Guia 1. Introduccion Al TCE
Guia 1. Introduccion Al TCE
Guia 1. Introduccion Al TCE
Clinical Guidebook
Table of Contents
The Evidence-Based Review of Acquired Brain Injury (ERABI) Clinical Guidebook is designed to include
clinical information relevant to the identification of ABI sequelae, diagnosis of ABI deficits, outcome
measures appropriate for diagnosis of specific deficits, and valuable evidence-based interventions for ABI
rehabilitation. In addition to this framework, we have also included supplementary materials to support
education in ABI rehabilitation such as clinical algorithms, key studies from the literature, quiz questions,
and case studies to consolidate your knowledge. There are 14 Chapters contained in the ERABI Clinical
Guidebook, focused around ABI specific deficits. Although we recognize research from other but similar
disease states, such as stroke rehabilitation, may be relevant, we have restricted our content to ABI
specific populations in order to maximize applications for ABI rehabilitation.
The ERABI Clinical Guidebook has been developed to provide a summary document of best evidence, and
hopefully serve to help minimize the gaps that exist in evidence-based ABI rehabilitation clinical care.
Some articles cite that the time lag from research to practice in healthcare may be as long as 17 years
(Hanney et al., 2015; Morris et al., 2011; Munro & Savel, 2016). Canada has recognized the need for high
impact tools to support evidence-based care as stated in the Federal Mandate to the Minister of Health
in 2017. This project also directly supports access to care, increased efficiency, and patient outcomes
through technology, knowledge translation, and open access material.
Click HERE to access the INESSS-ONF Clinical Practice Guideline for the Rehabilitation of adults with
Moderate to Severe TBI
For the purposes of this Clinical Guidebook vascular causes of focal ABIs, such as stroke, will be excluded
from our evidence synthesis. Included and excluded aetiologies of ABI for the purposes of this
Guidebook can be found in Table 1.1.
The vast majority of ABIs result from trauma and the two are essentially synonymous. ABIs are one of the
leading causes of lifelong disability in North America (Greenwald et al., 2003; Pickett et al., 2001; Thurman
& Guerrero, 1999). In the United States between 1.4 and 1.7 million people sustain a TBI every year (Faul
et al., 2010; Zaloshnja et al., 2008), with more than 120,000 people expected to develop long-term
disabilities (Zaloshnja et al., 2008). In the province of Ontario, more than 80,000 individuals sustained a
TBI between 2002 and 2006 (Colantonio et al., 2010). Although the majority of individuals who sustain a
TBI have mild impairment, the frequency of moderate severity injury is increasing; moderate TBIs
accounted for 19% of injuries in 1992 compared with 37% of injuries in 2002 (Colantonio et al., 2009).
It is important to know which factors are significantly related to outcomes post ABI. Prognostic indicators
can include such variables as injury severity, etiology of injury, age, rehabilitation length of stay, duration
of post-traumatic amnesia, etc. Table 1.2 summarizes the most common ABI prognostic indicators
identified in the literature.
5
The majority of TBIs are related to motor vehicle or transportation accidents and falls, together accounting
for approximately 75% of TBIs (Colantonio et al., 2009). Based on literature, falls account for
approximately 35% to 42% of TBIs whereas MVAs are responsible for 12% to 17% (Colantonio et al., 2010;
Faul et al., 2010). These trends have been consistent for approximately ten years (Roozenbeek et al.,
2013).
The rate of TBIs are typically higher among males, with males in the United States being 1.4 times more
likely to sustain a TBI than a female (Faul et al., 2010). Data from Ontario, Canada is consistent with this
finding and also shows greater rates of TBI among males (Chan et al., 2013a). This may be due to greater
participation in risk-taking behaviors by males compared to females. A cohort study has shown that
females have higher rates of TBIs due to falls, while TBIs in males are more likely to be caused by being
struck by or against an object (Colantonio et al., 2010).
Another significant demographic factor when evaluating ABI incidence and outcomes is age. Evidence
suggests that etiology of TBIs and outcome varies with age. For children aged 0-14, 50.2% of TBIs in the
United States were caused by falls (Faul et al., 2010), however rates of abusive head trauma can be as
high as 66% (Greenwald et al., 2003). For older adults, aged 65-85, falls make up approximately 60.7% of
TBIs, with falls being the cause of 90% of TBIs for those over 85 (Chan et al., 2013b, 2013c).
As might be expected, the prognosis of good outcomes long term is also associated with age. Evidence
suggests that age influences the trajectory of one’s recovery following injury. Individuals in the older age
bracket generally had poorer outcomes when compared to younger individuals (Marquez de la Plata et
al., 2008). Older age at the time of injury has also been associated with poorer performance in various
cognitive domains (Senathi-Raja et al., 2010). A study by Ashman and Mascialino (2008) noted that deficits
in encoding and retention of verbal information as well as inattention were more common and more
serious post TBI in those over the age of 65 years. It has been postulated, for those who are older at the
time of injury, that less neuronal plasticity may negatively affect the brain’s ability to compensate or adapt
in the same way a younger brain does post injury (Senathi-Raja et al., 2010). Mosenthal et al. (2002) found
older subjects (>64 years of age) had a significantly higher mortality rate than their younger peers at all
levels of TBI severity (p<0.001). Study authors suggested this increase in mortality may be attributable to
multiple factors including pre-existing comorbidities, post injury complications, and the intrinsic
challenges of aging itself (Mosenthal et al., 2002).
Click HERE to see the ERABI Module on Epidemiology and Long-term Outcomes
6
1. The Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS) (best score within 24 hours of injury).
2. Duration of unconsciousness.
3. Duration of posttraumatic amnesia (PTA).
TBI severity is usually classified according to the level of altered consciousness experienced by the patient
following injury (Table 1.3). The use of level of consciousness as a measurement arose because the
primary outcome to understand the severity of an injury has been the GCS. Consciousness levels following
ABI can range from transient disorientation to deep coma. Patients are classified as having a mild,
moderate or severe ABI according to their level of consciousness at the time of initial assessment. Various
measures of altered consciousness are used in practice to determine injury severity. Common measures
include the GCS, the duration of loss of consciousness (LOC), and the duration of PTA.
PTA is the time period post trauma for which the conscious patient has no recall for events. PTA is formally
defined as the period following emergence from coma in which the patient may appear confused,
disoriented, or agitated (Campbell, 2000). Research indicates a dose-response relationship, with the
length of PTA frequently being proportional to the severity of injury. Injury severity is defined as mild if
the duration of PTA is less than 1 hour, moderate if between 1–24 hours, and severe if PTA is between 1–
7 days. PTA exceeding 7 days is considered to represent a very severe injury (Campbell, 2000; Russell,
1932).
Description
The GCS is a quick, simple and objective tool used during the initial examination to estimate TBI
severity.
The GCS consists of 15 items in three basic categories: (1) eye opening – 4 items, (2) verbal response
– 5 items, and (3) motor response – 6 items.
Minimum score of 3
Maximum score of 15
Strengths
1. Simple, straightforward and brief bedside assessment.
2. Most familiar and most widely used instrument in the assessment of level of consciousness.
3. Established categories related to depth of coma and severity of injury.
4. Significant predictor of outcome following head injury.
5. Can be used by various groups of healthcare professionals regardless of level of education or ICU
experience
Weaknesses
1. The application of painful stimulus is controversial.
2. Assessment can be compromised by early interventions such as intubation and sedation.
3. Use of a global score may result in loss of information that adversely affects the predictive accuracy
of the GCS.
4. The motor response sub score has the greatest influence on the total GCS score.
5. Individuals with the same GCS scores in varying permutations can have significantly different
survival outcomes.
6. Lack of experience may result in inaccurate assessment.
7. It is an ordinal scale whereby, for example, the difference between scores of 3 and 4 Is not the same
as the difference between scores of 13 and 14
The GCS score has been shown to have a significant correlation with outcome following severe TBI, both
as the summary score (Choi et al., 1994; Choi et al., 1988) and
as the motor sub score (Beca et al., 1995; Bhatty & Kapoor,
1993; Choi et al., 1988; Michaud et al., 1992). In a prospective
study, the positive predictive value for a poor outcome (dead,
vegetative, or severely disabled) was calculated to be 77% for
patients with a GCS score of 3-5 and 26% for a GCS score of 6-
8 (Table 1.5) (Narayan et al., 1981). As is commonly done, this
8
study grouped GCS measurements versus outcome. In a larger study each GCS level would have its own
predictive value. For example, in a series of 315 patients with TBI from Australia, a significant inverse
correlation was demonstrated between the initial GCS score (obtained 6-48 hours after injury) and
mortality (Fearnside et al., 1993).
Figure 1.1 A sagittal view of the human brain which identifies the major regions of function as well as
the common observed deficits which can arise from insult to each specific region of the brain (Adapted
from Excel Care Nurse Case Management, 2019).
Overall, there are six regions of the brain which can be impacted by a brain injury: the frontal, parietal,
occipital and temporal lobe, as well as the cerebellum and brain stem. Each of these regions is
characterized by the functions that they are responsible for. The frontal lobe (red in Figure 1.1) is
responsible for higher level cognition, motor skills, expressive language, and other functions. It terminates
at the central sulcus and includes the motor cortex. The parietal lobe (yellow in Figure 1.1) includes the
somatosensory cortex and is primarily responsible for processing tactile and sensory information such as
pain, pressure, and touch. The temporal lobe (blue in Figure 1.1), located at the bottom of each
9
hemisphere, controls the comprehension of language, auditory functions and memory. The occipital lobe
(purple in Figure 1.1) located at the posterior aspect of the brain, is associated with interpreting visual
stimuli and information. The cerebellum (green in Figure 1.1), which is part of the hindbrain, is responsible
for coordinating gross/fine movements and walking. Damage to this region can result in tremors,
dizziness, and slurred speech. Although these are not complete descriptions of what each area of the brain
is responsible for, it is sufficient background knowledge to put the existing ABI literature and relevant
rehabilitation interventions into context.
In addition to these regions of the brain, there are internal structures consisting of cranial nerves,
ventricles, and specialized structures. These structures can also be damaged, resulting in specific
consequences. Damage to one of the 12 cranial nerves can result in pain, tingling, or loss of sensation.
Damage to the pituitary can result in neuroendocrine dysfunction, while damage to the hippocampus can
result in memory deficits. For more details regarding deficits linked to the dysfunction or damage of a
specific region of the brain, please refer to the chapter related to that specific deficit (ex. The ERABI
module on Neuroendocrine Dysfunction discusses damage to both the anterior and posterior pituitary).
Figures 1.2 and 1.3 show the neuroanatomy of the 12 cranial nerves as well as the internal specialized
structures of the brain for reference.
10
(Original image attribution goes to Patrick J. Lynch; and was taken from https://my-
ms.org/anatomy_brain_part4.htm)
Figure 1.2 Ventral view of the brain with each of the 12 cranial nerves identified along with their primary
functions.
11
Figure 1.3 Sagittal view of the brain showing the internal structures of the brain including the ventricle,
hypothalamus, and pituitary gland.
When discussing primary and secondary injuries, the primary injury refers to the damage caused as a
direct result of the ABI; this can be the forces at impact, vascular injury (Subarachnoid hemorrhage; SAH),
benign tumors, cortical disruption or other aetiologies. Secondary injuries encompass the evolution of the
primary injury to include additional damage. Four categories of secondary injuries have been described
by Kochanek et al. (2013). The first three categories are independent and include 1) “ischemia,
excitotoxicity, energy failure”, 2) secondary cerebral swelling, and 3) axonal injury, with the fourth
category of inflammation and regeneration, contributing to the evolution of other secondary injury
categories. The most common resultants from secondary injury are increased intracranial pressure
(discussed in Module 16 of ERABI) and excitotoxicity leading to eventual apoptosis cascades (Kochanek,
2013). Regardless of the severity of the ABI both primary and secondary injuries can play a significant role
in determining the outcome of an individual with an ABI.
1. Diffuse axonal injury (DAI) is seen exclusively following TBI and results from acceleration-
deceleration and rotational forces associated with a high-velocity impact (i.e. motor vehicle
accidents).
2. Physical shearing of axons results in hemorrhage, tissue tears, axonal swelling and the formation of
axonal bulbs acutely. Subacute clusters of microglia and macrophages are seen. Chronically
Wallerian degeneration occurs.
3. DAI can be responsible for the initial loss of consciousness seen in acute TBI.
4. Damage is most often seen within midline structures and at interfaces between gray and white
matter. In particular, the corpus callosum, the parasagittal white matter, the interventricular
septum, the walls of the third ventricle and the brainstem, in particular the midbrain and the pons.
Figure 1.4 Two scans showing diffuse axonal injuries in the subcortical regions.
13
DAI is the distinguishing feature of traumatic brain injury due to high-speed accidents. The predominant
causes of DAI include:
1. High-speed motor vehicle collisions above 15 mph or 24 km/h.
2. Shaken baby syndrome.
3. High-speed collisions in sports (i.e. football, hockey, soccer, rugby) (see figure 1.5).
Rotational forces can cause diffuse tearing of neural processes and blood vessels throughout the white
matter resulting in diffuse axonal injury (Figure 1.5). Hemorrhagic changes involving the midline
structures are often associated with rotational acceleration and tend to involve the parasagittal white
matter, corpus callosum, structures in the walls of the 3rd ventricle and striatum (basal ganglia) (Goldberg,
2001). Strain tends to be concentrated at the interfaces between gray and white matter, at the midbrain
juncture between the brainstem and diencephalon, and at the juncture between the corpus callosum and
the cerebral hemispheres (Goldberg, 2001).
Figure 1.5 Image showing the axonal tearing and shearing that can occur as a result of rotational forces
on the cranium during a traumatic brain injury.
14
Q4. Describe some of the clinical features seen following diffuse axonal injuries.
1. Rostral brain stem involvement results in initial loss of consciousness, poor attention and
concentration.
2. Corticospinal tract involvement results in hemiparesis.
3. Shearing of the grey-white matter junction results in slowed mental processing and fatigue.
4. Cerebellar peduncle involvement results in ataxia.
5. Brainstem injury involvement results in dysarthria and dysphagia.
Given the breadth of symptoms that can result as a consequence of DAI, specific considerations must be
made in order to compensate for deficits in an appropriate manner during early recovery.
Q5. How does diffuse axonal injury impact recovery and rehabilitation?
1. Disrupted connections between nerves results in slowed mental processing, fatigue, poor attention
and concentration.
2. Rehabilitation must be organized in a manner that compensates for these difficulties such as by
providing a structured environment.
3. Physical and cognitive stamina may be reduced and proper pacing will need to be implemented.
4. Poor attention combined with memory difficulties and behavioural concerns may require attendant
care.
Q6. What are cerebral contusions? Where do they tend to occur most often?
Figure 1.6 Example of a coup-contrecoup injury that might occur where an individual strikes their head
twice on opposite poles of the cranium.
Cortical contusions are quite common following traumatic brain injury and in some cases can be quite
involved, extending through the cortex and into the subcortical white matter (Burke & Ordia, 2000).
Cortical contusions tend to occur in characteristic areas, in part because of the movement of the brain in
the skull with acceleration-deceleration and rotational forces and because of the location of bony
protrusions where the brain can strike inside the skull (Burke & Ordia, 2000). One common combination
of contusions is the coup-contrecoup injury (Figure 1.6). In this instance the patient strikes the front of
their head and the brain accelerates forward in the direction of the impact striking the skull. The brain
then rebounds in the opposite direction and strikes the back of the skull, creating the countercoup injury.
Contusions usually occur bilaterally in the frontal poles, the anterior tips of the temporal lobes and in
some cases the lateral parts of the temporal lobes and the occipital regions (Burke & Ordia, 2000).
Intracranial Hemorrhages
Figure 1.7 An illustration and two scans which exemplify both the nature of epidural hematomas as well
as their observed pathology.
Epidural hematomas (EDH) are often the result of an impact to the head that causes disruption of the
middle meningeal artery and/or its branches (Burke & Ordia, 2000). Less commonly an EDH can occur due
to a dural venous sinus injury. Blood collects between the dura and the skull. On imaging, an EDH has a
characteristic “lens-shaped” appearance that crosses the midline but does not cross the skull’s suture
lines. Symptom presentation can be delayed and may only become apparent 5 days post injury (Zasler et
al., 2007). However, if the EDH is related to an arterial laceration, it may evolve quickly resulting in rapid
deterioration and even death (Burke & Ordia, 2000). If treated promptly the underlying brain injury may
not be that severe (Burke & Ordia, 2000). Signs of an EDH include a decreased level of consciousness or
“clouding”. If lesions occur in the frontal lobe, onset of symptoms may be slow and vague (Zasler et al.,
2007). Patients must be monitored carefully.
1. Prognosis is poor with a high mortality because of the severity of the underlying brain injury
SDH are common in severe brain trauma and often occur in individuals who are on blood thinners or who
are older. SDH occur due to injuries of the cortical bridging veins or rarely the pial artery (Burke & Ordia,
2000). On imaging, a SDH has a crescent shape that does not cross the midline (it is confined by dural
reflections) but can cross skull suture lines. Due to the severity of injury to the underlying brain tissue, the
prognosis is poor and mortality rates are high.
17
Figure 1.8 An illustration and a scan which exemplifies both the nature and the observed pathology of
subdural hematomas.
Figure 1.9 An illustration and scan showing the nature and pathology of intracerebral hemorrhages.
Intracerebral haemorrhages may result from ruptured blood vessels in association with a penetrating or
non-penetrating head injury. Although they often occur in the frontal or temporal lobes, they may also be
found in the cerebellum and brain stem.
18
The subarachnoid space is located between the arachnoid membrane and the pia mater that covers the
surface of the brain. This space contains CSF, originating from the choroid plexus. The CSF flows from the
ventricular system to the basal cisterns, and to the surface of the brain. Small arachnoid granulations
within the dural venous sinuses absorb the CSF.
Intraventricular hemorrhage (IVH) can be classified as primary or secondary. Primary IVH occurs due to
injury of the supependymal veins surrounding the ventricles. Secondary IVH occurs due to extension of
adjacent SAH or Intracerebral haemorrhage. On CT, IVH appears bright and is often layered within the
dependent portions of the ventricles. IVH can obstruct cerebrospinal fluid flow resulting in hydrocephalus.
1. Consciousness is a function of the ascending reticular activating system (RAS) and the cerebral
cortex
2. The cell bodies of the RAS are located in the central reticular core of the upper brainstem (primarily
midbrain) and connect to the cerebral cortex via thalamic and extra-thalamic projections.
19
Consciousness can be defined as an awareness of self, and one’s environment (Plum & Posner, 1982).
Both arousal and awareness are required to be in a conscious state. ABI can significantly impact the
brainstem and the cortex which are responsible for these processes. Severe injuries result in disorders of
consciousness (DOC) defined as a coma, a vegetative state (VS) (Ashwal et al., 1994), or a minimally
conscious state (MCS) (Giacino et al., 2002). The American Academy of Neurology establishes a ≥28 day
loss of consciousness in order to diagnose a DOC. To confirm a diagnoses of either a VS or a MCS the
American Academy of Neurology Guidelines on Disorders of Consciousness (2018) state that
electromyography, evoked potentials, and functional MRIs are able to discriminate between a MCS and
VS and assist in the diagnosis of DOC. According to the American Academy of Neurology only 35% of
individuals with a DOC recover consciousness within the first three months, however this increases to 67%
of individuals recovering consciousness six months post-injury (Giaicino et al., 2018).
Click here to see the AAN Practice Guidelines for Disorders of Consciousness
The criteria for diagnosing a coma has remained relatively unchanged since first described by Plum and
Posner in 1982. The key features of a coma include a) the complete loss of spontaneous or stimulus
induced arousal, b) no detectible sleep/wake cycles on electroencephalography (EEG), c) no evidence of
intentional motor activity such as those in response to a command, d) no language ability, e) eyes remain
closed, even in the presence of adverse stimuli.
1. Loss of capacity to interact with the environment despite the preserved potential for spontaneous
or stimulus-induced arousal.
VS occurs when the capacity for stimulus-induced or spontaneous arousal is maintained while awareness
surrounding oneself or one’s environment is lost (Giacino, 2013). A diagnosis of VS is made with the re-
emergence of spontaneous eye opening, and the loss of intentional behavioral responses, as well as the
ability to engage in language expression or comprehension. A VS lasting more than one month can be
described as a persistent vegetative state.
1. Severely altered consciousness with minimal but definite behavioural evidence of self- or
environmental awareness (Giacino, 2013).
1. Sleep-wake cycles.
2. The patient demonstrates minimal evidence of self- and/or environmental-awareness.
3. Purposeful movements are reproducible but remain inconsistent: Simple command following, object
manipulation, and gestural or verbal yes/no responses.
4. Eyes will open spontaneously. The patient may also show visual fixation, smooth pursuit tracking
and emotional or motor behaviours contingent upon specific eliciting stimuli (ie. patient may
respond best to voices of family members).
MCS typically reflect a period of recovery from comas or vegetative state. In a post-mortem analysis of
those with TBI and confirmed MCS, typical lesions were found to be grade 2 or 3 axonal injuries (Jennett
et al., 2001). To diagnosis a MCS one or more of the following behaviors must be present and reproducible
(Zasler et al., 2007):
Emergence from a MCS is typically identified by functional object use as well as increased interactive
communication. These represent the criteria to recover from a MCS as both features require widely
distributed cortical connectivity. Socially, these behaviors also represent a significant increase in personal
autonomy, and therefore are used to determine if an individual has left a MCS.
21
1. PTA is defined as the “immediate and dramatic loss of memory that TBI can cause.”
2. PTA is the period during which individuals with a TBI “are unable to effectively encode and retain
new information and experiences.”
3. PTA varies in duration from a few moments to a few months, either retroactively, proactively, or
both.
4. Resolution of PTA clinically correlates with the period when incorporation of ongoing daily events
occurs in the working memory.
If an individual has post-traumatic amnesia they may appear confused, lack self-awareness of their
injuries, and even be agitated or combative. Individuals may even seem alert during the acute phases of
their injury and engage normally, however, later they may report that they were unconscious for this
period of time and have no memory of it (Eslinger, 2013).
Table 1.7 The relationship of PTA to likely outcomes in individuals with ABI.
Duration of PTA Likely Outcome
1 day or less Expect quick and full recovery with appropriate management (a few patients
may show persisting disability)
More than 1 day and Recovery period is more prolonged lasting weeks or months. However, for
less than 1 week most patients full recovery is possible with good management.
1-2 weeks Recovery occurs over many months. Many patients will be left with residual
problems even after the recovery process has ended, but one can be
reasonably optimistic about functional recovery with good management.
2-4 weeks Process of recovery is very prolonged – 1 year or longer is not unusual.
Permanent deficits are likely. There must be increasing pessimism about
functional recovery when PTA reaches these lengths.
More than 4 weeks Permanent deficits, indeed significant disability, are now certain. It is not just
a matter of recovery but of long-term retraining and management.
(Adapted from Brooks DN and McKinlay WW, Evidence and Quantification in Head Injury: Seminar notes.
Unpublished material, 1989).
PTA gradually improves over time in the majority of cases. The Galveston Orientation and Amnesia Test
(GOAT) is typically used to diagnose and track PTA (Eslinger, 2013). Overall, researchers conclude that
recovery can be predicted from early daily screening and the GOAT can be a moderately strong predictor
of length of stay, functional independence, and short-term memory (Eslinger, 2013).
22
Description
Advantages
1. The GOAT provides an objective rating of early cognitive recovery eliminating the need for
sometimes ambiguous terminology used to describe mental status, such as “confused” (Levin et al.,
1979).
2. Due to its design, the scale has been shown to be useful for assessing patients with a wide range of
cognitive impairments (Salter et al., 2008).
3. Can be used to guide timing of neuropsychological testing which should not be attempted until
GOAT score consistently >70.
Limitations
1. The standard GOAT response format makes administration difficult with nonverbal patients (Novack
et al., 2000). It is important to note that A-GOAT has been developed for use in aphasic patients but
requires further evaluation.
2. The requirement for oral or written expression may result in penalizing patients who are
experiencing deficits of expression but not in orientation or in the retrieval or consolidation of
memory (Jain et al., 2000).
3. While the GOAT does contain items intended to provide an assessment of memory, it is primarily a
measure of orientation. Eight of the 10 GOAT items evaluate orientation while only two examine
memory (Forrester et al., 1994).
The GOAT provides an objective assessment with a standardized cut-off for the presence of PTA. However,
in its original form, the GOAT is not well suited to assessment of patients with aphasia. It may be too
lengthy for a simple, repeated bedside assessment of mental status. However, it is freely available and
can be used by any healthcare professional (Salter et al., 2008).
Assessment consists of 10 items regarding orientation to person (name, address & birthdate), place
(city/town and building they are in) and time (current time, date, month, year & date of hospital
admission) as well as memory of events both after and prior to the injury (Bode et al., 2000). Oral
questions are posed directly to the patient who may respond either orally or in writing (Jain et al., 2000;
Levin et al., 1979). Error points are awarded for each incorrect response and are summed and deducted
from 100 to arrive at the total score. Both the scale and instructions for assigning error points are available
in Levin et al. (1979).
The GOAT was intended to evaluate orientation to time, place and person and to provide an estimation
of the intervals prior to and following the injury for which there is no recall (Levin et al., 1979). The
duration of PTA is defined as the period following coma in which the GOAT score is 75 or less (Levin et al.,
23
1979). PTA is considered to have ended if a score greater than 75 is achieved on 2 consecutive
administrations (Novack et al., 2000; Wade, 1992; Zafonte et al., 1997). In the initial standardization
study of Levin et al. (1979) using patients with mild head injury as a reference group, it was determined
that a score of 75 represented a level achieved by 92% of the standardization group. No patients with
mild head injury scored less than 65 on the GOAT. Scores between 66 and 75 are considered borderline-
abnormal while scores above 75 fall into the range considered normal within the reference group (Levin
et al., 1979; van Baalen et al., 2003).
24
1.9 References
Ashman, T. A., Cantor, J. B., Gordon, W. A., Sacks, A., Spielman, L., Egan, M., & Hibbard, M. R. (2008). A
comparison of cognitive functioning in older adults with and without traumatic brain injury. J
Head Trauma Rehabil, 23(3), 139-148.
Ashwal, S., Cranford, R., Bernat, J. L., Celesia, G., The Multi-Society Task Force on, P. V. S., & Multi-
Society Task Force on, P. V. S. (1994). Medical Aspects of the Persistent Vegetative State. The
New England Journal of Medicine, 330(21), 1499-1508.
Beca, J., Cox, P. N., Taylor, M. J., Bohn, D., Butt, W., Logan, W. J., Rutka, J. T., & BarkerG. (1995).
Somatosensory evoked potentials for prediction of outcome in acute severe brain injury. J
Pediatr, 126(1), 44-49.
Bhatty, G. B., & Kapoor, N. (1993). The Glasgow Coma Scale: a mathematical critique. Acta Neurochir
(Wien), 120(3-4), 132-135.
Bode, R. K., Heinemann, A. W., & Semik, P. (2000). Measurement properties of the Galveston
Orientation and Amnesia Test (GOAT) and improvement patterns during inpatient rehabilitation.
J Head Trauma Rehabil, 15(1), 637-655.
Burke, D., & Ordia, J. I. (2000). Pathophysiology of Traumatic Brain Injury. In N. S. Woo BH (Ed.), The
Rehabilitation of People with Traumatic Brain Injury (1st ed., pp. 19-34). Boston MA: Boston
Medical Center.
Campbell, M. (2000). Rehabilitation for traumatic brain injury: physical therapy practice in context:
Churchill Livingstone.
Chan, V., Zagorski, B., Parsons, D., & Colantonio, A. (2013a). Older adults with acquired brain injury: a
population based study. BMC Geriatr, 13, 97.
Chan, V., Zagorski, B., Parsons, D., & Colantonio, A. (2013b). Older adults with acquired brain injury:
Functional independence measures after inpatient rehabilitation. Archives of physical medicine
and rehabilitation, 94 (10), e9.
Chan, V., Zagorski, B., Parsons, D., & Colantonio, A. (2013c). Older adults with acquired brain injury:
outcomes after inpatient rehabilitation. Canadian Journal on Aging, 32(3), 278-286.
Choi, S. C., Barnes, T. Y., Bullock, R., Germanson, T. A., Marmarou, A., & Young, H. F. (1994). Temporal
profile of outcomes in severe head injury. J Neurosurg, 81(2), 169-173.
Choi, S. C., Narayan, R. K., Anderson, R. L., & Ward, J. D. (1988). Enhanced specificity of prognosis in
severe head injury. J Neurosurg, 69(3), 381-385.
Colantonio, A., McVittie, D., Lewko, J., & Yin, J. (2009). Traumatic brain injuries in the construction
industry. Brain Inj, 23(11), 873-878.
25
Colantonio, A., Saverino, C., Zagorski, B., Swaine, B., Lewko, J., Jaglal, S., & Vernich, L. (2010).
Hospitalizations and emergency department visits for TBI in Ontario. Can J Neurol Sci, 37(6),
783-790.
Eslinger, P. J., Zappala, G., Chakara, F., Barrett, A.M. . (2013). Cognitive Impairments. In K. D. Zasler ND,
Zafonte RD. (Ed.), Brain Injury Medicine: Principles and Practice (2nd ed., pp. 990-1001). New
York: Demos Medical Publishing.
Faul, M., Xu, L., Wald, M., & Coronado, V. (2010). Traumatic Brain Injury in the United States. Centers for
Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Injury Prevention and Control. Atlanta, GA.
Fearnside, M. R., Cook, R. J., McDougall, P., & McNeil, R. J. (1993). The Westmead Head Injury Project
outcome in severe head injury. A comparative analysis of pre-hospital, clinical and CT variables.
Br J Neurosurg, 7(3), 267-279.
Forrester, G., Encel, J., & Geffen, G. (1994). Measuring post-traumatic amnesia (PTA): an historical
review. Brain Inj, 8(2), 175-184.
Giacino, J. T., Ashwal, S., Childs, N., Cranford, R., Jennett, B., Katz, D. I., Kelly, J. P., Rosenberg, J. H.,
Whyte, J., Zafonte, R. D., & Zasler, N. D. (2002). The minimally conscious state: definition and
diagnostic criteria. Neurology, 58(3), 349-353.
Giacino, J. T., Katz, Douglas I., Garber, K., Schiff, Nicolas. (2013). Assessment and Rehabilitative
Management of Indiviuals with Disorders of Consciousness In K. D. Zasler ND, Zafonte RD. (Ed.),
Brain Injury Medicine: Principles and Practice (2nd ed., pp. 517-535). New York: Demos Medical
Publishing, LLC
Giaicino, J. T., Katz, D. I., Schiff, N. D., Whyte, J., Ashman, E. J., Ashwal, S., Barbano, R., Hammond, F.,
Laureys, S., Ling, G. S. F., Nakase-Richardson, R., Seel, R. T., Yablon, S., Getchius, T. S. D.,
Gronseth, G. S., & Armstrong, M. J. (2018). Report of the Guideline Development, Dissemination,
and Implementation Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology; the American
Congress of Rehabilitation Medicine; and the National Institute on Disability, Independent Living,
and Rehabilitation Research.
Goldberg, G. (2001). Mild Traumatic Brain Injury and Concussion. Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation:
State of the Art Reviews, 15, 363-398.
Greenwald, B. D., Burnett, D. M., & Miller, M. A. (2003). Congenital and acquired brain injury. 1. Brain
injury: epidemiology and pathophysiology. Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation, 84(3
Suppl 1), S3-7.
Hanney, S. R., Castle-Clarke, S., Grant, J., Guthrie, S., Henshall, C., Mestre-Ferrandiz, J., Pistollato, M.,
Pollitt, A., Sussex, J., & Wooding, S. (2015). How long does biomedical research take? Studying
the time taken between biomedical and health research and its translation into products, policy,
and practice. Health research policy and systems, 13(1), 1.
Jain, N. S., Layton, B. S., & Murray, P. K. (2000). Are aphasic patients who fail the GOAT in PTA? A
modified Galveston Orientation and Amnesia Test for persons with aphasia. Clin Neuropsychol,
14(1), 13-17.
26
Jennett, B., Adams, J. H., Murray, L. S., & Graham, D. I. (2001). Neuropathology in vegetative and
severely disabled patients after head injury. Neurology, 56(4), 486-490.
Kochanek, P. M., Clark, R.S.B., Jenkins, L.W. . (2013). Pathobiology of Secondary Brain Injury. In K. D.
Zasler ND, Zafonte RD. (Ed.), Brain Injury Medicine: Princples and Practice (2nd ed., pp. 148-161).
New York: Demos Medical Publishing.
Levin, H. S., O'Donnell, V. M., & Grossman, R. G. (1979). The Galveston Orientation and Amnesia Test. A
practical scale to assess cognition after head injury. J Nerv Ment Dis, 167(11), 675-684.
Marquez de la Plata, C. D., Hart, T., Hammond, F. M., Frol, A. B., Hudak, A., Harper, C. R., O'Neil-Pirozzi,
T. M., Whyte, J., Carlile, M., & Diaz-Arrastia, R. (2008). Impact of age on long-term recovery from
traumatic brain injury. Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation, 89(5), 896-903.
Michaud, L. J., Rivara, F. P., Grady, M. S., & Reay, D. T. (1992). Predictors of survival and severity of
disability after severe brain injury in children. Neurosurgery, 31(2), 254-264.
Morris, Z. S., Wooding, S., & Grant, J. (2011). The answer is 17 years, what is the question:
understanding time lags in translational research. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine,
104(12), 510-520.
Mosenthal, A. C., Lavery, R. F., Addis, M., Kaul, S., Ross, S., Marburger, R., Deitch, E. A., & Livingston, D.
H. (2002). Isolated traumatic brain injury: age is an independent predictor of mortality and early
outcome. Journal of Trauma-Injury, Infection, and Critical Care, 52(5), 907-911.
Munro, C. L., & Savel, R. H. (2016). Narrowing the 17-year research to practice gap: AACN.
Murdoch, B., & Theodoros, D. (2001). Introduction: Epidemiology, neuropathophysiology and medical
aspects of traumatic brain injury. San Diego, CA: Singular/Thomson Learning.
Narayan, R. K., Greenberg, R. P., Miller, J. D., Enas, G. G., Choi, S. C., Kishore, P. R., Selhorst, J. B., Lutz, H.
A., 3rd, & Becker, D. P. (1981). Improved confidence of outcome prediction in severe head
injury. A comparative analysis of the clinical examination, multimodality evoked potentials, CT
scanning, and intracranial pressure. J Neurosurg, 54(6), 751-762.
Novack, T. A., Dowler, R. N., Bush, B. A., Glen, T., & Schneider, J. J. (2000). Validity of the Orientation Log,
relative to the Galveston Orientation and Amnesia Test. J Head Trauma Rehabil, 15(3), 957-961.
Pickett, W., Ardern, C., & Brison, R. J. (2001). A population-based study of potential brain injuries
requiring emergency care. Cmaj, 165(3), 288-292.
Plum, F., & Posner, J. B. (1982). The Diagnosis of Stupor and Coma. Philadelphia: F.A. Davis Co. .
Roozenbeek, B., Maas, A. I., & Menon, D. K. (2013). Changing patterns in the epidemiology of traumatic
brain injury. Nat Rev Neurol, 9(4), 231-236.
Russell, W. R. (1932). Cerebral Involvement in Head Injury a Study Based on the Examination of Two-
hundred Cases. Brain, 55(4), 549-603.
27
Salter, K., Jutai, J., & Teasell, R. (2008). Assessment of Outcomes Following Acquired/Traumatic Brain
Injury. In M. S. Teasell R, Cullen N., Bayley M (Ed.), Evidence-Based Review of Moderate to
Severe Acquired Brain Injury (4th ed., pp. 1-73). London, ON: Ontario Neurotrauma Foundation.
Senathi-Raja, D., Ponsford, J., & Schönberger, M. (2010). Impact of age on long-term cognitive function
after traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology, 24(3), 336.
Teasdale, G., & Jennett, B. (1974). Assessment of coma and impaired consciousness: a practical scale.
The Lancet, 304(7872), 81-84.
Teasdale, G., & Jennett, B. (1976). Assessment and prognosis of coma after head injury. Acta
neurochirurgica, 34(1-4), 45-55.
Thurman, D., & Guerrero, J. (1999). Trends in hospitalization associated with traumatic brain injury.
Jama, 282(10), 954-957.
Toronto Acquired Brain Injury Network. (2005). Definition of acquired brain injury. Retrieved from
http://www.abinetwork.ca/downloads/binder-b3.pdf
van Baalen, B., Odding, E., Maas, A. I., Ribbers, G. M., Bergen, M. P., & Stam, H. J. (2003). Traumatic
brain injury: classification of initial severity and determination of functional outcome. Disabil
Rehabil, 25(1), 9-18.
Wade, D. T. (1992). Measurement in neurological rehabilitation. Curr Opin Neurol Neurosurg, 5(5), 682-
686.
Zafonte, R. D., Mann, N. R., Millis, S. R., Black, K. L., Wood, D. L., & Hammond, F. (1997). Posttraumatic
amnesia: its relation to functional outcome. Arch Phys Med Rehabil, 78(10), 1103-1106.
Zaloshnja, E., Miller, T., Langlois, J. A., & Selassie, A. W. (2008). Prevalence of long-term disability from
traumatic brain injury in the civilian population of the United States, 2005. The Journal of head
trauma rehabilitation, 23(6), 394-400.
Zasler, N. D., Katz, D. I., & Zafonte, R. D. (2007). Brain Injury Medicine. New York: Demos Medical
Publishing.