Fox 2017
Fox 2017
Fox 2017
To cite this article: Jesse Fox & Bree McEwan (2017): Distinguishing technologies for social
interaction: The perceived social affordances of communication channels scale, Communication
Monographs, DOI: 10.1080/03637751.2017.1332418
Article views: 85
Download by: [University of New Mexico] Date: 02 July 2017, At: 08:02
COMMUNICATION MONOGRAPHS, 2017
https://doi.org/10.1080/03637751.2017.1332418
back against claims and theorizing based on the channel itself (e.g., Walther et al., 2005),
instead advocating for approaches that account for distinct features (e.g., a variable-cen-
tered approach; Nass & Mason, 1990). Another issue is that assumptions in channel the-
ories are often based on the capabilities of channels at that point in time; both channels
and users’ perceptions can evolve (Ellison & Vitak, 2015). For example, the social identity
model of deindividuation effects (SIDE) suggests that disinhibited communication is more
likely in computer-mediated environments than face-to-face because in CMC channels
available at the time, fewer cues were able to be transmitted and participants were often
anonymous (Lea & Spears, 1991). Current CMC channels, such as social networking
sites and videoconferencing, challenge these original conceptualizations. Further, users
may be able to customize features within these channels, such as changing the privacy set-
tings on one’s social media profile. Thus, considering affordances instead of channels
should offer more nuanced – and durable – theorizing that is more flexible across contexts
(Ellison & Vitak, 2015). Whether or not a channel like Snapchat diminishes in popularity
like MySpace or the WELL, or fades to obsolescence like VideoTex, is less relevant than
what we can learn from affordances such as persistence, personalization, and privacy.
Of course, channel features have not been ignored entirely. Affordances have been inte-
grated in channel-focused theories for decades (e.g., social presence theory, Short, Wil-
liams, & Christie, 1976; media richness theory, Daft, Lengel, & Trevino, 1987) and a
considerable number of studies have identified relationships between channel affordances
and outcomes (e.g., Bazarova, 2012; Lee & Oh, 2012; Tanis & Postmes, 2003; Walther,
1994). Unfortunately, many approaches merely assume affordances differ meaningfully
between channels and they are not suitably operationalized in study design.
Another concern is that when affordances are included, they are treated as objective
properties of the channel and overlook the human experience. Numerous studies have
shown that people may have widely varying perceptions of channel affordances, particu-
larly in emerging media environments (e.g., Carlson & Zmud, 1999; DeAndrea, Van Der
Heide, Vendemia, & Vang, in press; Feaster, 2010; Schouten, Valkenburg, & Peter, 2007;
Tao & Bucy, 2007). For example, James may think choosing an avatar and handle not tied
to his real name on Twitter makes him anonymous, whereas Ashley may find this insuffi-
cient given that geolocated posts, connections within the network, and the content of posts
reveal sufficient clues about one’s identity. Indeed, previous research has found that objec-
tive manipulations of anonymity do not necessarily explain outcomes, whereas percep-
tions of anonymity can (Rains, 2007). Put simply, perceived affordances matter
regarding the use and effects of communication channels.
These issues could be addressed if researchers consistently assessed users’ perceptions
of affordances. The field has lacked validated ways to systematically assess perceptions of
affordances, however, limiting scholars’ ability to make comparisons across channels,
across studies, or between users. Thus, the goal of this project was to develop a measure
to assess a broad range of perceived social affordances across various communication
channels.
affordances are action possibilities that require both entities, but exist independent of the
actor’s perceptions. This conceptualization is similar to how media scholars have
described “features” or “attributes” of communication channels (e.g., Daft et al., 1987;
Eveland, 2003; Tao & Bucy, 2007).
In contrast to Gibson (1979), Norman (1990) argued for the importance of assessing
perceived affordances based on the user’s experience rather than the inherent properties
of an object. Users imbue objects with their own interpretations that may not correspond
with the intention of an object’s design. For example, although a gleaming silver tureen
may be designed as an aesthetic award, the award recipient may find it a suitable dog dish.
As noted, affordances are fundamental to many mediated communication theories,
although they are not always conceptualized as such. Medium theory, for example,
“focuses on such characteristics of each medium and on how each medium (or each
type of media) is physically, socially, and psychologically distinct from other media”
(Meyrowitz, 2009, p. 518). Similarly, early theorizing about CMC sought to clarify the
meaningful differences and similarities between CMC and face-to-face communication
(Walther & Parks, 2002). Given CMC at the time was generally text based, much of the
focus was on the lack of affordances to convey certain cues and communicate affective
states (e.g., Daft et al., 1987; Short et al., 1976).
As communication technologies become more widely adopted, researchers have delved
more deeply into variation among channels. Technological affordances identified as
having meaningful implications for human interaction have been labeled social affor-
dances (e.g., Bradner, Kellogg, & Erickson, 1999; Wellman et al., 2003), sociotechnical
affordances (e.g., Bazarova, 2012), or communicative affordances (Hutchby & Barnett,
2005; Parks, 2008). Hogan (2009) challenged previous applications of this term by
Wellman et al. and others, noting it typically overlooked individual perceptions. Hogan
redefined social affordances as “the perceptual cues that connote aspects of social structure
to individuals thereby creating a functional difference for the individual” (p. 27). Although
some scholars have argued for a more Gibsonian approach to affordances (e.g., Sundar,
2008), several scholars have argued that it is inadequate for communication research.
Schmitz and Fulk (1991) noted that treating a characteristic-like media richness as an
objective feature of the medium rather than considering users’ perceptions is a “specifica-
tion error” that “reduces our ability to accurately predict communication behavior in
organizations” (p. 491). Similarly, Feaster (2010) argued this approach “can in effect
exclude the communicator from the communication situation” (p. 119). Given our scale
is intended to be used in communicative contexts, we adhere to Hogan’s (2009) conceptual
definition and focus on perceptions.
sound design. From the perspective of communication research, this approach is also
problematic.
Given the persistence of digital divides and differences in media use (Pearce & Rice,
2013; Pew Research Center, 2015), it is presumptive to believe users’ perceptions are
aligned with the researcher’s. Individual differences such as cognitive capacity, media lit-
eracy, or physical limitations may influence a user’s ability to evaluate affordances of a
channel the way the researcher expects. People can perceive affordances that are not
inherent to an object or fail to perceive affordances that exist. For example, online
forum users may feel their posts are anonymous, but many sites record IP addresses,
which makes users identifiable. The mismatch between user perception and the object’s
inherent properties yields false affordances (Gaver, 1991). Another concern is hidden affor-
dances, in which users do not perceive an inherent affordance (Gaver, 1991). Someone
might resist using Twitter because of its public nature, not realizing privacy settings are
available. In both of these cases, it is likely users’ behaviors are better predicted by their
perceptions of affordances rather than these sites’ actual affordances.
Collectively, these arguments suggest that conceptualizing affordances as objectively
present or not, high or low, may overlook some crucial and theoretically important vari-
ation in the ways people experience communication channels. Such an approach may lead
to attributions toward a channel rather than clarifying the mechanisms driving effects.
Following many communication technology scholars (e.g., Feaster, 2010; Fulk et al.,
1990; Schmitz & Fulk, 1991; Schouten et al., 2007; Steinfield, 1986; Tao & Bucy, 2007),
we agree that assessing individuals’ perceptions of these affordances will provide greater
insight into social interaction.
and in terms of theoretical validity (Eveland, 2003; Walther, 2009). If researchers patch
together various existing measures, it is likely they may not represent clean factors
given that many affordances are closely related.
A final issue is that many “measures” of affordances are not derived using appropriate
scale development techniques. Although items and scale reliability are typically reported,
these are not sufficient indicators of measurement validity. Rarely do these measures
incorporate procedures such as developing item pools; identifying and refining factor
structure; establishing multiple aspects of validity; or testing items across multiple
samples (DeVellis, 2017). The assessment of perceived affordances requires a unified, psy-
chometrically sound measure.
Study 1
Sample and procedure
After IRB approval was obtained, Mechanical Turk workers (N = 875) who identified as
English speaking U.S. residents were recruited to participate in exchange for $2.
8 J. FOX AND B. MCEWAN
(Continued )
COMMUNICATION MONOGRAPHS 9
Table 1. Continued.
β Β
You can’t necessarily tell who is communicating through this channel. .78 .84
This channel makes it difficult to conceal one’s identity when communicating.a,b .70
Information Control (α = .61) CFA: χ 2/df = .75, CFI = 1.00, SRMR = .02, RMSEA = .00 (.00, .14)
This channel allows me to control how much information I am disclosing both verbally and nonverbally.
This channel allows me to control how much information the other person receives.
This channel allows me to avoid topics that I don’t want to discuss.
This channel allows me to control how much information I receive from others.
This channel allows me to filter out information if I want to.
Synchronicity (α = .85) CFA: χ 2/df = 1.67, CFI = .95, SRMR = .05, RMSEA = .06 (.03, .09)
The messages I send can be observed in real time – that is, the receiver can observe me while I am communicating.
There is a lapse of time in between the messages I send and the other person receiving the message.
This channel allows for instant communication.
It can be a long time before you get a response using this channel.a
This channel allows communicators to quickly send messages back and forth.
I should not expect to get my message right away when using this channel.a
When using this channel, I expect the other person will respond quickly.
This channel allows me to give and receive timely feedback.
This channel allows for back and forth interaction. a
Scale items: a indicates reverse scoring. Items in italics are not included in the final scale, including the entire information
control and synchronicity factors. b indicates items retained in Study 1 but removed from the final scale in Study 2.
Alphas: The reported Study 1 alpha includes all Study 1 items. Study 2 alphas are in boldface. CFAs: Reported CFAs are the
unidimensional CFA.
the SRMR was below .08 (Hu & Bentler, 1999), and the RMSEA was below .08 (taking into
account the associated confidence interval, Browne & Cudeck, 1993; Kline, 2016). Items
were examined if they did not fit the unidimensional model. Upon further consideration,
some of these items were determined to be vague or worded in a way that was awkward for
a particular channel and were removed. Initial models for synchronicity and anonymity
were acceptable. Three items each were trimmed from accessibility and social presence
and two from bandwidth and conversational control. One item each was trimmed from
privacy, network association, personalization, editability, and information control. The
complete list of items, unidimensional factors after item trimming, and final model fit stat-
istics can be found in Table 1.
Validation
We selected one existing measure for each our factors to examine convergent validity with
variables similar to each factor. We validated accessibility with the ease of use subscale
of the MOCA, given both scales concern the availability and convenience of channels
COMMUNICATION MONOGRAPHS 11
(α = .69, M = 5.24, SD = 1.24; Ledbetter, 2009); bandwidth with the clarity factor of Spitz-
berg’s (2006) CMC competence scale (α = .78, M = 5.11, SD = 1.04), given that the range of
available cues is associated with message ambiguity (e.g., SIP, Walther, 1996); privacy with
Buchanan, Paine, Joinson, and Reips’s (2007) measure of online privacy concern (α = .93,
M = 3.18, SD = 1.53); network association with Boster, Kotowski, Andrews, and Serota’s
(2011) connectivity scale (α = .85, M = 4.03, SD = 1.45); persistence with Suh and
Wagner’s (2013) deep-profiling items (α = .64, M = 3.75, SD = 1.52); editability with the
increased preparation and control subscale of Kelly and Keaten’s (2007) ACCS (α = .93,
M = 4.94, SD = 1.27); conversation control with Feaster’s (2010) information control
12 J. FOX AND B. MCEWAN
scale (α = .76, M = 4.94, SD = 1.43); personalization with the attentiveness factor of Spitz-
berg’s (2006) CMC competence scale (α = .73, M = 5.79, SD = .86); social presence with the
social richness subscale of Lombard, Ditton, and Weinstein’s (2009) Temple Presence
Inventory (α = .91, M = 5.38, SD = 1.14); and anonymity with Ritter’s (2014) perception
of online anonymity subscale (α = .59, M = 3.30, SD = .94), taken from her measure of
cybersexual harassment.
As seen in Table 2, the affordance factors were strongly and positively correlated with
their validation measures with two exceptions. The affordance of privacy was negatively
correlated with the privacy concern validation measure, but this was anticipated as the
more private a user perceives the channel, the less concerned they should be about
privacy issues while using that channel. The second exception was a relatively low cor-
relation between the anonymity affordance and anonymity validation measure, r(804)
= .34, p < .001. The online anonymity validation measure had low reliability, however;
the instability of the validation measure may have reduced the strength of the
correlation.
Comparing our measures across channels, the means generally reflect differences that
have been observed or theorized in previous scholarship. ANOVAs revealed that face-to-
face had significantly greater bandwidth and social presence, and significantly lower anon-
ymity, than all other channels (all ps < .01). The phone had the second-highest bandwidth
and social presence, and was significantly higher (p < .01) than all of the computer-
mediated channels (i.e., texting, email, and Facebook posts; all ps < .01). Facebook timeline
posts, a masspersonal channel, were significantly lower than all other channels on privacy
and personalization but also significantly higher on network association (all ps < .01).
These results from Study 1 can be viewed in Supplement Table 5 (http://www.
breemcewan.com/psaccs.html).
Study 2
After the factor structure was established and tested across channels in Study 1, we col-
lected a second sample to confirm the factor structure across media that existing research
has attributed with different affordances. We included the 10 affordances that emerged in
Study 1 (accessibility, bandwidth, social presence, privacy, network association, persona-
lization, persistence, editability, conversation control, and anonymity).
Additionally, we wanted to probe some affordances that appeared closely related in
Study 1 to ensure that they are indeed perceived as conceptually distinct by users. For
example, editability and persistence were rather highly correlated for our selected chan-
nels; both were low for face-to-face and phone, and higher for the CMC channels of
texting, Facebook posts, and email. Thus, we decided to test the measure with Snapchat,
which has similar functionality to texting, but is noted for deleting messages after they
have been received. Moreover, given that face-to-face is often heralded as an unparalleled
channel for many affordances such as bandwidth and social presence, we wanted to
examine participants’ perceptions of the closest approximation currently available: video-
conferencing. In addition to Snapchat and video conferencing, we examined instant mes-
saging given its inclusion in previous research (e.g., Ledbetter, 2009; Ramirez et al., 2008)
and also retained face-to-face communication.
COMMUNICATION MONOGRAPHS 13
Discriminant validity
To assess discriminant validity, we conducted channel comparisons using the final version
of the scale. As seen in Table 4, ANOVAs compared each affordance across all eight chan-
nels using both Study 1 and Study 2 data. Supplement Table 6 also provides comparisons
using solely Study 2 data (http://www.breemcewan.com/psaccs.html). Each ANOVA was
significant, and post hoc Scheffe tests indicated significant differences between at least
two channels on each affordance suggesting that the PSACCS was able to discriminate
between different media. Moreover, the new channels performed in largely predictable
ways. Snapchat was seen as significantly less persistent than other forms of CMC such as
texting, email, IM, and Facebook. Videoconferencing was perceived as providing higher
bandwidth and social presence than all other CMC channels, but not as high as face-to-
face communication. Instant messaging was considered the most anonymous channel, fol-
lowed by email. Given that these are predominantly text-based forms of interaction, users
are not physically visible, and typically users may choose any handle or avatar they wish,
these channels are perceived as offering fewer identifying cues than other channels.
14 J. FOX AND B. MCEWAN
Discussion
For decades, communication researchers have used affordances and related concepts to
predict aspects of channel use, yet there is no standard, validated measure for assessing
perceived affordances. The PSACCS comprises 41 items assessing perceptions of 10 dis-
tinct social affordances. The PSACCS can be implemented to assess perceived social
Table 4. ANOVA comparisons of perceived social affordances across channels.
Affordance (F ) F-t-F M (SD) Texting M (SD) Email M (SD) Facebook M (SD) Phone M (SD) Video conferencing M (SD) Snapchat M (SD) IM M (SD)
a e d,e a,b,c a,b,c a,b b,c,d
Accessibility (19.26) 5.56 (1.12) 6.39 (0.81) 6.31 (0.86) 5.72 (1.01) 5.89 (0.87) 5.57 (1.06) 5.92 (0.95) 6.07 (0.91)c,d,e
Bandwidth (175.70) 6.37 (0.71)a 4.10 (1.35)b 4.10 (1.23)b 4.41 (1.22)b,e 5.92 (0.82)c 5.96 (0.80)c 5.58 (1.07)d 4.52 (1.31)e
Privacy (120.24) 5.01 (1.37)a 5.04 (1.35)a 5.29 (1.21)a,c 2.78 (1.51)b 5.65 (1.00)c 5.34 (1.07)a,c 5.03 (1.37)a 4.99 (1.30)a
Net. Assoc. (208.40) 4.47 (1.50)a 2.87 (1.37)b 2.65 (1.19)b 5.51 (0.95)c 2.35 (1.07)b 4.61 (1.14)a 4.17 (1.44)a 4.23 (1.14)a
Persistence (322.65) 2.65 (1.49)a 5.98 (0.93)d,e 6.40 (0.69)e 5.94 (0.83)d,e 2.24 (1.18)a 4.15 (1.38)c 3.33 (1.65)b 5.45 (1.18)d
Editability (238.54) 3.00 (1.59)a 5.73 (0.95)e,f 6.01 (0.85)f 5.48 (0.97)d,e 2.33 (1.28)b 4.25 (1.56)c 5.17 (1.07)d 5.46 (0.86)d,e
Conv. Control (12.76) 5.34 (1.04)a,b 5.74 (0.84)c 5.64 (0.81)b,c 5.04 (1.13)a 5.81 (0.96)c 5.66 (0.90)b,c 5.45 (0.98)b,c 5.73 (0.97)c
Personaliz. (30.17) 6.02 (0.86)a,d 6.01 (0.81)a,d 6.25 (0.75)d 4.83 (1.22)c 6.08 (0.79)a,d 5.62 (0.85)b 5.76 (0.88)a,b 5.77 (0.80)a,b
Soc. Presence (220.62) 6.37 (0.72)a 3.75 (1.41)b 3.08 (1.22)f 3.71 (1.37)b 5.35 (1.05)d,e 5.65 (0.89)e 5.07 (1.26)d 4.94 (1.58)c
Anonymity (149.46) 1.98 (1.30)a 3.55 (1.52)c,d 4.20 (1.46)e,f 3.05 (1.32)b,c 2.87 (1.38)b 3.38 (1.44)b,c,d 3.76 (1.52)d,e 4.59 (1.63)f
COMMUNICATION MONOGRAPHS
Notes: Means and standard deviations were calculated using the final version of the PSACCS. All F-tests (7, 1476) are significant, p < .001. Significantly different means are indicated across rows by
different superscripts, p < .01.
15
16 J. FOX AND B. MCEWAN
affordances cited in the literature as crucial components of channel use, user experiences,
and communicative outcomes (e.g., boyd, 2011; Fulk et al., 1990; Treem & Leonardi,
2013). Further, the measure can be used to test or challenge fundamental assumptions
and predictions made by several channel theories and models.
Beyond the provision of the scale, our findings have some interesting implications. For
example, our results suggest that certain affordances outlined by previous literature may
be not be perceived distinctly by users. For example, the perceived affordance of synchro-
nicity is so tightly connected to perceptions of editability and social presence that synchro-
nicity failed to emerge as a distinct perceived affordance. Several models and theories
make attributions specific to asynchronicity, but many of these also relate its significance
to other affordances (e.g., the hyperpersonal model (Walther, 1996) suggests that asyn-
chronicity facilitates editability). Future research should explore whether asynchronicity
is distinctly meaningful, as the development of this scale is not sufficient to answer this
question completely. For scholars interested in pursuing work related to synchronicity,
our original items can be viewed in Table 1, although we must caution their use in con-
junction with other affordance measures.
Direct comparisons of channels on perceived social affordances also offered important
insights. Our findings indicate face-to-face communication has several perceived draw-
backs compared to other channels: it is less accessible, indicating communication is
more difficult to initiate; people feel less conversational control, so they may have difficulty
avoiding unpleasant interactions; and it is not persistent, so it may be hard to review what
was communicated in an important exchange. Early CMC researchers often privileged
face-to-face and considered it the preferred or desirable choice (Culnan & Markus,
1987; Daft et al., 1987). Although many CMC scholars have moved away from this
trend, face-to-face communication is often still heralded as superior by both scholars
and popular media (e.g., Turkle, 2015). Taking an affordance-based approach rather
than a channel-based approach may identify contexts in which face-to-face communi-
cation is not optimal and perhaps undesirable. It may also help overcome long-held
biases against certain channels, such as assuming that “lean” media like texting are inher-
ently low in bandwidth and thus insufficient for conveying emotion.
Measuring perceived social affordances could help identify explanatory mechanisms in
existing channel theories and findings. Perceived affordances could explain why or when
people switch channels in interpersonal relationships (modality switching; Ramirez &
Zhang, 2007) or why perceptions or effects of a message vary when delivered via different
channels (disclosure personalism; Bazarova, 2012). Measuring perceived affordances may
elucidate observed patterns, shifts, or outcomes of varied channel use in contexts such as
social support (High & Solomon, 2014) or everyday social interaction (Ruppel & Burke,
2015). Assessing perceived affordances could also help clarify boundary conditions for
theories that were developed before the emergence of certain technologies (Walther,
2009). For example, SIP suggests a slower process of information exchange during
relationship development via CMC. Is this pacing similar when people are interacting
through a social networking site like Facebook, where copious personal disclosures are
visible, previous interactions with others persist, and information can be obtained
through associated nodes?
In practice, when experimenters attempt to manipulate certain affordances, subscales of
this measure could also be employed as manipulation checks. They may also provide
COMMUNICATION MONOGRAPHS 17
greater explanatory power and account for some observed variance in outcomes. Tao and
Bucy (2007) recommend treating the media attribute as the independent variable and its
associated psychological state as a mediator to illuminate our understanding of media
effects; our perceived affordances measure provides a consistent way to assess such
psychological states.
Because this measure allows researchers to assess perceptions and account for individ-
ual variability, another interesting direction for future research could be identifying factors
that may predict the perception of affordances. Previous experience or training may influ-
ence perceptions; for example, levels of technological education have been associated with
different expectations about online privacy (Buchanan et al., 2007). Other traits and indi-
vidual differences may also play a role. For example, attachment anxiety may predict dif-
fering perceptions of social presence, self-monitors may perceive editability differently, or
communication competence may predict how individuals perceive bandwidth. Situational
factors related to the context, content of communication, or relationship to the commu-
nicator may also influence perceptions. For example, a user may see texting as typically
private, but not if the recipient is a friend who gossips and is likely to show a text to others.
In this study, we assessed commonly cited affordances associated with human com-
munication across mediated and non-mediated channels. No single measure could poss-
ibly encapsulate every perceived affordance, however, so inevitably some are not included
here. Further, because this measure was intended to test existing theoretical claims and be
suitable for making direct comparisons to face-to-face communication (i.e., address
potential type I technology errors; Walther, 2013), all items needed to make sense in
the face-to-face context. Some affordances identified in other work that have potential
for communication (e.g., locatability; Schrock, 2015) are only sensibly applied to certain
channels. Further, to attain parsimony and cohesiveness, we did not assess a number of
affordances related more directly to human–computer interaction, such as the navigability
of an interface (Sundar, 2008).
Our study selected channels reflecting previous channel-based research (e.g., Kelly &
Keaten, 2007; Ledbetter, 2014; Ruppel & Burke, 2015), but was limited to face-to-face,
texting, phone, email, Facebook posts, instant messaging, video conferencing, and Snap-
chat. Future research could incorporate previously tested channels such as postal mail
or expand to newer channels such as multiplayer video games or collaborative virtual
environments.
In conclusion, this measure represents an attempt to advance the scientific understand-
ing of individuals’ experiences with communication channels. Given that affordances are
embedded in several communication theories and models, a systematic way to examine
perceived social affordances is long overdue. Focusing on perceived affordances will
help communication researchers continue to progress our understanding of social inter-
action that is viably situated in human experience rather than the ever-moving target of
technology.
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank participants of The Ohio State School of Communication’s Communication
Technology Symposium for their feedback during early conceptualizations of this project. We would
also like to thank the Editor and three anonymous reviewers for their insights and helpful comments.
18 J. FOX AND B. MCEWAN
Disclosure statement
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.
Funding
This research was funded in part by The Ohio State University School of Communication Mattox
Award granted to the first author.
References
Bazarova, N. N. (2012). Public intimacy: Disclosure interpretation and social judgments on
Facebook. Journal of Communication, 62, 815–832. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2012.01664.x
Boster, F. J., Kotowski, M. R., Andrews, K. R., & Serota, K. (2011). Identifying influence:
Development and validation of the connectivity, persuasiveness, and maven scales. Journal of
Communication, 61, 178–196. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2010.01531.x
boyd, d. (2011). Social network sites as networked publics: Affordances, dynamics, and impli-
cations. In Z. Papacharissi (Ed.), A networked self: Identity, community, and culture on social
network sites (pp. 39–58). New York, NY: Routledge.
boyd, d. m., & Ellison, N. B. (2007). Social network sites: Definition, history, and scholarship.
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 13, 210–230. doi:10.1111/j/1083-6101.2007.
00393.x
Bradner, E., Kellogg, W. A., & Erickson, T. (1999). The adoption and use of ‘Babble’: A field study of
chat in the workplace. In Ecscw’99 (pp. 139–158). Copenhagen: Springer.
Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A. Bollen & J. S.
Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136–162). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Buchanan, T., Paine, C., Joinson, A. N., & Reips, U. D. (2007). Development of measures of online
privacy concern and protection for use on the Internet. Journal of the American Society for
Information Science and Technology, 58, 157–165. doi:10.1002/asi.20459
Carlson, J. R., & Zmud, R. W. (1999). Channel expansion theory and the experiential nature of
media richness perceptions. Academy of Management Journal, 42(2), 153–170. doi:10.2307/
257090
Clark, H. H., & Brennan, S. E. (1991). Grounding in communication. In L. B. Resnick, J. M. Levine,
& S. D. Teasley (Eds.), Perspectives on socially shared cognition (pp. 127–149). Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Culnan, M. J., & Markus, M. L. (1987). Information technologies. In F. M. Jablin, L. L. Putnam, K.
H. Roberts, & L. W. Porter (Eds.), Handbook of organizational communication (pp. 420–443).
Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Daft, R. L., Lengel, R. H., & Trevino, L. K. (1987). Message equivocality, media selection, and
manager performance: Implications for information systems. MIS Quarterly, 11, 355–366.
doi:10.2307/248682
DeAndrea, D. C., Van Der Heide, B., Vendemia, M. A., & Vang, M. H. (in press). How people evalu-
ate online reviews. Communication Research. doi:10.1177/0093650215573862
DeVellis, R. F. (2017). Scale development: Theory and applications. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Ellison, N. B., & Vitak, J. (2015). Social network site affordances and their relationship to social
capital processes. In S. S. Sundar (Ed.), The handbook of the psychology of communication tech-
nology (pp. 205–227). New York, NY: Wiley & Sons.
Eveland, W. P. (2003). A “mix of attributes” approach to the study of media effects and new com-
munication technologies. Journal of Communication, 53, 395–410. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.
2003.tb02598.x
Feaster, J. C. (2010). Expanding the impression management model of communication channels:
An information control scale. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 16, 115–138.
doi:10.1111/j.1083-6101.2010.01535.x
COMMUNICATION MONOGRAPHS 19
Fox, J., & Moreland, J. J. (2015). The dark side of social networking sites: An exploration of the rela-
tional and psychological stressors associated with Facebook use and affordances. Computers in
Human Behavior, 45, 168–176. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2014.11.083
Fulk, J., Schmitz, J., & Steinfield, C. W. (1990). A social influence model of technology use. In J. Fulk
& C. W. Steinfield (Eds.), Organizations and communication technology (pp. 117–140). Newbury
Park, CA: Sage.
Gaver, W. W. (1991). Technology affordances. Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human
Factors, 79–54. doi:10.1145/108844.108856
Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston, MA: Houghlin Mifflin.
Haythornthwaite, C. (2002). Strong, weak, and latent ties and the impact of new media. The
Information Society, 18, 385–401. doi:10.1080/01972240290108195
High, A. C., & Solomon, D. H. (2014). Communication channel, sex, and the immediate and longi-
tudinal outcomes of verbal person-centered support. Communication Monographs, 81, 439–468.
doi:10.1080/03637751.2014.933245
Hogan, B. J. (2009). Networking in everyday life (Doctoral dissertation). University of Toronto.
Retrieved from http://individual.utoronto.ca/berniehogan/Hogan_NIEL_10-29-2008_FINAL.
pdf
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis:
Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A
Multidisciplinary Journal, 6, 1–55. doi:10.1080/10705519909540118
Hutchby, I., & Barnett, S. (2005). Aspects of the sequential organization of mobile phone conversa-
tion. Discourse Studies, 7, 147–171. doi:10.1177/1461445605050364
Kelly, L., & Keaten, J. A. (2007). Development of the affect for communication channels scale.
Journal of Communication, 57, 349–365. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2007.00346.x
Kline, R. B. (2016). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (4th ed.). New York, NY:
Guilford.
Lea, M., & Spears, R. (1991). Computer-mediated communication, deindividuation, and group
decision-making. International Journal of Man-Machine Studies, 34, 283–301. doi:10.1016/
0020-7373(91)90045-9
Ledbetter, A. M. (2009). Measuring online communication attitude: Instrument development and
validation. Communication Monographs, 76, 463–486. doi:10.1080/03637750903300262
Ledbetter, A. M. (2014). Online communication attitude similarity in romantic dyads: Predicting
couples’ frequency of e-mail, instant messaging, and social networking site communication.
Communication Quarterly, 62, 233–252. doi:10.1080/01463373.2014.890120
Lee, E. J., & Oh, S. Y. (2012). To personalize or depersonalize? When and how politicians’ person-
alized tweets affect the public’s reactions. Journal of Communication, 62, 932–949. doi:10.1111/j.
1460-2466.2012.01681.x
Lombard, M., Ditton, T. B., & Weinstein, L. (2009, November). Measuring (tele)presence: The
Temple Presence Inventory. Presented at the Twelfth International Workshop on Presence, Los
Angeles, CA.
Marsh, H. W., Hau, K-T., & Wen, Z. (2004). In search of golden rules: Comment on hypothesis-
testing approaches to setting cutoff values for fit indexes and dangers in overgeneralizing Hu
and Bentler’s (1999) findings. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 11,
320–341. doi:10.1207/s15328007sem1103_2
McOmber, J. B. (1999). Technological autonomy and three definitions of technology. Journal of
Communication, 49, 137–153. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.1999.tb02809.x
Meyrowitz, J. (2009). Medium theory: An alternative to the dominant paradigm of media effects. In
R. L. Nabi & M. B. Oliver (Eds.), The Sage handbook of media processes and effects (pp. 517–530).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Nass, C., & Mason, L. (1990). On the study of technology and task: A variable-based approach. In J.
Fulk & C. Steinfield (Eds.), Organizations and communication technology (pp. 46–67). Newbury
Park, CA: Sage.
Norman, D. A. (1990). The design of everyday things. New York, NY: Doubleday.
20 J. FOX AND B. MCEWAN
O’Sullivan, P. B. (2000). What you don’t know won’t hurt me: Impression management functions of
communication channels in relationships. Human Communication Research, 26, 403–431.
doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2000.tb00763.x
Parks, M. R. (2008). Characterizing the communicative affordances of MySpace: A place for friends
or a friendless place? Paper presented at the Annual Conference of the International
Communication Association, Montreal.
Pearce, K. E., & Rice, R. E. (2013). Digital divides from access to activities: Comparing mobile and
personal computer Internet users. Journal of Communication, 63, 721–744. doi:10.1111/jcom.
12045
Pew Research Center. (2015). Technology device ownership 2015. Washington, DC: Author.
Rains, S. A. (2007). The impact of anonymity on perceptions of source credibility and influence in
computer-mediated group communication: A test of two competing hypotheses.
Communication Research, 34, 100–125. doi:10.1177/0093650206296084
Ramirez Jr., A., Dimmick, J., Feaster, J., & Lin, S. F. (2008). Revisiting interpersonal media compe-
tition: The gratification niches of instant messaging, e-mail, and the telephone. Communication
Research, 35, 529–547. doi:10.1177/0093650208315979
Ramirez Jr., A., & Zhang, S. (2007). When online meets offline: The effect of modality switching on
relational communication. Communication Monographs, 74, 287–310. doi:10.1080/
03637750701543493
Reid, F. J. M., & Reid, D. J. (2010). The expressive and conversational affordances of mobile messa-
ging. Behaviour & Information Technology, 29, 3–22. doi:10.1080/014492907014970
Resnick, P. (2001). Beyond bowling together: Sociotechnical capital. In J. Carroll (Ed.), HCI in the
new millennium (pp. 247–272). New York, NY: Addison-Wesley.
Rice, R. E. (1987). Computer-mediated communication and organizational innovation. Journal of
Communication, 37(4), 65–94. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.1987.tb01009.x
Rice, R. E., & Steinfield, C. (1994). Experiences with new forms of organizational communication
via electronic mail and voice messaging. In J. H. E. Andriesson & R. A. Roe (Eds.), Telematics and
work (pp. 109–132). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Ritter, B. A. (2014). Deviant behavior in computer-mediated communication: Development and
validation of a measure of cybersexual harassment. Journal of Computer-Mediated
Communication, 19, 197–214. doi:10.1111/jcc4.12039
Rogers, E. M. (1962). Diffusion of innovations. New York, NY: Free Press.
Ruppel, E. K., & Burke, T. J. (2015). Complementary channel use and the role of social competence.
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 20, 37–51. doi:10.1111/jcc4.12091
Schmitz, J., & Fulk, J. (1991). Organizational colleagues, media richness, and electronic mail: A test
of the social influence model of technology use. Communication Research, 18, 487–523. doi:10.
1177/009365091018004003
Schouten, A., Valkenburg, P. M., & Peter, J. (2007). Precursors and underlying processes of adoles-
cents’ online self-disclosure: Developing and testing an “Internet-attribute-perception” model.
Media Psychology, 10, 292–315. doi:10.1080/15213260701375686
Schrock, A. R. (2015). Communicative affordances of mobile media: Portability, availability, locat-
ability, and multimediality. International Journal of Communication, 9, 1229–1246. Retrieved
from http://ijoc.org/index.php/ijoc/article/view/3288
Short, J., Williams, E., & Christie, B. (1976). The social psychology of telecommunications. New York,
NY: Wiley.
Spitzberg, B. H. (2006). Preliminary development of a model and measure of computer-mediated
communication (CMC) competence. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 11, 629–
666. doi:10.1111/j.1083-6101.2006.00030.x
Steinfield, C. W. (1986). Computer-mediated communication in an organizational setting:
Explaining task-related and socioemotional uses. In M. L. McLaughlin (Ed.), Communication
yearbook (Vol. 9, pp. 777–804). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Stoycheff, E. (2016). Under surveillance: Examining Facebook’s spiral of silence effects in the wake
of NSA internet monitoring. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 93, 296–311. doi:10.
1177/1077699016630255
COMMUNICATION MONOGRAPHS 21
Suh, A., & Wagner, C. (2013, January). Factors affecting individual flaming in virtual communities.
In Proceedings of the 46th Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences (HICSS)
(pp. 3282–3291). IEEE. doi:10.1109/HICSS.2013.230
Sundar, S. S. (2008). The MAIN model: A heuristic approach to understanding technology effects
on credibility. In M. J. Metzger & A. J. Flanagin (Eds.), Digital media, youth, and credibility (pp.
72–100). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Sundar, S. S., & Bellur, S. (2011). Concept explication in the Internet age: The case of interactivity.
In E. P. Bucy & R. L. Holbert (Eds.), Sourcebook for political communication research: Methods,
measures, and analytical techniques (pp. 485–500). New York: Routledge.
Sundar, S. S., & Limperos, A. M. (2013). Uses and grats 2.0: New gratifications for new media.
Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 57, 504–525. doi:10.1080/08838151.2013.845827
Tanis, M., & Postmes, T. (2003). Social cues and impression formation in CMC. Journal of
Communication, 53, 676–693. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2003.tb02917.x
Tao, C. C., & Bucy, E. P. (2007). Conceptualizing media stimuli in experimental research:
Psychological versus attribute-based definitions. Human Communication Research, 33, 397–
426. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2007.00305.x
Toma, C. L., Hancock, J. T., & Ellison, N. B. (2008). Separating fact from fiction: An examination of
deceptive self-presentation in online dating profiles. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
34, 1023–1036. doi:10.1177/0146167208318067
Treem, J., & Leonardi, P. (2013). Social media use in organizations: Exploring the affordances of
visibility, editability, persistence, and association. In C. T. Salmon (Ed.), Communication year-
book (vol. 38, pp. 143–189). New York, NY: Routledge.
Turkle, S. (2015). Reclaiming conversation: The power of talk in the digital age. New York, NY:
Penguin Press.
Walther, J. B. (1994). Anticipated ongoing interaction versus channel effects on relational com-
munication in computer-mediated interaction. Human Communication Research, 20, 473–
501. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.1994.tb00332.x
Walther, J. B. (1996). Computer-mediated communication: Impersonal, interpersonal and hyper-
personal interaction. Communication Research, 23, 3–43. doi:10.1177/009365096023001001
Walther, J. B. (2009). Computer-mediated communication. In C. R. Berger, M. E. Roloff, & D. R.
Roskos-Ewoldsen (Eds.), Handbook of communication science (2nd ed., pp. 489–505). Newbury
Park, CA: Sage.
Walther, J. B. (2013). Affordances, effects, and technology errors. In C. T. Salmon (Ed.),
Communication yearbook (vol. 38, pp. 190–193). New York, NY: Routledge.
Walther, J. B., Gay, G., & Hancock, J. T. (2005). How do communication and technology research-
ers study the internet? Journal of Communication, 55, 632–657. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2005.
tb02688.x
Walther, J. B., & Parks, M. R. (2002). Cues filtered out, cues filtered in: Computer-mediated com-
munication and relationships. In M. L. Knapp & J. A. Daly (Eds.), Handbook of interpersonal
communication (3rd ed., pp. 529–563). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Wellman, B., Quan-Haase, A., Boase, J., Chen, W., Hampton, K., Díaz, I., & Miyata, K. (2003). The
social affordances of the Internet for networked individualism. Journal of Computer-Mediated
Communication, 8(3). doi:10.1111/j.1083-6101.2003.tb00216.x
Williams, F., & Rice, R. E. (1983). Communication research and the new media technologies. In R.
N. Bostrom (Ed.), Communication yearbook (Vol. 7, pp. 200–224). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.