Biofluorescence in Nocturnal Crepuscular Mammals
Biofluorescence in Nocturnal Crepuscular Mammals
Biofluorescence in Nocturnal Crepuscular Mammals
Tanyut Huidrom
15 May 2021
Glaucomys spp.1, the zoological community has seen a rise in interest towards this potentially
widespread “invisible” trait, its ecological function, and its presence in phylogenetically far
removed species fulfilling similar ecological niches. Researchers have discovered that quite a
few nocturnal crepuscular mammals showcase what appears to be innate biofluorescence. Recent
opossums (Didelphidae)3, New World placental flying squirrels (Glaucomys spp.), and most
recently, springhares (Pedetidae)4, being the first documented case of biofluorescence in an Old
World placental mammal. We will be reviewing the data available with regards to these four
species in an attempt to discern the role played by biofluorescence in sexual selection, predator
avoidance and foraging. We will also look at other intra and interspecific interactions occurring
Biofluorescence is the process by which short wavelengths of light are absorbed and
reemitted as longer wavelengths of light. It has been observed previously in a wide range of
invertebrates, fish, reptiles, and amphibians. In 1985, biofluorescence was discovered within
an abnormality and regarded as such until similar cases of biofluorescence were discovered in
2
Glaucomys spp., O. anatinus, and Pedetidae. These four animals inhabit 4 different continents,
live in environments which vary quite a bit, and are phylogenetically diverse. The common trait
between all four species is that they are nocturnal-crepuscular mammals. Being active in low
In the case of O. anatinus, three museum specimens were examined, captured from Tasmania,
New South Wales and Australia. All three specimens biofluoresced in response to UV light. The
male and female specimens biofluoresced in the same patterns and intensity. As the specimens
varied in age, and the fact that the specimens would have undergone washing in preparation for
being displayed, it is safe to assume that the biofluorescence was an intrinsic trait of the
specimens. The dorsal and ventral pelage of the specimens were green to cyan under UV light, in
contrast to brown under normal conditions. Using fluorescence spectroscopy it was determined
that the fur was absorbing short wavelengths around 200 to 400 nm and re-emitting visible light
A formal investigation was conducted following a chance encounter in the wild during which
researchers flashed a wild Glaucomys volans with a UV flashlight during a forest survey and
subsequently noted that the individual was fluorescing pink. Following the investigation it was
discovered that pink UV fluorescence was detected in 108 out of 109 museum specimens
studied. The ventral body surface of the animal, which appears white under normal lighting,
appeared pink under UV light. The specimens studied showcased substantial variation in terms
3
of the intensity and pattern, however, no meaningful relationships between species, month, sex,
The only study involving live specimens was conducted for springhares (Pedetidae). 14 museum
specimens ranging from Angola, Botswana, Kenya and Tanzania were studied, along with five
captive-bred specimens. Following the methods of Anich et al, they studied the intensity and
pattern of biofluorescence in the springhare specimens. The trait was determined not to be an
environmental one as the specimens came from across a 58 year period, and hailed from seven
separate locations. Museum specimens were also subject to heavy washing. Male and female
specimens fluoresced within the same regions with the same intensity. Intensity of fluorescence
tended to be reduced with older specimens, indicating a potential for degradation with time.
The methods used to determine biofluorescence were similar, photographs were taken of the
animals by applying a filter to the lens that blocked wavelengths below a certain limit so the
higher wavelengths (those emitted by the biofluorescent tissues) could be seen better. While the
ecological function of biofluorescence still remains uncertain, it has been hypothesized that the
trait may affect interspecific interactions5, along with mate-selection6 and intraspecific
communication7. As these animals are active in low light conditions, it is possible that it may
From the specimens studied, we can determine that biofluorescence as a trait is not sexually
dimorphic. The pattern and intensity of the fluorescent region does not appear to vary with sex.
4
Thus, it is unlikely that the trait plays a role in mate selection. While the trait may still be used
for intraspecific communication in other species, we know that this most likely isn’t the case for
the platypus. As the species navigates primarily by using mechanoreception and electroreception,
without much use of their eyes, it is unlikely that biofluorescence is significant in intraspecific
mammals, it is likely that its primary function is related to this behaviour and environment.8
Springhares are rather distant relatives to all known biofluorescent rodents within Sciuridae, and
also live in an environment that varies greatly from previously known examples of
biofluorescent nocturnal crepuscular mammals. They are mainly solitary individuals which
shelter in individual burrows and forage short-grass environments nocturnally. They also aren’t
dependent upon cover for predator avoidance, and benefit from the clear line-of-sight their
foraging environment provides, as they rely on early predator detection in order to escape using
their strong hind legs.4 Thus, it is likely that biofluorescence in springhares plays an interspecific
role.
It is possible that the absorbance of UV light and subsequent fluorescence of longer wavelengths
may reduce the visibility of the organism to UV-sensitive predators. During the hours
immediately after sunset and before sunrise, light environments tend to be dominated by UV
wavelengths.9 These wavelengths are present during daytime, but most likely don’t factor in
much as they are dominated by visible wavelengths. Glaucomys spp and O. anatinus are known
to be active during twilight. In the case of Glaucomys, it is possible that the biofluorescent
plumage on their ventral side allows them to blend into the backdrop of the sky better in order to
5
avoid predator detection. Sunlight present during these hours has a uniform intensity and colour,
which generally would allow for predators to be able to spot silhouettes of these organisms
relatively easily. However, by adapting to fluoresce similar wavelengths, they may be able to
camouflage, which has been observed in deep sea fish. By luminescing at a similar intensity and
wavelength to the light coming in from the top, they are able to blur their silhouettes from
predators lurking below.10 This would also explain the variation in wavelength emission
witnessed amongst the group. Further research in the wild habitats of these animals would be
Works Cited
1. Allison M Kohler, Erik R Olson, Jonathan G Martin, Paula Spaeth Anich, Ultraviolet fluorescence
discovered in New World flying squirrels (Glaucomys), Journal of Mammalogy, Volume 100, Issue 1, 28
6
2. Anich, Paula Spaeth, Anthony, Sharon, Carlson, Michaela, Gunnelson, Adam, Kohler, Allison M., Martin,
Jonathan G. and Olson, Erik R.. "Biofluorescence in the platypus (Ornithorhynchus anatinus)" Mammalia,
3. Pine, Ronald, Rice. J, Butcher. J, Tank. D, Greenhall. A (1985). Labile pigments and fluorescent pelage in
4. Olson, E.R., Carlson, M.R., Ramanujam, V.M.S. et al. Vivid biofluorescence discovered in the nocturnal
5. Olofsson, M., Vallin, A., Jakobsson, S. & Wiklund, C. Marginal eyespots on butterfly wings deflect bird
attacks under low light intensities with UV wavelengths. PLoS ONE 5, e10798.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0010798 (2010).
6. Pearn, S. M., Bennett, A. T. & Cuthill, I. C. Ultraviolet vision, fluorescence and mate choice in a parrot, the
https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2001.1813 (2001).
7. Honkavaara, J., Koivula, M., Korpimaki, E., Siitari, H. & Viitala, J. Ultraviolet vision and foraging in
8. Cronin T. W., and M. J.Bok. 2016. Photoreception and vision in the ultraviolet. Journal of Experimental
Biology 291:2790–2801.
9. Johnsen S., et al. 2006. Crepuscular and nocturnal illumination and its effects on color perception by the
10. Young, R.E,.; Roper, C.F.E. (1977). "Intensity Regulation of Bioluminescence during Countershading in
1251214.