Thesis Proposal Final Version
Thesis Proposal Final Version
Thesis Proposal Final Version
Thesis
Proposal
Self-‐monitoring
and
effective
leadership
The
moderating
role
of
team
identification
and
prosocial
motivation
Elisa
Roelofs
419185
Rotterdam
School
of
Management
MSc
Human
Resource
Management
2015
Supervisor:
Dr.
S.R.
Giessner
Co-‐reader:
…
1. Introduction
What makes a leader an effective leader? Behaviour associated with effective leadership is
extensively researched in literature (Judge & Piccolo, 2004). Effective leadership is important
as it is necessary for optimal performance of teams and organizations (Yukl, 2012). Two
dimensions that have recently regained attention in this respect are consideration and
initiating structure. These dimensions refer to the extent that leaders show task-oriented and
relationship-oriented behaviour towards their team members, and are associated with effective
leadership (Judge & Piccolo, 2004). Insights in antecedents of effective leadership behaviours
effectiveness.
To display effective leadership behaviour, leaders need to be able to understand the needs of
their followers and adapt their behaviour to the environment and external factors (Bass &
Riggio, 2005; Burns, 1978; Zaccaro, Foti & Kenny, 1991). A construct that is closely related
different social situations and can vary their behaviours accordingly. Consequently, a link
monitoring and leadership emergence (Snyder, 1987). Unfortunately, there has been little
research mostly focuses on leadership emergence (Day, Shleicher, Unckless & Hiller, 2002;
Zaccaro et al., 1991). This study extends previous literature by focusing on effectiveness. It is
because the concepts of effectiveness and emergence are not interchangeable. For example, it
has been shown that there is a difference between succesful managers (in terms of emergence)
and effective managers in how they spend their time on different activities (Luthans, 1988;
Luthans, Hodgetts & Rosenkrantz, 1988). To fill this gap, this study investigates the link
structure. The current study focuses on this link in the context of teams.
us that situational factors play an important role in the relationship between leadership
effectiveness and behaviour (Fiedler, 1967; House, 1971). Leadership does not occur in
isolation but in a social context (Zaccaro, Gilbert, Thor and Mumford, 1991). This is
especially relevant with regard to high self-monitors who easily adapt to situational cues. It is
with effective leadership. Therefore the question is raised: under what conditions is high self-
structure? In other words, under what situations do high (versus low) self-monitors adapt their
behaviours in the interest of the team? This study focuses on team identification as a
moderating factor. Based on previous literature, it can be concluded that leaders who identify
with a team are more likely to behave in the interest of the team (Giessner, Van Knippenberg,
Van Ginkel & Sleebos, 2013). The current study aims to determine whether this relationship
also exists when it comes to consideration and initiating structure. This study thus takes a
social identity perspective to investigate whether team identification plays a moderating role.
interaction effect of team identification on consideration. It can be argued that this effect is
stronger for highly (versus lowly) prosocially motivated leaders, because they are generally
more likely to show behaviour directed towards the team, and because this tendency is
strengthened if they are intrinsically motivated (Grant 2007; Grant, 2008). Because it is
expected that team identification provides such an intrinsic motivation, prosocial motivation
leadership in terms of consideration and initiating structure, and to what extent team
identification and prosocial motivation play a moderating role in this relation. This study
contributes to existing literature in the following ways. First of all, it extends knowledge
about antecedents of effective leadership behaviour and about the relationship between self-
perspective of social identity theory to leadership effectiveness, and applies this to a leader
perspective.
2. Literature review
(Fiedler, 1967; Geier, 1967; House, 1971; Kahn & Katz, 1960; Kirkpatrick & Locke, 1991;
Stogdill & Coons, 1951). Two main behavioral dimensions of leadership which are associated
with effective leadership have been identified in the Ohio State studies: consideration and
initiating structure (Stogdill & Coons, 1951). However, since their introduction these concepts
have moved from promising constructs to ‘forgotten’ ones. Methodological and conceptual
arguments challenged the validity of the construct (Fleischman, 1995; Schriesheim & Kerr,
1974). Yet, more recent research by Judge and colleagues (2004) reexamined the validity of
these constructs and concluded that the two constructs are associated with multiple aspects of
not be prematurely dismissed. The current study thus considers effective leadership in terms
of these two constructs. Consideration refers to the extent to which leaders show respect for
followers and their ideas, show concern for their feelings and welfare, provide support and
appreciation, and the extent to which their relationships with followers includes mutual trust
(Bass, 1990; Judge et al., 2004; Stogdill & Coons, 1951). Consideration thus reflects
task-oriented behaviour. This construct refers to the extent to which leaders define, structure
and organize their role and the role of their followers aiming at goal attainment, and the extent
to which leaders establish and structure communication channels and patterns towards this
Both consideration and intiating structure are associated with multiple attributes of
effective leadership. Judge and colleagues (2004) showed support for a relationship between
these behaviours and follower satisfaction (regarding their job and their leader) and leader
emphasis. High consideration in leaders is relatively more strongly associated with higher job
satisfaction and higher motivation, whereas high initiating structure is relatively more
strongly associated with higher levels of performance and productivity (Judge et al., 2004).
Therefore it is important to treat these two behaviours separately, reflecting different aspects
of effective leadership.
The question then remains, what kind of leaders display these behaviours and under
Self-monitoring
For a leader to be effective it is important to understand the needs of followers and to adapt to
external factors. This study examines the concept of self-monitoring as a potential antecedent
of leadership effectiveness in terms of consideration and initiating structure. Self-monitoring
refers to the extent to which a person is able to adjust his or her behaviour to situational and
external factors (Snyder, 1987). High self monitors are sensitive to social cues, are concerned
for social appropriateness and have high adaptability in their behaviour to different situations
(Briggs, Cheek & Buss, 1980; Snyder, 1974). A relationship has been indicated between self
monitoring and leadership emergence (Day et al., 2002; Snyder, 1987). However, the
relationship with effectiveness has received less attention, and sometimes the concepts of
effectiveness and emergence are used interchangeably. That these two constructs should not
be regarded as synonymous can be derived from a study by Luthans and colleagues (1988)
which indicates that succesful managers (in terms of emergence) spend their time differently
than effective managers. For example, succesful managers spend more time networking
whereas effective leaders spend more time communicating (Luthans et al., 1988). It is thus
important to separately research the link between effectiveness and leader emergence as well
as leadership effectiveness.
As identified by Zaccaro and colleagues (1991), social intelligence, social expertise and
behavioral flexibility towards different situational and social demands, which are associated
with leader emergence, are also predictors of leadership effectiveness. However, no research
on the relationship between self-monitoring and leadership effectiveness has been conducted
since then (Day et al., 2002). The current study therefore focuses on this relationship and
predicts that a similar relationship exists between self-monitoring and leader effectiveness in
terms of consideration and initiating structure. Therefore, the first hypothesis and second
expected that this relationship will be influenced by several moderating factors. Self-monitors
can disguise their private person and show different ‘faces’, leading to low behavioral
consistency (Snyder, 1987). It thus does not have to mean that because they are able to adapt,
that they will adapt in the interest of the team. Research indicated that high self-monitors are
associated with less commitment to their organization (Day et al., 2002). Furthermore, self-
montoring has mostly been associated with positive performance that is related to the leader
him or herself such as career mobility, promotion, position (Kilduff & Day, 1994; Mehra,
Kilduff & Brass, 2001). So the question should be asked: When do high self-monitoring
leaders use their ability to adapt their behaviour in the interest of the team and display
behaviours such as initiating structure and consideration? Given the inconsistency in literature
about the relationship between self-monitoring and effective leadership, this study considers
team identification as a moderating factor. Previous research indicates that team identification
draws attention to the team and directs behaviour (Richter, West, Van Dick & Dawson, 2006).
Consequently, it should play a crucial role to understand the relationship between self-
Team identification
The first moderating factor that will be considered is team identification of the leader. It is
predicted that if leaders identify with a team, they will use their abilities as high self-monitors
Based on the Social Identity Theory it is argued that people consider themselves as part of a
group and define themselves in terms of shared characteristics of a group (Tajfel & Turner,
group and its fate (Ashfort & Mael, 1989). With regard to teams, identification is the sense of
belonging with the team, the acknowledgement and valuement of being part of the team and
the sharing of norms and behaviour codes (Solansky, 2011). Identification directs one’s
values, attitudes and behaviours (Richter et al., 2006). A strong social identification with a
group is associated with behaviour in the group’s interest (Ashfort & Mael, 1989; Cicero,
Bonaiuto, Pierro, & Van Knippenberg, 2008; Haslam, 2001). Recent research has indicated a
similar relationship with regard to leaders’ identification and leader behaviour (Giessner et al.,
2013). This research shows that leaders who identify with a team, and who categorize
themselves as a group member, are more likely to behave in the interest of the team (Giessner
et al., 2013). It is assumed that leaders with a strong identification towards their team take the
interests of the team at heart and care more for the team than leaders who identify less with
the team. Furthermore, team identification implies that a leader feels part of the team and
regards the fate of the team as his or her own fate. This also has consequences for their
leadership behaviour towards the team: it is likely that leaders act in a more team-oriented
way in a situation of strong team identification than in a situation of weak team identification.
Given this assumption, high self-monitoring leaders compared to low self-monitoring leaders
are especially suited to understand and recognize the needs of the team and act upon these
needs. This will be particularly the case if they identify strongly with the team.
Therefore, this study predicts that under the condition of high team identification (versus low
identification) high self-monitoring leaders use their adaptability in altering their behaviour
towards serving the interest and needs of the team. Consideration and initiating structure are
regarded as behaviours that serve the interest and needs of the team. It is thus hypothesized
that a high self monitoring leader is more likely to show consideration and initiating structure
if he or she identifies with the team. Therefore the third and fourth hypothesis are as follows:
Hypothesis 3: The relationship between self-monitoring of a leader and consideration is
positively moderated by team identification; such that the greater the team identification of a
leader, the stronger the relationship between the level of self-monitoring of a leader and
consideration.
positively moderated by team identification; such that the greater the team identification of a
leader, the stronger the relationship between the level of self-monitoring of a leader and
initiating structure.
Prosocial motivation
The extent to which team identification moderates the relationship between self-monitoring
1987; De Dreu, 2006; Grant, 2007). The current study conceptualises prosocial motivation as
an individual difference measure, and defines prosocial motivation as a desire to expend effort
out of a concern for other people and to engage in work that is beneficial to others (Batson,
1998; De Dreu 2006; Grant, 2007). Prosocial motivation in a work context is associated with
job performance, personal initiative and organizational citizenship (De Dreu & Nauta, 2009).
This includes helping behaviour (Grant, 2007). Prosocial motivation also means a focus on
achieving a meaningful goal (Batson, 1998). Prosocial motivation can have different roots.
For example, people can be prosocially motivated because they generally care about others.
However, prosocial motivation does not automatically imply altruism, as people can also be
prosocially motivated because it makes them feel good about themselves, or because they care
for their membership in a group (Batson, Ahmad, Powell & Stocks, 2008; De Dreu, 2006).
High prosocial motivation thus means a stronger motivation to show behaviour directed
displayed especially when people are also intrinsically motivated. It is expected that a high
level of team identification may function as an intrinsic motivator for prosocial behaviour.
Since consideration is as an example of behaviour directed towards others, and since team
study expects a three-way interaction between prosocial motivation, team identification and
consideration. Specifically, this study predicts that for highly (versus lowly) prosocially
motivated leaders, the moderation effect of team identification on the relationship between
that for leaders high on prosocial motivation, the effect of team identification may be stronger
because team identification in turn fuels their prosocial motivation. This means that the
strongest effect of self-monitoring on consideration is expected for a leader with high team
identification and high prosocial motivation. The fifth hypothesis of this study is therefore as
follows:
and the relationship beween self-monitoring and consideration, such that a high (versus low)
figure 1.
identification
Leader team identification
Leader prosocial motivation
Consideration
Leader self-monitoring
Initiating structure
A quantitative field study will be conducted. A quantitative study is chosen because the aim
of this study is to test hypotheses based on previous literature. This implies that a quantitative
study is more suitable than a qualitative study whereby the aim is to generate new theory
(Neuman, 2001; Newman & Benz, 1998). Furthermore, this study aims to gather
generalizable objective data rather than subjective data and interpretations, which is another
reason why a quantitative study will be performed (Newman & Benz, 1998). A field study
(versus an experimental study) is chosen because this type of study allows for high external
2001). It should be kept in mind however that a field study implies that no causal inferences
A heterogeneous sample will be drawn from professional teams in various industries in The
collaboration with two other students. The unit of analysis will be the team leader. The
dependent variable will be collected from the followers, and the independent variable and
questionnaires will be handed out personally or sent via e-mail. It is a multi-source study:
team leaders and followers will receive different questionnaires. Questionnaires will include
individual codes to match the results of followers with their team leaders. Participants will be
informed about the procedure and aim of the survey, and will be debriefed afterwards about
First of all, from all participants the demographic measures of gender, age, industry and team
tenure will be included. Next to that, for the team leaders the variables team size and tenure in
Consideration
(LBDQ) will be used (Stogdill, Goode & Day, 1962). This is a self-report measure which
includes 100 questions about leader behaviour. For this study, the subscale referring to
consideration will be used. Since it is a self-report scale, and this study focuses on the
follower’s perspective, the items will be adapted such that they refer to the follower’s
perspective. The scale includes ten items to measure consideration, for example: “The team
leader is friendly and approachable” and “The team leader puts suggestions made by the
group into operation”. The measure uses a five-point Likert scale, ranging from A (1, never)
to E (5, always). Three items are formulated negatively and have a reversed scale.
Initiating structure
Initiating structure will also be measured by a subscale of the LBDQ (Stogdill et al., 1962).
The items will be adapted such that they refer to the follower’s perspective instead of a self-
report scale. The scale refers to structure and consists of ten items, such as: “The team leader
lets group members know what is expected of them” and “The team leader maintains definite
standards of performance. The measure uses a five-point Likert scale, ranging from A (1,
Self-monitoring
To measure self-monitoring the self-monitoring scale of Lennox and Wolfe (1984) will be
used. This scale is shown to have the best psychometric properties compared to other scales
measuring the construct of self-monitoring and is also the shortest scale (Day et al., 2002).
The self-report scale consists of 13 items, representing sensitivity to behavior of others and
ability to adapt behavior. Example items are: “I have found that I can adjust my behavior to
meet the requirements of any situation I find myself in.” and “My powers of intuition are
quite good when it comes to understanding others' emotions and motives”. The scale uses a
Team identification
Team identification will be measured with a self-report scale consisting of six items (Mael &
Ashfort, 1992; Van Knippenberg & Van Schie, 2000). The scale is adapted to the extent that
the object of the items will be the team. Furthermore, one item will be adapted in line with the
research of Giessner and colleagues (2013). The item: “If a story in the media criticized the
team, I would feel embarrassed” is changed into: “If a story criticized the team, I would feel
embarrassed” because the media are not likely to be involved in the sample of this study.
Examples of other items are: “When someone criticizes this team, it feels like a personal
insult” and “This team’s successes are my successes”. A five-point Likert scale is used,
Pro-social motivation
Prosocial motivation will be measured using a scale developed by Grant (2008). The scale is
based on the self-regulation scales as developed by Ryan and Connell (1989). The self-report
scale consists of an introductory question (Why are you motivated to do your work) and four
items such as “Because I care about benefiting others through my work” and “Because it is
important to me to do good for others through my work”. The original scale uses a seven-
point Likert scale. In this study this will be adapted to a five-point Likert scale in order to
Ashfort, B.E. & Mael, F. (1989). Social identity theory and the organization. The Academy of
Bass, B.M. (1990). Bass and Stogdill’s handbook of leadership. New York: Free Press.
Bass, B. M., & Riggio, R. E. (2005). Transformational leadership (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ:
Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (vol. 20, pp. 65–122). San Diego, CA:
Academic Press.
Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th ed., vol. 2, pp. 282–316). New
York: McGraw-Hill.
Batson, C. D., Ahmad, N., Powell, A. A., & Stocks, E. L. (2008). Prosocial motivation. In J.
Y. Shah & W. L. Gardner (Eds.), Handbook of motivation science (pp. 135–149). New
Briggs, S. R., Cheek, J. M., & Buss, A. H. (1980). An analysis of the Self-Monitoring Scale.
Cicero, L., Bonaiuto, M., Pierro, A., & Van Knippenberg, D. (2008). Employees’ work effort
Day, D. V., Shleicher, D. J., Unckless, A. L., & Hiller, N. J. (2002). Self-monitoring
behavior: Implications for job performance, prosocial behavior, and personal initiative.
Fielding, K.S. & Hogg, M.A. (1997). Social identity, self-categorization, and leadership: a
field study of small interactive groups. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and
Fleischman E.A. (1995). Consideration and structure: another look at their role in leadership
Gangestad, S., & Snyder, M. (1985). ‘To carve nature at its joints’: On the existence of
Geier, J.G. (1967) A trait approach to the study of leadership in small groups, Journal of
Giessner, S.R., van Knippenberg, D., van Ginkel, W.P. & Sleebos, E (2013). Team-oriented
Grant A. M. (2007) Relational job design and the motivation to make a prosocial difference.
Grant A. M. (2008) Does intrinsic motivation fuel the prosocial fire? Motivational synergy in
93(1), 48–58.
Haslam, S.A. (2001). Psychology in organisations: The social identity approach. London:
Sage.
Judge, T. A., Piccolo, R. F., & Ilies, R. (2004). The forgotten ones? The validity of consideration
and initiating structure in leadership research. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89(1), 36-
51.
Kahn, R. & Katz, D. (1960). Leadership practices in relation to productivity and morale. In D.
Cartwright and A. Zander (Eds.), Group Dynamics: Research and Theory (2nd ed., pp.
Kilduff, M. & Day, D.V. (1994). Do chameleons get ahead? The effects of self-monitoring on
Kirkpatrick, S.A. & Locke, E.A. (1991). Leadership: Do traits matter? Academy of
Lennox, R. D., & Wolfe, R. N. (1984). Revision of the Self-Monitoring Scale. Journal of
Luthans, F. (1988) Successful vs. effective real managers. The Academy of Management
Luthans, F., Hodgetts, R.M., & Rosenkrantz, S.A. (1988) Real managers. Cambridge, MA:
Ballinger.
Mael, F., & Ashforth, B. E. (1992). Alumni and their alma mater: A partial test of the
Mehra, A., Kilduff, M. & Brass, D.J. (2001). The social networks of high and low self-
46(1), 121-146.
Neuman, W. L. (2011). Social research methods: Qualitative and quantitative approaches
Richter, A.W., West, M.A., van Dick, R. & Dawson, J.F. (2006). Boundary spanners’
Ryan, R. M., & Connell, J. P. (1989). Perceived locus of causality and internalization:
Examining reasons for acting in two domains. Journal of Personality and Social
Schriesheim C.A. & Kerr, S. (1974). Psychometric properties of the Ohio State leadership
Stogdill, R.M. & Coons, A.E. (Eds.) (1951). Leader behavior: Its description and
Stogdill, R.M., Goode, O.S., & Day, D.R. (1962). New leader behavior description subscales.
Tajfel, H. & Turner, J.C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In S.
Worchel & W.G. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 7-24).
Van Knippenberg, D. & Van Schie, E.C.M. (2000). Foci and correlates of organizational
147.
Yukl, G.A. (2012). Leadership in organizations (8th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice
Hall.
Zaccaro, S. J., Foti, R. J., & Kenny, D. A. (1991). Self-monitoring and trait-based variance in
Zaccaro, S.J; Gilbert, J.A., Thor, K.K. & Mumford, M.D. (1991). Leadership and social
Leader Questionnaire
Follower Questionnaire
The team leader looks out for the personal welfare of group members
The team leader tries out his or her ideas in the group
The team leader makes his or her attitudes clear to the group
The team leader decides what shall be done and how it shall be done
The team leader makes sure that his or her part in the group is
understood by the group members
The team leader schedules the work to be done
The team leader asks group members to follow standard rules and
regulations