Piaget Theory
Piaget Theory
Development
Jean Piaget's theory of cognitive development suggests that intelligence changes
as children grow. A child's cognitive development is not just about acquiring
knowledge, the child has to develop or construct a mental model of the world.
Cognitive development occurs through the interaction of innate capacities and
environmental events, and children pass through a series of stages.
Piaget's theory of cognitive development proposes 4 stages of development.
The sequence of the stages is universal across cultures and follow the same
invariant (unchanging) order. All children go through the same stages in the
same order (but not all at the same rate).
The best way to understand children’s reasoning was to see things from their
point of view. What Piaget wanted to do was not to measure how well children
could count, spell or solve problems as a way of grading their I.Q. What he was
more interested in was the way in which fundamental concepts like the very idea
of number, time, quantity, causality, justice and so on emerged.
Piaget studied children from infancy to adolescence using naturalistic
observation of his own three babies and sometimes controlled observation too.
From these he wrote diary descriptions charting their development.
He also used clinical interviews and observations of older children who were able
to understand questions and hold conversations.
Although no stage can be missed out, there are individual differences in the rate
at which children progress through stages, and some individuals may never attain
the later stages.
Piaget did not claim that a particular stage was reached at a certain age - although
descriptions of the stages often include an indication of the age at which the
average child would reach each stage.
The infant learns about the world through their senses and through their
actions (moving around and exploring its environment).
During the sensorimotor stage a range of cognitive abilities develop. These
include: object permanence; self-recognition (the child realises that other
people are separate from them); deferred imitation; and representational
play.
They relate to the emergence of the general symbolic function, which is the
capacity to represent the world mentally
At about 8 months the infant will understand the permanence of objects
and that they will still exist even if they can’t see them and the infant will
search for them when they disappear.
During this stage the infant lives in the present. It does not yet have a mental
picture of the world stored in its memory therefore it does not have a sense of
object permanence.
If it cannot see something then it does not exist. This is why you can hide a toy
from an infant, while it watches, but it will not search for the object once it has
gone out of sight.
The main achievement during this stage is object permanence - knowing that an
object still exists, even if it is hidden. It requires the ability to form a mental
representation (i.e., a schema) of the object.
Towards the end of this stage the general symbolic function begins to appear
where children show in their play that they can use one object to stand for
another. Language starts to appear because they realise that words can be
used to represent objects and feelings.
The child begins to be able to store information that it knows about the world,
recall it and label it.
The Preoperational Stage
Ages: 2 - 7 Years
The pre-operational stage is one of Piaget's intellectual development stages. It
takes place between 2 and 7 years. At the beginning of this stage the child does
not use operations, so the thinking is influenced by the way things appear rather
than logical reasoning.
A child cannot conserve which means that the child does not understand that
quantity remains the same even if the appearance changes.
Furthermore, the child is egocentric; he assumes that other people see the world
as he does. This has been shown in the three mountains study.
Major Characteristics and Developmental Changes:
Toddlers and young children acquire the ability to internally represent the
world through language and mental imagery.
During this stage, young children can think about things symbolically. This
is the ability to make one thing, such as a word or an object, stand for
something other than itself.
A child’s thinking is dominated by how the world looks, not how the world
is. It is not yet capable of logical (problem solving) type of thought.
Moreover, the child has difficulties with class inclusion; he can classify
objects but cannot include objects in sub-sets, which involves classify
objects as belonging to two or more categories simultaneously
Infants at this stage also demonstrate animism. This is the tendency for the
child to think that non-living objects (such as toys) have life and feelings
like a person’s.
By 2 years, children have made some progress towards detaching their thought
from physical world. However have not yet developed logical (or 'operational')
thought characteristic of later stages.
Thinking is still intuitive (based on subjective judgements about situations) and
egocentric (centred on the child's own view of the world).
The stage is called concrete because children can think logically much more
successfully if they can manipulate real (concrete) materials or pictures of them.
Piaget considered the concrete stage a major turning point in the child's cognitive
development because it marks the beginning of logical or operational thought.
This means the child can work things out internally in their head (rather than
physically try things out in the real world).
Children can conserve number (age 6), mass (age 7), and weight (age 9).
Conservation is the understanding that something stays the same in quantity
even though its appearance changes.
But operational thought only effective here if child asked to reason about
materials that are physically present. Children at this stage will tend to make
mistakes or be overwhelmed when asked to reason about abstract or hypothetical
problems.
From about 12 years children can follow the form of a logical argument without
reference to its content. During this time, people develop the ability to think
about abstract concepts, and logically test hypotheses.
This stage sees emergence of scientific thinking, formulating abstract theories
and hypotheses when faced with a problem.
Schemas
Piaget claimed that knowledge cannot simply emerge from sensory experience;
some initial structure is necessary to make sense of the world.
According to Piaget, children are born with a very basic mental structure
(genetically inherited and evolved) on which all subsequent learning and
knowledge are based.
Schemas are the basic building blocks of such cognitive models, and enable us to
form a mental representation of the world.
Piaget (1952, p. 7) defined a schema as: "a cohesive, repeatable action sequence
possessing component actions that are tightly interconnected and governed by a
core meaning."
In more simple terms Piaget called the schema the basic building block of
intelligent behavior – a way of organizing knowledge. Indeed, it is useful to think
of schemas as “units” of knowledge, each relating to one aspect of the world,
including objects, actions, and abstract (i.e., theoretical) concepts.
Wadsworth (2004) suggests that schemata (the plural of schema) be thought of
as 'index cards' filed in the brain, each one telling an individual how to react to
incoming stimuli or information.
When Piaget talked about the development of a person's mental processes, he
was referring to increases in the number and complexity of the schemata that a
person had learned.
When a child's existing schemas are capable of explaining what it can perceive
around it, it is said to be in a state of equilibrium, i.e., a state of cognitive (i.e.,
mental) balance.
Operations are more sophisticated mental structures which allow us to combine
schemas in a logical (reasonable) way. As children grow they can carry out more
complex operations and begin to imagine hypothetical (imaginary) situations.
Apart from the schemas we are born with schemas and operations are learned
through interaction with other people and the environment.
Piaget emphasized the importance of schemas in cognitive development and
described how they were developed or acquired. A schema can be defined as a set
of linked mental representations of the world, which we use both to understand
and to respond to situations. The assumption is that we store these mental
representations and apply them when needed.
Examples of Schemas
A person might have a schema about buying a meal in a restaurant. The schema
is a stored form of the pattern of behavior which includes looking at a menu,
ordering food, eating it and paying the bill. This is an example of a type of schema
called a 'script.' Whenever they are in a restaurant, they retrieve this schema from
memory and apply it to the situation.
The schemas Piaget described tend to be simpler than this - especially those used
by infants. He described how - as a child gets older - his or her schemas become
more numerous and elaborate.
Piaget believed that newborn babies have a small number of innate schemas -
even before they have had many opportunities to experience the world. These
neonatal schemas are the cognitive structures underlying innate reflexes. These
reflexes are genetically programmed into us.
For example, babies have a sucking reflex, which is triggered by something
touching the baby's lips. A baby will suck a nipple, a comforter (dummy), or a
person's finger. Piaget, therefore, assumed that the baby has a 'sucking schema.'
Similarly, the grasping reflex which is elicited when something touches the palm
of a baby's hand, or the rooting reflex, in which a baby will turn its head towards
something which touches its cheek, are innate schemas. Shaking a rattle would be
the combination of two schemas, grasping and shaking.
Assimilation
Piaget defined assimilation as the cognitive process of fitting new information
into existing cognitive schemas, perceptions, and understanding. Overall beliefs
and understanding of the world do not change as a result of the new information.
Assimilation coccurs when the new experience is not very different form previous
experiences of a particular object or situation we assimilate the new situation by
adding information to a previous schema.
This means that when you are faced with new information, you make sense of
this information by referring to information you already have (information
processed and learned previously) and try to fit the new information into the
information you already have.
For example, a 2-year-old child sees a man who is bald on top of his head and has
long frizzy hair on the sides. To his father’s horror, the toddler shouts “Clown,
clown” (Siegler et al., 2003).
For example, a baby learns to pick up a rattle he or she will then use the same
schema (grasping) to pick up other objects.
Accommodation
Accommodation: when the new experience is very different from what we have
encountered before we need to change our schemas in a very radical way or
create a whole new schema.
Psychologist Jean Piaget defined accommodation as the cognitive process of
revising existing cognitive schemas, perceptions, and understanding so that new
information can be incorporated. This happens when the existing schema
(knowledge) does not work, and needs to be changed to deal with a new object or
situation.
In order to make sense of some new information, you actual adjust information
you already have (schemas you already have, etc.) to make room for this new
information.
For example, a baby tries to use the same schema for grasping to pick up a very
small object. It doesn’t work. The baby then changes the schema by now using the
forefinger and thumb to pick up the object.
Also, a child may have a schema for birds (feathers, flying, etc.) and then they see
a plane, which also flies, but would not fit into their bird schema.
In the “clown” incident, the boy’s father explained to his son that the man was not
a clown and that even though his hair was like a clown’s, he wasn’t wearing a
funny costume and wasn’t doing silly things to make people laugh.
With this new knowledge, the boy was able to change his schema of “clown” and
make this idea fit better to a standard concept of “clown”.
Equilibration
Piaget believed that all human thought seeks order and is uncomfortable with
contradictions and inconsistencies in knowledge structures. In other words, we
seek 'equilibrium' in our cognitive structures.
Equilibrium occurs when a child's schemas can deal with most new information
through assimilation. However, an unpleasant state of disequilibrium occurs
when new information cannot be fitted into existing schemas (assimilation).
Piaget believed that cognitive development did not progress at a steady rate, but
rather in leaps and bounds. Equilibration is the force which drives the learning
process as we do not like to be frustrated and will seek to restore balance by
mastering the new challenge (accommodation).
Once the new information is acquired the process of assimilation with the new
schema will continue until the next time we need to make an adjustment to it.
Equilibration is a regulatory process that maintains a balance between
assimilation and accommodation to facilitate cognitive growth. Think of it this
way: We can't merely assimilate all the time; if we did, we would never learn any
new concepts or principles.
Everything new we encountered would just get put in the same few "slots" we
already had. Neither can we accommodate all the time; if we did, everything we
encountered would seem new; there would be no recurring regularities in our
world. We'd be exhausted by the mental effort!
Applying Piaget's Theory to the Classroom
Think of old black and white films that you’ve seen in which children sat in rows
at desks, with ink wells, would learn by rote, all chanting in unison in response to
questions set by an authoritarian old biddy like Matilda!
Children who were unable to keep up were seen as slacking and would be
punished by variations on the theme of corporal punishment. Yes, it really did
happen and in some parts of the world still does today. Piaget is partly
responsible for the change that occurred in the 1960s and for your relatively
pleasurable and pain free school days!
How to teach
Within the classroom learning should be student-centered and accomplished
through active discovery learning. The role of the teacher is to facilitate learning,
rather than direct tuition.
Because Piaget's theory is based upon biological maturation and stages, the
notion of 'readiness' is important. Readiness concerns when certain information
or concepts should be taught. According to Piaget's theory children should not be
taught certain concepts until they have reached the appropriate stage of cognitive
development.
According to Piaget (1958), assimilation and accommodation require an active
learner, not a passive one, because problem-solving skills cannot be taught, they
must be discovered.
He was an inspiration to many who came after and took up his ideas.
Piaget's ideas have generated a huge amount of research which has
increased our understanding of cognitive development.
Piaget (1936) was one of the first psychologists to make a systematic study
of cognitive development. His contributions include a stage theory of child
cognitive development, detailed observational studies of cognition in
children, and a series of simple but ingenious tests to reveal different
cognitive abilities.
His ideas have been of practical use in understanding and communicating
with children, particularly in the field of education (re: Discovery
Learning). Piaget's theory has been applied across education. According to
Piaget's theory, educational programmes should be designed to correspond
to the stages of development.
For example, Keating (1979) reported that 40-60% of college students fail
at formal operation tasks, and Dasen (1994) states that only one-third of
adults ever reach the formal operational stage.
The fact that the formal operational stage is not reached in all cultures and
not all individuals within cultures suggests that it might not be biologically
based.
Reference
McLeod, S. A. (2018, June 06). Jean piaget's theory of cognitive development. Simply
Psychology. www.simplypsychology.org/piaget.html
Baillargeon, R., & DeVos, J. (1991). Object permanence in young infants: Further
evidence. Child development, 1227-1246.
Bruner, J. S. (1966). Toward a theory of instruction. Cambridge, Mass.: Belkapp
Press.
Dasen, P. (1994). Culture and cognitive development from a Piagetian
perspective. In W .J. Lonner & R.S. Malpass (Eds.), Psychology and culture (pp.
145–149). Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.
Hughes , M. (1975). Egocentrism in preschool children. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation. Edinburgh University.
Inhelder, B., & Piaget, J. (1958). The growth of logical thinking from childhood
to adolescence. New York: Basic Books.
Keating, D. (1979). Adolescent thinking. In J. Adelson (Ed.), Handbook of
adolescent psychology (pp. 211-246). New York: Wiley.
Piaget, J. (1932). The moral judgment of the child. London: Routledge & Kegan
Paul.
Piaget, J. (1936). Origins of intelligence in the child. London: Routledge & Kegan
Paul.
Piaget, J. (1945). Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. London: Heinemann.
Piaget, J. (1957). Construction of reality in the child. London: Routledge & Kegan
Paul.
Piaget, J., & Cook, M. T. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York,
NY: International University Press.
Plowden, B. H. P. (1967). Children and their primary schools: A
report (Research and Surveys). London, England: HM Stationery Office.
Siegler, R. S., DeLoache, J. S., & Eisenberg, N. (2003). How children develop.
New York: Worth.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological
processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Wadsworth, B. J. (2004). Piaget's theory of cognitive and affective
development: Foundations of constructivism. New York: Longman.