GerbnerGross-1976-Journal of Communication
GerbnerGross-1976-Journal of Communication
GerbnerGross-1976-Journal of Communication
net/publication/22223200
CITATIONS READS
1,740 26,448
2 authors, including:
L. Gross
University of Southern California
53 PUBLICATIONS 6,662 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
All content following this page was uploaded by L. Gross on 11 October 2017.
172
Living With Television:
The Violence Profile
173
Journal of Communication, Spring 1976
groups of young and old and isolated people who have never before joined any
mass public. Television is likely to remain for a long time the chief source of
repetitive and ritualized symbol systems cultivating the common consciousness
of the most far-flung and heterogenous mass publics in history.
174
Lioing With Teleoision: The Violcwrc, Prtijlu
2A summary of the cultivation studies also appears in our article in the April 1976 Psychology
Todrry (10).
175
Joumul of C~omtntrtiication,Spring J9i6
essential elements of art, science, technology, statecraft, and public (as well as
most family) story-telling. T h e information-poor (children and less educated
adults) are again the entertainment-rich held in thrall by the myths and legends
of a new electronic priesthood.
If you were born before, say, 1950, television came into your life after the
formative years as just another medium. Even if you are now an “addict,” it will
be difficult for you to comprehend the transformations it has wrought. Could
you, as a twelve-year old, have contemplated spending an average of six hours a
day at the local movie house? Not only would most parents not have permitted
such behavior but most children would not have imagined the possibility. Yet,
in our sample of children, nearly half the twelve-year-olds watch at least six
hours of television every day.
Unlike print, television does not require literacy. Unlike the movies, tele-
vision is “free” (supported by a privately imposed tax on all goods), and it is
always running. Unlike radio, television can show as well as tell. Unlike the
theater, concerts, movies, and even churches, television does not require mobil-
ity. It comes into the home and reaches individuals directly. With its virtually
unlimited access from cradle to grave, television both precedes reading and,
increasingly, preempts it.
Television is the first centralized cultural influence to permeate both the
initial and the final years of life-as well as the years between. Most infants are
exposed to television long before reading. By the time a child reaches school,
television will have occupied more time than would be spent in a college
classroom. At the other end of the lifelong curriculum, television is there to keep
the elderly company when all else fails.
A11 societies have evolved ways of explaining the world to themselves and to
their children. Socially constructed “reality” gives a coherent picture of what
exists, what is important, what is related to what, and what is right. T h e
constant cultivation of such “realities” is the task of mainstream rituals a n d
mythologies. They legitimize action along socially functional and convention-
ally acceptable lines.
T h e social, political, and economic integration of modern industrial society
has created a system in which few communities, if any, can maintain an
independent integrity. W e are parts of a Leviathan and its nervous system is
telecommunications. Publicly shared knowledge of the “wide world” is what
this nervous system transmits to us.
Television is the chief common ground among the different groups that
make up a large and heterogeneous national community. No national achieve-
ment, celebration, or mourning seems real until it is confirmed and shared on
television.
Never before have all classes and groups (as well as ages) shared so much of
the same culture and t h e same perspectives while having so little to d o with
their creation. Representation in the world of television gives an idea, a cause, a
group its sense of public identity, importance, and relevance. No movement can
get going without some visibility in that world or long withstand television’s
power to discredit, insulate, or undercut. Other media, used selectively and by
176
Lining W t h Telecision: Thr Violencr Projle
177
Journal of Communication, Spring 1976
178
Prosecutor is badgering the witness!” The judge replied that he, too, had seen
that objection raised on the Perry Mason show but, unfortunately, it was not
included in the California code.
179
lourno1 of Communicafion, Spring 1976
180
Living With Television: The Violence Projile
Much of the research on media violence, for example, has focused on the
observation and measurement of behavior which occurs after a viewer has seen a
particular program or even isolated scenes from programs. All such studies, no
matter how clean the design and clear the results, are of limited value because
they ignore a fundamental fact: the world of TV drama consists of a complex
and integrated system of characters, events, actions, and relationships whose
effects cannot be measured with regard to any single element or program seen in
isolation.
We believe that the key to the answer rests in a search for those assumptions
about the “facts” of life and society that television cultivates in its more faithful
viewers. That search requires two different methods of research. T h e relation-
ship between t h e two is one of the special characteristics of t h e Cultural
Indicators approach.’
T h e first method of research is the periodic analysis of large and representa-
tive aggregates of television output (rather than individual segments) as the
system of messages to which total communities are exposed. The purpose of
message system analysis is to establish to composition and structure of the
symbolic world. W e have begun that analysis with the most ubiquitous, trans-
lucent, and instructive part of television (or any cultural) fare, the dramatic
programs (series, cartoons, movies on television) that populate and animate for
most viewers t h e heartland of the symbolic world. Instead of guessing or
assuming the contours a n d dynamics of that world, message system analysis
maps its geography, demography, thematic and action structure, time and space
3 F ~ ar more detailed description of the conceptual framework for this research see “Cultural
Indicators, The Third Voice” (8)
181
Jortmol of Conirnrrnication, Spring 1976
I82
Many times a day, seven days a week, the dramatic pattern defines situations
and cultivates premises about society, people, and issues. Casting the symbolic
world thus has a meaning of its own: the lion’s share of representation goes to
the types that dominate the social order. About three-quarters of all leading
characters are male, American, middle- and upper-class, and in the prime of
life. Symbolic independence requires freedom relatively uninhabited by real-
life constraints. Less fully represented are those lower in the domestic and
global power hierarchy and characters involved in familiar social contexts,
human dependencies, and other situations that impose the real-life burdens of
human relationships and obligations upon freewheeling activity.
Women typically represent romantic or family interest, close human con-
tact, love. Males can act in nearly any role, but rare is the female part that does
not involve at least the suggestion of sex. While only one in three male leads is
shown as intending to or ever having been married, two of every three females
are married or expect to marry in the story. Female “specialties” limit the
proportion of TV’s women to about one-fourth of the total population.
Nearly half of all females are concentrated in the most sexually eligible
young adult population, to which only one-fifth of males are assigned; women
are also disproportionately represented among the very young and old. Chil-
dren, adolescents, and old people together account for less than 15 percent of
the total fictional population
Approximately five in ten characters can be unambiguously identified as
gainfully employed. Of these, three are proprietors, managers, and profes-
sionals. The fourth comes from the ranks of labor-including all those employed
in factories, farms, offices, shops, stores, mining, transportation, service stations,
restaurants, and households, and working in unskilled, skilled, clerical, sales,
and domestic service capacities. The fifth serves to enforce the law or preserve
the peace on behalf of public or private clients.
Types of activity-paid and unpaid-also reflect dramatic and social pur-
poses. Six in ten characters are engaged in discernible occupational activity and
can be roughly divided into three groups of two each. The first group represents
the world of legitimate private business, industry, agriculture, finance, etc. The
second group is engaged in activity related to art, science, religion, health,
education, and welfare, as professionals, amateurs, patients, students, or clients.
The third makes up the forces of official or semiofficialauthority and the army of
criminals, outlaws, spies, and other enemies arrayed against them. One in every
four leading characters acts out a drama of some sort of transgression and its
suppression at home and abroad.
Violence plays a key role in such a world. It is the simplest and cheapest
dramatic means available to demonstrate the rules of the game of power. In real
life much violence is subtle, slow, circumstantial, invisible, even impersonal.
Encounters with physical violence in real life are rare, more sickening than
thrilling. But in the symbolic world, overt physical motion makes dramatically
visible that which in the real world is usually hidden. Symbolic violence, as any
show of force, typically does the job of real violence more cheaply and, of
course, entertainingly.
183
Journal of Communication, Spring I976
Geared for independent action in loosely-knit and often remote social con-
texts, half of all characters are free to engage in violence. One-fifth “specialize”
in violence as law breakers or law enforcers. Violence on television, unlike in
real-life, rarely stems from close personal relationships. Most of it is between
strangers, set u p to drive home lessons of social typing. Violence is often just a
specialty-a skill, a craft, an efficient means to test the norms of and settle any
challenge to the existing structure of power.
Four specific types of indicators have been developed. Three come from
message system analysis: (1) the context of programming trends against which
any aspect of the world of television can be seen; (2)several specific measures of
violence given separately and also combined in the Violence Index; and (3)
structural characteristics of the dramatic world indicating social relationships
depicted in it, (in the present report, “risk ratios”). The fourth type of indicator
comes from cultivation analysis and will be shown in this report as the “cultiva-
tion differential.” Although the Violence Profile is the most developed, the
Cultural Indicators project is constructing similar profiles of other aspects and
relationships of the media world.
Before we present the indicators, let us briefly note the definitions, terms,
and some procedures employed in generating t h e TV violence measures.‘
Message system analysis has been performed on annual sample-weeks of
prime time and weekend daytime network dramatic programming since 1967 by
trained analysts who observe and code many aspects of TV content T h e
definition of violence employed in this analysis is “ t h e overt expression of
physical force against self or other, compelling action against one’s will on pain
of being hurt or killed, or actually hurting or killing.” T h e research focuses on a
clear-cut and commonly understood definition of violence, and yields indicators
of trends in the programming context in which violence occurs; in the preva-
lence, rate, and characterizations involved in violence; a n d in the power rela-
tionships expressed by the differential risks found in the world of television
drama.
All observations are recorded in three types of unite: the program (play) as a
whole, each specific violent action (if any) in the program, a n d each dramatic
character appearing in the program.
“Program” means a single fictional story presented in dramatic form. This
may be a play produced for televisjon, a feature film telecast during the period
‘For a more detailed methodological description and all tabulations not included here, see
“Violence Profile: A Technical Report” (3). available for $12.00 (checks to be made out to the
Trustees of the University of Pennsylvania) from The Annenberg School of Communications.
University of Pennsylvania, Philadclphia, PA 19174.
184
of the study, or a cartoon story (of which there may be one or more in a single
program).
Violent action means a scene of some violence confined to the same parties.
If a scene is interrupted (by flashback or shift to another scene) but continues in
“real time,” it is still the same act. However, if a new agent of violence enters
the scene, that begins another act. These units are also called violent episodes.
Characters analyzed in all programs (whether violent or not) are of two
types. Major characters ark the principal roles essential to the story. Minor
characters (subjected to a less detailed analysis) are all other speaking roles.
(The findings summarized in this report include the analysis of major characters
only. )
Samples of programming. Network dramatic programs transmitted in eve-
ning prime time (8 p.m. to 11 p.m. each day), and network children’s dramatic
programs transmitted weekend mornings (Saturday and Sunday between 8 a.m.
and 2 p.m.) comprise the analytical source material.5 With respect to four basic
sample dimensions (network, program format, type and tone), the solid week
sample is at least as generalizable to a year‘s programming as larger randomly
drawn samples (2).
Coder training and reliability. For the analysis of each program sample, a
staff of 12 to 18 coders is recruited. After about three weeks of training and
testing, coders analyze the season’s videotaped program sample.
During both the training and data-collection phases, coders work in inde-
pendent pairs and monitor their assigned videotaped programs as often as
necessary. All programs in the sample are coded by two separate coder-pairs to
provide double-coded data for reliability comparisons. Final measures, com-
puted on the study’s entire corpus of double-coded data, determine the accept-
ability of information for analysis and provide guidelines to its interpretation
(11, 12).
Three sets of violence measures have been computed from the direct obser-
vational data of the message system analysis. They show the percent of pro-
grams with any violence at all, the frequency and rate of violent episodes, and
the number of roles calling for characterizations as violents, victims, or both.
These measures are called prevalence, rate, and role, respectively. Each is given
separately in all the tabulations that follow.
For ease of illustration and comparison, the three types of measures are also
combined to form the Violence Index. The Index itself is not a statistical finding
but serves as a convenient illustrator of trends and facilitates gross comparisons.
The Index is obtained by adding measures of prevalence, rates (doubled to raise
their relatively low numerical value) and roles. The formula can be seen on
Tables 1 through 4.
‘In 1967 and 1968, the hours included were 7 : 3 0to 10 p . m . Monday through Saturday, 7 to 10
p.m. Sunday. and children’s programs 8 a.m. to noon Saturday. Beginning in 1969, these hours were
expanded until 11 p.m. each evening and from 7 a.m. to 2:30 p . m . Saturday and Sunday. As of 1971
however, network evening programming has been reduced by the FCC‘s prime-time access rule.
T h e effective evening parameters since 1971 are therefore 8 to 11 p.m. Monday through Saturday
and 7:30 to 10:30 p.m. Sunday.
185
Journal of Communication, Spring 1976
30t
t 1 I I I I I I 1
1967 68 69 70 71 72 73 74 75
Figure 1: “Action“ (crime, western, adventure) programs as percent of cartoon and of other
(general) programs analyzed
Before presenting the trends indicated by the measures just discussed, let us
glance at the first indicator, that of program mix. “Action” programs contribute
most violence to the world of television drama. Figure 1 shows that such
programs comprise more than half of all prime-time and weekend daytime
programming, and their proportion of the total has not changed much in recent
years. In fact, while general (non-cartoon) crime and adventure plays dropped
from their 1974 high of 62 percent to 54 percent in 1975, cartoon crime and
adventure rose in the same period from 47 percent to 66 percent of all cartoons.
These programming trends foreshadow the violence findings that follow. We
can summarize them by noting that there has been no significant reduction in
the overall Violence Index despite some fluctuations in the specific measures
and a definite drop in “family hour” violence, especially on CBS, in the current
season. The “family hour” decline has been matched by a sharp increase in
violence during children’s (weekend daytime) programming in the current
season and by an even larger two-year rise in violence after 9 p.m. EST.
Figure 2 shows these trends in greater detail. Figure 3 provides similar infor-
mation for each network separately, showing that late evening violence shot
up on all three networks in the past two or three years (with minor dips on CBS
and ABC in 1975), and that children’s (weekend daytime) programs became
more violent on ABC and NBC in the past season. Figure 4 is a direct com-
parison of the Violence Index for each network, showing remarkable long-term
stability and similarity among them. Figure 5 is a direct comparison of the
“family hour” Violence Index for each network, showing little change over a
two-year period for ABC and NBC, substantial reduction for the second year
in a row for CBS.
186
Lioing With Teleaixbn: The Violencv Projili.
Tables 1 through 4 (found at the end of the article) present all measures
for the different hours of programming. They show how the specific measures
of prevalence, rate, and role fluctuate and combine each year to make up the
composite Violence Index. More complete tabulations, including network and
format breakdowns, can be found in the Technical Report (3).
The indicators reflected in the Violence Index are clear manifestations of
what network programmers actually do as compared to what they say or intend
to do. Network executives and their censorship (“Standards and Practices”)
offices maintain close control over the assembly line production process that
results in the particular program mix of a season (6). While our data permit
many specific qualifications to any generalization that might be made, it is safe
to say that network policy seems to have responded in narrow terms, when at all,
to very specific pressure, and only while the heat was on. After nine years of
investigations, hearings, and commissions (or since we have been tracking
violence on television), eight out of every ten programs (nine out of every ten
weekend children’s hour programs) still contain some violence. The overall rate
of violent episodes, 8 per hour, is, if anything, higher than at any time since
1969. (The violence saturation of weekend children’s programs declined from
the 1969 high but increased from its 1974 low to 16 per hour, double that of
overall programming, as can be seen on Table 4 . ) Between six and seven out of
every ten leading characters (eight and nine for children) are still involved in
-
some violence. Between one and two out of every ten are still involved in killing.
--
-- All hours an sample
260
- ”Family hour’’ (before 9 p.m. EST1
Late evening (9-11 p.m. EST)
Weekend daytime (children’s) hours
220
I80
I40
\
\
\
100
1
I I I I I I I I I
1967 1968 1969 1970 1971 1972 1973 1974 1975
Figure 2: Violence Index for different hours of dramatic programming
187
Journal of Communication, Spring 1976
220-
I80 ~
ABC
140 -
100-
260 -
220 -
I80 -
CBS
140 -
100-
6ok , I
\
260 -
220 ~
NBC 180-
II00
4Ol
J
I I I I I 1 I
1967 1968 1969 1970 1971 1972 1973 1974 1975
188
Lioing With Television: The Violence Projilc,
I I I I I I I I I
I967 1968 1969 1970 1971 1972 1973 1974 1975
Figure 4: Violence Index for each network, all programs in sample
220
I80
I40
100
\<SS
\
60 \
I I I I I I I I I
1967 1968 1969 1970 1971 1972 1973 1974 1975
Figure 5: Violence Index for each network, family hour only
290
Living With Television: The Violence Projile
Figure 6: Percent giving the “television answer” to a question about the proportion of
people employed in law enforcement
the light viewers (those viewing an average of two hours a day or less). Figure 7
shows similar results for the question “ C a n most people be trusted?” and Figure
8 for the question “During any given week, what are your chances of being
involved in some type of violence?” One in ten (the “television answer”) or one
in a hundred?”
Let us take education as probably the best index of a complex of social
circumstances that provide alternative informational and cultural opportunities.
Those of our respondents who have had some college education are less likely to
choose the “television answer” than those who have had none. But within each
group, television viewing “biases” conceptions in the direction of the “facts” it
presents. When we compared light and heavy viewers within the “college” and
the “ n o college” groups, we got a typical step-wise pattern of the percentage of
“television answers.” Regular reading of newspapers makes a similar difference.
Both college education and regular newspaper reading seem to reduce the
percentage of “television answers,” but heavy viewing boosts it within both
groups. This appears to be the general pattern of TV’s ability to cultivate its own
“reality.”
An exaggerated impression of the actual number of law enforcement work-
ers seems t o be a consequence of viewing television. Of greater concern,
however, would be the cultivation of a concomitantly exaggerated demand for
their services. The world of television drama is, above all, a violent one in which
more than half of all characters are involved in some violence, at least one-tenth
65 %
Figure 7: Percent responding “Can’t be too careful” to the question “Can most people be
trusted?”
192
in some killing, and in which over three-fourths of prime-time hours contain
some violence. As we have suggested, the cultivation of fear and a sense of
danger may well be a prime residue of the show of violence.
Questions about feelings of trust and safety may be used to test that
suggestion. T h e National Opinion Research Corporation’s 1975 General Social
Survey asked “Can most people be trusted?” Living in the world of television
seems to strengthen the conclusion that they cannot. Heavy viewers chose the
answer “Can’t be too careful” in significantly greater proportions than did light
viewers in the same groups, as shown in Figure 7. Those who d o not read
newspapers regularly have a high level of mistrust regardless of TV viewing.
But, not surprisingly, women are the most likely to absorb t h e message of
distrust.
Focusing directly on violence, we asked a national sample of adults about
people’s chances of being involved in violence in any given week. Figure 8
shows the patterns of overestimations in line with television’s view of the world.
It may explain why in recent surveys, such as the Detroit study conducted by
the Institute of Social Research (13), respondents’ estimates of danger in their
neighborhoods had little to do with crime statistics or even with their own
personal experience. T h e pattern of our findings suggests that television and
other media exposure may be as important as demographic and other ex-
periential factors in explaining why people view the world as they do.
Television certainly appears to condition the view of the generation that
knew no world without it. All the figures show that the “under 30” respondents
exhibit consistently higher levels of “television responses,” despite the fact that
they tend to be better educated than the “over 30” respondents. We may all live
in a dangerous world, hut young people (including children tested hut not
reported on here), the less educated, women, and heavy viewers within all these
groups sense greater danger than light viewers in the same groups. College
education (and its social correlates) may counter the television view, but heavy
exposure to TV will counteract that too.
;iflflfiflflflfi
TV TV L H L H L H L H L H L H L H L H
C0ll.p. NO R4q~lOl Nap O“., UDd., HOI. Fernol.
A I/ C O I I II. Rigulor 30 30
Respondent 5 Education News Read,nv A ve Gender
Figure 8: Percent giving the “television answer” (exaggerating) their own chances of being
involved in violence
Journal of Communication, Spring 1976
may cultivate exaggerated assumptions about the extent of threat and danger in
the world and lead to demands for protection.
What is the net result? A heightened sense of risk and insecurity (different
for groups of varying power) is more likely to increase acquiescence to and
dependence upon established authority, and to legitimize its use of force, than it
is to threaten the social order through occasional non-legitimized imitations.
Risky for their perpetrators and costly for their victims, media-incited criminal
violence may be a price industrial cultures extract from some citizens for the
general pacification of most others.
As with violence, so with other aspects of social reality we are investigating,
TV appears to cultivate assumptions that fit its socially functional myths. Our
chief instrument of enculturation and social control, television may function as
the established religion of the industrial order, relating to governance as the
church did to the state in earlier times.
REFERENCES
1. Cater, Douglass, and Stephen Strickland. TV Violence and the Child; The Evolution and Fate of
the Surgeon General’s Report. New York: Russell Sage Foundation, 1975.
2. Eleey, Michael F. “Variations in Generalizability Resulting from Sampling Characteristics of
Content Analysis Data: A Case Study.” Unpublished manuscript, Annenberg School of
Communications, University of Pennsylvania, 1969.
3. Gerbner, George, and Larry Gross, with the assistance of Michael F. Eleey, Suzanne K. Fox,
Marilyn Jackson-Beeck, and Nancy Signorielli. “Violence Profile No. 7: A Technical Report.”
Annenberg School of Communications, 1976.
4. Gerbner, George. “Dimensions of Violence in Television Drama.” Chapter 15 in Violence and
the Media edited by Robert K. Baker and Sandra J. Ball, a staff report to the National
Commission on the Causes and Prevention of Violence, U.S. Government Printing Office,
1969.
5. Gerbner, George. “Violence in Television Drama: Trends and Symbolic Functions.” In G. A .
Comstock and E. A. Rubinstein (Eds.) Television and Social Behaoior, Vol. 1. Washington
D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office, 1972.
6. Gerbner, George, Larry Gross, and William H. Melody. Communications Technology and
Social Policy. New York: John Wiley and Sons, 1973.
7. Gerbner, George. “The Structure and Process of Television Program Content Regulation in the
U . S . ” In G. A. Comstock and E. A. Rubinstein (Eds.) Television and Social Behaoior, Vol. 1.
Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office, 1972.
8. Gerbner, George. “Cultural Indicators: The Third Voice.” In Communications Technology and
Social Policy. New York: John Wiley and Sons, 1973.
9. Gerbner, George. “Scenario for Violence.” Human Behavior, October 1975.
10. Gerbner, George, and Larry Gross. “The Scary World of Television,” Psychology Today,
April 1976.
11. Krippendofi, Klaus. “Bivariate Agreement Coefficients for the Reliability of Data.” In E. F.
Borgatta and G. W. Bohrnstedt (Eds.), Sociological Methodology. San Francisco: Jossey Bass,
1970.
12. Krippendorf, Klaus. “ A Computer Program for Agreement Analysis of Reliability Data, Version
4.” Mimeographed. Annenberg School of Communications, July 1973.
13. “Personal Safety a Major Concern: Public Perceptions of Quality of Life in Metropolitan
Detroit Examined in ISR Study.” ZSR Newsletter, Winter 1976, p. 4.
14. Rubinstein, Eli A. “The TV Violerke Report: What’s Next?” Journal of Communication,
Winter 1974, pp. 80-88.
15. Social Science Research Council. “ A Profile of Television Violence.” Report submitted by the
Committee on Television and Social Behavior of the SSRC, July 1975.
16. Weinberger, Caspar W. Letter to Senator John 0. Pastore dated November 13, 1973.
194
Table 1: Violence measures for all programs in sample
1967 1968 1969 1970 1971 1972 1973 1974 1975 TOTAL
SAMPLES (100%) N N N N N N N N N N
PREVALENCE x % 9. % % % x % % %
(%P) ~
Proqranls c u n t a ~ n i r .vlolcnce 81.3 81.6 83.5 77.5 80.6 79.0 72.7 83.3 78.4 79.8
Prosram t.ours containlng violence 83.2 87.0 83.2 78.3 87.2 84.2 79.7 86.8 83.0 83.6
RATE N N N N N N N N N N
Number of violent episodes 478 394 630 498 483 539 524 522 626 4694
lR/P) Rate per all programs Iplays1 5.0 4.5 5.2 4.5 4.7 5.4 5.3 5.4 5.6 5.1
IR/Hl Rate per all h w r s 7.7 6.7 8.8 7.4 6.9 7.5 7.0 6.9 a. 1 7.4
Ouratlcn of Violent Eoisodes ihrs) .. .. ~. -_ .. .. 3.2 3.8 3.6 10.6
VIolents Iconirn~tttngv ~ ole n cei 55.8 49.3 16.5 52.0 46.0 39.3 34.5 40.8 43.1 44.6
Victims (suojected to violence1 64.6 55 8 58.9 56.6 50.8 49.7 48.2 51.2 53.8 56.0
i%V) Any involvement 1 1 1 violence 73.3 65.1 66.3 62.8 61.5 58.3 55.7 60.7 64.8 62.9
Killers (committing fatal ;iolenceJ 12.5 10 7 3.7 6.6 8.7 7.7 5.8 9.8 6.3 7.7
Killed l v i c t i r s o f lethal violence1 7.1 3.7 2.1 4.6 3.2 4.7 3.3 5.8 3.8 4.2
(%Y) hny involvement in hilling 18.7 1l.G 5.6 8.7 9.9 9.7 7.5 13.6 9 .I 10.2
INDICATORSor VIOLENCE
Program Score: PS=[XP1+21R/P)+2(R/HI 106.6 104.1 111.4 101.3 103.7 104.8 97.3 107.9 105.6 104.8
Character V-Score: C s = ( X V J + IXKi 92.1 76.7 71 .9 71 .4 71.4 68.0 63.2 74.3 73.9 73.0
Violence Index: V1 E PS + CS 198.7 180.9 183.3 172.7 175.1 172.8 160.5 182.2 179.7 177.8
Table 2: Violence measures for family hour only
1967 1968 1969 1970 1971 1972 1973 1974 1975 TOTAL
SAMPLES 1100%) N N N N N N N N N N
Programs ( l a y s 1 a n a l y ? d 38 36 38 35 28 27 32 29 31 294
Program Hours A n a l y z e d 30.0 27.0 27.3 26.0 25.0 23.5 29.0 27.0 21.5 236.3
Leadlng characters analyzed 103 102 130 76 78 98 110 109 105 91 1
PREVALENCE % % % % % % % x % Y
(%P) Piyograms c o n t a i n i n g v i o l e n c e 78.9 75.0 63.2 57.1 75.0 74.1 56.3 69.0 51.6 66.7
Program h o u r s c o n t a i n i n g v l o l e n c e 86.7 83.3 74.3 67.3 86.0 85.1 70.7 77.8 60.5 77.1
RATE N N N N N N N N N N
V i o l e n t s ( c o n n n r t t ~ n g\ l o l e n c e l 58.3 39.2 36.2 32.9 37.2 37.8 29.1 29.4 16.2 35.0
Victims (subjected t o violence1 68.9 46. 1 40.8 39.5 38.5 40.8 33.6 36.7 27.6 41 .4
(%V) Any I n v o l v e m e n t i n v i o l e n c e 75.7 56.9 49.2 40.8 50.0 50.0 40.9 45.0 36.2 49.5
Killers (corrnitt;ng fatal vlolence) 22.3 10.8 6.2 3.9 9.0 4.1 6.4 12.8 1 .o 8.6
K i l l e d Ivictims of lethal violence) 7.8 4.9 3.1 1.3 2.6 3.1 4.5 7.3 0.0 4.0
I%K) Any l n v o l v e m e n t i n h i l l i n g 28.2 12.7 9.2 3.9 10.3 5.1 10.0 16.5 1 .o 11 .o
I N D I C A T O R S OF V I O L E N C E
Program Score: PS=(%P)+2lR/P)+2(R/H) 107.6 90.9 78.5 68.7 91.7 93.5 75.6 84.4 63.7 84 .O
Character V-Score' CS = (%\/I + (XKI 103.9 69.6 58.5 44.7 60.3 55.1 50.9 61.5 37.1 60.5
V\olence Index: V1 = P5 + CS 211.5 160.6 137.0 113.4 151.9 148.6 126.5 145.9 100.9 144.5
Table 3: Violence measures for late evening (9-11 p.m. EST)
1967 1968 1969 1970 1971 1972 1973 1974 1975 TOTAL
SAMPLES 1100%) N N N N N N N E, N
N.
Programs ( p l a y s ) a n a l y z e d 26 21 26 26 34 33 30 29 35 260
Program H o u r s A n a l y z e d 25.0 24.0 30.5 28.0 30.3 33.0 27.5 33.0 39.5 270.7
Leading characters analyzed 75 60 88 56 91 119 104 I15 133 841
PREVALENCE x % % % % % % x x x
l%P) Programs c o n t a i n i n g v i o l e n c e 69.2 76.2 80.8 69.2 76.5 69.7 63.3 86.2 85.7 75.4
Program h o u r s c o n t a i n i n g v i o l e n c e 76.0 89.6 84.4 80.4 87.6 79.8 79.1 92.4 92.4 85.1
RATE N N N N N N N N N N
R O L E S ( % O F L E A D I h G CHARACTER5I % % % % % % % x % x
Violents Icomnitting violence) 38.7 55.0 34.1 46.4 44.0 37.8 32.7 56.5 51.1 44.0
V i c t i m s (sub;ectcd t o v + o l e n c e ) 42.7 55.0 44.3 50.0 48.4 45.4 36.5 61.7 59.4 49.7
1°C) Any i n v o l v e m e n t i n v i o l e n c e 56.0 68.3 52.3 57.1 59.3 55.5 41 . 3 71.3 68.4 59.1
K i l l e r s (committing f a t a l violencel 5.3 16.7 5.7 14.3 15.4 16.0 12.5 16.5 16.5 13.6 cF.
K i l l e d (vlctirns of lethal violencel 4.0 5.0 2.3 12.5 5.5 8.4 6.7 10.4 9.8 7.4
(%K) Any i n v o l v e m e n t i n k i l l i n g 9.3 16.7 6.8 21.4 17.6 19.3 14.4 24.3 23.3 17.6 TQ
f
INDlCATORS O r VIOLENCE >
Program S c o r e : P S = ( % P ) + Z I R / P 1 + 2 ( R / H ) 82.9 93.9 96.4 86.4 92.6 90.5 81.5 114.7 116.3 95.7
Character V-Score: C S = ( % V ) + (%KJ 65.3 85.0 59.1 78.6 76.9 74.8 55.8 95.7 91.7 76.7
Vlolence Index: V I = PS + CS 148.2 178.9 155.5 165.0 169.5 165.3 137.2 210.4 208.1 172.4
Table 4: Violence measures for weekend daytime (children's) hours
1967 1968 1969 1970 1971 1972 1973 1974 1975 TOTAL
(0
SAMPLES (100%) N N N N N N N N N N
.;
h
PREVALENCE % % % % % % % x % x
(%PI Programs contalnrng violence 93.8 93.3 98.2 96.0 87.8 90.0 94.6 92.1 91.1 93.2
Program hours containing violence 94.0 92.2 97.6 95.6 88.5 92.3 94.6 90.6 89.8 92.7
RATE N N N N N N N N N N
Number o f violent epis~cies 151 172 398 296 244 245 247 194 265 2212
(R/P) Rate per all programs (plays) 4.7 5.7 7.0 5.9 6.0 6.1 6.7 5.1 5.9 6.0
IR/Hl Rate per all hours 21 . 6 22.9 28.4 22.5 16.2 15.8 13.2 12.1 16.2 18.0
Violents f c o m i t t I rig violence) 72.6 62.3 66.7 79.7 50.6 43.4 40.0 36.1 57.1 54.8
Victims (subjected t o violence) 03.9 75.5 81.8 82.8 65.1 66.3 67.6 54.1 69.8 70.9
f%Vl Any involvement ~n v7olence 90.3 77.4 88.1 93.8 74.7 72.3 77.2 64.8 84.9 79.9
Killers (committing fatal vlolencel 4.8 3.8 0.6 3.1 1.2 0.0 0.7 0.8 0.0 1'.2
Killed (victims o f lethal violencel 9.7 0.0 1.3 1.6 1.2 1.2 0.0 0.0 0.8 1'.3
l%Kl Any involvement in hilling 14.5 3.8 1.9 3.1 1.2 1.2 0.7 0.8 0.8 2.3
INOICATORS O F VIOLENCE
Program Score: PS=l%P1+2lR/PI+2(R/HI 146.3 150.7 169.1 152.8 132.2 133.9 134.4 126.6 135.3 141 .1
character V-Score: cs = ( X V I+ ( % K I 104.8 81.1 89.9 96.9 75.9 73.5 77.9 65.5 85.7 87.3
Violence Index: v1 i Ps + CS 251 .2 231 .8 259.0 249.7 208.1 205.4 212.3 192.1 221.1 223.4
Table 5: Risk ratios for all programs studied 1967-75
Nationality
U.S. 1505 75.0 -1.19 +2.39 503 46.1 -1.39 - 1.08
Other 276 96.7 -1.22 +1.13 66 60.6 -1.55 +3.00
Race r
8.
White 1533 77.6 -1.20 f2.12 541 49.9 -1.29 +1.07
CS
Other 264 83.3 -1.27 +1.33 50 38.0 -2.43 -0.00'
Character type**
E
s
"Good" (heroes) 928 69.3 -1.26 f3.47 314 43.3 -1.56 -6.00
Mixed type 432 71.1 -1.31 +1.09 156 43.6 -1.37 1.oo
g
"Bad" (v iIla ins) 291 114.1 - 1.03 +1.80 41 s.
82.9 +1.14 +2.00 $.
?
violents nor victims. If 0.00 i s preceded by a sign, group has either no violents or no victims; +O.OO means only violent(s1
* Group has neither
but no victims(s); -0.00 means only victim(s) but no violent(s).
2
** This classification was introduced in 1969. s
Note: Character score is the percent of characters involved in any violence plus the percent involved in any killing. V-v ration is of violents (+) and
victims (-). K-k ration is of killers (+) and killed (-).
s
300
?
5
rc