Articles Arnold Bakker 191
Articles Arnold Bakker 191
Articles Arnold Bakker 191
This study, conducted on a nation-wide sample of Dutch junior doctors (also called medical
residents) (N2115), investigated the unique relationships of workaholism with burnout and
well-being, and hypothesized that (inter- and intra-) role conflict would mediate these effects.
The results of multi-group structural equation modelling analyses offered support for this
model. Specifically, role conflict fully mediated the relationships between workaholism (i.e.
working excessively and working compulsively) and job demands (i.e. emotional, mental and
organizational demands) on the one hand, and burnout (i.e. emotional exhaustion,
depersonalization and reduced medical accomplishment) and well-being (job satisfaction,
happiness and perceived health) on the other hand. This indicates that workaholism
contributed incrementally to explaining positive (well-being) and negative (burnout) outcomes
beyond common indicators of job demands.
Keywords: burnout; role conflict; workaholism; junior doctors; medical residents; work-related
stress
Introduction
Since the introduction of the concept in the late 1960s (Oates, 1968), scientific
interest in workaholism has been growing, particularly during the last 15 years or so.
A literature search using PsychInfo revealed that over that period 184 articles on
workaholism haven been published, and that the publication rate has doubled every
5 years from 1990 onwards (Taris & Schaufeli, 2007). Nevertheless, our knowledge
about workaholism is still quite limited because most studies have dealt with the
measurement of workaholism and with its correlates, rather than with the processes
involved (McMillan, O’Driscoll, & Burke, 2003).
The current study tries to fill this void by proposing a model assuming that
workaholism is indirectly related to employee burnout and well-being through intra-
and inter-role conflict. Moreover, the model proposes that job demands are related
to these outcomes in a similar indirectly way, and that both workaholism and job
Mudrack & Naughton, 2001). For instance, North American workaholics work on
average 5060 hours per week (Brett & Stroh, 2003). However, conceiving
workaholism exclusively in terms of the number of working hours would be wrong
because it would neglect its addictive nature. After all, people may work long hours
without being addicted to it for many reasons, including financial problems, poor
marriage, social pressure or career advancement. Rather than being motivated by
such external factors, a typical work addict is motivated by an obsessive internal
drive that (s)he cannot resist. Hence, we define workaholism as an irresistible inner
drive to work excessively hard (Schaufeli, Taris, & Bakker, 2008). So in our view,
workaholism includes two elements: a strong inner drive and working hard.
Our two-dimensional conceptualization of workaholism corresponds with the
original meaning of the term as it was used by Oates (1971), who described
workaholism as ‘‘ . . . the compulsion or the uncontrollable need to work inces-
santly’’ (p. 11). Analogously to alcoholics, for workaholics their need to work is so
exaggerated that it endangers their health, reduces their happiness and causes a
deterioration in their interpersonal relations and social functioning, he argued. In
addition, various overviews confirm that both dimensions feature across most
definitions of workaholism. For instance, Scott et al. (1997) observed that virtually
all definitions assume that workaholics: (1) spend a great deal of time on work
activities when given the discretion to do so*they are excessively hard workers; (2)
Workaholism Well-being
_
+
+ Role conflicts
_
+ +
are reluctant to disengage from work and they persistently and frequently think
about work when they are not at work*they are obsessed workers; and (3) work
beyond what is reasonably expected from them to meet organizational or economic
requirements. The latter is in fact a specification of the first and the second features,
because it deals with the motivation to spend an excessive amount of time to work.
Taken together, Scott et al.’s conceptual analyses revealed that workaholics work
harder than is required out of an obsessive inner drive, and not because of external
factors. In a similar vein, in seven of the nine workaholism definitions that were
recently listed by McMillan and O’Driscoll (2006), working excessively hard and
being propelled by an obsessive inner drive are mentioned as core characteristics.
Finally, a recent analysis of scholarly definitions by Ng et al. (2007) concludes that
hard work at the expense of other important life roles and an obsessive internal drive
to work are the two core aspects of workaholism.
Taken together, it seems that a common denominator exists of workaholism: the
tendency to work excessively hard in a compulsive way. Working excessively hard
represents its behavioural component that indicates that workaholics tend to allocate
Downloaded By: [Universiteit Utrecht] At: 13:57 24 July 2009
an exceptional amount of their time and energy to work and that they work beyond
what is reasonably expected to meet organizational or economic requirements.
Working compulsively represents the cognitive component of workaholism and
indicates that workaholics are obsessed with their work and persistently and
frequently think about work, even when not being at work.
extra time that might interfere with efficient patient care that they are expected to
deliver as a doctor. At any rate, resources that are spent while performing one role
cannot be spent while performing the other (cf. the role scarcity hypothesis; Edwards
& Rothbard, 2000). Typically, workaholics make their work (in either role) more
complicated than necessary. They create difficulties for their co-workers, and they
refuse to delegate work (Burke, 2001; Kanai & Wakabayashi, 2001; Machlowitz,
1980). Moreover, they are characterized by orderliness, rigidity and a high need for
achievement (Mudrack, 2006; Mudrack & Naughton, 2001), and by inflexibility and
perfectionism (Kanai & Wakabayashi, 2001; Killinger, 2006; Porter 2001). Hence, it
is likely that workaholic junior doctors are caught between their roles as doctor and
trainee. They like to spend an excessive amount of time and energy in both roles, but
resources spent in one role go at the expense of the other role and vice versa, thus
causing an intra-role conflict at work. Compared to the literature on workaholism
and inter-role conflict, studies on intra-role conflict are scarce. However, a study
among Japanese automotive workers showed that role conflict was positively
associated with the compulsive component of workaholism, after controlling for
Downloaded By: [Universiteit Utrecht] At: 13:57 24 July 2009
among Dutch junior doctors (the group under study in this article) was estimated at
13% in a preliminary study and was thus somewhat lower (Prins et al., 2007c).
Nevertheless, this percentage is substantially higher than the 4% burnout cases that
has been estimated using a Dutch sample of over 13,000 employees from 29 different
professions (Bakker, Schaufeli, & Van Dierendonck, 2000). So taken together, there
is convincing evidence that burnout is particularly prevalent among junior doctors.
In our study we investigate to what extent burnout levels are uniquely related (via
role conflicts) with workaholism, controlling for the effects of job demands.
Method
Sample and procedure
All 5245 Dutch junior doctors who were included in the national register of the
Royal Dutch Medical Association on 1 October 2005 received a questionnaire by
mail. In the Dutch system medical students follow a 6-year general training in
Downloaded By: [Universiteit Utrecht] At: 13:57 24 July 2009
Measures
Workaholism was measured with the 10-item DUWAS (Dutch WorkAholism Scale;
Schaufeli et al., in press) that includes two scales: Working Excessively (5 items,
a .67) and Working Compulsively (5 items, a .77). Example items are: ‘‘I seem to
be in a hurry and racing against the clock’’ (working excessively) and ‘‘I feel that
there’s something inside me that drives me to work hard’’ (working compulsively).
Both scales were scored on a 5-point rating scale, ranging from 1 (‘‘never’’) to 5
(‘‘always’’). A confirmatory factor-analyses revealed that the hypothesized two-
factor structure of the DUWAS fit well to the data (x2(34 df) 360.46, pB.001;
GFI .97; AGFI .95; RMSEA .07, NFI 92; NNFI.91, CFI .93). The
correlation between both latent factors was .55 (pB.001).
Three qualitative job demands were assessed: Mental Demands (4 items; a .77),
Organizational Demands (5 items; a .64), Emotional Demands (4 items; a .71),
using shortened scales (e.g. Bakker, Demerouti, Taris, Schaufeli, & Schreurs, 2003) of
the Questionnaire on the Experience and Evaluation of Work (QEEW). The QEEW
is widely employed by applied researchers in The Netherlands (Van Veldhoven, De
Jonge, Broersen, Kompier, & Meijman, 2002). Example items are: ‘‘Does your work
Work & Stress 161
Analyses
In order to reduce change capitalization, the total sample was randomly split into a
model development sample (Sample 1, N 1058) that was used to test the
162 W.B. Schaufeli et al.
measurement model and the research model (see Figure 1) and a model validation
sample (Sample 2, N 1057) in which the final model was cross-validated. Structural
equation modelling methods as implemented by AMOS 5 (Arbuckle, 2003) were
used to test the fit of the measurement model and the research model. Maximum
likelihood estimation methods were used and the input for the analysis was the
covariance matrix of the items. The goodness-of-fit of the model was evaluated using
four absolute fit indices (cf. Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1986): x2 goodness-of-fit statistic,
Goodness-of-Fit Index (GFI), Adjusted Goodness-of-Fit Index (AGFI) and the
Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA). Because x2 is sensitive to
sample size, three relative goodness-of-fit indices were calculated (Hu & Bentler,
1999): Normed Fit Index (NFI), Non-Normed Fit Index (NNFI) and Comparative
Fit Index (CFI). For these three fit-indices, as a rule of thumb, values greater than
.90 are considered as indicating acceptable fit, whereas values smaller than .08 for
RMSEA indicate acceptable fit.
Downloaded By: [Universiteit Utrecht] At: 13:57 24 July 2009
Results
Preliminary analysis
The means, standard deviations and correlations between the study variables of
Samples 1 and 2 are shown in Table 1.
First the measurement model was tested in Sample 1. The fit of the 1-factor
model with all 13 study variables loading on a single latent variable was compared
with that of a 5-factor model that included workaholism (working excessively and
working compulsively), job demands (mental, emotional, and organizational
demands), intra- and inter-role conflict, burnout (emotional exhaustion, deperso-
nalization, reduced medical accomplishment) and well-being (job satisfaction,
happiness, perceived health). The 5-factor model fitted well to the data
(x2(55 df) 307.04, p B.001, GFI .96; AGFI .93; RMSEA .07; NFI93;
NNFI .91; CFI .94), and its fit was superior to that of the 1-factor model.
(^x2(10 df)274.77; p B.001). Factor loading were all significant, ranging from
.35 to .91. So it is concluded that instead of loading on a single factor the study
variables load, as expected, on the five latent variables that have been specified in
our model.
Sample 1 Sample 2
(N 1058) (N 1057) PM correlations
M SD M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13
1 Working excessively 2.67 .45 2.66 .44 1.0 .44 .32 .31 .27 .43 .38 .42 .17 .28 .14 .22 .20
2 Working compulsively 2.00 .60 1.96 .58 .48 1.0 .30 .25 .27 .45 .39 .50 .34 .24 .22 .31 .24
3 Emotional demands 2.45 .57 2.47 .57 .29 .27 1.0 .28 .36 .38 .40 .43 .31 .30 .18 .22 .28
4 Mental demands 4.01 .59 3.99 .58 .27 .18 .28 1.0 .18 .23 .21 .22 .10 .13 .06 .09 .08
5 Organizational 2.81 .67 2.85 .66 .35 .31 .31 15 1.0 .36 .44 .35 .19 .20
7 Intra role conflict 2.08 .55 2.06 .56 .41 .36 .35 .19 .40 .51 1.0. .47 .30 .27 .22 .31 .32
8 Emotional exhaustion 2.02 1.06 1.96 1.04 .48 .50 .42 .23 .38 .57 .47 1.0 .49 .37 .34 .50 .45
9 Depersonalization 1.45 .89 1.40 .87 .26 .33 .34 .13 .24 .33 .31 .56 1.0 .41 .15 .25 .26
10 Reduced medical 2.16 .56 2.17 .54 .28 .21 .26 .12 .22 .24 .35 .42 .43 1.0 .07 .16 .20
accomplishment
11 Perceived health 3.41 .62 3.41 .62 .16 .24 .17 .05 .17 .25 .19 .37 .19 .12 1.0 .38 .22
12 Happiness 7.69 1.08 7.71 1.01 .26 .33 .26 .07 .20 .41 .25 .52 .34 .18 .37 1.0 .39
13 Job satisfaction 3.32 .92 3.33 .90. .29 .27 .27 .03 .30 .31 .29 .49 .33 .31 .23 .38 1.0
Note: All correlations.06; pB.05.
163
164 W.B. Schaufeli et al.
well-being (z2.98; pB.01); and supported the mediating role of role conflicts
in the relationship between job demands and burnout (z3.45; p B.01) and well-
being (z 2.53; p B.05). Hence, it is concluded that, as expected, role conflicts
fully mediate the relationships between workaholism and job demands on the one
hand and burnout and well-being on the other hand.
Workaholism Well-being
.53 -.70
.79 -.59
Role conflicts
.48 .80
Discussion
We set out to test a process model that assumed that both workaholism and job
demands have an indirect impact (via inter-role and intra-role conflicts) on negative
(burnout) as well as positive (well-being) outcomes. The model that is depicted in
Figure 1 was successfully tested and cross-validated in a large national sample of
Dutch junior doctors. This group was selected because junior doctors have been
considered a group at-risk for workaholism (Harpaz & Snir, 2003; Scott et al., 1997),
because they experience an inherent conflict between their role as a doctor and a
trainee, and because their levels of burnout are relatively high (Thomas, 2004). To
the best of our knowledge this is the first study that specifically focuses on
workaholism among junior doctors.
The results obtained in the current study add to the literature in at least six ways.
First, our model suggests that workaholism contributes incrementally to explaining
burnout and well-being beyond emotional, mental and organizational demands. Or
put differently, workaholism seems to act as an individual risk-factor that
Downloaded By: [Universiteit Utrecht] At: 13:57 24 July 2009
contributes, independently from the job context, to burnout and well-being. The
effects of workaholism and job demands on role-problems are of similar magnitude.
Obviously, being driven by a strong inner need to work excessively is associated with
burnout and poor wellbeing, independently from the experienced job demands. This
agrees with studies both on the Type A behavioural pattern and on overcommitment
as conceptualized in the Effort-Reward Imbalance model (Siegrist, 1998). For
instance, Day and Jreige (2002) showed in a Canadian sample that Type A
behavioural pattern, which is characterized by achievement striving and impa-
tience-irritability, was positively related to perceived stress and negatively related to
job satisfaction, after controlling for various job stressors, such as overload and
conflict. Furthermore, Tsutsumi, Kayaya, Theorell, and Siegrist (2001) showed in a
Japanese sample of employees who were threatened by job loss, that overcommit-
ment (i.e. an excessive striving in combination with a strong desire of being approved
and esteemed) was strongly related to depression, independently from an imbalance
between occupational efforts and rewards. It seems that workaholism, Type A
behavioural pattern and overcommitment have a strong inner drive in common, and
it is perhaps this drive that constitutes a risk-factor that threatens occupational
health and well-being.
Second, inter- and intra-role conflicts fully mediate the relationships between
workaholism and job demands on the one hand, and burnout and well-being at the
other hand. Model tests for partial mediation were unsuccessful and subsequent
Sobel tests yielded significant results so that we may conclude that role conflicts
indeed play a pivotal role. It remains to be seen whether or not this result may be
generalized beyond the group under study because role conflicts are particularly
salient for junior doctors. Not only do they (almost by definition) experience inter-
role conflicts between their roles as doctor and trainee, but many junior doctors are
also in the process of building a family, which aggravates work-to-family conflict.
However, our study suggests that in addition to crossover, the inter-individual
transmission of stress or strain from the job incumbent to his/her spouse (Bakker
et al., 2009), workaholism is also related to spillover, the intra-individual transmis-
sion of stress or strain from one domain (work) to another domain (home and
family).
166 W.B. Schaufeli et al.
of the one-factor model (^x2(1 df)28.86; pB.001). Hence, positive and negative
outcomes should not be collapsed into one overall factor. Yet, Figure 2 shows that
our model is rather symmetrical in the sense that the relationship of role problems
with burnout is comparable in strength with the relationship with well-being.
Fifth, many scholars differentiate between ‘‘good’’ and ‘‘bad’’ forms of
workaholism. For instance, Machlowitz (1980) distinguishes between ‘‘fulfilled’’
and ‘‘unfulfilled’’ workaholics, Scott et al. (1997) consider achievement-oriented
workaholics as ‘‘hyper performers’’ and Buelens and Poelmans (2004) write about
some workaholics as ‘‘happy hard workers.’’ Moreover, the most popular model of
workaholism assumes three underlying dimensions (the so-called workaholic-triad)
consisting of work involvement, drive and work enjoyment (Spence & Robbins,
1992). Different combinations of these three dimensions are assumed to produce
different workaholic types. For instance, ‘‘real workaholics’’ are high in involvement,
high in drive and low in enjoyment, whereas ‘‘work enthusiasts’’ are high in
involvement and enjoyment and low in drive. In contrast, we agree with Mudrack
(2006, p. 109), who concluded after reviewing various definitions of workaholism:
‘‘. . . work enjoyment, whether it is high or low, is simply not a defining characteristic
of workaholism.’’ Those who have been identified as ‘‘good’’ workaholics strongly
resemble engaged workers, as is explained in greater detail elsewhere (Schaufeli et al.,
2006). Moreover, recent studies in The Netherlands (Schaufeli, Taris, & Van Rhenen,
2008) and Japan (Schaufeli et al., in press) attested to the discriminant validity of
workaholism and work engagement. The results of the current study confirm that
workaholism (conceptualized as an irresistible inner drive to work excessively hard)
is negatively related to various positive indicators, notably job satisfaction and
happiness. This contradicts the notion of ‘‘good’’ workaholism.
Sixth, our results indicate that workaholism and perceived job demands are
strongly related. We did not assume a causal direction because it might work both
ways. On the one hand, workaholics are known to make their work more complicated
than necessary, to create more work than they actually have and not to delegate their
work (e.g. Machlowitz, 1980); to some extent they create their own (high) job
demands. On the other hand, high work demands may prompt workaholic behaviour,
especially among those with a particular ‘‘workaholic’’ predisposition characterized
Work & Stress 167
by dominance, high need for achievement, super ego (i.e. being guided by conscience)
and perfectionism (Mudrack, 2006). Because of this bidirectional nature, the strong
correlation between job demands and workaholism is not unexpected.
Given these six contributions to the literature, we may conclude that the current
study contributes to our understanding of the nature of workaholism, at least as far
as junior doctors are concerned. We can have particular confidence in the obtained
results because the research model was not only successfully tested but also cross-
validated in an independent sample. Hence, it is not likely that our findings have been
influenced by chance capitalization.
we assessed the fit of the measurement model, relative to that of a model that
assumed that all study variables would load on one single factor that may be
interpreted as a common method factor. It appeared that the fit of the latter was
inferior to that of the measurement model that assumes five independent latent
factors that corresponded with those in our research model. Moreover, recently
Spector (2006) has argued that common method variance is not that troublesome as
one might expect in studies such as the current one. He convincingly showed that
potentially biasing variables such as social desirability, negative affectivity and
acquiescence (the tendency to agree with items independent of their content) do not
systematically inflate correlations between self-reported variables. Nevertheless, in
order to avoid common method variance, future research on workaholism should
include non-self reports as well, such as peer ratings from colleagues (Burke & Ng,
2007) or acquaintances (Aziz & Zickar, 2006; Bakker et al., 2009).
Another limitation is our rather homogeneous sample that consists of highly
educated professionals, i.e. junior doctors. This might have caused restriction of
range in some variables and potentially threatens the generalizability of our findings.
However, the sample can also be considered a major strength of our study because
the entire population was included: all Dutch junior doctors were invited to
participate. If the results are biased, it is most likely that workaholics, being busy and
tired, rather than healthy (non-workaholic) workers were under-represented in the
sample. Nevertheless, future research should replicate our results in other occupa-
tional groups and show that the two-dimensional conceptualization of workaholism
is (more than) the sum of its parts.
Three scales had internal consistency coefficients that were lower than the
recommended criterion of .70 (Nunally & Bernstein, 1994), their a-values ranged
from .64 to .67. However, this criterion of .70 is an arbitrary value that is not
universally accepted. For instance, De Vellis (2003) in his handbook on scale
construction, proposed .65 as a minimum threshold for an acceptable coefficient a.
As an example of the arbitrariness of his criterion, Nunally (1967) mentioned that a’s
ranging from .50.60 would be acceptable, but in the second edition of his book
(Nunally, 1967) he suggests a value .70 without further justification. Moreover, the
minimally required degree of reliability is a function of the research purpose; for
168 W.B. Schaufeli et al.
individual-level, diagnostic research, a should be much higher than for the basic,
group-level research reported in our study (Peterson, 1994).
The most important limitation, however, is the cross-sectional nature of our
study that precludes causeeffect relationships being uncovered. We employed
structural equation modelling analyses because it is a feasible way to examine
simultaneously a set of structural associations between latent factors that are
constituted by various indicators. Despite the use of arrows in Figures 1 and 2, our
strategy of analysis should not be taken to suggest that we actually investigated
causal relationships. So this would mean that future longitudinal research should
replicate the cross-sectional findings that are reported in the current study. Clearly, a
longitudinal study spanning a relatively long period of 2 years or so would be
necessary because it is likely that the processes involved unfold only gradually.
Workaholics do not burn out instantaneously.
Practical implications
Downloaded By: [Universiteit Utrecht] At: 13:57 24 July 2009
The current study suggests that workaholism and job demands are associated (via
role problems) with burnout. This means that, eventually, workaholism in
combination with high job demands may lead to burnout though increased inter-
and intra-role conflict. Burned-out junior doctors are exhausted, relate to their
patients in a more callous and cynical way (depersonalization) and accomplish less,
medically speaking. These are serious indications that patient care might suffer when
junior doctors feel burned out (Thomas, 2004). Hence, it is important to prevent and
combat burnout, by targeting workaholic behaviour, role problems and job demands
among junior doctors in hospital settings. We will focus next on how to reduce
workaholism because individual-based and organizational-based interventions to
reduce burnout in health care, such as stress-management training and job-redesign,
have been discussed elsewhere (Schaufeli, 2007).
Following Fassel and Schaef (1989), supervisors should pay attention to the
performance and work habits of junior doctors and be alert to early warning signs of
workaholism. Instead of rewarding excess work they should try to encourage living a
balanced life, for instance by doing their best to ensure that junior doctors regularly
take time off work. Insecurity, self-doubt, work overload and career opportunities
may make junior doctors feel compelled to put more time and effort into their work.
In that case, their supervisors should try to minimize their impact on the atmosphere
in the work setting. Alternatively, workaholic junior doctors could be referred to an
employee assistance program or a recovery program for treatment. However, based
on a survey among American employee assistance programs, Porter and Herring
(2007) conclude that it is not very likely that workaholics will be referred to employee
assistance programs, at least not under a referral labelled ‘‘workaholism.’’ However,
once they find their way to an employee assistance program there is some chance that
their symptoms will be interpreted as possibly part of a larger workaholic pattern.
Furthermore, Haas (1991) formulated some interesting ideas on the role of
supervisors that it may be possible to apply to hospital settings as well. For instance,
supervisors should help junior doctors to prioritize and encourage them to delegate
their work whenever possible. Also, junior doctors should be given specific times to
take breaks and to leave work. Finally, supervisors should coach junior doctors in
changing their harmful workaholic behaviours and cognitions as part of their
Work & Stress 169
Acknowledgements
This study was supported by unrestricted grants of SWG Arts en Werk, Stichting
Capaciteitsorgaan, Dutch Association of Medical Residents (LVAG) and University Medical
Center Groningen.
Downloaded By: [Universiteit Utrecht] At: 13:57 24 July 2009
References
Andreassen, C.S., Ursin, H., & Eriksen, H.R. (2007). The relationship between strong
motivation to work, ‘‘workaholism,’’ and health. Psychology & Health, 22, 625629.
Arbuckle, J.L. (2003). AMOS 5.0 (Computer Software). Chicago, IL: SPSS.
Aziz, S., & Zickar, M.J. (2006). A cluster analysis investigation of workaholism as a syndrome.
Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 11, 5262.
Bakker, A.B., & Demerouty, E. (2007). The Job Demands-Resources model: State of the art.
Journal of Managerial Psychology, 22, 309328.
Bakker, A.B., Demerouti, E., & Burke, R. (2009). Workaholism and relationship quality: A
spillover-crossover perspective. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 14, 2333.
Bakker, A.B., Demerouti, E., Taris, T.W., Schaufeli, W.B., & Schreurs, P.J.G. (2003). A multi-
group analysis of the Job Demands-Resources model in four home care organizations.
International Journal of Stress Management, 10, 1638.
Bakker, A.B., & Schaufeli, W.B. (2008). Positive organizational behavior: Engaged employees
in flourishing organizations. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 29, 147154.
Bakker, A.B., Schaufeli, W.B., & Van Dierendonck, D. (2000). Burnout: prevalentie,
risicogroepen en risicofactoren [Burnout: prevalence, risk groups and risk factors]. In
I.L.D. Houtman, W.B. Schaufeli & T.W. Taris (Eds.), Psychische vermoeidheid en werk (pp.
6582). Alphen a/d Rijn, The Netherlands: Samsom.
Becker, G.S. (1991). A treatise on the family. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Becker, G.S. (1993). Human capital theory. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Bonebright, C.A., Clay, D.L., & Ankenmann, R.D. (2000). The relationship of workaholism
with work-life conflict, life satisfaction, and purpose in life. Journal of Counseling
Psychology, 47, 469477.
Bresó, E., Salanova, M., & Schaufeli, W.B (2007). In search of the ‘‘third dimension’’ of
burnout: Efficacy or inefficacy? Applied Psychology: An International review, 56, 460478.
Brett, J.M., & Stroh, L.K. (2003). Working 61 plus hours per week: Why do managers do it?
Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 6778.
Buelens, M., & Poelmans, S.A.Y. (2004). Enriching the Spence and Robbins’ typology of
workaholism: Demographic, motivational and organizational correlates. Organizational
Change Management, 17, 459470.
Burke, R.J. (2000). Workaholism in organizations: Psychological and physical well-being
consequences. Stress Medicine, 16, 1116.
170 W.B. Schaufeli et al.
Burke, R.J. (2001). Workaholism components, job satisfaction, and career progress. Journal of
Applied Social Psychology, 31, 23392356.
Burke, R.J. (2008). Work motivations, satisfactions, and health: Passion versus addiction. In
R.J. Burke & C.L. Cooper (Eds.), The long working hours culture. Causes, consequences and
choices (pp. 227251). Bingley, UK: Emerald.
Burke, R.J., & Koksal, H. (2002). Workaholism among a sample of Turkish managers and
professionals: An exploratory study. Psychological Reports, 91, 6068.
Burke, R.J., & Ng, E.S.W. (2007). Workaholic behaviours: Do colleagues agree? International
Journal of Stress Management, 24, 312320.
Burke, R.J., Richardsen, A.M., & Mortinussen, M. (2004). Workaholism among Norwegian
managers: Work and well-being outcomes. Journal of Organizational Change Management,
7, 459470.
Byron, K. (2005). A meta-analytic review of workfamily conflict and its antecedents. Journal
of Vocational Behavior, 67, 169198.
Conway, J.M. (2002). Method variance and method bias in industrial and organizational
psychology. In S.G. Rogelberg (Ed.), Handbook of research methods in organizational and
industrial psychology (pp. 344365). Malden, NJ: Blackwell Publishers.
Downloaded By: [Universiteit Utrecht] At: 13:57 24 July 2009
Daugherty, S.R., DeWitt, C.B., & Rowley, B.D. (1998). Learning, satisfaction, and
mistreatment during medical internship: A national survey of working conditions. Journal
of the American Medical Association, 279, 11941199.
Day, A.L., & Jreige, S. (2002). Examining Type A behavior pattern to explain the relationship
between job stressors and psychosocial outcomes. Journal of Occupational Health
Psychology, 7, 109120.
Demerouti, E., Bakker, A.B., & Bulters, A. (2004). The loss spiral of work pressure, work
home interference and exhaustion: Reciprocal relationships in a three-wave study. Journal of
Vocational Behavior, 64, 131149.
De Vellis, R.F. (2003). Scale development: Theory and applications (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks,
CA: Sage.
Diener, E., Suh, E.M., Lucas, R.E., & Smith, H.L. (1999). Subjective well-being: Three
decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 267302.
Edwards, J.R., & Rothbard, N.P. (2000). Mechanisms linking work and family: Clarifying the
relationship between work and family constructs. Academy of Management Review, 25,
178199.
Fassel, D., & Schaef, A.W. (1989). The high cost of workaholism. Business and Health, 34,
842.
Geurts, S.A.E., Taris, T.W., Kompier, M.A.J., Dikkers, S.J.E., Van Hooff, M., & Kinnunen, U.
(2005). Workhome interaction from a work-psychological perspective: Development and
validation of a new questionnaire, the SWING. Work & Stress, 19, 319339.
Haas, R. (1991). Strategies to cope with a cultural phenomenon*Workaholism. Business and
Health, 36, 4.
Harpaz, I., & Snir, R. (2003). Workaholism: Its definition and nature. Human Relations, 56,
291319.
Hu, L.T., & Bentler, P.M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indices in covariance structure analyses:
Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 155.
Jackson, S.E., & Schuler, R.S. (1985). A meta-analysis and conceptual critique or research on
role-ambiguity and role conflict in work settings. Organizational Behavior and Human
Decision Processes, 36, 1678.
Jöreskog, K.G., & Sörbom, D. (1986). LISREL user guide version VI, (4th ed.). Mooresville,
IL: Scientific Software International.
Kanai, A., & Wakabayashi, M. (2001). Workaholism among Japanese blue-collar employees.
International Journal of Stress Management, 8, 129203.
Karasek, R.A., & Theorell, T. (1990). Healthy work. New York: Basic Books.
Work & Stress 171
Killinger, B. (2006). The workaholic breakdown syndrome. In R.J. Burke (Ed.), Research
companion to working time and work addiction (pp. 6188). Northampton, MA: Edward
Elgar.
Lee, R.T., & Ashforth, B.E. (1996). A meta-analytic examination of the correlates of the three
dimensions of job burnout. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, 123133.
Luthans, F., & Youssef, C.M. (2007). Emerging positive organizational behavior. Journal of
Management, 33, 321349.
Machlowitz, M. (1980). Workaholics: Living with them, working with them. New York: Simon
& Schuster.
Maslach, C. (1986). Stress, burnout and workaholism. In R.R. Killberg, P.E. Nathan & R.W.
Thoreson (Eds.), Professionals in distress: Issues, syndromes and solutions in psychology (pp.
5373). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Maslach, C., Leiter, M.P., & Jackson, S.E. (1996). Maslach Burnout Inventory Manual,
(3rd ed.). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
McMillan, L.H.W., & O’Driscoll, M.P. (2006). Exploring new frontiers to generate an
integrated definition of workaholism. In R.J. Burke (Ed.), Research companion to working
Downloaded By: [Universiteit Utrecht] At: 13:57 24 July 2009
time and work addiction (pp. 89107). Northampton, MA: Edward Elgar.
McMillan, L.H.W., O’Driscoll, M.P., & Burke, R.J. (2003). Workaholism: A review of theory,
research, and future directions. In C.L. Cooper & I.T. Robertson (Eds.), International review
of industrial and organizational psychology (Vol. 18, pp. 167189). New York: Wiley.
Mudrack, P.E. (2006). Understanding workaholism: The case of behavioral tendencies. In R.
Burke (Ed.), Work hours and work addiction (pp. 108128). Northampton, MA: Edward
Elgar.
Mudrack, P.E., & Naughton, T.J. (2001). The assessment of workaholism as behavioral
tendencies: Scale development and preliminary testing. International Journal of Stress
Management, 8, 93112.
Ng, T.W.H., Sorensen, K.L., & Feldman, D.C. (2007). Dimensions, antecedents, and
consequences of workaholism: a conceptual integration and extension. Journal of
Organizational Behavior, 28, 111136.
Nunally, J.C. (1967). Psychometric theory, (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Nunally, J.C., & Bernstein, I.H. (1994). Psychometric theory, (3rd. ed.). New York: McGraw-
Hill.
Oates, W. (1968). On being a ‘‘workaholic’’ (A serious jest). Pastoral Psychology, 19, 1620.
Oates, W. (1971). Confessions of a workaholic: The facts about work addiction. New York:
World Publishing Co.
Perkin, M.B., Higton, A., & Witcomb, M. (2003). Do junior doctors take sick leave?
Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 60, 699700.
Peterson, R.A. (1994). A meta-analysis of Cronbach’s alpha. Journal of Consumer Research,
21, 381391.
Porter, G. (2001). Workaholic tendencies and the high potential for stress among co-workers.
International Journal of Stress Management, 8, 147164.
Porter, G., & Herring, R.A. (2007). The unlikely referral of workaholics to an employee
assistance program. In R. Burke (Ed.), Work hours and work addiction (pp. 242269).
Northampton, MA: Edward Elgar.
Prins, J.T., Gazendam-Donofrio, S.M., Tubben, B.T., Van der Heijden, F.M.M.A., Van de
Wiel, H.B.M., & Hoekstra-Weebers, J.E.H.M. (2007a). Burnout in medical residents: A
review. Medical Education, 41, 788800.
Prins, J.T., Hoekstra-Weebers, J.E.H.M., Gazendam-Donofrio, S.M., Van de Wiel, H.B.M.,
Sprangers, F., Jaspers, F.C.A., et al. (2007b). The role of social support in burnout among
Dutch medical residents. Psychology. Health & Medicine, 12, 16.
172 W.B. Schaufeli et al.
long work hours culture. Causes, consequences and choices (pp. 203226). Bingley, UK:
Emerald.
Schaufeli, W.B., Taris, T.W., & Van Rhenen, W. (2008). Workaholism, burnout and
engagement: Three of a kind or three different kinds of employee well-being? Applied
Psychology: An International Review, 57, 173203.
Schaufeli, W.B., & Van Dierendonck, D. (2000). Handleiding van de Utrechtse Burnout Schaal
(UBOS) [Manual Utrecht Burnout Scale]. Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets Test Services.
Scott, K.S., Moore, K.S., & Miceli, M.P. (1997). An exploration of the meaning and
consequences of workaholism. Human Relations, 50, 287314.
Shanafelt, T., Bradley, K., Wipf, J., & Back, A. (2002). Burnout and self-reported patient care
in an internal medicine residency program. Annals of Internal Medicine, 136, 358367.
Siegrist, J. (1998). Adverse health effects of effort-reward imbalance at work. In C. Cooper
(Ed.), Theories of organizational stress (pp. 190204). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Sonnentag, S., & Zijlstra, F.R. (2006). Job characteristics and off-job activities as predictors of
need for recovery, well-being, and fatigue. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 330350.
Spector, P.E. (2006). Method variance in organizational research: Truth or urban legend?
Organizational Research Methods, 2, 221232.
Spence, J.T., & Robbins, A.S. (1992). Workaholism: Definition, measurement, and preliminary
results. Journal of Personality Assessment, 58, 160178.
Taris, T.W., & Schaufeli, W.B. (2007). Workaholisme [Workaholism]. In W.B. Schaufeli & A.B.
Bakker (Eds.), De psychologie van arbeid en gezondheid (pp. 259271). Houten, The
Netherlands: Bohn Satfleu van Loghum.
Taris, T.W., Schaufeli, W.B., & Verhoeven, L.C. (2005). Internal and external validation of the
Dutch Work Addiction Risk Test: Implications for jobs and non-work conflict. Applied
Psychology: An international Review, 54, 3760.
Thomas, N.K. (2004). Resident burnout. Journal of the American Medical Association, 292,
28802889.
Tsutsumi, A., Kayaya, K., Theorell, T., & Siegrist, J. (2001). Association between job stress
and depression among Japanese employees threatened by job loss in a comparison between
two complementary job-stress models. Scandinavian Journal of Work Environment and
Health, 27, 146153.
Van Veldhoven, M., De Jonge, J., Broersen, S., Kompier, M., & Meijman, T.F. (2002). Specific
relations between psychosocial job conditions and job-related stress: A three-level analytic
approach. Work & Stress, 16, 207228.