Sustainability 11 02666 With Cover

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 20

3.9 5.

Article

What Do Coffee Shop


Entrepreneurs Need to Do to Raise
Pro-Environmental Customer
Behavioral Intentions?

Sunmi Yun and Taeuk Kim

Special Issue
Environmental Behaviour and Collective Decision Making
Edited by
Dr. Ralph Hansmann

https://doi.org/10.3390/su11092666
sustainability

Article
What Do Coffee Shop Entrepreneurs Need to
Do to Raise Pro-Environmental Customer
Behavioral Intentions?
Sunmi Yun and Taeuk Kim *
International Center for Hospitality Research & Development, Dedman School of Hospitality, Florida State
University, Tallahassee, FL 32306, USA; [email protected]
* Correspondence: [email protected]; Tel.: +1-850-345-2971

Received: 10 April 2019; Accepted: 30 April 2019; Published: 10 May 2019 

Abstract: Our research framework, built on the norm activation model (NAM), was designed to
provide a comprehensive understanding of the formation of consumers’ pro-environmental behavioral
intentions in an eco-friendly coffee shop. We employed the NAM to test its mediating effect of personal
environmental norms (PEN), social environmental norms (SEN), and ascription of responsibility
(AR) and the moderating effect of the overall green image (OGI) on pro-environmental behavioral
intentions. Data were collected through a survey of 530 customers who frequently visited a coffee
shop in Korea, and structural equation modeling (SEM) was used to test the research hypotheses. The
findings generally supported the hypothesized associations of the study variables within our proposed
theoretical framework (PEN, SEN, and AR in order of the mediating effect on pro-environmental
behavioral intentions) and confirmed OGI’s moderating effect. In addition, the study’s results have
important (1) theoretical and (2) practical implications for the environment. (1) They expand the
original NAM by explaining the effect of the relationship between SEN and PEN on pro-environmental
customer behavioral intentions (PCBI) and confirm the mediating effect of the NAM (SEN, PEN, AR)
on PCBI, as demonstrated in previous studies. (2) Moreover, the findings herein may encourage coffee
shops to participate in the prevention of environmental problems by restricting the use of products
such as plastic coffee cups and straws.

Keywords: norm activation (NAM); environmental problem concern (EPC); social environmental
norms (SEN); personal environmental norms (PEN); ascription of responsibility (AR);
pro-environmental customer behavioral intentions (PCBI); overall green image (OGI); coffee shops

1. Introduction
The global environment has become a primary focus of concern as more people are aware of
sustainability-related issues than ever before. Reflecting the awareness of ecological issues that
have been steadily increasing over the last two decades, the general public is now beginning to
comprehend the impact these issues will have on their lives [1–4]. As consumers have recognized the
importance of a sustainable green environment, environmental protection has become an essential
issue in the consumer market [5]. Governments all over the globe are executing environmental policies
as environmental protection is gathering global interest. Over fifty nations participated in the ocean
plastics reducing campaign. For instance, India prohibited the use of disposable plastic material.
Chile, Peru prohibited plastic bags. Nigeria set up waste recycling factories. Private organizations
such as Internal Olympic Committee (IOC), World Wildlife Fund (WWF), LG electronics, and Volvo
also joined plastic regulation campaigns. Additionally, global citizens are showing their interest
via SNS, expressing their opinion about disposable plastic materials’ rejection in the form of a viral

Sustainability 2019, 11, 2666; doi:10.3390/su11092666 www.mdpi.com/journal/sustainability


Sustainability 2019, 11, 2666 2 of 19

campaign [6]. Environmentally-conscious consumers recognize that our society faces a severe crisis
due to environmental problems (e.g., climate change, water scarcity, and air pollution). Customers
with environmental concerns prefer to buy and use pro-environmental goods and services, even if
they are less convenient than the alternatives [7,8]. Therefore, it is necessary to understand the needs
of eco-friendly consumers and strive for pro-environmental management in the consumer market
by, for example, implementing environmentally-friendly technologies, initiating diverse eco-friendly
programs, encouraging eco-friendly practices among customers and employees, modifying operation
processes, and developing environmental policies and guidelines [2,7,9–11]. Notably, the global growth
of the coffee shop industry has inevitably increased energy and resource consumption, from the amount
of coffee that is harvested to the electricity needed to switch on the lights in the actual shops. This
increased consumption correlates with a rise in the amount of waste produced, including packaging
materials and coffee grounds [12]. The issue of environmental sustainability in the coffee shop industry
is frequently discussed in the mainstream media in terms of disposable waste, often focused on coffee
cups and grounds [13,14]. Therefore, as coffee shops attempt to become environmentally friendly
in order to respond to consumers’ enhanced awareness, an increasing number of coffee shops are
proactively changing their operations by focusing on more eco-friendly practices. For example,
Starbucks is working to shrink its environmental footprint and meet its customers’ expectations by
increasing the use of recycled cups, reducing waste, and conserving energy/water [15]. Dunkin’ Donuts
announced its plan to eliminate all polystyrene foam cups in its global supply chain beginning in
spring 2018, with a targeted completion date of 2020 [16]. Costa Coffee has put great effort into using
sustainable and recyclable products and equipment. For instance, the company’s paper cups are made
of a sustainable wood pulp material from northern European forests; the company does not use smart
coffee machines in order to help reduce carbon emissions, and it recycles all of its coffee sacks into the
shops’ carpet underlay [16]. Despite this growing interest in pro-environmental behaviors, however,
relatively little attention has been paid to the identification of the essential factors that influence
pro-environmental customer behavioral intentions (PCBI) in environmentally-friendly coffee shops.
PCBI can reduce severe environmental pollution and enable customers to improve their quality of
life in a clean environment and move toward a sustainable future [2,3,17–19]. PCBI also improves
business in coffee shops by increasing customer revisits due to the higher demand for eco-friendly
goods and enhanced customer satisfaction and employee commitment [20]. Therefore, this study aims
to expand the research on PCBI and benefit both the environment and coffee shops’ ability to conduct
business successfully.
Previous studies have relied heavily on Ajzen’s [21] theory of planned behavior (TPB), Stern’s [22]
value-belief-norm (VBN) theory, and Schwartz’s [23] norm activation model (NAM) as the most
important predictors of PCBI within the domain of environmental psychology [3,24–26]. Of these,
the NAM is considered the most influential theory [3,24,27,28]. The NAM used in this study is
based on the NAM that researchers [3,27] developed by extending the VBN theory. The NAM
implies the decision-making process of activating norms for pro-social behavior [3,29]. Schwartz’s [23]
NAM comprises three concepts: awareness of problems, ascription of responsibility (AR), and
personal norms [3,22,23,30,31]. Although the NAM has been extensively used in PCBI [30], many
researchers asserted the need to expand the original NAM to better explain individuals’ eco-friendly
intentions/behaviors [1,3,12,22,32–35]. In this study, the critical variable of the existing NAM is personal
norms. In Schwartz’s [23] study, personal norms are defined as “internalized rules of conduct that
are socially learned [and that] vary among individuals within the same society and direct behavior
in a particular situation”. Hence, personal norms are behaviors that appear differently depending
on individual tendencies and situations based on behaviors learned in society. Social norms affect
individuals, and then personal norms appear. Therefore, unlike the previous studies mentioned above,
social environmental norms (SEN) are applied to the NAM, and social norms influence personal
norms and identify relationships with PCBI. Besides, previous studies on marketing and consumer
behavior have indicated that the overall image of a firm plays a critical role in customers’ behavioral
Sustainability 2019, 11, 2666 3 of 19

intentions [36]. References [5,37,38] found that the higher the overall green image (OGI), the more
people behave in an eco-friendly manner.
To fill the existing research gaps, this study’s purpose is to develop a theoretical framework that
clearly explains PCBI in the environmentally-responsible coffee shop business, which has not yet
been well documented. Moreover, we want to use customers’ environmental awareness as a basis for
marketing to improve the coffee shop business by identifying the role of personal and social norms to
explain PCBI. Specifically, we aim to (1) broaden the NAM by incorporating the VBN framework and
normative process; (2) test the mediating impact of personal environmental norms (PEN, SEN, and
AR); (3) deepen the NAM by considering the moderating impact of OGI; (4) identify the adequacy
of the proposed model by conducting a model comparison; and (5) examine the relative importance
among constructs in building intentions in the model.

2. Conceptual Framework

2.1. Environmental Problem Concern


Protecting the environment is paramount, and increasing concern about environmental protection
may facilitate proper discussion about this issue. Thus, various studies have been conducted to
define the true nature of environmental concern and find its precise measurement methodology [35].
Environmental problem concern (EPC) can be classified as a user-defined term. However, it normally
refers to “awareness about humans’ capability to damage nature”. EPC can induce people to protect
nature through three phases: seeing, feeling, and acting [39]. Mostafa [40] insisted that environmental
concern can positively influence consumers’ intent to choose eco-friendly products. Evidence is offered
in the results of numerous studies aimed at shedding light on the relationship between environmental
concern and pro-environmental intentions/behaviors [41,42].

2.2. Norm Activation Model


Norm activation model (NAM) can be defined as “the standard of value judgment that a person
should follow when that person thinks and acts”. Amendments to people’s thoughts and behaviors
are made based on two types of norms: personal norms function from within and social norms
that are constructed in the surrounding society [43]. In 1977, Schwartz developed the NAM in the
context of altruistic behavior. The model was designed to investigate the true nature of altruistic
behavior and to examine people’s pro-environmental behaviors and intentions [34] in order to explain
moral norm/pro-environmental action transfer [23]. Hopper and Nielsen [44] applied the NAM to
recycling behavior, arguing that recycling should be interpreted as an altruistic behavior derived
from norms. The researchers argued that recycling behavior could be described as altruistic behavior
since sorting/moving recyclable goods does not bring anything to oneself but only benefits future
society [44].
The NAM has played a critical role in research on pro-social intentions, and it contains three
cardinal variables: environmental problem concern (EPC), personal environmental norms (PEN),
and ascription of responsibility (AR) [23,45]. Specifically, PEN is the NAM’s core element, described
as a “moral obligation to perform or refrain from specific actions” [46]. PEN works as an index of
pro-social behavior. EPC can be defined as “whether someone is aware of the negative consequences
for others or for other things one values when not acting pro-socially” [45]. AR refers to “feelings of
responsibility for the negative consequences of not acting pro-socially” [45]. For example, wasting
electricity may lead to a number of negative consequences. However, people who have EPC may
develop AR to feel joint responsibility for its negative consequences. Inversely, the development of AR
is difficult if EPC is not yet developed. Studies have found that EPC positively affects the development
of AR [3,34]. According to studies performed using empirical methods, EPC, AR, and PEN play an
essential role in individuals’ environmental intentions/behaviors [3,28,45,47–49]. Additionally, De
Groot and Steg [41] found that the cooperation between EPC and AR positively affected people’s
Sustainability 2019, 11, 2666 4 of 19

acceptance of several energy policies. Moreover, Guagnano [50] reported that the cooperation between
EPC and AR leads people to buy recycled paper products. From this perspective, we can suggest that
the cooperation of EPC and AR may boost people’s AR for energy saving and develop further research
on the relationship between EPC and AR. According to Han’s study, evidence has also been produced
through the empirical method, given the factors suggesting that EPC can affect AR, which can influence
PEN to promote pro-environmental customer behavioral intentions (PCBI) [3]. Continuing the research
stream on the relationships among variables within the norm activation model framework, this study
proposes that EPC influences AR and PEN and that AR, in turn, affects PEN and PCBI.

Hypothesis 1 (H1). Environmental problem concern is positively related to ascription of responsibility.

Hypothesis 2 (H2). Environmental problem concern is positively related to personal environmental norm.

Hypothesis 3 (H3). Ascription of responsibility is positively related to personal environmental norm.

Hypothesis 4 (H4). Ascription of responsibility is positively related to pro-environmental customer


behavioral intentions.

2.3. Relationships among Environmental Problem Concern, Ascription of Responsibility, Personal


Environmental Norms, and Pro-Environmental Customer Behavioral Intentions
Environmental problem concern (EPC) refers to a sense of “knowing of the impact of human
behavior on the environment” [50]. Since many environmental problems are slow-paced, complex,
and indirectly related to individuals’ lives, a cognitive limitation occurs that makes it difficult
for people to recognize these problems. People’s indifference to and emotional reactions against
environmental problems work as emotional limitations [51]. Studies have found that the solution
to this is related to the positive effect of pro-environmental behaviors/intentions on EPC [52]. For
instance, people with increased EPC prefer to purchase eco-friendly products or organic foods and
tend to participate in recycling programs [53]. Some people stop using hairspray after learning that
chlorofluorocarbon damages the ozone. In association with the eco-friendly hotel management system,
Chan and Hawkins [54] indicated that increased EPC and understanding of the system might lead to
eco-friendly acts.
A belief that environmental problems will threaten individual values can activate
pro-environmental actions derived from personal environmental norm (PEN). In this case, PEN
creates a general predisposition that influences all kinds of pro-environmental behaviors/intentions [55].
For example, it is known that personal energy waste can create some negative consequences in the
long-term (e.g., power failure, environmental damage, and even global warming). In this case, EPC
can lead individuals who might be concerned to actively approach energy saving [56]. Prior research
also reported that EPC has a significant influence on PEN. For instance, De Groot and Steg [45] found
that respondents with high EPC had higher PEN than respondents with low EPC. Harland et al. [57]
also found that high EPC can positively influence PEN in normal households, inducing the usage of
public transportation and water conservation. Similarly, high EPC can positively influence a hotel
employee’s PEN to save energy [56].
In the norm activation model (NAM), EPC and ascription of responsibility (AR) are emphasized
as key factors that promote one’s personal obligation to engage in altruistic behavior. However,
it is also true that AR was defined as a moderator between PEN and pro-environmental
behaviors/intentions [46,58]. Scholars have found that the relationship between AR and PEN can be
positive [47]. For example, Bamberg et al. [58] showed that AR exerts a strong positive effect on PEN in
the context of car use. Han et al. [59] found a positive influence of AR on personal norms when travelers
intended to stay at an environmentally responsible lodging. Stern’s [22] value-belief-norm (VAN) theory
was an attempt to link the NAM to the relationship between general values and pro-environmental
intentions/behaviors. The VAN is thereby also an integrative theory in itself and it assumes that
Sustainability 2019, 11, 2666 5 of 19

pro-environmental intentions/behaviors can be directly determined by PEN, which is based on the


NAM. In addition, Stern assumed that PEN has to be activated by AR and EPC. However, he also
assumed EPC as a prerequisite of AR. According to the VBN theory, EPC is related to one’s general
ecological worldview, which is measured by the new environmental paradigm [60]. In the proposed
research model, PEN is activated by EPC and AR; moreover, EPC is posited to be an antecedent of
AR. People with AR are likely to feel responsible for their acts and be less likely to perform certain
behaviors [34]. A person’s intention to choose organic food is formulated by attitude, subjective norms,
perceived behavioral control, and PEN [61,62]. Privileging organic food, in turn, promotes visiting
restaurants that feature organic menu items [34].
Ultimately, PEN is activated by a certain level of EPC and people’s feeling of responsibility to
alleviate the problem. When people understand the severity of an environmental problem, they are
more likely to undertake pro-environmental behavior [29]. For instance, people who are aware of the
relationship between car usage and air pollution develop a sense of responsibility for alleviating air
pollution, which leads them to reduce their car use [63]. Likewise, the NAM has been successfully
used to explain many kinds of pro-environmental behaviors, such as willingness to choose organic
food [64], intentions to revisit an eco-cruise [3,7], intentions to reduce energy use [59,65], and recycling
behaviors [44,66–68]. According to Schwartz [23], behaviors reflect a person’s internalized value
system, and certain behaviors are performed when the relevant values and norms are about to be
activated. The NAM posits that people with EPC and AR for their activities are more likely to display
altruistic behavior. When EPC and AR are present, people tend to act in ways that benefit others [69].
Accordingly, we hypothesize:

Hypothesis 5 (H5). Personal environmental norm is positively related to pro-environmental customer


behavioral intentions.

2.4. Relationships among Environmental Problem Concern, Ascription of Responsibility, Social Environmental
Norms, and Pro-Environmental Customer Behavioral Intentions
Environmental problem concern (EPC) is a concept interest in environmental problems and/or
consequences [70]. Social environmental norm (SEN) can be compared to ethos, an air that induces
people to have certain environmental problem concern (EPC). SEN is mentioned as an essential concept
that helps explain individuals’ pro-environmental customer behavioral intentions (PCBI) [2,3,9,67,69,70].
Moreover, in the examination of environmentally-friendly tourism behaviors, SEN has been used as
an index of individual behavior [32,47,68–75]. The norm activation model (NAM) proposes that SEN
can influence an individual’s actual behaviors through its influence on EPC [23]. In conjunction with
self-concept, personal environmental norm (PEN) is expressed as a moral obligation to perform a
certain behavior [23,76]. It can be interpreted as “a longing to do certain acts” and a behavior motivated
by a will to act according to one’s values [77]. In this case, SEN promotes a compulsion derived from
society’s current ethos to engage in certain acts, like environmental protection. For example, a tidy and
organized space creates an ethos in itself that encourages people to keep that place unsoiled. The role
of injunctive social norms is to suggest to people that they must follow specific social rules or there
would be a sanction [78]. A person who visits a tourist site may avoid littering for these reasons.
Experimental studies have shown that SEN can have powerful effects on willingness to engage in
pro-environmental behavior [43,79], and a significant relationship between SEN and PEN and their
impact on environmentally-friendly purchasing intentions have been demonstrated in the context of
hospitality [3]. According to the NAM, SEN and PEN are interrelated; SEN influences PEN to modify
individuals’ actual behaviors [23]. On the other hand, personal norms are defined as individuals’ own
beliefs linked to their self-concepts. The NAM suggests various ways to understand SEN’s effect on
pro-environmental intentions/behaviors [30,78,80]. The NAM was widely used to shed light on “a
decision-making process through which personal and social norms mediate the influences of general
values on altruistic and/or helping behavior” [46] in the context of pro-environmental behavior [41].
Sustainability 2019, 11, 2666 6 of 19

However, ever since the potential of the NAM’s extensive use in the environmental domain was
uncovered [81], behaviors such as the burning of garden waste [60], recycling [44,49,67], and energy
conservation [82] have been studied using the NAM.
Most studies interpret the NAM using either a mediation model or a moderation model. The
mediation model assumes that EPC influences PEN through ascription of responsibility (AR) [33,45].
In the moderation model, PEN’s influence on behavior is moderated by EPC and AR. In this study, we
interpret the NAM as a mediation model, as De Groot and Steg [45] provided strong evidence of the
NAM as a mediation model via five recent studies comparing the two models.
Researchers proposing a mediation model assume that PEN and AR have indirect effects on
intentions and behaviors via PEN [66,83,84]. More specifically, PEN is assumed to mediate the
relationship between AR and pro-social intentions and behaviors, and AR is assumed to mediate the
relationship between EPC and PEN. This interpretation of the NAM has been supported by several
studies [45]. Stern et al. [85] showed that PEN can be predicted by the level of AR. It has also been
shown that PEN can be a significant predictor of several pro-environmental behaviors. The application
of the NAM enabled the identification of PEN as a mediator of behaviors’ situational factors [29], and
environmental studies found influential factors of pro-environmental behaviors. For instance, Vining
and Ebreo [49] found that EPC influenced recycling behavior even when PEN’s central role was not yet
identified. Moreover, some researchers suggest that PEN mediates the relationship between all of the
NAM’s independent variable components including EPC, AR, and PEN. In addition, SEN is generally
believed to be an effective predictor of PEN and PCBI [2,3,7,10,47]. According to Kim et al. [86],
SEN effectively mediates the relationship between green identity and customers’ purchase intentions.
Ultimately, it can be assumed that EPC influences SEN while SEN affects PCBI and PEN within our
conceptual framework. Hence, the following hypotheses were formulated:

Hypothesis 6 (H6). Environmental problem concern is positively related to social environmental norm.

Hypothesis 7 (H7). Social environmental norm is positively related to pro-environmental customer


behavioral intentions.

Hypothesis 8 (H8). Social environmental norm is positively related to personal environmental norm.

Hypothesis 9 (H9). Ascription of responsibility, social environmental norm, and personal environmental norm
significantly mediate the relationship between environmental problem concern and pro-environmental customer
behavioral intentions.

2.5. Moderating Effect of Overall Green Image


According to Martineau [87], “store image” is the reflection of a shopper’s awareness about
a store, partly based on function and partly based on its atmosphere made up of psychological
attributes. Subsequent studies have focused on the relationship between store image and customers’
perceptions. Moreover, brand image creates brand awareness, which prompts consumers to make
purchase decisions [88]. Ur and Ishaq [89] proved that store image has a significant moderating
effect on the relationship between brand image and purchase intention. Furthermore, store image
became known as a manipulator of consumers’ price assessments, perceptions, satisfaction, and
intentions/behaviors. Therefore, store image can be treated as a critical concept in consumer behavior
research [36]. In addition, Bloemer and De Ruyter [36] have insisted that store image is a firm’s most
important attribute, alluding to consumers’ core element of overall perceptions of a firm. According
to Keller [90], “firm image” is “a series of perceptions about a firm as reflected by its associations in
consumers’ memories”.
Customers’ perceptions of a store’s overall image can be converted to overall green image
(OGI) [36]. Wang et al. [37] evaluated whether customers perceived a restaurant’s green image as its
green practices. OGI, which refers to the tendency to perform eco-friendly practices, can be used in
Sustainability 2019, 11, 2666 7 of 19

brand advertising since the consumers’ minds can personally link the green image with environmental
commitments and concerns [91]. Customers in the hotel industry with OGI are more likely to engage
in eco-friendly practices [38]. Similarly, a restaurant’s OGI can relate consumers’ perceptions of the
restaurant with pro-environmental customer behavioral intentions (PCBI).
In consumer behavior, scholars have stressed the role of image for firms and products.
Jeong et al. [38] tested the impacts of eco-friendly practices on a cafe’s green image and its customers’
attitudes. The results showed that a green image fostered positive customer attitudes toward the café,
eventually affecting consumption decisions. Han [5] suggested that enhancing a hotel’s green image
can manipulate customers’ pro-environmental intentions, which affects consumption decisions. Thus,
this strategy can significantly benefit hoteliers in the green hotel industry. Based on these research
findings, it can be assumed that the strength of the relationship between ascription of responsibility
(AR), personal environmental norm (PEN), social environmental norm (SEN), and pro-environmental
customer behavioral intentions (PCBI) depends on the level of overall green image. Based on the
discussion of the relationship between SEN, PEN, AR, and PCBI, three hypotheses are proposed
as follows:

Hypothesis 10a (H10a). Overall green image has a significant moderating role in the relationship between
ascription of responsibility and pro-environmental customer behavioral intentions.

Hypothesis 10b (H10b). Overall green image has a significant moderating role in the relationship between
personal environmental norm and pro-environmental customer behavioral intentions.

Hypothesis 10c (H10c). Overall green image has a significant moderating role in the relationship between
social environmental norm and pro-environmental customer behavioral intentions.

3. Materials and Methods

3.1. Measures and Questionnaire


The study’s online questionnaire included two sections. The first part of the survey requested
demographic information (e.g., gender, marital status, age, educational level, annual income level,
purpose of visits, and number of visits). The second part of the conceptual model constructs
environmental problem concern (EPC), ascription of responsibility (AR), personal environmental norm
(PEN), social environmental norm (SEN), overall green image (OGI), and pro-environmental customer
behavioral intentions (PCBI). The measurement scales can be found in the literature [2,3,10,22,23,29–
31,34]. Specifically, EPC featured three items (e.g., “I try not to buy from a brand of coffee shop that
strongly pollutes”) adopted from Choi et al. [1]; AR featured four items (e.g., “I feel jointly responsible
for the exhaustion of energy sources”) obtained from Steg and De Groot [31]; PEN featured three
items (e.g., “I feel an obligation to visit environmentally responsible coffee shops rather than regular
coffee shops”) adopted from Berenguer [24]; SEN featured three items (e.g., “Most people who are
important to me would believe that I visit an environmentally responsible coffee shop”) obtained from
Han et al. [5]; OGI featured three items (e.g., “My overall impression regarding a pro-environmental
coffee shop is important”) adopted from Han, Hsu, and Lee [5]; and, finally, PCBI featured eight items
(e.g., “To protect the environment, I would expend effort to visit an environmentally responsible coffee
shop rather than a general coffee shop in the future”) obtained from Han et al.[3]. The measurement
items used in this study are presented in the Appendix A. A seven-point Likert scale was used for all
the measurement items in this research, ranging from “Extremely disagree” (1) to “Extremely agree”
(7). In sum, we used 24 items for the assessment of the six variables in this study.

3.2. Data Collection and the Sample


The questionnaire was conducted with general coffee shop customers in Seoul, Republic of Korea
via an online, self-administered questionnaire between 4 and 24 February 2019. In order to reduce the
Sustainability 2019, 11, 2666 8 of 19

common method bias (CMB) [92,93], procedural remedies were used in this study. First, in order to
improve scale items before participants received the questionnaire, we supplemented the ambiguous
expressions and questions under the guidance of five professors affiliated to Department of Foodservice
Management from 21–24 January 2019. In addition, to reduce the CMB, the questionnaire was designed
to protect respondent anonymity and reduce evaluation apprehension. Moreover, the question order
was counterbalanced. Survey distribution and collection were performed with the help of Embrain, an
online survey company that provides reliable access to a large participant pool of randomly selected,
voluntary participants from national consumer panel groups. In addition, our survey questionnaire
was distributed to various coffee shop customers in Seoul, Republic of Korea who had visited a coffee
shop in the month prior to the survey. After the exclusion of insincere and inappropriate responses, a
total of 530 usable responses were collected. These cases were used to evaluate the adequacy and test
the hypothesized relationships of the proposed theoretical framework.
Regarding the demographic characteristics of the 530 participants, 57.9% (n = 307) were female
customers and 42.1% (n = 223) were male customers. Moreover, approximately 55.7% (n = 295) of the
respondents were single while 37.9% were married (n = 201), 4.2% were divorced (n = 22), 0.9% were
separated (n = 5), and 2.1% were widowed (n = 7). In addition, 79.4% (n = 421) of respondents were
20–40 years old, and the remaining 20.6% (n = 109) were over 40 years old. Their levels of education
were 2-year college (43.6%, n = 231), 4-year college (33.4%, n = 231), high school graduate and below
(18.7%, n = 99), and postgraduate or higher (4.4%, n = 23). Most of the respondents had an annual
income of 20,000,000–29,999,999 won (35.1%, n = 186), followed by 20,000,000 won or below (24.7%,
n = 24.7), 30,000,000–39,999,999 won (23.8%, n = 126), and 40,000,000 won and over (17.4%, n = 87). The
most common responses regarding the purpose of coffee shop visits were studying (38.9%, n = 206),
meeting friends (28.3%, n = 150), business meetings (23.2%, n = 123), and private time (9.6%, n = 51).
The numbers of visits to coffee shops per week were 1–2 times (44.2%, n = 234), 3–4 times (39.8%,
n = 211), 5–6 times (13.2%, n = 70), and 6+ times (2.9%, n = 15).

3.3. Data Analysis and Tools


To analyze the data, this study used IBM SPSS 20.0 program (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA)
and AMOS 24.0 program (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). First of all, demographic characteristics
and correlation were used by IBM SPSS 20.0 program. Furthermore, the measurement model with
confirmatory factor analysis (CFA), construct structural equation modeling (SEM), chi-square difference
test for modeling comparison, and metric invariance for moderating effect was evaluated by AMOS
24.0 program.

4. Results

4.1. Common Method Variance


This study intended to mitigate possible common method variance (CMV) by using two procedural
remedies in the survey design and using the analysis method. First of all, this study used different cover
stories when the respondents answered using the same scale to facilitate the psychological separation
between criterion variables and predictors. For example, the cover story for the pro-environmental
concern scale was “The following statements are irrelevant to the above questions. Please read carefully
each statement and then mark from extremely disagree to agree with your recent feeling”. Another
method of reducing CMV was dealing with item ambiguity, as suggested by [94]. For example, this
study provided specific definitions of imprecise terms to help respondents’ comprehension. Moreover,
the results of Harman’s one-factor analysis, which is a post hoc test to detect possible CMV [94],
showed that CMV of the unmeasured latent methods factor was 1.1% and the one-factor measurement
model was confirmed to fit the data satisfactorily (goodness-of-fit statistics for the measurement model:
χ2 = 237.284, df = 121, p < 0.001, χ2 /df = 1.961, RMSEA = 0.043, CFI = 0.983, IFI = 0.983, TLI = 0.976).
Sustainability 2019, 11, 2666 9 of 19

This study made an effort to reduce CMV by employing procedural remedies in the survey design
stage. Therefore, CMV did not influence the parameter estimations.

4.2. Reliability and Validity Assessments and Confirmatory Factor Analysis


The study’s measurement model was generated by conducting a confirmatory factory analysis.
We used the item parceling strategy (IPS) to increase the scale’s reliability [95,96]. If the measurement
index is composed of multiple items, the items are grouped into two, three, or more groups according
to structural concepts, and then the respective items are summed or averaged to be used as indicators
of the new item bundles (parcels) method [97]. Thus, to construct a balanced method, we used eight
items in three factors based on the PCBI factor load derived from the items’ confirmatory factor analysis.
A maximum likelihood estimation approach was used for the analysis. The model was confirmed to fit
the data satisfactorily (goodness-of-fit statistics for the measurement model: χ2 = 327.878, df = 137,
p <0.000, χ2 /df = 2.393, RMSEA = 0.051, CFI = 0.972, IFI = 0.972, TLI = 0.965). The RMSEA should
range between 0.04 and 0.08 [98] and the TLI, IFI, and CFI values should be close to 1.00, which
indicates an acceptable fit [99]. The average variance extracted (AVE) value was then estimated. The
calculation of the AVE revealed that all AVE values exceeded the minimum threshold of 0.50 [100] and
they ranged from 0.928 to 0.968 (as displayed in Table 1). These values were greater than the square of
the correlation between two variables. Therefore, discriminant validity was supported. The construct
reliability (CR) for the six study constructs was also calculated. The values of CR ranged from 0.974 to
0.990 (as shown in Table 1). These values highly exceeded Bagozzi and Yi’s [101] suggested cutoff of
0.70, thus supporting convergent validity.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the constructs and correlations.

Construct and Standardized MEAN AVE


EPC AR PEN SEN OGI PCBI
Scale Item Loading (SD) (CR)
EPC1 0.797
3.888 0.928 - - - - -
EPC EPC2 0.882 1
(1.183) (0.974)
EPC3 0.773
AR1 0.873
AR2 0.826 4.159 0.928 0.657 *** - - - -
AR 1
AR3 0.849 (0.999) (0.974) (0.431)
AR4 0.741
PEN1 0.832
4.236 0.940 0.699 *** 0.770 *** - - -
PEN PEN2 0.791 1
(1.016) (0.979) (0.489) (0.593)
PEN3 0.753
SEN1 0.821
4.090 0.935 0.660 *** 0.628 *** 0.834 *** - -
SEN SEN2 0.742 1
(1.005) (0.977) (0.435) (0.394) (0.696)
SEN3 0.786
OGI1 0.781
4.045 0.953 0.495 *** 0.498 *** 0.613 *** 0.519 *** -
OGI OGI2 0.835 1
(1.019) (0.984) (0.245) (0.248) (0.376) (0.269)
OGI3 0.889
WOMI 0.890
4.077 0.964 0.576 *** 0.664 *** 0.739 *** 0.730 *** 0.755 ***
PCBI WTPI 0.912 1
(0.982) (0.988) (0.332) (0.441) (0.546) (0.633) (0.570)
RI 0.755
Note 1. EPC = environmental problem concern, AR = ascription of responsibility, PEN = personal environmental
norm, SEN = social environmental norm, PCBI = pro-environmental customer behavioral intentions, OGI = overall
green image, AVE = average variance extracted, AVE = average variance extracted, CR = composite reliability,
SD = standard deviation. Note 2. Goodness-of-fit statistics for the measurement model: χ2 = 327.878, df = 137,
p < 0.001, χ2 /df = 2.393, RMSEA = 0.051, CFI = 0.972, IFI = 0.972, TLI = 0.965. Note 3. All factor loadings are
significant at *** p < 0.001.

4.3. Research Hypotheses Testing and Structural Equation Modeling (SEM)


The structural equation model (SEM) was generated using the hypothesized interrelationships
among variables and the maximum likelihood estimation method. Moreover, SEM analysis was
performed by using the maximum likelihood method [99] as an estimation method for the evaluation
of both the model and procedures. SEM was shown to fit the data adequately (goodness-of-fit statistics
for the structural model: χ2 = 323.864, df = 96, p < 0.001, χ2 /df = 3.374, RMSEA = 0.069, CFI = 0.959,
Sustainability 2019, 11, 2666 10 of 19

IFI = 0.959, TLI = 0.949). Moreover, SEM showed high prediction power for pro-environmental
customer behavioral intentions (PCBI) in general (R2 = 0.599). The standardized path coefficients and
t-values are shown in Table 2. In addition, the hypothesis test results are provided in Figure 1. The
path estimates show that environmental problem concern (EPC) had a significantly positive direct
effect on ascription of responsibility (AR) (β = 0.721, p < 0.001); thus, H1 was supported. The result
of estimation indicated that EPC had a not significant effect on personal environmental norm (PEN);
thus, H2 was not supported. Moreover, H3 was not supported because EPC was not positively and
significantly associated with PEN. The result of estimation indicated that AR had a significant positive
effect on PCBI (β = 0.273, p < 0.001); thus, H4 was supported. The impact of PEN on PCBI (β = 0.384,
p < 0.001) was assessed; thus, H5 was supported. The proposed impact of EPC on social environmental
norm (SEN) was assessed. As expected, EPC had an impact on SEN (β = 0.761, p < 0.01); thus, H6 was
supported. The influence of SEN on PCBI was also evaluated. It was found that SEN had a significant
positive effect on PCBI (β = 0.230, p < 0.01); thus, H7 was supported. As expected, the link between
SEN and PEN was significant (β SEN-PEN = 0.384, p < 0.01); thus, H8 was supported. Moreover, the
results of the analysis on mediating effects are shown by significant indirect effects in Table 3. This
study used the bootstrapping by [102] to verify the significance of the mediation effect. The findings
revealed that EPC significantly affected PCBI (β EPC → AR & SEN & PEN → PCBI = 0.640, p < 0.001)
indirectly through AR, SEN, and PEN, thus confirming all of them as partial mediating variables. Thus,
H9 was supported.

Table 2. Structural model results and hypotheses testing.

Hypothesized Paths Coefficients t-Values


H1: EPC → AR 0.721 13.426 ***
H2: EPC → PEN 0.128 1.492
H3: AR → PEN 0.038 1.765
H4: AR → PCBI 0.273 6.106 ***
H5: PEN → PCBI 0.384 4.437 ***
H6: EPC → SEN 0.761 15.109 ***
H7: SEN → PCBI 0.230 2.652 **
H8: SEN → PEN 0.384 4.437 ***
Explained variable: R2 (AR) = 0.520 R2 (PEN) = 0.580
R2 (PCBI) = 0.599 R2 (SEN) = 0.701
Note 1. EPC = environmental problem concern, AR = ascription of responsibility, PEN = personal environmental
norm, SEN = social environmental norm, PCBI = pro-environmental customer behavioral intentions, AVE = average
variance extracted, CR = composite reliability, SD = standard deviation. Note 2. Goodness-of-fit statistics for
the structural model: χ2 = 323.864, df = 96, χ2 /df = 3.374, RMSEA = 0.069, CFI = 0.959, IFI = 0.959, TLI = 0.949.
** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.

Table 3. Results of mediation effect ascription of responsibility, personal environmental norm, and
social environmental norm.

Indirect Effect: β(EPC → AR & PEN & SEN → PCBI) = 0.640 ***
Total Effect on PCBI: β(SEN) = 0.640 ***
The Results: H9: supported
Note 1. EPC = environmental problem concern, AR = ascription of responsibility, PEN = personal environmental
norm, SEN = social environmental norm, PCBI = pro-environmental customer behavioral intentions, AVE = average
variance extracted, CR = composite reliability, SD = standard deviation. *** p < 0.001.
Sustainability 2019, 11, 2666 11 of 19

Figure 1. Structural equation model estimation and test for structural metric invariance. Note 1.
EPC = environmental problem concern, AR = ascription of responsibility, PEN = personal environmental
norms, SEN = social environmental norms, PCBI = pro-environmental customer behavioral intentions,
OGI = overall green image, WOMI = word of mouth intention, WTPI = willingness to pay intention,
SI = sacrifice intention. Note 2. Goodness-of-fit statistics for the measurement model: χ2 = 327.878,
df = 137, p < 0.001, χ2 /df = 2.393, RMSEA = 0.051, CFI = 0.972, IFI = 0.972, TLI = 0.965. Note 3.
χ
Goodness-of-fit statistics for the structural model: χ2 = 323.864, df = 96, p < 0.001, χ2 /dfχ= 3.374,
RMSEA = 0.069, CFI = 0.959, IFI = 0.959, TLI = 0.949. Note 4. Goodness-of-fit statistics for the baseline
χ
model: χ2 = 409.868, df = 192, p < 0.001, χ2 /df = 2.135, RMSEA = 0.046, CFI = χ0.953, IFI = 0.954,
TLI = 0.942. Note 5. Two identical structural models were evaluated (model for high- [n = 322] and
low- [n = 207] OGI groups). χ χ

4.4. Moderating Effect of Overall Green Image


To assess the proposed moderating impact of overall green image (OGI), a test for metric invariance
was conducted. In this case, a continuous variable—such as an interval scale or ratio scale—was used
as a control variable to evaluate a metric variable. The sample was divided into high (n = 322) and low
(n = 207) overall image groups through K-mean cluster analysis. Since the significance probability for
the nested model was 0.152, which exceeds the significance level of 0.05, OGI was found to have an
effect. Moreover, the baseline model showed an acceptable level for data suitability (goodness-of-fit
statistics for the baseline model: χ2 = 409.868, df = 192, p < 0.001, χ2 /df = 2.135, RMSEA = 0.046,
CFI = 0.953, IFI = 0.954, TLI = 0.942). The details are shown in Table 4 and Figure 1. The chi-square test
results with measurement invariance revealed that the path from AR to pro-environmental customer
behavioral intentions (PCBI) for coffee shop practices was not significantly different between groups
(∆χ2 = 0.199, p > 0.05). However, while the ascription of the responsibility (AR)–PCBI link for the
low-OGI group (β = 0.335, p < 0.05) was significant, the AR–PCBI path for the high-OGI group was
not significant. Thus, the chi-square test results did not differ significantly across groups; thus, H10a
was supported. The study’s findings also indicated that, although the relationship between personal
environmental norm (PEN) and PCBI differed significantly between the high (β = 0.306, p < 0.001)
and low (β = 0.189, p < 0.05) OGI groups, there was no significant difference in the PEN–PCBI link
(∆χ2 = 0.402, p > 0.05); thus, H10b was not supported. Moreover, the study uncovered that the
SEN–PCBI link was significantly different between the high- (β = 0.495, p < 0.001) and low (β = 0.324,
p < 0.05) OGI groups, but that there was no significant difference in the SEN–PCBI link (∆χ2 = 2.501,
p > 0.05); thus, H10c was not supported.
Sustainability 2019, 11, 2666 12 of 19

Table 4. Results of the moderating effect of overall green image.

High-OGI Group (n = 322) Low-OGI Group (n = 207) Baseline Model Nested Model
Paths
Coefficients t-Values Coefficients t-Values (Freely Estimated) (Constrained to Be Equal)

H10a: AR–PCBI 0.006 0.050 0.335 2.096 * χ2 (192) = 409.868 χ2 (193) = 410.067a
H10b: PEN–PCBI 0.306 4.805 *** 0.189 2.582 * χ2 (192) = 409.868 χ2 (193) = 410.270b
H10c: SEN–PCBI 0.496 4.292 *** 0.324 2.047 * χ2 (192) = 409.868 χ2 (193) = 412.369c
Chi-square difference test: Test results: Goodness-of-fit statistics for the baseline model:
a ∆χ2 (1) = 0.199, p > 0.05 H10a: Supported χ2 = 409.868, df = 192, p < 0.001, χ2 /df = 2.135,
b ∆χ2 (1) = 0.402, p > 0.05 H10b: Not supported RMSEA = 0.046, CFI = 0.953, IFI = 0.954, TLI = 0.942.
c ∆χ2 (1) = 2.501, p > 0.05 H10c: Not supported * p < 0.05, *** p < 0.001

Note 1. AR = ascription of responsibility, PEN = personal environmental norms, SEN = social environmental norms,
PCBI = pro-environmental customer behavioral intentions, OGI = overall green image.

5. Discussion and Implications


The study’s proposed conceptual model was designed to clearly explain pro-environmental
customer behavioral intentions (PCBI) of coffee shop customers. In the eco-friendly coffee shop context,
there is limited scholarly research on PCBI for environmentally responsible practices during coffee shop
visits. The proposed theoretical framework comprising coffee shop practices of environmental problem
concern (EPC) as independent variables; ascription of responsibility (AR), personal environmental
norms (PEN), and social environmental norms (SEN) as mediators; and overall green image (OGI)
as a moderator was demonstrated to be useful and to satisfactorily predict PCBI. The hypothesized
relationships among study constructs were generally supported, but PEN was not affected by EPC or
AR. A significant mediating impact of PEN, SEN, and AR was also found. Moreover, the test for metric
invariance demonstrated OGI’s moderating role. This study’s findings generally provided a study
framework for research constructs and their relationships in the area of eco-friendly coffee shops and
customers’ pro-environmental behaviors. Thus, useful theoretical implications for research related to
PCBI and the practical implications of PCBI are suggested below.

5.1. Theoretical Implications


This study has several theoretical implications. First, the research establishes a specific set of
consumer pro-environmental psychographics (concern, responsibility, and norms) as clear predictors
of PCBI, which expands the current literature and provides guidelines for a clearer understanding
of PCBI. In this sense, for instance, it can be deduced that increased consumption of organic coffee
and foods in the region can lead to a concurrent increase in the use of recycled coffee cups [15–17].
Second, the original NAM has been used for over 40 years; however, more recently, the expanded
version of the norm activation model (NAM) [3,4,29,33] based on theory of planned behavior (TPB)
and value-belief-norm (VBN) has become popular. This study builds upon the original NAM, showing
EPC’s influence on PEN and SEN and its correlated effect on PCBI. This means that the effect of the
relationship between PEN and SEN on PCBI provides a basis for future research examining PCBI in
relation to PEN and SEN. Third, the study’s empirical results indicate that PEN has the strongest direct
effect on PCBI. The importance of PEN in explaining individuals’ pro-environmental intentions and
behaviors has been demonstrated by various studies [10,27,48]. These findings suggested that PEN
was a major factor in determining PCBI. Fourth, by verifying the mediating effects of the NAM (SEN,
PEN, and AR), this study supports a number of previous studies [3,4,29,31,45,66,88] in contrast to other
previous studies [3,46,57,62] that used moderating effects. Fifth, this study reveals OGI’s moderating
role in the effects of AR, PEN, and SEN on PCBI, legitimizing OGI and supporting its inclusion in the
model of PCBI as an important situational factor. This study is the first to apply the OGI variable that
affects the eco-friendly behavior of coffee shop customers to the NAM.
Sustainability 2019, 11, 2666 13 of 19

5.2. Practical Implications


This study offers a number of practical implications for improving PCBI that can provide insights
to coffee shop managers. First of all, the study’s findings can inform coffee shops managers that the
enhancement of EPC has a significant impact on AR and PCBI but no effect on PEN by pro-environmental
decision-making. These results are different from those of the extended NAM. In the extended NAM,
EPC, AR, and PEN all play an essential role in PCBI [4,28,34,45,47–50]. EPC and AR are highlighted
as key factors that promote one’s personal obligation to altruistic behavior. Stern [22] assumed EPC
as a prerequisite of AR. People with AR are likely to feel responsible for their acts and be less likely
to perform certain behaviors [34]. That is, people’s intentions to choose pro-environmental coffee
shops are formulated by attitudes, subjective norms, perceived behavioral control, and PEN [58,59].
Therefore, in order to raise customers’ AR and induce them to visit eco-friendly coffee shops, managers
have to ensure sustainable management and inform customers about it.
Secondly, SEN has a significant positive effect on PEN. This supports the existing research [3,4,
10,28,32,47,58]. According to Han’s [27] study, a significant relationship between social and personal
norms and environmentally-friendly purchasing intentions has been demonstrated in the context of
hospitality. In addition, the NAM has shown that the SEN and PEN are interrelated and that SEN
influences PEN to modify individuals’ actual behaviors [23]. Therefore, given SEN’s strong influence
on PEN, it is important to recognize the problem of the social environment. For example, we now
know that SEN has a strong impact on PEN. Therefore, we can insist that increasing PEN is possible by
the recognition of individuals/coffee shop managers/social environmental activists of EPC in advance
and related to SEN.
Third, the study results show that the effects of factors on pro-environmental behavior intention
have a descending order, from PEN to AR to SEN. This fact indicates that SEN’s effect on customer
behavior is relatively lower than that of PEN, which is actually the most effective factor. Therefore,
coffee shop managers should recognize the importance of environmental protection in order to raise
the PEN of their customers.
Fourth, we can predict that an increase in coffee shop customers’ AR can influence their
pro-environmental behavior intention. In other words, AR may generate pro-environmental
intentions/behavior among people, which will also lead to responsibility regarding the use of disposable
products or plastic and its consequences. Thus, pro-environmental coffee-shop managers should run
related campaigns to raise AR of customers and enhance the value of environmental protection in our
society. Thereby, the number of coffee-shops under sustainable management can be further expanded
to encourage pro-environmental customer.
Lastly, OGI for eco-friendly coffee shops did not moderate the effect of the relationship between
SEN and PEN on PCBI. This implies that if SEN and PEN are given, it affects PCBI regardless of coffee
shops’ OGI. That is, when customers visiting coffee shops recognize EPC, they show PCBI irrespective
of the coffee shops’ OGI. However, those with low OGI in eco-friendly coffee shops can see that the
behavior of eco-friendly customers is higher based on their AR for the environment. Therefore, we can
see that the behavior of low-OGI groups can positively enhance PCBI. In other words, the efforts of
coffee shop enterprises to protect and preserve the environment are important, but if the consumers try
to raise their environmental responsibility, AR for the environment can be improved.

5.3. Limitations and Future Studies


Some limitations are as follows: First, future research should consider the impact of
national/cultural differences on the proposed theoretical framework. This study was conducted
at coffee shops located in Seoul, Republic of Korea. One of the remaining questions is whether the
results could be applied to other regional/cultural environments such as Japan, China, or other Asian
countries. In addition, a cross-cultural study could be beneficial to investigate PCBI in coffee shops.
Second, most of the moderating effects of OGI were not supported in this study. In this study, OGI
was interpreted as having a mediating effect between AR and PCBI only because the low-OGI group
Sustainability 2019, 11, 2666 14 of 19

showed a significant link between AR and PCBI and the high-OGI group did not show a significant
link between AR and PCBI. Beyond these findings, no study has demonstrated OGI’s mediating effect
through a chi-square test. The present study’s results may or may not indicate a moderating effect
because they can be interpreted in various ways depending on different viewpoints. Therefore, in
the future, it will be possible to provide more precise theoretical and practical implications through
continuous studies on the moderating effects of OGI on the link between EPC and PCBI.
Third, this study did not divide consumers based on their different levels of EPC or the different
levels of service they received from the coffee shops they visited. Future studies could investigate
different consumer profiles based on different levels of EPC and service and then test the same model
to see if any discernible differences between them can be identified.

Author Contributions: Conceptualization, S.Y. and T.K.; methodology, S.Y.; formal analysis, S.Y.; investigation,
T.K.; resources, T.K.; data curation, T.K.; writing—original draft preparation, S.Y. and T.K.; writing—review and
editing, S.Y. and T.K.
Funding: This research received no external funding.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A. Measurement Items

Environmental problem concern


I try not to buy from brand of coffee shop that strongly pollute.
When possible, I systematically choose the coffee shop that has the lowest negative impact on the environment.
When I have the choice between two equivalent brand of coffee shop, I always wonder which one pollutes less
Ascription of responsibility
I feel jointly responsible for the energy problems.
I feel jointly responsible for the exhaustion of energy sources.
I feel jointly responsible for global warming.
My contribution to the energy problem is not negligible.
Personal environmental norm
I feel an obligation to visit an environmentally responsible coffee shops rather than regular coffee shops.
I feel that it is important to make coffee shops environmentally sustainable, reducing the harm to wider
environment.
Regardless of what other people do, because of my own values/principles I feel that I should attend an
environmentally responsible coffee shop.
Social environmental norm
Most people who are important to me think I should visit an environmentally coffee shop.
Most people who are important to me would me to visit with an environmentally responsible coffee shop.
People whose opinions I value would prefer me visit with an environmentally responsible coffee shop.
Pro-environmental customer behavioral intentions
I will encourage my friends and relatives to choose an eco-friendly coffee shop.
If someone is looking for a coffee shop, I will advise him/her to choose an environmentally responsible
coffee shop.
I will say positive things about an environmentally responsible coffee shop.
I am willing to pay coffees shops with an environmentally responsible coffee shop when deciding on visit
coffee shop in the future.
I plan to pay coffee shops by environmentally responsible coffee shop instead of a regular coffee shop in the
future. I switch to other brands for ecological reasons.
I would be willing to pay more for an environmentally responsible coffee shop.
To protect the environment, I would expend effort on visiting by an environmentally responsible coffee shop
instead of a general coffee shop in the future
To protect the environment, I would be willing to accept any inconvenience (e.g., recycling, reducing water/
energy use, decreasing waste, using the recycling coffee cup) in an environmentally responsible coffee shop.
Overall green image
My overall desire to frequent a pro-environmental coffee shop is strong.
My overall impression regarding a pro-environmental coffee shop is important.
My overall desire to visit for the first time a pro-environmental coffee shop is strong.
Sustainability 2019, 11, 2666 15 of 19

References
1. Choi, H.Y.; Jang, J.H.; Kandampully, J. Application of the extended VBN theory to understand consumers’
decisions about green hotels. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 2015, 51, 87–95. [CrossRef]
2. Han, H.S.; Hwang, J.S.; Kim, J.J.; Jung, H.K. Guests’ pro-environmental decision-making process: Broadening
the norm activation framework in a lodging context. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 2015, 47, 96–107. [CrossRef]
3. Han, H.S.; Hwang, J.S.; Lee, M.J.; Kim, J.H. Word-of-mouth, buying, and sacrifice intentions for eco-cruises:
Exploring the function of norm activation and value-attitude-behavior. Tour. Manag. 2019, 70, 30–443.
[CrossRef]
4. Ottman, J. The New Rules of Green Marketing: Strategies, Tools, and Inspiration for Sustainable Branding; Routledge:
Abingdon-on-Thames, UK, 2017.
5. Han, H.S.; Hsu, L.T.; Lee, J.S. Empirical investigation of the roles of attitudes toward green behaviors, overall
image, gender, and age in hotel customers’ eco-friendly decision-making process. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 2009,
28, 519–528. [CrossRef]
6. United Nations Environment Programme. Communities Unite against Plastic Pollution on World
Environment Day. Available online: https://www.unenvironment.org/news-and-stories/press-release/
communities-unite-against-plastic-pollution-world-environment-day (accessed on 5 June 2018).
7. Han, H.S.; Hwang, J.S. Cruise travelers’ environmentally responsible decision-making: An integrative
framework of goal-directed behavior and norm activation process. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 2016, 53, 94–105.
[CrossRef]
8. Laroche, M.; Bergeron, J.; Barbaro-Forleo, G. Targeting consumers who are willing to pay more for
environmentally friendly products. J. Consum. Mark. 2001, 18, 503–520. [CrossRef]
9. Bhuian, S.; Sharma, J.K. Predicting consumer pro-environmental behavioral intention: The moderating role
of religiosity. Rev. Int. Bus. Strategy 2017, 27, 352–368. [CrossRef]
10. Chen, M.F.; Tung, P.J. Developing an extended theory of planned behavior model to predict consumers’
intention to visit green hotels. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 2014, 3, 221–230. [CrossRef]
11. Kim, Y.J.; Njite, D.; Hancer, M. Anticipated emotion in consumers’ intentions to select eco-friendly restaurants:
Augmenting the theory of planned behavior. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 2013, 34, 255–262. [CrossRef]
12. Jang, Y.J.; Woo, G.K.; Lee, H.Y. Coffee shop consumers’ emotional attachment and loyalty to green stores:
The moderating role of green consciousness. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 2015, 44, 146–156. [CrossRef]
13. BBC News. Single-Use Coffee Cups: Wales Urged to Bring in ‘Latte Levy’. Available online: https:
//www.bbc.com/news/uk-wales-politics-46918869 (accessed on 20 January 2019).
14. FOX NEWS. Berkeley Follows Plastic Straw Ban by Charging Diners an Extra Quarter for Disposable Cups.
Available online: http://www.foxnews/us/berketley-followa-plastic-staw-ban-by-changing-diners-an-extra-
quarter-for-disposable-cups (accessed on 24 January 2019).
15. strarucucks.com. Environment: Pioneering Sustainable Solutions. Available online: https://www.starbucks.
com/responsibility/environment (accessed on 27 January 2019).
16. Dinkins’s Brand. Sustainable Packaging. Available online: https://www.dunkinbrands.com/community/
corporate-social-responsibility/our-planet (accessed on 27 January 2019).
17. costa.co.uk. Great Taste without the Waste. Available online: https://www.costa.co.uk/responsibility/
environment (accessed on 27 January 2019).
18. Bhuian, S.N.; Sharma, S.K.; Butt, I.; Ahmardm, Z.U. Antecedents and pro-environmental consumer behavior
(PECB): The moderating role of religiosity. J. Consum. Mark. 2018, 35, 287–299. [CrossRef]
19. Lita, R.P.; Surya, S.; Maruf, M.; Syahurl, L. Green attitude and behavior of local tourists towards hotels and
restaurants in West Sumatra, Indonesia. Procedia Environ. Sci. 2014, 20, 261–270. [CrossRef]
20. Swaim, J.A.; Maloni, S.A.; Napshin, S.A. Influences on student intention and behavior toward environmental
sustainability. J. Bus. Ethics 2014, 124, 465–484. [CrossRef]
21. Ajzen, I. The theory of planned behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process 1991, 50, 179–211. [CrossRef]
22. Stern, P.C. New environmental theories: Toward a coherent theory of environmentally significant behavior.
J. Soc. Issues 2000, 50, 407–424. [CrossRef]
23. Schwartz, S.H. Normative influences on altruism. In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology; Academic
Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1977; Volume 10, pp. 221–279.
Sustainability 2019, 11, 2666 16 of 19

24. Berenguer, J. The effect of empathy in environmental moral reasoning. Environ. Behav. 2010, 42, 110–134.
[CrossRef]
25. Kang, J.H.; Tang, J.Y.; Bosselman, R.H. Understanding customer behavior in name-brand Korean coffee
shops: The role of self-congruity and functional congruity. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 2012, 31, 809–818. [CrossRef]
26. Schubert, F. Exploring and Predicting Consumers’ Attitudes and Behaviors toward Green Restaurants.
Master’s Thesis, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, USA, 2008.
27. Han, H.S. The norm activation model and theory-broadening: Individuals’ decision-making on
environmentally-responsible convention attendance. J. Environ. Psychol. 2014, 40, 462–471. [CrossRef]
28. Hunecke, M.; Blobaum, A.; Matthies, E. Responsibility and environment: Ecological norm orientation and
external factors in the domain of travel mode choice behavior. Environ. Behav. 2001, 33, 830–852. [CrossRef]
29. Harland, P.; Staats, H.; Wilke, H.A.M. Situational and personality factors as direct or personal norm mediated
predictors of pro-environmental behavior: Questions derived from norm-activation theory. Basic Appl. Soc.
Psychol. 2007, 29, 323–334. [CrossRef]
30. Milfont, T.L.; Duckitt, J.; Wagner, C. A cross-cultural test of the value–attitude-behavior hierarchy. J. Appl.
Soc. Psychol. 2010, 40, 2791–2813. [CrossRef]
31. Steg, L.; De Groot, J. Explaining prosocial intentions: Testing causal relationships in the norm activation
model. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 2010, 49, 725–743. [CrossRef]
32. Matthies, E.; Selge, S.; Klöckner, C.A. The role of parental behavior for the development of behavior specific
environmental norms—The example of recycling and re-use behavior. J. Environ. Psychol. 2012, 32, 277–284.
[CrossRef]
33. Onwezen, M.C.; Antonides, G.; Bartels, J. The Norm Activation Model: An exploration of the functions of
anticipated pride and guilt in pro-environmental behavior. J. Econ. Psychol. 2013, 39, 141–153. [CrossRef]
34. Zhang, Y.; Wang, Z.; Zhou, G. Antecedents of employee electricity saving behavior in organizations: An
empirical study based on norm activation model. Energy Policy 2013, 62, 1120–1127. [CrossRef]
35. Diekmann, A.; Franzen, A. Environmental concern: A global perspective. In Einstellungen und Verhalten in
der Empirischen Sozialforschung; Springer: Wiesbaden, Germany, 2019; pp. 253–272.
36. Bloemer, J.; Ruyter, K.D. On the relationship between store image, store satisfaction, and store loyalty. Eur. J.
Mark. 1998, 32, 499–513. [CrossRef]
37. Wang, J.; Wang, S.; Xuw, H.; Wang, Y.; Li, J. Green image and consumers’ word-of-mouth intention in the
green hotel industry: The moderating effect of Millennials. J. Clean. Prod. 2018, 181, 426–436. [CrossRef]
38. Jeong, E.H.; Jang, S.C.S.; Day, J.; Ha, S. The impact of eco-friendly practices on green image and customer
attitudes: An investigation in a café setting. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 2014, 41, 10–20. [CrossRef]
39. Hansla, A.; Gamble, A.; Juliusson, A.; Garling, T. The relationships between awareness of consequences,
environmental concern, and value orientations. J. Environ. Psychol. 2008, 28, 1–9. [CrossRef]
40. Mostafa, M.M. Antecedents of Egyptian consumers’ green purchase intentions: A hierarchical multivariate
regression model. J. Int. Consum. Mark. 2006, 19, 97–126. [CrossRef]
41. Pierce, J.C.; Dalton, R.; Zaitsev, A. Public perceptions of environmental conditions. In Critical Masses: Citizens,
Nuclear Weapons Production, and Environmental Destruction in the United States and Russia; MIT: Cambridge,
MA, USA, 1999; pp. 97–129.
42. Stern, P.C.; Dietz, T.; Kalof, L. Value orientations, gender, and environmental concern. Environ. Behav. 1993,
25, 322–348. [CrossRef]
43. Park, S.Y.; Shon, S.H. Exploring the normative influences of social norms on individual environmental
behavior. J. Glob. Sch. Mark. Sci. 2012, 22, 183–194. [CrossRef]
44. Hopper, J.R.; Nielsen, J.M.C. Recycling as altruistic behavior: Normative and behavioral strategies to expand
participation in a community recycling program. Environ. Behav. 1991, 23, 195–220. [CrossRef]
45. De Groot, J.I.M.; Steg, L. Morality and prosocial behavior: The role of awareness, responsibility, and norms
in the norm activation model. J. Soc. Psychol. 2009, 149, 425–449. [CrossRef]
46. Schwartz, S.H.; Howard, J.A. A. A normative decision-making model of altruism. In Altruism and Helping
Behavior; Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1981; pp. 189–211.
47. Klöckner, C.A. A comprehensive model of the psychology of environmental behavior—A meta-analysis.
Glob. Environ. Chang. 2013, 23, 1028–1038. [CrossRef]
Sustainability 2019, 11, 2666 17 of 19

48. Zhang, X.; Geng, G.; Sun, P. Determinants and implications of citizens’ environmental complaints in China:
Integrating theory of planned behavior and norm activation model. J. Clean. Prod. 2017, 166, 148–156.
[CrossRef]
49. Vining, J.; Ebreo, A. Predicting recycling behavior from global and specific environmental attitudes and
changes in recycling opportunities 1. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 1992, 22, 1580–1607. [CrossRef]
50. Guagnano, G.A. Altruism and market-like behavior: An FFanalysis of willingness to pay for recycled paper
products. Popul. Environ. 2001, 22, 425–438. [CrossRef]
51. Kollmuss, A.; Agyeman, J. Mind the gap: Why do people act environmentally and what are the barriers to
pro-environmental behavior? Environ. Educ. Res. 2002, 8, 239–260. [CrossRef]
52. Bronfman, N.; Cisternas, P.; Lopez-Vazquez, E.; Maza, C.; Oyanedel, J. Understanding attitudes and
pro-environmental behaviors in a Chilean community. Sustainability 2015, 7, 14133–14152. [CrossRef]
53. Aman, A.H.L.; Harun, A.; Hussein, Z. The influence of environmental knowledge and concern on green
purchase intention the role of attitude as a mediating variable. Br. J. Arts Soc. Sci. 2012, 7, 145–167.
54. Chan, E.S.W.; Hawkins, R. Attitude towards EMSs in an international hotel: An exploratory case study. Int.
J. Hosp. Manag. 2010, 29, 641–651. [CrossRef]
55. He, X.; Zhan, W. How to activate the moral norm to adopt electric vehicles in China? An empirical study
based on extended norm activation theory. J. Clean. Prod. 2018, 172, 3546–3556. [CrossRef]
56. Wells, V.K.; Taheri, D.; Gregory-Smith, D.; Manika, D. The role of generativity and attitudes on employees’
home and workplace water and energy saving behaviors. Tour. Manag. 2016, 56, 63–74. [CrossRef]
57. Osterhus, T.L. Pro-social consumer influence strategies: When and how do they work? J. Mark. 1997, 61,
16–29. [CrossRef]
58. Bamberg, S.; Ajzen, I.; Schmidt, P. Choice of travel mode in the theory of planned behavior: The roles of past
behavior, habit, and reasoned action. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2003, 25, 175–187. [CrossRef]
59. Han, H.S.; Hsu, L.T.J.; Lee, J.S.; Sheu, C. Are lodging customers ready to go green? An examination of
attitudes, demographics, and eco-friendly intentions. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 2011, 30, 345–355. [CrossRef]
60. Dunlap, R.E. The new environmental paradigm scale: From marginality to worldwide use. J. Environ. Educ.
2008, 40, 3–18. [CrossRef]
61. Asif, M.; Wang, X.; Nasiri, S.; Ayyub, S. Determinant factors are influencing organic food purchase intention
and the moderating role of awareness: A comparative analysis. Food Qual. Prefer. 2018, 63, 144–150.
[CrossRef]
62. Shin, Y.H.; Im, J.; Jung, S.E.; Severt, K. The theory of planned behavior and the norm activation model
approach to consumer behavior regarding organic menus. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 2018, 69, 21–29. [CrossRef]
63. López-Mosquera, N.; Lera-López, F.; Sánchez, M. Key factors to explain recycling, car use, and
environmentally responsible purchase behaviors: A comparative perspective. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 2015,
99, 29–39. [CrossRef]
64. Abrahamse, W.; Steg, W.; Gifford, R.; Vlek, C. Factors influencing car use for commuting and the intention to
reduce it: A question of self-interest or morality? Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2009, 12, 317–324.
[CrossRef]
65. Black, J.S.; Stern, P.C.; Elworth, J.T. Personal and contextual influences on household energy adaptations. J.
Appl. Psychol. 1985, 70, 3–21. [CrossRef]
66. Bratt, C. The impact of norms and assumed consequences on recycling behavior. Environ. Behav. 1999, 31,
630–656. [CrossRef]
67. Park, J.Y.; Ha, S.J. Understanding consumer recycling behavior: Combining the theory of planned behavior
and the norm activation model. Fam. Consum. Sci. Res. J. 2014, 42, 278–291. [CrossRef]
68. Klöckner, C.A.; Ohms, S. The importance of personal norms for purchasing organic milk. Br. Food J. 2009,
111, 1173–1187. [CrossRef]
69. Ebreo, A.; Vining, J.; Cristancho, S. Responsibility for environmental problems and the consequences of
waste reduction: A test of the norm-activation model. J. Environ. Syst. 2003, 29, 219–244. [CrossRef]
70. Weigel, R.H. Environmental attitudes and the prediction of behavior. In Environmental Psychology: Directions
and Perspectives; Praeger: New York, NY, USA, 1983; pp. 257–287.
71. Bamberg, S.; Möser, G. Twenty years after Hines, Hungerford, and Tomera: A new meta-analysis of
psycho-social determinants of pro-environmental behavior. J. Environ. Psychol. 2007, 27, 15–25. [CrossRef]
Sustainability 2019, 11, 2666 18 of 19

72. Berkowitz, L. Social norms, feelings, and other factors affecting helping and altruism. In Advances in
Experimental Social Psychology; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1972; Volume 6, pp. 62–108.
73. Ajzen, I.; Fishbein, M. Attitude-behavior relations: A theoretical analysis and review of empirical research.
Psychol. Bull. 1977, 84, 888–918. [CrossRef]
74. Triandis, H.C. Interpersonal Behavior; Brooks/Cole Pub. Co.: Grove, CA, USA, 1977.
75. Doran, R.; Larsen, S. The relative importance of social and personal norms in explaining intentions to choose
eco-friendly travel options. Int. J. Tour. Res. 2016, 18, 159–166. [CrossRef]
76. Schwartz, S.H. Normative explanations of helping behavior: A critique, proposal, and empirical test. J. Exp.
Soc. Psychol. 1973, 9, 349–364. [CrossRef]
77. Minton, A.P.; Rose, R.L. The effects of environmental concern on environmentally friendly consumer behavior:
An exploratory study. J. Bus. Res. 1997, 40, 37–48. [CrossRef]
78. Cialdini, R.B.; Reno, R.R.; Kallgren, C.A. A focus theory of normative conduct: Recycling the concept of
norms to reduce littering in public places. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1990, 58, 1015–1026. [CrossRef]
79. Nolan, J.M.; Schultz, P.W.; Cialdini, R.B. Normative social influence is under-detected. Personal. Soc. Psychol.
Bull. 2008, 34, 913–923. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
80. Ebreo, A.; Hershey, J.; Vining, J. Reducing solid waste: Linking recycling to environmentally responsible
consumerism. Environ. Behav. 1999, 31, 107–135. [CrossRef]
81. Thøgersen, J. Recycling and morality: A critical review of the literature. Environ. Behav. 1996, 28, 536–558.
[CrossRef]
82. Tyler, T.R.; Orwin, R.; Schurer, L. Defensive denial and high-cost prosocial behavior. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol.
1982, 3, 267–281. [CrossRef]
83. Steg, L.; Dreijerink, L.; Abrahamse, W. Factors influencing the acceptability of energy policies: A test of VBN
theory. J. Environ. Psychol. 2005, 25, 415–425. [CrossRef]
84. Stern, P.C.; Dietz, T. The value basis of environmental concern. J. Soc. Issues 1994, 50, 65–84. [CrossRef]
85. Stern, P.C.; Dietz, T.; Black, J.S. Support for environmental protection: The role of moral norms. Popul.
Environ. 1985, 8, 204–222. [CrossRef]
86. Kim, H.N.; Lee, E.J.; Hur, W.M. The mediating role of norms in the relationship between green identity and
purchase intention of eco-friendly products. Hum. Ecol. Rev. 2012, 19, 125–135.
87. Martineau, P. Sharper focus for the corporate image. Harv. Bus. Rev. 1958, 36, 49–58.
88. Aaker, D.A. Capitalizing on the Value of a Brand Name; The Free Press: New York, NY, USA, 1991.
89. Ur, R.; Ihsan, H.; Ishaq, Z. The Impact of Brand Image on Purchase Intention: Moderating Role of Store
Image in Pakistan’s Retail Sector. IUP J. Brand Manag. 2017, 14, 54–66.
90. Keller, K.L. Conceptualizing, measuring, and managing customer-based brand equity. J. Mark. 1993, 57,
1–22. [CrossRef]
91. Chen, Y.S. The drivers of green brand equity: Green brand image, green satisfaction, and green trust. J. Bus.
Ethics 2010, 93, 307–319. [CrossRef]
92. Han, H.S. Travelers’ pro-environmental behavior in a green lodging context: Converging value-belief-norm
theory and the theory of planned behavior. Tour. Manag. 2015, 47, 164–177. [CrossRef]
93. Campbell, D.T.; Fiske, D.W. Convergent and discriminate validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix.
Psychol. Bull. 1959, 56, 81–105. [CrossRef]
94. MacKenzie, S.B.; Podsakoff, P.M. Common method bias in marketing: Causes, mechanisms, and procedural
remedies. J. Retail. 2012, 88, 542–555. [CrossRef]
95. Cattell, R.B.; Burdsal, C.A., Jr. The radial parcel double factoring design: A solution to the item-vs-parcel
controversy. Multivar. Behav. Res. 1975, 10, 165–179. [CrossRef]
96. Floyd, F.J.; Widaman, K.F. Factor analysis in the development and refinement of clinical assessment
instruments. Psychol. Assess. 1995, 7, 286–299. [CrossRef]
97. Cattell, R.B. Validation and intensification of the sixteen-personality factor questionnaire. J. Clin. Psychol.
1956, 12, 205–214. [CrossRef]
98. Turner, L.W.; Reisinger, Y. Shopping satisfaction for domestic tourists. J. Retail. Consum. Serv. 2001, 8, 15–27.
[CrossRef]
99. Byrne, B.M. Structural equation modeling with AMOS, EQS, and LISREL: Comparative approaches to testing
for the factorial validity of a measuring instrument. Int. J. Test. 2001, 1, 55–86. [CrossRef]
Sustainability 2019, 11, 2666 19 of 19

100. Fornell, C.; Larcker, D.F. Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement
error. J. Mark. Res. 1981, 18, 39–50. [CrossRef]
101. Bagozzi, R.P.; Yi, Y. On the evaluation of structural equation models. J. Acad. Mark. Sci. 1988, 16, 74–94.
[CrossRef]
102. Baron, R.M.; Kenny, D.A. The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research:
Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1986, 51–56, 1173–1182.
[CrossRef]

© 2019 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access
article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution
(CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).

You might also like