal
regions and areas involved in resolving competition between alternative actions (Chen et al., 2009;
Goel, 2007; Simmonds, Pekar & Mostofsky, 2008; Speer et al., 2009; Yarkoni, Speer & Zacks,
2008). Urrutia et al. (2012) investigated whether the involvement of the body in counterfactual
sentences affected processing and found overlapping activations in premotor and parietal regions.
Counterfactual language has implications for cognitive processes like planning, causal reasoning,
8
problem-solving, and decision-making (McNamara, Durso, Brown & Lynch, 2003). Older adults
with Parkinson's disease produce fewer counterfactual thoughts, suggesting a relationship between
counterfactual thinking and healthy cognitive aging (McNamara et al., 2003). Age-related
differences in memory processes during counterfactual reasoning have been observed, with older
adults showing vulnerability to false recall (Gerlach, Dornblaser & Schacter, 2014). Older adults
also exhibit lower inhibitory capacity for linguistic materials, impacting memory systems
(Radvansky, Zacks & Hasher, 2005). Counterfactual language imposes higher processing costs on
older adults, particularly working memory (Radvansky, Zacks & Hasher, 2005).
Multiple representation theories of abstract concepts have emerged as valuable and innovative
additions to current research (Gentner & Boroditsky, 2001; Barsalou, 2008). These theories suggest
that abstract concepts, although grounded in perception, action, and emotion, heavily rely on the
recreation of linguistic experiences (Paivio, 2007; Dove, 2009). According to proponents of these
representation of concrete and abstract concepts, but their distribution varies depending on the type
of concept (Binder & Desai, 2011). While perception and action information are more crucial for
concrete concepts, emotion and linguistically conveyed information play a greater role in abstract
multiple representation approaches (Mahon & Caramazza, 2008; Dove, 2016). Some consider
language as a mere shortcut to access word meaning, while others assign it a more significant role
in enhancing thought processes or as a complex bodily and social experience (Pulvermüller, 2005;
Dove, 2016). This review focuses on four influential or potentially influential multiple
representation theories, including the Language and Sit Simulation (LSS) approach (Zwaan &
9
Madden, 2005), Dove's representational pluralistic approach (Dove, 2009), and the Words as Social
The PCHS approach proposes that concepts are expressed through representations derived from
both perceptual/motor and linguistic experiences (Borghi & Cimatti, 2010). It emphasizes the
social dimension of word acquisition and considers both concrete and abstract words as social tools
for interaction (Borghi & Binkofski, 2014). The PCHS approach introduces four central principles,
which will be examined in light of recent evidence. These principles relate to the acquisition
modalities (Pulvermüller et al., 2014), conceptual representation in the brain (Barsalou, 2016),
embodied counterparts of different modalities (Glenberg, 2015), and the influence of social factors
on abstract word learning (Borghi et al., 2018). Research using fMRI (Binder et al., 2009), TMS
(Pulvermüller et al., 2005), and behavioral studies (Meteyard et al., 2012) has provided support for
the predictions made by the PCHS approach, highlighting the involvement of language processing
Overall, these findings contribute to our understanding of the multimodal nature of abstract concept
representation and the role of language and social factors in this process (Kiefer & Pulvermüller,
2.1. Participants
The sample consisted of 27 older adults from the city of Concepción, Chile (M = 70.34 years, SD
= 4.82, 15 women and 12 men). All of them were selected for showing a right hand preference,
according to the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Albayay et al., 2019). Only right-handed people
with a laterality coefficient greater than 60 were accepted. One participant was eliminated due to
all participants passed the psychological tests applied. None of them evidenced alterations in the
central nervous system, abuse or problematic use of alcohol and other substances, learning or
memory problems, mental health problems or serious medical illness; and none were taking
medications known to have central effects. All participants had normal or corrected vision and were
Before the experiment, each participant was evaluated with cognitive tests, screening and
depression, dementia, etc.). The Minimental State Examination (Folstein et al., 1983), with a
sensitivity of 93.6% and specificity of 46.1% (Quiroga et al., 2004), and the Yesavage Depression
Scale (Yesavage et al., 1982) were applied, with an internal consistency of 0.78 and construct
reliability of 0.87 (Angulo & Arias, 2011). In addition, a brief questionnaire was applied that
assessed functionality (especially in the sensorimotor plane). Participation was free and consented,
which was registered through a protocol validated by an Ethics Committee. The average duration
One hundred and sixty sentences corresponding to four types of semantics were elaborated: 40 high
mental effort factual sentences (HFS), 40 low mental effort factual sentences (LFS), 40 high mental
effort counterfactual sentences (HCS) and 40 low mental effort counterfactual sentences (LCS).
At the beginning of the task, the participants observed dashes and spaces on the computer screen.
The dashes corresponded to the letters of each word and the spaces to the segmentation places of
the sentence. Pressing a key unmasked the next segment and masked the one they just read. After
reading each sentence, participants were presented with a test word, which they had to decide
whether or not it was in the sentence they had just read by pressing the letter P (with a red sticker
on it) if their answer was affirmative or the letter Q (with a green sticker on it) if their answer was
negative.
In each experimental sentence, the intention was to record the event-related potentials (ERPs) of a
word (gerund/participle) and two syntagmas (direct object and adverbial). Technical problems
arose when measuring the direct object, so the results will refer to the gerund/participle as
POSITION 1 and the adverbial as POSITION 2. POSITION 1 reflects the effects associated with
the processing of the main verb of the sentence; therefore, it indicates the type of action performed
and implies, early on, the level of effort associated with it, as can be seen in the examples in Table
1. POSITION 2 captures the effects related to the closing of the sentence and the effects that are
For example, in the sentence: “Now you are counting the trucks on the road”, a synchronous
signal was sent to the EEG corresponding to the beginning of the word “counting” (gerund) and
another signal corresponding to the syntagma “on the road” (adverbial). The purpose of recording
12
both positions was to check the time course of the effort level processing in contexts of different
abstraction levels throughout the sentence. To analyze the electrophysiological signal at position 1,
200 ms prior to the analyzed position were designated as the baseline, and the total recording
window was extended to 1200 ms after the presentation of the stimulus. For position 2, 200 ms
were also designated as the baseline but the time window was extended to 1000 ms.
The experiment was self-administered using the E-Prime 2.0 Professional software.
2.3. Design
The experimental conditions were analyzed in a factorial design of two types of context (factual
vs. counterfactual) and two types of effort (low mental effort vs. high mental effort).
A high-density EEG set-up was used, with 58 thin Ag/AgCl electrodes mounted on an elastic cap
that fitted the size of the head. Five electrodes were of the cup-type (10 mm in diameter) and were
placed in the ocular area: one under the left eye and another next to the ridge of the right eye to
measure eye movements. Two electrodes were placed in the mastoid area (under each ear), being
used as an average reference for the rest of the electrodes (monopolar recording) and one on the
Electrode locations on the scalp were: FP1, FP2, F3, F4, C3, C4, P3, P4, O1, O2, F7, F8, T7, T8,
P7, P8, FZ, CZ, PZ, F1, F2, P1, P2, AF3, AF4, P5, P6, FC5, FC6, C5, C6, TP7, TP8, PO5, PO6,
FPZ, FCZ, CPZ, POZ, OZ, PO3, PO4, CP1, CP2, CP3, CP4, C1, C2, F5, F6, FC3, FC4, FC1, FC2,
CP5, CP6, PO7 and PO8. These locations followed the standard 10/20 electrode location system.
The inter-electrode impedance was kept below 5 KΩ. The biosignals were processed by a Neuronic
amplifier in a band between 0.02-100 Hz. In the ERP analysis, all those windows that contained
eye movements (EOG greater than 80ų) and those artifacts that contaminated the measurements
13
such as noise of body movement or facial muscles, among others, were eliminated from the
individual averages. This procedure was done thanks to the filter that the Neuronic analysis
program performs automatically, but then each window was manually corrected to verify that the
elimination criteria were always the same and thus perform a stricter cleaning of each window. The
stimuli for the EEG recording system were presented through the E-Prime 2.0 Professional
software. The EEG signals were prepared for ERP analysis to reveal whether endogenous
components, such as the N400, distinguished between factual and counterfactual meaning, and
between high and low levels of mental effort. The choice of time windows for the ERP/EEG
analysis was based on theoretical and statistical criteria. Specifically, the nonparametric statistical
estimate pairwise t-test comparisons between the levels of a variable at each data point (Khosla et
al., 2020). The ERP segments that reached the statistical criteria in the permutation tests were
selected as time windows for their analysis, according to the topographic distribution of the ERP
3. Results
200-600 ms window. There were significant effects on the set of 58 electrodes, with a significant
interaction Context x Effort F(1, 25) = 4.476, MSe = 4384465334.7, p < 0.05; η2 = 0.152; η2p/ƒ =
0.423; 1-β = 1.0 (Greenhouse-Geisser correction). In this time window, it is expected to find the
N400 component, with a negativity between 200 and 600 ms, greater in central-parietal sites, with
a bias towards the right hemisphere for written language (Issa et al., 2018). Thirty-four channels
were chosen as region of interest (ROI): C3, C4, P3, P4, P7, P8, P1, P2, P5, P6, C5, C6, PO5, PO6,
PO3, PO4, CP1, CP2, CP3, CP4, C1, C2, CP5, CP6, PO7, PO8, Fz, Cz, Pz, FPz, FCz, CPz, POz
and Oz, where significant interaction was found between the factors Context x Effort F (1, 25) =
14
4.207, MSe = 330401238.1, p < 0.05; η2 = 0.144; η2p/ƒ = 0.41; 1-β = 1.0 (Greenhouse-Geisser
The medial channels were analyzed separately and a significant interaction was found between the
factors Context x Effort F (1, 25) = 4.238, MSe = 63211215.87, p < 0.05; η2 = 0.145; η2p/ƒ = 0.41;
1-β = 1.0 (Greenhouse-Geisser correction). In this time window, N400 was expressed mainly in the
posterior parietal and central areas and to a lesser extent in the parieto-occipital areas.
Figure 3 shows the voltage differences (amplitude) between the different experimental conditions.
It can be seen that the positivity of the high mental effort counterfactual predominates, followed
by the low mental effort factual. The topographic distribution of this temporal window is mainly
observed in the right central parietal areas, according to the topography of a classic N400. However,
the brain wave has a morphology with several voltage changes within the temporal range, which is
350-500 ms window. There were no significant effects on the set of 58 electrodes, finding only a
marginal trend (p = 0.08 > 0.05) in the Context x Effort interaction. As in the previous case, it was
expected to find a negativity around 400 ms (N400). Even though there are few and inconsistent
data on the effect of age on the N400 component, the literature suggests that healthy older adults
show a decrease in the effects of N400 compared to young adults (Joyal et al., 2020). Studies
indicate that the reduction in the N400 effect is due to smaller amplitudes of N400 for semantically
unrelated or incongruous stimuli (Joyal et al., 2020). There is also evidence that indicates that in
the older population the N400 component becomes more frontal when context information is
restricted to interpret or reinterpret a sentence (Wlotko & Federmeier, 2012), especially for those
older adults with greater verbal fluency (Wlotko et al., 2010), which is relevant given that at the
15
sentence level to which this study was circumscribed, there was no contextual information before
Based on these empirical arguments, the ROI was extended to frontal areas, choosing the following
32 channels: F3, F4, C3, C4, P3, P4, T7, T8, F1, F2, P1, P2, C5, C6, TP7, TP8, PO5, PO6, PO3,
PO4, CP1, CP2, CP3, CP4, C1, C2, FC3, FC4, FC1, FC2, CP6, CP5, Cz, FCz and CPz. In these, a
significant interaction was found between the factors Context x Effort F(1, 25) = 4.220, MSe =
390132201.0, p < 0.05; η2 = 0.144; η2p/ƒ = 0.41; 1-β = 1.0 (Greenhouse-Geisser correction). Figure
The medial channels Cz, FCz and CPz were analyzed separately and a significant interaction was
found in them between the factors Context x Effort F(1, 25) = 4.797, MSe = 31794213.87, p <
On the other hand, differences in amplitude were observed between the four experimental
conditions (Figure 5). Low-effort counterfactuals and high-effort factuals triggered the N400 effect.
In this temporal range this effect had a more frontal topographic distribution than in the previous
window.
400-600 ms window. These results, in a way, were the extension of the results in the 200-600 ms
window. There were no significant effects on the set of 58 electrodes, finding only a marginal trend
(p = 0.058 > 0.05) in the Context x Effort interaction. The same channels were taken from the 200-
600 ms window (for the same reasons) and a significant interaction was found between the factors
Context x Effort F(1, 25) = 4.079, MSe = 333835235.8; p < 0.05; η2 = 0.14; η2p/ƒ = 0.42; 1-β = 1.0
(Greenhouse-Geisser correction). Figure 6 shows the direction of this effect and Figure 7 shows
Based on statistical criteria, the medial channels taken in this window were Cz, Pz, FCz, CPz, and
POz (not the eight medial channels in the 200-600 ms window). These were analyzed separately
and a significant interaction was found between the factors Context x Effort F(1, 25) = 4.396, MSe=
39918962.82, p < 0.05; η2 = 0.15; η2p/ƒ = 0.42; 1-β = 1.0 (Greenhouse-Geisser correction).
This negativity pointed in a direction similar to that of the findings of Zhu et al. (2018). These
authors found that N400 is delayed and has a smaller amplitude in older adults. It is important to
remember that the N400 component is not only modulated by the degree of semantic incongruity.
Among the various factors is the N400 concretion effect (Kounios & Holcomb, 1994). This effect
is typically seen in relation to concrete and abstract noun processing, with concrete nouns eliciting
improved frontal N400 responses compared to abstract nouns; and tends to be more pronounced
on the frontal scalp than on the parietal scalp (as opposed to the more central-parietal distribution
of the N400 semantic context effect for written words). The effect is also higher when word
processing goes beyond surface-level characteristics and when contextual constraints are weak
The region of interest for this particular window is presented below, showing a right central-parietal
distribution. Due to the sinuous morphology of the brain wave, that we assume was characteristic
of the studied population, effects were found in different topographies in the N400 component.
17
4. Discussion
This study explored the ERP components that are produced while processing sentences with
different levels of mental effort, in factual and counterfactual linguistic contexts, in elderly
population. The conceptual discussion of these results was carried out under the paradigm of
embodied and situated cognition (Schilhab, 2017; Winter, 2019). It is important to consider that
the analysis criteria will be framed in several aspects such as the characteristics of the population,
in terms of its cognitive decline, form of social and interpersonal interaction, as well as the
subjective emotional well-being of the elderly through the development of wisdom. This study also
provides elements for the analysis of both cognitive and affective processes involved in the
processing of these linguistic structures. The motor aspect will be used as a criterion and we will
The results of each time window will be discussed below, following a sequence that allows a deeper
3.
4.
Given the elicitation of the ERP N400 component in the three time windows, it is necessary to
discuss it in an integrated and sequential way. Based on the results found, the most striking thing
about this component lies in its latency. Negativity was expressed early at 200 ms and it was
In relation to these experimental antecedents, our results are novel as they show an onset 180 ms
earlier and 160 ms extended, compared to the typical time window in young people. The extension
18
of the latency of the N400 has been interpreted as a consequence of the increase in the size of the
semantic networks related to the greater lexical-conceptual volume (Joyal et al., 2020). This
linguistic aspect is formed from the sensorimotor information present in the degree of mental effort,
producing greater negativity in actions of low factual mental effort than sentences of high mental
effort. On the other hand, the counterfactual also modulates the degree of mental effort, with low
Another notable aspect is that the sentences used in our study did not include words semantically
incongruous with the immediate linguistic context in the sentence final position. Our results show
that the N400 component was modulated, early and primarily, by the level of mental effort and the
counterfactual context, that is, embodied, emotional and linguistic aspects integrated with each
If we observe the three moments of the N400 component in succession from its earliest stages of
onset (200 ms) to its latest (400 ms), we can observe that the high mental effort factual sentences
become more negative as the timeline advances. This implies that older adults process the
little adjusted to the body-context relationship. On the other hand, in the high mental effort
counterfactual sentences, the positivity of the potentials decreased, probably due to the fact that the
reinterpretation of the events loses degrees of veracity due to the low semantic integration of the
events. There, older adults began to process the double counterfactual meaning, leaning towards
the reality of events. The N400 component, therefore, is modulated by linguistic and embodiment
factors.
A cognitive and embodied explanation for these effects can be found in the idea that high-effort
factual sentences are interpreted as events that are difficult to carry out, in relation to the effort that
it implies for an older person to generate mental actions that demand high mental energy levels.
19
These types of actions are mentally represented or simulated as more demanding, and therefore
In parallel, the low effort factuals seem to offer greater opportunities to be simulated, represented
and semantically integrated, which is reflected in the positive voltages obtained. This facilitates
their processing and corroborates the idea that they function as more concrete elements than the
rest of the semantics expressed as experimental conditions of the study. On the other hand, it is also
striking that the high-effort counterfactual sentences were constantly more positive than the rest of
the sentences, reflecting that older adults feel, perhaps, stimulated to imagine challenging past
positive emotions. The positivity when processing these sentences would therefore be linked to the
In relation to the brain regions that participate directly in the expression of the found N400
components, the early N400 effect was modulated by the right central and parietal zones. The
“standard” N400 component is regulated by mainly frontal and central zones. In contrast, late N400
parieto-occipital areas.
The greater negativity of the high mental effort factual would imply that, by progressively building
the situation model of the sentence and reaching the critical zone where the gerund connects with
the direct object, the high effort in indicative grammatical mode tries to recruit personal
dispositions that the person unconsciously rejects or does not prefer, which would be interpreted
as implausibility or semantic/pragmatic incoherence. The opposite case occurs with the high mental
effort counterfactual, which continues to be salient, probably due to emotional and social factors.
The social aspect would be explained because the mandate contained in the sentence that begins
with “You should have...” is ultimately a social mandate oriented to the “should be” that comes
20
from moral standards introjected from the culture and that could, eventually, imply the review of
behavior from the gaze of other people who judge or evaluate behavior.
Finally, it seems relevant to comment on these results as a whole, in relation to other studies. If the
assumption is accepted that low mental effort factual sentences are processed in a more concrete
way than high mental effort factual sentences, given their similarity to the everyday life of older
people (component N400) and that, on the other hand, the processing of high mental effort
counterfactuals is more salient for reasons related to their emotional load and double semantic
representation, the results in these time windows (component N400) are similar to those obtained
by Scorolli et al. (2011). These authors found that the processing of concrete or abstract words does
not only depend on the level of imaginability. It would also depend on how they are used in
linguistic-social contexts and different levels of self-relationship (Buccino et al., 2019; Scorolli et
al., 2011). This seems to be the case in our study since, despite the fact that mental verbs are
abstract, they can be embodied based on their high frequency of personal and social use, which
would provide evidence for the Theory of Words as Social Tools (Borghi et al., 2019).
21
5. Conclusion
The present research provides evidence on the time course in which the aging brain processes
language. Both linguistic contexts -factual and counterfactual-, as well as the embodied parameter
of mental effort modulate the processing and participate with greater or lesser preponderance in
Counterfactual language seems to induce emotions that facilitate the processing of high effort,
whereas the low effort - counterfactual interaction represents a negativity that is associated with
the N400 component. The same goes for high-effort factual language. The appearance of an early
N400 is noteworthy, and could be related to both the high lexical knowledge of the elderly and the
failure of inhibitory processes, phenomena that seem to be stimulated by the high effort factual
Older adults present a proportionally higher N400 for the high mental effort factual language than
for the low mental effort factual language, depending on the fact that they process the embodied
aspects of the sentence as less plausible, incoherent situations or less adjusted to their life context,
which coincides with the evidence that shows that language processing is facilitated when an action
is perceived in its usual context (Beauprez & Bidet-Ildei, 2019; Beauprez et al., 2020). On the other
hand, the counterfactual sentences behave in a different way, since their initial positivity
progressively declines given the reinterpretation of the double meaning of the events and leaning
towards the factual or real version of them. This effect could be of a linguistic-semantic nature,
Our analysis suggests that age-related declines in cognitive abilities, particularly those associated
with the mirror neuron system extending over the inferior frontal lobe, inferior parietal, and
superior temporal areas (Buccino et al., 2001), have the potential to impact various processes such
22
as action observation and prediction, theory of mind, empathic resonance, motor resonance, and
sensorimotor simulation in the elderly. These findings have significant clinical relevance, as they
highlight the importance of considering the influence of age-related changes in the mirror neuron
Understanding how age-related cognitive changes affect language comprehension can have
important implications for the development of interventions and therapies aimed at improving
communication and linguistic abilities in older adults. By recognizing the specific linguistic
challenges that arise due to compromised mirror neuron system functioning, clinicians and
language therapists can tailor their approaches to better address the needs of older individuals.
Moreover, this research opens new avenues for investigating the potential benefits of targeted
cognitive training programs and rehabilitation strategies that aim to enhance the mirror neuron
system and its associated processes. By exploring ways to mitigate the effects of aging on action
language comprehension, we may be able to improve overall communication skills and quality of
In summary, delving into the interplay between neural mechanisms, language processing, and
cognitive aging not only expands our theoretical understanding but also provides valuable insights
for clinical interventions and language therapies targeting older populations. Such endeavors hold
great promise in addressing the communication challenges faced by the elderly and improving their
Funding: This work was supported by the Conicyt National Scholarship No. 21120957 of the
Doctorate in Linguistics of the University of Concepción, and the Project for the Acquisition and
University of Bio Bio", code 2250315 AD/EQ of the University of Bío Bío, Chillán Campus. Chile.
Excellence FB0003.
6. References
Albayay, J., Villarroel-Gruner, P., Bascour-Sandoval, C., Parma, V., & Gálvez-García, G. (2019).
Psychometric properties of the Spanish version of the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory in a sample
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2019.103618
Allport, D. A., & Funnell, E. (1981). Components of the mental lexicon. Philosophical
https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1981.0148
Angulo, C. B. G., & Arias, A. C. (2011). Escala de Yesavage para Depresión Geriátrica (GDS-15
735-743. https://doi.org/10.11144/javeriana.upsy10-3.eydg
Beauprez, S. A., & Bidet-Ildei, C. (2018). The kinematics, not the orientation, of an action
Beauprez, S. A., & Bidet-Ildei, C. (2019). Perceiving a Biological Human Movement Facilitates
017-9694-5
Beauprez, S. A., Blandin, Y., Almecija, Y., & Bidet-Ildei, C. (2020). Physical and observational
practices of unusual actions prime action verb processing. Brain and Cognition, 138, 103630.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2019.103630
Bidet-Ildei, C., Beauprez, S. A., & Badets, A. (2020a). A review of literature on the link between
action observation and action language: advancing a shared semantic theory. New Ideas in
Bidet-Ildei, C., Beauprez, S. A., & Boucard, G. (2020b). The link between language and action in
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.archger.2020.104099
Borghi, A. M., Barca, L., Binkofski, F., Castelfranchi, C., Pezzulo, G., & Tummolini, L. (2019).
Words as social tools: Language, sociality, and inner grounding in abstract concepts. Physics of
Borghi, A. M., Binkofski, F., Castelfranchi, C., Cimatti, F., Scorolli, C., & Tummolini, L. (2017).
https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000089
Buccino, G., Colagè, I., Silipo, F., & D’Ambrosio, P. (2019). The concreteness of abstract language:
an ancient issue and a new perspective. Brain Structure and Function, 224(4), 1385-1401.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-019-01851-7
Cappuccio, M. L. (Ed.). (2019). Handbook of embodied cognition and sport psychology. MIT press.
Cox, A. (2016). Music and embodied cognition: Listening, moving, feeling, and thinking. Indiana
University Press.
de Vega, M. (2021). Revisitando la corporeidad del lenguaje narrativo. Revista signos, 54(107),
985-1003. https://doi.org/10.4067/S0718-09342021000300985
Folstein, M. F., Robins, L. N., & Helzer, J. E. (1983). The mini-mental state examination. Archives
Ionescu, T., & Vasc, D. (2014). Embodied cognition: challenges for psychology and education.
Issa, M. F., Juhasz, Z. & Kozmann, G. (2018). EEG analysis methods in neurolinguistics: A short
Joyal, M., Groleau, C., Bouchard, C., Wilson, M. A., & Fecteau, S. (2020). Semantic Processing in
Healthy Aging and Alzheimer’s Disease: A Systematic Review of the N400 Differences. Brain
Khosla, A., Khandnor, P., & Chand, T. (2020). A comparative analysis of signal processing and
classification methods for different applications based on EEG signals. Biocybernetics and
Kounios, J., & Holcomb, P. J. (1994). Concreteness effects in semantic processing: ERP evidence
Loeffler, J., Raab, M., & Cañal-Bruland, R. (2016). A Lifespan Perspective on Embodied
Marinescu, I., Lawlor, P., & Kording, K. (2018). Quasi-experimental causality in neuroscience and
Meteyard, L., Cuadrado, S. R., Bahrami, B., & Vigliocco, G. (2012). Coming of age: A review of
Miles, L., Nind, L., & Macrae, C. (2010). Moving through time. Psychol. Sci. 21, 222–223.
Muffato, V., Meneghetti, C., Di Ruocco, V., & De Beni, R. (2017). When young and older adults
learn a map: The influence of individual visuo-spatial factors. Learning and Individual.
Portegijs, E., Karavirta, L., Saajanaho, M., Rantalainen, T., & Rantanen, T. (2019). Assessing
physical performance and physical activity in large population-based aging studies: home-based
assessments or visits to the research center? BMC Public Health, 19(1), 1-16.
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-019-7869-8
Quiroga, P., Albala, C., & Klaasen, G. (2004). Validación de un test de tamizaje para el diagnóstico
http://doi.org/10.4067/S0034-98872004000400009
Royle, P., Steinhauer, K., Dessureault, É., Herbay, A. C., & Brambati, S. M. (2019). Aging and
Salas-Herrera, J. L., Martínez, M. U., Araneda, R. M., & De Vos, M. V. (2020). Comprensión de
oraciones de esfuerzo en jóvenes y adultos mayores desde una perspectiva corpórea. Universitas
Samu, D., Campbell, K. L., Tsvetanov, K. A., Shafto, M. A., & Tyler, L. K. (2017). Preserved
cognitive functions with age are determined by domain-dependent shifts in network responsivity.
Scorolli, C., Binkofski, F., Buccino, G., Nicoletti, R., Riggio, L., & Borghi, A. M. (2011). Abstract
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00227
Tirado, C., Khatin-Zadeh, O., Gastelum, M., Leigh-Jones, N., & Marmolejo-Ramos, F. (2018). The
https://doi.org/10.21500/20112084.3420
Urrutia, M. & de Vega, M. (2012). Lenguaje y acción: Una revisión actual a las teorías corpóreas.
Vukovic, N., Feurra, M., Shpektor, A., Myachykov, A., & Shtyrov, Y. (2017). Primary motor cortex
Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review (9, 4): 625-
636.
28
Winter, B. (2019). Sensory linguistics: Language, perception and metaphor (Vol. 20). John
Wlotko, E.W., Federmeier, K.D. 2012. So that's what you meant! Event-related potentials reveal
multiple aspects of context use during construction of message-level meaning. Neuroimage, 62(1),
356-366. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.04.054
Wlotko, E. W., Lee, C. L., & Federmeier, K. D. (2010). Language of the Aging Brain: Event‐
Related Potential Studies of Comprehension in Older Adults. Language and Linguistics Compass,
Yang, Y., Dickey, M. W., Fiez, J., Murphy, B., Mitchell, T., Collinger, J., Tyler-Kabara, E.,
Boninger, M., & Wang, W. (2017). Sensorimotor experience and verb-category mapping in human
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2017.04.021
Yesavage, J. A., Brink, T. L., Rose, T. L., Lum, O., Huang, V., Adey, M., & Leirer, V. O. (1982).
Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: A preliminary report. Journal
Zhu, Z., Hou, X., & Yang, Y. (2018). Reduced Syntactic Processing Efficiency in Older Adults
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00243
29
7. Figure Legend