Haydraulic Structure Lecture Note
Haydraulic Structure Lecture Note
Haydraulic Structure Lecture Note
in Classical Conditioning *
FRANCES K. McSWEENEY
CALVIN BIERLEY**
conditioning for consumer research are discussed tioning. In operant conditioning, it is the relation between
throughout the paper. the animal's behavior and the reinforcer or US that
We do not argue that classical conditioning necessarily changes the animal's future behavior. In classical con-
plays an important role in consumer behavior, or that ditioning, it is the relation between some arbitrary stim-
the specific implications discussed here are necessarily ulus (the CS) and the US or reinforcer which changes
correct. The role of classical conditioning in consumer behavior.
behavior and the accuracy of specific implications need Many experiments had already shown that key pecking
to be established by careful experiments. However, initial could be operantly conditioned (i.e., that pecking would
experiments are promising (e.g., Gom 1982), and more increase in frequency when it was followed by a rein-
conclusive experiments cannot be conducted without a forcer). However, the key pecks observed by Brown and
thorough understanding of classical conditioning pro- Jenkins could not be attributed to operant conditioning.
cedures. This paper tries to provide such an understanding Not only had Brown and Jenkins failed to arrange the
in the hope that future experiments will be able to establish relation between the pecking response and its reinforcer,
the role of classical conditioning in consumer research which is necessary for operant conditioning, but also,
in greater detail. later experiments showed that the key peck would still
occur during a Brown and Jenkins procedure even when
THE FORM OF the procedure was modified so that the key peck actually
THE CONDITIONED RESPONSE cancelled the presentation of food (e.g., Williams and
tolerance for pain, a tone which preceded morphine made and that the form of the CR need not resemble the form
them less tolerant of pain (but see also Eikelboom and of the UR.
Stewart 1982 for a different point of view). One implication of these changes for consumer research
Second, Holland (1980) conducted a series of experi- is that if skeletal responses can be classically conditioned,
ments with long-duration CSs in which he found several then classical conditioning can do more for a product
CRs occurring during different parts of the CS. Although than make people feel good about it or make them salivate
these responses took many forms that were difficult to in its presence. If Hearst and Jenkins are correct, then
describe, Holland argued that responses which occurred people will approach a product which is followed by a
in the early part of a long CS took a form that was de- US that is also a reinforcer. To use Gom's example, pre-
termined by the CS, and that those which occured later senting a pen to a person followed by pleasant music
in the CS took a form determined by the US. For example, may produce more approaches to the pen in the store,
if the CS was localizable (e.g., a light), the animal usually as well as a greater consumer preference for the pen.
physically approached and contacted the stimulus during Inducing approach to a product may have several benefits.
the early part of its presentation. If the US was food, then People may be more likely to buy a product that they
the animal usually bit, chewed, or salivated during the approach, and they may be more susceptible to other
later part of the CS. Holland's results have been interpreted forms of advertising or incentives that may also accom-
to mean that more than one conditioned response may pany the product.
occur and that they need not all resemble the UR. A second implication of the revised view of the CR is
conditioning to occur. This statement is almost correct showed that no conditioning occurred when the proba-
and it can be used to correctly identify some situations bility that a US would occur was equal in the presence
in which classical conditioning will not occur. For ex- and absence of the CS. This finding is also incompatible
ample, classical conditioning will not occur if the US with the temporal priority view, which predicts-incor-
precedes the CS. This is called backward conditioning. rectly-that conditioning should occur. The temporal
Although a few laboratory studies do report some back- priority view predicts conditioning if the US occasionally
ward conditioning (e.g., Shurtleff and Ayres 1981), back- occurs after a CS, regardless of what happens in the ab-
ward procedures (US before CS) usually do not work and sence of the CS.
they are generally less effective than equivalent forward Rescorla summarized his findings by saying that clas-
procedures (CS before US). Because backward condi- sical conditioning occurs when a CS predicts rather than
tioning is usually ineffective, playing a jingle (US) and precedes a US. He also argued that a CS predicts a US
then displaying a soft drink (CS) should produce little when the conditional probability of the US is higher in
change in preference (CR) for the soft drink. the presence of the CS than in its absence (Rescorla 1967).
The temporal priority view of classical conditioning Many additional experiments have supported his predic-
also predicts little conditioning when the CS and US are tiveness view over the traditional temporal priority view
presented simultaneously. Although this procedure has of conditioning (e.g., Thomas and Wagner 1964; Wagner
been used in some past studies of classical conditioning et al. 1964), but his definition of predictiveness has not
in marketing (e.g., Gom 1982), laboratory research in- gone unchallenged. Unfortunately, no formal statements
conditioning, it is now necessary to use the two-group A final implication of the predictiveness view is that
experimental design. Otherwise, true classical condition- the number of exposures to the US in the absence of the
ing may be confused with changes in behavior resulting CS should be minimized. To condition preferences using
from CS or US familiarity, or from an interaction between music, a novel tune should be used. If a familiar song is
the two. used, then the US may frequently occur when it has not
The predictiveness view also has some practical im- been predicted by the CS, decreasing the effectiveness of
plications for altering consumer behavior. These impli- conditioning.
cations should be tested experimentally. For example, the To summarize, the CS must predict the US for classical
temporal priority view may imply that a preference for conditioning to be effective. The better the CS predicts
a soft drink could be conditioned by presenting the drink the US, the better the conditioning will be. The CS will
constantly with a jingle played occasionally. Sight of the not predict the US if the CS and US are presented si-
drink would occasionally precede the music. However, multaneously or if the CS is presented constantly with
experiments on classical conditioning have shown that the US introduced only occasionally. The predictiveness
presenting the CS constantly and introducing the US in- of the CS will also be reduced if the CS is encountered
termittently does not produce a conditioned response frequently in the absence of the US or if the US is en-
(Brown and Jenkins 1968). These experiments are con- countered frequently in the absence of the CS.
sistent with the predictiveness view of conditioning. In
this situation, the presence of the CS does not predict
TABLE 1
r
Type Procedure Results Implications
Overshadowing CR develops to CS1 but not CS2 because Remove other salient stimuli which coincide
CS1 US
CS 2 CS1 is more salient. with the CS from the advertisement.
Blocking CS 1 -> US. CR develops to CS1 • but not CS2 • Do not use something familiar as a US.
then CS1 ~US because of the past experience that
CS2 CS1 forecasts the US.
US pre-exposure US only. then CR fails to develop to CS because US Do not use something familiar as a US.
effect CS -> US loses its effectiveness through prior
presentation alone.
Latent inhibition CS only. then CR fails to develop to the CS because Use classical conditioning to change
CS -> US the CS loses its effectiveness through preferences for new products rather than
prior presentation alone. old ones.
Garcia effect CS1 -> US1 CR develops to CS 1 when followed by Pretest your choices of CS and US to make
CS2 US 1 US1 but not when followed by US2 • CR sure conditioning will occur for these
tioning occurs to the new CS (CS2) even though it precedes blocking experiments. The US pre-exposure effect also
and predicts the US. It is as if prior experience with the suggests that a familiar stimulus (e.g., a familiar song)
old CS (CS 1) "blocked" conditioning to the new CS (CS 2). should not be used as a US. Familiarity with the US will
For example, suppose that over many years you have decrease the effectiveness of a classical conditioning pro-
learned that clouds forecast rain. Now you acquire a ba- cedure in altering consumer behavior.
rometer so that you can potentially forecast rain on the
basis of either clouds or barometric pressure. If blocking Latent Inhibition
occurs you will not acquire a classically conditioned re-
sponse (e.g., a disliking) to low barometric pressure even Classical conditioning will not occur if latent inhibition
though it is now a good and salient predictor of rain. In occurs (Lubow 1973; Lubow and Moore 1959). In a latent
a sense, your past experience that clouds are a valid pre- inhibition procedure, the CS is presented several times
dictor of rain prevents you from learning a conditioned without the US. When the CS is later followed by the
response to the barometric pressure. There is no need to US, little conditioning occurs. Latent inhibition implies
condition to the barometric pressure because it provides that it will be easier to classically condition behaviors to
little new information about the US (the chance of rain). new products (CSs) than to products with which people
Blocking suggests that familiar USs should not be used have had previous experience.
if a product is to be presented with stimuli that have
predicted that US in the past. For example, people may Garcia Effect
learn that listening to a particular radio station predicts
that a particular popular song may be played. If a product Classical conditioning does not occur in some cases
(CS 2) is advertised on that station (CS 1) using that par- because of improper choice of the CS that is paired with
ticular song as a US, previous experience may block con- the US. Garcia's experiments on poison-based avoidance
ditioning to the product. That is, people may show an conditioning provide the classic demonstrations of this
increased preference for that radio station, but not for failure of conditioning (Garcia and Koelling 1966). Ba-
the product, even though the product also predicted the sically, Garcia found that an aversion (CR) was easily
song. conditioned to a flavor (CS 1) when that flavor was followed
by sickness (US 1), but an aversion (CR) did not develop
US Pre-Exposure Effect to the same flavor (CS 1) when it was followed by shock
(US 2 ). Likewise, an aversion (CR) did develop to a light
Classical conditioning will not occur if subjects are and a noise (CS 2 ) that were followed by shock (US 2 ), but
given prior exposure to the US presented alone (Mis and it did not develop to the same light and noise (CS 2) when
Moore 1973; Rescorla 1973). Although familarity with a they were followed by sickness (US 1).
stimulus is frequently reported to produce greater liking No one has yet provided a list of rules that would
for that stimulus (Zajonc 1968), familiarity also decreases predict which choices of CSs and USs will produce con-
the effectiveness of that stimulus as a US. The US pre- ditioning when they are paired. One popular speculation
exposure effect is similar to blocking, but the US is pre- is that stimuli that "biologically" belong together, such
sented alone instead of predicted by a CS, as it is in as tastes and sickness, lead to the best conditioning. Stim-
DEVELOPMENTS IN CLASSICAL CONDITIONING 625
TABLE 2
CHARACTERISTICS OF CLASSICAL CONDITIONING
Acquisition CS- US Strength of CR increases Use a salient CS and a strong US. Space commercials
gradually with increases well in time. Do not expect a change in preference
in the number of pairing until several commercials have been given. Present
of the CS with the US. the commercial only rarely after full conditioning has
been achieved.
Extinction CS - US, then CS Strength of CR progressively Do not expect the change in preference to last forever
alone or CS and US decreases. if advertisements are completely discontinued.
presented
randomly
Higher order CS, - US, then CR develops to CS 2 because You may use people and objects associated with
conditioning CS 2 - CS, CS, has acquired the pleasant events as USs.
ability to act as a US.
Discrimination CS, - US and CS 2 CR develops to CS" not to CS 2 . Present rival products without the US, as well as your
uli which are not "biologically" linked, such as tastes and CHARACTERISTICS OF CLASSICALLY
shocks, produce the worst conditioning. However, this CONDITIONED BEHAVIOR
rule is too vague to use in practice, and the implications
of the Garcia effect for altering consumer behavior are Although many of the characteristics of classically con-
clear without such a rule. The Garcia effect implies that ditioned behavior have been known for many years, we
it cannot be assumed that any stimulus can be used as discuss them here because they have obvious implications
a CS and any other stimulus can be used as a US. Instead, for consumer research. These characteristics are sum-
once a CS and US have been chosen, they must be pre- marized in Table 2.
tested to make sure that conditioning does occur when
they are paired. Acquisition
Summary One characteristic of conditioning is that classically
conditioned responses do not appear full blown the first
Taken together, these situations in which conditioning time the CS is followed by the US. Instead, the strength
fails to occur imply that careful attention must be paid of the conditioned response increases as a negatively ac-
to the procedure used if classical conditioning is to occur. celerated function of the number of pairings of the CS
For example, commercials cannot be put together solely with the US (Anrep 1920). This means that the strength
on the basis of intuition or artistic preference. Instead, of the CR increases greatly the first time the CS and US
they should be arranged very carefully to avoid situations are paired. The next pairing produces a slightly smaller
in which conditioning fails to occur. increase, the third pairing a still smaller increase, and so
626 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH
on. Eventually, the strength of the CR reaches an asymp- suggestion already offered to use salient CSs, strong USs,
tote. That is, it does not meaningfully increase with further and longer intertrial intervals to increase the speed of
pairings with the US. conditioning. Again, the only way to determine exactly
The speed of acquisition of CRs can range from one how quickly conditioning will occur is by pretesting the
to many trials, and it is influenced by many factors. For commercial using test groups.
example, the more salient the CS, the faster the condi-
tioning will be and the stronger the CR will be (Kamin Extinction
and Brimer 1963; Rescorla 1972). Conditioning will also
be better for stronger USs (Pavlov 1927; Wagner et al. A second characteristic of classically conditioned be-
1964) and for longer intertrial intervals (Terrace et al. havior is that it will disappear if the predictive relation
1975). The intertrial interval is the time between suc- between the CS and the US is broken (Pavlov 1927). This
cesssive CS-US pairings. Although no precise definition is called extinction, and the CS-US relation may be bro-
of CS salience can be given, CSs that are more intense ken in either of two ways. First, the US may be omitted
physically (e.g., brighter lights) are generally more salient entirely; second, the CS and US may both be presented
than less intense stimuli, and stimuli that have acquired randomly with respect to each other. For example, sup-
some psychological importance (e.g., your name) are more pose a preference for a soft drink has been conditioned
salient than those that have not. USs that are more intense by following that drink (CS) by a jingle (US). That con-
physically (e.g., more severe footshocks) are generally ditioned preference will disappear if the soft drink is pre-
and US will be broken and extinction will occur rapidly other breakfast cereals which resemble the one actually
when the commercial is discontinued. Using novel USs used.
in commercials which employ conditioning will avoid In practical situations, generalization may be a two-
this problem and prolong the life of the conditioned re- edged sword. On the one hand, a product can be made
sponse even after the commercials are discontinued. as distinctive as possible in order to decrease generalization
and to restrict the benefits of advertising to that particular
Higher Order Conditioning product, but at the same time, that product will not benefit
from the advertising of other products. On the other hand,
A third characteristic of classical conditioning is higher a product can be made as similar as possible to other
order conditioning. In higher order conditioning, one CS products in order to increase generalization and the ben-
(CS 1) is followed by a US until a response is conditioned. efits from other products' advertising, but at the same
Then a new CS (CS2 ) is followed by the old CS (CSt). A time, the other products will also benefit from the ad-
response becomes conditioned to the new CS (CS 2) even vertising of the first product. Decisions regarding which
though that CS has never been followed by the US itself approach to take may depend on whether a company or
(Pavlov 1927). The old CS (CS 1) has acquired the ability its competitors are doing more advertising. If other com-
to act as a "higher order" US. Although higher order panies are doing more advertising, then a company may
conditioning was once thought to be a fragile phenomenon wish to increase generalization by making its product
which might not occur outside of the laboratory, recent more similar to the more heavily advertised products. If
it will evoke a response which is opposite to that evoked at the present time. For example, at first glance, it might
by the CS that does predict the US. A commercial in seem that the question of whether awareness is needed
which a product is followed by music and a rival's product for conditioning could be easily answered by conducting
is not should not only increase preference for the first a conditioning experiment and then asking subjects to
product, it should also decrease preference for the rival's decribe what happened. The role of awareness would be
product. demonstrated if the subjects who described the CS-US
relation showed conditioning and those who did not de-
Conditioned Inhibition. The conditioned inhibition scribe the relation showed no conditioning. However,
procedure is another situation in which inhibition occurs such an experiment would actually establish very little.
(Pavlov 1927). In a conditioned inhibition procedure, First, the results could be used to support almost any
one stimulus is presented alone and then followed by a interpretation. Suppose, for example, that conditioning
US. The same stimulus is not followed by the US when was found in subjects who could not report the CS-US
it is accompanied by another stimulus. The second stim- relation. Then the proponents of the view that awareness
ulus becomes inhibitory. To use our example, a com- is necessary for conditioning could argue that some
mercial in which a pen is followed by music when it is "aware" subjects were actually classified as "unaware."
presented alone and is not followed by music when it is According to this argument, more careful questioning
presented with a rival pen should condition inhibition to would have revealed this rudimentary awareness.
the rival pen. That is, not only will preference increase This example should illustrate the fact that the role of
cies. Classical conditioning has been observed in many mental processes" are necessary for conditioning when
nonhuman species (see Thorpe 1963 for a review). It is people serve as subjects. But we are arguing that the role
hard to argue that the CR is produced by the contem- of higher mental processes in conditioning cannot be
plation of the CS-US relation when rats or rabbits serve conclusively established by current techniques. Focusing
as subjects. Therefore, if one argues that conditioning on this question now may divert attention from questions
occurs in people because of "higher mental processes," which can be answered, to the detriment of research on
one must also argue that classical conditioning occurs for conditioning.
qualitatively different reasons at different points on the
evolutionary scale. Such a claim should be avoided unless CONCLUDING COMMENTS
the case for the role of "higher mental processes" in hu-
mans is overwhelming, and it is not. This paper has presented some of our current knowl-
The weakness of the case for "higher mental processes" edge about classical conditioning and its potential im-
in human conditioning becomes more apparent when plications for consumer research. Some words of caution
one realizes that analogues of many of the experiments are needed before concluding.
cited by Brewer have been conducted, using nonhuman First, we are not arguing that classical conditioning
animals, with similar results. For example, Brewer showed necessarily plays an important role in consumer behavior.
that instructing human subjects that a CS will be followed Most of our information about classical conditioning
by a US produced a CR. But a similar phenomenon can comes from carefully controlled laboratory studies. These
Skinner, B. F. (1938), The Behavior of Organisms: An Exper- the Rabbit," Journal of Comparative and Physiological
imental Analysis, Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Psychology, 58 (I), 157-158.
Smith, Marius c., Steven R. Coleman, and I. Gormezano (1969), Thorpe, W. H. (1963), Learning and Instinct in Animals. Cam-
"Classical Conditioning of the Rabbit's Nictitating Mem- bridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press.
brane Response at Backward, Simultaneous, and Foreward Wagner, Allan R., Shepard Siegel, Earl Thomas, and Gaylord
CS-US Intervals," Journal of Comparative and Physio- D. Ellison (1964), "Reinforcement History and the Ex-
logical Psychology, 69 (2), 226-231. tinction of a Conditioned Salivary Response," Journal of
Steinhauer, G. D., G. H. Davol, and A. Lee (1976), "Acquisition Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 58 (3), 354-
ofthe Autoshaped Key Peck as a Function of Amount of 358.
Preliminary Magazine Training," Journal of the Experi- Williams, David R. and Harriet Williams (1969), "Auto-
mental Analysis of Behavior, 25 (3), 355-359. Maintenance in the Pigeon: Sustained Pecking Despite
Terrace, H. S., J. Gibbon, L. Farrell, and M. D. Baldock (1975), Contingent Non-Reinforcement," Journal of the Experi-
"Temporal Factors Influencing the Acquisition and mental Analysis of Behavior, 12 (4),511-520.
Maintenance of an Autoshaped Keypeck," Animal Learn- Zajonc, Robert B. (1968), "Attitudinal Effects of Mere Expo-
ing and Behavior, 3 (I), 53-62. sure," Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology Mono-
Thomas, Earl and Allan R. Wagner (1964), "Partial Reinforce- graph Supplement, 9 (Part 2, June), 1-27.
ment of the Classically Conditioned Eyelid Response in