Attachment Theory
Attachment Theory
Attachment Theory
Adult Attachment:
Theory, Research, and Clinical Implications
Edited by W. Steven Rholes and Jeffry A. Simpson
edited by
Jeffry A. Simpson
W. Steven Rholes
v
Contributors
3 Fooled Around and Fell in Love: The Role of Sex in Adult Romantic 68
Attachment Formation
Vivian Zayas, Sarah Merrill, and Cindy Hazan
xi
xii Contents
W. Steven Rholes
Jeffry A. Simpson
1
2 Introduction
Classic Themes
One long-standing theme in the attachment field is whether and how early
experiences with caregivers affect social development into adulthood, espe-
cially romantic relationships. In Chapter 1, Fraley and Roisman explore
how early attachment experiences shape differences in the way in which
people feel, think, and behave in romantic relationships in early adult-
hood. They focus on two developmental pathways that may connect early
attachment experiences with romantic relationship functioning in early
adulthood: (1) the development of social competence and (2) the forma-
tion of intimate friendships. They also discuss several emerging themes
in developmental research, such as identifying the developmental factors
that link early social experiences with later developmental outcomes, dis-
tinguishing different mediation pathways between early experiences and
later outcomes, and using multiple-wave studies to test whether and how
associations between early experiences and later outcomes either decay or
stabilize over time.
Another perennial theme is how attachment processes are implicated
in the development of sexual pair bonding in adult romantic relationships.
In Chapter 3, Zayas, Merrill, and Hazan note that romantic partners often
act as both attachment figures and sexual partners, yet research on sex
and adult attachment have advanced independently. As a consequence, not
much is known about whether and how the sexual mating system interfaces
Introduction 3
develops one disorder, whereas another person with the same profile devel-
ops another disorder or no disorder at all (divergent trajectories). Ein-Dor
and Doron develop and present a transdiagnostic model to address these
issues. Within the context of this model, they discuss contexts and mediat-
ing processes that may explain multifinality and divergent trajectories.
Two chapters address psychotherapy issues. In Chapter 15, Johnson,
Lafontaine, and Dalgleish describe how and why attachment theory serves
as the basis for emotionally focused therapy (EFT). The authors then review
then empirical studies that provide support for some of EFT’s core prin-
ciples. Among other things, they explore changes in attachment responses
and styles, and examine how strengthening the attachment bond can affect
other important aspects of the relationship. The authors also describe ways
in which therapists can use attachment theory to guide their understanding
of their clients’ communications during therapy sessions. A brief case study
is included to illustrate the change processes that can be produced by EFT.
In Chapter 14, Dozier and Roben discuss attachment-based interven-
tions with at-risk infants and young children. The authors then cover three
major issues regarding interventions. The first centers on foundational
issues needed to create successful attachment-based interventions. The sec-
ond involves a description of several current attachment-based intervention
programs; this section shows a remarkable variety of programs all based
to a greater or lesser degree on attachment theory. The third addresses dis-
semination of successful programs from the laboratory to field settings,
and explains the great difficulties in crossing this boundary.
Conclusion
As the chapters in this book confirm, one of the most dramatic changes
in the past decade has been the rapid extension of attachment principles,
ideas, processes, and findings to myriad topics in areas outside develop-
mental and social/personality psychology (where attachment theory and
research originally flourished). Core attachment principles, hypotheses,
and ideas—those stemming from both the normative and the individual-
difference components of attachment theory—have infiltrated areas within
the neurosciences, family social science, the health sciences, the clinical sci-
ences (including therapy and intervention work), the scientific study of sex
and sexuality, and even the organizational sciences. Attachment theory and
its empirical findings, in other words, are now being more broadly applied
to address significant questions, issues, and problems in a wide variety of
areas within the purview of the psychological sciences. This is an important
new direction, because many of these areas can benefit from a comprehen-
sive lifespan theory that explains not only how normative principles and
processes, but also how individual variations around normative principles
Introduction 7
and processes, can help us to better understand why people think, feel, and
behave as they do in different situations and at different stages of life. The
next decade is likely to reveal how well attachment theory and its core prin-
ciples are able to clarify, resolve, and answer some of these long-standing
questions, and where other theories, models, or principles are needed to
more fully explain certain phenomena or outcomes more fully. If Bowlby
were alive today, we suspect that he would be pleased to see his grand
theory being incorporated into so many different fields that are attempting
to solve so many diverse and important problems and issues.
In conclusion, when we started planning this book, our aspiration was
that the authors of each invited chapter would provide broad and com-
prehensive coverage of the most important theories, models, principles,
and research findings relevant to the topic focused on by each chapter. In
addition, we hoped that the authors would offer useful “roadmaps” that
future researchers interested in each topic could navigate. As editors, we are
delighted with what this outstanding set of authors has delivered. We sin-
cerely hope that you—the reader of this novel and cutting-edge volume—
will concur.
References
Bowlby, J. (1969/1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York:
Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation. New York: Basic
Books.
Bowlby, J. (1979). The making and breaking of affectional bonds. New York:
Methuen.
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Vol. 3. Loss. New York: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base. New York: Basic Books.
1
Early Attachment Experiences
and Romantic Functioning
Developmental Pathways, Emerging Issues,
and Future Directions
R. Chris Fraley
Glenn I. Roisman
9
10 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
safety and protection, and ultimately his or her reproductive fitness (also
see Zayas, Merrill, & Hazan, Chapter 3, this volume).
During the early months of life, the degree of security an infant experi-
ences is believed to depend largely on exogenous signals, such as the proxi-
mate availability and responsiveness of primary caregivers. Over repeated
interactions, however, children develop a set of knowledge structures, or
internal working models, that represent those interactions and contrib-
ute to the endogenous regulation of the system (Bretherton & Munhol-
land, 1999). These representations enable a child to simulate a variety of
response options to determine which course of action might best facilitate
certain goals (e.g., regaining the attention of the caregiver).
Importantly, these cognitive structures also reflect what the child has
learned about the responsiveness and availability of caregivers over the
course of repeated interactions. If caregivers are generally warm, respon-
sive, and consistently available, the child learns that others can be counted
on when needed. Consequently, he or she is likely to explore the world
confidently, initiate warm and sociable interactions with others, and find
security in the knowledge that a caregiver is available if needed. In short,
the child has developed a secure working model of attachment. If attach-
ment figures are cold, rejecting, unpredictable, frightening, or insensitive,
however, the child learns that others cannot be counted on for support and
comfort, and this knowledge is embodied in insecure working models of
attachment. The child is likely to regulate his or her behavior accordingly—
either by excessively demanding attention and care, or by withdrawing
from others and/or attempting to achieve a high degree of self-sufficiency
(DeWolff & van IJzendoorn, 1997).
A key idea in Bowlby’s theory is that early attachment experiences set the
stage for the way in which the child navigates interpersonal contexts. This
16 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
includes not only the child’s interactions with primary attachment figures,
but other contexts in which issues concerning trust and intimacy emerge,
including relations with teachers and mentors (Ainsworth, 1989), siblings
(Teti & Ablard, 1989), and, importantly, close friends.
Close friendships serve a number of important developmental func-
tions. For example, friendships can provide a context in which one
explores new skills and interests, builds alliances, bolsters self-esteem,
and reinforces emerging identities (Shulman et al., 1994). The process
of forming and maintaining a close friendship, however, is not a triv-
ial matter. One must be capable of managing conflict, offering support,
adopting the other’s perspective, and engaging in appropriate levels of
self-disclosure and reciprocity. Indeed, research indicates that these kinds
of social competencies play a role in facilitating the development of well-
functioning friendships (e.g., Boling, Barry, Kotchick, & Lowry, 2011;
Simpson et al., 2007).
Close friendships are also of special interest from an attachment per-
spective because they are some of the first extrafamilial contexts in which
issues concerning intimacy, trust, and support are explored. According to
some theorists, close friendships are often testing grounds for transferring
attachment-related features and functions away from parents (Zeifman &
Hazan, 2008). As such, the experiences that take place in the context of
friendship relationships may constitute a key “transfer station” in shaping
the development of expectations for trust, intimacy, and support in roman-
tic relationships.
According to attachment theorists, repeated experiences of supportive,
responsive care in early attachment relationships may support the develop-
ment of the skills necessary for developing and maintaining high-quality
friendships. In the sections below, we review research on the pathways
between early attachment experiences and the quality of friendship rela-
tionships in childhood and adolescence. In addition, we examine research
on how friendship experiences and perceptions are related to views of
romantic relationships and relationship functioning.
FIGURE 1.1. Alternative ways of organizing the pathways among early caregiving
experiences, social competence, friendship quality, and romantic functioning.
20 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
early attachment experiences help shape romantic functioning via two key
processes: the development of social competence and the development of
close friendships. But there are alternative ways to arrange the pieces of
this map, and, conceptually, these are equally compelling. For example, the
second panel of Figure 1.1 illustrates a model articulated by Simpson et al.
(2007). This model is similar to the first, with one exception. Namely, it
holds that the development of social competence also shapes the develop-
ment of close friendships. Simpson and his colleagues (2007) refer to this
as a “double mediation model,” because it assumes two mediational steps
in the link between early experiences and romantic relationship function-
ing. A different way to organize these variables is to assume that each one
has the potential to influence romantic functioning, but without specifying
any explicit causal relations among them. The third panel of Figure 1.1
illustrates a basic model assuming that all three factors may contribute
to variation in romantic functioning, and that the three predictors may
be related to one another for unspecified reasons. A fourth way to frame
these relations is to assume that they are all causally related to one another
within the constraints provided by the temporal ordering of the variables.
The fourth panel of Figure 1.1 illustrates a case in which each temporally
prior variable influences all the variables that follow it in time. Romantic
functioning is affected by social competence both directly and indirectly
through its influence on friendship quality.
Which of these models is the most appropriate way to conceptual-
ize the pathways that lead from early attachment experiences to romantic
functioning? We are not sure that this question can be answered adequately
on the basis of existing research. Simpson and his colleagues have advo-
cated for the double mediation model illustrated in the second panel of
Figure 1.1. They also examined some alternative models, including the one
illustrated in the third panel. From their analyses, they concluded that the
double mediation model best fits the data. Drawing upon data from the
same sample, Englund, Kuo, Puig, and Collins (2011) examined a direct
path between early attachment and friendship functioning, but did not find
statistically significant paths between early attachment and social compe-
tence or between social competence and friendship quality in the context of
the other constructs in the model. We and our colleagues (Fraley, Roisman,
Booth-LaForce, et al., 2013) have adopted the third model because we feel
that it makes the fewest assumptions. However, we did not find an asso-
ciation between early caregiving experiences and friendship relationships.
That said, we found that increases in the quality of caregiving over time
were associated prospectively with increases in the quality of friendships.
In summary, the jury is still out regarding the optimal way to model
the relations among these various factors. We believe that resolving these
issues is a valuable direction for future research. Having said that, we wish
to emphasize two caveats. First, the specific models under discussion do
Early Attachment and Romantic Functioning 21
assume that the value of the paths (b) are equivalent and are equal to .30.
Using covariance algebra, we find in this simple three-variable model that
the expected association between x and y is now .30 × .30 = .09. If we
hold everything constant, adding a single juncture or mediator reduces the
expected correlation between x and y. If we add a second mediator (e.g., a
double mediation model) such that x → z1 → z 2 → y, the expected correla-
tion grows even smaller (i.e., .30 × .30 × .30 = .027). These dynamics are
illustrated in the upper portion of Figure 1.2.
In short, if we attempt to flesh out the developmental pathway between
early attachment experiences and romantic functioning in adulthood by
adding mediators along a common developmental pathway, the expected
association between early experiences and later outcomes necessarily
decreases. In fact, if we assume that we do not adjust the value of the paths,
the expected correlation between x and y approaches zero as the number
of mediators increases. We refer to such mediational chains as horizontally
x z1 y rxy = b2
x z1 z2 y rxy = b3
x z1 z2 z3 y rxy = b4
z1
x y rxy = b2
z1
x y rxy = b2 + b2
z3
z1
x z2 y rxy = b2 + b2 + b2
z3
mediated pathways from this point forward. This result implies that a
model positing various junctures along a common pathway is more likely
to create discontinuities in development than to create sustained effects
of early experiences. Horizontally mediated pathways are incompatible
with the theoretical assumption that these various mechanisms sustain the
effects of early experiences across time.
There is an alternative way to specify mediators, however. Consider
the lower portion of Figure 1.2. These diagrams illustrate models in which
the mediators are added vertically. As such, we refer to such chains as verti-
cally mediated pathways. Notice that as we add mediators vertically, the
expected association between early attachment experiences and romantic
functioning increases. For example, if we assume that all the paths are
equal to .30 in the four-variable model, the expected correlation between
early experience and adult functioning is .09 + .09 = .18. If we make the
same assumptions for the five-variable model, the expected correlation rises
to .09 + .09 + .09 = .27. Why do vertically mediated pathways make the
expected correlation between x and y more robust? In this kind of model,
early experiences play an organizing role across multiple life domains,
including social competence, peer relations, and a variety of other factors.
If those domains also influence the outcome of interest, then those mecha-
nisms buffer the relationship between x and y.
In summary, there are alternative ways to formalize developmental
pathways. When one adds horizontally mediated pathways to a model,
one is implicitly claiming that such factors function to reduce the expected
association between early experiences and romantic functioning. When
one adds vertically mediated pathways, in contrast, one is implicitly claim-
ing that such factors serve to buttress or support the association between
early experiences and romantic functioning. Statistically, these additions
provide multiple pathways through which the indirect effects of early expe-
rience can accumulate and shape later outcomes. By calling attention to this
issue, we hope not only to make researchers aware of the difference, but to
alert researchers to the possibility of alternative developmental models that
can be examined.
Developmental Cascades
Developmental cascades “refer to the cumulative consequences for develop-
ment of the many interactions and transactions occurring in developmental
systems that result in spreading effects across levels, among domains at
the same level, and across different systems or generations” (Masten &
Cicchetti, 2010, p. 491). Developmental cascades represent one process
by which the effects of one variable may become manifest in various out-
comes downstream. The middle panel of Figure 1.3 represents an example
of potential cascade effects that could exist among the kinds of variables
24 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
care1
peer3
FIGURE 1.3. Developmental cascades and their effects on promoting the associa-
tions between developmental outcomes. Abbreviations: care, caregiving envi-
ronment; socc, social competence; peer, quality of peer friendships. The vari-
ous subscripts refer to different time points.
are correlated with one another at the initial assessment (r = .30). In the
absence of any cascading effects, the expected correlations among the three
variables are .30 at the first time wave, .11 at the second, .04 at the third,
.01 at the fourth, and .01 at the fifth (see the top panel of Figure 1.3).
Now assume that the quality of the caregiving environment at the first
wave predicts social competence at the second wave, such that the regres-
sion coefficient is .30. In addition, using the same magnitude of coefficient,
let us assume that social competence, in turn, influences friendship func-
tioning, and eventually trickles back to social competence downstream.
Now the correlation between caregiving and social competence in the first
wave is .30, .29 in the second, .10 in the third, .05 in the fourth, and .02
in the fifth. In other words, compared to a situation in which there are no
cascading effects, the presence of developmental cascades helps to boost the
association between the two variables of interest.
One reason why the concept of developmental cascades is important is
that cascade effects, if specified correctly, function as vertically mediated
pathways. If early experiences are modeled such that they organize mul-
tiple potential constructs (e.g., social competence and friendship quality),
those factors will help sustain certain effects for a longer period of time
than they would subsist in the absence of such effects. However, cascade
effects alone, when specified in this fashion, cannot sustain coordinated
effects indefinitely. As the predicted associations reveal, the shared asso-
ciations among caregiving and social competence gradually decrease and
will continue to do so unless there are ongoing and persistent transactional
pathways among the constructs. Moreover, in such a model, the effects of
early experiences will eventually wash out. This kind of model, while build-
ing in vertically mediated pathways, also includes horizontally mediated
pathways and their respective consequences compete with one another (see
below).
There is a lot to be learned about developmental pathways by studying
cascade effects across time. Moreover, although the concept of developmental
cascades has been discussed most extensively in the developmental literature
(e.g., Masten et al., 2005), there is potentially a lot of value in studying cas-
cades in intensive longitudinal studies in the social-psychological literature.
Adopting such models, for example, would allow researchers to investigate
the ways in which conflicts in romantic relationships exert downstream
effects indirectly via their influence on other resources (e.g., time spent
apart, verbal communication).
1.0
0.8
0.8
Coefficient
Coefficient
0.6
0.6
0.4
0.4
0.2
0.2
0.0
0.0
0 5 10 15 20 25 30 0 5 10 15 20 25 30
Age Age
1.0
1.0
Correlation (Early, Later Outcome)
0.8
0.6
0.6
0.4
0.4
0.2
0.2
0.0
0.0
0 50 100 150 0 10 15 20 25 30
Age (months) Age (years)
We have explored these ideas with data from the NICHD SECCYD.
For example, we (Fraley, Roisman, & Haltigan, 2013) examined whether
the long-term effects of early maternal sensitivity on social competence and
academic ability were more likely to fade gradually over time (suggesting
that early caregiving supports development early in life, but does not have
canalizing effects) or to stabilize at a nonzero value as children aged (sug-
gesting canalization effects). We found that variation in early parental sen-
sitivity predicted social competence in a relatively stable way across time
(see panel C of Figure 1.4).
Although the Fraley, Roisman, and Haltigan (2013) analyses illustrate
the potential value in examining the implications of alternative models of
developmental pathways, the research was not able to examine the associa-
tion between early experiences and attachment functioning in adulthood per
se. In a project better suited for that purpose, we (Raby, Roisman, Fraley, &
Simpson, in press) used data from the MLSRA to determine whether early
caregiving experiences were prospectively related to measures of social
competence—including romantic functioning in adulthood—across mul-
tiple assessment occasions. Parental sensitivity was assessed as a composite
of sensitive parenting coded from parent–child interactions across several
occasions when children were between the ages of 3 and 42 months. Social
competence was assessed by using teachers’ rankings of target children’s
social competence on multiple occasions between kindergarten and age 16.
Importantly, measures of romantic functioning at ages 23 and 32 years
were used as indicators of social competence in adulthood. This is based
on the developmental view that part of what early attachment experiences
help to organize is optimal functioning for age-appropriate developmental
tasks. In young and middle adulthood, that includes functioning in roman-
tic relationships. These analyses revealed that early caregiving experiences
predicted social competence in an enduring manner over time. Specifically,
children who had supportive caregiving experiences early in life were more
likely than those who did not to have well-functioning romantic relation-
ships in adulthood. And, importantly, those effects were more consistent
with a developmental model assuming that the effects of early experiences
become canalized than with a model not making that assumption.
one cannot separate the potential effects of early experience per se from the
amount of time that has elapsed since the event took place.
and longitudinal study of children. In one study (Roisman & Fraley, 2006),
for example, we used standard behavior genetic models to partition the
variance in the observed quality of infant–caregiver relationships when
children were 9 months of age. We found that the estimated contribution of
additive genetic effects to the quality of infant–caregiver relationships was
close to zero. In contrast, the estimated contribution of shared environment
effects was close to 40%, and the estimate of the nonshared environmental
effects was 59%.
In follow-up analyses from this sample, we (Roisman & Fraley, 2008)
found that infant attachment security observed at 24 months was largely
a function of shared and nonshared environmental influences; the esti-
mated additive genetic variance in security was close to zero. In addition,
the estimated additive genetic variance in parenting quality rated at 24
months was also close to zero. This study further decomposed the covaria-
tion between parenting quality and attachment security, and found that
the association between these two variables was largely due to shared and
nonshared environmental influences (Roisman & Fraley, 2008; see also
Fearon et al., 2006).
In summary, although it is premature to rule out the hypothesis that
heritable factors might explain some of the variation in early attachment
experiences, the findings to date suggest that this hypothesis does not offer
a viable account for variation in the quality of early parent–child relation-
ships or in the security of a child’s attachment pattern. Moreover, analyses
do not support the assumption that additive genetic factors account for the
covariation between parenting quality and attachment security. Although
heritable factors might explain some portion of individual differences in
romantic functioning later in life (e.g., Donnellan, Burt, Levendowsky, &
Klump. 2008), they do not appear to confound the inference that parent–
child experiences also contribute to interpersonal development.
example, genes code for the production of mRNA, which plays a role in
the development of proteins, which play a role in the organization of the
nervous system, and so on (Slavich & Cole, 2013). Effects at any of these
junctures can be modulated by various factors that influence transcription
and methylation. The existence of so many links (i.e., horizontally mediated
links) makes it unlikely a priori that the association between any one genetic
variant and a specific, multiply determined outcome will be very large.
Indeed, there is a growing consensus in the study of molecular genetics that
the association between specific polymorphisms and phenotypic outcomes
is extremely small—smaller, in some cases, than a Pearson correlation of
.02 (see Gibson, 2012). Thus the only way to reliably detect such findings
in the context of null hypothesis significance testing is to use extremely
large sample sizes—sample sizes that far exceed what is routine in social,
developmental, and personality research. To detect an effect of .02 with
80% power, for example, one would need at least 15,455 participants. (And
that estimate does not account for the alpha adjustments that one might
wish to use for testing multiple genetic variants instead of a single variant.)
Moreover, with small sample sizes, it is easy to falsely detect an association
that does not exist (e.g., Simmons, Nelson, & Simonsohn, 2011; Roisman,
Booth-LaForce, Belsky, Burt, & Groh, 2013). Thus, even if bivariate genetic
effects are really small, it should not be surprising if there are a few sizable
ones reported in the literature. Nonetheless, the point is that nonreplicated
findings of genetic associations are not particularly informative. If research-
ers are to pursue the way in which specific genetic variants shape romantic
functioning, we strongly recommend the use of large sample sizes.
Another exciting direction is the examination of G × E interactions.
One reason why G × E research has the potential to be valuable for studying
the ways in which early experiences might shape interpersonal functioning
later in life is that such models treat both genetic and environmental factors
as legitimate contributors to phenotypic variation. In contrast, biometric
models based on twin designs often pit genetic and environmental factors
against one another, such that higher variance estimates of one component
necessitate lower variance estimates for other components.
One emerging area of investigation in G × E research involves the
examination of alternative interaction models. The classic model, often
referred to as a dual-risk or a diathesis–stress model, assumes that some
individuals possess vulnerabilities that may make them more susceptible to
negative environmental experiences (e.g., neglectful caregiving) than other
individuals. In the context of G × E research, these vulnerabilities are often
construed as genetic risk factors, but historically they have often been stud-
ied as cognitive risk factors (e.g., Abramson et al., 2002). Another model
that has been gaining attention in recent years is differential susceptibility
theory (e.g., Belsky & Pleuss, 2009) or the theory of biological sensitiv-
ity to context (e.g., Boyce & Ellis, 2005). According to these frameworks,
34 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
many of the factors that investigators have identified as potential risk fac-
tors are not risk factors per se, but are better construed as plasticity fac-
tors. In other words, although some individuals might be at greater risk for
developing poor interpersonal skills in difficult caregiving environments,
those same people are more likely than others to thrive in supportive care-
giving environments. They are differentially susceptible to environmental
influences, for better and for worse.
Salo et al. (2011) found an interaction between variation in HTR2A
and maternal nurturance, such that individuals who carried two T alleles of
the HTR2A rs6313 SNP exhibited a stronger inverse association between
maternal nurturance at age 10 and avoidant attachment measured 21–27
years later. These individuals were more likely both to benefit from early
maternal nurturance and to be harmed by the relative lack of nurturance.
We (Fraley, Roisman, Booth-LaForce, et al., 2013) found a similar effect—
that increases in maternal sensitivity over childhood and adolescence pre-
dicted lower levels of avoidance at age 18 among individuals who carried
two T alleles of HTR2A than among C allele carriers. However, we found
that even C carriers exhibited a negative association between increases in
maternal sensitivity and avoidance, suggesting that the basic effect held for
people with both genetic variants.
We believe that this kind of work has the potential to be of great inter-
est in the near future, but again we offer some caveats. As in the molecular
genetic work discussed previously, there is a lot of room for “discovering”
things that are not replicable. Specifically, the number of genetic variants
that can be studied is large, and the set of G × E interactions that can be
tested is even larger. Because this work is relatively new, it is difficult to
articulate and defend strong predictions a priori. This makes it particularly
important that researchers think carefully about sample sizes and attempt
to replicate any findings that they uncover before weaving those findings
into the knowledge base of psychology.
References
Abramson, L. Y., Alloy, L. B., Hankin, B. L., Haeffel, G. J., MacCoon, D. G., &
Gibb, B. E. (2002). Cognitive vulnerability–stress models of depression in a
self-regulatory and psychobiological context. In I. H. Gotlib & C. L. Hammen
(Eds.), Handbook of depression (pp. 268–294). New York: Guilford Press.
Ainsworth, M. S. (1989). Attachments beyond infancy. American Psychologist,
44, 709–716.
Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. (1991). Attachment styles among young adults:
A test of the four-category model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 61, 226–245.
Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2009). Beyond diathesis stress: Differential susceptibility
to environmental influences. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 885–908.
Early Attachment and Romantic Functioning 35
Boling, M. W., Barry, C. M., Kotchick, B. A., & Lowry, J. (2011). Relations among
early adolescents’ parent–adolescent attachment, perceived social compe-
tence, and friendship quality. Psychological Reports, 109, 819–841.
Bowlby, J. (1969/1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York:
Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation: Anxiety and anger.
New York: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J., Robertson, J., & Rosenbluth, D. (1952). A two-year-old goes to the hos-
pital. In R. S. Eisler, A. Freud, H. Hartmann, & E. Kris (Eds.), Psychoanalytic
study of the child (Vol. 7, pp. 82–94). New York: International Universities
Press.
Boyce, W. T., & Ellis, B. J. (2005). Biological sensitivity to context: A. An evolu-
tionary–developmental theory of the origins and functions of stress reactivity.
Development and Psychopathology, 17, 271–301.
Bretherton, I., & Munholland, K. A. (1999). Internal working models in attachment
relationships: A construct revisited. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Hand-
book of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 89–111).
New York: Guilford Press.
Caughlin, J. P., Huston, T. L., & Houts, R. M. (2000). How does personality mat-
ter in marriage?: An examination of trait anxiety, interpersonal negativity,
and marital satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78,
326–336.
Collins, N. L., & Feeney, B. C. (2000). A safe haven: An attachment theory per-
spective on support seeking and caregiving in intimate relationships. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 1053–1073.
Crowell, J., & Owens, G. (1996). Current Relationship Interview. Unpublished
manuscript, State University of New York at Stony Brook.
DeWolff, M., & van IJzendoorn, M. (1997). Sensitivity and attachment: A meta-
analysis on parental antecedents of infant attachment. Child Development,
68, 571–591.
Donnellan, M. B., Burt, S. A., Levendosky, A., & Klump, K. (2008). Genes, per-
sonality, and attachment in adults: A multivariate behavioral genetic analysis.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 3–16.
Ellis, B. J., Figueredo, A. J., Brumbach, B. H., & Schlomer, G. L. (2009). Funda-
mental dimensions of environmental risk. Human Nature, 20, 204–268.
Englund, M. M., Kuo, S. I., Puig, J., & Collins, W. A. (2011). Early roots of adult
competence: The significance of close relationships from infancy to early
adulthood. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 35, 490–496.
Fearon, R. M. P., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Fonagy, P., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.
J., Schuengel, C., & Bokhorst, C. L. (2006). In search of shared and non-
shared environmental factors in security of attachment: A behavior-genetic
study of the association between sensitivity and attachment security. Devel-
opmental Psychology, 42, 1026–1040.
Fraley, R. C., & Brumbaugh, C. C. (2004). A dynamical systems approach to
understanding stability and change in attachment security. In W. S. Rholes &
J. A. Simpson (Eds.), Adult attachment: Theory, research, and clinical impli-
cations (pp. 86–132). New York: Guilford Press.
Fraley, R. C., & Heffernan, M. E. (2013). Attachment and parental divorce: A test
36 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
of the diffusion and sensitive period hypotheses. Personality and Social Psy-
chology Bulletin, 39, 1199–1213.
Fraley, R. C., Roisman, G. I., Booth-LaForce, C., Owen, M. T., & Holland, A. S.
(2013). Interpersonal and genetic origins of adult attachment styles: A lon-
gitudinal study from infancy to early adulthood. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 104, 817–838.
Fraley, R. C., Roisman, G. I., & Haltigan, J. D. (2013). The legacy of early
experiences in development: Formalizing alternative models of how early
experiences are carried forward over time. Developmental Psychology, 49,
109–126.
Furman, W., Simon, V. A., Shaffer, L., & Bouchey, H. A. (2002). Adolescents’
working models and styles with parents, friends, and romantic partners.
Child Development, 73, 241–255.
Gibson, G. (2012). Rare and common variants: Twenty arguments. Nature Reviews
Genetics, 13(2), 135–145.
Gillath, O., Shaver, P. R., Baek, J. M., & Chun, S. D. (2008). Genetic correlates
of adult attachment style. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34,
1396–1405.
Groh, A. M., Fearon, R. P., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van IJzendoorn, M.
H., Steele, R. D., & Roisman, G. I. (2014). The significance of attachment
security for children’s social competence with peers: A meta-analytic study.
Attachment and Human Development, 16(2), 103–136.
Kaplan, H. S., & Gangestad, S. W. (2005). Life history theory and evolutionary
psychology. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology
(pp. 68–95). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Karney, B. R., & Bradbury, T. N. (1997). Neuroticism, marital interaction, and the
trajectory of marital satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 72, 1075–1092.
Kestenbaum, R., Farber, E., & Sroufe, L. A. (1989). Individual differences in empa-
thy among preschoolers’ concurrent and predictive validity. In N. Eisenberg
(Ed.), New Directions for Child Development: No. 44. Empathy and related
emotional responses (pp. 51–56). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Laible, D., & Thompson, R. (1998). Attachment and emotional understanding in
preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 34, 1038–1045.
Masten, A. S., & Cicchetti, D. (Eds.). (2010). Developmental cascades [Special
issue]. Development and Psychopathology, 22(3), 491–715 (Part 1); 22(4),
717–983 (Part 2).
Masten, A. S., Roisman, G. I., Long, J. D., Burt, K. B., Obradović, J., Riley, J.
R., et al. (2005). Developmental cascades: Linking academic achievement and
externalizing and internalizing symptoms over 20 years. Developmental Psy-
chology, 41, 733–746.
McElwain, N. L., Booth-LaForce, C., & Wu, X. (2011). Infant–mother attachment
and children’s friendship quality: Maternal mental-state talk as an interven-
ing mechanism. Developmental Psychology, 47, 1295–1311.
McElwain, N. L., Cox, M. J., Burchinal, M. R., & Macfie, J. (2003). Differenti-
ating among insecure mother–infant attachment classifications: A focus on
child–friend interaction and exploration during solitary play at 36 months.
Attachment and Human Development, 5, 136–164.
Early Attachment and Romantic Functioning 37
Parker, J. G., & Asher, S. R. (1993). Friendship and friendship quality in middle
childhood: Links with peer group acceptance and feelings of loneliness and
social dissatisfaction. Developmental Psychology, 29, 611–621.
Raby, K. L., Roisman, G. I., Fraley, R. C., & Simpson, J. A. (in press). The endur-
ing predictive significance of early sensitivity: Social and academic compe-
tence through age 32 years. Child Development.
Roisman, G. I., Booth-LaForce, C., Belsky, J., Burt, K. B., & Groh, A. M. (2013).
Molecular-genetic correlates of infant attachment: A cautionary tale. Attach-
ment and Human Development, 15, 384–406.
Roisman, G. I., Collins, W. A., Sroufe, L. A., & Egeland, B. (2005). Predictors
of young adults’ representations of and behavior in their current romantic
relationship: Prospective tests of the prototype hypothesis. Attachment and
Human Development, 7, 105–121.
Roisman, G. I., & Fraley, R. C. (2006). The limits of genetic influence: A behavior-
genetic analysis of infant–caregiver relationship quality and temperament.
Child Development, 77, 1656–1667.
Roisman, G. I., & Fraley, R. C. (2008). A behavior-genetic study of parenting qual-
ity, infant attachment security, and their covariation in a nationally represen-
tative sample. Developmental Psychology, 44, 831–839.
Salo, J., Jokela, M., Lehtimäki, T., & Keltikangas-Järvinen, L. (2011) Serotonin
receptor 2A gene moderates the effect of childhood maternal nurturance on
adulthood social attachment. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 10, 702–709.
Salvatore, J. E., Kuo, S. I., Steele, R. D., Simpson, J. A., & Collins, W. A. (2011).
Recovering from conflict in romantic relationships: A developmental perspec-
tive. Psychological Science, 22, 376–383.
Schneider, B. H., Atkinson, L., & Tardif, C. (2001). Child–parent attachment and
children’s peer relations: A quantitative review. Developmental Psychology,
37, 86–100.
Shulman, S., Elicker, J., & Sroufe, L. A. (1994). Stages of friendship growth in pre-
adolescence as related to attachment history. Journal of Social and Personal
Relationships, 11, 341–361.
Simmons, J., Nelson, L., & Simonsohn, U. (2011). False-positive psychology:
Undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis allow presenting any-
thing as significant. Psychological Science, 22, 1359–1366.
Simpson, J. A., Collins, W. A., Tran, S., & Haydon, K. C. (2007). Attachment
and the experience and expression of emotions in romantic relationships: A
developmental perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92,
355–367.
Simpson, J. A., Griskevicius, V., Kuo, S. I., Sung, S., & Collins, W. A. (2012). Evo-
lution, stress, and sensitive periods: The influence of unpredictability in early
versus late childhood on sex and risky behavior. Developmental Psychology,
48, 674–686.
Simpson, J. A., Kim, J. S., Fillo, J., Ickes, W., Rholes, W. S., Oriña, M. M., et al.
(2011). Attachment and the management of empathic accuracy in relation-
ship-threatening situations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37,
242–254.
Slavich, G. M., & Cole, S. W. (2013). The emerging field of human social genom-
ics. Clinical Psychological Science, 1, 331–348.
38 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Omri Gillath
W hat can electrical activity along the scalp, blood flow to specific
brain areas, levels of chemicals in a synapse or the bloodstream, and the
structure of one’s double helix tell us about abstract concepts such as love,
security, and attachment? If we consider the recent upsurge in research
focusing on the micro-level analysis of attachment, the answer is—a lot.
Building on the knowledge base and methods developed within cogni-
tive psychology, neuroscience, psychophysiology, genetics, endocrinology,
and immunology, scholars have started to provide a new and exciting set
of answers to fundamental questions related to attachment theory and
research. Issues such as “How do attachment bonds develop?”, “Why do
people have a specific attachment style?”, “What is attachment security?”,
and “Is attachment an emotion or a motivation?” have gained renewed
interest while being considered from new angles. Research can now begin
to answer questions such as these by examining the systems and processes
underlying attachment.
Neuroscience is an interdisciplinary area, which builds on and
exchanges ideas with such fields as psychology, computer science, psycho-
neuroimmunology, neuroendocrinology, and genetics. In the current chap-
ter, I start with a brief summary of the leading principles and ideas relevant
to attachment and its investigation via neuroscientific tools. I follow this
summary with a literature review that highlights different techniques (such
39
40 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Not all interactions with attachment figures are positive and result in a
sense of greater attachment security. According to Ainsworth et al. (1978),
there are significant individual differences in attachment system dynamics
and functioning, based on the repeated interactions people experience with
their attachment figures. When attachment figures are sensitive, available,
and responsive in times of need, people feel they can confidently rely on
them. This, in turn, facilitates the development of a sense of connectedness
and security. In contrast, when attachment figures are not reliably available
and supportive, people are likely to feel a sense of attachment insecurity.
Being chronically insecure, in turn, is associated with pursuing secondary
attachment strategies—those that deactivate or hyperactivate the attach-
ment system—instead of the primary strategy of proximity seeking.
Ainsworth et al. (1978) identified two types of insecurity. When care-
givers tend to be cold and rejecting in times of need, people who seek prox-
imity and support from them are likely to develop an avoidant attachment
style characterized by distrust of relationship partners, strong striving for
independence, and emotional distancing. People high on attachment avoid-
ance tend to downplay the importance of emotions and relationship-related
issues and to use deactivating strategies. These strategies involve dismissal
of threat and attachment-related cues and suppression of attachment-
related thoughts, emotions, and memories.
When caregivers tend to be intrusive and to provide inconsistent and
insensitive support, people are likely to develop an anxious attachment style
characterized by chronic worries related to relationship partners not being
available in times of need. People high on attachment anxiety tend to per-
ceive themselves as worthless and helpless, are hypervigilant to relationship-
related cues, and use hyperactivating secondary strategies. These strategies
include high sensitivity to signs of rejection, intense appeals to attachment
figures, and obsessive reliance on them as a source of safety and support.
Attachment styles, which are often conceptualized in terms of a two-
dimensional space with attachment anxiety and avoidance as its axes, can
be assessed with reliable and valid self-report scales such as the Experiences
in Close Relationships (ECR; Brennan, Clark, & Shaver, 1998). In studies
using such measures, attachment styles have been associated in theoretically
predictable ways with relationship variables such as quality and length,
affect regulation strategies, and many other outcomes (for reviews, see Cas-
sidy & Shaver, 2008; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007a). People who score low
on both attachment dimensions tend to be secure or have a secure attach-
ment style. Attachment security is associated with greater self-confidence,
better personal adjustment, more effective emotion regulation, good com-
munication with relationship partners, generous and helpful behavior, and
more satisfying relationships. When I refer to people as anxious, avoidant,
or secure throughout this chapter, I mean that they score relatively high on
attachment anxiety or avoidance, or they score low on both (secure).
42 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
form better attachment bonds and help those who have problems with cre-
ating such bonds (e.g., Romanian orphans; Chisholm, 1998).
A third example of the value of neuroscience to the understanding of
attachment involves emotion regulation. People who have different attach-
ment styles cope differently and exhibit different emotion regulation strat-
egies (e.g., suppression, enhancement). For example, anxiously attached
people tend to be highly emotional and overwhelmed by their emotions,
whereas securely attached people have a weaker emotional reaction to dis-
tressing information (Nash, Prentice, Hirsh, McGregor, & Inzlicht, 2014).
A number of explanations have been suggested for these behaviors, but
it remains unclear why anxiously attached people manifest emotions so
intensely—is it due to higher sensitivity? Lower ability to control? Or both?
Using neuroimaging, we (Gillath et al., 2005) have found that when people
are asked to suppress their negative thoughts and emotions during an emo-
tion regulation task, anxiously attached people exhibit lower activation in
the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC). The OFC is associated with emotion regu-
lation skills, the lower activation found in anxiously attached people could
hence be interpreted as lesser engagement of this area in the process among
anxiously attached people. This, in turn, suggests that the extreme emo-
tional reactions of anxiously attached people are due to their lack of ability
to regulate emotions (Gillath et al., 2005; Warren et al., 2010).
A final example involves our understanding of security priming.
Whereas most of the research on attachment in general, and attachment
neuroscience more specifically, has focused on attachment styles (anxiety
and avoidance), less is known about the enhancement of attachment secu-
rity and especially its underlying neural mechanisms. To address this gap,
we (Canterberry & Gillath, 2012) recently examined people’s brain activa-
tion as we exposed them to attachment-security-related primes. Behavioral
studies have provided ample evidence to suggest that the enhancement of
attachment security has a host of beneficial outcomes for personal and rela-
tional well-being (for reviews, see Gillath et al., 2008; Mikulincer & Shaver,
2007b). In our study we suggested and tested the proposal that the benefits
associated with security are the outcome of three processes—cognitive,
affective, and behavioral. Indeed, we found that security priming led to dis-
tributed, co-occurring activation in brain areas reflective of these cognitive,
affective, and behavioral processes (e.g., the PFC, parahippocampus, and
temporal and parietal gyri). These patterns of activation related to security
were moderated by attachment styles. For example, attachment avoidance
was associated with activation in areas related to encoding and retrieval
(parahippocampal gyrus), suggesting that avoidantly attached people were
making increased memory retrieval attempts during the exposure to the
prime, perhaps reflecting a lack of easily accessible secure models.
These findings, while consistent with the existing attachment literature,
go beyond behavioral findings to demonstrate that all three components
The Neuroscience of Attachment 47
information in the brain. Cohen and Shaver suggest that because avoid-
antly attached people have less experience with positive attachment-related
information, they are more likely to make more errors, especially in the
hemisphere that has less to do with processing of positive information.
EEG can also be used to study specific neural reactions to events by
investigating event-related potentials (ERPs), such as P3 or N1 (the letter
represents positive–negative polarity, and the number represents the latency
in hundreds of milliseconds from the event). ERPs are caused by cognitive
processes that involve, among others, memory, expectation, attention, and
change in mental states. For example, when Zhang et al. (2008) examined
people’s reactions to facial expressions, they found that attachment styles
were related to differences in several components (N1, N2, P2, and N400),
suggesting that people who had different attachment styles differed in terms
of both early automatic encoding and late elaborative retrieval of emotional
content. Thus avoidant participants showed a less negative N1 and N400
compared to anxious and secure participants. N1 is thought to represent
level of attention (Hillyard, Teder-Sälejärvi, & Münte, 1998). From these
results, one might conclude that avoidant individuals devote less attention
to emotional stimuli than secure or anxious people do.
In a similar manner, Dan and Raz (2012) found differences on C1 (C
for Component; it can be either positive or negative; C1 is the first visual
ERP component that peaks between 50–100ms) and P1 mean amplitudes
at occipital and posterior-parietal channels in response to angry faces ver-
sus neutral faces, but only among people high on avoidance. The processing
biases toward angry faces (in the P1 component) and toward neutral faces
(in the C1 component) among avoidant people suggest that only avoidant
participants have the capacity to identify cues at such early stages of infor-
mation processing, which allows them to apply their deactivating strategies
(also see Niedenthal, Brauer, Robin, & Innes-Ker, 2002). Focusing on anx-
ious individuals, Zayas, Shoda, Mischel, Osterhout, and Takahashi (2009)
and Zilber, Goldstein, and Mikulincer (2007) demonstrated attachment
anxiety to be associated with later ERP components, such as N400 and
the late positive potential (LPP). For example, Zayas et al. (2009) found
that when participants were exposed to attachment-related cues, rejection-
related words (e.g., dismissing) elicited greater N400 amplitudes than
acceptance-related words (e.g., supporting) among women high on anxiety
and low on avoidance. N400 is thought to reflect the amount of semantic
processing elicited by a stimulus. People tend to process more when the
stimulus is unexpected or have a greater personal significance. Zayas et al.
concluded that anxiously attached women perceive rejection cues as more
personally significant, posing greater threat to the self.
In addition to looking at brain activation, either per region (fMRI),
per hemisphere (in laterality studies), or at a specific time point (ERPs),
researchers have also investigated brain structure or volume. For instance,
The Neuroscience of Attachment 49
Neurotransmitters/
hormones
Cognitive abilities/
mechanisms
FIGURE 2.1. Attachment as a function of genes and brain structure and function.
Genes include dopamine (DRD2, DRD4, DAT), serotonin (5HT), oxytocin
(OXTR), and catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT), among others. Brain
structure/function includes volume, connectivity, and mechanisms in areas
such as the hippocampus, amygdala, dorsal ACC, SCC, and OFC. Cognitive
abilities/mechanisms include attention, emotion regulation, thought control,
self-regulation, and IWMs.
Animal Models
There is a very broad literature on animal models of bonding, attachment,
and close relationships (e.g., Carter et al., 2005), which I only briefly touch
upon. Animal models constitute a powerful method for studying the social
brain and the neurobiological mechanisms underlying social relationships,
attachment included (e.g., Bales, Maninger, & Hinde, 2012). For instance,
oxytocin, which is thought to be a central player in human attachment and
bonding, was first examined in animal models (see Carter et al., 2005;
Insel & Young, 2001). In studies using animal models, researchers use
observational methods to identify bonding (social or pair-bonding) behav-
iors such as separation distress and soothing, or relationship/attachment
styles. Animal models of attachment and pair bonding created by Michael
Meaney and others are crucial in our understanding of the roles epigenetics
and neural mechanisms play in these systems and behaviors (see Bagot et
al., 2009; Bales et al., 2012; Carter et al., 2005; Lim & Young, 2006).
Meaney’s work demonstrated that parental behavior affects gene expres-
sion in the rat pup, which in turn affects the future parenting behavior of
the pup when it reaches adulthood. The major advantages of this approach
over work based on humans are the abilities to (1) study intergenerational
effects in much shorter time frames, (2) use genetic or chemical manipula-
tions that would be hard or impossible to use in humans, (3) inflict lesions,
and (4) perform postmortem analysis that would be prohibited, more dif-
ficult, or unethical in humans—all of which permit clearer inferences about
54 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
causality. Thus animal models provide additional angles that permit better
and deeper understanding of the structures, mechanisms, and functions
involved in attachment processes and outcomes in ways that typically are
not possible with human participants.
Theoretical Models
Although the research in the domain of attachment neuroscience is rela-
tively young, important findings have started to accumulate, and research-
ers have developed preliminary conceptual models to organize these
findings. For example, Fonagy, Luyten, and Strathearn (2011) suggest a
developmental, biobehavioral switch model, not focused specifically on
attachment, but rather on its associations with mentalization (i.e., the abil-
ity to understand the mental state of oneself and others) and stress. The
model is based on early work by Panksepp (1998) and Insel (e.g., Insel &
Young, 2001). The work, focusing on animal models, links attachment
bonds with substance dependence and opioids, suggesting that attachment
might be based on the same mechanisms as addictive disorders (Burkett
& Young, 2012). These mechanisms involve two neural systems, which
are the same systems that Fonagy et al. focus on in their model: (1) the
dopaminergic system (Ferris et al., 2005; Strathearn, Fonagy, Amico,
& Montague, 2009), and (2) the oxytocinergic system (Bartels & Zeki,
2004; Champagne, Diorio, Sharma, & Meaney, 2001; Feldman, Weller,
Zagoory-Sharon, & Levine, 2007). The dopaminergic system is associ-
ated with sensitivity to cues, and both the dopaminergic and oxytocinergic
systems are associated with responding to social cues and with rewarding
social and relational behaviors.
Tying their model to personality disorders, Fonagy et al. (2011) sug-
gest that complex interactions among environmental, biological, and psy-
chosocial factors affect the two neural systems, which in turn shape the
attachment system, and more specifically its threshold of activation. These
interactions also affect people’s ability to differentiate the mental states of
self and others, which decreases the sensitivity and susceptibility to conta-
gion from other people’s mental states, reduces integration of cognitive and
affective aspects of mentalization, and increases dysfunctions in stress reg-
ulation systems. These, in turn, affect the ability of people to regulate their
behavior. Together, the changes in threshold level and regulation or control
can lead to the development of insecure or even disorganized attachment.
Fonagy et al.’s model focuses on attachment and its association with mental
disorders. It draws a lot of its evidence from findings relevant to mothers’
behaviors in response to their offspring, which are more closely related
to the activation of the caregiving system than to that of the attachment
system (for similar models, see Atzil, Hendler, & Feldman, 2011; Galynker
The Neuroscience of Attachment 55
et al., 2012). Hence I turn next to Vrticka and Vuilleumier’s (2012) model,
which focuses less on mental disorders and the caregiving system.
Vrticka and Vuilleumier (2012) suggest that individual differences in
attachment styles correlate with various affective and cognitive processes,
particularly in attachment-relevant or social contexts. Their model of the
influence of adult attachment on social processing, which incorporates
Fonagy et al.’s (2011) model, involves two core networks: one network that
is associated with affective evaluation processes (such as threat or reward),
and includes approach and avoidance components; and another network
that is associated with cognitive control and mentalizing abilities, and
includes emotion regulation and mental state representation components).
Their model is similar to the attachment model suggested by Pietromonaco
and Barrett (2000) in terms of its affective and emotion regulation compo-
nents, and to more general models of social cognition and emotion process-
ing (e.g., Lieberman, 2007).
When describing the neuroscientific aspect of their model, Vrticka
and Vuilleumier (2012) add the serotonergic and cortisol systems to the
dopaminergic and oxytocinergic systems suggested by Fonagy et al. (2011),
and discuss a set of specific brain regions for each network’s component:
approach (the ventral tegmental, hypothalamus, striatum, and ventral
medial OFC), avoidance (the amygdala, hippocampus, insula, ACC, and
anterior temporal pole), emotion regulation (the dorsolateral PFC and lat-
eral OFC), and mental state representation (the medial PFC, posterior cin-
gulate cortex, precuneus, posterior superior temporal sulcus, temporopari-
etal junction, and anterior superior temporal gyrus).
Vrticka and Vuilleumier (2012) further suggest that there is a dynamic
balance between the threat-sensitive system motivating social aversion and
the attachment system that promotes a sense of safety via close relation-
ships and approach (MacDonald & MacDonald, 2011). According to this
explanation, attachment bonds serve as social rewards in the approach sys-
tem. Both approach and aversion are thought to be shaped by genes and the
environment, and modulated by attachment avoidance and anxiety. Thus
people high on attachment avoidance are thought to have weaker brain
activation in areas related to both the approach and the avoidance systems,
in line with their use of deactivating strategies; by contrast, people high
on anxiety are thought to have stronger brain activation, but mainly with
regard to the aversion system and the processing of negative social cues,
in line with their use of hyperactivating strategies. People who are low on
both dimensions are thought to have weaker reactions as compared with
anxiously attached individuals, but due to their effective regulation rather
than their deactivation of the attachment system (for a similar model and
findings, see Warren et al., 2010).
Coan (2010) suggests a different model—one that focuses on the
regulatory role of the attachment system via overt emotional and social
56 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
behavior. His model describes the neural systems involved in the forma-
tion and maintenance of adult attachment relationships and the way the
brain supports attachment behaviors. As Vrticka and Vuilleumier (2012)
do, Coan (2010) builds on research regarding the neural systems support-
ing emotion, emotion regulation, motivation, and social behavior. He also
introduces the social baseline model of social affect regulation. The model
integrates existing models of attachment with a neuroscientific principle—
economy of action—in the management of metabolic resources devoted to
emotional and social behavior. According to the model, adult attachment
relationships conserve brain metabolic resources, especially those of the
PFC.
Coan’s (2010) model, which is an attempt to bridge the gap between
the broad animal literature on bonding and the extended work on human
attachment behavior, depicts the attachment behavioral system as a higher-
order construct. This construct includes basic behaviors, such as recog-
nition and familiarity, proximity seeking, separation distress, soothing
behaviors, and maternal caregiving. Like Vrticka and Vuilleumier (2012)
and Fonagy et al. (2011), Coan talks about emotion and emotion regula-
tion systems used for attachment behaviors, the relevance of threat- and
reward-related systems, and associations between attachment and cognitive
processes, such as attention and memory. However, he adds an economic
aspect above and beyond these other models. Accordingly, attachment is
tied to the brain’s energy expenditure management, and being together
or feeling securely attached “saves” brain energy. Being alone is straining
and costly (Beckes & Coan, 2011), whereas interacting with others—the
default setting of human existence, according to Coan—is less effortful.
Being with others allows people to spend fewer resources on activities such
as threat detection and emotion regulation, because it involves load sharing
via familiarity, interdependence, and interpersonal conditioning.
more components (or puzzle pieces) as the evidence for their role accumu-
lates.
While neuroscience provides researchers with a preview of the micro
level of attachment, combining micro-level research with the macro-level
studies will be necessary for a full understanding of the attachment system
(see Figure 2.2, and Gillath et al., 2012). For instance, adopting a cultural
perspective can allow researchers to grasp how the brain adapts to fit bet-
ter with specific contexts or demands (e.g., Wilson, 2010). Understanding
the functions of attachment in the culture-ready brain (Whitehead, 2010)
can position attachment at the forefront of the new domain of cultural neu-
roscience (Chiao, 2010). Some preliminary work in this direction already
exists. For example, Eisenberg et al. (2010) describe the role of D4 dopa-
mine receptors in pair-bonding processes across different cultures/con-
texts, and Ray et al. (2010) describe differences in neural representations of
self and other (specifically, the mother) as a function of a specific cultural
context—interdependent self-construal.
Any model that seeks to explain the neuroscience of attachment should
also deal with the construct of attachment security and its underlying mech-
anisms. As mentioned above, we (Canterberry & Gillath, 2012) recently
conducted a study focusing on this aspect, showing that security involves
affective (increased positive mood and relaxation), cognitive (increased self-
and emotion regulation), and behavioral (prosocial tendencies) components.
Macro Micro
(interindividual) (intraindividual)
Context Neuroscience
Environment of Developmental
Current Brain Brain Hormones/
Culture evolutionary environment Genes
environment volume activation neurotransmitters
adaptedness (prenatal)
Attachment
system
Cognitive Regulatory
Behavior Attitudes Emotions
mechanisms mechanisms
FIGURE 2.2. Combining the micro and macro levels to gain a better understand-
ing of attachment.
The Neuroscience of Attachment 59
Baseline
FIGURE 2.3. The relations between attachment security and insecurity. The
dashed lines represent alternatives to the insecurity–security continuum, and
more generally the potential for two separate processes or systems for security
and insecurity.
60 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
References
Ahern, G. L., & Schwartz, G. E. (1985). Differential lateralization for positive and
negative emotion in the human brain: EEG spectral analysis. Neuropsycho-
logia, 23, 745–755.
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of
attachment: A psychological study of the Strange Situation. Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Anderson, M. C., Ochsner, K. N., Kuhl, B., Cooper, J., Robertson, E., Gabrieli,
S. W., et al. (2004). Neural systems underlying the suppression of unwanted
memories. Science, 303, 232–235.
Aron, A., Fisher, H., Mashek, D. J., Strong, G., Li, H. F., & Brown, L. L. (2005).
Reward, motivation, and emotion systems associated with early-stage intense
romantic love. Journal of Neurophysiology, 94, 327–337.
Atzil, S., Hendler, T., & Feldman, R. (2011). Specifying the neurobiological basis
of human attachment: Brain, hormones, and behavior in synchronous and
intrusive mothers. Neuropsychopharmacology, 36, 2603–2615.
Bagot, R. C., van Hasselt, F. N., Champagne, D. L., Meaney, M. J., Krugers, H. J.,
& Joels, M. (2009). Maternal care determines rapid effects of stress mediators
on synaptic plasticity in adult rat hippocampal dentate gyrus. Neurobiology
of Learning and Memory, 92, 292–300.
The Neuroscience of Attachment 61
Dawson, G., Ashman, S. B., Hessl, D., Spieker, S., Frey, K., Panagiotides, H., et al.
(2001). Autonomic and brain electrical activity in securely- and insecurely-
attached infants of depressed mothers. Infant Behavior and Development,
24, 135–149.
De Dreu, C. K. (2012). Oxytocin modulates the link between adult attachment and
cooperation through reduced betrayal aversion. Psychoneuroendocrinology,
37, 871–880.
DeWall, C. N., Masten, C. L., Powell, C., Combs, D., Schurtz, D. R., & Eisen-
berger, N. I. (2012). Do neural responses to rejection depend on attachment
style?: An fMRI study. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7, 184–
192.
Del Giudice, M. (2009). Sex, attachment, and the development of reproductive
strategies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32, 1–21.
Diamond, L. M., & Fagundes, C. P. (2010). Psychobiological research on attach-
ment. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 27, 218–225.
Donnellan, M. B., Burt, S. A., Levendosky, A. A., & Klump, K. L. (2008). Genes,
personality, and attachment in adults: A multivariate behavioral genetic anal-
ysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 3–16.
Drevets, W. C. (2000). Neuroimaging studies of mood disorders. Biological Psy-
chiatry, 48, 813–829.
Eisenberg, D. T. A., Apicella, C. L., Campbell, B. C., Dreber, A., Garcia, J. R., &
Lum, J. K. (2010). Assortative human pair-bonding for partner ancestry and
allelic variation of the dopamine receptor D4 (DRD4) gene. Social Cognitive
and Affective Neuroscience, 5, 194–202.
Eisenberger, N. I., & Lieberman, M. D. (2004). Why rejection hurts: A common
neural alarm system for physical and social pain. Trends in Cognitive Sci-
ences, 8, 294–300.
Eisenberger, N. I., Master, S. L., Inagaki, T. K., Taylor, S. E., Shirinyan, D., Lieber-
man, M. D., et al. (2011). Attachment figures activate a safety signal-related
neural region and reduce pain experience. Proceedings of the National Acad-
emy of Sciences USA, 108, 11721–11726.
Elliot, A. J., & Reis, H. T. (2003). Attachment and exploration in adulthood. Jour-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 317–331.
Feldman, R., Weller, A., Zagoory-Sharon, O., & Levine, A. (2007). Evidence for a
neuroendocrinological foundation of human affiliation plasma oxytocin lev-
els across pregnancy and the postpartum period predict mother–infant bond-
ing. Psychological Science, 18, 965–970.
Ferris, C. F., Kulkarni, P., Sullivan, J. M., Harder, J. A., Messenger, T. L., & Febo,
M. (2005). Pup suckling is more rewarding than cocaine: Evidence from func-
tional magnetic resonance imaging and three-dimensional computational
analysis. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 149–156.
Fonagy, P., Luyten, P., & Strathearn, L. (2011). Borderline personality disorder,
mentalization, and the neurobiology of attachment. Infant Mental Health
Journal, 32, 47–69.
Fraley, R. C., Roisman, G. I., Booth-LaForce, C., Owen, M. T., & Holland, A. S.
(2013). Interpersonal and genetic origins of adult attachment styles: A lon-
gitudinal study from infancy to early adulthood. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 104, 817–838.
64 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Galynker, I. I., Yaseen, Z. S., Katz, C., Zhang, X., Jennings-Donovan, G., Dash-
naw, S., et al. (2012). Distinct but overlapping neural networks subserve
depression and insecure attachment. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuro-
science, 7, 896–908.
Gillath, O., Atchley, R., Imran, A., & El-Hodiri, M. (2014). Attachment, game
theory, and neuroscience: Examining the enhancement and experience of
generous behavior. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Gillath, O., Bunge, S. A., Shaver, P. R., Wendelken, C., & Mikulincer, M. (2005).
Attachment-style differences in the ability to suppress negative thoughts:
Exploring the neural correlates. NeuroImage, 28, 835–847.
Gillath, O., Canterberry, M., & Collins, T. J. (2012). A multilevel multimethod
interdisciplinary approach to the understanding of attachment? In O. Gillath,
G. Adams, & A. D. Kunkel (Eds.), Relationship science: Integrating evolu-
tionary, neuroscience, and sociocultural approaches (pp. 219–240). Wash-
ington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Gillath, O., Giesbrecht, B., & Shaver, P. R. (2009). Attachment, attention, and cog-
nitive control: Attachment style and performance on general attention tasks.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 647–654.
Gillath, O., Hart, J., Noftle, E. E., & Stockdale, G. D. (2009). Development and
validation of a state adult attachment measure (SAAM). Journal of Research
in Personality, 43, 362–373.
Gillath, O., Pressman, S., Stetler, D., & Moskovitz, J. (2014). Attachment security
and metabolic energy: Enhancement of security results with increased blood
glucose. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Gillath, O., Selcuk, E., & Shaver, P. R. (2008). Moving toward a secure attachment
style: Can repeated security priming help? Social and Personality Psychology
Compass, 2(4), 1651–1666.
Gillath, O., Sesko, A. K., Shaver, P. R., & Chun, D. S. (2010). Attachment, authen-
ticity, and honesty: Dispositional and experimentally induced security can
reduce self- and other-deception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 98, 841–855.
Gross, C., Zhuang, X., Stark, K., Ramboz, S., Oosting, R., Kirby, L., et al. (2002).
Serotonin 1A receptor acts during development to establish normal anxiety-
like behavior in the adult. Nature, 416, 396–400.
Hellige, J. B. (1993). Hemispheric asymmetry. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univer-
sity Press.
Hillyard, S. A., Teder-Sälejärvi, W. A., & Münte, T. F. (1998). Temporal dynamics
of early perceptual processing. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 8, 202–
210.
Insel, T. R., & Young, L. J. (2001). The neurobiology of attachment. Nature
Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 129–136.
Karremans, J. C., Heslenfeld, D. J., van Dillen, L. F., & Van Lange, P. A. (2011).
Secure attachment partners attenuate neural responses to social exclusion: An
fMRI investigation. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 81, 44–50.
Kennedy, P. J., & Shapiro, M. L. (2004). Retrieving memories via internal context
requires the hippocampus. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 6979–6985.
Lakatos, K., Nemoda, Z., Toth, I., Ronai, Z., Ney, K., Sasvari-Szekely, M., et al.
(2002). Further evidence for the role of the dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4)
The Neuroscience of Attachment 65
Warren, S. L., Bost, K. K., Roisman, G. I., Silton, R. L., Spielberg, J. M., Engels,
A. S., et al. (2010). Effects of adult attachment and emotional distractors on
brain mechanisms of cognitive control. Psychological Science, 21, 1818–1826.
Whitehead, C. (2010). The culture ready brain. Social Cognitive and Affective
Neuroscience, 5, 168–179.
Wilson, M. (2010). The re-tooled mind: How culture re-engineers cognition. Social
Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5, 180–187.
Young, L. J., & Wang, Z. (2004). The neurobiology of pair bonding. Nature Neu-
roscience, 7, 1048–1054.
Zayas, V., & Hazan, C. (Eds.). (2015). Bases of adult attachment: Linking brain,
mind, and behavior. New York: Springer.
Zayas, V., Shoda, Y., Mischel, W., Osterhout, L., & Takahashi, M. (2009). Neural
responses to partner rejection cues. Psychological Science, 20, 813–821.
Zhang, X., Li, T., & Zhou, X. (2008). Brain responses to facial expressions by
adults with different attachment-orientations. NeuroReport, 19, 437–441.
Zilber, A., Goldstein, A., & Mikulincer, M. (2007). Adult attachment orienta-
tions and the processing of emotional pictures–ERP correlates. Personality
and Individual Differences, 43, 1898–1907.
3
Fooled Around and Fell in Love
The Role of Sex in Adult Romantic
Attachment Formation
Vivian Zayas
Sarah Merrill
Cindy Hazan
68
Sex and Romantic Attachment Formation 69
this initial sexual attraction sets the stage for potentially developing a full-
fledged attachment relationship.
Moreover, as the relationship gets under way, the sexual mating sys-
tem plays a key role in transforming the relationship from one of simple
attraction between two strangers to one between two attached partners.
In this respect, there are two issues of central importance. First, although
the sexual mating and attachment systems are distinct and independent,
and have evolved to serve different functions (Diamond, 2004), there is
still considerable overlap in basic structures of the two systems (Diamond
& Dickenson, 2012). Second, having sex repeatedly with the same person
engages these specific (and overlapping) brain structures and activates spe-
cific neurochemical systems that facilitate the formation of an adult attach-
ment bond. In what follows, we focus specifically on the dopaminergic
reward system and the oxytocinergic/opioid arousal-relieving systems. The
take-home message of the extant findings, which are discussed below, is
that through repeated sexual exchanges with the same person, these sys-
tems become conditioned. The effect of the conditioning is that this other
person, whether physically present or just mentally conjured, can automati-
cally activate these systems. This process of the conditioning of specific
reward and relief neural systems, in our view, is the core of adult attach-
ment formation.
First, a few caveats. Exactly how sex affects adult attachment is com-
plicated, and is likely to vary as a function of multiple individual and
group, and possibly gender, factors. We intentionally suspend commentary
on these possible variations (although we return to them at the end of the
chapter) in order to focus attention on a set of processes and mechanisms
that have yet to be articulated or explored in the attachment literature, and
that hold promise for shedding light on the role of sexual intercourse in the
formation of adult attachment bonds.
The chapter is organized as follows. We begin by providing a brief
summary of the behavioral, psychological, physiological, and neural corre-
lates assumed to characterize an adult attachment bond. We then describe
the basic physiological and neural processes involved in sexual intercourse,
and highlight systems that overlap with the attachment system. We con-
clude by raising many interesting, but as yet unanswered, questions with
regard to personal and situational moderators of the purported model that
we hope will fuel future investigations.
patterns (Selcuk, Zayas, & Hazan, 2010). There is a rich literature delin-
eating the precise psychological and neural systems that underlie adult pair
bonds and give rise to attachment-related behaviors (proximity mainte-
nance, safe haven, secure base, and separation distress) (see Mikulincer &
Shaver, 2007, for review).
These manifestations of adult pair bonds in terms of physiology,
emotion, and behavior are assumed to reflect the functioning of mental
representations, or internal working models. A core idea of attachment
theory is that the residue of past interactions with the particular part-
ner, as well as of interactions in other past and present relationships, is
stored in memory (e.g., Bowlby, 1973, 1982; Bretherton & Munholland,
2008; Collins, Guichard, Ford, & Feeney, 2004; Pietromonaco & Barrett,
2000; Zayas, Mischel, Shoda, & Aber, 2011). Mental representations con-
sist of detailed memories of interactions with, and conscious and noncon-
scious affective evaluations of, attachment figures (e.g., Zayas & Shoda,
2005, 2014), as well as strategies to regulate negative affect (e.g., turning
to attachment figures to alleviate negative affect, or turning away from
attachment figures and coping through other means; e.g., Zayas, Shoda,
Osterhout, Takahashi, & Mischel, 2009) in stressful and threatening situ-
ations (e.g., Collins et al., 2004; Pietromonaco, Barrett, & Powers, 2006).
Mental representations are impactful because they implicitly affect percep-
tions and expectations about likely events (e.g., “If I seek help, then I will
be supported”; see, e.g., Baldwin et al., 1993; Zayas et al., 2009), which
in turn affect physiology, emotion, and behavior, and may do so without a
person’s having any conscious awareness of the process (Günaydin, Zayas,
Selcuk, & Hazan, 2012).
originally induced by actual interactions with the figure (e.g., Depue &
Morrone-Strupinsky, 2005; Uvnäs-Moberg, 1998).
Indeed, numerous studies provide support for the proposition that
partners enhance affect regulation (for reviews, see Sbarra & Hazan, 2008;
Selcuk et al., 2010). For instance, intimate and supportive interactions
with a romantic partner, compared to nonsupportive interactions with a
partner or with being alone, lead to greater calmness while anticipating a
stressor (e.g., Simpson, Rholes, & Nelligan, 1992); smaller elevations in
self-reported anxiety and physiological reactivity (i.e., systolic blood pres-
sure, diastolic blood pressure, heart rate, and cortisol level; e.g., Collins
& Ford, 2010; Ditzen et al., 2007; Grewen, Anderson, Girdler, & Light,
2003); attenuation of neural threat responses while experiencing a stressor
(e.g., Coan, Schaefer, & Davidson, 2006); and faster emotional recovery
following a stressor (e.g., Collins & Ford, 2010). Moreover, some of the
distress-relieving benefits are realized simply by activating the mental rep-
resentation of the partner, even in the absence of his or her actual presence.
For example, simply viewing a photograph of one’s partner diminishes the
experience of a mildly painful stimulus (Eisenberger et al., 2011; Master et
al., 2009) and lessens the deleterious affective and cognitive consequences
of relieving a distressing autobiographical memory (Selcuk, Zayas, Günay-
din, Hazan, & Kross, 2012).
A growing body of research has focused on identifying the neural and
endocrinological mechanisms that confer these distress-alleviating effects.
This work, from both the human and animal literatures, converges on the
idea that interactions with attachment figures, whether actual or symbolic,
increase activity of two neurotransmitter systems: oxytocin, which pro-
motes feelings of trust, love, security, and affiliation; and endogenous opi-
oids such as beta-endorphins, which promote relaxation and well-being,
and (most importantly) decrease the experience of physical and emotional
pain (Depue & Morrone-Strupinsky, 2005; Sbarra & Hazan, 2008; Young
& Wang, 2004; Zak, Kurzban, & Matzner, 2005). The release of these
neurotransmitters, in turn, serves to down-regulate threat-related reactiv-
ity of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical (HPA) axis and the auto-
nomic nervous system (ANS) (Diamond, 2001; McCubbin, 1993; Uvnäs-
Moberg, 1998).
The threat response of the HPA axis and ANS has been well docu-
mented. In response to an external or internal threat, the hippocampus,
involved in memory, and the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST) of
the amygdala, an important brain region in the anxiety pathway, become
activated and trigger a cascade of physiological responses to signal potential
danger. In particular, the BNST, which provides threat feedback to the hip-
pocampus, produces corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH)—a precursor
to cortisol, the stress hormone (Aguilera & Liu, 2012). This activates an
ANS response by releasing cortisol (a glucocorticoid) into the bloodstream.
74 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
arousal, and courtship behavior (Woolley, Sakata, Gupta, & Crews, 2001);
and the consummatory system implicated in the consummatory phase,
which is involved in feelings of satiation and sedation following attainment
of the goal (Hinde, 1970).
To illustrate the basic activation of the sexual mating system (and later
how it may promote adult attachment) in the sections that follow, we refer
to a hypothetical scenario in which two individuals, Sam and Alex, meet
for the first time and experience sexual attraction. A number of cues, such
as facial shape and appearance, scent, body posture, and so on, will affect
whether Sam will find Alex attractive.
are strong predictors of actual behavioral intentions to date the person later
(in a speed-dating paradigm; Cooper, Dunne, Furey, & O’Doherty, 2012).
So, for Sam, cues associated with Alex—someone Sam inherently finds
physically attractive—serve to activate the appetitive neural and behav-
ioral systems. Subjectively, Sam’s simply seeing Alex is likely to elicit an
ecstatic high from dopamine release, which is a subjective high associated
with stimuli that are the most rewarding. This actually occurs before Sam
reaches Alex, in anticipation of the reward Sam will receive by interacting,
possibly successfully, with an attractive potential mate. Behaviorally, this
anticipatory dopaminergic reward occurs in order to propel an individual
toward a rewarding stimulus, not to receive the reward itself (Depue & Col-
lins, 1999). Sam will probably desire to engage in a number of approach-
oriented behaviors, such as physically get closer to, talk with, and maintain
eye contact with Alex (Breiter, Aharon, Kahneman, Dale, & Shizgal, 2001;
Aharon et al., 2001).
oxytocin from around the time of orgasm until about 5 minutes afterward
(Carmichael et al., 1987). Though the exact role, magnitude, and longevity
of oxytocin release during and after orgasm have been matters of debate
in the literature, especially in regard to male participants (Kruger et al.,
2003; Murphy, Seckl, Burton, Checkley, & Lightman, 1987), there is gen-
eral agreement that oxytocin levels do increase due to orgasm in men and
women (Blaicher et al., 1999; Caldwell, 2002). However, it is also true that
oxytocin is released throughout most sexual activity due to the physical
intimacy of sexual intercourse (Meston & Frohlich, 2000).
1992). The other acts that release comparable amounts of oxytocin are
lactation, parturition, and regulation of maternal behavior (Carter, 1992).
Given the large role that oxytocin plays in sexual functioning and
release, along with its interaction with dopamine in the NAS, it is reason-
able to hypothesize that the anxiolytic and intimacy-promoting effect of
oxytocin is amplified during positive sexual encounters—especially ones
resulting in orgasm for one or both of the partners. This continued distress
relief and increase in trust and intimacy from repeated sexual encounters
may then act to create the adult attachment bond, much as distress relief
does in infancy.
itself is not very satisfying. Thus the specific question of whether timing
of sex plays a role in moderating the hypothesized effect of sex on adult
attachment formation requires empirical investigation. Future work, ide-
ally using a longitudinal framework, should be used to examine more pre-
cisely how the timing of sexual intercourse affects subsequent components
of the attachment bond (e.g., alleviation distress).
Does the proposed model work for people who engage in casual sex or who are
serial monogamists?
We speculate that both people who engage in casual sex or who are serial
monogamists may find the incentive motivation dopaminergic reward to
be more valuable than the consummatory opioid reward. A preference for
dopamine-related activities over opioid-related ones may reflect individual
differences. For example, people who prefer casual sex or the beginning of
relationships may be more sensitive to dopaminergic (vs. opioid) rewards,
which would promote a preference for high-dopaminergic activities, such
as sex, compared to high-opioid activities associated with longer relation-
ships. Additionally, they may have fewer mu-opioid receptors due to early
life experiences. In fact, people who prefer casual or brief encounters may
be the neurochemical opposites of people who identify as asexual—that
is, who experience consummatory reward, but have no sexual incentive
motivation.
Moreover, those who prefer casual sex or who are serial monogamists
may have a preference for dopamine-related activities over opioid-related
ones because of the release of oxytocin. Although oxytocin is released by
86 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
women, and women on average tend to enjoy sexual intimacy more than
do men. We can see this sex difference in interactions with sexual stimuli
in a study by Festjens, Bruyneel, and Dewitte (2013), in which heterosexual
men and women were presented with underwear or a t-shirt of the opposite
sex and were allowed either to touch or only to look at them. Men exhibited
more reward seeking after both visual and tactile cues of women’s under-
wear. However, women only exhibited reward seeking after being allowed
to touch the men’s underwear, as visual cues alone were unable to elicit an
appetitive response. This sex difference is perhaps due to a difference in the
dopaminergic threshold that is required to experience sexual motivation;
that is, perhaps having an additional sensory experience was necessary for
the women in the study to reach this threshold. Alternatively, given the
behavioral conditioning response associated with the reward system, per-
haps the majority of women in this study had experientially learned not to
associate male undergarments with the magnitude of anticipatory reward
that men had learned in regard to female undergarments. This experiential
learning could stem from the saturation of sexualization of women and
women’s underwear in mainstream culture, or even from an antiquated yet
still pervasive view of women not acting as sexual aggressors.
Another possibility for the sex differences we find between men and
women’s sexual inclinations toward novelty versus intimacy, respectively,
may be due to the gender difference in internal hormonal milieu. The ste-
roid/peptide theory of social bonds (van Anders, Goldey, & Kuo, 2011)
calls attention to the seemingly opposing processes of oxytocin and testos-
terone in social bonding: Whereas oxytocin promotes trust, testosterone
inhibits it; whereas oxytocin promotes empathy, testosterone inhibits it;
whereas oxytocin inhibits stress, testosterone promotes it (Bos, Panksepp,
Bluthé, & Honk, 2012). van Anders et al. (2011) even go so far as to say
that testosterone and oxytocin seem to act as hormonal antagonists to
one another. From this antagonistic relationship between testosterone and
oxytocin presented in the steroid/peptide theory, it would be reasonable to
extrapolate that because men have more testosterone than women do, men
may need a larger amount of oxytocin in order for the neurotransmitter to
have the same effect in promoting pair bonding as it does in women. This
may be one reason why, separate from social-constructionist viewpoints, it
is more common for men to reach orgasm during a sexual experience than
women, as it may allow for a more equitable action of oxytocin in the two
partners. Future research might investigate sex differences in hormone and
neurotransmitter interactions and their effects on attachment formation.
Conclusion
Although it is the norm that romantic partners function as attachment fig-
ures and sex partners, and pair bonds in theory are characterized by an
integration of the attachment and sexual mating systems, sex has largely
been ignored by researchers focusing on adult romantic attachment. Birn-
baum and colleagues’ (Birnbaum & Reis, 2006; Birnbaum, 2010) work,
Sex and Romantic Attachment Formation 89
though its focus is on individual differences and not the role of sex in
attachment formation, is a notable exception.
From the evidence reviewed above, it is clear that the neural and physi-
ological systems operating during sexual exchanges overlap significantly
with those underlying attachment bonds. Notably, oxytocin is released
most strongly in the context of the two types of interpersonal relationships
that typically function as primary attachments—that is, infant–caregiver
bonds in early life (including parturition and lactation) and romantic/sex-
ual bonds in adulthood.
The neural systems that motivate us to engage in sexual interactions
and then reward us so intensely for doing so appear to play a central role
in adult attachment formation. Specifically, repeated sexual contact with
the same individual over time conditions these systems to a rich mental
representation of this individual that includes facial and bodily features,
voice, smell, touch, and so forth. In the normal course of romantic rela-
tionship development, the dopaminergic reward declines in intensity—but,
thanks to oxytocin, the opiate reward does not. Indeed, the point of “clear-
cut” attachment in adulthood (i.e., the marker of a qualitative change from
infatuation to full-blown attachment) may be when an opioid-based sense
of calm and satiety overtakes dopamine-driven feelings of desire. Further-
more, the action of oxytocin in preventing habituation to the rewarding
effects of opioids is what helps maintain pair bonds over the long term.
We hope the ideas presented in this chapter will inspire adult attach-
ment researchers to tackle the many fascinating and empirically testable
questions that the proposed model suggests, and thereby help move the
field of adult attachment theory forward.
References
Aguilera, G., & Liu, Y. (2012). The molecular physiology of CRH neurons. Fron-
tiers in Neuroendocrinology, 33, 67–84.
Aharon, I., Etcoff, N., Ariely, D., Chabris, C. F., O’Connor, E., & Breiter, H. C.
(2001). Beautiful faces have variable reward value: fMRI and behavioral evi-
dence, Neuron, 3(8), 537–551.
Argiolas, A. (1999). Neuropeptides and sexual behaviour. Neuroscience and
Biobehavioral Reviews, 23, 1127–1142.
Argiolas, A., & Gessa, G. L. (1991). Central functions of oxytocin. Neuroscience
and Biobehavioral Reviews, 15, 217–231.
Aron, A., Fisher, H., Mashek, D., Strong, G., Li, H., & Brown, L. (2005). Reward,
motivation and emotion systems associated with early-stage intense romantic
love. Journal of Neurophysiology, 93, 327–337.
Baldwin, M. W., Fehr, B., Keedian, E., Seidel, M., & Thomson, D. W. (1993).
An exploration of the relational schemata underlying attachment styles: Self-
report and lexical decision approaches. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 19, 746–754.
90 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Baldwin, M. W., Keelan, J. P. R., Fehr, B., Enns, V., & Koh-Rangarajoo, E. (1996).
Social-cognitive conceptualization of attachment working models: Availabil-
ity and accessibility effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71,
94–109.
Balfour, M. E., Yu, L., & Coolen, L. M. (2004). Sexual behavior and sex-associ-
ated environmental cues activate the mesolimbic system in male rats. Neuro-
psychopharmacology, 29, 718–730.
Bartels, A., & Zeki, S. (2004). The neural correlates of maternal and romantic
love. NeuroImage, 21, 1155–1166.
Beckes, L., Simpson, J. A., & Erickson, A. (2010). Of snakes and succor: Learning
secure attachment associations with novel faces via negative stimulus pair-
ings. Psychological Science, 21, 721–728.
Berridge, K. C. (1999). Pleasure, pain, desire, and dread: Hidden core processes of
emotion. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The
foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 525–557). New York: Russell Sage
Foundation.
Berscheid, E., & Walster, E. H. (1969). Interpersonal attraction. Reading, MA:
Addison-Wesley.
Birnbaum, G. E. (2007). Attachment orientations, sexual functioning, and rela-
tionship satisfaction in a community sample of women. Journal of Social and
Personal Relationships, 24, 21–35.
Birnbaum, G. E. (2010). Bound to interact: The divergent goals and complex inter-
play of attachment and sex within romantic relationships. Journal of Social
and Personal Relationships, 27, 245–252.
Birnbaum, G. E., & Reis, H. T. (2006). Women’s sexual working models: An evolu-
tionary-attachment perspective. Journal of Sex Research, 43, 328–342.
Birnbaum, G. E., & Reis, H. T. (2012). When does responsiveness pique sexual
interest?: Attachment and sexual desire in initial acquaintanceships. Person-
ality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 946–958.
Birnbaum, G. E., Reis, H. T., Mikulincer, M., Gillath, O., & Orpaz, A. (2006). When
sex is more than just sex: Attachment orientations, sexual experience, and rela-
tionship quality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 929–943.
Blaicher, W., Gruber, D., Bieglmayer, C., Blaicher, A. M., Knogler, W., & Huber,
J. C. (1999). The role of oxytocin in relation to female sexual arousal. Gyne-
cologic and Obstetric Investigation, 47, 125–126.
Bos, P. A., Panksepp, J., Bluthé, R. M., & Honk, J. V. (2012). Acute effects of
steroid hormones and neuropeptides on human social–emotional behavior:
A review of single administration studies. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology,
33, 17–35.
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation: Anxiety and anger.
New York: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1979). The making and breaking of affectional bonds. London: Tavis-
tock.
Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: Retrospect and prospect. American Jour-
nal of Orthopsychiatry, 52, 664–678.
Breiter H. C., Aharon, I., Kahneman, D., Dale, A., & Shizgal, P. (2001). Functional
imaging of neural responses to expectancy and experience of monetary gains
and losses. Neuron, 30(2), 619–639.
Sex and Romantic Attachment Formation 91
Ditzen, B., Neumann, I. D., Bodenmann, G., von Dawans, B., Turner, R. A.,
Ehlert, U., et al. (2007). Effects of different kinds of couple interaction on
cortisol and heart rate responses to stress in women. Psychoneuroendocrinol-
ogy, 32, 565–574.
Eiler, W. J., Hardy, L., Goergen, J., Seyoum, R., Mensah-Zoe, B., & June, H. L.
(2007). Responding for brain stimulation reward in the bed nucleus of the
stria terminalis in alcohol-preferring rats following alcohol and amphetamine
pretreatments. Synapse, 61, 912–924.
Eisenberger, N. I., Master, S. L., Inagaki, T. K., Taylor, S. E., Shirinyan, D., Lieber-
man, M. D., et al. (2011). Attachment figures activate a safety signal-related
neural region and reduce pain experience. Proceedings of the National Acad-
emy of Sciences USA, 108, 11721–11726.
Festjens, A., Bruyneel, S., & Dewitte, S. (2013). What a feeling!: Touching sexually
laden stimuli makes women seek rewards. Journal of Consumer Psychology,
24(3), 387–393.
Fisher, H., Aron, A., & Brown, L. L. (2005). Romantic love: An fMRI study of a
neural mechanism for mate choice. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 493,
58–62.
Gray, J. A. (1973). Causal theories of personality and how to test them. In J. R.
Royce (Ed.), Multivariate analysis and psychological theory (pp. 409–463).
New York: Academic Press.
Grewen, K. M., Anderson, B. J., Girdler, S. S., & Light, K. C. (2003). Warm part-
ner contact is related to lower cardiovascular reactivity. Behavioral Medi-
cine, 29, 123–130.
Günaydin, G., Zayas, V., Selcuk, E., & Hazan, C. (2012). I like you but I don’t
know why: Objective facial resemblance to significant others influences snap
judgments. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 250–353.
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment
process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 511–524.
Hazan, C., & Zeifman, D. (1994). Sex and the psychological tether. In K. Bar-
tholomew & D. Perlman (Eds.), Advances in personal relationships: Vol. 5.
Attachment processes in adulthood (pp. 151–178). London: Jessica Kings-
ley.
Herbert, J. (1993). Peptides in the limbic system: Neurochemical codes for co-ordi-
nated adaptive responses to behavioural and physiological demand. Progress
in Neurobiology, 41, 723–791.
Hilliard, S., Domjan, M., Nguyen, M., & Cusato, B. (1998). Dissociation of con-
ditioned appetitive and consummatory sexual behavior: Satiation and extinc-
tion tests. Animal Learning and Behavior, 26, 20–33.
Hinde, R. A. (1970). Animal behaviour: A synthesis of ethology and comparative
psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Impett, E. A., Muise, A., & Peragine, D. (2014). Sexuality in the context of rela-
tionships. In L. M. Diamond & D. L. Tolman (Eds.), APA handbook of sexu-
ality and psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 269–316). Washington, DC: American Psy-
chological Association.
Insel, T. R. (1992). Oxytocin—a neuropeptide for affiliation: Evidence from behav-
ioral, receptor autoradiographic, and comparative studies. Psychoneuroendo-
crinology, 17, 3–35.
Sex and Romantic Attachment Formation 93
Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P. R., & Pereg, D. (2003). Attachment theory and affect
regulation: The dynamics, development, and cognitive consequences of
attachment-related strategies. Motivation and Emotion, 27, 77–102.
Murphy, M. R., Checkley, S. A., Seckl, J. R., & Lightman, S. L. (1990). Naloxone
inhibits oxytocin release at orgasm in men. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology
and Metabolism, 71, 1056–1058.
Murphy, M. R., Seckl, J. R., Burton, S., Checkley, S. A., & Lightman, S. L. (1987).
Changes in oxytocin and vasopressin secretion during sexual activity in
men. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 65, 738–741.
Nelson, E. E., & Panksepp, J. (1998). Brain substrates of infant–mother attach-
ment: Contributions of opioids, oxytocin, and norepinephrine. Neuroscience
and Biobehavioral Reviews, 22, 437–452.
Oliet, C., Oliva, A., Castro, J., & Pérez-Segarra, C. D. (2007). Parametric studies
on automotive radiators. Applied Thermal Engineering, 27, 2033–2043.
Ostrowski, N. L. (1998). Oxytocin receptor mRNA expression in rat brain: Impli-
cations for behavioral integration and reproductive success. Psychoneuroen-
docrinology, 23, 989–1004.
Panksepp, J. (1986). The anatomy of emotions. In R. Plutchik & D. Kellerman
(Eds.), Emotion: Theory, research and experience (Vol. 3, pp. 91–124). New
York: Academic Press
Pfaff, D. W. (1999). Drive: Neurobiological and molecular mechanisms of sexual
motivation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Pfaus, J. G., Damsma, G., Wenkstern, D., & Fibiger, H. C. (1995). Sexual activity
increases dopamine transmission in the nucleus accumbens and striatum of
female rats. Brain Research, 693(1), 21–30.
Phillips-Farfán, B. V., & Fernández-Guasti, A. (2009). Endocrine, neural and
pharmacological aspects of sexual satiety in male rats. Neuroscience and
Biobehavioral Reviews, 33, 442–455.
Pietromonaco, P. R., & Barrett, L. F. (2000). The internal working models concept:
What do we really know about the self in relation to others? Review of Gen-
eral Psychology, 4, 155–175.
Pietromonaco, P. R., Barrett, L. F., & Powers, S. I. (2006). Adult attachment the-
ory and affective reactivity and regulation. In D. K. Snyder, J. A. Simpson, &
J. N. Hughes (Eds.), Emotion regulation in families and close relationships:
Pathways to dysfunction and health (pp. 57–74). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Porges, S. W. (1998). Love: An emergent property of the mammalian autonomic
nervous system. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 23, 837–861.
Porges, S. W. (2001). The polyvagal theory: Phylogenetic substrates of a social ner-
vous system. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 42, 123–146.
Sarnyai, Z., & Kovács, G. L. (1994). Role of oxytocin in the neuroadaptation to
drugs of abuse. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 19, 85–117.
Sbarra, D. A., & Hazan, C. (2008). Coregulation, dysregulation, self-regulation:
An integrative analysis and empirical agenda for understanding adult attach-
ment, separation, loss, and recovery. Personality and Social Psychology
Review, 12, 141–167.
Scantamburlo, G., Hansenne, M., Fuchs, S., Pitchot, W., Maréchal, P., Pequeux,
Sex and Romantic Attachment Formation 95
C., et al. (2007). Plasma oxytocin levels and anxiety in patients with major
depression. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 32, 407–410.
Selcuk, E., Zayas, V., Günaydin, G., Hazan, C., & Kross, E. (2012). Mental rep-
resentations of attachment figures facilitate emotional recovery following
upsetting autobiographical memory recall. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 103, 362–378.
Selcuk, E., Zayas, V., & Hazan, C. (2010). Beyond satisfaction: The role of attach-
ment in marital functioning. Journal of Family Theory and Review, 2, 258–
279.
Shahrokh, D. K., Zhang, T. Y., Diorio, J., Gratton, A., & Meaney, M. J. (2010).
Oxytocin–dopamine interactions mediate variations in maternal behavior in
the rat. Endocrinology, 151, 2276–2286.
Silk, J. B., Alberts, S. C., & Altmann, J. (2003). Social bonds of female baboons
enhance infant survival. Science, 302, 1231–1234.
Simpson, J. A., Rholes, W. S., & Nelligan, J. S. (1992). Support seeking and support
giving within couples in an anxiety-provoking situation: The role of attach-
ment styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 434–446.
Smillie, L. D. (2013). Extraversion and reward processing. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 22, 167–172.
Sroufe, L. A., & Waters, E. (1977). Attachment as an organizational construct.
Child Development, 48, 1184–1199.
Taylor, S. E., Gonzaga, G. C., Klein, L. C., Hu, P., Greendale, G. A., & Seeman,
T. E. (2006). Relation of oxytocin to psychological stress responses and hypo-
thalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical axis activity in older women. Psychoso-
matic Medicine, 68, 238–245.
Uvnäs-Moberg, K. (1998). Antistress pattern induced by oxytocin. Physiology,
13(1), 22–25.
van Anders, S. M., Goldey, K. L., & Kuo, P. X. (2011). The steroid/peptide theory
of social bonds: Integrating testosterone and peptide responses for classifying
social behavioral contexts. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 36, 1265–1275.
Whipple, B., & Komisaruk, B. R. (1985). Elevation of pain threshold by vaginal
stimulation in women. Pain, 21, 357–367.
Wiedenmayer, C. P., & Barr, G. A. (2000). Mu opioid receptors in the ventrolateral
periaqueductal gray mediate stress-induced analgesia but not immobility in
rat pups. Behavioral Neuroscience, 114, 125–136.
Willoughby, B. J., Carroll, J. S., & Busby, D. M. (2014). Differing relationship
outcomes when sex happens before, on, or after first dates. Journal of Sex
Research, 51, 52–61.
Windle, R. J., Shanks, N., Lightman, S. L., & Ingram, C. D. (1997). Central oxyto-
cin administration reduces stress-induced corticosterone release and anxiety
behavior in rats: 1. Endocrinology, 138, 2829–2834.
Woolley, S. C., Sakata, J. T., Gupta, A., & Crews, D. (2001). Evolutionary changes
in dopaminergic modulation of courtship behavior in Cnemidophorus whip-
tail lizards. Hormone and Behavior, 40, 483–489.
Young, L. J., & Wang, Z. (2004). The neurobiology of pair bonding. Nature Neu-
roscience, 7, 1048–1054.
Younger, J., Aron, A., Parke, S., Chatterjee, N., & Mackey, S. (2010). Viewing
96 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Lisa M. Diamond
97
98 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
PNS as “the” singular adaptive pattern of PNS activity during stress, given
that an increasing body of research has found that PNS activity sometimes
increases during stress, especially in tasks that call for active regulatory
effort (Beauchaine, 2001; Segerstrom & Nes, 2007; Thayer & Lane, 2000).
It has been theorized that this pattern of increased PNS activity may serve
to facilitate attention and vigilance to environmental demands by slowing
down cardiovascular activity. Hence both PNS increases and decreases in
response to stress can be viewed as adaptive responses, depending on the
conditions, and researchers have not yet identified a stable set of criteria by
which we might view one pattern or the other as “more appropriate.”
In addition to situational variation in PNS responses to stress, there
also appear to be individual differences. Katz (2007) has argued that indi-
viduals exposed to chronically stressful environments may develop an
enduring pattern of PNS engagement during stress, which may help them
to monitor their environment and maintain control over their emotions and
behavior. Although such a pattern may prove adaptive in the short term,
it may prove taxing over the long term. Many researchers have begun to
conceptualize regulatory capacity as being relatively finite, analogous to a
muscle that tires upon repeated use (Muraven & Baumeister, 2000). Hence
individuals who show chronic patterns of increased RSA in response to
stress may experience chronic regulatory “fatigue,” leaving them vulnerable
to frequent failures of self-control (Vohs, Baumeister, & Ciarocco, 2005).
This may explain why individuals who show heightened RSA during stress
also show multiple indicators of emotion dysregulation, such as depression,
anxiety, and hostility (Hessler & Katz, 2007; Neumann, Sollers, Thayer,
& Waldstein, 2004).
with that regarding the HPA axis: Early adversity predicts poorer stress-
related regulation in the PNS (Lengua, 2012; Propper, 2012) as well as
heightened SNS stress reactivity (Gunnar et al., 2009; Oosterman et al.,
2010), and the specific postnatal neural mechanisms underlying these asso-
ciations have been well elaborated by Rinaman, Banihashemi, and Koehnle
(2011). It also bears noting that there is extensive evidence for links between
ANS stress reactivity and early child health indicators, such as low birth
weight, poor maternal nutrition, preterm birth, and poor rate of growth
(see Kajantie & Räikkönen, 2010), suggesting that health-related correlates
of adversity may prove particularly important for tuning the ANS.
been found to be associated with lower vagal tone (Diamond & Hicks,
2005; Maunder, Lancee, Nolan, Hunter, & Tannenbaum, 2006).
One shortcoming of this body of research is that few studies assess or
control for childhood exposure to adversity, which (as noted above) may
partially explain the shared variance between attachment insecurity and
stress dysregulation. One exception is a study by Pierrehumbert, Torrisi,
Ansermet, Borghini, and Halfon (2012), which evaluated HPA stress reac-
tivity by using the Trier Social Stress Test in a sample of adults, more than
half of whom had experienced some form of childhood adversity (such
as abuse, trauma, or life-threatening illness). Independently of adversity
exposure, those who had been classified as dismissing according to the
Adult Attachment Interview (AAI)—an interview method for discerning
individuals’ childhood attachment security on the basis of their narra-
tive recollections and reconstructions in adulthood (Hesse, 1999; Main,
Kaplan, & Cassidy, 1985)—reported moderate levels of subjective stress
but significantly elevated levels of HPA activity, whereas those classified as
preoccupied on the AAI did not show elevated HPA activity. Notably, those
classified as unresolved on the AAI (a pattern often associated with child-
hood trauma) reported significantly elevated levels of subjective stress and
dampened levels of HPA activity.
A number of studies have investigated links between attachment inse-
curity and HPA or ANS stress reactivity in adulthood. For example, Powers,
Pietromonaco, Gunlicks, and Sayer (2006) found that insecurely attached
individuals showed greater HPA reactivity to laboratory-induced roman-
tic conflict than did securely attached individuals. Specifically, attachment
avoidance in female participants was related to increased HPA reactivity to
couple conflict, whereas men showed elevated reactivity if they had high
levels of anxiety combined with high avoidance. Individuals who had high
attachment anxiety also showed poorer recovery of HPA levels after the
conflict (Laurent & Powers, 2007). Dewitte, De Houwer, Goubert, and
Buysse (2010) led participants to believe that each was going to have to
watch a tape of his or her partner being interviewed about previous sexual
and romantic relationships by an attractive opposite-sex experimenter.
Participants who had higher attachment anxiety showed heightened HPA
reactivity while anticipating this stressful event, with the largest increases
found in women who were highly anxious and avoidant. Finally, during an
actual 4- to 7-day physical separation from their romantic partners, indi-
viduals who had high attachment anxiety showed tonically elevated levels
of cortisol (Diamond, Hicks, & Otter-Henderson, 2008).
All of these findings are consistent with the notion that anxiety is
associated with a lower threshold for attachment-related threats (conflict,
partner unavailability, jealousy, etc.; see Simpson & Rholes, 1994), which
manifests itself in heightened physiological response. Yet it is also possible
that insecurely attached individuals possess a generalized predisposition
Stress and Attachment 107
Thus the very features of HPA and ANS functioning that have historically
been viewed as risk factors from the perspective of the ALM are viewed
as either risk or resiliency factors from the perspective of the ACM. This
implies that we can no longer casually use the word adaptive: Adaptive is
a fundamentally relative construct, defined with respect to the challenges
and environments at hand.
Scholars considering these two models generally concur that there is
insufficient empirical evidence to determine whether the effects of early
adversity are best conceptualized in terms of cumulative risk or adaptive
calibration (Hostinar & Gunnar, 2013a; O’Connor & Spagnola, 2009).
However, a growing body of findings suggests that heightened physiologi-
cal stress reactivity is associated with differential risk in adverse condi-
tions, but (in some cases and for some outcomes) differential benefit in
highly nurturant conditions (see Belsky et al., 2007; Conradt, Measelle, &
Ablow, 2013; Del Giudice, Hinnant, Ellis, & El-Sheikh, 2012; Diamond,
Fagundes, & Cribbet, 2012; Pluess & Belsky, 2009, 2010, 2013). Overall,
the pattern of findings of differential risk is more robust than the pattern of
findings of differential benefit (Belsky & Pluess, 2012; Boyce et al., 1995;
Cummings et al., 2007; Diamond et al., 2012; El-Sheikh et al., 2009; Katz,
2007); this difference is partially attributable to the fact that most studies
examining stress reactivity as a moderator of children’s vulnerability to
environmental deficits have not even tested whether stress reactivity simul-
taneously moderates the benefits of enhanced environments.
patterns are laid down during the first year of life and largely “grow up”
into adult romantic attachment styles, establishing robust working models
of adult love dynamics before an individual has even had his or her first
romantic relationship. From this perspective, subsequent romantic experi-
ences usually strengthen and confirm the individual’s initial attachment
style because working models function as self-fulfilling prophecies, reli-
ably altering individuals’ selection of romantic partners and their ongoing
appraisals of partners’ responsiveness and availability.
The evidence for this “strong trait” perspective is mixed, given that
longitudinal studies have detected varying degrees of continuity in attach-
ment styles from childhood to adulthood (Hamilton, 2000; Lewis, Feiring,
& Rosenthal, 2000; Roisman et al., 2005; Waters et al., 2000; Weinfield
et al., 2000) and over adulthood from relationship to relationship (Bald-
win & Fehr, 1995; Davila, Burge, & Hammen, 1997; Davila et al., 1999;
Fraley, 2007; Klohnen & Bera, 1998; Lopez & Gormley, 2002; Mitch-
ell, 2007; Scharfe & Bartholomew, 1995; Zhang & Labouvie-Vief, 2004).
Many researchers have sidestepped this debate by gravitating toward a
“two-pronged” conceptualization of adult attachment styles, in which indi-
viduals have both a global working model (which is carried forward from
childhood) that provides a general, trait-like template for an individual’s
relationship expectations, and also a relationship-specific model based on
particular attachment figures, such as current or recent romantic partners
(Baldwin & Fehr, 1995; Baldwin, Keelan, Fehr, Enns, & Koh-Rangarajoo,
1996; La Guardia, Ryan, Couchman, & Deci, 2000). This perspective
takes more seriously the phenomenon of reciprocal influence between prior
and current attachment expectations and experiences, and hence it holds
more promise for the development of lifespan models of attachment that
take into account an individual’s entire cumulative trajectory of attach-
ment-relevant experiences.
The unique contribution of the ACM is the notion that cumulative
trajectories of attachment-relevant experiences may be more influential for
some individuals than others due to individual differences in stress reactiv-
ity, which render certain individuals disproportionately sensitive to envi-
ronmental and interpersonal threats and resources. Hence an intriguing
direction for future research involves longitudinal assessment of individual
differences in stress reactivity as predictors of the correspondence between
changes in social–environmental conditions and concurrent changes in psy-
chosocial functioning.
Conclusion
The increasing body of psychobiological research on attachment and stress
regulation underscores the critical role of attachment relationships in fos-
tering psychological, physical, and interpersonal functioning at all stages
114 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
References
Alkon, A., Boyce, W. T., Davis, N. V., & Eskenazi, B. (2011). Developmental
changes in autonomic nervous system resting and reactivity measures in
Latino children from 6 to 60 months of age. Journal of Developmental and
Behavioral Pediatrics, 32, 668–677.
Baldwin, M. W., & Fehr, B. (1995). On the instability of attachment style ratings.
Personal Relationships, 2, 247–261.
Baldwin, M. W., Keelan, J. P. R., Fehr, B., Enns, V., & Koh-Rangarajoo, E. (1996).
Social-cognitive conceptualization of attachment working models: Availabil-
ity and accessibility effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71,
94–109.
Bateson, P., Barker, D., Clutton-Brock, T., Deb, D., D’Udine, B., Foley, R. A., et al.
(2004). Developmental plasticity and human health. Nature, 430, 419–421.
Beauchaine, T. P. (2001). Vagal tone, development, and Gray’s motivational theory:
Toward an integrated model of autonomic nervous system functioning in psy-
chopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 183–214.
Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). For
better and for worse: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 300–304.
Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2009). Beyond diathesis stress: Differential susceptibility
to environmental influences. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 885–908.
Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2012). Differential susceptibility to long-term effects of
quality of child care on externalizing behavior in adolescence? International
Journal of Behavioral Development, 36, 2–10.
Berntson, G. G., Cacioppo, J. T., & Fieldstone, A. (1996). Illusions, arithmetic, and
the bidirectional modulation of vagal control of the heart. Biological Psychol-
ogy, 44, 1–17.
Blandon, A. Y., Calkins, S. D., Keane, S. P., & O’Brien, M. (2010). Contributions
of child’s physiology and maternal behavior to children’s trajectories of tem-
peramental reactivity. Developmental Psychology, 46, 1089–1102.
Stress and Attachment 115
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation: Anxiety and anger.
New York: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1977). The making and breaking of affectional bonds: I. Aetiology and
psychopathology in the light of attachment theory. British Journal of Psychia-
try, 130, 201–210.
Boyce, W. T., Chesney, M., Alkon, A., & Tschann, J. M. (1995). Psychobiologic
reactivity to stress and childhood respiratory illnesses: Results of two pro-
spective studies. Psychosomatic Medicine, 57, 411–422.
Boyce, W. T., & Ellis, B. J. (2005). Biological sensitivity to context: I. An evolu-
tionary-developmental theory of the origins and functions of stress reactivity.
Development and Psychopathology, 17, 271–301.
Bruce, J., Fisher, P. A., Pears, K. C., & Levine, S. (2009). Morning cortisol levels
in preschool-aged foster children: Differential effects of maltreatment type.
Developmental Psychobiology, 51, 14–23.
Bubier, J. L., Drabick, D. A. G., & Breiner, T. (2009). Autonomic functioning mod-
erates the relations between contextual factors and externalizing behaviors
among inner-city children. Journal of Family Psychology, 23, 500–510.
Buss, C., Lord, C., Wadiwalla, M., Hellhammer, D. H., Lupien, S. J., Meaney,
M. J., & Pruessner, J. C. (2007). Maternal care modulates the relationship
between prenatal risk and hippocampal volume in women but not in men.
Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 2592–2595.
Byrd-Craven, J., Auer, B. J., Granger, D. A., & Massey, A. R. (2012). The father–
daughter dance: The relationship between father–daughter relationship
quality and daughters’ stress response. Journal of Family Psychology, 26,
87–94.
Cacioppo, J. T. (1994). Social neuroscience: Autonomic, neuroendocrine, and
immune responses to stress. Psychophysiology, 31, 113–128.
Cacioppo, J. T., Uchino, B. N., & Berntson, G. G. (1994). Individual differences
in the autonomic origins of heart rate reactivity: The psychometrics of respi-
ratory sinus arrhythmia and preejection period. Psychophysiology, 31, 412–
419.
Calkins, S. D. (1997). Cardiac vagal tone indices of temperamental reactivity and
behavioral regulation in young children. Developmental Psychobiology, 31,
125–135.
Calkins, S. D., Smith, C. L., Gill, K. L., & Johnson, M. C. (1998). Maternal inter-
active style across contexts: Relations to emotional, behavioral, and physi-
ological regulation during toddlerhood. Social Development, 7, 350–369.
Chisholm, J. S., Quinlivan, J. A., Petersen, R. W., & Coall, D. A. (2005). Early
stress predicts age at menarche and first birth, adult attachment, and expected
lifespan. Human Nature, 16, 233–265.
Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., Gunnar, M. R., & Toth, S. L. (2010). The differ-
ential impacts of early physical and sexual abuse and internalizing problems
on daytime cortisol rhythm in school-aged children. Child Development, 81,
252–269.
Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., & Toth, S. L. (1998). Maternal depressive disorder
and contextual risk: Contributions to the development of attachment insecu-
rity and behavior problems in toddlerhood. Development and Psychopathol-
ogy, 10, 283–300.
116 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Conradt, E., Measelle, J., & Ablow, J. C. (2013). Poverty, problem behavior, and
promise: Differential susceptibility among infants reared in poverty. Psycho-
logical Science, 24, 235–242.
Crittenden, P. M. (2000). A dynamic-maturational exploration of the meaning of
security and adaptation. In P. M. Crittenden & A. H. Claussen (Eds.), The
organization of attachment relationships: Maturation, culture, and context
(pp. 358–383). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Cummings, E. M., El-Sheikh, C. D., Kouros, C. D., & Keller, P. S. (2007). Chil-
dren’s skin conductance reactivity as a mechanism of risk in the context of
parental depressive symptoms. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
48, 436–445.
Davila, J., Burge, D., & Hammen, C. (1997). Why does attachment style change?
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 826–838.
Davila, J., Karney, B. R., & Bradbury, T. N. (1999). Attachment change processes
in the early years of marriage. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
76, 783–802.
Del Giudice, M., Ellis, B. J., & Shirtcliff, E. A. (2011). The adaptive calibration
model of stress responsivity. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35,
1562–1592.
Del Giudice, M., Hinnant, J. B., Ellis, B. J., & El-Sheikh, M. (2012). Adaptive
patterns of stress responsivity: A preliminary investigation. Developmental
Psychology, 48, 775–790.
Dewitte, M., De Houwer, J., Goubert, L., & Buysse, A. (2010). A multi-modal
approach to the study of attachment-related distress. Biological Psychology,
85, 149–162.
Diamond, L. M., & Cribbet, M. R. (2013). Links between adolescent sympathetic
and parasympathetic nervous system functioning and interpersonal behavior
over time. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 88, 339–348.
Diamond, L. M., Fagundes, C. P., & Cribbet, M. R. (2012). Individual differences
in adolescent sympathetic and parasympathetic functioning moderate asso-
ciations between family environment and psychosocial adjustment. Develop-
mental Psychology, 48, 918–931.
Diamond, L. M., & Hicks, A. M. (2005). Attachment style, current relationship
security, and negative emotions: The mediating role of physiological regula-
tion. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 22, 499–518.
Diamond, L. M., Hicks, A. M., & Otter-Henderson, K. A. (2008). Every time you
go away: Changes in affect, behavior, and physiology associated with travel-
related separations from romantic partners. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 95, 385–403.
Dickerson, S. S., & Kemeny, M. E. (2004). Acute stressors and cortisol responses:
A theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychological
Bulletin, 130, 355–391.
El-Sheikh, M., Kouros, C. D., Erath, S., Cummings, E. M., Keller, P., Staton, L.,
et al. (2009). Marital conflict and children’s externalizing behavior: Inter-
actions between parasympathetic and sympathetic nervous system activity.
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 74(1, Serial
No. 292), 1–79.
El-Sheikh, M., & Whitson, S. A. (2006). Longitudinal relations between marital
Stress and Attachment 117
Lopez, F. G., & Gormley, B. (2002). Stability and change in adult attachment style
over the first-year college transition: Relations to self-confidence, coping, and
distress patterns. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 49, 355–364.
Luecken, L. J. (1998). Childhood attachment and loss experiences affect adult car-
diovascular and cortisol function. Psychosomatic Medicine, 60, 765–772.
Luecken, L. J., Rodriguez, A. P., & Appelhans, B. M. (2005). Cardiovascular stress
responses in young adulthood associated with family-of-origin relationship
experiences. Psychosomatic Medicine, 67, 514–521.
Madigan, S., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Moran, G.,
Pederson, D. R., & Benoit, D. (2006). Unresolved states of mind, anomalous
parental behavior, and disorganized attachment: A review and meta-analysis
of a transmission gap. Attachment and Human Development, 8, 89–111.
Main, M. (1981). Avoidance in the service of attachment: A working paper. In K.
Immelmann, G. Barlow, L. Petrinovich, & M. Main (Eds.), Behavioral devel-
opment: The Bielefeld interdisciplinary project (pp. 651–693). New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in infancy, childhood,
and adulthood: A move to the level of representation. In I. Bretherton & E.
Waters (Eds.), Growing points of attachment theory and research. Mono-
graphs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50(1–2, Serial No.
209), 66–104.
Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1990). Procedures for identifying infants as disorga-
nized/disoriented during the Ainsworth Strange Situation. In M. T. Green-
berg, D. Cicchetti, & E. M. Cummings (Eds.), Attachment in the preschool
years: Theory, research, and intervention (pp. 121–160). Chicago: University
of Chicago Press.
Maunder, R. G., Lancee, W. J., Nolan, R. P., Hunter, J. J., & Tannenbaum, D.
W. (2006). The relationship of attachment insecurity to subjective stress and
autonomic function during standardized acute stress in healthy adults. Jour-
nal of Psychosomatic Research, 60, 283–290.
McEwen, B. S. (1998). Stress, adaptation, and disease: Allostasis and allostatic
load. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 840, 33–44.
McEwen, B. S., & Stellar, E. (1993). Stress and the individual: Mechanisms leading
to disease. Archives of Internal Medicine, 153, 2093–2101.
Mikulincer, M., & Florian, V. (2004). Attachment style and affect regulation:
Implications for coping with stress and mental health. In M. B. Brewer &
M. Hewstone (Eds.), Applied social psychology (pp. 28–49). Malden, MA:
Blackwell.
Miller, G. E., Chen, E., & Parker, K. J. (2011). Psychological stress in childhood
and susceptibility to the chronic diseases of aging: Moving toward a model of
behavioral and biological mechanisms. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 959–997.
Miller, G. E., Chen, E., & Zhou, E. S. (2007). If it goes up, must it come down?:
Chronic stress and the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical axis in
humans. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 25–45.
Mills-Koonce, W. R., Appleyard, K., Barnett, M., Deng, M., Putallaz, M., & Cox,
M. (2011). Adult attachment style and stress as risk factors for early maternal
sensitivity and negativity. Infant Mental Health Journal, 32, 277–285.
Mills-Koonce, W. R., Garrett-Peters, P., Barnett, M., Granger, D. A., Blair, C., &
120 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Mario Mikulincer
Phillip R. Shaver
124
Boosting Attachment Security in Adulthood 125
(Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007b); this makes it possible to study the causal
effects of an experimentally primed sense of security within the confines
of a social psychological laboratory, or to examine the long-term effects of
real-life security-enhancing interpersonal contexts.
We (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007a) have proposed that individuals’ loca-
tion in the two-dimensional anxiety × avoidance space reflects both their
sense of attachment security and the way they deal with threats and stress-
ors. People who score low on the two insecurity dimensions are generally
secure, hold positive working models of self and others, and tend to employ
constructive and effective affect regulation strategies. Those who score high
on either attachment anxiety or avoidance suffer from attachment insecuri-
ties, worries about self-worth, or distrust of others’ goodwill and respon-
siveness in times of need. Moreover, insecure people tend to use secondary
attachment strategies that we, following Cassidy and Kobak (1988), char-
acterize as attachment system hyperactivation or deactivation when coping
with threats, frustrations, rejections, and losses. People who score high on
attachment anxiety rely on hyperactivating strategies—energetic attempts
to achieve support and love, combined with lack of confidence that these
resources will be provided and with feelings of anger and despair when they
are not provided (Cassidy & Kobak, 1988). In contrast, people who score
high on attachment-related avoidance tend to use deactivating strategies—
trying not to seek proximity to others when threatened, denying vulnerabil-
ity and needs for other people, and avoiding closeness and interdependence
in relationships.
With these ideas in mind, we can provide an overview of the cogni-
tive, affective, and relational outcomes associated with attachment system
functioning in adulthood. On the one hand, interactions with security-
enhancing attachment figures contribute to a stable and solid sense of
attachment security, which is an important aspect of healthy personality
development, favorable psychological functioning, and good social and
personal adjustment (see Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007a, for a review). On
the other hand, adoption of hyperactivating or deactivating strategies influ-
ences the specific defenses used by insecure people to regulate distress and
manage doubts about their self-worth and others’ availability, sensitivity,
and responsiveness. Adoption of a particular insecure strategy also shapes
the different emotional and relational problems that result from anxious
and avoidant forms of attachment. In the next section, we summarize the
positive effects of attachment security and related mental representations
on a person’s social motives, cognitions, and behaviors.
with sensitive and available attachment figures, individuals learn that dis-
tress is manageable, that external obstacles can be overcome, and that the
course and outcome of most threatening events are at least partially con-
trollable. Adult attachment studies provide extensive evidence that secure
individuals, as identified by self-report measures, appraise a wide variety of
stressful events in less threatening terms than insecure people (either anx-
ious or avoidant) do, and that they hold more optimistic expectations about
their ability to cope with stressors (e.g., Berant, Mikulincer, & Florian,
2001; Mikulincer & Florian, 1995; Radecki-Bush, Farrell, & Bush, 1993).
In addition, during interactions with supportive attachment figures,
individuals learn about others’ potential sensitivity, responsiveness, and
goodwill. They also learn to view themselves as active, strong, and com-
petent because they can effectively mobilize a partner’s support and over-
come threats that activate attachment behavior. Moreover, they perceive
themselves as valuable, lovable, and special, thanks to being valued, loved,
and regarded as special by caring attachment figures. Research has consis-
tently shown that such positive mental representations of self and others
are characteristic of secure persons (e.g., Baldwin, Fehr, Keedian, Seidel,
& Thomson, 1993; Collins, 1996; Collins & Read, 1990; Cooper, Shaver,
& Collins, 1998; Mickelson, Kessler, & Shaver, 1997; Mikulincer, 1995;
Simpson, 1990).
The broaden-and-build cycle of attachment security is renewed every
time a person notices that an actual or imaginary caring and loving attach-
ment figure is available in times of stress. To examine the psychological
reality of this cycle, we cannot rely solely on correlational studies examin-
ing cross-sectional or even prospective longitudinal associations between
dispositional measures of attachment orientations and measures of psycho-
logical functioning and mental health. Rather, we need to test whether
momentary or more prolonged experiences with actual or imaginary
responsive and supportive attachment figures, which we expect to increase
a person’s sense of security even if he or she is dispositionally insecure, can
activate the broaden-and-build cycle of attachment security and its positive
effects on psychological functioning. In the following sections of this chap-
ter we review studies that examined the psychological effects of momentary
or more prolonged boosts in attachment security. We first review findings
from laboratory experiments in which the security-enhancing mental rep-
resentations were primed in various ways. We then review findings from
both laboratory and field studies that have examined the effects of security-
heightening interpersonal experiences.
(2006) examined brain responses (via fMRI) of married women who under-
went a laboratory stressor (the threat of electric shock) while each one was
holding her husband’s hand, holding the hand of an unfamiliar male experi-
menter, or holding no hand at all. Holding a spouse’s hand reduced activa-
tion in brain regions associated with stress and distress (i.e., the right ante-
rior insula, superior frontal gyrus, and hypothalamus). The stress-reducing
effects of hand holding were greater for women who were more satisfied
with their marriages, probably because of the stronger sense of security
induced by physical contact with a responsive and supportive husband. In
another study, Master et al. (2009) found that holding the hand of a roman-
tic partner reduced perceptions of pain in response to heat stimuli.
Following this line of research, Kane, McCall, Collins, and Blascovich
(2012) asked young adults to complete a threatening cliff-walking task in
an immersive virtual environment. In this virtual world, each participant’s
romantic partner was, in three different experimental conditions, absent
from the virtual world; present in the world and attentive to the participant
during the task (waving, clapping at successes, head nodding, and actively
orienting his or her body toward the participant); or present but inattentive
(looking away from the participant). Participants in the attentive-partner
condition experienced the task as less stressful than those who were alone;
they also reported feeling more secure during the task and were less vigilant
of their partners’ behavior, compared to those in the inattentive-partner
condition. These findings suggest that a romantic partner can alleviate dis-
tress, particularly if he or she acts in an attentive and responsive manner—
that is, as a security-enhancing attachment figure.
Conceptually similar findings were reported by Guichard and Col-
lins (2008), who manipulated the quality of a romantic partner’s support
by having the partner send messages (actually written by the researchers)
before and after the focal person participated in a stressful speech-deliver-
ing task. Participants who received highly supportive messages were in a
better mood after their speech, had higher state self-esteem, and felt more
satisfied with their relationships, compared to those who received low-
support messages or no message from their partners. In a similar study,
Collins, Jaremka, and Kane (2009) found that experimentally manipulated
supportive messages from a romantic partner during a stressful speech task
(as compared to low-support messages) yielded lower cortisol levels and
more rapid emotional recovery from the stressful task.
There is also evidence that a responsive and supportive romantic
partner can enhance a person’s autonomous exploration and goal pursuit,
which is a component of the broaden-and-build cycle of attachment secu-
rity. According to Bowlby (1988), an important function of an attachment
figure is to provide a secure base from which another person can “make
sorties into the outside world” (p. 11), with confidence that he or she can
return for assistance and comfort if obstacles arise. Indeed, Feeney (2007)
found that participants’ perceptions of their romantic partners’ availability
Boosting Attachment Security in Adulthood 137
in times of need, and to accept and care for their soldiers rather than
rejecting and criticizing them). Two months after that (i.e., 4 months after
combat training began), soldiers once again evaluated their mental health.
The findings indicated that appraisals of officers as security providers (by
their soldiers) predicted desirable changes in soldiers’ mental health dur-
ing combat training. At the beginning of training, baseline mental health
was exclusively associated with soldiers’ own level of attachment security.
However, appraisals of officers’ provisions of security during combat train-
ing produced significant changes in soldiers’ mental health across the train-
ing (taking the baseline assessment into account). The higher the officers
were appraised by their soldiers as being more sensitive and responsive, the
more the soldiers’ mental health improved over 2 and 4 months of intensive
combat training. These findings highlight the important effects of lead-
ers’ functioning as security providers on their followers’ mental health and
emotional well-being under stressful conditions.
This research has been conceptually extended from the military set-
ting to other organizational settings. For example, Ronen and Mikulincer
(2012) collected data from subordinates and their direct managers in a
variety of business organizations and found that managers’ responsive-
ness and supportiveness as caregivers (as measured by a self-report scale)
predicted lower job burnout and higher job satisfaction among subordi-
nates. Importantly, these effects were not moderated by subordinates’ own
attachment insecurities.
There is also evidence that a counselor’s functioning as a security pro-
vider has beneficial effects on a client’s outcomes during and after career
counseling. In a 3-session career counseling study, Littman-Ovadia (2008)
found that counselees’ appraisal of their counselors as security-enhancing
attachment figures (following the second session) was a significant predic-
tor of heightened career exploration following counseling compared to
baseline career exploration, even after the researcher controlled for coun-
selees’ own attachment orientations. This appraisal of the therapist as a
security-enhancing attachment figure also mitigated the detrimental effects
of attachment anxiety and avoidance on career exploration. In another
study based on data from the National Institute of Mental Health Treat-
ment of Depression Collaborative Research Program, Zuroff and Blatt
(2006) found that a client’s positive appraisals of his or her therapist’s sensi-
tivity and supportiveness significantly predicted relief from depression and
maintenance of therapeutic benefits 18 months later. And these results were
not attributable to patient characteristics or severity of depression.
Conclusions
In line with Bowlby’s theorizing, the diverse research findings reviewed
in this chapter indicate that attachment security is key to the optimal
Boosting Attachment Security in Adulthood 139
functioning not only of the attachment behavioral system, but also of other
behavioral systems such as exploration and caregiving. This research also
shows that a person’s sense of security, which allows creative, undefensive
interactions with other people and fosters accurate perception of and effec-
tive reactions to others’ needs, is an important asset in both personal and
organizational relationships.
Fortunately, research conducted to date suggests that a person’s attach-
ment system, along with his or her core sense of security, can be changed for
the better. Moreover, the change can occur both immediately (and presum-
ably temporarily) and more permanently, and can occur both consciously
and unconsciously. The strong emphasis in the attachment research field on
individual differences—including much of our own work—may have made
those differences seem too deep-seated and robust to alter, even though the
documented benefits of good relationships and skilled psychotherapy have
always indicated that change for the better is possible. Fortunately, in both
behavioral and neuroscience research, it is becoming increasingly possible
to measure positive changes in systematically coded interpersonal behavior
and in functional brain images. This is therefore a hopeful time for testing
the efficacy of a variety of security-enhancing procedures.
One of the key remaining questions concerns how best to conceptual-
ize the sense of security and its role in mental and social processes. Is it a
“feeling,” an emotion, a background mood? How is it sustained when a
person is under pressure or suffering injuries or losses? How does it regu-
late defenses, allowing a person to be generous and supportive even when
mentally depleted—a process with important implications for parents, lov-
ers, teachers, and care providers of all kinds? More specifically, how does a
subliminal security prime, presumably acting through associative networks
in memory, alter a person’s sense of security, reduce defenses, and counter
compassion fatigue? Are its effects mainly cognitive, emotional, or neuro-
chemical? The well-documented effects reviewed in this chapter are major
discoveries, in our opinion, but their nature is still largely mysterious at
several different levels of analysis.
Acknowledgment
Preparation of this chapter was facilitated by a grant from the Fetzer Institute.
References
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of
attachment: A psychological study of the Strange Situation. Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Arndt, J., Schimel, J., Greenberg, J., & Pyszczynski, T. (2002). The intrinsic
self and defensiveness: Evidence that activating the intrinsic self reduces
140 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Master, S. L., Eisenberger, N. I., Taylor, S. E., Naliboff, B. D., Shirinyan, D., &
Lieberman, M. D. (2009). A picture’s worth: Partner photographs reduce
experimentally induced pain. Psychological Science, 20, 1316–1318.
Mickelson, K. D., Kessler, R. C., & Shaver, P. R. (1997). Adult attachment in a
nationally representative sample. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 73, 1092–1106.
Mikulincer, M. (1995). Attachment style and the mental representation of the self.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 1203–1215.
Mikulincer, M., & Florian, V. (1995). Appraisal of and coping with a real-life
stressful situation: The contribution of attachment styles. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 406–414.
Mikulincer, M., Gillath, O., Halevy, V., Avihou, N., Avidan, S., & Eshkoli, N.
(2001). Attachment theory and reactions to others’ needs: Evidence that acti-
vation of the sense of attachment security promotes empathic responses. Jour-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 1205–1224.
Mikulincer, M., Gillath, O., Sapir-Lavid, Y., Yaakobi, E., Arias, K., Tal-Aloni, L.,
et al. (2003). Attachment theory and concern for others’ welfare: Evidence
that activation of the sense of secure base promotes endorsement of self-tran-
scendence values. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 25, 299–312.
Mikulincer, M., Hirschberger, G., Nachmias, O., & Gillath, O. (2001). The affec-
tive component of the secure base schema: Affective priming with representa-
tions of attachment security. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
81, 305–321.
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2001). Attachment theory and intergroup bias:
Evidence that priming the secure base schema attenuates negative reactions to
out-groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 97–115.
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2003). The attachment behavioral system in
adulthood: Activation, psychodynamics, and interpersonal processes. In
M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 35,
pp. 53–152). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007a). Attachment in adulthood: Structure,
dynamics, and change. New York: Guilford Press.
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007b). Boosting attachment security to pro-
mote mental health, prosocial values, and inter-group tolerance. Psychologi-
cal Inquiry, 18, 139–156.
Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P. R., Gillath, O., & Nitzberg, R. A. (2005). Attachment,
caregiving, and altruism: Boosting attachment security increases compassion
and helping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 817–839.
Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P. R., & Horesh, N. (2006). Attachment bases of emotion
regulation and posttraumatic adjustment. In D. K. Snyder, J. A. Simpson, &
J. N. Hughes (Eds.), Emotion regulation in families: Pathways to dysfunction
and health (pp. 77–99). Washington, DC: American Psychological Associa-
tion.
Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P. R., & Rom, E. (2011). The effects of implicit and
explicit security priming on creative problem solving. Cognition and Emo-
tion, 25, 519–531.
Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P. R., Sahdra, B. K., & Bar-On, N. (2013). Can
Boosting Attachment Security in Adulthood 143
Nickola C. Overall
Edward P. Lemay, Jr.
A nnette loves her partner, David, but worries about whether David
truly loves her. Annette’s fears are particularly acute when David
appears to be unhappy with her in some way, such as when they dis-
agree. During these times, Annette’s distress can be overwhelming,
and she can’t stop thinking about what David’s reactions mean in
terms of his feelings toward her. She becomes propelled to ensure that
David still loves her, and often expresses to David how much she is
hurting and how much she needs him. David typically responds by
comforting and reassuring Annette, which helps her feel better and
more secure. Over the course of their relationship, David learns that he
needs to be careful not to trigger Annette’s fears, and so he sometimes
hides his negative feelings and makes a special effort to be affection-
ate and loving. This helps Annette feel happier and more confident in
David’s commitment to her.
Annette is high in attachment anxiety. Research investigating the
effects of attachment anxiety suggests that the difficulties Annette has
in regulating her emotions will cause problems in her relationship.
However, Annette and David’s responses to each other also involve
dyadic regulation. Annette’s efforts to draw David closer and gain
reassurance involve Annette trying to influence or regulate how David
is feeling, thinking, and behaving. Similarly, David’s efforts to soothe
Annette’s concerns and help her feel loved involve David trying to reg-
ulate Annette’s thoughts, feelings, and behavior.
145
146 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
TABLE 6.1. Examples of the Regulation Strategies Enacted by Insecurely Attached Individuals
and Their Partners
Regulation of the partner Regulation by the partner
Regulation Potential Potential Regulation Potential Potential
strategies benefits costs strategies benefits costs
Anxiety Guilt Reassurance Partner Exagger- Reassurance Partner
induction of partner’s dissatisfaction ated of partner’s dissatisfac-
love and and rejection affection love and tion and
commitment commitment rejection
low self-esteem, and high proneness to hurt feelings. We focus here on the
analyses examining attachment anxiety.
In their first study, Lemay and Dudley (2011) asked friends and roman-
tic partners to privately rate each other’s attachment anxiety and senti-
ments toward each other. Consistent with their prediction that partners
can and do detect insecure individuals’ high anxiety, there was a significant
link between the partners’ perceptions of individuals’ attachment anxiety
and individuals’ reports of their own attachment anxiety. To test whether
perceiving individuals’ anxiety led partners to conceal their negative feel-
ings about those individuals, toward the end of the experimental session
participants were unexpectedly asked to report their feelings toward each
other again. This time, however, half of the participants were assigned to a
“public” condition in which they were told that they would see each other’s
responses. As expected, partners who believed their responses would be
public and perceived they were in relationships with highly anxious individ-
uals concealed their negative evaluations by providing more positive ratings
than they had first provided. In contrast, partners who perceived they were
in relationships with individuals low in anxiety exhibited very few differ-
ences between their private and public responses, illustrating that they did
not attempt to conceal negative feelings.
A follow-up diary study with romantic couples provided further sup-
port that partners detect and regulate anxious individuals’ insecurity dur-
ing their daily interactions (Lemay & Dudley, 2011, Study 3). Replicating
their first study, partners accurately detected when they were involved in
relationships with anxious individuals. This detection of anxiety in turn
predicted efforts to regulate the insecurity of highly anxious individuals.
Across their daily interactions, partners who detected individuals’ attach-
ment anxiety reported more vigilance about upsetting anxious individuals
and greater motivations to up-regulate their feelings of relationship security.
Self-reports of vigilance were also corroborated by partners demonstrating
greater accuracy in anxious individuals’ daily sentiments. In particular,
partners who reported greater vigilance about upsetting their mates were
more accurate at detecting and remembering anxious individuals’ feelings
about their relationships. Thus partners’ vigilance appeared to optimize
detection of anxious individuals’ feelings of insecurity.
Lemay and Dudley (2011) also found that individuals who perceived
their partners to be more anxious reported a series of behaviors intended
to regulate their partners’ feelings of security, including concealing nega-
tive sentiments about anxious partners, exaggerating affection and positive
sentiments, and providing unmitigated forms of caregiving that involved
attending to the needs of anxious individuals at the expense of their own
needs. These regulation strategies were also effective; highly anxious
individuals felt more secure in their partners’ care and regard when their
partners enacted these daily regulation behaviors. Combined, the results
154 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
reported by Lemay and Dudley (2011) provide strong evidence that (1)
partners try to avoid triggering anxious individuals’ insecurities by cau-
tiously camouflaging discontent and accentuating how positive they feel in
their relationships, and (2) these regulation strategies are effective at help-
ing insecure individuals feel more valued and regarded.
The novel results reported by Lemay and Dudley (2011) highlight that
the impact of attachment insecurity on relationships depends on whether
and how successfully partners regulate the insecurity of anxious individu-
als. Their findings also show that partners’ regulation efforts appear to be
intentional and calibrated to the assessed need for regulation. In particular,
partners attempted to conceal negative feelings and reassure anxious indi-
viduals when they detected feelings of insecurity and when they encoun-
tered situations that were likely to exacerbate those anxieties, such as expo-
sure to negative evaluations. Simpson and Overall (2014) also highlight
that partners may be most likely to enact regulation behaviors in stressful
contexts that activate the attachment system, and in turn the rejection sen-
sitivity and emotion-focused strategies arising from attachment anxiety.
For example, confronting the pronounced distress and guilt induction strat-
egies that highly anxious individuals exhibit during conflict should initiate
counterattempts by their partners to down-regulate these potentially dam-
aging emotions and behaviors.
A study by Tran and Simpson (2009) provides further support for the
role partners play in down-regulating insecurities in conflict situations.
Tran and Simpson (2009) video-recorded couples discussing areas of rela-
tionship conflict, gathered measures of emotional reactions during the dis-
cussion, and objectively coded the presence of accommodation behaviors.
Accommodation involves resisting hurtful impulses and instead trying to
resolve a problem in a calm, forgiving, and supportive manner (Rusbult,
Verette, Whitney, Slovik, & Lipkus, 1991). Accommodation is good for
relationships, but it is hard to do; it requires self-control and high levels
of commitment (Arriaga & Rusbult, 1998; Finkel & Campbell, 2001).
Because of this, accommodation also conveys the qualities that highly
anxious individuals desire—love, commitment, and trustworthiness (Wie-
selquist, Rusbult, Foster, & Agnew, 1999)—all of which should soothe
anxious reactivity.
As expected, Tran and Simpson (2009) found that highly anxious indi-
viduals felt less accepted, reported more negative emotions, and in turn
behaved in less accommodating ways during the discussion. The partners
of highly anxious individuals also tended to respond with greater negative
emotions and less accommodation. However, when partners were highly
committed, they were able to prevent the reactivity of their highly anxious
mates from infecting their own responses and displayed more accommo-
dation. Additional dyadic analyses reported by Tran and Simpson (2011)
indicated that partners’ accommodation helped to counteract the effects
Attachment and Dyadic Regulation Processes 155
for example, partners may be able to curtail the anger and withdrawal
that highly avoidant individuals typically display by “softening” their com-
munication through (1) reducing direct influence attempts that challenge
avoidant targets (by downplaying problem severity, acknowledging prog-
ress made, and validating targets’ point of view); and (2) offering clear
evidence that avoidant targets are valued (by reducing friction, inhibiting
negativity, and expressing positive regard). In the study described above,
Overall et al. (2013) measured these types of softening behaviors. Avoidant
targets whose partners displayed more softening communication exhibited
less anger and withdrawal, which in turn generated more success in resolv-
ing the relationship problems couples discussed.
These types of softening behaviors should be particularly effective at
alleviating avoidant defenses because they are less autonomy-threatening
and they contradict the hostile intentions that avoidant individuals often
anticipate from their partners. Other research also shows that behaviors
sidestepping the emotional dependence that highly avoidant individuals
find disconcerting help in down-regulating avoidant reactivity. Simpson et
al. (2007) assessed the degree to which individuals were visibly calmed by
different types of caregiving at moments during conflict discussions when
individuals were most visibly upset. Individuals low in avoidance (i.e.,
those who were more securely attached as assessed by the Adult Attach-
ment Interview) were rated as more calmed when their partners gave them
more emotional care, such as encouraging them to talk about their emo-
tions/experiences; by contrast, individuals high in avoidance were more
calmed when their partners delivered instrumental caregiving, such as giv-
ing specific advice and concrete solutions to problems (see also Mikulincer
& Florian, 1997). Although high levels of emotional caregiving may coun-
teract avoidant individuals’ negative expectations, the emotionally laden
and intimacy-inducing nature of this type of caregiving requires too much
vulnerability and intimacy for highly avoidant people to lower their self-
protective defenses. Thus partners of avoidant individuals need to convey
their trustworthiness and availability, while simultaneously being sensitive
to the avoidant individuals’ needs to maintain emotional independence and
autonomy.
Salvatore, Kuo, Steele, Simpson, and Collins (2011) also demon-
strated that recovery following couples’ conflict interactions is important
in minimizing the damage that insecure attachment can cause to relation-
ships. Salvatore et al. (2011) examined conflict recovery during a 4-minute
“cool-down” task that immediately followed adult couples’ discussions of
a major relationship problem. Better conflict recovery was evident when
partners focused on the positive aspects of their relationships and were
responsive to each other’s repair attempts. Attachment insecurity (primar-
ily avoidance) during infancy as assessed in the Strange Situation approxi-
mately 20 years earlier predicted poorer conflict recovery, and insecure
Attachment and Dyadic Regulation Processes 163
individuals whose partners could not “move beyond the conflict” were
less likely to be together 2 years later. In contrast, insecure participants
involved with partners who exhibited better conflict recovery were more
likely to be together 2 years later. As with softening and instrumental care-
giving behaviors, recovering from conflict should signal that their partners
can be trusted to let go of negativity, which should provide avoidant indi-
viduals with the room they need to restore comfortable levels of autonomy.
These critical ingredients are what may have helped couples maintain their
relationships.
References
Arriaga, X. B., Kumashiro, M., Finkel, E. J., VanderDrift, L. E., & Luchies, L. B.
(2014). Filling the void: Bolstering attachment security in committed relation-
ships. Social Psychological and Personal Science, 5(4), 398–406.
Arriaga, X. B., & Rusbult, C. E. (1998). Standing in my partner’s shoes: Partner
perspective taking and reactions to accommodative dilemmas. Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 927–948.
Baumeister, R. F., Stillwell, A. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (1994). Guilt: An interper-
sonal approach. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 243–267.
Bouthillier, D., Julien, D., Dube, M., Belanger, I., & Hamelin, M. (2002). Predictive
166 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Gurit E. Birnbaum
170
Convergence of Sexual Urges and Emotional Bonds 171
al., 2006). On the other hand, attachment-related worries may lead simul-
taneously to aversive feelings during sexual intercourse (Birnbaum, 2007a).
Indeed, when describing their experiences of sexual intercourse, anxiously
attached people report relatively high levels of a “letting go” state of mind
and desire for emotional involvement, warmth, and attention from their
partners. At the same time, they also report negative emotions and doubts
about being loved (Birnbaum et al., 2006). Unsurprisingly, this pattern of
sexual experiences often fail to meet their unrealistic relationship expecta-
tions, resulting in frustrated attachment needs and sexual disappointment
(Birnbaum, 2007a; Birnbaum et al., 2006).
More avoidant individuals, in contrast, feel uncomfortable being close
to others (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007b) and may thus be threatened by
the demand for personal disclosure implied by sexual interactions. These
intimacy fears may motivate them either to abstain from sexual activity
altogether (or at least delay its onset; Gentzler & Kerns, 2004; Kalichman
et al., 1993; Tracy et al., 2003) or to approach sex in various distancing
ways. Avoidant people typically downplay sexual motives associated with
the promotion of emotional closeness and instead pursue self-serving goals,
such as having sex to feel good about oneself (see Cooper et al., 2006;
Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007a, for reviews). Consequently, they are less
likely than less avoidant people to be sexually interested in potential part-
ners who want to be close, and are more likely to be attracted to partners
with similar needs for independence (Birnbaum & Reis, 2012; Holmes &
Johnson, 2009).
Avoidant people are also more likely than less avoidant people to be
promiscuous and nonexclusive in intimate relationships. They hold more
permissive attitudes about casual sex and are more likely to engage in
uncommitted sex with different partners (Brennan & Shaver, 1995; Fee-
ney, Noller, & Patty, 1993; Gentzler & Kerns, 2004). Consistent with this
pattern, they tend to respond favorably to mate-poaching attempts (i.e.,
attempts to lure them away from their current partners) in a short-term con-
text, but not when the poaching is for a long-term relationship (Schachner
& Shaver, 2002). As might be expected, avoidant people’s tendency to have
sex outside their relationships is explained by low levels of commitment to
their relationship partners (DeWall et al., 2011) and is reinforced by their
primary partners’ desire for intimacy (Beaulieu-Pelletier, Philippe, Lecours,
& Couture, 2011).
Avoidant people distance themselves from their partners not only by
engaging in extradyadic sex, but also by relying on the solitary sexual activ-
ity of masturbation (Bogaert & Sadava, 2002) and by having less frequent
sex with their partners (Brassard, Shaver, & Lussier, 2007). When avoid-
ant people do have sex with their partners, they display relatively low lev-
els of physical affection and experience difficulties in attending to their
partners’ needs. In addition, they experience relatively strong feelings of
Convergence of Sexual Urges and Emotional Bonds 175
are more salient. Among men, attachment anxiety is associated with rela-
tively restricted sexual expression. For example, anxiously attached men
are less likely than less anxiously attached men to have sex to bolster their
self-esteem or to cheat on their partners (Cooper et al., 2006), and they
have fewer sex partners overall (Gentzler & Kerns, 2004). At the same
time, they are particularly likely to exert pressure on their current partners
to have sex (Brassard et al., 2007). Among women, attachment anxiety
is associated with unrestricted and risky sexual behaviors. For example,
anxiously attached women are inclined to engage in extrapair sex (Bogaert
& Sadava, 2002; Gangestad & Thornhill, 1997), as well as in unprotected
and consensual unwanted sex (e.g., Feeney & Noller, 2004; Impett & Pep-
lau, 2002). Expectedly, they also report higher rates of unplanned preg-
nancy than less anxiously attached women do (Cooper, Shapiro, & Powers,
1998).
These findings suggest that the same relational worries that inhibit
initiating sex with new partners among men and lead them to invest more
in current relationships also lead men to use coercive sex as a means for
regaining proximity to partners who are perceived as unresponsive. Similar
relational worries appear to create difficulty in negotiating sexual encoun-
ters among women and lead them to secure alternatives to their current
partners, both in the real and in the virtual worlds. Indeed, a corresponding
pattern emerges in the content of sexual fantasies: More anxiously attached
women are more likely than less anxiously attached women to experience
fantasies that involve unrestricted sex and are less likely to fantasize about
their current partners. More anxiously attached men, by comparison, are
more likely than less anxiously attached men to experience sexual fanta-
sies that involve romantic themes and to express the desire to satisfy their
partners (Birnbaum, 2007b). The content of these fantasies seems to reflect
the typical defense mechanisms employed by anxiously attached men and
women, further supporting the notion that sexual expressions constitute
a route by which men and women uniquely cope with their attachment
insecurities.
(e.g., insecurity regarding the love of one’s partner, possible mate poaching,
prospective separation). Such threatening conditions are likely to activate
attachment concerns that call for distress regulation (e.g., Davis, Shaver, &
Vernon, 2003; Mikulincer, Gillath, & Shaver, 2002; Simpson, Rholes, &
Phillips, 1996) and may therefore elicit attachment defensive strategies in
insecurely attached individuals (e.g., Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007b; Simp-
son & Rholes, 1994). Given that behaviors characteristic of the sexual sys-
tem may serve the goals of deactivation and hyperactivation strategies (e.g.,
maintaining distance by engaging in emotionless sex, and avoiding aban-
donment by deferring to partners’ sexual needs, respectively; Birnbaum,
2010; Davis et al., 2006), it is reasonable to expect that under relationship-
threatening conditions, the sexual manifestations of these defensive strate-
gies may be particularly pronounced.
This reasoning has received support from studies that examined
sexual responses to a variety of relationship-threatening conditions (e.g.,
hypothetical relationship threat scenarios, actual troubled interactions).
For example, in two series of experiments, participants imagined relation-
ship-threatening scenes (e.g., a partner’s considering breaking up, a part-
ner’s infidelity), non-relationship-threatening scenes (failure on an exam),
or nonthreatening scenarios (e.g., a partner going to a grocery store).
Following this procedure, participants rated or described their desire to
have sex, reasons for engaging in sex (Birnbaum, Weisberg, & Simpson,
2011), and what sexual activities they would like to engage in (i.e., their
sexual fantasies) (Birnbaum, Svitelman, Bar-Shalom, & Porat, 2008). The
results indicated that relationship threat generated mixed emotions in anx-
iously attached people. Specifically, it decreased their pursuit of sexual
pleasure (Birnbaum, Weisberg, et al., 2011) and increased their habitual
negative self-representations, such that highly anxious people represented
themselves as more alienated and hostile in their fantasies (Birnbaum et
al., 2008). This negative reaction, which may reflect the intensification of
anger toward the threatening partners and self-relevant thoughts typical of
relationship-threatening events (e.g., Campbell & Marshall, 2011; Miku-
lincer & Shaver, 2007b), was accompanied by a heightened desire to satisfy
others sexually (Birnbaum et al., 2008).
The findings of these studies suggest that anxiously attached people
have trouble enjoying sex when being flooded with the relationship worries
imposed by threat. Threats to their relationships exacerbate their habit-
ual insecurity, motivating them to respond to threats with relationship-
maintaining behaviors (e.g., using sex to please their partners) while deny-
ing their own sexual needs. The ensuing frustration may pose additional
obstacles to their erotic pleasure. Tragically, the ambivalent reaction that
relationship threat invokes in anxiously attached people may lead them to
display relationship-destructive behaviors (e.g., recurring bouts of uncon-
trollable anger, partner surveillance; Guerrero, 1998; Mikulincer & Shaver,
Convergence of Sexual Urges and Emotional Bonds 179
2005). Such behaviors may eventually sabotage their attempts to repair the
threatened relationship and lead them to realize their worst fear—relation-
ship loss (e.g., Campbell & Marshall, 2011; Campbell, Simpson, Boldry, &
Kashy, 2005; Simpson et al., 1996).
The studies on sexual reactions to threats also indicate that, as
expected, avoidant people use distancing strategies when being threatened.
Specifically, relationship threat lessened avoidant people’s desire to have
sex with their partners (Birnbaum, Weisberg, et al., 2011). In addition,
reminders of death—the final separation and possibly the ultimate threat—
increased the likelihood that avoidant people have sex for self-serving rea-
sons (e.g., affirming their self-worth) as well as the likelihood that avoidant
men engage in casual sex (Birnbaum, Hirschberger, et al., 2011). These
findings show that unlike anxiously attached people, who use sex to repair
threatened relationships, avoidant people react to threat by withdrawing
sexually from their partners and by using sex as a means to feel better about
themselves. That is, avoidant people attempt to protect themselves against
anticipated rejection by engaging in compensatory self-enhancement and
defensive distancing from their potentially rejecting partners.
A similar pattern was observed in a dyadic study conducted in a more
natural context (Birnbaum, Mikulincer, et al., 2011). In this study, mem-
bers of heterosexual cohabiting couples reported their attachment orienta-
tions and then provided daily diary measures of their relationship interac-
tions for 21 consecutive days. In addition, immediately after every occasion
in which they experienced a sexual fantasy during the 21-day study period,
participants described it in narrative form. The results revealed that nega-
tive couple interactions increased habitual attachment-related wishes and
self-representations. Specifically, distressful relationship events led anx-
iously attached people to shift from fantasizing about mutually nurturing
themes to fantasizing about submission themes that emphasize their needi-
ness (e.g., representations of the self as weak and helpless), which could
serve the goal of eliciting caregiving from a powerful partner (Birnbaum,
2007b; Davis et al., 2004). Avoidant people, in contrast, reacted to trou-
bled interactions with distancing themes and enhancement fantasies. They
expressed more avoidant wishes (escaping reality) and represented them-
selves as less weak and helpless.
Considered together, these findings demonstrate the involvement of
sexual mental imagery in handling attachment-related stressful events
and imply that such negative events amplify habitual defensive tenden-
cies. These events elicit compensatory self-enhancement (i.e., protective
responses in which the self is represented as sexually potent) among avoid-
ant people, and compensatory relational restoration strategies (i.e., pro-
tective responses designed to improve the relationship) among anxiously
attached people.
This conclusion is tempered, however, by results from a recent series of
180 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
to their partners for sexual consolation if they realize that their partners
are not likely to respond positively to such advances. Corroborating this
view, a study examining the effects of relational conflict on sexual motiva-
tion showed that major conflict inhibited self-serving sexual motives (e.g.,
having sex to obtain relief from stress) among people with avoidant part-
ners (Birnbaum et al., 2013). These findings imply that people are unlikely
to use avoidant partners as a source of comfort while engaging in so-called
“make-up” sex, because they do not view them as responsive figures that
can alleviate their distress, either inside or outside the bedroom. Indeed,
avoidant people habitually distance themselves from emotional partners,
dismiss their signals of sexual and nonsexual needs, and provide them with
less support (Birnbaum et al., 2006; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007b; Reis,
2007).
Other studies have demonstrated the value of adopting a dyadic per-
spective for understanding the manifestations of attachment dynamics in
everyday sexual experience. These studies show, for example, that partners
of anxiously attached people do not report greater levels of sexual dis-
satisfaction than partners of less anxiously attached people do, at least in
nonclinical samples (Butzer & Campbell, 2008; Impett & Peplau, 2002;
but see Brassard, Péloquin, Dupuy, Wright, & Shaver, 2012, for different
results in couples seeking marital therapy). This is probably because anx-
iously attached people tend to please their partners and defer to their sexual
needs (e.g., Davis et al., 2006). Still, partners of anxiously attached women
do experience relational distress following negative sexual interactions—a
finding that may reflect their negative reaction to the destructive behavior
that anxiously attached women exhibit after having experienced negative
feelings during sex (Birnbaum et al., 2006). In contrast, and as expected,
partners of avoidant people report greater levels of sexual dissatisfaction
than do partners of less avoidant people (e.g., Butzer & Campbell, 2008).
Furthermore, partners of avoidant men, compared to partners of less avoid-
ant men, show fewer reductions in relationship-damaging behaviors fol-
lowing sexual interactions, possibly because sex with an avoidant male
may contribute minimally to women’s intimacy goals in close relationships
(Birnbaum et al., 2006).
Taking into consideration the unique configuration of both partners’
attachment orientations extends these studies by unraveling the interac-
tive nature of couples’ sexuality. The predictive power of this approach
has been demonstrated in a study in which members of established couples
reported their attachment orientations and sexual experiences (Brassard
et al., 2007). This study revealed dyadic interaction patterns in which two
anxiously attached partners have a relatively high rate of sexual inter-
course. However, anxiously attached men have sex less often if their female
partners are less anxiously attached. This pattern suggests that anxiously
attached men’s intense efforts to have sex are well received by partners with
182 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
similar intimacy needs, but deter less anxiously attached partners, who
may perceive such excessive demands as irritating. Somewhat similarly,
anxiously attached women have sex less often if their male partners are
avoidantly attached, probably because anxiously attached women’s needs
for reassurance clash with their avoidant partners’ intimacy fears. Avoidant
men are also more likely to avoid sexual activities in their current relation-
ships, to experience sexual difficulties, and to have sex less often if their
female partners are avoidantly attached. It seems that fears of intimacy that
burden both partners in a couple may be particularly detrimental to their
sexuality, because neither of them is motivated to resolve their relationship
difficulties.
preceding orgasm (e.g., Carmichael et al., 1987; Carter, 1992; Filippi et al.,
2003; Murphy, Seckl, Burton, Checkley, & Lightman, 1987). Moreover,
in contrast to most mammalian species, humans have sex on every day of
the menstrual cycle; they may therefore experience an extended release of
oxytocin and vasopressin that further reinforces sexual bonding, which
over time promotes enduring attachment bonds between sexual partners
(Young & Wang, 2004).
Research assessing mental representations of the sexual aspect of
romantic relationships provides additional support for the theorized link
between sex and attachment. Specifically, studies exploring people’s
accounts of their motives for having sex show that some of the most fre-
quently endorsed reasons for having sex involve relationship-related motives,
such as the desire for emotional closeness and the desire to intensify the
relationship (e.g., Meston & Buss, 2007). Similarly, research examining the
functional meaning of sex has indicated that prevalent meanings attached
to sex reflect the beliefs that sexual activity promotes intimacy between
partners and enhances their emotional bond (e.g., Birnbaum, 2003; Birn-
baum & Gillath, 2006; Birnbaum & Reis, 2006). Subsequent research has
extended these studies by providing evidence for the hypothesized causal
pathway from activation of the sexual system to attachment formation
and maintenance (Gillath, Mikulincer, Birnbaum, & Shaver, 2008). In
this series of experiments, participants were subliminally exposed to erotic
stimuli (vs. neutral stimuli). The results revealed that subliminal exposure
to sexually arousing stimuli increased the willingness to self-disclose inti-
mate information to a potential new partner, as well as the willingness to
engage in relationship-promoting behaviors with existing partners. These
findings imply that sexual arousal leads people to employ strategies that
allow them to get closer to potential new partners or to strengthen relation-
ships with existing ones.
future interactions with this person (Birnbaum & Reis, 2006, 2012). Once
a suitable partner is found, sexual desire for this partner may motivate the
individual to form a relationship that extends beyond a single sexual epi-
sode (Birnbaum & Gillath, 2006).
As a relationship progresses from initial encounters to casual dating to
steady dating, sex may serve as a binding force that fosters emotional bond-
ing between sexual partners and strengthens their emerging relationship. A
recent longitudinal study has supported this theorizing (Mizrahi, Birnbaum,
Hirschberger, Mikulincer, & Szepsenwol, 2015). In this study, members of
couples who had been dating for less than 4 months completed measures
of sexual desire, frequency of sexual intercourse, and relationship-specific
attachment avoidance and anxiety three times over an 8-month period. The
results indicated that relationship-specific attachment insecurities declined
over time, but only among participants who reported relatively high levels
of sexual desire and high frequency of sexual intercourse. These findings
suggest that sexual activity reduces attachment defenses in the early stages
of dating, thereby fostering the formation of genuine intimacy (see also
Rubin & Campbell, 2012).
In later phases of relationship development, sex may still help maintain
a relationship (e.g., Bell, Daly, & Gonzalez, 1987; Birnbaum et al., 2006),
but may become less important to its quality and stability than other aspects
of the relationship, such as the provision of mutual support, warmth, and
interdependence (Kotler, 1985; Reedy, Birren, & Schaie, 1981; Sternberg,
1986). Nevertheless, sex may turn out to be especially beneficial to the
relationship of most people in relationship-threatening situations, which
provoke anxiety and elicit proximity seeking. In these situations, people
may use sex to repair their threatened relationships (e.g., Birnbaum, 2014;
Birnbaum et al., 2008). Frequent sexual activity can also buffer against
the detrimental relational implications of potentially destructive personal-
ity traits of romantic partners (e.g., neuroticism; Russell & McNulty, 2011)
or deficits in nonsexual relational dimensions (e.g., poor communication;
Litzinger & Gordon, 2005). It is possible that the intimacy inherent in sex-
ual contact provides an alternative, compensatory route for satisfying the
otherwise unmet attachment needs for security and love.
Yet relationship restoration is not always feasible, such as in the case
of major and insoluble relational conflict. Hence sex may eventually tear
partners apart. For example, prolonged major conflict may lead to a decline
in the desire for sex with one’s partner (e.g., Birnbaum et al., 2013), which
in turn may contribute to reevaluation of his or her suitability. When loss of
sexual interest signals incompatibility with the partner’s relationship goals,
it may motivate the individual to seek resolution of these interpersonal
problems, either with the current partner or by looking for a more suitable
one (Birnbaum & Reis, 2006).
186 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
how and why the sexual system develops either optimally or nonoptimally.
Individual differences in the functioning of any behavioral system were
theorized to result from a history of positive and negative outcomes of sys-
tem activation in various contexts across the lifespan (Bowlby, 1969/1982,
1973). Behavioral genetic studies have challenged this view by revealing
that, at least in the case of the attachment system, heritable factors may
account for some of these individual differences (e.g., Crawford et al., 2007;
see also Fraley & Roisman, Chapter 1, this volume). It is possible, of course,
that genetic factors interact with early life experiences (e.g., parental atti-
tudes toward sex, early sexual experiences) and current changes in the social
context (e.g., partners’ responses, social norms of sexuality) to shape the
functioning of the sexual system. Future studies should identify the ori-
gins of individual differences in the sexual system’s functioning, and should
clarify the extent to which these differences are heritable and the extent to
which they are produced by historical and contemporary contextual factors.
Future work should also more fully explore the dual role of sex: on
the one hand, as a potent relationship maintenance mechanism, and on the
other hand, as a force motivating people to pursue alternative sexual part-
ners. Several studies have delineated some of the conditions that encour-
age the pursuit of these seemingly conflicting goals. For example, a recent
study has indicated that women who are strongly attached to their part-
ners are more likely to desire sexual intimacy with the partners when they
themselves are fertile than when nonfertile, whereas women who are not
strongly attached to their long-term partners show the opposite pattern
(Eastwick & Finkel, 2012). Another study revealed that men’s investment
in their spouses is associated with lower testosterone levels (Gray, Kahlen-
berg, Barrett, Lipson, & Ellison, 2002). These findings suggest that activa-
tion of attachment processes can inhibit relationship-threatening tenden-
cies (i.e., extradyadic mating efforts near ovulation, extrapair desires that
are enhanced by higher testosterone levels).
Studies that pursue this line of investigation should take into account
the new challenges for traditional committed romantic relationships pre-
sented by the increase in social acceptability of alternative lifestyles (e.g.,
swinging, open relationships, polyamory; Conley, Ziegler, Moors, Matsick,
& Valentine, 2013) and the rise of Internet technology and social media
(e.g., greater availability of alternative partners, engaging in online sexual
activities without a partner). Clearly, these changing societal trends may
also create new opportunities for sexual expression in existing relation-
ships. For example, sexting and engaging in partnered online sexual activi-
ties may connect romantic partners across geographical distance, enabling
them to interact more intimately than at any time in the past. Future studies
should examine whether the new patterns of sexual communication pro-
mote the quality of previously challenged relationships, whether virtual
sexual expressions have similar beneficial effects on attachment processes
188 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
References
Andersen, S. M., & Chen, S. (2002). The relational self: An interpersonal social–
cognitive theory. Psychological Review, 109, 619–645.
Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among young
adults: A test of a four-category model. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 61, 226–244.
Bartz, J. A., & Lydon, J. E. (2004). Close relationships and the working self-con-
cept: Implicit and explicit effects of priming attachment on agency and com-
munion. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1389–1401.
Basson, R. (2000). The female sexual response: A different model. Journal of Sex
and Marital Therapy, 26, 51–65.
Baumeister, R. F., & Bratslavsky, E. (1999). Passion, intimacy, and time: Passionate
love as a function of change in intimacy. Personality and Social Psychology
Review, 3, 49–67.
Beaulieu-Pelletier, G., Philippe, F., Lecours, S., & Couture, S. (2011). The role of
attachment avoidance in extradyadic sex. Attachment and Human Develop-
ment, 13, 293–313.
Bell, R. A., Daly, J. A., & Gonzalez, C. (1987). Affinity-maintenance in marriage
and its relationship to women’s marital satisfaction. Journal of Marriage and
the Family, 49, 445–454.
Berscheid, E., & Reis, H. T. (1998). Attraction and close relationships. In D. T.
Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology
(3rd ed., Vol. 2, pp. 193–281). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Birnbaum, G. E. (2003). The meaning of heterosexual intercourse among women
with female orgasmic disorder. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 32, 61–71.
Birnbaum, G. E. (2007a). Attachment orientations, sexual functioning, and rela-
tionship satisfaction in a community sample of women. Journal of Social and
Personal Relationships, 24, 21–35.
Birnbaum, G. E. (2007b). Beyond the borders of reality: Attachment orientations
and sexual fantasies. Personal Relationships, 14, 321–342.
Birnbaum, G. E. (2010). Bound to interact: The divergent goals and complex inter-
play of attachment and sex within romantic relationships. Journal of Social
and Personal Relationships, 27, 245–252.
Birnbaum, G. E. (2014). Sexy building blocks: The contribution of the sexual
system to attachment formation and maintenance. In M. Mikulincer & P.
R. Shaver (Eds.), Mechanisms of social connection: From brain to group
(pp. 315–332). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Birnbaum, G. E., & Gillath, O. (2006). Measuring subgoals of the sexual behav-
ioral system: What is sex good for? Journal of Social and Personal Relation-
ships, 23, 675–701.
Birnbaum, G. E., Hirschberger, G., & Goldenberg, J. L. (2011). Desire in the face
of death: Terror management, attachment, and sexual motivation. Personal
Relationships, 18, 1–19.
Convergence of Sexual Urges and Emotional Bonds 189
Butzer, B., & Campbell, L. (2008). Adult attachment, sexual satisfaction, and rela-
tionship satisfaction: A study of married couples. Personal Relationships, 15,
141–154.
Byers, E. S., & Heinlein, L. (1989). Predicting initiations and refusals of sexual
activities in married and cohabiting heterosexual couples. Journal of Sex
Research, 26, 210–231.
Campbell, L., & Marshall, T. (2011). Anxious attachment and relationship pro-
cesses: An interactionist perspective. Journal of Personality, 79, 917–947.
Campbell, L., Simpson, J. A., Boldry, J. G., & Kashy, D. (2005). Perceptions of
conflict and support in romantic relationships: The role of attachment anxi-
ety. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 510–531.
Carmichael, M. S., Humbert, R., Dixen, J., Palmisano, G., Greenleaf, W., & David-
son, J. M. (1987). Plasma oxytocin increases in the human sexual response.
Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 64, 27–31.
Carroll, J. L., Volk, K. D., & Hyde, J. S. (1985). Differences between males and
females in motives for engaging in sexual intercourse. Archives of Sexual
Behavior, 14, 131–139.
Carter, C. S. (1992). Oxytocin and sexual behavior. Neuroscience and Biobehav-
ioral Reviews, 16, 131–144.
Carter, C. S., Ahnert, L., Grossmann, K. E., Hrdy, S. B., Lamb, M. E., Porges, S.
W., et al. (Eds.). (2005). Attachment and bonding: A new synthesis. Cam-
bridge, MA: MIT Press.
Cohen, D., & Belsky, J. (2008). Avoidant romantic attachment and female orgasm:
Testing an emotion-regulation hypothesis. Attachment and Human Develop-
ment, 10, 1–11.
Conley, T. D., Ziegler, A., Moors, A. C., Matsick, J. L., & Valentine, B. (2013). A
critical examination of popular assumptions about the benefits and outcomes
of monogamous relationships. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 17,
124–141.
Cooper, M. L., Pioli, M., Levitt, A., Talley, A., Micheas, L., & Collins, N. L.
(2006). Attachment styles, sex motives, and sexual behavior: Evidence for
gender-specific expressions of attachment dynamics. In M. Mikulincer & G.
S. Goodman (Eds.), Dynamics of romantic love: Attachment, caregiving, and
sex (pp. 243–274). New York: Guilford Press.
Cooper, M. L., Shapiro, C. M., & Powers, A. M. (1998). Motivations for sex and
risky sexual behavior among adolescents and young adults: A functional per-
spective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1528–1558.
Crawford, T. N., Livesley, W. J., Jang, K. L., Shaver, P. R., Cohen, P., & Gani-
ban, J. (2007). Insecure attachment and personality disorder: A twin study of
adults. European Journal of Personality, 21, 191–208.
Cyranowski, J. M., & Andersen, B. L. (1998). Schemas, sexuality, and romantic
attachment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1364–1379.
Davis, D., Shaver, P. R., & Vernon, M. L. (2003). Physical, emotional, and behav-
ioral reactions to breaking up. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
29, 871–884.
Davis, D., Shaver, P. R., & Vernon, M. L. (2004). Attachment style and subjective
motivations for sex. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1076–
1090.
Convergence of Sexual Urges and Emotional Bonds 191
Davis, D., Shaver, P. R., Widaman, K. F., Vernon, M. L., Follette, W. C., & Beitz,
K. (2006). “I can’t get no satisfaction”: Insecure attachment, inhibited sex-
ual communication, and sexual dissatisfaction. Personal Relationships, 13,
465–483.
DeLamater, J. D. (1987). Gender differences in sexual scenarios. In K. Kelley (Ed.),
Females, males, and sexuality (pp. 127–140). Albany: State University of New
York Press.
DeWall, C., Lambert, N., Slotter, E., Pond, R., Deckman, T., Finkel, E., et al.
(2011). So far away from one’s partner, yet so close to romantic alternatives:
Avoidant attachment, interest in alternatives, and infidelity. Journal of Per-
sonality and Social Psychology, 101, 1302–1316.
Diamond, L. M. (2003). What does sexual orientation orient?: A biobehavioral
model distinguishing romantic love and sexual desire. Psychological Review,
110, 173–192.
Eastwick, P. W., & Finkel, E. J. (2012). The evolutionary armistice: Attachment
bonds moderate the function of ovulatory cycle adaptations. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 174–184.
Ellis, B. J., & Symons, D. (1990). Sexual differences in sexual fantasy: An evolu-
tionary psychological approach. Journal of Sex Research, 27, 527–555.
Feeney, J. A., & Noller, P. (2004). Attachment and sexuality in close relationships.
In J. H. Harvey, A. Wenzel, & S. Sprecher (Eds.), The handbook of sexuality
in close relationships (pp. 183–201). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Feeney, J. A., Noller, P., & Patty, J. (1993). Adolescents’ interactions with the oppo-
site sex: Influence of attachment style and gender. Journal of Adolescence, 16,
169–186.
Feeney, J. A., Peterson, C., Gallois, C., & Terry, D. J. (2000). Attachment style as
a predictor of sexual attitudes and behavior in late adolescence. Psychology
and Health, 14, 1105–1122.
Filippi, S., Vignozzi, L., Vannelli, G. B., Ledda, F., Forti, G., & Maggi, M. (2003).
Role of oxytocin in the ejaculatory process. Journal of Endocrinological
Investigation, 26, 82–86.
Fisher, H. E. (1998). Lust, attraction, and attachment in mammalian reproduction.
Human Nature, 9, 23–52.
Fisher, H. E., Aron, A., Mashek, D., Li, H., & Brown, L. L. (2002). Defining the
brain systems of lust, romantic attraction, and attachment. Archives of Sexual
Behavior, 31, 413–419.
Ford, C. S., & Beach, F. A. (1951). Patterns of sexual behavior. New York: Harper
& Row.
Gagnon, J. H., & Simon, W. (1973). Sexual conduct: The social sources of human
sexuality. Chicago: Aldine.
Gangestad, S. W., & Thornhill, R. (1997). The evolutionary psychology of extra-
pair sex: The role of fluctuating asymmetry. Evolution and Human Behavior,
18, 69–88.
Gentzler, A. L., & Kerns, K. A. (2004). Associations between insecure attachment
and sexual experiences. Personal Relationships, 11, 249–265.
Gillath, O., Mikulincer, M., Birnbaum, G. E., & Shaver, P. R. (2008). When sex
primes love: Subliminal sexual priming motivates relational goal pursuit. Per-
sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 1057–1069.
192 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Gray, P. B., Kahlenberg, S. M., Barrett, E. S., Lipson, S. F., & Ellison, P. T. (2002).
Marriage and fatherhood are associated with lower testosterone in males.
Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 193–201.
Guerrero, L. K. (1998). Attachment-style differences in the experience and expres-
sion of romantic jealousy. Personal Relationships, 5, 273–291.
Hazan, C., & Zeifman, D. (1994). Sex and the psychological tether. In K. Bar-
tholomew & D. Perlman (Eds.), Advances in personal relationships: Vol. 5.
Attachment processes in adulthood (pp. 151–177). London: Jessica Kingsley.
Hazan, C., & Zeifman, D. (1999). Pair bonds as attachments: Evaluating the evi-
dence. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory,
research, and clinical applications (pp. 336–354). New York: Guilford Press.
Hazan, C., Zeifman, D., & Middleton, K. (1994, July). Adult romantic attach-
ment, affection, and sex. Paper presented at the 7th International Conference
on Personal Relationships, Groningen, The Netherlands.
Holmes, B. M., & Johnson, K. R. (2009). Adult attachment and romantic part-
ner preference: A review. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 26,
33–52.
Impett, E. A., Gordon, A. M., & Strachman, A. (2008). Attachment and daily
sexual goals: A study of dating couples. Personal Relationships, 15, 375–
390.
Impett, E. A., & Peplau, L. A. (2002). Why some women consent to unwanted
sex with a dating partner: Insights from attachment theory. Psychology of
Women Quarterly, 26, 359–369.
Kalichman, S. C., Sarwer, D. B., Johnson, J. R., Ali, S. A., Early, J., & Tuten, J. T.
(1993). Sexually coercive behavior and love styles: A replication and exten-
sion. Journal of Psychology and Human Sexuality, 6, 93–106.
Kotler, T. (1985). Security and autonomy within marriage. Human Relations, 38,
299–321.
Little, K. C., McNulty, J. K., & Russell, V. M. (2010). Sex buffers intimates against
the negative implications of attachment insecurity. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 36, 484–498.
Litzinger, S., & Gordon, K. C. (2005). Exploring relationships among communica-
tion, sexual satisfaction, and marital satisfaction. Journal of Sex and Marital
Therapy, 31, 409–424.
Main, M. (1990). Cross-cultural studies of attachment organization: Recent
studies, changing methodologies, and the concept of conditional strategies.
Human Development, 33, 48–61.
Mellen, S. L. W. (1981). The evolution of love. Oxford, UK: Freeman.
Meston, C. M., & Buss, D. M. (2007). Why humans have sex. Archives of Sexual
Behavior, 36, 477–507.
Mikulincer, M., Gillath, O., & Shaver, P. R. (2002). Activation of the attachment
system in adulthood: Threat-related primes increase the accessibility of men-
tal representations of attachment figures. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 83, 881–895.
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2005). Attachment theory and emotions in close
relationships: Exploring the attachment-related dynamics of emotional reac-
tions to relational events. Personal Relationships, 12, 149–168.
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007a). A behavioral systems perspective on the
Convergence of Sexual Urges and Emotional Bonds 193
Stefanou, C., & McCabe, M. P. (2012). Adult attachment and sexual functioning:
A review of past research. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 9, 2499–2507.
Stephan, C. W., & Bachman, G. F. (1999). What’s sex got to do with it?: Attach-
ment, love schemas, and sexuality. Personal Relationships, 6, 111–123.
Sternberg, R. J. (1986). A triangular theory of love. Psychological Review, 93,
119–135.
Tracy, J. L., Shaver, P. R., Albino, A. W., & Cooper, M. L. (2003). Attachment
styles and adolescent sexuality. In P. Florsheim (Ed.), Adolescent romance
and sexual behavior: Theory, research, and practical implications (pp. 137–
159). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Wei, M., Mallinckrodt, B., Larson, L. A., & Zakalik, R. A. (2005). Attachment,
depressive symptoms, and validation from self versus others. Journal of Coun-
seling Psychology, 52, 368–377.
Young, K. A., Gobrogge K. L., Liu, Y., & Wang, Z. X. (2011). The neurobiology
of pair bonding: Insights from a socially monogamous rodent. Frontiers in
Neuroendocrinology, 32, 53–69.
Young, L. J., & Wang, Z. (2004). The neurobiology of pair-bonding. Nature Neu-
roscience, 7, 1048–1054.
8
An Attachment-Theoretical Perspective
on Optimal Dependence
in Close Relationships
Brooke C. Feeney
Meredith Van Vleet
Brittany K. Jakubiak
195
196 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
is a need for it. We view dependence that occurs in the absence of need
as overdependence, and a complete lack of dependence on others (even
in times of need) as underdependence, which has also been referred to
as compulsive self-reliance or defensive self-reliance (Feeney & Collins,
2014). Theoretically, both overdependence and underdependence result
from having attachment figures (or close relationship partners) who are
not accepting of dependence or who do not consistently provide sensitive/
responsive support when needed.
A recent theoretical paper (Feeney & Collins, 2014) describes unre-
sponsive and insensitive support behaviors as undermining thriving because
they promote either overdependence or underdependence. Overdependence
(an overreliance on a significant other to do what can be done by oneself)
represents a means of clinging to a person whose availability and accep-
tance are perceived to be uncertain (e.g., inconsistently responsive support
providers) or who provides support when it is not needed (e.g., compulsively
overinvolved support providers). Underdependence (defensive self-reliance
and lack of dependence on others) represents a means of coping with a sup-
port environment or relationship history in which significant others have
been consistently unresponsive, insensitive to, or unaccepting of depen-
dence needs (e.g., neglectful/disengaged or negative/demeaning support
providers). Optimal dependence (a normative and healthy dependence on
others that occurs in response to genuine need), optimal independence (a
healthy autonomy to pursue opportunities for growth), and optimal inter-
dependence (a relationship in which each member is mutually dependent on
the other) is made possible when relationship partners provide sensitive and
responsive support for both attachment needs and autonomous exploration
(see Feeney & Collins, 2014).
Overdependence, underdependence, and optimal dependence may be
based in one’s relationship history and carried forward into new relation-
ships where the strategy is no longer adaptive, or they may arise from new
experiences in one’s current relationship. We now discuss various types of
support experiences that are likely to underlie each type of dependence. In
doing so, we emphasize the need, with regard to attaining optimal levels
of dependence in relationships, for partners to balance sensitive/responsive
support for attachment needs (safehaven support) and sensitive/responsive
support for autonomous exploration (secure base support). These support
experiences are presumed to have the greatest impact when enacted by
attachment figures with whom one has a strong emotional bond.
of support for both attachment needs (a safe haven) and autonomous explo-
ration behavior (a secure base). A balance of these two support functions
is needed to promote healthy levels of dependence, which should underlie
healthy and optimal human functioning. Figure 8.1 depicts a model of out-
comes related to the receipt of responsive safe haven and secure base sup-
port (elaborated below) that should underlie optimal levels of dependence.
204
Support Recipient’s Recipient’s Recipient’s
Provider’s perception of Immediate Outcomes Long-Term Outcomes
behavior as (Optimal Dependence) (Optimal Dependence)
Responsive supportive •• Reduced anxiety/stress •• Relationship quality/satisfaction
Safe Haven •• Increased well-being •• Psychological health
•• Improved coping capacity •• Physical health
•• Enhanced security •• Felt security of home base
•• Problem resolution •• Reduced reactivity to stressors
•• Positive perceptions of self, partner, and •• Greater coping capacity
relationship •• Resilience
•• Attachment security
•• View of support seeking as beneficial
•• Prosocial caring for others
Secure Base Processes: Response to Exploration Opportunities
FIGURE 8.1. Proposed links between responsive safe haven and secure base behavior and recipient perceptions and outcomes.
Attachment and Optimal Dependence 205
her feel valued, understood, and accepted; Reis & Shaver, 1988) (see also
Feeney & Collins, 2014).
Immediate consequences of receiving responsive safe haven support
that have implications for a healthy dependence on others include reduced
feelings of anxiety or distress; increases in the recipient’s well-being (e.g.,
increases in positive mood, decreases in negative mood, or freeing of cogni-
tive and emotional resources expended on ruminating about a problem);
improved coping capacity; enhanced feelings of safety or security; and
better problem resolution. Additional consequences of receiving respon-
sive safe haven support include positive perceptions of one’s partner and
relationship (e.g., perceptions that the partner is available, and that seek-
ing support and showing vulnerability is beneficial and will be met with
compassion). Responsive safe haven support should also increase feelings
of emotional closeness, trust, and satisfaction with the relationship because
it provides diagnostic information about a partner’s love and concern for
one’s welfare (Collins & Feeney, 2004), and it should also lead recipients to
have positive perceptions of themselves as a result of feeling cared for (Reis
& Shaver, 1988). These immediate outcomes reflect a healthy dependence
on a partner, because they signify the recipient’s and the relationship’s posi-
tive well-being.
These outcomes are supported by studies showing that caring sup-
port from a partner in times of stress has immediate positive effects on
emotional well-being and relationship functioning (e.g., Collins & Feeney,
2000, 2005; Cutrona, 1986; Jaremka, Kane, Guichard, Ford, & Collins,
2010; Kane, McCall, Collins, & Blascovich, 2012; Simpson et al., 1992;
Winstead & Derlega, 1985). These studies indicate a strong link between
receipt of responsive safe haven support and immediate improvements in
mood, relationship satisfaction, feelings of being loved and valued, and
feelings of security. Additional evidence comes from studies showing
that cardiovascular reactivity is buffered in individuals who experience a
stressor in the presence of a close, nonevaluative support provider, rela-
tive to individuals who experience the stressor alone, with a stranger, or
with an evaluative other (e.g., Allen, Blascovich, Tomaka, & Kelsey, 1991;
Edens, Larkin, & Abel, 1992; Fontana, Diegnan, Villeneuve, & Lepore,
1999; Kamarck, Manuck, & Jennings, 1990; Snydersmith & Cacioppo,
1992). Likewise, other studies show that soothing touch or close physical
contact with a close partner during a stressful task decreases heart rate and
blood pressure (e.g., Ditzen et al., 2007; Fishman, Turkheimer, & DeGood,
1995; Grewen, Anderson, Girdler, & Light, 2003; Lynch, Thomas, Paske-
witz, Katcher, & Weir, 1977; Whitcher & Fisher, 1979), and attenuates
neural activation in brain regions associated with emotional and behavioral
responses to threat (Coan, Schaefer, & Davidson, 2006).
These immediate outcomes of receiving responsive safe haven sup-
port should, over many interactions, contribute to long-term tendencies
206 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
212
Support Recipient’s Recipient’s Recipient’s
Provider’s perception of Immediate Outcomes Long-Term Outcomes
behavior as (Overdependence) (Overdependence)
Compulsive/ supportive and •• Attenuated anxiety •• Perceived security of home base
Overinvolved unsupportive •• Felt security •• Perceived benefit of seeking support
Safe Haven (ambivalence) •• Feeling cared for and validated •• Greater dependence on partner
•• Negative view of own ability to cope with •• Poor coping and emotion regulation
and resolve problems •• Intense distress if partner absent
•• Ambivalent perceptions of self, partner, •• Controlling and dominating
and relationship •• Poor relationship quality
FIGURE 8.2. Proposed links between compulsively overinvolved support provider behavior and recipient perceptions and outcomes.
Attachment and Optimal Dependence 213
their spouses viewed the messages as frustrating and insensitive, and they
perceived the spouses as both intrusive/interfering and helpful (provid-
ing further evidence of ambivalent feelings toward their spouses); and (3)
intrusive/interfering support provision during exploration was predictive
of decreases in state self-esteem (Feeney, 2004; Feeney & Thrush, 2010).
These results for adults are consistent with research showing that parental
interference in children’s exploratory activities is associated with negative
outcomes for children, including disrupted concentration, less persistence
and enthusiasm during exploration, more passivity, more negative emotion,
less competence, and less curiosity (e.g., Ainsworth, Bell, & Stayton, 1974;
Cassidy & Berlin, 1994; Egeland & Farber, 1984; Main, 1983; Matas,
Arend, & Sroufe, 1978).
Over time, the recipients of compulsively overinvolved support provi-
sion are likely to become overly dependent on their partners and less will-
ing to engage in autonomous exploration. They may become less willing to
venture away from their secure base (their partners) to pursue autonomous
goals because they have developed fears and concerns about autonomous
exploration, or because they have grown accustomed to having their part-
ners do everything for them. The overdependence on their partners and
resulting lack of exploration may lead to decreases in learning/discovery
(development of competencies), and therefore to decreases in perceived self-
efficacy, perceived competency, confidence in the recipients’ own abilities,
and trait self-esteem. Overdependence and lack of exploration may also
result in decreased psychological health over time (e.g., lower life satisfac-
tion, greater depression) because the recipients are not living up to their full
potential and growing as individuals. The relationship may be character-
ized by increases in conflict over time, especially if the recipients grow more
demanding or feel stifled by their partners. This consistently overinvolved
support pattern may also contribute to the development of codependence
within the relationship, in which the compulsively overinvolved partner
may depend on the recipient to fill his or her need to compulsively care for
others, and the recipient may in turn depend on the partner to do things for
him or her that the recipient can accomplish unaided (see Figure 8.2 for a
summary).
Negative/Demeaning Behavior
Negative and demeaning support provider behavior in safe haven and/or
secure base support contexts should undermine optimal dependence by
creating a defensive or compulsive self-reliance in the recipients. We next
describe processes involving negative/demeaning support provider behavior
that have implications for an unhealthy lack of dependence on others, as
depicted in Figure 8.3.
216 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
217
FIGURE 8.3. Proposed links between negative/demeaning support provider behavior and recipient perceptions and outcomes.
218 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Neglectful/Disengaged Behavior
Neglectful/disengaged support provider behavior in safe haven and/
or secure base support contexts is also likely to hinder one’s ability to
establish a healthy dependence on others, and it is also likely to result
in defensive self-reliance, albeit for different reasons than in the case of
negative/demeaning support provider behavior. We now describe pro-
cesses involving neglectful/disengaged support provider behavior that
have implications for a suboptimal lack of dependence on others, as
depicted in Figure 8.4.
Safe Haven Processes: Response in Times of Adversity
221
FIGURE 8.4. Proposed links between neglectful/disengaged support provider behavior and recipient perceptions and outcomes.
222 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
References
Ainsworth, M. D. (1982). Attachment: Retrospect and prospect. In C. M. Parkes
& J. Stevenson-Hinde (Eds.), The place of attachment in human behavior
(pp. 3–30). New York: Basic Books.
Ainsworth, M. D., Bell, S. M., & Stayton, D. (1974). Infant–mother attachment and
social development: Socialization as a product of reciprocal responsiveness to
226 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Feeney, B. C., & Collins, N. L. (2014). A new look at social support: A theoretical
perspective on thriving through relationships. Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy Review. Advance online publication.
Feeney, B. C., & Lemay, E. P. (2012). Surviving relationship threats: The role of
emotional capital. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 1004–
1017.
Feeney, B. C., & Thrush, R. L. (2010). Relationship influences on exploration in
adulthood: The characteristics and function of a secure base. Journal of Per-
sonality and Social Psychology, 98, 57–76.
Feeney, B. C., & Van Vleet, M. (2010). Growing through attachment: The inter-
play of attachment and exploration in adulthood. Journal of Social and Per-
sonal Relationships, 27, 226–234.
Feeney, J. A. (1996). Attachment, caregiving, and marital satisfaction. Personal
Relationships, 3, 401–416.
Fine, M., & Glendinning, C. (2005). Dependence, independence or inter-depen-
dence?: Revisiting the concepts of “care” and “dependency.” Aging and Soci-
ety, 25, 601–621.
Fincham, F. D., & Bradbury, T. N. (1990). Social support in marriage: The role of
social cognition. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 9, 31–42.
Fishman, E., Turkheimer, E., & DeGood, D. (1995). Touch relieves stress and pain.
Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 18, 69–79.
Fitzsimons, G. M., & Fishbach, A. (2010). Shifting closeness: Interpersonal effects
of personal goal progress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98,
535–549.
Fitzsimons, G. M., & Shah, J. Y. (2008). How goal instrumentality shapes rela-
tionship evaluations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 319–
337.
Fontana, A. M., Diegnan, T., Villeneuve, A., & Lepore, S. J. (1999). Nonevalua-
tive social support reduces cardiovascular reactivity in young women during
acutely stressful performance situations. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 22,
75–91.
Fritz, H. L., Nagurney, A. J., & Helgeson, V. S. (2003). Social interactions and car-
diovascular reactivity during problem disclosure among friends. Personality
and Social Personality Bulletin, 29, 713–725.
Gable, S. L., Gonzaga, G. C., & Strachman, A. (2006). Will you be there for me
when things go right?: Supportive responses to positive event disclosures.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 904–917.
Gable, S. L., Reis, H. T., Impett, E. A., & Asher, E. R. (2004). What do you
do when things go right?: The intrapersonal and interpersonal benefits of
sharing positive events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87,
228–245.
Gore, J. S., & Cross, S. E. (2006). Pursuing goals for us: Relationally autonomous
reasons in long-term goal pursuit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 90, 848–861.
Gottman, J. M., Coan, J., Carrere, S., & Swanson, C. (1998). Predicting marital
happiness and stability from newlywed interactions. Journal of Marriage and
the Family, 60, 5–22.
Gottman, J. M., & Levenson, R. W. (2000). The timing of divorce: Predicting
230 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
when a couple will divorce over a 14-year period. Journal of Marriage and the
Family, 62, 737–745.
Grewen, K., Anderson, B., Girdler, S., & Light, K. C. (2003). Warm partner con-
tact is related to lower cardiovascular reactivity. Behavioral Medicine, 29,
123–130.
Hawley, P. H. (1999). The ontogenesis of social dominance: A strategy-based evo-
lutionary perspective. Developmental Review, 19, 97–132.
Jaremka, L., Kane, H. S., Guichard, A. C., Ford, M. B., & Collins, N. L. (2010).
Perceived partner responsiveness and the development and maintenance of
felt-security in intimate relationships. Unpublished manuscript, University of
California, Santa Barbara.
Kamarck, T. W., Manuck, S. B., & Jennings, J. R. (1990). Social support reduces
cardiovascular reactivity to psychological challenge: A laboratory model. Psy-
chosomatic Medicine, 52, 42–58.
Kane, H. S., Jaremka, L. M., Guichard, A. C., Ford, M. B., Collins, N. L., &
Feeney, B. C. (2007). Feeling supported and feeling secure: How one part-
ner’s attachment style predicts the other partner’s perceived support and
relationship satisfaction. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 24,
535–555.
Kane, H. S., McCall, C., Collins, N. L., & Blascovich, J. (2012). Mere presence is
not enough: Responsive support in a virtual world. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology, 48, 37–44.
Kaplan, M., & Maddux, J. E. (2002). Goals and marital satisfaction: Perceived
support for personal goals and collective efficacy for collective goals. Journal
of Social and Clinical Psychology, 21, 157–164.
Katz, J., Beach, S. R. H., & Anderson, P. (1996). Self-enhancement versus self-veri-
fication: Does spousal support always help? Cognitive Therapy and Research,
20, 345–360.
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Loving, T. J., Stowell, J. R., Malarkey, W. B., Lemeshow, S.,
Dickinson, S. L., et al. (2005). Hostile marital interactions, proinflammatory
cytokine production, and wound healing. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62,
1377–1384.
Kotler, T. (1985). Security and autonomy within marriage. Human Relations, 38,
299–321.
Kunce, L. J., & Shaver, P. R. (1994). An attachment-theoretical approach to care-
giving in romantic relationships. In K. Bartholomew & D. Perlman (Eds.),
Advances in personal relationships (Vol. 5, pp. 205–237). London: Jessica
Kingsley.
La Guardia, J. G., Ryan, R. M., Couchman, C. E., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Within-
person variation in security of attachment: A self-determination theory per-
spective on attachment, need fulfillment, and well-being. Journal of Personal-
ity and Social Psychology, 79, 367–384.
Lakey, B., Tardiff, T. A., & Drew, J. B. (1994). Negative social interactions: Assess-
ment and relations to social support, cognition, and psychological distress.
Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 13, 42–62.
Little, T. D. (1998). Sociocultural influences on the development of children’s
action-control beliefs. In J. Heckhausen & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Motivation
Attachment and Optimal Dependence 231
and self-regulation across the lifespan (pp. 281–315). New York: University
Press.
Little, T. D., Hawley, P. H., Heinrich, C. C., & Marsland, K. (2002). Three views
of the agentic self: A developmental synthesis. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan
(Eds.), Handbook of self-determination research (pp. 389–404). Rochester,
NY: University of Rochester Press.
Lynch, J. J., Thomas, S. A., Paskewitz, D. A., Katcher, A. H., & Weir, L. O. (1977).
Human contact and cardiac arrhythmia in a coronary care unit. Psychoso-
matic Medicine, 39, 188–192.
Main, M. (1983). Exploration, play, and cognitive functioning related to infant–
mother attachment. Infant Behavior and Development, 6, 167–174.
Marvin, R., Cooper, G., Hoffman, K., & Powell, B. (2002). The Circle of Security
Project: Attachment-based intervention with caregiver–preschool child dyads.
Attachment and Human Development, 4, 107–124.
Matas, L., Arend, R., & Sroufe, L. A. (1978). Continuity of adaptation in the
second year: The relationship between quality of attachment and later compe-
tence. Child Development, 49, 547–556.
McEwen, B. S., & Stellar, E. (1993). Stress and the individual: Mechanisms leading
to disease. Archives of Internal Medicine, 153, 2093–2101.
Moore, D. (1987). Parent–adolescent separation: The construction of adulthood by
late adolescents. Developmental Psychology, 23, 298–307.
Noom, M. M., Dekovic, M., & Meeus, W. H. J. (1999). Autonomy, attachment
and psychosocial adjustment during adolescence: A double-edged sword?
Journal of Adolescence, 22, 771–783.
Omodei, M. M., & Wearing, A. J. (1990). Need satisfaction and involvement in
personal projects: Toward an integrative model of subjective well-being. Jour-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 762–769.
Pagel, M. D., Erdly, W. W., & Becker, J. (1987). Social networks: We get by with
(and in spite of) a little help from our friends. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 53, 793–804.
Palys, T. S., & Little, B. R. (1983). Perceived life satisfaction and the organization
of personal project systems. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
44, 1221–1230.
Park, L. E., & Crocker, J. (2005). Interpersonal consequences of seeking self-
esteem. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 1587–1598.
Park, L. E., Crocker, J., & Kiefer, A. K. (2007). Contingencies of self-worth, aca-
demic failure, and goal pursuit. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
33, 1503–1517.
Pierce, G. R., Baldwin, M. W., & Lydon, J. E. (1997). A relational schema approach
to social support. In G. R. Pierce, B. Lakey, I. G. Sarason, & B. R. Sarason
(Eds.), Sourcebook of social support and personality (pp. 19–47). New York:
Plenum Press.
Pressman, S. D., & Cohen, S. (2005). Does positive affect influence health? Psy-
chological Bulletin, 131, 925–971.
Rasmussen, P. R. (2005). The dependent prototype. In P. R. Rasmussen (Ed.),
Personality-guided cognitive-behavioral therapy (pp. 215–234). Washington,
DC: American Psychological Association.
232 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Jason D. Jones
Jude Cassidy
Phillip R. Shaver
234
Adult Attachment Style and Parenting 235
adult attachment (i.e., the AAI and self-reports). The link between attach-
ment styles and parenting was not the main focus of the first attachment
style researchers (Hazan & Shaver, 1987); thus we explain how both the-
ory and subsequent attachment style research provide a basis for expecting
this link. We then review the empirically established associations between
attachment styles and three aspects of parenting—parental behaviors,
emotions, and cognitions—and discuss important factors to consider when
interpreting the findings in this literature. Next, we describe our current
work focusing on the links between attachment styles and caregiving in
parents of adolescents. Finally, we propose directions for future research.
the self, attachment figures, and relationships (see Bretherton & Munhol-
land, 2008, for a review). According to theory, these IWMs serve as tem-
plates for current and future relationships, and as such are one of the mech-
anisms by which early attachment experiences influence later relationships,
including the parent–child relationship. Importantly, individual differences
in the nature and quality of IWMs emerge as a function of the type of care
received from attachment figures. Thus an infant who receives sensitive
and responsive care from an attachment figure will likely form representa-
tions of the self as worthy of care, and of attachment figures as people who
can be relied upon in times of need. On the other hand, an infant whose
attachment-related needs are responded to only inconsistently or in a reject-
ing manner will likely form representations of the self as unworthy of care,
and of attachment figures as unavailable or inconsistently available. These
representations then affect thoughts, feelings, and behaviors in subsequent
relationships, including ones with offspring.
Attachment in Adulthood
During the 1980s, two independent lines of research were initiated to
explore the nature of attachment in adulthood. Main and her colleagues
(George et al., 1984; Main, Kaplan, & Cassidy, 1985) developed an inter-
view procedure, the AAI, to assess current state of mind with respect to
attachment, inferred from the linguistic properties (e.g., coherence) of
responses to questions about early attachment experiences, recent losses,
and current relationships with one’s parents and children. AAI transcripts
are coded in detail and then assigned to one of three primary catego-
ries (secure, dismissing, preoccupied) that parallel the infant categories in
Ainsworth’s Strange Situation procedure (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, &
Wall, 1978; see Hesse, 2008, for a discussion of the AAI categories). Many
studies have found that an adult’s state of mind with respect to attachment
in the AAI is related to his or her child’s attachment classification in the
Strange Situation and to parenting behavior that partially mediates this
connection (see van IJzendoorn, 1995, and Madigan et al., 2006, for meta-
analyses).
Also in the 1980s, two social psychologists (Hazan & Shaver, 1987)
suggested that there are adolescent and adult parallels of Ainsworth’s
infant attachment categories—which they labeled attachment styles—
that influence a person’s experiences and behavior in romantic relation-
ships. They found that these styles could be assessed with self-report
measures, and subsequent psychometric research showed that the three
(or four, according to some researchers) adult attachment styles are bet-
ter captured by dimensional than by categorical measures (Brennan,
Clark, & Shaver, 1998). Following Ainsworth et al.’s (1978) diagram of
238 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
differences and the weak relation between the two kinds of measures, the
AAI and attachment style measures are similarly associated with a variety
of attachment-related constructs such as emotion regulation (Mikulincer
& Shaver, 2007), romantic relationship functioning (e.g., Simpson, Rholes,
Oriña, & Grich, 2002), and social information processing (Dykas & Cas-
sidy, 2011). To date, there has been little effort to integrate what is known
about the use of self-report measures of adult attachment style to predict
cognitive, emotional, and behavioral aspects of parenting. This chapter, as
well as our review paper (Jones et al., 2014), performs that service.
fathers were at higher risk for abuse than secure fathers. Finally, two stud-
ies found that insecure maternal attachment styles were weakly related to
greater use of corporal punishment (i.e., spanking; Berlin et al., 2011; Coyl,
Newland, & Freeman, 2010).
Parental Satisfaction
Six studies on this topic have yielded inconsistent results. Four found that
avoidance was related to lower parental satisfaction (Cohen & Finzi-Dot-
tan, 2005; Cohen et al., 2011; Rholes et al., 2006; Vieira, Ávila, & Matos,
2012), but in one study this effect emerged only for mothers (Cohen &
Adult Attachment Style and Parenting 243
Parental Stress
Eleven studies yielded significant associations between attachment style
and parental stress (Alexander et al., 2001; Fernandes, Muller, & Rodin,
2012; Howard, 2010; Kor, Mikulincer, & Pirutinsky, 2012; Kwako, Noll,
Putnam, & Trickett, 2010; Mills-Koonce et al., 2011; Nygren, Carstensen,
Ludvigsson, & Frostell, 2012; Rholes et al., 2006; Trillingsgaard et al.,
2011; Vasquez, Durik, & Hyde, 2002; Vieira et al., 2012). The majority
found that both avoidance and anxiety were related to greater parenting
stress. Studies that examined the effect of parent gender generally found
no differences (Kor et al., 2012; Nygren et al., 2012; Rholes et al., 2006;
Vasquez et al., 2002).
244 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Adam, Gunnar, & Tanaka, 2004; Cohn, Cowan, Cowan, & Pearson,
1992), but have not devoted much attention to emotions related to specific
aspects of parenting (e.g., stress, desire to have children). It would be inter-
esting to examine how parental states of mind assessed by the AAI relate
to these specific parental feelings and to compare the findings with those in
the attachment style literature.
Given the well-documented link between parental emotions and par-
enting outcomes (Dix, 1991), future research should examine how various
parental emotions mediate and moderate associations between attachment
styles and parenting behavior. In addition, researchers should go beyond
self-reports of parental emotion and include physiological and behavioral
indices of emotion. For example, how do parental attachment styles relate
to physiological responses during interactions with children or in response
to child distress?
Additional Considerations
Across all three broad parenting domains reviewed here, the empirical evi-
dence suggests that self-reported attachment security is associated with
more positive parenting characteristics and outcomes, whereas self-reported
attachment insecurity is associated with more negative parenting character-
istics and outcomes. Thus the conclusion that parents’ self-reported adult
attachment styles can be used profitably to study thoughts, feelings, and
behaviors in parent–child relationships seems to be justified by the available
empirical evidence. However, there are several important factors to consider
if one intends to understand the more nuanced aspects of this literature.
First, although it is true that insecurity is related to more negative par-
enting behaviors, emotions, and cognitions, the literature is less consistent
in terms of how the specific subtypes or dimensions of insecurity relate
to particular parenting outcomes. Attachment-related avoidance and anxi-
ety reflect very different approaches to close relationships; therefore, some
aspects of parenting may be particularly influenced by avoidance rather
than anxiety or vice versa, and this may account for some of the variabil-
ity in findings. For example, most attachment researchers would probably
expect the desire to have children (i.e., the desire for a close relationship
characterized by intense emotion and dependency) to be particularly low
for avoidant individuals, who value their independence and are uncom-
fortable with intimacy, and this is what the literature shows. In contrast,
Adult Attachment Style and Parenting 249
for sex differences in attachment styles (Del Giudice, 2011), which vary
across cultures, and the initial evidence reported in this review for potential
differences in how attachment styles relate to some aspects of parenting as
a function of parent gender, future research should include both mothers
and fathers, and any gender differences should be reported.
A Focus on Adolescence
Our extensive review of the literature on attachment styles and parenting
has revealed that virtually all of the studies to date have focused on parents
of young children or college-age children. Noticeably lacking in this area
of research are studies examining links between attachment styles and par-
enting in samples of parents of adolescents. Thus we have begun a line of
research to address this gap in the literature.
In an initial study of adolescents (mean age = 16 years) and their par-
ents (Jones & Cassidy, 2014), we examined how mothers’ and fathers’
attachment styles related to observed parental secure base provision and
adolescent secure base use in the context of a parent–adolescent conflict
discussion task. We also examined how parents’ attachment styles related
to parent-reported hostile behavior toward their adolescents and to ado-
lescents’ perceptions of their parents, and tested whether these variables
mediated the link between parental attachment styles and secure base use.
Parental attachment styles were related to observed adolescent secure base
use, but not to parental secure base provision; yet the pattern of findings
differed for mothers and fathers. At the bivariate level, maternal avoid-
ance, but not anxiety, was negatively related to adolescent secure base
use. Furthermore, maternal avoidance was indirectly related to less ado-
lescent secure base use through greater mother-reported hostile behavior
toward the adolescents and through adolescents’ negative perceptions of
their mothers (i.e., a composite indicating less secure base availability, less
warmth, less understanding, and more hostility). For fathers, attachment
anxiety, but not avoidance, was indirectly related to less adolescent secure
base use through greater father-reported hostility toward the adolescents.
In a second study with parents and adolescents (mean age = 14 years),
we (Jones, Ehrlich, Lejuez, & Cassidy, 2014) examined how parental
attachment styles related to mothers’, fathers’, and adolescents’ perceptions
of the degree to which parents were knowledgeable about their adolescents’
whereabouts and activities—a variable well known to predict adolescent
risk behavior and delinquency (e.g., Stattin & Kerr, 2000). For both moth-
ers and fathers, attachment anxiety and avoidance were negatively corre-
lated with their own reports of parental knowledge. With regard to ado-
lescents’ reports of parental knowledge, the pattern of results differed for
mothers and fathers: Maternal avoidance (but not anxiety) and paternal
Adult Attachment Style and Parenting 251
Future Directions
Throughout this chapter, we have mentioned many future directions for
this area of research. Below, we suggest several additional avenues for
future research.
One issue that remains to be investigated is whether individual differ-
ences in attachment style relate to differences in physiological and neuro-
biological responses to stimuli relevant to caregiving. Several studies have
found that variation in state of mind in the AAI is related to differences
in neural responses to infant cues, as well as to differences in peripheral
oxytocin response to infant contact (Riem, Bakermans-Kranenburg, van
IJzendoorn, Out, & Rombouts, 2012; Strathearn, 2011; Strathearn, Fon-
agy, Amico, & Montague, 2009). Given that attachment styles are related
to differences in physiological and neuroendocrine responding during
romantic partner interactions (Gouin et al., 2009; Powers, Pietromonaco,
252 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Gunlicks, & Sayer, 2006), it is possible that they also relate to neurobio-
logical responses to one’s children.
Future research should include greater consideration of the role of par-
ents’ romantic relationship quality in regard to the links between attach-
ment styles and parenting. It has long been recognized that marital relation-
ships both influence and are influenced by parent–child relationships (e.g.,
Belsky, 1981). Given substantial evidence for strong associations between
adult attachment styles and romantic relationship quality (Mikulincer &
Shaver, 2007), as well as evidence for links between marital quality and the
quality of parent–child relationships (see Erel & Burman, 1995, for a meta-
analysis), future research should examine the additive and interactive influ-
ences of attachment styles and marital quality on parenting (see Rholes et
al., 1995, Study 1, for some initial evidence).
Future research should examine not only how parental attachment
styles relate to parenting, but also how the experience of parenthood may
change parents’ attachment styles. The transition to parenthood is a major
life event that likely leads individuals to reflect upon, reevaluate, and pos-
sibly change their orientation toward close relationships (Bowlby, 1988;
Simpson, Rholes, Campbell, & Wilson, 2003). Furthermore, researchers
should consider how characteristics of the parents, of the parents’ romantic
relationship, and of the child relate to changes in parents’ attachment styles
across the transition to parenthood.
Another issue is experimentation. Much of the attachment style litera-
ture is based on studies in which contexts, stressors, subliminal security
or insecurity primes, and experimental tasks for couples are manipulated,
and the interaction of the manipulations and attachment styles is assessed.
Similar research techniques could be used in studies of parent–child rela-
tionships.
Finally, research on adult attachment and parenting would benefit
greatly from increased collaboration among researchers from the social
and developmental attachment research traditions. The modest empirical
association between the AAI and self-report attachment style measures
indicates that one measure is not simply a substitute for the other. Yet both
seem to be reliably associated with various facets of parenting. Therefore,
researchers should strive to include both the AAI and self-report mea-
sures of attachment style in their studies. It should be particularly easy for
researchers already administering the AAI to add a brief self-report attach-
ment style measure to their studies.
Acknowledgments
Preparation of this chapter was supported by awards to Jason D. Jones from the
National Institute on Drug Abuse (No. F31 DA033848), and to Phillip R. Shaver
from the Fetzer Institute.
Adult Attachment Style and Parenting 253
References
Abaied, J. L., & Rudolph, K. D. (2010). Contributions of maternal adult attach-
ment to socialization of coping. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships,
27, 637–657.
Adam, E. K., Gunnar, M. R., & Tanaka, A. (2004). Adult attachment, parent
emotion, and observed parenting behavior: Mediator and moderator models.
Child Development, 75, 110–122.
Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1989). Attachments beyond infancy. American Psychologist,
44, 709–716.
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of
attachment: A psychological study of the Strange Situation. Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Alexander, R., Feeney, J., Hohaus, L., & Noller, P. (2001). Attachment style and
coping resources as predictors of coping strategies in the transition to parent-
hood. Personal Relationships, 8, 137–152.
Belsky, J. (1981). Early human experience: A family perspective. Developmental
Psychology, 17, 3–23.
Berant, E., Mikulincer, M., & Florian, V. (2001a). The association of mothers’
attachment style and their psychological reactions to the diagnosis of infant’s
congenital heart disease. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 20, 208–
232.
Berant, E., Mikulincer, M., & Florian, V. (2001b). Attachment style and mental
health: A 1-year follow-up study of mothers of infants with congenital heart
disease. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 956–968.
Berlin, L. J., Whiteside-Mansell, L., Roggman, L. A., Green, B. L., Robinson, J., &
Spieker, S. (2011). Testing maternal depression and attachment style as mod-
erators of Early Head Start’s effects on parenting. Attachment and Human
Development, 13, 49–67.
Bowlby, J. (1969/1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York:
Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1979). The making and breaking of affectional bonds. London: Tavis-
tock.
Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Parent–child attachment and healthy human
development. New York: Basic Books.
Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measurement of
adult romantic attachment: An integrative overview. In J. A. Simpson & W.
S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 46–76). New
York: Guilford Press.
Bretherton, I., & Munholland, K. A. (2008). Internal working models in attach-
ment relationships: Elaborating a central construct in attachment theory. In
J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research,
and clinical applications (2nd ed., pp. 102–130). New York: Guilford Press.
Caldwell, J. G., Shaver, P. R., Li, C., & Minzenberg, M. J. (2011). Childhood mal-
treatment, adult attachment, and depression as predictors of parental self-effi-
cacy in at-risk mothers. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment and Trauma,
20, 595–616.
Cassidy, J. (1999). The nature of the child’s ties. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver
254 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Fernandes, C., Muller, R., & Rodin, G. (2012). Predictors of parenting stress in
patients with haematological cancer. Journal of Psychosocial Oncology, 30,
81–96.
Finzi-Dottan, R., Cohen, O., Iwaniec, D., Sapir, Y., & Weizman, A. (2006). The
child in the family of a drug-using father: Attachment styles and family char-
acteristics. Journal of Social Work Practice in the Addictions, 6, 89–111.
Fraley, R. C., Heffernan, M. E., Vicary, A. M., & Brumbaugh, C. C. (2011). The
Experiences in Close Relationships—Relationship Structures Questionnaire:
A method for assessing attachment orientations across relationships. Psycho-
logical Assessment, 23, 615–625.
George, C., Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1984). Adult Attachment Interview proto-
col. Unpublished manuscript, University of California at Berkeley.
George, C., Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1996). Adult Attachment Interview proto-
col (3rd ed.). Unpublished manuscript, University of California at Berkeley.
George, C., & Solomon, J. (2008). The caregiving system: A behavioral systems
approach to parenting. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of
attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (2nd ed., pp. 833–
856). New York: Guilford Press.
Goodman, G. S., Quas, J. A., Batterman-Faunce, J. M., Riddlesberger, M. M.,
& Kuhn, J. (1997). Children’s reactions to and memory for a stressful event:
Influence of age, anatomical dolls, knowledge, and parental attachment.
Applied Developmental Science, 1, 54–75.
Gouin, J., Glaser, R., Loving, T. J., Malarkey, W. B., Stowell, J., Houts, C., et
al. (2009). Attachment avoidance predicts inflammatory responses to marital
conflict. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 23, 898–904.
Green, B. L., Furrer, C., & McAllister, C. (2007). How do relationships support
parenting?: Effects of attachment style and social support on parenting behav-
ior in an at-risk population. American Journal of Community Psychology,
40, 96–108.
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. R. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment
process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 511–524.
Hesse, E. (2008). The Adult Attachment Interview: Protocol, method of analy-
sis, and empirical studies. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of
attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (2nd ed., pp. 552–
598). New York: Guilford Press.
Howard, K. S. (2010). Paternal attachment, parenting beliefs and children’s attach-
ment. Early Child Development and Care, 180, 157–171.
Jones, J. D., Brett, B. E., Ehrlich, K. B., Lejuez, C. W., & Cassidy, J. (2014). Mater-
nal attachment style and responses to adolescents’ negative emotions: The
mediating role of maternal emotion regulation. Parenting: Science and Prac-
tice. Advance online publication.
Jones, J. D., & Cassidy, J. (2014). Parental attachment style: Examination of links
with parent secure base provision and adolescent secure base use. Attachment
and Human Development, 16, 437–461.
Jones, J. D., Cassidy, J., & Shaver, P. R. (2014). Parents’ self-reported attachment
styles: A review of links with parenting behaviors, emotions, and cognitions.
Personality and Social Psychology Review. Advance online publication.
Jones, J. D., Ehrlich, K. B., Lejuez, C. W., & Cassidy, J. (2014). Parental knowledge
256 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Meredith, P., & Noller, P. (2003). Attachment and infant difficultness in postnatal
depression. Journal of Family Issues, 24, 668–686.
Mikulincer, M., & Florian, V. (1998). The relationship between adult attach-
ment styles and emotional and cognitive reactions to stressful events. In J. A.
Simpson & W. S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships
(pp. 143–165). New York: Guilford Press.
Mikulincer, M., & Florian, V. (1999a). The association between spouses’ self-
reports of attachment styles and representations of family dynamics. Family
Process, 38, 69–83.
Mikulincer, M., & Florian, V. (1999b). The association between parental reports
of attachment style and family dynamics, and offspring’s reports of adult
attachment style. Family Process, 38, 243–257.
Mikulincer, M., & Florian, V. (1999c). Maternal–fetal bonding, coping strategies,
and mental health during pregnancy: The contribution of attachment style.
Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 18, 255–276.
Mikulincer, M., Gillath, O., Halevy, V., Avihou, N., Avidan, S., & Eshkoli, N.
(2001). Attachment theory and reactions to others’ needs: Evidence that acti-
vation of the sense of attachment security promotes empathic responses. Jour-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 1205–1224.
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). Attachment in adulthood: Structure,
dynamics, and change. New York: Guilford Press.
Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P. R., Gillath, O., & Nitzberg, R. A. (2005). Attach-
ment, caregiving, and altruism: Boosting attachment security increases
compassion and helping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89,
817–839.
Millings, A., Walsh, J., Hepper, E., & O’Brien, M. (2013). Good partner, good
parent: Responsiveness mediates the link between romantic attachment and
parenting style. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39, 170–180.
Mills-Koonce, W., Appleyard, K., Barnett, M., Deng, M., Putallaz, M., & Cox,
M. (2011). Adult attachment style and stress as risk factors for early maternal
sensitivity and negativity. Infant Mental Health Journal, 32, 277–285.
Moncher, F. J. (1996). The relationship of maternal adult attachment style and risk
of physical child abuse. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 11, 335–350.
Nathanson, A. I., & Manohar, U. (2012). Attachment, working models of parent-
ing, and expectations for using television in childrearing. Family Relations:
An Interdisciplinary Journal of Applied Family Studies, 61, 441–454.
Nygren, M., Carstensen, J., Ludvigsson, J., & Frostell, A. (2012). Adult attach-
ment and parenting stress among parents of toddlers. Journal of Reproduc-
tive and Infant Psychology, 30, 289–302.
Oppenheim, D., & Koren-Karie, N. (2009). Infant–parent relationship assessment:
Parents’ insightfulness regarding their young children’s internal worlds. In C.
R. Zeanah (Ed.), Handbook of infant mental health (3rd ed., pp. 266–280).
New York: Guilford Press.
Pesonen, A., Räikkönen, K., Keltikangas-Järvinen, L., Strandberg, T., & Järven-
pää, A. (2003). Parental perception of infant temperament: Does parents’
joint attachment matter? Infant Behavior and Development, 26, 167–182.
Pesonen, A., Räikkönen, K., Strandberg, T., Kelitikangas-Järvinen, L., & Järv-
enpää, A. (2004). Insecure adult attachment style and depressive symptoms:
258 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Selcuk, E., Günaydin, G., Sumer, N., Harma, M., Salman, S., Hazan, C., et al.
(2010). Self-reported romantic attachment style predicts everyday maternal
caregiving behavior at home. Journal of Research in Personality, 44, 544–
549.
Shaver, P. R., & Mikulincer, M. (2002). Attachment-related psychodynamics.
Attachment and Human Development, 4, 133–161.
Shaver, P. R., Mikulincer, M., Lavy, S., & Cassidy, J. (2009). Understanding and
altering hurt feelings: An attachment-theoretical perspective on the genera-
tion and regulation of emotions. In A. L. Vangelisti (Ed.), Feeling hurt in close
relationships (pp. 92–122). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Simpson, J. A., Rholes, W. S., Campbell, L., & Wilson, C. L. (2003). Changes
in attachment orientations across the transitions to parenthood. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 317–331.
Simpson, J. A., Rholes, W. S., & Nelligan, J. S. (1992). Support seeking and support
giving within couples in an anxiety-provoking situation: The role of attach-
ment styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 434–446.
Simpson, J. A., Rholes, W. S., Oriña, M., & Grich, J. (2002). Working models of
attachment, support giving, and support seeking in a stressful situation. Per-
sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 598–608.
Simpson, J. A., Rholes, W. S., & Phillips, D. (1996). Conflict in close relationships:
An attachment perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71,
899–914.
Slade, A. (2005). Parental reflective functioning: An introduction. Attachment and
Human Development, 7, 269–281.
Snell, W. R., Overbey, G. A., & Brewer, A. (2005). Parenting perfectionism and the
parenting role. Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 613–624.
Solomon, J., & George, C. (1996). Defining the caregiving system: Toward a the-
ory of caregiving. Infant Mental Health Journal, 17, 183–197.
Stattin, H., & Kerr, M. (2000). Parental monitoring: A reinterpretation. Child
Development, 71, 1072–1085.
Stevenson-Hinde, J. (1994). An ethological perspective. Psychological Inquiry, 5,
62–65.
Strathearn, L. (2011). Maternal neglect: Oxytocin, dopamine and the neurobiol-
ogy of attachment. Journal of Neuroendocrinology, 23, 1054–1065.
Strathearn, L., Fonagy, P., Amico, J., & Montague, P. (2009). Adult attachment
predicts maternal brain and oxytocin response to infant cues. Neuropsycho-
pharmacology, 34, 2655–2666.
Trillingsgaard, T., Elklit, A., Shevlin, M., & Maimburg, R. D. (2011). Adult
attachment at the transition to motherhood: Predicting worry, health care
utility and relationship functioning. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psy-
chology, 29, 354–363.
van IJzendoorn, M. (1995). Adult attachment representations, parental responsive-
ness, and infant attachment: A meta-analysis on the predictive validity of the
Adult Attachment Interview. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 387–403.
Vasquez, K., Durik, A. M., & Hyde, J. (2002). Family and work: Implications
of adult attachment styles. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28,
874–886.
260 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Vieira, J., Ávila, M., & Matos, P. (2012). Attachment and parenting: The mediat-
ing role of work–family balance in Portuguese parents of preschool children.
Family Relations: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Applied Family Studies,
61, 31–50.
Wilson, C. L., Rholes, W. S., Simpson, J. A., & Tran, S. (2007). Labor, delivery,
and early parenthood: An attachment theory perspective. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 505–518.
10
Attachment Theory
in Organizational Settings
Ramona L. Paetzold
261
262 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
proposed and measured by Smith, Murphy, and Coats (1999). Work set-
tings or entire organizations can serve as groups, which can vary in size,
cohesion, and entitativity. Smith et al. (1999) demonstrated, using their
Social Group Attachment Scale, that group-level attachment style is dis-
tinct from romantic relationship attachment style and from group identifi-
cation. Thus a combination of the two types of attachment style could be
useful for understanding attachment theory’s role in organizations.
not often worry about being left in the lurch at work”; sample items on the
IAW subscale are “I often worry that people will not want to stay on my
work team” and “I get nervous when anyone at work confides too much.”
Thus it appears that each dimension of the AAW combines elements of
anxiety (i.e., worrying about rejection/abandonment) and avoidance (i.e.,
handling issues of physical/psychological closeness), posing problems for
interpretation.
One often-used self-report scale is substantially different from others
in the organizational literature. The Self-Reliance Inventory (SRI; Quick,
Joplin, Nelson, & Quick, 1992) appears to have been developed at about
the same time as early dimensional scales in the social-psychological litera-
ture. It generates three factors: Counterdependence (dismissing), Overde-
pendence (preoccupied), and Interdependence (secure) (Joplin, Nelson, &
Quick, 1999; Little, Nelson, Wallace, & Johnson, 2011). The SRI contains
items such as “Needing someone is a sign of weakness” (Counterdepen-
dence), “My desire to be close to my coworkers sometimes scares them
away” (Overdependence), and “I can usually take care of my own work but
I don’t mind getting help if I need it” (Interdependence) (Little et al., 2011).
Although an early version of the SRI has been validated and recommended
for use by organizational researchers (Hinkin, 1995), a question remains
as to how this scale can be compared to two-dimensional scales focusing
on anxiety and avoidance, where security represents being low in either or
both.
Throughout this chapter, the ECR (either the short or the long form)
has been used to measure attachment style unless another measure is des-
ignated.
1 It should be noted that because Daus and Joplin (1999) were apparently unaware of
Smith et al.’s (1999) work, they did not consider a group-based measure of attachment
orientation, but averaged individual group members’ scores to represent a group-level
attachment style.
Attachment Theory in Organizational Settings 267
attachment style and changes in the soldiers’ mental health levels. After
only 2 months, leader avoidance was directly related to follower mental
health, which was lower for insecure followers than for those who were
more secure. Followers who were more secure managed to maintain a
higher level of mental health after the 2-month period, regardless of the
insecurity of their leader. This buffering of the followers’ own attachment
style appeared to disappear after 4 months, however, so that negative
effects of the leader’s avoidance were present for all soldiers, regardless of
their own attachment style.
The relationship between leaders and followers has also been studied
through the lens of leader–member exchange (LMX), which focuses on
the process of dyadic relationship development via role and social exchange
theories (Wayne, Shore, & Linden, 1997). For example, Towler and Stuhl-
macher (2013), in a study of career-oriented working women, found that
although anxiety was not related to the quality of employee relationships
with supervisors (via LMX), avoidance was negatively related to LMX
quality; these results support the notion that more avoidant persons do
not make investments in their relationships with supervisors, perhaps as a
means of maintaining distance and autonomy. In addition, having good-
quality relationships with supervisors (for employees low in avoidance)
was associated with less work conflict. However, Towler and Stuhlmacher
(2013) did not consider the role of supervisor attachment style in determin-
ing LMX quality.
Richards and Hackett (2012)—using the ERS to measure both super-
visor and subordinate attachment styles in a sample obtained through the
StudyResponse project (Stanton & Weiss, 2002), and using the actor–part-
ner independence model (APIM) for data analysis—found that LMX qual-
ity perceptions were negatively related to both anxiety and avoidance in an
actor (i.e., there was a lack of trust, there was too much clinginess in the
relationship, and individuation was hindered), but there were no partner
effects. However, low levels of anxiety in actors and partners interacted
to predict higher perceived levels of LMX quality, with quality deteriorat-
ing as a function of the difference between levels of anxiety in actors and
partners (leaders and their subordinates). Thus there was support for a con-
gruence effect for anxiety in LMX quality (Keller, 2003), but because there
was no significant interaction for avoidance, congruence was not supported
for that attachment style.
Richards and Hackett (2012) also found that cognitive and behavioral
strategies such as reappraisal and suppression moderated the connection
between attachment styles and LMX quality, due to either reducing the
emotional impact of a situation (appraisal) or changing behavioral response
to a felt emotion (suppression). A higher quality of LMX was found in
dyads with highly anxious partners, when both supervisors and subor-
dinates used reappraisal to reframe threats that triggered the attachment
270 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Work Engagement
Work engagement is often represented by two constructs: vigor at work
and dedication (Schaufeli, Salanova, González-Romá, & Bakker, 2002).
Little et al. (2011) have defined vigor as a “positive affective state in a work
Attachment Theory in Organizational Settings 273
context that combines elements of an emotion and a mood state” (p. 467),
and that provides physical and emotional energy as well as mental alertness
for performing the job. Dedication, on the other hand, represents a sense of
purpose and pride at work (Moss, 2009).
Using the AAQ, Moss (2009) found that vigor and dedication were
both inversely correlated with anxiety and avoidance. In a more specialized
study of vigor based on the SRI, Little et al. (2011) found that security was
positively related to vigor, and, like Moss (2009), found that both counter-
dependence and overdependence were negatively related to vigor at work.
They interpreted these results as being consistent with the characteristics
of anxiety and avoidance. For example, counterdependent workers may
engage in defense mechanisms of denial or repression to avoid activation of
their attachment systems, which in turn may deplete their resource avail-
ability for work. Overdependent individuals may be in a state of hyperac-
tivation, with heightened levels of distress and rumination that leave few
resources available for vigor at work. In total, these results indicate that
employees higher in attachment security may be less engaged (or more dis-
engaged) from work, which is consistent with the findings on job burnout
(which may be loosely interpreted as a form of work disengagement).
Job Satisfaction
Studies have tended to show that more insecure workers tend to be dis-
satisfied with at least some aspects of their jobs (e.g., lack of recognition,
coworkers, overall job) (Hardy & Barkham, 1994; Hazan & Shaver, 1990;
Ronen & Mikulincer, 2012; Sumer & Knight, 2001). Both moderators
and mediators have been investigated to explain the link between attach-
ment insecurity and job dissatisfaction. In a survey study of employees,
Schirmer and Lopez (2001), who incorporated both the RQ and the ECR
into their research, found that avoidance interacted with supervisory sup-
port to predict levels of job satisfaction. In particular, under low levels of
support, workers who were more highly avoidant (ECR) reported higher
job satisfaction than did those who were lower in avoidance, but under
high levels of support, the group means were the same. 3 Towler and Stuhl-
macher (2013), in their study of working women, found that the association
between avoidance and job dissatisfaction was fully mediated by LMX.4
Avoidance was negatively related to LMX, which was positively related
to job satisfaction, such that more secure women reported higher levels of
job satisfaction as a result of having higher-quality relationships with their
3 TheRQ, however, did not detect differences in job satisfaction among preoccupied,
secure, and dismissing/avoidant workers.
4 Anxietywas not related to any of the mediators, and thus not to job satisfaction in the
mediation model.
274 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
supervisors. These two studies indicate the role that supervisory support
and the quality of supervisory relationships can play in determining job
satisfaction. More avoidant individuals are more satisfied without much
support from supervisors (an interaction effect), but because they tend to
have poor-quality relationships with their supervisors (as viewed through
an LMX lens), they tend to be less satisfied with their jobs (a mediation
effect). In other words, highly avoidant employees tend to prefer that their
supervisors allow them to work unaided, but relational leadership, as pro-
vided through LMX, does not permit that outcome.
Managerial/supervisory attachment styles are also related to subordi-
nate–employee job satisfaction levels. Ronen and Mikulincer (2012) found
that a manager’s anxiety was negatively related to his or her subordinates’
job satisfaction level, and that the hyperactivated caregiving associated
with the level of managerial anxiety accounted for the lowered job satisfac-
tion of the subordinates. Managers’ avoidance bore no connection to the
job satisfaction of their subordinates, however, and no dyadic interactions
of attachment styles predicted the job satisfaction levels of subordinates.
Work–Family Issues
Work–family balance (or work–family conflict) continues to be a major
area of organizational research, with models of the work–family interface
becoming increasingly complex over time (Ferguson, Carlson, Zivnuska, &
Whitten, 2012). Research in this area largely supports a spillover model,
indicating that work experiences flow into family life (and vice versa) (Kin-
nunen, Feldt, Geurts, & Pulkkinen, 2006). Crossover effects based on
workplace support for not only the employee, but also the spouse/part-
ner and children, can also be important in work–family issues (Ransford,
Crouter, & McHale, 2008).
Attachment research related to work–family issues is in the early stages,
with most of the focus on just an employee’s attachment style, and without
sufficient consideration of a partner’s roles or the presence of children. For
example, Sumer and Knight (2001), using the RQ, reported support for the
spillover model based on a measure they developed to assess work–family
linkage (the Work–Family Linkage Questionnaire). On this measure, pre-
occupied employees reported higher levels of negative spillover from home
to work than did secure or dismissing/avoidant employees (with fearful/
avoidant employees not differing from secure or dismissing/avoidant ones).
Secure employees were more likely to experience positive spillover from
family to work than were any of the other three groups. They were also
more likely to have positive spillover from work to family (compared to
dismissing and fearful employees), but they did not differ from preoccupied
employees on this dimension. In addition, preoccupied employees were less
likely than all other groups to experience segmentation (independence of
Attachment Theory in Organizational Settings 275
work and family lives), which indicates that such workers may use both life
aspects as a means of getting their safety and other needs met.
Towler and Stuhlmacher (2013) found that having higher-quality home
relationships (e.g., higher satisfaction with the partner/spouse, greater rela-
tionship cohesion)—which was related to higher levels of attachment secu-
rity—was positively related to a woman’s job satisfaction and lower levels
of conflict in the workplace, such that being more secure was associated
with more positive family–work spillover.
The most complex study to date was completed by Vasquez, Durik,
and Hyde (2002), who conducted a longitudinal study to examine the
experience of parenthood on work functioning. Using the RQ, they inves-
tigated mothers and fathers separately to determine how attachment styles
related to variables such as marital rewards, parental stress, working hours,
depression, and job satisfaction. At 1 year postpartum, secure and dismiss-
ing/avoidant mothers reported the least stress and greatest rewards from
their family relationships, whereas fearful/avoidant mothers reported the
most stress and fewest rewards. Depression was highest among fearful/
avoidant mothers. There were no differences across the attachment style
groups with regard to work functioning, however, so no spillover effects
related to attachment styles were found. Among men, secure fathers tended
to view family salience and parental role quality more positively and as less
stressful than did fearful/avoidant fathers. Both preoccupied and fearful/
avoidant fathers were more depressed than secure or dismissing/avoidant
fathers. Work functioning varied across the attachment style groups, with
secure fathers reporting fewer work-related concerns than either preoccu-
pied or fearful/avoidant fathers (i.e., less negative family-work spillover).
Secure fathers also reported less role overload than fearful/avoidant fathers
(i.e., they reported better work–family balance).
Results were somewhat different at 4.5 years postpartum. At this fol-
low-up, secure and dismissing/avoidant mothers were less stressed about
their families and felt more rewarded in family relationships than fearful/
avoidant mothers, just as at 1 year postpartum. Also, fearful/avoidant
mothers at this stage reported fewer work rewards than did the other three
attachment types, indicating greater negative family–work spillover for
them. Differences between fearful/avoidant mothers and the other groups
persisted with regard to depression, but the other three groups did not dif-
fer among themselves. For men, differences in family functioning were still
present at 4.5 years postpartum, with fearful/avoidant fathers reporting
the most stress and less family salience, and with secure fathers report-
ing the least stress and more family salience. There was no difference in
depression levels among the four attachment style groups. With regard to
work, however, preoccupied and fearful/avoidant fathers showed higher
work-related concerns (more negative family–work spillover) and more role
overload (less balance) than secure and dismissing/avoidant fathers did.
276 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Support Seeking
Again, little research has explicitly examined support-seeking behavior in
the workplace, although work difficulties might be expected to activate
the attachment system, even for individuals with lower levels of chronic
Attachment Theory in Organizational Settings 277
Prosocial Behaviors
The construct most commonly used to measure prosocial behavior (support
providing behavior) in the workplace is that of organizational citizenship
behaviors (OCBs). OCBs are behaviors intended to help in the “maintenance
and enhancement of the social and psychological context that supports
task performance” (Organ, 1997, p. 91). These behaviors, therefore, are
not task behaviors, but are generally viewed as discretionary and support-
ive of the organizational context (Motowidlo, 2000). OCBs are sometimes
operationalized as two dimensions: OCB-I, which measures prosocial or
supportive behaviors directed toward individuals within the organization,
and OCB-O, which measures prosocial behaviors geared toward helping
the organization (Coleman & Borman, 2000). Other researchers, however,
sometimes use a five-dimensional measure for OCBs representing altruism,
courtesy, sportsmanship, civic virtue, and conscientiousness (Niehoff &
Moorman, 1993). Because these behaviors are generally viewed as volun-
tary, persons higher in attachment security should engage in more of them
(Erez, Mikulincer, van IJzendoorn, & Kroonenberg, 2008).
A study by Desivilya, Sabag, and Ashton (2006) involving undergradu-
ate employees and their reports of their OCBs found that both higher levels
of anxiety and higher levels of avoidance were both associated with lower
self-reports of OCBs. To explain this connection, the authors examined
perceptions of interactional justice (a measure of organizational justice that
assesses an employee’s perceptions of fairness during interactions with his
or her supervisor) as a potential moderator, but these perceptions did not
moderate any of these relations. Richards and Schat (2011), using the ERS,
found that OCB-O was negatively related to anxiety. This result was also
supported by Little et al. (2011), using the SRI, who found that having an
278 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Antisocial Behavior
In the workplace, behaviors are sometimes viewed as counter to the best
interests of the organization. Various labels have been attached to these
behaviors: They are commonly termed counterproductive work behaviors
(CWBs), antisocial work behavior, workplace incivility, or workplace devi-
ance (Berry, Ones, & Sackett, 2007; Hershcovis, 2011). These constructs
differ slightly in operationalization, but they all convey that the behaviors
are undesirable and antithetical to the norms and goals of the organiza-
tion. The attachment-style-related results are so far mixed. Richards and
Schat (2011), using the ERS, found that anxiety was positively related, and
avoidance was negatively related, to CWBs. Little et al. (2011), using the
SRI, found instead that counterdependence positively predicted workplace
deviance, with the relation being fully mediated by vigor at work (which
was negatively related to deviance). They found no effects for either overde-
pendence or interdependence (security).
References
Barrick, M. R., & Mount, M. K. (1991). The Big Five personality dimensions and
job performance: A meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 44, 1–26.
Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among young
adults: A test of a four-category model. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 61, 226–244.
Berry, C. M., Ones, D. S., & Sackett, P. R. (2007). Interpersonal deviance, organi-
zational deviance, and their common correlates: A review and meta-analysis.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 410–424.
Berson, Y., Dan, O., & Yammarino, F. J. (2006). Attachment style and individual
differences in leadership perceptions and emergence. Journal of Social Psy-
chology, 146, 165–182.
Boatwright, K. J., Lopez, F. G., Sauer, E. M., VanDerWege, A., & Huber, D. M.
(2010). The influence of adult attachment styles on workers’ preferences for
relational leadership behaviors. Psychologist–Manager Journal, 13, 1–14.
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation: Anxiety and anger.
New York: Basic Books.
Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measurement of
adult romantic attachment: An integrative overview. In J. A. Simpson & W.
S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 46–76). New
York: Guilford Press.
Christiansen, N., & Tett, R. (Eds.). (2013). Handbook of personality at work.
New York: Routledge.
Coleman, V. I., & Borman, W. C. (2000). Investigating the underlying structure of
the citizenship performance domain. Human Resource Management Review,
10, 25–44.
Collins, N. L., & Read, S. J. (1990). Adult attachment, working models, and
Attachment Theory in Organizational Settings 283
Schaufeli, W., Leiter, M. P., Maslach, C., & Jackson, S. E. (1996). MBI-General
Survey. In C. Maslach, S. E. Jackson, & M. P. Leiter (Eds.), Maslach Burnout
Inventory manual (3rd ed., pp. 6–42). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psycholo-
gists Press.
Schaufeli, W. B., Salanova, M., González-Romá, V., & Bakker, A. B. (2002). The
measurement of engagement and burnout: A two sample confirmatory factor
analytic approach. Journal of Happiness Studies, 3, 71–92.
Schirmer, L. L., & Lopez, F. G. (2001). Probing the social support and work strain
relationship among adult workers: Contributions of adult attachment orienta-
tions. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 59, 17–33.
Shalit, A., Popper, M., & Zakay, D. (2010). Followers’ attachment styles and their
preference for social or for personal charismatic leaders. Leadership and
Organization Development Journal, 31, 458–472.
Simmons, B. L., Gooty, J., Nelson, D., & Little, L. M. (2009). Secure attachment:
Implications for hope, trust, burnout, and performance. Journal of Organiza-
tional Behavior, 30, 233–247.
Simpson, J. A. (1990). Influence of attachment styles on romantic relationships.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 971–980.
Simpson, J. A., Rholes, W. S., & Phillips, D. (1996). Conflict in close relationships:
An attachment perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71,
899–914.
Smith, E. R., Murphy, J., & Coats, S. (1999). Attachment to groups: Theory and
measurement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 94–110.
Stanton, J. M., & Weiss, E. M. (2002). Online panels for social science research:
An introduction to the StudyResponse project (Syracuse University, School
of Information Studies, Tech. Rep. No. 13001). Retrieved July 5, 2013, from
www.studyresponse.net/TechRpt13001.pdf.
Sumer, H. C., & Knight, P. A. (2001). How do people with different attachment
styles balance work and family?: A personality perspective on work–family
linkage. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 653–663.
Towler, A. (2005). Charismatic leadership development: Role of parental attach-
ment style and parental psychological control. Journal of Leadership and
Organizational Studies, 11, 15–25.
Towler, A. J., & Stuhlmacher, A. F. (2013). Attachment styles, relationship sat-
isfaction, and well-being in working women. Journal of Social Psychology,
153, 297–298.
Vasquez, K., Durik, A. M., & Hyde, J. S. (2002). Family and work: Implications
of adult attachment styles. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28,
874–886.
Wayne, S. J., Shore, L. M., & Linden, R. C. (1997). Perceived organizational sup-
port and leader–member exchange: A social exchange perspective. Academy
of Management Journal, 40, 82–111.
11
Health and Attachment Processes
Paula R. Pietromonaco
Cassandra C. DeVito
Fiona Ge
Jana Lembke
287
288 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
and health-related biological responses, health behavior, and health and dis-
ease outcomes. Throughout our review, we evaluate how attachment pro-
cesses across the lifespan—from childhood through adulthood—contribute
to health-related outcomes. Finally, we discuss several emerging themes, as
well as directions for future research that will enhance our understanding of
the mechanisms linking attachment processes and health.
PARTNER A PARTNER B
HPA Responses
A key physiological system subject to the influence of attachment is the
HPA axis. Upon activation by a stressor, the HPA axis governs the release
of cortisol into the bloodstream. Although this response prepares the body
292 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Mangelsdorf, Parritz, & Buss, 1996; Spangler & Schieche, 1998). Still other
work has examined attachment in early childhood and later patterns of
stress reactivity in adolescence among individuals at risk for asthma; in this
sample, insecure attachment in early childhood predicted flatter patterns
of cortisol reactivity over the course of a standard laboratory stressor, sug-
gesting dysregulated HPA responses (Kelsay, Leung, Mrazek, & Klinnert,
2013). Conversely, attachment security may have a health-protective effect.
One study found that attachment security buffered infants from elevations
in cortisol when the infants were facing threatening stimuli, such as receiv-
ing an immunization (Gunnar et al., 1996). Taken together, the research
indicates that childhood attachment insecurity is linked to greater dysregula-
tion in physiological responses to stress. It is possible that these dysregulated
physiological patterns, such as the frequency and magnitude of HPA activa-
tion, may serve as precursors to childhood health and disease problems.
SAM Responses
Attachment also may influence health outcomes through the SAM sys-
tem, which includes the autonomic nervous system (Diamond & Fagundes,
2010). As with HPA axis responses, dysregulated reactivity in the SAM
Health and Attachment Processes 295
Immune Responses
Immune system functioning is another biomarker of health outcomes that
can fluctuate depending on the quality of relationship experiences, which
are closely associated with attachment style. The literature examining
attachment and immune responses is in its infancy, with only a few studies
examining these processes in early childhood or adulthood.
Health Behavior
Attachment orientations are strongly tied to the strategies people use to
regulate their thoughts, feelings, and behavior (see Mikulincer & Shaver,
2007, and Pietromonaco & Beck, 2015); as a result, attachment security or
insecurity should predict the extent to which individuals engage in preven-
tive health behaviors or behaviors that increase health risks. Despite the
importance of this issue, relatively few studies have examined the extent to
which attachment in either childhood or adulthood predicts specific health
behaviors. Figure 11.1 illustrates that self-regulation, affect regulation, and
relationship processes may be implicated in the link between attachment
and health behavior, but this idea has yet to be directly tested.
Physical Health
All of the factors discussed so far may ultimately contribute to physical
health and disease risks and outcomes, as illustrated in Figure 11.1. We
now discuss the research linking attachment in childhood and adulthood
to known health risks and health conditions.
experiences shape health in later life (e.g., Boyce, 1985), few studies have
examined this question by using prospective, longitudinal designs. A recent
landmark study, however, used a prospective design to follow individuals
from infancy to age 32, providing the most compelling evidence yet for a
connection between attachment in childhood and health outcomes in adult-
hood (Puig, Englund, Simpson, & Collins, 2013). In this study, childhood
attachment was assessed when participants were 12 and 18 months of age
with an objective measure: the infants’ behavior (coded by trained observ-
ers) in response to separation and reunion with their mothers in the Strange
Situation. Physical health was assessed many years later, when partici-
pants were 32 years old. Remarkably, individuals’ infant attachment clas-
sifications predicted their physical health outcomes in middle adulthood,
even after potentially related variables (e.g., life stress, negative emotion,
body mass index) were taken into account. Specifically, individuals whose
attachments had been classified as anxious-resistant in infancy were more
likely to report physical illness as adults 32 years later than were individu-
als who had been classified as securely attached in infancy. Adults whose
attachments had previously been classified as anxious-avoidant or anxious-
resistant were more likely to report having an inflammation-related illness
at age 32 than their secure peers. In addition, participants classified as inse-
curely attached at both times (12 months and 18 months) during infancy
were more likely to report having a physical illness, inflammation-related
illness, and nonspecific symptoms at age 32 than participants classified as
insecurely attached at one time or as securely attached at both times during
infancy.
The Puig et al. (2013) study also examined whether several factors
mediated the link between early attachment and later health outcomes—
including variables that our model (Figure 11.1) suggests may be impor-
tant mechanisms, such as the role of childhood emotion regulation ability,
interpersonal competence with peers, and emotional health. None of the
variables tested significantly mediated the attachment–health link. As Puig
et al. note, however, the small percentage of participants with a physical
illness may have made it difficult to detect mediation.
This prospective research suggests that the quality of early parent–child
relationships is an important antecedent of later physical health. Additional
prospective longitudinal studies are needed for replicating this effect and
assessing its generality across samples. The infants in the Puig et al. (2013)
study were originally from low-SES backgrounds; such individuals carry
a greater risk for adverse health outcomes in adulthood, a process that
may occur in part through alterations beginning in childhood in the body’s
physiological stress response systems (Miller, Lachman, et al., 2011). Thus
it will be important for additional prospective studies to determine whether
the link between childhood attachment and later health outcomes is inten-
sified by early adverse circumstances (e.g., low SES), or whether a similar
Health and Attachment Processes 303
link emerges among individuals who have not experienced early childhood
adversity. Furthermore, research in this area will be advanced by assessing
potential physiological pathways (Miller, Chen, & Parker, 2011), as well
as further examining possible behavioral and emotional mechanisms (e.g.,
learned strategies for regulating emotion) through which early experiences
shape downstream health outcomes. This knowledge will be vital for devel-
oping early intervention programs to target processes that are implicated
in the link between early experience and later health and disease outcomes.
Physical Symptoms
Anxiously attached individuals, who show greater affective reactivity
(Pietromonaco, Barrett, & Powers, 2006; Pietromonaco & Barrett, 1997),
also may be more sensitive to physical distress and pain. Consistent with this
idea, individuals characterized by anxious attachment report more physical
and somatic symptoms than individuals with other forms of attachment do
(Feeney & Ryan, 1994; Kidd & Sheffield, 2005; Ciechanowski, Walker,
Katon, & Russo, 2002).
Most of the studies have examined adult attachment and reported
physical symptoms at the same point in time, making it difficult to know
whether attachment style influences symptom reporting, or whether expe-
riencing unpleasant physical symptoms creates conditions that lead to
attachment insecurity. For example, people who are in pain may find it
difficult to engage in positive interactions with others, which over time may
increase their attachment insecurity.
Studies of experimentally induced pain suggest that attachment anxi-
ety is associated not only with anxiously attached individuals’ reports
of pain experienced in their daily lives, but also with their reactions to
induced pain. Individuals higher in attachment anxiety show more intense
reactions to experimentally induced pain, including a lower pain threshold,
greater catastrophizing about the pain (e.g., ruminating about it, feeling
overwhelmed by it), and greater perceptions of pain (Meredith, Strong, &
Feeney, 2006; Wilson & Ruben, 2011), although some work has not found
this association (Andrews, Meredith, & Strong, 2011).
The link between attachment avoidance and experimentally induced
pain is less clear. Some work has found greater pain tolerance among avoid-
antly attached individuals exposed to an acute laboratory pain induction
304 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
(Andrews et al., 2011; Wilson & Ruben, 2011). Other work, however, has
found that both attachment anxiety and avoidance predict pain intensity.
In a diary study of women with chronic pain, those high in attachment
anxiety reported greater pain intensity and showed more pain catastroph-
izing on days when they reported more intense pain; more avoidant women
also reported greater pain intensity and catastrophizing, but they were less
likely to cope by relying on others on days when they showed higher cata-
strophizing (Kratz, Davis, & Zautra, 2012).
These findings suggest that for women with chronic pain, both attach-
ment anxiety and avoidance are associated with perceptions of pain and
coping strategies, especially on days that are more difficult (the pain is
more intense, or worries about the pain are high). Findings vary somewhat
across studies, possibly because the extent to which either attachment anxi-
ety, avoidance, or both are associated with pain responses may depend on
the context (e.g., an acute laboratory stressor or chronic pain), the nature of
the sample (e.g., individuals with or without chronic pain, age of sample),
and other contextual variables (e.g., the extent to which the pain sufferers
feel supported or rejected) (Andrews et al., 2011; MacDonald, 2008). For
example, anxiously attached individuals who were reminded of rejection
evidenced a lower pain threshold than anxiously attached individuals in
a control condition did (MacDonald, 2008), suggesting that social pain
may trigger greater sensitivity to physical pain among those with anxious
attachments.
Another symptom associated with attachment is difficulty sleeping,
which is linked to greater health risks such as contracting the common cold
(Cohen, Doyle, Alper, Janicki-Deverts, & Turner, 2009), poorer immune
function (Prather et al., 2012), and metabolic and cardiovascular diseases
(Grandner, Jackson, Pak, & Gehrman, 2012). Attachment anxiety in adults
may be relevant for sleep quality, because an individual’s worries about
the relationship and partner (e.g., concerns about closeness, rejection) may
become salient when the individual attempts to fall asleep (Carmichael &
Reis, 2005). In line with this reasoning, one study of married couples found
that attachment anxiety predicted difficulties with sleeping for husbands
and wives, even after the researchers controlled for individuals’ symptoms
of depression, which also are linked to sleep problems (Carmichael & Reis,
2005). Similarly, research using a diverse sample of older adults (ages 60–85
years) found that individuals who were preoccupied with attachment (high
in anxiety and low in avoidance) were more likely to use medication to help
them sleep and more likely to take naps during the daytime, possibly as a
side effect of the medications or because they had trouble sleeping at night
(Verdecias, Jean-Louis, Zizi, Casimir, & Browne, 2009).
Other work has found a link between anxious attachment and objec-
tive sleep quality, but, surprisingly, not subjective sleep quality (Troxel,
Cyranowski, Hall, Frank, & Buysse, 2007; Troxel & Germain, 2011). In
Health and Attachment Processes 305
this work, anxiously attached women with major depression showed poorer
sleep quality on objective measures: They showed the lowest percentage of
sleep during stages 3 and 4 (deep sleep), especially if they had lost a spouse
through divorce, separation, or death (Troxel et al., 2007). Similarly, mili-
tary veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder who were more anxiously
attached showed less sleep in stages 3 and 4 (Troxel & Germain, 2011).
Overall, the findings consistently show that attachment anxiety is
associated with sleep disturbances, whereas few studies have shown a simi-
lar link between avoidance and sleep disturbances (Maunder, Hunter, &
Lancee, 2011). Importantly, the link between relationship functioning and
sleep disruptions appears to be bidirectional (Hasler & Troxel, 2010). As
a result, individuals who are anxiously attached may experience a vicious
cycle in which attachment-related worries lead to poorer sleep quality, and
being fatigued may increase the likelihood of problematic interactions with
their partners, which can then interfere with subsequent sleep. This idea
fits with findings from a diary study showing that more anxiously attached
couple members reported greater sleep problems on mornings following
days of greater conflict with their partners, and fewer sleep problems on
mornings following days of lower conflict (Hicks & Diamond, 2011). In
contrast, individuals higher in avoidance showed a weaker association
between experiencing conflict on the previous day and sleep problems the
next morning than did those low in avoidance, perhaps because highly
avoidant individuals are better able to suppress potentially disturbing
thoughts about conflict.
affect health behavior change. Recent work highlights the role of dyadic
processes in predicting patients’ outcomes: Anxiously attached patients
with Alzheimer’s disease reported more physical and psychological symp-
toms, especially when their spouses/caregivers were also anxiously attached
(Monin, Schulz, & Kershaw, 2013).
Another important set of questions concern the extent to which attach-
ment patterns in childhood versus adulthood contribute to health outcomes
via the same or different pathways. For example, little is known about the
stability of health-related physiological responses from childhood to adult-
hood. To what extent are stability between childhood and adult attach-
ment, and the physiological correlates of each, able to account for the links
between adult attachment and health outcomes? And to what extent does
attachment at different points in development uniquely predict such out-
comes? In addition, experiences in early childhood may be more likely than
those in adulthood to alter underlying physiological stress response systems
(e.g., see Schore, 2001). If so, then early experiences may be more potent
predictors of later physical health.
Finally, it will be important to place the connection between attach-
ment and health within the larger context of other kinds of relationships,
such as those with health care providers. For example, insecurely attached
patients often desire close, supportive relationships with their physi-
cians (Noyes et al., 2003) and trust their physicians less (Holwerda et al.,
2013). Such perceptions may contribute to patients’ disease outcomes. For
instance, patients with diabetes and with dismissing/avoidant attachments
who reported lower-quality communications with their provider showed
poorer metabolic control (Ciechanowski, Katon, Russo, & Walker, 2001).
Research incorporating the perspectives of both health care providers and
patients, as well as spouses or close others who may be involved in the care
process, will inform the development of tailored interventions that take
into account which kinds of communication strategies promote health for
which patients.
Conclusions
Attachment patterns from childhood through adulthood are associated
with a range of health-related outcomes, including physiological stress
responses, health behavior, and health and disease conditions. Although
research in this area is growing, many questions remain about how attach-
ment patterns translate into later health and disease outcomes. We hope
that our model illustrating potential pathways underlying the attachment–
health link, as well as our recommendations for future research, will serve
as a roadmap to guide the next generation of studies.
310 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Acknowledgment
Preparation of this chapter was facilitated by a grant from the National Cancer
Institute of the National Institutes of Health (Grant No. R01CA133908) to Paula
R. Pietromonaco.
References
Ahrens, K. R., Ciechanowski, P. S., & Katon, W. (2012). Associations between
adult attachment style and health risk behaviors in an adult female primary
care population. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 72(5), 364–370.
Anderson, S. E., Gooze, R. A., Lemeshow, S., & Whitaker, R. C. (2012). Quality
of early maternal–child relationship and risk of adolescent obesity. Pediatrics,
129(1), 132–140.
Anderson, S. E., & Whitaker, R. C. (2011). Attachment security and obesity in
US preschool-aged children. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine,
165(3), 235–242.
Andrews, N. E., Meredith, P. J., & Strong, J. (2011). Adult attachment and reports
of pain in experimentally-induced pain. European Journal of Pain, 15(5),
523–530.
Antoni, M. H., Lutgendorf, S. K., Cole, S. W., Dhabhar, F. S., Sephton, S. E.,
McDonald, P. G., et al. (2006). The influence of bio-behavioural factors on
tumour biology: Pathways and mechanisms. Nature Reviews Cancer, 6(3),
240–248.
August, K. J., Rook, K. S., Franks, M. M., & Stephens, M. A. P. (2013). Spouses’
involvement in their partners’ diabetes management: Associations with
spouse stress and perceived marital quality. Journal of Family Psychology,
27(5), 712–721.
Beck, L. A., Pietromonaco, P. R., DeBuse, C. J., Powers, S. I., & Sayer, A. G.
(2013). Spouses’ attachment pairings predict neuroendocrine, behavioral, and
psychological responses to marital conflict. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 105(3), 388–424.
Beck, L. A., Pietromonaco, P. R., DeVito, C. C., Powers, S. I., & Boyle, A. M.
(2014). Congruence between spouses’ perceptions and observers’ ratings of
responsiveness: The role of attachment avoidance. Personality and Social Psy-
chology Bulletin, 40(2), 164–174.
Ben-Naim, S., Hirschberger, G., Ein-Dor, T., & Mikulincer, M. (2013). An experi-
mental study of emotion regulation during relationship conflict interactions:
The moderating role of attachment orientations. Emotion, 13(3), 506–519.
Blair, C., & Raver, C. C. (2012). Child development in the context of adversity:
Experiential canalization of brain and behavior. American Psychologist,
67(4), 309–318.
Boyce, W. T. (1985). Social support, family relations, and children. In S. Cohen &
S. L. Syme (Eds.), Social support and health (pp. 151–173). San Diego, CA:
Academic Press.
Branstetter, S. A., Furman, W., & Cottrell, L. (2009). The influence of representa-
tions of attachment, maternal–adolescent relationship quality, and maternal
Health and Attachment Processes 311
Crosby, R., DiClemente, R., Wingood, G., Cobb, B., Harrington, K., Davies, S., et
al. (2001). HIV/STD-protective benefits of living with mothers in perceived
supportive families: A study of high-risk African American female teens. Pre-
ventive Medicine, 33(3), 175–178.
Diamond, L. M., & Fagundes, C. P. (2010). Psychobiological research on attach-
ment. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 27(2), 218–225.
Diamond, L. M., Hicks, A. M., & Otter-Henderson, K. D. (2008). Every time you
go away: Changes in affect, behavior, and physiology associated with travel-
related separations from romantic partners. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 95(2), 385–403.
Dowd, J. B., Palermo, T. M., & Aiello, A. E. (2012). Family poverty is associated
with cytomegalovirus antibody titers in U.S. children. Health Psychology,
31(1), 5–10.
Dowd, J. B., Zajacova, A., & Aiello, A. E. (2010). Predictors of inflammation
in U.S. children aged 3–16 years. American Journal of Preventive Medicine,
39(4), 314–320.
Drake, K., Belsky, J., & Fearon, R. M. P. (2014). From early attachment to engage-
ment with learning in school: The role of self-regulation and persistence.
Developmental Psychology, 50(5), 1350–1361.
Emerson, E., Donenberg, G. R., & Wilson, H. W. (2012). Health-protective effects
of attachment among African American girls in psychiatric care. Journal of
Family Psychology, 26(1), 124–132.
Fagundes, C. P., Glaser, R., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K. (2013). Stressful early life expe-
riences and immune dysregulation across the lifespan. Brain, Behavior, and
Immunity, 27, 8–12.
Feeney, B. C., & Kirkpatrick, L. A. (1996). Effects of adult attachment and pres-
ence of romantic partners on physiological responses to stress. Journal of Per-
sonality and Social Psychology, 70(2), 255–270.
Feeney, J. A., Peterson, C., Gallois, C., & Terry, D. (2000). Attachment style as a
predictor of sexual attitudes and behavior in late adolescence. Psychology and
Health, 14(6), 1105–1122.
Feeney, J. A., & Ryan, S. M. (1994). Attachment style and affect regulation: Rela-
tionships with health behavior and family experiences of illness in a student
sample. Health Psychology, 13(4), 334–345.
Felitti, V. J., Anda, R. F., Nordenberg, D., Williamson, D. F., Spitz, A. M., Edwards,
V., et al. (1998). Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to
many of the leading causes of death in adults: The Adverse Childhood Experi-
ences (ACE) Study. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 14(4), 245–258.
Foshee, V., & Bauman, K. E. (1994). Parental attachment and adolescent cigarette
smoking initiation. Journal of Adolescent Research, 9(1), 88–104.
Fraley, R. C., & Brumbaugh, C. C. (2004). A dynamical systems approach to con-
ceptualizing and studying stability and change in attachment security. In W.
S. Rholes & J. A. Simpson (Eds.), Adult attachment: Theory, research, and
clinical implications (pp. 86–132). New York: Guilford Press.
Frigerio, A., Ceppi, E., Rusconi, M., Giorda, R., Raggi, M. E., & Fearon, P. (2009).
The role played by the interaction between genetic factors and attachment in
the stress response in infancy. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
50(12), 1513–1522.
Health and Attachment Processes 313
Kerns, K. A., Klepac, L., & Cole, A. (1996). Peer relationships and preadolescents’
perceptions of security in the child–mother relationship. Developmental Psy-
chology, 32(3), 457–466.
Kidd, T., & Sheffield, D. (2005). Attachment style and symptom reporting: Exam-
ining the mediating effects of anger and social support. British Journal of
Health Psychology, 10(4), 531–541.
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Malarkey, W. B., Chee, M., Newton, T., Cacioppo, J. T.,
Mao, H. Y., et al. (1993). Negative behavior during marital conflict is associ-
ated with immunological down-regulation. Psychosomatic Medicine, 55(5),
395–409.
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Newton, T. L. (2001). Marriage and health: His and hers.
Psychological Bulletin, 127(4), 472–503.
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Newton, T., Cacioppo, J. T., MacCallum, R. C., Glaser, R.,
& Malarkey, W. B. (1996). Marital conflict and endocrine function: Are men
really more physiologically affected than women? Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 64(2), 324–332.
Kochanska, G., Philibert, R. A., & Barry, R. A. (2009). Interplay of genes and
early mother–child relationship in the development of self-regulation from
toddler to preschool age. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(11),
1331–1338.
Kratz, A. L., Davis, M. C., & Zautra, A. J. (2012). Attachment predicts daily
catastrophizing and social coping in women with pain. Health Psychology,
31(3), 278–285.
Laws, H., Sayer, A. G., Pietromonaco, P. R., & Powers, S. I. (2014). Longitudinal
changes in spouses’ cortisol response patterns: Physiological convergence in
newlywed couples. Manuscript under review.
Lehman, B. J., Taylor, S. E., Kiefe, C. I., & Seeman, T. E. (2005). Relation of
childhood socioeconomic status and family environment to adult metabolic
functioning in the CARDIA study. Psychosomatic Medicine, 67(6), 846–854.
Lewis, M. A., & Butterfield, R. M. (2007). Social control in marital relationships:
Effect of one’s partner on health behaviors. Journal of Applied Social Psy-
chology, 37(2), 298–319.
Luecken, L. J. (1998). Childhood attachment and loss experiences affect adult car-
diovascular and cortisol function. Psychosomatic Medicine, 60(6), 765–772.
Luster, T., & Small, S. A. (1994). Factors associated with sexual risk-taking behav-
iors among adolescents. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 56(3), 622–632.
MacDonald, G. (2008). Use of pain threshold reports to satisfy social needs. Pain
Research and Management, 13(4), 309–319.
Mangelsdorf, S., Gunnar, M., Kestenbaum, R., Lang, S., & Andreas, D. (1990).
Infant proneness-to-distress temperament, maternal personality, and mother–
infant attachment: Associations and goodness of fit. Child Development,
61(3), 820–831.
Maunder, R. G., & Hunter, J. J. (2001). Attachment and psychosomatic medicine:
Developmental contributions to stress and disease. Psychosomatic Medicine,
63(4), 556–567.
Maunder, R. G., & Hunter, J. J. (2008). Attachment relationships as determinants
of physical health. Journal of the American Academy of Psychoanalysis and
Dynamic Psychiatry, 36(1), 11–32.
Health and Attachment Processes 315
Maunder, R. G., Hunter, J. J., & Lancee, W. J. (2011). The impact of attach-
ment insecurity and sleep disturbance on symptoms and sick days in hospi-
tal-based health-care workers. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 70(1),
11–17.
McWilliams, L., & Bailey, S. (2010). Associations between adult attachment rat-
ings and health conditions: Evidence from the National Comorbidity Survey
Replication. Health Psychology, 29(4), 446–453.
Meredith, P. J., Strong, J., & Feeney, J. A. (2006). The relationship of adult attach-
ment to emotion, catastrophizing, control, threshold and tolerance, in exper-
imentally-induced pain. Pain, 120(1–2), 44–52.
Mickelson, K. D., Kessler, R. C., & Shaver, P. R. (1997). Adult attachment in a
nationally representative sample. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 73, 1092–1106.
Mikulincer, M. (1998). Adult attachment style and individual differences in func-
tional versus dysfunctional experiences of anger. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 74(2), 513–524.
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). Attachment in adulthood: Structure,
dynamics, and change. New York: Guilford Press.
Miller, A. H. (2010). Depression and immunity: A role for T cells? Brain, Behavior,
and Immunity, 24(1), 1–8.
Miller, G. E., & Chen, E. (2010). Harsh family climate in early life presages the
emergence of a proinflammatory phenotype in adolescence. Psychological
Science, 21(6), 848–856.
Miller, G. E., Chen, E., & Parker, K. J. (2011). Psychological stress in childhood
and susceptibility to the chronic diseases of aging: Moving toward a model
of behavioral and biological mechanisms. Psychological Bulletin, 137(6),
959–997.
Miller, G. E., Chen, E., & Zhou, E. S. (2007). If it goes up, must it come down?:
Chronic stress and the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical axis in
humans. Psychological Bulletin, 133(1), 25–45.
Miller, G. E., & Cole, S. W. (2012). Clustering of depression and inflammation in
adolescents previously exposed to childhood adversity. Biological Psychiatry,
72(1), 34–40.
Miller, G. E., Lachman, M. E., Chen, E., Gruenewald, T. L., Karlamangla, A. S.,
& Seeman, T. E. (2011). Pathways to resilience: Maternal nurturance as a buf-
fer against the effects of childhood poverty on metabolic syndrome at midlife.
Psychological Science, 22(12), 1591–1599.
Monin, J. K., Schulz, R., & Kershaw, T. S. (2013). Caregiving spouses’ attachment
orientations and the physical and psychological health of individuals with
Alzheimer’s disease. Aging and Mental Health, 17(4), 508–516.
Moore, M. R., & Chase-Lansdale, P. L. (2001). Sexual intercourse and pregnancy
among African American girls in high-poverty neighborhoods: The role of
family and perceived community environment. Journal of Marriage and Fam-
ily, 63(4), 1146–1157.
Mrazek, D. A., Casey, B., & Anderson, I. (1987). Insecure attachment in severely
asthmatic preschool children: Is it a risk factor? Journal of the American
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26, 516–520.
Nachmias, M., Gunnar, M., Mangelsdorf, S., Parritz, R. H., & Buss, K. (1996).
316 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Robles, T. F., Slatcher, R. B., Trombello, J. M., & McGinn, M. M. (2014). Marital
quality and health: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 140(1),
140–187.
Rosenberg, T., & Shields, C. (2009). The role of parent–adolescent attachment in
the glycemic control of adolescents with type 1 diabetes: A pilot study. Fami-
lies, Systems and Health: Journal of Collaborative Family HealthCare, 27(3),
237–248.
Salvatore, J. E., Kuo, S. I.-C., Steele, R. D., Simpson, J. A., & Collins, W. A. (2011).
Recovering from conflict in romantic relationships: A developmental perspec-
tive. Psychological Science, 22(3), 376–383.
Saxbe, D., & Repetti, R. L. (2010). For better or worse?: Coregulation of couples’
cortisol levels and mood states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
98(1), 92–103.
Sbarra, D. A., & Hazan, C. (2008). Coregulation, dysregulation, self-regulation:
An integrative analysis and empirical agenda for understanding adult attach-
ment, separation, loss, and recovery. Personality and Social Psychology
Review, 12(2), 141–167.
Schore, A. N. (2001). Effects of a secure attachment relationship on right brain
development, affect regulation, and infant mental health. Infant Mental
Health Journal, 22(1–2), 7–66.
Shonkoff, J., Boyce, W., & McEwen, B. (2009). Neuroscience, molecular biology,
and the childhood roots of health disparities: Building a new framework for
health promotion and disease prevention. Journal of the American Medical
Association, 301(21), 2252–2259.
Simpson, J. A., Rholes, W. S., Campbell, L., Tran, S., & Wilson, C. L. (2003).
Adult attachment, the transition to parenthood, and depressive symptoms.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(6), 1172–1187.
Simpson, J. A., Rholes, W. S., & Phillips, D. (1996). Conflict in close relationships:
An attachment perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
71(5), 899–914.
Simpson, J. A., Winterheld, H. A., Rholes, W. S., & Oriña, M. M. (2007). Work-
ing models of attachment and reactions to different forms of caregiving from
romantic partners. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(3), 466–
477.
Slopen, N., Koenen, K. C., & Kubzansky, L. D. (2012). Childhood adversity and
immune and inflammatory biomarkers associated with cardiovascular risk in
youth: A systematic review. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 26(2), 239–250.
Spangler, G., & Grossmann, K. E. (1993). Biobehavioral organization in securely
and insecurely attached infants. Child Development, 64(5), 1439–1450.
Spangler, G., & Schieche, M. (1998). Emotional and adrenocortical responses of
infants to the strange situation: The differential function of emotional expres-
sion. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 22(4), 681–706.
Stephens, M. A. P., Franks, M. M., Rook, K. S., Iida, M., Hemphill, R. C., &
Salem, J. K. (2013). Spouses’ attempts to regulate day-to-day dietary adher-
ence among patients with type 2 diabetes. Health Psychology, 32(10), 1029–
1037.
Steptoe, A., & Kivimäki, M. (2013). Stress and cardiovascular disease: An update
on current knowledge. Annual Review of Public Health, 34, 337–354.
318 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Gery C. Karantzas
Jeffry A. Simpson
A pproximately 524 million people are over the age of 65, and by
the year 2050, this figure will rise to 1.5 billion (National Institute on
Aging [NIA], 2011; World Health Organization [WHO], 2012). Further-
more, about 80% of people within this age group report having at least
one chronic health condition (National Center for Chronic Disease Pre-
vention and Health Promotion, 2011). Because of these trends, the aging
of the population has introduced new challenges for couples and families
regarding the care of ailing older adults. No longer is aged care the pri-
mary responsibility of the state or government. Instead, family members
(whether these are aging adults’ spouses/partners, or grown children and
their partners) are increasingly becoming the primary carers of aging
adults, as federal and state governments in most parts of the globe struggle
to meet the health care demands of their aging societies (Karantzas, Evans,
& Foddy, 2010; NIA, 2011; WHO, 2012). In the coming decades, caring
for an older adult is likely to become a normal life task for many—and
perhaps most—adult children and their spouses. Caring for an older per-
son, however, is a highly stressful and challenging responsibility, even for
family members; it involves coming to terms with the eventual decline and
ultimate loss of someone who often has been a primary source of love,
comfort, and support across a carer’s entire life. From this perspective,
attachment theory (Bowlby, 1969/1982) provides a particularly useful and
powerful framework for understanding the processes of caregiving and
care receiving, as well as the mental health outcomes experienced by both
carers and care recipients. Because it is a lifespan theory of development,
319
320 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
state in which a person feels safe and protected. When this state is compro-
mised (by experiencing a stressful event or being exposed to a threatening
situation), most individuals try to seek out their attachment figures in order
to reestablish felt security (these efforts are termed proximity seeking). In
childhood, felt security is achieved by engaging in rather direct proximity-
seeking behaviors, such as when an upset child maintains close physical
distance to his or her parent/guardian (e.g., Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, &
Wall, 1978; Vaughn, Egeland, Sroufe, & Waters, 1979).
Compared to young children, adults do not require direct or frequent
physical contact with their parents in order to feel secure and safe in threat-
ening situations. Rather, adults can achieve a sense of comfort and security
by simply thinking about (symbolically representing) their parents—spe-
cifically, through generating thoughts or memories of closeness, internal-
ized shared values, goals, or common interests with their parents (Cicire-
lli, 1993). As Koski and Shaver (1997) point out, “availability becomes
more abstract and no longer requires constant, immediate physical pres-
ence” (p. 29). This symbolic representation of contact can be periodically
reinforced by direct communication with parents during visits, or via tele-
phone calls or other forms of contact. These behaviors can be conceptual-
ized as an extension of an infant’s original working models of his or her
parents. Attachment during adulthood, in other words, does not always
require actual physical proximity, because felt security can be achieved by
stimulated closeness via thoughts, fantasies, and imagery of parents (Cici-
relli, 1991, 1993; Shaver & Mikulincer, 2004). This symbolic aspect of
attachment has been supported by Troll and Smith (1976), who found that
strong familial attachments between older parents and their adult children
are often maintained, regardless of their contact frequency or proximity
maintenance. Therefore, positive recollections of a parent as an attachment
figure, coupled with phone calls, e-mails, and other nonphysical contact,
can sustain the parent as a secure base and safe haven, even when a child
becomes an adult.
Cicirelli (1998) suggests that in later life, a powerful attachment threat
for an adult child is the current or imminent ill health of a parent. When
parents become ill, most adult children will engage in proximity seeking
manifested in caregiving actions, which Cicirelli has termed protective
behavior. Protective behavior is designed to preserve or restore the exis-
tence of the threatened attachment figure (Bowlby, 1979, 1980; Cicirelli,
1983, 1985). As the vulnerability of a child’s attachment bond with a par-
ent becomes salient due to the onset of age-related illnesses, the adult child
may become motivated to protect the parent, especially if the parent con-
tinues to be an important source of emotional security. Through caregiv-
ing and other forms of helping behavior, the adult child should attempt
to delay the eventual loss of the parent for as long as possible. To put this
another way, the parent’s ill health and potential dependency pose a threat
322 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Behavioral Systems
Implicit in much of the work linking attachment to aged care is the inter-
play between different behavioral systems—namely, the attachment behav-
ioral system in relation to the caregiving system. According to Bowlby
(1969/1982), the caregiving system is complementary to the attachment
system, in that it motivates an individual to offer assistance, comfort, and
support when another person is distressed and needs help. As such, the goal
of the caregiving system is to respond to another individual’s need for felt
security by providing sensitive and responsive care (Canterberry & Gillath,
2012; Gillath, Shaver, & Mikulincer, 2005). The caregiving system is acti-
vated when an individual detects that another is distressed or needs help,
and is deactivated when the care recipient’s need is met or his or her sense
of security is reestablished (Canterberry & Gillath, 2012; Gillath et al.,
2005). To date, a considerable amount of early childhood and adult attach-
ment research has examined the associations between these two behavioral
systems. For example, research on adult attachment orientations and the
experimental enhancement of people’s sense of security by priming attach-
ment security have provided important insights into the dynamic interplay
of these two systems (for reviews, see Canterberry & Gillath, 2012; George
& Solomon, 2008; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007a, 2007b).
Recently, Canterberry and Gillath (2012) have proposed a model of
caregiving system activation and dynamics that directly maps onto Miku-
lincer and Shaver’s (2003, 2007a) model of attachment system dynamics.
The Canterberry and Gillath model provides an organizational framework
that articulates the role that certain individual differences play in the func-
tioning of the caregiving system, and how they align with other individual
differences associated with the functioning of the attachment system. Spe-
cifically, the model posits that people can engage in one of three broad care-
giving strategies: (1) sensitive and responsive caregiving, (2) hyperactivating
caregiving, or (3) deactivating caregiving. Sensitive and responsive caregiv-
ing strategies reflect caregiving that is delivered in an appropriate manner
and that meets the care recipient’s specific needs. Hyperactivating care-
giving strategies entail caregiving behaviors that are intrusive, compulsive,
Attachment and Aged Care 323
and persistent in nature and are delivered in a way that usually intensifies
the care recipient’s distress or fails to meet the person’s needs. Deactivat-
ing strategies refer to caregiving that is distant, minimal, and lacking in
emotional content. Sensitive and responsive caregiving tends to be enacted
by securely attached individuals. Hyperactivating caregiving strategies are
typically enacted by anxiously attached individuals, who rely on hyperac-
tivating attachment strategies to regulate their emotions when they are dis-
tressed (i.e., strategies in which distress and proximity-seeking efforts are
intensified). Deactivating caregiving strategies are displayed by avoidantly
attached individuals, who use deactivating attachment strategies when they
become upset (i.e., strategies in which distress is minimized and proximity
seeking is inhibited).
Despite the utility of the Canterberry and Gillath (2012) model and
research that has attempted to test connections between the attachment
and caregiving systems, aged-care research has not systematically inves-
tigated the links between attachment and caregiving from a behavioral
systems perspective. The aged-care field needs to incorporate behavioral
systems approaches and concepts in studies linking attachment principles
to the care of older adults. The framing of research and testing of assump-
tions proposed in the Canterberry and Gillath model and related models
(e.g., Feeney & Collins, 2004) is particularly important, given Cicirelli’s
(1998) conceptualization of attachment theory and protective behavior. For
instance, from a behavioral systems perspective, one can argue that protec-
tive behavior represents the functioning of the attachment system. That is,
wanting to be near and wanting to assist an ailing attachment figure (parent
or partner) could be a form of proximity seeking that is motivated by the
need to feel more secure when faced with the impending loss of an attach-
ment figure (parent or partner). Alternatively, protective behavior may
reflect the functioning of the caregiving system, in which a carer notices
the older adult’s distress, concern, or calls for help, which then motivates
the carer to alleviate the older adult’s distress or suffering. These competing
but equally plausible explanations of protective behavior constitute merely
one example of the utility of integrating behavioral systems approaches to
elucidate the pathways linking attachment behavior with caregiving behav-
ior in the context of aged care.
& Cohen, 1998; Nelis, Clare, & Whitaker, 2012). In contrast, attachment
security is positively associated with the amount and quality of care pro-
vided by adult children to their aging parents, and negatively associated
with the carers’ burden and mental health outcomes (e.g., Carpenter, 2001;
Cooper, Owens, Katona, & Livingston, 2008; Karantzas, Evans, et al.,
2010; Magai & Cohen, 1998; Nelis et al., 2012).
In regard to the type of care rendered to older adults, research has
found that attachment anxiety is negatively associated with adult chil-
dren’s provision of both emotional and instrumental support (Carpenter,
2001; Kim & Carver, 2007). Studies of attachment avoidance show less
consistent findings, with some reporting negative associations and oth-
ers reporting no link between adult children’s attachment avoidance and
the provision of either emotional or instrumental support to older parents
(e.g., Carpenter, 2001; Kim & Carver, 2007; Pohl et al., 1995). However,
attachment avoidance is positively associated with adult children’s ten-
dency to place their aging parents in residential care facilities (Markiewicz,
Reis, & Gold, 1997). In contrast, attachment security is positively associ-
ated with adult children’s provision of emotional and instrumental help to
their aging parents, along with a tendency to keep their parents in their
own homes rather than put them in residential care facilities (Markiewicz
et al., 1997).
In the only study to date investigating attachment and the style (the
manner) in which care is provided, Braun et al. (2012) found that in older
couples dealing with cancer, carers’ attachment avoidance was negatively
associated with the provision of sensitive care, whereas carers’ attachment
anxiety was associated with the provision of compulsive caregiving. In addi-
tion, both attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance were associated
with carers’ engaging in more controlling care toward their older spouses.
During the last decade, a handful of studies have examined how
attachment is related to the future care of older adults. Specifically, attach-
ment security and attachment strength are positively correlated with adult
children’s preparedness and willingness to provide future care to older
adults (Cicirelli, 1983; Sörensen, Webster, & Roggman, 2002). In contrast,
attachment insecurity is negatively associated with carers’ willingness to
plan or their intentions to give care to older adults in the future (Karantzas,
Evans, et al., 2010; Sörensen et al., 2002). Our work and that of others
has also found that attachment avoidance is strongly associated with adult
children’s unwillingness to provide future care to older parents (Karantzas,
Evans, et al., 2010; Sörensen et al., 2002).
physical and mental health outcomes. This is true despite the fact that
many studies of attachment and aged care appear to have collected data on
care recipients’ physical and/or emotional well-being.
In our systematic review of the literature (Karantzas et al., 2014),
we found only five studies that reported associations between and among
attachment, care receipt/care seeking, and health outcomes from the per-
spective of the care recipients. In relation to attachment strength, Anto-
nucci (1994) found that older women who reported stronger attachment to
their daughters received more emotional support from them. In an innova-
tive study, Steele, Phibbs, and Woods (2004) examined how the behavior of
older adults with dementia, when reunited with their adult daughters after
separation, predicted the daughters’ attachment mental states as assessed
by the Adult Attachment Interview (AAI; Main, Kaplan, & Cassidy, 1985).
Steele et al. found that older mothers’ behaviors during these reunions with
their daughters were positively correlated with their daughters’ coherence
of mind as assessed by the AAI, even when the researchers controlled for
the severity of the mothers’ dementia symptoms. Specifically, mothers’ dis-
play of secure reunion behavior (i.e., proximity seeking, maintenance of
contact, and responsiveness) was associated with their daughters’ being
securely attached on the AAI. In another study focusing on people with
dementia, Nelis et al. (2012) found that for such people, attachment secu-
rity was related to having a more positive self-concept and fewer symp-
toms of dementia-related anxiety. In one of the few longitudinal studies on
attachment and caregiving in aged care, Perren, Schmid, Herrmann, and
Wettstein (2007) found that among older couples dealing with dementia
care, the caregivers’ attachment avoidance and the care recipients’ insecure
attachment (i.e., attachment anxiety or attachment avoidance) were asso-
ciated with increased levels of dementia-related problem behavior in care
recipients.
Our own work has also examined attachment and familial caregiv-
ing from the care recipients’ perspective. In one study of older parents’
perceptions of seeking care and its effect on carers, we found that attach-
ment anxiety was positively associated with older parents’ current receipt
of care, their future willingness to receive care, and their perceptions of
carer burden (Karantzas, Evans, et al., 2010). We also found that older par-
ents’ attachment anxiety was positively associated with their perceptions of
the sense of obligation that adult children should have in providing care to
older parents (i.e., filial obligation). In a path-analytic model, we confirmed
that the covariation between older parents’ views about filial obligation
and their attachment orientation predicted older parents’ actual seeking of
care from their adult children. This suggests that filial obligation may be
interconnected with attachment anxiety. If so, older parents who are highly
anxious may impose filial responsibilities on their adult children as a means
of safeguarding and controlling their relationship with them.
328 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
A Word of Caution
Although this brief review of the current literature on attachment and aged
care offers some valuable insights, caution must be exercised regarding how
much to read into these findings. A recurrent theme in this review and
recent others is the significant variability in how attachment is conceptu-
alized and measured in most aged-care research (see Bradley & Cafferty,
2001; Karantzas, Evans, et al., 2010; Van Assche et al., 2013). We address
the issue of the conceptualization and assessment of attachment in later
life and aged care in the next section. In doing so, we discuss various con-
ceptualizations of attachment and describe particular measures associated
with each one. We also discuss the strengths and weaknesses of the various
conceptualizations and assessments as they relate to aged care.
Strength of Attachment
Several studies of attachment and aged care have conceptualized attach-
ment in terms of the strength of the “bond” between older parents and
their adult children (e.g., Cicirelli, 1995; Thompson & Walker, 1984; Troll
& Smith, 1976), with few measures of this type assessing the romantic
attachment orientations of older adults. This conceptualization of attach-
ment places little, if any, emphasis on the distinction between attachment
individual differences in the form of attachment orientations or styles;
rather, strength is measured as a unidimensional construct, with higher
scores reflecting a tighter/closer perceived bond between an older adult
and an adult child. A common inference associated with these measures
is that a higher score indicates a more secure attachment (Cicirelli, 1993;
Thompson & Walker, 1984). However, this inference (as we discuss later
in this section) is somewhat tenuous, as a “tighter” or “closer” bond does
not necessarily reflect a “secure” attachment. Rather, a secure attachment
bond is characterized by a relationship in which closeness is balanced with
autonomy and independence (Karantzas, Evans, et al., 2010).
Nearly all of these unidimensional measures are self-report in nature,
Attachment and Aged Care 329
(coupled with their scoring procedures) casts some doubt over their validity
as good indicators of attachment patterns in later life. As cases in point,
higher scores on both Thompson and Walker’s (1984) measure and Cicirelli’s
(1995) AAS are presumed to reflect stronger (and more secure) attachment,
whereas lower scores are believed to reflect weaker (and more insecure)
attachment. However, this scoring procedure is likely to be inappropriate,
because both of these measures were specifically designed to assess attach-
ment bonding between older parents and their adult children (especially
aging mothers and adult daughters) within the context of aged care. Several
items in these measures—such as “We’re emotionally dependent on one
another,” “The thought of losing my mother is deeply disturbing to me,” or
“I feel lonely when I don’t see my mother often”—suggest that a high score
may not reflect attachment security, but attachment insecurity. Within the
context of family members involved in aged care, the loss of an older adult
is inevitable. According to Bowlby (1980) and Fraley and Shaver (1999),
individuals who are securely attached typically go through a cognitive reor-
ganization of their working models after the death of an attachment figure,
which allows them to come to terms with the loss and eventually reengage
in exploratory behavior. In doing so, the reorganization of their working
models is likely to commence prior to death, as in the case of familial care-
giving during which an older spouse or adult child witnesses an attachment
figure endure a protracted illness (see Fraley & Shaver, 1999).
Thus it seems erroneous to interpret high scores on these unidimen-
sional measures as indexing secure attachment. In fact, it seems more
appropriate to infer that moderate scores on these measures may be more
indicative of secure familial attachment, because secure adult children,
while distressed about the eventual loss of their parents, should have
started reorganizing their attachment working models during the course
of their parents’ ill health. Therefore, high scores on these measures are
probably indicative of an overly anxious form of attachment characterized
by clinginess, a high degree of separation protest, and cognitive inflex-
ibility in the reorganization of attachment working models. Moreover, the
items on the AAS and Thompson and Walker’s measure assess attachment
anxiety, with little emphasis on attachment avoidance. In contrast, the Bar-
nas et al. (1991) interview measure conflates scores on attachment security
and attachment avoidance. As a result, it is difficult to determine whether
scores on this measure reflect either the presence or absence of security or
the presence or absence of avoidance. Moreover, with no explicit assess-
ment of attachment anxiety, this measure excludes a fundamental type of
attachment insecurity. Finally, while Antonucci and colleagues’ social con-
voy measure is not solely a measure of attachment strength, the suggestion
that inner-circle network members are bona fide “attachment figures” is
an assertion rather than a fact. Thus, although it may be true that some
“very close” network members are actual attachment figures, there is no
Attachment and Aged Care 331
definitive way of determining whether this is true for the social convoy
measure. In sum, even though these unidimensional assessments of attach-
ment have been used to study aging families and aged care, whether and
the extent to which these measures validly capture attachment orientations
or styles remain uncertain.
the caregiver: (1) supports the [care recipient’s] exploration; (2) remains
available and responsive and serves as a resource as necessary; (3) the
[care recipient] encounters an obstacle or threat and becomes distressed;
(4) either the [care recipient] retreats to the caregiver or the caregiver
goes to the [care recipient]; (5) the difficulty is resolved or removed; (6)
proximity and/or contact with the caregiver effectively comforts the
[care recipient]; (7) the [care recipient] (possibly with the caregiver’s
assistance) returns to constructive [activity] (or ends [the activity] com-
fortably and makes a transition to another activity). (p. 1)
Attachment Orientations/Styles
The most widely used of the validated attachment measures in aging and
aged-care research are self-report assessments of attachment orientations/
styles. Generally speaking, these self-report measures have been either cat-
egorical assessments or dimensional assessments of attachment. The most
commonly used categorical measures are Hazan and Shaver’s (1987) three
category descriptors (i.e., secure, anxious, and avoidant) and Bartholomew
and colleagues’ prototype measures of attachment (i.e., secure, preoccu-
pied, dismissing, and fearful, assessed by the Relationship Questionnaire
[the RQ; Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991] or the Relationship Styles Ques-
tionnaire [the RSQ; Griffin & Bartholomew, 1994]). The most popular
dimensional measures have been the Experiences in Close Relationships
(ECR) scale (Brennan, Clark, & Shaver, 1998) and revisions of it (e.g., the
ECR-R; Fraley, Waller, & Brennan, 2000), and the Attachment Style Ques-
tionnaire (ASQ; Feeney, Noller, & Hanrahan, 1994; Karantzas, Feeney, &
Wilkinson, 2010). Both the ECR and the ASQ tap the two primary dimen-
sions underlying attachment orientations/styles—attachment anxiety and
attachment avoidance. The ASQ also taps specific facets of attachment
insecurity (for reviews, see Feeney et al., 1994; Karantzas, Feeney, et al.,
2010; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007a). Scores derived from these categori-
cal and dimensional assessments have been linked to caregiving behavior
outcomes, carer outcomes, and anticipated caregiving behavior (e.g., Braun
334 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
et al., 2012; Carpenter, 2001; Karantzas, Evans, et al., 2010; Magai &
Cohen, 1998; Nelis et al., 2012).
The adoption of these self-report measures in aged-care research has
introduced more uniformity in how individual differences in attachment
are conceptualized and measured (Bradley & Cafferty, 2001). They have
also provided greater psychometric rigor, and these measures are yielding
important insights into the links between attachment and aged care. As
a result, differential predictions can be made regarding how attachment
security and different forms of insecurity are likely to affect the provision
of care and the seeking and/or receipt of care.
Given that these measures were designed to measure adult romantic
or global attachment orientations/styles, various gerontological researchers
have modified or adapted their instructions or reworded the items to focus
on attachments between adult children and older adults. Although these
adaptations are presumed to be more context-specific and ecologically
valid assessments of attachment, research is mixed regarding the extent
to which alterations of instructions and items yield assessments that are
similar to general attachment representations (see Cameron, Finnegan, &
Morry, 2012; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007a). Given these mixed findings,
the use of self-report measures originally designed to assess romantic or
general attachment orientations during young and middle adulthood raises
questions about the validity of these assessments when they are applied
to aged-care contexts, especially when assessments target familial attach-
ments between older parents and their adult children. For instance, items
that typically capture attachment insecurity in romantic relationships—
such as “I want to merge completely with another person” and “I want to
be completely emotionally intimate with others” (i.e., attachment anxiety),
and “I am nervous when partners get too close to me” and “I am too busy
with other activities to put much time into relationships” (i.e., attachment
avoidance)—may not apply very well to how older parents or their adult
children perceive their relationships. The use of these self-report measures
in aged-care research, in fact, has often yielded low reliability coefficients,
indicating poor internal consistency and/or factor structures that do not
neatly map onto the dimensions of the original measures (e.g., Carpenter,
2001; Magai et al., 2001). These findings suggest that these revised mea-
sures may not capture attachment insecurity in a way that is age-appro-
priate and/or relationship-appropriate when researchers are investigating
bonds between older adults and their adult children.
Therefore, considerable caution needs to be taken when self-report
measures are implemented in the aging and aged-care contexts, especially
for the purpose of measuring attachment relationships between older par-
ents and their adult children. One way forward may be to develop self-
report measures that assess the critical features of attachment security,
anxiety, and avoidance, but that contain items worded in a manner that
Attachment and Aged Care 335
References
Ainsworth, M. D. (1985). Attachments across the life-span. Bulletin of the New
York Academy of Medicine, 61, 792–812.
Ainsworth, M. D. (1989). Attachments beyond infancy. American Psychologist,
44, 709–716.
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of
attachment: A psychological study of the Strange Situation. Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Allen, K. R., Blieszner, R., & Roberto, K. A. (2000). Families in the middle and
340 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
later years: A review and critique of research in the 1990s. Journal of Mar-
riage and Family, 62, 911–926.
Antonucci, T. C. (1986). Social support networks: A hierarchical mapping tech-
nique. Generations: Journal of the American Society on Aging, 10, 10–12.
Antonucci, T. C. (1994). Attachment in adulthood and aging. In M. B. Sperling
& W. H. Berman (Eds.), Attachment in adults: Clinical and developmental
perspectives (pp. 256–272). New York: Guilford Press.
Antonucci, T. C., & Akiyama, H. (1987). Social networks in adult life and a pre-
liminary examination of the convoy model. Journal of Gerontology, 42,
519–527.
Antonucci, T. C., Akiyama, H., & Takahashi, K. (2004). Attachment and close
relationships across the life span. Attachment and Human Development, 6,
353–370.
Antonucci, T. C., Kahn, R., & Akiyama, H. (1989). Psychosocial factors and the
response to cancer symptoms. In R. Yancik & J. W. Yates (Eds.), Cancer in
the elderly: Approaches to early detection and treatment (pp. 40–52). New
York: Springer.
Barnas, M. V., Pollina, L., & Cummings, E. (1991). Life-span attachment: Rela-
tions between attachment and socioemotional functioning in adult women.
Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 117, 175–202.
Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among young
adults: A test of a four-category model. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 61, 226–244.
Bengtson, V. L. (2001). Beyond the nuclear family: The increasing importance of
multigenerational bonds. Journal of Marriage and Family, 63, 1–16.
Blieszner, R., & Mancini, J. A. (1987). Enduring ties: Older adults’ parental role
and responsibilities. Family Relations, 36, 176–180.
Bowlby, J. (1969/1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York:
Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1979). The making and breaking of affectional bonds. London: Tavis-
tock.
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Vol. 3. Sadness and depression. New
York: Basic Books.
Bradley, J. M., & Cafferty, T. P. (2001). Attachment among older adults: Current
issues and directions for future research. Attachment and Human Develop-
ment, 3, 200–221.
Braun, M., Hales, S., Gilad, L., Mikulincer, M., Rydall, A., & Rodin, G. (2012).
Caregiving styles and attachment orientations in couples facing advanced can-
cer. Psycho-Oncology, 21, 935–943.
Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measurement of
adult romantic attachment: An integrative overview. In J. A. Simpson & W.
S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 46–76). New
York: Guilford Press.
Brodaty, H., & Green, A. (2002). Who cares for the carer?: The often forgotten
patient. Clinical Practice: Therapeutic Review, 31, 1–4.
Cameron, J. J., Finnegan, H., & Morry, M. M. (2012). Orthogonal dreams in an
oblique world: A meta-analysis of the association between attachment anxiety
and avoidance. Journal of Research in Personality, 46, 472–476.
Attachment and Aged Care 341
Canterberry, M., & Gillath, O. (2012). Attachment and caregiving. In P. Noller &
G. C. Karantzas (Eds.), The Wiley–Blackwell handbook of couple and family
relationships (pp. 207–219). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Carpenter, B. D. (2001). Attachment bonds between adult daughters and their
older mothers: Associations with contemporary caregiving. Journals of Ger-
ontology, Series B, 56B, 257–266.
Chen, C. K., Waters, H. S., Hartman, M., Zimmerman, S., Miklowitz, D. J., &
Waters, E. (2013). The secure base script and the task of caring for elderly
parents: Implications for attachment theory and clinical practice. Attachment
and Human Development, 15, 332–348.
Cicirelli, V. G. (1983). Adult children’s attachment and helping behaviour to elderly
parents: A path model. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 45, 815–822.
Cicirelli, V. G. (1985). The role of siblings as family caregivers. In W. J. Sauer &
R. T. Cowards, (Eds.), Social support networks and the care of the elderly
(pp. 93–107). New York: Springer.
Cicirelli, V. G. (1991). Family caregiving: Autonomous and paternalistic decision
making. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Cicirelli, V. G. (1993). Attachment and obligation as daughters’ motives for care-
giving behavior and subsequent effect on subjective burden. Psychology and
Aging, 8, 144–155.
Cicirelli, V. G. (1995). A measure of caregiving daughters’ attachment to elderly
mothers. Journal of Family Psychology, 9, 89–94.
Cicirelli, V. G. (1998). A frame of reference for guiding research regarding the
relationship between adult attachment and mental health in aging families. In
J. Lomranz (Ed.), Handbook of aging and mental health: An integrative
approach (pp. 341–353). New York: Plenum Press.
Cooke, D. D., McNally, L., Mulligan, K. T., Harrison, M. J. G., & Newman, S. P.
(2001). Psychosocial interventions for caregivers of people with dementia: A
systematic review. Aging and Mental Health, 5, 120–135.
Cooper, C., Owens, C., Katona, C., & Livingston, G. (2008). Attachment style
and anxiety in carers of people with Alzheimer’s disease: Results from the
LASER-AD study. International Psychogeriatrics, 20, 494–507.
Coppola, G., Vaughn, B. E., Cassibba, R., & Costantini, A. (2006). The attach-
ment script representation procedure in an Italian sample: Associations with
adult attachment interview scales and with maternal sensitivity. Attachment
and Human Development, 8, 209–219.
Crispi, E. L., Schiaffino, K., & Berman, W. H. (1997). The contribution of attach-
ment to burden in adult children of institutionalized parents with dementia.
The Gerontologist, 37, 52–60.
Dilworth-Anderson, P., Williams, I. C., & Gibson, B. E. (2002). Issues of race,
ethnicity, and culture in caregiving research: A 20-year review (1980–2000).
The Gerontologist, 42, 237–272.
Dykas, M. J., Woodhouse, S. S., Cassidy, J., & Waters, H. S. (2006). Narrative
assessment of attachment representations: Links between secure base scripts
and adolescent attachment. Attachment and Human Development, 8, 221–
240.
Feeney, B. C., & Collins, N. L. (2004). Interpersonal safe haven and secure base
caregiving processes in adulthood. In W. S. Rholes & J. A. Simpson (Eds.),
342 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Kim, Y., & Carver, C. S. (2007). Frequency and difficulty in caregiving among
spouses of individuals with cancer: Effects of adult attachment and gender.
Psycho-Oncology, 16, 714–723.
Knight, B., Silverstein, M., McCallum, T., & Fox, L. (2000). A sociocultural stress
and coping model for mental health outcomes among African American
caregivers in Southern California. Journals of Gerontology, Series B, 55B,
142–150.
Koski, L. R., & Shaver, P. R. (1997). Attachment and relationship satisfaction
across the lifespan. In R. J. Sternberg & M. Hojjat (Eds.), Satisfaction in close
relationships (pp. 26–55). New York: Guilford Press.
Levitt, M. J. (1991). Attachment and close relationships: A life-span perspec-
tive. In J. Gewirtz, & W. M. Kurtines (Eds.), Intersections with attachment
(pp. 183–205). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Lopez-Hartmann, M., Wens, J., Verhoeven, V., & Remmen, R. (2012). The effect
of caregiver support interventions for informal caregivers of community-
dwelling frail elderly: A systematic review. International Journal of Inte-
grated Care, 12, 1–16.
Magai, C., & Cohen, C. I. (1998). Attachment style and emotion regulation in
dementia patients and their relation to caregiver burden. Journals of Geron-
tology, Series B, 53B, 147–154.
Magai, C., Cohen, C. I., Milburn, N., Thorpe, B., McPherson, R., & Peralta, D.
(2001). Attachment styles in older European American and African American
adults. Journals of Gerontology, Series B, 56B, 28–35.
Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in infancy, childhood, and
adulthood: A move to the level of representation. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters
(Eds.), Growing points of attachment theory and research. Monographs of the
Society for Research in Child Development, 50(1–2, Serial No. 209), 66–104.
Markiewicz, D., Reis, M., & Gold, D. P. (1997). An exploration of attachment
styles and personality traits in caregiving for dementia patients. International
Journal of Aging and Human Development, 45, 111–132.
Marvin, R. S., & Stewart, R. B. (1990). A family systems framework for the
study of attachment. In M. T. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, & E. M. Cummings
(Eds.), Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research, and intervention
(pp. 51–86). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2003). The attachment behavioral system in
adulthood: Activation, psychodynamics, and interpersonal processes. In
M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 35,
pp. 53–152). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007a). Attachment in adulthood: Structure,
dynamics, and change. New York: Guilford Press.
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007b). Boosting attachment security to pro-
mote mental health, prosocial values, and inter-group tolerance. Psychologi-
cal Inquiry, 18, 139–156.
Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P. R., Gillath, O., & Nitzberg, R. A. (2005). Attachment,
caregiving, and altruism: Boosting attachment security increases compassion
and helping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 817–839.
Miller, B., & Cafasso, L. (1992). Gender differences in caregiving: Fact or artifact?
The Gerontologist, 32, 498–507.
344 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion. (2011).
Healthy aging: Helping people to live long and productive lives and enjoy a
good quality life. Atlanta, GA: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention.
National Institute on Aging (NIA). (2011). Global health and aging (NIH Publica-
tion No. 11-7737). Bethesda, MD: National Institutes of Health.
Nelis, S. M., Clare, L., & Whitaker, C. J. (2012). Attachment representations in
people with dementia and their carers: Implications for well-being within the
dyad. Aging and Mental Health, 16, 845–854.
Perren, S., Schmid, R., Herrmann, S., & Wettstein, A. (2007). The impact of
attachment on dementia-related problem behavior and spousal caregivers’
well-being. Attachment and Human Development, 9, 163–178.
Pohl, J. M., Boyd, C., Liang, J., & Given, C. W. (1995). Analysis of the impact
of mother–daughter relationships on the commitment to caregiving. Nursing
Research, 44, 68–75.
Rholes, W. S., Simpson, J. A., Campbell, L., & Grich, J. (2001). Adult attachment
and the transition to parenthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 81, 421–435.
Roisman, G. I., Fraley, R. C., & Belsky, J. (2007). A taxometric study of the Adult
Attachment Interview. Developmental Psychology, 43, 675–686.
Shaver, P. R., & Mikulincer, M. (2004). What do self-report attachment measures
assess? In W. S. Rholes & J. A. Simpson (Eds.), Adult attachment: Theory,
research, and clinical implications (pp. 17–54). New York: Guilford Press.
Silverstein, M., & Bengtson, V. L. (1997). Intergenerational solidarity and the
structure of adult child–parent relationships in American families. American
Journal of Sociology, 103, 429–460.
Silverstein, M., Gans, D., & Yang, F. M. (2006). Intergenerational support to aging
parents: The role of norms and needs. Journal of Family Issues, 27, 1068–
1084.
Simpson, J. A., & Rholes, W. S. (1994). Stress and secure base relationships in
adulthood. In K. Bartholomew & D. Perlman (Eds.), Advances in personal
relationships: Vol. 5. Attachment processes in adulthood (pp. 181–204). Lon-
don: Jessica Kingsley.
Simpson, J. A., & Rholes, W. S. (2012). Adult attachment orientations, stress, and
romantic relationships. In T. Devine & A. Plante (Eds.), Advances in experi-
mental social psychology (Vol. 45, pp. 279–328). New York: Elsevier.
Simpson, J. A., Rholes, W. S., Campbell, L., Tran, S., & Wilson, C. L. (2003).
Adult attachment, the transition to parenthood, and depressive symptoms.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 1172–1187.
Simpson, J. A., Rholes, W. S., & Nelligan, J. S. (1992). Support seeking and support
giving within couples in an anxiety-provoking situation: The role of attach-
ment styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 434–446.
Sörensen, S., Webster, J. D., & Roggman, L. A. (2002). Adult attachment and
preparing to provide care for older relatives. Attachment and Human Devel-
opment, 4, 84–106.
Steele, H., Phibbs, E., & Woods, R. T. (2004). Coherence of mind in daughter
caregivers of mothers with dementia: Links with their mothers’ joy and relat-
edness on reunion in a strange situation. Attachment and Human Develop-
ment, 6, 439–450.
Attachment and Aged Care 345
Thompson, L., & Walker, A. J. (1984). Mothers and daughters: Aid patterns and
attachment. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 46, 313–322.
Troll, E., & Smith, J. (1976). Attachment through the life-span: Some questions
about dyadic bonds among adults. Human Development, 19, 156–170.
Van Assche, L., Luyten, P., Bruffaerts, R., Persoons, P., van de Ven, L., & Vanden-
bulcke, M. (2013). Attachment in old age: Theoretical assumptions, empirical
findings and implications for clinical practice. Clinical Psychology Review,
33, 67–81.
Vaughn, B. E., Egeland, B. R., Sroufe, L. A., & Waters, E. (1979). Individual differ-
ences in infant–mother attachment at 12 and 18 months: Stability and change
in families under stress. Child Development, 50, 971–975.
Waters, H. S., & Rodrigues-Doolabh, L. M. (2001). Narrative assessment of
adult attachment representations: The scoring of Secure Base Script content.
Unpublished manuscript, State University of New York at Stony Brook.
Waters, H. S., & Waters, E. (2006). The attachment working models concept:
Among other things, we build script-like representations of secure base expe-
riences. Attachment and Human Development, 8, 185–197.
Weiss, R. S. (1982). Attachment in adult life. In C. M. Parkes & J. Stevenson-Hinde
(Eds.), The place of attachment in human behavior (pp. 171–184). New York:
Basic Books.
World Health Organization (WHO). (2012). Knowledge translation on ageing and
health: Policy framework. Geneva, Switzerland: Author.
13
Psychopathology and Attachment
Tsachi Ein-Dor
Guy Doron
A lbert Einstein once stated, “One knows from daily life that one
exists for other people—first of all for those upon whose smiles and well-
being our own happiness is wholly dependent” (1931, p. 193). In keeping
with this view, theory and research have indicated that the roots of mental
health and psychopathology may be traced to a person’s history of interac-
tions with other people, specifically in times of need (Bowlby, 1980; Sroufe,
2005; Sroufe, Egeland, Carlson, & Collins, 2005). When people, particu-
larly close others, regularly respond sensitively to the person’s needs, he or
she develops a sense of attachment security that includes acquiring con-
structive strategies for coping with threats and for regulating negative emo-
tions (see Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007, for an extensive review). But when
other people are often unavailable, unreliable, or rejecting of bids for sup-
port, the person may become chronically insecure with respect to close
relationships. One of the main insecure attachment patterns in adulthood
is avoidance, which relates to the extent to which a person distrusts rela-
tionship partners’ goodwill, strives to maintain independence, and relies
on deactivating strategies for dealing with threats and negative emotions
(e.g., Fraley & Shaver, 1997). The second main insecure pattern is anxiety,
which relates to the extent to which a person worries that others will not be
available or helpful in times of need. Anxious individuals exaggerate their
sense of vulnerability and insistently call on others for help and care, some-
times to the point of being intrusive (e.g., Feeney & Noller, 1990).
Research has indicated that attachment insecurities (both anxiety and
avoidance) are associated with a general vulnerability to mental disorders
346
Psychopathology and Attachment 347
(Mikulincer & Shaver, 2012; Sroufe, Duggal, Weinfield, & Carlson, 2000).
For example, attachment insecurities are linked with depression (e.g., Cat-
anzaro & Wei, 2010), generalized anxiety disorder (e.g., Marganska, Gal-
lagher, & Miranda, 2013), obsessive–compulsive disorder (e.g., Doron,
Moulding, Kyrios, Nedeljkovic, & Mikulincer, 2009), posttraumatic stress
disorder (PTSD) (e.g., Ein-Dor, Doron, Solomon, Mikulincer, & Shaver,
2010), eating disorders (e.g., Illing, Tasca, Balfour, & Bissada, 2010), and
suicide ideation (e.g., Davaji, Valizadeh, & Nikamal, 2010). Attachment
insecurity is also related to many personality disorders (Crawford et al.,
2007; Meyer & Pilkonis, 2005). For example, people high on attachment
anxiety have higher prevalences of dependent, histrionic, and borderline
personality disorders, which often comprise identity confusion, anxiety,
emotional liability, cognitive distortions, submissiveness, self-harm, and
suspiciousness (the “emotional dysregulation” component of personality
disorders; Livesley, 1991). Conversely, avoidant individuals have higher
prevalences of schizoid and avoidant personality disorders, which consist
of restricted expression of emotions, problems with intimacy, and social
avoidance (the “inhibitedness” component of personality problems; Lives-
ley, 1991).
Attachment theory, however, has difficulty simultaneously explaining
the mechanisms by which attachment insecurities lead to multiple disor-
ders (i.e., the question of multifinality; Cicchetti, 1984; Egeland, Pianta, &
Ogawa, 1996), and why one individual with a particular attachment orien-
tation develops one set of symptoms while another with the same attach-
ment vulnerability develops another set of symptoms (i.e., the question of
divergent trajectories; Nolen-Hoeksema & Watkins, 2011). In addition,
attachment research has yet to explore the dyadic processes that play a role
in exacerbating or mitigating psychopathology. In this chapter, we unfold
a transdiagnostic model of attachment insecurities (in line with Nolen-
Hoeksema and Watkins’s [2011] heuristic for developing such models; see
Figure 13.1 for an outline of the heuristic) that refers to the possible causal
processes linking attachment orientation to multiple disorders.
We first review studies of both clinical and nonclinical samples that
link attachment dispositions to the two primary dimensions of psycho-
pathology: internalizing (including mood and anxiety disorders, such as
major depression, generalized anxiety disorder, panic disorder, and social
anxiety disorder [social phobia]; Krueger & Markon, 2006, 2011), and
externalizing (including substance use and antisocial disorders; Krueger &
Markon, 2006, 2011).
Next, we address the question of multifinality by speculating on the
processes (i.e., proximal risk factors) that mediate the linkage between
attachment insecurities and multiple disorders, and the question of diver-
gent trajectories by indicating the different contexts (e.g., genetic predispo-
sition, family environment, and cultural environment) that might set one
348 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Anxiety Disorders
Bowlby (1973) was the first to contend that attachment insecurities can,
under specific circumstances, lead to anxiety disorders. When caregivers
Psychopathology and Attachment 349
Sroufe et al., 2005, for an extensive review), which set out to explore devel-
opmental trajectories of early attachment orientations, revealed that infants
classified as anxious-resistant were more likely than their secure counter-
parts to endorse anxiety disorders at age 17 (Warren, Huston, Egeland,
& Sroufe, 1997). This prospective prediction was not apparent among
anxious-avoidant infants. In a different study, attachment anxiety, but not
avoidance, was also linked with a history of anxiety-related disorders—
from emotional dysregulation at age 3.5, through childhood anxiety prob-
lems, to anxiety disorders in adolescence (Bosquet & Egeland, 2006).
Depression
Bowlby (1980) posited that attachment insecurities relate to the develop-
ment of depressive disorders, as well as to that of anxiety. He contended
that a loss of a primary attachment figure or a repeated failure to form a
secure relationship with a caregiver encourages the formation of pessimis-
tic, hopeless representations of self, other, and world. When an insecure
individual encounters subsequent losses, traumatic events, or hardships, his
or her abandonment-related cognitions and feelings may trigger symptoms
of depression.
To date, more than 100 studies have examined the links between
attachment dispositions and the severity of depressive symptoms. For
example, studies in special populations with a heightened risk for depres-
sion (i.e., low socioeconomic status, stressful life history, and poor health)
revealed that people high on either attachment anxiety or avoidance suffer
from more depressive symptoms. Specifically, dismissing and preoccupied
states of mind according to the AAI, and higher scores on self-report mea-
sures of attachment avoidance or anxiety, have been associated with more
severe symptoms of depression in samples of psychiatric inpatients (Fowler,
Allen, Oldham, & Frueh, 2013); patients with chronic pain (Ciechanowski,
Sullivan, Jensen, Romano, & Summers, 2003); patients with diabetes
(Ciechanowski, Katon, & Russo, 2005); patients who are HIV-positive
(e.g., Hinnen et al., 2012); and outpatients suffering from eating disorders,
drug abuse, or major depression (e.g., Eng, Heimberg, Hart, Schneier, &
Liebowitz, 2001; Visioli, Senatore, Lepri, & Tondo, 2012).
Studies in nonclinical samples have indicated that a preoccupied state
of mind on the AAI (Cole-Detke & Kobak, 1996), endorsement of an
anxious (categorical) attachment style in close relationships (e.g., Muris,
Meesters, van Melick, & Zwambag, 2001), or higher ratings of attachment
anxiety on self-report scales (e.g., Brenning, Soenens, Braet, & Bosmans,
2012; Wei, Mallinckrodt, Larson, & Zakalik, 2005) are associated with
heightened severity of depressive symptoms. In keeping with the findings
regarding anxiety disorders, the evidence for the link between attachment
avoidance and depression is conflicting: About half of the studies have
Psychopathology and Attachment 351
who were not held in captivity (Ein-Dor, Doron, et al., 2010). It may be
that when individuals are challenged over a prolonged period of time under
extreme conditions such as captivity, the otherwise relatively effective
avoidant strategies of suppressing attachment needs and distress tend to
break down, causing attachment-related avoidance to be associated with
emotional problems and psychopathology.
have found that conduct problems are associated with an anxious (pre-
occupied) state of mind (e.g., Caspers, Cadoret, Langbehn, Yucuis, &
Troutman, 2005; McElhaney et al., 2006), others relate these problems
to an avoidant (dismissing) state of mind (e.g., Allen, Hauser, & Borman-
Spurrell, 1996).
The developmental trajectory of conduct problems with respect to early
attachment dispositions has yet to be directly examined. With that being
said, poor-quality parenting at infancy (age 42 months), which resulted
from maternal attachment avoidance and alienation (Sroufe et al., 2005),
was found to be a risk factor for externalizing problems in adulthood in the
Minnesota Study (Lorber & Egeland, 2009, 2011).
Socia
l- -
antis pressure ent
ocial -r vem s D
ing pote elated chie ed los an ista
ten ntial A lat tis nc
a
Inte loss
ea t re oc ing
Traum
Tra
t hr men um ial -r Moderators
ic on a po ela
rper
l
al
for a “dark triad” of processes that link attachment anxiety with multiple
psychopathological disorders: (1) maladaptive emotion regulation pro-
cesses, with a tendency to up-regulate negative affectivity; (2) greater vigi-
lance to threat-related cues and heightened empathic accuracy; and (3) a
lower level of perceived others’ responsiveness—that is, seeing others as
less responsive and supportive and as less understanding of one’s needs (a
broader notion than Reis and colleagues’ perceived partner responsiveness;
e.g., Reis, Clark, & Holmes, 2004).
Emotion regulation has been conceptualized as processes through
which individuals modulate their emotions to respond appropriately to
environmental demands (e.g., Campbell-Sills & Barlow, 2007). Individuals
deploy regulatory strategies to modify the magnitude and/or type of their
emotional experience or to adjust an emotion-eliciting event itself (e.g., Dia-
mond & Aspinwall, 2003). The attachment system plays an important role
in emotion regulation: It is activated by physiological and psychological
threats, and causes a threatened individual to seek proximity to others as a
means of managing the threat and restoring emotional equilibrium (Miku-
lincer & Shaver, 2003). Among secure people, these strategies reduce the
impact of distressing events, allowing them to experience and acknowledge
356 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
their partners’ true thoughts and feelings (e.g., when their partners rated
and discussed an attractive person with them; Simpson, Ickes, & Grich,
1999; Simpson et al., 2011). According to Mikulincer and Shaver (2003),
these abilities stem from the use of a hyperactivating strategy by people
high in attachment anxiety—ruminating about worst-case scenarios and
remaining vigilant to signs that close others might not be committed to
them.
Attentiveness to threat-related information has been assigned a promi-
nent role in the etiology and maintenance of anxiety and related disorders
(e.g., Eysenck, 1992; Mathews & MacLeod, 2002), such as PTSD (Buckley,
Blanchard, & Neill, 2000), generalized anxiety disorder (Mogg & Bradley,
2005), and panic disorder and phobias (McNally, 1999). The attentional
system of people high on attachment anxiety seems to be distinctively sensi-
tive to and biased in favor of threat-related stimuli in the environment (see
Bar-Haim, Lamy, Pergamin, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & van IJzendoorn,
2007, for a recent meta-analysis). This tendency may confer adaptational
advantages (Ein-Dor, Mikulincer, et al., 2010), but may also create disad-
vantages in the form of psychopathology.
Aside from perceiving more threats in the environment and intensively
reacting to them, people high on attachment anxiety fear that they may
need to face these threats alone: They tend to regard others as less respon-
sive and supportive, and as less understanding to their needs (i.e., they have
low perceived others’ responsiveness). Research has indicated that perceived
social support reflects a personality characteristic rather than the actual
helpfulness of others when needed (Sarason et al., 1991). For example, Col-
lins and Feeney (2004) found that people high on attachment anxiety were
predisposed to perceive and remember a partner’s helpful behavior as less
supportive, especially if the behavior was ambiguous, open to subjective
construal, and likely to reactivate worries about a partner’s availability and
supportiveness. Moreover, they often choose unsupportive partners and
tend to act in ways that cause partners to act unsupportively, thus con-
firming and strengthening their doubts about other people’s supportiveness
(Rholes, Simpson, Campbell, & Grich, 2001).
Perceived availability of social support (and not actual support received)
has been found to play an important role in the prediction of specific psy-
chopathologies, including PTSD (e.g., Besser & Neria, 2012), depression
(e.g., Grav, Hellzèn, Romild, & Stordal, 2012), and externalizing disorders
(e.g., Lifrak, McKay, Rostain, Alterman, & O’Brien, 1997).
We therefore contend that the dark triad of people high on attachment
anxiety—hypervigilance to threats, intensified negative affectivity, and
low perceived others’ responsiveness—may constitute the proximal trans-
diagnostic risk factors (see Nolen-Hoeksema & Watkins, 2011; also called
intermediate phenotypes) that mediate the relationships between attach-
ment anxiety and multiple psychopathologies, and that launch anxious
358 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Attachment Avoidance
Theory and research have indicated that attachment avoidance is organized
around deactivating strategies of affect regulation, which involve deempha-
sizing threats and trying to cope with them alone, without seeking help or
support from other people (e.g., Kobak, Cole, Ferenz-Gillies, & Fleming,
1993; Shaver & Mikulincer, 2002). Avoidant people also deny attachment
needs and suppress attachment-related thoughts and emotions (Mikulincer
& Shaver, 2003). These tendencies may be the initiating conditions for a
second “dark triad” of processes—ones that, in this case, link attachment
avoidance with multiple psychopathological disorders: (1) maladjusted
emotion regulation processes, with a tendency to down-regulate affectivity
and employing distancing strategies; (2) compulsive self-reliance; and (3)
lower levels of social support and perceived others’ responsiveness.
The attachment behavioral system fosters emotion regulation in a
socially based way (Coan, 2008; Sroufe et al., 2004): It motivates a per-
son to alleviate his or her distress by seeking actual or symbolic proximity
to significant others. People high on attachment avoidance tend to forgo
this tendency and to maintain a defensive façade of security and compo-
sure, while managing cognitive and emotional avoidance and dealing with
threats without seeking help from others (Shaver & Mikulincer, 2002). To
independently overcome life’s many challenges, they tend to block access
to emotions and to cope with stress by ignoring, suppressing, or denying it
(e.g., Dozier & Kobak, 1992). These deactivation strategies may be effec-
tive in regulating mild levels of stress (e.g., Ein-Dor, Doron, et al., 2010),
but they leave suppressed distress unresolved nonetheless. When avoidant
persons are faced with prolonged and demanding stressful experiences that
require active confrontation of a problem and mobilization of external
sources of support, the suppressed distress can impair their ability to deal
with inevitable adversities. In these cases, avoidant people may feel inad-
equate to cope and may undergo a marked decline in functioning (Horow-
itz, 1982).
Cognitive and emotional avoidance, together with suppression of neg-
ative affectivity, have been found to predict a large array of psychopatho-
logical disorders: depression and anxiety (Blalock & Joiner, 2000; Hola-
han, Moos, Holahan, Brennan, & Schutte, 2005), eating disorders (Engler,
Crowther, Dalton, & Sanftner, 2006), and conduct problems (Cooper,
Wood, Orcutt, & Albino, 2003). For example, Holahan and colleagues
(2005) found that cognitive and emotional avoidance predicted increases in
depressive symptoms over 10 years in a sample of late-middle-aged adults.
Aside from coping with life’s challenges by cognitively and emotionally
Psychopathology and Attachment 359
Concluding Comments
Numerous studies have linked attachment dispositions (anxiety and avoid-
ance) to multiple psychopathological disorders. In the present chapter, we
have unfolded a transdiagnostic model of attachment insecurities to address
two open questions in attachment research: (1) What are the potential
mechanisms by which attachment dispositions cause all the different disor-
ders they are associated with; and (2) why does a given disposition lead to
different disorders in different people, or to different disorders within the
same person over time? We have suggested that each attachment orienta-
tion has its own “dark triad” of processes that link it to psychopathology,
and that by interacting with a specific moderator, these processes launch an
individual on a pathway for a particular disorder.
Two key aspects fall outside the scope of the present model and ought
to be considered in future transdiagnostic models of attachment disposi-
tions: the moderating effect of genetic predispositions, and the dyadic and
interpersonal processes that may affect the liability to psychopathology.
Specifically, we contend that particular genetic predispositions may inter-
act with the dark triads of people high on anxiety or avoidance as well as
with environmental moderators to predict the likelihood for psychopathol-
ogy. Of specific interest are the polymorphisms of the receptor genes for
oxytocin (OT) and arginine vasopressin (AVP). OT and AVP are mamma-
lian neuropeptides that have been found to be linked not only with devel-
opment and expression of social behavior and emotion regulation (Bartz,
Psychopathology and Attachment 363
Zaki, Bolger, & Ochsner, 2011), but also with stress-related behavior and
disorders, including anxiety disorders, comorbid depression, and their
neuroendocrine concomitants (see Meyer-Lindenberg, Domes, Kirsch,
& Heinrichs, 2011, for a review). For example, Thompson, Parker, Hall-
mayer, Waugh, and Gotlib (2011) have shown that the AA/AG genotype
of the OTR rs2254298 polymorphism, which has been associated with
insecure attachment (Chen, Barth, Johnson, Gotlib, & Johnson, 2011) was
also associated with physical, social, and separation anxieties among peo-
ple with adverse childhood experiences. Therefore, specific OT and AVP
polymorphisms may interact with a specific dark triad and environmental
moderator to launch an individual on a pathway for a particular psycho-
pathology.
In addition, dyadic processes may play a role in exacerbating or miti-
gating psychopathology. Specifically, the attachment orientations of a per-
son presenting with psychopathological symptoms may influence his or
her spouse’s symptom severity. For example, research has indicated that
spouses of anxious people may present with greater secondary traumatiza-
tion and more psychopathology-related symptoms because of the tendency
of anxious people to up-regulate their emotional states. Conversely, spouses
of avoidant people may paradoxically present with fewer psychopathology
symptoms because avoidant people tend to down-regulate their emotional
states (e.g., Ein-Dor, Doron, et al., 2010).
Despite these shortcomings, if our model is supported in future stud-
ies, it will enable a more comprehensive yet efficient view on the develop-
mental trajectory linking early environmental influences with adult psy-
chopathology.
References
Adam, E. K., Gunnar, M. R., & Tanaka, A. (2004). Adult attachment, parent
emotion, and observed parenting behavior: Mediator and moderator models.
Child Development, 75, 110–122.
Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion regulation strat-
egies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology
Review, 30, 217–237.
Allen, J. P., Hauser, S. T., & Borman-Spurrell, E. (1996). Attachment theory as a
framework for understanding sequelae of severe adolescent psychopathology:
An 11-year follow-up study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
64, 254–263.
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of
mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: Author.
Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van
IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and
nonanxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133,
1–24.
364 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Barlow, D. H., Farchione, T. J., Fairholme, C. P., Ellard, K. K., Boisseau, C. L.,
Allen, L. B., et al. (2011). The unified protocol for transdiagnostic treat-
ment of emotional disorders: Therapist guide. New York: Oxford Univer-
sity Press.
Bartz, J. A., Zaki, J., Bolger, N., & Ochsner, K. N. (2011). Social effects of oxy-
tocin in humans: Context and person matter. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
15, 301–309.
Batgos, J., & Leadbeater, B. J. (1994). Parental attachment, peer relations, and
dysphoria in adolescence. In M. B. Sperling & W. H. Berman (Eds.), Attach-
ment in adults: Clinical and developmental perspectives (pp. 155–178). New
York: Guilford Press.
Berant, E., Mikulincer, M., & Florian, V. (2001). The association of mothers’
attachment style and their reactions to the diagnosis of infant’s congenital
heart disease. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 20, 208–232.
Berenbaum, H., Raghavan, C., Le, H. N., Vernon, L. L., & Gomez, J. J. (2003). A
taxonomy of emotional disturbances. Clinical Psychology: Science and Prac-
tice, 10, 206–226.
Besser, A., & Neria, Y. (2012). When home isn’t a safe haven: Insecure attachment
orientations, perceived social support, and PTSD symptoms among Israeli
evacuees under missile threat. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research,
Practice, and Policy, 4, 34–46.
Blalock, A., & Joiner, T. (2000). Interaction of cognitive avoidance coping and
stress in predicting depression/anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 24,
47–65.
Blatt, S. J. (1974). Levels of object representation in anaclitic and introjective
depression. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 24, 107–157.
Blatt, S. J., & Blass, R. B. (1996). Interpersonal relatedness and self-definition:
Two basic dimensions in personality development and psychopathology. In G.
G. Noam & K. W. Fischer (Eds.), Development and vulnerabilities in close
relationships (pp. 309–338). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bosquet, M., & Egeland, B. (2006). The development and maintenance of anxiety
symptoms from infancy through adolescence in a longitudinal sample. Devel-
opment and Psychopathology, 18, 517–550.
Bowlby, J. (1944). Forty-four juvenile thieves: Their character and home-life. Inter-
national Journal of Psychoanalysis, 25, 19–52.
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation: Anxiety and anger.
New York: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1978). Attachment theory and its therapeutic implications. In S. C.
Feinstein & P. Giovacchini (Eds.), Adolescent psychiatry: Developmental and
clinical studies (Vol. 6, pp. 5–23). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Vol. 3. Sadness and depression. New
York: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Clinical applications of attachment theory. Lon-
don: Routledge.
Brenning, K. M., Soenens, B., Braet, C., & Bosmans, G. (2012). Attachment and
depressive symptoms in middle childhood and early adolescence: Testing the
validity of the emotion regulation model of attachment. Personal Relation-
ships, 19, 445–464.
Psychopathology and Attachment 365
Brown, G. W., & Harris, T. O. (1986). Establishing causal links: The Bedford Col-
lege studies of depression. In H. Katsching (Ed.), Life events and psychiatric
disorders (pp. 107–187). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Buckley, T. C., Blanchard, E. B., & Neill, W. T. (2000). Information processing
and PTSD: A review of the empirical literature. Clinical Psychology Review,
20, 1041–1065.
Campbell-Sills, L., & Barlow, D. H. (2007). Incorporating emotion regulation
into conceptualizations and treatments of anxiety and mood disorders. In J.
J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 542–559). New York:
Guilford Press.
Caspers, K. M., Cadoret, R. J., Langbehn, D., Yucuis, R., & Troutman, B. (2005).
Contributions of attachment style and perceived social support to lifetime use
of illicit substances. Addictive Behaviors, 30, 1007–1011.
Catalano, R., Aldrete, E., Vega, W., Kolody, B., & Aguilar-Gaxiola, S. (2000). Job
loss and major depression among Mexican Americans. Social Science Quar-
terly, 81, 477–487.
Catanzaro, A., & Wei, M. (2010). Adult attachment, dependence, self-criticism,
and depressive symptoms: A test of a mediational model. Journal of Personal-
ity, 78, 1135–1162.
Charuvastra, A., & Cloitre, M. (2008). Social bonds and posttraumatic stress dis-
order. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 301–328.
Chen, F. S., Barth, M. E., Johnson, S. L., Gotlib, I. H., & Johnson, S. C. (2011).
Oxytocin receptor (OXTR) polymorphisms and attachment in human infants.
Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 200.
Cicchetti, D. (1984). The emergence of developmental psychopathology. Child
Development, 55, 1–7.
Ciechanowski, P. S., Katon, W. J., & Russo, J. E. (2005). The association of depres-
sion and perceptions of interpersonal relationships in patients with diabetes.
Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 58, 139–144.
Ciechanowski, P. S., Sullivan, M., Jensen, M., Romano, J., & Summers, H. (2003).
The relationship of attachment style to depression, catastrophizing and health
care utilization in patients with chronic pain. Pain, 104, 627–637.
Coan, J. A. (2008). Toward a neuroscience of attachment. In J. Cassidy & P. R.
Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical appli-
cations (2nd ed., pp. 241–265). New York: Guilford Press.
Cole-Detke, H., & Kobak, R. (1996). Attachment processes in eating disorder and
depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 282–290.
Collins, N. L., & Feeney, B. C. (2004). Working models of attachment shape per-
ceptions of social support: Evidence from experimental and observational
studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 363–383.
Connor, D. F. (2002). Aggression and antisocial behavior in children and adoles-
cents. New York: Guilford Press.
Cooper, M. L., Wood, P. K., Orcutt, H. K., & Albino, A. (2003). Personality and
predisposition to engage in risky behaviors or problem behaviors during ado-
lescence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 390–410.
Costa, N. M., & Weems, C. F. (2005). Maternal and child anxiety: Do attachment
beliefs or children’s perceptions of maternal control mediate their association?
Social Development, 14, 574–590.
366 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Crawford, T. N., Livesley, W. J., Jang, K. L., Shaver, P. R., Cohen, P., & Gani-
ban, J. (2007). Insecure attachment and personality disorder: A twin study of
adults. European Journal of Personality, 21, 191–208.
Davaji, R. B. O., Valizadeh, S., & Nikamal, M. (2010). The relationship between
attachment styles and suicide ideation: The study of Turkmen students, Iran.
Procedia—Social and Behavioral Sciences, 5, 1190–1194.
Davila, J. (2001). Refining the association between excessive reassurance seeking
and depressive symptoms: The role of related interpersonal constructs. Jour-
nal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 20, 538–559.
Dekel, R., Solomon, Z., Ginzburg, K., & Neria, Y. (2004). Long-term adjustment
among Israeli war veterans: The role of attachment style. Journal of Stress,
Anxiety and Coping, 17, 141–152.
Diamond, L. M., & Aspinwall, L. G. (2003). Emotion regulation across the life
span: An integrative perspective emphasizing self-regulation, positive affect,
and dyadic processes. Motivation and Emotion, 27, 125–156.
Dieperink, M., Leskela, J., Thuras, P., & Engdahl, B. (2001). Attachment style
classification and posttraumatic stress disorder in former prisoners of war.
American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 71, 374–378.
Doron, G., Moulding, R., Kyrios, M., Nedeljkovic, M., & Mikulincer, M. (2009).
Adult attachment insecurities are related to obsessive compulsive phenomena.
Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 28, 1022–1049.
Dozier, M., & Kobak, R. (1992). Psychophysiology in attachment interviews: Con-
verging evidence for deactivating strategies. Child Development, 63, 1473–
1480.
Duggal, S., Carlson, E. A., Sroufe, L. A., & Egeland, B. (2001). Depressive symp-
tomatology in childhood and adolescence. Development and Psychopathol-
ogy, 13, 143–164.
Egeland, B., Pianta, R., & Ogawa, J. (1996). Early behavior problems: Pathways
to mental disorders in adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 8,
735–749.
Ein-Dor, T., Doron, G., Solomon, Z., Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2010).
Together in pain: Attachment-related dyadic processes and posttraumatic
stress disorder. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 57(3), 317–327.
Ein-Dor, T., Mikulincer, M., Doron, G., & Shaver, P. R. (2010). The attachment
paradox: How can so many of us (the insecure ones) have no adaptive advan-
tages? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 123–141.
Ein-Dor, T., Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2011a). Attachment insecurities and
the processing of threat-related information: Studying the schemas involved in
insecure people’s coping strategies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 101, 78–93.
Ein-Dor, T., Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2011b). Effective reaction to dan-
ger: Attachment insecurities predict behavioral reactions to an experimentally
induced threat above and beyond general personality traits. Social Psycho-
logical and Personality Science, 2, 467–473.
Ein-Dor, T., & Orgad, T. (2012). Scared saviors: Evidence that people high in
attachment anxiety are more effective in alerting others to threat. European
Journal of Social Psychology, 42, 667–671.
Ein-Dor, T., & Perry, A. (2014). Full house of fears: Evidence that people high in
Psychopathology and Attachment 367
Lifrak, P. D., McKay, J. R., Rostain, A., Alterman, A. I., & O’Brien, C. P. (1997).
Relationship of perceived competencies, perceived social support, and gender
to substance use in young adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 933–940.
Lipsey, M. W., & Derzon, J. H. (1998). Predictors of violent or serious delinquency
in adolescence and early adulthood: A synthesis of longitudinal research. In
R. Loeber & D. P. Farrington (Eds.), Serious and violent juvenile offenders:
Risk factors and successful interventions (pp. 86–105). Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage.
Livesley, W. J. (1991). Classifying personality disorders: Ideal types, prototypes, or
dimensions? Journal of Personality Disorders, 5, 52–59.
Lorber, M. F., & Egeland, B. (2009). Infancy parenting and externalizing psycho-
pathology from childhood through adulthood: Developmental trends. Devel-
opmental Psychology, 45, 909–912.
Lorber, M. F., & Egeland, B. (2011). Parenting and infant difficulty: Testing a
mutual exacerbation hypothesis to predict early onset conduct problems.
Child Development, 82, 2006–2020.
Mansell, W., Harvey, A., Watkins, E., & Shafran, R. (2009). Conceptual founda-
tions of the transdiagnostic approach to CBT. Journal of Cognitive Psycho-
therapy, 23, 6–19.
Marganska, A., Gallagher, M., & Miranda, R. (2013). Adult attachment, emotion
dysregulation, and symptoms of depression and generalized anxiety disorder.
American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 83, 131–141.
Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (2002). Induced processing biases have causal effects
on anxiety. Cognition and Emotion, 16, 331–354.
Maunder, R. G., Lancee, W. J., Hunter, J. J., Greenberg, G. R., & Steinhart, A.
H. (2005). Attachment insecurity moderates the relationship between disease
activity and depressive symptoms in ulcerative colitis. Inflammatory Bowel
Diseases, 11, 919–926.
McElhaney, K. B., Immele, A., Smith, F. D., & Allen, J. P. (2006). Attachment
organization as a moderator of the link between friendship quality and ado-
lescent delinquency. Attachment and Human Development, 8, 33–46.
McNally, A. M., Palfai, T. P., Levine, R. V., & Moore, B. M. (2003). Attachment
dimensions and drinking-related problems among young adults: The medita-
tional role of coping motives. Addictive Behaviors, 28, 1115–1127.
McNally, R. J. (1999). Panic and phobias. In T. Dalgleish & M. J. Power (Eds.),
Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 479–496). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Mennin, D. S., Holoway, R. M., Fresco, D. M., Moore, M. T., & Heimberg, R. G.
(2007). Delineating components of emotion and its dysregulation in anxiety
and mood psychopathology. Behavior Therapy, 38, 284–302.
Meyer, B., & Pilkonis, P. A. (2005). An attachment model of personality disorders.
In M. F. Lenzenweger & J. F. Clarkin (Eds.), Major theories of personality
disorder (2nd ed., pp. 231–281). New York: Guilford Press.
Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Domes, G., Kirsch, P., & Heinrichs, M. (2011). Oxytocin
and vasopressin in the human brain: Social neuropeptides for translational
medicine. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12, 524–538.
Mikulincer, M., Birnbaum, G., Woddis, D., & Nachmias, O. (2000). Stress and
accessibility of proximity-related thoughts: Exploring the normative and
370 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Simpson, J. A., Kim, J. S., Fillo, J., Ickes, W., Rholes, W. S., Orina, M. M., & Win-
terheld, H. A. (2011). Attachment and the management of empathic accuracy
in relationship-threatening situations. Personality and Social Psychology Bul-
letin, 37, 242–254.
Simpson, J. A., Rholes, W. S., Campbell, L., Tran, S., & Wilson, C. L. (2003).
Adult attachment, the transition to parenthood, and depressive symptoms.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 1172–1187.
Solomon, Z., Ginzburg, K., Mikulincer, M., Neria, Y., & Ohry, A. (1998). Coping
with war captivity: The role of attachment style. European Journal of Person-
ality, 12, 271–285.
Sroufe, L. A. (2005). Attachment and development: A prospective, longitudinal
study from birth to adulthood. Attachment and Human Development, 7,
349–367.
Sroufe, L. A., Duggal, S., Weinfield, N., & Carlson, E. A. (2000). Relation-
ships, development, and psychopathology. In A. J. Sameroff, M. Lewis, &
S. M. Miller (Eds.), Handbook of developmental psychopathology (2nd ed.,
pp. 75–91). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum.
Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., Carlson, E. A., & Collins, A. (2005). The development
of the person: The Minnesota Study of Risk and Adaptation from Birth to
Adulthood. New York: Guilford Press.
Thompson, R. J., Parker, K. J., Hallmayer, J. F., Waugh, C. E., & Gotlib, I. H.
(2011). Oxytocin receptor gene polymorphism (rs2254298) interacts with
familial risk for psychopathology to predict symptoms of depression and anx-
iety in adolescent girls. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 36, 144–147.
Twaite, J. A., & Rodriguez-Srednicki, O. (2004). Childhood sexual and physical
abuse and adult vulnerability to PTSD: The mediating effects of attachment
and dissociation. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 13, 17–38.
Visioli, C., Senatore, R., Lepri, B., & Tondo, L. (2012). Low quality in early attach-
ment bondings as possible predictor of adult depressive disorder. Journal of
Psychopathology, 18, 273–280.
Vungkhanching, M., Sher, K. J., Jackson, K. M., & Parra, G. R. (2004). Relation
of attachment style to family history of alcoholism and alcohol use disorders
in early adulthood. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 75, 47–53.
Wade, T. J., & Brannigan, A. (1998). The genesis of adolescent risk-taking: Path-
ways through family, school and peers. Canadian Journal of Sociology, 23,
1–20.
Warren, S. L., Huston, L., Egeland, B., & Sroufe, L. A. (1997). Childhood anxiety
disorders and attachment. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 637–644.
Wei, M., Heppner, P. P., & Mallinckrodt, B. (2003). Perceived coping as a mediator
between attachment and psychological distress: A structural equation model-
ing approach. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 50, 438–447.
Wei, M., Mallinckrodt, B., Larson, L. M., & Zakalik, R. A. (2005). Attachment,
depressive symptoms, and validation from self versus others. Journal of Coun-
seling Psychology, 52, 368–377.
Whiffen, V. E. (2005). The role of partner characteristics in attachment insecurity
and depressive symptoms. Personal Relationships, 12, 407–423.
Wijngaards-de Meij, L., Stroebe, M., Schut, H., Stroebe, W., van den Bout, J.,
Psychopathology and Attachment 373
van der Heijden, P. G. M., et al. (2007). Patterns of attachment and parents’
adjustment to the death of their child. Personality and Social Psychology Bul-
letin, 33, 537–548.
Zakin, G., Solomon, Z., & Neria, Y. (2003). Hardiness, attachment style, and
long-term distress among Israeli ex-POWs and combat veterans. Personality
and Individual Differences, 34, 819–829.
Zuroff, D. C., & Fitzpatrick, D. K. (1995). Depressive personality styles: Impli-
cations for adult attachment. Personality and Individual Differences, 18,
253–265.
14
Attachment-Related
Preventive Interventions
Mary Dozier
Caroline K. P. Roben
Y oung children are dependent upon their parents for help in reg-
ulating attachment, emotions, behaviors, and physiology. When parents
themselves experience harsh, adverse conditions and/or have histories of
problematic relationships, it is often difficult for them to provide the care
children need to flourish. The consideration of clinical phenomena and
interventions was central to the development of attachment theory (Bowlby,
1944, 1969/1982). Nonetheless, only relatively recently have attachment-
based interventions for parents of young children been developed and tested
through experimental design. This chapter focuses upon early attachment-
based interventions for parents of infants and young children. Enhancing
parenting for infants and young children, especially among parents who
are faced with adversity that compromises caregiving, should be a rich and
productive avenue for change. Indeed, a number of interventions appear to
have important effects on parenting and on child outcomes.
In this chapter, we first review several key principles of attachment,
and describe themes specifically relevant to early intervention. We then
describe several promising preventive interventions for at-risk children
based upon these principles; in particular, we provide an in-depth descrip-
tion of the intervention developed in our lab. We conclude with directions
for future research.
374
Attachment-Related Preventive Interventions 375
for survival. By the time infants are capable of moving away from their
parents, the attachment system has developed fully, leading children to
seek out proximity under threat (Bowlby, 1969/1982). The caregiving sys-
tem functions in complementary ways to the attachment system, serving
to promote parents’ protection of their young under conditions of threat
(George & Solomon, 1999; Insel, 2000). Although parents differ in how
they respond to infants’ distress, most nonetheless behave in ways to pro-
tect their young from danger.
Nurturance
First, young children need nurturing care, and this is especially true for
children who have experienced early adversity. Although children from
low-risk environments can usually organize attachment around the avail-
ability of non-nurturing caregivers, high-risk children have greater diffi-
culty doing so (Cyr, Euser, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & van IJzendoorn,
2010; Dozier, Stovall, Albus, & Bates, 2001). Therefore, an objective of
most attachment-based interventions is to help parents behave in nurturing
ways when their children are distressed.
Parental Delight
Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, and Wall (1978) emphasized the importance
of parents’ feeling and showing delight toward their infants. Bernard and
Dozier (2011) found that foster parents who indicated that they felt more
committed to their foster children showed more delight behaviorally than
parents who indicated that they felt less committed. When parents display
positive affect to their children, children are more likely to display positive
affect themselves (e.g., Forbes, Cohn, Allen, & Lewinsohn, 2004; Tron-
ick, 1989). In contrast, when children’s parents (such as parents who are
depressed) display emotions characterized as either flat or withdrawn, chil-
dren are more likely to have dysregulated emotions and behaviors (e.g.,
Beardslee, Bemporad, Keller, & Klerman, 1983; Cohn & Tronick, 1989;
Field, 1984). Delight is explicitly addressed in some interventions.
Nonfrightening Behavior
When parents behave in frightening ways, children have difficulty orga-
nizing their attachment behaviors (Schuengel, Bakermans-Kranenburg,
& van IJzendoorn, 1999); indeed, such children develop disorganized
attachments at high rates (Carlson, Cicchetti, Barnett, & Braunwald,
1989). Children with disorganized attachments have difficulty regulating
behavior and physiology (Bernard & Dozier, 2010; Hertsgaard, Gunnar,
Erickson, & Nachmias, 1995). Main and Hesse (1990) have suggested
that when parents behave in frightening ways, children experience “fright
without solution” (p. 163) because they are frightened of the persons from
whom they need to seek reassurance. This issue is addressed directly by
some interventions.
comments, described in more detail below, are considered the most critical
aspect of the program.
The ABC intervention has been used with both foster parents and
maltreating birth parents referred by child protective services as part of
a foster care diversion program. The intervention is best suited to parents
of children who are between about 6 and 24 months of age. Although it is
possible to intervene with infants between the ages of birth and 6 months,
the intervention is likely to be more powerful with children older than 6
months, because there are more opportunities for practice on intervention
targets. When the intervention is conducted with newborns and young
infants, frequent napping and fewer spontaneous behaviors (vocalizing,
reaching for objects, etc.) provide parents with relatively few opportunities
to practice targeted behaviors. We are currently testing the efficacy of an
intervention for children older than 24 months, which includes attention
to helping parents help their children to develop self-regulatory strategies
under challenging conditions.
“In-the-Moment” Comments
Parent coaches use “in-the-moment” comments to provide feedback to
parents regarding their behaviors that relate to intervention targets. These
comments focus attention on parents’ opportunities for behaving in nur-
turing and synchronous ways during the sessions, and help them recognize
and practice the key targeted behaviors. Parent coaches are expected to
make in-the-moment comments at least once per minute. Parent coaches
are expected to pause or interrupt themselves during discussions of manual
content to bring the focus of the session back to the parent–child interac-
tion. Manual content is expected to be secondary to in-the-moment com-
ments.
In-the-moment comments can address one or more of three possible
components: describing the parent’s behavior (e.g., “He bumped his head,
and you said, ‘Oh, sweetie, are you OK?’ ”); relating the behavior to an
intervention target (e.g., “That’s such a good example of your providing
her with nurturance”); and/or relating the behavior to long-term outcomes
(e.g., “That’s the kind of thing that will let him know you’ll be there when
he needs you”). The first of these three components, describing the behav-
ior, helps the parent clearly identify the behavior that is addressed. Without
a clear description, parents are sometimes confused about exactly what the
parent coach is focusing upon. Anecdotally, when parent coaches fail to
provide clear descriptions, we have seen parents increase behaviors other
than those intended. The second of these components, relating the behavior
to an intervention target, helps the parent see links between the more gen-
eral concept and specific behaviors. In other words, it makes the discussion
of a general construct (e.g., following the child’s lead, nurturance) more
Attachment-Related Preventive Interventions 381
specific and concrete, which helps the parent associate his or her own cur-
rent actions with the manual content being discussed. The third of these
components, relating the behavior to a child outcome, provides examples
of how the parent’s behavior affects child outcomes. The target for parent
coaches is an average of one component per comment. Although it is pos-
sible (and sometimes desirable) to make comments that include all three
components, it can be overwhelming for all comments to include three
components.
In the first several intervention sessions, parent coaches are expected
to make comments that point out only those times when nurturance, fol-
lowing the child’s lead, or delight are observed (even if this means missing
many opportunities for making comments when parents fail to nurture or
follow the lead). After the first several sessions, when parent coaches have
developed sufficient rapport with the parents, they are expected to begin
to make comments that point out times when parents have neglected to
nurture or follow the lead. These comments can be made gently and in a
scaffolding way: “This is one of those times when he might need you to
pick him up,” or “Are you following or leading right now?”
We have developed a system of coding parent coaches’ in the moment
comments. Any parental behavior that is relevant to an intervention tar-
get is a trigger for a comment. These behaviors include nurturing (or non-
nurturing) behavior, synchronous (or nonsynchronous) behavior, delight,
and nonfrightening (or frightening) behavior. Parent coaches’ behaviors are
coded with regard to whether they commented on each relevant parental
behavior, along with the number of components included in each comment
(description, identification of intervention target, link with child outcome).
An automated spreadsheet calculates rates of on-target commenting, aver-
age number of components included in comments, and a number of other
variables.
Parent coaches are also trained to code their own sessions. A trained
coder provides feedback on the reliability of coding and suggests alterna-
tive strategies for comments. We have been able to use carefully trained
and supervised undergraduate students in this role. The students work with
parent coaches to reach predetermined levels of proficiency by the end of
a year of supervision. In order to be certified as implementing ABC with
fidelity, coaches are expected to reliably make one comment every minute
(or miss fewer than 50% of the opportunities available to make a com-
ment); to be on target with their comments at least 80% of the time (e.g.,
to comment on nurturance when the parent behavior is nurturance instead
of a different type of behavior); and to have an average of at least one com-
ponent (description of the behavior, labeling the target, or providing an
outcome) for every comment. An experienced clinician (PhD) also provides
clinical supervision.
Randomized clinical trials have found ABC to be effective in
382 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Child–Parent Psychotherapy
Alicia Lieberman and colleagues at the University of California, San Fran-
cisco, developed CPP. CPP grew from the Infant–Parent Psychotherapy
intervention developed by Selma Fraiberg (Fraiberg, 1980; Fraiberg, Adel-
son, & Shapiro, 1975), which considered how parents’ “ghosts in the nurs-
ery,” or challenging attachment experiences, affect parenting. Lieberman
adapted CPP to be broader than Infant–Parent Psychotherapy and manual-
ized it (Lieberman, Ghosh Ippen, & Van Horn, 2006). The intervention is
designed for parents and their young children who have experienced chal-
lenging early attachment experiences, including trauma. The intervention
focuses on helping parents become aware of their issues that interfere with
sensitivity to their children’s needs. The intervention works to help par-
ents provide supportive, safe relationships so that children can cope effec-
tively with trauma, and can develop secure, organized attachments. Among
infants, the focus is almost exclusively on parents helping their young chil-
dren cope, but the intervention increasingly includes children as partners as
they become older. Delivered in weekly sessions over about a year, this rela-
tively intensive intervention is intended to help parents gain understanding
of their children’s challenges in a safe, playful context. The intervention is
delivered in the home in some cases and in a clinic-based setting in others.
Through randomized clinical trials, CPP has been shown to reduce
disorganized attachment (Cicchetti, Rogosch, & Toth, 2006), reduce nega-
tive self-representations (Toth, Maughan, Manly, Spagnola, & Cicchetti,
2002), and reduce posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms among children
(Lieberman et al., 2006).
Circle of Security
The Circle of Security (Hoffman, Marvin, Cooper, & Powell, 2006) was
developed by three clinicians in Spokane, Washington: Kent Hoffman,
Glen Cooper, and Bert Powell. For a number of years, Hoffman, Cooper,
and Powell worked with Robert Marvin from the University of Virginia;
Attachment-Related Preventive Interventions 383
more recently, they have worked with Jude Cassidy from the University of
Maryland. Two versions of the Circle of Security have been developed—a
longer version that requires close work with the model developers to imple-
ment, and a briefer, more recent version that is DVD-based and can be
implemented independently following training. Both versions of the inter-
vention use a conceptual model of relationship challenges to focus par-
ents’ and interventionists’ attention. Attachment issues are considered to
represent a “circle of security,” with parents providing a safe haven when
children are distressed, and a secure base when children feel prepared to
explore the world. Children are expected to develop confidence in their
parents’ availability and develop stronger self-esteem when they find that
they can venture out into the world supported by their parents, and that
they can return to their parents for support when needed.
Parents are asked to think about when their own issues interfere more:
when they need to respond to a child’s needs for reassurance, or when they
need to allow the child to move away to explore. In the longer version,
attachment quality is assessed at preintervention to guide and inform the
intervention approach, and at postintervention to assess change. In an
attempt to reduce the time and resources needed for the longer version,
an eight-session parent reflection protocol was developed that uses DVD
footage created by the model’s founders, rather than individualized video
reviews. Groups of parents are encouraged to discuss their own strengths
and struggles in parenting in relation to the video clips of other parents and
children, and to reflect on parenting behaviors that could serve to maintain
attachment problems.
The longer version has been tested in several pre- to postintervention
blinded studies, and has been shown to enhance attachment security (Hoff-
man et al., 2006; Cassidy et al., 2010). At this point, randomized clinical
trials have not been reported in the literature, but such trials are under way
for both the longer and DVD versions of the model.
Interaction Guidance
Susan McDonough developed Interaction Guidance as a brief model using
video feedback to enhance parents’ awareness of infants’ signals and
responses, so that children could develop trusting, secure relationships
(McDonough, 2004). Targeting parent–infant dyads resistant to other
treatments, this model emphasizes the importance of parents’ representa-
tions of the world more generally, and of their infants more specifically, as
the mechanisms of change in parent sensitivity/child attachment. Interven-
tionists are expected to be very positive in their approach to parents, par-
ticularly in making suggestions of alternative explanations for children’s
behaviors.
Robert-Tissot et al. (1996) compared Interaction Guidance with psy-
chodynamic therapy in a randomized clinical trial. Reductions in symptoms
and improvements in mothers’ self-esteem were seen for both groups, with
no significant differences between the groups. In a small matched-com-
parison-group study, Benoit, Madigan, Lecce, Shea, and Goldberg (2001)
found lower levels of maternal atypical behaviors following the interven-
tion among mothers receiving Interaction Guidance than among mothers
receiving a feeding-focused intervention.
and strategies for improving the quantity and quality of comments. Prelim-
inary evidence from eight parent coaches at one training site suggests that
adding the coding feedback to regular small-group supervision increased
the coaches’ rate of commenting across subsequent months (Dozier, Meade,
Wallin, & Bernard, 2013).
Finally, we have established clear, quantified criteria that specify how
frequently we expect to see in the moment comments and the average num-
ber of components expected. Parent coach trainees can therefore gauge
their progress toward these criteria. When they meet criteria, they are cer-
tified as ABC parent coaches. Since the institution of this system, 80–90%
of the parent coaches who have trained in the system have met the criteria
and been certified.
Pre- to postintervention assessments provide support for the inter-
vention’s effectiveness in changing parental sensitivity in the community
(Meade, Dozier, Weston-Lee, & Haggerty, 2014). A randomized clinical
trial of ABC is being conducted at another site, but data are not yet avail-
able.
Conclusion
A number of attachment-based preventive interventions for parents of
young children have been developed in the last several decades. Some of
these interventions focus primarily on the parents’ own attachment issues
that may interfere with caregiving, and others focus primarily on parenting
behaviors themselves. In all interventions, goals involve enhancing paren-
tal sensitivity and responsiveness, as well as reducing frightening behavior.
A growing evidence base supports both of these approaches to enhanc-
ing parental sensitivity and responsiveness, with effects seen on children’s
attachment and self-regulation. This work is exciting in demonstrating the
openness of the caregiving and attachment systems to change.
References
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attach-
ment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Beardslee, W. R., Bemporad, J., Keller, M. B., & Klerman, G. L. (1983). Children
of parents with major affective disorder: A review. American Journal of Psy-
chiatry, 140, 825–832.
Beebe, B., Jaffe, J., Markese, S., Buck, K., Chen, H., Cohen, P., et al. (2010). The
origins of 12-month attachment: A microanalysis of 4-month mother–infant
interaction. Attachment and Human Development, 12, 3–141.
Beebe, B., & Lachmann, F. M. (1984). Representation and internalization in
infancy: Three principles of salience. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 11, 127–165.
388 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Benoit, D., Madigan, S., Lecce, S., Shea, B., & Goldberg, S. (2001). Atypical mater-
nal behavior toward feeding-disordered infants before and after intervention.
Infant Mental Health Journal, 22, 611–626.
Bernard, K., Butzin-Dozier, Z., Rittenhouse, J., & Dozier, M. (2010). Young chil-
dren living with neglecting birth parents show more blunted daytime patterns
of cortisol production than children in foster care and comparison children.
Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 164, 438–443.
Bernard, K., & Dozier, M. (2010). Examining infants’ cortisol responses to labo-
ratory tasks among children varying in attachment disorganization: Stress
reactivity or return to baseline? Developmental Psychology, 46, 1771–1778.
Bernard, K., & Dozier, M. (2011). This is my baby: Foster parents’ feelings of
commitment and displays of delight. Infant Mental Health Journal, 32,
251–262.
Bernard, K., Dozier, M., Bick, J., & Gordon, K. A. (2014). Normalizing blunted
diurnal cortisol rhythms among children at risk for neglect: The effects of an
early intervention. Development and Psychopathology. Advance online pub-
lication.
Bernard, K., Dozier, M., Bick, J., Lewis-Morrarty, E., Lindhiem, O., & Carlson,
E. (2012). Enhancing attachment organization among maltreated infants:
Results of a randomized clinical trial. Child Development, 83, 623–636.
Bernard, K., Hostinar, C., & Dozier, M. (in press). Intervention effects on diurnal
cortisol rhythms of CPS-referred infants persist into early childhood: Pre-
school follow-up results of a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Pediatrics.
Bernard, K., Simons, R., & Dozier, M. (2014). Effects of an attachment-based
intervention on high-risk mothers’ event related potentials to children’s emo-
tions. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Bick, J., & Dozier, M. (2013). The effectiveness of an attachment-based interven-
tion in promoting foster mothers’ sensitivity toward foster infants. Infant
Mental Health Journal, 34, 95–103.
Blair, C., & Raver, C. C. (2012). Child development in the context of adversity:
Experiential canalization of brain and behavior. American Psychologist, 67,
309–318.
Bowlby, J. (1944). Forty-four juvenile thieves: Their character and home-life. Inter-
national Journal of Psychoanalysis, 25, 19–52.
Bowlby J. (1969/1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York: Basic
Books.
Calkins, S. D., & Leerkes, E. M. (2011). Early attachment processes and the devel-
opment of emotional self-regulation. In K. D. Vohs & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.),
Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (2nd ed., pp.
355–373). New York: Guilford Press.
Carlson, V., Cicchetti, D., Barnett, D., & Braunwald, K. (1989). Disorganized/
disoriented attachment relationships in maltreated infants. Developmental
Psychology, 25, 525–531.
Carroll, K. (2012). Evidence-based practices: How far we’ve come, and how much
further we’ve got to go. Addiction, 107, 1031–1033.
Cassidy, J., Ziv, Y., Stupica, B., Sherman, L., Butler, H., Karfgin, A., et al. (2010).
Enhancing maternal sensitivity and attachment security in the infants of
Attachment-Related Preventive Interventions 389
Hertsgaard, L., Gunnar, M., Erickson, M. R., & Nachmias, M. (1995). Adreno-
cortical responses to the Strange Situation in infants with disorganized/disori-
ented attachment relationships. Child Development, 66, 1100–1106.
Hesse, E., & Main, M. (1999). Second-generation effects of unresolved trauma
in nonmaltreating parents: Dissociated, frightened, and threatening parental
behavior. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 19, 481–540.
Hoffman, K. T., Marvin, R. S., Cooper, G., & Powell, B. (2006). Changing tod-
dlers’ and preschoolers’ attachment classifications: The Circle of Security
intervention. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74, 1017–
1026.
Insel, T. R. (2000) Toward a neurobiology of attachment. Review of General Psy-
chology, 4, 176–185.
Johnson, A. E., Bruce, J., Tarullo, A. R., & Gunnar, M. R. (2011). Growth delay as
an index of allostatic load in young children: Predictions to disinhibited social
approach and diurnal cortisol activity. Development and Psychopathology,
23, 859–871.
Johnson, M. H., & Munakata, Y. (2005). Processes of change in brain and cogni-
tive development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 152–188.
Juffer, F., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2008). Pro-
moting positive parenting: An attachment-based intervention. New York:
Erlbaum.
Kelly, J. F., Zuckerman, T., & Rosenblatt, S. (2008). Promoting first relationships:
A relationship-focused early intervention approach. Infants and Young Chil-
dren, 21, 285–295.
Kopp, C. B. (1982). Antecedents of self-regulation: A developmental perspective.
Developmental Psychology, 18, 199–214.
Korfmacher, J., Adam, E., Ogawa, J., & Egeland, B. (1997). Adult attachment:
Implications for the therapeutic process in a home visitation intervention.
Applied Developmental Science, 1, 43–52.
Landsverk, J. (2013). Reflections on treatment integrity in a dissemination and
implementation framework. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 20,
114–119.
Lewis-Morrarty, E., Dozier, M., Bernard, K., Terraciano, S., & Moore, S. (2012).
Cognitive flexibility and theory of mind outcomes among foster children: Pre-
school follow-up results of a randomized clinical trial. Journal of Adolescent
Health, 52, S17–S22.
Lieberman, A. F., Ghosh Ippen, C., & Van Horn, P. (2006). Child–Parent Psycho-
therapy: 6-month follow-up of a randomized controlled trial. Journal of the
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 45, 913–918.
Lind, T., Bernard, K., Ross, E., & Dozier, M. (2014). Intervention effects on nega-
tive affect of CPS-referred children: Results of a randomized clinical trial.
Child Abuse and Neglect, 9, 1459–1467.
Loman, M. M., Johnson, A. E., Westerlund, A., Pollak, S. D., Nelson, C. A., &
Gunnar, M. R. (2013). The effect of early deprivation on executive attention
in middle childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54, 37–45.
Lorenz, L. (1935). Der Kumpan in der Umwelt des Vogels. Journal of Ornithology,
83, 137–213.
Lyons-Ruth, K., & Block, D. (1996). The disturbed caregiving system: Relations
Attachment-Related Preventive Interventions 391
among childhood trauma, maternal caregiving, and infant affect and attach-
ment. Infant Mental Health Journal, 17, 257–275.
Main, M., & Hesse, E. (1990). Parents’ unresolved traumatic experiences are
related to infant disorganized attachment status: Is frightened and/or fright-
ening parental behavior the linking mechanism? In M. T. Greenberg, D. Cic-
chetti, & E. Cummings (Eds.), Attachment in the preschool years: Theory,
research, and intervention (pp. 161–182). Chicago: University of Chicago
Press.
McDonough, S. C. (2004). Interaction guidance. In A. J. Sameroff, S. C.
McDonough, & K. L. Rosenblum (Eds.), Treating parent–infant relationship
problems: Strategies for intervention (pp. 79–96). New York: Guilford Press.
Meade, E. B., Dozier, M., & Bernard, K. (2014). Using video feedback as a tool in
training parent coaches: Promising results from a single-case design. Attach-
ment and Human Development, 16, 356–370.
Meade, E., Dozier, M., Weston-Lee, P., & Haggerty, D. (2014). Effectiveness of
community implementation of Attachment and Biobehavioral Catch-up.
Manuscript submitted for review.
Meade, E., Roben, C. K. P., & Dozier, M. (2013). Screening interventionists for
a parent training program. Unpublished manuscript, University of Delaware.
Nelson, C. A., Zeanah, C. H., Fox, N. A., Marshall, P. J., Smyke, A. T., & Guthrie,
D. (2007). Cognitive recovery in socially deprived young children: The Bucha-
rest Early Intervention Project. Science, 318, 1937–1940.
Nelson, E. M., & Spieker, S. J. (2013). Intervention effects on morning and stimu-
lated cortisol responses among toddlers in foster care. Infant Mental Health
Journal, 34, 211–221.
Perez, S. M., & Gauvain, M. (2007). The sociocultural context of transitions in
early socioemotional development. In C. A. Brownell & C. B. Kopp (Eds.),
Socioemotional development in the toddler years: Transitions and transfor-
mations (pp. 396–419). New York: Guilford Press.
Ramey, C. T., & Ramey, S. L. (1998). Early intervention and early experience.
American Psychologist, 53, 109–120.
Raver, C. C. (1996). Relations between social contingency in mother–child inter-
actions and 2-year-olds’ social competence. Developmental Psychology, 32,
850–859.
Robert-Tissot, C., Cramer, B., Stern, D. N., Serpa, S. R., Bachmann, J. P., Palacio-
Espasa, F., et al. (1996). Outcome evaluation in brief mother–infant psycho-
therapies: Report on 75 cases. Infant Mental Health Journal, 17(2), 97–114.
Schuengel, C., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1999).
Frightening maternal behavior linking unresolved loss and disorganized infant
attachment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67, 54–63.
Shonkoff, J. P., Garner, A. S., & The Committee on Psychosocial Aspects of Child
and Family Health, Committee on Early Childhood, Adoption, and Depen-
dent Care, and Section on Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. (2012).
The lifelong effects of early childhood adversity and toxic stress. Pediatrics,
129, e232–e242.
Shore, R. (1997). Rethinking the brain: New insights into early development. New
York: Families and Work Institute.
Southham-Gerow, M. A., Rodriguez, A., Chorpita, B. F., & Daleiden, E. L. (2012).
392 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Susan M. Johnson
Marie-France Lafontaine
Tracy L. Dalgleish
393
394 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
distressed relationship, often never explicitly stated, are “Are you there for
me; am I important to you; and will you come when I call?” Relationship
conflict is then seen as separation distress that is continually perpetuated
by the negative ways in which a couple deals with attachment signals. As
Bowlby himself suggested, through the lens of attachment every apparently
dysfunctional response to a partner makes sense, and so the multilevel,
often confusing drama of distress is laid bare and becomes amenable to
intervention. Partners criticize to evoke responses from each other, but end
up pushing each other away; or they shut down and withdraw to avoid
rejection, and end up shutting each other out and elicit fears of abandon-
ment. Attachment theory allows therapist to grasp the essential nature of
relationship distress on both individual and interactional levels in a manner
that clients find salient and compelling and creates a path for the curtail-
ment of negative cycles of interaction and the creation of positive bonding
interactions.
The attachment perspective also offers a coherent picture of a healthy,
stable relationship that provides a direction for therapy and keeps the thera-
pist on track in the process of change. EFT is conducted in three stages.
Stage 1, deescalation, involves offering the members of a couple a new
understanding of how they influence each other and what they need from
each other; the therapist helps them to identify negative patterns of inter-
action that constantly elicit disconnection, distance, and despair. Once
partners can help each other out of such cycles, the relationship becomes
a secure base from which to explore the path to deeper connection and
Stage 2, restructuring the bond. Here partners are guided into a process of
becoming more open and responsive to each other, to the point where they
can ask for comfort in regard to their attachment fears and clearly state
their attachment needs in a manner that invites responsiveness. Stage 3, the
last stage, is consolidation, where the partners form a coherent narrative of
how they changed their relationship and how they can continue to enhance
their bond. Process studies of key attachment interactions in Stage 2 inform
therapists of the shifts in emotional processing and interactional responses
that are necessary and sufficient to create lasting change. The goal in EFT
is not simply to lessen negativity and offer a couple some new resources. It
is specifically to shape the emotional synchrony typical of positive bonded
relationships in the session, and to offer the couple a corrective emotional
experience of secure connection. Clarity as to the nature of love and bond-
ing allows for the identification of pivotal moments in therapy—moments
where focused, systematic intervention can help partners create not just a
more generally positive dance, but one that will answer their primary needs
for connection and care, as well as building ongoing commitment and sat-
isfaction.
It is also important to note that this goal, which is admittedly more
ambitious than interventions aimed at simply reducing relationship hostility
Attachment and Couple Interventions 397
bond. These results support the notion that EFT is creating unconscious
changes in attachment security that show up in explicit responses in seek-
ing and providing support in love relationships.
Our team also examined changes in self-reported attachment security
over the course of EFT, using the relationship-specific version of the ECR
(administered pre- and posttherapy) and the ECR-S (administered after
each therapy session). First, we found that as partners’ relationship-specific
attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance decreased, relationship sat-
isfaction increased (Burgess Moser et al., in press). This is consonant with
the theoretical underpinnings of EFT, suggesting that attachment theory
is on target in terms of guiding therapists’ interventions that are effec-
tive in improving relationship satisfaction. On the ECR-S, we found that
reported attachment avoidance significantly decreased over the course of
EFT. This suggests that the interventions in EFT are specifically helping
avoidant partners to develop more adaptive models and perspectives when
interacting with their loved ones. Instead of using deactivation strategies
(such as viewing their partners as dangerous and preferring to shut down
attachment needs), these individuals are learning to view connection with
their partners as a resource, and they begin to depend on them and be
more open to sharing fears and needs with their significant others. These
results highlight the importance of therapists’ helping individuals with
higher levels of attachment avoidance (typical of more withdrawn partners)
at the start of therapy to turn to their partner rather than inward, and so
to develop more adaptive coping mechanisms. These results are contrary
to previous research, which suggests that individuals with avoidant attach-
ment may be more difficult to engage in therapy as a result of their deacti-
vating coping strategies (Meyer, Pilkonis, Proietti, Heape, & Egan, 2001;
Horowitz, Rosenberg, & Bartholomew, 1993). Rather, EFT and therapists’
use of key interventions seem to help such partners modify their internal
working models, which paint others as unsafe and minimize attachment
needs. Based on these results, therapists should take pains to ensure that
withdrawn clients are engaged at the beginning of therapy and are given
help to begin to be open to new perceptions of and revised behaviors from
their partners.
When we looked at attachment anxiety over the course of EFT, our
study at first found no significant change for partners (Burgess Moser et
al., in press). However, in a second study, Burgess Moser et al., 2014 dem-
onstrated that significant changes in attachment anxiety did indeed occur
by the end of therapy for a subset of partners (16 out of 32)—namely, those
who were able to explicitly engage in a key change event in EFT, the blamer-
softening event. The blamer-softening event occurs when a previously hos-
tile/critical partner is able to openly ask for his or her attachment needs to
be met from a position of soft vulnerability and a high level of emotional
engagement (Johnson, 2004; Johnson & Best, 2002). A partner expressing
406 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
needs in this manner pulls the previously withdrawn partner toward him
or her, engages this partner in the process, and enables the partner to hear
and respond to these needs. The blamer-softening event is a corrective emo-
tional experience in which a new level of felt security is experienced with
the partner. Since the withdrawer is helped to reengage before the blamer is
asked to take a risk by openly asking for attachment needs to be met, both
partners are responsive once the blamer-softening event occurs, and are
able to risk, reach, and share attachment vulnerabilities and needs.
The task was now to reach a further understanding of how attach-
ment orientations shift as a result of this key change event (Burgess Moser
et al., 2014), which has been consistently linked to changes in relationship
satisfaction and other positive outcomes at the end of EFT and at follow-up
(Johnson & Greenberg, 1988; Dalgleish et al., 2014). A deeper understand-
ing of this shift and its impact will enable therapists to guide partners confi-
dently as they regulate difficult and unfamiliar affect, mine their emotional
vulnerability, and take risks with their loved ones. Interestingly, we found
that couples who were able to complete a softening (as coded from the
audio-recorded interactions in session) also reported a significant increase
in relationship satisfaction and a decrease in attachment avoidance at the
end of the softening session. Although these couples reported an initial
increase in relationship-specific attachment anxiety in the softening session,
this was followed by a significant decrease in attachment anxiety in the ses-
sions that followed. It seems that anxiously attached partners have pressing
and urgent fears about whether they matter to their loved ones; they are
preoccupied with the fear of abandonment and of being unloved (Collins
& Read, 1990; Davila & Kashy, 2009). For these individuals, the anger/
protest in the negative interactional cycle of relationship distress arises as
a result of not being able to seek comfort or have their normal needs for
contact and intimacy met by their partners (Johnson, 2004). The softening
event appears to disconfirm their belief that their partners will abandon
them, and provides them with an experience of soothing responsiveness
that directly leads to decreased attachment anxiety. Therapy modalities
that emphasize and elaborate the importance of close relationships with
significant others and emotions may fit particularly well with highly anx-
ious individuals (Daniels, 2006). EFT interventions are designed to be
soothing and provide an alternative to these partners’ usual hyperactivating
relationship strategies. In EFT, partners are constantly exploring, access-
ing, and reprocessing emotions such as reactive anger, sadness, loss, shame,
and fears of rejection and abandonment, and formulating their attachment
longings with their partners. Throughout EFT, these partners develop more
emotional balance. They find more positive, less angry, and less controlling
ways of expressing their emotions and needs and of inviting their partners
to engage with them. In a softening event, a previously withdrawn part-
ner’s new accessibility and responsiveness are carefully made explicit by
Attachment and Couple Interventions 407
the EFT therapist, and this actively challenges the more anxiously attached
partner’s cognitive belief that he or she will be abandoned and is essentially
defective and therefore unlovable. This belief is a key element of the nega-
tive model of self in anxious attachment (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007). The
more anxious partner’s awareness of vulnerability and the emotional risk
associated with reaching for a partner in a softening event (Johnson, 2004)
seems to explain the temporary increase in attachment anxiety reported by
such a partner in this specific session.
The results of these two key studies from our lab support the notion
that key events and interventions in the process of change in EFT are
effective in facilitating changes in relationship-specific attachment secu-
rity and relationship satisfaction. It is crucial for therapists to understand
the process of change, so that they are able to select their interventions
appropriately for the many couples dealing with significant issues of inse-
cure attachment and chronic emotional disconnection, and to focus on the
completion of successful softening events where both partners are open,
engaged, and responsive. Understanding the probability that attachment
anxiety will peak in a softening session should help therapists normalize,
validate, and soothe this anxiety. In contrast to the effects on attachment
anxiety, partners’ attachment avoidance slowly decreases in every session
over the entire course of EFT. This result emphasizes the importance of
constantly supporting the more withdrawn member of a couple to slowly
but surely become more actively engaged in the therapeutic process, and to
gradually learn to express emotions to the partner. A therapist should also
ensure that the avoidant, withdrawn partner is able to hear that his or her
lack of emotional presence is a trigger for the other partner to experience
panic and rejection, and that this other indeed truly values and desires the
avoidant partner’s love and care.
These findings support the idea that the softening event acts as a classic
corrective emotional experience, as described in the general psychotherapy
literature (Johnson & Best, 2002). This corrective experience demonstrates
the powerful impact of sharing fears and vulnerabilities in an emotionally
expressive and affiliative manner that elicits attuned caregiving rather than
avoidance or rejection. The softening event is, then, a pivotal moment of
intrapsychic and interpersonal change that therapists must actively shape in
order to create changes in relationship-specific attachment orientations and
relationship satisfaction. Preliminary follow-up analyses suggest that the
changes discussed here, as found in other EFT follow-up studies, remain
stable across time. Once partners have found the path to a deeply satisfying
felt sense of security, they are likely to seek and find this path again and
again. Thus attachment is amenable to change—to the integration of new
experiences, and to revised internal working models of self and other—
throughout EFT in general and through the blamer-softening event in par-
ticular.
408 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
supportive actions, and are less apt to consider others as deserving of their
care, in comparison to individuals embodying other attachment patterns.
Partners with higher levels of attachment anxiety may have difficulty
responding to the needs of their loved ones, as their cognitive resources
tend to be exhausted on their preoccupation with their own distress and
attachment-related needs. In opposition, partners with secure attachment
are not worried with regulating anxiety and doubts about self-worth, and
they have more attention and resources to offer their partners. Securely
attached individuals also perceive their partners to be available in times of
need or distress, and in turn may be more likely to consider their partners as
meriting compassion and help when needed (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007).
In this vein, it is only when a sense of security is established or restored
within a romantic relationship that the caregiving system may be effectively
activated in response to a partner’s distress (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2005).
As mentioned earlier, results from clinical studies support the notion
that EFT provokes changes toward attachment security in partners’ views
of themselves and others. These major changes will then be noticeable in
terms of seeking connection and in terms of sensitive caregiving and con-
tingent responsive behaviors in their relationships. In Stage 1 of EFT, where
members of a couple are guided to reframe their problems in the terms of
how they are stuck in cycles of distance that spark emotional starvation,
separation distress, and deprivation around attachment needs, this meta-
perspective “sets the table” for becoming more attuned and supportive of
each other. For instance, Carol states in session, “I never realized that he
was lonely too and felt rejected. I guess I have complained and blamed a
lot. I feel more generous now, more caring toward him. He needs positive
messages from me. We are not so different after all.” In Stage 2 of EFT, the
therapist helps the withdrawn partner to reengage in the relationship and
to assertively state the conditions of this deeper engagement. The therapist
will also encourage the critical partner to take a more vulnerable position
that facilitates attempts to have the other partner respond to his or her
attachment needs. By the end of Stage 2 of EFT, each partner is more able
to trust and find comfort with the loved one, who is now more accessible,
attentive, and supportive. For example, Ted, Carol’s partner, is able to tell
her, “I want some acceptance from you. I want you to support me when I
am stressed and not assume I am going to let you down. I need caring too.
Then I can let you in.” Later, Carol can softly ask Ted, “When I get all
lonely, I need you to be there. I don’t need advice. I need you to take me in
your arms and really comfort me.” In Stage 3 of EFT, consolidation, part-
ners can actively empathize with each other and develop new solutions to
old problems that take each person’s needs for closeness, security, and car-
ing into account. These problems are no longer tainted with overwhelming
negative emotions and active triggers for rejection and abandonment; they
can be solved cooperatively.
Attachment and Couple Interventions 411
wife to perceive him in a new way and fosters reciprocal sharing about their
sexual and emotional relationship.
Improving attachment security and relationship satisfaction leads to
new avenues in a couple’s sexual interaction. Understanding love as attach-
ment gives a picture of optimal, healthy relatedness and sexuality. In a
secure relationship, positive emotional experiences of joy and excitement,
tender touch, and erotic playfulness can all come together (Johnson & Zuc-
carini, 2010). The first step for the EFT therapist is to increase emotional
safety and secure connection between partners, regardless of whether their
sex life plays a role in their relationship distress (Johnson & Zuccarini,
2010). In Stage 1 of EFT, the therapist will first explore the quality of
the couple’s physical relationship and integrate this information into the
context of the negative interaction cycle. Here it is important for the EFT
therapist to give an attachment frame to partners’ sexual responses—for
instance, by connecting the lack of satisfaction and difficulty having an
orgasm to the lack of safety and fears of abandonment. If partners do not
report any sexual difficulty, but sex has deteriorated as a result of the nega-
tive cycle, their sex life begins to improve at the end of Stage 1, when both
partners can work together against the negative impact of their cycle both
inside and outside the bedroom. In Stage 2, the EFT therapist helps the
partners initiate positive cycles of emotional responsiveness, as they are
able to risk, confide attachment needs and fears (i.e., physical closeness and
sexuality), and reach for and respond to each other. In a case where sexual-
ity is experienced as dangerous, partners will be invited to preclude having
intercourse and focus on safe, pleasurable touch. In order to normalize
sexual experiences, the EFT therapist may need to offer the couple some
information, with the goal of supporting the transfer of safe emotional
engagement and exploration into the sexual area. For example, a husband
may be reassured to find out that orgasms vary in intensity and character,
and that it’s perfectly natural for him to feel different from time to time.
Mutual accessibility and responsiveness between partners helps them to
engage in a new kind of satisfying and connected sexual experience. In
Stage 3 of EFT, the therapist helps partners to create a joint story of the
repair of their relationship that includes the enhancement of their sexual
bond. The therapist may also help partners solve concrete problems, such as
modifying a lifestyle that excludes time for enjoyable and pleasurable sexual
play. Satisfying sexual encounters now strengthen the couple’s bond, and a
more secure bond continues to build more erotic and more satisfying sex.
In sum, by creating changes in attachment security, EFT helps roman-
tic partners alter the explicit ways in which they seek support and provide
support and care, as well as their sexual connection. Couple therapists will
find in EFT a powerful guide that can help partners integrate attachment,
caregiving, and sex, in a way that leads to a powerful, resilient, and satisfy-
ing bond.
Attachment and Couple Interventions 413
1 The case and the incident described in this section were first outlined in modified form
in Johnson (2013).
414 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
and other. Both are more trusting and able to deal with their emotions in a
way that fosters open engagement and allows for empathic responsiveness
to each other. Both now frame themselves as more confident and compe-
tent, and as able to offer more sensitive caregiving. Larry reports being less
driven to exercise compulsively and less anxious in the relationship. Prue
emerges from her clinical depression and becomes more assertive about her
own needs with Larry and with others. In the final session, the partners
also report that their new ways to communicate with each other seem
to have improved their sexual relationship. In summary, the deepening
of key attachment-oriented experiences in this case has resulted in new
perspectives and new ways to send signals to each other that have pulled
these partners closer and shaped a new dance of mutual accessibility and
responsiveness—the elements of a secure bond.
Conclusion
Attachment theory and science are changing the way we view and treat
adult love relationships. Attachment offers a systematic protocol for rela-
tionship repair that has already proven effective on many different levels
and is more and more broadly adopted by couple therapists across the
globe. It also expands the scope of couple therapy as a modality. If couple
therapy can help partners not only repair their relationships, but shift from
basically insecure working models and affect regulation strategies to secure
connection, this therapy modality can begin a cascade of change and indi-
vidual growth, evoking all the positive effects associated with more secure
loving bonds. This not only offers a new direction for couple therapists and
their clients; it also validates attachment theorists and researchers in their
formulations of exactly how the most precious connections people have
with others work, and how they may be honored and fostered in the future.
References
Acevedo, B., & Aron, A. (2009). Does a long-term relationship kill romantic love?
Review of General Psychology, 13, 59–65.
Baldwin, M. W., Keelan, J. P. R., Fehr, B., Enns, V., & Koh-Rangarajoo, E. (1996).
Social-cognitive conceptualization of attachment working models: Availabil-
ity and accessibility effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71,
94–109.
Basson, R. (2007). Sexual desire arousal disorders in women. In S. Leiblum (Ed.),
Principles and practice of sex therapy (4th ed., pp. 25–53). New York: Guil-
ford Press.
Benjamin, L. S. (1981). Manual for coding social interaction in terms of Structural
Analysis of Social Behavior (SASB). Madison: University of Wisconsin Press.
416 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Collins, N. L., Guichard, A. C., Ford, M. B., & Feeney, B. C. (2006). Responding
to need in intimate relationships: Normative processes and individual differ-
ences. In M. Mikulincer & G. S. Goodman (Eds.), Dynamics of romantic
love: Attachment, caregiving, and sex (pp. 149–189). New York: Guilford
Press.
Collins, N. L., & Read, S. J. (1990). Adult attachment models and relationship
quality in dating couples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58,
644–663.
Crowell, J. A., Treboux, D., Gao, Y., Fyffe, C., Pan, H., & Waters, E. (2002).
Assessing secure base behavior in adulthood: Development of a measure, links
to adult attachment representations, and relations to couples’ communication
and reports of relationships. Developmental Psychology, 38, 679–693.
Crowell, J. A., Treboux, D., & Waters, E. (2002). Stability of attachment represen-
tations: The transition to marriage. Developmental Psychology, 38, 467–479.
Dalgleish, T. L., Johnson, S. M., Burgess Moser, M., Wiebe, S. A., & Tasca, G. A.
(2014). Predicting key change events in emotionally focused couple therapy.
Journal of Marital and Family Therapy. Advance online publication.
Dalton, J., Greenman, P., Classen, C., & Johnson, S. M. (2013). Nurturing con-
nections in the aftermath of childhood trauma: A randomized control trial of
emotionally focused couple therapy (EFT) for female survivors of childhood
abuse. Couple and Family Psychology: Research and Practice, 2(3), 209–221.
Daniels, S. I. F. (2006). Adult attachment patterns and individual psychotherapy: A
review. Clinical Psychology Review, 26, 968–984.
Davila, J., & Cobb, R. (2003). Predicting change in self-reported and interviewer-
assessed attachment security: Tests of the individual and life-stress model.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 859–870.
Davila, J., & Cobb, R. (2004). Predictors of change in attachment security dur-
ing adulthood. In W. S. Rholes & J. A. Simpson (Eds.), Adult attachment:
Theory, research, and clinical implications (pp. 133–156). New York: Guil-
ford Press.
Davila, J., Karney, B. R., & Bradbury, T. N. (1999). Attachment change processes
in the early years of marriage. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
76, 783–802.
Davila, J., & Kashy, D. A. (2009). Secure base processes in couples: Daily associa-
tions between support experiences and attachment security. Journal of Family
Psychology, 23, 76–88.
Davis, D., Shaver, P. R., & Vernon, M. L. (2004). Attachment style and subjective
motivations for sex. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1076–
1090.
Denton, W., Wittenborn, A., & Golden, R. (2012). Augmenting antidepressant
medication treatment of depressed women with emotionally focused therapy
for couples: A randomized pilot study. Journal of Marital and Family Ther-
apy, 38, 23–38.
Dewitte, M. (2012). Different perspectives on the sex–attachment link: Towards an
emotion-motivational account. Journal of Sex Research, 49, 105–124.
Diamond, D., Stovall-McCloush, C., Clarkin, J. F., & Levy, K. (2003). Patient–
therapist attachment in the treatment of borderline personality disorder. Bul-
letin of the Menninger Clinic, 67, 227–260.
418 ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH
Eisenberger, N., Lieberman, M., Matthew, D., & Williams K. (2003). Does rejec-
tion hurt?: An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science, 302, 290–293.
Ekman. P. (2003). Emotions revealed. New York: Holt.
Feeney, B. C., & Collins, N. L. (2001). Predictors of caregiving in adult intimate
relationships: An attachment theoretical perspective. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 80, 972–994.
Feeney, J. A. (1996). Attachment, caregiving, and marital satisfaction. Personal
Relationships, 3, 401–416.
Fonagy, P., Steele, M., Steele, H., Leigh, T., Kennedy, R., Matton, G., et al. (1995).
Attachment, the reflective self and borderline states: The predictive specificity
of the Adult Attachment Interview and pathological emotional development.
In S. Goldberg, R. Muir, & J. Kerr (Eds.), Attachment theory: Social, develop-
mental and clinical perspectives (pp. 233–279). Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press.
Fosha, D. (2000). The transforming power of affect: A model for accelerated
change. New York: Basic Books.
Fraley, R. C. (2002). Attachment stability from infancy to adulthood: Meta-analysis
and dynamic modeling of developmental mechanisms. Personality and Social
Psychology Review, 6, 123–151.
Fraley, R. C., Vicary, A. M., Brumbaugh, C. C., & Roisman, G. I. (2011). Patterns
of stability in adult attachment: An empirical test of two models of continu-
ity and change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 974–992.
Gottman, J., Coan, J., Carriere, S., & Swanson, C. (1998). Predicting marital hap-
piness and stability from newlywed interactions. Journal of Marriage and the
Family, 60, 5–22.
Greenman, P., & Johnson, S. M. (2013). Process research on emotionally focused
therapy: Linking theory to practice. Family Process, 52, 46–61.
Gross, P. (2001). Emotional regulation in adulthood: Timing is everything. Cur-
rent Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 214–219.
Hawkley, L., Masi, C., Berry, J., & Cacioppo, J. (2006). Loneliness is a unique
predictor of age-related differences in systolic blood pressure. Journal of Psy-
chology and Aging, 21, 152–164.
Heiman, J. (2007). Orgasmic disorders in women. In S. Leiblum (Ed.), Principles
and practice of sex therapy (4th ed., pp. 84–123). New York: Guilford Press.
Horowitz, L. M., Rosenberg, S. E., & Bartholomew, K. (1993). Interpersonal prob-
lems, attachment styles, and outcome in brief dynamic psychotherapy. Jour-
nal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 549–560.
Johnson, S. M. (2004). The practice of emotionally focused couple therapy: Creat-
ing connection. New York: Routledge.
Johnson, S. M. (2008a). Hold me tight: Seven conversations for a lifetime of love.
New York. Little, Brown.
Johnson, S. M. (2008b). Couple and family therapy: An attachment perspective. In
J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research
and clinical applications (2nd ed., pp. 811–829). New York: Guilford Press.
Johnson, S. M. (2009a). Extravagant emotion: Understanding and transforming
love relationships in emotionally focused therapy. In D. Fosha, D. Siegel, &
M. Solomon (Eds.), The healing power of emotion: Affective neuroscience,
development and clinical practice (pp. 257–279). New York: Norton.
Johnson, S. M. (2009b). Attachment and emotionally focused therapy: Perfect
Attachment and Couple Interventions 419
Singer, J. D., & Willett, J. B. (2003). Applied longitudinal data analysis: Meth-
ods for studying change and event occurrence. New York: Oxford University
Press.
Spanier, G. B. (1976). Measuring relational adjustment: New scales for assessing
the quality of marriage and similar dyads. Journal of Marriage and the Fam-
ily, 38, 15–28.
Stefanou, C., & McCabe, M. P. (2012). Adult attachment and sexual functioning:
A review of past research. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 9, 2499–2507.
Tasca, G. A., Balfour, L., Ritchie, K., & Bissada, H. (2007). Change in attachment
anxiety is associated with improved depression among women with binge eat-
ing disorder. Psychotherapy, 44, 423–433.
Tracy, J. L., Shaver, P. R., Albino, A. W., & Cooper, M. L. (2003). Attachment
styles and adolescent sexuality. In P. Florsheim (Ed.), Adolescent romantic
relations and sexual behavior: Theory, research, and practical implications
(pp. 137–159). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Travis, L. A., Binder, J. L., Bliwise, N. G., & Horne-Moyer, H. L. (2001). Changes
in clients’ attachment orientations over the course of time-limited dynamic
psychotherapy. Psychotherapy, 38, 149–159.
Wallin, D. J. (2007) Attachment in psychotherapy. New York: Guilford Press.
Wei, M., Russell, D. W., Mallinckrodt, B., & Vogel, D. L. (2007). The Experiences
in Close Relationships Scale (ECR)—Short Form: Reliability, validity, and
factor structure. Journal of Personality Assessment, 88, 187–204.
Zuccarini, D., Johnson, S. M., Dalgleish, T. L., & Makinen, J. A. (2013). Forgive-
ness and reconciliation in emotionally focused therapy for couples: The client
change process and therapist interventions. Journal of Marital and Family
Therapy, 39, 148–162.
Author Index
423
424 Author Index
Boomsma, D. I., 101 Brumbaugh, C. C., 11, 44, 238, 279, 291, 402
Boone, A. L., 289 Brunstein, J. C., 209, 210, 220
Booth-LaForce, C., 17, 18, 20, 33, 34, 51, 279 Bruyneel, S., 87
Borghini, A., 106 Bubier, J. L., 108
Borman, W. C., 277 Buchheim, A., 45
Borman-Spurrell, E., 354 Buckley, T. C., 357
Bornstein, R. F., 199, 200 Bugental, D. B., 292
Bos, P. A., 87 Buhrmester, D., 206
Bosmans, G., 298, 350 Bunge, S. A., 44
Bosquet, M., 350 Burchinal, M. R., 14
Bouaziz, A.-R., 409 Bureau, J. F., 402
Bouchey, H. A., 17 Burge, D., 113
Bouthillier, D., 148 Burgess Moser, M., 399, 401, 402, 404, 405,
Bowlby, J., 1, 7, 10, 11, 12, 40, 42, 44, 52, 68, 406
69, 71, 97, 112, 124, 125, 126, 138, 146, Burkett, J. P., 54, 60
148, 161, 170, 171, 173, 187, 196, 197, Burman, B., 252
198, 200, 201, 203, 206, 207, 223, 234, Burt, K. B., 33
235, 236, 252, 261, 279, 319, 320, 321, Burt, S. A., 32, 51
322, 323, 329, 330, 339, 346, 348, 350, Burton, J. P., 278
353, 359, 374, 375, 376, 393, 397, 398, Burton, S., 80, 183
401, 402, 403, 404, 409 Busby, D. M., 84
Boyce, W. T., 33, 107, 108, 110, 291, 302 Bush, J. P., 129
Boyd, C., 325 Buss, C., 101
Boyle, A. M., 307 Buss, D. M., 76, 175, 182, 183
Bradbury, T. N., 31, 107, 203, 394, 402 Buss, K., 105, 292, 293
Bradley, B. P., 352, 357 Butterfield, R. M., 299, 308
Bradley, J. M., 328, 334 Butzer, B., 181
Bradshaw, D., 170, 408 Butzin-Dozier, Z., 379
Braet, C., 298, 350 Buysse, A., 106
Brammer, G. L., 51 Buysse, D. J., 304
Brannigan, A., 353 Byers, E. S., 175
Branstetter, S. A., 298 Byrd-Craven, J., 102
Brassard, A., 174, 176, 181, 409
Bratslavsky, E., 186 Cacioppe, R., 266
Brauer, M., 48 Cacioppo, J. T., 98, 100, 205, 305, 394
Braun, M., 326, 333 Cadoret, R. J., 354
Braunwald, K., 378 Cafasso, L., 324
Bredin, S. S. D., 209 Cafferty, T. P., 328, 334
Breiner, T., 108 Caldwell, J. D., 80
Breiter H. C., 76 Caldwell, J. G., 246
Brennan, K. A., 41, 126, 172, 174, 237, 238, Calhoun, V. D., 57
249, 263, 333, 404 Calkins, S. D., 100, 102, 376
Brennan, P. L., 358 Cameron, J. J., 334
Brenning, K. M., 350 Campbell, K., 300
Bretherton, I., 11, 44, 71, 237 Campbell, L., 148, 152, 178, 179, 181, 185,
Brett, B. E., 251 252, 288, 320, 351, 357, 403
Brewer, A., 246 Campbell, W. K., 154
Brodaty, H., 339 Campbell-Sills, L., 355
Brodersen, L., 292 Canterberry, M., 43, 46, 57, 58, 132, 322, 323
Brody, G. H., 299 Cantor, J. M., 79
Brook, A. T., 224 Carlson, D., 274
Brooks, K. P., 297 Carlson, E., 375
Brown, G. W., 360 Carlson, E. A., 21, 346, 347, 351
Brown, L. L., 72, 184 Carlson, V., 378
Brown, S. L., 289 Carmichael, C. L., 304
Browne, R. C., 304 Carmichael, M. S., 80, 183
Bruce, J., 103, 375 Carnelley, K. B., 132, 206, 288, 289, 306
Brumbach, B. H., 30 Caron, A., 402, 409
426 Author Index
Eastwick, P. W., 187 Fehr, B., 42, 68, 72, 113, 129, 402
Edelstein, R. S., 239, 246, 249, 251 Feiring, C., 113
Edens, J. L., 205 Feldman, R., 54, 377, 378
Egan, M., 405 Feldt, T., 274
Egeland, B., 12, 21, 105, 107, 112, 215, 321, Felitti, V. J., 291
346, 347, 350, 351, 354, 384 Felmlee, D., 160
Ehrlich, K. B., 250, 251 Ferenz-Gillies, R., 148, 358
Eiler, W. J., 82 Ferguson, M., 274
Ein-Dor, T., 5, 6, 308, 346, 347, 352, 353, 356, Fernandes, C., 243
357, 358, 361, 363 Fernández-Guasti, A., 82
Einstein, A., 346 Ferris, C. F., 54
Eisenberger, N. I., 47, 58, 73, 130, 398 Festjens, A., 87
Ekman, P., 398 Fibiger, H. C., 76
El-Hodiri, M., 59 Field, T., 378
Elicker, J., 15 Fieldstone, A., 100
Elklit, A., 243 Figueredo, A. J., 30
Elliot, A. J., 42, 199 Filippi, S., 183
Ellis, B. J., 30, 33, 98, 107, 108, 110, 175 Fincham, F. D., 203
Ellison, P. T., 187 Fine, M., 200
Elmore-Staton, L., 107 Finkel, E. J., 154, 157, 187
El-Sheikh, C. D., 108 Finnegan, H., 334
El-Sheikh, M., 100, 107, 108, 110 Finzi-Dottan, R., 242, 243, 247
Emerson, E., 298 Fishbach, A., 210
Emilien, G., 131 Fisher, H., 72, 182, 184
Emmons, R. A., 209 Fisher, J. D., 205
Eng, W., 350 Fisher, P., 102, 103
Engdahl, B., 352 Fishman, E., 205
Engler, P. A., 358 Fitzgerald, M. E., 79
Englund, M. M., 20, 302 Fitzpatrick, D. K., 351
Enns, V., 42, 68, 113, 402 Fitzsimons, G. M., 210
Enrico, P., 77 Fleming, C. B., 385
Erdly, W. W., 218 Fleming, W., 148, 358
Erel, O., 252 Fletcher, G. J. O., 137, 149
Erez, A., 277 Fletcher, L., 301
Erickson, A., 72 Florian, V., 111, 129, 162, 238, 242, 243, 245,
Erickson, M. F., 292, 384 247, 249, 265, 267, 349, 351, 352, 354
Erickson, M. R., 378 Foddy, M., 319
Esbjørn, B. H., 349 Folkman, S., 203
Eskenazi, B., 107 Fonagy, P., 54, 55, 56, 97, 251, 393, 403
Euser, E. M., 377 Fontana, A. M., 205
Evans, L., 319, 324, 325, 326, 327, 328, 334, Forbes, E. E., 378
338 Ford, C. S., 182
Eysenck, M. W., 357 Ford, M. B., 71, 73, 203, 205, 222, 408
Fosha, D., 393
Fagundes, C. P., 50, 110, 294, 295, 296, 307 Foshee, V., 298
Faig, H. G., 101 Foster, C. A., 154
Farber, E., 14, 215 Fowler, J. C., 350
Farrell, A., 129, 159 Fowler, R. S., 300
Farrington, D. P., 361 Fox, L., 324
Fazzari, D. A., 352 Fraiberg, S., 382
Fearon, R. M. P., 32, 289 Fraley, R. C., 2, 9, 11, 12, 15, 18, 20, 26, 27,
Feeney, B. C., 3, 15, 71, 135, 136, 137, 146, 28, 29, 32, 34, 44, 51, 52, 107, 113, 126,
157, 195, 196, 198, 199, 200, 201, 202, 158, 187, 238, 249, 279, 291, 330, 333,
203, 205, 206, 207, 208, 209, 210, 214, 346, 352, 402
215, 216, 218, 219, 222, 223, 224, 238, Frank, E., 304
241, 289, 305, 307, 323, 357, 359, 408 Frankenhuis, W. E., 111
Feeney, J. A., 173, 174, 176, 206, 240, 241, Franks, M. M., 299
243, 299, 303, 333, 346, 408 Fredrickson, B. L., 128
Author Index 429
Warren, S. L., 46, 47, 55, 350 Winterheld, H. A., 135, 158, 203, 289
Waters, E., 13, 40, 72, 107, 113, 125, 126, 128, Wittenberg, M. T., 206
196, 237, 321, 331, 332, 378, 403 Wittenborn, A., 397
Waters, H. S., 128, 331, 332, 333 Woddis, D., 354
Watkins, E. R., 347, 348, 354, 357, 360 Wolchik, S. A., 209
Waugh, C. E., 363 Wood, P. K., 358
Way, B. M., 291 Woodhouse, S. S., 333
Wayne, S. J., 269 Woods, R. T., 327
Wearing, A. J., 209 Woolley, S. C., 75
Webster, G. D., 299 Wouden-Miller, M., 102
Webster, J. D., 326 Wright, J., 181, 409
Weems, C. F., 349 Wu, X., 17
Wei, M., 173, 288, 347, 349, 350, 404 Wust, S., 101
Weimer, B. L., 299
Weinfield, N., 107, 113, 347 Yammarino, F. J., 267
Weir, L. O., 205 Yang, F. M., 324
Weisberg, Y. J., 178, 179, 180, 184 Yetim, U., 209
Weiss, E. M., 269, 277 Young, E. A., 74, 82
Weiss, R. S., 329 Young, K. A., 182
Weizman, A., 247 Young, L. J., 51, 53, 54, 60, 73, 76, 77, 83,
Weller, A., 54, 351 183
Wendelken, C., 44 Youngblade, L. M., 16
Wenkstern, D., 76 Younger, J., 78, 131
Wens, J., 339 Yu, L., 72, 79
Westen, D., 352 Yucuis, R., 354
Weston-Lee, P., 387
Wettstein, A., 327 Zagoory-Sharon, O., 54
Wewerka, S. S., 103 Zajacova, A., 296
Whiffen, V. E., 351 Zak, P. J., 73
Whipple, B., 78 Zakalik, R. A., 173, 288, 350
Whitaker, C. J., 326 Zakay, D., 267
Whitaker, R. C., 298 Zaki, J., 363
Whitcher, S. J., 205 Zakin, G., 352
Whitehead, C., 58 Zaleski, Z., 209
Whitney, G. A., 154 Zautra, A. J., 304
Whitson, S. A., 100 Zayas, V., 2, 3, 11, 13, 42, 48, 68, 69, 71, 73,
Whitten, D., 274 81, 130
Wiebe, S. A., 399 Zeanah, C. H., 375
Wiedenmayer, C. P., 74 Zeifman, D., 16, 81, 170, 171, 173, 182
Wieselquist, J., 154 Zeki, S., 54, 72
Wijngaards-de Meij, L., 352, 361 Zerach, G., 241
Wilkinson, R. B., 333 Zhang, F., 107, 113
Willett, J. B., 404 Zhang, Q., 362
Williams, E., 150 Zhang, T. Y., 77
Williams, I. C., 324 Zhang, X., 43, 48
Williams K., 398 Zhou, E. S., 99, 292
Willoughby, B. J., 84 Zhou, X., 43
Wills, T. A., 203, 209 Ziegler, A., 187
Wilson, C. L., 242, 244, 246, 252, 288, 303, Zilber, A., 48
304, 320, 351, 403 Zivnuska, S., 274
Wilson, H. W., 298 Zizi, F., 304
Wilson, M., 58 Zuccarini, D., 400, 409, 411, 412
Wilson, P., 102 Zuckerman, T., 385
Windle, R. J., 74 Zuroff, D. C., 138, 351
Winstead, B. A., 205 Zwambag, L., 350
Subject Index
The letter f following a page number indicates figure; the letter t indicates table.
441
442 Subject Index
Attachment styles. See also Anxious health and, 293, 299–300, 303–304,
attachment style; Attachment processes; 304–305
Avoidant attachment style; Insecure immune system and, 297
attachment; Secure attachment neuroscience and, 55
adolescence and, 250–251 organizational settings and, 268,
adult attachment system and, 237–238 271–272, 273–274, 281–282
animal models and, 53–54 parenting and, 239–240, 249–251
EFT and the stability of, 402–407 regulation by the partner, 161–165
health and, 288–291, 290f, 295–296, regulation of the partner, 157–161,
298–300 164–165
in later life and aged care, 328–335 regulation processes and, 146–147, 147f
organizational settings and, 273–274, secure-base script and, 128
279–282 stress and, 105–106
overview, 41–42 transdiagnostic model and, 358–359
parenting and, 234–237, 239–248, Avoidant attachment style. See also
250–251, 252 Attachment styles; Avoidance; Insecure
sympathetic–adrenal medullary (SAM) attachment
axis and, 295–296 adolescence and, 250–251
Attachment systems, 270–272 adult attachment system and, 238
Attachment theory aged care and, 326, 337–338
aged care and, 320–323 genetic factors and, 51
neuroscience and, 40–42 health and, 288–291, 299–300, 304–305
organizational research and, 264–279 immune system and, 297
overview, 124–127, 375, 393–395 neuroimaging technologies and, 46,
Attachments, 68–70 47–48, 49–50
Attention, 44–45, 60 overview, 41–42
Attitudes, 58f, 270–276 parenting and, 246, 249–251
Attraction, 69–70, 171. See also Sexual psychopathology and, 346–347, 348–354
mating system regulation by the partner, 161–165
Authoritarian parenting, 241. See also regulation of the partner, 157–161,
Parenting 164–165
Autonomic nervous system (ANS) sexual mating system and, 88, 174–175,
adult attachment system and, 73–74 177, 179
attachment insecurity and, 105–108 transdiagnostic model of, 354–362, 355f
differential susceptibility and adaptive Avoidant personality disorder, 347
calibration and, 108–112
early caregiving and, 101–104 Basal ganglia, 49, 49–50
health and, 290f Bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST),
role of sex in adult attachment formation, 73–74, 82–83
82–83 Behavior inhibition, 292–293
stress and, 97–98, 99–104, 105–108 Behavioral genetics, 50–51. See also Genetic
Autonomy factors
early attachment experiences and, 198 Behavioral system. See also Attachment
optimal dependence and, 204f behavioral system
romantic relationships and, 136–137 aged care and, 322–323
Avoidance. See also Avoidant attachment health and, 290f, 298–300
style neuroscience and, 40–42, 58f
adolescence and, 250–251 Big Five traits, 261–262
attachment system and, 71–72, 126–127, Biological factors, 54–55
238 Biological sensitivity to context theory,
couple therapy and, 398–399, 409–410 33–34, 107–108
genetic factors and, 51 Blamer-softening event, 404–405
444 Subject Index
Bonding. See also Pair bonding; Sexual pair immune system and, 296–297
bonding sympathetic–adrenal medullary (SAM)
animal models and, 53–54 axis and, 295–296
couple therapy and, 408–412 Child–Parent Psychotherapy (CPP), 379, 382
emotionally focused therapy (EFT) and, Cigarette smoking, 298
402 Circle of Security intervention, 379, 382–383
neuroscience and, 54 Classic themes in the attachment field, 2–4
Bonds, attachment. See also Bonding Client Attachment to Therapist Scale, 270
neuroimaging technologies and, 45–46 Cognitive mechanisms, 44–45, 58f, 247–248
relationships and, 196–204 Communication, 307–308
Borderline personality disorder, 347 Compassion, 238
Brain volume, 49 Compulsively overinvolved support,
Broaden-and-build cycle 211–215, 212f
interpersonal contexts, 135–138 Computed tomography (CT) scans, 43
overview, 128–129 Concentric mapping approaches, 329
security priming and, 130–135 Conditioning, 70, 72–73
Burnout, 270–272 Conduct behavior problems, 353–354
Confidence, 207
Cardiovascular disease, 305–306 Conflict
Care receiving, 326–327, 336–339 parenting and, 240
Care seeking regulation by the partner, 162–163
aged care and, 326–327, 336–339 regulation of the partner, 159–160
health and, 307–308 sexual mating system and, 177–180
Caregivers, 40–41, 375. See also Attachment Consolidation stage of EFT, 396
figure Consummatory phase, 75, 77–80
Caregiving environment. See also Early Control, 241
attachment experiences; Environmental Coping strategies
factors; Parenting neglectful/disengaged support and, 223
adult attachment system and, 139 optimal dependence and, 204f
aged care and, 321–322, 325–328, organizational settings and, 272
336–339 parenting and, 241
couple therapy and, 408–412 transdiagnostic model and, 359
developmental cascades and, 24–25, 24f Corporal punishment, 240–241
developmental trajectories and, 12 Corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH),
genetic factors and, 31–34 73–74
health and, 307–308 Cortisol
intervention and, 376–379 adult attachment system and, 73–74
optimal dependence and, 202–210, 204f health and, 293
research regarding, 239–248 role of sex in adult attachment formation,
security priming and, 133 82–83
unpredictability and valance of, 30–31 stress and, 98–99
Caregiving system Counseling, 138
adult attachment system and, 238 Counterproductive work behaviors (CWBs),
aged care and, 336–339 278
overview, 236 Couple therapy. See also Relationships
Caring behavior, 241 attachment-oriented innovations in,
Catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT), 397–401
53f, 60 caregiving and sex and, 408–412
Childhood attachment. See also Early case example of, 413–415
attachment experiences overview, 393–394, 415
health and, 295, 300–303, 309 secure base for the practice of, 395–397
health behavior and, 298–299 stability of attachment and, 402–407
Subject Index 445