The Future of Parenting Programs I Design PDF
The Future of Parenting Programs I Design PDF
The Future of Parenting Programs I Design PDF
To cite this article: Marc H. Bornstein, Lucie Cluver, Kirby Deater-Deckard, Nancy E. Hill, Justin
Jager, Sonya Krutikova, Richard M. Lerner & Hirokazu Yoshikawa (2022) The Future of Parenting
Programs: I Design, Parenting, 22:3, 201-234, DOI: 10.1080/15295192.2022.2087040
SYNOPSIS
Parenting programs worldwide (and especially in low- and mid
dle-income countries) support parents in their caregiving roles.
Parenting programs are popular and prolific, but many outright
fail to deliver meaningful effects or eventuate in only small
effects. Incomplete consideration and execution of many design
features of programs can account for these shortfalls. This article
delimits several critical criteria surrounding successful design
and evaluation of evidence-based parenting programs.
Specific factors include important preliminary questions con
cerning details of program design, such as whether the topic
of the parenting program specifies the aspect(s) of parenting to
be encouraged or discouraged and what theory of change
underlies the program; program design contents concern sub
ject matter development, sources, and messages; program
design components specify the delivery mode, effectiveness,
location, and alignment; program design targeting and sam
pling concern whom the program is addressing, why, and
whether the program is designed to be universal or targeted
to a specific population; ensuring reliable and valid program
measurement; and rigorous experimental standards that
encompass evaluating program effectiveness, including rando
mized control trial or quasi-experimental designs and the selec
tion of control and comparison conditions. Policy makers,
program leaders, investigators, and, of course, parents and chil
dren all benefit when parenting programs are well designed.
INTRODUCTION
Parenting programs are common globally and implemented by basic science
investigators, nongovernmental organizations, and governments to support
and improve caregiving and optimize the lives of children. Investigators in the
medical, social, and behavioral sciences have developed best practices of good
program design, but these principles are nowhere collected and codified for
parenting programs specifically and, in consequence, are not always fully
applied to the detriment of parenting program success. What should the
next generation of parenting program designers know to take advantage of
knowledge gained from past and extant parenting programs? What are “best
practices” in parenting program design? As the first of three companion
articles on the future of parenting programs, this article focuses on program
design. The complementary two articles focus on program implementation
© 2022 The Author(s). Published with license by Taylor & Francis Group, LLC.
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/
licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly
cited.
202 BORNSTEIN ET AL.
and uptake/scaling. This article addresses seven key issues in parenting pro
gram design: design preliminary questions, design contents, design compo
nents, design targeting and sampling, measurement in design, and controls
and comparisons in evaluating design effectiveness. The main aims of this
article are to describe these key issues in the design of parenting programs and
best practices in determining whether parenting programs maximize effective
ness. Parenting program successes, and thereby optimal caregiving, child
development, and family life, will be improved through wider-spread recogni
tion and adherence to best practices in program design.
Necessity
Why is the parenting program necessary? What about the sample or popula
tion, community or locale requires support or prevention or intervention?
Who is determining the need for the program? Will the program design
resolve the requirement? How?
Known Knowledge
Level
At what level is the parenting program targeted? At a psychological or beha
vioral level (e.g., change in parent cognitions or practices; Bornstein, 2015) or
in mental health and well-being (Dix & Moed, 2019; Nelson-Coffey & Stewart,
2019)? At a family systems level (e.g., change in parent-child responsive and
play interactions; Bornstein, 2012; Tamis-LeMonda, Kuchirko, Escobar, &
Bornstein, 2019) or the co-parenting relationship (McHale & Sirotkin,
2019)? At a macro-level (e.g., change in a parent-community relation)?
Theory of Change
What theory of change underlies the parenting program? A theory of change
explains the effective process by specifying causal linkages in the program design
that yield the desired outcome (Douglass, Halle, & Tout, 2019). For instance,
a theory of change might specify that, if a program promotes warm, engaged,
knowledgeable, and responsive parenting, then the child will thrive. Parenting
programs that aim to encourage positive outcome(s) or discourage negative
outcome(s) downstream operate upstream via changes and/or improvements in
implementation (e.g., text-based self-implemented media or staff cognitions or
practices where staff deliver programs). Thus, program design theories of change
rely on indirect or mediator models (Figure 1). Although mediational chains
204 BORNSTEIN ET AL.
A logic model follows from the theory of change, with specific actions indexed
in specific ways at specific times with specific people to result in specific
outcomes. The logic model for the above-noted theory of change would
Figure 2. A moderated-mediation model. The strength of the Mediator effect varies across levels or
subgroups of the Moderator. The moderator alters path 1 and so changes the strength of the
independent variable → mediator link in the mediational chain (Path 3). An example is when
a program functions differently for different parents in a targeted group; concretely, maternal
verbal responsiveness training might proceed differently with more versus less verbal mothers. As
such, constructs that systematically impact program effectiveness (i.e., moderator effects) are
anticipated and should be identified.
PARENTING 205
Prevention/Intervention Scope
Is the program designed for all parents (i.e., universal) or parents with specific
needs or backgrounds (targeted)? Prevention/Intervention Scope refers to the
population for the intervention (e.g., Universal/Primary; Targeted/Secondary;
and Indicated/Tertiary). Programs designed for everyone are referred to as
“Universal” or “Primary” prevention programs. These include home-visiting
programs for new parents that all new parents receive. Targeted or Secondary
prevention/intervention programs are programs that are designed to support
parents and families who are at risk because of some identified factor (e.g.,
parenting in a high-crime neighborhood). Indicated/Tertiary programs are
deeper still and focused on parents, families, or children who are experiencing
adverse outcomes (Simeonsson, 1991).
For example, Brody et al. (2006) used quantitative studies of rural
African American families to develop a universal prevention known as
the Strong African American Families Program. Given the increasing
sophistication of machine learning and other adaptive computational
approaches, developing specific programs targeted to specific parents’
specific needs is becoming more feasible, fortuitously increasing program
designers’ ability to provide parents with “just-in-time” knowledge and
supports (Hill, 2021). A recurring challenge to parenting program uptake
is that programs do not meet parents where they are with the issues that
they confront at the moment (see Article III: Uptake and Scaling; Murry,
Berkel, & Liu, 2018).
Content Sources
Content Messages
Program content messages may be specific and prescriptive (e.g., when
a parenting program directs what specifically to do to encourage some cogni
tion or practice in a parent), or program content messages may be general and
based on broad principles (e.g., when a parenting program offers generic rules
for encouraging some cognition or practice in a parent). Whichever, content
messages should be erected on a faithful understanding of parents’ strengths
and needs.
The delivery mode (aka modality or platform) of the parenting program needs
to be specified as (1) interpersonal in a one-to-one, group, or combined format
or (2) not interpersonal in a format as self-instructional or based on technology
or media. Each mode has advantages and disadvantages. For example, groups
are useful to incorporate peer support for parent well-being when peer support
and community building are explicitly targeted in the theory of change. With the
increase in machine learning and individualized adaptive computer algorithms,
individualized, and targeted programs are both possible and feasible. Moreover,
some modes lend themselves to specific outcomes: Lectures, brochures, and
videos might prove more effective when delivered using active rather than
passive learning techniques, such as role-playing or actual interactions with
participants. In the face of the COVID-19 pandemic, parenting programs were
designed to accommodate to physical distancing using a variety of remote
platforms (e.g., digital/internet, telephone, text messaging, radio, and others)
to reach parents during lockdown periods (Hackett, Proulx, & Omoeva, 2021).
Internet programs can mitigate challenges to in-person attendance, including
transportation and childcare, but modes that rely on technology/remote pro
gramming can still pose technical difficulties related to access and connectivity
depending on the context. Moreover, internet-based programs inhibit the crea
tion of social support networks that are ancillary to program content but some
times integral to program success. Social supports and friendships that arise
from being in a program together often constitute significant program benefits
(Task Force for Child Survival and Development, 2003).
degrees offer parents still more intensive training in how to manage a range of
child behavioral problems through eight to 10 face-to-face or self-directed sessions
(degree four) or actual practice sessions with parents (degree five). A meta-analysis
of 70 studies of parenting interventions focused on maternal sensitivity and
attachment found that fewer than five sessions were equally effective to five to
16 sessions and more effective than more than 16 sessions (Bakermans-
Kranenburg, van IJzendoorn, & Juffer, 2003). Booster sessions that extend pro
gram duration are helpful to reinforce and remind parents about program content
after programs conclude (Breiner, Miller, & Hormes, 2021; Buchanan-Pascall,
Melvin, Gordon, & Gray, 2021; Zhang et al., 2021).
Program Location
Program Alignment
Program components should align with the needs and characteristics of the
participants and adjust appropriately to available human, technical, and
financial resources. Also, outcome variables are strongest when closely aligned
with program content (Pinquart & Teubert, 2010); but see the discussion of
indirect effects below.
Trade-offs
Adjudicating among these several criteria entails trade-offs. Parenting pro
grams with small doses with concise messaging might reach large numbers of
parents and achieve successful outcomes around more targeted knowledge and
information. Trade-offs are always an empirical question. In program design
generally, to design is to choose.
Parenting program designs need to specify the target sample, keeping in mind
that the sample is meant to represent a specific population. Results of a sample
are meaningful, not because they apply to the specific sample, but because the
sample represents, and hence generalize to, a larger population (which is the
reason for sampling in the first place; Bornstein, Jager, & Putnick, 2013; Jager,
Putnick, & Bornstein, 2017). Specify the sociodemographic characteristics of
the target sample (e.g., sample size, gender; Bornstein, 2013; Brown & Tam,
2019), ethnicity (Bornstein, 2019), socioeconomic status (Hoff & Laursen,
2019; Magnuson & Duncan, 2019) as well the presence and sizes of any
subsamples (e.g., the disadvantaged or at-risk; Bornstein, 2013, 2019; Brown
& Tam, 2019; Hoff & Laursen, 2019; Magnuson & Duncan, 2019). The target
sample of parenting programs is usually adult parents (both mothers and
fathers are children’s significant caregivers) or co-parents (McHale &
Sirotkin, 2019) but may be someone else in or outside the family in
a parenting role (e.g., grandparents, other members of the extended family,
non-parental caregivers, et al.; Kramer & Hamilton, 2019; Raikes et al., 2019;
Smith & Wild, 2019). The target sample could also be children or adolescents,
in which case the sample age or stage needs to be identified (Sandler, Ingram,
Wolchik, Tein, & Winslow, 2015). Every program must tailor its contents and
its methodology to the cognitive and social-emotional developmental levels of
the target sample. Samples should also be comprehensive; UNICEF Standards
for ECD Parenting Programs note: “Parenting programmes should involve all
parents and caregivers so that messaging about the function of parenting is
cohesive and coherent within the household” (UNICEF, 2017, p. 16).
Unit of Randomization
Parenting program designs need to specify the unit of randomization (if the
sample is a probability sample). (1) The unit of randomization might be an
individual parent or child, adolescent, family, or community (e.g., neighbor
hood, municipality, state, or province). (2) The unit of randomization might be
embedded in a cluster (e.g., families in a neighborhood, parent groups in village,
schools in a community). Clustering minimizes contamination effects from one
participant or group to another (Song & Herman, 2010a, 2010b). If clusters are
randomized, moreover, confounds are less an issue (Lachman et al., 2020).
However, randomizing at the cluster level can lead to “design effects” which
artificially deflate standard errors, but can be appropriately adjusted (Davis-
Kean, Jager, & Maslowsky, 2015). If clusters are not randomized, analyses need
to ensure balance across clusters and consider variation due to clustering in
examining effects (Raudenbush, Martinez, & Spybrook, 2007).
212 BORNSTEIN ET AL.
(Cook, 2014; Heckman, Urzua, & Vytlacil, 2006; Heckman & Vytlacil, 1999;
Murnane & Willett, 2010). An alternative to the traditional pretest–posttest
design involving participant self-reports is the retrospective pretest–posttest
design (Geldhof et al., 2018). When, as in most instances, prior or preliminary
evidence for the main pathways and processes in the theory of change are
based on correlational evidence, it is important to evaluate that background
correlational evidence to ensure that the evidence is based on rigorous mea
surement and sampling methods.
How does a parenting program designer know that the design will be
effective? Just because an “effect” is detected at the conclusion of the
program does not prove the program had efficacy (in the case of an
efficacy trial) or was effective (in the case of an effectiveness study; for
discussions of standards of evidence see Flay et al., 2005; Gottfredson
et al., 2015). An efficacy trial is an experiment or proof-of-concept study
to test whether a program works under researcher-controlled circum
stances. Program design therefore includes evaluation to ensure success
and meaningfulness. Three evaluation questions are: (1) when and how
to evaluate; (2) what measures to use in evaluation; and (3) how to judge
success and meaningfulness. (For a discussion of general standards of
evidence in evaluation, see Crowley et al., 2018.)
218 BORNSTEIN ET AL.
With respect to the first evaluation question of when and how, multi-phase
optimization models prior to finalization or adaptation of a model have been
advanced (Collins, 2018; Collins, Murphy, & Strecher, 2007). Evaluation data
collectors and analysts should be blind to all research conditions, and assess
ments should be made by researchers who are independent of the parenting
program developer (Flay et al., 2005). Although some independent program
evaluations have produced findings like those reported by program developers
(Baker-Henningham, Scott, Jones, & Walker, 2012), other evaluations of
programs have indicated that studies led by evaluators who had a higher
degree of influence on the design and implementation report substantially
larger effects than studies in which the evaluators have a lesser degree of
influence on the design and implementation (Petrosino & Soydan, 2005).
Measurement (and the corresponding measurement model) represents the
selections made by program personnel and stakeholders (e.g., families, com
munity leaders) of specific indicators (surveys, interviews, standardized tests,
observations, behavioral/performance tests, or biomarkers) that are used to
quantitatively and/or qualitatively measure or index each facet of the logic
model (Lerner, 2018).
Program effectiveness is usually judged with respect to program treatment
versus control and comparison conditions, and meaningfulness is usually
judged with respect to effect size of the difference between treatment and
control or comparison conditions (Cohen, 1988). (Large effect sizes are nor
mally sought, but even small to moderate effect sizes may still have practical
meaning, and small early effects can aggregate to later large effects; Bornstein,
2014; Dishion et al., 2008). Alongside the treatment group, five types of control
or comparison conditions test and defeat specific potential criticisms or
weaknesses in parenting program design regarding causal inference.
Replication is an additional evaluation consideration.
(1) The no-treatment condition. Parents in this group are not offered
program resources, but they are free to make use of other available
(even related) resources on their own. A mathematics intervention
program with Head Start families provided eight biweekly classes
for mother–child dyads plus access to a library of mathematics kits
for use at home (Starkey & Klein, 2000). Participants assigned to
the no-treatment (aka business-as-usual) condition in this study
received neither the classes nor access to the mathematics kits. But
the no-treatment condition participants could find or continue to
use other resources to promote their child’s mathematics knowl
edge (e.g., borrow mathematics-related materials from their local
library). Some researchers contend that a business-as-usual condi
tion is ethically problematic and opt to offer a nominal level
(Condition 2) program for all participants as a comparison
PARENTING 219
Replication
Replication studies are essential in science to rule out chance findings and the
potential influences of specific times and unique contextual factors, which is
also applicable to parenting program evaluation (Duncan, Engel, Claessens, &
Dowsett, 2014). Current best practices to exclude chance findings include
preregistration of design (including sample, hypotheses, measures, and analy
tic approach) and open-access data (Naudet et al., 2018; Nosek, Ebersole,
DeHaven, & Mellor, 2018). As for replication across contexts, in practice, it
is often impractical to run a trial every time a change is made to a program, or
a program is implemented in a new context. Replication studies are unfortu
nately rare in the design of parenting programs (Pashler & Harris, 2012). An
exception is systematic program research as in Olds’s Nurse-Family
Partnership (NFP) which investigated replication in a series of sequential
efficacy trials, each conducted with a different population in a different com
munity. The first trial was implemented with a small European American
sample of women in a semi-rural community (Elmira, NY) that was followed
up 15 years after the intervention (Olds et al., 1997, 1998). A second efficacy
trial was conducted with a larger sample of African American women
(Memphis, TN) and followed up 9 years post-intervention (Olds et al.,
2007a, 2007b). A third RCT was conducted (Denver, CO) with a Mexican
American sample and followed up 4 years post-intervention (Olds et al., 2004).
Each follow-up study replicated generally positive parenting program effects
(e.g., reductions in child abuse, neglect, and injuries), although some differ
ences were evident across the three sites and populations in types of impacts.
Alongside replication, where possible, systematic research on drivers of
impacts is key because knowing the drivers of impacts fortifies an assumption
that those drivers will be robust to variation in time and context. Rather than
running full trials through replication exercises at different times and in
different contexts, it is often more feasible to measure and document key
drivers.
program to promote children’s reading Klaus and Gray (1968) had home
visitors school mothers in a program versus a comparison with a popular
children’s magazine. When they got no program effect, the investigators
discovered that local merchants in the community sold unusually large quan
tities of the same children’s magazine. They then graphed overlapping family
and friendship networks of participants to understand the flow of information
(“horizontal diffusion”) between their program and comparison groups. With
the advent of modern social media, it is also feasible that experiences with
a parenting program even at great distances can contaminate or diffuse.
Another benefit of fidelity monitoring is assurance that control and compar
ison participants are not exposed to or participate in program services
(Gottfredson et al., 2015). Contamination and diffusion are non-trivial in
that both undermine the possibility to detect effects between program versus
control and comparison conditions.
(3) Condition 3 deletes the program and tests the effects of pretest priming on
the posttest. A comparison of Conditions 1 versus 3 on the pretest-posttest
difference reveals the unique effect (or non-effect) of the program.
(4) Condition 4 deletes the pretest and the program, retaining only the
posttest. Between the times of the pretest and the posttest in Condition
1, participants grow and change (possibly also affected by contamina
tion or diffusion). A comparison of Conditions 1 and versus 4 reveals
the effects of development or other non-program influences (such as the
“Hawthorne Effect” or contamination and diffusion).
CONCLUSIONS
Designing parenting programs calls for a wide array of upstream decisions that
have implications for downstream outcomes. Parenting programs are multi-
dimensional and dynamic but always occur with specific aims involving
specific populations and in specific contexts. Specific designs therefore differ
224 BORNSTEIN ET AL.
by contents, participants, and situations, but still share many common over
arching meta-principles described here. Evidence-based research and scientific
rigor constitute the core of quality parenting program design. Article I focuses
on a broad range of design considerations for parenting program preventions,
supports, and interventions. It is not thought that all parenting programs,
especially those undertaken in challenging contexts of low- and middle-
income countries, can implement all of the design considerations detailed in
this article. However, it is best for parenting program designers to be fully
cognizant of all these considerations of program design so that priorities
among them can be adjudicated thoughtfully and trade-offs clearly understood
in advance of any parenting program field implementation. Building and
disseminating knowledge about good program design is vital to ensuring
future parenting programs that enhance caregiving, child development, and
family life. Design challenges span a wide range of decisions about how to
devise, implement, evaluate, and scale up parenting programs. This article on
Design is accompanied by companion articles on parenting program imple
mentation and parenting program up-take and scaling.
ARTICLE INFORMATION
Conflict of Interest Disclosures
Each author signed a form for disclosure of potential conflicts of interest. No
authors reported any financial or other conflicts of interest in relation to the
work described.
Ethical Principles
Funding
MHB was supported by the Intramural Research Program of the NIH/NICHD, UNICEF, and
an International Research Fellowship at the Institute for Fiscal Studies, funded by the European
Research Council under the Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme. LC was
supported by the UKRI Global Challenges Research Fund, the European Research Council,
the LEGO Foundation, and the Oak Foundation. RML was supported by the Templeton World
Charity Foundation, the Chan Zuckerberg Initiative, Compassion International, and the
National 4-H Council. HY was supported by funding from the NYU Abu Dhabi Research
Institute to the Global TIES for Children Center.
Acknowledgements
The ideas and opinions expressed herein are those of the authors alone, and endorsement by
the authors’ Institutions or funding agencies is not intended and should not be inferred. We
thank L. Henry for keen assistance.
ORCID
Marc H. Bornstein http://orcid.org/0000-0002-6810-8427
Lucie Cluver http://orcid.org/0000-0002-0418-835X
Kirby Deater-Deckard http://orcid.org/0000-0003-4151-2152
Nancy E. Hill http://orcid.org/0000-0002-2489-3582
Justin Jager http://orcid.org/0000-0003-0067-0920
Sonya Krutikova http://orcid.org/0000-0003-2488-7489
Richard M. Lerner http://orcid.org/0000-0002-0429-8719
Hirokazu Yoshikawa http://orcid.org/0000-0001-5607-8526
REFERENCES
Adair, J. G. (1984). The Hawthorne effect: A reconsideration of the methodological artifact.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 69(2), 334–345. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.69.2.334
Adler-Baeder, F., Garneau, C., Vaughn, B., McGill, J., Harcourt, K. T., Ketring, S., & Smith, T.
(2018). The effects of mother participation in relationship education on coparenting,
parenting, and child social competence: Modeling spillover effects for low-income minority
preschool children. Family Process, 57(1), 113–130. doi:10.1111/famp.12267
Altafim, E. R. P., McCoy, D. C., & Linhares, M. B. M. (2021). Unpacking the impacts of
a universal parenting program on child behavior. Child Development, 92(2), 626–637.
doi:10.1111/cdev.13491
226 BORNSTEIN ET AL.
Attanasio, O., Cattan, S., Fitzsimons, E., Meghir, C., & Rubio-Codina, M. (2020).
Estimating the production function for human capital: Results from a randomized
controlled trial in Colombia. American Economic Review, 110(1), 48–85. doi:10.1257/
aer.20150183
Bailey, D., Duncan, G. J., Odgers, C. L., & Yu, W. (2017). Persistence and fadeout in the impacts
of child and adolescent interventions. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 10(1),
7–39. doi:10.1080/19345747.2016.1232459
Baker-Henningham, H., Scott, S., Jones, K., & Walker, S. (2012). Reducing child conduct
problems and promoting social skills in a middle-income country: Cluster randomised
controlled trial. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 201(2), 101–108. doi:10.1192/bjp.
bp.111.096834
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van Ijzendoorn, M. H., & Juffer, F. (2003). Less is more:
Meta-analyses of sensitivity and attachment interventions in early childhood. Psychological
Bulletin, 129(2), 195–215. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.129.2.195
Beigel, J. H., Tomashek, K. M., Dodd, L. E., Mehta, A. K., Zingman, B. S., Kalil, A. C., . . .
Ohmagari, N. (2020). Remdesivir for the treatment of Covid-19 — Final report. The New
England Journal of Medicine, 383(19), 1813–1826. doi:10.1056/NEJMoa2007764
Bernal, G., & Adames, C. (2017). Cultural adaptations: Conceptual, ethical, contextual, and
methodological issues for working with ethnocultural and majority-world populations.
Prevention Science, 18(6), 681–688. doi:10.1007/s11121-017-0806-0
Bornstein, M. H. (1995). Form and function: Implications for studies of culture and human
development. Culture & Psychology, 1(1), 123–137. doi:10.1177/1354067X9511009
Bornstein, M. H. (2012). Caregiver responsiveness and child development and learning: From
theory to research to practice. In P. L. Mangione (Ed.), A guide to cognitive development and
learning (pp. 11–25). California Department of Education’s Bureau of Publications.
Bornstein, M. H. (2013). Parenting X gender X culture X time. In W. B. Wilcox & K. K. Kline
(Eds.), Gender and parenthood: Biological and social scientific perspectives (pp. 91–119).
Columbia University Press.
Bornstein, M. H., Jager, J., & Putnick, D. L. (2013). Sampling in developmental science:
Situations, shortcomings, solutions, and standards. Developmental Review, 33(4), 357–370.
doi:10.1016/j.dr.2013.08.003
Bornstein, M. H. (2014). Human infancy . . . and the rest of the lifespan. Annual Review of
Psychology, 65(1), 121–158. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100359
Bornstein, M. H. (2015). Children’s parents. In M. H. Bornstein & T. Leventhal Eds.,
Ecological settings and processes in developmental systems. handbook of child psychology
and developmental science (Vol. 4, pp. 55–132). Wiley. doi:10.1002/9781118963418.
childpsy403
Bornstein, M. H., Putnick, D. L., Lansford, J. E., Pastorelli, C., Skinner, A. T., Sorbring, E., . . .
Oburu, P. (2015). Mother and father socially desirable responding in nine countries: Two
kinds of agreement and relations to parenting self-reports. International Journal of
Psychology, 50(3), 174–185. doi:10.1002/ijop.12084
Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.), (2019). Handbook of parenting (3e). Volume 4: Social conditions and
applied parenting. Routledge. doi:10.4324/9780429398995
Breiner, C. E., Miller, M. L., & Hormes, J. M. (2021). ARFID parent training protocol:
A randomized pilot trial evaluating a brief, parent-training program for avoidant/restrictive
food intake disorder. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 54(12), 2229–2235.
doi:10.1002/eat.23643
Breitenstein, S. M., Gross, D., & Christophersen, R. (2014). Digital delivery methods of
parenting training interventions: A systematic review. Worldviews on Evidence-Based
Nursing, 11(3), 168–176. doi:10.1111/wvn.12040
PARENTING 227
Brody, G. H., Murry, V. M., Gerrard, M., Gibbons, F. X., McNair, L., Brown, A. C., . . .
Chen, Y.-F. (2006). The strong African American families program: Prevention of youths’
high-risk behavior and a test of a model of change. Journal of Family Psychology, 20(1), 1–11.
doi:10.1037/0893-3200.20.1.1
Brown, C. S., & Tam, M. (2019). Parenting girls and boys. In M. H. Bornstein Ed., Children and
parenting. Handbook of parenting (Vol. 1, pp. 258–387). Routledge. doi:10.4324/
9780429440847-8
Buchanan-Pascall, S., Melvin, G. A., Gordon, M. S., & Gray, K. M. (2021). Evaluating the effect
of parent–child interactive groups in a school-based parent training program: Parenting
behavior, parenting stress and sense of competence. Child Psychiatry and Human
Development, 1–19. doi:10.1007/s10578-021-01276-6
Card, N. A. (2017). Methodological issues in measuring the development of character. Journal
of Character Education, 13(2), 29–45.
Cartwright, N., & Munro, E. (2010). The limitations of randomized controlled trials in
predicting effectiveness. Journal of Evaluation in Clinical Practice, 16(2), 260–266.
doi:10.1111/j.1365-2753.2010.01382.x
Cluver, L., Lachman, J. M., Sherr, L., Wessels, I., Krug, E., Rakotomalala, S., . . . McDonald, K.
(2020). Parenting in a time of COVID-19. The Lancet, 395(10231) Article e64, e64.
doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30736-4.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Erlbaum.
Collins, L. M., Murphy, S. A., & Strecher, V. (2007). The multiphase optimization strategy
(most) and the sequential multiple assignment randomized trial (SMART): New methods
for more potent eHealth interventions. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 32(Suppl
5), S112–S118. doi:10.1016/j.amepre.2007.01.022
Collins, L. M. (2018). Optimization of behavioral, biobehavioral, and biomedical interventions.
Springer. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-72206-1
Conaway, C., & Goldhaber, D. (2020). Appropriate standards of evidence for education policy
decision making. Education Finance and Policy, 15(2), 383–396. doi:10.1162/edfp_a_00301
Cook, T. D., Campbell, D. T., & Shadish, W. (2002). Experimental and quasi-experimental
designs for generalized causal inference. Houghton Mifflin. doi:10.1198/jasa.2005.s22
Cook, T. D. (2014). Quasi-experimental design. In C. L. Cooper (Ed.), Wiley encyclopedia of
management (pp. 1–2). Wiley Online Library. doi:10.1002/9781118785317.weom110227
Crowley, D. M., Dodge, K. A., Barnett, W. S., Corso, P., Duffy, S., Graham, P., . . . Plotnick, R.
(2018). Standards of evidence for conducting and reporting economic evaluations in pre
vention science. Prevention Science, 19(3), 366–390. doi:10.1007/s11121-017-0858-1
Davis-Kean, P. E., Jager, J., & Maslowsky, J. (2015). Answering developmental questions using
secondary data. Child Development Perspectives, 9(4), 256–261. doi:10.1111/cdep.12151
Dishion, T. J., Shaw, D., Connell, A., Gardner, F., Weaver, C., & Wilson, M. (2008). The family
check-up with high-risk indigent families: Preventing problem behavior by increasing
parents’ positive behavior support in early childhood. Child Development, 79(5),
1395–1414. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01195.x
Dishion, T., Forgatch, M., Chamberlain, P., & Pelham, W. E. (2016). The Oregon model of
behavior family therapy: From intervention design to promoting large-scale system change.
Behavior Therapy, 47(6), 812–837. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2016.02.002
Dix, T., & Moed, A. (2019). Parenting and depression. In M. H. Bornstein Ed., Special
conditions and applied parenting. Handbook of parenting (Vol. 4, pp. 449–483). Routledge.
doi:10.4324/9780429398995-15
Domenech-Rodríguez, M. M., Baumann, A. A., & Schwartz, A. L. (2011). Cultural adaptation of
an evidence based intervention: From theory to practice in a Latino/a community context.
American Journal of Community Psychology, 47(1), 170–186. doi:10.1007/s10464-010-9371-4
228 BORNSTEIN ET AL.
Douglass, A., Halle, T., & Tout, K. (2019). The culture of continuous learning project:
A breakthrough series collaborative for improving child care and head start quality:
Theory of change. In Office of planning, research and evaluation, administration for children
and families. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. https://www.acf.hhs.gov/
sites/default/files/documents/opre/ccl_theory_of_change_brief.pdf
Drake, K. L., & Ginsburg, G. S. (2011). Parenting practices of anxious and nonanxious mothers:
A multi-method, multi-informant approach. Child & Family Behavior Therapy, 33(4),
299–321. doi:10.1080/07317107.2011.623101
Duncan, G. J., Engel, M., Claessens, A., & Dowsett, C. J. (2014). Replication and robustness in
developmental research. Developmental Psychology, 50(11), 2417–2425. doi:10.1037/a0037996
Durlak, J. A., & DuPre, E. P. (2008). Implementation matters: A review of research on the
influence of implementation on program outcomes and the factors affecting
implementation. American Journal of Community Psychology, 41(3), 327–350. doi:10.1007/
s10464-008-9165-0
Erkut, S. (2010). Developing multiple language versions of instruments for intercultural
research. Child Development Perspectives, 4(1), 19–24. doi:10.1111/j.1750-8606.2009.00111.x
Flay, B. R., Biglan, A., Boruch, R. F., Castro, F. G., Gottfredson, D., Kellam, S., . . . Ji, P. (2005).
Standards of evidence: Criteria for efficacy, effectiveness and dissemination. Prevention
Science, 6(3), 151–175. doi:10.1007/s11121-005-5553-y
Forgatch, M. S., & DeGarmo, D. S. (1999). Parenting through change: An effective prevention
program for single mothers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67(5), 711–724.
doi:10.1037/0022-006X.67.5.711
Fredrick, J. W., & Luebbe, A. M. (2022). A multi-method, multi-informant test of maternal
emotion socialization in relation to adolescent fears of social evaluation. Research on Child
and Adolescent Psychopathology, 50(2), 177–192. doi:10.1007/s10802-021-00786-0
Gardner, F., Leijten, P., Melendez-Torres, G. J., Landau, S., Harris, V., Mann, J., . . . Scott, S.
(2019). The earlier the better? Individual participant data and traditional meta-analysis of
age effects of parenting interventions. Child Development, 90(1), 7–19. doi:10.1111/
cdev.13138
Geldhof, G. J., Warner, D. A., Finders, J. K., Thogmartin, A. A., Clark, A., & Longway, K. A.
(2018). Revisiting the utility of retrospective pre-post designs: The need for mixed-method
pilot data. Evaluation and Program Planning, 70, 83–89. doi:10.1016/j.
evalprogplan.2018.05.002
Gottfredson, D. C., Cook, T. D., Gardner, F. E., Gorman-Smith, D., Howe, G. W., Sandler, I. N.,
& Zafft, K. M. (2015). Standards of evidence for efficacy, effectiveness, and scale-up research
in prevention science: Next generation. Prevention Science, 16(7), 893–926. doi:10.1007/
s11121-015-0555-x
Graham, J. W. (2009). Missing data analysis: Making it work in the real world. Annual Review
of Psychology, 60(1), 549–576. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085530
Grantham-McGregor, S., & Smith, J. A. (2016). Extending the Jamaican early childhood
development intervention. Journal of Applied Research on Children: Informing Policy for
Children at Risk, 7(2), Article 4.
Grindal, T., Bowne, J. B., Yoshikawa, H., Schindler, H. S., Duncan, G. J., Magnuson, K., &
Shonkoff, J. P. (2016). The added impact of parenting education in early childhood educa
tion programs: A meta-analysis. Children and Youth Services Review, 70, 238–249.
doi:10.1016/j.childyouth.2016.09.018
Gross, D., Julion, W., & Fogg, L. (2001). What motivates participation and dropout among
low-income urban families of color in a prevention intervention? Family Relations: An
Interdisciplinary Journal of Applied Family Studies, 50(3), 246–254. doi:10.1111/j.1741-
3729.2001.00246.x
PARENTING 229
Gross, D., Garvey, C., Julion, W., Fogg, L., Tucker, S., & Mokros, H. (2009). Efficacy of the
Chicago parent program with low-income African American and Latino parents of young
children. Prevention Science, 10(1), 54–65. doi:10.1007/s11121-008-0116-7
Gul, R. B., & Ali, P. A. (2009). Clinical trials: The challenge of recruitment and retention of
participants. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 19(1–2), 227–233. doi:10.1111/j.1365-
2702.2009.03041.x
Gutiérrez, K. D., & Jurow, A. S. (2016). Social design experiments: Toward equity by
design. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 25(4), 565–598. doi:10.1080/10508406.2016.
1204548
Hackett, K., Proulx, K., & Omoeva, C. (2021). Case studies of programs to promote and protect
nurturing care during the COVID-19 pandemic. LEGO Foundation. Retrieved June 30, 2022,
from https://www.fhi360.org/sites/default/files/media/documents/resource-lego-case-study
-nurturing-care-report.pdf
Hebbeler, K. M., & Gerlach-Downie, S. G. (2002). Inside the black box of home visiting:
A qualitative analysis of why intended outcomes were not achieved. Early Childhood
Research Quarterly, 17(1), 28–51. doi:10.1016/S0885-2006(02)00128-X
Heckman, J. J., & Vytlacil, E. J. (1999). Local instrumental variables and latent variable models
for identifying and bounding treatment effects. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 96(8), 4730–4734. doi:10.1073/pnas.96.8.4730
Heckman, J. J., Urzua, S., & Vytlacil, E. (2006). Understanding instrumental variables in
models with essential heterogeneity. The Review of Economics and Statistics, 88(3),
389–432. doi:10.3386/w12574
Heckman, J. J., Moon, S. H., Pinto, R., Savelyev, P. A., & Yavitz, A. (2010). The rate of return to
the highscope perry preschool program. Journal of Public Economics, 94(1–2), 114–128.
doi:10.3386/w15471
Hill, N. E. (2021). In search of the individual embedded in context: Applications of the
specificity principle. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 75, 101289. Article
101289 doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2021.101289
Hoff, E., & Laursen, B. (2019). Socioeconomic status and parenting. In M. H. Bornstein Ed.,
Biology and ecology of parenting. Handbook of parenting (Vol. 2, pp. 421–447). Routledge.
doi:10.4324/9780429401459-13
Hulley, S. B., Cummings, S. R., Browner, W. S., Grady, D. G., & Newman, T. B. (2013).
Designing clinical research. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
Jager, J., Putnick, D. L., & Bornstein, M. H. (2017). Developmental methodology: II. More than
just convenient: The scientific merits of homogeneous convenience samples. Monographs of
the Society for Research in Child Development, 82(2), 13–30. doi:10.1111/mono.12296
Jensen, P. S., Hoagwood, K., & Trickett, E. J. (1999). Ivory towers or earthen trenches?
Community collaborations to foster real-world research. Applied Developmental Science, 3
(4), 206–212. doi:10.1207/s1532480xads0304_4
Kitzman, H. J., Cole, R., Yoos, H. L., & Olds, D. (1997). Challenges experienced by home
visitors: A qualitative study of program implementation. Journal of Community Psychology,
25(1), 95–109.
Klaus, R. A., & Gray, S. W. (1968). The early training project for disadvantaged children:
A report after five years. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 33(4),
iii–66. doi:10.2307/1165717
Knight, G., & Hill, N. (1998). Measurement equivalence in research involving minority
adolescents. In V. C. McLoyd & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Studying minority adolescents:
Conceptual, methodological, and theoretical issues (pp. 183–210). Erlbaum. doi:10.4324/
9781410601506-18
230 BORNSTEIN ET AL.
Korfmacher, J., O’Brien, R., Hiatt, S., & Olds, D. (1999). Differences in program implementa
tion between nurses and paraprofessionals providing home visits during pregnancy and
infancy: A randomized trial. American Journal of Public Health, 89(12), 1847–1851.
doi:10.2105/ajph.89.12.1847
Kramer, L., & Hamilton, T. (2019). Sibling caregiving. In M. H. Bornstein Ed., Being and
becoming a parent. Handbook of parenting (Vol. 3, pp. 372–408). Routledge. doi:10.4324/
9780429433214-11
Lachman, J., Wamoyi, J., Spreckelsen, T., Wight, D., Maganga, J., & Gardner, F. (2020).
Combining parenting and economic strengthening programmes to reduce violence against
children: A cluster randomised controlled trial with predominantly male caregivers in rural
Tanzania. BMJ Global Health, 5(7) Article e002349, e002349. doi:10.1136/bmjgh-2020-
002349.
Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., & Swank, P. R. (2006). Responsive parenting: Establishing early
foundations for social, communication, and independent problem-solving skills.
Developmental Psychology, 42(4), 627–642. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.42.4.627
Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Swank, P. R., & Guttentag, C. (2008). A responsive parenting
intervention: The optimal timing across early childhood for impacting maternal beha
viors and child outcomes. Developmental Psychology, 44(5), 1335–1353. doi:10.1037/
a0013030
Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Swank, P. R., Zucker, T., Crawford, A. D., & Solari, E. F. (2012). The
effects of a responsive parenting intervention on parent–child interactions during shared
book reading. Developmental Psychology, 48(4), 969–986. doi:10.1037/a0026400
Laosa, L. M. (1983). Social policies toward children of diverse ethnic, racial, and language
groups in the United States. ETS Research Report Series, 1983(2), i–202. doi:10.1002/j.2330-
8516.1983.tb00029.x
Lerner, R. M. (2018). Concepts and theories of human development. (4th edn). Routledge.
doi:10.4324/9780203581629
Lessac, M. S., & Solomon, R. L. (1969). Effects of early isolation on the later adaptive behavior
of beagles: A methodological demonstration. Developmental Psychology, 1(1), 14–25.
doi:10.1037/h0026778
Lunkenheimer, E. S., Dishion, T. J., Shaw, D. S., Connell, A. M., Gardner, F., Wilson, M. N., &
Skuban, E. M. (2008). Collateral benefits of the family check-up on early childhood school
readiness: Indirect effects of parents’ positive behavior support. Developmental Psychology,
44(6), 1737–1752. doi:10.1037/a0013858
Luthar, S. S., & Eisenberg, N. (2017). Resilient adaptation among at-risk children: Harnessing
science toward maximizing salutary environments. Child Development, 88(2), 337–349.
doi:10.1111/cdev.12737
Magnuson, K. A., & Duncan, G. J. (2019). Parents in poverty. In M. H. Bornstein Ed., Special
conditions and applied parenting. Handbook of parenting (Vol. 4, pp. 301–328). Routledge.
doi:10.4324/9780429398995-9
Mauricio, A. M., Mazza, G. L., Berkel, C., Tein, J.-Y., Sandler, I. N., Wolchik, S. A., &
Winslow, E. (2018). Attendance trajectory classes among divorced and separated mothers
and fathers in the new beginnings program. Prevention Science, 19(5), 620–629. doi:10.1007/
s11121-017-0783-3
McCall, R. B., & Green, B. L. (2004). Beyond the methodological gold standards of behavioral
research: Considerations for practice and policy. SRCD: Social Policy Report, 18(2), 1–20.
doi:10.1002/J.2379-3988.2004.TB00025.X
McCurdy, K., & Daro, D. (2001). Parent involvement in family support programs: An inte
grated theory. Family Relations: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Applied Family Studies, 50
(2), 113–121. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3729.2001.00113.x
PARENTING 231
Parra-Cardona, J. R., Bybee, D., Sullivan, C. M., Rodríguez, M. M. D., Dates, B., Tams, L., &
Bernal, G. (2017). Examining the impact of differential cultural adaptation with Latina/o
immigrants exposed to adapted parent training interventions. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 85(1), 58–71. doi:10.1037/ccp0000160
Pashler, H., & Harris, C. R. (2012). Is the replicability crisis overblown? Three arguments
examined. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(6), 531–536. doi:10.1177/
1745691612463401
Patterson, G. R., Chamberlain, P., & Reid, J. B. (1982). A comparative evaluation of a
parent-training program. Behavior Therapy, 13(5), 638–650. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(82)
80021-X
Paulsell, D., Del Grosso, P., & Supplee, L. (2014). Supporting replication and scale-up of
evidence-based home visiting programs: Assessing the implementation knowledge base.
American Journal of Public Health, 104(9), 1624–1632. doi:10.2105/AJPH.2014.301962
Peña, E. D. (2007). Lost in translation: Methodological considerations in cross-cultural
research. Child Development, 78(4), 1255–1264. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01064.x
Petrosino, A., & Soydan, H. (2005). The impact of program developers as evaluators on criminal
recidivism: Results from meta-analyses of experimental and quasi-experimental research.
Journal of Experimental Criminology, 1(4), 435–450. doi:10.1007/s11292-005-3540-8
Pinquart, M., & Teubert, D. (2010). Effects of parenting education with expectant and new
parents: A meta-analysis. Journal of Family Psychology, 24(3), 316–327. doi:10.1037/a0019691
Powell, D. R. (2005). Searches for what works in parenting interventions. In T. Luster &
L. Okagaki Eds., Parenting: An ecological perspective (2nd edn, 343–373). Erlbaum.
doi:10.4324/9781410613394-21
Powell, D. R., & Carey, A. J. (2012). Approaches to program fidelity in family literacy research.
In B. H. Wasik Ed., Handbook of family literacy (2nd edn, 387–400). Routledge. doi:10.4324/
9780203841495.ch25
Putnick, D. L., & Bornstein, M. H. (2016). Measurement invariance conventions and reporting:
The state of the art and future directions for psychological research. Developmental Review,
41, 71–90. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2016.06.004
Raikes, H., Raikes, A., Esteraich, J., Encinger, A., Garcia, A. S., Ucus, S., & Escalante, E. (2019).
Nonparental caregiving. In M. H. Bornstein Ed., Being and becoming a parent. Handbook of
parenting (Vol. 3, pp. 409–440). Routledge. doi:10.4324/9780429433214-12
Raudenbush, S. W., Martinez, A., & Spybrook, J. (2007). Strategies for improving precision in
group-randomized experiments. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 29(1), 5–29.
doi:10.3102/0162373707299460
Rioux, C., & Little, T. D. (2020). Underused methods in developmental science to inform policy
and practice. Child Development Perspectives, 14(2), 97–103. doi:10.1111/cdep.12364
Rubio-Codina, M., Dormal, M., & Araujo, M. C. (2019). Observing home-visiting quality at
scale with the home visit rating scales and a supervisor checklist in peru. Infant Mental
Health Journal, 40(3), 343–362. doi:10.1002/imhj.21775
Salari, R., & Filus, A. (2017). Using the health belief model to explain mothers’ and fathers’
intention to participate in universal parenting programs. Prevention Science, 18(1), 83–94.
doi:10.1007/s11121-016-0696-6
Sanders, M. R., Markie-Dadds, C., Tully, L. A., & Bor, W. (2000). The triple P-positive
parenting program: A comparison of enhanced, standard, and self-directed behavioral
family intervention for parents of children with early onset conduct problems. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68(4), 624–640. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.68.4.624
Sanders, M. R., Bor, W., & Morawska, A. (2007). Maintenance of treatment gains: A comparison
of enhanced, standard, and self-directed triple P-positive parenting program. Journal of
Abnormal Child Psychology, 35(6), 983–998. doi:10.1007/s10802-007-9148-x
PARENTING 233
Wagner, M. M., & Clayton, S. L. (1999). The parents as teachers program: Results from two
demonstrations. The Future of Children, 9(1), 91–115. doi:10.2307/1602723
Webster-Stratton, C., Reid, M. J., & Hammond, M. (2001). Preventing conduct problems,
promoting social competence: A parent and teacher training partnership in Head Start.
Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 30(3), 283–302. doi:10.1207/S15374424JCCP3003_2
Westfall, J. M., VanVorst, R. F., Main, D. S., & Herbert, C. (2006). Community-based
participatory research in practice-based research networks. Annals of Family Medicine, 4
(1), 8–14. doi:10.1370/afm.511
Yaremych, H. E, & Persky, S. (2022). Recruiting fathers for parenting research: An evaluation
of eight recruitment methods and an exploration of fathers’ motivations for participation.
Parenting, 22, 1–32. Published online: 25 Feb 2022. doi:10.1080/15295192.2022.2036940
Yousafzai, A. K., & Aboud, F. (2014). Review of implementation processes for integrated
nutrition and psychosocial stimulation interventions. Annals of the New York Academy of
Sciences, 1308(1), 33–45. doi:10.1111/nyas.12313
Zhang, L., Ssewanyana, D., Martin, M. C., Lye, S., Moran, G., Abubakar, A., . . . Malti, T. (2021).
Supporting child development through parenting interventions in low-to middle-income
countries: An updated systematic review. Frontiers in Public Health, 9. Article 671988
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2021.671988
Zhou, J., Connell, L. E., & Graham, J. W. (2014). Handling missing data in cluster randomized
trials: A demonstration of multiple imputation with PAN through SAS. The Quantitative
Methods for Psychology, 10(2), 153–166. doi:10.20982/tqmp.10.2.p153