0% found this document useful (0 votes)
92 views23 pages

Piaget Theory

Jean Piaget's theory of cognitive development proposes four stages of development: sensorimotor, preoperational, concrete operational, and formal operational. Each stage involves different types of thinking and problem solving abilities. The stages follow a universal sequence but children progress through them at different rates.

Uploaded by

Ogechi Ajaa
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as DOCX, PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
92 views23 pages

Piaget Theory

Jean Piaget's theory of cognitive development proposes four stages of development: sensorimotor, preoperational, concrete operational, and formal operational. Each stage involves different types of thinking and problem solving abilities. The stages follow a universal sequence but children progress through them at different rates.

Uploaded by

Ogechi Ajaa
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as DOCX, PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 23

Piaget's Stages of Cognitive

Development
Jean Piaget's theory of cognitive development suggests that intelligence changes
as children grow. A child's cognitive development is not just about acquiring
knowledge, the child has to develop or construct a mental model of the world.
Cognitive development occurs through the interaction of innate capacities and
environmental events, and children pass through a series of stages.
Piaget's theory of cognitive development proposes 4 stages of development.

 sensorimotor stage: birth to 2 years


 preoperational stage: 2 to 7 years
 concrete operational stage: 7 to 11 years
 formal operational stage: ages 12 and up

The sequence of the stages is universal across cultures and follow the same
invariant (unchanging) order. All children go through the same stages in the
same order (but not all at the same rate).

How Piaget Developed the Theory


Piaget was employed at the Binet Institute in the 1920s, where his job was to
develop French versions of questions on English intelligence tests. He became
intrigued with the reasons children gave for their wrong answers to the questions
that required logical thinking.
He believed that these incorrect answers revealed important differences between
the thinking of adults and children.
Piaget branched out on his own with a new set of assumptions about children’s
intelligence:

 Children’s intelligence differs from an adult’s in quality rather than in


quantity. This means that children reason (think) differently from adults
and see the world in different ways.
 Children actively build up their knowledge about the world. They are not
passive creatures waiting for someone to fill their heads with knowledge.

The best way to understand children’s reasoning was to see things from their
point of view. What Piaget wanted to do was not to measure how well children
could count, spell or solve problems as a way of grading their I.Q. What he was
more interested in was the way in which fundamental concepts like the very idea
of number, time, quantity, causality, justice and so on emerged.
Piaget studied children from infancy to adolescence using naturalistic
observation of his own three babies and sometimes controlled observation too.
From these he wrote diary descriptions charting their development.
He also used clinical interviews and observations of older children who were able
to understand questions and hold conversations.

Piaget’s Four Stages


Jean Piaget's theory of cognitive development suggests that children move
through four different stages of intellectual development which reflect the
increasing sophistication of children's thoughts.
Each child goes through the stages in the same order, and child development is
determined by biological maturation and interaction with the environment.
At each stage of development, the child’s thinking is qualitatively different from
the other stages, that is, each stage involves a different type of intelligence.

Stage Age Goal

Sensorimotor Birth to 18-24 months Object permanence

Preoperational 2 to 7 years old Symbolic thought

Concrete operational Ages 7 to 11 years Logical thought

Formal operational Adolescence to adulthood Scientific reasoning

Although no stage can be missed out, there are individual differences in the rate
at which children progress through stages, and some individuals may never attain
the later stages.
Piaget did not claim that a particular stage was reached at a certain age - although
descriptions of the stages often include an indication of the age at which the
average child would reach each stage.

The Sensorimotor Stage


Ages: Birth to 2 Years
The first stage is the sensory motor stage, and during this stage the infant focuses
on physical sensations and on learning to co-ordinate his body.
Major Characteristics and Developmental Changes:

 The infant learns about the world through their senses and through their
actions (moving around and exploring its environment).
 During the sensorimotor stage a range of cognitive abilities develop. These
include: object permanence; self-recognition (the child realises that other
people are separate from them); deferred imitation; and representational
play.
 They relate to the emergence of the general symbolic function, which is the
capacity to represent the world mentally
 At about 8 months the infant will understand the permanence of objects
and that they will still exist even if they can’t see them and the infant will
search for them when they disappear.

During this stage the infant lives in the present. It does not yet have a mental
picture of the world stored in its memory therefore it does not have a sense of
object permanence.
If it cannot see something then it does not exist. This is why you can hide a toy
from an infant, while it watches, but it will not search for the object once it has
gone out of sight.
The main achievement during this stage is object permanence - knowing that an
object still exists, even if it is hidden. It requires the ability to form a mental
representation (i.e., a schema) of the object.
Towards the end of this stage the general symbolic function begins to appear
where children show in their play that they can use one object to stand for
another. Language starts to appear because they realise that words can be
used to represent objects and feelings.
The child begins to be able to store information that it knows about the world,
recall it and label it.
The Preoperational Stage
Ages: 2 - 7 Years
The pre-operational stage is one of Piaget's intellectual development stages. It
takes place between 2 and 7 years. At the beginning of this stage the child does
not use operations, so the thinking is influenced by the way things appear rather
than logical reasoning.
A child cannot conserve which means that the child does not understand that
quantity remains the same even if the appearance changes.
Furthermore, the child is egocentric; he assumes that other people see the world
as he does. This has been shown in the three mountains study.
Major Characteristics and Developmental Changes:

 Toddlers and young children acquire the ability to internally represent the
world through language and mental imagery.
 During this stage, young children can think about things symbolically. This
is the ability to make one thing, such as a word or an object, stand for
something other than itself.
 A child’s thinking is dominated by how the world looks, not how the world
is. It is not yet capable of logical (problem solving) type of thought.
 Moreover, the child has difficulties with class inclusion; he can classify
objects but cannot include objects in sub-sets, which involves classify
objects as belonging to two or more categories simultaneously
 Infants at this stage also demonstrate animism. This is the tendency for the
child to think that non-living objects (such as toys) have life and feelings
like a person’s.

By 2 years, children have made some progress towards detaching their thought
from physical world. However have not yet developed logical (or 'operational')
thought characteristic of later stages.
Thinking is still intuitive (based on subjective judgements about situations) and
egocentric (centred on the child's own view of the world).

The Concrete Operational Stage


Ages: 7 - 11 Years
By the beginning of the concrete operational stage, the child can use operations
( a set of logical rules) so he can conserve quantities, he realises that people see
the world in a different way than he does (decentring) and he has improved in
inclusion tasks. Children still have difficulties with abstract thinking.
Major Characteristics and Developmental Changes:

 During this stage, children begin to thinking logically about concrete


events.
 Children begin to understand the concept of conservation; understanding
that, although things may change in appearance, certain properties remain
the same.
 During this stage, children can mentally reverse things (e.g. picture a ball
of plasticine returning to its original shape).
 During this stage, children also become less egocentric and begin to think
about how other people might think and feel.

The stage is called concrete because children can think logically much more
successfully if they can manipulate real (concrete) materials or pictures of them.
Piaget considered the concrete stage a major turning point in the child's cognitive
development because it marks the beginning of logical or operational thought.
This means the child can work things out internally in their head (rather than
physically try things out in the real world).
Children can conserve number (age 6), mass (age 7), and weight (age 9).
Conservation is the understanding that something stays the same in quantity
even though its appearance changes.
But operational thought only effective here if child asked to reason about
materials that are physically present. Children at this stage will tend to make
mistakes or be overwhelmed when asked to reason about abstract or hypothetical
problems.

The Formal Operational Stage


Ages: 12 and Over
The formal operational period begins at about age 11. As adolescents enter this
stage, they gain the ability to think in an abstract manner, the ability to combine
and classify items in a more sophisticated way, and the capacity for higher-order
reasoning.
Adolescents can think systematically and reason about what might be as well as
what is (not everyone achieves this stage).. This allows them to understand
politics, ethics, and science fiction, as well as to engage in scientific reasoning.
Adolescents can deal with abstract ideas: e.g. they can understand division and
fractions without having to actually divide things up. Solve hypothetical
(imaginary) problems.
Major Characteristics and Developmental Changes:

 Concrete operations are carried out on things whereas formal operations


are carried out on ideas. Formal operational thought is entirely freed from
physical and perceptual constraints.
 During this stage, adolescents can deal with abstract ideas (e.g. no longer
needing to think about slicing up cakes or sharing sweets to understand
division and fractions).
 They can follow the form of an argument without having to think in terms
of specific examples.
 Adolescents can deal with hypothetical problems with many possible
solutions. E.g. if asked ‘What would happen if money were abolished in one
hour’s time? they could speculate about many possible consequences.

From about 12 years children can follow the form of a logical argument without
reference to its content. During this time, people develop the ability to think
about abstract concepts, and logically test hypotheses.
This stage sees emergence of scientific thinking, formulating abstract theories
and hypotheses when faced with a problem.

Piaget's Theory Differs From Others In Several


Ways:
Piaget's (1936, 1950) theory of cognitive development explains how a child
constructs a mental model of the world. He disagreed with the idea that
intelligence was a fixed trait, and regarded cognitive development as a process
which occurs due to biological maturation and interaction with the environment.
Children’s ability to understand, think about and solve problems in the world
develops in a stop-start, discontinuous manner (rather than gradual changes over
time).
▪ It is concerned with children, rather than all learners.
▪ It focuses on development, rather than learning per se, so it does not address
learning of information or specific behaviors.
▪ It proposes discrete stages of development, marked by qualitative differences,
rather than a gradual increase in number and complexity of behaviors, concepts,
ideas, etc.
The goal of the theory is to explain the mechanisms and processes by which the
infant, and then the child, develops into an individual who can reason and think
using hypotheses. 
To Piaget, cognitive development was a progressive reorganization of mental
processes as a result of biological maturation and environmental experience.
Children construct an understanding of the world around them, then experience
discrepancies between what they already know and what they discover in their
environment.

Schemas
Piaget claimed that knowledge cannot simply emerge from sensory experience;
some initial structure is necessary to make sense of the world.
According to Piaget, children are born with a very basic mental structure
(genetically inherited and evolved) on which all subsequent learning and
knowledge are based.
Schemas are the basic building blocks of such cognitive models, and enable us to
form a mental representation of the world.
Piaget (1952, p. 7) defined a schema as: "a cohesive, repeatable action sequence
possessing component actions that are tightly interconnected and governed by a
core meaning."
In more simple terms Piaget called the schema the basic building block of
intelligent behavior – a way of organizing knowledge. Indeed, it is useful to think
of schemas as “units” of knowledge, each relating to one aspect of the world,
including objects, actions, and abstract (i.e., theoretical) concepts.
Wadsworth (2004) suggests that schemata (the plural of schema) be thought of
as 'index cards' filed in the brain, each one telling an individual how to react to
incoming stimuli or information.
When Piaget talked about the development of a person's mental processes, he
was referring to increases in the number and complexity of the schemata that a
person had learned.
When a child's existing schemas are capable of explaining what it can perceive
around it, it is said to be in a state of equilibrium, i.e., a state of cognitive (i.e.,
mental) balance.
Operations are more sophisticated mental structures which allow us to combine
schemas in a logical (reasonable) way. As children grow they can carry out more
complex operations and begin to imagine hypothetical (imaginary) situations.
Apart from the schemas we are born with schemas and operations are learned
through interaction with other people and the environment.
Piaget emphasized the importance of schemas in cognitive development and
described how they were developed or acquired. A schema can be defined as a set
of linked mental representations of the world, which we use both to understand
and to respond to situations. The assumption is that we store these mental
representations and apply them when needed.
Examples of Schemas
A person might have a schema about buying a meal in a restaurant. The schema
is a stored form of the pattern of behavior which includes looking at a menu,
ordering food, eating it and paying the bill. This is an example of a type of schema
called a 'script.' Whenever they are in a restaurant, they retrieve this schema from
memory and apply it to the situation.
The schemas Piaget described tend to be simpler than this - especially those used
by infants. He described how - as a child gets older - his or her schemas become
more numerous and elaborate.
Piaget believed that newborn babies have a small number of innate schemas -
even before they have had many opportunities to experience the world. These
neonatal schemas are the cognitive structures underlying innate reflexes. These
reflexes are genetically programmed into us.
For example, babies have a sucking reflex, which is triggered by something
touching the baby's lips. A baby will suck a nipple, a comforter (dummy), or a
person's finger. Piaget, therefore, assumed that the baby has a 'sucking schema.'
Similarly, the grasping reflex which is elicited when something touches the palm
of a baby's hand, or the rooting reflex, in which a baby will turn its head towards
something which touches its cheek, are innate schemas. Shaking a rattle would be
the combination of two schemas, grasping and shaking.

The Process of Adaptation: How Children Learn


Adaptation is the process by which the child changes its mental models of the
world to match more closely how the world actually is.
When our existing schemas can explain what we perceive around us, we are in a
state of equilibration. However, when we meet a new situation that we cannot
explain it creates disequilibrium, this is an unpleasant sensation which we try to
escape, this gives the motivation for learning.
According to Piaget, reorganization to higher levels of thinking is not
accomplished easily. The child must "rethink" his or her view of the world. An
important step in the process is the experience of cognitive conflict. In other
words, the child becomes aware that he or she holds two contradictory views
about a situation and they both cannot be true. This step is referred to
as disequilibrium.
Jean Piaget (1952; see also Wadsworth, 2004) viewed intellectual growth as a
process of adaptation (adjustment) to the world. This happens through
assimilation, accommodation, and equilibration.
To get back to a state of equilibration we need to modify our existing schemas, to
learn and adapt to the new situation. This is done through the processes
of accommodation and assimilation. This is how our schemas evolve and become
more sophisticated.

Assimilation
Piaget defined assimilation as the cognitive process of fitting new information
into existing cognitive schemas, perceptions, and understanding. Overall beliefs
and understanding of the world do not change as a result of the new information.
Assimilation coccurs when the new experience is not very different form previous
experiences of a particular object or situation we assimilate the new situation by
adding information to a previous schema.
This means that when you are faced with new information, you make sense of
this information by referring to information you already have (information
processed and learned previously) and try to fit the new information into the
information you already have.
For example, a 2-year-old child sees a man who is bald on top of his head and has
long frizzy hair on the sides. To his father’s horror, the toddler shouts “Clown,
clown” (Siegler et al., 2003).
For example, a baby learns to pick up a rattle he or she will then use the same
schema (grasping) to pick up other objects.

Accommodation
Accommodation: when the new experience is very different from what we have
encountered before we need to change our schemas in a very radical way or
create a whole new schema.
Psychologist Jean Piaget defined accommodation as the cognitive process of
revising existing cognitive schemas, perceptions, and understanding so that new
information can be incorporated. This happens when the existing schema
(knowledge) does not work, and needs to be changed to deal with a new object or
situation.
In order to make sense of some new information, you actual adjust information
you already have (schemas you already have, etc.) to make room for this new
information.
For example, a baby tries to use the same schema for grasping to pick up a very
small object. It doesn’t work. The baby then changes the schema by now using the
forefinger and thumb to pick up the object.
Also, a child may have a schema for birds (feathers, flying, etc.) and then they see
a plane, which also flies, but would not fit into their bird schema.
In the “clown” incident, the boy’s father explained to his son that the man was not
a clown and that even though his hair was like a clown’s, he wasn’t wearing a
funny costume and wasn’t doing silly things to make people laugh.
With this new knowledge, the boy was able to change his schema of “clown” and
make this idea fit better to a standard concept of “clown”.

Equilibration
Piaget believed that all human thought seeks order and is uncomfortable with
contradictions and inconsistencies in knowledge structures. In other words, we
seek 'equilibrium' in our cognitive structures.
Equilibrium occurs when a child's schemas can deal with most new information
through assimilation. However, an unpleasant state of disequilibrium occurs
when new information cannot be fitted into existing schemas (assimilation).
Piaget believed that cognitive development did not progress at a steady rate, but
rather in leaps and bounds. Equilibration is the force which drives the learning
process as we do not like to be frustrated and will seek to restore balance by
mastering the new challenge (accommodation).
Once the new information is acquired the process of assimilation with the new
schema will continue until the next time we need to make an adjustment to it.
Equilibration is a regulatory process that maintains a balance between
assimilation and accommodation to facilitate cognitive growth. Think of it this
way: We can't merely assimilate all the time; if we did, we would never learn any
new concepts or principles.
Everything new we encountered would just get put in the same few "slots" we
already had. Neither can we accommodate all the time; if we did, everything we
encountered would seem new; there would be no recurring regularities in our
world. We'd be exhausted by the mental effort!
Applying Piaget's Theory to the Classroom
Think of old black and white films that you’ve seen in which children sat in rows
at desks, with ink wells, would learn by rote, all chanting in unison in response to
questions set by an authoritarian old biddy like Matilda!
Children who were unable to keep up were seen as slacking and would be
punished by variations on the theme of corporal punishment. Yes, it really did
happen and in some parts of the world still does today. Piaget is partly
responsible for the change that occurred in the 1960s and for your relatively
pleasurable and pain free school days!

'Children should be able to do their own experimenting and their own


research. Teachers, of course, can guide them by providing appropriate
materials, but the essential thing is that in order for a child to understand
something, he must construct it himself, he must re-invent it. Every time we
teach a child something, we keep him from inventing it himself. On the other
hand that which we allow him to discover by himself will remain with him
visibly'.
Piaget (1972, p. 27)
Plowden Report
Piaget (1952) did not explicitly relate his theory to education, although later
researchers have explained how features of Piaget's theory can be applied to
teaching and learning.
Piaget has been extremely influential in developing educational policy and
teaching practice. For example, a review of primary education by the UK
government in 1966 was based strongly on Piaget’s theory. The result of this
review led to the publication of the Plowden report (1967).
In the 1960s the Plowden Committee investigated the deficiencies in education
and decided to incorporate many of Piaget’s ideas in to its final report published
in 1967, even though Piaget’s work was not really designed for education. The
report makes three Piaget-associated recommendations:

1. Children should be given individual attention and it should be realised that


they need to be treated differently.
2. Children should only be taught things that they are capable of learning
3. Children mature at different rates and the teacher needs to be aware of the
stage of development of each child so teaching can be tailored to their
individual needs.

'The report's recurring themes are individual learning, flexibility in the


curriculum, the centrality of play in children's learning, the use of the
environment, learning by discovery and the importance of the evaluation of
children's progress - teachers should 'not assume that only what is measurable is
valuable.'
Discovery learning – the idea that children learn best through doing and actively
exploring - was seen as central to the transformation of the primary school
curriculum.

How to teach
Within the classroom learning should be student-centered and accomplished
through active discovery learning. The role of the teacher is to facilitate learning,
rather than direct tuition.
Because Piaget's theory is based upon biological maturation and stages, the
notion of 'readiness' is important. Readiness concerns when certain information
or concepts should be taught. According to Piaget's theory children should not be
taught certain concepts until they have reached the appropriate stage of cognitive
development.
According to Piaget (1958), assimilation and accommodation require an active
learner, not a passive one, because problem-solving skills cannot be taught, they
must be discovered.

Therefore, teachers should encourage the


following within the classroom:
 Educational programmes should be designed to correspond to Piaget's
stages of development. Children in the concrete operational stage should
be given concrete means to learn new concepts e.g. tokens for counting.
 Devising situations that present useful problems, and create disequilibrium
in the child.
 Focus on the process of learning, rather than the end product of it. Instead
of checking if children have the right answer, the teacher should focus on
the student's understanding and the processes they used to get to the
answer.
 Child-centred approach. Learning must be active (discovery learning).
Children should be encouraged to discover for themselves and to interact
with the material instead of being given ready-made knowledge.
 Accepting that children develop at different rate so arrange activities for
individual children or small groups rather than assume that all the children
can cope with a particular activity.
 Using active methods that require rediscovering or reconstructing "truths."
 Using collaborative, as well as individual activities (so children can learn
from each other).

Role of the Teacher


 Evaluate the level of the child's development so suitable tasks can be set.
 Adapt lessons to suit the needs of the individual child (i.e. differentiated
teaching).
 Be aware of the child’s stage of development (testing).
 Teach only when the child is ready. i.e. has the child reached the
appropriate stage.
 Providing support for the "spontaneous research" of the child.
 Using collaborative, as well as individual activities.
 Devising situations that present useful problems, and create disequilibrium
in the child.
Curriculum Development
According to Piaget children cognitive development is determined by a process of
maturation which cannot be altered by tuition so education should be stage-
specific. For example, a child in the concrete operational stage should not be
taught abstract concepts and should be given concrete aid such as tokens to count
with.
According to Piaget children learn through the process of accommodation and
assimilation so the role of the teacher should be to provide opportunities for
these processes to occur such as new material and experiences which challenge
the children’s existing schemas. Furthermore, according to this theory, children
should be encouraged to discover for themselves and to interact with the material
instead of being given ready-made knowledge.
Curricula need to be developed that take into account the age and stage of
thinking of the child. For example there is no point in teaching abstract concepts
such as algebra or atomic structure to children in primary school. Curricula also
need to be sufficiently flexible to allow for variations in ability of different
students of the same age. In Britain the National Curriculum and Key Stages
broadly reflect the stages that Piaget laid down.
For example, egocentricism dominates a child’s thinking in the sensori-motor
and preoperational stages. Piaget would therefore predict that using group
activities would not be appropriate since children are not capable of
understanding the views of others.
However, Smith et al. (1998), point out that some children develop earlier than
Piaget predicted and that by using group work children can learn to appreciate
the views of others in preparation for the concrete operational stage. The national
curriculum emphasises the need for using concrete examples in the primary
classroom.
Shayer (1997), reported that abstract thought was necessary for success in
secondary school (and co-developed the CASE system of teaching science).
Recently the National curriculum has been updated to encourage the teaching of
some abstract concepts towards the end of primary education, in preparation for
secondary courses. (DfEE, 1999).
Child-centred teaching is regarded by some as a child of the ‘liberal sixties.’ In the
1980s the Thatcher government introduced the National Curriculum in an
attempt to move away from this and bring more central government control into
the teaching of children.
So, although the British National Curriculum in some ways supports the work of
Piaget, (in that it dictates the order of teaching), it can also be seen as prescriptive
to the point where it counters Piaget’s child-oriented approach. However, it does
still allow for flexibility in teaching methods, allowing teachers to tailor lessons to
the needs of their students.

Support for Piaget's Theory


 The influence of Piaget’s ideas in developmental psychology has been
enormous. He changed how people viewed the child’s world and their
methods of studying children.

He was an inspiration to many who came after and took up his ideas.
Piaget's ideas have generated a huge amount of research which has
increased our understanding of cognitive development.

 Piaget (1936) was one of the first psychologists to make a systematic study
of cognitive development. His contributions include a stage theory of child
cognitive development, detailed observational studies of cognition in
children, and a series of simple but ingenious tests to reveal different
cognitive abilities.
 His ideas have been of practical use in understanding and communicating
with children, particularly in the field of education (re: Discovery
Learning). Piaget's theory has been applied across education. According to
Piaget's theory, educational programmes should be designed to correspond
to the stages of development.

Cons of Piaget's Theory


 Are the stages real? Vygotsky and Bruner would rather not talk about
stages at all, preferring to see development as a continuous process. Others
have queried the age ranges of the stages. Some studies have shown that
progress to the formal operational stage is not guaranteed.

For example, Keating (1979) reported that 40-60% of college students fail
at formal operation tasks, and Dasen (1994) states that only one-third of
adults ever reach the formal operational stage.
The fact that the formal operational stage is not reached in all cultures and
not all individuals within cultures suggests that it might not be biologically
based.

 According to Piaget the rate of cognitive development cannot be


accelerated as it is based on biological processes however, direct tuition can
speed up the development which suggests that it is not entirely based on
biological factors.
 Because Piaget concentrated on the universal stages of cognitive
development and biological maturation, he failed to consider the effect that
the social setting and culture may have on cognitive development.

Cross-cultural studies show that the stages of development (except the


formal operational stage) occur in the same order in all cultures suggesting
that cognitive development is a product of a biological process of
maturation.
However the age at which the stages are reached varies between cultures
and individuals which suggests that social and cultural factors and
individual differences influence cognitive development. 
Dasen (1994) cites studies he conducted in remote parts of the central
Australian desert with 8-14 year old Indigenous Australians. He gave them
conservation of liquid tasks and spatial awareness tasks. He found that the
ability to conserve came later in the Aboriginal children, between aged 10
and 13 ( as opposed to between 5 and 7, with Piaget’s Swiss sample).
However, he found that spatial awareness abilities developed earlier
amongst the Aboriginal children than the Swiss children. Such a study
demonstrates cognitive development is not purely dependent on
maturation but on cultural factors too – spatial awareness is crucial for
nomadic groups of people.
Vygotsky, a contemporary of Piaget, argued that social interaction is crucial
for cognitive development. According to Vygotsky the child's learning
always occurs in a social context in co-operation with someone more
skillful (MKO). This social interaction provides language opportunities and
Vygotksy conisdered language the foundation of thought.

 Piaget’s methods (observation and clinical interviews) are more open to


biased interpretation than other methods. Piaget made careful, detailed
naturalistic observations of children, and from these he wrote diary
descriptions charting their development. He also used clinical interviews
and observations of older children who were able to understand questions
and hold conversations.
Because Piaget conducted the observations alone the data collected are
based on his own subjective interpretation of events. It would have been
more reliable if Piaget conducted the observations with another researcher
and compared the results afterward to check if they are similar (i.e., have
inter-rater reliability).
Although clinical interviews allow the researcher to explore data in more
depth, the interpretation of the interviewer may be biased. For example,
children may not understand the question/s, they have short attention
spans, they cannot express themselves very well and may be trying to
please the experimenter. Such methods meant that Piaget may have
formed inaccurate conclusions.

 As several studies have shown Piaget underestimated the abilities of


children because his tests were sometimes confusing or difficult to
understand (e.g., Hughes, 1975).

Piaget failed to distinguish between competence (what a child is capable of


doing) and performance (what a child can show when given a particular
task). When tasks were altered, performance (and therefore competence)
was affected. Therefore, Piaget might have underestimated children’s
cognitive abilities.
For example, a child might have object permanence (competence) but still
not be able to search for objects (performance). When Piaget hid objects
from babies he found that it wasn’t till after nine months that they looked
for it. However, Piaget relied on manual search methods – whether the
child was looking for the object or not.
Later, research such as Baillargeon and Devos (1991) reported that infants
as young as four months looked longer at a moving carrot that didn’t do
what it expected, suggesting they had some sense of permanence,
otherwise they wouldn’t have had any expectation of what it should or
shouldn’t do.

 The concept of schema is incompatible with the theories of Bruner (1966)


and Vygotsky (1978). Behaviorism would also refute Piaget’s schema theory
because is cannot be directly observed as it is an internal process.
Therefore, they would claim it cannot be objectively measured.
 Piaget studied his own children and the children of his colleagues in
Geneva in order to deduce general principles about the intellectual
development of all children. Not only was his sample very small, but it was
composed solely of European children from families of high socio-
economic status. Researchers have therefore questioned the
generalisability of his data.
 For Piaget, language is seen as secondary to action, i.e., thought precedes
language. The Russian psychologist Lev Vygotsky (1978) argues that the
development of language and thought go together and that the origin of
reasoning is more to do with our ability to communicate with others than
with our interaction with the material world.

Reference
McLeod, S. A. (2018, June 06). Jean piaget's theory of cognitive development. Simply
Psychology. www.simplypsychology.org/piaget.html
Baillargeon, R., & DeVos, J. (1991). Object permanence in young infants: Further
evidence. Child development, 1227-1246.
Bruner, J. S. (1966). Toward a theory of instruction. Cambridge, Mass.: Belkapp
Press.
Dasen, P. (1994). Culture and cognitive development from a Piagetian
perspective. In W .J. Lonner & R.S. Malpass (Eds.), Psychology and culture (pp.
145–149). Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.
Hughes , M. (1975). Egocentrism in preschool children. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation. Edinburgh University.
Inhelder, B., & Piaget, J. (1958). The growth of logical thinking from childhood
to adolescence. New York: Basic Books.
Keating, D. (1979). Adolescent thinking. In J. Adelson (Ed.), Handbook of
adolescent psychology (pp. 211-246). New York: Wiley.
Piaget, J. (1932). The moral judgment of the child. London: Routledge & Kegan
Paul.
Piaget, J. (1936). Origins of intelligence in the child. London: Routledge & Kegan
Paul.
Piaget, J. (1945). Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. London: Heinemann.
Piaget, J. (1957). Construction of reality in the child. London: Routledge & Kegan
Paul.
Piaget, J., & Cook, M. T. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York,
NY: International University Press.
Plowden, B. H. P. (1967). Children and their primary schools: A
report (Research and Surveys). London, England: HM Stationery Office.
Siegler, R. S., DeLoache, J. S., & Eisenberg, N. (2003). How children develop.
New York: Worth.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological
processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Wadsworth, B. J. (2004). Piaget's theory of cognitive and affective
development: Foundations of constructivism. New York: Longman.

You might also like