Van Dijck - Mediated Memories in The Digital Age
Van Dijck - Mediated Memories in The Digital Age
Van Dijck - Mediated Memories in The Digital Age
Cultural Memory
in
the
Present
TA1637.D57 2007
153.1'3—dc22
2007008641
Preface xi
Acknowledgments xvii
Notes 183
Bibliography 217
Index 229
Preface
but it also would involve agonizing decisions about what items to store and
what to throw away after digitization.
At first, I tackled the problem as a practical one: a mess of objects in
need of systematic sorting and filing. When I sat down to think about it,
the problem was not the mess in my shoebox and by extension on my
desktop; these objects and technologies generated questions concerning
memory and media that were far more intricate than I had initially
thought. The contents of my shoebox indeed posed the challenge of fitting
digital memories into analog frames for storage and retrieval, but beyond
my idiosyncratic dilemmas, it raised poignant concerns about the relation
between material objects and autobiographical memory, between media
technologies and our habits and rituals of remembrance. My collection of
personal items made me reflect on how we present and preserve images of
ourselves to others; it caused me to speculate on how private collections tap
into the much larger phenomenon of communal rites of storing and re-
trieving. I wondered whether my switch from analog to digital memory
objects was the result of a general technological-commercial push or the
desire to be more creatively engaged with my treasured artifacts. The
microuniverse of my shoebox opened up a Pandora’s box of unexpected
philosophical questions pertaining to the nature, culture, and politics of
what I dubbed “mediated memories.” Why and how do we create and save
mediated items for later reminiscence? What is the function of mediated
memories in our personal lives? What is the role of media technologies and
material objects in capturing both individual and collective memory? Are
analog and digital objects interchangeable in the making, storing, and
recalling of memories? Do digital objects change our inscription and re-
membrance of lived experience, and do they affect the memory process in
our brains?
Articulating these questions was a first step toward acknowledging
the complexity of the term I had casually coined. “Mediated memories”
refers to both to the concrete objects in my shoebox and a mental concept—
a concept that encompasses aspects of mind and body as well as of tech-
nology and culture. I also realized that memory and media both comprise
vast and exhaustively mined research subjects, to the extent that the terms
themselves are at risk of becoming empty signifiers. To say that human
memory is a complex problem is an understatement; it is such a daunting,
intricate object of research that generations of scholars can hardly be
Preface xiii
By the time one writes the acknowledgments, a book has almost be-
come a material artifact, and the writing process is about to become a
memory. But the ultimate memory product is full of traces of collabora-
tion, inspiration, and affection. I would like to extend my gratefulness to
some of the people who have contributed to this book.
Students and colleagues at the Department of Media Studies at the
University of Amsterdam have offered invaluable support and stimuli. My
graduate students always shared my enthusiasm for this topic, and if not,
they never showed it. Thanks to my dear colleagues Thomas Elsaesser,
Frank van Vree, and Eric Ketelaar for co-teaching a seminar on media and
memory. Patricia Pisters subtly mended the Deleuze-gaps in my education
and has proven a wonderful ally. Without the department’s support staff,
life would be half as memorable: Jobien Kuiper; Piet van Wijk, Henny
Bouwmeester, who also provided valuable administrative support; and
Joost Bolten, who is a Word whiz. Being the chair of such a great team of
faculty and staff has been an honor and a total delight; the department’s
growth and flourishing in the past five years was the accomplishment of a
superb group.
Some chapters in this book have been the outcome of or input for
various projects. I thank Karin Bijsterveld for our collaboration in Sound
Technologies and Cultural Practice, a project funded by the Netherlands
Organization for Scientific Research (NWO). Sonja Neef’s organizing qual-
ities in our handwriting project cannot be praised enough. American col-
leagues, most notably Richard Grusin, Hugh Crawford, Jonathan Sterne,
Phillip Thurtle, Robert Mitchell, Herman Gray, George Lipsitz, and Lisa
Cartwright offered valuable criticism to early drafts or lectures on this
topic.
xviii Acknowledgments
Mediated Memories as
a Conceptual Tool
Evidently, the same scene gives rise to two sets of inquiries into
memory formation, each highlighting different aspects, and yet, the per-
sonal and cultural can hardly be disentangled because there is a constant
productive tension between our (personal) inclinations to stake out certain
events and the (social) frameworks through which we do so—between the
(individual) activities of remembering and the (cultural) products of auto-
biographical recall. Acts and products of memory are far from arbitrary. In
Western culture, filming and photographing baby’s first steps are consid-
ered common ritualized attempts to freeze and store a milestone in a hu-
man being’s development; hence, the decision of these parents to catch the
event on film and arrest the moment in photographs is in tune with pre-
vailing norms—norms that, naturally, change with every new generation
and also vary culturally. Western European and American practices of
remembering and recording significantly diverge from Asian or African
mores in this area, due to diverging cultural norms and social relation-
ships.8 In general, personal memory stems from the altercation of individ-
ual acts and cultural norms—a tension we can trace in both the activity of
remembering and in the object of memory.
Therefore, I want to define “personal cultural memory” as the acts
and products of remembering in which individuals engage to make sense of
their lives in relation to the lives of others and to their surroundings, situating
themselves in time and place.9 According to my definition, “personal” and
“cultural” are the threads that bind memory’s texture: they can be distin-
guished, but they never can be separated. We usually mark events because
their significance is already ingrained in our conscious: first steps are an
important happening in a child’s life, just as birthdays and first school days
are. The decision to record such events is already, to a large extent, stipu-
lated by conventions prescribing which occurrences are symbolic or ritual
highlights and thus worth flagging. Some events, such as conflicts or de-
pressions, may seem unsuitable for video recording, but they may instead
be amenable subjects for diary entries. Other events, such as household
routines or intense emotions, are perhaps too dull or too poignant for any
kind of inscription, yet that does not mean they cannot be recalled—most
of our life’s experiences, after all, go undocumented, and often deliber-
ately so. Parents who decide not to take out their video camera may do so
because they prefer to enjoy and remember the first-step experience with-
out the camera’s intervention. At various moments, people decide what to
Mediated Memories as a Conceptual Tool 7
I am less concerned with what these reservoirs do or do not consist of; in-
stead, this book concentrates on how memory works in constructing a
sense of individual identity and collectivity at the same time. To set up this
claim, I first need to sketch how prevailing notions of collective memory
have structured academic thinking, most notably in sociological and his-
torical accounts.
Just as individual or autobiographical memory is almost automati-
cally associated with theory formation in the area of psychology, collec-
tive memory, since the early twentieth century, has been the privileged
domain of sociologists, historians, and cultural theorists. Originated in
late nineteenth-century French and German sociology, the concept of col-
lective memory was most prominently theorized by Maurice Halbwachs, a
critical student of both Henri Bergson and Emile Durkheim. In Les cadres
sociaux de la memoire, first published in 1925, Halbwachs sketches the par-
tially overlapping cadres (spheres) of individual and larger communities,
such as family, community, and nation. In contending that memory needs
social frames, he distances himself from more physiological approaches to
memory, particularly those insisting on the isolated enframing capacity of
the human mind. Far from being a cognitive trajectory activated by inter-
nal or external stimuli, human memory “needs constant feeding from col-
lective sources, just as collective memories are always sustained by social
and moral props.”12 Halbwachs thus emphasizes the recursive nature of in-
dividual and collective memory, one always inhabiting the other. Collec-
tivity, he claims, arises in the variable contexts of groups who share an
orientation in time and space. Our memories organize themselves accord-
ing to our actual or perceived participation in a (temporal) collectivity—a
group vacation, a school class, a family, a generation—and recall tends to
lean on a sense of belonging or sharing rather than on a relocation in real
time or space. We may remember events chronologically or spatially, but
quite often we remember in terms of connectivity. As social creatures, hu-
mans experience events in relation to others, whether or not these commu-
nal events affect them personally.
One of Halbwachs’s important observations is that collective memory
is never the plain sum of individual remembrances: every personal memory
is cemented in an idiosyncratic perspective, but these perspectives never cul-
minate into a singular collective view. The memories of both parent and
child participating in the same event are not necessarily the same or even
10 Mediated Memories as a Conceptual Tool
inscribe memories for oneself and the desire to share them only with desig-
nated recipients as it is about publicness, or the inclination to share experi-
ences with a number of unknown viewers or readers. There is not, nor has
there ever been, a sharp distinction between private and public, but every
act of memory involves a negotiation of these spheres’ boundaries. The in-
tention to inscribe or recall a memory exclusively for private use may change
over time, as personal memory may acquire a larger significance against a
background of evolving social mores or personal growth. Control over one’s
memory may also change in the course of time; one may lose command
over either one’s mental capacities for remembering, as a result of disease or
death, or one may lose ownership over material inscriptions of former ex-
periences, whether voluntary or involuntary. Intentions and control change
along with our revisions of memories in the passage of time, and revisions,
in turn, reset the boundaries for what counts as public or private. Those
boundaries are concurrently the outcome and stakes in the act of cultural
memory.
Let me illustrate this specified concept of cultural memory using an
example—an example I further elaborate in Chapter 3. Anne Frank’s diary
is most commonly typified as the poignant personal lens through which we
experience a collective memory of the Holocaust. From my perspective,
though, Frank’s diary stands for a continuous and ongoing struggle be-
tween individual and collective acts of memory. Defined as personal
cultural memory, it signifies a Dutch teenager’s choice to narrate her expe-
rience in a cultural form—a handwritten daily account, trusted to a note-
book, that she later revised; Otto Frank’s decision to publish selected parts
of his daughter’s journal turned the diary into a public, collective item.
Anne’s aspiration to become a novelist as well as her father’s judgment to
censor the first editions should be understood in the context of the larger
cultural arena in which these mandates were negotiated. Naturally, the en-
suing Anne Frank industry—the museum, the objects in the museum, the
play, the movies, television series—are part of the (collective) cultural act
of remembrance, but they are also products of the memory industry.27 All
past, present, and future choices made in the service of inscribing and pre-
serving Anne Frank’s legacy are in fact collusions of individuality and col-
lectivity. Memory filtered through the prism of culture acknowledges the
idea that individual expressions are articulated as part of, as much as in
spite of, larger collectivities; individuality can be traced in every negotiation
14 Mediated Memories as a Conceptual Tool
and even help construct, realities.”37 Mediated memory objects never repre-
sent a fixed moment; they serve to fix temporal notions and relations be-
tween past and present. Steven Rose, a British scientist who discusses the
physiological complexity of human memory and consciousness at length,
makes the case that mnemonic aids, such as photos or videos, are con-
founded with our individual memories to such extent that we can hardly dis-
tinguish between the two.38 And anthropologist Richard Chalfen, in his
study of how home media help communicate individuals’ perceptions of
self to others, argues that our “snapshots are us.”39 Personal cultural memory
seems to be predatory on media technologies’ and media objects’ shaping
power.
A similar notion of mediation can be found among historians dis-
cussing the infiltration of mass media in collective memory. British sociol-
ogist John Urry explains in his essay “How Societies Remember the Past”
that electronic media intrinsically change the way we create images of the
past in the present.40 Mass media, according to historian of popular cul-
ture George Lipsitz, embody some of our deepest hopes and engage some
of our most profound sympathies; films, records, or other cultural expres-
sions constitute “a repository of collective memory that places immediate
experience in the context of change over time.”41 Media like television
and, more recently, computers are devices that produce, store, and reshape
earlier versions of history. With the recent explosion of electronic and dig-
ital devices, the tools for memory have shifted in nature and function—a
subject I return to in the next chapter.
We can witness this contrived interlocking most poignantly at the
metaphorical level; the term “mediation of memory” refers equally to our
understanding of media in terms of memory (as illustrated by historians’
accounts above) as well as to our comprehension of physiological memory
in terms of media, evidenced by the many metaphors explaining certain
features of human memory. Ever since the invention of writing tools, but
most noticeably since the emergence of photography in the nineteenth
century, the human capacity to remember has been indexed in daily lan-
guage by referring to technical tools for reproduction. Dutch psychologist
Douwe Draaisma, who extensively researched the historical evolution of
memory metaphors, notes that media are a special conceptual category
for envisioning memory’s mechanics.42 For instance, the term “flashbulb
memory”—the proclivity to remember an impacting moment in full
18 Mediated Memories as a Conceptual Tool
detail, including the circumstances, time, and place in which the mes-
sage was received—derived its signifier from the realm of photography.
In the twentieth century, the terminology of film and video started to in-
vade the discourse of memory and memory research: life is said to be re-
played like a film in the seconds before death, and psychologists have
extensively examined the phenomenon known as “deathbed flash.” By the
same token, we are now firmly grilled by the media’s convention to visu-
alize a character’s recall of past experience as a slow-motion replay or
flashback. Metaphors are not simply means of expression, but are concep-
tual images that structure and give meaning to our lives.43 As media have
become our foremost tools for memory, metaphorical reciprocity signals
their constitutive quality.
However compelling and valid, most mediation of memory theories
still hinge on a few premises that funnel the scope of this concept, restrict-
ing its explanatory range. First, an exclusive focus on either home media or
mass media often presume a symbiotic union of home media with individ-
ual memory and of mass media with its collective counterpart; such rigid
distinctions hamper a fuller understanding of how individual and collec-
tive memory are shaped in conjunction, with media serving as relational
means connecting self to others. Second, even though most theories ac-
knowledge the convergence of memory and media, the “mediation” con-
cept frequently favors a single vector: media shape our memories, but we
seldom find testimony of media being shaped by memory, indicating an
implicit hierarchy. Let me address each of these conceptual deficiencies in
more detail.
It is practical to assume that personal cultural memory is generated
by what we call home media (family photography, home videos, tape
recorders) whereas collective cultural memory is produced by mass media
(television, music records, professional photography), implying that the
first type of media is confined to the private sphere, whereas the latter per-
tains to the public realm. But that simple division, even if functional, is
also conceptually flawed: it obscures the fact that people derive their auto-
biographical memories from both personal and collective media sources.
Media sociologist John Thompson, who highlights the role of individual
agency in media reception, explains the hermeneutic nature of this rela-
tionship.44 He argues that “lived experience,” in our contemporary cul-
ture, is interlaced with “mediated experience”; mediation, then, comprises
Mediated Memories as a Conceptual Tool 19
not only the media tools we wield in the private sphere but also the active
choices of individuals to incorporate parts of culture into their lives. Expe-
rience is neither completely lived nor entirely mediated, as the encounter
between the two is a continuously evolving life-project to define the self
in a larger cultural context. What makes mediated experience today differ
from lived experience two hundred years ago is the fact that individuals
need no longer share a common locale to pursue commonality; the grow-
ing availability of mediated experience creates “new opportunities, new
options, new arenas for self-experimentation.”45 If we accept a prelimi-
nary distinction between home and mass media, we not only fail to ac-
count for media shaping our sense of individuality and collectivity in
conjunction, but we equally obscure how individuals actively contribute to
the collective media that shape their individuality.
A strict delineation of home and mass media has also become im-
practicable because each type informs the other. For instance, America’s
Funniest Home Videos (AFHV ), a program format that has been success-
fully franchised to television stations around the globe for over two de-
cades, is made up entirely of home media footage, woven into a glitzy
commercial production; by the same token, many home video enthusiasts
have taken AFV ’s narrative structure (or its announced themes) as direc-
tive models to film their cute children and pets. Amateur videos of the
tsunami hitting the shores of Thailand, Sri Lanka, and Indonesia in De-
cember 2004 made the news worldwide; the power of amateur footage
probably motivates individuals who buy and carry a camera to create news
and offer it to television stations. There are many examples of documen-
taries based largely on compilations of amateur or home movies, and this
trend is likely to increase with the growing availability of affordable digital
cameras, computers, and editing equipment. In the next chapter, I further
elaborate on the meaning of media technologies (digital and analog) as
tools that mediate between personal and collective cultural memory.
The second conceptual deficiency in the mediation of memory con-
cerns the implied hierarchy between external and internal memory, or in
plain terms, between technology and the human mind. Understandably,
our metaphors often explain the invisible in terms of the visible and know-
able; that is why the mysteries of mental processing are often elucidated in
terms of media technology—technology that is at once transparent and
mechanically predictable. But how about turning the vector back on its
20 Mediated Memories as a Conceptual Tool
always implied in the other. The brain steers and stimulates the camera
as much as the camera stimulates the brain. Mediated experience, as film
scholar Thomas Elsaesser remarks, is subjected to a generalized cultural
condition:
In our mobility, we are “tour”ists of life: we use the camcorder in our hand or of-
ten merely in our head, to reassure ourselves that this is “me, now, here.” Our ex-
perience of the present is always already (media) memory, and this memory
represents the recaptured attempt at self-presence: possessing the experience in or-
der to possess the memory, in order to possess the self.48
In other words, memory is not mediated by media, but media and
memory transform each other. In the next chapter, I further elaborate
on how changing (digital) technologies and objects embody changing
memories.
A revised concept of memory’s mediation thus needs to avoid the
pitfalls of fallacious binary and hierarchical structures. Media are not con-
fined to private or public areas, and neither do they store or distort the past
in relation to the present or future. Like memories, media’s dynamic na-
ture constitutes constantly evolving relations between self and others, pri-
vate and public, past and future. The term “mediation of memory,” if we
attend to its conceptual flaws, may be rearticulated as “mediated memo-
ries,” a concept that may add to a better understanding of the mutual
shaping of memory and media.
Mediated memories are the activities and objects we produce and appro-
priate by means of media technologies, for creating and re-creating a sense of
past, present, and future of ourselves in relation to others. Mediated memory
objects and acts are crucial sites for negotiating the relationship between
self and culture at large, between what counts as private and what as pub-
lic, and how individuality relates to collectivity. As stilled moments in the
present, mediated memories reflect and construct intersections between
past and future—remembering and projecting lived experience. Mediated
memories are not static objects or repositories but dynamic relationships
that evolve along two axes: a horizontal axis expressing relational identity
and a vertical axis articulating time. Neither axis is immobile: memories
22 Mediated Memories as a Conceptual Tool
Future
Self Others
Individual Collective
Past
figure 1
move back and forth between the personal and collective, and they travel
up and down between past and future. The commingling of both axes sig-
nals memories should be understood as processes—mediating self to cul-
ture to past to future. The dynamic nature of this analytical model shows
in Figure 1.
Mediated memories refers both to acts of memory (construing a re-
lational identity etched in dimensions of time) and to memory products
(personal memory objects as sites where individual minds and collective
cultures meet). The term is neither a displacement of a psychological def-
inition of (personal) cultural memory, nor is it a dislodgment of the histo-
rian’s notion of (collective) cultural memory. Instead, it offers a tool for
analyzing complex cases of memory formation and transformation, taking
personal shoebox items as culturally relevant objects.
Taken at face value, my mediated memories concept appears quite
similar to the term “tangled memories” coined by American media theorist
Marita Sturken. Sturken deploys a concept of cultural memory that comes
close to Aleida Assmann’s, but unlike the German historian, she includes
media (objects) in her theoretical model, even turning them into a central
focus of analysis.49 Sturken explains how items of popular culture—from
the Vietnam Memorial to the AIDS Memorial Quilt—are compiled from
personal acts of commemoration out of which arises a collective statement
Mediated Memories as a Conceptual Tool 23
about a shared trauma. Media play a principal role in this process, both as
personal instruments (e.g., private photos or videos) and as mass conveyors
of social narratives (e.g., news coverage or documentaries). Individuals
root their personal memories in cultural objects and activities that at once
give meaning to their own experience and attribute historical or political
significance to the larger events of which they are part. Sturken correctly
observes that personal memory, cultural memory, and history do not exist
within neatly defined boundaries: “Memories and memory objects can
move from one realm to another, shifting meaning and context. Thus per-
sonal memories can sometimes be subsumed into history and elements of
cultural memory can exist in concert with historical narratives.”50 If we
compare Sturken’s concept of tangled memories to my concept of mediated
memories figured in the diagram, they differ on two accounts. As figure 1
shows, it is possible to advance mediated memories from both the right and
the left angle. Sturken approaches manifestations of cultural memory from
the (right) angle of collectivity. What is more, she explicitly excludes “shoe-
box contents” insofar as they remain private possessions, not having gained
any cultural or political relevance in the public, collective realm.51 Sturken’s
approach is similar to Alison Landsberg’s who proposes the term “pros-
thetic memory” to argue that memories are not so much socially con-
structed or individual occurrences but emerge at the interface of individual
and collective experience; she states that prosthetic memory is “less inter-
ested in large-scale social implications and dialectics than in the experiential
quality of prosthetic memory and in the ramifications of these memories
for individual subjectivity and political consciousness.”52
Both Sturken and Landsberg approach cultural memory from the
right angle of collectivity, showing how memories have the ability to alter
a person’s (political) outlook or action. In contrast, this book approaches
memories from the opposite direction, privileging private memory objects,
regardless of whether they have gained recognition in the public realm. By
emphasizing the left angle, we acknowledge cognitive or psychosocial di-
mensions of remembering as complementary to historical, political or
cultural dimensions of memory. My thesis that we cannot separate the in-
dividual psyche from culture is articulated in the relational (mediating)
nature of the diagram, hence prompting the integration of psychological
and cognitive perspectives into cultural theory.
24 Mediated Memories as a Conceptual Tool
In the movie Eternal Sunshine of the Spotless Mind, the company La-
cuna Inc. advertises its method for focused memory removal with the fol-
lowing slogan: “Why remember a destructive love affair if you can erase
it?”1 When Joel Barish (Jim Carrey) incidentally finds out that his ex-
girlfriend Clementine Kruczynski (Kate Winslet) has undergone the
Lacuna procedure to wipe their bitterly ended relationship from her mem-
ory, he requests Dr. Howard Mierzwiak to perform the same procedure on
his brain. Joel is instructed to go home and collect any objects or memen-
tos that have any ties to Clementine (“photos, gifts, CDs you bought to-
gether, journal pages”) and to bring them to the doctor’s office. Upon his
return, Lacuna-technician Stan wires Joel’s brain to a computerized head-
set; the doctor holds up each separate object (drawings from his diary, a
mug with Clementine’s picture, etc.) and tells Joel to let each object trigger
spontaneous memories. Stan subsequently tags each object-related mem-
ory and punches it into a computer, apparently recording Joel’s mental as-
sociations on a digital map of Clementine. That same night, Stan and his
assistant come to Joel’s house, hook up their drug-induced sleeping client
to a machine that looks like a hairdryer but generates images similar to
those produced by functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), and
start the erasure process. As the Lacuna Inc. website explains: “The proce-
dure works on a reverse timeline, which means it begins with the most re-
cent memories and goes backward in time. This approach is designed to
28 Memory Matters in the Digital Age
target the emotional core that every memory builds on. By eradicating the
core, Dr. Mierzwiak is able to make the entire memory dissolve.”2 One by
one, Joel’s memories of Clementine are erased—a fairly automatic process
that would have been finished by early morning if not for Joel’s realization,
halfway through the procedure, that he wants to keep the good memories
of his love affair, so he actively starts to resist the erasure guys. Incapaci-
tated by drugs, he embarks on a dreamlike, psychic journey with a remem-
bered Clementine, creatively hiding her in unconscious, untargeted corners
of his memory where she does not belong, in an attempt to escape the
high-tech apparatus that is slowly stripping away Joel’s recollection of his
former girlfriend.
Michel Gondry’s and Charlie Kaufman’s fictional treatise of modern
science’s struggle to erase undesirable autobiographical memories raises im-
portant questions: First, what is the “matter” of personal memories? Mem-
ory is obviously embodied, but neurobiologists, cognitive philosophers,
and cultural theorists hold different—even if complementary—views on
what “substance” memories are made of. Scientific concepts of memory
have evolved significantly in recent decades, and the movie actually reflects
on some recent neurocognitive theories on memory formation and re-
trieval. Second, Eternal Sunshine of the Spotless Mind presents an ambigu-
ous answer to the issue of where memory is located. On the one hand,
personal memory is situated inside the brain—the deepest, most intimate
physical space of the human body. On the other hand, personal memories
seem to be located in the many objects Joel and Clementine (like most of
us) create to serve as reminders of lived experiences. Most of these items
are what, in the previous chapter, I dubbed “mediated memory objects,”
such as pictures, videos, recorded music, diaries, and so on; people have a
vested interest in them because they come to serve as material triggers of
personal memories. Mediated memory objects, however, are not simply
prostheses of the mind, as the movie wants us to believe. Mediated mem-
ories, as I argue in this chapter, can be located neither strictly in the brain
nor wholly outside in (material) culture but exist in both concurrently, for
they are manifestations of a complex interaction between brain, material
objects, and the cultural matrix from which they arise.
After exploring how mediated memories are concurrently embodied
through the mind and brain, enabled by media technologies, and embed-
ded in a cultural context, the question arises, what happens when memory
Memory Matters in the Digital Age 29
production enters the digital era? What makes the movie Eternal Sunshine
interesting in this respect is the conversion of Joel’s painful memories into
digital brain scans. The technology deployed by Lacuna’s technicians tran-
scribes memories triggered by material objects onto a digital map that looks
like a series of brain scans. Like ordinary computer files, the Clementine
files can be erased and thus be made to disappear from the place in Joel’s
brain where they are stored. The proposed translations from experiences
into memory objects, back into actual memories, and then into information
files—files that can subsequently be stored or deleted—propel sophisticated
perspectives on the (de)materialization and (dis)embodiment of memory.
In contrast, the movie’s depiction of memory objects in the digital age is
rather simplistic: photographs, scrapbooks, and cassette tapes still seem to
dominate Joel’s and Clementine’s mutual recollections in a period when
digital pictures, weblogs, compact disks and MP3 files are rapidly replacing
their analog precursors. In light of recent revisionist theory on memory for-
mation, the question arises how digital technologies may accommodate the
“matter” of both mind and media. Even if memory in the digital age is just
as embodied and mediated through artifacts as before, the very notions of
embodiment and materiality need upgrading, in order to account for mem-
ory’s morphing nature. Upon entering the digital era, the question of where
mediated memories are located or produced—how they are embodied, en-
abled, and embedded—becomes even more poignant.
entists studying its neurological and genetic workings; they point at the
brain as the nucleus of all our mental activity and consciousness.7 Genes,
neurons, and living cells all constitute the bodily apparatus needed to carry
out mental functions, for instance cognitive tasks such as factual recall, or
affective tasks such as emotions or feelings. In spite of putting the center
for memory activity in the brain, scientists assert there is no such thing as
a single location for memory. Even though some parts of the brain are spe-
cialized in specific memory tasks—such as the hippocampus for retaining
short- and long-term memory, the amygdala for emotional learning—there
is no single vector between one brain system and one type of memory. Au-
tobiographical memory is usually associated with emotional matters that
are in turn sheltered by the two amygdalae, yet this does not mean they are
solely confined to this part of the brain. Instead, the establishment of mem-
ories depends on the working of the entire brain network, consisting in
turn of several memory systems, including semantic and episodic memory,
declarative or procedural memory.8 The hunt for the location of memory,
undertaken by scientists of various disciplines, has come up with a stagger-
ing distributed answer to that question, in fact defying the very possibility
of pinning down one type of memory to a single place in the brain. Facili-
tated by neurological circuits, the brain sets the mind to work, stimulating
a perception or a mode of thinking—a mental image, a feeling—that in
turn affects our bodily state. The brain is thus the generator of reflexes, re-
sponses, drives, emotions and, ultimately, feelings; memory involves both
(the perception of) a certain body state and a certain mind state.
In more recent philosophies of mind, connectionist metaphors tend
to conceive of memory as a distributed agency that leaves traces of an on-
going process. Of all connectionist metaphors that philosophers and neu-
roscientists have introduced over the years, the networked computer is
probably the most prominent one, but it is not necessarily the best one.9
Perhaps the symphony orchestra is a more appropriate metaphor than the
computer when it comes to explaining the function of memory and how
the brain’s matter is responsible for the personal memories it produces.10
Like a performance of Mahler’s Seventh Symphony by an orchestra re-
quires a brass section, a string section, and a percussion section, memory is
a function of the brain that manifests itself through the mind and directs
our consciousness or conscious acts, such as self-reflection or autobio-
graphical reminiscence; it is a consortium of concerted efforts resulting in
32 Memory Matters in the Digital Age
extent that emotions inform our memories, the stuff of memory may be
partly derived from the external object itself (a scenic landscape or a pic-
ture thereof) and partly from the construction the brain makes of it (the
auditory, visual, tactile, or olfactory perceptions in our minds). Neurosci-
entist Antonio Damasio calls the latter an “emotionally competent object,”
referring to the event or object (e.g., seeing a painting, a landscape; hearing
a song) that is at the origin of a brain map and elicits a certain feeling:
“this picture makes me feel happy” or “this music makes me sad.” Invok-
ing a scene or scenery through one’s memory may not change the actual
object (the painting, the photograph, the record), but it certainly changes
the internalized “map” of the initial trigger.15 Recall and permanent re-
arrangement of our personal experiences, according to Damasio, play a
role in the unfolding of desire. The very desire to re-create an original
emotion may be the motivation for changing the brain map: “There is a
rich interplay between the object of desire and a wealth of personal mem-
ories pertinent to the object—past occasions of desire, past aspirations,
and past pleasures, real or imagined.”16
As neuroscientific research indicates, memory and imagination are
not the distant cousins they once seemed: both derive from the same cel-
lular and neurological processes and are intricately intertwined in the
matter memories are made of. Memory can be creative in reconstructing
the past, just as imagination can be reconstructive in memorizing the
present—think only of the many visual tricks people play to perform the
cognitive task of factual recall. The function of personal memory, even if
restricted to studying its “mindware,” is not simply about re-creating an
accurate picture of one’s past, but it is about creating a mental map of
one’s past through the lens of the present. The contents of memory are
configurations of body states represented in somatosensing maps. Living
cells producing this mindware are all but indifferent to the processes they
condition, and thus, we could conclude, memory is only the trick the
mind plays on the brain. As humans, we even tinker with these processes,
for instance by inserting chemical substances (drugs) that alter the body’s
emotional state. Or, as in the science fiction of Eternal Sunshine, techni-
cians artificially remove unpleasant memories by deactivating those neu-
rological circuits responsible for undesirable responses conditioned in
brain maps. The erasure of the mental image of an experience in Joel’s
brain activates a desire to thwart the procedure, which in turn causes the
Memory Matters in the Digital Age 35
Many of us appreciate these items for more than contents only: we treasure
the fading colors on yellowed paper, the fumes of tobacco attached to old
diaries, the irritating scratches on self-compiled tapes. Apparently, physical
appearances—including smell, looks, taste, and feel—render mediated
memory objects somehow precious.
Some cultural theorists have located the matter of memory precisely—
and often exclusively—in the tangibility of mediated objects. Walter Ben-
jamin, writing on reproducible memorabilia like personal photographs,
called them the “modern relics of nostalgia,” the meaning of which lies
hidden in the layers of time affecting their appearance.19 Some contempo-
rary scholars argue that memory materializes primarily through the tech-
nology used to produce mediated objects. Media theorist Belinda Barnet,
for instance, prefers technology as the main focus for memory research
when she writes: “There is no lived memory, no originary, internal experi-
ence stored somewhere that corresponds to a certain event in our lives.
Memory is entirely reconstructed by the machine of memory, by the pro-
cess of writing; it retreats into a prosthetic experience, and this experience
in turn retreats as we try to locate it. But the important point is this: our
perception, and our perception of the past, is merely an experience of the
technical substrate.”20 Whereas both Benjamin and Barnet acknowledge
that memories actually change over time—one in terms of the object get-
ting older, taking on a sheen of authenticity and invoking nostalgia, the
other in terms of technology defining and replacing the very experience of
memory—they are adamant in restricting their focus on memory to its ma-
terial and technical strata only. Barnet argues the primacy of technologies
in our production and reproduction of memories. Quite a few mediated
memory objects require the original technological apparatus upon later re-
call because that equipment is indispensable for viewing their contents.
Think, for instance, about the projector and roll-down screen needed to
show your old slides, an 8-track recorder for playing these antique tapes, or,
to stay closer to the present, the hardware and software to read the large
floppy disks on which you diligently continued writing your diary after
buying your first word processor.
Clearly, the inscription and invocation of personal memory is often
contingent on technologies and objects, but unlike Barnet, I locate mem-
ory not in the matter of items per se but rather in the items’ agency, the
way they interact with the mind. Paradoxically, the real value of mediated
Memory Matters in the Digital Age 37
objects and their enabling technologies is often thought to lie in their sup-
posedly static meaning, despite their obvious physical decay, and in their
supposed fixity as triggers, despite our constant intervention in their mate-
riality. Memory objects serve as representations of a past or former self,
and their robust materiality seems to guarantee a stable anchor of memory
retrieval—an index to lived experience. But the hypothesis that mediated
memory objects remain constant each time we use them as triggers is
equally fallacious as the outdated theory that memories remain unaffected
upon retrieval—a theory meticulously refuted by neuroscientists. After all,
photo chemicals and ink on paper tend to fade, and home videos lose qual-
ity as a result of frequent replay (and even if left unused, their quality de-
teriorates). In fact, it is exactly this material transformation—its decay or
decomposing—that becomes part of a mutating memory: the growing im-
perfect state of these items connotes continuity between past and present.
Their materiality alters as time passes, but could it be the very combina-
tion of material aging and supposed representational inertia that accounts
for their growing emotional value?
Besides a sort of natural physical decay, there is a decisive human
factor in the modification of (external) memory objects. Like human
brains tend to select, reconfigure, and reorder memories upon recall, peo-
ple also consciously manipulate their memory deposits over time: they de-
stroy pictures, burn their diaries, or simply change the order of pictures in
their photo books. Memory deposits are prone to revision as their owners
continue to dictate their reinterpretation: a grown-up woman ashamed of
her teenage scribbles revises details in her diary; a bitter man erases videos
of his ex-wife; a grandmother takes apart her carefully composed photo al-
bum to divide its pictures among her numerous grandchildren.
The double paradox of a stable yet changing external object trigger-
ing a stable yet retouched mental image appears all too persistent in our
cultural imagination. In Eternal Sunshine, Joel’s and Clementine’s desire
to destroy their reminders testifies to the human inclination to constantly
revise our past. An endearing scene in Dr. Mierzwiak’s waiting room, show-
ing tearful clients holding their bags filled with treasured items to be de-
stroyed, signals the intrinsic modifiability of objects as they are constantly
prone to manipulation and reinterpretation. People have always used ma-
terial objects not just to store memories but also to alter them, annihilate
them, or reassign meaning to them. Mediated memory objects never stay
38 Memory Matters in the Digital Age
put for once and for all: on the contrary, the deposits themselves are agents
in an ongoing process of memory (re)construction, motivated by desire.
Memory allows for both preservation and erasure, and media objects can be
manipulated to facilitate and substantiate (new) versions of past experience.
The parallel between neuroscientific theories of memory formation
and cultural conjectures of memory is far from coincidental; I dare to ar-
gue the two processes intersect. For neuroscientists, the mediation of
memory happens in the brain where various interacting neurosensory ap-
paratuses account for their inherent mutability. Cognitive philosophers
add to this theory that memories are mediated not only by the intricate
brain-mind orchestration but also by the interaction between the brain
with physical, external objects it encounters, including the technologies
that help make them manifest. Australian philosopher John Sutton, for in-
stance, defines the locus of memory in the hermeneutics of mind and mat-
ter; the biggest challenge in analyzing the cognitive life of memory objects
is “to acknowledge the diversity of feedback relations between objects and
embodied brain.”21 This view is corroborated by Andy Clark, who argues
that memory and its enabling technologies are mutually constitutive; he pro-
poses a cognitive science that includes “body and brains” as well as “props
and aids (pens, papers, PCs) in which our biological brains learn, mature,
and operate.”22 Both Sutton and Clark regard a mutual shaping of the
brain/mind and object/technology the inescapable consequence of new neu-
roscientific insights, and they advocate a concerted interdisciplinary research
effort to face the challenge of new paradigms created by these findings.
Indeed, I agree with both philosophers’ view that memory is not sim-
ply triggered by objects but happens through these objects; brain, mind,
technology, and materiality are inextricably intertwined in producing and
revising a coherent picture of one’s past. However, this double-edged con-
cept can still not fully account for the matter of memory. Memories, in my
view, are not only embodied by the brain/mind and enabled by the
object/technology, but they are also mediated by the sociocultural practices
and forms through which they manifest themselves. Although practices and
forms are commonly squarely located in the realm of culture, they cannot
be studied separately from the other two conceptual pairs. But before we
take that layer further apart into its cultural components, let us first look at
how media technologies and objects “matter” as instruments for inscribing
personal memory and identity.
Memory Matters in the Digital Age 39
the person performs a cognitive task, the machine measures the metabolic
changes that are linked up with neural activity. In this way, emotions such
as fear, aggression, or sexual urge—emotions essential to survival—can be
shown to emerge through the paleocortex, the middle brain, whereas the
“higher” cognitive and behavioral functions, including reason, are regu-
lated in the neocortex, the outer brain layer.
But what do these scans exactly figure? Do they visualize the matter
of memory? Not really. What these machines do is to measure increases in
blood flows through the brain; if the brain is more active, it needs more
blood and oxygen, resulting in more intensive blood circulation. This ac-
tivity shows up as red and yellow blots on the screen; representations of the
brain at work can subsequently be translated into knowledge about mental
processes—that is, by those properly trained in reading them. Medical im-
aging involves a series of translations in which the body, technology, and
expert scientists each play a constitutive role; the beautiful graphics of
fMRI, like many techniques heretofore developed in medical diagnostics,
imply much more precision, interpretative clarity, and transparency than
there actually is.30 Although the development of fMRI is still in its in-
fancy, the apparatus could theoretically be employed to trace memories as
physiological activities in the brain; “trace” is the word here, not “locate,”
because what neuroscientists actually capture in these images are the
changes in neural activity resulting from a specific cognitive, emotive, or
conscious task encoded in colored signals. However, most fMRI studies
use univariate processing—highlighting only one variable in the brain—a
method that shortchanges the distributed nature of neurodynamics. The
apparatus tends to confine activity to a specific location in the brain, thus
favoring the legitimacy of linking complex mental functions to particular
brain regions.
If powerful imaging technologies sway professionals to design re-
search questions supportive of simplified paradigms, it is easy to see how
nonexperts are persuaded by the machine’s potential to appear like a trans-
parent diagnostic apparatus, linking diseases to exact locations in the brain.
Ever since the emergence of X-ray technology, photography has been the
dominant model for all kinds of medical imaging—what you see is what
you get. It is an increasingly popular tenet—especially in court circles—
that complex phenomena like schizophrenia, drug addiction, criminality,
or for that matter, “traumatic” personal memories, can show up on digital
44 Memory Matters in the Digital Age
definitely affect the body.37 If we accept the premise that memory is not lo-
cated either inside the body or outside it in culture but is an embodied ex-
perience in which mind, computer, and object form the distributed agency,
then the idea of intervention in the brain’s function by means of informa-
tion technology becomes much more realistic. In the long run, not one com-
ponent in the chain lacing mind, machine, and memory will be left
untouched. In fact, brain modification by means of information processing
does occur, albeit more slowly and weakly than this movie makes us believe.
Technologies of memory are in and of themselves technologies of affect.
treasures of every genre (music, pictures, texts), but no one guarantees the
preservation of electronic materials for generations to come. Machines and
software formats may become obsolete, hard drives are anything but ro-
bust, and digital files may start to degrade or become indecipherable. Iron-
ically, problems of preservation and access to personal memories, as a
result of their digital condition, could become even more complex than
before. Even digital memories can fade—their fate determined by their in
silico conception—as the durability of hard drives, compact disks, and
memory sticks has yet to be proven.40 Memory does matter, perhaps even
more so in the digital age.
culture; weblogs only partly overlap with the conventional use of paper di-
aries, laminated pictures are still printed despite the rise of digital photog-
raphy, and MP3 files are not exactly replacing our tangible music
collections. New practices gradually transform the way we collect, read,
look at, or listen to our cherished personal items. The word “gradually” is
important to emphasize here, because the ongoing digitization of memory
tools and objects all but annihilates analog forms and practices. On the
contrary, various theorists emphasize the dynamics of remediation: the way
in which new technologies tend to absorb and revamp older forms or genres
without completely replacing the old.41 Photography “remediated” paint-
ing, but never took its place, even though both cultural practices repeatedly
had to adjust their ontological and epistemological claims in the face of new
technologies. Diaries and photo albums are currently undergoing a meta-
morphosis, although it is hard to predict which status and function familiar
paper forms will adopt in conjunction with lifelogs and various kinds of
web-based pictorial repositories. In fact, it is likely that analog and digital
forms and practices will always coexist, albeit in varying configurations.
New hybrid forms and fused practices are likely to inform the larger cul-
tural tendencies that propel their use.
Can we conclude from the above that digitization is, ultimately, a cul-
tural process that is slowly changing the way we remember our selves? The
problem with this thesis, as stated earlier, is its deceptive primacy of tech-
nology as the cause for change. The matter, nature, and function of memory
never changes as a result of technology; rather, the concomitant transition of
mind, technology, practices, and forms gradually impinge on our very acts
of memory. The first chapter explained how mediated memories manifest
themselves along two axes: a horizontal axis expressing relational identities
and a vertical axis articulating time. Being active producers and collectors of
mediated memories, we carve out our personal niches in the vast sea of cul-
ture surrounding us, thus creating a continuum between past and present.
In this chapter, I have argued to add a third (diagonal) axis to this model,
configuring how memories are mediated through functions of body and
mind, technology and materiality, and practices and forms (see Figure 2).
Tied in with the horizontal relational axis, it emphasizes how acts and ob-
jects of memory are concurrently embodied in individual brains and minds,
enabled by instruments and embedded in cultural dynamics. And offset
against a vector of time, the model builds in a reflection on transformation.
50 Memory Matters in the Digital Age
Self Others
Mediated Memories
Private (enabling technologies)
Public
Individual Collective
Embodied
Recollection Past Preservation
figure 2
apparently misjudged or neglected the fact that their diary entries were also
accessible to other readers. It did not take long for their peers to trace their
revelations and the male students’ revenge was more than a little awkward
for the authors, who presumed their readership to be limited to one friend
only. Like my own diary, the teenagers’ lifelog was directed to one other
pair of eyes but it was unwittingly exposed to the public eye.
The paper diary and the electronic weblog have much in common in
terms of function and use. For many centuries, the diary has been charac-
terized as a (hand)written document that chronicles the experiences, obser-
vations, and reflections of a single person at the moment of inscription.
The diary as a cultural form is varied and heterogeneous, yet it is typically
thought to represent the record of a single person constructing an autobi-
ographical account. Inviting the translation of thoughts into words via the
technologies of pen and paper, the diary symbolizes a haven for a person’s
most private thoughts, even if published in print later on. Diary writing,
as a quotidian cultural practice, involves both reflection and expression,
but it is also a peculiarly hybrid act of remembrance and communication,
always intended for private use, yet often betraying an awareness of its po-
tential to be read by others.
A similar ambiguity can be found in what is considered the diary’s
digital equivalent. Since 1995, weblogs have become a popular genre on the
Internet, as millions of people are now heavily engaged in blogging—
expressing and exchanging their personal accounts through the technolo-
gies of hardware and software.1 The term “weblog” or “blog” is a rather
general label for a variety of interactive forms; of all weblog variations, the
so-called lifelog seems to come closest to the traditional diary genre.2 Life-
logs are multimedial online experiments in self-expression, but they are
often cross-linked to other lifelogs, creating blog communities.3 Searching
on the Internet today, one can find a plethora of digital diaries, everything
from travel blogs chronicling the climbing of Mount Everest to personal
blogs documenting the spiritual journey of a born-again Christian, and
from the intimate exchange of sexual experiences between teenagers to the
shared stories of earthquake victims.
How do old-fashioned paper diaries compare with digital lifelogs?
My own experience and the experience of the two female students appear
strikingly similar in showing how diary writing and blogging are both am-
biguous acts, rife with ambiguities concerning privacy and publicness. And
Writing the Self 55
private phenomena, and as much as they offer public signs of their exis-
tence, these utterances should not be equated to subjective experiences
processed by the mind. When individuals express their intimate thoughts,
these verbal articulations are interlaced with behavioral, social, and cul-
tural aspects of what we call enunciation. Written records of personal ex-
perience are always mitigated by the tools and conventions of writing. In
order to deconstruct the myth of diaries forming a verbal template for the
outpour of reflections and feelings, we need to look more closely at the
mental process involving subjectivity and affectivity.
American psychologist Silvan Tomkins has argued that the emo-
tional formation of the subject occurs not in isolation but happens in
complex interactions or “affects” with others and with thought; affects, ac-
cording to Tomkins, are “neuro-physiological events” that are part of a
larger cognitive system operating as a series of distributed functions, in-
cluding sensory perception and memory.6 The affective constitution of
personal memories is well recognized by psychologists: when people read
or hear reminiscences narrated by others, they often feel triggered or in-
vited to contribute their own memories. Based on Tomkins’ theory, Aus-
tralian psychologist Anna Gibbs labels this phenomenon “contagious
feelings,” which she describes as a process of “neural firing,” eliciting a
positive feedback loop “in which more of the same affect will be evoked
in both the person experiencing the affect and in the observer (a phenom-
enon known as ‘affective resonance’).”7 In other words, subjectivity and
affectivity constitute each other in a constant feedback loop between self
and others, where the narration of experiences, memories, and feelings of
others contribute to the formation of self. Media, as Gibbs contends, in-
troduce a powerful new element in this state of affairs: they literally and
metaphorically act as amplifiers of affect while dramatically increasing ra-
pidity of communication and audience reach. Mediated affect may thus
at any point add to the feedback loop of affective subjectivity: personal
stories told on camera and broadcast by television may trigger associated
feelings and memories in individual viewers, contributing to that subject’s
emotional formation.
Gibbs implicitly transfers the notion of affective subjectivity from
autobiographical memory onto cultural memory by appointing contempo-
rary electronic media as the amplifiers of affect: stories told on television,
radio, or the Internet encourage public exchange of intimate feelings and
Writing the Self 57
emotions. But the mediated affect she describes did not originate with the
advent of electronic media such as television; for centuries, paper diaries
have provided this function. Diaries or personal notebooks, as the material
signifier of subjective reflection, never have been simply expressions of the
individual’s emotional state or psyche but are themselves cultural forms
that trigger affective response. Needless to say, there is no single, uniform
definition of what a diary is, and over the past centuries, its classification
has been anything but homogeneous.8 The diary obviously finds its roots
in the daily recording of events, transactions, feelings, and reflections; in
contrast to the journal, the diary is commonly referred to as a private kind
of reflective writing produced by a single author and closed to public
scrutiny. In general, the taxonomy of the old-fashioned paper diary tends
to be based either on its contents (personal, intimate self-expressions as op-
posed to daily records of fact) or on its directionality (intended for private
reading versus public use).9 However, this opposition is fallacious.
If we closely look at how paper diaries have been used in the past,
the characterization of diaries as enunciations written strictly for oneself is
hardly tenable. Keeping a diary is at once a creative and a communicative
act, and it also serves as a memory tool: writing the self constructs continuity
between past and present while keeping an eye on the future. Reading one’s
own scribbles at a later moment understandably elicits a tendency to rework
hindsight experiences into one’s personal reflection and to edit the original
entries not only for grammar and punctuation but also for content. The di-
ary’s contents, when reread at a later stage in life, may either yield nostalgic
yearning or retroactive embarrassment, in some cases even leading to a de-
finitive destruction of the object. Anne Frank’s famous diary is a case in
point: she began revising the first version of her personal account in March
of 1944, several months before she was deported to a concentration camp,
inspired by a government’s official radio address, urging Dutch citizens suf-
fering under Nazi occupation to keep personal notebooks for later publica-
tion. The various drafts have now become an essential part of her legacy:
they show how keen she was to revise her diary to include hindsight observa-
tions just a year after writing her first draft, as evidence of her evolving or
growing self. Equally important in this regard is the fact that Anne never in-
tended to keep the diary strictly to her (future) self: she wanted to become a
published author. Anne Frank’s desire to share experiences and evoke affects
with (potential) others was very much at the heart of her effort. Affective
58 Writing the Self
subjectivity is thus embodied in the Diary of Anne Frank, the published ver-
sion whose mediated affect has inspired many teenagers and young adults to
articulate their own personal struggles of coming of age in the face of op-
pression or diaspora.
A similar amalgamation of subjectivity and affectivity can be dis-
cerned in online blogging, although in a distinctly different form. Specify-
ing the mediated affect of contemporary blogs, I turn to a specific type of
lifelogs produced by patients suffering from Alzheimer’s disease (AD).10
By means of therapy, patients suffering from dementia and AD have long
been encouraged to try to retain a sense of self by looking at old photo-
graphs and rereading old notebooks and letters. Supported by relatives and
friends who want them to remember their (shared) pasts, one of the most
frustrating problems these patients face is their growing inability to recog-
nize themselves and their loved ones as depicted in pictures or described in
words. Adding to this frustration is the increasing distance, due to mem-
ory loss, between a patient’s mental world and the (social) world surround-
ing them. To an AD patient, the past is disconnected from the present,
and often relatives inevitably lose track of the patient’s mindset, thus sev-
ering ties between the worlds of self and others. Some patients, particu-
larly those coping with an early onset of the disease, have begun to explore
lifelogs as a new means to express themselves and share their experience
with others. Upon his own confrontation with the disease, Morris Friedell,
a retired professor from California, cofounded the Dementia and
Alzheimer’s Support Network International (DASNI) and initiated a blog
community of AD patients in 1999.11
Looking at various AD patients’ lifelogs, their most observable role is
in the sustenance of subjectivity. Some patients poignantly describe their
first symbolic confrontation with their “new self”: they find themselves
staring at the evidence of cerebral atrophy in the diagnostic scans made in
the hospital, while the doctor explains the signified meanings of the slices
on the screen. The look into the brain often leads to confusion and discus-
sion. As Friedell discovers, brain scans (PET, fMRI) are often indecisive
and ambiguous; a discussion on the Internet forum may help patients un-
derstand what medical images mean in terms of diagnosis and prospects.12
Once the initial shock of their confrontation with a shrinking brain vol-
ume subsides, patients discover the blog as a tool for self-preservation;
as the disease progresses, it becomes vital to document what can still be
Writing the Self 59
next one, which in turn adds to the communal effort. From this perspec-
tive, blogging becomes a sort of feedback loop in which subjectivity and
affect work reciprocally to constitute the formation of self in constant in-
teraction with others. And yet, the need to express oneself as an individual
and train the brain’s cognitive functions never appears to be at odds with
the need to connect and communicate. Consider, for instance, the blog of
Mary Lockhardt, an Oklahoma woman who ran a licensed day-care home
for infants until she was diagnosed with Alzheimer’s at age 55. She had to
give up working and started her blog in addition to opening a chat room
for patients like her. These online activities, according to Lockhart, helped
redirect her purpose in life: “This is a very lonely disease. I don’t want any-
one to feel as lonely as I did when I was diagnosed (I thought I was the
only one over 50 that had Alzheimer’s). If I can help one person with my
blog it will all be worth it.”15 Bound together by the same ordeal—people
“who are in the same leaky boat,” as AD-patient Morris Friedell phrases
it—each blogger trusts personal emotions to this novel medium and shares
his or her experience with relative strangers who are held captive by the
same neurological conditions. Yet the fact that this is a shared weblog does
not keep Mary Lockhart, or other bloggers, from listing the birthdays of
relatives and friends and reporting how she celebrated each of them. These
comments read like a typical boring diary entry—until you realize, as an
outside reader, that remembering birthdays is a great struggle in the life of
a person coping with dementia or Alzheimer’s.
The hybrid notion of the individual yet collective nature of remem-
brance is crucial to the understanding of why and how lifelogs work the
way they do. Australian media theorist Robert Payne argues how the on-
line dynamics of collective recollection stimulates the articulation and
rearticulation of the self. Analyzing an Internet-based collective project
named Random Access Memory, Payne relates how reading through other
people’s assorted memories—organized by themes or years—activated him
to trust his own personal memories to the screen, thus contributing to
the overall project and in turn stimulating others to revise or reenact their
memories in narrated form.16 Random Access Memory, according to
Payne, forms the online materialization of “interaffectivity”: an exchange
of individual stories making a community of records.17 Of course, Ran-
dom Access Memory entries do not have the same function as the entries
posted by Alzheimer’s patients in their lifelogs, but they equally enhance
Writing the Self 61
affect, are striking; their historical continuity is also palpable in their func-
tions to mediate between past and future. And yet, networked lifelogs are
distinctly different in that their default mode seems to be connectivity and
communication, as opposed to the default mode of diaries, which is isola-
tion and reflection. Notions of subjectivity and connectivity are equally im-
portant in AD patients’ fight against memory fading and isolation. In
addition, the Internet-based tool appears to diffuse the boundaries between
privacy and publicness. Whereas Anne Frank’s intimate reflections took a
long time to move from private diary to public memory object, the built-in
potential of lifelogs to open up one’s writings to selected audiences changes
their intent and impact. To further explore these changes, we need to turn to
the technological and material aspects of diaries and lifelogs.
Diaries are commonly valued for their contents rather than for their
look or feel. But the materiality of diaries and the technologies by which
they have come into being are crucial factors in their signification. When
referring to paper diaries, two typical concepts spring to mind: the empty
diary, preformatted for daily use, which we can buy at stationary stores;
and the original, handwritten manuscripts of diaries that have later ap-
peared in print and became widely read. The physical appearance of a pre-
fab diary prefigures the functions of its intended use: empty pages, with or
without lines, bound or unbound, dated or undated, offer the author stim-
uli to fill the more or less blank surface with personal inscriptions and
thoughts. In some cases, the diary is completed by a lock and key—a po-
tent symbol of its private nature. The preformatted diary has always been,
to some extent, a product of contemporary fashion, its design and layout
representing a particular style and catering to a specific age or taste. A di-
ary’s materiality forms an essential part of its content. Over the years,
diarists often grow fond of the look, feel, and smell of their notebooks;
fading paper, youthful handwriting, and ink blobs trigger reminiscences
the way photographs do.
Arguably, diary writing is not necessarily inspired by prefab formats:
many diaries that were discovered many years after they were written and
Writing the Self 63
subsequently published in print had first been scribbled onto single sheets
or in simple notebooks. The actual manuscript of such a diary, its original
inscription, becomes a vital sign of authenticity—often stored in special
places and only accessible to owners or researchers. In the case of Anne
Frank’s diary, written partly in notebooks and partly on single sheets of
paper, the written pages have become the centerpiece of the Dutch
teenager’s legacy. The manuscript, stored in Amsterdam, appeared in such
demand that the Anne Frank Foundation had two exact duplicates made:
one to replace the original on display at the museum, the other to satisfy
the many requests from film media directors, researchers, and documen-
tary makers for pictures of the original. The original manuscript was
locked in a safe place to protect it from further deterioration. Its faithful
copies concurrently underscored the idea of the diary as a public commod-
ity and withdrew the original’s unique materiality from the media’s or
public’s eye. The manuscript’s materiality constitutes an intricate part of
the diary’s genesis and forms the stake in controversial claims to its au-
thenticity, (uncensored) originality, and completeness.20
Pivotal to the materiality of diaries, up to the age of computers, has
been the notion of script: the concept of the diary is commonly associated
with (hand)writing, signifying not just authenticity but also personality.
Handwriting has historically been believed to betray the corporeality of its
producer—graphology being the study that yields clues to the writer’s char-
acter, such as age and even personality traits. Regarded as the first technol-
ogizing of the word, the availability of pen and paper facilitated the need to
make oneself legible to the other or to the future self. Writing is often tied
in with a stage in one’s personal development: a teenager’s scrawls betray his
or her inexperience with the prime tool of literacy—the immaturity of
body or mind.21 Sonja Neef, a German media scholar, claims handwriting is
an embodied practice enabled by technology: moving a pen onto paper in-
volves a direct connection between body and script, an act in which the eye
and hand are intimately interwoven with the technology of paper and pen
and the techniques of deploying them; the hand—a body part instrumen-
tal to the “Verkörperung” (embodiment) of thoughts—fixes the inner self
to the outside world.22
As our technologies for writing change, so do our ways of creating
self-reflective records; memory, in other words, is always implicated in the
act and technology of writing. Handwritten diaries as material artifacts are
64 Writing the Self
basically serve the same purpose, the formats may differ in layout and digi-
tal possibilities. To some extent, these different designs resemble the prefor-
matted paper diaries for sale at stationary stores. It seems like the various
software formats attract different audiences, catering to heterogeneous
tastes and lifestyles, much like brand names of fashion products. As Emily
Nussbaum points out in her ethnographic, journalistic report of bloggers,
the formats vary only slightly, but users’ choice of software packages may
be based on particular technical features.26 For instance, websites like Xanga
offer the possibility to give “aProps,” a kind of gold star rating for particu-
larly good posts, whereas LiveJournal allows for selecting features such as
“current mood music” and “embedded polls or surveys.” In a way, blog soft-
ware resembles the preference for jeans: nuances in style and brand name are
important to individuals who seek to belong to a group.
Digital weblogs, in terms of their materiality, may not even remotely
resemble their paper precursor, but there is still a distinct continuity in
their personal signature. If handwriting betrays a diary writer’s character
and level of maturity, the typewriter and later the word processor had al-
ready erased that trademark of personality; and yet, through word choice,
style, punctuation, and the use of emoticons it is remarkable how much
lifelog entries give away a person’s character. On top of that, the personal-
ity of a diarist is even more traceable through his or her prolific choices of
cultural contents. A blogger may attach references to (or actually attach)
songs, pictures, movies, books, and so forth. Despite prescriptive software
formats, weblogs offer a relatively high degree of creative freedom; users
can cut and paste from all kinds of media sources, thus exploring their taste
by testing it against the taste of others. Weblog architecture encourages this
type of self-exploration through encoded features such as “my current
mood,” “mini biography,” and “my interests.”
The most profound difference between a paper diary and a blog may
be found in the blog’s networked condition; lifelogs, by default, are tech-
nologies of affect—connecting the individual to others, whether selected
groups or an anonymous audience. Some websites offering lifelog software
(like OpenDiary.com) allow users to search entries by age group, gender,
theme of the week, subject, and cultural preferences; standard features
such as “friend groups,” “syndication,” and “communities” (LiveJournal),
prefigure a connected exploration of a user’s personal life. What the Internet
does best is to create a forum for collective discourses. In this culture of
Writing the Self 67
sharing, the lifelog finds its natural habitat: the digital diary becomes in-
strumental as its multimedial modality equally allows for the creation of
one’s personal entries as well as for the exchange of cultural contents (clip-
pings, files, songs). Blogging software and Internet hardware, in this argu-
ment, are neither neutral technical conduits nor simple commodities, but
they are technological tools facilitating a social process in which exchange
and participation are conditions of enacting citizenship.27 The flipside of the
culture of reciprocity is instant marketability: personal taste and cultural
choices become instantaneously traceable and marketable to commercial
ventures. Whereas many diaries (like OpenDiary.com and DearDiary.com)
started out as small communities of like-minded individuals, many of
these services have been bought by corporations.28 In a networked envi-
ronment, where information is constantly cached, weblogs have become
gold mines for data diggers. Nostalgic notions of personality still persist,
even if new media prompt a keen awareness of technological strategies di-
recting individual taste and community building.
Although the multimedial lifelog appears very different from the
preformatted lock-and-key paper diary, each materiality gives away dis-
tinctive clues to an author’s subjectivity and personality. Just as paper di-
aries reflect someone’s age, taste, and preference at a particular moment in
one’s life, the software and signature of blogs seem to accommodate the
needs of especially contemporary teens and young adults to express and
sort out their identity in an increasingly wired, mediated world. Digital
technologies are imperative to the creation of blogs, but technology does
not tell the whole story; in our focus on technologies, we often tend to un-
derstate the importance of their uses. In conjunction to the technological
script, we hence need to look at how social practices and cultural forms
transpire through the concrete manifestations of diary writing and lifelogs.
The myth that the diary is a private object, written strictly for oneself,
contrasting a journal of fact, which is written for others, is as misleading as
it is persistent. As literary scholar Thomas Mallon argues, no one ever kept
a diary just for him- or herself; pointing out the continuity between the
68 Writing the Self
journal and the diary, he concludes that both are directed toward an audi-
ence and “both [are] rooted in the idea of dailiness, but perhaps because of
the journal’s link to the newspaper trade and the diary’s etymological kin-
ship to ‘dear,’ the latter seems more intimate than the former.”29 Of all the
varieties within the diary genre, some are written more with a reader in
mind than others, but an essential feature of all diaries is their (imagined)
addressee. Some authors direct their diaries to God, to an imagined
friend—like Anne Frank’s “Kitty” or André Gide’s mysterious addressee—
or to the world at large; in any case, the notion of addressing is crucial to
the recognition of diary writing as an act of communication.30 Even as a
form of self-expression, diary writing signals the need to connect, either to
someone or something else or to oneself later in life.31
Another misperception we can trace in the genealogy of diaries con-
cerns their authorship. They were commonly ascribed individual voice and
authorship—supposedly written not only for one person but also by one
person. However, there are a number of historical examples in which the
genre was deployed as a communal means of expressing and remembering.
Many religious congregations, for instance, considered the diary a semi-
public record, shared within but never outside a community.32 In the history
of diary writing, the genre as a communal means of expression has found
many practitioners, from South Pole explorers to POWs held in captivity.33
For groups bound together by an adventurous ordeal, a collaborative diary
was often a means to trust one’s personal emotions to a relatively safe medium
and share the experience with mates held captive under the same conditions.
We often encounter preconceived notions of the diary as a private ob-
ject written by a single author when it is contrasted to the lifelog; the digital
networked variant is thought to privilege connection over individual expres-
sion and group formation over private authorship. Indeed, exploring one’s
identity in the various scenarios allowed by online groups is part and parcel
of experimenting with multiple aspects of a personality, specifically among
teenaged bloggers.34 Many young bloggers engage in diary writing to be
seen or to see themselves through the lenses of others—a kind of validating
experience even if no one else ever reads the diary. And as we witnessed in
the blogs fostered by AD patients, they obviously value connectedness: the
potential to address others. Connectivity and sharing apparently form the
default mode of blogging, as opposed to individual expression and mono-
logue, which seem to form the default mode of paper diaries. And yet, as
Writing the Self 69
argued above, neither the historical genre of the diary nor its contemporary
digital variant fits this binary genre frame: the functions of self-expression
and communication have never been at odds but have always been (and still
are) copresent in the genre. Diaries have historically been produced as a sort
of materialized feedback loop—dialogic rather than monologist—in which
subjective emotions trigger responses that contribute to an individual’s for-
mation. Lifelogs continue to espouse this hybridity, which is less a feature of
the genre itself as it is an effect of its cultural use.
From our reading of historical diaries, we may deduce many details
concerning the social practice of diary keeping; by the same token, we can
tell from reading online lifelogs (complemented by ethnographic research)
how contemporary teenagers deploy the new technologies to perform daily
rites. Both diary writing and blogging are ritualized activities that gradu-
ally receive a place in a person’s life. Writing a diary, of course, never hap-
pened in a social vacuum; the ritual occupies its own niche alongside other
acts of communication, such as talking, listening, reading, and so forth.
As a quotidian habit, diary keeping gives meaning and structure to some-
one’s life, and therefore it is important to regard this activity in the context
of everyday activities. In the case of Anne Frank, writing a journal created
a zone of silence and refuge in a small space, densely crowded and heavily
trafficked by human interaction. Her daily ritual was an act of self-
protection as much as of self-expression. By carving out a discursive space,
she was able to articulate her private thoughts and define her position in
relation to others and to the world at large. Diary writers fashion a habit
by choosing a medium; the creation of that mediated habit is always in-
spired by cultural conventions and prevailing fashions.35 Quotidian acts
such as diary writing should thus be regarded not only as stilled reflections
but also as ways of constructing life. They always coexist amid a number
of other communicative habits and culturally determined practices.
Cultural practices have become increasingly mediated in the past
century: watching television, talking on the phone, taking pictures, and
writing letters are only few of many potential communicative acts through
which people articulate themselves. With the introduction of the Internet,
some of these daily rituals are gradually changing, often fusing old prac-
tices with new conventions. For instance, the emergence of e-mail substan-
tially transformed people’s daily rituals of communication and interaction
along with their sense of physical or psychological presence, just as the
70 Writing the Self
its authentic form and retrieved to invoke a past experience. When a diary’s
contents were published through an intermediate process of editing, print-
ing, and distribution, we were mostly concerned with how the original
words—presumably the recordings of experiences—matched the words
published in print. The fusion of old and new technologies results in a hy-
brid tool that seamlessly combines editing and archival functions; blogging
allows for preserving and revising entries at the same time. Blogging itself
becomes a real-life experience, a construction of self that is mediated by
tools for reflection and communication. In the life of bloggers, the medium
is not the message but the medium is the experience. If the meaning of experi-
ence is slowly changing, so is the meaning of memory. As time proceeds,
memories of experiences inevitably evolve; revising one’s past inscriptions is
a natural part of a process of personal growth. Rather than being fixed in
objects, memory mutates through digital materiality. Although the Internet
is often characterized as a transient, evanescent medium, lifelogs have both
the ability to fix and the potential to morph.
It is exactly this hybrid function of the blog that helps Alzheimer’s
patients to sustain a sense of continuity in the face of harrowing mental
changes in their self-consciousness. The lifelog becomes literally a memory
device through which they can speak their minds, even as their states of
mind appear increasingly confused in the eyes of relatives and loved ones.
In the past, Alzheimer’s patients were often recommended to look at old
photographs to regain and retain an idea of past self, but in a way this
meant a constant confrontation with the person they realized they would
never be again, and for relatives these photographs carried the resentment
of their “lost” loved one. Contrastingly, the lifelog is equipped to bespeak
a changing self in the various stages of mental deterioration. Rather than
lamenting the loss of a past persona—a memory to someone who has men-
tally if not yet physically disappeared from our lives—this enunciation at
least refurbishes memory back into a real-life experience. Digital media, as
amplifiers of affect, dramatically increase the rapidity and extent of inter-
affectivity to reach global proportions, and this affect may in turn em-
power patients to keep themselves composed in their regular reports.
Bloggers are retooling the current practice of diary writing, mean-
while creating new types of mental scripts, cultural knowledge, and social
interaction via their tools. The reciprocity inherent in networked systems
points at a profound reorganization in social consciousness. Examining
76 Writing the Self
sion plays, and so forth may directly derive from the wordless stories first
created in our minds.15
Damasio’s approach highlighting internal narratives is surprisingly
complementary to cultural-semiotic theory relating musical memory to
individual and social identity. Thomas Turino has attempted to counter
the intriguing problem of musical memory by approaching music as a
system of signs; he uses Charles S. Peirce’s notion of indexicality to ex-
plain how music is not about feelings but rather involves signs of feeling
and experience.16 Musical signs, he says, are sonic events that create ef-
fects and affects in a perceiver the way a falling tree creates waves
through the air. Rational effects or conscious responses are responses
that involve reasoning: the interpretation or appreciation of music. The
“secondness” or affect of music lies not in the sounds or words per se,
but in the emotions, feelings, and experiences attached to hearing a par-
ticular song. Musical signs thus carry strong personal connotations be-
traying an emotional investment; at the same time, however, members of
a social group share indices proportional to common experiences.17 In
sum, musical signs integrate affective and identity-forming meanings in a
direct manner, and it is through the recollection of songs that we may
come to see the nature of this integration.
Damasio’s neurocognitive speculation and Turino’s cultural semiotic
conjecture cannot be empricially tested, but applying narrative analysis to
stories that relate how people feel affected by recorded popular music of-
fers an insight into the connection of personal and collective memory.
Many respondents to the Top 2000 create images or stories around certain
songs to help them communicate a particular feeling or mood or to express
their affective ties to particular songs. Through these stories, we learn how
people came to invest emotionally in a song, how the song came to mean
anything to them in the first place, and how they retained that attached
meaning—a meaning they like to share with a large, anonymous audience.
As Damasio predicts recall includes the experience of listening and the
emotive state at the time of apprehending. Compare, for instance the first
reaction to John Lennon’s song “Imagine” with the next reaction, posted
in response to the U2 song “With or Without You”:
It was 1971, I was waiting on a boat someplace in Norway when I heard this song
for the first time. It was such a perfect day, everything was right: the weather, the
blue sky, the peaceful tidal waves in the fjord matching the melodious waves of
Record and Hold 83
music. There are moments in life that you feel thoroughly, profoundly happy.
This was such moment, believe me. (posted by Jan from Eindhoven)
My father died suddenly in November of 1986. That night we all stayed awake.
I isolated myself from my family by putting on the headphones and listening to
this song. The intense sorrow I felt that night was expressed in Bono’s intense
screams. I will never forget this experience, and each time I hear this song I get
tears in my eyes. (posted by Jelle van Netten from Woudsend)
In this comment, a general longing for the mood of a past era is as-
sociated with lived experience, even though the experience is somewhat
blurred and sensuous. Mental maps, as neuroscientific theories show, are
derived partly from the object itself and partly from the auditory, visual,
olfactory, or other perceptions triggered in our minds.18 Respondents also
frequently report smells and tastes to be invoked by familiar songs.
The comment above implicitly suggests that the memory aroused
upon hearing the song is an exact repeat of the original listening experi-
ence. The idea of a recording reiterating its exact same content each time it
is played is subconsciously transposed to the experience attached to hear-
ing the music. People’s expectation to feel the same response each time a
song is played stems from a craving to relive the past as it was—as if the
84 Record and Hold
past were a recording. Many of us want our memory of the original listen-
ing experience to be untainted by time, age, or life’s emotional toll. And
yet, it is improbable that repeated listening over a lifespan would leave the
original emotion (if there ever was such thing) intact. Instead, the original
listening experience may be substituted by a fixed pattern of associa-
tions, a pattern that is likely to become more brightly and intensely col-
ored over the years.19 Memories change each time they are recalled, and
their contents are determined more by the present than by the past. As
Geoffrey O’Brien eloquently puts it in his musical memoir: “The age of
recording is necessarily an age of nostalgia—when was the past so haunt-
ingly accessible?—but its bitterest insight is the incapacity of even the
most perfectly captured sound to restore the moment of its first inscribing.
That world is no longer there.”20
Cognitive research confirms that musical remembrance alters with
age. An American clinical study shows a significant difference between
how older and younger people remember recorded music; whereas young
adults tie in recorded music with memories of specific autobiographical
events, seniors use familiar songs as stimuli to summon more general
memories and moods from the past.21 Recorded music infested with feel-
ings elicits stronger—even if less specific—autobiographical memories later
on in life. Because the narratives posted to the Top 2000 website do not sys-
tematically disclose the respondents’ ages, I cannot use them to confirm or
disprove the researchers’ empirical observation. In general, though, respon-
dents who give clues to their age as being over forty-five tend to refer more
to nostalgic moods triggered by specific songs than respondents who iden-
tify themselves as being young adults. However, this might just as well be
attributed to the fact that a majority of songs featured in this collective
ranking were popular in the era when baby boomers came of age.
The observation of neuroscientists and cognitive psychologists that
musical memory is an emotional investment has been corroborated by both
sociologists and cultural theorists. Australian cultural geographers John
Connell and Chris Gibson argue, for instance, that “music can evoke mem-
ories of youth and act as a reminder of earlier freedoms, attitudes, events;
its emotive power . . . serves to intensify feelings of nostalgia, regret or rem-
iniscence.”22 And yet, it is too simple to assume that music triggers memo-
ries; recorded music may also construct a cognitive framework through
which (collectively) constructed meanings are transposed onto individual
Record and Hold 85
And another listener attributes her fondness for the pop song “You’re
So Vain” by Carly Simon to fetal exposure:
When my mother was pregnant with me, in 1973, my father bought her this al-
bum as a present. They played the record innumerable times. As long as I am
aware, this has been my favorite pop song, but I only found out about my parents’
story several years ago. Who knows, listening to music in the womb may have an
effect on a person’s musical taste! (posted by Harriette Hofstede from Dordrecht)
Musical memories can thus be understood as an intergenerational
transfer of personal and collective heritage, not only by sharing music but
also by sharing stories; many comments posted on the website testify to
this circular cultural process. Like photographs, recorded songs relate
personal memories. That older people are eager to pass on their stories
along with their preference for certain recorded music is therefore not
surprising.
Damasio’s conjecture about the mind’s involvement in autobiograph-
ical recall, suggests that narrated audio impressions help glue recorded mu-
sic to people’s cultural memory. Understood in terms of bodily affect, the
mind is a sewing machine that quilts personal memory onto recorded mu-
sic, stitched together by emotion and feelings. Whether tied in with specific
experiences or general moods, stories appear a distinct aid in remembering
the mental associations attached to a particular kind of music. These sto-
ries, like the memories themselves, are likely to change with age, and as
much as we like to capture the original affection triggered by music, we
want the story to hold that feeling for future recollection. Yet stories, like
records, are mere resources in the process of reminiscence, a process that
often involves imagination as much as retention. In other words, our per-
sonal musical repertoire is a living memory that stimulates narrative en-
gagement from the first time we hear a song up to each time we replay it at
later stages in life. It is this vivid process of narrative recall that gives mean-
ing to an album and assigns personal and cultural value to a song.
generating resentment, their aging may partially account for our very at-
tachment to these objects. Personal memory evolves through our interac-
tions with these apparatuses (such as record players, compact disc players,
radios) and material things (such as records, cassettes, digital files), as both
are agents in the process of remembering. Media technologies and objects
are often deployed as metaphors, expressing a cultural desire for personal
memory to function like an archive or a storage facility for lived experience.
When it comes to music, it is easy to see where this metaphorical notion
originated. The record’s presumed ability to register—to record and hold—
a particular mood, experience, or emotional response can be traced back to
the its historically ascribed function as a material-mechanical inscription of
a single musical performance. It is almost a truism to expect recording
equipment to replay the presumed original sound of a song, notwithstand-
ing our awareness that objects and apparatuses—like bodies—wear out,
age, and thus change over time. The “thingness” of recorded music is un-
stable and yet this knowledge does not prevent a peculiar yearning for the
re-creation of audio quality as it was first perceived, evidenced for instance
by the recent vinyl nostalgia accompanying the surge in compact disc
sales.26 People who use recorded music as a vehicle for memories often
yearn for more than mere retro appeal: they want these apparatuses to reen-
act their cherished, often magical experiences of listening.
It may be illustrative to filter this kind of techno-stalgia from the
comments posted on the Top 2000 website, espousing the integrality of
technology to people’s reminiscing. Many respondents recall the sound
equipment through which they first heard a particular song, emphasizing
how it defined their listening experience. Writing in reaction to the Beatles
song “The Long and Winding Road” a woman writes:
The first time I heard this song I almost snuggled into my transistor radio. This was
the most beautiful thing I had ever heard. When I got the Beatles’ album, I re-
member pushing the little Lenco-speakers against my ear (they were sort of the
precursor of the walkman). Whenever this record is played again, I get on my
knees, direct my ears downward, pushing them toward the speakers on the floor.
I still want to live in this song. (posted by Karin de Groot from Rotterdam)
and context of the original sound—a fact the respondent is very aware
of—but certainly brings about the intended affect.27
In other instances of reminiscence, the role of technology should be
understood indexically rather than metaphorically—adhering to Thomas
Turino’s Peircian apparatus—as it stands for taking control over one’s
sonic space. Memories of the original listening experience often include
allusions to the newly acquired freedom to listen to these songs, alone or
with friends, outside the living room where the soundscape was usually
controlled by the musical taste of parents. The 1960s ushered in a period
of “private mobilization,” to borrow Raymond Williams’s renowned
term, so it is not surprising to find that many respondents, recalling im-
pressions from that era, take notice of sound technology’s bearing upon
their coming of age.28 Many are still committed to the sounds mounting
from the radio (especially transistors and car radios), a medium that first
confronted the baby-boom generation with pop music. In contrast to mu-
sic played on personal stereos (record players or tape recorders), radio
sound is ephemeral. Listening to music on the radio often allows for a mo-
mentary inner sensation that the listener is part of something larger; it cre-
ates relationships between self and others that contribute to an individual’s
sociality.29 Narratives that testify to the liberating role of music technol-
ogy abound on the website postings to the Top 2000. Read, for instance,
this reaction to the Herman Hermits song “No Milk Today”:
Because this was the first song to wake me up to the phenomenon of pop music in
the years 1966–1970, it reminds me of how magical it felt to just listen to my small
transistor radio, often secretively, because I needed to hide it away from my par-
ents. When I listen to this song now, I turn up the sound as much as I can, prefer-
ably when I am driving my car and listening to old tapes. (posted by Maarten
Storm from Leusden)
process of remembering. The term “recorded music” has become the rather
generic container for vinyl albums, cassette tapes, compact discs, and MP3
files stored in computers. But the status of these items varies and that varia-
tion affects their function in memory formation. Each song listened to from
live radio, records, cassette tapes, or MP3 players has a different emotion at-
tached to it. Prerecorded CDs and records are more valuable as objects to
hold on to and collect, whereas MP3s and cassette tapes have a different
function: they are more often a backup or backlog. As music theorist Mark
Katz shows in his study on sound capturing, which includes a survey of
young downloaders of recorded music, a large majority of respondents still
buys prerecorded CDs, often after having listened to them in rerecorded
form or after having shared them in whatever mechanical or digital form:
“The tangibility of the CD is part of its charm. A collection is meant to be
displayed, and has a visual impact that confers a degree of expertise on its
owner.”32 In semiotic terms, the indexical function of the musical sign is
bound up with its auditory materiality: hearing a familiar song on the radio
constitutes a different memory experience than playing that very song from
one’s own collection, perhaps even more so when these recordings are played
from MP3 formats. As one respondent to the Top 2000 puts it:
It is so strange: I keep most songs [featured in the Top 2000] on CDs and I have
the entire list of songs stored in MP3 format on the hard disk of my PC, so I can
listen to these songs any day any time. And yet, I only swing and sing along with
my favorite songs when I hear them on the radio, during this yearly end-of-the-
year broadcast. (posted by Jaap Timmer from Winterswijk)
The transfer of emotive affection from the brain onto the technology
and materiality of audio recordings shows how memory acts out in the
spaces between individual reminiscences and shared experiences. These same
narratives disclose how materiality and technology often become integral to
memory, something that is unlikely to change with the advent of digital
equipment. As long as listening to music remains a mediated experience,
memory will be enabled and constructed through its material constituents.
The radio event engenders collectivity at the same time and by the same
means that it generates collective memories; the actual sharing of music
and singing along with a group hence becomes part of the emotion trig-
gered by a song.
Some sound technologies, by nature of their hardware, promote lis-
tening to music as a solo activity, but can still be deployed in social activi-
ties. Ever since the emergence of the Walkman in the 1970s, personal stereos
have been associated with the construction of individual sonic space. As
British sociologist Michael Bull argues, personal stereos can function as a
form of “auditory mnemonic” in which users attempt to reconstruct a
sense of narrative within urban spaces that have in themselves no narra-
tive sense to them.35 And while it is true that the Walkman—and more
recently the MP3 player and Discman—are designed with individual ur-
ban listeners in mind, these recording technologies can also be put to so-
cial use and serve as collective listening instruments. As we can read in
the posting of an eighteen-year-old respondent to the Top 2000 website,
who commends the 1961 song “Non, je ne regrette rien” performed by
Edith Piaf:
Last summer, half a dozen of my classmates drove to France to celebrate our high
school graduation. We played a lot of oldies, and as both cars had their own iPods
attached to the stereo system, we sang along as loud as we could with our self-
compiled repertoire. Now we’ve all gone off to different colleges, but next month
we’ll have a reunion and I’m sure we’ll bring our iPods along, so we can bring
back some cherished memories. (posted by Willem van Oostrum from Utrecht)
The rather novel act of plugging the iPod into the car’s stereo sys-
tem, allowing the youths to collectively listen to and sing along with the
songs stored on the device, is inscribed in the narrative recollection of a
generation of young adults; they consciously create their own sonic mem-
ories, using the newest devices to re-create golden oldies. The MP3 player,
rather than being a mere vehicle for individual listening and storage of fa-
vorite songs, thus figures as agency in the conscious process of building up
a collective memory.
Besides collective listening, remixing and exchanging songs are im-
portant means for constructing a collective platform for shared memories.
Memory and identity are ineffably bound to us in the way we re-create
given formats; interventions in prerecorded cultural forms propelled by
the music industry are more than symbols of individual appropriation.
Record and Hold 93
live recordings of the songs played on the radio. The chatbox at times
functions as a platform for the exchange of homemade selections, thus be-
coming a venue for sharing creative re-collections—a way of embedding
one’s idiosyncratic choice in a community of listeners.
To sum up, cultural practices like communal listening, mixing, and
swapping recorded music appear crucial in understanding how and why
we construct shared memories through embedded experiences: musical
memories are shaped through social practices and cultural forms as much
as through individual emotions. New digital technologies allow music fans
to customize their favorite songs and use them as symbolic resources in the
construction of collective identity and community. But let us now turn to
the role the Top 2000, as a cultural form, plays in inculcating a sense of
collectivity in its listeners. Should we look upon this event the way we re-
gard rankings of pop music and radio programs? In what sense do the Top
2000 stories of individual reminiscence contribute to a sense of collective
memory and communal cultural heritage?
The Dutch Top 2000 nicely illustrates how mediated memories are
shaped precisely at the intersections of personal and collective memory.
Mental mappings, sound technologies, and sociocultural practices consti-
tute the channels for shaping individuality while concurrently defining the
larger collectivity we (want to) belong to—ensuring autobiographical as
well as historical continuity. Through embodied affection, enabling tech-
nologies, and cultural embedding, recorded music becomes part of our
collective memory at the same time and by the same means as it gets settled
into our personal memories. Viewed from a neurocognitive angle, we of-
ten engage with recorded music by stitching emotion or lived experience
onto musical impressions, hence conjuring up mental maps—internal sto-
ries that are later recalled as part of our musical memory. Theorized from
a semiotic-cultural perspective, personal emotions and affects attached to
songs are articulated in explicit memory narratives that people like to
exchange—reminiscences of lived experience expressed through musical
preferences. These stories not only are about emotions triggered by music,
but they also directly bespeak musical memory as it relates to personal
and group identity, not seldom handed down generation to generation.
Record and Hold 95
versus collective identity is far more important than the ultimate result; in-
teractive participation of listeners is the goal, not the means by which a rank-
ing is compiled.
One might argue that the collective nature of the Top 2000 can just as
well be explained by its trendy catering to a participatory, (inter)active audi-
ence; the event, after all, fits in well with the current boom of audience par-
ticipation contest shows on radio and television. However, there are several
arguments to rebut this explanation. For one thing, it is not a coincidence
that the event is staged through public rather than commercial radio. Stag-
ing the Top 2000 is unlike oldies listings by commercial stations catering to
a retro experience. As stated earlier, many if not all of the songs featured in
the Top 2000 can be heard by tuning into one of the many commercial
oldies stations abounding on the airwaves, and they can be listened to on de-
mand by downloading or pulling them from one’s private collection. Cul-
tural historian Paul Grainge proposes the relevant distinction between
nostalgia as a commercial mode and a collective mood. In the example of the
Top 2000, nostalgia emanates from a collectively experienced mood, in con-
trast to a conception of nostalgia as a consumable stylistic mode espoused by
commercial outlets such as the Top 40 or oldies stations.42 I concur with
Grainge that the oldies station phenomenon can be better explained by the
industry’s imperative to find profitable market segments and niche con-
sumption than by a presumed generational longing for an idealized past.
Grainge’s concept of nostalgia as an experienced mood links up with my
definition of collective cultural memory: it connects personal affect and
emotion to collective identity and heritage via recorded music.
In addition, I think the participatory, interactive nature of the Top
2000 is geared toward discussion and exchange rather than commercial
call-in activities and revenue-generating voting strategies. Through a com-
bination of a radio event, website, and television broadcast, this multime-
dia platform offers space for consensus building on a national heritage of
pop songs, and it simultaneously serves as a podium for collective nostalgia
and communal reminiscence. Audience participation is of course a consti-
tutive element of the ultimate ranking list—indeed, without individuals
sending in their favorite top fives there would be no Top 2000. And with-
out the hundreds of thousands of comments filling the chatbox and par-
ticipants sharing stories through web postings, there would be no event. It
is rather the desire to couple personal memories onto collective experience
Record and Hold 97
and the need for a platform for the exchange of musical memories that
constitutes the repeated success and public impact of this event.
The extensive archive of responses generated by the Dutch Top 2000
constitutes an interesting source of data on personal musical memory and
cultural heritage formation. It opens up new perspectives on the impor-
tance of public space for sharing personal stories and constructing a collec-
tive musical kinship, which in turn feeds our individual creativity and
identity. The commercial domain, although an important provider of re-
sources for a common culture, seems to have less and less tolerance for the
necessity of building up collective reservoirs, public involvement, and cre-
ative exchange. Virtually void of commercial push-and-pull mechanisms,
the Top 2000 offers space for narrative engagement and gives room to what
American legal scholar Lawrence Lessig calls a “creative commons.”43
Whereas (digital) culture, according to Lessig, opens up ample opportuni-
ties for strengthening the public domain and for promoting individual
creativity, the commercial music industry often impedes exchange and par-
ticipation.44 The Top 2000 encourages both individual memories and col-
lective reminiscence; attaching emotion to recorded music is not only
imperative to the formation of personal identity, but it is instrumental in
imagining collectivity. Indeed, the public domain, or the creative com-
mons, is vital to keep alive a vibrant heritage of old and new music, because
it provides individuals with cultural resources to understand their own
pasts and guarantee a shared interest in a communal future—both essential
forces in people’s long-term commitment to music. By ignoring insights in
how recorded music functions as cultural memory—the linchpin between
individual remembrance and collective cultural heritage—we may deprive
ourselves of an important enticement for future growth.
5
retrograde projections, and these mental image maps, in turn, feed a desire
to impact external (camera) visions of ourselves.6
The role photographs play in the complex construction of one’s per-
sonal memory and identity has been theorized in cognitive theory as well
as in cultural theory, particularly semiotics. Cognitive psychologists have
investigated the intriguing question of how photographs can influence
personal memories.7 The human mind actively produces visual autobio-
graphical evidence through photographs, but it also modifies this evidence
through pictures—cutting off estranged spouses or throwing away depress-
ing images depicting them when they were still seriously overweight. Re-
search has shown that people are easily seduced into creating false
memories of their pasts on the basis of unaltered as well as doctored pic-
tures. In the early 1990s, researchers from America and New Zealand per-
suaded experimental subjects into believing false narratives about their
childhoods, written or told by family members and substantiated by “true”
photographs.8 Over the next decade, these findings were corroborated by
experiments in which doctored pictures were used; more than 50 percent
of all subjects constructed false memories out of old personal photographs
that were carefully retouched to depict a scene that had never happened in
that person’s life.9 There is a continuing debate whether it is narratives or
photographs (or a combination of both) that trigger most false memories,
but the conclusion that people’s autobiographical memories are prone to
either self-induced intervention or secret manipulation is well estab-
lished.10 Not surprisingly, these scientific insights are gratefully deployed
in marketing and advertising departments to advance sales by manipulat-
ing customers’ memories about their pasts and thus influence their future
(buying) behavior. What customers recall about prior product or shopping
experiences often differ from their actual experiences if marketers refer to
those past experiences in positive ways.11
The close interweaving of memory, imagination, and desire in cre-
ating a picture of one’s past has also been subject to theoretical probing
by cultural theorists, most notably Roland Barthes. When exploring the
intricacies of the camera lucida, Barthes testifies to this complex loop of
images/pictures informing desire/memory when describing the discom-
fort he feels the moment he succumbs to being the camera’s object.
Having one’s photograph taken, as Barthes observes, is a closed field of
forces where four image repertoires intersect: “the one that I think I am”
102 Pictures of Life, Living Pictures
(the mental self-image); “the one I want others to think I am” (the idealized
self-image); “the one the photographer thinks I am” (the photographed
self-image); and “the one the photographer makes use of when exhibiting
his art” (the public self-image or imago).12 Whereas the first two levels rep-
resent the stages of mental, internal image processing, the third and fourth
levels refer to the external process of picture taking and presentation—the
photographer’s frame of reference and cultural perspective. In contrast to
psychologists, Barthes’s semiotic perspective emphasizes that cognition
does not necessarily reside inside our brains but extends into the social
and cultural realm.
Barthes’s exploration of analog photography elucidates how the
four image repertoires of self intersect and yet never match. They collide
at various moments: at the instant of capturing, when evaluating the out-
come or photographed object, or while reminiscing at a later point in
time, reviewing the picture. When a picture is taken, we want those pho-
tographs to match our idealized self-image—flattering, without pimples,
happy, attractive—so we attempt to influence the process by posing, smil-
ing, or giving instructions to the photographer. At a later stage, we can try
to change the undesired outcome by selecting, refusing, or destroying the
actual print. A photographed person exerts only limited control over the
resulting picture. The photographer’s choice of frame and angle defines
the portraiture, and the referent can be further modified at the stage of de-
velopment by applying retouching techniques. Barthes obviously feels
powerless in the face of the photographer’s decisions, lacking control over
the picture’s referent, which he wants to equal his idealized self. Its fate is
in the hands of the photographer who is taking the picture and of the
chemical, mechanical, and publishing forces involved in its ultimate mate-
rialization. Barthes’s discomfort signals a fundamental resentment about
his inability to fashion pictures in his own image. Because the four levels
never coincide, portrait photographs are profoundly alienating, even to the
extent of giving the French philosopher a sense of imposture.
Paradoxically, Barthes perceives a lack of control over his photo-
graphed image and imago and yet he feels confident he can exert power over
the mental and idealized images entering his mind. According to Barthes,
our mental capacities determine which images are allowed to enter our
minds and memories. Photos that “work” are those you still remember when
you no longer see them. To test the affect of a picture, Barthes suggests
Pictures of Life, Living Pictures 103
that you close your eyes and wait to see if the photographed image is incul-
cated in the mind—that is, if it conjures up mental images. Barthes’s now
famous concept of the photograph’s punctum uses memory as a litmus test
for admitting pictures to the mental reservoir. In a similar vein, the auto-
biographical self selects those photographed images that work the mind,
though they do not necessarily “work” to accommodate or feed its idealized
self-images. On the contrary, old photographs of self may counteract such
projections. For instance, when looking at a group portrait of myself amidst
a class of sixth graders, I may identify myself as the one who looks dumb or
innocent, or I may point at the stupid dress my mother made me wear to
school. In the process of recalling, photographs may block or parcel out spe-
cific mental images. We can apparently resist photographs the way we resist
memories, not just by literally destroying them but also by simply barring
them from entering the mind’s eye. The photograph is never a simple con-
duit but is made to signify something by the mind’s ability to frame a picture
each time it is (re)viewed.
Barthes’s perceived powerlessness over the photographer’s perspec-
tive and the black box of the camera in relation to the assumed autonomy
over his mental images and memories appears entirely plausible, and yet
neither perception can go undisputed. The photographed image—the
manipulation of one’s public imago—has never been outside the subject’s
influence. Since the late 1840s, commercial portrait photographers have
succumbed to their patrons’ desire for idealized self-images the way paint-
ers did before the advent of photography: by adopting flattering perspec-
tives and applying chemical magic. However, the subject’s power over
images entering the mind may not be as manageable as it appears. Cultural
ideals of physical appearance, displayed through photographs and evolv-
ing over time, often unconsciously influence the mind’s (idealized) images
of self.13 Control over photographic images is hence not inscribed in the
machine’s ontology, and neither does the mind have full sovereignty over
the images it allows to enter memory. Instead, control over one’s self-
portrait is a subtle choreography of the four image repertoires, a balancing
act in which photographic images enculturate personal memory and a sub-
ject’s memory evolves through cultural engagement.14 Individual cognitive
editing and processes of image manipulation are intertwined at every level
of cultural memory.
Now when we replace the analog camera with a digital one, and
104 Pictures of Life, Living Pictures
laminated photos with pixeled shots, how does this affect the intertwining
of mental-cognitive and cultural-material image processes in photogra-
phy? The attempt to answer this question reveals a conspicuous absence
of interdisciplinary research in this area. None of the cognitive studies
discussed above pay attention to ways in which individuals use digital
photography to manipulate their own personal pictures and memory; the
cultural, material, and technological aspects of memory morphing appear
strikingly irrelevant to cognitive science. This is striking because scientists
often mention how their academic interest in manipulated pictures gains
relevance in the face of a growing ubiquitous use of digital photography
and its endless potential to reconstruct and retouch one’s childhood
memories; skills once monopolized by Hollywood studios and advertising
agencies are now within the reach of every individual who owns a “digital
camera, image editing software, a computer, and the capacity to follow
instructions.”15 Indeed, without digital photo enhancement equipment,
cognitive psychologists would have a hard time conducting their research
on manipulated autobiographical memory in the first place; only with the
help of computer paintbrush programs can they make doctored photo-
graphs look immaculate. Mutatis mutandis, when turning to cultural the-
orists for enlightenment, their disregard of psychological and cognitive
studies in this area is rather remarkable; semioticians and constructivists
typically analyze the intricacies of technological devices to connect them
to social and cultural agency.16 Yet without acknowledging the profound
interlacing of mental, technical, and cultural levels involved in digital
photography, we may never understand the intricate connection between
memory, identity formation, and photography.
It may be instructive to spell out a few significant differences between
analog and digital photography in terms of their (cognitive and technical)
mechanisms. At first sight, digital photography provides more access to
the imaging process between the stages of taking the picture and looking at
its printed result. Only seconds after its taking, the picture may be pre-
viewed via the camera’s small screen. The display shows a tentative result,
an image that can be deleted or stored. Because this sneak preview allows
the photographer to instantly share the results with the photographed sub-
ject, there is room for negotiation: the subject’s evaluation of his or her self-
image may influence the next posture. A second review takes place at the
computer, in which images, stored as digital code, are susceptible to editing
Pictures of Life, Living Pictures 105
also produces material keepsakes that embody routines and generate com-
mercial products.
toddler’s latest achievements may have been distributed with the same in-
tent as laminated pictures enclosed with a letter, but that does not mean
they convey the same message. The connotation of e-mailing pictures as
attachments is one of transience rather than of permanence, a mere update
rather than a record. Along similar lines, showing a slide show on a laptop
is a performative act ardently reminiscent of an old-fashioned slide show,
and yet the casualness of the gesture implied in the laptop presentation
sharply contrasts the cerebral efforts involved in setting up the slide pro-
jector, pulling out the screen, and sorting through the slide racks.
Digital photography elicits a performativity and materiality that de-
serves to be evaluated in its own right, not just as a virtualization of former
analog products and practices. The virtual image, as a screen object, is grad-
ually acquiring a new status beside the laminated object, which in turn ad-
justs its meaning to suit this pixeled neophyte. We can see this also in
relation to methods of storing and presenting personal picture collections.
Software engineers increasingly begin to realize that the design of picture
management systems requires a profound understanding of why and how
users interact with their pictures: the acts of storing pictures in a shoebox or
sticking them into albums cannot simply be transposed onto digital plat-
forms.31 In terms of hardware, the single-purpose camera for taking still
pictures gives way to multifunctional appliances, combining the camera
function with the personal digital assistant (PDA), the mobile phone, MP3
players, and global positioning devices. But the so-called camera phone also
permits entirely new performative rituals, such as shooting a picture at a live
concert and instantly e-mailing the image to a friend. Emerging digital tools
are thus deeply affecting the way people socialize and interact and, by ex-
tension, the way they maintain relationships and consolidate them into per-
sonal memory.
Because personal photography is the pivot of a large industrial branch,
commercial stakes in the digital refurbishing of material keepsakes are high.
The logic of consumption and replenishment very much underlies digital
photography, hence the huge economic interest in creating and tracing new
material and performative uses of photography. Hardware industries are
gradually shaking off their chemical and laboratory divisions, instead con-
centrating on corporate strategies that concurrently target digital and pa-
per image processing. Software developers respond to the camera phone
trend by integrating photography into multifunctional scripts for online
Pictures of Life, Living Pictures 111
taking and storing photographs to document their lives but also by partic-
ipating in communal photographic exchanges that mark their identity as
interactive producers and consumers of culture.
From the above observations, it is tempting to draw the conclusion
that digital cameras are moving away from their prime functions as mem-
ory tools, instead becoming tools for identity formation, communication,
and experience. If photographs were always a medium for remembering
scenes and objects from the past, digital cameras particularly encourage
users to imagine and invent the present. Digital personal photography gives
rise to new social practices in which pictures are considered visual resources
in the microcultures of everyday life.45 In these microcultures, memory
does not so much disappear from the spectrum of social use as it takes on a
different meaning. In the networked reality of people’s everyday life, the
default mode of personal photography becomes sharing. However, few
people realize shared experience almost by definition implies distributed
storage: personal live pictures distributed through the Internet may remain
there for an indefinite period, turning up in unforeseen contexts, reframed
and repurposed. A well-known example may clarify the meaning of distrib-
uted memory and demonstrate the intertwined meanings of personal and
collective cultural memory: the Abu Ghraib pictures.
In May 2004, a series of the most horrific, graphic scenes of torture
and violence used by American guards stationed at the Abu Ghraib prison
against Iraqi detainees appeared in the press, and they were subsequently
disseminated through the Internet.46 Most pictures were made by prison
guards and frequently featured two lower ranked members of the armed
forces, Charles Graner and Lynndie England; they often posed giving the
thumbs-up in front of individual or piled up prisoners who invariably
showed signs of torture or sexual assault. The hundreds of pictures taken
by prison guards of detainees communicate an arduous casualness in the
act of photographing. Clearly, these picture were made by digital cameras
(or camera phones) deployed by Army personnel as part of their daily
work routines—perfectly in tune with the popular function of photogra-
phy as a ritual of everyday communication. As Sontag poignantly describes
in her essay on the case: “The pictures taken by American soldiers in Abu
Ghraib reflect a recent shift in the use made of pictures—less objects to be
saved than messages to be disseminated, circulated. A digital camera is a
common possession among soldiers. Where once photographing war was
Pictures of Life, Living Pictures 117
apply to bodies. Memory, like photographs and bodies, can now be made
picture perfect; memory and photography change in conjunction, adapting
to contemporary expectations and prevailing norms. Our photographs tell
us who we want to be and how we want to be remembered. Just as before-
and-after photos on the Internet or on television normalize the need for cos-
metic surgery, the abundance of editing tools available will make the
inclination to brush up our past selves even more acceptable. Personal pho-
tography may become a lifelong exercise in revising past desires and adjust-
ing them to new expectations. Even if still a memory tool, the digital camera
is now pushed as an instrument for identity construction, allowing more
shaping power over autobiographical memories.
And yet, this same manipulative potential that empowers people to
shape their identity and memory may be also used by others to reshape
that image. The consequence of digital technology is that personal pictures
can be retouched without leaving traces and be manipulated regardless of
ownership or intent of the original picture, as evidenced at the beginning
of this chapter by the anecdote about the student who was unpleasantly
surprised to find a doctored picture of her and her friends electronically
distributed to (anonymous) recipients. Personal photographs are increas-
ingly pulled out of the shoebox to be used as public signifiers. Pictures
once bound to remain in personal archives increasingly enter the public do-
main, where they are invariably brushed up or retouched to (retro)fit con-
temporary narratives. It is quite plausible to see personal pictures emerge in
entirely different public contexts, either as testimony to a criminal on the
run, as a memorial to a soldier who died in the war, or as evidence of a
politician’s excessive alcohol use in college.48 Like the subjects who were
shown altered pictures in the psychologists’ experiments, we may be un-
able to determine whether they are true or false: is it memory that manip-
ulates pictures, or do we use pictures to create or adjust memory? The
digital age is setting new standards for remembrance and recall: the value
of the terms “true” and “doctored” in the context of memory will have to
be reconsidered accordingly.
The ability of photographic objects to evoke personal memories, is
increasingly giving way to its communicative and experiential uses. In
addition to photographs’ function as material keepsakes, once primarily
intended for veneration or ritual use—stored in the family archive or ex-
posed on the walls of the home gallery—photographs metamorphose into
120 Pictures of Life, Living Pictures
diminish control over what happens once the pictures become part of a
networked environment, which changes their performative function each
time they are retrieved. Due to this networked condition, the definition
of personal cultural memory is gravitating toward a distributed presence.
We can no longer keep the lid on the shoebox we used to store in our at-
tic: its pictorial contents will increasingly spill out into the virtual corners
of the World Wide Web, where it seamlessly blends in with our collec-
tive pictorial heritage. Once again, pictures of life will become living
pictures—even if unwittingly.
6
more recently the digital camcorder have been used to capture the rou-
tines of everyday life. At the same time, “real families” transpire in cul-
tural products everywhere. Families played out in television series shape
our mental concepts of family interaction, and home movies provide in-
put for media products. In the age of camcorders, webcams, and multi-
media productions, family lives are becoming an insidious part of our
combined mental, technological, and cultural fabric of memory. We need
to differentiate between these three levels in order to understand how, in
the digital age, filmed family life always involves remembrance, fabrication
and projection. Various examples—a feature film, a documentary, and
websites—will illustrate this combined theoretical approach. From the
analyses of contemporary mediated memories, I argue that the future of
memory will be determined by our tools for reconstruction as much as by
our imaginative capacities.
Let us return for a moment to the scene in which the children de-
manded their playful activities to be taped by their father’s camera. The
youngsters were keenly aware of how video footage potentially steers their
public image as they grasp the essence of raw images serving as input for
memories that have yet to be shaped. But could such awareness be the re-
sult of their minds’ projections or, perhaps more likely, is it the result of
the movie camera’s infiltration into their mindsets—the result of the ubiq-
uitous presence of camcorders in their everyday lives? Mind and technol-
ogy, as I argued in earlier chapters, are closely interwoven in our projections
and memories of self. The desire to identify oneself as belonging to a
family may be deeply implicated in the private camera as a mnemonic
tool—to save visual evidence of family life for later reference—but it may
just as well be a function of the brain to funnel conscious perceptions of
family into desirable or idealized (moving) images. I succinctly explore
various hypothetical angles to account for the complex interrelation be-
tween mind and audiovisual technology in the construction of personal
cultural memory.
Some neurobiologists who study the physiological mechanisms of
autobiographical memory concentrate on the brain as an explanatory
framework, and choose to ignore the constitutive function of technology or
Projecting the Family’s Future Past 125
inherently defined by the input of actual moving images, which are always
partly constructions of the brain.4 Whereas Damasio’s term “movie-in-the-
brain” implies a figural equation (to understand the brain’s mechanism in
terms of film productions), Deleuze explicitly connects cognitive mecha-
nisms to the movement-image of cinema. Cinema is as much a production
of the individual mind as it is a production of a mechanical apparatus.
Echoing Bergson, Deleuze distinguishes between different categories of im-
ages in motion: the perception-image, the affection-image, and the action-
image.5 “External images” act upon the mind and become “internal images,”
moving images that stir action or affection, converging into experience.
Memory, in Deleuze’s concept, is never a retrieval of past images but
always a function of the present, a function that embodies the essential
continuation of time. Body and cinema are part of the same organic, con-
nective system: just as the body constantly renews itself molecularly, im-
ages never remain the same when processed in an individual’s mind. In
other words, moving images produced by film are input for the brain, al-
ways resulting in updated output. Such a dynamic concept of movement-
image sharply contrasts semiotic theories that consider film footage as
representations or signs. “Perception-images” of the present determine
how actual images of the past are interpreted, and yet, both are inevitably
injected with projections of the future: idealized images, virtual images,
desire. It is instructive to quote Deleuze’s words in full here: “But instead
of a constituted memory, as function of the past which reports a story, we
witness the birth of memory, as function of the future which retains what
happens in order to make it the object to come of the other memory. . . .
[M]emory could never evoke and report the past if it had not already been
constituted at the moment when past was still present, hence in an aim to
come. It is in fact for this reason that it is behavior: it is in the present that
we make a memory, in order to make use of it in the future when the pres-
ent will be past.”6 Films are imagined in the brain—a process involving
the convergence of mental projections and technological scripts. If the past
is a filmic product of the present, so is the future; according to Deleuze,
memory is always in a “state of becoming.”
It takes little effort to translate this part of Deleuze’s theory into the
mediated memories model outlined in figure 2 (see Chapter 2). Applied to
the specificity of home movies, one could argue that the film apparatus is
inseparable from the individual who deploys the camera to articulate a
Projecting the Family’s Future Past 127
the eyes—an audiovisual recording straight from the brain. The implanted
camera starts rolling the moment the fetus passes its mother’s birth canal,
and it is not removed until that person’s death. In Naim’s imagined soci-
ety, one out of five people carry an implant, often unwittingly, as parents
are not supposed to inform their children of this surprise until the chil-
dren turn twenty-one. The Zoe implant is one of the most precious gifts
a parent can donate to a child; it replaces the need for personal photo-
graphs or home movies because the eye camera offers a full and impecca-
ble registration of someone’s life. After the person’s death, the chip is
removed to be edited into a so-called rememorial, a ninety-minute film
reminiscent of a made-in-Hollywood biopic. The implant comes with a
new ritual: the “Re-Memory” is a commemorative gathering taking place
forty days after a person’s death at a cinema-alias-funeral parlor where the
feature-length retrospective of the deceased is premiered to an invited
audience.
The main character of The Final Cut is Alan Hakman (Robin
Williams), one of the best professional cutter’s of Re-Memory movies; his
job is to turn the lifetime reels, removed from the deceased’s brain, into con-
ventional audio-visual productions, a sort of edited and anthologized digest
presented as the ultimate obituary. In the main plot line, Hakman is asked
to perform the “final cut” on the life movie of Charles Bannister, one of Eye
Tech’s attorneys who recently died. His widow Jennifer instructs Hakman
to edit her husband’s implant footage into a glossy retrospective, honoring
the principles “family, community, career” and thus carefully omitting any
scenes that would compromise his public image. As all cutters do, Hakman
betrays the cutter’s code proscribing to refrain from manipulation, instead
accommodating the wishes of surviving relatives. While sorting through
Bannister’s life files, the editor is not only confronted with dubious money
laundering schemes and fraud, but he also witnesses scenes in which the
attorney commits adultery and incest with his daughter Isabel. Hakman
discretely erases all compromising evidence from the deceased’s ultimate
portrait: the delete function turns out to be the cutter’s most important Re-
Memory tool. The result is a sanitized life in review, a public version of a
family man with a brilliant career. Although every Re-Memory visitor un-
derstands the subtext of this genre, the dark side of Bannister’s life remains
invisible to the public eye. The ultimate home movie is everything but a
true memory of the deceased’s life. Like any film, this one is a mediated
130 Projecting the Family’s Future Past
series that premiered on January 11, 1973, captured seven months in the life
of a real California family—Pat and Bill Loud and their five teenaged chil-
dren, Lance, Kevin, Grant, Delilah, and Michele. Producer Craig Gilbert
and filmmakers Alan and Susan Raymond followed each member of the
household at a turbulent time of their lives: the Louds’ marriage ended in a
divorce; the oldest son, Lance, announced he was gay; and the family unit
was literally splitting up. A PBS announcement described this new televi-
sion format as it invited viewers to “meet TV’s first real family tonight and
share their lives in the 11 weeks to follow.”24 The immensely popular series
struck a chord with the American audience, as the Louds’ turmoil mirrored
quite a few experiences that real families encountered in this decade of
change. The center was not holding—to echo Yeats’s famous dictum—and
television was recording its falling apart.
Because the series itself, as a unique and revolutionary media phe-
nomenon, has received due scholarly attention, here I concentrate on the
construction of the home mode in An American Family, because it exem-
plifies the struggle with codes inscribed in the home movies of the 1950s
in favor of the newer, more immediate style of home video and cinema
vérité.25 An American Family subscribed to this new style as it suited the
function of capturing family life as it is. In the words of the PBS announce-
ment: “The Louds are not actors. They had no scripts. They simply lived.
And were filmed.”26 Film crews that followed the Louds made themselves
unobtrusive and their presence went almost unnoticed by the Loud family
members, even while recording the couple’s rows over Bill’s extramarital
affairs and their decision to file for divorce. Lance, the oldest son living in
New York, received a light-weight Portapak recorder to document his own
observations in addition to being filmed. This direct cinema style sharply
contrasted the nostalgic photographs and home movie reels edited into the
series as a way to visualize the Louds’ past family life. As Ruoff explains in
his reconstruction of the series: “The home movies and family photo-
graphs themselves represent an important detour from the observational
focus on images and sounds recorded in the present. The Louds’ home
movies chronicle festive occasions such as Thanksgiving dinners and baby
Lance taking his first steps. These nostalgic recollections suggest happier
times, offering a powerful contrast to the Louds’ contemporary lives.”27
Those incremental snippets of home movies and photographs not only sig-
nify life as it was for the Louds, but they also serve as a confirmation of
136 Projecting the Family’s Future Past
techniques set the standards for arresting reality in the era of analog
video, the webcam may currently count as the symbolic catching hook for
life as it is. A naturalistic mode of filming gives way to a surveillance
mode of recording: fixed webcams cover an unwitting subject’s move-
ments. Even when the subjects are aware of the cameras’ presence, there is
no actual intervention from a camera crew. This mode of surveillance is
not only frequently employed in public areas, where cameras control the
movements of passengers, but with the webcam installed in a home, it is
also rapidly becoming a common means of self-exposure on the World
Wide Web.29
The digital mode seems to better suit contemporary fractured no-
tions of family and more prominent individualism. Extending the home
video logic of the 1980s—the real family captured in their rebellion against
normative domestic values—the camcorder of the 1990s allots even more
power to individual users to construct their idiosyncratic views of family.30
As Moran observes, families in the 1990s and the twentieth-first century
are no longer “natural” units: they are “families we choose”—domestic
relationships between individuals construed as family ties.31 The family
we choose is the family that chooses to film itself as a unit, using an in-
sider’s perspective and camera, giving each member a direct voice in his
or her representation. New conventions of television programming also
define what constitutes a real family: a number of individuals who volun-
tarily move into a house, succumb to a regime of created conditions, and
are continuously monitored by numerous surveillance cameras. The Big
Brother effect, in a way, is the televised, formatted counterpart of circuited
webcams installed in a family’s home, continuously beaming pictures of
real family life on the Internet.
Let us look at one example of a turn-of-the-millennium webcast fea-
turing a regular family and compare it with a typical television series inte-
grating the surveillance home mode into its program format. The webcam
site of the Jacobs family from Alta Loma, a Los Angeles suburb, is one of
thousands of sites featuring regular family life on the Internet these days.32
Consisting of two parents and seven children, the Jacobs keep up an ex-
tensive home page where each member presents him- or herself in pictures
and texts. The site presents an interesting mixture of the 1950s idealized
family–style pictures and the digital surveillance home mode. When turned
on, the webcam beams live footage from the living room: the children
138 Projecting the Family’s Future Past
joking, the mother showing things to the webcam, the father leaning
backward in his office chair.33 The webcam footage seems to verify the real
happiness professed in the website’s texts and still pictures, as if saying,
“Look, this happiness is authentic; you can watch us live from our home,
every day.”
Interestingly, the Jacobs family is literally made possible through the
Internet. On their home page, they introduce themselves as follows: “We
are the Jacobs Family. Clarence and DeShawn were married on November
27, 1999. We met on the internet [sic] in August of 1999, fell in love, and
were married in Las Vegas. When me met, DeShawn had her son Dustin,
and Clarence had Jennifer, CJ, Kyle, and Davey. Since then, we have
‘added’ Jarrett and Julia. A true ‘Brady Bunch’ as we are sometimes called.”34
The Jacobs, as we can see and read, are a typical postmodern fractured
family: DeShawn writes about her birth mother and the search for her bi-
ological father, daughter Jennifer lives with her mother and stepfather in
Arizona, and Clarence’s sons are getting used to their new half brother and
half sister. We can safely assume digital technology not only facilitates but
actually enables family life for the Jacobs: without the webcam, their
daughter Jenny in Arizona could not participate in everyday life, and with-
out the Internet, DeShawn would not have been able to contact her bio-
logical father. In short, the Jacobs construct their “bunch” through digital
media, while at the same time (and by the same means) they also beam this
family’s domestic bliss to a potentially worldwide audience.
Perhaps not coincidentally, the Jacobses’ favorite pastime, as we can
read on their site, is watching reality series on television. One of the more
popular domestic reality series featuring in the early twenty-first century
is The Osbournes. Ozzie Osbourne, once the notorious band leader of the
heavy metal group Black Sabbath; his wife and manager Sharon; and their
teenage children Kelly and Jack, according to MTVs announcement, al-
low surveillance cameras to continuously peek into their private lives,
and that footage is turned into “an addictive new docu-series.”35 Even if
the announcement promises the series to be a continuous window onto the
lives of a famed rock star and his family, the net result is a traditional tele-
vision format. On the one hand, The Osbournes deploys technological de-
vices to create a sense of reality: furtive handicam footage is alternated by
fixed steadicam shots, which are occasionally used by a family member to
stage an intimate tête-à-tête with the television audience. On the other
Projecting the Family’s Future Past 139
hand, the MTV docu-series is edited into an episodic format that undeni-
ably reminds us of conventional genres—in this case a hybrid of a domes-
tic sitcom and a video clip.36 The docu-reels in the television series bring
the traditional format of the domestic sitcom (and traditional family
values) up to the current technical standards of reality capturing.37 Natu-
rally, the home mode and family concept featured in reality series like The
Osbournes inspire many family webcam sites.
Following the social constructivist logic of James Moran, audiovisual
representations of the Bunkers, the Louds, the Osbournes, and the Jacobs,
reveal the interlocking of home movie technologies and cultural forms
such as (reality) television series. In contrast to Deleuze and Hansen, Moran
considers filmed family portraits to be technical substrates interwoven with
sociocultural norms and conventions. While (cognitive) philosophers show
little interest in the sociocultural component of converging brains-cum-
apparatuses, cultural theorists such as Moran tend to disregard mental-
cognitive functions when describing the home mode. And yet, I think we
need a merger of both approaches in order to fully comprehend the intri-
cateness of current filmic constructions of hindsight, especially now that
digital technologies prompt us to develop a renewed awareness of medi-
ated memory as a fabric woven of cognitive, technological, and cultural
threads.
and reshaping family life; director Andrew Jarecki’s production is not just a
documentary; the DVD-version, which I focus on here, renders a much
more comprehensive view of the harrowing family saga he is trying to tell.39
The Friedmans are a typical middle-class Jewish American family living in
Great Neck, Long Island, where Arnold and Elaine, both into their fifties,
have raised three sons: David, Seth, and Jesse. A retired schoolteacher,
Arnold teaches computer classes for kids in his home basement and is helped
by his youngest son Jesse, then eighteen. In November 1987 they are both ar-
rested on charges of repeated sexual abuse of boys who attended their
classes. The police raid heralds months of denigrating incarceration, release
on bail, a media frenzy, a neighborhood witch-hunt, and—within the Fried-
mans’ home—family rows over the best strategy to keep Arnold and Jesse
out of jail. The family is torn apart by conflicting emotions of guilt, doubt,
suspicion, and loyalty; David chooses the side of his father and brother and
resents his mother Elaine, who is in more than one way the family’s outsider.
She is never convinced of Arnold’s innocence, as he has lied to her in the
past about his pedophilic inclinations and molestations. When her endearing
attempts to save Jesse, by urging both father and son to plead guilty, inad-
vertently backfire, her oldest son David bitterly turns against her. In separate
hearings, Arnold and later his youngest son enter their guilty pleas and are
sentenced to substantial jail time; Arnold commits suicide in 1995 while im-
prisoned, and Jesse is released in 2001 after having served thirteen years of
his sentence.
Although the events unfolding in retrospect before the viewers’ eyes
are dramatic by themselves, it is the hypertextual nature of this documen-
tary and particularly the DVD that prevents the story from turning either
sensationalist or partisan. Rather than following a chronological narrative
logic, the documentary relies on the viewer’s ability to identify three dif-
ferent technical types of film and to intertwine the distinct historical and
contemporary home modes to which they refer: the home movies shot by
Arnold in the 1950s through 1970s; David’s home video footage recorded
after the arrest in 1987; and interviews taped in the present by Jarecki.
For starters, the documentary features home movies and family
pictures shot by Arnold Friedman primarily in the 1950s through the 1970s.
These images are in perfect accordance with the conventions of home
movies at the time: happy scenes of birthday parties, beach fun, and fam-
ily vacations. Filming appears to be a family tradition—a narcissistic way
142 Projecting the Family’s Future Past
And the documentary is extended on the Internet via a website that updates
its viewers on the continuous saga of the Friedman family. David and his
brother Jesse—now released from prison—try to get a retrial, on the basis
of Jarecki’s film (especially his interviews with witnesses), to prove their
father’s and Jesse’s innocence.
In line with Moran’s constructivist theory, technical and socio-
cultural codes codetermine the construction of family and our personal
and collective memory of it. Yet beyond this constructivist analysis, we
need to acknowledge that Capturing the Friedmans is also a coproduction
of mind and technology, involving both the reconstruction of past images
and the projection of future memories. Future construction of cinematic
hindsight is already inscribed at the moment of each home movie shoot-
ing, most notably when David Friedman, in the middle of the turmoil in
1988, takes up the video camera and turns it onto himself. Sitting on his
bed, and starts a monologue: “This is a private thing, you know . . . if you’re
not me, you’re not supposed to be watching this. This is between me and
me, between me now and me in the future.” In this scene, David is ad-
dressing himself in the future. Indeed, why make a video if there was no
intention of telling the family story someday, in some form? Another in-
stance illuminating the intentional inflection of future memories of the
family’s past comes in David’s response when director Jarecki asked him
why he started to film his family’s ordeal: “Maybe I shot the video tape so
I wouldn’t have to remember it myself. It’s a possibility. Because I don’t
really remember it outside the tape. Like your parents take pictures of you
but you don’t remember being there but just the photographs hanging on
the wall.” David cogently identifies the power of home video and home
movies as dual instruments for constructing and remembering family life.
On the one hand, he needs to record his own version of reality because his
father is going to jail and he does not want his own future children to re-
member their grandfather from newspaper pictures. On the other hand, he
wants to document his father’s and brother’s innocence. For instance,
David films Jesse while driving the car on his way to the courthouse where
Jesse intends to enter his guilty plea and hopes to obtain a reduced prison
sentence; David forces his brother to state his innocence when he asks,
“Did you do it, Jess?” to which Jesse solemnly responds, “I never touched a
kid.” This home video footage painfully contrasts the official court video
later in the documentary, showing a crying, remorseful Jesse admitting his
Projecting the Family’s Future Past 145
guilt to the judge—an act so convincing you no longer know which docu-
mentary evidence to believe. David and Jesse undoubtedly utilize the home
video to assert some measure of control over the events as they are unfold-
ing, perhaps in an attempt to avoid the family breaking up. But in doing
so, they consciously build their future defense—their personal memory of
a torment that was, in their version, uncalled for and unjust.
As we learn from Capturing the Friedmans, family is a production
of media as well as a product of memory, involving projection and re-
construction.42 Through our home modes, we construct the memories of
tomorrow, and via our technologies, we create projections of the past.
Family is remembered through mental, technological, and cultural means
of mediation, and every generation chooses its own tools to understand
and reframe that concept. If we want to understand how both media and
memory change along with notions of family, we may depart from Jarecki’s
improvised definition of memory in an interview included in the DVD:
“We think we can put our memories away in a box and we can go check on
them later and they will be the same, but they are never the same; they are
these electro-chemical bubbles that continue to bubble over time.” Mem-
ories never just are; they are always in the process of becoming. Our
memories of family evolve in conjunction with home movie technologies
and social codes and conventions; the real challenge of cultural analysis is
to recognize mediated memories’ historical complexity in order to explain
their contemporary relevance.
vibrations” where the image “must produce a shock, a nerve wave which
gives rise to thought.”43 Memory moves along the axis of time: we are always
in a state of becoming. The cultural forms we produce, whether home
movies or science fiction films, are at once the result of, and input for, our
brain waves. The future of home movies is contingent on the shaping power
of past and present (filmic) productions—a shaping power that leads Hansen
to shift his focus to the “post-cinematic problem of framing information in
order to create (embodied) digital images.”44
That same circularity applies to the model’s horizontal axis relating
self to others. Individual family members produce and project home modes
while the home mode defines the concept of family as a social unit of be-
longing. What we call a home movie is in fact a coproduction in which
mental and cultural concepts of family and home constantly evolve, at
once revising old notions and anticipating new ones. Changing technolo-
gies (8 mm film, video, camcorders) are instrumental in the construction
of familial memories—images of how a family was, how it presents itself,
and how it wants to remember in the future. But the filmic perspective on
family is always embodied in individual minds: individual members whose
subjective pairs of eyes provide points of view that may at any time di-
verge or converge from siblings or other relatives. A home movie, like
memory itself, is not a self-evident filmic document that chronicles a fam-
ily’s past. Instead, by analyzing the discrepancies and tensions between its
various makers and producers, we acknowledge moving images stored in
our family archives to be input for—rather than output of—memory acts.
The meaning and impact of these documents may always be subject to fu-
ture reuse and reinterpretation, and hence our notion of family is never es-
tablished for once and for all.
With the advent of camcorders and advanced editing facilities on
personal computers, the awareness of moving images serving as input for
future memories is likely to become more prevalent. The rapidly growing
cultural practice to record one’s life audiovisually by means of ever more
digital instruments, combined with the innate inclination of human mem-
ory to select and reinterpret the past, presages the immanent expansion of
cinematic retrospection. Bolstering this trend is the growing interest of
people in multimedia productions that galvanize their remembrance after
death; like artists or actors, we want to secure an eternal place in the vir-
tual universe.45 Personal recordings of someone’s life increasingly resemble
Projecting the Family’s Future Past 147
the mechanical logic espoused by Bush and regard the brain and the ma-
chine (memory and media) to be ontologically distinct entities.
Because a main thesis of this book is to show the mutual shaping of
human cognitive memory and media technologies in everyday cultural
contexts, I address the questions of how new digital memory machines
may affect the nature of our recollections and the process of remember-
ing and, vice versa, how a connectionist model of human memory and its
recycling mechanism influence the conceptualization of new tools. The
last section of this chapter argues that networked computers serve less as
repositories and more as agents of change. Digital storage-retrieval facil-
ities, such as search engines, are not merely new metaphors that mold
our concepts of memory; they actually define the performative nature of
memory. Identifying three major types of transformation—digitization,
multimediatization and googlization—I argue these developments are in-
tegrated in the ways we store, retrieve, and adjust memories in the course
of living.
the digital age, the brain has been frequently invoked as a source of meta-
phorical imagery, mirroring the frequent use of (media) technologies in
neurobiological and cognitive circles, as described in the previous chapter.8
Bush’s concept of the memory machine is both mechanical and para-
doxical. Mechanical because he presumes an unambiguous vector between
technology and the human mind: the memex ought to function as a human
mind. Memory, to his regret, is fallible (“transitory”) and therefore, a ma-
chine should take over part of the brain’s function to prevent amnesia due
to information overload. Ideas, memories, and thoughts are stored in docu-
ments or other recordings, and these recordings should be randomly (“asso-
ciatively”) retrievable. When talking about data, Bush equates bits of
information to ideas, memories, and thoughts that can be put away in a
repository and be pulled out in random order. However, retrieval of docu-
ments from a database and retrieval of memories from a human brain are
fundamentally different processes with very distinctive goals. Documents or
recordings can be stored in a database, and we want them to be there, un-
changed, as we retrieve them and subject them to (re)interpretation; mem-
ories are never unchanging data that can be stored and retrieved in original
shape. As German media theorist Hartmut Winkler puts it: “Material stor-
age devices are supposed to preserve their contents faithfully. Human
memories, on the other hand, tend to select, reconfigure, and forget their
contents—and we know from theory that this is the real achievement of
human memory. Forgetting, in that sense, is not a defect, but an absolute
necessary form of protection.”9 Even if human memory and material infor-
mation deposits are distinctly different entities, they are inextricably inter-
twined in the process of remembering: retrieved documents constantly feed
upon a twisting and changing memory, whereas the human mind tends to
alter information in a depository by replacing, renaming, or deleting its
content files. As most people know from experience, reordering shoebox
contents or reorganizing one’s files on a desktop always involves both a
mental and a material exercise. Of course, deleting or destroying items may
cause intense regret later, but that regret is part of the integral act of re-
membering one’s past.
Returning to Bush’s concept of memory, we can conclude that, be-
sides being mechanical, it is also paradoxical. Machines cannot simply be
modeled after the human brain, because the brain interacts with the ma-
chine and vice versa. In Bush’s vision, there is no room for the logic that
From Shoebox to Digital Memory Machine 153
personal collections. There have been many more initiatives in this area
than I have room to describe here, so instead of a general overview, I fo-
cus on four projects in more detail: Shoebox, the Living Memory Box,
Lifestreams, and MyLifeBits. Some of these projects are technical in na-
ture, concentrating on the development of hardware and software tools that
help tackle the management of personal data; others are more visionary, es-
pousing a comprehensive perspective on the nature and structure of per-
sonal memory machines—more in line with Bush’s historic effort. Despite
the different goals and outcomes of these various projects, their common
assumptions about the functioning of personal collections in relation to
memory betray a peculiar desire to control and manage the human brain
like a computer system or, vice versa, to model the machine after the brain.
Shoebox
“Shoebox” is the name of a digital photo management system devel-
oped by AT&T labs in Cambridge, England.11 Its software package pro-
vides a range of browsing and searching facilities for the storage and
retrieval of digital photographs, utilizing spoken and written annotations
as well as a content-based image retrieval method.12 The AT&T researchers
specifically tested the usability of Shoebox as a retrieval system in the con-
text of personal photo collections, involving audio and textual labels be-
sides photographic images. Although they rarely talk about memory as
such, their research is built on the assumption that personal photo col-
lections primarily—if not solely—serve as reminiscing tools in people’s
everyday life. Perfectly ordered photo collections save the user time and
annoyance: easy retrieval depends on logical storage and labeling.
For instance, the easiest way to index series of photographs would be
to order them chronologically (when taken) or geographically (where taken).
Yet the designers resist the notion that personal collections of photos are ex-
clusively stored or retrieved by factual data. Shoebox tacitly subscribes to the
idea that people remember a picture topically (the Tower of Pisa) or recall a
series of pictures thematically (our vacation in Italy). Adding key words or
short descriptions, users can also retrieve specific photos by combining ob-
jects and persons (Uncle Sam at the Eiffel Tower). Frequent users may
vaguely remember the content or image shape of the picture, but they may
have forgotten when or where it was taken and thus, where it is stored.
Image-based indexing, in that case, may be a helpful retrieval tool. The
From Shoebox to Digital Memory Machine 155
Lifestreams
Whereas Shoebox and the Living Memory Box offer designs of con-
crete software-supported memory machines, the next two projects are
more in line with Bush’s visionary scheme. Introducing their Lifestreams
project, four researchers from the Department of Computer Science at
Yale University define their modest goal: “to change the world, computa-
tionally speaking.”18 A lifestream is a time-ordered stream of documents
158 From Shoebox to Digital Memory Machine
MyLifeBits
Whereas Lifestreams sprouts from the minds of computer scientists,
MyLifeBits is an American commercial venture, but the two projects operate
from remarkably similar premises. MyLifeBits was launched in 2002 by a
group of Microsoft engineers at the MS Media Presence Lab in San Fran-
cisco led by Gordon Bell.21 The project leaders work on a comprehensive
software system and communicate their goals and mission to a broad audi-
ence.22 The engineers at Microsoft aim to build multimedia tools that allow
people to chronicle their lives’ events and make them searchable, because
memories deceive us: “Experiences get exaggerated, we muddle the timing
of events, and simply forget stuff,” says one of the project leaders. “What we
want and need is a faithful memory, one that records and builds on the reli-
ability of the PC.”23 Bell and his team consider memory’s ability to forget
and revise a weakness rather than a strength or necessity. In the many inter-
views Bell issued to the news media, he pitches MyLifeBits not only as the
solution to the giant shoebox problem but as an organizer of life: everyday
events will be fully recorded in text, images, and audio and stored orderly in
a computer. Each item will be tagged by audio or text annotations—tags
that may also be cross-linked. To test the program, Bell has downloaded all
his personal bits of life, including his parents’ photographs, onto a hard
160 From Shoebox to Digital Memory Machine
drive; MyLifeBits, he claims, is more than a memory, it’s “an accurate surro-
gate brain,” the realization of Bush’s memex machine, which equally fea-
tured automated retrieval as its highest ambition. Bell imagines how, someday
in the future, a compulsive recorder will call up a single day in his or her life
and get an hour-by-hour breakdown of what he or she did, said, and saw.
Besides chronological retrieval, one major advantage of MyLifeBits software
is its ability to allow a “Google-like search on your life”: to retrieve random
memories by typing in a tag.
Unlike Shoebox, but much in accordance with the Living Memory
Box, MyLifeBits software departs from the notion of stories or memory
narratives as key-ingredients of the remembering process. The shoebox of
digital items is viewed as a nonhierarchical repository of annotated data,
out of which users construct a story every time they retrieve a single bit.
Annotated stories may be browsed like the World Wide Web, that is, the
user simply follows the links connecting one resource to another by means
of keyword. Thus, memory is conceived as an associative journey through
linked bits of life, which may subsequently be re-presented in any medi-
ated form: as a Powerpoint presentation, a slide show, or a photo album.
Unlike other systems heretofore described, the presentation of stories en-
abled by item retrieval constitutes the conceptual heart of this project. The
most valuable inheritance to our children or grandchildren, Bell claims, is
not a shoebox of assorted items but a selection and representation of
annotated stories. “Your cinematic deathbed flashback will already be
uploaded to your hard drive,” one of his interviewers concludes.24 In
MyLifeBits, the idea of the computer as a model for the brain is extended
from its storage and retrieval capacities to its presentation capacities. It is
interesting to notice how Microsoft engineers construct the notion of life
as a story, while simultaneously equating life stories with mediated formats.
Personal memories cast in narrative, using images as material signposts, con-
ceptually morph into preformatted media presentations—preferably copy-
righted by Microsoft, of course.
MyLifeBits’s design deftly reflects (and smartly caters to) two con-
temporary anxieties: worrying about managing one’s life and worrying
about amnesia. For an upscale Western audience, managing data has be-
come an attractive metaphor for controlling life. To live an experience at a
date and time of one’s choosing—rather like a television program recorded
on a VCR—takes some pressure off life’s fast pace, regulated by the clock.
From Shoebox to Digital Memory Machine 161
upon through projections and desires. Our ability to create stories of the
past does not depend on our ability to recollect precise facts; on the contrary,
stories create memories. The morphing nature of episodic memory may lead
machines to enhance that quality. According to the reconsolidation theory,
memory is a creative amalgamation of fact and fantasy, and the biggest ad-
vantage of computers is that they may support this essential feature of
episodic memory better than any previous technology. In addition to storing
past recordings, we can use digital machines to transform records into new
stories that better suit our present understanding of memory and reality. In-
deed, although most projects focus on memories as narratives rather than
facts, they still tend to stress the accuracy of retrieved recordings. Terms like
“accurate,” “manipulated,” and “false” memories—in opposition to “authen-
tic” and “true” recollections—govern the discourses of psychologists and
cognitive researchers, and they also prevail in the design strategy of com-
puter engineers. The obdurate modernist belief in authentic versus manipu-
lated memories obviously echoes in MyLifeBits and other projects.
Instead of confirming fallacious binary frameworks, I have tried to
outline how digital tools may help reconceptualize memory as a process
etched in time—a process continuously prone to the vagaries of reinter-
pretation and reordering. If we consider the latest research results coming
from neurobiologists and cognitive psychologists, we have to accept hu-
man memory as an amalgamation of creative projection, factual retrieval,
and narrative recollection of past events. By the same token, we may look
upon media tools as creative reminiscing instruments in addition to them
being mnemonic aids. Defining memory concurrently as a product and a
process, we may acknowledge the versatility and morphing quality of dig-
ital memory machines as a positive element that is integral to human rem-
iniscing. Why not create multiple story lines out of stored documents and
images while still giving recourse to the original records? Why disregard
the creative and transformative potential of memory, as it is such integral
element of our identity as human beings? As earlier chapters show, lifelogs
may help people keep track of their changing personalities, manipulating
digital photographs supports a person’s identity formation, mix-and-burn
software may customize existing sounds to particular moods, and digitized
home movies can offer a reframing of a family’s contrived past.
Indeed, digitization of our entire collection of personal documents
may elicit new cultural forms and habits besides putting photos in a shoebox
164 From Shoebox to Digital Memory Machine
that triggers particular emotions or sensations. For instance, a song may help
invoke a specific mood, whereas a diary is more suitable to inscribe and recall
reflective thoughts. The recording medium once dictated the choice for a
particular sensory inscription, but multimedia computers and digital record-
ing devices expand the choices now that digital cameras combined with soft-
ware packages promote their multiple usage as recorders of sound, text, still
pictures, and moving images. I doubt whether people suddenly want to use
digital apparatuses to exhaustively record sensory experiences, but they may
start experimenting with the synesthetic qualities of the new machines. As
users of the Living Memory Box made clear, parents still want to save a tod-
dler’s crayon drawings in their shoebox, but their digital equipment enables
them to document a child’s cute hairdo, first words, and first steps in all their
respective visual, auditory, and textual dimensions. The multifunctionality
of the digital photo camera unwittingly adds moving images and sound to
the recording repertoire of eager parents.
Capturing multiple dimensions of experience—even if inadvertently—
may shift a person’s propensity to privilege a singular sense in the process of
remembering and even lead to new cultural practices. Chapter 5 noted how
camera phones do not simply alter the preference for still images in the act of
reminiscence by adding text but change social codes and cultural forms of
teenage communication as well. In other words, digital technologies poten-
tially change the way we choose to frame our pasts in new sensory modes;
but more profoundly, those individual preferences inevitably affect the con-
ventions for remembering and communicating. Sound may undergo a re-
vival in the creation of ego-documents now that audio tags and recorded
voices are almost as ubiquitous as image files or text labels. Perhaps personal
digital assistants will be deployed to tape dinner-table discussions, which will
be disseminated afterward as podcasts—the audio equivalent of e-mailed
photographs. We can imagine a large variety of new modes of recording and
storing memories, exploiting the new variability of multimedial equipment.
It is remarkable to observe how computer engineers bet on multimedia com-
puters as instruments of exhaustive recording and retention, while the real
innovation of personal memory machines most likely lies in the combination
of new sensorial mind frames with innovative cultural uses.
Finally, most contemporary memory machines (except for Lifestreams)
understate the importance of connectivity in their designs of storage and
retrieval systems. When Bell introduces MyLifeBits, he states that the
166 From Shoebox to Digital Memory Machine
human mind ought to work like the World Wide Web—the most efficient
and effective navigation environment emerging from the digital age. He
suggests Google, Silicon Valley’s celebrated search engine, as the prime
conceptual model for running and activating the human mind. Yet on
closer inspection, MyLifeBits designers interpret Google as a mere retrieval
tool capable of recovering pieces from a vast but static environment, be-
traying a rather modernist notion of human memory as well as a remark-
ably limited understanding of the capabilities of search engines. In Bell’s
conceptual model, memories are fixed entities patiently awaiting their re-
trieval from the shelves of the mental library, triggered by a coincidental or
conscious thought. Today, these assumptions have been discredited in fa-
vor of memory as a complicated encoding process, where memories are
preserved through and affected by elaborate mental, social, and media
schemata. Specific needs, interests, or desires on the part of the remem-
berer significantly structure the content of memories while at the same
time they define communication and expression. Human memory is a
flexible agency through which identity development, and thus personal
growth, is made possible. Microsoft engineers ignore the inherent trans-
gressive qualities of human memory; they stress the functionality of mem-
ory as a storage machine, at the expense of its creative, communicative,
and connectionist capabilities. Moreover, Bell’s metaphorical equation of
memory to an intelligent software agent (that is, Google) does not do jus-
tice to the transformative power inscribed in this search engine; Google’s
power lies not in its ability to search fixed sets of databases, but in its abil-
ity to navigate a person through a vast repository of mutant items, yielding
different content depending on when and how they are retrieved, reshap-
ing the order of its data upon each usage. Two unique Google qualities—
its navigability and its constantly changing inventory—are conspicuously
absent in MyLifeBits and other projections of future memory machines.
Ignoring the inherent mutability of human memory may seriously
hamper the high ambitions set by Microsoft and other designers. Projects
like Shoebox, the Living Memory Box, and MyLifeBits tend to regard soft-
ware programs as self-contained nostalgia machines—jukeboxes of individ-
ual memory—that perform a mnemonic function: retrieve that particular
song or recall that image. But thanks to the networked computer, memory
becomes more of a topological skill: to navigate personal memory not only
highlights the process of remembering but also allows the user to make
From Shoebox to Digital Memory Machine 167
connections that would have never been discovered without the computer.
For instance, a person may detect patterns in his or her musical preference
or a specific development in the family’s vacation photographs over the
years, particularly if relating personal items to historical trends. Chapter 6
explained how various types of home movies and videos, after being juxta-
posed and connected to publicly available documents via digital media
technology, led to ambiguous interpretations and innovative representa-
tions of a family’s past records. Topology and navigation, in addition to
retrievability, make the memory process a more intriguing effort than ever
before; the networked computer is a performative agent in the act of re-
membering. Reconnection is perfectly compatible with recollection, as in-
dividual memories are always inherently embedded in cultural contexts.
The googlization of memory, in Microsoft terms, is a callow conceptualiza-
tion of what networked modalities may produce; the emergence of new
genres that connect private memories to reflections of others or to public
resources—and thus produce new thoughts—is the true innovative poten-
tial of a digital memory machine.
Contemporary designs for personal memory machines assume tech-
nological innovations to provide a new model for human memory, such
as the computer or the World Wide Web, but they fail to acknowledge the
mutual shaping of human memory and machine. A basic flaw in the dig-
ital machine’s software design is that it models the brain after the com-
puter and assumes the private mind to be searchable like the World Wide
Web is searchable; personal memory is thus considered a repository that is
completely separate from collective memory. Projects like Shoebox and
the Living Memory Box concentrate on the collection of personal mem-
ory items only. Lifestreams and MyLifeBits duly acknowledge the inter-
lacing of personal and public records (or records sent by others), but at
best they address the issue of ubiquitous connectivity in terms of privacy
infringement and personal integrity. However, they fail to recognize the
innovative potential of new digital environments; the World Wide Web
opens up space for new cultural practices fulfilling a social need for con-
necting the self to larger contexts of community, society, and history.
Memory is neither exclusively cemented in the recall of individual experi-
ences nor in the remembrance of collective experiences, but a human be-
ing has a vested interest in connecting both poles if he or she wants to
pursue personal growth.
168 From Shoebox to Digital Memory Machine
his everyday world and to find a new way to inscribe his personal life in di-
mensions of time and order.
The performative nature of memory is, I believe, much under-
exposed in current research on memory machines. Memories are narratives
as well as artifacts, performances as well as objects—things that work in
every day lives and cultures of people. In their search for the perfect digi-
tal memory machine, engineers and entrepreneurs have systematically fo-
cused on the products of memory and ignored the role of technologies in
the active staging of memory. They assume mind and machine to be sepa-
rate entities, one serving as the model for the other, generally disregarding
their perpetual mutual encroachment. Computer engineers, as designers
of digital tools, could profit from neuroscientific research to obtain in-
sights in how autobiographical memory works and evolves; they could also
benefit from the wealth of ethnographic inquiries into the individual’s use
of both material and digital memories in the process of collecting, storing,
and retrieving.27 Working at the crossroads of various disciplinary perspec-
tives, we may try to find out how digital materiality impinges on our
everyday habits of preserving and presenting personal heritages. Digital
technologies seem to promote a different materiality that both complements
and partially replaces analog objects embodying memory; most important,
they shape the very nature of remembering as they become (literally) incor-
porated in our daily routines of self-formation. In spite of current project
designers’ projections, the ultimate goal of memory is not to end up as a
Powerpoint presentation on your grandchild’s desktop; the ultimate goal
of memory is to make sense of one’s life.
8
Epilogue
I must have been around twenty years old, when I saw Francis Pi-
cabia’s painting The Acrobat in a Scandinavian museum—I cannot remem-
ber whether it was Oslo or Stockholm. What struck me in the painting
were the thick brush strokes accentuating the unnatural upside-down pos-
ture of the acrobat. The poster I bought of this picture unfortunately at-
tenuated the brush strokes, but it nevertheless covered the wall of my
student room for some time. More than ten years later, while browsing in a
bookstore in Los Angeles, I hit upon a reproduction of Picabia’s painting in
an art book. I only remember that particular browsing experience—as dis-
tinct from hundreds of hours of browsing in American bookstores—because
of the reproduction. Not until recently, almost twenty-five years after first
seeing the painting, did these two moments come back to me via an old
photograph of myself on a couch in my student room, in which the poster
on the wall figures as background. The friend who had taken the picture
had kept it in her shoebox for many years; when she found the photo-
graph, she scanned it and e-mailed it to me. I looked on the Internet for a
clean reproduction of my beloved acrobat and reworked both images into
a humorous story about my peculiar run-ins with this painting, which I
then e-mailed back to my friend.
Mediated memories, as proposed in the first chapter, are the activi-
ties and objects we produce and appropriate by means of media technolo-
gies for creating and re-creating a sense of our past, present, and future
Epilogue 171
always been a playground for sorting and appropriating cultural items. Dig-
ital technologies empower consumers of culture, arming them with ad-
vanced means to construct collective identities. Taking photographs and
buying posters are both acts of appropriation: by defining their idiosyn-
cratic choices in the sea of cultural products, individuals mark their pref-
erences but also communicate a sense of belonging to a (peer) group or a
(sub)cultural league. Chapter 4 described how digital music files lend
themselves to easy exchange of pop songs and thus contribute to the for-
mation of one’s personal taste in relation to collective musical heritage. In
more than one way, the distinction between individual and collective mem-
ory evaporates as individuals obtain and wield the (digital) instruments that
lie at the heart of cultural production. Home movies inspire television se-
ries as much as feature films inspire home video productions (Chapter 6).
In short, personal and collective cultural products are almost seamlessly in-
terwoven into the symbolic fabric of everyday life.
Memories also mediate between what is private and what is public,
concurrently modifying the meanings of privacy and publicness. The de-
velopment of every new medium—beginning with print—has reconsti-
tuted the boundaries between public and private life. Dissemination of
personal memory is increasingly an online activity: from the creation of
lifelogs to the distribution of virtual images, the already thin line between
private and public has only become more diffuse. Personal cultural mem-
ory, of course, was never squarely located within the private realm, because
individuals made conscious decisions to publish their shoebox contents.
That private shoebox is gradually integrated in a global, digital bazaar of
documents, music, and pictures, where files appear almost indistinguish-
able. So when I put my picture of the Picabia poster on the Internet, cut-
ting and pasting freely from digitized museum reproductions, I should not
be surprised by claims of copyright infringement. The opposite is also true:
a specific entry in your lifelog or a picture sent by a camera phone (Chapter 5)
may later show up in public contexts as a result of having shared it via a
public forum, even if unwittingly. Personal and collective cultural items
thus become progressively interwoven in memory discourses.
The vertical axis in the proposed model signifies how memories
mediate between past and future. One of autobiographical memory’s
most important functions is that of self-continuity: in spite of the body
being a constant physical transmutation of cellular tissue, appearance, and
Epilogue 173
discover the creative potential of hardware and software into their new ma-
chines. It will be interesting to experiment with the multimodal potential
of the new technologies in terms of memory enhancement.
Another major omission in the design of these digital memory ma-
chines is their remarkable indifference to contextual use; personal memory
is always embedded in sociocultural forms and practices, which are partly
responsible for its shaping. When we store a picture for later retrieval, this
act is part of a social custom to save and store photographs for later remi-
niscence; the photo album or shoebox is the conventional cultural form in
which we preserve memories. The photograph of my student room featur-
ing the poster was preserved in somebody else’s shoebox, but was recol-
lected because of a new connective custom: to electronically distribute
pictures to friends—a social practice grounded in the networked condition
of connectivity. It may be safe to assume that the current transformation
of technology in conjunction with sociocultural forms and practices poses
a challenge to the human mind and how it selects, understands, and re-
members information about the self and relates this information to others
and to culture at large. A camera phone, as elucidated in Chapter 5, liter-
ally merges the functions of memory and communication; the resulting
image-text is neither an exclusive memory object nor an ephemeral by-
product of a casual conversation. New social practices and cultural forms
emerge at the crossroads of memory and media, and even if we cannot yet
label them, it is important to mark their appearance. It is too early to tell
how cultural forms will develop further into the digital age, but there is no
denying that the functions of memory, communication, and identity for-
mation are becoming evermore intertwined.
Recognizing the connection between memories’ embodiment, their
enabling technologies, and their embedding in social practices and cultural
forms motivates my call for an interdisciplinary approach to memory. Rather
than considering technology the engine of a multifaceted transformation,
this book opted for a more complex analysis of personal cultural memory,
one that advocates neither the teleology of change nor a compartmentalized
patchwork of separate evolutions. Instead, I show how results from neurobi-
ological and cognitive research may complement social-constructivist para-
digms concerning media technologies and how software designers may
profit from insights in sociocultural patterns of media use. By defining mem-
ory as a phenomenon that needs biological-cognitive, material-technological,
Epilogue 177
products resulting from this collaboration are the designs of new virtual
environments that serve as simulations of real experiences.3 Multimodal
3-D environments are currently tested in the field of psychotherapy, for in-
stance to treat phobic disorders, or in courtroom settings to reconstruct ac-
tual crime scenes.4 In both cases, digital apparatuses help human memory
to perform. The vulnerability of autobiographical memory to guided
imagination in addition to the capabilities of new digital media to paint
memories in digital multimedial productions may necessitate new stan-
dards for litigation or successful therapy. Will we accept a multimedia re-
construction in the courtroom, for instance a graphic 3-D reconstruction
of a crime scene or the scene of an accident, knowing that a multimedial
representation also imposes and induces a certain version of what hap-
pened? How is human recollection affected by multimedial input? How do
cultural forms, such as televised crime-scene investigations, influence con-
crete personal memories of criminal scenes? How do computer games fea-
turing virtual chases and enemy shoot-outs affect (or interfere with) the
treatment of post-traumatic stress disorders?
If morphibility and connectivity are becoming the default mode of
mediated memories in the digital age, we need to adjust our research ques-
tions accordingly. The ontology of memory, like the ontology of media,
used to be firmly rooted in metaphors such as the archive or the repository.
Memory was considered something we have or lack, retain or lose, but it
was never a state of becoming. In conjunction with this metaphor, we speak
of retrieving or recalling past events, implying that the thing to be recalled
is already there waiting to be retrieved. But with the implementation of dig-
ital media tools in the everyday construction of memory, our very concept
of how memory functions is technically and metaphorically grounded in
different parameters. Does this mean we should now define autobiographi-
cal memory by its “track changes” mode in addition to its “save file” mode?
Acknowledging the track changes mode as a valid mindset may boost the
appreciation of new cultural practices, such as the lifelog and the photo-
blog, which favor the dynamics of recording over the desire to freeze the
past. Adjunct to its morphibility, the networked lifelog emphasizes mem-
ory’s connective function in addition to its commemorative use. A collec-
tion of digitized personal files stands in perpetual dialogue with the infinite
basin of files on the Internet—a repository that is by definition dynamic.
Instead of retrieving memories, we may now speak of navigating memory
180 Epilogue
at the same time, I have argued how the morphing of memory is part of a
more general cultural condition—a tendency to control and stimulate the
active shaping of identity. People identify themselves and the way they
look by studying the looks of ancestors, relatives, and peers, creating con-
tinuity by discovering roots. Media technologies are a means to help us
discover and create, reconstruct and re-create a sense of self in the context
of historical continuities.
Trauma is another term monopolized by psychiatrists and psycholo-
gists but inevitably appropriated by historians and cultural theorists to the-
orize the propensities of human memory. Psychologists, for instance, are
interested in how the experience of war or violence in a child’s past may
affect that person’s present and future state of mind, whereas historians
transfer the term trauma to study collective wounds caused by war or vio-
lence and how we cope with them in revising our histories. The proposed
connectionist model encourages the study of trauma as a mediated experi-
ence: the Afghan refugee’s angst or Somalian war child’s trauma are both
mediated by the (lack of) personal photographs showing a happy family
and by the news photographs documenting scenes of continuous warmon-
gering. The exposure to—but also the manipulation of—public photo-
graphs with the help of media technologies creates building blocks in the
construction of personal identity and cultural heritage. By the same token,
the (manipulability of) pictures and personal narratives may be a creative
tool in the process of healing and coming to terms with traumatic memo-
ries. “Connectivity” and “communicating” may be new key words in the
definition of personal cultural memory, terms that remind us of the rele-
vance of memory to our well-being as humans.
When researchers face new phenomena and thus new challenges,
they tend to look for common ground that may help them test proven hy-
potheses against new paradigms. In the future, memory research may in-
clude horizons that currently escape a scientist’s immediate disciplinary
scope. That is why I never attempted to define what memory is but instead
focused on its mediatedness. In designing an expansive model for memory
research, I worked from the assumption that memory always serves a pur-
pose and that it works toward a certain goal: we remember because we
want to make meaning out of life. Memory makes meaning by mediating
between disparate abstract and concrete entities: the self and the world, the
mortal individual and the immortal collective, the family’s past and the
182 Epilogue
chapter 1
1. Although there are some subtle differences between the words “personal”
and “autobiographical” in connection to memory, most psychologists and neuro-
scientists use them interchangeably. For stylistic reasons I alternate between the
terms “autobiographical” and “personal,” yet I strongly prefer the term “personal”
in connection to cultural memory, for reasons to be explained later in this chapter,
toward the end of the second section.
2. For more information on the development of the autobiographical self (in
relation to the biological self) see Antonio Damasio, The Feeling of What Happens:
Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness (Orlando, FL: Harcourt, 1999),
222–33. I further elaborate on this theory in Chapters 2 and 4.
3. Susan Bluck, “Autobiographical Memory: Exploring Its Functions in Every-
day Life,” Memory 11, no. 2 (2003): 113–23.
4. Katherine Nelson, “Narrative and Self, Myth and Memory: Emergence of
the Cultural Self,” in Autobiographical Memory and the Construction of a Narrative
Self: Developmental and Cultural Perspectives, ed. Robyn Fivush and Catherine
Haden (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 2003), 3–25. In this article, Nelson lists
the different levels of self-understanding psychologists have long recognized: in
addition to a cognitive and social sense of self, experts have distinguished repre-
sentational and narrative levels of understanding. See also Katherine Nelson, “Self
and Social Functions: Individual Autobiographical Memory and Collective Nar-
rative,” Memory 11, no. 2 (2003): 125–36.
5. Nelson, “Narrative and Self,” 7.
6. Nelson, “Self and Social Functions,” 127.
7. See Qi Wang and Jens Brockmeier, “Autobiographical Remembering as
Cultural Practice: Understanding the Interplay between Memory, Self and Cul-
ture,” Culture and Psychology 8, no. 1 (2002): 45–64, 45.
8. Cultural differences of remembering between parents and children of Chinese
and American descent have been studied by Wang and Brockmeier, “Autobiograph-
ical Remembering as Cultural Practice,” 54–60. Differences between American and
184 Notes to Chapter 1
36. This double take is not unlike the modernist tendency, observed by
Bruno Latour in We Have Never Been Modern (Cambridge: Harvard University
Press, 1993), to simultaneously insist on hybridity and purification—holding on
to the ontological division between human and nonhumans (things, machines)
while also canceling out their separation. The invincibility of these arguments is
possible only because they hold on to the absolute dichotomy between the order
of Nature and that of Society, a dichotomy that “is itself only possible because
they never consider the work of purification and that of mediation together”
(40). On the theory of technology and social change, see Latour’s Aramis, or the
Love of Technology (London: Harvard University Press, 1996).
37. Kuhn, “A Journey through Memory,” 183.
38. Steven Rose, The Making of Memory (London: Bantam Press, 1992),
91–96.
39. Richard Chalfen, “Snapshots ‘R’ Us: The Evidentiary Problematic of
Home Media,” Visual Studies 17, no. 2 (2002): 141–49, 144.
40. John Urry, “How Societies Remember the Past,” in Theorizing Muse-
ums: Representing Identity and Diversity in a Changing World, ed. S. Macdonald
and G. Fyfe (Oxford: Blackwell, 1996), 45–68.
41. George Lipsitz, Time Passages: Collective Memory and American Popular
Culture (Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 1990), 5.
42. For a very interesting historical exploration of the notion of time in indi-
vidual remembering, see the work of Douwe Draaisma, particularly his book
Waarom de tijd sneller gaat als je ouder wordt (Groningen: Historische Uitgeverij,
2001). For a specific study on the (historical) use of metaphors in relation to mem-
ory, see Draaisma, Metaphors of Memory: A History of Ideas about the Mind (Cam-
bridge: Cambridge University Press, 2000).
43. I am referring here to George Lakoff and Mark Johnson’s renowned the-
ory of metaphors as described in Metaphors We Live By (Chicago: Chicago Uni-
versity Press, 1980).
44. John B. Thompson, The Media and Modernity: A Social Theory of the Me-
dia (Cambridge: Polity Press, 1995).
45. Thompson, in The Media and Modernity, is obviously concerned with the
double-bind that constitutes the process of self-formation in modernity—where
the self is caught between an increasing dependency on media and an increasing
need for self-reflexivity to define itself as part of a larger world (233).
46. The obvious theoretical link to Gilles Deleuze’s theory on the relation be-
tween time, movement, and image comes to mind. I extensively elaborate on this
theory in Chapter 6.
47. Besides sight and sound, other sensory perceptions, such as smell or touch,
form a trigger for later recall. Few contemporary theorists have stressed the role of
the senses in cultural memory. See, for instance, C. Nadia Seremetakis, ed., The
188 Notes to Chapter 2
chapter 2
1. Charlie Kaufman, Eternal Sunshine of the Spotless Mind, directed by Michel
Gondry (Universal City, CA: Focus Features, 2004).
2. The movie’s producers have created a mock website of the company Lacuna
Inc., available at: http://www.lacunainc.com (accessed December 23, 2006).
3. See John Sutton, Philosophy and Memory Traces: Descartes to Connectionism
(Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1998).
4. For an eminent and thorough historical analysis of metaphors of memory,
see Douwe Draaisma, Metaphors of Memory: A History of Ideas about the Mind
(Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2000). The library and archive as meta-
phors are described in chapter 2, “Memoria: Memory as Writing.”
5. See Henri Bergson, Matter and Memory (London: George Allen and Unwin,
1911). Originally published in 1896. The original manuscript of Matter and Mem-
ory was published in 1896.
6. Bergson, Matter and Memory, 197.
7. Of course this view is widely disputed as the age-old distinction of mind
and brain remains a hotly contested issue. For a neuroscientific take on this issue,
see Antonio Damasio, Looking for Spinoza: Joy, Sorrow, and the Feeling Brain (Or-
lando, FL: Harcourt, 2003), chapter 5.
Notes to Chapter 2 189
17. Strictly speaking, the destruction of physical objects patients bring into
Lacuna’s office should be redundant, because the internal, mental pictures from
which the unpleasant memories derive no longer constitute the link between the
brain and the feeling or emotion proper.
18. Don Slater, quoted by Deborah Chambers in Representing the Family (Lon-
don: Sage, 2001), refers to the results of a market research survey in which 39 per-
cent of respondents claimed their family photos to be their most treasured
possessions (82).
19. Walter Benjamin, One-Way Street and Other Writings (London: Verso, 1979).
On Benjamin’s writings on memory objects, see also Esther Leslie, “Souvenirs and
Forgetting: Walter Benjamin’s Memory-Work,” in Material Memories, ed. Marius
Kwint, Christopher Breward, and Jeremy Aynsley (Oxford: Berg, 1999), 107–23.
20. Belinda Barnet, “The Erasure of Technology in Cultural Critique,” Fi-
breculture 1 (2003), http://journal.fibreculture.org/ (accessed December 23, 2006).
21. John Sutton, “Porous Memory and the Cognitive Life of Things,” in Pre-
figuring Cyberculture: An Intellectual History, ed. Darren Tofts, Annemarie Jonson,
and Alessio Cavallara (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2002), 130–41, 138.
22. Clark, Mindware, 141.
23. L. H. Martin, Technologies of the Self: A Seminar with Michel Foucault
(London: Tavistock, 1988), 16.
24. My notion of sociocultural practices finds a middle ground between what
sociologist Pierre Bourdieu, in Outline of a Theory of Practice (Cambridge: Cam-
bridge University Press, 1977), refers to as “habitus” and what philosopher Michel
de Certeau, in The Practice of Everyday Life (Berkeley: University of California
Press, 1984), rearticulates as “the practice of everyday life.” Bourdieu’s “habitus” is
associated with the internalized, practical knowledge by which people operate in
stable, social structures and situations; De Certeau uses the term “practice” to em-
phasize the dynamics of people evolving in social structures, changing them and
adapting to new ones. When I use the term “sociocultural practices,” I am refer-
ring to both static structures and dynamic changes. However, I am much more
specific in my denotation of the word “practice,” referring to a set of practical,
technical, social, and cultural skills needed to operate the “technologies of self”
Foucault identifies. These sociocultural practices are grounded both in materiality
and technology (in this case media technologies) as well as in the knowledge of
their practical use (e.g., social norms and discourses).
25. Hartmut Winkler, a German media scholar, presents a theory of cultural
continuity by explaining the translation of certain cultural practices into “deposits”
(defined by technology and its use) that turn back into practices. Through constant
reinterpretation and reshaping of practices and objects, the continuity of culture is
secured, even if constantly morphing. See “Discourses, Schemata, Technology,
Monuments: Outline for a Theory of Cultural continuity,” Configurations, 10
(2002): 91–109.
Notes to Chapter 2 191
Chicago Press, 1999), rightly criticizes theorists such as Hans Moravec and Ray
Kurzweil whose affection for “disembodied minds” and “virtual brains” seems to
dispose of the body as a locus for mental activity. “Posthumanists,” as Hayles calls
them, are blind to the materiality of informatics and indifferent to the embodi-
ment of digital media. The idea of human memory being digitized and transposed
to a locus outside the brain is an immensely popular trope in the twenty-first cen-
tury, informing both visionary science projects and science fiction movies like
Brain Destroyer and Fantastic Voyage II: Destination Brain.
34. Eugene Thacker, “What is Biomedia?” Configurations 11 (2003): 47–79.
35. Ibid., 76–77.
36. Genomics is a case in point: the computations of genetic sequences are car-
ried out by computers, and thus digital information becomes an impetus for re-
dressing our knowledge of genetic defects. For a detailed explanation of how
genomics and information interact, see José van Dijck, ImagEnation: Popular Im-
ages of Genetics (New York: New York University Press, 1998), chapter 6.
37. For an extensive analysis of how ultrasound imaging not just works to re-
configure our conceptualization of the fetus, but also affects pregnancy and the
development of the fetus, see Van Dijck, “Ultrasound and the Visible Fetus,” in
The Transparent Body, 100–117.
38. There is a hilarious scene in Eternal Sunshine where Dr. Mierzwiak asks Joel
to unleash his painful memories of Clementine by talking about her into the micro-
phone of an old-fashioned cassette recorder. Later in the movie, Joel and Clemen-
tine are confronted with their embarrassing monologues when the magnetic tapes
with their voices are returned to them through a disgruntled, revengeful secretary
after she has discovered the “erased” love affair with her boss, Dr. Mierzwiak.
39. Gregory Ulmer, for instance, treats (digital) memory as a reservoir for cre-
ative invention and intervention—new media technologies allowing the reorder-
ing and reshaping of digital imprints of the past, whether pictures, sounds, or
texts. See Gregory Ulmer, Heuretics: The Logic of Invention (Baltimore, MD:
Johns Hopkins University Press, 1994).
40. A concise and insightful article in the New York Times provides an
overview of the many problems involved in storing, preserving, and retrieving
digital memory files for the next generation. See Katie Hafner, “Even Digital
Memories Can Fade,” New York Times, November 10, 2004 (online edition,
www.nytimes.com).
41. See Jay Bolter and Richard Grusin, Remediation: Understanding New Me-
dia (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1999).
chapter 3
1. Susan Herring, an American sociologist from Indiana University specializing
in computer-mediated communication, in a 2005 presentation, quotes the number
Notes to Chapter 3 193
of weblog users from the statistics of the Perseus group at 4.12 million. This num-
ber of bloggers also includes hosted weblog services; 34 percent of these logs are
used actively. Herring, “Weblog as Genre, Weblog as Sociability,” available at:
http://ella.slis.indiana.edu/~herring/ssc.ppt (accessed April 19, 2006).
2. Initially, blogs were either personal homepages operated by individuals,
mostly people who were interested in sharing technical and personal knowledge,
or they were websites consisting of chronological lists of links, interspersed with
information and editorialized and personal asides. For a description of the early
development of weblogs, see, for instance, We’ve Got Blog: How Weblogs Are
Changing Our Culture, ed. John Rodzvilla (Cambridge, MA: Perseus Publishers,
2002); see also Charles Cheung, “A Home on the Web: Presentations of Self on
Personal Homepages,” in Web Studies: Rewiring Media Studies for the Digital Age,
ed. David Gauntlett (London: Arnold, 2000), 43–51.
3. Researchers of the blogosphere distinguish lifelogs from linklogs; linklog-
gers primarily post links to other websites, whereas lifeloggers primarily post de-
tails about their personal lives and everyday experiences. See Frank Schaap,
“Links, Lives, Logs: Presentation in the Dutch Blogosphere,” in Into the Blogo-
shphere: Rhetoric, Community and Culture of Weblogs, ed. Laura Gurak, Smiljana
Antonijevio, Laurie Johnson, Clanoy Ratliff, and Jessica Reyman, (Minneapolis:
University of Minnesota, 2004), http://blog.lib.umn.edu/blogosphere/ (accessed
April 18, 2006).
4. German media theorist Andreas Kitzmann, in his article “That Different
Place: Documenting the Self within Online Environments,” Biography 26, no. 1
(Winter 2003): 48–65, proposes to study media change in the context of the much
wider phenomenon of “material complexification” to understand the continuities
and changes between old and new media such as diaries and weblogs. He argues
that change is not cumulative “but [measured by] structural shifts that may lead
to growth, contraction, stasis, or a combination of all three” (51).
5. For an extensive elaboration of this theory, see Antonio Damasio, The Feel-
ing of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness (Orlando,
FL: Harcourt, 1999), 308, chapter 2.
6. Silvan Tomkins, Affect, Imagery, Consciousness (New York: Springer, 1962),
quoted by Anna Gibbs in “Contagious Feelings: Pauline Hanson and the Epidemi-
ology of Affect,” Australian Humanities Review 24 (2001). Available at: http://www
.lib.latrobe.edu.au/AHR/archive/Issue-December-2001/gibbs.html (accessed April
18, 2006).
7. See Gibbs, “Contagious Feelings.”
8. The diary genre has been defined as therapy or self-help, as a means of con-
fession, as a chronicle of adventurous journeys (both spiritual and physical), and
as a scrapbook for creative endeavors. Thomas Mallon, author of the standard
work, A Book of One’s Own: People and Their Diaries (New York: Ticknor and
Fields, 1984), distinguishes at least seven types of diaries and labels the various
194 Notes to Chapter 3
14. Chip Gerber, “My Journey,” March 2005, part 2, available at: http://
www.zarcrom.com/users/alzheimers/chip.html (accessed April 18, 2006).
15. See Mary Lockhart’s blog, available at: http://www.angelfire.com/ok4/
mari5113/index.html (accessed April 18, 2006).
16. Robert Payne, “Digital Memories, Analogues of Affect,” Scan: Journal of
Media Arts Culture 2 (2004), http://scan.net.au/scan/journal/display.php?journal
_id=42 (accessed April 18, 2006).
17. The term “community of records” is used by Eric Ketelaar in the context
of collective archiving. See Eric Ketelaar, “Sharing: Collected Memories in Com-
munities of Records,” Archives and Manuscripts 33 (2005): 44–61.
18. Gerber, “My Journey,” March 2005, part 3.
19. See Friedell’s weblog, available at: http://members.aol.com/MorrisFF?.
20. For a detailed description of the authenticity debate concerning Anne
Frank’s manuscript in contrast to the fabricated Adolf Hitler diaries that were dis-
covered in the 1980s, see Sonja Neef: “Authentic Events: The Diaries of Anne
Frank and the Alleged Diaries of Adolf Hitler,” in Sign Here! Handwriting in the
Age of Technological Reproduction, ed. Sonja Neef, José van Dijck, and Eric Kete-
laar (Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press, 2006), 23–50.
21. As Canadian archivist Jane Zhang claims in “The Lingering of Handwritten
Records,” Proceedings of I-Chora Conference (International Conference on the His-
tory of Records and Archives, University of Toronto, October 2–4, 2003), 38–45, an
individual’s handwriting is habitually viewed as “his own personal mark, which dis-
tinguishes him not only from others, but also from his own past and future” (43).
22. Sonja Neef, “Die (rechte) Schrift und die (linke) Hand,” Kodikas/Ars
Semiotica 25, no. 1 (2002): 159–76.
23. Jacques Derrida, Archive Fever: A Freudian Impression (Chicago: University
of Chicago Press, 1995).
24. Friedrich Kittler, in his famed Film, Gramophone, Typewriter (Stanford,
CA: Stanford University Press, 1999), states that the typewriter disrupted the inti-
macy of handwritten expression, as it “tears writing from the essential realm of
the hand, i.e., the realm of the word” (198). It should be noted, though, that this
idea does not originally stem from Kittler; he is referring to Martin Heidegger’s
Parmenides lecture.
25. For an intricate philosophical explanation of how technology and body are
intertwined in the digital act of writing, see Mark Hansen, Embodying Technesis:
Technology beyond Writing (Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press, 2000).
26. Emily Nussbaum, in “My So-Called Blog,” New York Times Magazine,
January 11, 2004, online version at (www.nytimes.com), states: “A LiveJournal is a
Blurty is a Xanga is a DiaryLand.”
27. However, there is another side to this techno-cultural transformation that of-
ten is underemphasized. The culture of reciprocation is not solely based on linking the
self to the Internet, but it is also based on linking the Internet to the self. Tracing
196 Notes to Chapter 3
cultural or political preferences of other bloggers, one can decide to connect to people
with similar tastes and preferences; it is precisely this technological feature that makes
weblogs interesting for marketers. With the use of fairly simple software applications
like AllConsuming.net, it becomes increasingly easy to find correlations between blog-
gers and the cultural products they mention via links or sidebars: books, music, televi-
sion programs, movies, and so forth. On the interlinking of weblogs for commercial
purposes, see Erik Benson, “All Consuming Web Services,” O’Reilly Webservices,
May 7, 2003, http://www.xml.com/pub/a/ws/2003/05/27/allconsuming.html (ac-
cessed April 18, 2006).
28. Tracking software allows a glimpse of the patterns and trends that emerge
out of the topics shared by a group. Coupled with vast databases like those of Ama-
zon and Google, the possibilities for polling and marketing research are endless, ex-
plaining Google’s eagerness to buy start-up companies like Blogger. Google bought
Pyra labs, one of the first start-up companies that designed blogger software, in
2003. For details on this transaction, see Leander Kahney, “Why Did Google Want
Blogger?” Wired News, February 22, 2003, http://www.wired.com/news/technology.
29. Mallon, A Book of One’s Own, xvi.
30. On the importance of the addressee in diaries, see Marty, L’ecriture du
jour, 87.
31. William M. Decker, in Epistolary Practices: Letter Writing in America before
Telecommunications (Raleigh: University of North Carolina Press, 1998), theorizes
the evolution of epistolary writing in the United States; he observes that letters,
much like diaries, carry the aura of a private genre, whereas the diary genre en-
codes itself according to public standards: “What we identify as the private life is
a conventionalized and hence public construction” (6).
32. Elizabeth Yakel, in “Reading, Reporting, and Remembering: A Case Study
of the Maryknoll Sisters Diaries,” Proceedings of I-Chora Conference (International
Conference on the History of Records and Archives, University of Toronto, Oc-
tober 2–4, 2003), 142–50, describes an intriguing account of how the Maryknoll
Sisters, a religious community active between 1912 and 1967, adapted the genre as
a collective means of expression to record and exchange spiritual and intellectual
journeys to each other. Their record-keeping practices suited various goals, from
expressing individual beliefs to communicating information across time and space
with like-minded congregations. As Yakel phrases it: “The diaries had multiple
audiences—they were a means of internal communication within the community
and also served as a mechanism for external communication to Catholics and oth-
ers interested in their mission activities” (143).
33. Michael Piggott, archivist at the University of Melbourne, discovered
Australian archives to contain many such collective ego-documents, chronicling
important episodes from the sixteenth to the nineteenth centuries through the
eyes of transient groups. See Michael Piggott, “Towards a History of the Aus-
tralian Diary,” Proceedings of I-Chora Conference (International Conference on
Notes to Chapter 3 197
in Into the Blogoshphere: Rhetoric, Community and Culture of Weblogs, ed. Laura
Gurak, Smiljana Antonijevio, Laurie Johnson, Clanoy Ratliff, and Jessica Reyman
(Minneapolis: University of Minnesota, 2004). Available at: http://blog.lib.umn
.edu/blogosphere/ (accessed April 18, 2006).
43. In the case of phone conversations and text messaging, Taylor and Harper,
in “The Gift of the Gab?” found that some teenagers express the wish to store
each SMS exchange on a memory card in order to recall the experience later: the
message’s physical properties (form, content, and time and date stamp) all work in
combination to instill meaning onto the physical object.
44. According to Viegas’s “Blog Survey,” almost 75 percent of all bloggers in-
deed edit their past entries, with changes varying from punctuation and grammar
to contents and names.
45. Nussbaum notes in “My So-Called Blog” that bloggers have a “degraded
or relaxed sense of privacy,” depending on your perspective: “Their experiences
may be personal, but there is no shame in sharing . . . [and they get back] a new
kind of intimacy, a sense that they are known and listened to.”
46. Not surprisingly, more than one-third of all bloggers have gotten into
trouble because of things they have written in their blogs and the majority forgets
about defamation or liability when writing about others in networked environ-
ments. As the aforementioned MIT Media Lab Survey by Viegas shows, bloggers
are hardly concerned with the persistent nature of what they publish; the over-
whelming majority of them publish private information about themselves or
other people without thinking about legal or moral consequences.
chapter 4
1. There have been a number of psychological and cognitive studies of the
connection between emotion and individual meanings attached to music, and sev-
eral of the more important ones are surveyed in the course of this chapter. One of
the oldest and often cited studies in this respect is Leonard B. Meyer, Emotion and
Meaning in Music (Chicago: Chicago University Press, 1961).
2. The list of studies on popular music and cultural memory and identity is re-
markably long. Most studies on collective memory and identity cover recorded mu-
sic as part of popular culture. See, for instance, George Lipsitz, Time Passages:
Collective Memory and American Popular Culture (Minneapolis: University of
Minnesota Press, 1990). For an extensive bibliography and an overview of academic
work on popular music and collective identity, see John Connell and Chris Gibson,
Sound Tracks: Popular Music, Identity, and Place (New York: Routledge, 2003).
3. This chapter concentrates on popular (rather than classical or experimental-
artistic) music and its affective commitment to memory, because pop music is
probably more conducive to the kind of mental mapping and narrative recall fun-
damental to the argument developed here. It is beyond the scope of this chapter to
Notes to Chapter 4 199
theorize how these processes would extend to other kinds of music, which may
create a different connection to identity and memory.
4. See Jeffrey Prager, Presenting the Past: Psychoanalysis and the Sociology of
Misremembering (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1998), 215.
5. Timothy Taylor, in Strange Sounds: Music, Technology and Culture (New
York: Routledge, 2001), puts more emphasis on the sociotechnical systems from
which recorded music emanates and how this becomes part of its history and col-
lective memory. See also David Morton, Off the Record: The Technology and Cul-
ture of Sound Recording in America (New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University
Press, 2000). Morton defines recording culture as follows: “The recording of mu-
sic is an activity that combines a very old form of cultures, the performance of
music, with a variety of technological processes to create a new form of cul-
ture. . . . What we are concerned with here is not only music captured on record
as an example of mass-produced culture but also recording as a cultural process;
not only the meaning of the content of a record, but the meaning of the practices
which developed around the act of recording” (13).
6. Started as a onetime millennium event in 1999, the Dutch national public
radio station (Radio 2) invited listeners to send in their personal top-five favorite
songs of all time, resulting in a collective Top 2000. (Available at: http://top2000
.radio2.nl/2005/site/page/homepage.) The response to this event was so over-
whelming that the station decided to repeat it the next year, and a tradition began.
In December 2004 and 2005, the national Top 2000 was selected by well over one
million Dutch citizens who sent in their personal top-five songs. The number of
public participants is unprecedented in the history of mediated events in The
Netherlands. In 2004, almost 6.5 million people listened to the radio broadcast,
5 million people watched the accompanying daily television shows, and the web-
site registered 9.2 million page views in just five days. Cast against a population of
16 million, the event engaged more than half of all Dutch people twelve years and
older. The comments used in this chapter are derived from the 2004 database; this
database is no longer publicly available but is archived by Radio 2. Comments
were originally in Dutch; they were translated by me and I have identified the re-
spondents in the same way they identified themselves on the (public) website. I
thank Kees Toering, station manager and initiator of the Top 2000 for making all
statistics and archives available to me.
7. Music turns out to be an important contextual element in human recall; clin-
ical research shows that recall is optimal when people hear the same music during
the experiencing and recalling of events. See W. R. Balch and B. S. Lewis, “Threads
of Music in the Tapestry of Memory,” Memory and Cognition 21 (1996): 21–8.
8. Thomas Turino, in “Signs of Imagination, Identity, and Experience: A
Peircian Semiotic Theory for Music,” Ethnomusicology 43, no. 2 (1999): 221–55, ar-
gues that identities are at once individual and social and that music is a key re-
source for realizing personal and collective identities at the same time.
200 Notes to Chapter 4
9. Patrick Colm Hogan, Cognitive Science, Literature, and the Arts (New York:
Routledge, 2003), 14.
10. Friedrich Kittler, in Gramophone, Film, Typewriter (Stanford, CA: Stanford
University Press, 1999) describes the historical tendency to regard the gramophone
as an instrument of repetition and faithfulness; Freud took this very literally by ex-
plaining how “the unconscious coincides with electric oscillations” (89).
11. Lisa Gitelman, “How Users Define New Media: A History of the Amuse-
ment Phonograph,” in Rethinking Media Change: The Aesthetics of Transition, ed.
David Thorburn and Henry Jenkins (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 61–80, 65.
12. American neuroscientist Rusiko Bourtchouladze argues in Memories Are
Made of This: How Memory Works in Humans and Animals (New York: Columbia
University Press, 2002) that all memories start as episodic, but only unique expe-
riences survive as time goes by. “Those that do not have freshness and characteris-
tic flavor tend to go downhill with time” (28).
13. For a detailed explanation of this argument, see H. Baumgartner, “Re-
membrances of Things Past: Music, Autobiographical Memory, and Emotion,”
Advances in Consumer Research 19 (1992): 613–20.
14. I have tried to paraphrase the points Antonio Damasio makes about emo-
tions and feelings with regard to memory in The Feeling of What Happens: Body
and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness (Orlando, FL: Harcourt, 1999),
183–94.
15. See Damasio, The Feeling of What Happens, 188.
16. See Turino, “Signs of Imagination, Identity, and Experience,” 224.
17. See also Simon Frith, who, in Performing Rites: On the Value of Popular
Music (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1996), observes that music, especially
during adolescence and teenage years, tends to be retained in connection to in-
tense personal experiences; particular recordings are often considered “our songs”
by a group or collective focused on identity building and enhancement.
18. See Damasio, The Feeling of What Happens, 123–24.
19. A few cognitive psychological studies have shown how older adults’ mem-
ory grows more positive over the years. As Quinn Kennedy, Mara Mather, and
Laura Carstensen show in “The Role of Motivations in the Age–Related Positivity
Effect in Autobiographical Memory,” Psychological Science 15, no. 3 (1994):
208–14, older adults are more motivated than younger adults to remember their
past in emotionally satisfying ways, and older adults’ positive bias in reconstruc-
tive memory reflects their motivation to regulate emotional experience.
20. Geoffrey O’Brien, Sonata for Jukebox: Pop Music, Memory, and the Imag-
ined Life (New York: Counterpoint, 2004), 16.
21. American clinical psychologists Matthew Schulkind, Laura Hennis, and
David Rubin, in “Music, Emotion and Autobiographical Memory: They’re Playing
Your Song,” Memory and Cognition 27, no. 6 (1999): 948–55, tested how various age
groups remember through music. For their experiment, the researchers tested two
Notes to Chapter 4 201
groups of adults: younger adults between eighteen and twenty-one years of age and
older adults between sixty-five and seventy years of age. They made them listen to a
series of songs that were popular between 1935 and 1994 but only appeared on the hit
lists during a defined period (in contrast to evergreens). The subjects were asked
whether each song reminded them of a general period or a specific event from their
lives.
22. Connell and Gibson, Sound Tracks, 222–23.
23. At this point, I will not enter the discussion whether this intergenerational
longing concerns a generation or a specific time frame, such as a decade. Joseph
Kotarba, in “Rock ’n’ Roll Music as a Timepiece,” Symbolic Interaction 25, no. 3
(2002): 397–404, argues that the concept of cohort is more useful than the con-
cept of the decade for an interpretive analysis of musical reminiscence.
24. See Antonio Damasio, Looking for Spinoza. Joy, Sorrow and the Feeling
Brain (Orlando, FL: Harcourt, 2003), 93–96. Also see the more detailed descrip-
tion in Chapter 2.
25. Barbra Misztal, in Theories of Social Remembering (Maidenhead UK: Open
University Press, 2003) identifies this as generational memory: “As generation fol-
lows generation, each receives an inheritance from its predecessor, and this intergen-
erational transmission, or tradition, is a foundation of societal continuity” (84).
26. Mark Katz, in Capturing Sound: How Technology Has Changed Music
(Berkeley: University of California Press, 2004) and Eric W. Rothenbuhler and
John Durham Peters, in “Defining Phonography: An Experiment in Theory,”
Musical Quarterly 81, no. 2 (1997): 242–64, all discuss the significance of (vinyl)
materiality in the age of phonography in contrast to the pre-phonographic and
post-phonographic era. Like Katz, Rothenbuhler and Peters emphasize the impor-
tant role of technology in the history of recorded music.
27. On the material temporality of recording, see the discussion on “triple
temporality” in chapter 6 of Jonathan Sterne’s The Audible Past (Durham, NC:
Duke University Press, 2003).
28. In chapter 4 of his book Television: Technology and Cultural Form
(Hanover, NH: University Press of New England, 1974), Raymond Williams ex-
plains the term “private mobilization.”
29. Joe Tacchi, in his article “Radio Texture: Between Self and Others,” in Ma-
terial Cultures: Why Some Things Matter, ed. Daniel Miller (London: UCL Press,
1998), 25–45, points at how hearing favorite songs on the radio is different than
playing them on your own stereo. Hearing a song on the radio, as Eric W. Rothen-
buhler argues in “Commercial Radio as Communication,” Journal of Communica-
tion 46 (1996): 125–43, is a moment when the symbolic activity of the other enters
into the field of contingencies of the self.
30. Joseph Auner, “Making Old Machines Speak: Images of Technology in Re-
cent Music,” ECHO: A Music-Centered Journal 2, no. 2 (2000), http://www.humnet
.ucla.edu/echo (accessed April 19, 2006).
202 Notes to Chapter 4
chapter 5
1. I prefer the term “personal photography” over commonly used terms like
“amateur photography” or “family photography.” The word “personal” is meant
to distinguish it from professional photography, but it also avoids the troubling
connotation of “amateurish” in relation to camera use. Family photography mis-
takenly presupposes the presence of a familial context, whereas photography has
always been and is increasingly used for personal identity formation.
2. Norwegian researcher C. M. Stuhlmiller, in “Narrative Picturing: Ushering
Experiential Recall,” Nursing Inquiry 3 (1996): 183–84, argues that narratives of re-
membering always involve elements of imagining and picturing, feeding verbal
stories.
3. Susan Sontag’s On Photography (New York: Delta, 1973) and Roland
Barthes’s Camera Lucida: Reflections on Photography (New York: Hill and Wang,
1981) were not the first but were certainly the most notable theories of photogra-
phy. Both essayists claim memory to be the most important function of personal
photography, but they also acknowledge photography’s material, ritual, and com-
municative meaning in the everyday lives of people.
4. “My pictures are my memories” is a cliché still resonant in many anthropo-
logical and sociological studies of family photography. See, for instance, Richard
Chalfen, “Snapshots ‘R’ Us: The Evidentiary Problematic of Home Media,” Vi-
sual Studies 17, no. 2 (2002): 141–49.
5. Barthes, Camera Lucida, 80.
6. According to neurobiologist Steven Rose in The Making of Memory (Lon-
don: Bantam Press, 1992), the still photograph is not a fixed object, but it forms
the input of a constantly changing and evolving autobiographical memory.
204 Notes to Chapter 5
7. For a very insightful overview of research in this area, see Deryn Strange,
Matthew Gerrie, and Maryanne Garry, “A Few Seemingly Harmless Routes to a
False Memory,” Cognitive Process 6 (2005): 237–42.
8. There are a large number of research groups reporting on the issue of false
memory as it is related to both narrative and visual evidence. For instance, see He-
lene Intraub and James Hoffman, “Reading and Visual Memory: Remembering
Scenes that Were Never Seen,” American Journal of Psychology 105, no. 1 (1992):
101–14. See also Elisabeth Loftus, “The Reality of Repressed Memories,” Ameri-
can Psychologist 48 (1993): 508–37; Elisabeth Loftus and J. Pickrell, “The Forma-
tion of False Memories,” Psychiatric Annals 25 (1995): 720–25. On the role of true
pictures in the creation of false memories, see Stephen Lindsay, Lisa Hagen, Don
Read, Kimberley Wade, and Maryanne Garry, “True Photographs and False
Memories,” Psychological Science 15, no. 3 (2004): 149.
9. Research by cognitive psychologists focusing particularly on the role of
doctored photographs in relation to false memory is also widely available. See, for
instance, Maryanne Garry and Matthew Gerrie, “When Photographs Create False
Memories,” Current Directions in Psychological Science 14 (2005): 321. See also
Kimberley Wade, Maryanne Garry, Don Read, and Stephen Lindsay, “A Picture
Is Worth a Thousand Lies: Using False Photographs to Create False Childhood
Memories,” Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 9, no. 3 (2002): 597–603. In their ar-
ticle, Wade and her colleagues confronted experimental subjects with doctored
pictures of the subjects as children in hot-air balloons; half the adults were per-
suaded into believing they actually remembered the balloon ride.
10. For the ongoing debate on whether narratives or pictures are more con-
ducive to false memories, see Maryanne Garry and Kimberley Wade, “Actually, a
Picture Is Worth Less Than 45 Words: Narratives Produce More False Memories
Than Photographs Do,” Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 12, no. 2 (2005): 359–66.
11. See, for instance, Gerald Zaltman, How Customers Think: Essential Insights
into the Mind of the Market (Boston: Harvard Business School Press, 2003). An ex-
periment has shown how easy it is for advertisements to plant false memories: re-
searchers presented participants with fake ads for Disney that feature them
meeting Bugs Bunny at a Disney Resort (although Bugs Bunny is a Warner Broth-
ers character). Most participants believed this was a true experience. See Kathryn
Braun, Rhiannon Ellis, and Elizabeth Loftus, “Make My Memory: How Adver-
tising Can Change Our Memories of the Past,” Psychology and Marketing 19
(2002): 1–23.
12. Barthes, Camera Lucida, 13.
13. Sontag, in On Photography, also touches upon this issue when she explains
how retouching techniques were prominent in commercial portrait photography
from the onset: “People want the idealized image: a photograph of themselves at
their best. They feel rebuked when the camera does not return an image of them-
selves as more attractive than they really are” (85).
Notes to Chapter 5 205
14. Jacques Aumont, in The Image (London: British Film Institute, 1997), pro-
vides a semiotic model that expands Barthes’s theory of the photographic image to
the contextual level, including the relationship between not only image and viewer
but also image and its referent in terms of aesthetics and of representation. For a
subtle critique of Barthes’s paradoxes concerning his control and lack of control
over the photographic image, see Ron Burnett, Cultures of Vision: Images, Media
and the Imaginary (Bloomington: Indiana University Press, 1995), 32–71.
15. Strange, Gerrie, and Gary, “A Few Seemingly Harmless Routes to a False
Memory,” 237.
16. In recent years, there has been an explosion of theory on the semiotics and
ontology of the digital image, but it is beyond the scope of this chapter to review
the literature in this area. As a general introduction to the digitization of visual
culture in general and photography in particular, consult The Photographic Image
in Digital Culture, ed. Martin Lister (New York: Routledge, 1995) and Warwick
Mules, “Lines, Dots and Pixels: The Making and Remaking of the Printed Image
in Visual Culture,” Continuum, Journal of Media and Cultural Studies, 14, no. 3
(2000): 303–16. A more philosophical introduction to ontology of the image can
be found in D. N. Rodowick, Reading the Figural, or, Philosophy after the New Me-
dia (Durham, NC: Duke University Press, 2001). Lev Manovich, in The Language
of New Media (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2001), addresses digital photography
as a technical-cultural construct. One notable exception to the negligence of cog-
nitive perspectives by humanists is the work of Canadian art and design scholar
Ron Burnett, who weaves cognitive perspectives into his semiotic approach to dig-
ital photography. In his recent book How Images Think (Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press, 2004), Burnett scrutinizes mental-cognitive processes (with a specific em-
phasis on memory) in relation to digital technology.
17. William J. T. Mitchell, in The Reconfigured Eye: Visual Truth in the Post-
photographic Era (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1992), identifies the pictorial tra-
dition of realism with the essence of photographic technology, and he identifies
the tradition of montage and collage with the essence of digital imaging. Lev
Manovich, in his essay “The Paradoxes of Digital Photography,” published in
Photography after Photography, Exhibition catalogue, Germany 1995 (available
at http://www.manovich.net/TEXT/digital_photo.html, accessed December 26,
2006), refutes this claim, countering that both traditions existed before photog-
raphy and that “normal” or “straight” photography never existed.
18. For a more technical and institutional exploration of photography’s move
toward digitization, see Fred Ritchin, In Our Own Image: The Coming Revolution
in Photography (New York: Aperture, 1999).
19. For an interesting introduction to issues of image manipulation and stock
photography, see, for instance, Paul Frosh, The Image Factory: Consumer Culture,
Photography, and the Visual Content Industry (Oxford: Berg, 2003). In chapter 7,
Frosh quotes directors from stock photography firms, who estimate that in the
206 Notes to Chapter 5
late 1990s, 80 percent to 90 percent of all photographs they promoted had been
digitally manipulated. As Frosh concludes: “Stock photographers and their clients
in design forms and advertising agencies assume that consumers, long accustomed
to the formal conventions and promotional goals of advertising images, do not ex-
pect such fidelity from their photographs” (175).
20. See, for instance, the software offered by VisionQuest Images, available at:
http://www.visionquestimages.com/index.htm (accessed April 8, 2006).
21. The photoblog of Chris Line, available at: http://a.trendyname.org/archives/
category/personal/ (accessed April 8, 2006).
22. For more details on the work of Nancy Burson, see http://www.nancyburson
.com/human_fr.html (accessed April 8, 2006). Her Human Race Machine featured
in many magazines and television programs in the spring of 2006, most notably on
the Oprah Winfrey Show.
23. For instance, a package called Picture Yourself Graphics (available at:
http://www.pygraphics.com) encourages playful collages and manipulation of pic-
tures on wedding announcements, featuring the bride and groom as five-year-olds
holding hands; other software design favors the use of personal pictures in combi-
nation with snapshots of famous tourist sites. For software that encourages the mix
of personal photographs with general stock photography, promoting playful per-
sonalization of tourist snapshots, see http://www.fotosearch.com/photodisc/picture-
yourself-here (accessed April 8, 2006).
24. Burnett, How Images Think, 28.
25. For a more elaborate argument on how ultrasound helps to turn the fetus
into an object to be worked on, see José van Dijck, Manufacturing Babies and Pub-
lic Consent: Debating the New Reproductive Technologies (New York: New York
University Press, 1995), chapter 5.
26. Sontag, in On Photography, already called attention to photography’s ma-
terialness by pointing at the logic of consumption underpinning the need to pho-
tograph things and people, converting sights into tangible, mobile objects: “To
consume means to burn up—and therefore, the need to be replenished” (179).
27. Don Slater, “Domestic Photography and Digital Culture,” in The Photographic
Image in Digital Culture, ed. Martin Lister (New York: Routledge: 1995), 129–46, 130.
28. Barthes, in his Camera Lucida, already noticed how we tend to look
through the laminated object because only the referent adheres, but he stresses
how it is chemistry and light that turn a picture into a fetish-object to be looked at
and to hold on to (p. 80).
29. For an interesting anthropological perspective on the materiality and ritu-
ality of photographs, see Elizabeth Edwards, “Photographs as Objects of Mem-
ory,” in Material Memories, ed. Markus Kwint, Christopher Breward, and Jeremy
Aymsley (Oxford: Berg, 1999), 221–48; see also Chris Wright, “Material and
Memory. Photography in the Western Solomon Islands,” Journal of Material Cul-
ture 9, no. 1 (2004): 73–85.
Notes to Chapter 5 207
30. For an interesting take on the material objects accompanying virtual desk-
tops, see Anna McCarthy, “Cyberculture or material culture?” Etnofoor 15, no. 1
(2002): 47–63.
31. There are a number of articles, most written by computer engineers, that
grapple with the (new) performative meanings of digital photo management. See
for instance, Kerry Rodden and Kenneth Wood, “How Do People Manage Their
Digital Photographs?” Computer Human Interaction 5, no. 1 (2003): 409–16. See
also Nancy Van House, Marc Davis, and Yuri Takhteyev, “From ‘What’ to ‘Why’:
The Social Uses of Personal Photos” (paper presented at the CSCW Conference
in Chicago, November 6–10, 2004).
32. In reaction to Sontag’s pejorative interpretation of the touristic photographic
experience, Steve Garlick argues in “Revealing the Unseen: Tourism, Art, and Pho-
tography,” Cultural Studies 16, no. 2 (2002): 289–305, that tourist photography is “a
mechanism that leads the subject to engage with the world in a creative act that
opens and re-opens spaces, each time people take or review their pictures” (296).
33. Sontag, On Photography, 8.
34. Quite a few cultural theorists and anthropologists have taken up Sontag’s
insights to write about photography and family. See, for instance, Marianne Hirsch,
Family Frames: Photography, Narrative, and Postmemory (Cambridge, MA: Har-
vard University Press, 1997); P. Holland, “Introduction: History, Memory, and
the Family Album,” in Family Snaps: The Meaning of Domestic Photography, ed. J.
Spence and P. Holland (London: Virago, 1991): 1–14; and Deborah Chambers,
Representing the Family (London: Sage, 2001).
35. Barbara Harrison, in “Photographic Visions and Narrative Inquiry,” Narra-
tive Inquiry 12, no.1 (2002): 87–111, notices a recent shift in photography’s social use:
“Images that have a place in everyday life have become less bound up with memory
or commemoration, but with forms of practice that are happening now. . . . Self-
presentation rather than self-representation is more important in identity forma-
tion” (107). Testifying to these trends is the pin board, where private pictures are
often combined with public ones and personal pictures are put on display.
36. Barbara Harrison, “Photographic Visions,” 107.
37. According to the New York Times, American sales of digital cameras sur-
passed the sales of analog film cameras for the first time in March 2003. See Katie
Hafner, “Recording Another Day in America, Aided by Digital Cameras,” New
York Times, May 12, 2003 www.nytimes.com.
38. Diane Schiano, Coreena Chen, and Ellen Isaacs, “How Teens Take, View,
Share, and Store Photos,” in Proceedings of the Conference on Computer-Supported
Co-operative Work (New York: ACM, 2002). Interestingly, the researchers con-
clude that teenagers are less inclined than adults to label pictures with captions, ei-
ther because they don’t think it is relevant or because they put great trust in their
future memory capacity; they feel confident they will always be able to remember
what is in the pictures.
208 Notes to Chapter 5
chapter 6
1. For an interesting exploration of the discussions concerning the construc-
tion of family in film and television, see Shooting the Family: Cultural Values and
Transnational Media, ed. Patricia Pisters and Wim Staat (Amsterdam: Amsterdam
University Press, 2005), particularly the introduction.
2. Antonio Damasio, The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the
Making of Consciousness (Orlando, FL: Harcourt, 1999), 11.
3. Gilles Deleuze, Cinema 2: The Time Image (Minneapolis: University of
Minnesota Press, 2003). Originally published as Cinema 2: L’ímage-temps (Paris:
Athlone Press, 1989).
4. There are a large number of books explaining the importance and meaning
of Deleuze’s philosophical concepts for film theory. See, for instance, Gregory
Flaxman, ed., The Brain is the Screen: Deleuze and the Philosophy of Cinema (Min-
neapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 2000); Barbara M. Kennedy, Deleuze
and Cinema: The Aesthetics of Sensation (Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press,
2000); and Ronald Bogue, Deleuze on Cinema (New York: Routledge, 2003).
5. Patricia Pisters, in The Matrix of Visual Culture: Working with Deleuze in
Film Theory (Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press, 2003), provides an in-
sightful analysis of Deleuze’s theory and film, particularly his conjecture that the
apparatus of cinema and the dynamics of brain activity and neural patterns are
fundamentally interlaced.
6. Deleuze, Cinema 2: The Time Image, 52.
7. For a detailed critique of Deleuze’s work, see Mark B. Hansen, New Philos-
ophy for New Media (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2004). A more elaborate theory
of “virtual embodiment” particularly in relation to virtual environments can be
found in Mark B. Hansen, Bodies in Code: Interfaces with Digital Media (New
York: Routledge, 2006).
8. Hansen, New Philosophy for New Media, 194.
9. One of my criticisms of Deleuze’s theory concerns his disregard of movies as
cultural forms and watching movies as a sociocultural practice. I am not saying,
though, that Deleuze completely ignores culture and politics in his writings; the
micro-politics of culture are discussed more generally in his works A Thousand
Plateaus: Capitalism and Schizophrenia (London: Continuum, 1988) and, with Felix
210 Notes to Chapter 6
21. On the change of the nuclear family in the 1960s, see for instance Arlene
Skolnick, Embattled Paradise: The American Family in an Age of Uncertainty (New
York: Basic Books, 1991).
22. As Moran aptly observes in There’s No Place Like Home Video, “Each
medium attempts to provide a home audience’s hankering for audiovisual images
of themselves, borrowing from each other over time, thus inventing and reinvent-
ing each other’s conventions of representation and patterns of interpersonal com-
munication” (106).
23. On the new genre of family portrait and documentary techniques, see Jim
Lane, The Autobiographical Documentary in America (Madison: University of
Wisconsin Press, 2002), 94–95.
24. Jeffrey Ruoff, An American Family: A Televised Life (Minneapolis: Univer-
sity of Minnesota Press, 2002), xvii.
25. The series was exhaustively reviewed in the press when it aired in 1973 and
afterward; there have also been numerous academic and scholarly articles written
that deal with An American Family and its documentary mode. For an overview, I
refer to the extensive bibliography included in Ruoff’s An American Family.
26. Ruoff, An American Family, 29.
27. Ibid., 88.
28. All family members, but most notably Pat Loud and her oldest son Lance,
conceded in hindsight that the presence of a film crew in their house forever
changed family life, even decades after the series was aired. Pat Loud wrote a book
on her experiences and frequently appeared on television, including when the fam-
ily was revisited by camera crews ten, fifteen, and twenty years after the actual
shooting. Lance Loud, who became a filmmaker himself, even furbished the last
episode in the series: WNET/PBS aired a production of his struggle with, and
eventual succumbing to, HIV/AIDS in 2001.
29. On the use of webcams in the private sphere, see Sheila Murphy, “Lurking
and Looking: Webcams and the Construction of Cybervisuality,” in Moving Im-
ages: From Edison to Webcam, ed. John Fullerton and Astrid Söderbergh Widding
(London: John Libbey, 2000), 173–80. See also Michele White, “Too Close to See:
Men, Women, and Webcams,” New Media and Society 5, no. 1 (2003): 7–28.
30. According to Australian film theorist Keith Beattie, in Documentary Screens:
Nonfiction Film and Television (Hampshire: Palgrave Macmillan, 2004), the digi-
tal camcorder is “creating new visual styles that situate the viewer in an intimate
relationship with the subject of autobiography” (105).
31. Moran, There’s No Place Like Home Video, 47.
32. See the Jacobs Family Website, available at: http://jacobsusa.com/main/
(accessed April 13, 2006).
33. This peculiar manifestation of what Arild Fetveit, in “Reality TV in the
Digital Era: A Paradox in Visual Culture?” in Reality Squared: Televisual Discourse
on the Real, ed. James Friedman (New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press,
212 Notes to Chapter 6
2002), 119–37, has called the “ambiguous coexistence of digital manipulation and
‘reality footage’ can be explained by a desire to reclaim a sense of reality that is vir-
tually absent in any static collage of digital photos and texts” (130).
34. Jacobs Family Website, http://jacobsusa.com/main/.
35. The Osbournes was produced and broadcast by MTV. Subsequent DVD
versions of the various seasons’ series were distributed by Miramax Home Enter-
tainment. Not the entire Osbourne family participates in the series: one of their
three children refused to appear on television. The first season premiered in 2003.
36. Although real life usually does not present itself thematically, each twenty-
minute episode of The Osbournes is organized as a fast-paced edited sequence cap-
italizing on a single theme, such as “like father, like daughter” or “won’t you be
my neighbor.”
37. For an early discussion on the construction of reality in documentary, see Bill
Nichols, Representing Reality: Issues and Concepts in Documentary (Bloomington: In-
diana University Press, 1991). In postmodern television culture, the conventions of
reality are increasingly informed by standards of so-called reality TV, where a con-
trived family—whether ten people living in a Big Brother house or a group conven-
ing on a deserted island—is captured on a reality show by ubiquitous cameras. The
legacy of home movie and video is still at work in documentary, just as the legacy of
documentary is still at work in reality TV. In coming to terms with these new forms
of reality on the screen, media theorist John Corner has coined the term “postdocu-
mentary” to describe new forms of tele-factuality. See John Corner, “Performing the
Real: Documentary Diversions,” Television and New Media 3 (2002): 255–69.
38. Moran, in There’s No Place Like Home Video, appropriately describes this
difference: “Digital icons undermine the authority of the video image and dis-
tance the artist from the actual process of image creation: whereas analogue video’s
aesthetic has been valued as immediate, literal, and naturalistic, digitized video is
more often construed as contrived, synthetic, and analytic” (13).
39. The documentary Capturing the Friedmans, directed by Andrew Jarecki
(New York: Magnolia Pictures) came out in 2003. In that same year, the DVD
containing two discs with the documentary and lots of extra materials was distrib-
uted by HBO Video.
40. While David had bought and frequently used the video camera, his
brother Jesse had a habit of audio-taping the family’s rows at the dining table.
Some of these audio-taped fights can be heard in the documentary.
41. In the interview with Andrew Jarecki featured on the DVD, the director
relates how in the middle of shooting the film, David Friedman came up with
twenty-five hours of taped home video and consented to its being used for the
documentary.
42. Andrew Jarecki, in an interview also included on the DVD, states that by
making this film, he took an explicit stance against currently reigning notions of
reality TV: “We are so tuned now to reality on television, but you watch the
Notes to Chapter 7 213
Friedmans for a minute and you see the incredible power of ‘real’ reality as op-
posed to that reality on CBS-TV where you see people starving on an island, but
you know all the while they are surrounded by cameras.”
43. In this quote, Deleuze echoes Antoni Artaud’s beliefs in cinema and the
world. See Deleuze, Cinema 2: The Time-Image, 167. For an extensive explanation
and commentary on this observation, see Patricia Pisters, From Eye to Brain. Gilles
Deleuze: Refiguring the Subject in Film Theory (Ph-Diss, University of Amsterdam,
1999), 76–85.
44. Hansen, New Philosophy for New Media, 270.
45. Kyle Veale has meticulously described this phenomenon in “Online Memo-
rialisation: The Web as a Collective Memorial Landscape for Remembering the
Dead” Fibreculture 3. Online journal available at: http://journal.fibreculture.org/
issue3/issue3_veale.html (accessed December 28, 2006).
46. Life on Tape, a Dutch producer, specializes in memorial videos, available
at: http://www.lifeontape.nl (accessed December 28, 2006). Precious Memories
and More, an American company, offers memorial DVDs to “help survivors memo-
rialize their loved ones.” Available at: http://www.preciousmemoriesandmore.com
(accessed December 28, 2006).
chapter 7
1. See Scott McQuire, Visions of Modernity: Representations, Memory, Time
and Space in the Age of the Camera (London: Sage, 1998), 127–38.
2. Gottfried Wilhelm Leibnitz (1646–1716), a German philosopher and mathe-
matician, was the inventor of differential calculus; he wanted to design a universal
language, which would facilitate communication via a network of universities. Leib-
nitz’s cylindrical computer, though never built, signified an important step forward
from dead mechanical calculations to a flexible “ars combinatoria,” which would
differentiate between the feeding in of data and the calculation itself. Leibniz also
philosophized about a computer based on a binary numerical system. Charles Bab-
bage (1791–1871), a British engineer, is known as the “Father of Computing” for his
contributions to the basic design of the computer through his “analytical engine.”
His previous “difference engine” was a special-purpose device intended for the pro-
duction of tables. He never turned his prototypes into working devices.
3. Vannevar Bush (1890–1974) was president of the Carnegie Institute in
Washington, DC (1939) and chair of National Advisory Committee for Aeronau-
tics (1939) before he became the director of the Office of Scientific Research and
Development. This last role was a presidential appointment and made him re-
sponsible for the six thousand scientists involved in the WWII effort.
4. Vannevar Bush’s famous, canonized article “As We May Think” first appeared
in Atlantic Monthly, July 1945, 101–8. Available at: http://www.theatlantic.com/
doc/194507/bush, accessed December 30, 2006.
214 Notes to Chapter 7
chapter 8
1. See, for instance, some marketers of preformatted scrapbooks (digital and pa-
per) such as Creative Memories (available at: http://www.creativememories.com,
accessed December 30, 2006) whose mission statement says: “Creative Memories
believes in and teaches the importance of preserving the past, enriching the pres-
ent, and inspiring hope for the future. We promote the tradition of historian/story-
teller and the importance of memory preservation and journaling for future
generations. We offer quality, photo-safe scrapbook and album-making products
and information.”
2. Elisabeth Loftus, “Make-Believe Memories,” American Psychologist (2003):
867–73, 872.
3. Cockpits of Starfighter jets or surgical units help students train their skills in
handling harrowing situations that involve all possible senses: sight, touch, smell,
sound.
4. See, for instance, Francesco Vincelli, “From Imagination to Virtual Reality:
The Future of Clinical Psychology,” CyberPsychology and Behavior 2, no. 3 (1999):
241–48. See also Barbara Rotbaum and Larry Hodges, “A Controlled Study of
Virtual Reality Exposure Therapy for the Fear of Flying,” Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology 68, no. 6 (2000): 1020–26.
Bibliography
Adatto, Kiku. Picture Perfect: The Art and Artifice of Public Image Making. New
York: HarperCollins, 1993.
Alphen, Ernst van. “Symptoms of Discursivity: Experience, Memory, and Trauma”
In Acts of Memory: Cultural Recall in the Present, edited by Mieke Bal, Jonathan
Crew, and Leo Spitzer, 24–38. Hanover, NH: University Press of New England,
1999.
Assmann, Jan, and John Czaplicka. “Collective Memory and Cultural Identity.”
New German Critique 65 (1995): 125–33.
Aumont, Jacques. The Image. London: British Film Institute, 1997.
Auner, Joseph. “Making Old Machines Speak: Images of Technology in Recent
Music,” ECHO: A Music-Centered Journal 2, no. 2 (2000). http://www.humnet
.ucla.edu/echo, accessed December 30, 2006.
Bal, Mieke, Jonathan Crew, and Leo Spitzer, eds. Acts of Memory: Cultural Recall
in the Present. Hanover, NH: University Press of New England, 1999.
Balch, W. R. and B. S. Lewis. “Threads of Music in the Tapestry of Memory.”
Memory and Cognition 21 (1996): 21–28.
Barnet, Belinda. “Pack-Rat or Amnesiac? Memory, the Archive and the Birth of
the Internet.” Continuum: Journal of Media and Cultural Studies 15, no. 2
(2001): 217–31.
———. “The Erasure of Technology in Cultural Critique.” Fibreculture 1 (2003).
http://journal.fibreculture.org/, accessed December 30, 2006.
Barthes, Roland. Camera Lucida: Reflections on Photography. New York: Hill and
Wang, 1981.
Baumgartner, H. “Remembrances of Things Past: Music, Autobiographical Mem-
ory, and Emotion.” Advances in Consumer Research 19 (1992): 613–20.
Beattie, Keith. Documentary Screens: Nonfiction Film and Television. Hampshire:
Palgrave Macmillan, 2004.
Benjamin, Walter. One-Way Street and Other Writings. London: Verso, 1979.
Bergson, Henri. Matter and Memory. London: George Allen and Unwin, 1911.
Bjarkman, Kim. “To Have and to Hold: The Video Collector’s Relationship with
an Eternal medium.” Television and New Media 5, no. 3 (2004): 217–46.
218 Bibliography
Chaney, David. Cultural Change and Everyday Life. Houndmills, UK: Palgrave,
2002.
Cheung, Charles. “A Home on the Web: Presentations of Self on Personal Home-
pages.” In Web.Studies: Rewiring Media Studies for the Digital Age, edited by
David Gauntlett, 43–51. London: Arnold, 2000.
Clark, Andy. Mindware: An Introduction to the Philosophy of Cognitive Science.
Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2001.
Cohen, Kris. “What Does the Photoblog Want?” Media, Culture and Society 27,
no. 6 (2005): 883–901.
Connell, John, and Chris Gibson. Sound Tracks: Popular Music, Identity and Place.
London: Routledge, 2003.
Connerton, Paul. How Societies Remember. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press, 1989.
Corner, John. “Performing the Real: Documentary Diversions.” Television and
New Media 3 (2002): 255–69.
Cubitt, Sean. Timeshift: On Video Culture. London: Routledge, 1991.
Damasio, Antonio. The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making
of Consciousness. Orlando, FL: Harcourt, 1999.
———. Looking for Spinoza: Joy, Sorrow, and the Feeling Brain. Orlando, FL:
Harcourt, 2003.
Danet, Brenda. “Books, Letters, Documents: The Changing Aesthetics of Texts in
Late Print Culture.” Journal of Material Culture 2 (1997): 5–38.
Decker, William M. Epistolary Practices: Letter Writing in America before Telecom-
munications. Raleigh: University of North Carolina Press, 1998.
Deleuze, Gilles. A Thousand Plateaus: Capitalism and Schizophrenia. London:
Continuum, 1988.
———. Cinema 2: The Time Image. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press,
2003.
Deleuze, Gilles, and Felix Guattari. Anti-Oedipus: Capitalism and Schizophrenia.
Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 2003.
Derrida, Jacques. Archive Fever: A Freudian Impression. Chicago: University of
Chicago Press, 1995.
Didier, Beatrice. Le journal intime. Paris: Editions du Seuil, 1976.
Dijck, José van. Manufacturing Babies and Public Consent: Debating the New Re-
productive Technologies. New York: New York University Press, 1995.
———. ImagEnation: Popular Images of Genetics. New York: New York Univer-
sity Press, 1998.
———. “No Images without Words.” In The Image Society: Essays on Visual Cul-
ture, edited by Frits Gierstberg and Warna Oosterbaan, 36–47. Rotterdam: Nai
Publishers, 2002.
———. The Transparent Body: A Cultural Analysis of Medical Imaging. Seattle:
University of Washington Press, 2005.
220 Bibliography
Draaisma, Douwe. Metaphors of Memory: A History of Ideas about the Mind. Cam-
bridge: Cambridge University Press, 2000.
———. Waarom de tijd sneller gaat als je ouder wordt. Groningen: Historische
Uitgeverij, 2001.
Dumit, Joseph. “Objective Brains, Prejudicial Images.” Science in Context 12, no. 1
(1999): 173–201.
Edwards, Elizabeth. “Photographs as Objects of Memory.” In Material Memories,
edited by Markus Kwint, Christopher Breward, and Jeremy Aymsley, 221–48.
Oxford: Berg, 1999.
Elsaesser, Thomas. “ ‘Where were you when . . . ?’; or, ‘I Phone, therefore I am.’ ”
PMLA 118, no. 1 (2003): 120–22.
Fernback, Jan. “Legends on the Net: An Examination of Computer-Mediated
Communication as a Locus of Oral Culture.” New Media and Society 5, no. 1
(2003): 29–45.
Fetveit, Arild. “Reality TV in the Digital Era: A Paradox in Visual Culture?” In
Reality Squared: Televisual Discourse on the Real, edited by James Friedman,
119–37. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press, 2002.
Flaxman, Gregory, ed. The Brain Is the Screen: Deleuze and the Philosophy of Cin-
ema. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 2000.
Foucault, Michel. The Archaeology of Knowledge and the Discourse on Language.
New York: Pantheon, 1972.
Frith, Simon. Performing Rites: On the Value of Popular Music. Oxford: Oxford
University Press, 1996.
Frosh, Paul. The Image Factory: Consumer Culture, Photography, and the Visual
Content Industry. Oxford: Berg, 2003.
Garland, Brent, ed. Neuroscience and the Law: Brain, Mind, and the Scales of Jus-
tice. New York: Dana Press, 2004.
Garlick, Steve. “Revealing the Unseen: Tourism, Art, and Photography.” Cultural
Studies 16, no. 2 (2002): 289–305.
Garry, Maryanne, and Matthew Gerrie. “When Photographs Create False Mem-
ories.” Current Directions in Psychological Science 14 (2005): 321.
Garry, Maryanne, and Kimberley Wade. “Actually, a Picture Is Worth Less Than
45 Words: Narratives Produce More False Memories Than Photographs Do.”
Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 12, no. 2 (2005): 359–66.
Gere, Charlie. “Brains-in-Vats, Giant Brains and World Brains: The Brain as
Metaphor in Digital Culture.” Studies in the History and Philosophy of Biologi-
cal and Biomedical Sciences 35 (2004): 351–66.
Gibbs, Anna. “Contagious Feelings: Pauline Hanson and the Epidemiology of Af-
fect.” Australian Humanities Review 24 (2001). http://www.lib.latrobe.edu.au/
AHR/archive/Issue-December-2001/gibbs.html, accessed December 30, 2006.
Gitelman, Lisa. “How Users Define New Media: A History of the Amusement
Phonograph.” In Rethinking Media Change: The Aesthetics of Transition, edited
Bibliography 221
by David Thorburn and Henry Jenkins, 61–80. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press,
2003.
Goodwin, Alan. “Sample and Hold: Pop Music in the Digital Age of Reproduc-
tion.” Critical Quarterly 30, no. 3 (1988): 34–49.
Grainge, Paul. “Nostalgia and Style in Retro America: Moods, Modes, and Media
Recycling.” Journal of American and Comparative Cultures 23, no. 1 (2000): 27–34.
Gross, David. Lost Time: On Remembering and Forgetting in Late Modern Culture.
Amherst: University of Massachusetts Press, 2000.
Halbwachs, Maurice. On Collective Memory. Chicago: University of Chicago
Press, 1992.
Hansen, Mark. Embodying Technesis: Technology beyond Writing. Ann Arbor: Uni-
versity of Michigan Press, 2000.
———. New Philosophy for New Media. Cambridge MA: MIT Press, 2004.
———. Bodies in Code. Interfaces with Digital Media. New York: Routledge, 2006.
Harrison, Barbara. “Photographic Visions and Narrative Inquiry.” Narrative In-
quiry 12, no. 1 (2002): 87–111.
Hayles, N. Katherine. How We Became Posthuman: Virtual Bodies in Cybernetics,
Literature and Informatics. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1999.
———. Writing Machines. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2002.
Hirsch, Marianne. Family Frames: Photography, Narrative, and Postmemory. Cam-
bridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1997.
Hogan, Patrick Colm. Cognitive Science, Literature, and the Arts. New York: Rout-
ledge, 2003.
Holland, P. “Introduction: History, Memory, and the Family Album.” In Family
Snaps: The Meaning of Domestic Photography, edited by J. Spence and P. Hol-
land, 1–14. London: Virago, 1991.
Hoskins, Andrew. “New Memory: Mediating History.” Historical Journal of Film,
Radio and Television 21, no. 4 (2001): 333–46.
———. “Signs of the Holocaust: Exhibiting Memory in a Mediated Age.” Me-
dia, Culture and Society 25, no. 1 (2003): 7–22.
Hutchins, Edwin. Cognition in the Wild. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1996.
Huyssen, Andreas. Twilight Memories: Marking Time in a Culture of Amnesia.
New York: Routledge, 1995.
Intraub, Helene, and James Hoffman. “Reading and Visual Memory: Remember-
ing Scenes that Were Never Seen.” American Journal of Psychology 105, no. 1
(1992): 101–14.
Johnson, Steven. Mind Wide Open: Your Brain and the Neuroscience of Everyday
Life. New York: Scribner, 2004.
Jones, Steve. “Music that Moves: Popular Music, Distribution and Network Tech-
nologies.” Cultural Studies 16, no. 2 (2002): 213–32.
Katz, Mark. Capturing Sound: How Technology Has Changed Music. Berkeley:
University of California, 2004.
222 Bibliography
Rotbaum, Barbara, and Larry Hodges. “A Controlled Study of Virtual Reality Ex-
posure Therapy for the Fear of Flying.” Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psy-
chology 68, no. 6 (2000): 1020–26.
Rothenbuhler, Eric W. “Commercial Radio as Communication.” Journal of Com-
munication 46 (1996): 125–43.
Rothenbuhler, Eric W. and John D. Peters, “Defining Phonography: An Experi-
ment in Theory.” Musical Quarterly 81, no. 2 (1997): 242–64.
Ruoff, Jeffrey. An American Family: A Televised Life. Minneapolis: University of
Minnesota Press, 2002.
Samuel, Raphael. Theatres of Memory. Volume 1: Past and Present in Contemporary
Culture. London: Verso, 1994.
Schaap, Frank. “Links, Lives, Logs: Presentation in the Dutch Blogosphere.” In
Into the Blogoshphere: Rhetoric, Community and Culture of Weblogs, edited by
Laura Gurak, Smiljana Antonijevio, Laurie Johnson, Clanoy Ratliff, and Jessica
Reyman. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota, 2004. http://blog.lib.umn
.edu/blogosphere/, accessed December 30, 2006.
Schiano, Diane J., Coreena Chen, and Ellen Isaacs. “How Teens Take, View,
Share, and Store Photos.” In Proceedings of the Conference on Computer-
Supported Co-operative Work. New York: ACM, 2002.
Schulkind, Matthew D., Laura K. Hennis, and David C. Rubin. “Music, Emo-
tion and Autobiographical Memory: They’re Playing Your Song.” Memory and
Cognition 27, no. 6 (1999): 948–55.
Seremetakis, C. Nadia, ed. The Senses Still: Perception and Memory as Material
Culture in Modernity. Boulder, CO: Westview Press, 1994.
Silverstone, Roger, Eric Hirsch, and David Morley. “Information and Communi-
cation Technologies and the Moral Economy of the Household.” In Consum-
ing Technologies: Media and Information in Domestic Spaces, edited by Roger
Silverstone and Eric Hirsch, 14–31. London: Routledge, 1992.
Skolnick, Arlene. Embattled Paradise: The American Family in an Age of Uncer-
tainty. New York: Basic Books, 1991.
Slater, Don. “Domestic Photography and Digital Culture.” In The Photographic
Image in Digital Culture, edited by Martin Lister, 129–46. New York: Rout-
ledge, 1995.
Sontag, Susan. On Photography. New York: Delta, 1973.
Spigel, Lynn. Make Room for TV: Television and the Family Ideal in Postwar Amer-
ica. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1992.
Stephens, Mitchell. The Rise of the Image, the Fall of the Word. New York: Oxford
University Press, 1998.
Sterne, Jonathan. The Audible Past: Cultural Origins of Sound Reproduction.
Durham, NC: Duke University Press, 2003.
———. “MP3 as Cultural Artifact.” New Media and Society, 8 no. 5 (2006): 825–42.
Stevens, Molly, Gregory Abowd, Khai Truong, and Florian Vollmer, “Getting
Bibliography 227
into the Living Memory Box: Family Archives and Holistic Design.” Personal
Ubiqutous Computing 7 (2003): 210–16.
Strange, Deryn, Matthew Gerrie, and Maryanne Garry. “A Few Seemingly
Harmless Routes to a False Memory.” Cognitive Process 6 (2005): 237–42.
Stuhlmiller, C. M. “Narrative Picturing: Ushering Experiential Recall.” Nursing
Inquiry 3: (1996): 183–84.
Sturken, Marita. Tangled Memories: The Vietnam War, The AIDS Epidemic, and
the Politics of Remembering. Berkeley: University of California Press, 1997.
———. “The Image as Memorial: Personal Photographs in Cultural Memory.”
In The Familial Gaze, edited by Marianne Hirsch, 178–95. Hanover, NH: Uni-
versity Press of New England, 1999.
Sutton, John. Philosophy and Memory Traces: Descartes to Connectionism. Cam-
bridge: Cambridge University Press, 1998.
———. “Porous Memory and the Cognitive Life of Things.” In Prefiguring Cy-
berculture: An Intellectual History, edited by Darren Tofts, Annemarie Johnson,
and Alessio Cavallara, 130–41. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2002.
Tacchi, Joe. “Radio Texture: Between Self and Others.” In Material Cultures:
Why Some Things Matter, edited by Daniel Miller, 25–45. London: UCL Press,
1998.
Taylor, Alex S., and Richard Harper. “The Gift of the Gab? A Design-Oriented
Sociology of Young People’s Use of Mobiles.” Journal of Computer Supported
Cooperative Work 12, no. 3 (2003): 267–96.
Taylor, Ella. Prime-Time Families: Television Cultures in Postwar America. Berke-
ley: University of California Press, 1989.
Taylor, Timothy D. Sounds: Music, Technologies, and Culture. New York: Rout-
ledge, 2001.
Thacker, Eugene. “What is Biomedia?” Configurations 11 (2003): 47–79.
Thompson, John B. The Media and Modernity: A Social Theory of the Media.
Cambridge UK: Polity Press, 1995.
Thorburn, David, and Henry Jenkins, eds. Rethinking Media Change: The Aesthet-
ics of Transition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2003.
Tofts, Darren, Annemarie Johnson, and Alessio Cavallara, eds. Prefiguring Cyber-
culture: An Intellectual History. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2002.
Tomkins, Silvan. Affect, Imagery, Consciousness. New York: Springer, 1962.
Turino, Thomas. “Signs of Imagination, Identity, and Experience: A Peircian
Semiotic Theory for Music.” Ethnomusicology 43, no. 2 (1999): 221–55.
Ulmer, Gregory. Heuretics: The Logic of Invention. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hop-
kins University Press, 1994.
Urry, John. “How Societies Remember the Past.” In Theorizing Museums: Repre-
senting Identity and Diversity in a Changing World, edited by Sharon Macdon-
ald and Gordon Fyfe, 45–68. Oxford: Blackwell, 1996.
Uttal, William R. The New Phrenology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2003.
228 Bibliography
Van House, Nancy, Marc Davis, and Morgan Ames. “The Uses of Personal Net-
worked Digital Imaging: An Empirical Study of Cameraphone Photos and
Sharing.” In Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, 1853–36. New
York: ACM, 2005.
Veale, Kylie. “Online Memorialisation: The Web as a Collective Memorial
Landscape for Remembering the Dead.” Fibreculture 3 (2004). http://journal.
fibreculture.org/issue3/issue3_veale.html, accessed December 30, 2006.
Vincelli, Francesco. “From Imagination to Virtual Reality: The Future of Clinical
Psychology.” CyberPsychology and Behavior 2, no. 3 (1999): 241–48.
Wade, Kimberley, Maryanne Garry, Don Read, and Stephen Lindsay. “A Picture
Is Worth a Thousand Lies: Using False Photographs to Create False Child-
hood Memories.” Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 9, no. 3 (2002): 597–603.
Wang, Qi, and Jens Brockmeier. “Autobiographical Remembering as Cultural
Practice: Understanding the Interplay between Memory, Self and Culture.”
Culture and Psychology 8, no. 1 (2002): 45–64.
White, Michele. “Too Close to See: Men, Women, and Webcams.” New Media
and Society 5, no. 1 (2003): 7–28.
Williams, Raymond. Television: Technology and Cultural Form. Hanover, NH:
University of New England Press, 1974.
Willis, Paul. Common Culture: Symbolic Work at Play in the Everyday Cultures of
Young. London: Open University Press, 1990.
Winkler, Hartmut. “Discourses, Schemata, Technology, Monuments: Outline for
a Theory of Cultural Continuity.” Configurations 10 (2002): 91–109.
Wright, Chris. “Material and Memory. Photography in the Western Solomon Is-
lands.” Journal of Material Culture 9, no. 1 (2004): 73–85.
Yakel, Elizabeth. “Reading, Reporting, and Remembering: A Case Study of the
Maryknoll Sisters Diaries.” In Proceedings of I-Chora Conference, 142–50.
Toronto: University of Toronto, 2003.
Zak, Albin J. The Poetics of Rock: Cutting Tracks, Making Records. Berkeley: Uni-
versity of California Press, 2001.
Zaltman, Gerald. How Customers Think: Essential Insights into the Mind of the
Market. Boston: Harvard Business School Press, 2003.
Zhang, Jane. “The Lingering of Handwritten Records.” In Proceedings of I-Chora
Conference, 38–45. Toronto: University of Toronto, 2003.
Zimmerman, Patricia R. Reel Families: A Social History of Amateur Film. Bloom-
ington: Indiana University Press, 1995.
Index