Ebi Scale
Ebi Scale
Ebi Scale
A Thesis by
Benjamin A. Walter
July 2009
© Copyright 2009 by Benjamin A. Walter
The following faculty members have examined the final copy of this thesis for form and
content, and recommend that it be accepted in partial fulfillment of the requirement for
the degree of Master of Education with a major in Educational Psychology.
Epistemological beliefs have been found to have important implications for learning, for
(Schommer, Calvert, Gargliette, & Bajaj, 1997), and may predict teaching practices
(White 2001). This study examined the epistemological beliefs of 83 (M= 16, F = 67)
This study utilized the Epistemic Beliefs Inventory (EBI) (Schraw, Bendixen, & Dunkle,
2002) and the four-quadrant method (Schraw & Olafson, 2008) to measure participants’
level, education program, gender, age, licensure type, and order in which participants
received the instruments. The four-quadrant method was found to have small correlations
Chapter Page
1. THE PROBLEM 1
1.1 Definitions 2
1.1.1 Epistemology 2
1.1.2 Ontology 2
1.2 Historical Background 3
1.3 A Belief System 4
1.4 Purpose 5
1.5 Overview 5
3. METHOD 21
3.1 Participants 21
3.2 Instruments 22
3.3 Procedure 23
4. RESULTS 26
4.1 Descriptive Analysis 26
4.2 Statistical Analysis 29
4.3 Ancillary Analysis 31
4.4 Concurrent Validity of Four-Quadrant Scale 34
5. DISCUSSION 35
5.2. Limitations 38
TABLE OF CONTENTS (continued)
Chapter Page
LIST OF REFERENCES 41
APPENDICES 45
Appendix A 46
Appendix B 47
Appendix C 48
Appendix D 51
Appendix E 53
LIST OF TABLES
Table Page
1. Means, Standard Deviations, Numbers for Gender, Age Groups, 28
Education Level, Instrument Order, Licensure, and Program of Study
2. MANOVA for Education Level 29
3. Zero-Order Correlations of EBI dimensions and four-quadrant scale 30
dimensions
4. MANOVA for Program of Study 31
5. MANOVA for Gender 32
6. MANOVA for Licensure Type 33
7. MANOVA for Age Category 33
8. MANOVA for Instrument Order 34
CHAPTER 1
THE PROBLEM
Fostering critical thinking and exploring the nature of knowledge with students
are ways teachers can create the right environment to enhance the development of
students’ epistemological beliefs (Hofer, 2001). The teacher can only explore the nature
Schraw, 2006). Research has indicated that epistemological beliefs are important to
2002) has concluded that epistemological beliefs are important predictors of mathematics
and problem solving skill. Epistemological beliefs have also been linked with reading
comprehension (Schommer, 1990; Schommer, Crouse, & Rhodes, 1992), academic goal
setting (Braten & Stromso, 2004) attitudes toward school (Schommer & Walker, 1997),
and grade-point average (Schommer, Calvert, Gargliette, & Bajaj, 1997). Some studies
(e.g., Levitt, 2001) have indicated that teachers tend to engage in simplistic teaching
practices that do not enhance epistemological belief development in their students and
their educational level. Evidence (e.g., Lehman, Lempert, & Nisbett, 1988) suggests that
sophisticated and less naïve. Before addressing the purpose of this study, definitions
Epistemological beliefs are defined as beliefs about the nature and acquisition of
knowledge (Bruning, Schraw, Norby, & Ronning, 2004). Schommer (1994) pioneered an
epistemological beliefs system of five more or less independent beliefs, which are:
knowledge acquisition and the speed of knowledge acquisition. Each dimension ranges
from simplistic, naïve beliefs to more sophisticated and complex beliefs. For instance,
within the dimension of structure of knowledge, a naïve belief would be that knowledge
is best described as isolated facts, while a more sophisticated belief would be that
dimensions that can be represented on a scale. One extreme of the scale is labeled
Epistemological Realist (representing more naïve beliefs), describing a belief that there is
an objective body of knowledge that must be acquired. The other extreme of this scale is
Schraw and Olafson also incorporated Ontology into the study of epistemological beliefs.
Ontology is the study of beliefs about the nature of reality and can also be represented on
a scale. One end of the scale is labeled Ontological Realist, which describes assumptions
that there is one underlying reality that is the same for everyone. The opposite end of the
the study of epistemological beliefs. The following brief review of this history is taken
from her work. The study of epistemological beliefs began in 1968, when Perry examined
concluded that the subjects in the early years of college tended to believe in simple,
certain knowledge that is handed down by authority. Nearing the end of their college
experience, most students believed in more tentative, complex knowledge that is derived
from their reason and observation. Following Perry, Ryan (1984) linked epistemological
beliefs to comprehension monitoring. Ryan found that those with naïve epistemological
beliefs were more likely to monitor comprehension by reciting facts rather than reporting
Judgment Model, proposing that learners move from beliefs of absolute and concrete
knowledge that is validated by authority to the final stage of the belief that knowledge is
concluded that children hold beliefs about the ability to learn, and those beliefs influence
their behavior during a learning task. Children who believe that learning ability is fixed at
birth will give up on a problem faster than children who believe that the ability to learn
can be improved. Schoenfeld (1983, 1985) found evidence that seemed to show that
A Belief System
Schommer (1994) developed the notion of epistemology into a belief system that
included five different domains: (a) certainty of knowledge, (b) structure of knowledge,
(c) source of knowledge, (d) control of knowledge acquisition, and (e) speed of
knowledge acquisition. Certainty of knowledge belief ranges from personal beliefs that
knowledge is static throughout time to the view that knowledge is tentative and changes
over time. The structure of knowledge belief ranges from beliefs that knowledge is
simply facts to the belief that knowledge is better represented as complex theories.
Source of knowledge beliefs are the beliefs that people hold regarding where knowledge
comes from: Whether it comes from those in authority to something that can be
discovered and learned by anyone. The control of knowledge belief relates to the beliefs
individuals hold about the ability to learn, ranging from the belief that ability to learn is
fixed at birth or that the ability to learn changes throughout an individual’s life. The
speed of knowledge acquisition belief refers to the belief in how quickly knowledge can
be acquired. Individuals hold beliefs that range from the perception that knowledge will
only be learned in a small amount of time or it won’t be learned at all to the belief that
most things can be learned by most people if enough time is dedicated. These domains
are proposed to be more or less independent of each other, suggesting that an individual
can hold sophisticated beliefs in one domain and more naïve beliefs in another.
In contrast to Schommer (1994), Hofer and Pintrich (1997) indicated that some of
The authors stated that fixed ability and quick learning do not fall into the construct of
epistemological beliefs because they do not purely concern beliefs about knowledge but
This work will assume the definition of epistemological beliefs as beliefs about
the nature and acquisition of knowledge (Bruning et al., 2004) and include the five
Purpose
must be epistemologically sophisticated before they can engage in teaching practices that
sophistication. The educational programs that may have led to the development of these
Overview
programs, (who are also called pre-service teachers). Research on practicing teachers was
also presented. Ontology and its relation to education was shown, and Shraw’s (2008)
four-quadrant scale was exhibited. Literature explaining the effect education has on
reasoning and epistemological beliefs was discussed. Chapter three discusses the
methods, participants, instruments and procedure of study. Chapter four describes the
Chapter two of this thesis presents evidence of the importance of these beliefs
and their connection to teaching practices. The effect of graduate education on teachers’
epistemological beliefs will be discussed, and the notion of ontology will be introduced.
Epistemological beliefs have been linked with comprehension (e.g., Schommer, 1990;
cited in De Corte, et al., 2002), attitudes toward school (Schommer &Walker (1997),
academic goal setting (Braten & Stromso, 2004) and grade-point average (Schommer et
al. (1997).
Johnson, & Anderson, 1993) and levels of education (Schommer, 1998). As students
progress through college and graduate school, epistemological beliefs become more
sophisticated. Similar findings have been made about reasoning abilities, level of
Literature Review
Whereas the importance and meaning for examining the epistemological beliefs
of students has been established (e.g., Broten & Stromso, 2004; Schommer et al., 1992;
Schommer, 1990), a more recent exploration of epistemological beliefs has begun in two
areas. First, a number of studies have been conducted using students in a teacher
White’s (2000) line of inquiry explored the beliefs of pre-service teachers in the
context of problematic classroom situations. Specifically, White wanted to find out what
beliefs pre-service teachers held about the certainty of knowledge, how pre-service
teachers view problematic classroom situations, what source of knowledge the pre-
service teachers would use to make their choice of a solution to follow in a problematic
classroom situation, and how pre-service teachers would justify their choice of action in a
case studies of first-year teachers. The students then discussed and gave individual
written responses to the case studies. These written responses were what White then used
as a framework for interviews that were conducted individually at the end of the
semester. The participants in White’s study were 20 members of the teacher education
during the interview according to King and Kitchener’s stages of Reflective Judgment,
knowledge and use of evidence in responses (King & Kitchener, 1994; cited in White,
2000). Of these participants, two were judged by White to be in the departing absolutist
category, which is the category that describes the most naïve beliefs. The final stage,
called the reflected relative, is the stage that describes the most sophisticated beliefs.
these beliefs do not develop in a stage-like manner and are interconnected in a web-like
fashion.
service teachers, Gill, Ashton, and Algina (2004) created and performed a study that
provided pre-service teachers with instructional intervention. The purpose of this study
was to determine how pre-service teachers would adopt the philosophy of the National
epistemological belief that knowledge is uncertain and complex, and that mathematics
teachers, the researchers used three measures of epistemological beliefs. The first
measure was the Cognitively Guided Instruction Belief Survey (CGI) that measured the
were administered a revised version (Quain & Alverman, 1995, cited in Gill et al., 2004)
epistemological beliefs the students held about mathematics, the researchers used
epistemological beliefs these pre-service teachers had about teaching mathematics. The
participants were divided into a control and experimental group; the experimental group
received a treatment of a text that explained how and why a constructivist teacher would
teach a mathematics class and how a procedural teacher would teach the class and
believed reasons a teacher would teach that way. Results of this study provided support
for the use of persuasive text to foster the development of epistemological beliefs.
Differences in experimental and control group were statistically significant for all three of
Sinatra and Kardash (2004) examined epistemological beliefs and their relation to
the views pre-service teachers had about teaching as persuasion. Teaching as persuasion
can be defined as a metaphor for teaching in which teachers acknowledge that students
have (a) knowledge or beliefs that conflict with classroom content; (b) alternative points
of views are worth considering; (c) messages can be structured to influence students’
understanding; and (d) learning results from a change in students’ knowledge and beliefs.
However, the term persuasion often has a negative connotation. The purpose of this study
was to determine if pre-service teachers would be open to change and learning about
administered two scales from the Epistemological Beliefs Survey (EBS) (Kardash
&Wood, 2002; Wood & Kardash, 2002; cited in Sinatra & Kardash, 2004). The two
scales were the Speed of Knowledge Acquisition and the Knowledge Construction and
Modification scale. The Speed of Knowledge scale measures students’ beliefs about how
quickly and how complicated learning is. The Knowledge Construction and Modification
scale seems to measure beliefs about the certainty of knowledge, the integration of
knowledge from various sources, and the degree to which knowledge is actively
constructed. The study found that epistemological beliefs among pre-service teachers
conducted by Chan and Elliot (2004). This study utilized 385 teacher education students
from a post-secondary institution in Hong Kong. The purpose of this study was to
determine if there were any relationships between explicit epistemological beliefs and
explicit teaching beliefs. Schommer’s EBQ (1990) was administered as well as the
Teaching and Learning Conceptions Questionnaire (TCCQ) (Chan & Elliot, 2004). This
conception sees learning as the acquisition of knowledge by the learner through reasoning
and justification, and teaching is the facilitation of the learning process rather than the
scale to statements such as “it is important that a teacher understands the feelings of the
students” and “during the lesson, it is important to keep students confined to the
textbooks and the desks.” The results of the EBQ and the TCCQ were correlated. The
Traditional Conception of teaching was positively and significantly correlated with three
Authority/Expert Knowledge and Certainty of Knowledge but to a lesser degree than the
Traditional Conception. Learning Effort/Process was negatively related to the
Constructivist Conception.
and how these beliefs are related to various forms of teacher behavior. Levitt (2001)
studied the beliefs teachers had about teaching and learning science among elementary
school science teachers. The purpose of the study was to ascertain how consistent
teachers’ beliefs were with current reform standards in science education. This reform
which can be generally described as the learning of answers, memorizing facts, and
questions, thinking critically, and making arguments to explore science. Levitt explained
that the success of any reform depended on teachers incorporating the new philosophies
of the reform into their own teaching practices and philosophies. Teachers from two
different school districts (10 from each district) were observed during the teaching of a
science lesson. The teachers were then interviewed based on what the researcher had seen
during the observations. Levitt found that eight of the teachers fell in a transitional range
that was somewhere between a positivist teacher and a constructivist teacher. There were
five teachers whose beliefs were traditional, and three whose beliefs were
transformational. As a whole, Levitt believed that the teachers who were sampled
(2006) determined the epistemological beliefs of elementary teachers and then attempted
to look for a correlation between their epistemological beliefs and their actual teaching
practices. The researchers used vignettes that described what they called epistemological
world views. These were called realist, contextualist, and relativist world views. Realists
posit that students must construct their own knowledge and that the teacher serves as a
indicate that students need to construct their own knowledge and teachers should build an
environment where students construct their knowledge and learn to think independently.
Olafson and Schraw divided their research into two phases. Phase I of the study
curriculum. These teachers were administered the Epistemic Belief Inventory (Schraw,
Bendixen, & Dunkle, 2002). The researchers focused on simple knowledge (how simple
or complex knowledge is), certain knowledge (to what degree is knowledge tentative),
and omniscient authority (to what degree) subscales of the instrument. The participants
then completed the Need for Cognition Scale (Cacioppo, Petty, Feinstein, & Jarvis, 1996,
cited in Olafson & Schraw, 2006), measuring the participants’ preference for complex
thinking. The elementary teachers were also administered a Motivation for Teaching
Scale (Schraw & Olafson, 2002, cited in Olafson & Schraw, 2006), that measured
intrinsic and extrinsic motivation for teaching. The participants were then read the
epistemological world views vignettes and were categorized into a world view based on
an interview conducted after the reading of the vignettes. Teachers were then interviewed
regarding their teaching practices, specifically their selection of curriculum. The authors
reported that 95% of participants identified with the contextualist position, 85% disagreed
with the realist position, and 30% gave support to the relativist world view. All of the
teachers used the curriculum the school district provided; however, there was large
variability in how much the teachers relied on the district-provided curriculum. Teachers
who relied heavily on the district curriculum had less student-centered classrooms than
In Phase II of the study, participants completed the same three scales as Phase I
memorable teaching experience. Using PowerPoint, teachers made the storyboard that
reflection paper of their beliefs and the relationship of their beliefs to their practices.
Phase II results were similar to participants in Phase I; that is, Phase II participants had
indicated that epistemological beliefs and worldviews are correlated. The contextualist
beliefs. Also, more sophisticated epistemological beliefs were correlated with more
intrinsic motivation. The authors also noted that, according to the Phase II teachers’
storyboards and reflection papers, epistemological beliefs and worldviews are not closely
aligned with teaching practices. Of the nine storyboards and reflection papers that were
submitted, 59% were contextualist, 36% were realist, and 5% were relativist. This is in
contrast to the Phase II teachers’ reported world view beliefs, where 89% gave support to
contextualist position, 55% gave support for the relativist, and no teacher gave support to
Most recently, the study of beliefs and their effects on learning has expanded to
include the notion of ontology, the study of the beliefs about the nature of reality. Slotta
and Chi (2006) explored the conceptualizations that students hold about physics concepts.
The authors hypothesized that students have difficulty learning about topics such as heat,
light and electricity because students consider these concepts to be material substances
rather than emergent processes. This incorrect classification results from an incorrect
emergent processes. Participants (n=24) were undergraduate students and were equally
divided into an experimental group and a control group, the experimental group receiving
training from the emergent process ontology training module while the control group
received training on the same computer and on the same topic as the experimental group
but without the focus on emergent processes. Both groups completed a pre-test and a
text to read and then an electricity post-test. These tests contained prompts for the
participants to expound on their answers. The results of this study indicated that the
students who received emergent processes training reduced their errors on the electricity
post-test by 20% more than the control group. Also, experimental participants’ responses
to the prompts to elaborate on their answers to the post-test were judged to be more
instructions on how to rate their epistemological and ontological beliefs on the four-
ontological and epistemological positions. The participants made their mark on the four-
quadrant scale where they believed their epistemological and ontological views were best
represented. The participants were then asked to give written justification for their mark.
Results of this study showed that approximately 45% of the participants rated themselves
themselves as ontological relativists. The authors also noted that the content of
alignment.
Graduate Education
education. Advancing education requires individuals to think in more complex ways and
deal with areas of knowledge that are less certain. Different graduate programs have been
shown to elicit different reasoning skills and methods of thinking (Lehman, Lempert, &
Nisbett, 1988). These authors investigated the reasoning abilities of 213 first-year law
students, 127 medical students, 25 psychology students, and 31 chemistry students in the
areas of statistics, methodology, conditional reasoning, and verbal reasoning. Third year
students in the same disciplines were given the same instrument to measure their
reasoning in the same areas as the first year students, with 50 law students, 48 medical
students, 33 psychology students and 26 chemistry students. Lehman and colleagues also
used a longitudinal design to explore the reasoning abilities of students. Students were
utilized both in their first year and third year, with 77 law students, 87 medical students,
24 psychology students, and 18 chemistry students. The results of the study showed that
in the first year of the students’ programs, their reasoning abilities differed only
reasoning saw considerable improvement for third year law, medicine, and psychology
school are related to epistemological beliefs. The authors modified Schommer’s (1990)
showed that academic field of study and level of education were related to
Schommer sought to determine what relationship age has with the development of
beliefs of 418 adults from 170 occupations, including farmers, firefighters, executives,
approximately one-third of the adults having a high school education only, one-third
possessing a bachelor’s degree only, and one-third having graduate education. Results
showed that age and level of education affect epistemological beliefs in their own ways.
Education was related to beliefs about structure and the stability of knowledge whereas
age was related to beliefs about the ability to learn (Schommer, 1998).
Summary
graduate students, teacher education students, and various practicing professionals, most
prominently, practicing teachers. Personal beliefs about knowledge and learning have
shown their importance in how, and how much, individuals learn. Research using
practicing teachers as participants has shown that epistemological beliefs are related to
endeavor, more studies and data need to be collected to further assess the beliefs of
practicing teachers. Ontological beliefs have only recently been incorporated with
teacher beliefs. The four quadrant scale method for assessing epistemological and
ontological beliefs is a recently constructed measure that has not established any forms of
reliability. Also, effects of graduate education have not been specifically assessed
regarding special education teachers and the effects of specific graduate programs within
within their students, the teachers must hold sophisticated epistemological beliefs
This literature review suggests that both academic major and level of education
are related to epistemological beliefs. The questions for this study will be:
found?
3. Will there be replication of earlier results (Schraw & Olafson, 2008) regarding
the four quadrant scale method of measuring ontological and epistemological beliefs?
CHAPTER 3
METHOD
Participants
teacher education program at Wichita State University or teachers that were contacted via
email. There were 233 teachers who were emailed the consent form and EBI. After two
follow-up emails, 21 teachers responded, giving a response rate of 9%. There were 52
students in graduate courses in the college of education who also participated in this
study. There were 83 total participants in the study (M= 16, F= 67). Gender differences in
this study reflect the general male to female ratio in elementary education (“Education
and 1 did not provide ethnic information. Age information was collected on 70 of the
participants. Ages ranged from 23-62 (m=38). The number of participants whose
educational level was less than a Master’s degree was 59. The number of participants
who had Master’s degrees or higher education was 22. Teaching position was provided
by 79 participants. The number of special education teachers was 33, the number of
gifted teachers was 11, and the number of general education teachers was 35. The type of
licensure held by the teachers was provided by 81 participants. The number of teachers
with provisional licenses was 24, the number of teachers with conditional licenses was 3,
The Four-Quadrant Scale. The four-quadrant scale (Schraw & Olafson, 2008)
relativist on the x axis and ontological realist to ontological relativist on the y axis. This
study utilized a modified version of this scale (see Appendix C). Numbered grid lines
were added to give a frame of reference for participants and make scoring of the
instrument quicker for the researcher. The grid was numbered from 0 to 10 on both the x
and y axis, 0 beginning on the realist end of the scales and 10 labeling the most
relativistic end of the scales. Participants were given descriptions of ontological and
epistemological realist and relativist positions (see Appendix C). Participants were then
asked to make their mark on the four-quadrant scale. The instrument measures
epistemological beliefs by the distance the participants’ mark is from the origin of the
four quadrant scale. The instrument was scored by recording the coordinates of the
Belief Inventory (Schraw, Bendixen, & Dunkle, 2002) (see Appendix D). This 28 item 5
point Likert scale questionnaire has yielded Cronbach Alphas for items ranging from .58
disagree’ to a 5 corresponding to ‘strongly agree’ (See appendix D). Items are totaled,
and a mean score for each of the five factors is computed, resulting in a minimum score
of 1 to a maximum score of 5. Items 2, 6, 19, 24, and 28 are reversed scored (G. Schraw,
personal communication, January 15, 2009). Higher scores are meant to represent more
naïve epistemological beliefs. The Epistemological Beliefs Inventory (EBI) uses the five
factors proposed by Schommer (1990). There are seven items measuring simple
knowledge, five items measuring certain knowledge, five items measuring omniscient
authority, six items measuring innate ability, and five items measuring quick learning (G.
Schraw, personal communication, January 15, 2009). Unlike Schommer (1990) the EBI
indicates evidence of loadings of the omniscient authority dimension. This may be due to
unique items that were developed by Schraw, Dunkle, and Bendixen (1995). Schraw,
Dunkle, and Bendixen suggested that this is evidence of the existence of the omniscient
authority dimension. The EBI has shown predictive validity. College students who
questionnaire that will assess: sex, age, teaching experience, current teaching assignment,
Procedure
participants. The school districts of Valley Center and Maize granted permission for the
researcher to email teachers. The Director of the Sedgwick County Area Educational
Services Interlocal cooperative (SCAESIC) was contacted and granted permission for the
researcher to email all special education teachers employed by the SCAESIC. Teachers’
email addresses were obtained via each school district’s website and the website of the
SCAESIC. Teachers were then given an email from the researcher. In the body of this
email (Appendix A) were brief directions for completing the study, a consent form and an
internet link to an online version of the EBI and demographics questionnaire. After
reading the consent form, participants signified their consent to participate in this study
by opening an internet link to the online versions of the EBI and demographics
questionnaire. GoogleDocs™ was used to create and administer the online versions of the
EBI and demographics questionnaire. A total of 233 teachers were emailed the study.
Two follow-up emails were given to participants who had not yet participated in the
study. Because so few of the teachers responded, the sample size was supplemented by
requesting graduate students to participate in the study. To answer the research questions
Wichita State University were surveyed. Three instructors were asked permission for the
researcher to come to their classes before instruction began. All instructors granted
permission to the researcher. The researcher then prepared packets for participants
containing paper copies of the consent form (Appendix B), EBI, four quadrant scale, and
demographics questionnaire (Appendix E). The order of the copies of EBI and four-
quadrant scale was alternated, so half of the class received the EBI first in their packet
while the other half received the four-quadrant scale first. The order in which the EBI and
the four quadrant method was given in the packet was recorded in 33 of the participants.
The researcher gave a brief explanation of the study and provided the packet to students
who volunteered their participation. Further explanation of the four-quadrant scale was
given to participants who had questions about how to complete the four-quadrant method.
CHAPTER 4
RESULTS
The results of the data analysis are presented in this chapter. The research
addressed the following research questions: (a) Will there be differences in educators
based on their level of education? (b) Will a relationship between ontological beliefs and
epistemological beliefs be found? (c) Will there be replication of earlier results (Schraw
& Olafson, 2008) regarding the four quadrant scale method of measuring ontological and
epistemological beliefs?
Descriptive Analysis
Before presenting the statistical analysis, Table 1 presents means and standard
deviations for the 5 EBI dimensions, results of 4-quadrant scale, based on the
Licensure, and Program of Study. Participants were divided by age into four age groups:
22-28, 29-37, 38-45, and 46 and older (Levinson, Darrow, Klein, Levinson & McKeel,
1976; Roberts & Newton, 1987). Levinson, et al. indicate that these age groupings typify
specific developmental tasks that need to be accomplished during that time frame and
Education level by degree and credit hours earned. Bachelor’s degree and credit hours
earned up to a Master’s degree was one group, Master’s degree and credits and degrees
earned post-Master’s was the second group. Participants were grouped in the Instrument
Order variable according to which instrument in the study the participants completed
first. The Program of Study variable categorized participants by their chosen educational
field, if they are in a program to earn a special education degree or general education
degree.
Table 1.
Means, Standard Deviations, Numbers for Gender, Age Groups, Educational Level,
Instrument Order, Licensure, and Program of Study
Ontology 6.8(2.6) 8.5(2.4) 8.2(2.5) 8.4(2.2) 8.1(2.6) 8.0(2.6) 9.0(2.7) 8.5(2.5) 7.7(2.3) 9.6(2.6) 8.2(2.5)
Simple Know. 3.1(.6) 3.0(.3) 3.0(.5) 3.0(.5) 2.8(.5) 3.0(.7) 2.9(.5) 2.9(.4) 3.1(.5) 3.0(.5)
Certain 2.6(.7) 2.6(.6) 2.6(.6) 2.6(.6) 2.7(.6) 2.2(.2) 2.6(.6) 2.6(.6) 2.7(.7) 2.7(.6)
Know.
Omniscient 3.2(.5) 3.4(.6) 3.3(.6) 3.1(.6) 3.4(.5) 3.5(.8) 3.2(.6) 3.2(.6) 3.4(.5) 3.3(.5)
Auth
Quick 1.6(.3) 1.9(.4) 1.7(.4) 1.8(.4) 1.7(.4) 1.4(0) 1.8(.4) 1.8(.5) 1.7(.4) 1.8(.5)
Learning
Quandrant
Scale
Epistemology 7.6(2.3) 6.1(2.1) 6.9(2.3) 8.1(2.9) 7.1(2.3) 6.3(1.1) 7.7(2.7) 6.1(2.3) 7.0(2.2) 6.4(2.3)
Ontology 8.0(2.1) 7.2(2.1) 7.6(2.1) 8.6(2.5) 6.7(2.1) 5.7(1.1) 8.3(2.4) 6.9(2.2) 7.7(2.1) 7.2(2.2)
Statistical Analyses
To answer the first research question (will there be differences in the 5 dimensions of the
EBI and 2 measures of the four-quadrant scale between educators based on their level of
education), MANOVA was used. Table 2 presents the F values of the MANOVA.
Table 2.
Source df F Sig.
Innate Ability 1 0.01 0.00 0.90
Simple Knowledge 1 3.90* 0.05 0.05
Certain Knowledge 1 1.59 0.02 0.21
Omniscient Authority 1 7.82** 0.09 0.01
Quick Learning 1 0.29 0.00 0.59
Epistemology 1 14.53** 0.16 0.00
Ontology 1 10.16** 0.11 0.00
Significant F values were found for the Simple Knowledge and Omniscient Authority
dimensions of the EBI, and for both measures of the four-quadrant scale, indicating that
participants with a master’s degree or higher education were less likely to believe in
Because the issue of teaching assignment was a relevant variable (gifted and
special education teachers would be more likely to have masters’ degrees), a MANOVA
was also conducted to determine if there was significant differences between EBI
Innate 1.00
Ability
Simple .19 1.00
Knowledge
Certain .25* -.01 1.00
Knowledge
Omniscient .22* .16 .47** 1.00
Authority
Quick .29* .35** -.01 .04 1.00
Learning
.06 -.06 -.04 -.04 -.03 1.00
Epistemology
Ontology -.03 -.12 .08 -.04 -.12 .76** 1.00
p<.05
**p<.01
among the five dimensions of the EBI and both of the measures of the four-quadrant
scale. Zero-order correlations of the 5 dimensions of the EBI and the two measures of the
four-quadrant scale are presented in Table 3. Significant relationships were found among
Innate Ability and Certain Knowledge, Omniscient Authority, and Quick Learning.
Certain Knowledge was related to Omniscient authority, and Quick Learning and Simple
Olafson, 2008) were found (research question 3), percentages of participants’ self ratings
on the four quadrant scale were calculated. Of the participants, 24% (n=58) classified
themselves as epistemological realists, 76% as epistemological relativists, 12% as
Ancillary Analysis
Differences in EBI and Four-quadrant scale scores were also analyzed using
differences in teacher’s graduate program of study, gender, licensure type, and age. Table
4 presents the F values of the Graduate program of study MANOVA. Table 5 presents the
F values from MANOVA using Gender as the independent variable. Table 6 shows the F
values of MANOVA with Licensure Type as the independent variable. Table 7 presents
the F values of MANOVA based on age categories. Table 8 presents the F values of
Table 4.
Source df F Sig.
Innate Ability 1 0.01 0.00 0.94
Simple Knowledge 1 4.34* 0.07 0.04
Certain Knowledge 1 0.74 0.01 0.39
Omniscient Authority 1 2.72 0.04 0.10
Quick Learning 1 1.82 0.03 0.18
Epistemology 1 2.57 0.04 0.11
Ontology 1 2.38 0.04 0.13
Graduate program of study was found to have significant difference in the EBI
dimension of Simple Knowledge, indicating that students in masters’ degree program for
general education were more likely to believe in simple knowledge than students in the
special education program. Although not significant, there is a trend suggesting that
students in general education are also more likely to have stronger beliefs in omniscient
authority.
Table 5.
Source Df F Sig.
Innate Ability 1 2.27 0.03 0.14
Simple Knowledge 1 0.32 0.00 0.57
Certain Knowledge 1 1.13 0.01 0.29
Omniscient Authority 1 8.68** 0.10 0.00
Quick Learning 1 0.34 0.00 0.56
Epistemology 1 4.10* 0.05 0.05
Ontology 1 6.70** 0.08 0.01
Gender differences were found to be significant for both measures of the four
quadrant scale and the Omniscient Authority dimension of the EBI. Females are more
likely to believe in omniscient authority; however, females are also more likely to have
more relativistic beliefs in ontology and epistemology as measured by the four quadrant
scale.
Table 6.
Source df F Sig.
Innate Ability 2 0.85 0.02 0.43
Simple Knowledge 2 0.89 0.02 0.42
Certain Knowledge 2 0.79 0.02 0.46
Omniscient Authority 2 1.37 0.03 0.26
Quick Learning 2 1.46 0.04 0.24
Epistemology 2 1.49 0.04 0.23
Ontology 2 3.12* 0.07 0.05
The only significant difference found for licensure type was with the ontology
measure of the four-quadrant scale. Participants who had full licensure were more likely
Table 7.
df F Sig.
omniscient authority. The youngest age group was less likely to endorse beliefs in
omniscient authority.
Table 8.
df F Sig.
Innate Ability 1 2.97 0.09 0.09
Simple Knowledge 1 0.57 0.02 0.45
Certain Knowledge 1 0.04 0.00 0.85
Omniscient Authority 1 1.67 0.05 0.21
Quick Learning 1 4.93* 0.14 0.03
Epistemology 1 3.52 0.10 0.07
Ontology 1 1.30 0.04 0.26
Significant differences were found in the EBI dimension of quick learning. Participants
who completed the four-quadrant method first were more likely to have lower EBI scores
results (Schraw & Olafson, 2008) were found and to investigate the concurrent validity of
the Four-quadrant scale by determining the correlation between the EBI dimensions and
DISCUSSION
The purpose of this study was to examine teacher’s epistemological beliefs and
the factors that may have contributed to the development of those beliefs. The first
education?) indicated that there were indeed differences among educators. In the EBI
and epistemological beliefs be found?) was investigated. Schraw & Olafson (2008)
the four-quadrant scale. As reported in this study, a correlation of r=.76 was found
between the two measures of the scale (see table 3). This evidence could support the
more accurately describes a relationship between the measures of the four-quadrant scale.
This relationship cannot be generalized due to the correlations that were found between
the EBI and the four-quadrant scale. As reported in table 3, the epistemology measure of
the four-quadrant scale had negative correlations with Simple Knowledge, Certain
support a positive relationship between the EBI and the four-quadrant scale because a
low EBI score supports sophisticated epistemological beliefs and a high four quadrant
correlation with the Innate Ability dimension of the EBI. The ontological scale of the
four-quadrant method was found to have slight, negative correlations between the Innate
the EBI. The ontological measure had a positive correlation with Certain Knowledge.
These findings indicated a weak relationship between the four-quadrant method and the
EBI and did not support a relationship between ontological and epistemological beliefs.
The third research question asked if earlier results (Schraw & Olafson, 2008)
would be found regarding the four quadrant scale method of measuring ontological and
epistemological beliefs. Similar results were found in this study. Schraw & Olafson’s
(2008) reported that 45% of participants (n= 24) rated themselves as epistemological
independent variables. It was expected for Gender, Age, and Level of Education to have
effects on epistemological beliefs. Schommer (1998) found Age to affect beliefs about
the ability to learn and Level of Education to affect beliefs about the stability and
structure of knowledge. In this study, Level of Education had an effect on the Simple
Knowledge and Omniscient Authority dimensions of the EBI. This evidence supports
this study, Age was found to have an affect on the Omniscient Authority dimension of the
EBI. While this study did not find evidence to support that Age affected beliefs about the
ability to learn, it did show that Age affected epistemological beliefs in different ways
than Level of Education. Age only affected the Omniscient Authority dimension the EBI,
while Level of Education had an affect on both the Omniscient Authority and the Simple
Knowledge dimensions.
Schommer, (1990) found females to be more likely to believe in gradual learning rather
than quick learning. Braten & Stromso, (2004) found female teachers to be more likely to
use mastery goals rather than performance goals. This study did not replicate previous
findings but did find significant differences in beliefs in omniscient authority, with
Educational Program was also found to have a significant affect on one of the
participants whose chosen field of study would gain them a degree in special education,
beliefs (e.g. Jehng, et. al, (1993), and reasoning skills (Lehman, Lempert & Nisbett,
1988) but it was a somewhat surprising finding in this study due to the perceived
the four quadrant scale, Teachers with full licensure were more likely to have higher
scores on the ontological measure. Due to the lack of concurrent validity between the
four quadrant scale and the EBI, conclusions cannot be drawn by this finding until
Limitations
This study was limited in various ways. Low response rate from the online survey
led this study to have a small sample size. Trends in differences in mean EBI and four-
sample size would have been obtained. The sample also did not have equal numbers of
participants based on gender, making conclusions about gender differences found in this
study very tentative. Modifications made to the four-quadrant scale limit the conclusions
that can be drawn about the reliability and validity of the instrument. Further limiting the
conclusions that can be drawn about the four-quadrant scale is the small sample of
participants who completed the measure. This researcher discovered no practical way for
the four-quadrant scale to be administered electronically, and so the measure was not sent
out to online participants. The four-quadrant scale was also confusing to some
participants. Some participants required further explanation about the measure before
beliefs should naturally have teaching practices that support and promote sophisticated
more readily support constructivist activities and discussions that promote beliefs in the
complexity and tentativeness of knowledge and the possibility for anyone to obtain it.
However, Olafson and Schraw (2006) did not find epistemological beliefs to be closely
aligned with actual teaching practices when teachers self reported classroom activities,
teaching strategies, and instructional objectives. Future research should further examine
This study found that there were differences in educators based on the type of
graduate education program they were seeking degrees in. Because participants in this
study were all involved in classes that were about teaching, this study suggests that even
small differences in the type of education or subject matter can affect epistemological
beliefs. Future research should address factors within different graduate education
Women in this study were found to endorse beliefs in omniscient authority more
so than men. This could support the idea that women are socialized in Western culture to
obey rules and follow authority more so than men. Due to the small sample of men in this
study and the lack of this finding reported by earlier researchers, future research should
confusing to some participants and did not show strong concurrent validity with the EBI.
Conclusions about the Four-quadrant method cannot be made due to the modifications
made by this researcher. It may be the case that future research of the Four-quadrant
method may require more explanation of the measure in order for participants to better
Participants who completed the Four-quadrant method prior to the EBI were less
likely to score high on the Quick Learning dimension of the EBI. This may show that
reading the descriptions of epistemological and ontological realists and relativists found
may have detected from the Four-quadrant method descriptions that relativists teachers
are more effective and desirable teachers and so their true quick learning beliefs were not
reported. Future research should keep in mind this effect of the four-quadrant method or
any other descriptions or definitions given before the administration of the EBI.
REFERENCES
List of References
Bruning, R., Schraw, G., Norby, M. & Ronning, R. (2004). Cognitive psychology and
instruction. Columbus, OH: Merrill.
Chan, K. & Elliot, R. G. (2004). Relational analysis of personal epistemology a
conceptions about teaching and learning. Teaching and Teacher Education, 20,
817-831.
De Corte, E., Op ‘T Eynde, P., & Verchaffel, L. (2002). “Knowing what to believe”: the
relevance of students’ mathematical beliefs for mathematics education. In B. K.
Hofer & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Personal epistemology: The psychology of beliefs
about knowledge and knowing (pp. 297- 320). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Duell, O.K. & Schommer-Aikins, M. (2001). Measures of People’s Beliefs About
Knowledge and Learning. Educational Psychology Review, 13, 419-449.
Gill, M. G., Ashton, P.T., & Algina, J. (2004). Changing preservice teachers’
epistemological beliefs about teaching and learning in mathematics: An
intervention study. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 29, 164-185.
Hofer, B. K. (2001). Personal epistemology research: implications for learning and
teaching. Journal of Educational Psychology Review, 13(4), 353-383.
Hofer, B.K., & Pintrich, P. R. (1997). The development of epistemological theories:
Beliefs about knowledge and knowing and their relation to learning. Review
of Educational Research, 67(1), 88-140.
Jehng, J. J., Johnson, S. D., & Anderson, R. C. (1993). Schooling and
students’epistemological beliefs about learning. Contemporary Educational
Psychology, 18(1), 23-35.
Lehman, D. R., Lempert, R. O. & Nisbett, R. E. (1988). The effects of graduate training
on reasoning. American Psychologist, 43, 431-442.
Levitt, K. E. (2001). An analysis of elementary teachers’ beliefs regarding the teaching
and learning of science. Science Education, 86, 1-22.
Levinson, D.J., Darrow, C.M., Klein, E.B., Levinson, M. H. & McKee, B. (1976).
Periods in the adult development of men; Ages 18-45. The Counseling Psychologist, 6,
21-25.
Male elementary teachers harder to find. (2007, April). Education Reporter, 255.
Olafson, L. & Schraw, G. (2006). Teachers’ beliefs and practices within and across
domains. International Journal of Educational Research, 45, 71-84.
Roberts, P. , & Newton, P.M. (1987) Levinsonian studies of women’s adult development.
Psychology and Aging, 2, 154-163.
Schommer-Aikins, M. (2002). An evolving theoretical framework for an epistemological
belief system. In B. K. Hofer & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Personal epistemology: The
psychology of beliefs about knowledge and knowing (pp. 103-118). Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Schommer, M. (1998). The influence of age and education on epistemological beliefs.
British Journal of Educational Psychology, 68, 551-562.
Schommer, M. (1994). An emerging conceptualization of epistemological beliefs and
their role in learning. In R. Garner & P. Alexander (Eds.), Beliefs about text
instruction, (pp. 25-39). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Schommer, M. (1990). Effects of beliefs about the nature of knowledge on
comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76(2), 1226-1238.
Schommer, M., Calvert, C., Gargliette, G., & Bajaj, A. (1997). The development of
epistemological beliefs among secondary students: A longitudinal study. Journal
of Educational Psychology, 89, 37-40.
Schommer, M., Crouse, A. & Rhodes, N. (1992). Epistemological beliefs and
mathematical text comprehension: believing it is simple does not make it so.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 84(4), 435-443.
Schommer, M. & Walker, K. (1997). Are epistemological beliefs similar across domains?
Journal of Educational Psychology, 87(3), 424-432.
Schraw, G., Bendixen, L.D., & Dunkle, M.E. (2002). Development and validation of the
epistemic belief inventory (EBI). In B. K. Hofer & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Personal
epistemology: The psychology of beliefs about knowledge and knowing (pp. 261
275). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Schraw, G., Dunkle, M.E., & Bendixen, L.D. (1995). Cognitive processes in well-defined
and ill-defined problem solving. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 9, 523-538.
Schraw, G. J. & Olafson, L.J. (2008). Assessing teachers’ epistemological and
ontological worldviews. In M.S. Khine (ed.) Knowing, knowledge, and beliefs:
Epistemological studies across diverse cultures (25-44). Netherlands: Springer.
Sinatra, G.M. & Kardash, C.M. (2004). Teacher candidates’ epistemological beliefs,
dispositions, and views on teaching as persuasion. Contemporary Educational
Psychology, 29, 483-498.
Slotta, J.D. & Chi, M.T. (2006). Helping students understand challenging topics in
science through ontology training. Cognition and Instruction, 24(2), 261-289.
White, B. C. (2000). Pre-service teachers’ epistemology viewed through perspectives on
problematic classroom situations. Journal of Education for Teaching, 26, (3),
279-305.
APPENDICES
Appendix A
If you decide to participate, you will proceed by opening the link below in this email
message. You will then follow a set of directions within the link that will guide you through a
set of 28 questions to assess your personal beliefs about knowledge. An example item is:
“The best ideas are often the most simple.” There are no anticipated discomforts or risks
involved with this study. Your participation will require approximately 15 minutes.
The completion of this research will benefit society by increase understanding of how
personal knowledge beliefs are developed. Any information obtained in this study in which
you can be identified will remain confidential and will be disclosed only with your
permission. If you would like, results of this study may be provided to you at your request.
Participation in this study is entirely voluntary. Your decision whether or not to participate
will not affect your future relations with Wichita State University or your school employer. If
you agree to participate in this study, you are free to withdraw from the study at any time
without penalty.
If you have any questions about this research, you can contact Ben Walter,
[email protected], 316-217-4709 or Dr. Linda Bakken, 151 Corbin Education
Center,1845 Fairmount, Box 28 Wichita, Kansas 67260-0028, (316) 978-3322.
If you have questions pertaining to your rights as a research subject, or about research-related
injury, you can contact the Office of Research Administration at Wichita State University,
Wichita, KS 67260-0007, telephone (316) 978-3285.
You are under no obligation to participate in this study. To participate in this study, open the
link contained in this email and follow the directions within the link. By opening the link,
you signify that you have read the information provided above and have voluntarily decided
to participate.
<iframe
src="http://spreadsheets.google.com/embeddedform?key=pWW3C6oCVDNH9zIF48IslSw"
width="500" height="6980" frameborder="0" marginheight="0"
marginwidth="0">Loading...</iframe>
You will be given a copy of this consent form to keep upon request.
Thank you,
Ben Walter
Appendix B
Spring 2009
You are invited to participate in this study about personal knowledge beliefs. We hope to learn
more about factors that contribute to the development of these beliefs. You were selected as a
possible participant in this study because of your occupation as an educator and your graduate
education. You are in a group of approximately 40 graduate students participating in this study.
If you decide to participate, continue by following the provided directions that will guide you
through a set of 29 questions that will assess your personal beliefs about knowledge. A sample
item is: “The best ideas are often the most simple There are no anticipated discomforts or risks
involved with this study. Your participation will require approximately 15 minutes.
The completion of this research will benefit society by increasing the understanding of how
personal knowledge beliefs are developed. Any information obtained in this study in which you
can be identified will remain confidential and will be disclosed only with your permission. If you
would like, results of this study may be provided to you at your request. Participation in this study
is entirely voluntary. Your decision whether or not to participate will not affect your future
relations with Wichita State University or your school employment. If you agree to participate in
this study, you are free to withdraw from the study at any time without penalty.
If you have any questions about this research, you can contact Ben Walter, [email protected],
316-217-4709 or Dr. Linda Bakken, 151 Corbin Education Center,1845 Fairmount, Box 28
Wichita, Kansas 67260-0028, (316) 978-3322,
If you have questions pertaining to your rights as a research subject, or about research-related
injury, you can contact the Office of Research Administration at Wichita State University,
Wichita, KS 67260-0007, telephone (316) 978-3285.
You are under no obligation to participate in this study. Your signature on this consent form
indicates that you have read the information above and voluntarily agree to participate in this
study. You will be given a copy of this consent form to keep.
Appendix C
We want you to rate and explain your epistemological and ontological worldviews.
Please read the following descriptions of terms used in this study. Then indicate where
you would place yourself in the four quadrants by moving the oval from the center of the
axes to where your ratings intersect on the epistemology and ontology dimensions.
Descriptions: Please note that the following descriptions represent endpoints on each of
the scales. Your own beliefs may lie anywhere between these two endpoints. You may
use any part of the four quadrant area.
Epistemology
Epistemological Realist
Epistemological Relativist
The content of the curriculum should be responsive to the needs of the community.
It is useful for students to engage in tasks in which there is no indisputably correct
answer. Students design their own problems to solve.
Ontology
Ontology is the study of beliefs about the nature of reality. The personal ontology of
teachers is characterized by a set of beliefs regarding whether students share a common
reality and what a classroom reality should look like.
Ontological Realist
A teacher who is an ontological realist assumes one underlying reality that is the same for
everyone. Instructionally, this means that all children should receive the same type of
instruction at the same time regardless of their individual circumstances and context. An
ontological realist would agree with the following:
Ontological Relativist
An ontological relativist assumes that different people have different realities. From an
instructional perspective, teachers are seen as collaborators, co-participants, and
facilitators of learning who work to meet the individual needs of students. Instructional
practices are less teacher-directed, such as:
7. Parents should teach their children all there is to know about life.
9. If a person tries too hard to understand a problem, they will most likely end up being
confused.
13. Some people are born with special gifts and talents.
14. How well you do in school depends on how smart you are.
15. If you don’t learn something quickly, you won’t ever learn it.
16. Some people just have a knack for learning and others don’t.
17. Things are simpler than most professors would have you believe.
18. If two people are arguing about something, at least one of them must be wrong.
20. If you haven’t understood a chapter the first time through, going back over it won’t
help.
Thank you for completing the questionnaire. Please briefly provide the following
demographic information:
Sex:
Age:
Ethnicity:
School District:
Education Level: