The Therapeutic Process in Psychodynamic Therapy With Children With Different Capacities For Mentalizing
The Therapeutic Process in Psychodynamic Therapy With Children With Different Capacities For Mentalizing
The Therapeutic Process in Psychodynamic Therapy With Children With Different Capacities For Mentalizing
Vera Regina Rohnelt Ramires, Cibele Carvalho, Rodrigo Gabbi Polli, Geoff
Goodman & Nick Midgley
To cite this article: Vera Regina Rohnelt Ramires, Cibele Carvalho, Rodrigo Gabbi Polli,
Geoff Goodman & Nick Midgley (2020) The Therapeutic Process in Psychodynamic Therapy
with Children with Different Capacities for Mentalizing, Journal of Infant, Child, and Adolescent
Psychotherapy, 19:4, 358-370, DOI: 10.1080/15289168.2020.1812323
ABSTRACT
Children’s mentalizing capacity may influence the therapeutic process in
psychodynamic therapy. The literature points to the need to tailor the ther
apeutic intervention to the particular configuration of clinical and develop
mental problems presented by the children and recommends a shift in
technique in the case of severely disturbed or traumatized patients with
impaired mentalizing capacity. The aim of this study was to explore the
therapeutic process in psychodynamic therapy with school-age children with
different kinds of difficulties and mentalizing profiles. A longitudinal design
based on the systematic case study method was adopted. Three children with
different baseline mentalizing capacities and their psychotherapists partici
pated. Two hundred seventy-three psychotherapy sessions were analyzed
using the Child Psychotherapy Q-Set (CPQ) in order to identify the relationship
patterns between patient and therapist. Different interaction structures were
identified. When the child presented with a more developed mentalizing
capacity at baseline, the treatment was more similar to a standard psychody
namic approach. When mentalization capacity was impaired, more directive,
supportive and empathic interventions were observed. A child’s capacity for
mentalization appears to impact the therapeutic process, with the adoption of
certain therapeutic approaches and specific techniques depending on the
child’s own baseline capacity to mentalize.
Introduction
Psychodynamic child therapy is a well-recognized treatment and there is growing evidence of its
effectiveness (Abbass et al., 2013; Hanley & Noble, 2017; Midgley & Kennedy, 2011; Midgley, O’Keeffe,
et al., 2017; Palmer et al., 2013). Although child psychotherapy research has not received as much
attention as adult psychotherapy research, an increasing number of studies have been developed,
focusing on the therapeutic process with young patients (Goodman & Midgley, 2019). However, to
date, there has been little empirical research on how the therapeutic process, and in particular
therapeutic interactions, may vary depending on the nature of the child’s mental functioning.
Vera Regina Rohnelt Ramires [email protected] Rua Luzitana 1148 apartamento 1002, CEP 90520-080, Porto
Alegre, Brazil
Vera Regina Rohnelt Ramires, MSc, PhD – University of Rio dos Sinos Valley, Unisinos, Brazil. Associate professor at the Graduate
Program in Clinical Psychology. Member of the Steering Committee of the Child, Adolescent, and Family Therapy Research Special
Interest Group of the Society for Psychotherapy Research.
Cibele Carvalho, MSc, PhD - University of Rio dos Sinos Valley, Unisinos, Brazil. Child and Adolescent Psychotherapist. Postdoctoral
Fellowship at Federal University of Rio Grande do Sul, Brazil.
Rodrigo Gabbi Polli, MSc, PhD - University of Rio dos Sinos Valley, Unisinos, Brazil. Child and Adolescent Psychotherapist.
Geoff Goodman, MSc, PhD – Long Island University, USA. Associate Professor of Psychology in the Clinical Psychology Doctoral
Program at Long Island University, Brookville, NY, and Chair of the Child, Adolescent, and Family Therapy Research Special Interest
Group of the Society for Psychotherapy Research.
Nick Midgley, MSc, PsychD, PhD – University College London and Anna Freud National Centre for Children and Families, UK. Associate
Professor at UCL, and co-director of the Child Attachment and Psychological Therapies Research Unit (ChAPTRe), at the Anna Freud
Centre/UCL.
© 2020 JICAP Foundation, Inc.
JOURNAL OF INFANT, CHILD, AND ADOLESCENT PSYCHOTHERAPY 359
Two decades ago, Fonagy and Target (1998) reported a detailed analysis of over 750 case records of
children and adolescents, who participated in psychoanalysis and psychodynamic therapy at the Anna
Freud Center, in the United Kingdom. They found that interpretations of unconscious conflict aimed
at promoting insight had limited value to the children who presented with more complex disorders,
whereas for the group of less severely disturbed children, a more traditional, interpretive approach was
beneficial. In the first group, designated as Cluster A, the youngsters showed fragile reality testing and
thought organization, magical thinking, bizarre fantasies, mistrust, and limited relationship skills. In
the second group, designated as Cluster B, the youngsters showed intense affect and hunger for social
response, hyperactivity and temper tantrums.
Fonagy and Target (1998) suggested that the fundamental difference between these two groups was
their capacity for reflective functioning, or what they came to call “mentalizing”. The authors
suggested that the findings of their study indicated that it would be necessary to tailor the therapeutic
intervention to the particular configuration of clinical and developmental problems presented by these
children. They recommended a shift in analytic technique in the case of severely disturbed or
traumatized patients, with a low reflective capacity. Instead of a conflict and insight-oriented approach
they suggested it would be helpful to adopt a focused, mentalization-building therapy.
Mentalization is a construct that has its roots in psychoanalysis, attachment theory, developmental
psychopathology and cognitive psychology (Fonagy et al., 2012). In order to be observed and assessed,
it has been operationalized as reflective functioning (RF). Mentalizing is the capacity to understand
self and others according to underlying mental states (e.g., wishes, intentions, beliefs). It develops in
the context of a secure attachment relationship, and it can be implicit or explicit (i.e., automatic or
controlled), based on internal or external features of self and others, directed toward the self or others,
and cognitively or affectively oriented, according to Fonagy et al. (2012). It is better understood as
a process instead of a fixed capacity, and it may vary according to interactional context and degree of
arousal or anxiety. Difficulties with different polarities of mentalizing are an essential feature of
psychological disorders, and there is some indication that psychotherapies may have a therapeutic
effect through a benign impact on the patient’s mentalizing capacity (Adshead & Fonagy, 2012; Fonagy
& Adshead, 2012; Sharp & Venta, 2012).
Sharp and Venta (2012) have discussed how different aspects of mentalizing are reflected in
different childhood disorders. They describe difficulties of mentalizing as “no mentalizing”,
“under-mentalizing”, “hyper-mentalizing”, “pseudo-mentalizing” and “distorted mentalizing”.
These categories refer to different types of impairments in the ability to mentalize, ranging from
its absence to distortions or different levels of this ability. These authors highlighted the need to
adapt therapeutic technique in response to the careful assessment of mentalizing problems in child
psychotherapy.
Verheugt-Pleiter (2008), building on Fonagy and Target’s study (1998), have highlighted the need
to change the aims of psychotherapy when children present with more complex disorders, where there
underlying capacity to mentalize is limited. Nowadays, for those working with children, a manualized
mentalization-based treatment exists for children (MBT-C; Midgley, Ensink, et al., 2017). The aim of
this treatment, more than achieving insight, is to enhance mentalizing capacity, for both the child and
parents, and to promote their skills to manage emotions and relationships.
Furthermore, Muñoz Specht et al. (2016) have shown that psychodynamic therapists working with
children who have experienced early trauma or maltreatment often use mentalizing techniques in their
work with these children and early adolescents. However, it is still unclear to what degree the child’s
basic capacity to mentalize informs the process of psychodynamic therapy with children, and whether
therapists adapt their technique depending on the basic mentalizing capacity of the child.
To our knowledge, there are few empirical studies with children that have focused on this issue.
Halfon et al. (2017) compared the treatment process of two children with anxiety disorder in
psychodynamic therapy. These children presented with different capacities for mentalizing before
treatment. The authors found that the child who had a more explicit mentalizing ability before
treatment began was able to make clinically significant change in internalizing problems at the end
360 V. R. R. RAMIRES ET AL.
of the treatment. However, the therapeutic process was less traditionally psychodynamic with the child
who had more significant mentalization deficits, and treatment outcomes were poorer.
Tuber (2012) has analyzed extensive clinical vignettes of two children who presented with different
mentalizing characteristics. He aimed at discussing the clinical implications of these problems for their
psychodynamic psychotherapies. One child, described as an oppositional, easily frustrated and
aggressive child, was, according to the author, agnostic toward processes of mentalizing, while the
other child was overtly antagonistic toward mentalizing. Emphasizing the importance of language and
play in psychotherapy, and discussing in detail the therapist’s interventions, Tuber showed the
importance of “articulating a frame, a method, or hypothesis about the notion of being psychologically
minded” (p. 13) in the first case, and of “finding a way to respond to overt challenges to any frame
where self-reflection might flourish” (p. 14) in the second case. Despite the richness of this analysis, it
is focused on a fragment of each psychotherapy, and not on the therapy process as a whole.
Based on these previous studies, we hypothesize that children’s mental functioning, and specifically
their baseline mentalizing abilities may impact the therapeutic process. Considering the characteristics
of psychodynamic child psychotherapy, one could expect that when a child has developed an ability to
mentalize, the child-therapist dyad will be able to focus on mental contents related to the child’s
conflicts and defenses. On the other hand, an impaired mentalizing ability can change the analytical
process, making it necessary to approach mental processes rather than mental content, in order to help
a child to face his or her psychological and relationship problems. This means that the therapist
deliberately or implicitly would focus their interventions on helping the child to observe and identify
his or her emotions, thoughts, wishes and so on, which is akin to a mentalization-based approach.
The aim of this observational study was to explore the therapeutic process in psychodynamic
psychotherapy with children presenting with different capacities to mentalize at baseline. We hypothe
sized that these different characteristics would be associated with differences in the therapeutic
process, as assessed by the relationship patterns between the child and the therapist, and that
psychodynamic therapies with children who have a greater mentalizing capacity will be more similar
to a traditional psychodynamic approach, whereas work with children with more impaired mentaliz
ing may lead to adaptations in ways of working and interaction structures.
Method
A longitudinal, repeated-measures design, based on the systematic case-study method (McLeod,
2010).
Participants
Three children and their three psychodynamic therapists participated in this study. We have called the
three patients Alice, Peter and Walter (pseudonyms).
Alice
Alice was 8 years old when she began her psychotherapy. She was referred because of anxiety and
psychosomatic symptoms. She had headaches and stomach pain with no organic causes identified. The
mother sought care, referred by the daughter’s neuropediatrician. Alice suddenly lost her father five
months before starting psychotherapy, and the family faced financial difficulties and a change of
school. She was described by the mother and the teacher as a very smart, communicative and caring
girl, and also perfectionistic and afraid of making mistakes.
During the diagnostic assessment, it was observed that Alice was experiencing a state of emotional
overload that limited her ability to identify and deal with affects. She struggled to exercise a strong
control of affects, showed a self-centeredness and reduced interest in the outside world and in contact
with the other. She was also observed to be excessively self-demanding, to have a capacity for
sophisticated and complex cognitive work, and to have perfectionist characteristics.
JOURNAL OF INFANT, CHILD, AND ADOLESCENT PSYCHOTHERAPY 361
Based on the clinical evaluation and the Rorschach Method, it was also observed that Alice revealed
a good empathic ability, a trend to establish positive bonds, and an ability to reflect on her own and
others’ mental states. In other words, it was observed that Alice was able to mentalize, especially in the
cognitive dimension of mentalization.
Alice was diagnosed with an adjustment disorder with anxiety (AD; DSM-5; American Psychiatric
Association, 2013), and she attended weekly 50-minute sessions for 15 months (40 sessions). Alice’s
psychotherapy had a break because of her therapist’s maternity leave.
Peter
Peter was 8 years old when he began his psychotherapy. His parents sought treatment because he did
not perform his school activities, showed signs of anxiety, and was very afraid when he had to separate
from his parents. He had many food restrictions, and his interests were quite limited. He was a very
quiet boy who spoke little and low and was very inhibited in his play.
During the diagnostic assessment, he was observed to have significant difficulties in thought as well
as distorted, confused and negative self-representations and representations of others. The Rorschach
protocol was also indicative of social isolation, difficulties in processes of analysis and synthesis, and
insecure and dependent behavior. He presented difficulties with identification processes. His difficul
ties in self-perception were closely related to impairments in affective processing, which indicated
significant limitations in recognizing and expressing his emotions and excessively internalizing his
affects, which increased his internal tension and made him emotionally and socially withdrawn. He
had immature and regressive postures in his relationships, showing limitations in establishing positive
bonds. Peter showed little evidence of interest in the human mind and probably a significant impair
ment in mentalization capacity.
Peter was diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder, level 1, without language or intellectual
impairment (ASD; DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Peter’s psychotherapy lasted
151 sessions for 43 months.
Walter
Walter was 7 years old when he began his psychotherapy. His parents sought treatment because of his
relationship difficulties and aggressive behavior at school. He had no friends and was bullied at school.
He presented as very childish in his behavior and demanding.
During the diagnostic assessment, it was observed that Walter exhibited limited internal resources to
deal with daily stressors, little sensitivity to his own needs and limitations in identifying internal tension,
indicating difficulties in self-control. These impairments were manifested mainly in the interpersonal
sphere, in difficulty in establishing positive bonds, and in relating in a superficial, dependent and less
sensitive way to the needs and interests of others, making him vulnerable to rejection.
Walter exhibited a lack of confidence in himself, associated with a clearly undervalued self-image,
and an important distortion in his body image. In the affective sphere, he demonstrated a strong denial
of affects, associated with hostile behaviors, with marked opposition and negativism. Such character
istics indicated the possibility of unstable and unpredictable conduct. He had no impairment in the
cognitive sphere.
Considering his difficulties, the confused and distorted representations of self and others, his
dependent behavior, opposition style and negativity, an impairment in his mentalizing ability was
indicated. His impaired ability to modulate affect, the difficulties in the perception of self and others, in
emotion regulation, and in relationships with others also supported this view.
Walter was diagnosed with disruptive mood dysregulation disorder (DMDD; DSM-5; American
Psychiatric Association, 2013). His psychotherapy lasted 158 sessions for 44 months.
Therapists
The three therapists were all female and had between two and 23 years of experience at the beginning
of the treatments. Two of them were trained in psychodynamic psychotherapy; two of them held
362 V. R. R. RAMIRES ET AL.
master’s degrees in Clinical Psychology; and one of them held a doctoral degree in Clinical Psychology.
All therapists performed individual psychoanalytic therapy and received clinical supervision during
these psychotherapies.
Instruments
The Child Psychotherapy Q-Set (CPQ) – The CPQ (Schneider & Jones, 2006, 2012) is based on
Q-sort methodology. It is used to analyze the psychotherapeutic process among 3- to 13-year-olds,
and consists of 100 items, each containing a statement that describes a relevant feature of the
treatment process corresponding to: a) the child’s attitudes (i.e., feelings, behaviors, or experi
ence); b) the therapist’s actions and attitudes; and c) the nature of the patient-therapist interac
tion. To improve the instrument’s reliability, a coding manual provides clear definitions and
examples of each item, with descriptions of behaviors that might be identified from videos of
sessions. After watching the videotape of the sessions, raters sort the items into nine categories,
according to how characteristic each item is of the session. Forced-choice coding (ipsative) is used,
establishing a normal distribution. Several studies have confirmed interrater reliability and dis
criminant validity of the CPQ (Goodman, 2015; Goodman & Athey-Lloyd, 2011; Goodman et al.,
2016, 2015; Ramires et al., 2015; Schmidt et al., 2018; Schneider et al., 2010). The CPQ has been
used to identify specific relationship patterns between patient and therapist, or interaction
structures (ISs), in a range of diagnoses like Autism Spectrum Disorder (Goodman & Athey-
Lloyd, 2011; Ramires et al., 2015), borderline personality disorder (BPD; Goodman, 2015),
disruptive mood dysregulation disorder (DMDD; Ramires et al., 2017), adjustment disorder
(AD; Schmidt et al., 2018), major depressive disorder and generalized anxiety disorder
(Schneider et al., 2010), as well as in a study of children with externalizing and internalizing
problems (Halfon et al., 2020).
Rorschach Method – The Rorschach method is used for the exploration of the structure and
dynamics of personality, and has been used in many studies to analyze changes during the therapeutic
process and after termination (Bram, 2010; Exner & Andronikof-Sanglade, 1992; Gronnerod, 2004;
Heedea, Rungea, Storeboa, Rowleyb, & Hansen, 2009; Weiner, 2004; Weiner & Exner, 1991). It was
used according to Exner’s (2003) Comprehensive System (CS). Although not directly aimed at
assessing reflective functioning, the Rorschach method was suggested as an instrument capable of
clinically researching and evaluating many psychological processes involving implicit mentalization
(Berant & Zim, 2013; Conklin et al., 2012; Pineda et al., 2011; Porcelli & Kleiger, 2016). Conklin et al.
(2012), based on an extensive literature review, conceptually proposed that texture variables (T) could
reflect an individual’s ability to establish attachment bonds with others; human movement responses
(M) would be related to empathic ability; and the quality of the human response (GHR vs. PHR) would
be related to the accuracy of perceptions about other people. The authors proposed profiles based on
these Rorschach variables that describe different levels of the ability to mentalize, according to specific
combinations of these variables. In this study, the Rorschach method was used to analyze children’s
mentalizing capacity at the onset of therapy, in addition to the clinical evaluation.
Child Behavior Check List for Children and Adolescents between 6 and 18 years of age (CBCL/6–18)
and Teachers Report Form (TRF/6–18). The CBCL/6–18 and the TRF/6–18 are evidence-based
instruments which are part of the ASEBA Assessment System (Achenbach System of Empirically
Based Assessment) (Achenbach & Rescorla, 1991). Their scales provide wide coverage of the psycho
pathological symptoms found in childhood and adolescence (Achenbach & Ruffle, 2000). The CBCL is
considered to be the most efficient quantification instrument for parental concerns about children’s
behavior (Wielewicki et al., 2011). The version of the CBCL used in this study exhibited good
sensitivity (87%), correctly identifying 75% of mild, 95% of moderate and 100% of serious cases.
The CBCL protocols were completed by the children’s mothers and fathers, and the TRF form by the
children’s teachers.
JOURNAL OF INFANT, CHILD, AND ADOLESCENT PSYCHOTHERAPY 363
Procedure
When they came to psychotherapy, the children were assessed based on the parents’ interviews,
children’s interviews, the CBCL and TRF forms, and the Rorschach method. Once psychotherapy
was recommended, they were consulted about their participation in the research project. The
therapists had previously been consulted about their availability. The study was approved by the
Research Ethics Committee of the university of the first author, linked to the corresponding National
Research Ethics Committee (N. 039/2012). Therapists and parents signed informed consent forms,
and children provided their assent. All the sessions with children were videotaped, and they added up
to 349 sessions. The psychotherapies took place in the therapists’ consulting rooms.
Data analysis
A team of eight psychologists, trained to use the CPQ, coded 273 sessions (all sessions of Alice’s and
Walter’s treatments were coded; one out of every two sessions of Peter’s treatment was coded [n = 75]).
The videos were coded in random order by two raters who were randomly assigned to and independently
coded each session. Interrater reliability ranged from.62 to .81 (mean = .73). After that, the means of
these assessments were calculated in order to generate the composite score, which was used subsequently.
The composite scores were submitted to a principal components factor analysis to determine the
relationship patterns of each psychotherapy, or, in other words, the interaction structures (ISs).
Pearson correlations were used to analyze whether the ISs changed over time. All statistical analysis
in the study were performed using SPSS 23.0.
The Rorschach protocols were independently coded by two trained psychologists and subsequently
compared. The intraclass correlation coefficients ranged from .97 to .99 (mean = .98). The interpreta
tion of the results in each Rorschach variable was based on normative data for the population under
study (Ribeiro et al., 2012). The criterion used to interpret the results was one standard deviation
above or below average.
Results
In this study, we analyzed the psychotherapies of three children who presented predominantly with
difficulties in three different spheres: internalizing symptoms, externalizing symptoms, and
a developmental disorder. Alongside these difficulties, their mentalization capacities at psychotherapy
onset were varied. The Rorschach assessments led to the development of a mentalizing profile for each
child, which can broadly be described as “able to mentalize” (Alice), “mentalization impairments”
(Walter) and “significant mentalization impairments” (Peter). We provide detailed information on
these assessments and the corresponding Rorschach variables in the Appendix.
The therapeutic processes of these children were analyzed focusing on the relationship patterns
between patients and therapists, conceptualized as interaction structures (ISs). To determine these ISs
we performed a principal components factor analysis, that yielded four conceptually distinct factors in
Peter’s and Walter’s treatments. In Alice’s psychotherapy we identified five distinct factors. These
factors explained 36.9%, 37.3% and 31.9% of the variance of the items in Alice’s, Peter’s and Walter’s
treatments, respectively, which is consistent with the literature (Goodman, 2015; Goodman & Athey-
Lloyd, 2011; Ramires et al., 2015, 2017; Schmidt et al., 2018). The factors were conceptualized as ISs,
describing repetitive patterns of interaction between each therapeutic dyad. Table 1 summarizes these
ISs, their internal consistency, and changes over time.
Discussion
We can observe that differences in children’s difficulties and in their baseline mentalizing capacity
seem to be associated with differences in the therapeutic process, as assessed by the interaction
364 V. R. R. RAMIRES ET AL.
Table 1. Interaction structures in the three case studies, internal consistency, and changes over time.
Internal Changes over
Interaction Structure consistency Time
Alice, age 8, diagnosed with AD
IS 1 – Attuned and interpretive T with active, expressive and demanding C α =.81 r =.41**↑
IS 2 – Sensitive and supportive T with withdrawn and distant C α =.85 –
IS 3 – Didactic, directive T with aggressive and defensive C α =.83 –
IS 4 – Directive and limiting T with dependent, resistant and ashamed C α =.78 r = −.53**↓
IS 5 – Receptive and supportive T with anxious C α =.76 r =.35*↑
Peter, age 8, diagnosed with ASD
IS 1 – Resistant and defensive C with uncertain, unresponsive and didactic T α =.95 r = −.52**↓
IS 2 – Active, confident and lively C, competing with connected and reflective T α =.88 r =.39**↑
IS 3 – Articulated C seeking proximity with an active and not neutral T α =.74 –
IS 4 – Provocative, hostile C with reassuring and accepting T α =.65 –
Walter, age 7, diagnosed with DMDD
IS 1 – Connected C, attached to T, expressing mental contents and fantasies, with α =.94 –
a supportive T
IS 2 – Sensitive, accepting and supportive T with an understood C α =.84 –
IS 3 – Directive and didactic T with a C aware of his difficulties α =.82 r =.78**↑
IS 4 – Demanding, competitive and blaming C with a supportive T α =.67 –
T: therapist; C: Child; *: p <.05; **: p <.01; ↑: IS became more characteristic over time; ↓: IS became less characteristic over time.
structures between the patient and therapist. The three psychotherapies were all psychodynamic
treatments. However, we found indicators that the therapists probably had to adapt their interventions
to respond to the children’s needs and difficulties, sometimes moving away from the standard
technique of psychodynamic therapy.
Unlike the other psychotherapies, in Alice’s treatment we observed an attuned and interpretive
therapist interacting with an expressive child (IS 1), and this relationship pattern had increased over
the psychotherapy (r = .41, p < .01). We also observed a sensitive, supportive and receptive therapist
interacting with a distant or anxious girl (IS 2 and 5). In the IS 5 (receptive and supportive therapist
with an anxious child), the IS became more characteristic (r = .35, p < .05), and we should remember
that this patient had suddenly lost her father and had to face the therapist’s maternity leave. We
hypothesize that these factors could help to explain the relationship patterns in which Alice was
aggressive and defensive (IS 3) or dependent and resistant (IS 4), when the therapist acted in a more
directive and limiting way, although the IS 4 became less characteristic over time (r = −.53, p < .01).
Overall Alice’s psychotherapy, according to the identified ISs, was similar to a traditional psycho
dynamic approach. Considering her psychic resources and a therapist with an accurate understanding
of the therapeutic process, it was possible for her therapist to analyze and formulate an understanding
of her unconscious conflicts and contents.
Walter presented with very disruptive behavior and emotional dysregulation, and his mentalizing
capacity was impaired at psychotherapy onset. Only IS 3 became significantly more characteristic over
time (r = .78, p < .01). It describes a “CBT-style” therapist, which may have been necessary in order to
help an impulsive and dysregulated child with limited mentalizing capacity. Items pertaining to the
cognitive-behavioral therapy model described by Goodman et al. (2016) were present in this IS, like
item 57 (therapist attempts to modify distortions in child’s beliefs) and item 27 (there is a focus on
helping child plan behavior outside the session). The therapist also directly rewarded desirable
behaviors (item 55), was directly reassuring (item 66), and informed the child of the potential impact
of his behavior on others (item 87), among other items.
We can also observe in this psychotherapy the interactions between a connected and expressive
child and a supportive therapist (IS 1), a sensitive and accepting therapist with an understood child
(IS 2), and a demanding and blaming child with a supportive therapist (IS 4). These three relationship
patterns were consistent across the treatment, and they seemed to be related to a very emotionally
dysregulated child who needed containment and a framework that, to some extent, enabled the
JOURNAL OF INFANT, CHILD, AND ADOLESCENT PSYCHOTHERAPY 365
experience of being recognized and contained in his therapist’s mind. We did not observe, in the ISs,
an interpretive therapist, as in Alice’s treatment, which suggests that the focus on explicit mentalizing
was less part of this therapy.
Peter, the boy diagnosed with ASD, presented with significant impairment in his mentalizing
capacity at psychotherapy onset. In cases like this, the literature has stated the need to adopt a focused,
mentalization-oriented therapy (Fonagy & Target, 1998; Midgley, Ensink, et al., 2017; Verheugt-
Pleiter, 2008). Two ISs in Peter’s treatment corroborate the role of such an approach in the
psychotherapy process. IS 2 (active, confident and lively child, competing with connected and
reflective therapist) suggests a mentalizing approach becoming more characteristic over time (r =
.39, p < .01). On the other hand, IS 1 (resistant and defensive child with uncertain, unresponsive and
didactic therapist) decreased over time (r = – .52, p < .01) and suggests a relationship pattern in which
a reflective functioning was gained by the therapist and the child, and we hypothesize that the child’s
difficulties in mentalizing may have initially had an impact on the therapist’s mentalizing capacity.
Probably, when the therapist was able to figure out what was going on and recovered this capacity, she
was effective in helping the boy to slowly start to mentalize, and we could observe a change in the
interaction patterns in the later phases of this treatment.
Walter and Peter’s treatments were the longest and showed greater variability in the quality of their
relationship patterns. Children who present with disruptive behavior and autism spectrum difficulties
usually show significant impairment in reflective capacity and hence impairments in emotional
regulation (Goodman, 2015; Goodman & Athey-Lloyd, 2011; Hoffman et al., 2016). In Walter’s
case, these impairments were reflected in uncontrolled behaviors, feelings of being misunderstood,
and difficulties in reflecting on his conflicts. In Peter’s case there was a mental impermeability and
a total lack of awareness of mental states of self and others. More directive strategies are used when
there are moments of no mentalization during the sessions. At such times, it is important for the
therapist to focus on points where the mentalization was lost, to help the patient to explore and think
about what happened (Fonagy et al., 2014).
Directive ISs were observed in all treatments in this study. In Walter’s case, such ISs became
significantly more characteristic (IS 3), and we would suggest that it was necessary, considering his
disruptive behavior and dysregulated emotions. Such directive strategies are common with more
regressive patients like Peter, or patients who show affective outbursts like Walter. Children with
limited internal resources demand the therapist’s flexibility to adapt to their needs (Hoffman et al.,
2016).
Hence, children might need a different approach depending on their presentation in a particular
session, which has been observed previously in the literature (e.g., Goodman, 2015; Goodman &
Athey-Lloyd, 2011). As different children may need different approaches, the same child may need
different techniques in different therapeutic moments. Another interpretation might be that counter
transference pressures pushed the therapists “off-model” such that they were induced at various
moments to be more controlling or directive in the sessions. Maybe more directive strategies were
used when there were moments of no mentalization during the sessions. It would be necessary to
conduct time series analysis to identify whether directive strategies precede or follow these moments,
and who initiates such non-mentalizing moments: the therapist or the child.
Muñoz Specht et al. (2016) had examined the mentalizing techniques used by psychodynamic
therapists in their work with children and had categorized such interventions into three main groups:
“supporting mentalization-stance interventions”, “basic mentalizing techniques”, and “mentalizing
the play context”. In the first type, the authors described therapeutic framework interventions,
supportive and empathic interventions, exploration and clarification techniques and psychoeducation,
which are not directly mentalization-focused, but which are necessary to stimulate a mentalizing
stance. For instance, when applying therapeutic framework interventions, the therapist may address
the rules, the therapeutic contract and the limits. We can observe these kinds of interventions in
Walter’s IS 3 and even in Alice’s IS 3 and 4, in this case probably when her arousal levels were higher.
Supportive and empathic interventions were present in all treatments: IS 2 and 5 in Alice’s
366 V. R. R. RAMIRES ET AL.
Conclusion
The findings of this study suggest that when a child presents with an impaired mentalizing capacity,
this impairment can hinder the progress of the therapeutic process in general, until some capacity to
reflect on mental states can be enhanced. On the other hand, if some mentalizing ability is present,
a psychodynamic approach is likely to be followed more closely.
This study corroborated the literature, which considers mentalization as an important aspect in
psychodynamic therapeutic process (Goodman et al., 2016, 2015; Halfon et al., 2017; Halfon &
JOURNAL OF INFANT, CHILD, AND ADOLESCENT PSYCHOTHERAPY 367
Bulut, 2019; Muñoz Specht et al., 2016; Tessier et al., 2016). The findings support the hypothesis that
a child’s baseline mentalizing capacity should be one factor, among others, to be taken into account
in explaining the differences among these therapeutic processes. Future studies could explore if
improving mentalization would contribute to a change in the direction of the therapeutic process, in
terms of a more insight-oriented approach and vice versa, in a feedback loop, and the role of other
techniques as well. That would require repeated measures to assess children’s mentalizing capacity
across treatment.
The results of this study lead us to consider that mentalizing can be thought of as an individual
capacity (even if it is not fixed) and as an important factor in shaping the therapeutic process. It can
play a different role in the therapeutic process, depending on the child’s characteristics and diagnosis,
probably becoming more salient in treatment the more regressive and emotionally dysregulated the
child. Whilst more traditional psychodynamic techniques may be found in the treatment of children
who already have a basic capacity to mentalize, it may be that psychodynamic therapists working with
children with greater impairments in mentalizing are likely to draw on a broader range of ways of
working, including more directive, supportive and empathic interventions.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank the National Council for Scientific and Technological Development of Brazil - CNPq (grants 309544/
2017-3 and 422602/2018-3) and the CAPES Foundation, Ministry of Education of Brazil, for supporting this study.
Disclosure statement
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.
ORCID
Nick Midgley, MSc, PsychD, PhD http://orcid.org/0000-0002-6263-5058
References
Abbass, A. A., Rabung, S., Leichsenring, F., Refseth, J. S., & Midgley, N. (2013). Psychodynamic psychotherapy for
children and adolescents: A meta-analysis of short-term psychodynamic models. Journal of the American Academy of
Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 52(8), 863–875. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2013.05.014
Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (1991). Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & pro- files. University of Vermont,
Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families.
Achenbach, T. M., & Ruffle, T. M. (2000). The child behavior checklist and related forms for assessing behavioural/
emotional problems and competencies. Pediatrics in Review, 21(8), 265–271. https://doi.org/10.1542/pir.21-
8-265
Adshead, G., & Fonagy, P. (2012). How does psychotherapy work? The self and its disorders. Advances in Psychiatric
Treatment, 18(4), 242–249. https://doi.org/10.1192/qpt.bp.111.009274
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). American
Psychiatric Publishing.
Berant, E., & Zim, S. (2013). Integration of attachment self-report scales with the Rorschach. Rorschachiana, 34(2),
156–187. https://doi.org/10.1027/1192-5604/a000047
Bram, A. D. (2010). The relevance of the Rorschach and patient–examiner relationship in treatment planning and
outcome assessment. Journal of Personality Assessment, 92(2), 91–115.https://doi.org/10.1080/00223890903508112
Conklin, A. C., Malone, J. C., & Fowler, J. T. (2012). Mentalization and the Rorschach. Rorschachiana, 33(2), 189–213.
https://doi.org/10.1027/1192-5604/a000035
Exner, J. E., Jr. (2003). The Rorschach: A comprehensive system (4th ed.). Wiley.
Exner Jr., J. E., & Andronikof-Sanglade, A. (1992). Rorschach changes following brief and short-term therapy. Journal of
Personality Assessment, 59(1), 59–71. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa5901_6
Fonagy, P., Bateman, A. W., & Luyten, P. (2012). Introduction and overview. In A. W. Bateman & P. Fonagy (Eds.),
Handbook of mentalizing in mental health practice (pp. 3–42). American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc.
368 V. R. R. RAMIRES ET AL.
Fonagy, P., & Adshead, G. (2012). How mentalisation changes the mind. Advances in Psychiatric Treatment, 18(5),
353–362. https://doi.org/10.1192/apt.bp.108.005876
Fonagy, P., Rossouw, T., Sharp, C., Bateman, A., Allison, L., & Farrar, C. (2014). Mentalization-based treatment for
adolescents with borderline traits. In: C. Sharp & J. L. Tackett (Eds.), Handbook of borderline personality disorder in
children and adolescents (pp. 313–332). New York, NY: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4939-0591-1_21
Fonagy, P., & Target, M. (1998). Mentalization and the changing aims of child psychoanalysis. Psychoanalytic Dialogues,
8(1), 87–114. https://doi.org/10.1080/10481889809539235
Goodman, G. (2015). Interaction structures between a child and two therapists in the psychodynamic treatment of
a child with borderline personality disorder. Journal of Child Psychotherapy, 41(2), 141–161. https://doi.org/10.1080/
0075417X.2015.1048124
Goodman, G., & Midgley, N. (2019). Child psychodynamic therapy: Contemporary trends in treatment development. In
D. Kealy & J. S. Ogrodiniczuk (Eds.), Contemporary psychodynamic psychotherapy (pp. 237–252). Elsevier.
Goodman, G., & Athey-Lloyd, L. (2011). Interaction structures between a child and two therapists in the psychodynamic
treatment of a child with Asperger’s disorder. Journal of Child Psychotherapy, 37(3), 311–326. https://doi.org/10.1080/
0075417X.2011.614749
Goodman, G., Midgley, N., & Schneider, C. (2016). Expert clinicians’ prototypes of an ideal child treatment in
psychodynamic and cognitive-behavioral therapy: Is mentalization seen as a common process factor?
Psychotherapy Research, 26(5), 590–601. https://doi.org/10.1080/10503307.2015.1049672
Goodman, G., Reed, P., & Athey-Lloyd, L. (2015). Mentalization and play therapy process between two therapists and
a child with Asperger’s disorder. International Journal of Play Therapy, 24(1), 13–29. https://doi.org/10.1037/
a0038660
Gronnerod, C. (2004). Rorschach assessment of changes following psychotherapy: A meta-analytic review. Journal of
Personality Assessment, 83(3), 256–276. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa8303_09
Halfon, S., Bekar, O., & Gurleyen, B. (2017). An empirical analysis of mental state talk and affect regulation in two
single-cases pf psychodynamic child therapy. Psychotherapy, 54(2), 207–219. https://doi.org/10.1037/pst0000113
Halfon, S., & Bulut, P. (2019). Mentalization and the growth of symbolic play and affect regulation in psychodynamic
therapy for children with behavioral problems. Psychotherapy Research, 29(5), 666–678. https://doi.org/10.1080/
10503307.2017.1393577
Halfon, S., Goodman, G., & Bulut, P. (2020). Interaction structures as predictors of outcome in a naturalistic study of
psychodynamic child psychotherapy. Psychotherapy Research, 30(2), 251–266. https://doi.org/10.1080/10503307.
2018.1519267
Hanley, T., & Noble, J. (2017). Therapy outcomes: Is child therapy effective? In N. Midgley, J. Hayes, & M. Cooper
(Eds.), Essential research findings in child and adolescent counselling and psychotherapy (pp. 59–78). Sage
Publications Ltd.
Heede, T., Runge, H., Storebø, O. J., Rowley, E., & Hansen, K. G. (2009). Psychodynamic milieu-therapy and changes in
personality – what is the connection? Journal of Child Psychotherapy, 35(3), 276–289. https://doi.org/10.1080/
00754170903237286
Hoffman, L., Rice, T., & Prout, T. (2016). Manual of Regulation – Focused Psychotherapy for Children (RFP-C) with
externalizing behaviors. Routledge.
McLeod, J. (2010). Case study research in counselling and psychotherapy. Sage.
Midgley, N., Ensink, K., Lindqvist, K., Malberg, N., & Muller, N. (2017). Mentalization-based treatment for children.
A time-limited approach. American Psychological Association.
Midgley, N., & Kennedy, E. (2011). Psychodynamic psychotherapy for children and adolescents: A critical review of the
evidence base. Journal of Child Psychotherapy, 37(3), 232–260. https://doi.org/10.1080/0075417X.2011.614738
Midgley, N., O’Keeffe, S. O., French, L., & Kennedy, E. (2017). Psychodynamic psychotherapy for children and
adolescents: An updated narrative review of the evidence base. Journal of Child Psychotherapy, 43(3), 307–329.
https://doi.org/10.1080/0075417X.2017.1323945
Muñoz Specht, P., Ensink, K., Normandin, L., & Midgley, N. (2016). Mentalization techniques used by psychodynamic
therapists working with children and early adolescents. Bulletin of Menninger Clinic, 80(4), 281–315. https://doi.org/
10.1521/bumc.2016.80.4.281
Palmer, R., Nascimento, L. N., & Fonagy, P. (2013). The state of the evidence base for psychodynamic psychotherapy for
children and adolescents. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 22(2), 149–214. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.chc.2012.12.001
Pineda, J. A., Giromini, L., Porcelli, P., Parolin, L., & Viglione, D. J. (2011). Mu suppression and human movement
responses to the Rorschach test. NeuroReport, 22(5), 223–226. https://doi.org/10.1097/WNR.0b013e328344f45c
Porcelli, P., & Kleiger, J. H. (2016). The “feeling of movement”: Notes on the Rorschach human movement response.
Journal of Personality Assessment, 98(2), 124–134. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2015.1102146
Ramires, V. R. R., Carvalho, C., Schmidt, F. D., Fiorini, G. P., & Goodman, G. (2015). Interaction structures in the
psychodynamic therapy of a boy diagnosed with Asperger’s disorder: A single-case study. Research in Psychotherapy:
Psychopathology, Process and Outcome, 18(2), 129–140. https://doi.org/10.7411/RP.2015.112
JOURNAL OF INFANT, CHILD, AND ADOLESCENT PSYCHOTHERAPY 369
Ramires, V. R. R., Godinho, L. R., & Goodman, G. (2017). The therapeutic process of a child diagnosed with disruptive
mood dysregulation disorder. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 34(4), pap0000134. https://doi.org/10.1037/pap0000134
Ribeiro, R. K. S. M., Semer, N. L., & Yazigi, L. (2012). Rorschach comprehensive system data from a sample of 211
nonpatient children in Brazil. Journal of Personality Assessment, 94(3), 267–275. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.
2011.653727
Schmidt, F. D., Gastaud, M., & Ramires, V. R. R. (2018). Interaction structures in the psychodynamic psychotherapy of
a girl diagnosed with adjustment disorder. Trends in Psychology, 26(2), 719–734. https://doi.org/10.9788/tp2018.
207pt
Schneider, C., & Jones, E. E. (2012). Appendix IB. Child psychotherapy Q-set. Coding manual. In R. A. Levy, J. S. Ablon,
& H. Kächele (Eds.), Psychodynamic psychotherapy research: Evidence-based practice and practice-based evidence (pp.
611–626). Humana Press.
Schneider, C., & Jones, E. E. (2006). Child psychotherapy Q-set. Coding manual. University of California. Unpublished
Manual.
Schneider, C., Midgley, N., & Duncan, A. (2010). A “motion portrait” of a psychodynamic treatment of an 11-year-old
girl: Exploring interrelations of psychotherapy process and outcome using the child psychotherapy Q-set. Journal of
Infant, Child and Adolescent Psychotherapy, 9(2–3), 94–107. https://doi.org/10.1080/15289168.2010.510979
Sharp, C., & Venta, A. (2012). Mentalizing problems in children and adolescents. In N. Midgley & I. Vrouva (Eds.),
Minding the child: Mentalization-based interventions with children, young people and their families (pp. 35–53).
Routledge.
Tessier, V. P., Normandin, L., Ensink, K., & Fonagy, P. (2016). Fact or fiction? A longitudinal study of play and the
development of reflective functioning. Bulletin of Menninger Clinic, 80(1), 60–79. https://doi.org/10.1521/bumc.2016.
80.1.60
Tuber, S. (2012). The clinical implications of aspects of a child’s degree of psychological mindedness in dynamically
oriented child psychotherapy. Journal of Infant, Child, and Adolescent Psychotherapy, 11(1), 3–20. https://doi.org/10.
1080/15289168.2012.648886
Verheugt-Pleiter, A. J. E. (2008). Treatment strategy. In A. J. E. Verheugt-Pleiter, J. Zelvalkink, & M. G. C. Schmets
(Eds.), Mentalizing in children therapy (pp. 41–68). Karnac.
Weiner, I. B. (2004). Monitoring psychotherapy with performance-based measures of personality functioning. Journal of
Personality Assessment, 83(3): 323–331. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa8303_13
Weiner, I. B., & Exner Jr., J. E. (1991). Rorschach changes in long-term and short-term psychotherapy. Journal of
Personality Assessment, 53(3): 453–465. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa5603_7
Wielewicki, A., Gallo, A. E., & Grossi, R. (2011). Clinical practice instruments: CBCL as a functional analysis and
intervention planning facilitator. Trends in Psychology, 19(2), 513–523.
Zevalkink, J. (2008). Assessment of mentalizing problems in children. In: A. J. E. Verheugt-Pleiter, J. Zelvalkink, & M. G.
C. Schmets (Eds.), Mentalizing in children therapy (pp. 22–40). London: Karnac.
370 V. R. R. RAMIRES ET AL.
Table A1. Children’s outcomes in the variables assessing mentalization, according to the Rorschach Method.
Expected scores for nonclinical chil Expected scores for nonclinical chil
Alice’s Peter’s dren Walter’s dren
Variables Scores Scores (Age 8) Scores (Age 7)
M 3↑ 1 .79(SD =.1.20) 1 .46(SD =.89)
H:(H)+Hd 1: 2↑ 1: 5↑ H: 1.11(SD = 1.07) 1: 3↑ H:.96(SD = 1.16)
+(Hd) (H)+Hd+(Hd):.64(SD =.83) (H)+Hd+(Hd):.78(SD = 1.07)
COP 1↑ 0 .19(SD =.40) 0 .08(SD =.34)
GHR: PHR 3↑: 2 1: ↑5 GHR: 1.25(SD = 1.14) 2: 2 GHR: 1.34(SD = 1.45)
PHR: 1.91(SD = 1.92) PHR: 1.44(SD = 1.11)
Fd 0 1↑ .25(SD =.68) 1↑ .04(SD =.28)
Sum T 0 0 .21(SD =.49) 0 .24(SD =.62)
Human 3 6↑ 2.85(SD = 2.24) 4 2.68(SD = 1.87)
content
Pure H 1 1 1.11(SD = 1.07) 1 .96(SD = 1.16)
DQ+ 7↑ 0↓ 2.74(SD = 2.44) 0 1.90(SD = 2.05)
DQv 1 1 .75(SD = 1.07) 0 .80(SD = 1.46)
M- 0 1↑ .25(SD =.48) 0 .20(SD =.57)
M+ 0 0 .00(SD =.00) 0 .00(SD =.00)
Mo 1↑ 0 .18(SD =.44) 0 .18(SD =.44)
Mu 2↑ 0 .08(SD =.27) 1 .08(SD =.27)
SD: standard deviation; ↑ increased result compared with the mean and standard deviation of the age group; ↓ decreased result
compared with the mean and standard deviation of the age group; M: response in which human movement was determinant; H:
(H)+Hd+(Hd): ratio between responses of human content and responses of parahuman content and partial perceptions; COP:
special code, indicating a trend to establish positive bonds; GHR: PHR: proportion between good human representations and poor
human representations; Fd: responses of food content, indicate dependent behavior; Sum T: related to closeness and emotional
contact; Human Content: total responses of human content; Pure H: responses of human content. Involves the percept of whole
human form; DQ+: reflects the completion of analysis and synthesis processes; DQv: reflects immature information processing M-:
response in which human movement was determinant and the formal quality was poor; indicates significant difficulties in thought;
M+: response in which human movement was determinant and the formal quality was good; Mo: response in which human
movement was determinant and the formal quality was ordinary; Mu: response in which human movement was determinant and
the formal quality was unusual.