ARTICLE
A Review of Attachment
Theory in the Context of
Adolescent Parenting
Serena Cherry Flaherty, MSN, RN, CPNP,
& Lois S. Sadler, PhD, PNP-BC, FAAN
ABSTRACT This article provides a review of attachment theory
The purpose of this article is to review attachment theory and re- and relates the attachment perspective to the unique
late the attachment perspective to adolescent mothers and their challenges of clinical work with adolescent mothers
children. Attachment theory explains positive maternal-infant and their children. Infants of adolescent mothers are
attachment as a dyadic relationship between the infant and
at risk for poor attachment outcomes that are associated
mother that provides the infant with a secure base from which
to explore the world. With respect to cognitive, social, and be-
with long-term adverse consequences in cognitive,
havioral domains, securely attached infants tend to have more adaptive, and behavioral domains (Belsky & Fearon,
favorable long-term outcomes, while insecurely attached infants 2002; Karen, 1990). A secure attachment relationship
are more likely to have adverse outcomes. Adolescent parent- evolves from a mother’s ability to be reflective,
hood can disrupt normal adolescent development, and this dis- responsive, and sensitive to her infant’s needs
ruption influences development of the emotional and cognitive and results in the infant’s development of trust,
capacities necessary for maternal behaviors that foster secure at- confidence, and resilience in later life (Karen, 1990).
tachment. However, it appears that if specialized supports are in Adolescent mothers
place to facilitate the process of developing attachment, infants may not intuitively be
of adolescent mothers can obtain higher rates of secure attach-
It is critical for
able to assume these
ment than normative samples in this population. J Pediatr Health
characteristics that fos-
clinicians to be able
Care. (2011) 25, 114-121.
ter secure attachment to recognize signs
because of their own of poor
KEY WORDS
developmental stage
Attachment, maternal-infant attachment, adolescent mother
(Sadler & Cowlin,
attachment.and
2003). It is critical for to learn to model
Serena Cherry Flaherty, Pediatric Nurse Practitioner, The Door -
clinicians to be able to favorable parenting
Adolescent Health Center, New York, NY.
recognize signs of
Lois S. Sadler, Associate Professor Yale School of Nursing and poor attachment that
behaviors that
Yale Child Study Center, Assitant Dean for Academic Affairs, Yale
School of Nursing.
can most easily be enhance
picked up by observa- attachment.
This work was supported by the following grants: NIH/NINR
tions of the mother’s in-
(P30NR08999), NIH/NICHD (R21HD048591), and NIH/NICHD
(R01HD057947). teractions with her infant and to learn to model
favorable parenting behaviors that enhance attach-
Conflicts of interest: None to report.
ment. Secure attachment is a critical part of the founda-
Correspondence: Serena Cherry Flaherty, MSN, RN, CPNP, The tion for a healthy life. Therefore, maternal-infant
Door - Adolescent Health Center, 121 Avenue of the Americas,
interactions, particularly in this high-risk adolescent
New York, NY 10013; e-mail:
[email protected].
population, need to be assessed in the context of pedi-
0891-5245/$36.00 atric clinical care.
Copyright Q 2011 by the National Association of Pediatric Attachment theory has evolved from work by numer-
Nurse Practitioners. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights ous researchers, primarily John Bowlby and Mary Ains-
reserved.
worth, and later Mary Main (Ainsworth, 1982;
doi:10.1016/j.pedhc.2010.02.005 Ainsworth, 1985; Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall,
114 Volume 25 Number 2 Journal of Pediatric Health Care
1978; Bowlby, 1969/1982; Bowlby, 1973; Bowlby, 1980; The many challenges and consequences associated
Main, Kaplan, & Cassidy, 1985). Attachment, according with teen pregnancy and parenthood are well
to Ainsworth (1963) provides a secure base from which documented, but less is known about attachment rela-
the child can explore the world, and this idea has since tionships among adolescent mother-infant dyads
remained a fundamental principle of attachment the- (Manlove, Ikramullah, Mincieli, Holcombe, & Danish,
ory. Bowlby (1969/1982) subsequently described at- 2009; Moore & Brooks-Gunn, 2002; Patterson, 1997).
tachment as a unique relationship between an infant Many of the background and developmental
and his or her caregiver that is the foundation for further characteristics of adolescent mothers also may be
healthy development. Bowlby described attachment linked to poor attachment outcomes in their infants.
theory as an inherent biological response and behav- Poverty, poor parental modeling, growing up in
ioral system in place to provide satisfaction of basic hu- single-parent homes, and lack of educational opportu-
man needs. Mary Main, a student of Ainsworth’s, found nities and career goals often are associated with teen
that adult attachment representations, the construct of pregnancy and early parenthood (Coley & Chase-
how adults remember their own childhood experi- Lansdale, 1998; Manlove et al., 2009; Moore & Brooks-
ences, might influence the attachment categorization Gunn, 2002; Patterson, 1997). Adolescent mothers are
of their children (Main et al., 1985). less likely to receive adequate prenatal care and are
Attachment security and the theory of the internal more likely to experience pregnancy and birth
working model are two hallmark ideas that comprise at- complications, often because they are likely to be
tachment theory and influence how the child views living in poverty (Coley & Chase-Lansdale, 1998). An
himself or herself and other relationships (Belsky & analysis of the National Longitudinal Study of
Fearon, 2002; Cassidy, 2008). Whether mother-child Adolescent Health from 1994 through 2008 revealed
interactions are positive or negative, some level of that adolescent parents are more likely to be from fam-
attachment security and subsequent internal working ilies that report incomes below 200% of the federal
model develops (Carlson & Sroufe, 1995). According poverty level (FPL) (59% of survey respondents
to Bowlby (1969/1982), individuals develop ‘‘internal reported living in families with incomes below 200%
working models’’ of attachment that describe the rela- of the FPL, and 41% reported living in families with
tionship between the infant’s self and his or her attach- incomes greater than or equal to 200% of the FPL)
ment figure. In response to experiences and behavior of (The National Campaign to Prevent Teen and
the attachment figure toward the infant, the infant is Unplanned Pregnancy, 2009). Adolescent mothers are
able to formulate mental responses to the attachment more likely to suffer from depression and have a higher
figure’s behavior that are catalogued as mental repre- substance abuse potential (Clemmens, 2001;
sentations of the infant’s view of himself or herself Panzarine, Slater, & Sharps, 1995; Reid & Meadows-
and understanding of the attachment figure Oliver, 2007; Spieker, Gillmore, Lewis, Morrison, &
(Bretherton, 1992). The infant’s ability to explore the Lohr, 2001). In addition, they often have fewer
world and relationships within it hinges on the type resources, less social support, and an increased
of attachment security that develops during the first potential for child abuse and neglect (de Paul &
year of life (Belsky & Cassidy, 1994). In the context of Domenech, 2000; Panzarine et al., 1995; Turner,
attachment theory, it is important to distinguish Grindstaff, & Phillips, 1990; Whitman, Borkowski,
attachment behavior and attachment bond. Attachment Keogh, & Weed,, 2001; Zuravin & DiBlasio, 1992).
behavior is behavior on the part of the infant that pro- These factors, independently and collectively, heighten
motes proximity to the attachment figure, such as smil- the risk of compromised parenting behaviors with
ing and vocalization (Carlson & Sroufe, 1995; Cassidy, these young families. Parenting behaviors among ado-
2008). Attachment bond, however, is described by lescent mothers vary, but many of them experience
Ainsworth and Bowlby not as a dyadic and reciprocal higher degrees of stress related to parenting and tend
relationship existing between the infant and his or her to be less responsive, less sensitive, more detached,
caregiver, but rather as the infant’s interpretation of and more likely to exhibit intrusive behaviors with their
his or her relationship to his or her mother (Cassidy, infants (Berlin, Brady-Smith, & Brooks-Gunn, 2002;
2008). Evidence supports the positive influence of Whitman et al., 2001). These parenting characteristics
secure mother-child attachment on later development specific to adolescent parents are precisely what place
and aptitude (Slade & Aber, 1992). A secure attachment them at risk for compromised attachment
system serves as a foundation for expression of relationships. We are only beginning to learn about
emotions and communication in future relationships, the quality of attachment relationships among infants
provides opportunities for self-regulation of affect of adolescent mothers, but it appears they often are
(the ability to consider emotional processes before compromised, leading to less optimal infant outcomes
responding), and creates potential for resilience in developmental and socioemotional domains, all of
(Belsky & Cassidy, 1994; Carlson & Sroufe, 1995; which are more likely to be the case when there are
Cassidy, 2008; Karen, 1990). limited supportive programs or family members
www.jpedhc.org March/April 2011 115
available to help the young mother in her new and anxious-avoidant attachment (Group A), and anxious-
complex parenting roles. resistant attachment (Group C) (Ainsworth et al.,
In addition to the socioeconomic profile of many 1978). Subsequently, Mary Main later classified an
adolescent mothers that may contribute to poor attach- additional insecure group as Group D, which repre-
ment outcomes, adolescent mothers differ develop- sents disorganized-disoriented attachment (Main &
mentally from most adult mothers because they are Solomon, 1990).
working to combine their adolescent developmental Securely attached infants account for 65% to 70% of
tasks with the new tasks and roles of parenthood infants, and they actively seek attention from their
(Flanagan, McGrath, Meyer, & Garcia Coll, 1995; mothers, may become distressed upon the mother’s de-
Moriarty Daley, Sadler & Reynolds, in press; Sadler & parture or may decrease play and exploration, and are
Cowlin, 2003). When a pregnancy occurs during able to be consoled during the reunion episode of the
adolescence, the period of development during SSP (Ainsworth et al., 1978). Anxious-avoidant infants
which adolescents develop the cognitive skills to represent about 20% to 25% of U.S. samples and can
assume parenting responsibilities is interrupted behave similarly to securely attached infants until the
(Whitman et al., 2001). As a result, many adolescent separation period of the SSP, when they do not appear
mothers may not have the developmental capacity to to be affected by mother’s departure. During the
adopt parenting behaviors that enhance the maternal- reunion episode, anxious-avoidant infants will not
infant attachment relationship. Adolescents tend to be approach their mother and may protest her return by
idealistic, have a diminished capacity for reflection, avoiding her (Ainsworth et al., 1978; Slade & Aber,
and tend to embody egocentricity, individuality, and 1992). Anxious-resistant infants account for less than
independence (Hamburg, 1998). Adolescent develop- 10% of U.S. infants (Slade & Aber, 1992). They tend to
ment allows for the transition to higher levels of cogni- lack comfort in exploration in a strange environment,
tive function and ability to appreciate more abstract are significantly distressed during the separation
processes (Elkind, 1998). Maternal characteristics episode, and at the point of reunion are torn between
that enhance the attachment relationship, such as sensi- a desire for proximity and resistance to comfort
tive parenting, reflectivity, and responsiveness, are (Ainsworth et al., 1978; Carlson & Sroufe, 1995; Slade
challenging for adolescent mothers to intuitively adopt & Aber, 1992). Disorganized-disoriented infants have
because often they do not have the cognitive awareness no standard response to stress of separation and
afforded by full adult development (Moriarty Daley reunion and appear to have disorganized and unpre-
et al., in press; Sadler, Anderson, & Sabatelli, 2001). dictable patterns of behavior (Main & Solomon, 1990).
Literature exists to suggest cross-cultural application
MEASUREMENT OF ATTACHMENT of these attachment patterns. Sagi-Schwartz and Van
Historically, the Strange Situation Procedure (SSP) has IJzendoorn (2008) reviewed cross-cultural attachment
been the standard method for assessing attachment research and determined that the three attachment pat-
(Ainsworth et al., 1978). In a laboratory setting with terns applied using the SSP are universal. Attachment
a mother and her infant present, the attachment classification, therefore, is dependent on the relation-
relationship is stressed and the attachment system is ac- ship of the infant and mother, and although there may
tivated by various episodes of the mother and a stranger be cultural factors that affect emotional responses and
coming and going. The infant’s response to the behaviors, it does not appear that attachment outcomes
situation, particularly during the mother-child reunion, are directly associated with specific race or culture.
is reflective of the child’s attachment pattern (Ainsworth
et al., 1978; Van IJzendoorn & Kroonenberg, 1988). The PATTERNS OF ATTACHMENT AND
SSP is videotaped, and the attachment category is SUBSEQUENT OUTCOMES
determined by standardized procedures for scoring Secure attachment can be protective and provide a foun-
(Ainsworth et al., 1978). Mary Main later developed dation for exploration and normal development, while
the Adult Attachment Interview to assess adult compromised attachment can lead to negative develop-
attachment representations, which reveals how adults mental outcomes in these domains (Greenberg, Speltz,
interpret their own childhood experiences (Hesse, & DeKlyen, 1993). Additionally, secure attachment
1999; Main et al., 1985; van IJzendoorn, 1995). relationships are associated with appropriate social
development and the ability to interact with others
ATTACHMENT CLASSIFICATIONS throughout life, and persons with insecure attachment
Patterns of attachment and attachment classifications are more likely to lack social aptitude (Belsky &
are associated with various outcomes. Ainsworth Cassidy, 1994; Belsky & Fearon, 2002). Specifically,
(1978) established a rating system based on the infant’s children with anxious-avoidant and anxious-resistant
behavior during the SSP, particularly during the reunion attachment styles have been reported to have behavior
episodes with the mother. She described three cate- problems, emotional difficulties, and social incompe-
gories of attachment: secure attachment (Group B), tence (Belsky & Cassidy, 1994; Carlson & Sroufe, 1995).
116 Volume 25 Number 2 Journal of Pediatric Health Care
It also is understood that compromised attachment both to affect the quality of attachment (Calkins & Fox,
relationships often are correlated with developmental 1992) and to have no influence on attachment security
cognitive delays (Carlson & Sroufe, 1995). Behavioral (Belsky & Rovine, 1987; Mangelsdorf & Frosch, 2000).
problems, particularly aggression, have been associated There is speculation that infant gender influences the
with disorganized-disoriented attachment (Lyons-Ruth, quality of attachment (Greenberg, 1999). Furthermore,
Alpern, & Repacholi, 1993). The idea that attachment poor infant health is thought to compromise the quality
classifications correlate with later developmental course of attachment. Literature on this topic specifically ad-
is central to attachment theory (Bowlby, 1969/1982). Re- dresses the influence of preterm status and low-birth
searchers acknowledge the contribution of environmen- weight on maternal-infant attachment (Greenberg,
tal factors, but consistent with Bowlby’s notion of the 1999; Mangelsdorf, Plunkett, Dedrick, & Berlin, 1996;
important developmental considerations related to at- Wille, 1991). Preterm and low-birth weight is not inher-
tachment, researchers continue to assess the impact of ently associated with poor attachment, but those infants
the quality of attachment (Belsky & Cassidy, 1994; may be more at risk for developmental delays and
Greenberg et al., 1993). Belsky & Fearon (2002) found physical, cognitive, and visual impairments. As a result,
correlations between different patterns of attachment attachment behaviors such as smiling, crying, and resis-
and socioemotional and language development but tance to separations may look very different in these in-
also stressed the influence of contextual risk factors fants. However, the maternal response designed to
that contribute to general cognitive development. promote security can still be observed. It is important to
remember that attach-
RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS ment is a reciprocal .although an
A number of risk and protective factors contribute to relationship between
maternal-infant attachment relationships. Maternal the caregiver and the in- infant with
responsiveness and sensitivity refers to the mother’s fant, and although an in- compromised
ability to respond to her infant’s cues and her ability to fant with compromised health status may
understand the infant’s experiences and own mental health status may not
state (Slade, 2005). The greater ability a mother has to respond with the same not respond with
be sensitive and responsive to her infant, the greater attachment behavior the same
likelihood the infant will develop a secure attachment patterns as a healthy attachment
(Bornstein & Tamis LeMonda, 1989; Bornstein & Tamis infant, attachment rela-
LeMonda, 1990; Bornstein & Tamis LeMonda, 1997; tionships can still be as- behavior patterns
Dunham & Dunham, 1990; Goodman, Aber, Berlin, & sessed and enhanced. as a healthy infant,
Brooks-Gunn, 1998; Koren-Karie, Oppenheim, Dolev, Environmental fac- attachment
Sher, & Etzion-Carasso, 2002; Landry, Smith, Miller tors, such as social sup-
Loncar, & Swank, 1998; Olson, Bates, & Kaskie, 1992; port and the influence relationships can
Raval et al., 2001; Seifer & Schiller, 1995; Van Egeren, of multiple caregivers, still be assessed
Barratt, & Roach, 2001). Additionally, a mother’s ability may influence the qual- and enhanced.
to assume the maternal role can affect the quality of ity of attachment. Social
attachment (Aber, Belsky, Slade, & Crnic, 1999; Benoit, support is thought to be
Parker, & Zeanah., 1997). a protective factor in maternal-infant attachment relation-
Conversely, maternal depression and psychological dis- ships (Belsky, 1999; Berlin & Cassidy, 1999; Huth-Bocks,
tress can adversely affect the mother-infant attachment Levendosky, Bogat, & von Eye, 2004; Simpson, 1999).
relationship (Murray, Kempton, Woolgar, & Hooper, Particularly relevant to adolescent mothers is the
1993; Murray, Fiori Cowley, Hooper, & Cooper, 1996; presence of the maternal grandmother, which alters the
Poehlmann & Fiese, 2001; Tronick & Weinberg, 1997). A maternal-infant attachment relationship and provides
mother’s ability to recollect her own childhood and the child with the possibility of multiple attachment rela-
recall her experiences with her primary caregiver tionships (Cassidy, 2008; Howes, 1999; Patterson, 1997;
(maternal representation) also affects the maternal-infant Poehlmann & Fiese, 2001). Mothers have been most
attachment relationship. Poor maternal attachment repre- frequently studied in attachment research, primarily
sentations, when a mother recalls unfavorable relation- because they are most often the parent providing the
ships in her past, may compromise the development of secure base from which the reciprocal attachment
secure infant attachment (Fonagy, Steele, & Steele, 1991; relationship can develop. However, it is increasingly
Goldberg, Benoit, Blokland, & Madigan, 2003; Huth- being recognized that the attachment relationship is not
Bocks, Levendosky, Bogat, & von Eye, 2004; Slade, 2005). limited to mothers and infants, and an infant can
Some literature suggests that infant characteristics develop a positive attachment with other consistent
may play a role in the development of attachment caregivers, including the infant’s father, and often the
relationships. Difficult temperaments have been shown maternal grandmother (Cassidy, 2008).
www.jpedhc.org March/April 2011 117
Although there is not a significant amount of research that the dyadic attachment relationship has room to
related to attachment patterns of infants of adolescent evolve in a healthy direction. Furthermore, because
mothers, existing research reveals that adolescent adolescent mothers sometimes have unreasonable
mothers are at risk for poor attachment outcomes. developmental or behavioral expectations for their
Among this group, adolescent mothers living in infants, it also can be helpful to coach or demonstrate
poverty, those with lower educational achievements, the infant’s developmental capacity at different ages
and those with poor social support have lower rates and stages of development.
of secure attachment in their infants. Although there are increasingly more demands and
time constraints around pediatric well child care visits,
CLINICAL APPLICATION introducing attachment behaviors can easily be incor-
Attachment theory provides a framework from which to porated into the course of a routine examination. A
understand and care for adolescent mothers and their well child care visit is a perfect opportunity to assess
infants. For the first time since 1991, the birth rate for the attachment relationship and explain and model
adolescents living in the United States increased in some of the behaviors on the part of the mother in re-
2006, and it increased again in 2007 (Hamilton, sponse to the infant’s behaviors that can improve this
Martin, & Ventura, 2009; Martin et al., 2009; Moore, relationship. Separations and reunions between the in-
2008). As a result of this rise, more pediatric clinicians fant and mother that take place over the course of a visit
will be caring for adolescent parents and their are a simple means by which the clinician can assess
children. Secure attachment relationships can lead to and coach adolescent mothers on ways to improve
favorable outcomes both in the early years and as the the attachment relationship. A securely attached infant
child matures into adulthood, and it is an important will show some suspicion around the stranger (the
foundation that should not be overlooked in the clinician) and will continue to make contact with his
clinical setting. While attachment theory certainly has or her mother, or check in with her to ensure his or
psychological applications, pediatric clinicians in the her safety (Slade & Aber, 1992). When this behavior
primary care setting are on the front lines and in occurs (for example, when the clinician enters the
a position to identify, assess, and enhance the room or begins the physical examination), the clinician
attachment relationship. Ideally, pediatric clinicians can explain to the mother that the infant is looking
can begin working with young mothers and their around to make sure his or her mother is still present
infants immediately after birth when the attachment and that he or she feels safe with her in the room.
relationship is first being established. When the child is removed from the mother’s presence,
To begin, clinicians can simply explain the attach- perhaps simply by stepping out of the room with the in-
ment relationship as a reflection of how the infant and fant, he or she may begin crying and looking for the
mother respond to one another. Additionally, the clini- mother (Slade & Aber, 1992). A simple means to assess
cian can explain the infant’s attachment behaviors, such gross motor skills and understand the infant’s attach-
as smiling, crying, reaching, and resisting separations ment security simultaneously is to bring the infant out
from the mother. Attachment behaviors ought to be of the examination room and out of the mother’s sight
met with a maternal response that will promote a feeling and let him or her crawl or walk back to the mother. It
of security in the child. Clinicians can look for attach- is important for the adolescent mother to understand
ment behaviors in the visit, such as when the infant that the crying infant is a strong indicator as to the secu-
cries, follows (or tracks the mother), or how the child rity the child feels with respect to his or her mother.
references the mother during greetings and reunions Finally, when the infant reunites with the mother, the
after short separations. During the visit, clinicians can infant often allows the mother to pick him or her up
model caregiver-infant relationships or coach and and provide comfort before returning to play (Slade &
encourage young mothers to assume some of the Aber, 1992). This can be a very powerful message for
behaviors that favor more secure attachment outcomes. a young teen mother who may not understand the ex-
Adolescent mothers may need help reinterpreting the tent of her role as a secure figure in her infant’s life. In
infant’s behaviors as cues to the parent about the contrast to the secure infant, insecurely attached infants
infant’s state and needs. For example, when an infant may show little wariness around strangers, often make
cries, a young mother may become frustrated with no protest when removed from their mother’s sight, and
what can be a disrupting physical state and either offer little attention to their mother when reunited
ignore the infant or rush the pick the infant up for a feed- (Slade & Aber, 1992). It can be very striking to observe
ing. Rather, the clinician can guide the mother to appre- infant behavior, particularly as it relates to the mother’s
ciate that the infant is crying to express an emotional presence, and it can offer the clinician insight into the
state, ask the mother how she feels (with an ‘‘I won- existing attachment relationship and provide teachable
der.’’ statement) when the infant cries, and how she moments during the visit to enhance the relationship.
thinks the baby might be feeling. It is with recognition Observation of attachment behaviors is not limited to
of maternal and infant emotional and physical states the primary care setting; pediatric clinicians in acute
118 Volume 25 Number 2 Journal of Pediatric Health Care
care and urgent care settings also can use knowledge of attachment in human behavior (pp. 3-30). New York, NY: Basic
attachment behaviors to aid in enhancing the attach- Books.
Ainsworth, M. D. (1963). The development of infant-mother interac-
ment relationship. An acute care setting may in fact re- tion among the Ganda. In B. M. Foss (Ed.), Determinants of
veal even more about the nature of the attachment infant behavior (pp. 67-112). New York, NY: Wiley.
relationship, because it is in times of stress and separa- Ainsworth, M. D. (1985). Patterns of attachment. Clinical Psycholo-
tion that poor attachment behaviors often are revealed. gist, 38(2), 27-29.
Clinicians in an acute care setting can easily adapt the Ainsworth, M. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978).
Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange
guidelines for encouraging attachment behaviors as situation. Oxford, England: Lawrence Erlbaum.
described for a primary care setting, because infant at- Belsky, J., & Cassidy, J. (1994). Attachment: Theory and evidence. In
tachment behaviors such as crying, smiling, and resist- M. Rutter & D. Hay (Eds.), Development through life: A hand-
ing separations and the maternal response to these book for clinicians (pp. 373-402). Oxford: Blackwell.
behaviors can be observed in any environment. Belsky, J. (1999). Interactional and contextual determinants of
attachment security. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Hand-
Especially in stressful environments such as hospital book of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications
settings, the attachment system affords the clinician (pp. 249-264). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
with a way of explaining the child’s behavior to the par- Belsky, J., & Fearon, R. M. P. (2002). Infant-mother attachment secu-
ent and offers opportunities for coaching to enhance rity, contextual risk, and early development: A moderational
the attachment relationship. analysis. Development & Psychopathology, 14(2), 293-310.
Belsky, J., & Rovine, M. (1987). Temperament and attachment
security in the strange situation: An empirical rapprochement.
CONCLUSION Child Development, 58(3), 787-795.
Positive maternal-infant attachment can enhance Benoit, D., Parker, K. C. H., & Zeanah, C. H. (1997). Mothers’ repre-
a child’s development and ability to explore the world sentations of their infants assessed prenatally: Stability and
from a secure emotional foundation. While little re- association with infants’ attachment classifications. Journal of
Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 38(3), 307-313.
search is available that Berlin, L. J., Brady-Smith, C., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2002). Links be-
is specifically related Young mothers tween childbearing age and observed maternal behaviors with
to attachment out- often benefit from 14-month-olds in the early head start research and evaluation
comes of infants of ad- project. Infant Mental Health Journal, 23(1-2), 104-129.
olescent mothers, at guidance in the Berlin, L. J., & Cassidy, J. (1999). Relations among relationships:
Contributions from attachment theory and research. In
present we know that process of J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: The-
adolescent mothers assuming the ory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 688-712). New
and their infants gener- York, NY: Guilford Press.
ally have lower rates of maternal role in Bornstein, M. H., & Tamis LeMonda, C. S. (1997). Maternal respon-
secure attachment. a way that will siveness and infant mental abilities: Specific predictive relations.
Infant Behavior & Development, 20(3), 283-296.
These young dyads of- facilitate secure Bornstein, M. H., & Tamis LeMonda, C. S. (1990). Activities and inter-
ten lack the cognitive actions of mothers and their firstborn infants in the first six
maturity and resources attachment, and months of life: Covariation, stability, continuity, correspon-
to appreciate the ma- this can be dence, and prediction. Child Development, 61(4), 1206-1217.
ternal and infant be- Bornstein, M. H., & Tamis LeMonda, C. S. (1989, Spring). Maternal
accomplished in responsiveness and cognitive development in children. New Di-
haviors that are
associated with more the course of a well- rections for Child Development, 43, 49-61.
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Vol. 3. Sadness and depres-
favorable attachment child visit. sion (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Basic Books.
relationships. Young Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation: Anxiety
mothers often benefit from guidance in the process of and anger. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1969/1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment
assuming the maternal role in a way that will facilitate (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Basic Books.
secure attachment, and this can be accomplished in Bretherton, I. (1992). The origins of attachment theory: John Bowlby
the course of a well-child visit. Secure attachment is as- and Mary Ainsworth. Developmental Psychology, 28(5), 759-775.
sociated with positive long-term cognitive, social, and Calkins, S. D., & Fox, N. A. (1992). The relations among infant tem-
behavioral outcomes, and it is worth bringing attach- perament, security of attachment, and behavioral inhibition at
twenty-four months. Child Development, 63(6), 1456-1472.
ment theory into practice as a means to facilitate secure Carlson, E. A., & Sroufe, L. A. (1995). Contribution of attachment
attachment in a population particularly at risk for poor theory to developmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti &
attachment patterns. D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology, Vol. 1:
Theory and methods (pp. 581-617). Oxford, England: John
Wiley & Sons.
REFERENCES Cassidy, J. (2008). The nature of the child’s ties. In J. Cassidy &
Aber, J. L., Belsky, J., Slade, A., & Crnic, K. (1999). Stability and P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research,
change in mothers’ representations of their relationship with and clinical applications (pp. 2-22). New York, NY: Guilford
their toddlers. Developmental Psychology, 35(4), 1038-1047. Press.
Ainsworth, M. D. (1982). Attachment: Retrospect and prospect. In Clemmens, D. (2001). The relationship between social support
C. M. Parkes & J. Stevenson-Hinde (Eds.), The place of and adolescent mothers’ interactions with their infants:
www.jpedhc.org March/April 2011 119
A meta-analysis. Journal of Obstetric, Gynecologic, & Neonatal Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in infancy, child-
Nursing, 30(4), 410-420. hood, and adulthood: A move to the level of representation.
Coley, R. L., & Chase-Lansdale, P. L. (1998). Adolescent pregnancy Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development,
and parenthood: Recent evidence and future directions. 50(1-2), 66-104.
American Psychologist, 53(2), 152-166. Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1990). Procedures for identifying infants as
de Paul, J., & Domenech, L. (2000). Childhood history of abuse and disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth strange situa-
child abuse potential in adolescent mothers: A longitudinal tion. In M. T. Greenberg & D. Cicchetti (Eds.), Attachment in
study. Child Abuse & Neglect, 24(5), 701-713. the preschool years: Theory, research, and intervention (pp.
Dunham, P., & Dunham, F. (1990). Effects of mother-infant social 121-160). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
interactions on infants’ subsequent contingency task perfor- Mangelsdorf, S. C., & Frosch, C. A. (2000). Temperament and
mance. Child Development, 61(3), 785-793. attachment: One construct or two? In H. W. Reese (Ed.),
Elkind, D. (1998). Cognitive development. In S. B. Friedman, Advances in child development and behavior (vol. 27, pp.
K. Schonberg, E. M. Alderman, & M. M. Fisher (Eds.), Compre- 181-220) San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
hensive adolescent health care (2nd ed). St. Louis, MO: Mosby. Mangelsdorf, S. C., Plunkett, J. W., Dedrick, C. F., & Berlin, M.
Flanagan, P. F., McGrath, M. M., Meyer, E. C., & Garcia Coll, C. (1996). Attachment security in very low birth weight infants.
(1995). Adolescent development and the transitions to mother- Developmental Psychology, 32(5), 914-920.
hood. Pediatrics, 96, 272-277. Manlove, J., Ikramullah, E., Mincieli, L., Holcombe, E., & Danish, S.
Fonagy, P., Steele, H., & Steele, M. (1991). Maternal representations (2009). Trends in sexual experience, contraceptive use and
of attachment during pregnancy predict the organization of teenage childbearing 1992-2002. Journal of Adolescent Health,
infant-mother attachment at one year of age. Child Develop- 44, 413-423.
ment, 62(5), 891-905. Martin, J., Hamilton, B., Sutton, P., Ventura, S., Menacker, F., Kir-
Goldberg, S., Benoit, D., Blokland, K., & Madigan, S. (2003). Atypical meyer, S., . Matthews, T. J. (2009). Births: Final data for
maternal behavior, maternal representations, and infant 2006. National Vital Statistics Reports, 57(7), 1-13.
disorganized attachment. Development & Psychopathology, Moore, K. (2008). Teen births: Examining the recent increase. Re-
15(2), 239-257. trieved from National Campaign toPrevent Teen and Unplanned
Goodman, G., Aber, J. L., Berlin, L., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1998). The Pregnancy website: http://www.thenationalcampaign.org/
relations between maternal behaviors and urban preschool chil- resources/pdf/TeenBirths_ExamIncrease.pdf
dren’s internal working models of attachment security. Infant Moore, M. R., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2002). Adolescent parenthood.
Mental Health Journal, 19(4), 378-393. In E. In M. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting (2nd ed.,
Greenberg, M. T. (1999). Attachment and psychopathology in child- Vol. 4, pp. 173-214). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
hood. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attach- Moriarty Daley, A.M., Sadler, L.S., & Reynolds, H. (in press). Adoles-
ment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 469-496). cent pregnancy and parenthood. In I. Alexander (Ed.), Women’s
New York, NY: Guilford Press. health care. New York, NY: Blackwell.
Greenberg, M. T., Speltz, M. L., & DeKlyen, M. (1993). The role of at- Murray, L., Kempton, C., Woolgar, M., & Hooper, R. (1993). Depressed
tachment in the early development of disruptive behavior prob- mothers’ speech to their infants and its relation to infant gender
lems. Development & Psychopathology, 5(1-2), 191-213. and cognitive development. Journal of Child Psychology &
Hamburg, B. A. (1998). Psychosocial development. In S. B. Friedman, Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 34(7), 1083-1101.
K. Schonberg, E. M. Alderman, & M. M. Fisher (Eds.), Compre- Murray, L., Fiori Cowley, A., Hooper, R., & Cooper, P. (1996). The im-
hensive adolescent health care (2nd ed). St. Louis, MO: Mosby. pact of postnatal depression and associated adversity on early
Hamilton, B., Martin, J., & Ventura, S. (2009). Births: Preliminary data mother-infant interactions and later infant outcomes. Child
for 2007. National Vital Statistics Reports, 57(12), 1-23. Development, 67(5), 2512-2526.
Hesse, E. (1999). The adult attachment interview: Historical and cur- Olson, S. L., Bates, J. E., & Kaskie, B. (1992). Caregiver-infant inter-
rent perspectives. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Hand- action antecedents of children’s school-age cognitive ability.
book of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 38(3), 309-330.
applications (pp. 395-433). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Panzarine, S., Slater, E., & Sharps, P. (1995). Coping, social support,
Howes, C. (1999). Attachment relationships in the context of multiple and depressive symptoms in adolescent mothers. Journal of
caregivers. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of at- Adolescent Health, 17(2), 113-119.
tachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 671- Patterson, D. (1997). Adolescent mothering: Child-grandmother
687). New York, NY: Guilford Press. attachment. Journal of Pediatric Nursing, 12, 228-237.
Huth-Bocks, A. C., Levendosky, A. A., Bogat, G. A., & von Eye, A. Poehlmann, J., & Fiese, B. H. (2001). The interaction of maternal and
(2004). The impact of maternal characteristics and contextual infant vulnerabilities on developing attachment relationships.
variables on infant-mother attachment. Child Development, Development & Psychopathology, 13(1), 1-11.
75(2), 480-496. Raval, V., Goldberg, S., Atkinson, L., Benoit, D., Myhal, N., Poulton,
Karen, R. (1990, February). Becoming attached. Atlantic Monthly, L., . Zwiers, M. (2001). Maternal attachment, maternal respon-
35-70. siveness and infant attachment. Infant Behavior & Develop-
Koren-Karie, N., Oppenheim, D., Dolev, S., Sher, E., & Etzion-Car- ment, 24(3), 281-304.
asso, A. (2002). Mothers’ insightfulness regarding their infants’ Reid, V., & Meadows-Oliver, M. (2007). Postpartum depression in
internal experience: Relations with maternal sensitivity adolescent mothers: An integrative literature review. Journal
and infant attachment. Developmental Psychology, 38(4), of Pediatric Health Care, 21, 289-298.
534-542. Sadler, L. S., Anderson, S. A., & Sabatelli, R. M. (2001). Parental
Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Miller Loncar, C. L., & Swank, P. R. competence among African American adolescent mothers
(1998). The relation of change in maternal interactive styles to and grandmothers. Journal of Pediatric Nursing, 16, 217-233.
the developing social competence of full-term and Sadler, L. S., & Cowlin, A. (2003). Moving into parenthood: A program
preterm children. Child Development, 69(1), 105-123. for new adolescent mothers combining parent education with
Lyons-Ruth, K., Alpern, L., & Repacholi, B. (1993). Disorganized in- creative physical activity. Journal of Specialists in Pediatric
fant attachment classification and maternal psychosocial prob- Nursing, 8, 62-70.
lems as predictors of hostile-aggressive behavior in the Sagi-Schwartz, A., & Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2008). Cross cultural
preschool classroom. Child Development, 64(2), 572-585. patterns of attachment: universal and contextual dimensions.
120 Volume 25 Number 2 Journal of Pediatric Health Care
In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Tronick, E. Z., & Weinberg, M. K. (1997). Depressed mothers and
Theory, research, and clinical applications (2nd ed., pp. 880- infants: Failure to form dyadic states of consciousness. In
905) New York, NY: Guilford Press. L. Murray & P. J. Cooper (Eds.), Postpartum depression
Seifer, R., & Schiller, M. (1995). The role of parenting sensitivity, infant and child development (pp. 54-81). New York, NY: Guilford
temperament, and dyadic interaction in attachment theory and Press.
assessment. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Turner, R. J., Grindstaff, C. F., & Phillips, N. (1990). Social support
Development, 60(2-3), 146-174. and outcome in teenage pregnancy. Journal of Health & Social
Simpson, J. A. (1999). Attachment theory in modern evolutionary Behavior, 31(1), 43-57.
perspective. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of Van Egeren, L. A., Barratt, M. S., & Roach, M. A. (2001). Mother-
attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. infant responsiveness: Timing, mutual regulation, and interac-
115-140). New York, NY: Guilford Press. tional context. Developmental Psychology, 37(5), 684-697.
Slade, A. (2005). Parental reflective functioning: An introduction. At- Van IJzendoorn, M. (1995). Adult attachment representations,
tachment & Human Development, 7(3), 269-281. parental responsiveness, and infant attachment: A meta-
Slade, A., & Aber, J. L. (1992). Attachments, drives, and development: analysis on the predictive validity of the adult attachment
Conflicts and convergences in theory. In J. W. Barron & interview. Psychological Bulletin, 117(3), 387-403.
M. N. Eagle (Eds.), Interface of psychoanalysis and psychology (pp. Van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Kroonenberg, P. M. (1988). Cross-cultural
154-185). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. patterns of attachment: A meta-analysis of the strange situa-
Spieker, S. J., Gillmore, M. R., Lewis, S. M., Morrison, D. M., & Lohr, tion. Child Development, 59(1), 147-156.
M. J. (2001). Psychological distress and substance use by ad- Whitman, T. L., Borkowski, J. G., Keogh, D. A., & Weed, K. (2001).
olescent mothers: Associations with parenting attitudes and the Interwoven lives: Adolescent mothers and their children. Mah-
quality of mother-child interaction. Journal of Psychoactive wah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Drugs, 33(1), 83-93. Wille, D. E. (1991). Relation of preterm birth with quality of infant-
The National Campaign to Prevent Teen and Unplanned Pregnancy. mother attachment at one year. Infant Behavior & Development,
(2009). Socio-economic and family characteristics of teen 14(2), 227-240.
childbearing. Retrieved from http://www.thenationalcampaign. Zuravin, S. J., & DiBlasio, F. A. (1992). Child-neglecting adolescent
org/resources/pdf/SS/SS41_SocioEconomicFamilyCharacteristics. mothers: How do they differ from their nonmaltreating counter-
pdf parts? Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 7(4), 471-489.
www.jpedhc.org March/April 2011 121