Linguistics - Chapter 07
Linguistics - Chapter 07
are of relatively little use, a fact known by anyone who has visited
a foreign country armed only with a dictionary, and no
knowledge of the language. Does me - bus mean 'I am a bus', 'A
bus hit me', 'I carne by bus', or 'I want to go by bus'? So let us
now look at how words may be combined together into longe r
utterances.
In this chapter, we shall consider, first, the ways in which words
may be linked together to form larger units. Second, we shall
discuss how to analyze sentences into their component parts, or
constituents in linguistic terminology. Third, we shall suggest
ways of representing this analysis. o
•••••••
Word order
The device used most frequently in English is word order:
The large spider (rightened Aunt Matilda.
Aunt Matilda frightened the large spider.
The words themselves in these two sentences are identical. It is
the word order which indicates who frightened whom, and that
it is the spider which is large, not Aunt Matilda. Languages
which rely heavily on word order are known as configurational
languages.
Inflections
In a language such as Latin, word endings or inflections,
indica te the relationship bet'Neen words. In the sentence:
Magna aranea perterruit Matildam amitam.
Large spider frightened Matilda aunt
This chapter discusses the 'The large spider frightened Aunt Ma tilda' .
ways in which words can be the word order is irrelevant. The sentence would still mean the
linked together to form larger
units. It explains how to
same if the words were arranged quite differently as in:
analyze sentences into their Magna Matildam perterruit amitam aranea.
'constituents' (component Large Matilda frightened aunt spider
parts), and shows ways of
representing this type of
analvsis.
70 The endings alone show that it was the spider whicn terrificd ln this sentence, the and duck ean be replaced by a singlc word 71
Aunt Matilda, not the rcverse, and that it is the spider, not Aunt su~h as Dana/d, it, without altering the basic sentenee pattcrn.
Matilda, which is large. ln linguistic terminology, Latin is a non- Thls suggests that these two words are closely linked and
configurational language. Word order is not critical, though together constitute a single, larger eomponent. 5imilarl~, the
some word order preferenees are found. words the and burglar go together, since they also eould be
replaeed by a word sueh as Albert, him. 50 as a first stage, we
Function words have reduced a sentence with five original eomponents down to
three more basie ones (Figure 7.1):
Another eommon device, used to some extent in both English
and Latin, is the use of function words. These are words sueh
as 0(, by, that, which indica te relationships between parts of I the burglar
o
•••••••
the sentence:
Aunt Matilda was terri(ied by a spider .
[ The duck
figure 7.1
o
•••••••
The advantage of a tree diagram is that each join or node on
the tree can be labelled, so that the whole construction becomes
means 'Replace the symbol VP by V NP'.
NP-D N
~
DAUGHTER DAUGHTER
~~
SISTERS
figure 7.5
S
belong together as a constituent elsewhere, since words that are
75
NP----------- VP
grou~ed tog.ether in
constltuent 111 other
one sentence are likely to recur as a single
sentences. One way of checking this is to
~ ~ construct sentcnces in which the original words occur in a
o N V NP diffcrent order:
As we have seen, every sentenee ean be broken down into The mouse ate up the cheese.
successive layers of eonstituents. However, not all sentences can We can show the difference by switching the sentence around:
be analyzed with as little trouble as The duck bit the burglar. *Up the cheese ate the mouse.
Consider the sentence:
(Compare: Up the clock ran the mouse.)
The mouse ran up the clock. The mouse ate the cheese up.
How should this be analyzed? Should we braeket [ran up] (Compare: *The mouse ran the clock up.)
together, on the assumption that these words eould be replaeed We may therefore analyze the second sentence as:
by a word such as climbed? Or should we braeket [up the
[The mouse] [ate up] [the cheese.]
clock] together, noting that the whole phrase could be replaced
by a single word such as upwards? Problems of this type are and draw the tree diagram as in Figure 7.9, using the extra
solved by seeing whether the groups of words in question node-Iabe1s VB for 'phrasal verb' and PRT for 'particle':
76
s Of course, other types of phrase can occur in some of thesc 77
positions. But an NP such as the cat can occur in all of them.
;
~
~
N VP
---------- NP
Consequently, if we find a phrase which we suspect might be an
NP, we can apply these (and other) tests. For example, consider
I
::l
o O N VB the sentences below:
CIl
lhe mouse
10 order to find out whcther thc first two words in each seoteoce
ate up the cheese
are an NP, we cao apply the NP tests listed below:
o
••••••••
figure 7.9
changes on a few recurring patterns. It is therefore possible to • At the end of a sentence after a verb:
build up a store of specific 'tests' for the presence of a particular The cat scratched Uncle Harry.
constituent in a given language. As up the clock suggests, one test *The cat scratched suddenly Harry.
for a PP (preposition phrase = phrase containing a preposition) is • After by in a passive senteoce:
that a preposition cannot immediately follow its NP. Just as you The cat was kicked by Uncle Harry.
cannot say: *The cat was kicked by suddenly Harry.
*The mouse ran the clock up. • After an auxiliary verb in questions:
Did Uncle Harry kick the cat?
so you cannot say:
*Fene/la went the woods into. ••Did sudd~trly Harry kick the cat?
The failure of suddenly Harry to pass most of these NP tests
*Doris swam the bridge under.
shows that it cannot be an NP, whereas the success af Uncle
Let us now go on to consider this notion of 'tests' further, by Harry indicates that it probably is an NP.
considering 'NP tests'.
NP
------------- VP
VP -. V NP PP
So far, then, we have three different rcwritc rulcs for ElIglish
~ ~ VPs:
D N V PP
VP -. V NP The duck bit the burglar.
~
P NP VP -. V (PP) The duck slept. The duck slept in the bat/;o
VP -. V NP PP The burglar put the duck in a sack.
~
D N It would be useful to combine these three separate rulcs. As a
I I first suggestion, one might simply number the types of verb (Vi
The duck slept in the bath for a verb such as bit, V2 for slept, V3 for put), and enclose
them in another type of bracket { } which is used to denote
alternative possibilities:
figure 7.10
N
above, since one can slot in a verb only if it fits the structure
choscn. For cxample, suppose we had chosen both the optional
items, NP and PP, we must then slot in a verb followed by NP
I I PP, in this case put. Similarly, if we had chosen V alone, the only
80 V which fits in this case is sleep. Maurice took a photograph of a p/atypus. 81
I
le
With a detailed lexicon of this type, which can be cxpanded to
include other word classes also, we no longe r need substitution
~
S
I
rules such as: V -t bit, N -t burglar. NP VP
Let us therefore summarize the rewrite rules and lexical cntries
for: ~
V NP
The duck bit the burglar.
~
The duck slept. O ?
The duck s/ept in the bath.
The burglar put the duck in a sack.
A Rewrite rules N
~
~
o
••••••••
P NP
S -tNP VP
VP -t V (NP) (PP) ~
NP-tD N O N
PP -tp NP \ I
Maurice took a photograph of a platypus
B Lexicon
burg/ar N figure 7.12
duck N
sack N The sequence a photograph of a platypus is c1early a noun
bath N phrase (NP) and the words photograph and p/atypus are no uns
bit V [-NP] (N). But what of the intervening node, comprising photogra{Jh
s/ept V [-(PP)] af a platypus? It seems to be something between an N and a fu!,!
put V [-NP PP] NP, so what is it? A useful solution is to give the label N
the D (pronounced N-bar, since it has a bar along the top) to
a D something that is neither ª
simple N, nor a whole NP. Some
tn P people ais o give the label N (N-double bar) to a whole NP. (In
Figure 7.13, a triangle has been drawn in place of the details of
af course, if more data had been considered, the rules and the the PP. This is a standard procedure which avoids wasting time
lexicon would have to be complicated further. For example, if and space when the details are irrelevant to the point under
we had included a proper name such as Donald, the lexicon discussion. )
wouId have to specify which nouns are found with a determiner
(D), as in the duck, a sack, and which not, as in Donald not *a NP ar fJ
Donald. However, we set out to write ruIes for the sentence
patterns in question, and we have done this as economically as ~
possibIe. O N
~
N PP
Layers of branches I ~
a photograph of a platypus
The tree diagrams we have considered so far have reIatively few
Iayers. But consider a sentence such as:
figure 7.13
The use of bars has one further major advantage: they can be IP
82 83
used with adjectives (A), verbs (V), and prepositions (P), as welJ
~
as with nouns (N). It is then easy to see similarities in structure I V
between NPs, APs (adjective phrascs), VPs and PPs which wcre
not so evident before. It turns out that the head (main word) in I 1
to fish
one type of phrase is in a very similar position to the head in
will
another. ln other words, a noun in an NP is likely to be in a
-ed
paralJellocation to an adjective in an AP, a verb in a VP and a
preposition in a PP. For example, the AP very proud of the figure 7.15
platypus has a structure that is similar in its branching pattern
to the NP a photograph of the platypus. (ln Figure 7.14, DEG
stands for 'degree'.) Complex sentences
AP arà $0 far, we have assumed that ali sentences are simple ones such as:
~ The duck bit the burglar.
DEG A The mouse ran up the c/ock.
~ In practice, however, many sentences have one or more sentence-
A PP like structures attached to them or inserted inside them. Consider:
I ~ Archibald played tennis, and Peter went fishing.
very proud of the platypus
Here we have two sub-sentences of equal importance attached
together to form a single one. This process is known as
figure 7.14 conjoining. lntheory an indefinite number of sentences could be
joinecl together:
A number of details still have to be worked out concerning X- Archibald played tennis, and Peter went fishing, and Pip
bar syntax or X-bar theory, the na me of this method of dealing played cricket, and Mary washed her hair, and Drusilla
with sentence patterns. For example, there is some controversy cIimbed the EiffeI tower ...
as to how many layers of bars it is useful to set up. But the
theory appears to be here to stay, and it plays an important role However, conjoining is not the only process by which sentence-
in Chomsky's work. like structures are linkecl together. More often subsidiary
sentences are inserted into one main sentence. This is known as
Yet another way of handling layers has emerged in recent years, embedding (Figure 7.16):
partly combined with X-bar syntax. Functional phrases, that is,
phrases introduced by function words (Chapter 6) have a The rumour that the dinosaur had escaped worried the publico
structure similar to lexical phrases, it has been claimed. For
example, inflections (verb attachments) and the accompanying I The rumour worried the public I
verb can be labelled an inflectional phrase (IP). 50 to fish, will
fish, and fished are ali IPs. In English, the inflection, I,
some times known as INFL, mostly comes before the verb, as
with to, will, though some times aner it, as with fished (Figure figure 7.16
7.15).
In theory, a sentence can have an indefinite number of sentences Mavis believes the burglar took the duck. (Figure 7.19). 85
embedded in it. In The (act that the rumour that the dinosaur
had escaped worried the lJUblic is not surprising, the simple s ~
:J
..•
<li
sentence has two others embedded in it (Figure 7.17). ~
:J
(')
<li
NP VP 'O
Simple senlence: DI
I lhe fact is not surprising I I~ ~
N V S 3cn
Embedding 1: worried the public
II~ believes
Mavis lhe burglar took the duck
Embedding 2: (that) the dinosaur had escaped
o
•••••••
figure 7.17 figure 7.19 o
•••••••
Another example of embedding is the old nursery rhyme (Figure 50 far, this chapter has shown how linguists analyze sentence
7.18). patterns, with particular attention to configurationallanguages
(those which rely on word order). There are extra problems
involved in the investigation of non-configurational languages,
lhis is the cat ] but the notion of checking whether one constituent (component
part of sentence) can be substituted for another is basic to a11
syntactic analyses.
Questions
1 Suggest three ways in which languages show the relationship
of one word to another.
2 What is a tree diagram, and why is it usefu/?
3 What are rewrite rules?
4 Draw a tree diagram for each of the following sentences:
The bus ran down the dog.
The boy ran down the street.
5 Distinguísh between conjoining and embedding. Give
examples.
6 What are thematic relations?