Sex Differences in Cognitive Functioning With Aging in The Netherlands
Sex Differences in Cognitive Functioning With Aging in The Netherlands
aCentre for Nutrition, Prevention and Health Services, National Institute for Public Health and the Environment,
Bilthoven, The Netherlands; bDepartment of Health Sciences, Faculty of Science, Amsterdam Public Health
Research Institute, Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam, The Netherlands; cAmsterdam UMC, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam,
Department of Epidemiology & Biostatistics, Amsterdam Public Health, Amsterdam, The Netherlands;
dJulius Center for Health Sciences and Primary Care, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht University, Utrecht,
The Netherlands
Keywords cognitive function than men (p’s < 0.01). However, women
Aging · Cognitive function · Cohort effect · Longitudinal showed up to 10% faster decline in these cognitive domains,
study · Sex except for flexibility, where women showed 9% slower de-
cline. In the LASA, women scored poorer on fluid intelligence
(p < 0.01), but their decline was 10% slower than that in men.
Abstract Female advantage was larger in later born cohorts; adjust-
Introduction: Dementia prevalence in older women is high- ment for the educational level increased the female advan-
er than that in men. The purpose of the present study was to tage. Conclusion: Women have better memory and process-
investigate whether there is a female disadvantage in cogni- ing speed than men at middle age. This female advantage
tive functioning at adult age and/or whether a female disad- becomes smaller with aging and has increased in more re-
vantage develops with age. Methods: Data of 5,135 women cent birth cohorts. © 2022 The Author(s).
and 4,756 men from the Longitudinal Aging Study Amster- Published by S. Karger AG, Basel
1002
birth cohort and level of education in LASA
Memory function Information processing speed Fluid intelligence Global cognitive function
(z-score) (z-score) (z-score) (MMSE score)
model 1 (level of model 2 (cognitive model 1 (level of model 2 (cognitive model 1 (level of model 2 (cognitive model 1 (level of model 2 (cognitive
cognitive functioning) decline) cognitive functioning) decline) cognitive functioning) decline) cognitive functioning) decline)
Overall (n = 4,632, 14,411 observations) (n = 4,579, 14,236 observations) (n = 3,860, 12,041 observations) (n = 5,123, 17,319 observations)
Sex† 0.422 [0.375, 0.469] 0.483 [0.409, 0.556] 0.106 [0.058, 0.154] 0.185 [0.122, 0.248] −0.143 [−0.194, −0.092] −0.216 [−0.296, −0.136] 0.002 [−0.110, 0.114] 0.044 [−0.132, 0.219]
Sex × age −0.004 [−0.008, −0.000] −0.006 [−0.009, −0.003] 0.005 [0.001, 0.009] −0.004 [−0.017, 0.009]
By birth cohort
1908–1917
Sex 0.30 [0.20, 0.40]a,b 0.37 [−0.08, 0.81] −0.03 [−0.14, 0.07]d −0.00 [−0.35, 0.35] −0.12 [−0.23, −0.01] −0.02 [−0.49, 0.45] −0.14 [−0.56, 0.29] 0.49 [−1.49, 2.46]
Sex × age −0.00 [−0.02, 0.01] −0.00 [−0.01, 0.01] −0.00 [−0.02, 0.01] −0.03 [−0.10, 0.05]j
DOI: 10.1159/000520318
1918–1927
Sex 0.47 [0.37, 0.58]a 0.60 [0.37, 0.82] 0.09 [−0.02, 0.20]e 0.13 [−0.06, 0.32] −0.03 [−0.14, 0.08]h,i −0.16 [−0.38 0.07] 0.05 [−0.22, 0.32] −0.09 [−0.78, 0.60]
Gerontology 2022;68:999–1009
Sex × age −0.01 [−0.02, 0.00] −0.00 [−0.01, 0.01] 0.01 [−0.00, 0.02] 0.01 [−0.03, 0.05]
1928–1937
Sex 0.44 [0.34, 0.54] 0.44 [0.32, 0.57] 0.07 [−0.04, 0.18]f 0.12 [−0.00, 0.24] −0.20 [−0.29, −0.10]h −0.21 [−0.33, −0.09] −0.09 [−0.30, 0.13] −0.16 [−0.44, 0.12]
Sex × age −0.00 [−0.01, 0.01] −0.00 [−0.01, 0.00] 0.00 [−0.01, 0.01] 0.01 [−0.01, 0.03]j
1938–1947
Sex 0.36 [0.26, 0.46]c 0.39 [0.26, 0.52] 0.11 [0.00, 0.21]g 0.18 [0.07, 0.30] −0.22 [−0.31, −0.12]i −0.25 [−0.39, −0.11] −0.07 [−0.32, 0.17] −0.04 [−0.35, 0.27]
Sex × age −0.00 [−0.01, 0.01] −0.01 [−0.02, −0.00] 0.01 [−0.01, 0.02] −0.00 [−0.03, 0.02]
1948–1957
Sex 0.55 [0.44, 0.66]b,c 0.54 [0.33, 0.75] 0.30 [0.20, 0.40]d,e,f,g 0.24 [0.09, 0.39] ‡ 0.19 [−0.02, 0.40] 0.42 [0.01, 0.83]
Sex × age 0.00 [−0.02, 0.03] 0.01 [−0.01, 0.03] −0.03 [−0.09, 0.02]
By educational level
Low
Sex 0.45 [0.36, 0.53] 0.61 [0.45, 0.76] 0.15 [0.07, 0.24]k 0.32 [0.18, 0.45] 0.03 [−0.06, 0.12] 0.12 [−0.06, 0.30] 0.32 [0.03, 0.60] 0.53 [0.02, 1.03]
Sex × age −0.01 [−0.02, −0.00] −0.01 [−0.01, −0.00] −0.00 [−0.01, 0.00]m −0.01 [−0.04, 0.01]n
Medium
Sex 0.55 [0.48, 0.63] 0.40 [0.24, 0.56] 0.34 [0.27, 0.42]k,l 0.39 [0.29, 0.49] 0.01 [−0.07, 0.09] −0.15 [−0.27, −0.02] 0.26 [0.09, 0.42] 0.04 [−0.22, 0.31]
Sex × age 0.03 [0.01, 0.04] −0.00 [−0.01, 0.00] 0.01 [0.00, 0.02]m 0.02 [0.00, 0.04]n
Sex × age2 −0.00 [−0.00, −0.00]
High
Sex 0.54 [0.46, 0.61] 0.60 [0.49, 0.71] 0.18 [0.11, 0.26]l 0.25 [0.16, 0.34] −0.07 [−0.14, −0.00] −0.14 [−0.24, −0.03] 0.12 [−0.00, 0.24] 0.09 [−0.13, 0.30]
Nooyens et al.
Sex × age −0.01 [−0.01, −0.00] −0.01 [−0.01, −0.00] 0.01 [−0.00, 0.01] 0.00 [−0.01, 0.02]
Estimates in bold are statistically significant at p < 0.05; estimates with same superscript letters are significantly different at p < 0.05 (between comparable models only). LASA, Longitudinal Aging Study Amsterdam; MMSE,
Mini-Mental State Examination. † Sex differences are presented as average [95% CL] and represent the average difference in cognitive function between men and women, regardless of age (model 1) or at age 55 years (model 2).
Positive values represent better scores (sex) or less decline for every year of aging (sex × age and sex × age2) for women compared to men. Sex differences are adjusted for age and age squared, with age centered at age 55 years.
‡
Fluid intelligence was not assessed in the latest birth cohort. For stratified analyses on the latest birth cohort (1948–1957), no random slope was included in the model.
Sensitivity Analyses Cognitive Flexibility
In order to test whether menopause influenced results, analyses Women scored better than men on cognitive flexibil-
for DCS were repeated, excluding participants younger than 55
years. Mixed model analyses are able to deal with missing values, ity (Table 3). At the age of 65 years, women were cogni-
based on the assumption that missings are at random. Since miss- tively 3 years younger than men. This female advantage
ings due to attrition are likely not at random, we repeated our anal- further increased with aging due to smaller decline in
yses based on data up to age 80 years (LASA and DCS), diminish- women (9% between ages 65 and 75 years, see also Fig. 1).
ing attrition not at random due to, e.g., illness and death, and test-
ed whether sex differences were different between participants
who did and did not participate for 10 years (LASA) or in the most Global Cognitive Function
recent measurement round (DCS). In the LASA, no significant differences in global cogni-
tive function or decline in global cognitive function (based
on MMSE) with aging were observed between men and
Results women (Table 2; Fig. 1). In the DCS, women scored better
than men on global cognitive function (based on all cogni-
In the LASA, the average age at baseline was 66 years and tive tests, Table 3). At the age of 65 years, women were
in the DCS, 55 years. In both cohorts, there were about as cognitively 4 years younger than men. This female advan-
many men as women. In all birth cohorts, men were higher tage became smaller with aging due to stronger decline in
educated than women and each subsequent birth cohort women (10% between ages 65 and 75 years). Despite this
was higher educated than the previous one (see online sup- convergence, the sex difference in global cognitive func-
pl. Table 3). More details on demographic characteristics of tion in the DCS remained statistically significant until af-
the study population are presented in Table 1. ter the age of 85 years (see also Fig. 1).
–0.5
23
(z-score)
–1.0
21
–1.5
19
–2.0
17 –2.5
15 –3.0
55 60 65 70 75 80 85 90 95 45 50 55 60 65 70 75 80 85
Age, years Age, years
1.0 1.0
Memory function (z-score)
1.0 1.0
Information processing speed
0.5 0.5
0 0
–0.5 –0.5
(z-score)
(z-score)
–1.0 –1.0
–1.5 –1.5
–2.0 –2.0
–2.5 –2.5
–3.0 –3.0
55 60 65 70 75 80 85 90 95 45 50 55 60 65 70 75 80 85
Age, years Age, years
1.0 1.0
Cognitive flexibility (z-score)
0.5 0.5
Fluid Intelligence (z-score)
0 0
–0.5 –0.5
–1.0 –1.0
–1.5 –1.5
–2.0 –2.0
–2.5 –2.5
–3.0 –3.0
55 60 65 70 75 80 85 90 95 45 50 55 60 65 70 75 80 85
Age, years Age, years
Fig. 1. Cognitive functions with aging for men and women in the LASA (plotted for age 55–99 years) and DCS
(plotted for age 45–85 years). Note that MMSE scores with aging of men and women in LASA are similar, and
therefore, only 1 line is visible in the figure. LASA, Longitudinal Aging Study Amsterdam; DCS, Doetinchem
Cohort Study; MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination.
Memory function Information processing speed Cognitive flexibility Global cognitive function
(z-score) (z-score) (z-score) (z-score)
model 1 (level of model 2 (cognitive model 1 (level of model 2 (cognitive model 1 (level of model 2 (cognitive model 1 (level of model 2 (cognitive
cognitive functioning) decline) cognitive functioning) decline) cognitive functioning) decline) cognitive functioning) decline)
By birth cohort
1928–1937
Sex 0.24 [0.12, 0.35]e,f,g 0.35 [0.16, 0.54] −0.04 [−0.17, 0.09]i,j,k 0.35 [0.03, 0.68] 0.08 [−0.05, 0.20] 0.30 [−0.07, 0.67] 0.00 [−0.12, 0.12]a,b,c 0.09 [−0.07, 0.26]
Sex × age −0.01 [−0.02, 0.00] −0.05 [−0.09, −0.01] −0.04 [−0.08, 0.01] −0.01 [−0.02, 0.00]
Sex × age2 0.00 [0.00, 0.00] 0.00 [0.00, 0.00]
1938–1947
Sex 0.48 [0.39, 0.57]e 0.48 [0.37, 0.59] 0.21 [0.11, 0.30]i 0.21 [0.11, 0.31] 0.18 [0.09, 0.27] 0.15 [0.05, 0.25] 0.33 [0.24, 0.42]a 0.36 [0.26, 0.46]
Sex × age 0.01 [−0.01, 0.02] −0.00 [−0.01, 0.00]l 0.01 [−0.00, 0.01]m −0.01 [−0.01, −0.00]
Sex × age2 −0.00 [−0.00, −0.00]
1948–1957
Sex 0.43 [0.35, 0.50]f,h 0.43 [0.35, 0.50] 0.23 [0.15, 0.31]j 0.22 [0.14, 0.30] 0.15 [0.07, 0.23] 0.15 [0.07, 0.23] 0.31 [0.23, 0.39]b,d 0.30 [0.22, 0.38]
Sex × age 0.00 [−0.01, 0.01] −0.01 [−0.01, −0.00]l −0.00 [−0.01, 0.00]m −0.01 [−0.01, −0.00]
1958–1967
Sex 0.57 [0.43, 0.70]g,h 0.56 [0.42, 0.70] 0.32 [0.18, 0.45]k 0.32 [0.18, 0.46] 0.19 [0.07, 0.31] 0.18 [0.06, 0.31] 0.45 [0.32, 0.57]c,d 0.45 [0.32, 0.58]
Sex × age −0.00 [−0.04, 0.03] 0.00 [−0.02, 0.03] −0.00 [−0.02, 0.02] 0.00 [−0.03, 0.03]
DOI: 10.1159/000520318
By educational level
Gerontology 2022;68:999–1009
Low
Sex 0.52 [0.33, 0.72] 0.54 [0.29, 0.80] 0.30 [0.03, 0.56] 0.36 [0.06, 0.65] 0.38 [0.13, 0.63]q 0.45 [0.16, 0.74] 0.41 [0.21, 0.62] 0.56 [0.31, 0.80]
Sex × age −0.00 [−0.02, 0.01] −0.01 [−0.03, 0.01] −0.01 [−0.03, 0.01]r −0.02 [−0.03, −0.00]
Medium
Sex 0.48 [0.41, 0.56] 0.48 [0.40, 0.56] 0.32 [0.24, 0.40]p 0.34 [0.25, 0.42] 0.24 [0.16, 0.32] 0.22 [0.14, 0.30] 0.38 [0.30, 0.45] 0.40 [0.32, 0.48]
Sex × age −0.00 [−0.01, 0.01] −0.00 [−0.01, 0.00] 0.01 [0.00, 0.01]r −0.01 [−0.01, −0.00]
High
Sex 0.52 [0.45, 0.58] 0.54 [0.47, 0.60] 0.23 [0.17, 0.29]p 0.24 [0.18, 0.30] 0.20 [0.14, 0.26]q 0.19 [0.13, 0.25] 0.41 [0.35, 0.47] 0.43 [0.36, 0.49]
Sex × age −0.01 [−0.01, −0.00] −0.00 [−0.01, −0.00] −0.00 [−0.01, 0.00] −0.01 [−0.01, −0.00]
Sex × age2 0.00 [0.00, 0.00]
Estimates in bold are statistically significant at p < 0.05; estimates with same superscript letters are significantly different at p < 0.05 (between comparable models only). For stratified analyses on the latest birth cohort (1958–
1967), no random slope was included in the model. DCS, Doetinchem Cohort Study. † Sex differences are presented as average [95% CL] and represent the average difference in cognitive function between men and women,
regardless of age (model 1) or at the age of 55 years (model 2). Positive values represent better scores (sex) or less decline for every year of aging (sex × age and sex × age2) for women than men. Sex differences are adjusted for
age and age squared, with age centered at the age of 55 years.
1005
ences in cognitive function, but sex differences in cogni- disadvantage on fluid intelligence with more recent birth
tive change were larger for memory, and decline in flex- cohort is not easily explained. Based on our results, exist-
ibility was now significantly larger in men than that in ing differences between men and women seem to have
women (online suppl. Table 6). Analyses on data from enlarged over time.
participants up to 80 years old yielded comparable results In agreement with the systematic review [5], earlier
to those based on all available data (online suppl. Table analyses on sex differences in cognitive decline over, re-
7). In the LASA, sex differences in memory function, but spectively, 6 and 16 years of follow-up in the LASA re-
not memory decline, were larger in the group who re- vealed no significant sex differences [27, 28]. However, in
mained >10 years in the study than persons who dropped the present study with 23 years of follow-up, we did ob-
out earlier. In the DCS, although persons who did not serve sex differences in cognitive decline in the LASA.
participate in the most recent measurement round had The disagreement may be explained by the lack of sex dif-
worse cognitive function and declined faster, attrition did ferences in cognitive decline in earlier birth cohorts in the
not affect sex differences in cognitive function or cogni- LASA (Table 2). Later studies observed either no sex dif-
tive decline. ferences in cognitive decline [6], or, in contrast to our
results, observed less cognitive decline among women [7,
8]. For instance, in the English Longitudinal Study of
Discussion Ageing [8], women aged 50 years and over showed sig-
nificantly less decline in memory and global cognitive
The objective of this study was to investigate sex dif- function than men over 8-year period of follow-up. One
ferences in cognitive function at middle age and in cogni- recent study among US adults [9] showed faster cognitive
tive decline with aging from middle to old age. We ob- decline in women for global cognitive function and ex-
served a female advantage for memory function, infor- ecutive function, but not for memory. In both our co-
mation processing speed, and cognitive flexibility; a horts, cognitive decline in memory function as well as
female disadvantage for fluid intelligence; and no sex dif- processing speed was faster in women than in men. The
ferences (in LASA) or a female advantage (in DCS) for differences in results can possibly be explained by the fact
global cognitive function. Overall, these sex differences in that we have more repeated measurements, and thus
cognitive function declined with aging, since women more power, for memory than in Levine’s study.
showed faster cognitive decline in memory and informa- Memory is typically first affected in Alzheimer’s dis-
tion processing speed than men. Sex differences in cogni- ease, while processing speed is, among other domains,
tive function were larger in more recent birth cohorts. affected in vascular dementia. Therefore, the stronger de-
After adjustment for the educational level, the female ad- cline in these domains in women may indicate higher risk
vantages were larger, indicating that, particularly in ear- of these types of dementia at older age in women. We did
lier cohorts, the lower average education level of women not investigate sex differences in different types of de-
suppressed part of the sex differences in cognitive func- mentia, but questioned whether sex differences in cogni-
tioning. tive function and/or cognitive decline at adult age exist.
In our study, men outperformed women in spatial These may explain sex differences in the total prevalence
tests (fluid intelligence) and women outperformed men of dementia, based on the assumption that lower cogni-
on verbal tests (memory test) and speed tests (substitu- tive reserve and/or stronger cognitive decline in general
tion test). In an opinion article, Hyde [4] wrote that such increases the risk of dementia at older age.
differences between men and women existed in psycho- In our analyses, with up to 23 years of follow-up, we
logical research from the 1930s through the 1970s, but observed that, in general, sex differences in cognitive
that these phenomena may have narrowed over time [4]. functioning became smaller from middle into old age.
In our longitudinal datasets, however, we observed that Cognitive function was modeled as a (nonlinear) func-
both the female advantages (memory and information tion of age, since age is the most important determinant
processing speed) as well as the female disadvantage (flu- of cognitive functioning [5]. Sex significantly interacted
id intelligence) became larger in later birth cohorts. The with age on cognitive functioning in the overall analyses.
larger female advantages in more recent birth cohorts In the stratified analyses, this interaction was less clear
may be explained by the fact that inequalities in educa- and significant in only few birth cohorts (Tables 2, 3). It
tional attainment between men and women declined was not clear whether the sex differences in cognitive de-
across birth cohorts. However, the increase of the female cline in the overall models actually were artifacts based on
The authors thank the respondents, epidemiologists, and field- The data of the Doetinchem Cohort Study pertaining to this
workers of the Longitudinal Aging Study Amsterdam and the Mu- manuscript are available upon request. We are unable to place data
nicipal Health Service in Doetinchem for their contribution to the in a public repository due to legal and ethical constraints. The par-
data collection for this study. ticipants’ informed consent did not include consent to public
availability of the data. However, the data are available upon re-
quest. We advise researchers to contact the corresponding author,
who will contact the scientific committee of the Doetinchem Co-
Statement of Ethics hort Study.
Both the DCS and LASA have been conducted in line with the
Helsinki Declaration. The LASA has been approved by the Medical
Ethics Committee of the VU University medical center (reference
numbers 92/138, 2002/141 and 2012/361) and the DCS by the ex-
ternal Medical Ethics Committee of the Netherlands Organization
for Applied Scientific Research (MEC codes 93/01 and 98/01) and