Zvara 2019
Zvara 2019
Zvara 2019
To cite this article: B. J. Zvara, M. Burchinal & the Family Life Project Key Contributors (2019):
Maternal history of childhood maltreatment and children's cognitive and social development, Early
Child Development and Care, DOI: 10.1080/03004430.2019.1621861
Article views: 20
Childhood sexual trauma (CST) results in significant and long-lasting consequences for the victim
(Banyard, 1997; Putnam, 2003). Women with CST histories report numerous psychosocial problems
in adulthood including elevated rates of depression and depressive symptoms (Briere & Elliott,
1994; Cry, McDuff, & Wright, 2006). Many women with CST histories report problems parenting
and nurturing their children (DiLillo, Giuffre, Tremblay, & Peterson, 2001; Trickett, Noll, & Putnam,
2011; Zvara, Mills-Koonce, Carmody, Cox, & Family Life Project Key Investigators, 2015). There is
increasing evidence that maternal history of CST places the next generation at risk for maladaptive
developmental outcomes (Collishaw, Dunn, O’Connor, & Golding, 2007; Roberts, O’Connor, Dunn,
& Golding, 2004; Zvara et al., 2015). Much of this research suggests a mediational pathway linking
maternal CST to behaviour problems and conduct disorders in offspring (Roberts et al., 2004;
Zvara et al., 2017) through parenting behaviour, parenting stress, and maternal mental health.
CONTACT Bharathi J. Zvara [email protected] Gillings School of Global Public Health, The University of North Carolina
at Chapel Hill, 220 Rosenau Hall, CB #7400, Chapel Hill, NC 27599-7400, USA
*The Family Life Project (FLP) Key Investigators include Lynne Vernon Feagans, The University of North Carolina; Martha Cox, The
University of North Carolina; Clancy Blair, New York University; Peg Burchinal, The University of North Carolina; Linda Burton, Duke
University; Keith Crnic, The Arizona State University; Ann Crouter, The Pennsylvania State University; Patricia Garrett-Peters, The
University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill; Mark Greenberg, The Pennsylvania State University; Stephanie Lanza, The Pennsylvania
State University; Roger Mills-Koonce, The University of North Carolina at Greensboro; Emily Werner, The Pennsylvania State Uni-
versity and Michael Willoughby, The University of North Carolina.
This article has been republished with minor changes. These changes do not impact the academic content of the article.
© 2019 Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group
2 B. J. ZVARA ET AL.
Despite the growing interest reflected in the literature, the field of study has numerous limitations,
including sampling, because much of the prior research has come from clinical samples or
samples of convenience (DiLillo, 2001), thereby weakening the generalizability of the results. In
the current study, a propensity matched design was used to extend current knowledge and under-
standing of adjustment of children whose mothers reported a history of CST compared to children
whose mothers did not report childhood trauma.
There is robust evidence that children with better socioemotional adjustment and rudimentary
academic skills at school entry may be better poised for later success (Duncan et al., 2007; Sabol &
Pianta, 2012). This body of work supports the premise that home environments characterized as
responsive and nurturing to child needs, lay the foundation to achieve language milestones
(Landry, Smith, Swank, Assel, & Vellet, 2001), score higher on cognitive tests (Landry, Smith, Swank,
& Miller-Loncar, 2000), develop better social skills (Calkins, Smith, Gill, & Johnson, 1998), and have
fewer emotional and behaviour problems (Goldberg, Corter, Lojkasek, & Minde, 1990). This may be
problematic for children of mothers reporting a history of CST given elevated rates of depression
and parenting difficulties among women with trauma histories. However, despite the growing inter-
est in the development of offspring of mothers with CST histories, little is known or understood about
the school adjustment for children of CST survivors. In the current study, we examine socioemotional
and cognitive development in middle childhood, a period when children are transitioning to formal
schooling and must adjust to the demands of classroom routines (Pianta, Rimm-Kaufman, & Cox,
1999). Given the multiple risk factors to children whose mothers report a history of CST, it is likely
that they may be at greater risk for early school difficulties, however, these associations have not
been well examined.
Furthermore, much of the current research examining the relations between maternal history
CST and offspring adjustment has centred on two developmental stages, infancy/toddlerhood
and adolescence and far less attention has been on other stages of adjustment such as early
school aged children. Middle childhood, defined as a period between ages six to 12, is also critical
years that carry long-lasting influence (Weisner, 1984). This developmental stage, as children tran-
sition to formal schooling, is marked by adaptation to the demands of classroom settings, nego-
tiating academic skills, and building peer relationships. Although early research posited middle
childhood as a dormant period (Long, Henderson, & Ziller, 1967), recent advances in neuroscience
suggest that during this developmental period, the brain is actively undergoing synaptic pruning,
which is a process whereby some areas are enhanced, while others are selectively diminished (For
review, See, Knudsen, 2004; Mah & Ford-Jones, 2012), reflecting a process of gradual consolidation
of abilities and behaviours. Research further supports that this process is heavily dependent on
the child’s environment (Feldman & Knudsen, 1998; Fox, Levitt, & Nelson, 2010).
problematic outcomes including greater behavioural problems, academic difficulties, and higher
dropout rates (Burchinal, Roberts, Zeisel, Hennon, & Hooper, 2006; Webster-Stratton, Reid, &
Hammond, 2004).
Similarly, there is growing evidence that children with language and general knowledge deficits
often experience problems gaining acceptance and avoiding rejection by peers (Pentimonti, Murphy,
Justice, Logan, & Kaderavek, 2016). The ability to communicate effectively aides children in under-
standing play activities with peers (Birch & Ladd, 1997). Further, evidence supports the premise
that social competence and academic skills are related (Graziano, Reavis, Keane, & Calkins, 2007)
with findings suggesting that behaviour problems undermine academic achievement and vice
versa across childhood and into adolescence (Masten et al., 2005).
Despite the clinical and research interest in the development of children whose mothers report a
history of childhood sexual trauma, much of the existing literature examining an intergenerational
consequence of CST examined childhood maltreatment more broadly (i.e. physical abuse, neglect)
(Noll, 2008; Zuravin & Fontanella, 1999). Although limited, existing research provides evidence that
having a mother with a history of CST leaves children at increased risk for maladjustment including
behaviour problems (Collishaw et al., 2007; Roberts et al., 2004; Zvara et al., 2017) as reported by
mothers. What is not yet known is if maternal trauma history is related to elevated risk for learning
deficits for children. This is particularly important given evidence achievement gaps as early as in kin-
dergarten forecast poorer performance throughout the academic and life trajectory (Alexander,
Entwisle, & Dauber, 1993; Baydar, Brooks-Gunn, & Furstenberg, 1993; Duncan et al., 2007; Gutman,
Sameroff, & Cole, 2003) and that early gaps in achievement tend to increase over time (Alexander,
Entwisle, & Olson, 2001). Given that approximately one of five women in the United States report
a history of CST, (Pereda, Guilera, Forns, & Gómez-Benito, 2009), understanding the classroom experi-
ences of children with mothers reporting CST has important implications for therapeutic intervention.
Moreover, although there is increasing interest in understanding the relations between maternal
history of CST and offspring adjustment, the current body of knowledge is correlational in nature. A
randomized controlled trial would allow for causal inference, but the very nature of CST would make
it impossible to use a randomized trial methodology to study the effects of maternal CST on offspring.
However, the use of econometric quasi-experimental methods, including propensity score analysis
have the potential to minimize selection bias allowing for stronger causal inference. This statistical
approach was developed by Rosenbaum and Rubin (1983) to draw causal inferences from observa-
tional data. The central premise of propensity score matching (PSM) is identifying a ‘treatment’ (e.g.
CST history or No CST history). It then matches the treated and the untreated on a variety of back-
ground and individual characteristics, thus achieving statistically what randomization to treatment
and control conditions would achieve by design.
Current study
The current study sought to address a significant gap in the literature on the associations between
maternal CST and offspring adjustment in the classroom setting. Using novel statistical methods, we
examined whether a maternal history of CST was related to their child’s (a) social competence and (b)
cognitive development at school entry. This study is among the first to test the associations between
maternal childhood trauma history and offspring adjustment using a quasi-experimental design and
has the potential to provide key information to inform targeted interventions.
Appalachian Mountains in Central Pennsylvania. A birth cohort of 1,292 families enrolled in the
FLP by completing the first home visit when the family’s infant was two months old. Of these,
144 biological mothers reported that they had experienced childhood sexual trauma at or before
the age of 14. We used propensity score matching procedures to create a contrast group of families
based on carefully chosen covariates (the procedures for the propensity matching are described in
greater detail in the analysis plan). Briefly, women who reported CST (as assessed by the Trauma
History Interview, Green, 1996) were matched on mothers’ childhood demographic variables
from the family of origin to a group of women (controls) who did not report a history of CST
(n = 204). Data for the matching procedures were collected from home visits at child age 2-, 36-
and 58- months old. At the Kindergarten and First grade assessments, teachers completed question-
naires about the child’s academic and social competence in the classroom setting, via a secure
online survey site.
Measures
Trauma history questionnaire
At either the 36- or 58-month home visit, all participants in the FLP study completed the THQ (Green,
1996) depending on time restriction for the visit. Participants only completed the THQ once. A 24-
item self-report measure, THQ examines experiences with potentially traumatic events, such as
crime, general disaster, and sexual and physical assault, using a yes/no format. For each event
endorsed, respondents were asked to provide the frequency of the event, their age at the time
the event occurred, and the nature of their relationship with the abuser. For the purposes of this
study, the item relating to sexual abuse/assault asked, ‘Has anyone ever done something sexual to
you against your will, such as made you have intercourse, oral or anal sex, touched private parts
of your body, or made you touch theirs, or otherwise forced you to have unwanted sexual
contact?’ If answered yes, follow-up questions asked about the age at the time of the sexual
trauma, frequency, and relationship of the victim to the perpetrator.
Covariates
For the current study we report two sets of covariates. First, to generate the propensity matched
sample, we used maternal report of family of origin demographics variables. The selection of match-
ing variables for the PSM were chosen based on theoretical and empirical considerations. Children
with verified sexual trauma histories often come from home environments that can be characterized
as having a lack of economic resources, parents with a low level of education, and adverse environ-
mental conditions such as social isolation (Erickson & Egeland, 2002; Ethier, Couture, & Lacharité,
2004). Thus, the covariates for the PSM procedures included maternal childhood demographic infor-
mation including whether the participants’ family of origin received AFDC (Aid to Families with
Dependent Children), food stamps, or Medicaid or lived in public housing. Mothers’ education
level from the family of origin and additional trauma’s experienced in childhood (e.g. physical
abuse) were used as the matching variables. By controlling for additional maltreatment and
trauma experiences of childhood through the matching procedures, we were able to isolate the
effects of CST. Details of matching procedures have been previously published (Zvara et al., 2015).
Second, for the analyses examining group differences for offspring of mothers with and without
CST histories, we included a broad range of covariates that were found in the previous research to be
associated with child socioemotional and cognitive development (Bradley & Corwyn, 2002; Dodge &
Pettit, 2003; McLoyd, 1998). Covariates included family’s income-to-needs ratio, maternal education,
race and child sex. Given that the overwhelming majority of African American families resided in one
study site, study location was also included as a covariate to address a potential confound between
site and ethnicity.
differences for children’s social competence and academic skills were tested using MANOVA to draw
inferences about the correlation between the groups. All tests were run with alpha = .05 and were
performed using SPSS (version 19.0).
Results
Preliminary analyses
As previously reported (Zvara et al., 2015) the PSM yielded a sample of 204 mothers 105 with CST
histories matched to 99 mothers without trauma histories. Although all 144 women reporting CST
were entered into the PSM procedures, thirty-nine (39) of these participants did not have exact
matches with women in the not abused group, and thus were not included in the final subsample
for this analysis. The final subsample for the current study after completion of the PSM procedures
was (n = 204) 105 who experienced childhood sexual trauma matched to 99 women who did not,
with similar background experiences.
In all, we ran two diagnostic tests to examine the validity of the PSM procedures. The results of the
diagnostic tests suggested that the matching methods performed well at reducing the standardized
mean difference with each covariate when compared to the unmatched data (Table 1). Meaning, that
based on the selected covariates, the two groups are more similar to each other than with the larger
sample (i.e. the full FLP sample from which the comparison group was drawn). A second diagnostic
test was with the use of logistic regression. All matching variables were regressed on childhood
trauma history. There were no significant differences between the groups post-matching on any
of these variables. This means that, based on childhood family of origin variables, the two groups
were statistically not different from each other leaving childhood sexual trauma (yes or no) as the
one differentiator among those variables considered.
Descriptive statistics
The children in the CST group were 50% male and 50% African American and in the not CST (NCST) group
were 49% male and 44% African American. Independent sample t-tests (see Table 2) indicated no signifi-
cant difference between the two groups with regard to maternal age or maternal education, however, the
NCST group had significantly lower 5–54 month mean income-to-needs ratio, t (204) = 2.53, p < .001.
Table 1. Balance checking before and after propensity matching for abused and not abused sample.
Before Matching After Matching
SMD p-value SMD p-value % reduction in bias
Childhood Demographics of the Mother
Received Aid to Dependent Children .019 .35 −.029 .32 12%
Received food stamps .15 .07 .03 .44 24%
Received Medicaid .05 .09 −.06 .14 22%
Received public housing .05 .004 −.017 .54 17%
Mother education (from family of origin) −.35 .002 −.19 .40 4.5%
*Other trauma before age 14 .04 .006 −.02 .37 6.5%
Note. SMD: Standardized mean difference; * traumas other than sexual abuse (e.g. physical abuse) were summed and used as a
covariate in the matching procedures.
EARLY CHILD DEVELOPMENT AND CARE 7
childsex and study location (Table 3). The overall model indicated significant CST group differ-
ences on the social competence measures, F (5, 89) = 2.57, p < .05; Wilkes Lambda = .90; Partial
eta squared = .10. Review of the findings revealed that Levene’s test of equality of error variances
were non-significant for all dependent variables. With a significant MANOVA, the individual
ANOVAs were examined for CST group differences. Findings suggest that children whose
mothers reported CST, were reported to exhibit greater oppositional behaviour ((β = .33, p
< .05), F (1, 89 = 5.7, p < .05)); peer problems, ((β = .14, p < .05), F (1, 89) = 4.3, p < .05); and
conduct problems, ((β = .28, p < .01). (F (1, 89 = 10.0, p < .01)).
Discussion
There is growing evidence to support the hypothesis that CST is associated with consequences across
the lifespan (Briere & Jordan, 2009; Briere & Runtz, 1990; Cicchetti & Toth, 2005). Less well understood
is how the impact of maternal CST history exerts its influence on the next generation. The current
study adds to the body of evidence that children of mothers with trauma histories may be at risk
for developmental difficulties across domains of functioning necessary for school success. Methodo-
logically, this study has several strengths. By using a large sample of mothers with extensive data, we
were able to employ propensity matching techniques to create groups of children whose mothers
had similar family of origin backgrounds but differed on whether they reported a history of CST.
Using measure of psychological adjustment and cognitive and academic skills collected from tea-
chers, this study adds additional rigour to the study of CST and its potential impact across
generations.
This study extends the current research on the relations between maternal history of CST by
demonstrating that maternal childhood trauma history is associated with significant risk to the
social and cognitive development of children whose mothers report a history of CST. Overall, children
with mothers reporting a history of CST manifested more problems in social competence than did
children in the control group. Specifically, after controlling for numerous child and family factors,
including maternal education, race, household income-to-needs, child sex, and study site, children
whose mother reported CST were observed by their teachers to show more aggressive and opposi-
tional behaviour, conduct problems, and have difficulties with peer relationships. Moreover, children
in the CST group were also reported by teachers as having scored lower on the picture vocabulary
scores of the Woodcock-Johnson (WJ-III). When considered against the backdrop of the larger litera-
ture on school readiness and achievement gaps, the findings from this study suggest that children of
mothers with CST histories may be at elevated risk for difficulties in the classroom setting and learn-
ing deficits. Given evidence that early difficulties in social and academic capabilities tend to increase
over time (Alexander et al., 2001), the findings from this study would suggest that intervention and
prevention efforts focused on mothers with CST histories should be expanded to include children.
These findings may best be interpreted within the larger literature on developmental psycho-
pathology that has shown that family environmental characteristics such as parenting behaviour
and parental depression are among the strongest predictors of social and cognitive adjustment in
young children (NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 1999). Given the existing evidence that
mothers with trauma histories report greater depression and parenting problems (Briere & Elliott,
1994; Cry et al., 2006; Zvara et al., 2015), it is likely that mothers in the CST group may engage in beha-
viours that impact children’s development by not providing the support, scaffolding,and modelling
necessary for children to acquire competency in these skills. Given previous work by Landry et al.
(2001) suggesting that consistent and predictable caregiving facilitates children’s engagement
with the learning and social environment, it may be that mothers struggling with own emotional
states may be limited in their capacity to foster a warm, stimulating, and enriching environment
for their child.
The growing evidence that middle childhood is a key developmental period during which children
master and consolidate skills critical for school success provides an opportunity to identify areas for
targeted intervention to mitigate the effects of maternal psychopathology. This brings into focus to
the need for developmental models to understand the complex mediating influences on children’s
successful adaptation to the school environment, particularly for children whose mothers report a
history of CST. Given the substantial number of children impacted by maternal trauma history,
early intervention in schools, aimed at enhancing self-regulatory and social skills may benefit children
across multiple domains. There is consistent evidence that behaviour problems and academic
success are related, with more recent reports suggesting a bidirectional relationship between the
two (Kremer, Flower, Huang, & Vaughn, 2016; Morgan, Farkas, Tufis, & Sperling, 2008). This would
suggest children’s social behaviour can promote or undermine their academic skills and their
EARLY CHILD DEVELOPMENT AND CARE 9
academic success may have implications for their behaviour, as well as their opportunities to develop
social relationships and skills. Identifying young children whose mothers report a history of CST and
providing targeted interventions to increase picture and word knowledge may improve academic
performance, both in the short and long term.
Disclosure statement
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.
Funding
Support for this research was provided by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development
(1R03HD090430-01A1, Zvara, PI) awarded to the lead author.
Notes on contributors
B. J. Zvara, Ph.D., is an Assistant Professor in the Department of Maternal and Child Health at the Gillings School of Global
Public Health at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill. Her research focuses on the family system as the primary
context for children’s socioemotional health and development with a particular focus on low income and high-risk
contexts.
M. Burchinal, Ph.D., is a Senior Scientist at the Frank Porter Graham Child Development Institute at the University of North
Carolina at Chapel Hill. She is a leading researcher and statistician in child care research and a widely recognized applied
statistician. Dr. Burchinal studies growth curve methodology and the short- and long-term impacts of early care and edu-
cation, especially for children at-risk due to poverty.
The Family Life Project (FLP) Key Investigators include Lynne Vernon-Feagans, The University of North Carolina; Mark
Greenberg, The Pennsylvania State University; Martha Cox, The University of North Carolina; Clancy Blair, New York
University; Margaret Burchinal, The University of North Carolina; Michael Willoughby, The University of North Carolina;
Patricia Garrett-Peters, The University of North Carolina; Roger Mills-Koonce, The University of North Carolina.
10 B. J. ZVARA ET AL.
ORCID
B. J. Zvara http://orcid.org/0000-0002-6254-7920
M. Burchinal http://orcid.org/0000-0002-3606-7843
References
Alexander, K. L., Entwisle, D. R., & Dauber, S. L. (1993). First-grade classroom behavior: Its short-and long-term conse-
quences for school performance. Child Development, 64(3), 801–814.
Alexander, K. L., Entwisle, D. R., & Olson, L. S. (2001). Schools, achievement, and inequality: A seasonal perspective.
Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 23(2), 171–191.
Banyard, V. L. (1997). The impact of childhood sexual abuse and family functioning on four dimensions of women’s later
parenting. Child Abuse & Neglect, 21(11), 1095–1107.
Baydar, N., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Furstenberg, F. F. (1993). Early warning signs of functional illiteracy: Predictors in childhood
and adolescence. Child Development, 64(3), 815–829.
Birch, S. H., & Ladd, G. W. (1997). The teacher-child relationship and children’s early school adjustment. Journal of School
Psychology, 35(1), 61–79.
Bradley, R. H., & Corwyn, R. F. (2002). Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1),
371–399.
Briere, J. N., & Elliott, D. M. (1994). Immediate and long-term impacts of child sexual abuse. The Future of Children, 4, 54–69.
Briere, J., & Jordan, C. E. (2009). Childhood maltreatment, intervening variables, and adult psychological difficulties in
women: An overview. Trauma, Violence, & Abuse, 10(4), 375–388.
Briere, J., & Runtz, M. (1990). Differential adult symptomatology associated with three types of child abuse histories. Child
Abuse & Neglect, 14(3), 357–364.
Buhs, E. S., & Ladd, G. W. (2001). Peer rejection as antecedent of young children’s school adjustment: An examination of
mediating processes. Developmental Psychology, 37(4), 550–560.
Burchinal, M., Roberts, J. E., Zeisel, S. A., Hennon, E. A., & Hooper, S. (2006). Social risk and protective child, parenting, and
child care factors in early elementary school years. Parenting: Science and Practice, 6(1), 79–113.
Calkins, S. D., Smith, C. L., Gill, K. L., & Johnson, M. C. (1998). Maternal interactive style across contexts: Relations to
emotional, behavioral and physiological regulation during toddlerhood. Social Development, 7(3), 350–369.
Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (2005). Child maltreatment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 409–438.
Collishaw, S., Dunn, J., O’connor, T. G., Golding, J., & Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children Study Team.
(2007). Maternal childhood abuse and offspring adjustment over time. Development and Psychopathology, 19(2),
367-383.
Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. (2002). Using the Fast Track randomized prevention trial to test the early-
starter model of the development of serious conduct problems. Development and Psychopathology, 14(4), 925–943.
Coolahan, K., Fantuzzo, J., Mendez, J., & McDermott, P. (2000). Preschool peer interactions and readiness to learn:
Relationships between classroom peer play and learning behaviors and conduct. Journal of Educational Psychology,
92(3), 458–465.
Cry, M., McDuff, P., & Wright, J. (2006). Prevalence and predictors of dating violence among adolescent female victims of
child sexual abuse. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 21(8), 1000–1017.
DiLillo, D. (2001). Interpersonal functioning among women reporting a history of childhood sexual abuse: Empirical
findings and methodological issues. Clinical Psychology Review, 21, 553–576.
DiLillo, D., Giuffre, D., Tremblay, G. C., & Peterson, L. (2001). A closer look at the nature of intimate partner violence
reported by women with a history of child sexual abuse. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 16(2), 116–132.
Dodge, K. A., & Pettit, G. S. (2003). A biopsychosocial model of the development of chronic conduct problems in adoles-
cence. Developmental Psychology, 39(2), 349–371.
Duncan, G. J., Dowsett, C. J., Claessens, A., Magnuson, K., Huston, A. C., Klebanov, P., … Sexton, H. (2007). School readiness
and later achievement. Developmental Psychology, 43(6), 1428–1446.
Erickson, M. F., & Egeland, B. (2002). Child neglect. The APSAC Handbook on Child Maltreatment, 2, 3–20.
Ethier, L. S., Couture, G., & Lacharité, C. (2004). Risk factors associated with the chronicity of high potential for child abuse
and neglect. Journal of Family Violence, 19(1), 13–24.
Feldman, D. E., & Knudsen, E. I. (1998). Experience-dependent plasticity and the maturation of glutamatergic synapses.
Neuron, 20(6), 1067–1071.
Fox, S. E., Levitt, P., & Nelson III, C. A. (2010). How the timing and quality of early experiences influence the development of
brain architecture. Child Development, 81(1), 28–40.
Goble, P., Hanish, L. D., Martin, C. L., Eggum-Wilkens, N. D., Foster, S. A., & Fabes, R. A. (2016). Preschool contexts and
teacher interactions: Relations with school readiness. Early Education and Development, 27(5), 623–641.
Goldberg, S., Corter, C., Lojkasek, M., & Minde, K. (1990). Prediction of behavior problems in 4-year-olds born prematurely.
Development and Psychopathology, 2(1), 15–30.
EARLY CHILD DEVELOPMENT AND CARE 11
Goodman, R. (2001). Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. Journal of the American
Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(11), 1337–1345.
Goodman, G. S., Ghetti, S., Quas, J. A., Edelstein, R. S., Alexander, K. W., Redlich, A. D., & Jones, D. P. (2003). A prospective
study ofmemory for child sexualabuse: New findings relevant to the repressed-memory controversy. Psychological
Science, 14(2), 113–118.
Graziano, P. A., Reavis, R. D., Keane, S. P., & Calkins, S. D. (2007). The role of emotion regulation in children’s early academic
success. Journal of School Psychology, 45(1), 3–19.
Green, B. L. (1996). Trauma history questionnaire. In B. H. Stamm (Ed.), Measurement of stress trauma and adaptation (pp.
366–369). Lutherville, MD: Sidran.
Gutman, L. M., Sameroff, A. J., & Cole, R. (2003). Academic growth curve trajectories from 1st grade to 12th grade: Effects
of multiple social risk factors and preschool child factors. Developmental Psychology, 39(4), 777–790.
Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, R. C. (2001). Early teacher-child relationships and the trajectory of children’s school outcomes
through eighth grade. Child Development, 72(2), 625–638.
Knudsen, E. I. (2004). Sensitive periods in the development of the brain and behavior. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16
(8), 1412–1425.
Kremer, K. P., Flower, A., Huang, J., & Vaughn, M. G. (2016). Behavior problems and children’s academic achievement: A
test of growth-curve models with gender and racial differences. Children and Youth Services Review, 67, 95–104.
Kupersmidt, J. B., Burchinal, M., & Patterson, C. J. (1995). Developmental patterns of childhood peer relations as predictors
of externalizing behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 825–843.
Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Swank, P. R., Assel, M. A., & Vellet, S. (2001). Does early responsive parenting have a special
importance for children’s development or is consistency across early childhood necessary? Developmental
Psychology, 37(3), 387–403.
Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Swank, P. R., & Miller-Loncar, C. L. (2000). Early maternal and child influences on children’s later
independent cognitive and social functioning. Child Development, 71(2), 358–375.
Long, B. H., Henderson, E. H., & Ziller, R. C. (1967). Developmental changes in the self-concept during middle childhood.
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of Behavior and Development, 13(3), 201–215.
Lonigan, C. J., Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (2007). TOPEL: Test of preschool early literacy. Austin, TX: Pro-
Ed.
Mah, V. K., & Ford-Jones, E. L. (2012). Spotlight on middle childhood: Rejuvenating the ‘forgotten years’. Paediatrics & Child
Health, 17(2), 81–83.
Masten, A. S., Roisman, G. I., Long, J. D., Burt, K. B., Obradović, J., Riley, J. R., … Tellegen, A. (2005). Developmental cascades:
Linking academic achievement and externalizing and internalizing symptoms over 20 years. Developmental
Psychology, 41(5), 733–746.
McClelland, M., Morrison, F., & Holmes, D. (2000). Children at risk for early academic problems: The role of learning-related
social skills. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 15(3), 307–329.
McLoyd, V. C. (1998). Socioeconomic disadvantage and child development. American Psychologist, 53(2), 185–204.
Mendez, J. L., Fantuzzo, J., & Cicchetti, D. (2002). Profiles of social competence among low–income African American pre-
school children. Child Development, 73(4), 1085–1100.
Morgan, P. L., Farkas, G., Tufis, P. A., & Sperling, R. A. (2008). Are reading and behavior problems risk factors for each other?
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41(5), 417–436.
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (1999). Chronicity of maternal depressive symptoms, maternal sensitivity, and
child functioning at 36 months. Developmental Psychology, 35(5), 1297–1310.
Noll, J. G. (2008). Sexual abuse of children: Unique in its effects on development? Child Abuse & Neglect, 32(6), 603–605.
Pentimonti, J. M., Murphy, K. A., Justice, L. M., Logan, J. A., & Kaderavek, J. N. (2016). School readiness of children with
language impairment: predicting literacy skills from pre-literacy and social–behavioural dimensions. International
Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 51(2), 148–161.
Pereda, N., Guilera, G., Forns, M., & Gómez-Benito, J. (2009). The prevalence of child sexual abuse in community and
student samples: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 29(4), 328–338.
Pianta, R. C., Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., & Cox, M. J. (1999). Introduction: An ecological approach to kindergarten transition. In
R. C. Pianta & M. J. Cox (Eds.), The transition to kindergarten (pp. 3–12). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing
Company.
Putnam, F. W. (2003). Ten-year research update review: Child sexual abuse. Journal of the American Academy of Child &
Adolescent Psychiatry, 42(3), 269–278.
Roberts, R., O’Connor, T, Dunn, J., Golding, J., & The ALSPAC Study Team. (2004). The effects of child sexual abuse in later
family life: Mental health, parenting and adjustment of offspring. Child Abuse & Neglect, 28, 525-545.
Rose-Krasnor, L. (1997). The nature of social competence: A theoretical review. Social Development, 6(1), 111–135.
Rosenbaum, P. R., & Rubin, D. B. (1983). The central role of the propensity score in observational studies for causal effects.
Biometrika, 70(1), 41–55.
Sabol, T. J., & Pianta, R. C. (2012). Patterns of school readiness forecast achievement and socioemotional development at
the end of elementary school. Child Development, 83(1), 282–299.
12 B. J. ZVARA ET AL.
Trickett, P. K., Noll, J. G., & Putnam, F. W. (2011). The impact of sexual abuse on female development: Lessons from amul-
tigenerational, longitudinal research study. Development and Psychopathology, 23(2), 453–476.
Webster-Stratton, C., Reid, M. J., & Hammond, M. (2004). Treating children with early-onset conduct problems:
Intervention outcomes for parent, child, and teacher training. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33
(1), 105–124.
Weisner, T. S. (1984). Eco cultural Niches of Middle Childhood: A Cross Cultural Perspective. In W. A. Collins (Ed.),
Development during middle childhood: The years from six to twelve (pp. 335–269). Washington, DC: National
Academy Science Press.
Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., & Mather, N. (2001). Woodcock-Johnson III tests of achievement. Itasca, IL: Riverside
Publishing.
Zuravin, S. J., & Fontanella, C. (1999). Parenting behaviors and perceived parenting competence of child sexual abuse
survivors. Child Abuse & Neglect, 23(7), 623–632.
Zvara, B. J., Mills-Koonce, W. R., Carmody, K. A., Cox, M., & Family Life Project Key Investigators. (2015). Childhood sexual
trauma and subsequent parenting beliefs and behaviors. Child Abuse & Neglect, 44, 87–97.
Zvara, B. J., Mills-Koonce, W. R., Carmody, K. A., Cox, M., & Family Life Project Key Investigators. (2017). Maternal childhood
sexual trauma and child conduct problems. Journal of Family Violence, 32(2), 231–242.