Decision Making Ob
Decision Making Ob
RELATIONSHIPS
REPORT
Submitted By
NANDHINI R (2213059)
MITHUN GANESH (2213057)
simple heuristics rather than on elaborate calculations (Hauser 2011). In the course of the last
decades, literature has identified many different types of heuristics that could be applied in
such simple decision situations, like the recognition heuristic (Gigerenzer and Goldstein 2011)
or the take-the-best heuristic (Gigerenzer and Goldstein 1996; Newell et al. 2003).
also include inaction. While it can be argued that management is decision making, half of the
decisions made by managers within organizations ultimately fail (Ireland & Miller, 2004; Nutt,
2002; Nutt, 1999). Therefore, increasing effectiveness in decision making is an important part
The process by which decision makers choose the decision parameters they will use to evaluate
decision alternatives usually involves cognitive biases constrained by past events and
individual cognitive styles. The decision-making process is the stage where all alternatives are
evaluated to produce a final choice. This process can be based on reasoning or it can be an
emotional process. It can be rational or irrational and can be based on explicit or tacit
assumptions. In the final step, actions need to be planned and decisions are implemented.
Group decisions free any single member from accountability for the group’s final choice, so
more extreme position can be taken. It’s also likely that people take on extreme positions
because they want to demonstrate how different they are from the outgroup.
Individual Vs Group Decision Making
Individual decision-making does not involve a group or even more than one person.
Individual decision-making is quick and generally cost-effective, because it does not require
gathering others and scheduling a meeting or multiple meetings or sending a single email. An
individual generally makes prompt decisions, while a group is dominated by various people,
consumes lots of time. Individuals do not escape responsibilities. They are accountable for their
acts and performance. In a group, it is not easy to hold any one person accountable for a wrong
decision. Individual decision-making saves time, money and energy as individuals usually
make prompt and logical decisions. There are also cons to individual decision-making. These
include: A group has the potential to collect more complete information, compared to an
individual, while making decisions. An individual uses his own intuition and views. A group
has many members, so its many views and many approaches result in better decision-making.
A group discovers the hidden talent and core competency of employees of an organization. An
individual will not take into consideration every member's interest, while a group will take into
involve multiple individuals that analyse various issues and situations and consider how to
solve them. The overall effectiveness of group decision-making depends on many factors. For
example, time constraints or any underlying conflicts can impact the group decision-making
outcomes. That’s why, before problems occur, it’s vital to establish group decision-making
1. Interacting Group
The most common form of group decision making takes place in interacting groups. Members
meet face to face and rely on both verbal and nonverbal interaction to communicate.
2. Brainstorming
Brainstorming overcomes the pressures for conformity that dampen creativity by encouraging
any and all alternatives while withholding criticism. (A. F. Osborn, 1963; McGlynn et al, 2004;
Litchfield, 2008). To encourage members “to think the unusual,” no criticism is allowed, even
of the most bizarre suggestions, and all ideas are recorded for later discussion and analysis.
Production blocking: When people are generating ideas in a group, many are talking at once,
which blocks the thought process and eventually impedes the sharing of ideas (Kerr and
Tindale, 2004).
The nominal group technique restricts discussion or interpersonal communication during the
decision-making process, hence the term nominal. Group members are all physically present,
and then the group takes the following steps: Before any discussion takes place, each member
independently writes down idea son the problem. After this silent period, each member presents
one idea to the group. No discussion takes place until all ideas have been presented and
recorded. The group discusses the ideas for clarity and evaluates them. Each group member
silently and independently rank-orders the ideas. The idea with the highest aggregate ranking
determines the final decision. Advantage: Permits a group to meet formally but does not restrict
independent thinking, as does an interacting group. Research generally shows nominal groups
The Electronic Meeting blends the nominal group technique with sophisticated computer
technology (Baltes, Dickson, Sherman, Bauer, and LaGanke, 2002). Up to 50 people sit around
a horseshoe-shaped table, empty except for a series of networked laptops. Issues are presented
to them, and they type their responses into their computers. These individual but anonymous
comments, as well as aggregate votes, are displayed on a projection screen. This technique also
allows people to be brutally honest without penalty, and it’s fast because chitchat is eliminated.
What is Heuristics?
A heuristic is a word from the Greek meaning ‘to discover’. It is an approach to problem-
solving that takes one’s personal experience into account. Heuristics provide strategies to
diminish the work of retrieving and storing information in memory and of streamlining the
making the choice or passing judgement. However, whilst heuristics can speed up our problem-
solving and decision-making processes, they can introduce errors and bias judgements.
fraction of subjects, and only in certain situations. For example, Newell et al. (2003) found that
roughly one third of subjects were actually using take-the best. Similarly, in the experiments
of Bro¨der (2000) and Bro¨der and Schiffer (2006), between 28 and 53 % of the subjects were
classified as using take-the-best. Other studies found even lower numbers, for example
Glo¨ckner and Bro¨der (2011) found that less than 10 % of their subjects used recognition, and
only about 15 % used take-the-best. Concerning the recognition heuristic, Pachur et al. (2008)
found that about half of their subjects used recognition in a strict sense (without incorporating
additional information). This limited extent of actual use of heuristics has led researchers to
study factors that could influence the application of heuristics. Gigerenzer and Goldstein (2011)
thus noted that decision makers seem to apply different decision strategies, but little is known
about the factors that influence their choice. One framework to explain the choice of different
off between the cognitive effort needed to make a decision, and the quality of the decision,
since methods leading to better decisions typically also require higher effort. However, since
the fast and frugal heuristics we consider here are all characterized by very little cognitive
effort, this framework cannot be applied to the choice between these heuristics. Two types of
factors can be distinguished: factors that refer to individual characteristics of the decision
maker, and factors related to the decision problem or the environment. Research on individual
factors has indicated that intelligence moderates the choice of decision strategies such as
heuristics. Researchers also studied the impact of the Big Five personality characteristics on
the use of heuristics and found that persons high in neuroticism are more likely to use the
maker has about alternatives has some impact on choices (Pohl 2006), although its impact
seems to be limited (Hilbig et al. 2009). The type of environment in which a decision is made
has also an impact on the decision-making strategy (Bro¨der 2003). In particular, a high amount
of uncertainty present in the environment seems to increase reliance on heuristics like take-the-
best (Hogarth and Karelaia 2006; Newell et al. 2003). The choice of strategies also depends on
the availability of additional cues, their validity and the cost at which such information can be
obtained. If the validity of the primary cue is high, take-the best is more often used (Newell et
al. 2003), while higher validity of other cues reduces its usage (Newell and Fernandez 2006).
Some authors also report that the cost of additional information increases the use of heuristics
(Newell et al. 2003), while others report that choices can still be better explained by alternative
models even if information acquisition has high costs. Decision making styles and the use of
heuristic. Another important factor that influences the use of heuristics is time pressure. A
positive effect of time pressure on the use of the recognition heuristic was found by Pachur and
Hertwig (2006). Furthermore, Hilbig et al. (2012) found that time pressure increases reliance
on the recognition heuristic even when violating time constraints does not have a negative
impact on the decision maker. It thus seems that the mere presence of time pressure has a strong
effect on the choice of a decision strategy. Time pressure was also used as an experimental
factor to induce heuristic choice behavior in consumer decision experiments. Since the
presence or absence of time pressure is likely to induce different levels of use of heuristics for
the same decision problem, it was taken as an experimental factor in the subsequent empirical
In contrast to factors related to the choice problem, research on individual factors that might
trigger the use of heuristics is so far limited. However, decision making behaviour in general
has been studied more intensively, and different classifications of decision-making styles can
be found in the literature. Many of them refer to specific settings like shopping situations
(Sproles and Kendall 1986). For this paper, we use a more general classification of decision-
making styles developed by Scott and Bruce (1995). They distinguish five dimensions of a
decision-making style:
1.Rational: This style is characterized by making decisions in a logical and systematic way, or
2. Intuitive: This style is characterized by relying on intuition and making decisions that ‘‘feel
right’’.
3.Dependent: This style represents decision makers who tend to consult others before making
4. Avoiding: This style is characterized as putting off decisions or making decisions only at the
last minute.
5.Spontaneous: This style refers to making quick and impulsive decisions. These five styles
Bruce (1995) does not classify decision makers uniquely into one style, but assigns each
decision maker a score in each dimension indicating how much that decision maker’s
individual style resembles each of the five prototypical styles. The instrument developed by
Scott and Bruce (1995) has been extensively used in studies on decision making behavior.
Among them, and in direct relation with the present paper, Crossley and Highhouse (2005)
used a variant of the instrument to classify different approaches applicants use in searching for
jobs. Curseu and Schruijer (2012) studied the relationship of decision making styles to actual
decision performance and found that subjects scoring high in the rational dimension were less
affected by decision biases, and subjects scoring high on the avoiding style were more
indecisive. Similarly, de Bruin et al. (2007) found a positive relationship between rational
decision making style and decision competence. Gettinger et al. (2013) used the instrument to
classify users of a decision support system and their preferences for different graphical
information representations.
The confirmation bias is the tendency to listen more often to information that confirms our
existing beliefs. Through this bias, people tend to favor information that reinforces the things
The hindsight bias is a common cognitive bias that involves the tendency to see events, even
random ones, as more predictable than they are. It's also commonly referred to as the "I knew
The anchoring bias is the tendency to be overly influenced by the first piece of information
that we hear. Some examples of how this works: The first number voiced during a price
negotiation typically becomes the anchoring point from which all further negotiations are
based.
Misinformation Effect
The misinformation effect is the tendency for information received after an event to interfere
with one's memory of the original happenings. Research has shown that the introduction of
even relatively subtle new information later on can have a dramatic effect on how people
Actor-Observer Bias
According to the actor-observer bias, people explain their own behavior with situational
causes and other people's behavior with internal causes. False Consensus Effect: The false
consensus effect is the tendency people have to overestimate how much other people agree
The halo effect is the tendency for an initial impression of a person to influence what we
think of them overall. Also known as the "physical attractiveness stereotype" or the "what is
beautiful is 'good' principle" we are either influenced by or use the halo to influence others
The self-serving bias is a tendency for people tend to give themselves credit for successes but
The availability heuristic is the tendency to estimate the probability of something happening
The optimism bias is a tendency to overestimate the likelihood that good things will happen
to us while underestimating the probability that negative events will impact our lives.
Heuristic Decision-Making
boundedly rational decision-makers, an idea originating from the work of the Carnegie School,
notably March and Simon (1958) and Cyert and March (1963). The central assumption of
bounded rationality is that decision-makers are constrained by their cognitive limitations and
pursue solutions that are satisfactory rather than objectively optimal (Cyert & March, 1963;
March & Simon, 1958). Indeed, the organizational sphere is too complex and uncertain to be
fully understood in every detail at the managerial level, who must, therefore, base their
1991; Gavetti & Levinthal, 2000). Building on the notion of bounded rationality, recent
research suggests that managers do not base their decisions on a careful assessment of all
available information in a given instance. Instead, they rely on heuristics that can reduce the
& Gaissmaier, 2011). Specifically, heuristics are learned rules, both conscious and
place of deliberately thinking about and processing large amounts of information (e.g.,
Gigerenzer & Gaissmaier, 2011). Heuristics are especially useful in situations where decisions
are made in conditions of limited information, processing capability, and time (Newell &
unfamiliar environments by providing direction and coherence (Bingham & Haleblian, 2012).
It is important to note, however, that this ability is dependent on prior managerial experience
of a particular situation (Maitland & Sammartino, 2015a). Recent research has also ascertained
this link by showing that firms develop heuristics that are idiosyncratic to their own operations
through process experience (Bingham & Eisenhardt, 2011), and that negative outcomes may
enhance the learning of heuristics (Bingham & Haleblian, 2012). Ultimately, past and
making functions.
After The Decision
After a decision is made, people experience a variety of reactions. In addition, present decisions
influence future decision making. Several of the outcomes that may result from a decision are
making. Interestingly, regret may shape the decision making process. According to Abraham
and Sheeran (2003), anticipated regret is the belief that the decision will be result of inaction.
Anticipated regret may prompt behavior; that is, when a person indicates they will do
something, such as exercise, they may follow through with their intended decision, to avoid
regret. Once the decision is made, the impact of the decision, if regret is experienced, will
impact future decisions. People can often get consumed with examining the other options that
were available; the path not taken (Sagi & Friedland, 2007).Sagi and Friedland (2007)
theorized people feel regret in accordance with how the decision was made; regret may be
dependent on the number of options that were available during the decision making process;
and how varied the options were may impact how regret is experienced after the decision was
made. Through a series of experiments, Sagi and Friedland concluded that people feel remorse
because they feel they were able to make a better choice by looking at more information,
previously disregarded, and carefully weighing the pros and cons of each choice. In addition,
regret is magnified when individuals revisit the other available options and considering what
satisfaction the other option would have brought them. Interestingly, people who are
dissatisfied with their decision feel obligated to embrace the decision, as a means to reducing
anxiety regarding the quality of the decision (Botti & Iyengar, 2004; see also Gilbert & Ebert,
2002). For example, when a job applicant does not get hired, he may restructure the experience,
and find many reasons that explain why he did not want to work for the company. In addition
to regret, individuals may also experience satisfaction with their decisions. Satisfaction refers
to how pleased the decision maker is with the outcome of the decision. There are many things
that impact levels of satisfaction. Botti and Iyengar (2004) observed individuals prefer to make
their own decisions and believe they will be more satisfied with their choices; however, when
people are given only undesirable options, decision makers are less satisfied than those who
have had the choice made for them. Botti and Iyengar posited the explanation for this
phenomenon is that the decision maker assumes responsibility for the decision made. As a
result, if the available choices are bad, they may feel as though they are responsible for making
poor choices.
Given that research on heuristics shows heuristic rules are used commonly in situations of
limited information, limited time, and limited cognitive capacity, it is plausible to assume that
heuristics are used extensively when actors interact in business relationships, since such
conditions are typical for interaction situations. To what extent heuristics are used and affect
empirical evidence is yet to be produced in future research. However, assuming heuristics are
used in business relationships has some implications for management practice. The first is that
management should become conscious of the power of interaction and of the role heuristics
can play in the adaptive capacity of their businesses. This requires understanding that the
behaviours, and that adaptive capacity and interaction effectiveness at company level are an
aggregate of the interaction capabilities at relationship level. Past research offers consistent
evidence of the link between interaction and relationship outcomes and also of the link between
business performance and the organization's capacity to adapt and innovate. Consciousness
about the role of heuristics in the adaptive capacity of an organization means being able to see
the limits and potential of the use of heuristics, and to cope with these. The use of heuristics
has drawbacks that are partly related to their tacit and personal nature; they can result in biases
when applied to situations in which there is scope for systematic analysis as in analysis of past
performance, for instance. One possible way to contain the negative consequences of the use
of heuristics is to make them less tacit and more explicit, which will make it easier to identify
when the use of heuristics is desirable or not. The potential for using heuristics relates to their
‘accuracy’ and time and data efficiency in situations where the scope for systematic analytical
approaches is limited. Some studies show that, under certain circumstances, heuristics are more
effective for producing strategically relevant business behaviours because they enhance the
capacity of an organization to adapt and innovate when the context is in motion (Astebro
&Elhedhli, 2006; Bingham & Eisenhardt, 2011; Davis, Eisenhardt, & Bingham, 2009;
Eisenhardt & Sull, 2001). These confirm indications from heuristic research that under certain
circumstances the use of heuristics produces more effective behavioural strategies than more
to other types of rule-based behaviours, such as following routines and norms, is related to the
‘local’ matching of heuristic rules to the context, on the one hand, and the actor's own features
and skills, on the other. Such an ‘ad hoc’ matching is likely to be a mechanism that ensures
flexibility in managing the myriad different issues arising during interactions and also in coping
with the variety of mindsets and toolboxes an organization faces when it interacts with, and
adapts to, others with whom it does business. It is plausible that similar effects can be found
regarding interaction effectiveness in business relationships. The above studies also point out
that a business's ability to adapt and innovate is related to the “portfolio of heuristics” and the
the toolboxes individual actors use and activate in various interaction episodes. Given the role
of the collective toolbox in company performance it should be effectively managed. That bring
us to the need to settle for a policy that ensures heuristics accompany uses are effective.
recommendations will depend on further research and insights from management practice. But,
on the basis of limited past research, we can make a few observations. Enhancing the adaptive
toolbox at organizational level requires accepting the temporary nature of rules and the fact
that their use is personal. Management should spare no effort in supporting the development
and refinement of the toolbox of the individuals involved in business relationships. Effective
experience based heuristics are acquired. That, in turn, has some organizational implications in
company's interactions involves at least four issues suggested in Fig. 1: (1) How to sustain the
acquisition of individual heuristic rules: this involves finding ways that favour the acquisition
and development of individual heuristics and aggregating them into an effective set (toolbox)
at collective level; (2) How to cope with the diffusion of certain heuristics within an
organization; should there be an effort to render the heuristics less personal, or should they be
left to the individuals to match to situations. Should the diversity in heuristics used be tolerated
or favoured? How, if at all, should heuristics be shared within an organization?; (3) Whether
and how to share heuristics externally; should clarification be given as to what heuristics are in
use in interaction across the boundaries of an organization? To what extent does sharing
heuristics externally favour effective interaction processes and adaptations?; (4) Is evolution in
heuristics in use desirable, and to what extent should the use of heuristics evolve both at the
individual level and at the level of the entire organization? How is the learning of heuristics to
be favoured? How can individual heuristics that are counterproductive be dealt with?
Opening the black box of interaction and finding that the space for systematic analysis and
decision making in interaction is severely limited do not mean management should give up
trying to behave sensibly in pursuit of its intended aims. In practice, management remains
accountable for the economic outcomes of business relationships, which depend on how
effectively the parties involved can cope in interaction in business relationships. Once we
reckon that the use of heuristics is widespread (rather than practicing extensive problem
solving), there is a need to focus on tools of management that might appear counterintuitive in
Conclusion
Heuristics are ingrained cognitive processes utilized by all humans, and can lead to a
concerning variety of biases. Both of these statements are established facts. However, this does
not mean that the biases that heuristics produce are unavoidable. As the wide ranging impacts
of such biases on societal institutions has become a popular research topic, psychologists have
emphasized techniques for reaching more sound, thoughtful and fair decisions in our daily