The Interaction Hypothesis PDF
The Interaction Hypothesis PDF
The Interaction Hypothesis PDF
Input processing
In his research with American university students learning foreign languages,
Bill VanPatten (2004) observed many cases of students misinterpreting sen
tences. For example, as predicted by the competition model discussed earlier
in this chapter, when English speakers heard sentences in Spanish, they used
word order to interpret the relationships among the nouns in the sentence.
Thus, they interpreted 'La sigue el señor' as 'She (subject pronoun) follows
the man'. The correct interpretation is 'Her (object pronoun) follows the man'
(subject of the sentence). In other words, the correct English translation would
be 'The man follows her'. In order to understand that, students need to learn
that in Spanish, a pronoun object often precedes the verb and that, rather than
rely on the word order alone, it is essential to pay attention to whether the
form of the pronoun indicates a subject or an object.
VanPatten argued that the problem arose in part from the fact that learners
have limited processing capacity and cannot pay attention to form and
meaning at the same time. Not surprisingly, they tend to give priority to
meaning, overlooking sorne features of the language form. When the context
in which they hear a sentence helps them make sense of it, that is a good
strategy for understanding the general idea, but it may interfere with learners'
progress in acquiring the language. In Chapter 6 we will see how VanPatten
developed instructional procedures that require learners to focus on the
specific language features in order to interpret the meaning, thus pushing them
to acquire those features.
Processability theory
Jürgen Meisel, Harald Clahsen, and Manfred Pienemann ( 1981) studied the
acquisition of German by a group of adult migrant workers who had little or
no second language instruction. They analysed large samples of their speech
and described the details of developmental sequences in their production of
simple and complex sentences. They concluded that the sequence of develop
ment for features of syntax and morphology was affected by how easy these
were to process. Ease of processing was found to depend to a large extent on
the position of those features in a sentence. Features that typically occurred at
the beginning or end of a sentence were easier to process (and learn) than
those in the middle. All learners acquired the features in the same sequence,
Explaining second languagelearning 118
even though they progressed at different rates. The researchers also found that
sorne language features did not seem to be affected by these constraints and
could be learned and used by learners who were at different developmen tal
stages. These were referred to asvariational features.
Pienemann (1999, 2003) developed processability theory on the basis of
research with learners of different languages in a variety of settings, both
instructional and informal. One important aspect of his theory is the inte
gration of developmental sequences with first language influence. He argues
that his theory explains why learners do not simply transfer features from their
first language at early stages of acquisition. Instead, they have to develop a
certain level of processing capacity in the second language before they can use
their knowledge of the features that already exist in their first language. We
saw examples of this in the acquisition of negatives and questions in Chapter
2.
Summary
In the end, what all theories oflanguage acquisition are intended to
account for is the ability of human learners to acquire language within
a variety of social and instructional environments. All of the theories
discussed in this chapter and in Chapter 1 use metaphors to represent
something that cannot be observed directly.
Linguists working from an innatist perspective draw much of their
evidence from studies of the complexities of proficient speakers'
knowledge of lan guage and from analysis of their own intuitions about
language. Cognitive and developmental psychologists argue that it is
not enough to know what the final state of knowledge is and that more
attention should be paid to cor pus-based studies of the input, as well as
to the developmental steps leading up to the achievement of high levels
ofproficiency.
Recent cognitive perspectives have often involved computer
simulations or controlled laboratory experiments where people learn
specific sets of care fully chosen linguistic features, often in an
invented language. Many linguists argue that this does not entitle
psychologists to generalize to the complexities of the linguistic
knowledge that learners eventually have.
lnteractionists emphasize the role of negotiation for meaning in
conversa tional interactions. This perspective and the sociocultural
perspective provide insights into the ways in which learners can gain
access to new knowledge about the language when they have support
from an interlocutor. Sorne linguists challenge the interactionist
position, arguing that much of what learners need to know is not
Explaining second languagelearning 122
available in the input, and so they put greater emphasis on innate
principies oflanguage that learners can draw on.
Both linguists and psychologists draw sorne of their evidence from neuro
logical research. At present, most of the research on language
representation in the brain and specific neurological activity during
language processing is inconclusive. However, advances in technology
are rapidly increasing oppor tunities to observe brain activity more
directly. Such research will eventually contribute to reinterpretations of
research that previously could examine only the observable behaviour
oflearners speaking or performing other lan guage tasks.
Educators who are hoping that language acquisition theories will give
thern insight into language teaching practice are often frustrated by the
lack of
agreement among the 'experts'. The complexities of second language acquisi
tion, like those of first language acquisition, represent puzzles that scientists
will continue to work on far a long time. Research that has theory develop
ment as its goal has important long-term significance far language teaching
and learning, but agreement on a 'complete' theory of language acquisition is
probably, at best, a long way off. Even if such agreement were reached, there
would still be questions about how the theory should be interpreted far
language teaching practice.
While sorne teachers watch theory development with interest, they must still
continue to teach and plan lessons and assess students' performance in the
absence of a comprehensive theory of second language learning. A growing
body of applied research draws on a wide range of theoretical orientations,
sometimes explicitly stated, sometimes merely implied. This research may
provide information that is more helpful in guiding teachers' reflections about
pedagogy. In Chapters 5 and 6, we will examine language acquisition research
that has focused on learning in the dassroom.