Autistic Spectrum Disorders in Children, Pediatric Habilitation Series Volume 12 (Marcel Dekker, 2004)
Autistic Spectrum Disorders in Children, Pediatric Habilitation Series Volume 12 (Marcel Dekker, 2004)
Autistic Spectrum Disorders in Children, Pediatric Habilitation Series Volume 12 (Marcel Dekker, 2004)
Disorders in Children
edited by
Vidya Bhushan Gupta
New York Medical College
and Columbia University
New York, New York, U.S.A.
MARCEL
MARCEL INC.
DEKKER, N E WYORK * BASEL
ISBN: 0-8247-5061-6
Headquarters
Marcel Dekker, Inc., 270 Madison Avenue, New York, NY 10016, U.S.A.
tel: 212-696-9000; fax: 212-685-4540
The publisher offers discounts on this book when ordered in bulk quantities. For more
information, write to Special Sales=Professional Marketing at the headquarters address
above.
Neither this book nor any part may be reproduced or transmitted in any form or by any
means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, microfilming, and recording, or
by any information storage and retrieval system, without permission in writing from the
publisher.
10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
Series Editor
ALFRED L. SCHEWER
Joan and Sanford 1. Weill Medical College
Cornell Universdy
New Yo&, New York
For over 25 years, the Pediatric Habilitation series has brought to readers the
latest information and significant emerging clinical approaches in the field of
developmental disabilities in children. Focus has always been on those specific
conditions demanding the greatest concern at the time. And developments have
been regularly updated with new editions when there has been rapid change, as in
the areas of cerebral palsy and attention deficit disorders.
Over this period of time there has been considerable shift in the disabilities
encountered by health professionals, from the high-morbidity, low-frequency
conditions, such as cerebral palsy and neural tube defects, to the low-morbidity,
high-frequency attention deficit disorders, learning disabilities, and, more re-
cently, the autistic spectrum disorders. These changes reflect advances in obstetric
and neonatal care and consequences of genetic, environmental, and social factors.
In addition, developmental disabilities in children are now seen as spectrum
disorders that usually occur with closely related comorbid conditions, rather than
as single and uniquely diagnosed entities that stand alone.
Since the inception of this series, nowhere has change been more striking
and, indeed, more stressful than in the area of autistic spectrum disorders.
Emerging data suggest a marked increase in incidence and prevalence, with a
corresponding demand for accurate diagnosis and more effective long-term
management. This comes at a time when accepted etiology of the autistic spec-
trum disorders has shifted dramatically from a psychiatric to a neurodevelop-
mental focus. Moreover, this field has been virtually flooded with literature
Alfred L. Scherzer
Only a few decades ago, autism was a medical curiosity, conjuring up visions of a
child who walks on his toes like a ballerina, staring vacantly into space but is
capable of performing complex mathematical operations at the speed of light. Its
prevalence was reported to range from 0.7 to 4.5=10,000 children. Today, autism
is being diagnosed in 5–6 of 1000 children and an epidemic is suspected. A
debate is raging as to whether the prevalence of autism has truly increased or data
merely reflect heightened awareness of the condition. It is now agreed that autism
occurs along a spectrum, with only a few children manifesting the full range of
symptoms. Inclusion of milder and atypical variants of autism, such as Asperger’s
and Rett’s syndromes, under the rubric of pervasive developmental disorders, has
also contributed to the increasing prevalence of autism. Nosology of the syn-
drome is still in flux, with terms such as autism, autistic spectrum disorder,
pervasive developmental disorder, and mutlisystem disorder in vogue.
Our thinking about the origins of autism has undergone radical change.
Long gone are Kannerian ideas of refrigerated mothers. Instead, autism is now
regarded as a neurobiological disorder. However, as with any common disorder
that does not have an incontrovertible candidate as its cause, autism has become a
fertile ground for theories. There are theories about what part of an autistic brain
is abnormal, the nature of the abnormality, and how the abnormality translates
into abnormal behavior. No single area of the brain has been found to be
consistently abnormal in children with autism, but abnormalities have been
noticed in the cerebellum and the limbic system. The nature of the defect is
3. Etiology of Autism
Vidya Bhushan Gupta
Laura Kresch Curran, B.A. Center for Autism and Developmental Disabil-
ities Epidemiology, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Balti-
more, Maryland, U.S.A.
Nora L. Lee, B.S. Research Program Coordinator, Center for Autism and
Developmental Disabilities Epidemiology, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of
Public Health, Baltimore, Maryland, U.S.A.
I. ORIGINS
The term autism, meaning “living in self” (in Greek aut means self, and ism refers to
a state), was coined by a Swiss psychiatrist, Eugen Bleuler, in 1911, to describe self-
absorption due to poor social relatedness in schizophrenia (1). Leo Kanner (Fig. 1), in
1943, borrowed this term to describe 11 children who “were oblivious to other people,
did not talk or who parroted speech, used idiosyncratic phrases, who lined up toys in long
rows, and who remembered meaningless facts.” In his classic paper, “Autistic
Disturbances of Affective Contact,” Kanner described the features of classic autism with
uncanny detail (2). Describing the social isolation of his first case, Kanner said, “Donald
got happiest when left alone, almost never cried to go with his mother, did not seem to
notice his father’s homecomings, and was indifferent to visiting relatives. The father
made a special point of mentioning that Donald even failed to pay the slightest attention
to Santa Claus in full regalia.” His second child played abnormally, “He never was very
good with cooperative play. He doesn’t care to play with the ordinary things that other
children play with, anything with wheels on.” The third child said “no recognizable
words, although he did make noises (3).” In 1956, Eisenberg and Kanner suggested two
essential criteria for the disorder: inability to relate in the ordinary way to people and
situations and failure to learn to speak or inability to convey meaning to others through
language, both occurring from the beginning of life (4).
In 1944, Hans Asperger, a Viennese pediatrician, independently described a
condition similar to that described by Leo Kanner and called it autistic psychopathy
Figure 1 Leo Kanner, M.D., is credited with the first description of autism.
Use of the term “autism” by both Kanner and Asperger led to confusion about
the relation of the newly described condition to schizophrenia. Although Kanner
commented on the similarity of the clinical picture of autism with “some of
the basic schizophrenic phenomena,” he underscored the difference that autism
was present since birth while schizophrenia occurred after years of normal
development. Asperger, too, emphasized that while both autistic children
and schizophrenics have “complete shutting off [of] relations between self
and the outside world,” the latter have a “gradual disintegration of personality”
while the former have social withdrawal “from the start.” To underscore this
point, Asperger called the condition he described a psychopathy rather than
psychosis.
Bender described a group of children similar to those described by Kanner
in 1942, labeling them “childhood schizophrenics of the pseudodefective type
(7).” Anthony (1958) described two types of autism, primary and secondary, both
as subtypes of childhood psychosis (8). Primary autism described by him was
similar to autism described by Kanner. In 1961, a group of British clinicians
(the British Working Party, or BWP) gave nine points—the Nine Points—for
the diagnosis of “schizophrenic children.” The criteria included symptoms of
autism described by Eisenberg and Kanner, but promoted the notion that autism
was a type of childhood psychosis (9). These nine points were incorporated in the
classification of childhood psychiatric disorders by the Group for the
Advancement of Psychiatry (GAP) in 1966. Autism as described by Kanner
(Kanner’s syndrome) was included in the category of psychosis of infancy and
early childhood (10).
Although clearly differentiated from childhood psychoses of late onset,
autism continued to be labeled a childhood psychosis, albeit of early onset, until
the early 1970s (11,12). Keeping in line with the then-prevalent views,
DSM (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of the American Psychiatric
Association) I and II did not have a separate category of autism (13,14). In
1956, Eisenberg and Kanner challenged the view that childhood schizophrenia
and autism were the same condition (4). In 1968, Ornitz and Ritvo, described
autism as a specific syndrome with a cluster of symptoms, including
abnormalities of perceptual integration (15). They clearly distinguished between
the two conditions by their age of onset, autism occurring before the age of 30
months and schizophrenia later. Makita (16) and Rutter (17) made a case for
separating autism from childhood schizophrenia. In his theoretical paper
“Childhood Schizophrenia Reconsidered,” Michael Rutter discussed the
confusion between the terms “childhood schizophrenia” and “autism.” He
laid to rest the notion that autism was a type of childhood psychosis and
argued that the term “childhood schizophrenia” be dropped forever, and that
children with schizophrenia be simply called schizophrenic (17). Kolvin et al.
(18) in England and Green et al. (19) in the United States restated the
differences between autism and schizophrenia: early onset of autism, presence
of positive symptoms such as hallucinations in schizophrenia, waxing and
waning of symptoms in schizophrenia, and generally higher intelligence of
children with schizophrenia. In 1978, Rutter gave criteria for autism that were
PDD (NOS), pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified; MR, mental retardation.
deprivation on the infant also suggested that mental disorders could be caused by
lack of maternal availability and responsivity (34 –36). For the next three decades
autism was attributed to “refrigerator mothers” who did not show affection to
their children. Widely publicized stories of feral children reinforced the view that
autism was due to faulty nurture and not faulty nature (37). But the social skills of
abandoned and neglected children often improve with nurturing care contrary to a
little improvement in social relatedness of most autistic children despite
intervention (38). Kasper Hauser, who was neglected and maltreated for many
years, recovered and acquired some communication skills even after the age of 17
years (39). A follow-up of children who suffered severe psychosocial deprivation
during the Ceausescu regime in Romania showed that with intervention many
such children learned to communicate and make social approach, although
deviant in quality (40). Moreover, abused or neglected children show unhealthy
attachment to their caregivers, while autistic children are attached to primary
caregivers (41).
The psychogenic theory of autism held sway until Bernard Rimland in his
book Infantile Autism proposed that autism was neurogenic and not psychogenic
(42). He argued that many autistic children were born to loving and caring parents
and many perfectionist professional parents had normal offspring. Moreover,
most autistic children did not acquire the disorder but were born with it. Kanner
himself changed his views about his theory of “refrigerator mothers” when in
1969, at the National Association of Autistic Children, he declared, “Herewith I
officially acquit of you people as parents” (43).
V. NOSOLOGY
Originally, Eugen Bleuler used the word autism to describe a state of extreme
social withdrawal in schizophrenia (1). Kanner and Asperger borrowed the term
as an adjective to describe a constellation of symptoms that included not only
social withdrawal but communication difficulties and rigid and stereotypic
behavior (2). The former called this syndrome “autistic disturbance of affective
contact” and the latter, “autistic psychopathy.” Although both Kanner and
Asperger had described high-functioning children, the syndrome described by
Kanner, consisting of severe impairments of social and communicative behavior,
became known as autism and the syndrome described by Asperger, with a wider
therefore, can improve with intervention. This term has not gained popularity,
because the DSM-IV category of PDD-NOS includes atypical and subthreshold
cases of autism (68).
Another term, multiple complex developmental disorder, has been
proposed both as a separate category or as a subtype of pervasive developmental
disorders. The defining characteristic of this disorder is deficit in affect regulation
with secondary deficits in relatedness and thought. The symptoms of this
condition include disturbed attachments, idiosyncratic anxiety reactions,
episodes of behavioral disorganization, and wide emotional variability (69 –
71). Although both multiplex complex disorder and multisystem disorder have
not become popular, they highlight abnormalities of affect regulation and sensory
processing as part of the litany of problems in pervasive developmental disorder.
The differential connotations of the terms used to describe pervasive
developmental disorders are given in Table 2.
VI. TAXONOMY
Communication Low
Social Sensory Affective nonverbal
deficit Qualitative Quantitative Stereotypies disintegration dysregulation IQ
Autism
High functioning þþ þ + þ + + 2
Low functioning þþ þ þ þ + + þ
Asperger’s syndrome þþ þ 2 þ + 2 2
Multisystem disorder + + + + þþ + +
Multiplex complex + + + + + þþ +
disorder
PDD-NOS þ þ + þ + + +
Language disorder 2 þ + 2 2 2 2
Affective dysregulation, lack of or inappropriate facial expression; þ, mild; þþ, severe; 2, absent.
High-functioning Low-functioning
REFERENCES
26. American Psychiatric Association. The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders. 4th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc., 1994.
27. International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems. 10th
ed. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization, 1994.
28. Volkmar FR, Klin A, Siegel B, et al. Field trial for autistic disorders in DSM-IV. Am
J Psychriatry 1994; 151:1361.
29. Little WJ. On the influence of abnormal parturition, difficult labours, premature birth,
and asphyxia neonatorum on the mental and physical condition of the child,
especially in relation to deformities. Trans Obstet Soc Lond 1861; 3:293.
30. Kanner L. Problems of nosology and psychodynamics of early infantile autism. Am J
Orthopsychiatry. 1949; 19:416– 426.
31. Kanner L, Eisenberg L. Early infantile autism: 1943– 1955. Am J Orthopsychiat
1956; 26:55 – 65.
32. Chess S. A remembrance. J Autism Dev Disord 1981; 11:259 – 263.
33. Bettelheim B. The Empty Fortress: Infantile Autism and the Birth of the Self.
New York: Free Press, 1967.
34. Bakwin H. Loneliness in infants. Am J Dis Child 1942; 63:30 – 40.
35. Spitz RA. Hospitalism. In: Fenichel O, ed. The Psychoanalytic Study of the Child.
New York: International Universities Press, 1945:53– 74.
36. Bowlby J. Maternal Care and Mental Health. Geneva: World Health Organization, 1951.
37. Schopler E. New developments in the definition and diagnosis of autism. In: Lahey
BB, Kazdin AE, eds. Advances in Clinical Child Psychology 1983; 6:93– 127.
38. Favazza AR. Feral and isolated children. Br J Med Psychol 1977; 50:105– 111.
39. Simon N. Kasper Hauser’s recovery and autopsy: a perspective on neurological and
sociological requirements for language development. J Autism Child Schizo 1978;
8:209 – 217.
40. Rutter M, Andersen-Wood L, Beckett C, Bredenkamp D, Castle J, Groothues C,
Kreppner J, Keaveney L, Lord C, O’Connor TG. Quasi-autistic patterns following
severe early global privation. English and Romanian Adoptees (ERA) Study Team.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1999; 40:537 –49.
41. Rogers SJ, Pennington BF. A theoretical approach to the deficits in infantile autism.
Dev Psychopathol 1991; 3:137– 162.
42. Rimland B. Infantile Autism: The Syndrome and Its Implications for a Neural
Theory of Behavior. New York: Appleton-Century Crofts, 1964.
43. Crawley CA. Infantile autism—an hypothesis. J Ir Med Assoc 1971; 64:335 –45.
44. Maudsley H. The Physiology and Pathology of Mind. London: Macmillan, 1867.
45. De Sanctis S. On some varities of dementia praecox. Riv Sper Freniatria 1906;
32:141 – 165. Translated and reprinted in Howells JG, ed. Modern Perspectives in
International Child Psychiatry. Edinburgh: Oliver & Boyd, 1969:509 – 609.
46. Mouridsen SE, Rich B, Isager T. A comparative study of genetic and neurobiological
findings in disintegrative psychosis and infantile autism. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci
2000; 54:441 – 446.
47. Witmer L. Orthogenic cases. XIV. Don: a curable case of arrested development due
to a fear psychosis the result of shock in a three-year-old infant. Am Assoc Adv Sci
1920; 401:97 – 111.
48. Ssucharewa GE, Wolff S. The first account of the syndrome Asperger described?
(Die shizolden Psychopathien lm Kindersalter). Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1996;
5 –119– 32.
49. Demorsier G. Wild children and wolf children. Psychol Ekon Praxi (Charles
University, Czechoslovakia) 1965; 24:148– 155.
50. Lane Harlan. Wild Boy of Averon. Boston: Harvard University Press, 1976.
51. “Autistic psychopathy” in childhood. Translated by Uta Frith. In: Frith U,
ed. Autism and Asperger Syndrome. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press,
1991.
52. Gayral L, Chabbert P, Baillaud-Citeau H. The first observations on the wild-boy of
Lacaune (called “Victor” or “the wild-boy of Aveyron”): new documents. Ann Med-
Psychol (Editions Elsevier, France) 1972; 2:465 – 490.
53. Stumpfe KD. The case of Kasper Hauser. Prax Kinderpsychol Kinderpsychiatr 1969;
18:292– 299.
54. Oliver Sacks. Henry Cavendish: an early case of Asperger’s syndrome? Neurology
2001; 57:1347.
55. Ledgin N, Temple Gardin. Diagnosing Jefferson. Arlington, TX: Future Horizons,
Inc., 2002.
56. Houston Rab A, Houston RA, Frith U. Autism in History: The Case of Hugh Blair of
Borgue. London: Blackwell Publishers, 2000.
57. Diagnosis and Detection: The Medical Iconography of Sherlock Holmes.
Rutherford, NJ: Farleigh Dickinson University Press, 1987.
58. Eisenmajer R, Prior M, Leekam M, Wing L, Gould J, Welham M, et al. Comparison
of clinical symptoms in autism and Asperger syndrome. J Am Acad Child Adolesc
Psychiatry 1996; 35:1523 – 1531.
59. Volkmar F. Can you explain the difference between autism and Asperger syndrome?
J Autism Dev Disord 1999; 29:185 – 186.
60. Mayes SD, Calhoun SL, Crites DL. Does DSM-IV Asperger’s disorder exist?
J Abnorm Child Psychol 2001; 29:263 –271.
61. Ghaziuddin M, Gerstein L. Pedantic speaking style differentiates Asperger syndrome
from high-functioning autism. J Autism Dev Disord 1996; 26:585 – 595.
62. Byrna Siegel. Toward DSM-IV: a developmental approach to autistic disorder.
Psychiatry Clin North Am 1991; 14:53 –68.
63. Wing L. The autistic spectrum. Lancet 1997; 350:1761– 1766.
64. Filipek PA, Accardo PJ, Baranek GT. The screening and diagnosis of autistic
spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 1999; 29:439 –484.
65. Rapin I. The autistic-spectrum disorders. N Engl J Med 2002; 347:302 – 303.
66. Piven J, Palmer P, Jacobi D, Childress D, Arndt S. Broader autism phenotype:
evidence from a family history study of multiple-incidence autism families. Am J
Psychiatry 1997; 154:185 – 190.
67. Bolton P, MacDonald H, Pickles A, et al. A case-control family history study of
autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1994; 35:877 –900.
68. Diagnostic Classification: 0 – 3. Diagnostic Classification of Mental Health and
Developmental Disorders of Infancy and Childhood. Arlington, VA: ZERO TO
THREE/National Center for Clinical Infant Programs, 1994.
69. Demb H B, Noskin O. The use of the term multiple complex developmental disorder in
a diagnostic clinic serving young children with developmental disabilities: a report of
15 cases. Mental Health Aspects Dev Disabil 2001; 4:49–60.
70. Zalsman G, Cohen DJ. Multiplex developmental disorder. Isr J Psychiatry Relat Sci
1998; 35:300 – 306.
71. Van der Gaag RJ, Buitelaar J, Van den Ban E, Bezemer M, Njio L, Van Engeland
H. A controlled multivariate chart review of multiple complex developmental
disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1995; 34:1096 – 1106.
72. Wing L, Gould J. Severe impairments of social interaction and associated
abnormalities in children: epidemiology and classification. J Autism Dev Disord
1979; 9:11 – 29.
73. Wing L. The autistic spectrum. Lancet 1997; 350:1761 – 1766.
74. Castelloe P, Dawson G. Subclassification of children with autism and pervasive
developmental disorder: a questionnaire based on Wing’s subgrouping scheme.
J Autism Dev Disord 1993; 23:229– 241.
75. Waterhouse L, Morris R, Allen D, Dunn M, Fein D, Feinstein C, Rapin I, Wing L.
Diagnosis and classification in autism. J Autism Dev Disord 1996; 26:59 – 86.
76. Borden MC, Ollendick TH. An examination of the validity of social subtypes in
autism. J Autism Dev Disord 1994; 24:23– 37.
77. Volkmar FR, Cohen DJ, Bregman JD, Hooks MY, Stevenson JM. An examination of
social typologies in autism. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1989; 28:82– 86.
78. Fein D, Stevens M, Dunn M, Waterhouse L, Allen D, Rapin I, Feinstein C. Subtypes
of pervasive developmental disorders: clinical characteristics. Child Neuropsychol
1999; 5:1 – 23.
79. Stevens MC, Fein DA, Dunn M, Allen D, Waterhouse LH, Feinstein C, Rapin I.
Subgroups of children with autism by cluster analysis: a longitudinal examination.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2000; 39:346 – 352.
80. Prior M, Eisenmajer R, Leekam S, Wing L, Gould J, Ong B, Dowe D. Are there
subgroups within the autistic spectrum? A cluster analysis of a group of children
with autistic spectrum disorders. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Disc 1998;
39:893 – 902.
81. Eaves LC, Ho HH, Eaves DM. Subtypes of autism by cluster analysis. J Autism Dev
Disord 1994; 24:3– 22.
82. Siegel B, Anders TF, Ciaranello RD, Bienenstock B, Kraemer HC. Empirically
derived subclassification of the autistic syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord 1986;
16:275 – 293.
83. Stevens MC, Fein DA, Dunn M, Allen D, Waterhouse LH, Feinstein C, Rapin I.
Subgroups of children with autism by cluster analysis: a longitudinal examination.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2000; 39:346– 352.
84. Waterhouse L, Morris R, Allen D, Dunn M, Fein D, Feinstein C, Rapin I, Wing
L. Diagnosis and classification in autism. J Autism Dev Disord 1996; 26:59 – 86.
85. DeLong GR. Autism: new data suggest a new hypothesis. Neurology 1999;
52(23):911– 916.
86. Rutter M, Schopler E. Classification of pervasive developmental disorders: some
concepts and practical considerations. J Autism Dev Disord 1992; 22:459 – 482.
Tanya Karapurkar
Battelle Memorial Institute and Centers for Disease Control and
Prevention, Atlanta, Georgia, U.S.A.
Nora L. Lee, Laura Kresch Curran, and Craig J. Newschaffer
Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, Maryland,
U.S.A.
Marshalyn Yeargin-Allsopp
National Center on Birth Defects and Developmental Disabilities, Centers
for Disease Control and Prevention, Atlanta, Georgia, U.S.A.
I. INTRODUCTION
In the 1940s, Dr. Leo Kanner described 11 children with “autistic disturbances of
affective contact,” and even today, that characterization continues to form the
basis of the diagnostic criteria for autistic disorder (1). Epidemiological studies
describing the prevalence and risk factors for autistic disorder and autism
spectrum disorders (ASDs) did not appear in the scientific literature until the late
1960s and early 1970s (2 –4). Since these earlier investigations, several
population-based prevalence studies have been conducted. However, these vary
in terms of their methods, case definitions, and population size; hence,
comparisons of estimates from these studies are difficult. Other factors
contributing to the lack of epidemiological understanding of autism stem from
the paucity of well-designed studies that are able to examine trends over time, as
well as a range of risk factors, both biological and sociodemographic, for autism.
For instance, there have been few studies that reflect racially heterogeneous
populations, such as from the United States.
To examine the current prevalence of autism and trends in prevalence over
time, we conducted a MEDLINE literature search for prevalence studies that
have been published between 1966 and the present (January 2003). The criteria
used for inclusion in the review included: peer-reviewed articles in the English
language, book chapters, and summaries of articles published in other languages
(1) that describe studies with temporally and geographically defined population-
based cohorts; inclusion of case definitions that used clinical examinations or
review by expert clinicians’ assessment and diagnosis; and (2) that used the
accepted clinical diagnostic criteria for autism/ASDs as a basis for case
definition. This chapter will describe these studies according to (1) diagnostic
criteria; (2) time period; (3) size of population; and (4) methods of ascertainment.
We will also present what is currently known about autism risk factors and
discuss challenges to conducting epidemiological studies of autism. In this
chapter, we will use the term ASD to refer to the Diagnostic and Statistical
Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edition (5) diagnosis of autistic disorder,
Asperger disorder, and pervasive developmental disorder –not otherwise
specified (PDD-NOS). Where possible, we will indicate those studies that
attempted to differentiate between autistic disorder and the broader spectrum
of ASDs.
change in the real risk of disease occurrence. Considering this, the evolution of
autism diagnostic criteria and the impact it has had on the prevalence of autism
and ASDs will be described in this chapter.
For the reasons described above, only four studies have attempted to report the
incidence of autism (8 – 11). The investigators in all of these studies used the year
of diagnosis as the year of onset of autism. However, the age of diagnosis in most
studies is much later than the age of recognition of the behaviors that are
characteristic of autism. In fact, although the current diagnostic criteria for autism
require that the onset of behaviors that define the condition occur prior to 3 years
of age, the mean age of recognition of the condition was closer to 4 years of age
(12) in one study and was as late as 6 years of age in an earlier study (13). In
addition to not having a precise time of onset, the diagnosis was not confirmed in
any of these studies and the investigators did not take into account that the
diagnostic criteria for autism that were used changed during the periods of study.
Nevertheless, all of these studies, covering birth cohorts from the 1980s and
1990s, report increases over time. However, only carefully designed prospective
studies after there was more certainty over how to detect the true biological onset
of ASDs would allow us to determine precisely the onset of autism in the
population, and thus, the true incidence of the disorder in a given population.
From our literature review, we were able to identify only three studies that
described trends in autism prevalence or incidence within a defined population.
In one study, the prevalence was estimated for two French birth cohorts (1972
and 1976) from the same region; no change in prevalence (5.1 and 4.9/10,000
children) occurred over that short period of time (14). In Göteborg, Sweden, the
prevalence of autism was estimated for two time periods, 1962 – 1976 and 1975–
1984 (15). Over the 1975 –1984 time period, the prevalence increased from 4.0 to
11.6/10,000 children. The Swedish investigators offered a possible explanation
for the increase. Rates of autism in children with mild mental retardation
remained relatively stable, while the rates increased in children with severe
mental retardation (IQ , 50) and in children with normal intelligence (IQ . 70).
The investigators postulated that changes in the overall prevalence were
influenced by the ability to better identify children with autism with very low as
well as normal to high functioning. A third study that examined trends in autism
occurrence measured incidence between 1991 and 1996 in preschool children in
two areas on the west Midlands, UK, and found that although the rates for
classical autism increased by 18% per year, there was a much larger increase for
the other ASDs, i.e., 55% per year (8). The investigators attributed this increase in
incidence to increased awareness among clinicians rather than to true changes in
disease occurrence.
V. PREVALENCE STUDIES
Lotter 1966 England 1964 8–10 78,000 Kanner Case enumeration and 4.5/— 84
direct exam
Brask 1970 Denmark 1962 2–14 46,500 Kanner Case enumeration 4.3/— NR
Treffert 1970 U.S.A. 1962– 1967 3–12 899,750 Kanner Case enumeration 0.7/2.4
Wing and 1979 England 1970 0–14 35,000 Kanner Case enumeration and 4.6/15.7 70
Gould direct exam
Hoshino et al.a 1982 Japan 1977 0–17 234,039 Kanner Case enumeration and 5.0/— NR
direct exam
Ishii and 1983 Japan 1981 6–12 35,000 Rutter Case enumeration and 16.0/— NR
Takahashi direct exam
Bohman et al. 1983 Sweden 1979 0–20 69,000 Rutter Case enumeration and 3.0/2.6 NR
direct exam
McCarthy et al. 1984 Ireland 1978 8–10 65,000 Kanner Case enumeration and 4.3/— NR
direct exam
Gillberg 1984 Sweden 1980 4–18 128,584 DSM-III Case enumeration and 2.0/1.9 80,77
direct exam
Steinhausen 1986 Germany 1982 0–14 279,616 Rutter Case enumeration and 1.9/— 44
et al. direct exam
Steffenberg and 1986 Sweden 1984 ,10 78,413 DSM-III Case enumeration and 4.5/2.2 88
Gillberg direct exam
Matsuishi et al. 1987 Japan 1983 4–12 32,834 DSM-III Case enumeration and 15.5/— NR
direct exam
Burd et al. 1987 U.S.A. 1985 2–18 180,986 DSM-III Case enumeration and 1.2/2.1 NR
direct exam
Prevalence
rate (PR) for
Time Age Number of autism/other
Year period range children in Criteria ASD per IQ , 70
Author published Country studied studied population used Methodology used 10,000 (%)
Magnusson and 2000 Iceland 1997 5–14 43,153 ICD-10 Population screen and 8.6/4.6 95/65
Saemundsen direct exam
Chakrabarti 2001 England 1998 2.5–6.5 15,500 DSM-IV Population screen and 16.8/45.8 24,70
and direct exam
Fombonne
Fombonne 2001 UK 1999 5–15 12,529 DSM-IV Population screen and 26.1 44
et al.b direct exam
Bertrand et al. 2001 USA 1998 3–10 8,996 DSM-IV Case enumeration and 40.0/67.0 49,58
direct exam
Croen et al. 2001 USA 1987–1999 0–21 4.6 million DSM-III-R Case enumeration 11.0/— NR
or DSM-IV
Yeargin- 2003 USA 1996 3–10 290,000 DSM-IV Case enumeration 34 64,68c
Allsopp
et al.b
mean prevalence rates of studies that used DSM-IV or ICD-10 diagnostic criteria
as compared with Kanner, Rutter, DSM-III, or DSM-III-R diagnostic criteria, the
weighted prevalence shows a less exaggerated difference in the prevalence by
diagnostic criteria, but more accurately shows the rise in prevalence based on the
criteria that have been used over time. Further, these data support the notion that
the broadening of the case definition at the time that the DSM-III or DSM-III-R
was introduced may have contributed to the rise in prevalence seen in more recent
investigations.
We examined trends in the weighted and mean prevalence rates of the 31 studies
by time period studied. For the studies that reported period or birth prevalence
rates over a specific range of time, we took the midpoint of that range of time to
represent the year studied. For the studies that did not provide such information,
we identified the study year as 2 years prior to the year of publication (31,44).
If we examine the weighted and mean prevalence rates of the 31 studies by
year studied, there is an increasing trend over the four decades of published
reports, with an exception in the weighted prevalence rate for those investigations
that took place from 1980 to 1989 (Fig. 2). The weighted prevalence was 0.9/
10,000 for the three studies published between 1960 and 1969 (2 –4); 3.8/10,000
for the three investigations that were studied between 1970 and 1979 (19,24,26);
2.6/10,000 for the 12 studies that were conducted between 1980 and 1989
(25,27 –37); and 10.1/10,000 for the 13 studies that were conducted between
1990 and 1999 (8,38 – 49). The mean rates that were established for each decade
that the studies were conducted show a similar pattern as the trend seen with the
weighted prevalence estimates; however, larger differences are seen in the mean
rates from one decade to the next. The mean prevalence rate ranges from 3.1 in
the 1960s to 19.8 in the 1990s (Fig. 2). These data, showing the rise in prevalence
by time period of study, may be reflective of the changing diagnostic criteria used
over these four decades, but may also be reflective of other factors such as
increased awareness and diagnosis of the disorder.
As has been noted by Fombonne (50), when we examine the mean prevalence
rates of the 31 studies by size of population (denominator), there is a trend toward
higher prevalence rates in smaller populations. The weighted prevalence for
studies with a population size of 100,000 or more children was 3.3/10,000, while
the mean rate was 5.5/10,000 (4,27,28,31,34,36,40,45,49); for the one study with
a population in the range of 80,000 – 99,999 (33), both the weighted and mean
prevalence rate was 13.8/10,000. The lowest weighted and mean prevalence rates
are seen in populations of 40,000 – 59,999 (weighted prevalence, 5.7/10,000 and
mean prevalence, 6.4/10,000) (3,46) and 60,000– 79,999 (weighted prevalence,
4.6/10,000 and mean prevalence, 4.9/10,000) (2,24,26,29,37,38,42). The
weighted and mean prevalence starts to increase as the population becomes
smaller, with a weighted prevalence of 9.2/10,000 and a mean prevalence of
11.2/10,000 for studies with a population of 20,000 –39,999 (8,19,25,30,32).
The prevalence is highest in the seven studies that had a population size of
0 –19,999 children, with a weighted prevalence of 20.8 and a mean prevalence
Of the 31 studies that were included in the review, over half (58%) of the
investigators identified the cases using case enumeration, based on record review
or surveys of a given population, followed by clinical examination of the
children. For studies that used this methodology, a weighted prevalence of 2.9/
10,000 and a mean prevalence of 10.9/10,000 was found (2,19,24 – 32,36–
38,40,42,43,48). However, although the number of studies using case
enumeration only (N ¼ 7) (3,4,8,33,34,45,49) was almost the same as the
number of studies using population screening followed by clinical examination
(N ¼ 6) (35,39,41,44,46,47), the weighted and mean prevalence rate of
the studies using population screening and direct examination was higher than
those using case enumeration only (Fig. 4). The weighted and mean prevalence
estimates among the studies that used population screening and direct
The vast majority of autism cases are idiopathic, and no specific causal factor
has been identified for ASDs. It is clear from twin (51 – 54) and family studies
(55 – 58) that there is a substantive heritable component to ASDs. Although
there is some consistent indication that suceptability genes may reside on
chromosomes 7q (59 – 63) and 15q (64 –67), no putative autism gene has been
identified. At the same time, few nonheritable factors have been implicated in
autism etiology. However, as much of the existing research on nonheritable
factors are small studies with case groups defined under older, more restrictive
diagnostic criteria, the evidence base ruling out particular factors of theoretical
appeal is also quite weak. Nonheritable risk factors have increasingly become of
concern in recent years owing to the apparent rise in autism prevalence. Should
the prevalence increase prove to be attributable in part to a real increase in risk,
this short-term fluctuation suggests the influence of a nonheritable as opposed to a
heritable risk factor or factors. The remainder of this section discusses each class
of potential nonheritable risk factors.
Another cluster of ASD cases was investigated in the 1990s in the New Jersey
community of Brick Township, but the concern was prenatal and postnatal
exposure to environmental chemicals. Unlike Leominster, however, the
prevalence of autistic disorder in Brick Township was higher than expected,
4/1000. After examining potential exposures such as swimming in the river,
the local landfill, and drinking water, the Agency for Toxic Substances and
Disease Registry reported “no apparent public health hazard” from these
sources (72,73). Levels of chemicals in the river were not sufficient to cause
adverse health effects. Trihalomethanes, tetrachloroethylene, and trichloroethy-
lene were measured in the drinking water, but the sites and times of high
measurements did not correspond with the residences of the cases during the
study period.
XIII. XENOBIOTICS
A. Thalidomide
The discovery of a high prevalence of autism among a thalidomide-exposed
cohort and subsequent follow-up of the autism subjects has established that
prenatal xenobiotic exposure, defined as chemicals foreign to the body, can cause
autism (74). In the mid-1990s a small, retrospective study of thalidomide
embryopathy (75) first reported a higher-than-expected number of autism cases
among a cohort prenatally exposed to the drug. The autism cases were all exposed
between days 20 and 24 of gestation, the period when the neural tube is formed
(76). Although today thalidomide exposure is not an important autism etiological
factor since the drug is only approved for use in extremely limited circumstances,
other in utero exposures during this critical window of embryonic development
may play a role in autism etiology.
B. Other Pharmaceuticals
Other prenatal and intrapartum pharmaceutical agents have not shown such a
strong correlation with autism as thalidomide, but the epidemiological
evidence is limited. A few case reports point to prenatal use of valproic acid
and other anticonvulsants as a possible cause of autism (77 – 79), but published
epidemiological studies on this suggested link were not found. Interestingly,
up to 30% of ASDs cases have comorbid epilepsy (80 – 82), and anti-
convulsants appear to ameliorate ASD symptoms among nonepileptic children
(83 – 85).
Two relatively large case-control studies, each with at least 50 cases,
examined any prenatal medication use. One study, based on self-reporting,
showed a slight increase in use among the ASDs group (86), but the other
study, based on obstetrical records review, did not find any association (87).
Epidemiological evidence on use of labor-inducing drugs and risk for autism is
just as mixed. A 10-year birth cohort from four hospitals in Japan had twice
the prevalence of autistic disorder from one hospital that routinely used labor-
inducing drugs as well as more frequent use of general anesthesia, sedatives,
and analgesics compared to the three other hospitals. In contrast, a case-control
study of 180 children with ASDs that used two control groups—language
impaired and cognitively impaired—found similar prevalences of labor
induction (about 20%) among all groups. Two other relatively large case-
control studies had conflicting results—one finding a significant difference in
labor induction rates between the autism group and the general population
(29% vs. 16%, respectively) (88) and the other finding no association with
labor induction in 17% of both the autistic probands and their nonautistic
siblings (87).
C. Vaccines
Beginning in the late 1990s, exposures via childhood vaccination, particularly
the measles-mumps-rubella (MMR) vaccine and the ethylmercury-containing
vaccine preservative thimerosal, have received considerable attention in both
the scientific and lay media as potential autism risk factors. The
epidemiological evidence that has since accumulated with respect to MMR
has been consistent in showing no association with ASD risk (9–11,89– 92).
Fewer epidemiological studies have been able to assess thimerosal exposure.
One unpublished analysis, based on CDC’s Vaccine Safety Datalink, found no
significant association (93). Reports on mercury-poisoned populations and
populations with long-term low-dose methylmercury exposure have been
reviewed and no unusual observations were found with regard to more frequent
occurrence of autism (7). Further, the characteristic pathological features of
brains of individuals with methylmercury poisoning do not appear to match
those of the brains of persons with autism (7) and the toxicokinetics of ethyl-
and methylmercury may differ with indications being that ethylmercury could
be more efficiently eliminated (94).
XIV. INFECTIONS
A. Maternal
One of the earliest published reports of prenatal maternal infection possibly being
associated with ASDs was a 1971 study of 243 preschool children with congenital
rubella, 18 of whom had ASDs (95). In a follow-up report, however, six of the 18
ASD cases were reclassified so that only 12 of the 243 children had ASDs. A few
other case reports of ASDs following prenatal maternal infection with herpes
simplex, rubeola, syphilis, and varicella-zoster have been published (96).
Using a broader definition of infection exposure status, which includes
documented fever in medical records to self-report of an ill person in the home,
Deykin and MacMahon (97) analyzed maternal infection during pregnancy and
found significantly increased risk of having a child with ASD following exposure
to measles, mumps, rubella, and influenza. Other studies with broadly defined
infection measures have not reported statistically significant associations,
although risks were elevated in infection-exposed groups (86 – 88).
B. Child
Several published case studies describe sudden onset of autistic symptoms,
or regressive autism, in older children after contracting herpes encephalitis
(98 –100). Secondary hydrocephalus (e.g., from bacterial meningitis) as a
precursor to late-onset autism has also been reported in a few cases (98,100,101).
Others have implicated the viral or bacterial infection, rather than the secondary
hydrocephalus, as the pathogenic factor in autism (97,102,103). Deykin and
MacMahon (97), using the broad definition of exposure as being in a household
with another person who is infected during the first 18 months of the subject’s
life, found more autism cases exposed to mumps, chickenpox, herpes, and fever
of unknown origin than their control siblings. The relative risks for autism when
there was actual clinical illness during the first 18 months of life, after adjustment
for sibship size, were even higher. These illnesses included measles, mumps,
rubella, chickenpox, CNS infections, influenza, ear infections, and fever of
unknown origin.
XVI. SUMMARY
Epidemiology is a tool that is useful for describing the occurrence of and risk
factors for a disease or condition in human populations. In this chapter, we have
described the epidemiology, specifically measures of occurrence, trends over
time, and risk factors for autism and ASDs.
The most commonly used measure of disease frequency for most
developmental disabilities, including autism, is prevalence, which has been used
to approximate incidence because measurement of the true incidence of autism is
problematic. Since the 1960s, with the conduct of the first prevalence studies of
autism, a solid body of literature examining the prevalence of autism from
population-based studies has been accumulating. We conducted a literature
review and identified 35 population-based prevalence studies of autism/ASD,
which we organized by diagnostic criteria, time period, size of population, and
method of ascertainment in an attempt to better understand trends in autism
prevalence over time; however, only 31 of the studies met our criteria for
inclusion in the examination of possible trends.
When we examined trends in prevalence by diagnostic criteria used over
time, we found that while there is a large difference in the mean prevalence rates
of studies that used DSM-IV or ICD-10 diagnostic criteria as compared with
other criteria, this trend toward increased prevalence rates based on use of these
diagnostic criteria persisted, although the pattern was attenuated when weighted
prevalence rates were used. When the 31 studies were examined by the year of
study, overall, we found that the prevalence rates increased over time; however,
the exact influences on such apparent trends cannot be easily elucidated. As noted
previously, there seems to be a trend toward higher prevalence rates associated
with smaller study populations. We were able to confirm this observation; i.e., we
found that both the weighted and mean prevalence rates of studies with
denominators of 20,000 or less yielded higher prevalence rates than study
populations of 100,000 or more. The type of case ascertainment method is also
REFERENCES
43. Kadesjö B, Gillberg C, Hagberg B. Brief report. Autism and Asperger syndrome in
seven-year-old children. J Autism Dev Disord 1999; 29:327 – 332.
44. Baird G, Charman T, Baron-Cohen S, et al. A screening instrument for autism at 18
months of age: a 6 year follow-up study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry
2000; 39:694 –702.
45. Kielinen M, Linna S, Moilanen I. Austism Northern Finland. Eur Child Adolesc
Psychiatry 2000; 9:162 – 167.
46. Mágnússon P, Sæmundsen E. Prevalence of autism in Iceland. J Autism Dev Disord
2001; 31:153 –163.
47. Chakrabarti S, Fombonne E. Pervasive developmental disorder in preschool
children. JAMA 2001; 285:3093 – 3099.
48. Bertrand J, Mars A, Boyle C, et al. Prevalence of autism in a United States
population: the Brick Township, New Jersey, investigation. Pediatrics 2001;
108:1155 –1161.
49. Croen L, Grether J, Hoogstrate J, et al. The changing prevalence of autism in
California. J Autism Dev Disord 2001; 32:207 – 215.
50. Fombonne E. The epidemiology of autism: a review. Psychol Med 1999;
29:769– 786.
51. Folstein S, Rutter M. Infantile autism: a genetic study of 21 twin pairs. J Child
Psychol Psychiatry 1977; 18(4):297– 321.
52. Ritvo ER, Freeman BJ, Mason-Brothers A, Mo A, Ritvo AM. Concordance for the
syndrome of autism in 40 pairs of afflicted twins. Am J Psychiatry 1985;
142(1):74– 77.
53. Steffenburg S, Gillberg C, Hellgren L, Andersson L, Gillberg IC, Jakobsson G, et al.
A twin study of autism in Denmark, Finland, Iceland, Norway and Sweden. J Child
Psychol Psychiatry 1989; 30(3):405– 416.
54. Bailey A, Le Couteur A, Gottesman I, Bolton P, Simonoff E, Yuzda E, et al. Autism
as a strongly genetic disorder: evidence from a British twin study. Psychol Med
1995; 25(1):63– 77.
55. Smalley SL, Asarnow RF, Spence MA. Autism and genetics: a decade of research.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1988; 45(10):953– 961.
56. Jorde LB, Hasstedt SJ, Ritvo ER, Mason-Brothers A, Freeman BJ, Pingree C, et al.
Complex segregation analysis of autism. Am J Hum Genet 1991; 49(5):932– 938.
57. Ritvo ER, Jorde LB, Mason-Brothers A, Freeman BJ, Pingree C, Jones MB, et al.
The UCLA – University of Utah epidemiologic survey of autism: recurrence risk
estimates and genetic counseling. Am J Psychiatry 1989; 146(8):1032– 1036.
58. Jorde LB, Mason-Brothers A, Waldmann R, Ritvo ER, Freeman BJ, Pingree C, et al.
The UCLA – University of Utah epidemiologic survey of autism: genealogical
analysis of familial aggregation. Am J Med Genet 1990; 36(1):85–88.
59. Philippe A, Martinez M, Guilloud-Bataille M, Gillberg C, Rastam M, Sponheim E,
et al. Genome-wide scan for autism susceptibility genes. Paris Autism Research
International Sibpair Study. Hum Mol Genet 1999; 8(5):805– 812.
60. A genomewide screen for autism: strong evidence for linkage to chromosomes 2q,
7q, and 16p. International Molecular Genetic Study of Autism Consortium. Am J
Hum Genet 2001; 69(3):570– 581.
77. Moore SJ, Turnpenny P, Quinn A, Glover S, Lloyd DJ, Montgomery T, et al.
A clinical study of 57 children with fetal anticonvulsant syndromes. J Med Genet
2000; 37:489 –497.
78. Williams G, King J, Cunningham M, Stephan M, Kern B, Hersh JH. Fetal valproate
syndrome and autism: additional evidence of an association. Dev Med Child Neurol
2001; 43:202 –206.
79. Bescoby-Chambers N, Forster P, Bates G. Foetal valproate syndrome and autism:
additional evidence of an association. Dev Med Child Neurol 2001; 43:847– 848.
80. Deykin EY, MacMahon B. The incidence of seizures among children with autistic
symptoms. Am J Psychiatry 1979; 136(10):1310– 1312.
81. Bryson SE, Smith IE. Epidemiology of autism: prevalence, associated
characteristics, and implications for research and service delivery. Ment Retard
Dev Disabil Res Rev 1998; 4:97– 103.
82. Berney TP. Autism-an evolving concept. Br J Psychiatry 2000; 176:20– 25.
83. DiMartino A, Tuchman RF. Antiepileptic drugs: affective use in autism spectrum
disorders. Pediatr Neurol 2001; 25(3):199– 207.
84. Hollander E, Dolgoff-Kaspar R, Cartwright C, Rawitt R, Novotny S. An open trial
of divalproex sodium in autism spectrum disorders. J Clin Psychiatry 2001;
62(7):530– 534.
85. Plioplys AV. Autism: electroencephalogram abnormalities and clinical improve-
ment with valproic acid. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 1994; 148(2):220– 222.
86. Deykin EY, MacMahon B. Pregnancy, delivery, and neonatal complications among
autistic children. Am J Dis Child 1980; 134:860 –864.
87. Mason-Brothers A, Ritvo ER, Pingree C, Petersen PB, Jenson WR, McMahon WM,
et al. The UCLA-University of Utah epidemiologic survey of autism: prenatal,
perinatal, and postnatal factors. Pediatrics 1990; 86(4):514– 519.
88. Juul-Dam N, Townsend J, Courchesne E. Prenatal, perinatal, and neonatal factors in
autism, pervasive developmental disorder-not otherwise specified, and the general
population. Pediatrics 2001; 107(4):E63.
89. Madsen KM, Hvid A, Vestergaard M, Schendel D, Wohlfahrt J, Thorsen P, et al.
A population-based study of measles, mumps, and rubella vaccination and autism.
N Engl J Med 2002; 347(19):1477– 1482.
90. Fombonne E, Chakrabarti. No evidence for a new variant of measles-mumps-
rubella-induced autism. Pediatrics 2001; 108(4):URL: http://www.pediatrics.org/
cgi/content/full/108/4/e58
91. Gillberg C, Heijbel H. MMR and autism. Autism 1998; 2(4):423–424.
92. Farrington CP, Miller E, Taylor B. MMR and autism: further evidence against a
causal association. Vaccine 2001; 19:3632 – 3635.
93. Institute of Medicine, Board on Health Promotion and Disease Prevention, Immu-
nization Safety Review Committee. Immunization Safety Review: Thimerosal-
Containing Vaccines and Neurodevelopmental Disorders. Washington, DC:
National Academy Press, 2001.
94. Pichichero ME, Cernichiari E, Lopreiato J, Treanor J. Mercury concentrations and
metabolism in infants receiving vaccines containing thiomersal: a descriptive study.
Lancet 2002; 360(9347):1737– 1741.
95. Chess S. Autism in children with congenital rubella. J Autism Child Schizophr
1971; 1(1):33 – 47.
96. Gillberg C, Coleman M. Infectious Diseases. The Biology of the Autistic
Syndromes. London: Mac Keith Press, 1992:218 –225.
97. Deykin EY, MacMahon B. Viral exposure and autism. Am J Epidemiol 1979;
109(6):628– 638.
98. Delong GR, Bean SC, Brown FR. Acquired reversible autistic syndrome in acute
encephalopathic illness in children. Arch Neurol 1981; 38(3):191– 194.
99. Gillberg C. Onset at age 14 of a typical autistic syndrome. a case report of a girl
with herpes simplex encephalitis. J Autism Dev Disord 1986; 16(3):369– 375.
100. Gillberg IC. Autistic syndrome with onset at age 31 years: herpes encephalitis
as a possible model for childhood autism. Dev Med Child Neurol 1991;
33(10):920– 924.
101. Knobloch H, Pasamanick B. Some etiologic and prognostic factors in early infantile
autism and psychosis. Pediatrics 1975; 55(2):182– 191.
102. Ritvo ER, Mason-Brothers A, Freeman BJ, Pingree C, Jenson WR, McMahon WM,
et al. The UCLA – University of Utah epidemiologic survey of autism: the etiologic
role of rare disease. Am J Psychiatry 1990; 147(12):1614– 1621.
103. Matarazzo EB. Treatment of late onset autism as a consequence of probable
autoimmune processes related to chronic bacterial infection. World J Biol
Psychiatry 2002; 3(3):162–166.
104. Gillberg C, Gillberg IC. Infantile autism: a total population study of reduced
optimality in the pre-, peri-, and neonatal period. J Autism Dev Disord 1983;
13(2):153– 166.
105. Eaton WW, Mortensen PB, Thomsen PH, Frydenberg M. Obstetric complications
and risk for severe psychopathology in childhood. J Autism Dev Disord 2001;
31(3):279– 285.
106. Torrey EF, Hersh SP, McCabe KD. Early childhood psychosis and bleeding during
pregnancy. J Autism Child Schizophr 1975; 5(4):287–297.
107. Finegan J, Quarrington B. Pre, peri, and neonatal factors and infantile autism.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1979; 20:119 – 128.
108. Bolton PF, Murphy M, Macdonald H, Whitlock B, Pickles A, Rutter M. Obstetric
complications in autism: consequences or causes of the condition? J Am Acad
Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1997; 36(2):272– 281.
109. Lord C, Mulloy C, Wendelboe M, Schopler E. Pre- and perinatal factors in
high-functioning females and males with autism. J Autism Dev Disord 1991;
21(2):197– 209.
110. Piven J, Simon J, Chase GA, Wzorek M, Landa R, Gayle, J, et al. The etiology of
autism: pre-, peri-, and neonatal factors. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1993;
32(6):1256– 1263.
111. Cryan E, Byrne M, O’Donovan A, O’Callaghan E. Brief report: a case-control study
of obstetric complications and later autistic disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 1996;
26(4):453– 460.
I. INTRODUCTION
Kanner, in his seminal paper, noted that the parents of children in his case series
were “highly intelligent, preoccupied with abstractions of a scientific, literary,
and artistic nature, limited in genuine interest in people, obsessive and lacking
warm-heartedness” (4,5). Although he wrongly attributed autism to this lack of
warmth in the parents, his observation was not without merit. Like Kanner, Piven
et al. have also reported that the parents of autistic subjects have higher rates of
aloof, rigid, hypersensitive, and anxious personality traits and of speech and
language deficits (6). Family studies suggest that there is an increased prevalence
of both autism and autistic-like behaviors in the first-degree relatives of persons
with autism (7 –9). The prevalence of autism in siblings is 3– 9% (10,11), while
the prevalence of a broad autism phenotype, comprising of subtle communication
and social impairments, is 12.4– 20.4% (7). Many families with multiple cases of
autism (multiplex families) have been described. Relatives of multiple-incidence
families have even higher rates of social and communication deficits and
stereotypic behaviors (8). The recurrence rate for autism after the birth of one
child is 8.6% and the relative risk of a sibling having autism is 50– 175 (12,13).
These data suggest that genetic factors cause subtle and obvious social,
communication, and sensorimotor deficits among family members of the
probands and influence the development of autism.
A genetic basis of autism is also suggested by twin studies, because
monozygotic twins have a higher concordance rate for autism than dizygotic
twins (14 – 16). However, the concordance for typical autistic disorder in
monozygotic twins is only 50%, and the symptoms of autism are often different in
monozygotic twin pairs (17,18). Concordance for a broad autism phenotype
consisting of social and language deficits, on the other hand, is much higher
(92%) (17). This suggests that either a broad autism phenotype or a susceptibility
the prevalence of autism in siblings is 3 –9% (10,11) while the broad autism
phenotype, comprising more subtle communication/social impairments or
stereotypic behaviors, can occur in as many as 12.4 – 20.4% of siblings (7). The
risk of autism is particularly high if one of the parents has autism or Asperger’s
syndrome (72).
The immune system and brain communicate with each other through
neurotransmitters, hormones, and cytokines. The cytokines and immune cells
can activate neuronal pathways and release tropic hormones such as ACTH.
The latter, in turn, can influence immunological function by stimulating the
release of end-organ hormones, such as corticosteroids. The immune, nervous,
and endocrine systems are, therefore, tightly interwoven to regulate
homeostasis and changes in one can affect the other, and it is biologically
plausible that immune dysfunction can cause neurological dysfunction. Two
broad categories of immunological abnormalities have been described in
autism—qualitative or quantitative abnormalities of immune cells, and
autoantibodies against neural elements. However, it is difficult to make
meaningful generalizations from the available studies because of small and
heterogeneous samples, selection bias, and lack of uniform diagnostic criteria
across studies. The findings are often inconsistent and contradictory. While
decreased lymphocyte proliferation in response to mitogens, such as
phytohemagglutinin (PHA), concanavalin A, and pokeweed mitogen, was
reported in a few studies (73,74), other studies found normal (75), and both
high and low rates of T-cell proliferation in response to mitogens (76).
Decreased number of T cells, proportional to the severity of symptoms, was
reported by one group (74,77), but another group reported normal numbers of
T and B cells (75). Changes in the distribution of T-cell subtypes have also
been described in autism. The T lymphocytes are characterized by cluster of
differentiation (CD) surface molecules. CD4þ T cells, also called helper cells,
stimulate the differentiation of B lymphocytes into plasma and memory cells
and induce suppressor/cytotoxic cells, thus helping both the cellular and
humoral components of immune response. CD8þ cells, also called T
suppressor cells, kill the infected cells and suppress autoimmune response.
The lack of T helper cells can impair cell-mediated and humoral immune
response, while the lack of suppressor cells can set the stage for autoimmune
mechanisms to occur. Warren et al. reported reduced numbers of CD4þ cells,
in particular of the CD4 þ CD45RA þ lymphocytes that induce suppressor/
cytotoxic cells (77). A reversal of T helper/suppressor ratio due to a selective
decrease in CD4 helper cells has also been reported (78,79). Plioplys et al., on
the contrary, have reported normal CD4:CD8 ratios for the whole group, with
increased ratio in some and decreased ratio in others (75). Gupta et al.
reported lower proportions of (Th1) T cells and increased proportions of (Th2)
T cells in autistic children as compared to healthy controls (80). T helper-1
cells promote the expansion of active T cells by producing cytokines IL-2 and
IFN-g. Decreased numbers of Th1 cells can affect cell-mediated immunity
and NK cell activity, making an individual more susceptible to infection,
particularly by viruses.
Other studies have focused on the activity of natural killer (NK) cells in
autistic patients. These cells have the ability to function without prior exposure
to a particular antigen and are involved in the removal of viral-infected cells
as well as tumor cells. It is believed that these cells may have a regulatory role
in the immune system, preventing autoimmunity, because their activity has
been demonstrated to be reduced in several autoimmune disorders, such as
systemic lupus erythematosus, rheumatoid arthritis, and multiple sclerosis.
Warren et al. reported significantly reduced killing activity by NK cells in 40%
of autistic subjects (81). Reduced NK cell activity can presumably place an
individual, or fetus, at an increased risk for the development of neurological
damage by viruses.
The second hypothesis linking the etiology of autism to the immune system
involves the breakdown of self-recognition mechanisms, or autoimmunity.
Autoimmunity is characterized by cellular and humoral immunological reactions
against components of the self. Production of autoantibodies to neuron-specific
antigens in autistic children has been described in several studies. Singh et al.
reported antibodies to neuron-axon filament proteins (NAFP) and myelin basic
proteins in children with autism (82,83), while Plioplys et al. reported antibodies
against cerebellar neurofilaments (84). Vojdani et al. measured autoantibodies
against nine different neuron-specific antigens, including myelin basic protein,
neurofilament proteins, and tubulin, and three cross-reactive peptides from
Chlamydia pneumonia, Streptococcus group A, and milk. In this study, autistic
children showed the highest levels of IgG, IgM, and IgA antibodies against all
neurological antigens as well as the three cross-reactive peptides compared to
controls (85).
Several studies have reported antibodies against neurotransmitter receptors
such as serotonin (5-HT) receptors (86,87), a2-adrenergic receptors (88), and
brain endothelia cell proteins (89) in children with autism. However, the
autoantibodies reported in the above studies are not specific to patients with
autism and are seen in demyelinating neuropathies such as multiple sclerosis and
Guillain-Barré syndrome as well. Antibodies reactive to CNS proteins are not
seen in the sera of most patients with autism. Unlike myasthenia gravis, in which
a clear antibody-receptor interaction has been identified, no consistent antibody
receptor interaction has been found in autism. There are few pathological findings
During the last two decades there has been a growing interest in the “central
nervous system and gut nexus.” The enteric nervous system develops from the
cells of neural crest (95) and shares neurotransmitters, such as serotonin,
vasoactive intestinal peptide (VIP), and secretin, with the main nervous system.
There is some evidence that VIP regulates immune response by microglia and
secretin affects GABA transmission in the Purkinje cells of the cerebellum
(96,97). Thus it is theoretically conceivable that the gut and brain can influence
each other.
The main premise of the “gut” theory of autism is that the gastrointestinal
(GI) tract is unable to adequately metabolize opioids derived from dietary
sources, in particular foods that contain gluten and casein, and permits them to be
absorbed via an abnormally permeable intestinal membrane (98 –102). These
peptides cross the blood-brain barrier and bind to the opioid receptors producing
symptoms of autism, such as inattention, inability to learn, and poor social
interaction (103,104). Children with autism have been reported to have increased
urinary excretion of low-molecular-weight peptides (99,105,106) and increased
opioid levels in cerebrospinal fluid (98,99).
The proponents of this theory cite increased rate of the gastrointestinal
symptoms, such as bulky, malodorous, loose stools or intermittent diarrhea, in
children with autistic spectrum disorder (103). A variety of gastrointestinal
abnormalities, such as lymphoid nodular hyperplasia of the terminal ileum,
enterocolitis with infiltration of T, plasma, and Paneth cells, mild duodenitis,
disaccharide malabsorption, and esophageal reflux, have been described in
children with autistic spectrum disorders (ASD) referred to the GI clinics
(103,107,108). The now largely discounted (109) association between MMR
vaccination and autism was allegedly mediated by vaccine-induced enterocolits
resulting in absorption of toxic peptides (101).
may include: stool studies for malabsorption and Clostridium, urine for opioid
and peptide levels, serum levels of IgG and IgM antibodies for casein,
lactalbumin, and b-lactoglobulin, and upper and lower endoscopic studies to
evaluate for lymphoid nodular hyperplasia and other signs of gut epithelial
dysfunction. Moreover, well-designed population-based studies are needed to
evaluate the prevalence of abnormalities of the GI tract and to test the hypothesis
that gut dysfunction can cause autism through the gut-brain nexus.
V. TERATOGENIC FACTORS
The saga of measles, mumps, and rubella (MMR) vaccination and autism started
when Wakefield et al. reported a series of 12 cases that allegedly developed
Cases of autism have been described in children who had intrauterine rubella
(142,143) and cytomegalovirus infection (144). Autism has also been described
after perinatal and postnatal herpes simplex encephalitis (145). Most of these
cases have clear evidence of brain damage with additional manifestations such as
mental retardation and seizures. Besides damaging the brain directly, viruses can
damage the brain indirectly by triggering an autoimmune response against the
neural elements of the host. Investigations are ongoing into the possibility of
previously unrecognized viruses specifically causing the syndrome of autism
without producing recognizable cytopathic effects. Hornig and Lipkin have
produced symptoms suggestive of autism in newborn Lewis rats by infecting
them with Borna virus (146). These rats have been noted to have abnormalities in
cerebellum and hippocampus that resemble the lesions reported in autism. Thus it
is biologically plausible for a viral infection occurring at a critical time of
development to result in neurodevelopmental disability with no overt signs of
encephalitis at the time of infection. Persistent viral infections can, theoretically,
cause neurotransmitter dysfunction without causing inflammation, histological
injury, or cytopathic effect and may not be detected by traditional neurological
investigations (147). Thus a viral infection at a key time in early stages of
neurological development can predispose an individual to autism, but no specific
viral cause of autism has been confirmed so far.
Perinatal and obstetric factors have not emerged as important in the etiology of
autism. Several case control studies have found no association between obstetric
factors and autism. Others have reported weak associations between various
obstetric factors and autism, but no single obstetric event has emerged as a
preeminent antecedent of autism. Higher incidence of second- or third-trimester
uterine bleeding and prolonged labor have been reported in several studies (148 –
151). Hyperbilirubinemia was noted more often in autistic children in three
studies (148 –151). Other complications that have been reported inconsistently
include induction of labor, prolonged and precipitous labor, and oxygen
requirement at birth. Data on vaginal infections during pregnancy are conflicting
and may be confounded by socioeconomic status and number of sexual partners,
factors not controlled for in the studies. Prematurity is not associated with autism.
Overall the complications that have been reported have been mild and none have
emerged as necessary or sufficient to cause autism (152).
Studies that have used the optimality approach to assess the obstetric
factors in autism have also reported mixed to negative results. This approach
looks at obstetric and neonatal risk as a composite score. Deb et al. found no
significant difference in the scores of obstetric optimality between the overall
group of autistic children and their siblings, but among children with severe
autism there was a significant correlation between severity of autism and
obstetric and neonatal complication scores. This observation supports the notion
of two types of autism: low-functioning, which is secondary to some known
X. SUMMARY
It seems from the foregoing that the etiology of autism is still elusive. Many
hypotheses have been proposed, some based on empirical observations, some by
analogy, and some by deductive reasoning. Only the salient hypotheses have been
presented in this chapter. Because autism is such a disheartening condition, there
is a tendency to say “eureka” whenever any association is observed. Explosion in
the prevalence of autism has made it a political disease. And the media smells for
rats, ready to sensationalize any anecdote, any suggestion, and any insinuation as
a potential cause. In this charged environment it is the responsibility of the
professionals to be rational while keeping an open mind. Instead of rushing to
judgment, the professionals should measure every association with Hill’s criteria
and plan and inform accordingly. Because of the fervor with which the scientific
community and the society at large are responding to this apparent epidemic of
autism, it is likely that we shall find the etiological factors associated with autism
in the near future and be able to stall the tide of autism.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
I wish to acknowledge the help of Rohit Kohli, M.D., in the section on gastro-
intestinal factors and of Divya Aggarwal, M.D., in the section on teratogenic
factors.
REFERENCES
1. DeLong GR. Autism: new data suggest a new hypothesis. Neurology 1999;
23;52:911– 916.
2. Fombonne E. The epidemiology of autism: a review. Psychol Med 1999;
29:769– 786.
3. Hill AB. The environment and disease: association or causation? Proc R Soc Med
1965; 58:295 –300.
4. Kanner L. Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child 1943;
2:217– 250.
5. www.ama.org.br/kannereng12.htm, accessed on 9.12.02.
6. Piven J, Palmer P, Landa R, Santangelo S, Jacobi D, Childress D. Personality and
language characteristics in parents from multiple-incidence autism families. Am J
Med Genet 1997; 74:398– 411.
7. Bolton P, MacDonald H, Pickles A, et al. A case-control family history study of
autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1994; 35:877 –900.
8. Piven J, Palmer P, Jacobi D, Childress D, Arndt S. Broader autism phenotype:
evidence from a family history study of multiple-incidence autism families. Am J
Psychiatry 1997; 154:185 – 190.
9. Spiker D, Lotspeich L, Kraemer HC, et al. Genetics of autism: characteristics of
affected and unaffected children from 37 multiplex families. Am J Med Genet 1994;
54:27– 35.
10. Smalley SL, McCracken J, Tanguay P. Autism, affective disorders, and social
phobia. Am J Med Genet 1995; 60:19 – 26.
11. Jones MB, Szatamari P. Stopagge rules and genetic studies of autism. Autism Dev
Disord 1988; 18:31 – 40.
12. Ritvo ER, Jorde LB, Mason-Brothers A, Freeman BJ, Pingree C, Jones MB,
McMahon WM, Petersen PB, Jenson WR, Mo A. The UCLA – University of Utah
epidemiologic survey of autism: recurrence risk estimates and genetic counseling.
Am J Psychiatry 1989; 146:1032 – 1036.
13. Silverman JM, Smith CJ, Schneidler J, et al. Symptom domains in autism and
related conditions: evidence for familiarity. Am J Med Genet (Neuropsychiatry)
2002; 114:64 –73.
14. Folstein S, Rutter M. Infantile autism: a genetic study of 21 twin pairs. J Child
Psychol Psychiatry 1977; 18:297– 321.
15. Ritvo ER, Freeman BJ, Mason-Brothers A, Mo A, Ritvo AM. Concordance of the
syndrome of autism in 40 pairs of afflicted twins. Am J Psychiatry 1985; 142:74– 77.
16. Steffenburg S, Gillberg C, Hellgren L, et al. A twin study of autism in Denmark,
Finland, Iceland, Norway and Sweden. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1989; 30:405–416.
17. Bailey A, Le Couteur A, Gottesman I, et al. Autism as a strongly genetic disorder:
evidence from a British twin study. Psychol Med 1995; 25:63– 77.
18. Le Couteur A, Bailey A, Goode S, Pickles A, Robertson S, Gottesman I, Rutter M.
A broader phenotype of autism: the clinical spectrum in twins. J Child Psychol
Psychiatry. 1996; 37:785 – 801.
19. Risch N, Spiker D, Lotspeich L, Nouri N, Hinds D. A genomic screen of autism:
evidence for a multilocus etiology. Am J Hum Genet 1999; 65:493 – 497.
20. International Molecular Genetic Study of Autism Consortium. A full genome
screen for autism with evidence for linkage to a region on chromosome 7q. Hum
Mol Genet 1998; 7:571– 578.
21. Shao Y, Wolpert CM, Raiford KL, Menold MM, Donnelly SL, Ravan SA, Bass MP,
McClain C, von Wendt L, Vance JM, Abramson RH, Wright HH, Ashley-Koch A,
Gilbert JR, DeLong RG, Cuccaro ML, Pericak-Vance MA. Genomic screen and
follow-up analysis for autistic disorder. Am J Med Genet. 2002; 114:99 – 105.
22. Wolpert CM, Menold MM, Bass MP, Qumsiyeh MB, Donnelly SL, Ravan SA,
Vance JM, Gilbert JR, Abramson RK, Wright HH, Cuccaro ML,
Pericak-Vance MA. Three probands with autistic disorder and isodicentric
chromosome 15. Am J Med Genet. 2000; 96:365– 372.
23. Cook EH Jr. Genetics of autism. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev 1998; 4:113–120.
24. Cook EH Jr, Lindgren V, Leventhal BL, Courchesne R, Lincoln A, Shulman C,
Lord C, Courchesne E. Autism or atypical autism in maternally but not paternally
derived proximal 15q duplication. Am J Hum Genet 1997; 60:928 –34
25. Nurmi EL, Bradford Y, Chen Y, Hall J, Arnone B, Gardiner MB, Hutcheson HB,
Gilbert IR, Pericak-Vance MA, Copeland-Yates SA, Michaelis RC, Wassink TH,
Santangelo SL, Sheffield VC, Piven J, Folstein SE, Haines JL, Sutcliffe JS. Linkage
disequilibrium at the Angelman syndrome gene UBE3A in autism families.
Genomics 2001; 77:105 – 113.
26. Sultana R, Yu J, Raskind W, Disteche C. Cloning of a candidate gene (ARG1) from
the breakpoint of t(7;20) in an autistic twin pair. In: Abstracts of the Annual
Meeting of the American Society for Human Genetics, program no. 230. Bethesda,
Maryland, 1999.
27. Fatemi SH. Dysregulation of reelin and Bcl-2 proteins in autistic cerebellum.
J Autism Dev Disord 2001; 31:529– 535.
28. Wolff DJ, Clifton K, Karr C, Charles J. Pilot assessment of the subtelomeric regions
of children with autism: detection of a 2q deletion. Genet Med 2002; 4:10 – 14.
29. Ghaziuddin M, Burmeister M. Deletion of chromosome 2q37 and autism: a distinct
subtype? J Autism Dev Disord 1999; 29:259 – 263.
30. Hallmayer J, Hebert JM, Spiker D et al. Autism and the X chromosome: multipoint
sib-pair analysis. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1996; 53:985– 989.
31. Gurling HM, Bolton PF, Vincent J, Melmer G, Rutter M. Molecular and
cytogenetic investigations of the fragile X region including the Frax A and Frax
ECGG trinucleotide repeat sequences in families multiplex for autism and related
phenotypes. Hum Hered 1997; 47:254– 262.
32. Vincent JB, Konecki DS, Munstermaun E, et al. Point mutation analysis of the
FMR-1 gene in autism. Mol Psychiatry 1996; 1:227– 231.
33. Ritvo ER, Mason-Brothers A, Freeman BJ, Pingree C, Jenson WR, McMahon WM,
Peterson PB, Jorde LB, Mo A, Ritvo A. The UCLA – University of Utah
epidemiologic survey of autism: the etiologic role of rare diseases. Am J Psychiatry
1990; 147:1614 – 1621.
34. Weidmer-Mikhail E, Sheldon S, Ghaziuddin M. Chromosomes in autism and
related pervasive developmental disorders: a cytogenetic study. J Intellect Disabil
Res 1998; 42 (Pt 1):8– 12.
35. Gillberg C, Coleman M. The Biology of the Autistic Syndromes. Clinics in
Developmental Medicine No. 126. 2nd ed. London: MacKeith Press, 1992.
36. Rasmussen P, Borjesson O, Wentz E, Gillberg C. Autistic disorders in Down
syndrome: background factors and clinical correlates. Dev Med Child Neurol 2001;
43:750– 754.
37. Bailey A, Bolton P, Butler L, LeCouteur A, Murphy M, Scott S, Webb T,
Rutter M. Prevalence of the fragile X anomaly amongst autistic twins and
singletons. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1993; 34:673 – 688.
38. Brown WT, Jenkins EC, Cohen IL, Fisch GS, Wolf-Schein EG, Gross A,
Waterhouse L, Fein D, Mason-Brothers A, Ritvo E, et al. Fragile X and autism: a
multicenter survey. Am J Med Genet 1986; 23:341 – 352.
39. Reiss AL, Freund L. Behavioral phenotype of fragile X syndrome: DSM-III-R
autistic behavior in male children. Am J Med Genet 1991; 43:35– 46.
40. Einfeld S, Molony H, Hall W. Autism is not associated with the fragile X syndrome.
Am J Med Genet 1989; 34:187– 193.
41. Rapin I. Legitimacy of comparing fragile X with autism questioned. J Autism Dev
Disord 2002; 32:60 – 61.
42. Sudhalter V, Cohen IL, Silverman W, Wolf-Schein EG. Conversational analyses of
males with fragile X, Down syndrome, and autism: comparison of the emergence of
deviant language. Am J Ment Retard 1990; 94:431 –441.
43. Cohen IL, Vietze PM, Sudhalter V, Jenkins EC, Brown WT. Parent – child dyadic
gaze patterns in fragile X males and in non-fragile X males with autistic disorder.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1989; 30:845– 856.
44. Gillberg C, Wahlstrom J. Chromosome abnormalities in infantile autism and other
childhood psychoses: a population study of 66 cases. Dev Med Child Neurol 1985;
27:293– 304.
45. Gutierrez GC, Smalley SL, Tanguay PE. Autism in tuberous sclerosis complex.
J Autism Dev Disord 1998; 28:97 – 103.
46. Smalley SL. Autism and tuberous sclerosis. J Autism Dev Disord 1998;
28:407– 414.
47. Bolton PF, Griffiths PD. Association of tuberous sclerosis of temporal lobes with
autism and atypical autism. Lancet 1997; 349:392– 395.
48. Kratter FE. The physiognomic, psychometric, behavioral and neurological aspects
of phenyketonuria. J Pediatr 1959; 55:182– 191.
49. Buck PS, Michener JA. The Child Who Never Grew. Bethesda, MD: Woodbine
House, 1992.
50. Tierney E, Nwokoro NA, Kelley RI. Behavioral phenotype of RSH/Smith-Lemli-
Opitz syndrome. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev 2000; 6:131– 134.
51. Fombonne E, Du Mazaubrun C, Cans C, Grandjean H. Autism and associated
medical disorders in a French epidemiological survey. J Am Acad Child Adolesc
Psychiatry 1997; 36:1561– 1569.
52. Mbarek O, Marouillat S, Martineau J, Barthelemy C, Muh JP, Andres C.
Association study of the NF1 gene and autistic disorder. Am J Med Genet 1999;
88:729 – 732.
53. Kent L, Evans J, Paul M, Sharp M. Comorbidity of autistic spectrum disorders in
children with Down syndrome. Dev Med Child Neurol 1999; 41:153 – 158.
54. Rasmussen P, Borjesson O, Wentz E, Gillberg C. Autistic disorders in Down
syndrome: background factors and clinical correlates. Dev Med Child Neurol 2001;
43:750 – 754.
55. Demb HB, Papola P. PDD and Prader-Willi syndrome. J Am Acad Child Adolesc
Psychiatry 1995; 34:539– 540.
56. Dykens EM, Cassidy SB, King BH. Maladaptive behavior differences in Prader-
Willi syndrome due to paternal deletion versus maternal uniparental disomy. Am J
Ment Retard 1999; 104:67 –77.
57. Steffenburg S, Gillberg CL, Steffenburg U, Kyllerman M. Autism in Angelman
syndrome: a population-based study. Pediatr Neurol 1996; 14:131 – 136.
58. Penner KA, Johnston J, Faircloth BH, Irish P, Williams CA. Communication,
cognition, and social interaction in the Angelman syndrome. Am J Med Genet
1993; 46:34 – 39.
59. Summers JA, Allison DB, Lynch PS, Sandler L. Behaviour problems in Angelman
syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res 1995; 39(Pt 2):97– 106.
60. Pascal-Castroviejo I, Roche C, Martinez-Bermejo A, Arcas J, Lopez-Martin V,
Tendero A, Equiroz JL, Pascal-Pascal SI. Hypomelanosis of Ito: a study of 76
infantile cases. Brain Dev 1998; 20:36 – 43.
61. Gosch A, Pankau R. “Autistic” behavior in two children with Williams-Beuren
syndrome. Am J Med Genet 1994; 15;53:83– 84.
62. Gillberg C, Rasmussen P. Brief report: four case histories and a literature review of
Williams syndrome and autistic behavior. J Autism Dev Disord. 1994; 24:381– 93.
63. Komoto J, Usui S, Otsuki S. Infantile autism and Duchenne muscular dystrophy.
J Autism Dev Disord 1984; 14:191– 195.
64. Gillberg C, Steffenburg S. Autistic behaviour in Moebius syndrome. Acta Paediatr
Scand 1989; 78:314– 316.
65. Stromland K, Sjogreen L, Miller M, Gillberg C, Wentz E, Johansson M, Nylen O,
Danielsson A, Jacobsson C, Andersson J, Fernell E. Mobius sequence—a Swedish
multidiscipline study. Eur J Paediatr Neurol 2002; 6:35 –45.
66. Holroyd S, Reiss AL, Bryan RN. Autistic features in Joubert syndrome: a genetic
disorder with agenesis of the cerebellar vermis. Biol Psychiatry 1993; 33:854– 855.
87. Todd RD, Ciaranello RD. Demonstration of inter- and intraspecies differences in
serotonin binding sites by antibodies from an autistic child. Proc Natl Acad Sci
USA 1985; 82:612 – 616.
88. Cook EH Jr., Perry BD, Dawson G, Wainwright MS, Leventhal BL. Receptor
inhibition by immunoglobulins: specific inhibition by autistic children, their
relatives, and control subjects. J Autism Dev Disord 1993; 23:67 – 78.
89. Connolly AM, Chez MG, Pestronk A, Arnold ST, Mehta, Deuel RK. Serum
autoantibodies to brain in Landau-Kleffner variant, autism, and other neurologic
disorders. J Pediatr 1999; 134:607– 613.
90. Comi AM, Zimmerman AW, Frye VH, Law PA, Peeden JN. Familial clustering of
autoimmune disorders and evaluation of medical risk factors in autism. J Child
Neurol 1999; 14:388– 394.
91. Warren RP, Yonk J, Burger RW, Odell D, Warren WL. DR-positive T cells in
autism: association with decreased plasma levels of the complement C4B protein.
Neuropsychobiology 1995; 31:53 – 57.
92. Gupta S. Treatment of children with autism with intravenous inimunoglobulin.
J Child Neurol 1999; 14:203 –205.
93. Plioplys AV. Intravenous immunoglobulin treatment in autism. J Autism Dev
Disord 2000; 30:73– 74.
94. DelGiudice-Asch G, Simon L, Schmeidler J, Cunningham-Rundles C, Hollander E.
Brief report: a pilot open clinical trial of intravenous immunoglobulin in childhood
autism. J Autism Dev Disord 1999; 29:157 –160.
95. Gershon MD. The enteric nervous system: a second brain. Hosp Pract (Off Ed).
1999; 34:31 – 32, 35– 38, 41– 42.
96. Yung WH, Leung PS, Ng SS, Zhang J, Chan SC, Chow BK. Secretin facilitates
GABA transmission in the cerebellum. J Neurosci 2001; 21:7063 –7068.
97. Delgado M. Vasoactive intestinal peptide and pituitary adenylate cyclase-activating
polypeptide inhibit CBP-NF-kappaB interaction in activated microglia. Biochem
Biophys Res Commun 2002; 297:1181– 1185.
98. Reichelt KL, Hole K, Hamberfer A, et al. Biologically active peptide containing
fractions in schizophrenia and childhood autism. Adv Biochem Psychopharmacol
1981; 28:627 – 643.
99. Israngkun PP, Newman HAI, Patel ST, DuRuibe VA, Abou-Issa H. Potential
biochemical markers for infantile autism. Neurochem Pathol 1986; 5:51– 70.
100. D’Eufemia P, Celli M, Finocchiaro R, et al. Abnormal intestinal permeability in
children with autism. Acta Paediatr 1996; 85:1076 –1079.
101. Wakefield AJ, Murch SH, Anthony A. Ileal-lymphoid-nodular hyperplasia, non-
specific colitis, and pervasive developmental disorder in children. Lancet 1998;
351(9103):637 –641.
102. Horvath K, Papadimitriou JC, Rabsztyn A, Drachenberg C, Tildon JT.
Gastrointestinal abnormalities in children with autistic disorder. J Pediatr 1999;
135:559 – 563.
103. Reichelt KL, Knivsberg A, Lind G, Nodland M. Probable etiology and possible
treatment of childhood autism. Brain Disfunct 1991; 4:308– 319.
104. Knivsberg A, Reichelt KL, Nodland M, et al. Autistic syndromes and diet:
a follow-up study. Scand J Educ Res 1995; 39:222 – 236.
105. Reichelt KL, Saelid, G, Lindback T, et al. Childhood autism: a complex disorder.
Biol Psychiatry 1986; 21:1279 – 1290.
106. Shattock P, Kennedy A, Rowell F, et al. The role of neuropeptides in autism
and their relationship with classical neurotransmitters. Brain Dysfunct 1990;
3:328– 345.
107. Furlano RI, Anthony A, Day R. Colonic CD8 and gammadelta T-cell infiltration
with epithelial damage in children with autism. J Pediatr 2001; 138:366 – 372.
108. Goodwin MS, Cowen MA, Goodwin TC. Malabsorption and cerebral dysfunction:
a multivariate and comparative study of autistic children. J Autism Child Schizophr
1971; 1:48– 62.
109. Madsen KM, Hviid A, Vestergaard M, Schendel D, Wohlfahrt J, Thorsen P, Olsen
J, Melbye M. A population-based study of measles, mumps, and rubella vaccination
and autism. N Engl J Med 2002; 7(347):1477– 1482.
110. Finegold SM, Molitoris D, Song Y, Liu C, Vaisanen ML, Bolte E, McTeague M,
Sandler R, Wexler H, Marlowe EM, Collins MD, Lawson PA, Summanen P,
Baysallar M, Tomzynski TJ, Read E, Johnson E, Rolfe R, Nasir P, Shah H, Haake
DA, Manning P, Kaul A. Gastrointestinal microflora studies in late-onset autism.
Clin Infect Dis 2002; 1(35)(suppl 1):S6– S16.
111. Sandler RH, Finegold SM, Bolte ER, Buchanan CP, Maxwell AP, Vaisanen ML,
Nelson MN, Wexler HM. Short-term benefit from oral vancomycin treatment of
regressive-onset autism. J Child Neurol 2000; 15:429– 435.
112. Megson MN. Is autism a G-alpha protein defect reversible with natural vitamin A?
Med Hypoth 2000; 54:979 – 983.
113. Fombonne E, Du Mazaubrun C, Cans C, Grandjean H. Autism and associated
medical disorders in a French epidemiological survey. J Am Acad Child Adolesc
Psychiatry 1997; 36:1561 – 1569.
114. Fombonne E. Inflammatory bowel disease and autism (letter). Lancet 1998;
351:955.
115. Lucarelli S, Frediani T, Zingoni AM, et al. Food allergy and infantile autism.
Panminerva Med 1995; 37:137 – 141.
116. Cornish E. Gluten and casein free diets in autism: a study of the effects on food
choice and nutrition. J Hum Nutr Diet 2002; 15:261 – 269.
117. Sandler AD, Sutton KA, DeWeese J, et al. Lack of benefit of a single dose of
synthetic human secretin in the treatment of autism and pervasive developmental
disorder. N Engl J Med 1999; 341:1801 –1806.
118. Lightdale JR, Hayer C, Duer A. Effects of intravenous secretin on language
and behavior of children with autism and gastrointestinal symptoms: a single-
blinded, open-label pilot study. URL: http://www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/
full/108/5/e90.
119. Feldman HM, Kolmen BK, Gonzaga AM. Naltrexone and communication skills
in young children with autism. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry, 1999;
38:587– 593.
120. Miller MT. Thalidomide embryopathy: an insight into autism? Teratology 1993;
47:387– 388.
121. Stromland K, Nordin V, Miller M, Akerstrom B, Gilberg C. Autism in
thalidomide embryopathy: a population study. Dev Med Child Neurol 1994;
36:351– 356.
122. Rodier PM, Ingram JL, Tisdale B, Croog VJ. Linking etiologies in humans and
animal models: studies of autism. Reprod Toxicol 1997; 11:417– 422.
123. Moore SJ, Turnpenny P, Quinn A, Glover S, Llyod DJ, Montgomery T, Dean JC. A
clinical study of 57 children with fetal anticonvulsant syndromes. J Med Genet
2000; 37:489 – 497.
124. Nanson JL. Autism in fetal alcohol syndrome: a report of five cases. Alcohol Clin
Exp Res 1990; 14:322.
125. Davis E, Fennoy I, Laraque D, Kanem N, Brown G, Mitchell J. Autism and
developmental abnormalities in children with perinatal cocaine exposure. J Natl
Med Assoc 1992; 84:315– 319.
126. Delaney-Black V, Covington C, Templin T, Compton S, Sokol R, Ager J, Martier S.
Influence of prenatal alcohol/cocaine exposure on the report of autistic-like behaviors
among first grade students. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1997; 21(3 suppl):117A.
127. Bertrand J, Mars A, Boyle C, Bove F, Yeargin-Allsopp M, Decoulfe P. Prevalence
of autism in a United States population: the Brick Township, New Jersey,
investigation. Pediatrics 2001; 108:1155 – 1161.
128. Trask CL, Kosofsky BE. Developmental considerations of neurotoxic exposures.
Neurol Clin. 2000; 18:541 –562.
129. Wakefield AJ, Anthony A, Murch SH, et al. Enterocolitis in children with
developmental disorders. Am J Gastroenterol 2000; 95:2285– 2295.
130. Wakefield AJ, Murch SH, Anthony A, et al. Ileal-lymphoid-nodular hyperplasia,
nonspecific colitis, and pervasive developmental disorder in children. Lancet 1998;
351:637 – 641.
131. Taylor B, Miller E, Farrington CP, et al. Autism and measles, mumps, and rubella
vaccine: no epidemiologic evidence for a causal association. Lancet 1999;
353:2026 – 2069.
132. Madsen KM, Hviid A, Vestergaard M, Schendel D, Wohlfahrt J, Thorsen P, Olsen
J, Melbye M. Population-based study of measles, mumps, and rubella vaccination
and autism. N Engl J Med. 2002; 347:1477– 1482.
133. Dales L, Hammer SJ, Smith NJ. Time trends in autism and in MMR immunization
coverage in California. JAMA 2001; 285:1183– 1185.
134. Thjodleifsson B, Davidsdottir K, Agnarsson U, Sigthorsson G, Kjeld M, Bjarnason
I. Effect of Pentavac and measles-mumps-rubella (MMR) vaccination on the
intestine. Gut 2002; 51:816– 817.
135. Makela A, Nuorti JP, Pelota H. Neurologic disorders after measles-mumps-rubella
vaccination. Pediatrics 2002; 110:957– 963.
136. Haga Y, Funakoshi O, Kuroe K, Kanazawa K, Nakajima H, Saito H, Murata Y,
Munakata A, Yoshida Y. Absence of measles viral genomic sequence in intestinal
tissues from Crohn’s disease by nested polymerase chain reaction. Gut 1996;
38:211 – 215.
137. Afzal MA, Minor PD, Begley J, Bentley ML, Armitage E, Ghosh S, Ferguson
A. Absence of measles-virus genome in inflammatory bowel disease. Lancet 1998;
351:646 – 647.
138. Bernard S, Enayati A, Redwood L, Roger H, Binstock T. Autism: a novel form of
mercury poisoning. Med Hypoth 2001; 56:462 –471.
I. INTRODUCTION
Brain is the final pathway to which all the etiological factors discussed in the last
chapter lead. There is sufficient evidence in the neuroscience literature that
autistic symptoms occur because of functional or structural abnormalities of the
brain. About 15 – 30% of children with autism have macrocephaly (1 –5), due to
larger brain volume (6,7), with increases in both gray and white matter (8). While
autistic individuals continue to have a larger head circumference throughout their
life, their brain volume decreases after increasing for a few years after birth
(8 – 10), suggesting a neuropathological process that begins before birth but
continues postnatally. Increased head circumference in autism is not correlated
with IQ, verbal ability, seizure disorder, or autistic symptoms.
Despite a litany of studies pointing to the neurogenic origin of autistic
symptoms, the core areas of the brain that are involved and the specifics of their
dysfunction are unknown. The attempts to find a single source of autistic
symptoms in the brain have been unsuccessful and it is likely that this syndrome
is neurologically heterogeneous with different symptoms originating from
different structures of the brain. Perhaps autism occurs due to an insult to the
developing nervous system at an earlier stage when a localized insult branches off
to other areas of the brain because of the “interdependent nature of early brain
development” (11). Depending on sensory inputs and other factors during
subsequent maturation, this “branching off” takes its unique course in every
individual, giving rise to unique pathology in every individual. Reported
abnormalities in the key brain structures that have been incriminated as the
“home of autism in the brain” (12) are described below.
II. CEREBELLUM
Several neuropathological studies have found low Purkinje and granular cell
count in the cerebellar hemispheres of individuals with autism (13 – 15). Smaller
Purkinje cells have been reported. Fatemi et al. attributed small this reduction in
the size of Purkinje cells to reduction in Reelin and Bcl-2 proteins in the
cerebellum (16,17). Proton magnetic resonance studies have found lower
concentration of N-acetyl aspartate (NAA) in the cerebellum, suggesting reduced
activity of Purkinje cells (18,19).
Hypoplasia of cerebellar vermal lobules VI and VII was reported as the
“eureka” of autism in 1988 by Courchesne et al. (20), but since then other
findings, such as hypoplasia of vermal lobuli VIII –X (21,22), hypoplasia of
vermal lobuli I– V (23), and a combination of vermal hypo- and hyperplasia, have
been described (24). A report of pervasive developmental disorder in two
children with Joubert’s syndrome, a condition characterized by the agenesis of
cerebellar vermis, provided some support to the cerebellar hypothesis of autism
(25), but a later study refuted this association (26). Not all individuals with autism
have vermal hypoplasia (27,28) and vermal hypoplasia has been described in
individuals without autism, such as acute lymphoblastic leukemia survivors
(29,30). According to Ciesielski and Knight, involvement of the cerebellum in
such diverse conditions may be due to its prolonged course of maturation, making
it vulnerable to injury (30).
Morphometric studies of cerebellum have also shown inconsistent and
contradictory results. Both small (31) and large (32) cerebella have been
described, and in studies that report large cerebella, increase in cerebellar size out
of proportion to the overall brain size (32) as well as increase in cerebellar size
proportional to the overall brain size has been reported (27,33).
Limited support for the cerebellar hypothesis of autism comes from an
animal model in which exposure to valproic acid early during gestation damages
the brainstem cranial nerve nuclei and reduces the number of neurons in the
deep cerebellar nuclei, changes analogous to thalidomide embryopathy with
autism (34).
Although it is difficult to conclude from the inconsistent and contradictory
data presented above that cerebellar abnormalities are the source of autistic
symptoms in every person with autism, they may be responsible for some
symptoms of autism in a subset of patients. The cerebellum plays an important
role in language, emotion, and motor-attentional systems through its connections
with frontal lobe, thalamus, olivary nuclei, and other areas of the brain. It is
IV. BRAINSTEM
Association between autism and Möbius sequence suggests that brainstem may
be involved in a few cases of autism (48,49). Möbius sequence is characterized by
hypoplasia of cranial nerve nuclei resulting in congenital palsy of the sixth and
seventh cranial nerves. Rodier et al. found cellular abnormalities of the facial and
other brainstem nuclei in the brain of a woman with autism. The association
between cranial nerve involvement and autism has also been seen in thalidomide
embryopathy (50). Rodier et al. reproduced the lesion seen in thalidomide
embryopathy in rats by exposing them to valproic acid at the time of neural
tube closure (50). Autopsy studies have revealed smaller pons, midbrain, and
medulla oblongata (51 – 53) and abnormalities of inferior olivary nucleus in the
brains of individuals with autism (54). It has been suggested that autism occurs
because of the persistence of a transitional zone (lamina desiccans) beneath the
Purkinje cells where the climbing olivary fibres synapse until 24 –30 weeks of
gestation. The lesions in brainstem at an early stage of embryogenesis perhaps
damage the cerebral-cerebellar connections that are necessary for the
development of higher cognitive functions (55). Such brainstem-cerebellar
dysfunction was suggested by the finding of abnormal oculomotor movements
and brainstem potentials in children with autism in a study (56). However, the
evidence for brainstem involvement in autism is far from conclusive. Studies of
auditory brainstem responses of autistic probands and their relatives report
contradictory results including prolongation, shortening (57,58), and no
abnormalities of transmission latencies (59). Few children with autism have
cranial nerve palsies.
The brainstem plays a role in arousal and shifting of attention, and
modulates both general sensory input and motor response to it. Ornitz et al. found
abnormal responses to vestibular stimulation in autistic children (60). Therefore,
it is plausible that brainstem dysfunction can cause some symptoms of autism
such as transitioning from one activity to another, vestibular dysfunction, and
abnormal sensory processing.
V. BASAL GANGLIA
Delayed maturation
Maturational arrest with persistence of fetal circuitry
Abnormal development of neuropil (reduced dendritic pruning or abnormal proliferation)
Early damage to neural circuits/networks
Abnormal neuronal migration
Abnormal differentiation (microcolumnar changes)
Early degeneration (neurofibrillay tangles)
Abnormal apoptosis
Disconnect between various areas of the brain
Abnormalities of cerebral regional blood flow
Source in
Symptom complex the brain Reported associations
REFERENCES
38. Baron-Cohen S, Ring HA, Bullmore ET, Wheelwright S, Ashwin C, Williams SC.
The amygdala theory of autism. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2000; 24:355 – 364.
39. Howard MA, Cowell PE, Boucher J, Broks P, Mayes A, Farrant A, Roberts N.
Convergent neuroanatomical and behavioural evidence of an amygdala hypothesis
of autism. Neuroreport 2000; 11:2931 – 2935.
40. Bachevalier J. Medial temporal lobe structures and autism: a review of clinical and
experimental findings. Neuropsychologia 1994; 32:627– 648.
41. Amaral DG, Corbett BA. The amygdale, autism, and anxiety. In: Autism: Neural
Basis and Treatment Possibilities. Wiley, Chichester, England (Novartis
Foundation Symposium 251), 2003:177 – 197.
42. Haznedar MM, Buchsbaum MS, Metzger M, Solimando A, Spiegel-Cohen J,
Hollander E. Anterior cingulate gyrus volume and glucose metabolism in autistic
disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1997, 154:1047 – 1050.
43. Saitoh O, Karns CM, Courchesne E. Development of the hippocampal formation
from 2 to 42 years: MRI evidence of smaller area dentate in autism. Brain 2001;
124(Pt 7):1317 –1324.
44. Hardenar MM, Buchsbaum MS, Wei TC, Hof PR, Cartwright C, Bienstock CA,
Hollander E. Limbic circuitry in patients with autism spectrum disorders studied
with positron emission tomography and magnetic resonance imaging. Am J
Psychiatry 2000; 157:1994– 2001.
45. Kemper TL, Bauman ML. The contribution of neuropathologic studies to the
understanding of autism. Neurol Clin 1993; 11:175 – 187.
46. Raymond GV, Bauman ML, Kemper TL. Hippocampus in autism: a Golgi analysis.
Acta Neuropathol (Berl) 1996; 91:117– 119.
47. Piven J, Bailey J, Ranson BJ, Arndt S. No difference in hippocampus volume
detected on magnetic resonance imaging in autistic individuals. J Autism Dev
Disord 1998; 28:105– 110.
48. Gillberg C, Steffenburg S. Autistic behaviour in Möbius syndrome. Acta Paediatr
Scand 1989; 78:314– 316.
49. Stromland K, Sjogreen L, Miller M, Gillberg C, Wentz E, Johansson M, Nylen O,
Danielsson A, Jacobsson C, Andersson J, Fernell E. Mobius sequence—a Swedish
multidiscipline study. Eur J Paediatr Neurol 2002; 6:35 –45.
50. Rodier PM, Ingram JL, Tisdale B, Nelson S, Romano J. Embryological origin for
autism: developmental anomalies of the cranial nerve motor nuclei. J Comp Neurol
1996 24; 370:247 –261.
51. Hashimoto T, Tayama M, Miyazaki M, Murakawa K, Kuroda Y. Brainstem and
cerebellar vermis involvement in autistic children. J Child Neurol 1993; 8:149– 153.
52. Hashimoto T, Tayama M, Miyazaki M, et al. Brainstem involvement in high
functioning autistic children. Acta Neurol Scand 1993; 88:123– 128.
53. Hashimoto T, Tayama M, Murakawa K, et al. Development of the brainstem and
cerebellum in autistic patients. J Autism Dev Disord 1995; 25:1– 18.
54. Bauman M, Kemper TL, Histoanatomic observations of the brain in early infantile
autism. Neurology 1985; 35:866 – 874.
55. Skoyles JR. Is autism due to cerebral-cerebellum disconnection? Med Hypoth
2002; 58:332 – 336.
72. Baron-Cohen S, Ring HA, Wheelwright S, Bullmore ET, Brammer MJ, Simmons A,
Williams SC. Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: an fMRI study.
Eur J Neurosci 1999; 11:1891 – 1898.
73. Bolton PF, Park RJ, Higgins JN, Griffiths PD, Pickles A. Neuro-epileptic
determinants of autism spectrum disorders in tuberous sclerosis complex. Brain
2002; 125(Pt 6):1247 – 1255.
74. Riikonen R. Long-term outcome of patients with West syndrome. Brain Dev 2001;
23:683 – 687.
75. DeLong GR. Autism: new data suggest a new hypothesis. Neurology 1999;
52:911 – 916.
76. Muller RA, Behen ME, Rothermel RD, Chugani DC, Muzik O, Mangner TJ,
Chugani HT. Brain mapping of language and auditory perception in high-
functioning autistic adults: a PET study. J Autism Dev Disord 1999; 29:19 – 31.
77. Chiron C, Leboyer M, Leon F, Jambaque I, Nuttin C, Syrota A. SPECT of the brain
in childhood autism: evidence for a lack of normal hemispheric asymmetry. Dev
Med Child Neurol 1995; 37:849– 860.
78. Minshew NJ, Luna B, Sweeney JA. Oculomotor evidence for neocortical systems
but not cerebellar dysfunction in autism. Neurology 1999; 52:917 – 922.
79. Zilbovicius M, Garreau B, Samson Y, et al. Delayed maturation of the frontal cortex
in childhood autism. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152:248 – 252.
80. Rossi PG, Parmeggiani A, Bach V, Santucci M, Visconti P. EEG features and
epilepsy in patients with autism. Brain Dev 1995; 17:169 – 174.
81. Lewine JD, Andrews R, Chez M, Patil AA, Devinsky O, Smith M, Kanner A,
Davis JT, Funke M, Jones G, Chong B, Provencal S, Weisend M, Lee RR,
Orrison WW Jr. Magnetoencephalographic patterns of epileptiform activity
in children with regressive autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics 1999;
104(3Pt 1):405 –418.
82. Rossi PG, Parmeggiani A, Bach V, Santucci M, Visconti P. EEG features and
epilepsy in patients with autism. Brain Dev 1995; 17:169 – 174.
83. Tuchman RF, Rapin I. Regression in pervasive developmental disorders:
seizures and epileptiform electroencephalogram correlates. Pediatrics 1997;
99:560 – 566.
84. Rapin I. Autistic regression and disintegrative disorder: how important the role of
epilepsy? Semin Pediatr Neurol 1995; 2:278 – 285.
85. Nass R, Gross A, Devinsky O. Autism and autistic epileptiform regression with
occipital spikes. Dev Med Child Neurol 1998; 40:453 – 458.
86. Wong V. Epilepsy in children with autistic spectrum disorder. J Child Neurol 1993;
8:316 – 322.
87. Giovanardi RP, Posar A, Parmeggiani A. Epilepsy in adolescents and young adults
with autistic disorder. Brain Dev 2000; 22:102 – 106.
88. Rossi PG, Parmeggiani A, Bach V, Santucci M, Visconti P. EEG features and
epilepsy in patients with autism. Brain Dev 1995; 17:169 – 174.
89. Olsson I, Steffenburg S, Gillberg C. Epilepsy in autism and autisticlike conditions: a
population-based study. Arch Neurol 1988; 45:666 – 668.
90. Tuchman RF, Rapin I, Shinnar S. Autistic and dysphasic children. II. Epilepsy.
Pediatrics 1991; 88:1219 – 1225.
91. Casanova MF, Buxhoeveden DP, Switala AE, Roy E. Minicolumnar pathology in
autism. Neurology 2002; 12;58:428– 432.
92. Cook EH, Leventhal BL. The serotonin system in autism. Curr Opin Pediatr 1996;
8:348– 354.
93. Cook EH. Autism: review of neurochemical investigation. Synapse 1990; 6:292–308.
94. Kuperman A, Beeghly JHL, Burns TL, Tsai LY. Association of serotonin
concentration to behavior and IQ in autistic children. J Autism Dev Disord 1987;
17:133– 140.
95. Herault J, Petit E, Martineau J, Perrot A, Lenoir P, Cherpi C, Barthelemy C,
Sauvage D, Mallet J, Muh JP. Autism and genetics: clinical approach and association
study with two markers of HRAS gene. Am J Med Genet 1995; 60:276–281.
96. Todd RD, Ciaranello RD. Demonstration of inter- and intraspecies differences in
serotonin binding sites by antibodies from an autistic child. Proc Natl Acad Sci
USA 1985; 82:612 – 616.
97. Singh VK, Singh EA, Warren RP. Hyperserotoninemia and serotonin receptor
antibodies in children with autism but not mental retardation. Biol Psychiatry 1997;
41:753– 755.
98. Cook EH Jr, Perry BD, Dawson G, Wainwright MS, Leventhal BL. Receptor
inhibition by immunoglobulins: specific inhibition by autistic children, their
relatives, and control subjects. J Autism Dev Disord 1993; 23:67 –78.
99. Yuwiler A, Shih JC, Chen CH, Ritvo ER, Hanna G, Ellison GW, King BH.
Hyperserotoninemia and antiserotonin antibodies in autism and other disorders.
J Autism Dev Disord 1992; 22:33 – 45.
100. Kim SJ, Cox N, Courchesne R, Lord C, Corsello C, Akshoomoff N, Guter S,
Leventhal BL, Courchesne E, Cook EH Jr. Transmission disequilibrium mapping at
the serotonin transporter gene (SLC6A4) region in autistic disorder. Mol Psychiatry
2002; 7:278 –288.
101. Yirmiya N, Pilowsky T, Nemanov L, Arbelle S, Feinsilver T, Fried I, Ebstein RP.
Evidence for an association with the serotonin transporter promoter region
polymorphism and autism. Am J Med Genet 2001; 105:381 –386.
102. Klauck SM, Poustka F, Benner A, Lesch KP, Poustka A. Serotonin transporter
(5-HTT) gene variants associated with autism? Hum Mol Genet 1997; 6:2233–2238.
103. Herault J, Petit E, Martineau J, Cherpi C, Perrot A, Barthelemy C, Lelord G,
Muh JP. Serotonin and autism: biochemical and molecular biology features.
Psychiatry Res 1996; 65:33– 43.
104. Tordjman S, Gutknecht L, Carlier M, Spitz E, Antoine C, Slama F, Carsalade V,
Cohen DJ, Ferrari P, Roubertoux PL, Anderson GM. Role of the serotonin
transporter gene in the behavioral expression of autism. Mol Psychiatry 2001;
6:434– 439.
105. Persico AM, Pascucci T, Puglisi-Allegra S, Militerni R, Bravaccio C, Schneider C,
Melmed R, Trillo S, Montecchi F, Palermo M, Rabinowitz D, Reichelt KL,
120. Bouvard MP, Leboyer M, Launay JM, Recasens C, Plumet MH, Waller-Perotte D,
Tabuteau F, Bondoux D, Dugas M, Lensing P, et al. Low-dose naltrexone effects on
plasma chemistries and clinical symptoms in autism: a double-blind, placebo-
controlled study. Psychiatry Res 1995; 16;58:191– 201.
121. Feldman HM, Kolmen BK, Gonzaga AM. Naltrexone and communication skills in
young children with autism. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1999; 38(5):587–593.
122. Reichelt KL, Knivsberg AM, Lind G, et al. Probable etiology and possible
treatment of childhood autism. Brain Dysfunct 1991; 4:308– 319.
123. Sahley, TL, Panksepp, J. Brain opioids and autism: an updated analysis of possible
linkages. J Autism Dev Disord 1987; 17, 201– 216.
124. Knivsberg A, Reichelt K, Høien T, et al. Parents’ observations after one year of
dietary intervention for children with autistic syndromes. In: Shattock P, Linfoot G,
eds. Psychobiology of Autism: Current Research and Practice. Sunderland: Autism
Research Unit, University of Sunderland and Autism North, 1998:13– 24.
125. Whiteley P, Rodgers J, Savery D, et al. A gluten-free diet as an intervention for
autism and associated spectrum disorders: preliminary findings. Autism 1999;
3:45– 65.
126. Tsai LY. Psychopharmacology in autism. Psychosom Med 1999; 61:651– 665.
127. Lee M, Martin-Ruiz C, Graham A, Court J, Jaros E, Perry R, Iversen P, Bauman M,
Perry E. Nicotinic receptor abnormalities in the cerebellar cortex in autism. Brain
2002; 125:1483 – 1495.
128. Aston-Jones G. Locus ceruleus and regulation of behavioral flexibility and
attention. Prog Brain Res 2000; 126:165 –82.
129. Cook EH. Autism: review of neurochemical investigation. Synapse 1990;
6:292– 308.
130. Fatemi SH, Halt AR, Stary JM, Kanodia R, Schulz SC, Realmuto GR. Glutamic
acid decarboxylase 65 and 67 kDa proteins are reduced in autistic parietal and
cerebellar cortices. Biol Psychiatry 2002; 52:805 – 810.
131. Insel TR, O’Brien DJ, Leckman JF. Oxytocin, vasopressin, and autism: is there a
connection? Biol Psychiatry 1999; 45:145 – 157.
I. INTRODUCTION
The authors state that all four criteria should be present to make the provisional
diagnosis of ASD in the very young child. It is important to note that three of the
four are social skill criteria. Recognizing the importance of social skills in
defining and detecting ASD in very young children, this chapter will focus on
social skill deficits and how they impact later development of language and play
skills. A short discussion of additional DSM criteria in the language and
restricted, repetitive play domains, as well as brief discussions regarding physical
characteristics, motor development, savant skills, and autism regression variant,
will follow.
“Social skills” as discussed in this chapter are defined more loosely than
in the DSM-IV. Indeed, behaviors described in this chapter overlap with some
of the DSM-IV criteria listed in the language and repetitive/restricted play
domains. Additionally, deficits in social skills negatively impact the later deve-
lopment of language and cognitive skills. Whereas very early social skills
depend on facial and gestural interactions, later ones depend on the child’s
ability to verbalize. Social development parallels cognitive development in
normal or globally delayed children; in children with ASD, the development of
social skills is characteristically “out of sync” with the child’s overall level of
functioning. This discrepancy between social and general levels of functioning
is one of the most important defining criteria of ASD. The discrepancy can be
first noted in infants aged 8 – 12 months, but it becomes more evident as the
child approaches 18 – 24 months. The purpose of this chapter is not to discuss
the social skills as strictly defined by the DSM-IV, but rather to discuss a
more inclusive set of social-emotional characteristics that impact not only
social skill development but also language, cognitive, and play development.
Ultimately, the goal is to help the clinician recognize the expanded range of
social deficits to raise the index of suspicion for ASD at an earlier age than
would occur if one focuses only on failure to achieve later-occurring language
milestones. This is extremely important as recent literature has demonstrated
that children with ASD, who are recognized early and referred to an appro-
priate and intensive intervention program, improve and demonstrate decreased
symptomatology (7 –9).
In the past, the definitive diagnosis of autistic disorder has usually not
been made until after 3 years of age, more likely between 4 and 6 years of age.
Howlin (10,11) demonstrated that parents usually become concerned by 18
months, but do not present to the child’s primary care provider with these
concerns until 6 months later. In one study published in 1997, over 50% of
parents were reassured and told “not to worry.” The usual interval between first
parental concern and the final definitive diagnosis was approximately 4 years.
Denial, lack of familiarity with the spectrum of distinguishing characteristics
that define ASD, and scarce subspecialty resources, among other reasons,
contributed to late diagnosis.
Parents usually first become concerned when they realize that their child’s
expressive language is delayed. Indeed, this has been the historical hallmark of
the disorder and will likely continue to be so as these deficits are easily
recognized. Although earlier social skill deficits are now better known in
professional circles, they are not as easily recognized by parents. There is one
exception. In families where there are two children with ASD, parents often
recognize the early social signs in the younger child prior to any concerns about
language. Indeed, now that experts in the field have begun to focus on these
earlier social deficits, often by the use of screening (e.g., the CHAT) or diagnostic
(e.g., the ADOS) tools that target such skills, the average age of diagnosis has
decreased. These instruments have facilitated a “provisional,” if not definitive,
diagnosis of ASD much earlier. In Europe, it has been reported that these
techniques have resulted in decreasing the average age of diagnosis from 4 years
to 30 months (12). The recent emphasis on social skill deficits has also led to
better ascertainment of children who are high functioning with few language
deficits, i.e., children with Asperger’s syndrome. Indeed, it is both the earlier
recognition of ASD and the recognition of additional children with ASD who are
high-functioning and/or more mildly affected that has been responsible, at least
in part, for the apparent rise in prevalence.
The recognition of the importance of deficits in social skills as defining
characteristics of ASD will not likely represent our final stage in the evolution of
our understanding of this disorder (Table 1). Our understanding of autism has
“come a long way,” especially during the 1990s. It has moved away from the
belief that most children with autism are nonverbal, make no eye contact, sit in
the corner, and engage in bizarre stereotypies. It is now known that more subtle
deficits in social skills, particularly in joint attention, may indicate a disorder
somewhere “on the spectrum.” But the journey is not yet over. At the time of this
publication, a multicenter study designed to evaluate signs in even younger
infants is in progress, but results are not available. The “Baby Sibs Project” has
been expanded from its original study site in Canada under the leadership of
Lonnie Zwaigenbaum to a multicenter project with international funding (l3).
The goal of this project is to identify ASD in siblings (between the ages of 3 and 6
months) of children with known ASD. Siblings have an increased incidence of
ASD—3– 9% when there is one older sibling with ASD (l4) and up to 25% when
there are two (15). The study infants will be prospectively observed for the above
distinguishing early social deficits as well as for new, potentially unknown early
behavioral signs of ASD.
As noted above, abnormalities in social relatedness are now the sine qua non of
autism; however, these are rarely defined (16). Rogers and Benneto suggest that it
is a kind of “interpersonal synchrony of bodies, voices, movements, expressions
and . . . complementary feeling states. It is the interpersonal coordination that
people feel, see and hear through the matching of their movements (face, body,
voice) with those of their social partners.” Infants with other disabilities, for
example cerebral palsy or blindness, may have difficulties with this synchrony
due to their respective motor and visual deficits; however, they do manage to
maintain social connectedness by using compensatory strategies. Infants who are
blind use voice, touch, and language through which emotions can be shared and
connectedness experienced. Those with severe cerebral palsy establish social
relatedness through eye contact, facial expressions, sounds, and conversations.
With time both partners ignore the asynchronies and attend to whatever
interpersonal coordinations are indeed present. Children with autism make no (or
very few) such attempts to compensate.
A. Joint Attention
The single most distinguishing characteristic of very young children with ASD is
a deficit in “joint attention.” Currently it is thought to be associated with
abnormalities in the amygdala. It is a core feature of the DSM-IV and includes a
limited inclination to share enjoyment, interests, or achievements with other
people. Joint attention is the triadic ability to coordinate one’s own attention
between an object and another person. It is dependent on four developmental
components: 1) orienting and attending to a social partner, 2) coordinating
attention between people and objects, 3) sharing affect or emotional states with
people, and 4) ultimately being able to draw others’ attention to objects or events
for the purpose of indicating a need of sharing experiences. Children with autism
may have difficulty with all of these components. Mastery of joint attention
reflects a child’s ability and motivation to share in mental states of others.
Joint attention serves both a communicative and a social function. In its
purely communicative function, it may be used to regulate the behavior of others
to get them to do something (request) or to stop doing something (protest).
Although children with ASD may occasionally use gestures to accomplish this,
they rarely alternate eye gaze between the object and the person’s face. When
they do happen to look at the adult’s face, they neither follow his/her gaze nor
use the adult’s facial expressions to influence their own behavior (see “Social
Orienting,” below). Even more rare is the use of joint attention for the pure social
function of drawing another’s attention to an object or event out of mere interest
(as in to comment or label).
with ASD rarely masters this skill at the usual time. Instead he is more likely to
take the caregiver’s hand and lead him to the object, for example, to the
refrigerator if he is hungry or thirsty. At that point the caregiver must open the
refrigerator door and guess what the child wants by offering him various items
and asking, “Is this what you want?” The child will then grasp the desired object.
Some children develop certain self-help skills (such as opening a refrigerator
door, climbing onto cabinets) at an advanced rate to circumvent the need to solicit
help. Another possibility might involve a more primitive pointing stage whereby
the child opens and closes his hand in repetitive grasping motion in the direction
of the refrigerator and cries or whines. There is little or no eye contact with the
caregiver. Some consider this to be a transitional skill leading to later more
mature protoimperative pointing.
At 14– 16 months of age, in a typically developing child, pointing and
accompanying verbalization may take on a new function. Instead of being used as
a “command,” pointing is used to “comment” or to point out an object/event of
interest. This type of gesturing is more social in nature and is often called
“protodeclarative pointing.” The same triad exists (child, caregiver, object), but
the goal is reversed. The child sees something of interest, perhaps a helicopter
flying overhead. He points to the sky and may vocalize to some degree
(depending on his language level) to get the caregiver’s attention. He will then
alternatively look at the object and the caregiver to make sure the caregiver has
seen the object of interest. The reward is the caregiver’s approval either by
smiling at the child or through some type of verbalization that acknowledges the
object of interest. Children with autism consistently fail to demonstrate
protodeclarative pointing skills at age-appropriate times. If and when these skills
finally emerge, there is often a qualitative difference in that the child is less likely
to show positive affect during acts of joint attention. Also around 14– 16 months,
children should master the social skill of “showing.” This occurs when the
child has found something or made something (a scribble on a piece of paper) and
holds it out to the parent as if to say, “Look at this!” This act is to be distinguished
from bringing an item to the parent to get help, for example, bringing a bottle of
bubbles to the parent and putting his/her hand on the lid to indicate that he wants
it opened. Although this skill is also often delayed in children with ASD, it is not
a pure joint-attention milestone. Instead, it is a “reenactment.” Reenactments can
be motor, as just described, or verbal (see discussion of echolalia, below). In
reenactment attempts, the child repeats an event to make it happen again. It may
serve as a building block and actually herald the emergence of joint-attention
skills in some children (17).
The ability to communicate using gestural joint attention (showing or
pointing to direct attention) emerges before words in typical development and
appears to be a core deficit in autism that impedes later functional language
development. Joint-attention skills (but not global social skills) appear to be
ToM, theory-of-mind.
proportionate to language skills including the correct use of I and you pronouns
(18,19). In one longitudinal study, its emergence was shown to be a significant
predictor of functional language development approximately 1 year later (9,20).
In this regard, many researchers consider joint attention a “pivotal skill”; that is,
its mastery leads to collateral changes in a broader range of deficits within the
autism phenotype. This recognition has led to important advances in early
intervention strategies. Several groups are developing intervention strategies that
target the development of joint attention and prelinguistic communication skills
(21). In one study (22), interventionists presented children with unpredictable or
predictable social stimulation. In the unpredictable condition, the teacher played
with toys that did not match the toy play activities of the child. In the predictable
condition, the teacher imitated the toy play of the child. Children with ASD
displayed more joint attention in the predictable (imitated) condition than in the
unpredictable condition. Another study (23) revealed that children with ASD are
more likely to learn new words when the parent “tunes in” to (or joins attention
with) what the child is looking at than when parents attempt to direct the child’s
gaze to an object that the parent is focusing on and labels. Said differently,
language comprehension seemed to be proportional to the frequency of maternal
follow-in and not to maternal-directed language (24). Once language is
established, the older child is able to maintain joint attention with pure
conversation regardless of visual or gestural cues (e.g., telling a parent about
what happened in school) (25). The importance of joint attention as a core deficit
in ASD was further substantiated in a larger study that demonstrated limitations
in joint attention as linked to not only communication but also to deficits in play,
emotional responsiveness, and peer interactions (26). Other studies have shown
that joint attention is a necessary precursor for the development of theory of mind
(see below) (27). Joint-attention deficits also appear to be specific to ASD. In one
study, joint-attention skills were evaluated in children with Down syndrome and
autism (matched for nonverbal cognitive ages of 18 months). Children with
Down syndrome performed normally (as predicted by their mental age); those
with autism did not. Joint-attention deficits reliably differentiate children with
ASD from children with other neurodevelopmental disorders (28) and is a core
DSM-IV criteria for autism. Currently deficits in joint attention is believed to be
associated with abnormalities in the amygdala.
B. Social Orienting
Social orienting is the ability to orient to social stimuli, in particular, turning to
respond to one’s own name (29,30). Although it was recognized as a core deficit
in ASD somewhat later than joint attention, social orienting emerges earlier in
typical development and may actually influence the emergence of gestural joint
attention (31). Most children will turn preferentially when their name is called at
about 8– 10 months of age. Although some children with ASD might sporadically
respond, most do not do so consistently. In fact, one of the early concerns of
parents of children with ASD is a potential hearing impairment. They are puzzled
because their child seems to hear quite well in some situations but not in others.
This dichotomy occurs because children with ASD often attend to environmental
sounds extremely well but tend to ignore sounds generated by humans such as
calling the child’s name and other speech utterances (32). Again, using children
with Down syndrome as a control group, it was found that children with ASD
more often failed to orient to both social (name being called) and nonsocial
stimuli (a musical jack-in-the-box being played or rattle being shaken); however,
the difference was significantly more extreme for social stimuli. Furthermore, it
was found that social orienting, but not object orienting, was significantly related
to joint attention among the children with autism (33).
Several studies have evaluated infant behavior, especially in regard to
responding to name. These have involved the retrospective evaluation of 1-year-
old birthday videos in children later diagnosed as having ASD.
bottle and then, spontaneously, lie the doll down and cover her with a cloth.
Depending on the child’s language level, she might even say, “all done,” “night
night” (examples of giant words), or “baby tired” (true two-word phrase). Both
types of complex pretend play represent a higher order of play that is consistently
absent in 18-month-old children with ASD. When one evaluates a child’s ability
to engage in pretend play, using tiny pieces of real food or a play bottle
containing an outer layer of milky liquid that appears to pour is discouraged since
this might not represent true symbolic play. Additionally, some children already
in intervention might have been “taught” to engage in certain pretend play
“routines.” These activities might be done repetitiously during daily therapy
sessions. However, until a “pretend play routine” can be generalized to other
situations, it should not be scored as “mastered.” Although, children with ASD
can be taught to imitate pretend play, no 20-month-old with ASD in at least one
study produced spontaneous pretend play (40). Novelty is important when
assessing a child’s skills, especially when he or she is already receiving formal
intervention services.
Imaginative play is the next level of play and is more sophisticated. True
imaginative play is not usually evident until well after the second birthday.
Emergence of this stage of play is more variable. When evaluating a child’s
ability to engage in imaginative play, no concrete representative or generic object
is used to engage the child. For example, as described in one evaluation tool
(ADOS), the child is asked to pretend to brush his teeth (41). The examiner draws
an imaginary circle with her finger on the tabletop to represent a sink. She then
points to an imaginary toothbrush and tube of toothpaste and says to the child,
“Show me how you would brush your teeth.” The typically developing child
would then “pretend” to pick up the imaginary brush and the tube of toothpaste,
open the lid, squeeze the paste out onto the brush, turn the water on, and brush his
teeth. Although some children with high-functioning ASD eventually master this
skill, albeit at a later age, many children with ASD never do.
Many children with severe autism never progress past the sensorimotor
play stage. They mouth and manipulate objects in stereotypic ways. They may
also twirl, bang, and throw objects. Often their favorite play toys are not typical
popular toys, but are instead string, sticks, rocks, ballpoint pens, books (for
carrying around, not reading), etc. Perhaps the only popular real toy is the puzzle,
especially shape-matching puzzles. Some children with ASD are quite proficient
in constructive play (e.g., using objects in combination to create a product, such
as stacking blocks, nesting cups, putting puzzles together, or solving computer
“puzzle” games) (42). Constructive play mastery depends on trial-and-error
problem solving and not imitation or observation of others. Children with ASD
excel at behaviors that do not depend on social interaction but can be instead
learned through trial and error. It is not uncommon for parents to state that the
child is an unusually “good” child, content to play by himself for hours, requiring
little or no attention from the parent. Often this “play” is either constructive
(puzzles, computer games, blocks), ritualistic (lining objects up or sorting/
matching shapes or colors), or sensorimotor in nature.
In addition to classifying play according to its developmental level, play
can be described in several other ways that are helpful in differentiating children
with ASD:
Play logistics: Is the child’s play ritualistic, functional, or creative?
Communicative aspects of play: Through play, is the child attempting to
communicate in some way?
Social characteristics of play: Is the play done in isolation, parallel to other
children, or is it interactive?
Children with autism often engage in ritualistic play with little or no
communicative or social intent. It is carried out in isolation of other children or in
parallel (side-by-side another child) with little interaction. However, these
children often do like (and may appear to even be interactive during) chase games
and roughhousing. It is the sensorimotor aspects of these active games that are
appealing to the child and not the interactive or social aspects.
an indication of fear in her facial expression. His facial expression will usually
mimic hers although he may not understand the full implication of the situation.
A child with ASD often fails to look at his mother’s facial expression and even so,
if he does, he engages in less imitation. Children with ASD are less likely to
imitate social behaviors of others overall. Such deficits in social referencing and
orienting have been found to correlate with measured deficits in joint attention.
Children who display a greater capacity to coordinate and imitate affect (social
referencing) are more likely to communicate for social reasons (44).
taking, children with ASD have difficulties with social emotional behaviors such
as empathy, sharing, and comforting. ToM is important in achieving acceptable
social functioning to maintain social relationships. Prior to the 1990s, it was felt
that children were largely unaware of the existence of states of mind until they
achieved a mental age of approximately 7 years; however, now it is generally
accepted that children have some beginning awareness of the mental states of
others around 3– 4 years of age (61). Although some feel it is one of the core
deficits defining ASD, its late appearance cannot explain the earlier deficits in
joint attention, social orienting, social referencing, etc. Instead these deficits
seem to be primary and likely contribute to subsequent impairments in ToM (62).
Its late appearance also prevents it from being helpful in the diagnosis of
ASD in the very young child, but may indeed be helpful, even critical, in the
diagnosis of the later-appearing Asperger’s syndrome. Besides being dependent
on the development of cognitive skills at approximately a 4-year-old level,
demonstration of intact ToM skills requires some language.
Hypothetically, ToM skills are dependent on a special type of cognition
called metarepresentation. Metarepresentational ability allows one to mentally
depict the psychosocial status of others: it involves the capacity of one individual
to mentally represent the mental representations of another individual. According
to the ToM theory, a disturbance in this metarepresentational thought process
gives rise to pragmatic language deficits seen in persons with ASD. This is also
linked to difficulties with understanding figures of speech (idioms like “two heads
are better than one”) and/or the communicative intent of an author who might
write headlines stating, “Iraqi Head Seeks Arms.” Without mastery of ToM, one
would also have difficulty gauging the constraints of discourse and perceiving the
informational needs of others. The fact that ToM skills are closely linked to
pragmatic functioning is also evident in very early prelinguistic gestural language
development. Whereas normal children develop requesting and commenting
skills concurrently, children with ASD develop the more social commenting
pointing skills significantly later than requesting ones (63). Commenting skills
are dependent on the ability to attribute attention to others.
ToM deficits are not specific to children with ASD. They have also been
found to be delayed in children with severe hearing impairment and in some
children with Down syndrome (64). However, it is important to note that neither
group demonstrated deficits in joint attention or social referencing. Unlike
persons with hearing impairment or Down syndrome, who eventually develop
some degree of ToM as their social experiences and mental age increase, adults
with autism and intelligence in the average range retain their inability to decouple
or segregate their own thoughts from others. Because ToM is the ability to infer
states of mind based on external behavior, Baron-Cohen (65) coined the term
“mindblindedness” to represent the most severe level of ToM deficit as seen in
persons with ASD.
False belief paradigms: One of the most popular test paradigms for 4– 5-
year-olds is the Sally-Anne Disbelief Test (Fig. 1). In this test situation, a
child is asked to watch “Sally” hide a toy in a box. Sally then leaves the
room, and Anne enters. She then moves the toy from the box and puts it
in a different covered container. Sally is then asked to return and the child
is asked, “Where will Sally look for the object?” To answer this question
correctly, the child must be able to disregard or set aside his own
knowledge about where the toy really is and think about where Sally
thinks the toy is. People with ASD manifest robust difficulty with false
belief and related ToM tasks as compared to language- and IQ-matched
controls (65). An alternative false belief task simply requires a Band-Aid
box, Band-Aids, and wrapped strips of chewing gum. It is logistically
less complex and does not require a “Sally” or “Anne,” which makes it
easier to implement in clinical practice. The clinician asks the parents to
leave the room momentarily. He/she then shows the box containing
Band-Aids to the child. He removes the Band-Aids, substitutes them with
strips of gum, and closes the lid. He gives the box to the child and says,
“Let’s bring the box to your parents and ask them what’s inside the box!
What do you think they will say?” Whereas most typically developing
4-year-old children will realize that their parents would expect to find
Band-Aids, the child with ASD is more likely to say “gum.”
Strange stories: Individuals are read short stories depicting each of the
following: pretend, joke, lie, white lie, double bluff, figure of speech,
irony persuasion, and a control (physical story). Afterward one is asked if
the story is true and why the person said what he/she did (66). Persons
with ASD often give quite different answers than other populations
owing to their inability to understand humor or idioms. A mental age of
at least 6 years is required.
Faux pas vignettes: A series of stories have been developed for children
with mental ages of 9 years and above. For example, one story describes
a mother and her daughter making an apple pie as a gift to welcome a
new neighbor. When it is baked, they box it and visit the neighbor. The
neighbor very graciously accepts the gift, saying, “A pie, how very
thoughtful of you. I just love pies, all except apple pies that is.” A set of
questions follows each vignette to determine whether or not the listener
understood the faux pas (67).
Judging mental states: One is asked to interpret the mental or emotional state of
others by observing their facial expression in photos of pairs of eyes. He/she
must choose the word from a list of four that most accurately describes the
person’s emotional state (i.e., angry, serious, happy, afraid) (68).
7. Attachment
Attachment does not seem to be primarily impaired in children with ASD, though
at one time it was thought to be a core feature. Occasionally children with
primary attachment disorders are confused with those having ASD. In a study of a
Romanian orphanage where children received less than optional attention from
multiple-shiftwork caregivers, several children demonstrated quasi-autistic
features and actually met full criteria based on the ADOS (71). However,
symptoms improved once the child was adopted into a nurturing family
environment. In an Ainsworth Strange Situations paradigm, it was demonstrated
that children with ASD seek proximity and contact with their mothers as often as
normal children matched by mental age (72).
8. Summary
The above discussion provides a broad overview of the social deficits in ASD.
Some of these deficits are consistently seen in young children with ASD; others
are not. The goal of this chapter is to help raise the clinician’s awareness of these
characteristics since they are more subtle and/or more difficult to evaluate than
failure to attain language milestones. Hopefully, a better understanding will allow
the clinician to recognize ASD at a younger age and to refer the child to an
appropriate intervention program earlier. It is thought that earlier intervention
utilizing strategies that address these social deficits as well as the more obvious
language deficits and behavior problems will result in better functional outcomes.
Recent outcome studies support this approach (7– 9).
autism are nonverbal, the remainder have some degree of speech, and in a few,
speech may “appear” to be advanced due to echolalia. Children with Asperger’s
syndrome may not demonstrate obvious speech delays but instead have more
subtle abnormalities in language pragmatics. As more and more children are
being diagnosed with milder conditions on the spectrum, those with speech
represent a growing proportion of children with ASD. For a much more in-depth
discussion of language development in children with ASD, the reader is referred
to Chapter 6 in Autism Spectrum Disorders, edited by Amy Wetherby and Barry
Prizant (73).
C. Echolalia
The vast majority of children with ASD who eventually demonstrate functional
symbolic language go through a period of using echolalia. Echolalia is classified
as immediate (child’s parroting occurs immediately after the partner’s
vocalization) or delayed (child’s parroting occurs at a time remote from the
original vocalization). Although a child with ASD may demonstrate both kinds of
echolalia, the delayed form is more distinguishing. Most typically developing
children go through a stage where they imitate other’s speech, particularly the last
one or two words of a sentence. It is usually immediate and occurs when children
are rapidly gaining new words during the “vocabulary burst stage” (73). Autistic
echolalia should be differentiated from normal imitation that occurs during this
period of rapid language acquisition. The echolalia is more exact, has a monotone
quality, and includes larger “chunks” of verbal utterances. It is also associated
with reversal of I and you pronouns since the child repeats the phrase or sentence
exactly as he himself hears it. For example, he may say, “Do you want a drink?”
when he actually is requesting a drink for himself. Additionally delayed echolalia
in children with ASD may include recitation of songs, advertisement jingles,
ABCs, etc. to a degree that far exceeds the child’s functional language ability.
These abilities are sometimes viewed as an indication of genius on the part of
parents and observers.
Echolalia may serve as a verbal type of reenactment (see discussion above
in the section on joint attention) whereby the child repeats a phrase or sentence to
make the event happen again. For example (73): “A 4-year old boy with ASD
repeatedly approached his teacher and stated, ‘Do ahhh’ while opening his
mouth. It was clear by his nonverbal behavior that he was trying to communicate
something, but his teacher was at a loss to understand his meaning. After school,
the teacher called the boy’s mother and explained the dilemma. Without
hesitation, his mother explained that when her son is not feeling well, she tells
him to open his mouth and ‘Do ahhh’ so that she can observe whether his throat is
inflamed. Recently, he had begun to initiate interactions with her using this same
phrase to ‘tell’ her that he was not feeling well. Thus, he used this rather
unconventional gestalt language form that he had come to associate with feeling
ill.” Often these and other reenactments might not be understood if the listener
does not know the history of how the behavior evolved. Reenactments are
performed with some sense of anticipation and may serve as building blocks to
symbolic communication.
In the past, echolalia was considered deviant and not to play a role in the
development of functional language. It is now recognized that most verbal
children with ASD go through a stage of using both immediate and delayed
echolalia. It may be a necessary first step for children with ASD in their unique
journey to meaningful language; they use echolalia to communicate rather than
just parrot. Parents are now encouraged to acknowledge it and help functional
language development by providing context and meaning to echolalic phrases
and sentences. Transforming seemingly meaningless echolalia into functional
language appears to take place in five stages (76):
First the child simply repeats the phrase spontaneously without
understanding its meaning or use.
Then, the child gradually learns the meaning of the phrases by using them
repeatedly and finding out how they “work.” In the example above, once
the parent deduces that “Do you want a drink?” might actually mean that
the child wants a drink, he/she may stop answering “no” and, instead
offers the child a drink while saying, Oh, so [name], you want a drink.
Here is a drink for you, [name], and here is a drink for me, too.
This, in turn, reinforces the utterance and the child begins to use the
sentence/phrase purposefully, yet the pronoun reversal persists since he
can only repeat it as he has heard it.
Next the child starts to break the sentence down into chunks . . . smaller
meaningful units that he might mix and match in various contexts.
Finally he transforms it into a rule-governed language system and he is able to
substitute I for you and make it a personal request: “Can I have a drink?”
Children with ASD who eventually develop functional language tend to develop
grammatical skills in the same general progression as normal children. However,
they lag far behind and, in fact, may never develop an understanding of the social
rules, how to take turns in conversation or how to “read” the listener’s interest or
response to what is being said. These are all aspects of pragmatic language (see
below).
H. Pragmatic Deficits
Some high-functioning children with ASD may have mild or very limited speech
delays and actually demonstrate rather fluent symbolic speech patterns. The only
defining abnormality may be in speech delivery and/or in the social use of
language (pragmatics). Their language may seem odd and not listener-
responsive. Early diagnosis of (later-appearing) Asperger’s syndrome might be
missed unless the clinician and parents have a good understanding of language
pragmatics. The description of the pragmatic components of language came
relatively late to the accumulated fund of knowledge on language development
(78). First described by Bates in 1976, the study of pragmatics revolutionized
language-learning literature (79). (For a comprehensive discussion of pragmatics,
the reader is referred to Chapter 10, in Autism Spectrum Disorders, edited by
Amy Wetherby and Barry Prizant (78). Pragmatics rest on three premises:
1. Speech acts have function; they express intentionality to accomplish a
given purpose. Simple functions might include making requests or
protests; more complex functions include negotiating and expressing
opinions. Although verbal children with ASD may learn to use
language for simple functions, they have much more difficulty with
more complex ones, especially those that require abstract reasoning
and discussion of thoughts and opinions of others.
2. Competent communication requires the speaker to make judgments
about what the listeners already know and what new facts they need to
be given in order to comprehend the intentions of the speaker. For most
people this is automatic and effortless. For those with ASD it is not. For
example, one teen with ASD simply exclaimed, “Florida!” Not
knowing what he meant, his teacher approached him and discovered he
was concerned about a lesion on his leg. It looked like a map of
Florida. Persons without pragmatic deficits would intuitively know that
they needed to provide more information to the listener than simply
“Florida” (80).
3. To engage in cooperative conversational exchanges, one must speak
according to the “rules of discourse.” This may include such things as:
how to choose a topic of conversation, understanding and producing
appropriate facial expression and body language during conversation,
politeness, recognizing when the partner has lost interest in a topic,
knowing when to start, sustain, and end a conversation based on
listener cues, knowing when and how to repair a communication
breakdown, and appropriate tempo and turn taking (78). For example,
a teen with Asperger’s syndrome suffered a minor injury at a fund-
raising event. The physician on duty approached him to inquire of his
status. Instead of answering the inquiries regarding the injury, the teen
Children with ASD often demonstrate little interest in the usual popular childhood
toys, often preferring everyday items such as string, rocks, dirt, feathers, and
chains. They may play with them in a stereotypic, sensorimotor manner for hours
at a time without seeking attention. Sometimes parents will state that the child is
exceptionally “good” and can entertain himself indefinitely, requiring little or no
supervision. Depending on the child’s developmental stage, he may line objects
up, put objects in and out of containers, complete board puzzles, turn pages of
books, or play for hours with constructive toys or computer games. Sometimes
children with ASD are, indeed, interested in typical toys but in unusual ways or in
only their parts. For example, rather than playing with a miniature truck in the
typical way, they instead turn it upside down and spin the wheels repeatedly. Still
others are intensely interested, even obsessed, with certain unusual items such as
fans, light switches, electric cords, etc. Although most children, at some time
during their early development, form attachments with a stuffed animal, a special
pillow, or a “blankee,” children with ASD often prefer hard items (ballpoint pens,
chains, fast-food giveaway toys, etc.). Moreover, the attachment is much more
robust; they may even insist on holding the object most of the day, even during
meals, and protest violently when the object is removed. Children may violently
protest other types of transitions as well, insisting on “sameness” in regard to
events and routines. When forced to change to a different activity or toy, they may
become extremely angry and quickly escalate to a prolonged temper tantrum
characterized by aggression or self-injurious behaviors.
Many children with ASD develop stereotypies (e.g., hand flapping,
twirling, finger movements, rocking, head nodding etc.). Although stereotypies
are often very distinctive and obvious, they are not specific to children with ASD.
They often do not occur until after 3 years of age and thus they are not helpful in
the early diagnosis of ASD. Normal toddlers, especially prior to the onset of
fluent language, sometimes flap briefly when they are excited or frustrated and
children with profound mental retardation and/or severe visual deficits also
demonstrate stereotypies. Somewhat unique to children with ASD, however, is
the demonstration of habitual toe walking and/or sensory stereotypies such as
persistent sniffing and licking of nonfood items.
V. SELF-INJURIOUS BEHAVIOR
It is estimated that 5– 17% of persons with ASD, especially those with comorbid
mental retardation, demonstrate some form of self-injurious behavior (SIB). SIB
is a symptom expressed in many different syndromes and in various forms. It is
also thought to have several etiologies: operant, metabolic, neurochemical (dopa,
serotonin, opioid, and GABA neural transmitter systems have all been
implicated), or anatomical. True SIB usually does not appear until after 5
years of age. A proto-SIB (head banging that is not injurious and, occasionally,
self-biting without breaking skin) occurs prior to 5 years. Reasons for SIB include
(81):
Frustration with skill deficits, primarily in language, whereby the SIB
serves a social-communication function.
Reaction to a stimulus where SIB is used to obtain a tangible, to cope with
an under- or overstimulating environment, to escape from an anxiety-
provoking situation (encroachment on one’s personal space or a
requirement to transition from one activity/toy to another); an
endogenous neurochemical abnormality in dopa, serotonin, opioid, or
GABA neural transmitter systems.
A trait that makes up the behavioral phenotype in specific genetically
determined syndromes (self-picking in Prader-Willi and self-hugging in
Smith-Magenis syndromes).
A chronic health problem: pain, illness, pruritus, sleep deprivation, etc.
Although not at all specific for ASD, “sensory integration” problems have
received much attention in the recent years. [For a comprehensive review of
sensory and motor processing in ASD, the reader is referred to Chapter 6 in this
book and/or Chapter 7 (Understanding the Nature of Communication and
Language Impairments) in Autism Spectrum Disorders, edited by Amy
Wetherby and Barry Prizant (85).] Children with ASD tend to demonstrate
simultaneous hyposensitivities and hypersensitivities for different stimuli even
within the same sensory modality. For example, the slightest sound of water
dripping may provoke a negative response, yet the child seems oblivious to his
mother calling his name loudly. Although part of the explanation may be rooted
to the tendency of children with ASD to selectively tune out social (human)
sounds (see above discussion of social orienting), some degree of physiological
deficit might also be responsible. As noted earlier in this chapter, often one of
the first concerns the parents have is the possibility of deafness. Yet they
eventually deny that the child has a hearing problem since he seems to hear
environmental sounds exceptionally well. Vision does not seem to be as
problematic, but parents will often report that their child explores toys visually
in unusual ways. He may hold the object very close to his eyes, look at the
object out of the corners of his eye, or demonstrate an unusual head tilt (which
would typically raise concerns about visual impairment and/or strabismus).
Additionally, some children seem annoyed with fluorescent lights and if verbal,
they might complain that the light makes things “jump around.” Although motor
stereotypies are seen in other disabilities, particularly severe mental retardation
and blindness, children with ASD demonstrate somewhat unique sniffing and
licking stereotypies. On the other hand, children with ASD may have oral
aversions and intolerance to certain textures that contribute to self-imposed
restricted diets. In addition to oral aversions, many children demonstrate “tactile
defensiveness” and are intolerant of soft touch and various clothing textures.
Conversely, they may be indifferent to significant injuries and other noxious
stimuli that would typically be quite painful to children without ASD. The
dichotomy is puzzling, but most experts feel that it is due to an abnormal arousal
level or sensory gating system in the cerebellum and/or brainstem. Recently, the
frontal cortex has been the focus of attention for its abnormal minicolumns
possibly resulting in less “insulation” of neural transmissions thus causing
overstimulation (86). Abnormalities in the gating of stimuli are thought to
account for two patterns of children with autism: (a) the hyporeactive child with
a high sensory threshold requiring excessive sensory input to achieve activation
of the system and (b) the hyperreactive child with a low sensory threshold who
is often overly focused on detail. Understanding these patterns has implications
for medical intervention, with SSRIs possibly being more helpful in the former
and stimulants in the latter (87).
One isolated video study of very young infants who were later diagnosed with
ASD (88) demonstrated unusual movement abnormalities of the mouth, trunk,
and extremities. Some investigators feel that these abnormalities may herald
the praxic deficits noted to be present in many older children with ASD.
Praxis refers to the planning, execution, and sequencing of movements (89).
Apraxia (severe deficits) and dyspraxia (milder deficits) are linked to deficits in
imitation and cannot be linked to neuromotor pathology. Praxic deficits affect
the imitation of speech, facial expressions, play, and/or motor patterns of
the extremities. Most children with ASD exhibit less imitation. In those
children with comorbid praxic deficits, imitation quality as well as quantity is
impaired; the movements appear awkward and inaccurate. The children are
often labeled as clumsy and uncoordinated, which seems to contradict earlier
reports that children with ASD usually had advanced gross motor skills.
Dyspraxia is not unique to ASD; oral-motor dyspraxia has long been known
to be a contributing factor to speech delay in children without ASD. Both
groups have difficulties imitating sequenced oral-motor movements (e.g.,
imitating “da-pa-ka” rapidly) necessary for fluent speech in spite of intact
neurological and oral cavity exams. Praxis problems also prevent automatic,
smooth synchronous, continuous motor matching of a partner and result in
problems in timing, speed, and grading of movements (90). Praxia has been
conceptualized as encompassing three steps: (l) ideation (formulating the
goal), (2) motor planning (figuring out how to accomplish the goal), and (3)
execution (the actual carrying out of the planned action) (85). Deficits can
occur at any step and result in failure. For example, when faced with the task
of playing in a carpeted play tunnel, a child must first develop some concept
that it is possible to crawl through a tunnel. Children with ASD often had
difficulty with this first step, ideation. They have little idea of the goal for
play in a tunnel. Others may have a deficit in the next step, motor planning;
although they desire to go through the tunnel, they cannot figure out how to
accomplish the task owing to a poor sensorimotor awareness. Still others will
be unable to execute the activity owing to poor motor control and
coordination. These children may also demonstrate gross and fine motor
delays.
In addition to abnormal quality of motor actions, children with ASD may
demonstrate abnormal amounts of activity. Some may appear to be “hyperactive”
and motor-driven with an exterior focus of attention. Others may be hypoactive,
move little, seem to have an interior focus of attention, and are withdrawn. This
may impact decisions regarding medication management at a later time. Whereas
the former may respond to stimulants, the latter may more likely respond to a
selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor.
As noted in the introduction to this chapter, ASD is not associated with a classic
physical phenotype. Fewer than 25% of children will have a comorbid genetic
syndrome and thus will manifest the physical signs that are characteristic for that
syndrome (91) (see Chapter 6). Most children have idiopathic autism and are
not dysmorphic. Approximately 25 –30% of children develop postnatal-onset
macrocephaly that is not associated with ventricular pathology (92). A few
studies have reported children with slightly posterior rotated ears.
XI. CONCLUSION
REFERENCES
1. Rogers SJ, Pennington BF. A theoretical approach to the deficits in infantile autism.
Dev Psychopathol 1991; 3:137– 162.
2. Rogers SJ, Benneto L. Intersubjectivity in Autism. In: Wetherby AM, Prizant BM,
eds. Autism Spectrum Disorders. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.,
2000:79 – 107.
3. Mundy P, Sheinkope S. Social behavior and the neurology of autism. Int Pediatr
1993; 8:205 – 210.
4. Travis L, Sigman M. Social deficits and interpersonal relationships in autism. Ment
Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev 1998; 4:65– 72.
5. Minshew N, Goldstein G. Autism as a disorder of complex information processing.
Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev 1998; 4:129– 136.
6. Stone WL. Can autism be diagnosed accurately in children under 3 years? J Child
Psychol Psychiatry 1999; 40:219– 226.
7. Dawson G, Osterling J. Early intervention in autism. In: Guralnick JM, ed. The
Effectiveness of Early Intervention. Baltimore, MD: Paul H Brookes Publishing Co.,
1997:307 – 326.
8. Hurth J, Sahw E, Izeman SG, Shaley K, Rogers SJ. Areas of agreement about
effective practices among programs serving young children with autism spectrum
disorders. Infants Young Child 1999; 12:17– 26.
9. Committee on Education Interventions for Children with Autism (Chair: Catherine
Lord). Educating Children with Autism. Washington, DC: National Academy of
Science, 2001.
10. Howlin P, Moore A. Diagnosis of autism: a survey of over 1200 patients in the UK.
Autism 1997; 1:135– 162.
11. Howlin P, Asgharian A. The diagnosis of autism and Asperger syndrome: findings
from a survey of 770 families. Dev Med Child Neurol 1999; 41:834– 839.
12. Fombonne E. The epidemiology of autism: a review. Psychol Med 1999; 29:769 –
786.
13. Zwaigenbaum L. Baby sibs studies expanded. Narrative 2002; Fall:7.
14. Bailey A, Le Courteur A, Gottesman I. Autism as a strongly genetic disorder:
evidence form a British twin study. Psychol Med 1995; 25:63– 77.
32. Leekam S, Lopez B. Attention and joint attention in preschool children with autism.
Dev Psychol 2000; 36:261 – 273.
33. Dawson G, Meltzoff A, Osterling J. Children with autism fail to orient to naturally
occurring social stimuli. Paper presented at the meeting of the Society for Research
in Child Development, Indianapolis, IN, March 1995.
34. Osterling J, Dawson G. Early recognition of children with autism: a study of first
birthday home videotapes. J Autism Dev Disord 1994; 24:247 – 257.
35. Osterling J, Dawson G. Early recognition of infants with autism versus mental
retardation. Poster presented at the meeting of the society for research in child
development. Albuquerque, NM, 1999.
36. Baranek GT. Autism during infancy: a retrospective video analysis of sensory-motor
and social behaviors at 9– 12 months of age. J Autism Dev Disord 1999;
29:213 – 224.
37. Maestro S, Muratori F, Cristina M, Pei F, Stern D, Golse B, Palacia-Espasa F.
Attentional skills during the first 6 months of age in autism spectrum disorder. J Am
Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2002; 41:1239– 1245.
38. Lord C. Follow-up of two year-olds referred for possible autism. J Child Psychol
Psychiat Allied Discipl 1995; 36:1365 – 1382.
39. Mundy P, Sigman M, Ungere J, Sherman T. Nonverbal communication and play
correlates of language development in autistic children. J Autism Dev Disord 1987;
17:349 – 364.
40. Charman T, Wettenham J, Baron-Cohen S. An experimental investigation of
cognitive abilities in infants with autism: clinical implications. Infant Mental Health
J 1998; 19:260– 275.
41. Lord C, Risi S, Lambert L, et al. The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule –
Generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the
spectrum of autism. J Autism Dev Disord 2000; 30:205 –223.
42. Wetherby AM, Prizant BM, Hutchinson T. Communicative, social-affective, and
symbolic profiles of young children with autism and pervasive developmental
disorder. Am J Speech Lang Pathol 1998; 7:79– 91.
43. Dawson G, Hill D, Spencer A, Galpert L, Watson L. Affective exchanges between
young autistic children and their mothers. J Abnorm Child Psychol 1990; 18:335–345.
44. Greenspan SI, Wieder S. Developmental patterns and outcomes in infants and
children with disorders in relating and communicating: a chart review of 200 cases of
children with autistic spectrum diagnoses. J Dev Learning Disord 1997; 1:87– 114.
45. Courchesne E, Yeung-Courchesne R, Press G, Hesselink JR, Jernigan TL.
Hypoplasia of cerebellar vermal lobules VI and VII in infantile autism. N Engl J
Med 1988; 318:1349– 1354.
46. Courchesne E. Infantile autism. Part 2. A new neurodevelopmental model. Int
Pediatr 1995; 10:155.
47. Courchesne E, Hesselink JR, Jernigan TL, Yeung-Courchesne R. Abnormal
neuroanatomy in a non-retarded person with autism: unusual findings with magnetic
resonance imaging. Arch Neurol 1987; 44:335 – 341.
48. Courchesne E, et al. Impairment in shifting attention in autistic and cerebellar
patients. Behav Neurosci 1994; 108:848 – 865.
83. Szatmari P, Mérette C, Bryson SE, Thivierge J, Roy MA, Cayer M, Maziade M.
Quantifying dimensions in autism: a factor-analytic study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc
Psychiatry 2002; 41:467 – 474.
84. Ozonoff S, McEvoy RE. A longitudinal study of executive function and theory of
mind development in autism. Dev Psychopathol 1994; 6:415 – 431.
85. Anzalone ME, Williamson G. Sensory processing and motor performance in autism
spectrum disorders. In: Wetherby AM, Prizant BM, eds. Autism Spectrum Disorders.
Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co., 2000:143 – 166.
86. Casonova P. Abnormal mini-columns in individuals with autism spectrum disorder.
Neurology 2002; 58:438 – 444.
87. Anzalone ME, Williamson G. Sensory processing and motor performance in autism
spectrum disorders. In: Wetherby AM, Prizant BM, eds. Autism Spectrum Disorders.
Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co., 2000:148 – 149.
88. Teitelbaum P. Movement analysis in infancy may be useful for early diagnosis of
autism. Proc Natl Acad Sci 1998; 95:1 – 6.
89. Rogers SJ, Benneto L. Intersubjectivity in autism. In: Wetherby AM, Prizant BM,
eds. Autism Spectrum Disorders. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.,
2000:84.
90. Rogers SJ, Benneto L. Intersubjectivity in autism. In: Wetherby AM, Prizant BM,
eds. Autism Spectrum Disorders. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.,
2000:101.
91. Gilberg C, Coleman M. Autism and medical disorders: a review of the literature. Dev
Med Child Neurol 1996; 38:191 – 202.
92. Bailey A. Luthert P, Bolton P, Le Courteur A, Rutter M, Harding B. Autism and
megalencephaly. Lancet 1993; 341:1225 – 1226.
93. Tuchman RF, Rapin I. Regression in pervasive developmental disorders: seizures
and epileptiform electroencephalogram correlates. Pediatrics 1997; 99:560 – 566.
94. van Gent T, Heijnen CJ, Treffers PD. Autism and the immune system. J Child
Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discipl 1997; 38:337 – 349.
95. Cook EH Jr, Perry BD, Dawson G, Wainwright MS, Leventhal BL. Receptor
inhibition by immunoglobulins: specific inhibition by autistic children, their
relatives, and control subjects. J Autism Dev Disord 1993; 23:67– 77.
Pasquale J. Accardo
Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, Virginia, U.S.A.
I. INTRODUCTION
Several clinical practice guidelines for the screening and diagnosis of autism and
autistic spectrum disorders are fairly similar in their components, and any variations
in the process reflect their target audience. Guidelines drafted by the American
Academy of Pediatrics (1) are intended for pediatricians; those by the American
Academy of Neurology (2,3), for child neurologists; those by the American Academy
of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry (4), for child psychiatrists; and those by the
New York State Department of Health (5), for professionals from diverse disciplines
as well as for parents. The present chapter will assume some familiarity with such
protocols and attempt to provide a rationale for the major steps in this clinical process.
Familiarity with the more traditional components of routine pediatric health care
provision as well as with child developmental surveillance will also be assumed. The
emphasis will be on how the medical practitioner can most effectively contribute.
The American Psychiatric Association’s DSM-IVTR (6) and the American
Academy of Pediatrics DSM-PC (7) place autism within the category of pervasive
developmental disorders (PDD). This category includes five separate conditions:
Autism occurs when the child displays significant impairment in all of three
separate areas of functioning: communication, socialization, and
repetitive, self-stimulatory, and restrictive behaviors, and these
impairments have their onset before the age of 3 years (Table 1).
The numbers in the last paragraph do not refer to footnotes but to the table.
Source: Refs. 6, 7.
All children who are receiving well-child health care have their developmental
progress routinely evaluated at each visit. The physician is less concerned to be
intimately familiar with all aspects of a given child’s developmental progress
than to identify significant developmental delays and neurological diagnoses that
warrant biomedical assessment and both general and specific intervention
strategies. From this perspective, screening procedures can be situated within a
larger framework that rationally delineates their use.
Focusing on which conditions can be diagnosed at specific ages can help
further to limit the investment of time needed to screen for different conditions.
With the partial effectiveness of early (prior to age 60 months) intensive
behavioral intervention strategies in the treatment of ASD (8), the importance of
early identification is increasingly recognized. Nevertheless, it is not unusual still
to encounter a child with developmental problems in whom the diagnosis of ASD
was not considered or confirmed until after the age of 60 months (9). Parents
usually become concerned by 18 months of age, but do not present to the primary
care provider with these concerns until 6 months later and considerable time
lapses between first parental concern and the final definitive diagnosis (10). In the
current state of knowledge, failure to diagnose ASD earlier is unacceptable.
While researchers pursue the diagnosis of ASD into the age group prior to 18
months (see Chapter 5), the clinician needs to be comfortably familiar with the
presentation of ASD between the ages of 18 and 36 months. Most children who
will qualify for a diagnosis of ASD will actually develop sufficient signs and
symptoms between 18 and 24 months, and the remainder will do so between 24
and 36 months of age. Eager to initiate therapy by 24 months of age, there is
concern that the age for diagnosis be lowered to 24 months. The average age of
diagnosis in Europe has, indeed, decreased to 30 months from 4 years (11). It has
been suggested that DSM criteria be modified for younger children to include
social skills deficits, such as decreased use of nonverbal behaviors, lack of social
and emotional reciprocity, and lack of seeking to share enjoyment, to account for
unreliability of some DSM criteria in children less than 3 years of age (12).
However, some children with ASD will simply not be diagnosable until closer to
age 36 months, the upper age limit as defined by DSM-IVTR.
Until the development of a screening tool based on the early social signs of
autism (see Chapter 5), it is acceptable for a working approach to screening for
this group of conditions to rely on the DSM system’s characterization of ASD as
disorders with significant impairments in communication, socialization, and
repetitive, stereotypic behaviors. Since the specific and often striking stereotypies
are not pathognomonic, and social interaction is difficult to assess in the pediatric
office setting, the screening process can best start with universal prospective
surveillance of communication skills in children.
Expressive Receptive
12 mo 1 word One-step command
with gesture
14 mo 3 words
Immature jargoning
16 mo 4 words One-step command
without gesture
18 mo 6 words Points to one picture
Points to one body part
Vocabulary explosion
22 mo 2 word phrases
24 mo 50 words Points to a half-dozen
body parts
2 – 3-word sentences Two-step command
30 mo Pronouns Points to a half-dozen
pictures
Children who present with delayed expressive language delay will typically fall
into one of the following broad diagnostic categories:
1. Isolated expressive language delay, slow talkers
2. Communication disorder with delays in both expressive and receptive
language
3. Global cognitive impairment (intellectual deficiency, mental retar-
dation)
4. Hearing impairment
5. ASD
There are a number of clinical observations by which these conditions might be
distinguished in the primary care setting (14). Although such clinical clues might
seem to adequately differentiate one condition from another, it is generally
accepted that any child who presents with some variant of expressive language
delay needs to have a formal audiological assessment and never just a hearing
screening.
The child with delayed expressive but intact receptive language milestones has
some version of isolated expressive language disorder. The child with expressive
and receptive language delays may have a more severe combined communication
disorder, global cognitive impairment such as mental retardation, or ASD.
cords usually reduce past neutral (19). While global cognitive limitation and most
other neurodevelopmental disabilities are frequently associated with some degree
of microcephaly, ASD is more often found with macrocephaly (20,21).
One screening test for ASD that has been proposed is the CHAT (22).
While statistics do not support its general usage, it does highlight several key
behaviors that can be used to help diagnose ASD (Table 5). An obvious weakness
in the construction of the CHAT is simply that many of the items are not specific
to ASD but rather are common to global cognitive impairment or mental
retardation. As the percentage of ASD children with mental retardation
decreases, so does the utility of the test. The CHAT behaviors more specific to
ASD are those that involve pretend play and joint attention (protodeclarative
pointing). (For a more detailed discussion of these behaviors see Chapter 5.) The
use of these markers can help discriminate ASD from other types of
communication disorders. Apart from issues with sensitivity and specificity,
the CHAT may be too long for routine use in the primary-care office setting,
particularly in the American managed-care setting.
Parent questions
1. Does your child enjoy being swung, bounced on your knee, etc.?
2. Does your child take an interest in other children?
3. Does your child like climbing on things such as up stairs?
4. Does your child enjoy playing peek-a-boo/hide-and-seek?
5. Does your child ever pretend, e.g., to make a cup of tea using a toy cup and
teapot, or pretend other things?
6. Does your child ever use his/her index finger to point, to ask for something?
7. Does your child ever use his/her index finger to point, to indicate interest in
something?
8. Can your child play properly with small toys without just mouthing, fiddling,
or dropping them?
9. Does your child ever bring objects over to you to show you something?
Professional observations
1. Has the child made eye contact with you?
2. Get the child’s attention, then point across the room at an interesting object and say,
“Oh look! There’s a. . .!” Watch the child’s face. Does the child look across to
see what you are pointing at? [and not just at your finger]
3. Get the child’s attention, then give a miniature toy cup/teapot, and say, “Can
you make a cup of tea?” Does the child pretend to pour out tea, drink it, etc.?
4. Say to child, “Where’s the light?” or “Show me the light.” Does the child point [with
his/her index finger] to the light?
5. Can the child build a tower of bricks? (If yes, of how many bricks?)
There is no single accepted diagnostic evaluation process for ASD. To answer the
question as to what such a battery should contain, the specific goal of the
diagnostic evaluation must be clarified. Specialists are often looking to answer
questions of little concern to the parents, while parents are often asking questions
that the most extensive test batteries do not address. There are four separate
questions that a diagnostic assessment might attempt to address:
1. Does this child have ASD or not?
2. If the child has ASD, how severe is the condition?
3. What tests are appropriate to investigate possible etiologies for the
ASD?
4. What are the interventions appropriate to this specific child?
1. The response to the first question is a straightforward clinical one. The
clinician familiar with the diagnostic criteria for ASD can usually answer it with
reasonable certainty after a developmental history and a period of observation
and interaction taking something on the order of 1 –2 hr. Not infrequently in more
classic cases the diagnosis can be strongly entertained after only several minutes
of observation. Needless to say, the diagnosis should never be based on such a
quick glance but rather such an initial impression needs to be supported by a
developmental history that confirms such behaviors to be pervasive across
settings and chronic in duration—in other words, that this atypical behavior is
actually typical for this child.
A number of formal ASD screening tools exist: Autism Behavior Checklist
(ABC), Autism Screening Questionnaire (ASQ), Checklist for Autism in
Toddlers (CHAT), Pervasive Developmental Disorder Screening Test (PDDST)
Stage 1, Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ), and Screening Tool for
Autism in Two-year-olds (STAT) (Table 6). Since it is usually not practical to
screen the general population of young children for autism using a specific autism
screening test, it is recommended instead to look for clinical clues (5). The
clinical decision tree outlined here does not allow a major role to the routine
employment of screening instruments.
Formal diagnostic instruments include: Autism Diagnostic Interview –
Revised (ADI-R), Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule– Generic (ADOS-
G), Behavioral Summarized Evaluation (BSE), Childhood Autism Rating Scale
(CARS), Diagnostic Instrument for Social and Communicative Disorders
(DISCO), Gilliam Autism Rating Scale (GARS), Parent Interview for Autism
(PIA), Pervasive Developmental Disorder Screening Test (PDDST) Stages 2 and 3,
Pre-Linguistic Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (PL-ADOS) (Table 7).
The ADI-R and ADOS-G are considered the “gold standard” for such diagnostic
instruments and the CARS has the longest history and the widest use. No single
instrument should be used as the sole basis for diagnosing ASD (5). On the one
hand, the better formal instruments require extensive training and experience; on
the other hand, the instruments are intended for use by practitioners not clinically
experienced in the diagnosis of ASD (3).
The confirmation of the clinical diagnosis of ASD by a formal instrument is
strongly recommended but will be subject to the reasonable availability of
professionals with the requisite training in the administration of such scales.
Reasonable availability can also refer to the fact that a diagnostic medical
evaluation by a child neurologist, child psychiatrist, or developmental pediatrician
may be covered by the family’s health insurance, but the performance of an ASD
rating scale by a child psychologist or other certified professional is rarely
covered. Neither is the requisite training expertise universally available through
either early-intervention programs or the public school system.
ASD are all found within the syndrome group with verbal skills more impaired
than nonverbal skills. Therefore, any attempt to measure general intellectual
ability in ASD children should always use instruments with a strong nonverbal
component such as the Leiter International Performance Scale –Revised (Leiter-
R), the Griffiths Mental Development Scale, the Raven’s Progressive Coloured
Matrices, the Seguin Formboard, and the Clinical Adaptive Test (CAT) of the
Capute Scales. The finding of nonverbal-problem-solving superior to verbal
abilities (e.g., a CAT score above the CLAMS score) should be considered
typical for ASD.
When a child with ASD tests out globally delayed with low scores in both
the verbal and nonverbal areas, the problem will be to differentiate an associated
mental retardation from difficulties in obtaining the cooperation of a child who is
not socially reciprocating. How can one differentiate superior nonverbal skills
that equate with overall better nonverbal intelligence from an isolated “splinter
skill” or two? Sometimes the examiner can note test behaviors that make one of
these two interpretations more probable, but it will often take time for the picture
to sort itself out. The difficulties in interpreting such data should not dissuade one
from obtaining such baseline information. Sometimes the use of the Vineland
Adaptive Behavior Scales can help in this differential diagnosis by providing
developmental age levels for several other functional areas. Modifications in the
use of the Vineland with ASD persons have been published, and this population
will be specifically addressed in the next revision of the instrument. Rating scales
for repetitive, obsessive compulsive, stereotypic, and tic behaviors (23) are less
diagnostic than treatment oriented by providing baseline measures for targeted
behaviors.
3. The pediatric medical evaluation for ASD should review and explore
all those prenatal, perinatal, and postnatal factors associated with the entire
spectrum of neurodevelopmental disabilities.
Since the incidence of seizures is increased in children with ASD (and
tends to increase further with age), electroencephalography needs to be
considered in the diagnostic medical assessment. Children with obvious or
suspicious seizure episodes should receive an electroencephalogram (EEG). This
recommendation does not support the routine use of the EEG.
Concern remains that even in the absence of overt seizure activity,
epileptiform discharges might represent a deleterious influence on certain brain
areas. The classic example of such an association is the Landau-Kleffner
syndrome (or acquired epileptic aphasia) (24). In this syndrome, children develop
a seizure focus directly over the language areas of the brain and lose previously
acquired speech (they develop aphasia). Most cases of Landau-Kleffner
syndrome occur later than the upper age limit for the onset of ASD (3 years),
and most have obvious clinical seizures. There are, however, some cases of
Landau-Kleffner syndrome with only electric discharges and no clinical seizure
activity. A key distinction between the regression that typically occurs in ASD and
the regression that occurs in acquired epileptic aphasia relates to the
accompanying impairment of social interaction and relatedness. In children
who are developing some form of ASD, the language loss is contemporaneous
with the onset of social withdrawal. In children with an acquired epileptic
aphasia, the emotional withdrawal will occur only after the language impairment
has had time to exert a secondary impact on socialization. In this latter case, the
effect and its time course are similar to the socialization difficulties that occur
with significant hearing impairment.
No consistent neuroimaging findings are associated with ASD. The criteria
for obtaining an MRI or CT scan should be the same as with children without
ASD: abnormalities of the head (other than mild macrocephaly), facial
dysmorphology (e.g., midface hypoplasia), positive or localizing findings on
neurological examination, or significant global developmental delay (mental
retardation) (17,25). The last reflects the importance of attempting an assessment
of general intelligence in ASD. Otherwise neuroimaging cannot be considered
routine in ASD.
ASD has been associated with a number of genetic syndromes (Table 8).
Often such syndromes will have been identified prior to the development
and diagnosis of the ASD. For a long time ASD was thought to be relatively
uncommon in children with Down syndrome (trisomy 21). Recent research supports
an incidence of ASD in Down syndrome on the order of magnitude of 10% (26,27).
Sometimes the diagnosis of ASD will lead the clinician to search more
closely for a genetic etiology (Table 9). The most common genetic disorder
associated with autism is fragile X syndrome. In fact it would be more correct to say
that fragile X syndrome is strongly associated with the presence of autistic-like
features and less so with ASD. And more importantly, the primary developmental
association of fragile X syndrome is with mental retardation and not with ASD. A
child who qualifies for a diagnosis of ASD but who is not mentally retarded rarely
needs to be screened for fragile X syndrome. Using a checklist in which each of six
items (mental retardation, family history of mental retardation, ADHD, large ears,
elongated face, autistic-like behaviors) is scored 0 (absent), 1 (borderline), or 2
(present), scores of 5 or more will identify all cases of fragile X syndrome (28,29).
The accumulation of various trace elements and heavy metals in the body
has been claimed to be associated with ASD, and attempts have been made
sometimes to measure their levels in hair, blood, and urine for the purpose of
recommending chelation therapy. With the exception of lead, there is no
scientific support for either the validity of these measures, their association with
ASD, or any responsiveness to the proposed therapy.
Lead poisoning (plumbism) is a disorder most commonly found in children
who engage in pica in settings where interior lead-based paint is flaking from the
walls or ceiling. Children with ASD often engage in pica and are therefore at
Angelman
Bardet-Biedl
Coffin-Siris
Cornelia de Lange
Down
Duchenne muscular dystrophy
Fragile X
Gilles de la Tourette
Goldenhar
Hurler
Hypomelanosis of Ito
Joubert
Laurence-Moon-Biedl
Lugan-Fryns (X-linked mental
retardation with marfinoid habitus)
Möbius
Myotonic dystrophy
Neurofibromatosis I
Noonan
Oculocutaneous albinism
Phenylketonuria
Prader-Willi
Rett
Rubella, congenital
Sanfillippo
Smith-Magenis
Tuberous sclerosis*
Velocardiofacial syndrome*
Williams
XYY
greater risk of developing lead poisoning. Generally the relationship with lead is
considered to be secondary rather than causative, but the possibility remains that
some cases of ASD might be caused or (more likely) exacerbated by toxic levels
of lead (30). Lead screening should be entertained in any medical assessment for
ASD in children with pica and possible exposure to lead-based paint. In addition,
such lead screening may need to be periodically repeated in ASD children with
persistent pica.
Copyright © 2004 Marcel Dekker, Inc.
Screening and Diagnosis 143
Feeding selectivity and tactile aversion are specific behaviors for which
desensitization procedures may be helpful.
VIII. CONCLUSION
Because of the many uncertainties that cloud the field of autism, there is a
tendency to rush to conclude that the latest research finding, instrument, test,
suspected cause, or even risk factor is the final key to having it all make sense.
The vast amount of clinical data that needs to be restructured by new
interpretations cannot yet be jettisoned by an oversimplification of the complex
phenomena that can present as ASD.
Despite the wide prevalence of out-of-home caregiving settings for infants
and young children (e.g., nursery, day care, preschool), the primary-health-care
provider remains the mainstay for early diagnosis of ASD. Given the importance
of early diagnosis for early intervention, the primary-care physician should
specifically rule out ASD at the 18– 36-month checkups. Expressive language
screening accompanied by several probes into social reciprocity can effectively
rule out most cases of ASD and alternately identify those children who need to be
referred for more in-depth assessment. It would also be appropriate for the
primary care physician to raise the possibility of autism with those patients
otherwise identified with various communication disorders or other patterns of
nonspecific developmental delay since it is not unusual for children with ASD to
be identified with neurodevelopmental problems but incorrectly categorized.
Although ASD is a serious neurodevelopmental disorder, the only formal
assessment that is routinely indicated in every case is a careful and
comprehensive audiological assessment. Every other possible medical test and
procedure needs some other specific indication. ASD by itself is not sufficient to
justify an expensive battery of biomedical and behavioral tests. When
accompanied by significant global developmental delay (mental retardation),
ASD becomes open to a much larger range of biomedical considerations.
Measured clinical judgment needs to collaborate with informed parents to select
the best approach to a comprehensive diagnosis.
REFERENCES
25. Filipek PA. Neuroimaging in the developmental disorders: the state of the art. J Child
Psychol Psychiatry 1999; 40:113– 128.
26. Kent L, Evans J, Paul M, Sharp M. Comorbidity of autistic spectrum disorders in
children with Down syndrome. Dev Med Child Neurol 1999; 41:153 –158.
27. Rasmussen P, Borjesson O, Wentz E, Gillberg C. Autistic disorders in down
syndrome: background factors and clinical correlates. Dev Med Child Neurol 2001:
43:750 – 754.
28. Giangreco CA, Steele MW, Aston CE, Cummings JH, Wenger SL. A simplified six-
item checklist for screening for fragile X syndrome in the pediatric population.
J Pediatr 1966; 129:611– 614.
29. Accardo PJ, Shapiro LR. FRAXA and FRAXE: when to test or not to test. J Pediatr
1998; 132:762 – 764.
30. Accardo PJ, Whitman BY, Caul J, Rolfe U. Autism and plumbism: a possible
etiological contribution. Clin Pediatr 1988; 27:41– 44.
31. Bostwick HE, Berezin SH, Halata MS, Jacobson R. Celiac disease presenting with
microcephaly. J Pediatr 2001; 138:589– 592.
32. Gordon N. Cerebellar ataxia and gluten sensitivity: a rare but possible cause of
ataxia, even in childhood. Dev Med Child Neurol 2000; 42:283– 286.
Alfred L. Scherzer
Joan and Sanford I. Weill Medical College, Cornell University, New York,
New York, U.S.A.
While the family plays an important role in the subsequent development of all
children with developmental disabilities, it has a unique and multifaceted
position when the child has autism. The interactive relationship involving
communication and behavior between the family and child is fundamental and
unique in the autistic spectrum disorders (ASD). It serves both as a basis for
alerting the family that something is awry, and an opportunity for involvement in
treatment and management. That a special kind of relationship exists between the
family and the child with ASD is apparent in the literature. Comparison with
family references to other major developmental disabilities from the National
Library of Medicine (PubMed) indicates that only mental retardation approaches
the frequency of ASD entries (Table 1).
The special relationship between a child with ASD and his or her family
has led to the widespread practice of direct parental involvement in the treatment
and management of ASD. The family is specifically an integral part of all the
currently described model treatment programs for children with autistic spectrum
disorder (1,2).
The unique interactive parent involvement in the subsequent development of
the child with ASD can greatly influence outcome. For example, there is a relation
between the development of communication skills of the child and parents’ ability
to synchronize interaction with the child’s attention and activity (3). At the same
time, the greater severity of the child with autism may have considerable impact on
parental stress (4). In turn, this can be related to less social responsiveness as the
child develops. On the other hand, more mutual play and positive feedback by the
parent is seen with more highly communicative children who have ASD (5).
The parents, therefore, are in a pivotal position with respect to the child with
autism. They are involved early because of the fundamental deviations in
behavior and communication, and have a role in identification by bringing their
concerns to the primary care provider. They have a major team role in the very
nature of the therapy and treatment processes that have evolved. At the same time,
it is clear that their interaction with the child in itself may significantly influence
the treatment process and ultimate development. Finally, parental coping ability,
stress, and mental health are all intimately intertwined in the process.
able to perform now and in the future. The process can be a kind of unstated
active negotiation about pessimism versus optimism, on either side, out of which
evolves a jointly constructed diagnosis. Though the physician or other pro-
fessional controls the interaction, there is great need to allow parents to freely
express feelings, doubts, and concerns during this informing process and at any
subsequent meetings (16). Parental dissatisfaction with how the diagnosis is dis-
closed is not inevitable (19 –21). Success requires appropriate understanding,
sensitivity, and technical skill on the part of the physician (9).
Training in “breaking the news” needs to be given much more emphasis in
the medical curriculum, and is increasingly essential in continuing medical
education programs (CME) (7,8). Targeted communication training programs
can be effective in changing physician attitudes (22,23).
In general, the broad steps to guide planning for a successful informing
experience include the following:
1. Planning the setting
2. Assessing the family’s background knowledge and experience
3. Individualizing the strategy of informing
4. Evaluating the family’s understanding
From a consensus of several studies, Table 2 summarizes specific guide-
lines to follow in developing optimum conditions to achieve an effective parent/
professional communication process for discussing the child’s diagnosis.
Of course, these guidelines should also provide the basis for all subsequent
communication with parents, and can help to develop the kind of rapport
necessary for long-term care. Remember that initial disclosure represents but the
first of many opportunities for communicating with parents. Subsequent contacts
will need to keep in mind the strategies outlined above, particularly when further
clinical contacts with the child may result in revisions in prognosis concerning
degree of involvement, functional capability, the presence of comorbidity, or
even the diagnosis itself.
The wide range of strategies used by parents for coping with the child and
dealing with stress includes: authoritarian versus permissive discipline (31),
variable sharing of responsibility with spouse and siblings (41), partnering with
the professional team (42), and even becoming a community advocate (43).
Heightened anxiety relating to unanticipated or delayed changes as the child
develops can also greatly affect both parents and professionals as the child develops
(6). Open exchange on each side is needed when this occurs. The clinician must be
sensitive to the tensions that appear and encourage expression of concerns by
the parent, yet be able to share his own feelings of uncertainty and anxiety. Where
there is need for emotional assistance or treatment of existing psychopathology,
counseling or psychiatric intervention should be strongly recommended.
Periodic respite care also provides an important alternative to ease the
constant demands of daily management, and can greatly relieve the stress to
which many families are often subjected (44).
To begin with, the stress of adjusting to life with a child who has autism, and
coping with long-term care and management, may be associated with significant
psychopathology in some families (38). Compared to normal populations and those
with Down syndrome, mothers of children with autism show depression (36%),
anxiety (46%), or both (9%) (25). Established maternal coping styles, attitudes, and
philosophy are found to be important determinants in later adjustment (26).
On the other hand, fathers are said to show relatively good adaptation,
comparable to those raising children with Down syndrome, but are frequently
under significant stress as well (45). Both parents of children with autism are
reported to experience significantly lower marital intimacy, associated with greater
stress and depression, than those with Down syndrome, or normal children (46).
Siblings, likewise, are at greater risk for emotional and behavioral problems
(47). In addition, preexisting personality abnormality and even psychopathology
is reported in some parents and families of children with autism (48 –50). Parents
have been noted to be more aloof, untactful, and less responsive than those with
children who have Down syndrome (51). Fathers are considered to have more
schizoid and intellectual traits (52,53). Compared to a normal population,
mothers of children with autism showed a greater degree of family problems
during the pregnancy (54). Lifetime prevalence rates for anxiety disorder and
major depressive disorder were likewise found to be greater in the group whose
children have autism (55).
These findings are reported in an effort to alert and sensitize professionals
to potential family mental health issues, rather than in any way to suggest an
etiological relation to autism. In fact, it is concluded in studies reported that there
Discussion elsewhere (see Chapter 12) reviews the major issue of complementary/
alternative medicine (CAM). The use of CAM has been increasing in the United
States over the past 50 years, and today is said to involve more than one-third of
the population (77). A burgeoning number of websites now provide extensive
information on CAM, and are easily accessible (78). The use of CAM is common
and significant among pediatric patients with acute illness seen in primary care
practices, and includes herbs/homeopathic medicines, prayer healing, high-dose
vitamin therapy, nutritional supplements, folk/home remedies, massage therapy,
chiropractic care, biofeedback, self-help groups, relaxation, hypnosis, and
acupuncture or acupressure (79–81). It is estimated that up to 50% of families of
children with autism are using CAM (82). Among the family factors identified in
this practice is maternal age over 31, foreign-born caretakers, religious affiliation,
and the use of CAM by parents (79). And more than 50% of pediatricians in
practice also reported using CAM (83). The extent to which caretakers inform
physicians about their use of CAM is estimated variously (80,84).
While there are many theories concerning the widespread use of CAM in
autism, the clinical basis of the (sometimes changing) diagnosis, massive
amounts and diverse quality of available information, and influence of well-
meaning person-to-person and family contacts are all factors in play. Perhaps
most important is inadequate or failed communication between physician and
family. In one respect it goes back to the initial diagnostic interview when trust,
sharing, and mutual acceptance form the basis for an ongoing relationship. If
successful it will help to shield the family effectively from turning to alter-
natives that either may be of little value or are possibly expensive and even
harmful.
Particularly with the child who has autism, there needs to be a medical
home setting that provides compassionate, family-centered care. An atmosphere
of mutual participation is essential between clinician and family, in which the
range of options for care is mutually explored, and a collaborative decision about
management is based on informed consent (84). This has been termed “shared
decision making and relationship-centered care” (85).
One principle to keep in mind is that the family has the right to choose
CAM, yet it is the physician’s obligation to be aware of any untoward effects and
to communicate them effectively (86). There may not be complete agreement, but
such discussions and how they are handled provide opportunities for developing
positive relationships (87).
Another principle is that coincidental use of both traditional medicine and
CAM may be acceptable provided the risks and benefits are well understood. The
bottom line is not whether one or the other is used exclusively, but rather finding a
solution acceptable to the family that is best for the child. The process of arriving
at such a plan is the essence of truly good medicine (85).
Assuming a major parental role as advocate at many levels flows naturally from
the unique involvement and relationship of the family with a child who has
autism. This often initially begins with the discussions and negotiations
concerning services for the infant under age 2, when the IFSP is formulated
(88,89). At that point the parent has the opportunity to express concerns regarding
available treatment and support services, and to press for additional or more
targeted programs. The process continues into the preschool level with the
parent-professional team preparation of the individual education plan (IEP).
Opportunities for parental involvement in influencing awareness of the unique
needs of their child, and the provision of relevant services, continues throughout
the entire subsequent school experience during periodic updating of the IEP.
In the course of participating in the school setting, many parents become
knowledgeable about their legal rights and are better able to initiate requests for
special services or a Section 504 Plan that will better meet their child’s needs.
Parents may also become involved when there are concerns about
availability and adequacy of medical, social, and mental health services. Either
individually or through the efforts of support groups, parents may bring their
concerns to government at various levels, help in fund raising, and work
toward changing or adopting needed legislation. This can result in stimulating the
community to influence legislation, organizing advocacy groups to educate the
public, and heighten awareness of the medical profession to the needs of children
with autism (90,91).
One area of special concern is the transition from school to employment,
and eventual integration into the adult world. For those children who will remain
totally dependent, parents will be stimulated to work for residential and day-care
services in a group setting where dignified and compassionate management is
available at a community level. The more challenging advocacy issue relates to
integration into the workplace, changing attitudes of employers, and ensuring fair
and equitable employment practices. Mobilization of parents to achieve these
goals will take on more immediacy as children with autism increasingly age into
the workforce.
The extent to which parents become personally involved in advocacy issues
will depend on many variables: education, socioeconomic status, gender, religion,
perceived local needs, and mental health status. Fostering a partnership with
professionals as a team member in caring for the child with autism will greatly
assist in developing a more realistic and productive advocacy role. Much-needed
research is essential to better understand parental involvement in advocacy, and
IX. SUMMARY
Children with autism differ from those with other developmental disabilities in
that they have aberrations in the basic, fundamental areas of communication,
behavior, and social interaction that affect their function. These are the areas
normally nurtured within the family context from the earliest age, and when
they have gone awry place both burdens and opportunities on the family for
resolution. For this reason it was early recognized that the family should have
a pivotal place in treatment programs and strategies as they evolved. No
wonder, then, that the family of the child with autism is perhaps more at the
center of early diagnosis and management than all the other major childhood
disabilities.
At the same time, there is likely to be a unique sensibility of the family
when their child is diagnosed with autism. Disclosure can greatly influence their
subsequent relationship with the child, other family members, and siblings, as
well as caregivers. The procedure for communicating the diagnosis, therefore,
requires careful planning and a sensitive, caring process.
Since the disabilities in autism are so basic to development, it is not sur-
prising that the family is likely to experience significant stress when the diag-
nosis is confirmed, and subsequently as daily care and treatment progress. In
addition, data currently available indicate that in some cases parents of children
with autism have preexisting personality abnormalities and emotional disorders.
Sensitive professional caregivers must assure families that their emotional or
psychiatric status is not a cause of the child’s condition, and help to provide
necessary mental health services where indicated. Management of the child with
autism must focus on family needs as a whole, and not simply target the affected
child.
The vast amount of information on autism through news media and the
Internet to which families are exposed can greatly affect their involvement
with the child, participation in treatment programs, as well as their use of
complementary/alternative therapies. This places great responsibility on pro-
fessionals to maintain close communication with families to help interpret
information and develop a partnership relationship in which there is shared
decision making in care of the child.
Finally, families that initially work to improve facilities and services for
their own affected child may go on to assist other parents through discussion
groups, attempt to educate professionals, work toward improving employment
opportunities for adults with autism, campaign for needed funds and legislation,
or even become involved in the political process itself. The opportunities are
extensive; the challenges are great, for there is much more yet to be learned about
this family-centered condition.
REFERENCES
1. Lord C, McGee JP, eds. National Research Council: Educating Children with
Autism. Washington, DC: National Academy Press, 2001:140 – 172.
2. Rogers SJ. Empirically supported comprehensive treatments for young children with
autism. J Clin Child Psychol 1998; 27:168 –179.
3. Siller M, Sigman M. The behaviors of parents of children with autism predict the
subsequent development of their children’s communication. J Autism Dev Disord
2002; 32:77 – 89.
4. Kasari C, Sigman M. Linking parental perceptions to interactions in young children
with autism. J Autism Dev Disord 1997; 27:39 –57.
5. Kasari C, Sigman M, Mundy P, Yirmiya N. Caregiver interactions with autistic
children. J Abnorm Child Psychol 1988; 16:45– 56.
6. Bax MCO. Disclosure. Dev Med Child Neurol 2002; 44:579.
7. Sloper P. Determinants of parental satisfaction with disclosure of disability. Dev
Med Child Neurol 1993; 35:816– 882.
8. Garwick AW, Patterson J, Bennett FC, Blum RW. Breaking the news: how families
first learn about their child’s chronic condition. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 1995;
149:991 – 997.
9. Hasnat MJ, Graves P. Disclosure of developmental disability—a study of parent
satisfaction and determinants of satisfaction. J Paediatr Child Health 2000;
36:32 – 35.
10. Baird G, McConachie H, Scrutton D. Parents’ perceptions of disclosure of the
diagnosis of cerebral palsy. Arch Dis Child 2000; 83:475 – 480.
11. Bartolo PA. Communicating a diagnosis of developmental disability to parents—
multiprofessional negotiation frameworks. Child Care Health Dev 2002; 28:65– 71.
12. Ahmann E. Review and commentary: two studies regarding giving “bad news.”
Pediatr Nurs 1998; 24:554 – 556.
13. Krauss-Mars AH, Lachman P. Breaking bad news to parents with disabled
children—a cross-cultural study. Child Care Health Dev 1994; 20:101 – 113.
14. Partridge JW. Putting it in writing: written assessment reports for parents. Arch Dis
Child 1984; 59:678 – 681.
15. McConachie H, Lingam S, Stiff B, Holt KS. Giving assessment reports to parents.
Arch Dis Child 1988; 63:209 – 210.
16. Sharp MC, Strauss RP, Lorch SC. Communicating medical bad news: parents’
experiences and preferences. J Pediatr 1992; 121:539 – 546.
17. Jan M, Girvin JP. The communication of medical bad news to parents. Can J Neurol
Sci 2002; 29:78 – 82.
18. Abrams EZ. Diagnosing developmental problems in children: parents and
professionals negotiate bad news. J Pediatr Psychol 1998; 23:87– 88.
19. Krahn GL, Hallum A, Kime C. Are there good ways to give “bad news”? Pediatrics
1993; 91:578 –582.
20. Cottrell DJ, Summers K. Communicating an evolutionary diagnosis of disability to
parents. Child Care Health Dev 1990; 16:211– 218.
21. Cunningham CC, Morgan PA, McGucken RB. Down’s syndrome: is dissatisfaction
with disclosure of diagnosis inevitable? Dev Med Child Neurol 1984; 26:33– 39.
22. Vaidya VU, Greenberg LW, Patel KM, Strauss LH. Teaching physicians how to
break bad news: a 1-day workshop using standardized parents. Arch Pediatr Adolesc
Med 1999; 153:419 – 422.
23. Greenberg LW, Ochsenschlager D, O’Donnell R, Mastruserio J, Cohen GJ.
Communicating bad news: a pediatric department’s evaluation of a simulated
intervention. Pediatrics 1999; 103:1210 –1217.
24. Fajardo B. Parenting a damaged child: mourning, regression, and disappointment.
Psychoanal Rev 1987; 74:19– 43.
25. Shu BC, Lung FW, Chang YY. Mental health of mothers with autistic children in
Taiwan. Kaosiung J Med Sci 2000; 16:308 – 314.
26. Dunn ME, Burbine T, Bowers CA, Tantliff-Dunn S. Moderators of stress in parents
of children with autism. Community Ment Health J 2001; 37:39– 52.
27. Kalyampur M, Rao SS. Empowering low-income black families of handicapped
children. Am J Orthopsychiatry 1991; 61:523 – 532.
28. Dyson LL. Fathers and mothers of children with developmental disabilities—stress.
Am J Ment Retard 1997; 102:267– 279.
29. Donavan AM. Family stress and ways of coping—maternal perceptions. Am J Ment
Retard 1988; 92:502– 509.
30. Holroyd J, McArthur D. Mental retardation and stress on the parents—
a contrast between Down’s syndrome and childhood autism. Am J Ment Defic
1976; 80:431 –436.
31. Holmes H, Carr J. The pattern of care in families of adults with a mental handicap:
a comparison between families of autistic adults and Down syndrome adults.
J Autism Dev Disord 1991; 21:159 – 176.
32. Bouma R. Impact of chronic childhood illness on family stress: autism vs cystic
fibrosis. J Clin Psychol 1990; 46:722– 730.
33. Henderson D, Vandenberg B. Factors influencing adjustment in families with autism.
Psychol Rep 1992; 71:167– 171.
34. Bristol MM. Mothers of children with autism or communication disorders:
successful adaptation and the double ABC X model. J Autism Dev Disord 1987;
17:469– 486.
35. Hastings RP, Johnson E. Stress in UK families conducting intensive home based
behavioral intervention for their young child with autism. J Autism Dev Disord 2001;
31:327– 336.
36. Moes D, Koegel RL, Schreibman L, Loos LM. Stress profiles of mothers and fathers
of children with autism. Psychol Rep 1992; 71:1272 – 1274.
37. Koegel RL, Schreibman L, Loos LM, Dirlich-Wilhelm H, Dunlap G, Robbins PR,
Plienis AJ. Consistent stress profiles in mothers of children with autism. J Autism
Dev Disord 1992; 22:205 – 216.
38. Firat S, Diler RS. Comparison of psychopathology in the mothers of autistic and
mentally retarded children. J Korean Med Sci 2002; 17:679 – 685.
39. Bogenholm A, Gilberg C. Psychological effects on siblings of children with autism.
J Ment Def Res 1991; 35:291– 307.
40. Roegers H, Myoke K. Siblings of children with autism. Child Care Health Dev 1995;
21:305 – 319.
41. Frey KS, Greenberg MT, Fewell RR. Stress and coping among parents
of handicapped children: a multidimensional approach. Am J Ment Retard 1989;
94:240 – 249.
42. Kolko DJ. Parents as behavior therapists for their autistic child. In: Schopler E,
Mesibov GB, eds. The Effects of Autism on the Family. New York: Plenum Press, 1984.
43. Tissot C. Parental advocacy. J Autism Dev Disord 1999; 29:345 –346.
44. Factor DC, Perry A, Freeman N. Stress, social support, and respite care use in
families with autistic children. J Autism Dev Disord 1990; 20:139 –146.
45. Rodrigue JR, Morgan SB, Geffken GR. Psychosocial adaptation of fathers of
children with autism, Down syndrome, and normal development. J Autism Dev
Disord 1992; 22:249– 263.
46. Fisman SN, Wolf LC, Noh S. Marital intimacy in parents of exceptional children.
Can J Psychiatry 1989; 34:519– 525.
47. Kaminisky L, Dewey D. Siblings relationships of children with autism. J Autism Dev
Disord 2001; 31:399– 410.
48. DeLong R, Nohria C. Psychiatric family history and neurological disease in autistic
spectrum disorders. Dev Med Child Neurol 1994; 36:441 – 448.
49. Piven J, Palmer P. Psychiatric disorder and the broad autism phenotype: evidence
from a family study of multiple-incidence autism families. Am J Psychiatry 1999;
156:557 – 563.
50. Murphy M, Bolton PF, Pickles A, Fombonne E, Piven J, Rutter M. Personality traits
of the relatives of autistic probands. Psychol Med 2000; 30:1411 –1424.
51. Piven J, Wzorek M, Landa R, Lainhart J, Bolton P, Chase GA, Folstein S.
Personality characteristics of the parents of autistic individuals. Psychol Med 1994;
24:783 – 795.
52. Wolff S, Narayan S, Moyes B. Personality characteristics of parents of autistic
children: a controlled study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1988; 29:143– 153.
53. Narayan S, Moyes B, Wolff S. Family characteristics of autistic children: a further
report. J Autism Dev Disord 1990; 20:523 – 535.
54. Ward AJ. Comparison of analysis of the presence of family problems during
pregnancy of mothers with autistic vs. mothers of normal children. Child Psychiatry
Hum Dev 1990; 20:279 –288.
55. Piven J, Chase GA, Landa R, Wzorek M, Gayle J, Cloud D, Folstein S. Psychiatric
disorders in the parents of autistic individuals. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry
1991; 30:471 – 478.
56. Lovaas OI, Koegel R, Simmons JQ, Long JS. Some generalization and follow-up
measures on autistic children in behavior therapy. J Appl Behav Anal 1973; 6:131–166.
57. Lovaas OI. Behavioral treatment and normal educational and intellectual functioning
in young autistic children. J Consult Clin Psychol 1987; 55:3– 9.
58. Neef NA. Pyramidal parent training by peers. J Appl Behav Anal 1995; 28:333 – 337.
59. Holmes N, Hemsley R, Rickett J, Likierman H. Parents as cotherapists: their
perceptions of a home-based behavioral treatment for autistic children. J Autism Dev
Disord 1982; 12:331 – 342.
60. Jocelyn LJ, Casiro OG, Beattie D, Boe J, Kniesz J. Treatment of children with
autism: a randomized control trial to evaluate a caregiver-based intervention pro-
gram in community day-care centers. J Dev Behav Pediatr 1998; 19:326 – 334.
61. Ozonoff S, Cathcart K. Effectiveness of a home program intervention for young
children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord 1998; 28:25– 32.
62. Shields J. The NAS Early Bird Programme: partnership with parents in early
intervention. The National Autistic Society. Autism 2001; 5:49– 56.
63. Salt J, Shemilt J, Sellars V, Boyd S, Coulson T, McCool S. The Scottish Centre for
autism preschool treatment programme. II. The results of a controlled treatment
outcome study. Autism 2002; 6:33 – 46.
64. Panerai S, Ferrante L, Zingale M. Benefits of the Treatment and Education of
Autistic and Communication Handicapped Children (TEACCH) programme as
compared with a non-specific approach. J Intellect Disabil Res 2002; 46:318– 327.
65. Koegel RL, Bimbela A, Schreibman L. Collateral effects of parent training on family
interactions. J Autism Dev Disord 1996; 26:347 – 359.
66. Sheinkopf SJ, Siegel B. Home-based behavioral treatment of young children with
autism. J Autism Dev Disord 1998; 28:15– 23.
67. Bibby P, Eikeseth S, Martin NT, Mudford OC, Reeves D. Progress and outcomes for
children with autism receiving parent-managed intensive interventions. Res Dev
Disabil 2001; 22:425 – 447.
68. Helm DT, Kozloff MA. Research on parent training: shortcomings and remedies.
J Autism Dev Disord 1986; 16:1 – 22.
69. Boyd RD, Corley MJ. Outcome survey of early intensive behavioral intervention for
young children with autism in a community setting. Autism 2001; 5:430 – 431.
70. Berney TP. Autism—an evolving concept. Br J Psychiatry 2000; 176:20 – 25.
71. Risdon C. Cybersearch. Quick clicks to answer clinical questions. Can Fam
Physician 2001; 47:1183.
72. Patel NC, Yeh JY, Shepherd MD, Crismon ML. Secretin treatment for autistic
disorder: a critical analysis. Pharmacotherapy 2002; 22:905– 914.
73. Ramsay S. UK starts campaign to reassure parents about MMR-vaccine safety.
Lancet 2001; 357:290.
74. Fischman J. Vaccine worries get shot down but parents still fret. US News World
Rep 2001; 130:61.
75. Allen A. The not-so-crackpot autism theory. NY Times Magazine. 2002; Nov 10:66.
76. American Academy of Pediatrics, Committee on Children with Disabilities. The
pediatrician’s role in the diagnosis and management of autistic spectrum disorder in
children. Pediatrics 2001; 107:1221– 1226.
77. Kessler RC, Davis RB, Foster DF, Van Rompay MI, Walters EE, Wilkey SA,
Kaptchuk TJ, Eisenberg DM. Long-term trends in the use of complementary
and alternative medical therapies in the United States. Ann Intern Med 2001;
135:262– 268.
78. Elliott B, Elliott G. Complementary or alternative medicine and the Internet. Del
Med J 1998; 70:479 –484.
79. Sawni-Sikand A, Schubiner H, Thomas RL. Use of complementary/alternative
therapies among children in primary care pediatrics. Ambul Pediatr 2002; 2:99– 103.
80. Pitetti R, Singh S, Hornyak D, Garcia SE, Herr S. Complementary and alternative
medicine use in children. Pediatr Emerg Care 2001; 17:165– 169.
81. Straus SE. Herbal medicines—what’s in a bottle? N Engl J Med 2002; 347:
1997– 1998.
82. Nickel RE. Controversial therapies for young children with developmental
disabilities. Infants Young Child 1996; 8:29 – 40.
83. Sikand A, Laken M. Pediatricians’ experience with and attitudes toward complementary/
alternative medicine. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 1998; 152:1059–1064.
84. American Academy of Pediatrics, Committee on Children with Disabilities.
Counseling families who choose complementary and alternative medicine for their
child with chronic illness or disability. Pediatrics 2001; 107:598– 601.
85. Perlman AI, Eisenberg DM, Panush RS. Talking with patients about alternative and
complementary medicine. Rheum Dis Clin North Am 1999; 25:815– 822.
86. Clark PA. The ethics of alternative medicine therapies. Public Health Policy
2000; 21:447 – 470.
87. Cauffield JS. The psychosocial aspects of complementary and alternative medicine.
Pharmacotherapy 2000; 20:1289 –1294.
88. Seligman M, Darling RB. Ordinary Families, Special Children. 2nd ed. New York:
Guilford Press, 1997.
89. Schopler E. Will your journal support parents advocating for intensive behavior
therapy (the Lovaas Method) as an entitlement under Part H of the IDEA? J Autism
Dev Disord 1999; 29:345– 346.
90. Aylott J. Understanding and listening to people with autism. Br J Nurs 2001;
10:1676 – 172.
91. American Academy of Pediatrics, Committee on Children with Disabilities.
Pediatrician’s role in the development and implementation of an individual
education plan (IEP) and/or an individual family service plan (IFSP). Pediatrics
1992; 89:340 – 342.
I. INTRODUCTION
Reinforcement: The procedure used to increase the likelihood that a behavior will occur
or to increase the rate of a behavior.
Reinforcer: The event that follows the occurrence of a behavior and increases the
probability or rate of that behavior. Generally, a reinforcer is most effective if delivered
immediately after the behavior occurs (immediacy) or if access to that reinforcer has
been limited or denied for some time (deprivation).
Schedules of reinforcement:
Continuous reinforcement: A behavior is reinforced every time it occurs.
Ratio schedule: A behavior is reinforced after the occurrence of a number of fixed
or variable responses.
Interval schedule: A reinforcer is administered after a fixed or variable period of time
and after the occurrence of a response. Variable ratio schedule builds behavior most
resistant to extinction.
Punishment: A procedure used to decrease the likelihood that a behavior will occur again
in the future (or to decrease the rate of the behavior).
Punisher: The event that follows the occurrence of a behavior and decreases the
probability or rate of that behavior. There are fixed and variable ratio and interval
schedules of punishment. Forms of punishment include contingent punishment,
extinction, time out, and overcorrection. Overapplication of punishments can create
conditions of aversion for the learner.
Establishing operation (EO): Refers to a condition that establishes the power of a
reinforcer and that evokes particular behavior(s). The effect of an EO is to change the
reinforcing effectiveness of some stimulus and to change the frequency of all behavior
that has been reinforced by that stimulus.
Motivation: A state of willingness to perform an action or a set of actions.
Response effort: The amount of effort that must be spent to perform a task. Familiar
tasks with fewer steps require less response effort than novel tasks with several steps.
Generalization: The occurrence of a behavior in the presence of a novel stimulus. A
behavior should be reinforced in different stimulus situations, such as across instructors
and settings until it generalizes to other related stimuli.
Shaping: A method of teaching successive approximations of behavior by reinforcing a
portion of a desired response or a behavior that approximates a desired response.
Chaining: The process of creating a sequence of two or more behaviors in which each
behavior produces a result that acts as a stimulus for the next behavior to occur and in
which the last behavior is reinforced.
Prompting: A process of providing an added stimulus to increase the likelihood of a
correct response.
Fading: Withdrawing a prompt quickly to avoid creating a condition under which
behavior occurs only when prompted. Prompts must be faded quickly from most to least
restrictive in order to build behavior that occurs when environmental conditions call for
its use.
Discriminative stimulus (or SD): A stimulus associated with reinforcement of a
particular behavior. Refers to the conditions under which a behavior is taught and/or is
expected to occur.
intervention that is highly effective in some and less so in other cases of autism.
Importantly, Lovaas’ specific results have not been replicated universally. While
the method of intensive one-to-one instruction that Lovaas described is the
mainstay of several highly effective programs, some aspects of his instruction,
such as the use of prompts and punishments, remain unclear. Moreover,
methodological weaknesses in the design of Lovaas’ study have been identified,
such as nonrandom assignment of subjects to treatment groups and no indication
of whether children in the treatment group received other kinds of treatment
simultaneously (11).
Lovaas’ method of intensive one-to-one instruction is known as discrete
trial training. A discrete trial follows the basic format of presentation of an
instruction or request (called a “discriminative stimulus”), an expected response
from the learner, and a consequence delivered by the instructor. If required, a
prompt (an additional stimulus that helps the learner to make the correct response)
may be administered as well. This general format allows for several interpre-
tations of how antecedents, learner responses, and consequents are to be rendered.
Proper training and education of individuals who provide ABA is essential
because a few inappropriate and unproven applications of the format, such as the
use of the “no-no” prompt, have found their way into widespread popular
application. The “no-no” prompt consists of the instructor providing the verbal
consequent “no” contingent on the learner giving an incorrect response, and then
informing the learner that “no” reinforcement will be provided. While there are
no data that support the use of the “no-no” prompt (12), research does support the
use of errorless learning approaches (13) for response acquisition. In the errorless
approach, the learner is prompted to make swift and correct responses that always
end with delivery of a reinforcer, as opposed to the “traditional” discrete trials in
which the learner obtains reinforcement only when a correct and independent
response is made. A clear distinction must be made between “behavioral” and
“educational” techniques that involve some element of reinforcement and those
that are truly grounded in ABA. There are several effective techniques that fall
under the latter category, such as discrete trial training, functional behavior
assessment (FBA), functional communication training (FCT), applied verbal
behavior (AVB), the picture exchange communication system (PECS), and
pivotal response training (PRT). Other behavioral interventions that have shown
promise include social stories and programs devoted to social skills training
(including modeling strategies).
the lack of a natural community that uses pictures, and difficulty in emitting the
appropriate communicative response exactly when it is needed (23).
G. Social Stories
Social stories (26) are intended to provide individuals with autism with
information about what can be expected in various social situations. Social stories
can provide information about what the physical setting may look like, what other
people in that setting might say and do, and offers suggestions about the
behavior(s) that may be expected in that setting. Stories normally consist of
words and pictures, and are rendered in a simple fashion without extraneous
detail. Social stories can be written to address virtually any social situation that
may arise, based on a basic “formula” that is offered. Social stories are used
prophylactically or preventively; that is, they are reviewed prior to entering social
situations in which the individual with autism encounters some difficulty, in an
attempt to prevent the difficulty. The use of social stories is supplemented with
modeling and role playing of appropriate behavior as well as corrective feedback.
Through the use of social stories, individuals with autism have the opportunity to
experience elements of a number of different problem settings, from a “safe”
environment in which they can practice responses to social situations without
negative consequences. However, social stories may not be able to capture the
nuances of all social situations and environmental settings.
Most educational programs for children with autism are based to varying degrees
on the principles of ABA (28,29). Few educational programs have empirical
support for their efficacy based on properly controlled studies. Methodological
flaws in outcome studies of educational programs for children with autism often
make it impossible to determine with certainty if the intervention (or some
component of the intervention) truly contributed to the putative outcome.
Therefore, the interventions below can strictly be considered as “unproven” (30).
his subjects, the study has been criticized for the punishment procedures.
Currently more positive approaches are emphasized.
D. Denver Model
This program is based on a developmental model of autism first described by
Rogers and Pennington (37). Since autism is a social disorder, building social
relationships should be the core of treatment. This is done by providing
opportunities for social interaction and play at home, in an integrated preschool,
and during one-to-one teaching. The program is geared toward children from
ages 2 to 5 years. More than 20 h/week of systematic instruction is provided with
preplanned objectives and ongoing data collection. Instructional approach in this
program is interdisciplinary and eclectic, including relationship-based, develop-
mental, sensory, and discrete trials-based approaches. Curriculum emphasizes
building communication, play, sensory, and motor skills, and promotes personal
independence and participation in social routines. The Denver Model is a
comprehensive “best practices” model without a narrow theoretical
underpinning.
Alpine Learning Group utilizes visual supports in the form of daily activity
schedules, a strategy made popular within Treatment and Education of Autistic
and Related Communication Handicapped Children (TEACCH). Initially the
program is strictly 1:1, but after mastering some prerequisite skills, the child is
allowed to participate in regular education programs with typically developing
peers through a supported inclusion program.
3 –5 h/day. In the data supporting DIR (45), the composition of the “comparison
group” raised the possibility of a significant placebo effect. The DIR approach
might better be considered more a theoretical framework for intervention rather
than a specific intervention: this framework incorporates a variety of approaches,
each adjusted to the individual child’s specific needs rather than to theoretical
needs. To varying degrees the application of DIR to a specific child might
therefore overlap with any number of other intervention approaches.
IV. CONCLUSION
On the one hand, there appear to be a wide variety of intervention approaches and
programs for children with autism. On the other hand, the presence of more than a
few (if any) options within a given community is rare. Theoretically, many of
these approaches differ significantly from one another. Practically, many
programs tend to be more eclectic and diverse in content. The components
common to effective programs could be derived from the above review of
available approaches. Research supports the early (prior to age 5 years)
application of a program with a significant component of applied behavioral
analysis (49 – 53). A recent study found that the specific nature of the intervention
is more important than merely the intensity (54). Table 3 presents a number of
questions that parents might use to assess specific programs.
REFERENCES
1. Lovaas OI. Behavioral treatment and normal educational and intellectual functioning
in young autistic children. J Consult Clin Psychol 1987; 55:3– 9.
2. Anderson SR, Avery DL, DiPietro EK, Edwards GL, Christian WP. Intensive
home-based early intervention with autistic children. Educ Treat Child 1987;
10:352– 366.
3. Birnbauer JS, Leach DJ. The Murdoch early intervention program after 2 years.
Behav Change 1987; 10:63 – 74.
4. McEachin JJ, Smith T, Lovaas OI. Long-term outcome for children with autism
who received early intensive behavioral treatment. Am J Ment Retard 1993;
97:359– 372.
5. Fenske EC, Zalenski S, Krantz PJ, McClannahan LE. Age at intervention and
treatment outcome for autistic children in a comprehensive intervention program.
Anal Intervent Dev Disabil 1985; 5:49 – 58.
6. Hoyson M, Jamieson B, Strain PS. Individualized group instruction of normally
developing and autistic-like children: the LEAP curriculum model. J Div Early Child
1984; 8:157– 172.
7. Eikeseth S, Smith T, Jahr E, Eldevik S. Intensive behavioral treatment at school for
4- to 7-year old children with autism: a 1-year comparison controlled study. Behav
Modif 2002; 49–68.
8. Rogers SJ. Empirically supported comprehensive treatments for young children with
autism. J Clin Child Psychol 1998; 27(2):168– 179.
9. Skinner BF. Science and Human Behavior. New York: Macmillan, 1953.
10. Lovaas OI. Behavioral treatment and normal educational and intellectual functioning
in young autistic children. J Consult Clin Psychol 1987; 55:3– 9.
11. Rogers SJ. Empirically supported comprehensive treatments for young children with
autism. J Clin Child Psychol 1998; 27(2):168– 179.
12. Heckaman K, Alber S, Hooper S, Heward W. A comparison of least to most and
progressive time delay on the disruptive behavior of students with autism. J Behav
Educ 1998; 8:171 – 202.
13. Touchette PE, Howard J. Errorless learning: reinforcement contingencies and
stimulus control transfer in delayed prompting. J Appl Behav Anal 1984;
17:175– 181.
14. Skinner BF. Verbal Behavior. MA: Copley, 1957.
15. Sundberg ML, Partington JW. Teaching Language to Children with Autism or Other
Developmental Disabilities. Version 7.1. Pleasant Hill, CA: Behavior Analysts, Inc.,
1998.
16. Partington JW, Sundberg ML. The Assessment of Basic Language and Learning
Skills. Version 1.0. Pleasant Hill, CA: Behavior Analysts, Inc., 1998.
17. Sundberg ML, Michael J. The benefits of Skinner’s analysis of verbal behavior for
children with autism. Behav Modif 2001; 25:698–724.
18. Iwata BA, Dorsey MF, Slifer KJ, Bauman KE, Richman GS. Toward a functional
analysis of self-injury. Anal Intervent Dev Disabil 1982; 2:3 –20.
19. Iwata BA, Dorsey MF, Slifer KJ, Bauman KE, Richman GS. Toward a functional
analysis of self-injury. Anal Intervent Dev Disabil 1982; 2:3 –20.
20. Carr EG, Durand VM. The social-communicative basis of severe behavior problems
in children. In: Reiss S, Bootzin RR, eds. Theoretical Issues in Behavior Therapy.
New York: Academic Press, 1985:219 – 254.
21. Frost LA, Bondy AS. The Picture Exchange Communication System Training
Manual. Cherry Hill, NJ: Pyramid Educational Consultants, 1994.
22. Charlop-Christy MH, Carpenter M, Le L, LeBlanc LA, Kellet K. Using the picture
exchange communication system (pecs) with children with autism: assessment of
pecs acquisition, speech, social-communicative behavior, and problem behavior.
J Appl Behav Anal 2002; 35:213 – 231.
23. Sundberg ML, Partington JW. Teaching Language to Children with Autism or Other
Developmental Disabilities. Version 7.1. Pleasant Hill, CA: Behavior Analysts, Inc.,
1998.
24. Koegel LK, Koegel RL, Harrower JK, Carter CM. Pivotal response interven-
tion. I. Overview of approach. J Assoc People Spec Handicaps 1999; 24:174 –
185.
25. Koegel R, O’Dell MC, Koegel LK. A natural language paradigm for teaching
nonverbal autistic children. J Autism Dev Disabil 1987; 17:187– 199.
26. Gray C. The New Social Story Book. Illustrated ed. Arlington, TX: Future Horizons,
Inc., 2000.
27. Charlop-Christy MH, Le L, Freeman KA. A comparison of video modeling with
in vivo modeling for teaching children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord 2000;
30:537 – 552.
28. Handleman JS, Harris SL, eds. Preschool Education Programs for Children with
Autism. 2nd ed. TX: Pro-Ed, 2001.
29. Lord C, McGee JP, eds. Educating Children with Autism. Washington, DC: National
Academy Press, 2001.
30. Division 12 Task Force. Training in and dissemination of empirically validated
psychological treatments: report and recommendations. Clin Psychol 1995;
48:3 –23.
31. Weiss MJ, Piccolo E. The Rutgers autism program. In: Handleman JS, Harris SL,
eds. Preschool Education Programs for Children with Autism. Austin, TX: Pro Ed,
2001:13 – 27.
32. Smith T, Donahoe PA, Davis BJ. The UCLA young autism project. In: Handleman JS,
Harris SL, eds. Preschool Education Programs for Children with Autism. Austin, TX:
Pro Ed, 2001:29 –48.
33. Romanczyk RG, Lockshin S, Matey L. The IGS Curriculum – Version 9. Vestal, NY:
CBTA, 1998.
34. Romanczyk RA, Lockshin SB, Matey L. The children’s unit for treatment and
evaluation. In: Handleman JS, Harris SL, eds. Preschool Education Programs for
Children with Autism. TX: Pro Ed, 2001:49 – 94.
35. Taylor J, Ekdahl M, Romanczyk RG, Miller M. Escape behavior in task situations:
task versus social antecedents. J Autism Dev Disord 1994; 24:331– 344.
36. Taylor J, Romanczyk RG. Generating hypotheses about the function of student problem
behavior by observing teacher behavior. J Appl Behav Anal 1994; 27:251–265.
37. Rogers SJ, Hall T, Osaki D, Reaven J, Herbison J. The Denver Model:
a comprehensive, integrated educational approach to young children with autism
and their families. In: Handleman JS, Harris SL, eds. Preschool Education Programs
for Children with Autism. Austin, TX: Pro Ed, 2001:95 –133.
38. Meyer LS, Taylor BA, Levin L, Fisher JR. Alpine learning group. In: Handleman JS,
Harris SL, eds. Preschool Education Programs for Children with Autism. Austin,
TX: Pro Ed, 2001:135 – 155.
39. McGee GG, Morrier MJ, Daly T. The Walden early childhood programs. In:
Handleman JS, Harris SL, eds. Preschool Education Programs for Children with
Autism. Austin, TX: Pro Ed, 2001:157 – 190.
40. McClannahan LE, Krantz PJ. Behavior analysis and intervention for preschoolers at
the Princeton child development institute. In: Handleman JS, Harris SL, eds. Preschool
Education Programs for Children with Autism. Austin, TX: Pro Ed, 2001:191–213.
41. Marcus L, Schopler E, Lord C. TEACCH services for preschool children. In:
Handleman JS, Harris SL, eds. Preschool Education Programs for Children with
Autism. Austin, TX: Pro Ed, 2001:215 – 232.
42. Lord C, McGee JP, eds. Educating Children with Autism. Washington, DC: National
Academy Press, 2001.
43. Harris SL, Handleman JS, Arnold MS, Gordon RF. The Douglass developmental
disabilities center: two models of service delivery. In: Handleman JS, Harris SL, eds.
Preschool Education Programs for Children with Autism. Austin, TX: Pro Ed,
2001:233– 260.
44. Greenspan SI, Wieder S. An integrated developmental approach to interventions for
young children with severe difficulties in relating and communicating. Zero to Three:
National Center for Infants, Toddlers, and Families 1997; 17:5– 18.
45. Greenspan SI, Wieder S. Developmental patterns and outcomes in infants and
children with disorders in relating and communicating: a chart review of 200 cases of
children with autistic spectrum diagnoses. J Dev Learning Disord 1997; 1:87– 141.
46. Strain PS, Hoyson M. The need for longitudinal, intensive social skill intervention:
LEAP follow-up outcomes for children with autism as a case in point. Topics Early
Child Spec Educ 2000; 20:116 –122.
47. McClannahan LE, Krantz PJ. Behavior analysis and intervention for preschoolers
at the Princeton child development institute. In: Handleman JS, Harris SL, eds.
Preschool Education Programs for Children with Autism. Austin, TX: Pro Ed,
2001:191– 213.
48. Quill K, Gurry S, Larkin A. Daily life therapy: a Japanese model for educating
children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord 1989; 19:625– 635.
49. Lovaas OI. Behavioral treatment and normal educational and intellectual functioning
in young autistic children. J Consult Clin Psychol 1987; 55:3– 9.
50. McEachin JJ, Smith T, Lovaas OI. Long-term outcome for children with autism who
received early intensive behavioral treatment. Am J Ment Retard 1993; 97:359 – 372.
51. Scheinkopf SJ, Siegel B. Home-based behavioral treatment of young children with
autism. J Autism Dev Disabil 1998; 28:15– 23.
52. Green G. Early behavioral intervention for autism: what does research tell us? In:
Maurice C, ed. Behavioral Intervention for Young Children with Autism. Austin,
TX: Pro-Ed, 1996:29 – 44.
53. Fenske EC, Zalenski S, Krantz PJ, McClannahan LE. Age at intervention and
treatment outcome for autistic children in a comprehensive treatment program.
Anal Intervent Dev Disabil 1985; 5:49 – 58.
54. Eikeseth S, Smith T, Jahr E, Eldevik S. Intensive behavioral treatment at school for
4- to 7-year old children with autism. Behav Modif 2002; 26:49– 68.
55. Clinical Practice Guidelines: The Guideline Technical Report: Autism/Pervasive
Developmental Disorder, Assessment and Treatment for Infants and Young Children
(Age 0 – 3 Years). Albany: New York State Department of Health Early Intervention
Program, 1999:IV – 12.
The field of speech language pathology encompasses the diagnosis and treatment
of communication disorders in a population ranging from infancy to geriatrics.
A pediatric speech pathologist has extensive training in the evaluation of speech
production and language development. Speech production is characterized as the
way one forms the sounds of one’s language (articulation), the rhythm and
inflection (prosody), the fluency of the production, and the vocal quality. These
qualities have a significant impact on a child’s ability to communicate effectively
and intelligibly.
Language is the meaningful use of words that are symbols that enable us to
convey our ideas and express our needs and wants. Language development
includes receptive and expressive language. Receptive language is the ability to
understand the meaning of verbal language. Expressive language is the ability to
use verbal behaviors.
Pragmatic language is the ability to use language in a social setting. This
skill develops in infancy as children use eye gaze to interact with their caregivers.
Children instinctively use nonverbal and verbal language to regulate the behavior
of others through protesting (No, don’t touch) and requesting objects (i.e., I want
cookie) and actions (i.e., Tie my shoe). Young children may socially interact by
greeting, showing off (i.e., Did you see that red fire truck?), requesting assistance
(i.e., Help me), answering questions, and posing questions to seek information.
Finally, children establish joint attention, which is defined as the ability to share
attention. Deficits in joint attention are considered by many researchers to be an
early predictor of childhood autism (1) and are considered to be pivotal to deficits
in language, play, and social development in the autism spectrum disorder (ASD)
population.
II. MUTISM
It is reported that up to 40% of children with ASD are nonverbal, that is, make
few sounds or words. Nonverbal children with ASD do not spontaneously
develop gestural or other nonverbal means of conveying complex messages as
nonverbal children with hearing impairment do. A nonverbal child who acquires
language prior to 5 years of age has a better prognosis.
Some nonverbal children with ASD cannot communicate due to verbal
apraxia. Children with verbal apraxia have difficulty coordinating and/or
initiating the sequential movements for speech in the absence of weakness or
paralysis of the speech musculature. Verbal dyspraxia is different from oral motor
apraxia in which the coordination of the movement of the articulators is
compromised for nonspeech (i.e., tongue movements) and feeding (i.e., chewing)
skills. Children with verbal apraxia, without ASD, possess intact comprehension
and acquire linguistic concepts consistent with their chronological age. Children
with verbal apraxia tend to be quiet as babies, with limited vocal play or babbling.
Depending on the severity of the apraxia, a child’s imitation skills are impaired.
Despite a model, children with severe verbal apraxia may not be able to imitate
oral movements, vowels, consonants, or consonant-vowel combinations. If the
child does possess speech, the most salient characteristic of apraxia is
the inconsistent production from one attempt to another. The prosody of speech,
including inflection, stress, and pitch, is often affected, particularly as the child’s
language skills increase. Children with ASD may have speech apraxia in addition
to the pragmatic language deficits, which will hamper speech development (2).
III. ECHOLALIA
preoccupation with the verbal exchange rather than the content of the
conversation and insensitivity to the nonverbal cues of boredom displayed by
the listener. They lack role taking and have poor perception of the listener and the
listener’s needs.
V. NONVERBAL COMMUNICATION
Parents of children with ASD often complain that their children are very picky
eaters. These children are overly selective in their choice of foods and will not
taste foods shared by other members of the family. Idiosyncratic differences in
processing of tactile, auditory, olfactory, proprioceptive, and visual information
are common in children with ASD and may be the basis of food selectivity.
Children who are hypersensitive to touch may react negatively and emotionally
when offered food. They may be unable to tolerate the food inside their mouth,
where touch may trigger a tonic bite reflex or a hypersensitive gag response.
These children may develop feeding aversions as a way of protecting themselves
from the negative feelings. Aversions include negative responses to specific
textures, tastes, smells, and temperatures of foods, and the children may avoid
eating. Ahearn et al. evaluated 30 individuals with ASD or PDD-NOS who had a
history of aberrant feeding patterns (6). More than half of the participants
exhibited low overall levels of food acceptance, while several individuals refused
all food presented.
VIII. ASSESSMENT
assessment needs of children with ASD. Each of these tools requires considerable
training to ensure reliability.
Direct observation of social behavior, communication, and play in children
suspected of having autism may be made using standardized tools such as the
Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) (11), the Pre-Linguistic
Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (PL-ADOS) (12), and the ADOS-G
(13). ADOS and ADOS-G are observational scales used for children and adults,
while the PL-ADOS (12) serves as a downward extension of the ADOS to
evaluate nonverbal young children. The ADOS-G is a semistructured assessment
of social interaction, communication, play, and imaginative use of materials to
evaluate individuals with ASD across their life span.
The CSBS model (10) evaluates children in seven areas of pragmatic
function. It looks at a child’s ability to respond with emotion and establish eye
contact during interactions. It examines a child’s ability to use language to
regulate the behavior of others, to interact socially, and to establish joint
attention. Use of natural gestures, sounds, words, and objects is evaluated, along
with a child’s understanding of words.
A. Background Information
Prior to an assessment, it is necessary to obtain as much background information
as possible. Parents serve as the primary source of information about a child’s
communication, play, and behavior in the first 2 years of life. Parent recollections
can be enhanced with a review of videotapes taken over the course of the 2 years.
Information pertaining to use of natural gestures, social eye gaze, understanding
of the spoken word, and vocalizations may be analyzed prior to an assessment.
B. Social Communication
Evaluation of a child’s social use of language is central to an assessment of
communication in ASD. Informal and formal tools are available to evaluate a
child’s conversational skills, ability to share information, and ability to
understand contextual cues.
The Test of Pragmatic Language (TOPL) is the only evaluation devoted
specifically to the assessment of language pragmatics. It is used for children 5 – 13
years of age (14).
The Prutting Pragmatic Protocol is a descriptive taxonomy of 30 pragmatic
parameters (i.e., variety of speech acts, topic selection, topic introduction, topic
maintenance) rated according to whether they are used “appropriately” or
“inappropriately” or “not observed” (15).
The Checklist of Communicative Function and Means is an evaluation of a
child’s intent gained through a natural play setting (16). Wetherby and Prizant
D. Play
A child’s cognitive, nonlinguistic abilities are often evaluated through play. The
goal of the play assessment is to determine whether the child is able to use objects
for truly imaginative play or is just preoccupied by unusual aspects of objects.
Is the play repetitive and stereotyped?
Westby described 10 stages in the development of symbolic play abilities.
The Symbolic Play Scale evolved from a Piagetian model (23).
Between the ages of 9 and 12 months (Stage I), the child is developing
object permanence and will find a toy if it is hidden under a scarf.
Between 13 and 17 months (Stage II), the child explores a toy, operates it
by pulling levers and strings, and will attempt to act upon it by pushing,
pulling, turning, and pounding. The child will hand a toy to an adult if
unable to operate independently.
Between 17 and 19 months (Stage III), Westby identified the emergence of
pretend skills. The child may pretend to go to sleep, drink from a cup, or
eat from a spoon. At this age level, true language is emerging and a
marked growth occurs in the number of words that a child uses.
By 19 –22 months (Stage IV), the child extends the symbolism beyond him
or herself to include the caregiver or a doll. The child may feed the doll,
brush the doll’s hair, or put the doll in the bed.
By 24 months (Stage V), the child will represent his daily experiences
through play. The child plays house and takes on roles. A true sequence
of events has not emerged.
By 21⁄2 years (Stage VI), the child begins to represent events less frequently
experienced. The child may pretend to be the doctor and take care of a
sick child. The child continues to play alongside his peers; however,
associative play appears.
By the age of 3 (Stage VII), the child’s actions have a sequence (i.e., the doctor
checks the patient, calls the ambulance, takes the patient to the hospital).
By 31⁄2 years (Stage VIII), the child is less dependent on realistic toys such
as a dollhouse, farm, parking garage. He is more interested in using
blocks for building and will use one object to represent another.
IX. INTERVENTION
A. Play
Play is a free-choice activity that is self-motivated, enjoyable, process-oriented,
and not literal. The materials, time, and roles of the “players” are made up by the
children. Children play because they like it. They do not play for praise, food,
money, or rewards.
Motor play helps the brain to develop gross and fine muscles, nerves, and
brain functions.
Social play encourages learning to give and take, cooperation, and sharing.
The children use play to learn to use moral reasoning and values.
Constructive play encourages children to manipulate their environment.
Building cities with blocks and making castles in the sand are forms of
constructive play.
Fantasy play helps the children to try out new roles, rehearse a variety of
possible situations, and experience language and emotions.
Teaching a child to play games with rules is helpful in addressing turn
taking, social interaction, and learning to play by the rules imposed by
another. Games with rules teach children a very important concept—we
all must follow rules.
B. Social Language
Children with ASD benefit from intervention that teaches specific social goals.
The role of the speech pathologist is to identify the complex social behavior (i.e.,
conversation) and analyze the prerequisite behaviors and rules that can be
memorized and practiced in a variety of settings. Several models and inter-
ventions have proven to be quite effective in addressing the social communi-
cation and language needs of young and older children with ASD.
1. SCERTS Model
The social communication, emotional regulation, and transactional support
(SCERTS) model was developed by Wetherby and Prizant as a tool to enhance
the communication and social emotional development in young children on the
autistic spectrum (24). This model addresses the challenges and issues at each
level of language development—including prelinguistic, emergent language, early
language, and the more advanced language stage—through an individualized and
developmental approach.
2. Social Stories
Social stories, developed by Gray and Garand, is a technique used to teach a
critical component of social interactions (25). Its development was based on the
understanding that children and adults with ASD are impaired in their ability to
consider the perspective of others. The intention of the social stories is to help the
person with ASD to learn strategies to interact in a variety of social situations.
These stories can be tailored to meet the individual needs of the person for whom
the story is written. The stories can be used to teach children social routines (i.e.,
greeting a new person), how to ask for help, and how to respond to their feelings.
Social stories are considered an effective tool to teach children social skills and
how to make sense of social situations.
3. Teach Me Language
Teach Me Language, by Freeman and Dake, identifies and helps to teach speech
and language concepts to children on the autistic spectrum, including AS (26).
This book was designed to meet the specific needs of children with ASD with
ABA teaching techniques. It is most effective for children who perceive visually
presented information better than orally presented information. The children
must be able to communicate in some way, either verbally or through total
communication.
C. Speech Production
Therapy to address speech production is dependent on the diagnosis of the speech
disorder, whether the problem is due to an apraxia (motor planning disorder) or a
dysarthria (speech disorder), or even a phonological disorder (linguistic basis).
Many children with ASD experience motor planning speech disorders.
Traditional articulation therapy is useful for teaching a child to produce one
sound at a time in isolation until he or she can master this sound. Once the child
masters the sound, the therapist will increase the complexity of the word until the
child can produce this sound on the sentence and conversational levels. However,
this approach does not address the significant obstacles presented by a child with
a motor planning disorder/apraxia or muscular weakness/dysarthria.
The preferred approach for treating children with verbal apraxia is to focus
on the motor movements in sequence for the production of a meaningful word.
The therapy will start with sounds already in the child’s phonemic repertoire and
use these sounds to increase the consistency of sound production in a variety of
syllables and words. For example, a child with apraxia may have learned to
produce the “o” sound; however, the child will state “apa” instead of “open.” The
goal of the therapy is to patiently repeat the sound in varying words and word
combinations and provide the child the opportunity to practice and repractice
until the new sounds are integrated.
Tactile cueing is an essential technique in working with children with
motor planning disorders. PROMPT (prompts for restructuring oral muscular
phonetic targets) is a cueing technique developed by Hayden (27). Using
tactile-kinesthetic-proprioceptive cues, a speech pathologist using the PROMPT
method would restructure the speech production output of children and adults
with speech disorders. The input presented to the individual helps the individual
achieve voluntary control of the motor speech system as he or she begins
to integrate the motor, cognitive-linguistic, and social-emotional aspects of
communication. The cues, which are applied externally to the muscles of the
face and the mylohyoid muscle under the chin, nose, and jaw, help to reshape
sounds on the individual (i.e., “a”), syllable (i.e., “ma”), word (i.e., mom,
mommy), phrase (i.e., my mommy), and sentence levels. As the length of the
word increases, the child who has an emergent speech system benefits from the
hands-on cues to stimulate the articulatory movements. As the child integrates
the correct sequence of movements to achieve speech sound production, the hand
cues are faded.
Speech disorders due to muscular involvement are best addressed through
an oral motor program. The children with a dysarthria will demonstrate weakness
through the jaw, tongue, lips, cheeks, and soft palate. This weakness will affect
all motor speech processes, including breathing, sound production, articulation,
resonance, and the prosody (melody) of speech.
D. Feeding Issues
Feeding issues tend to have both a motor and sensory basis. As noted, children
with feeding issues often have hypersensitivities and specific food preferences.
A team approach is recommended, comprised of the parent, teacher, occupational
therapist, and speech pathologist. A physical therapist may be part of the team
in the case of children with a neuromuscular disorder.
Interventions utilize techniques to normalize sensory defensiveness and
aversive responses to tactile, auditory, and olfactory stimulation. Calming music,
oral stimulation of the tongue, lips, and facial region, an explanation of
expectations, and graded modifications in texture and temperature can be pre-
sented as techniques to help reduce oral hypersensitivity.
E. Total Communication
A total communication approach with children with ASD would provide a range
of effective and accessible means available to communicate including alternative
and augmentative systems. Speech, when available, provides an effective and
accessible means for children to communicate, but augmentative systems may be
necessary for children who are preverbal and are experiencing difficulty in
acquiring speech. These systems encourage the children to communicate without
precluding the acquisition of verbalizations. Research suggests that the use of
alternative communication systems actually promotes the development of more
complex skills (28).
Augmentative and alternative communication (AAC) and assistive tech-
nology (AT) are used to provide new communication possibilities for the children
who are nonverbal and when speech is not an effective means of communication.
A wide variety of products are available, ranging from a device that has a few
words to more sophisticated devices that are programmable to include an
individualized vocabulary of pictures and/or written words.
The picture exchange communication system (PECS) developed by Frost
and Bondy, uses drawing to help young children with severe speech disorders to
communicate. PECS uses pictures to convey words and concepts (29).
Sign language is a viable intervention when working with nonverbal or
apraxic/dysarthric children until language emerges. It involves introducing
natural gestures along with oral language to build communication skills.
X. CONCLUSION
On November 12, 2002, Ms. Polly Morrice wrote about her struggle with autism
in The New York Times (30). Her son, who was 11 years at the time of this
op-ed article, was diagnosed with ASD and enrolled in a specialized preschool
program at the age of 3. She described autism as a “window-of-opportunity
disorder,” which she elaborated by stating that “the earlier in a child’s life you
intervene to adjust the faulty neural wiring that causes it, the better the outcome.”
According to many clinical practice guidelines developed by experts, such as the
Clinical Practice Guideline – Autism/Pervasive Developmental Disorders:
Assessment and Intervention for Young Children (Ages 0– 3 years) supported
by the New York State Early Intervention Program, treatment is most effective
when intensive training is provided to young children (31). Although the
applied behavioral analysis program, similar to the intervention developed
by Lovaas, has been endorsed by some of these guidelines, including the
New York State guidelines, other models, such as Greenspan’s floor time (32)
may be effective if applied early and intensely. Each of these interventions
encourages the integration of a speech pathology component in a comprehensive
program.
REFERENCES
10. Wetherby AM, Prizant BM. Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales
Developmental Profile (CSBS DP). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brooks Publishing Co.
Inc., 2002.
11. Lord C, Rutter M, Goode S, Heemsbergen J, Jordan H, Mawhood L, Schopler E.
Autism diagnostic observation schedule: a standardized observation of commu-
nicative and social behavior. J Autism Dev Disord 1989; 19:185 – 212.
12. DiLavore PC, Lord C, Rutter M. The pre-linguistic autism diagnostic observation
schedule. J Autism Dev Disord 1995; 25:355 – 379.
13. Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook EH Jr, Leventhal BL, DiLavore PC, Pickles A,
Rutter M. The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: a standard measure
of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism.
J Autism Dev Disord 2000; 30:205 – 223.
14. Phelps-Terasaki D, Phelps-Gunn T. Test of Pragmatic Language. Austin, TX:
Pro-Ed, Inc., 1992.
15. Prutting CA, Kirchner DM. A clinical appraisal of the pragmatic aspects of language.
J Speech Hear Disord 1987; 52:105 – 119.
16. Wetherby A, Prizant B. Infant/Toddler checklist for communication and language
development. Appl Symbolix, Langley, B.C., Canada: SKF Books, 1998.
17. Wetherby A, Prizant B. Profiling young children’s communicative competence.
In: Warren S, Reichle J, eds. Causes and Effects in Communication and Language
Intervention. Baltimore, MD: Paul Brookes, 1990; 217– 251.
18. Rossetti LM. Infant Toddler Language Scale. East Moline, IL: Linguisystems, Inc.,
1990.
19. Reynell JK. Reynell Developmental Language Scale. Wood Dale, IL: Stoelting Co.,
2001.
20. Zimmerman I, Steiner V, Evatt Pond R. Preschool Language Scale – Fourth Edition.
San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation, 2001.
21. Carrow Woolfolk E. Oral and Written Language Scales. Circle Pines, MN: American
Guidance Service, 1995.
22. Gardner M. Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test. Novato, CA: Academic
Therapy Publications, 2000.
23. Westby C. Language abilities through play. Lang Speech Hear Serv Schools, 1980;
XI: 154– 168.
24. Wetherby A, Prizant B. SCERTS model. Presented in Conference by Wetherby and
Prizant, August 2000, Providence, RI.
25. Gray C, Garand JD. Social stories: improving responses of students with autism with
accurate social information. Focus Autist Behav 1993; 8:1– 10.
26. Freeman S, Dake L. Teach Me Language. Langley, B.C., Canada: SKF Books, 1996/
1997: 26.
27. Hayden D. The PROMPT System — Therapeutic Intervention Hierarchy, August
1994.
28. Kangas KA, Lloyd L. Early cognitive skills as prerequisites to augmentative and
alternative communication use: what are we waiting for? Augment Altern Commun
1998; 4:211– 221.
29. Bondy A, Frost L. The picture exchange communication system. Focus Autist Behav
1994; 9:1– 19.
30. Morrice P. Few options for treating autism. New York Times, November 2002.
31. Autism/Pervasive Developmental Disorders Guideline Panel. Clinical practice
guideline – autism/pervasive developmental disorders: assessment and intervention
for young children (Ages 0 –3 years). Clinical Practice Guideline No. 1, NYDOH.
Albany: New York State Department of Health, Early Intervention Program, 1999.
32. Greenspan SI, Wider S. Developmental patterns and outcomes in infants and
children with disorders in relating and communicating: a chart review of 200 cases of
children with autistic spectrum diagnoses. J Dev Learning Disord 1997; 1:87– 141.
I. INTRODUCTION
I was destructive as a child. I drew all over the walls not once or twice, but
anytime I got my hands on a pencil or crayon. I remember really “catching” it
for peeing on the carpet. So the next time I had to go, instead of peeing on the
carpet, I put the drape between my legs. I thought it would dry quickly and
Mother wouldn’t notice. Normal children use clay for modeling; I used my
feces and then spread my creations all over the room. I chewed up puzzles
and spit the cardboard mush out on the floor. I had a violent temper, and when
thwarted, I’d throw anything handy—a museum quality vase or left-over
feces. I screamed continually, responded violently to noise and yet appeared
deaf on some occasions. (1)
The above quote graphically depicts the chaotic and overwhelming world of
individuals with autism. The overt behaviors that occur because of this inner
chaos can be daunting and overwhelming for parents, physicians, teachers, and
all who come in contact with them. Occupational therapists believe that much of
this behavioral chaos occurs because of a failure of individuals with autism to
organize their sensory experiences, or sensory integration dysfunction (DSI).
Sensory integration theory was developed over 50 years ago by a pioneering
occupational therapist, A. Jean Ayres, while she was working with children with
perceptual, learning, and behavioral problems of unknown etiology. She set out
to examine how the brain processed sensations, not just sensations of the eyes and
ears but other parts of the body as well (2). Ayers referred to sensory integration
(SI) as “the organization of sensory input for use, through sensory integration,
[of] the many parts of the nervous system [that] work together so that a person
can interact with the environment effectively and experience appropriate
satisfaction” (2). Carol Kranowitz broke down SI into four sequential steps:
receiving, organizing, and using sensory information, and reacting to sensory
information (3). Sensory information is picked up peripherally, and travels to the
CNS for organization and planning. The reaction and execution is then carried
out in the motor system. DSI occurs when sensory information is not processed,
integrated, or organized properly in the brain, resulting in dysfunction of
information processing, behavior, and the ability to meet the demands of the
environment (3). DSI makes it hard for the child to learn from his/her
experiences.
The five senses obtained via the sense organs of eyes, ears, mouth, hands, and
nose are vision, auditory, gustatory, touch, and olfactory, respectively. These are
often referred to as “far senses” because the input comes from outside one’s body.
SI also adds three “near senses,” or hidden senses. These are tactile, vestibular,
and proprioceptive. They are called hidden because the information comes from
within one’s body (3).
Tactile includes the sensations of touch, pressure, vibration, pain, and
temperature, which we obtain through the skin all over the body. These
sensations determine: Does the person like to be hugged, or shies away from a
hugging, kissing relative? Does the person like to walk in the surf at the beach
getting wet and sandy? Does the person have the label in the back of a shirt cut
out because it is bothersome? What is the person’s pain tolerance?
Vestibular is the sense that receives and responds to input from movement
of the body and from changes in the head position. This information is received in
the inner ear (3). Vestibular sense tells a person that he or she is moving—
forward, backward, upward, or downward. It tells one how fast he or she is
moving, or how slow he or she is moving. Vestibular sense tells one whether he or
she is upright or upside down. It also tells one when his or her head is tilted or
rotated looking at the horizon. The vestibular sense determines if a person is an
amusement park thrill-ride seeker or the thought of a roller coaster makes him
nauseous.
The last hidden sense is proprioception, or the internal “position sense.” It
tells us how the body and the body parts are positioned. This information comes
from the muscles, ligaments, and joints. The brain registers when muscles are
contracted or elongated; it also registers when the joints flex, extend, or when
joint traction occurs (2). This sense determines if a child maintains an upright
sitting posture at the table or shifts constantly, falling out of the chair.
The three basic senses, vestibular, proprioceptive, and tactile, are integral in
developing a child’s body perception, coordination of the two sides of the body,
motor planning, activity level, attention span, and emotional stability. Along with
the far senses, particularly vision and auditory, a child learns to make sense of his
or her world (2). Children with autism fail to organize their sensory information.
The inability to organize this information often leaves a person with autism appear
highly reactive to sensory information. Temple Grandin’s description of riding a
spinning amusement ride is an excellent example of sensory organization:
Frightened but dared, I bought a ticket for the ride and with trembling legs,
walked up the few steps to enter the barrel. My heart in my mouth, I leaned
against the side. The sound of the motor starting up sent a chill skittering
down my spine. Then the “rotor” picked up speed and the motor sounded like
a giant’s hum. The colors of the blue sky, the white clouds, the yellow sun
blended together like a spinning top. The smell of cotton candy, Karmel Corn
and tacos swirled around individually until they too, combined into the
carnival smell. Glued to the side of the barrel, I waited for the floor to drop
out. Fear tasted bitter in my mouth and I tried to press harder against the side.
With a creak on the hinges the floor opened to the ground below but now my
senses were so overwhelmed with stimulation that I didn’t react with anxiety
or fear. I felt only the sensation of comfort and relaxation. (1)
A. Sensory Modulation
In a review of recent research literature, Wilbarger and Stackhouse (4) define
modulation as “the intake of sensation via typical sensory processing mechanisms
such that the intensity and quality of response are graded to match environmental
demand and so that a range of optimal performance/adaptation is maintained.”
The human organism seeks to maintain a state of homeostasis, or sensory
balance. Typical children can modulate their level of arousal and alertness to be
able to attend to stimuli. They can regulate themselves to achieve, monitor, and
change a state of attention that matches the demands of the environment. They
can modulate a response in relation to the task required. They can discriminate
and attend to selected stimuli (sensitization) and accommodate to the constant
bombardment of stimuli in daily life (habituation). Modulation states can vary
widely during a typical day in both the neurotypical and atypical child, depending
on environment, stresses, and physiological needs (hunger, sleep).
Children with autistic spectrum disorder (ASD) demonstrate deficits of
sensory modulation. As they function through the day, their regulation states may
not match the level of intensity in a given situation. For example, the child may
have completed the morning routine of waking, eating, dressing, and trans-
itioning to school in a highly aroused state with very poor ability to focus on a
visual motor activity requiring attending and inhibition of extraneous stimuli
(auditory and tactile as well as emotional arousal). For this child to participate, he
or she will need to modulate or bring his or her arousal state to a level of matching
the environment. For the neurotypical child, this can be accomplished through
following a set of routines and verbal and social cues to “settle down” and sit at a
desk in the classroom. For the atypical child, the requirement to “settle down” has
no context or strategy to accomplish. This child may need sensorimotor cues to
ease the transition. It might well start with waking up with a deep pressure
massage and followed by clothing that is less irritating to the skin. A breakfast
with textures that provide proprioceptive input such as a chewy bagel and
drinking through a straw may assist in regulating the arousal level. Once the child
arrives at school, having a structured task involving controlled movement may
again assist in regulation of activity level. The modulation problems include low
registration of stimuli, hypo- and hyperresponse to stimuli, and sensory
avoidance.
B. Sensory Discrimination
Sensory discrimination is the ability to differentiate between incoming stimuli
based on prior experience to make meaningful adaptive responses. It provides the
foundation for sorting and classifying information. It allows the individual to
prioritize what information is important and what is not necessary at the moment.
At the sensory level, this happens unconsciously. An easy example is all the
auditory information received constantly (hums of electrical equipment, people
talking, background music, etc.), and the ability to screen it all out, yet still
be able to discern something unusual or important. Discrimination is necessary
for both habituation and sensitization. It allows the individual to put multiple
attributes to a sensory experience and integrate the information for further
learning.
C. Motor Planning
Praxis, or motor planning, is the ability to conceive, organize, and execute an
unfamiliar motor activity (2). It involves physically or mentally constructing the
movement in a goal-directed manner. Adequate movement skills require proper
A child can rarely describe his sensory experiences to others, for he has no
internal baseline with which to compare them.—T. Berry Brazelton
1. Low Registration
The child appears to show dull affect with little interest in the external
environment and requires a significant amount of stimulation to achieve an
alerting response. Threshold or the amount or quality of stimulus required to have
a CNS reaction is high (5).
2. Underresponsive
The child has a diminished perception of self as moving and interacting within his
environment. He seeks sensations to remain alert, but has difficulty reaching an
alert status because of impaired processing. He may appear to be very active and
disorganized as he attempts to gain enough sensory input to organize self and
respond; threshold is high.
3. Increased Registration
This results in poor ability to discriminate or select sensation to respond or
process to habituate. A child demonstrates defensive behaviors to protect from
incoming sensory information from the environment and may typically display
“fight, fright, or flight” behaviors in response to environmental demands;
threshold is low.
4. Hyperresponsive
Children with this behavior demonstrate overreaction or defensiveness to sensory
input. Their motor responses are fearful, cautious, withdrawing, or aggressive
and they may appear negative and angry.
B. Sensory Avoidance
The child avoids sensory input that is perceived as painful and overwhelming to
the nervous system and develops routines and rituals to avoid external stimuli.
Threshold is low (5).
C. Sensory Seeking
Self-stimulatory and self-injurious behaviors and constant motion fall into this
category. Within the concept of threshold and habituation, the self-stimulating
child may be seeking to meet the sensory need, but the arousal/threshold is not
met and the behavior recurs. The child may also be seeking to extinguish
unpleasant sensory or environmental stimuli by blocking them with stimuli that
turn his or her attention inwardly. Extremes of seeking proprioceptive or
vestibular stimulation may be observed with repeated pounding with the
extremities, head banging, pushing, and seeking of deep pressure or “squeezing”
(6). Temple Grandin and several others have reported relief with the “squeeze
machine.” Spinning and rocking are commonly seen as both alerting and calming
for the child. Through the visual-vestibular connection, the child may also engage
in seeking out the same sensory information through spinning toys and objects.
D. Sensory Defensiveness
This is a sensory disorder characterized by a “fight, flight, or fright” reaction to
sensory stimulation most individuals would consider harmless. The nervous
system does not adequately discriminate different sensory stimuli and responds in
a survival mode: aggressive response, withdrawal from stimuli, rigid routines to
avoid stimuli and maintain control over the environment, and other nonfunctional
responses to the environment.
E. Tactile Defensiveness
This is commonly seen in DSI. The child may be irritated by clothes and may
refuse to wear certain clothes. He or she may not like the texture of certain foods
and may refuse to eat them. Socially the child may be seen as aggressive when he
or she avoids contact with others or responds aggressively when touched
F. Auditory Defensiveness
This is an aversive reaction to sounds, common and unexpected. Sounds, such as
a vacuum cleaner, a telephone ringing, or a timer on a stove, may be enough to
disrupt the child, eliciting flight or withdrawal. Both sound intensity and pitch
may disturb the child.
G. Visual Defensiveness
This includes hypersensitivity to light or avoidance of gaze. Busy visual
environments may be overwhelming because of poor figure ground
discrimination. The child may have difficulty in transitioning from indoor to
outdoor because of the light sensitivity.
H. Oral Defensiveness
This child with ASD may refuse many foods because of oral defensiveness and
may become more selective as he or she gets older instead of using a broad
selection of foods. It may be difficult to clean the child’s face after eating,
creating a problem of hygiene. Olfactory defensiveness may also be present.
Food selections in oral sensitivity may be based on taste and/or texture and it is
important to note the taste and texture of preferred foods.
I. Gravitational Insecurity
This is a form of hyperresponsivity to vestibular sensations, particularly
sensations from the otolith organs, which detect linear movement through space
and the pull of gravity (2). The child may show an excessive fear to ordinary
movement and is challenged by changes in head position and movement,
particularly with head tilted back or up and down. The child may demonstrate
intolerance to movement imposed or on an unstable surface. Movement through
the environment is difficult and confusing for the child. The fear that the child
feels is very real and impacts movement and balance.
J. Decreased Imitation
Children with autism have difficulty in imitating others. Imitation and develop-
ment of praxis require adequate development of body awareness and kinesthetic
processing. The sensory inputs from the tactile, proprioceptive, and vestibular
systems all contribute to support motor skills. When the child is unable to plan a
movement through imitation or receives faulty feedback from the movement he
completes, his motor coordination is affected. Children with autism frequently
have difficulty with initiating movements and performing goal-directed motor
complex activities.
Here’s your hat, Temple. . . My ears felt as if they were being squashed
together into one giant ear. The band of the hat pressed tightly around my
head. I jerked the hat off and screamed. Screaming was the only way of
telling Mother that I didn’t want to wear the hat. It hurt. It smothered my
hair.—Temple Grandin
Tasks of daily living can be very challenging for a child with ASD and his
caregivers. Sensory processing and modulation disorder combined with language
impairment make the routines of daily living a constant challenge. Frequently,
the child falls into patterns of routines and unchanging rituals to self-regulate his
day. By controlling the environment in this manner, the child can have an
internalized set of expectations and outcomes with each activity. But when a new
challenge or change in routine is introduced, the child lacks the ability to plan,
compensate, or react in a meaningful manner to it.
Dressing can be a major issue for children with autism, especially as it
relates to tactile processing. Abnormal reactions may relate to hypersensitivity or
hyposensitivity. The hypersensitive child may refuse clothing altogether, or be
very selective in terms of textures, tags, or clothing that is loosely touching the
skin. The child does not become habituated to his perceived source of irritation
and seeks constant relief from it. The hypersensitive child may also demonstrate a
need to be covered on all extremities and body parts by layering of clothing. By
layering and wrapping clothing tightly, the child is self-regulating and providing
not only a protective barrier from touch, but deep pressure to inhibit irritating
stimuli and calm him. The hyposensitive child requires significant amounts of
stimuli for arousal and attention and seeks sensation. This child may tightly wrap
clothing on his or her body and extremities or may not want to wear clothing to
maximize the sensory input being received by the CNS.
Mealtimes also present significant issues in regard to sensory processing.
Environmentally, the modulation required to regulate sounds, smells, and body
position to stay at the table may present as a challenge. Decreased proprioceptive
awareness in the hands to hold a utensil may result in finger feeding or passively
waiting to be fed. The motor plan to scoop the spoon and bring it to the
mouth, while maintaining the grasp, is a challenge for many of the children.
Visual as well as tactile food aversions are commonly seen. Children with autism
may prefer certain food textures, may avoid foods that are multitextural
(casseroles, cereal in milk, salads, or oranges, for example), may select foods that
are all of similar textures (smooth, least resistive, less taste), and frequently may
prefer foods that are savory and sharp over sweet (barbeque flavor, bacon, etc.).
Sensory processing and modulation impact daily routines and require-
ments. Transitions in arousal states begin with waking in the morning and
progress throughout the day. Modulation is often on the high end of arousal and a
small amount of sensory input can cause the child to react strongly and
emotionally. From a sensory point of view, the child with autism seeks out the
routine and ritual to decrease the amount of input and novelty the system needs to
accommodate and act upon. Always walking against a wall and limiting food and
clothing preferences decrease tactile input. Vestibular dysfunction is character-
ized by gravitational insecurity and decreased movement from place to place. The
child may show extreme distress at a diaper change because of the head tilt and
body movement imposed by lying the child down. Toilet training may become
difficult as the child is perceiving fear of feet off the ground and the tactile and
auditory sensations of the bathroom are overwhelming to the nervous system.
Rearranged furniture in a familiar room may cause an extreme reaction, as
if the child has “lost” the visual motor plan of the room and is unable to recognize
the room out of context. Without the ability to “reorganize” internally, the point
of control becomes restoring the room to the familiar state.
Self-stimulatory and self-injurious behaviors may serve a sensory function
to the child with autism (6). The behaviors frequently center on tactile,
proprioceptive, and or vestibular input. The child may use these behaviors to alert
and arouse or to assist with calming and blocking other, more aversive stimuli.
Children are frequently noted to engage in oral stimulation by mouthing
nonfood items and hands, as well as extraoral stimulation with fabrics and light
touch. Hands are frequently engaged in light touch patting and rubbing preferred
textures and favorite items to hold. The self-seeking stimulation of this type often
appears with the tactile defensive child, yet that is the child that who refuses
touch imposed upon him or her. Once again, it appears to be an issue of
controlling the sensory input. This child may also seek to rub and hug against a
caregiver, but refuse reciprocation as a defensive measure. The extremes of this
behavior may be seen in biting, hair pulling, and slapping. The hyporesponsive
child may engage in extreme sensory-seeking behavior when in actuality, the
child may be trying to produce sensation that he or she can perceive.
Proprioceptive self-stimulatory behaviors are seen in a variety of ways.
Compressing and stretching the muscles over a joint gives the strong
proprioceptive feedback the body is seeking to assist with muscle tone, body
image, and regulation of movement effort. Flapping, jumping, slapping, and other
movements that involve jarring of the joints give intense proprioceptive
stimulation. Toe walking stretches the leg muscles, also giving the deep
proprioceptive input. Frequently, oral behaviors such as hitting against the face,
pulling against resistance with teeth, clenching teeth, and even guttural sounds
are providing proprioceptive input to the child. The chewing gum industry
has been aware of how people seek out this type of stimulation. The extremes of
this sensation seeking may be seen in head butting and hitting against walls,
biting oneself, and slapping and hitting oneself.
The focus of vestibular self-stimulation is responsively to head movement.
Rocking the body, isolating head movements in linear movements, twirling, and
spinning are all movements related to vestibular stimulation. The close
association of visual-vestibular processing gives rise to the concept of using
visual stimulation (watching spinning objects, finger flickering in the periphery)
to arouse the vestibular system.
no questions are asked and scored to provide the therapist with a profile as to the
areas where there are difficulties that need to be targeted (6). Other questionnaires
available include the Touch Inventory for Elementary School-aged Children
(TIE) and behavior checklists for families and teachers.
B. Standardized Tests
Only a few standardized tests have been developed to test sensory integration
specifically. It is important to evaluate fine motor, visual motor, and activities of
daily living (ADL) skills because sensory issues affect everyday life and skills.
The following section will comment on several of the available tests.
The Sensory Integration Praxis Tests (SIPT) is one of the main
standardized tests used to evaluate sensory dysfunction. It contains 17 subtests,
which measure tactile, vestibular, and proprioceptive sensory processing;
visuomotor perception (including position in space); motor planning; and
bilateral and sequencing skills. This test takes 21⁄2 hr to administer and score but it
can be broken into sections and administered separately (7).
A sensory test for younger children is the Test of Sensory Functions in
Infants (TSFI). It can be administered in 20 min. It is for ages 4 – 18 months and
will assist with children who are at risk for developing sensory issues later.
DeGangi-Berk and Berk also developed the TSI, which is the DeGangi-Berk Test
of Sensory Integration, and is for children 3– 5 years of age. This test requires
30 min to administer.
Testing of fine motor skills can be completed using the Peabody
Developmental Motor Scales –Second Edition (PDMS-2) or the Bruninks-
Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency, depending on the age of the child. The
PDMS-2 tests children from birth to 72 months of age in gross motor (reflexes,
locomotion, bilateral coordination) and fine motor skills (grasping and visual
motor integration). It requires approximately an hour for administration and
scoring (8). The Bruninks test is for ages 4– 17, and tests gross motor and fine
motor skills (response speed, bilateral coordination, upper-limb speed and
dexterity, and visual motor coordination). This test requires 11⁄2 hours, but can be
broken into sections (9).
Other tests include the Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual-
Motor Integration (VMI). This test is administered quickly and can be interpreted
in three components: visual motor integration, visual integration, and motor
skills. The age range is 3– 18 years and a short form is available that can be used
with children up to 8 years (10). The Developmental Test of Visual Perception – 2
(DVPT-2) is for ages 4– 10 years. It measures visual perceptual skills through
eight subtests (eye-hand coordination, position in space, copying, figure-ground,
spatial relations, visual closure, visual-motor speed, form constancy). It can be
administered in sections and the time frame can range from 30 min to 1 hr (11).
C. Clinical Observations
Clinical observations can be completed in several settings including the home, a
clinic, or any other natural environment for the child. Observations include play
skills and interactions with objects in the environment, muscle tone, reflex
integration, and strength.
VII. INTERVENTIONS
level. But not all intervention requires swings or other suspended equipment.
Assisting the child to move using scooter boards, changing running speed, and
movement through complex motor skills can also assist with integration of
sensation.
A key concept in therapy is providing structured input to organize the
nervous system. Proprioceptive input has a strong effect on modulation.
Trampolines and other equipment that provide deep pressure to the joints are
frequently used to provide input to promote sensory stimulation. Many of the
repetitive or self-stimulatory behaviors seen in autism have a basis in
proprioception-seeking behaviors. By increasing the amount and duration of
varied input, the need for this type of behavior can be decreased.
To provide a program of vestibular-proprioceptive input, the program must
be structured to meet the child’s sensory needs and be flexible to meet the child’s
changing status. Occupational therapy begins with finding the child’s arousal
state and establishing the child’s sensory responsiveness. Challenge to motor
skills and modulation is ongoing and toward the goal of increasing tolerance for
the changing environment and the child’s ability to make an adaptive response. A
“sensory diet” is frequently set up with the family. This refers to evaluating a
child’s day and keying into those times and activities that appear to be most
disruptive for the child. Evaluating whether incorporating a sensory component
into that activity would assist with completion, as well as increase the learning
potential for the child, is one of the roles of the occupational therapist. As an
example, a bedtime routine is frequently a challenge for families. The child may
be in a state of overarousal and unable to stay in bed or sleep. The sequence of
events prior to bedtime may be examined and evaluated with the parents. A
typical experience may be that the child has been free-playing until bedtime. The
parent tries to make the child brush his teeth, wash, change into nightclothes, and
settle with a book to sleep. However, the child is not at a place where transition to
sleep can be easily accomplished. Changing the routines to have a time to brush
teeth (a high-arousal activity), then providing deep proprioceptive input, slow
vestibular input (slow movement in a glider-rocker chair, wrapup in bath towels),
and evaluating the sleep area (use of weighted blanket or sleeping bag) may assist
with the transition. A sensory diet is an ongoing evaluation of the child’s status
and must be flexible. It requires task analysis from the therapist and the family for
it to be successful.
Other interventions focus on tactile and oral processing. The defensive
behaviors discussed previously are responsive to touch pressure as well as
increasing discrimination. Slow exposure to tactile experience and acceptance of
input is often the focus of therapy. Setting up the environment to support the
child’s ability to integrate sensory experience is essential for a successful
experience. Oral motor and feeding issues involve motor planning. A child with
ASD may not be able to plan the movements of bringing food to the mouth,
chewing, and swallowing. Tactile and visual aversions to food may be present. To
be successful in promoting good nutrition, the therapist requires a good
understanding of the child’s sensory status and needs to use a sensory model
within the context of a behavioral model.
Interventions include providing the opportunity to increase body awareness
and giving proprioceptive and tactile inputs for the body to recognize where it is
in relation to position in space. Incorporating sensory motor stimulation in a
directed manner assists in the development of motor skills, because successful
motor planning requires adequate sensory processing. The therapist teaches
motor planning and sequential motor skills so that the child is able to perform
complex motor skills and react in a functional manner to novel motor
requirements.
home and school, and then come back when new developmental or sensory issues
arise. Regular therapy services should include a home and school carryover
program and instructions should be provided to the parents and teachers. Therapy
performance and goals should be shared with the medical and instructional team
for optimal outcomes. Occupational therapy services with a sensory integration
focus could also occur via a consultation mode with the therapist observing the
child monthly in a school or home setting and provide ideas, activities, and
suggestions to be incorporated into regular daily activities. For children with
moderate to severe impairment, the occupational therapists should work closely
with educators and speech pathologists as a team. For children with mild
impairment, the therapists should work with the families to evaluate how sensory
issues impact functional performance and develop strategies to assist the child in
activities of daily living.
As with most forms of intervention, the earlier the child receives therapy,
the more effect it will have on learning and functional performance.
REFERENCES
1. Grandin T, Scariano MM. Emergence: Labeled Autistic. Novato, CA: Arena Press,
1986:20, 76.
2. Ayres AJ. Sensory Integration and the Child. Los Angeles, CA: Western
Psychological Services, 1979:5, 14, 59 – 66, 69 – 75, 82 – 83, 83 – 88, 91 – 101.
3. Kranowitz CS. The Out-of-Sync Child: Recognizing and Coping with Sensory
Integration Dysfunction. New York: Berkley Publishing Group, 1998:8, 40 – 42,
42 – 47, 95 –101.
4. www.ot-innovations.com/jerry.html
5. Dunn W. Sensory Profile User’s Manual. San Antonio, Texas: Psychological
Corporation, 1999:1– 5, 33– 36, 35.
6. Reisman JE, Hanschu B. Sensory Integration Inventory –Revised: For Individuals
with Developmental Disabilities. Hugo, MN: PDP Press, 1999:2 – 5, 8 – 9, 20–22.
7. Case-Smith J. Occupational Therapy for Children. 4th ed. St. Louis: Mosby,
2001:359.
8. Folio MR, Fewell RR. Peabody Developmental Motor Scales Examiner’s Manual.
2nd ed. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed, 2000:3, 4, 10.
9. Bruininks RH. Bruininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency Examiner’s Manual.
Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service, 1978:11, 43.
10. Beery KE. The Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration:
Administration Scoring and Teaching Manual. 4th ed. Parsippany, NJ: Modern
Curriculum Press, 1997:21 – 22.
11. Hammill DD, Pearson NA, Voress JK. Developmental Test of Visual Perception
Examiner’s Manual. 2nd ed. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed, 1993:5.
12. Sellin B. I Don’t Want to Be Inside Me Anymore: Messages from an Autistic Mind.
New York: Basic Books, 1995.
13. Berk RA, DeGang GA. Test of Sensory Function in Infants (TSFI). Los Angeles,
CA: Western Psychological Services, 1990:1, 7.
I. INTRODUCTION
A. Psychostimulants
Although psychostimulants, such as methylphenidate and dextroamphetamine,
have shown modest effects in reducing overactivity (2– 4), the effects on
attention and distractibility are not so obvious. Side effects of psychostimulants,
such as irritability, anxiety, agitation, insomnia, aggression, delusions, and
worsening of social withdrawal and stereotypies, have been reported more often
in individuals with autism (5). Handen et al. reported positive response in 8/13
subjects but adverse effects, such as social withdrawal and irritability, were noted
at higher doses (6). Others have reported therapeutic benefits without adverse side
effects (4). Stimulants are more useful in children with Asperger’s syndrome and
PDD-NOS than in autism, and are not so helpful in patients who persevere within
a narrow range of activities (6). Theoretically stimulants have the potential to
increase stereotypic behaviors.
B. a2 -Adrenergic Agonists
Clonidine, an a2 -adrenergic agonist, has a modest effect in reducing hyperactivity,
overarousal, and irritability, but has little effect on the social and communication
abnormalities (7,8). Fankhauser et al. reported improvement in stereotyped body
movements, self-stimulation, hypervigilance, and hyperactivity with weekly
clonidine patch treatment in a double-blind, placebo-crossover study, but side
effects such as sedation and fatigue occurred during the first 2 weeks of clonidine
treatment (7). Clonidine was modestly effective in the short-term treatment of
irritability and hyperactivity in children with autism in another study (8).
C. Antipsychotics (Neuroleptics)
Although older antipsychotics (neuroleptics), such as haloperidol, were effective
in treating hyperactivity and aggression (9), they resulted in significant adverse
effects, particularly tardive dyskinesia (10). Newer or atypical neuroleptics, such
as risperidone and olanzapine, hold more promise in reducing hyperactivity,
impulsivity, perseverative behaviors, and aggression with much less risk of
tardive dyskinesia. Risperidone, an HT2A and dopamine D2 antagonist, has been
studied the most frequently (11 – 14). Besides open-label trials (12,13), a multi-
center, double-blind, placebo-controlled study has indicated that risperidone may
be effective in children and adolescents in reducing temper tantrums, aggression,
Some children with autism have stereotypic or repetitive motor behaviors, such
as body rocking and spinning, flicking hands in front of the face, twisting of
hands, twirling an object, and gesticulations. They also repeat motor sequences or
scripts such as lining up toys, breaking the line, and lining them up again. Some
indulge in repetitive self-stimulatory behavior such as rocking, swaying, and
lying on the floor. Some repetitive behaviors, such head banging and hand biting,
cause injury but perpetuate themselves, perhaps because of endorphin release.
Other children with autism have compulsive behaviors such as hording or tearing
paper or turning the faucet on and off. The underlying basis of repetitive
behaviors in autism is unclear. Unlike typical obsessive compulsive disorder
(OCD), these are not due to uncontrollable and distressing obsessions, but
perhaps due to inflexibility, immaturity, or cognitive limitation. They might even
have some adaptive value like stress reduction, sensory stimulation, and social
attention, and escape from demands (31). Therefore, repetitive behaviors should
be treated only if they are maladaptive; that is, they interfere with programming,
worsen the quality of life, or are potentially harmful to the child.
Serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SRIs) and atypical neuroleptics are the
major drug groups that have been used to treat repetitive, ritualistic, and
stereoptypic behaviors in individuals with autism.
IV. AGGRESSION
A. Antipsychotics (Neuroleptics)
Increased dopaminergic activity has been postulated to be a cause of
aggression in animals (56). Extrapolating from the animal data, antipsychotics
that block dopaminergic receptors, such as haloperidol, have been the mainstay
of treatment of aggressive behavior in psychiatry. Campbell et al. conducted
several controlled drug trials of haloperidol in autism reporting beneficial
effects. Combination of haloperidol with behavior treatments resulted in
acquisition of imitative speech and social skills. Haloperidol also decreased
fidgetiness, withdrawal, and stereotypies and improved relatedness to the
examiner. However, on long-term follow-up, 34% of children developed
tardive or withdrawal dyskinesias, involving the face and mouth. Most of these
movements were reversible in the long run (57). The newer antipsychotics,
such as risperidone and olanzapine, cause fewer side effects and are replacing
haloperidol as medications of choice. Risperidone, a 5-HT2 receptor and
D2-receptor antagonist, has been found to reduce behavioral symptoms such as
aggression, self-injurious repetitive movements, and hyperactivity in many
studies, both controlled and uncontrolled. Adverse side effects include weight
gain, sedation, and galactorrhea (58). Olanzapine was found to be comparable
to haloperidol in an open-label trial (59). Quetiapine, on the other hand, was
not well tolerated by children with PDD (60), and side effects of clonazepine,
particularly agranulocytosis, preclude its use in autism, except as a last
resort (61).
C. Anticonvulsants
Theoretically anticonvulsants may improve the behavior of individuals with autism
by controlling epilepsy and underlying bipolar disorder (65). There are case reports
of individuals with autism whose behavior and language and social skills improved
after anticonvulsant therapy (66–68). Many of these had epileptiform discharge on
EEG with or without epilepsy. It has been suggested that some children with autism
may respond to anticonvulsants because of shared neuropathology, such as
abnormal electric activity in the amygdale (65). According to Rapin, although
epilepsy may play a minor role in a few children with regressive autism, it is
uncertain if anticonvulsants can stall autistic regression (69,70).
Affective anticonvulsants, valproic acid (VPA) and carbamazepine, are
used to treat aggression, irritability, and bipolar disorder in individuals with
mental retardation, with or without autism. A study of VPA in the treatment of
patients with intellectual disability and aggressive or self-injurious behavior
found a moderate to marked improvement in 71% of subjects. In 82% of the
subjects there was a significant reduction in aggression and self-injurious
behavior, in 46% other psychotropic medications could be discontinued, and in
39% the dose of other (71) psychotropic medication could be reduced. Although
it is difficult to diagnose bipolar disorder in individuals with autism, it is likely
that aggressive and irritability in some individuals with autism may be a
manifestation of underlying affective disorder. This view is strengthened by the
finding of higher prevalence of affective disorders in families of individuals with
autism (72). However, anticonvulsants can have serious side effects (73,74) and
should be used after careful risk-benefit analysis in individuals with autism also
who have epilepsy, epileptiform discharge on EEG, cyclic aggression and
irritability (75).
D. Trazodone (Desyrel)
Trazodone, an antidepressant that is neither tri- nor tetracyclic, has been
mentioned as treatment of aggression and violence in autistic children (76). The
response rate is 25– 30% and side effects, such as orthostatic hypotension and leg
swelling and pain, can occur. In males it can cause priapism.
E. Lithium
Lithium maybe used in individuals who are aggressive, agitated, and overaroused,
and have cycles of overarousal and withdrawal. But its side effects and the need
to draw blood for levels and tests limit its use in individuals with autism.
Buspirone and beta-blockers have also been used to treat aggressive
behavior in autism. These have been discussed in other sections.
V. ANXIETY
Children with autistic spectrum disorder often have symptoms of anxiety (77),
particularly in unfamiliar environments, when exposed to certain environmental
stimuli, when the familiar order in their environment is changed, or when their
rigidity and repetitive behavior is challenged (78). Buspirone, a 5-HT1A receptor
agonist, has been found to be safe and effective in controlling the symptoms of
anxiety in autism in double-blind, placebo-controlled crossover study (79). It
belongs to the azipirone group of medications, does not affect mental alertness as
other anxiolytics do, and is not addictive. At higher doses it inhibits 5-HT 2A
receptors, and decreases striatal levels of serotonin and its metabolites. Buspirone
has effects on dopamine, norepinephrine, and the GABA systems as well. It takes
about 2 – 4 weeks for its full effects, although mild effects are seen within 1 week
(80). In an open-label trial buspirone resulted in marked improvement in the
target symptoms of anxiety and irritability in children with autism (81). It has also
been found to decrease aggression, hyperactivity, stereotypies, and self-injurious
behavior (82). Others have shown mixed results (83,84). In an open-label study,
there was worsening of aggression with buspirone (84). Rarely increase in
agitation and aggression and involuntary movements of the mouth have also been
reported (85).
A. Opioid Antagonists
Positive results for self-injurious behavior have been reported with naltrexone, a
long-acting narcotic antagonist (87 –89). Naltrexone has been reported to
decrease hyperactivity, irritability, and withdrawal, and to increase verbal output
and attentiveness (90 – 93). Despite its initial promise, several double-blind
control trials have either failed to show significant effects of naltrexone on
behavioral symptoms of autism or have found rather small effects, particularly
on the core symptoms of social withdrawal and communication deficits
(94 –96). Others have argued that only those individuals who have evidence
of dysregulation of the propiomelanocortin system with elevated levels of
C-terminal b-endorphin and serotonin benefit from naltrexone (95,97).
B. b-Adrenergic Blockers
b-Adrenergic blockers, such as propranolol and nadolol, have been reported to
decrease aggressive outbursts and self-injurious and stereotypic behavior in a few
open-label studies and case reports of individuals with mental retardation and
autism. Secondary improvement in attention (98–100), language, and social skills
was also reported in the one study. Nadolol was reported to be better than
propranolol in a case report (101).
The use of b-adrenergic blockers is limited by their side effects of
hypotension, bradycardia, exacerbation of asthma, and nightmares. For a detailed
review of b-adrenergic blockers in aggression see Haspel (102).
Individuals with autism often have sleep problems, such as difficulties in settling
down, frequent and prolonged nighttime awakenings, night terrors, and distur-
bances of sleep-wake cycle (103). Sleep disturbances may be due to disturbance
of the sleep-wake cycle, underlying medical conditions, anxiety, or psychosocial
stress. Disturbances of nighttime sleep affect daytime behavior and also cause
parental stress (104). While behavioral interventions are the mainstay of manage-
ment of sleep problems, medications are sometimes necessary. Short-acting
benzodiazepines, such as triazolam (Halcion) and flurazepam (Dalmane), may
have a short-term role in the treatment of pediatric insomnia but are not approved
for use in children in the United States. However, tachyphylaxis and risk of
misuse preclude the long-term use of benzodiazepines for the treatment of
insomnia in children. Alimemazine (trimeprazine), a phenothiazine, has been
shown to be effective in the short-term treatment of insomnia in young children,
but does not have U.S. Food and Drug Administration approval for pediatric
insomnia (105). Niaprazine, a histamine H1-receptor antagonist with sedative
properties, was found to treat insomnia in an open-label study of 25 children and
young people with autism and a range of severity of mental retardation (age range
2 –20 years) (106). Individuals with mild-to-moderate mental retardation were
selectively better responders. Newer hypnotics, such as zolpidem (Ambien),
zopiclone (Imovane), and Zaleplon (Sonata), which appear better tolerated and
less habit-forming than the benzodiazepines in studies of adults, may have a role
when combined with psychosocial treatments for pediatric insomnia (107,108).
Melatonin, a pineal gland hormone, was found to be effective in treating
insomnia in a recent uncontrolled study of 50 children and young adults with
developmental disorders (age range 3– 28 years) (109). The results in people with
mental retardation have been mixed (110). Side effects, such as residual
drowsiness the next morning, awakening in the middle of the night, and excite-
ment after awakening and before going to bed, can occur but are usually mild.
There is no consensus about dosage and the mode of administration. It is
uncertain if melatonin needs to be given continuously or can be phased out after
sleep pattern has improved (111). The recommended dose is 0.5– 3 mg/day.
VIII. SUMMARY
Table 4 summarizes the major drug groups and their indications in autism. The
number of medications that have been or are being tried for autism is too large to
be discussed in this volume. Some, such as fenfluramine, have become history.
It was withdrawn from the market because of side effects. Others, such as
lofexidine, amantadine, cyproheptadine, famotidine, and donepezil, have been
tried in individual cases, with mixed results. Unless the neurochemical basis of
autism is discovered, a specific drug in unlikely to be available for autism. Until
then drug therapy will remain, at best, an adjunct to behavioral and educational
interventions.
Anxiety/
Repetitive Hyperactivity Aggression, affective
behavior impulsivity self-injury symptoms
SSRI X
Atypical X X X X
antipsychotics
Stimulants X
Naltrexone X
Clonidine X X X
Lithium X
b-blockers X
Anticonvulsants X
Buspirone X
Source: Adapted from Williemsen-Swinkels SHN, Buitelaar JK. The autistic spectrum: subgroups,
boundaries, and treatment. Psychiatr Clin North Am 2002; 25:811–836.
REFERENCES
16. Potenza MN, Holmes JP, Kanes SJ, McDougle CJ. Olanzapine treatment of
children, adolescents, and adults with pervasive developmental disorders: an open-
label pilot study. J Clin Psychopharmacol 1999; 19:37 – 44.
17. Malone RP, Cater J, Sheikh RM, Choudhury MS, Delaney MA. Olanzapine versus
haloperidol in children with autistic disorder: an open pilot study. J Am Acad Child
Adolesc Psychiatry 2001; 40:887– 894.
18. McDougle CJ, Kem DL, Posey DJ. Case series: use of ziprasidone for maladaptive
symptoms in youths with autism. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2002;
41:921 – 927.
19. Gobbi G, Pulvirenti L. Long-term treatment with clozapine in an adult with autistic
disorder accompanied by aggressive behaviour. J Psychiatry Neurosci 2001;
26:340 – 341.
20. Chen NC, Bedair HS, McKay B, Bowers MB Jr, Mazure C. Clozapine in the
treatment of aggression in an adolescent with autistic disorder. J Clin Psychiatry
2001; 62:479 – 480.
21. Martin A, Koenig K, Scahill L, Bregman J. Open-label quetiapine in the treatment
of children and adolescents with autistic disorder. J Child Adolesc Psychopharma-
col 1999; 9:99 – 107.
22. Gordon CT, Rappaport JL, Hamburger SD, State RC, Manheim GB. Differential
response of seven subjects with autistic disorder to clomipramine and desipramine.
Am J Psychiatry 1992; 149:363– 366.
23. Posey DJ, Guenin KD, Kohn AE, Swiezy NB, McDougle CJ. A naturalistic open-
label study of mirtazapine in autistic and other pervasive developmental disorders.
J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol 2001; 11:267 – 277.
24. Fatemi SH, Realmuto GM, Khan L, Thuras P. Fluoxetine in treatment of adolescent
patients with autism: a longitudinal open trial. J Autism Dev Disord 1998;
28:303 – 307.
25. Realmuto GM, August GJ, Garfinkel BD. Clinical effect of buspirone in autistic
children. J Clin Psychopharmacol 1989; 9:122 – 125.
26. Campbell M, Anderson LT, Small AM, Locascio JJ, Lynch NS, Choroco MC.
Naltrexone in autistic children: a double-blind and placebocontrolled trial.
Psychopharmacol Bull 1990; 26:130 – 135.
27. Kolmen BK, Feldman HM, Handen BL, Janosky JE. Naltrexone in young autistic
children: replication study and learning measures. J Am Acad Child Adolesc
Psychiatry 1997; 36:1570– 1578.
28. Willemsen-Swinkels SH, Buitelaar JK, van Berckelaer-Onnes IA, van
Engeland H. Brief report: six months continuation treatment in naltrexone-
responsive children with autism: an open-label case-control design. J Autism Dev
Disord 1999; 29:167– 169.
29. Willemsen-Swinkels SH, Buitelaar JK, van Engeland H. The effects of chronic
naltrexone treatment in young autistic children: a double-blind placebo-controlled
crossover study. Biol Psychiatry 1996; 39:1023– 1031.
30. Rugino TA, Samsock TC. Levetiracetam in autistic children: an open-label study.
J Dev Behav Pediatr 2002; 23:225 – 230.
31. Mason G. Stereotypis: a critical review. Anim Behav 1991; 41:1015– 1037.
32. Awad GA. The use of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors in young children with
pervasive developmental disorders: some clinical observations. Can J Psychiatry
1996; 41:361 –366.
33. Lewis MH, Bodfish JW, Powell SB, Parker DE, Golden RN. Clomipramine
treatment for self-injurious behavior of individuals with mental retardation:
a double-blind comparison with placebo. Am J Ment Retard 1996; 100:654 – 665.
34. Gordon CT, State RC, Nelson JE, Hamburger SD, Rapoport JL. A double-blind
comparison of clomipramine, desipramine, and placebo in the treatment of autistic
disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993; 50:441– 447.
35. McDougle CJ, Kresch LE, Posey DJ. Repetitive thoughts and behavior in pervasive
developmental disorders: treatment with serotonin reuptake inhibitors. J Autism
Dev Disord 2000; 30:427 – 435.
36. Garber HJ, McGonigle JJ, Slomka GT, Monteverde E. Clomipramine treatment of
stereotypic behaviors and self-injury in patients with developmental disabilities.
J Am Acad Child Adoclesc Psychiatry 1992; 31:1157 – 1160.
37. Brodkin ES, McDogle CJ, Naylor ST, Cohen DJ, Price LH. Clomipramine in adults
with pervasive developmental disorders: a prospective open-label investigation.
J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol 1997; 7:109– 121.
38. Brasic JR, Barnett JY, Sheitman BB, Lafargue RT, Kowalik S, Kaplan D, et al.
Behavioral effects of clomipramine on prepubertal boys with autistic disorder and
severe mental retardation. CNS Spectrum 1998; 3:39– 46.
39. Sanchez LE, Campbell M, Small AM, Cueva JE, Armenteros JL, Adams PB. A pilot
study of clomipramine in young autistic children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc
Psychiatry 1996; 35:537 – 544.
40. McDougle CJ, Naylor ST, Cohen DJ, Volkmar FR, Heninger GR, Proce LH.
A double blind placebo controlled study of fluvoxamine in adults with autistic
disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1996; 53:1001– 1008.
41. Kauffmann C, Vance H, Pumariega AJ, Miller B. Fluvoxamine treatment of a child
with severe PDD: a single case study. Psychiatry 2001; 64:268 – 277.
42. Cook-EHJ, Rowlett R, Jaselskis C, Leventhal BL. Fluoxetine treatment of children
and adults with autistic disorder and mental retardation. J Am Acad Child Adolesc
Psychiatry 1992; 31:739 – 745.
43. Fatemi SH, Realmuto GM, Khan L, Thuras P. Fluoxetine in treatment of adolescent
patients with autism: a longitudinal open trial. J Autism Dev Disord 1998; 28:303–307.
44. Bradford D, Bhaumik S, Naik B. Selective serotonin re-uptake inhibitors for the
treatment of perseverative and maladaptive behaviours of people with intellectual
disability. J Intellect Disabil Res 1998; 42(Pt 4):301 –306.
45. DeLong GR, Ritch CR, Burch S. Fluoxetine response in children with autistic
spectrum disorders: correlation with familial major affective disorder and
intellectual achievement. Dev Med Child Neurol 2002; 44:652 –659.
46. Posey DI, Litwiller M, Koburn M, McDougle CJ. Paroxetine in autism. J Am Acad
Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1999; 38:111 – 112.
47. Steingard RJ, Zimnitzky B, DeMaso DR, Bauman ML, Bucci JP. Sertraline
treatment of transition-associated anxiety and agitation in children with autistic
disorder. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol 1997; 7:9– 15.
48. Hellings JA, Kelley LA, Gabrielli WF, Kilgore E, Shah P. Sertraline response in
adults with mental retardation and autistic disorder. J Clin Psychiatry 1996;
57:333 – 336.
49. McDougle CJ, Kresch LE; Posey DJ Compulsive behavior: Repetitive thoughts and
behavior in pervasive developmental disorders: treatment with serotonin reuptake
inhibitors. J Autism Dev Disord 2000; 30:427 – 435.
50. Lindenmayer JP. The pathophysiology of agitation. J Clin Psychiatry 2000;
61(suppl 14):5– 10.
51. Eichelman B, Hartwig AC. Discussion: biological correlates of aggression. Ann NY
Acad Sci 1996; 794:78 – 81.
52. Eichelman B. Toward a rational pharmacotherapy for aggressive and violent
behavior. Hosp Commun Psychiatry 1988; 39:31– 39.
53. Miczek KA, Weerts E, Haney M, Tidey J. Neurobiological mechanisms controlling
aggression: preclinical developments for pharmacotherapeutic interventions.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1994; 18:97 – 110.
54. Barnard L, Young AH, Pearson J, Geddes J, O’Brien G. A systematic review of the
use of atypical antipsychotics in autism. J Psychopharmacol 2002; 16:93 – 101.
55. Kim E. The use of newer anticonvulsants in neuropsychiatric disorders. Curr
Psychiatry Rep 2002; 4:331 – 337.
56. van Erp AM, Miczec KA. Aggressive behavior, increased accumbal dopamine, and
decreased cortical serotonin in rats. J Neurosci 2000; 20:9320 – 9325.
57. Campbell M, Armenteros JL, Malone RP, et al. Neuroleptice-related dyskinesias in
autistic children.: a prospective , longitudinal study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc
Psychiatry 1997; 36:835– 843.
58. Masi G, Cosenza A, Mucci M, De Vito G. Risperidone monotherapy in
preschool children with pervasive developmental disorders. J Child Neurol 2001;
16:395 – 400.
59. Malone RP, Cater J, Sheikh RM, Choudhury MS, Delaney MA. Olanzapine versus
haloperidol in children with autistic disorder: an open pilot study. J Am Acad Child
Adolesc Psychiatry 2001; 40:887– 894.
60. Martin A. Koenig K, Scahill L, Bregman J. Open-label quetiapine in treatment of
children and adolescents with autistic disorder. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol
1999; 9:99 – 107.
61. Gobbi G, Pulvirenti L. Long-term treatment with clozapine in an adult with autistic
disorder accompanied by aggressive behaviour. J Psychiatry Neurosci 2001;
26:340 – 341.
62. Mitsis EM, Halperin JM, Newcorn JH. Serotonin and aggression in children. Curr
Psychiatry Rep 2000; 2:95 – 101.
63. Mehlman PT, Higley JD, Faucher I, Lilly AA, Taub DM, Vickers J, Suomi SJ,
Linnoila M. Low CSF 5-HIAA concentrations and severe aggression and
impaired impulse control in nonhuman primates. Am J Psychiatry 1994;
151:1485 – 1491.
64. Armenteros JL, Lewis JE. Citalopram treatment for impulsive aggression in children
and adolescents: an open pilot study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2002;
41:522–529.
65. Di Martino A, Tuchman RF. Antiepileptic drugs: affective use in autism spectrum
disorders. Pediatr Neurol 2001; 25:199 – 207.
66. Pilopys AV. Autism: electroencephalogram abnormalities and clinical improve-
ment with valproic acid. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 1994; 148:220 – 222.
67. Childs JA, Blair JL. Valproic acid treatment of epilepsy in autistic twins. J Neurosci
Nurs 1997; 29:244– 248.
68. Bardenstein R, Chez MG, Helfand BT, Buchanan C, Zucker M. Improvement in
EEG and clinical function in pervasive developmental disorder (PDD): effect of
valproic acid. Neurology 1998; 50:a86.
69. Rapin I. Autistic regression and disintegrative disorder: how important the role of
epilepsy? Semin Pediatr Neurol 1995; 2:278 –285.
70. Tuchman RF, Rapin I. Regression in pervasive developmental disorders: seizures
and epileptiform electroencephalogram correlates. Pediatrics 1997; 99:560 – 566.
71. Ruedrich S, Swales TP, Fossaceca C, Toliver J, Rutkowski A. Effect of divalproex
sodium on aggression and self-injurious behavior in adults with intellectual
disability: a retrospective review. J Intell Disabil Res 1999; 43:105– 111.
72. Bolton PF, Pickles A, Murphy M, Rutter M. Autism, affective and other psychiatric
disorders: patterns of familial aggregation. Psychol Med 1998; 28:385 –394.
73. Calabrese JR, Goethe JW, Kayser A, Marcotte DB, Monagin JA, Kimmel SE,
Brugger AM, Morris D, Fatemi SH. Adverse events in 583 valproate-treated
patients. Depression 1996; 3:257 – 262.
74. Beran RG, Gibson RJ. Aggressive behaviour in intellectually challenged patients
with epilepsy treated with lamotrigine. Epilepsia 1998; 39:280– 282.
75. Hollander E, Dolgoff-Kaspar R, Cartwright C, Rawitt R, Novotny S. An open trial
of divalproex sodium in autism spectrum disorders. J Clin Psychiatry 2001;
62:530– 534.
76. Gedye A, Trazodone reduced aggressive and self-injurious movements in a mentally
handicapped male patient with autism. J Clin Psychopharmacol 1991; 11:275 – 276.
77. Muris P, Steerneman P, Merckelbach H, Holdrinet I, Meesters C. Comorbid anxiety
symptoms in children with pervasive developmental disorders. J Anxiety Disord
1998; 12:387 –393.
78. Gillott A, Furniss F, Walter A. Anxiety in high-functioning children with autism.
Autism 2001; 5:277– 286.
79. McCormick LH. Treatment with buspirone in a patient with autism. Arch Fam Med
1997; 6:368 –370.
80. Schweitzer E, Rickels K. Strategies for treatment of generalized anxiety disorder in
the primary care setting. J Clin Psychiatry 1997; 58(suppl 3):27– 31.
81. Buitelaar JK, van der Gaag RJ, van der Hoeven J. Buspirone in the management of
anxiety and irritability in children with pervasive developmental disorders: results
of an open label study. J Clin Psychiatry 1999; 59:56 – 59.
82. Ratey JJ, Sovner R, Mikkelsen E, Chmielinski HE. Buspirone therapy for
maladaptive behavior and anxiety in developmentally disabled persons. J Clin
Psychiatry 1989; 50:382 – 384.
83. Realmuto GM, August GJ, Garfinkel BD. Clinical effect of buspirone in autistic
children. J Clin Psychopharmacol 1989; 9:122– 125.
84. King BH, Davanzo P. Buspirone treatment of aggression and self-injury in autistic
and non-autistic persons with severe mental retardation. Dev Brain Dysfunct 1996;
6:368 – 687.
85. Posey DJ. The pharmacotherapy of target symptoms associated with autistic
disorder and other pervasive developmental disorders. Harv Rev Psychiatry 2000;
8:45 – 63.
86. Sandman CA. beta-Endorphin dysregulation in autistic and self-injurious behavior:
a neurodevelopmental hypothesis. Synapse 1988; 2:193– 199.
87. Herman BH, Hammock MK, Arthur-Smith A, et al. Naltrexone decreases self-
injurious behavior. Ann Neurol 1987; 22:550– 552.
88. Lienemann J, Walker F. Reversal of self-abusive behavior with naltrexone (letter to
the editor). J Clin Psychopharmacol 1989; 9:448– 449.
89. Sandman CA, Barron JL, Colman H. An orally administered opiate blocker,
naltrexone, attenuates self-injurious behavior. Am J Ment Retard 1990; 95:93– 102.
90. Willemson-Swinkels SH, Buitelaar JK, vanEngeland H. The effects of chronic
naltrexone treatment in young autistic children: a double-blind placebo-controlled
crossover study. Biol Psychiatry 1996; 39:1023– 1031.
91. Panksepp J, Lensing P. Brief report: a synopsis of an open-trial of naltrexone
treatment of autism with four children. J Autism Dev Disord 1991;
21:243 – 249.
92. Campbell M, Anderson LT, Small AM, Adams P, Gonzalez NM, Ernst
M. Naltrexone in autistic children: behavioral symptoms and attentional learning.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1993; 32:1283– 1291.
93. Campbell M, Anderson LT, Small AM, Locascio JJ, Lynch NS, Choroco MC.
Naltrexone in autistic children: a double-blind and placebo controlled study.
Psychopharmacol Bull 1990; 26:130 – 135.
94. Willemson-Swinkels SHN, Buitelaar JK, Nijhof GJ, Van Engeland H. Failure of
naltrexone hydrochloride to reduce self-injurious and autistic behavior in mentally
retarded adults: double blind placebo-controlled studies. Arch Gen Psychiatry
1995; 52:766 – 773.
95. Bouvard MP, Leboyer M, Launav JM, Recasens C, Plumet MH, Waller-Perotte D,
Tabuteau F, Bondoux D, Dugas M, Lensing P, et al. Low-dose naltrexone effects on
plasma chemistries and clinical symptoms in autism: a double-blind, placebo-
controlled study. Psychiatry Res 1995; 58:191 – 201.
96. Kolmen BK, Feldman HM, Handen BL, Janosky JE. Naltrexone in young autistic
children: replication study and learning measures. J Am Acad Child Adolesc
Psychiatry 1997; 36:1570– 1578.
97. Sandman CA, Touchette P, Marion S, Lenjavi M, Chicz-Demet A. Disregulation of
propiomelanocortin and contagious maladaptive behavior. Regul Pept 2002;
108:179 – 185.
98. Williams DT, Mehl R, Yudofsky S, Adams D, Roseman B. The effect of
propranolol on uncontrolled rage outbursts in children and adolescents with organic
brain dysfunction. J Am Acad Child Psychiatry 1982; 21:129– 135.
99. Ratey JJ, Bemporad J, Sorgi P, Bick P, Polakoff S, O’Driscoll G, Mikkelsen E, et al.
Open trial effects of beta-blockers on speech and social behaviors in 8 autistic
adults. J Autism Dev Disord 1987; 17:439 – 446.
100. Cohen IL, Tsiouris JA, Pfadt A. Effects of long-acting propranolol on agonistic and
stereotyped behaviors in a man with pervasive developmental disorder and fragile
X syndrome: a double-blind, placebo-controlled study. J Clin Psychopharmacol
1991; 11:398 –399.
101. Connor DF, Ozbayrak KR, Benjamin S, Ma Y, Fletcher KE. A pilot study of
nadolol for overt aggression in developmentally delayed individuals. J Am Acad
Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1997; 36:826 – 834.
102. Haspel T. Beta-blockers and the treatment of aggression. Harv Rev Psychiatry
1995; 2:274 –281.
103. Richdale A. Sleep problems in autism: prevalence, cause and intervention. Dev
Med Child Neurol 1999; 41:60 – 66.
104. Younus M, Labellarte MJ. Insomnia in children: when are hypnotics indicated?
Paediatr Drugs 2002; 4:391– 403.
105. Lancioni GE, O’Reilly MF, Basili G. Review of strategies for treating sleep
problems in persons with severe or profound mental retardation or multiple
handicaps. Am J Ment Retard 1999; 104:170– 186.
106. Rossi PG, Posar A, Parmaggianni A, et al. Niaprazine in the treatment of autistic
disorder. J Child Neurol 1999; 14:547 – 550.
107. Kripke DF. Chronic hypnotic use: deadly risks, doubtful benefit. Sleep Med Rev
2000; 4:5– 20.
108. Patat A, Paty I, Hindmarch I. Pharmacodynamic profile of Zaleplon, a new non-
benzodiazepine hypnotic agent. Hum Psychopharmacol 2001; 16:369 – 392.
109. Ishizaki A, Sugama M, Takeuchi N. Usefulness of melatonin for developmental
sleep and emotional/behavior disorders: studies of melatonin trial on 50 patients
with developmental disorders. No to Hattatsu 1999; 31:428 – 437.
110. Mendelson WB. A critical evaluation of the hypnotic efficacy of melatonin. Sleep
1997; 20:916 –919.
111. Sack RL, Hughes RJ, Edgar DM, Lewy AJ. Sleep-promoting effects of melatonin:
at what dose, in whom, under what conditions, and by what mechanisms? Sleep
1997; 20:908 –915.
I. INTRODUCTION
A. Dietary Manipulation
Proponents of the gastrointestinal theory of autism posit that autism occurs
because of the absorption of toxic peptides from the gut and, therefore, can be
treated by eliminating the source of these toxins from the diet. See Chapter 2 for
details.
B. Dietary Supplementation
1. Megavitamin B6
In 1978, Rimland et al. reported that some autistic children responded favorably
to high doses of vitamin B6 (orthomolecular treatment) (12). In a subsequent trial,
Rimland observed that some children experienced increased irritability, sound
sensitivity, nausea, increased excitability, increased autistic symptoms, loose
stools, and upper respiratory infection when they were given large amounts of
vitamin B6 (pyridoxine), but these problems disappeared when increased
amounts of magnesium were added to the diet. Subsequent trials have, therefore,
used pyridoxine with magnesium. The advocates of this treatment claim that the
favorable changes in behavior are not due to general sedation but represent
improvement in symptoms associated with autism. Although vitamin B6
deficiency has not been documented in individuals with autism, according to the
proponents, subclinical deficiencies could be present in individuals with autism.
The effects could be mediated by neurotransmitters, because vitamin B6 is
involved in the formation of dopamine and other neurotransmitters (i.e.,
serotonin, g-aminobutyric acid, norepinephrine, and epinephrine) (13). Some
studies have, indeed, documented a tendency toward normalization of urinary
homovanillic acid levels and evoked potentials with pyridoxine and magnesium
(13,14).
Sensory neuropathy, a serious side effect, has been reported with very high
doses of pyridoxine in the literature at large (15), but in studies that use 1 g/
kg/day of vitamin B6, few participants reported adverse side effects and the
adverse effects were relatively minor and manageable. Once magnesium was
added to the megavitamin therapy, adverse effects were even less. In the trials,
dose of vitamin B6 ranged from 1 mg/kg to 30 mg/kg, and of magnesium, from
2. Other Vitamins
Supplementation with other vitamins, such as vitamin A, folic acid, vitamin E,
vitamin B1, vitamin B12, and vitamin C, is also recommended by some. As long
as the dose of vitamins is within the recommended dietary allowance or slightly
higher, the treatment may be harmless, but megadoses of vitamins, particularly
vitamin A, D, and C, can be toxic. Vitamin C in the dose of 8 g/70 kg/day in
divided dose was found to decrease stereotypic behaviors in a small sample of
individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and mental retardation in a
double-blind, placebo-controlled study. Except for a risk of renal stones this
therapy seems to be benign (17).
3. Minerals
Advocates of trace and other minerals claim that children with autism have low
levels of calcium, magnesium, copper, manganese, and chromium and higher
levels of lithium and mercury in their hair samples as compared to age-and-sex-
matched controls. Based on this premise, children with autism are given
supplements of common minerals (calcium, magnesium, copper, manganese,
zinc, chromium, and cobalt), and trace minerals (vanadium, germanium,
selenium, tungsten, molybdenum, and tin). Minerals, such as zinc, have
important functions in brain development and function, but dietary deficiencies
or metabolic abnormalities involving trace elements are rare and have not been
shown to cause autism. If the supplement contains doses within or slightly
beyond the recommended dietary allowance, the treatment is harmless.
4. Fatty Acids
Supplementation with long-chain fatty acids (especially omega-3 and omega-6)
has been alleged to improve the symptoms of autism in a few individuals, but
there is no empirical or theoretical proof of their efficacy. The proponents argue
that long-chain fatty acids restore the imbalance of prostaglandins and cytokines.
If the intervention consists of merely administering oils such as evening primrose
oil or borage seed oil, it is innocuous, but when the proponents recommend costly
analysis of fatty acids in their personal laboratories, it becomes exploitative.
5. Inositol
Inositol is a precursor in the second-messenger system of some serotonin
receptors. In animal studies and human trials, it has been found to improve
6. Dimethylglycine
Dimethylglycine, a tertiary amine, is claimed by some to be effective in about
50% of individuals with autism (21). It has antioxidant and immunomodulating
effects. In one study it was found to enhance humoral and cell-mediated
immunity (22). At one time it was considered to have anticonvulsant effect as
well, but controlled trials have failed to demonstrate its anticonvulsant effect
(23). Two placebo-controlled trials failed to demonstrate significant benefits of
dimethylglycine on behavior of patients with autistic disorder (24,25). One of
these studies was faulted for using a low dose of dimethylglycine. However,
dimethylglycine is a reasonably priced innocuous substance with few side effects.
The recommended dose is one to four 125-mg tablets for a child, and two to eight
tablets for an adult. It is also available as a liquid for younger children (26).
Other nutraceuticals, such as antioxidants, a-lipoic acid, peroxynitrate, and
urecholine, have also been suggested as potential treatments for autism, but do
not have any empirical or theoretical support.
Although physicians should be open to the idea of using neutraceuticals,
they should provide limited leadership to the families without compromising the
families’ autonomy. They should carefully check the neutraceuticals for harmful
ingredients because under the Dietary Supplement Health and Education Act of
1994 (DSHEA), ingredients used in dietary supplements are no longer subject to
the premarket safety evaluations by the Food and Drug Administration. They
should be vigilant about the side effects of neutraceuticals and should caution the
families about brand-name products that make tall claims, because there is
nothing unique in one concoction of minerals and vitamins over another.
C. Biologicals
1. Secretin
Fortuitous improvement in language and social behavior of three patients with
autism who received a single dose of porcine secretin during a GI procedure led
to an interest in secretin as a treatment for autism (27). It is a 27-amino-acid
polypeptide hormone released by the cells of upper intestinal tract in response to
a bolus of food to stimulate bicarbonate and bile production. Although it is widely
distributed in the brain, its role in the brain is unknown. Many well-done studies
have failed to demonstrate therapeutic benefits of single or multiple doses of
either synthetic human secretin (28 –31) or porcine secretin in individuals with
autism (32 – 34).
At present, secretin is approved for diagnostic purposes only and its use
in autism is off-label. Recommended dose for autism is 2 clinical units (CU)/kg
or 0.2 –0.4 mg/kg body weight (35). A sensitivity test with a smaller dose is
recommended before the full bolus is given. Although the proponents claim that
secretin is safe, hyperactivity and a few cases of diarrhea, seizures, and apnea
have been reported. Secretin is neither innocuous nor inexpensive.
2. Antiyeast Therapy
Candida albicans, a yeast, is normally found in various parts of the body,
including the gut. Generally, the amount of yeast in the gut is kept under control
by other microbes that compete for the same nourishment. However, exposure to
antibiotics, especially repeated exposure, can destroy these microbes, resulting in
an overgrowth of C. albicans. According to the proponents of the yeast theory of
ADD and autism, when the yeast multiplies, it releases toxins, such as aldehyde,
alcohol, tartaric acid, and other organic acids, which, in turn, impair the central
nervous system and the immune system. Only one laboratory in the country,
owned by one of the proponents of the Candida theory, tests the urine for the
toxic metabolites of Candida. The promoters of this theory recommend that this
test, costing about $200, be done at the initiation of therapy and then periodically.
No other commercial laboratory tests for these substances and medical insurers
do not reimburse the cost.
Some of the behavior problems that have been linked to an overgrowth of
C. albicans include confusion, hyperactivity, short attention span, lethargy,
irritability, and aggression. Health problems, such as headaches, stomachaches,
constipation, gas pains, fatigue, and depression, have also been attributed to
Candida. Treatment is begun with an antifungal antibiotic, nystatin. If this does
not work, other antifungal antibiotics, amphotericin B, ketoconazole,
fluconazole, and terbinafine are tried. All are given orally. Additionally, the
gut is replenished with a rich culture of probiotic agents or good microbes such as
acidophilus. Avoiding sugar and other carbohydrate-rich foods on which yeast
thrives, such as fruits, prevents overgrowth of the yeast. Interestingly, the child is
supposed to become ill and to show negative behaviors for a few days after
receiving antifungal treatment, because the yeast is destroyed and the debris is
circulated through the body until it is excreted. A child who shows such
deterioration is believed to have a good prognosis.
All evidence is anecdotal, written up in well-marketed books (36). There is
no empirical evidence that there is overgrowth of Candida in individuals with
autism (37). Whereas nystatin is relatively safe with mild risk of anemia and
diarrhea, the other drugs are not so benign and may cause serious side effects,
such as liver function abnormalities, headache, and rash. This treatment has no
scientific basis.
3. Immunomodulator Therapy
a. Intravenous g-Globulins (IVIG). There are two contradictory reports
about the efficacy of IVIG. Gupta reported improvement in 10 children with
autism who had evidence of immunodeficiency with IVIG at 400 mg/kg every 4
weeks for 6 months (38). Plioplys, on the other hand, reported benefit in only one
of 10 children who did not have evidence of immunodeficiency with IVIG at
154 –375 mg/kg every 6 weeks (39). Doses up to 5 g/kg every other day have
been suggested without any empirical evidence. IVIG is costly and carries the
risk of blood-borne infections such as hepatitis and HIV. Renal dysfunction
ranging from increased blood urea nitrogen and creatinine to renal failure has
been reported. IVIG is contraindicated in individuals with IgA deficiency. Thus
IVIG is neither innocuous nor inexpensive.
4. Detoxification Therapies
Because the increased prevalence of autism has coincided with increasing
industrial pollution, there is a considerable interest in the xenobiotic theory of
autism. According to this theory, autism is caused by accumulation of heavy
metals such as mercury, lead, cadmium, arsenic, and antimony in the body. The
proponents use unconventional commercial panels of questionable validity to
measure the body burden of heavy metals in hair and urine samples and
recommend chelation if the tests are positive (41). If mercury poisoning is
suspected, a careful environmental history, including the frequency, amount, and
types of seafood consumed, should be taken followed by measurement of
mercury concentrations in blood and urine at a reliable laboratory that uses
reference values in general agreement with the published values. Blood mercury
is the best test for current methylmercury exposure, while a 24-h urine collection
is the best indicator of recent or chronic exposure to elemental or inorganic
mercury (42). Spot collections are usually adequate but must be adjusted for
creatinine concentration. Provocative chelation should not be used as a primary
diagnostic test (42,43). In most cases hair testing is not required and has limited
utility (42). Unconventional commercial hair, urine, and other panels should not
be used (44).
Although no link has been found between patients’ symptoms and mercury
levels in individuals without known exposure (44), proponents of this theory
recommend chelating children with autism with DMSA (2,3-dimercaptosuccinic,
or Succimer) and DMPS ((2,3-dimercapto-1-propanesulfonic acid) even if there
is no clinical or laboratory evidence of mercury poisoning. Blood CBC, liver, and
kidney panels, and a sensitivity test are a prerequisite for this treatment. Succimer
is given in the dose of 10 mg/kg/day in three divided doses for a few days
followed by a rest period (45). Such cycles are repeated until heavy metals are
cleared from the urine.
The reports supporting this treatment are mainly anecdotal. The treatment
is neither innocuous, nor inexpensive. Both DMSA and DMPS can cause
adverse reactions, such as gastrointestinal (GI) symptoms, rashes, neutropenia,
and elevation of liver enzymes.
The proponents of the toxic theory also recommend supporting the
cytochrome 450 and sulfation system in the liver to promote metabolism of toxic
chemicals, but there is little theoretical or empirical support for such treatments.
Proponents recommend glutathione and its precursors, glutamine, N-acetylcys-
teine, glycine, a-lipoic acid. The list of substances that are mentioned, such as
methylsulfonylmethane, taurine, and molybdenum, is formidable and irrational.
Even probiotics, such as lactobacilli and bifidobacteria are recommended to assist
with mercury detoxification (46). The theory is pseudoscience in megawords and
can be intimidating for the families. The physician should assist the families in
researching the harmful effects of the ingredients offered in preparations sold on
the Internet or in health food stores.
5. Famotidine (Pepcid)
One double-blind, placebo-controlled study reported some behavioral improve-
ment in 9 children with ASD with 2 mg/kg of famotidine per day in divided
doses. Famotidine is a H2 (histamine) receptor antagonist used to treat
gastroesophageal reflux. H2 receptors in the brain are supposed to mediate
exploratory behavior in animals. Although more studies are needed, the treatment
is benign and worth a try (47).
A. Deep Pressure
It has been suggested that deep lateral pressure can reduce arousal and anxiety in
autism. Occupational therapists often use deep pressure to decrease anxiety level
and to increase sensory awareness of their subjects, but specific advantage of
contraptions such as the Grandin Hug machine or Velvasoft Deep Pressure
Sensory Tops and Shorts is not proven (48,49). Medline search came up with only
one pilot study that suggested reduction in tension and anxiety for children with
autism who received deep pressure (50). However, the treatment is innocuous and
not very costly.
D. Facilitated Communication
Facilitated communication (FC) involves supporting a nonverbal individual’s
hand to make it easier for him/her to type out words on a typewriter, computer
keyboard, or other communication device (56). Several scientific studies have
suggested that facilitators may unintentionally influence the communication,
perhaps to the extent of actually selecting the words themselves. There are no
correct responses from the subject unless the facilitator knows the response
(57,58). Many controlled studies have reported negative findings, indicating that
the technique is neither reliable nor valid (59,60). The families should be aware
that FC has been used to obtain allegations of abuse, particularly sexual abuse,
from individuals with autism against third persons, causing negative
consequences for families (61). For a detailed review of FC, see Ref. 62.
These include medical systems that have been practiced in other parts of the
world from antiquity. Each of these systems has its unique explanation of how
diseases are caused and how they should be treated. Except for homeopathic
doses of secretin, there are few reports of alternative medical systems for the
treatment of autism.
V. LIFESTYLE CHANGES
Lifestyle changes are helpful, actually essential, in coping with and caring for a
child with autism. Parents should be encouraged to join parent support groups,
obtain respite services, and practice behavioral relaxation.
A. Mind-Body Medicine
Mind-body medicine involves behavioral, psychological, social, and spiritual
attempts to treat the body by exerting the influence of the mind over the body.
The mind is the sum total of our thoughts, feelings, and memories. Each of our
thoughts, feelings, and memories is a chemical or electrical wave in the brain that
has the potential of circulating through the entire body. Thus, mind and body are
intricately interlinked and constantly affect each other. Mind-body treatments,
such as behavioral relaxation training, yoga, and mediation, are not effective for
autism per se, but can help the parents in coping with the condition of their
children.
VI. SUMMARY
The number of CAM treatments that are being promoted for autism is too large to
be covered in this book. Physicians should be cautious about cynically rejecting
these treatments, because some may have a placebo effect. If a treatment is not
harmful, even if not useful, one should not dissuade the patents from trying it.
Parents feel more empowered and in control if they make therapeutic decisions
about their children, right or wrong. However, if a treatment is potentially
harmful, one should caution the parents without paternalistically forbidding its
use. The parents should decide for themselves what is good for their child and
their family. The physician can assist them in this process but should not usurp
their autonomy. A few examples of CAM are given in Table 1 and a clinical
approach to CAM is presented in Table 2.
1. Keep an open mind. Instead of being overcritical, support parents in their quest for a
cure for their child’s condition.
2. Respect the parents’ autonomy in making decisions about their child. Avoid
paternalism. Do not judge and treat the parents adversely if the parents choose to try
CAM along with conventional medicine.
3. Stay informed about CAM and be willing to provide guidance and assistance to the
family in obtaining and reviewing information when the family is considering
controversial or unproven treatments.
4. Discuss CAM options with the parents when a treatment plan is being developed. State
whatever is known about a CAM, including its side effects, objectively and neutrally.
5. Discuss with the parents how to decide if CAM is based on hearsay or credible
scientific research performed by a number of researchers working independently.
Word of mouth, multisyllabic words, testimonials, and sponsored programs that make
tall claims for a product are red flags. Counsel the parents about the importance of
blinded controlled research with a comparison group. Newspapers and radio or TV
talk shows cannot achieve the rigor of science. Tell the parents about reputed sources
of information such as Medline Plus, the National Institute of Mental Health, the
MIND Institute of the University of California, EBSCO, and ERIC.
6. Support the parents in choosing a reliable practitioner of CAM and a reliable product
if they decide to try CAM. Help them develop some objective criteria to assess the
efficacy of the CAM. Encourage them to use conventional treatments as well when
CAM is being tried. Check for drug interactions.
7. Ensure that the treatments are not harmful to the child’s health and safety.
8. Check for fraud. Check the National Health Fraud Unit, the FDA, the National Center
for Complementary and Alternative Medicine (NCCAM) at www.nccam.nih.gov, the
Federal Drug Administration (FDA) at www.fda.gov, and Alternative Medicine Alert,
a monthly newsletter published by the American Health Consultants, to find out if the
claims of the alternative provider are extravagant and fraudulent. If it is an herb, check
the webpage of the Duke Center for Integrative Medicine at www.dukehealth.org/
int_med for its efficacy and toxicity. The Federal Trade Commission has a free
consumer alert about fraudulent health advertising on the Internet, called “Virtual
Treatments Can Be Real-World Deceptions.” It can be obtained by writing to the
Federal Trade Commission, Consumer Response Center, 600 Pennsylvania Ave.,
N.W., Washington, DC 20580, or by calling 877-FTC-HELP. Another useful site is
www.quackwatch.com.
9. Check that the treatments are cost-effective and that the family’s financial and
emotional resources are not compromised by the use of these treatments. Check the
cost of the treatment and suggest to the parents that they check with the insurance
company to see if it will pay for the treatment.
10. When in doubt, acknowledge ignorance and be willing to refer for consultation.
11. Do not take away hope. Even if the positive effect of a CAM is a placebo effect, let the
parents savor it, and positively acknowledge it.
12. Suggest that parents read the FDA Guide to Choosing Medical Treatments at
www.stopgettingsick.com.
REFERENCES
18. Einat H, Belmaker RH. The effects of inositol treatment in animal models of
psychiatric disorders. J Affect Disord 2001; 62:113– 121.
19. Levine J. Controlled trials of inositol in psychiatry. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol
1997; 7:147 – 155.
20. Levine J, Aviram A, Holan A, Ring A, Barak Y, Belmaker RH. Inositol treatment of
autism. J Neural Transm 1997; 104:307– 310.
21. http://www.autism.com/ari/editorials/dmg.1html, accessed on 2.23.03.
22. Graber CD, Goust JM, Glassman AD, Kendall R, Loadholt CB. Immunomodulating
properties of dimethylglycine in humans. J Infect Dis 1981; 143:101– 105.
23. Gascon G, Patterson B, Yearwood K, Slotnick H. N,N-Dimethylglycine and
epilepsy. Epilepsia 1989; 30:90– 93.
24. Kern JK, Miller VS, Cauller PL, Kendall PR, Mehta PJ, Dodd M. Effectiveness of
N,N-dimethylglycine in autism and pervasive developmental disorder. J Child
Neurol 2001; 16:169– 173.
25. Bolman WM, Richmond JA. A double-blind placebo-controlled, crossover pilot trial
of low dose Dimethylglycine in patients with autistic disorder. J Autism Dev Disord
1999; 29:191 – 194.
26. http://www.autism.org/dmg.html, accessed on 2.23.03.
27. Horvath K, Stefanatos G, Sokolski KN, Wachtel R, Nabors L, Tildon JT. Improved
social and language skills after secretin administration in patients with autistic
spectrum disorders. J Assoc Acad Minor Phys 1998; 9:9– 15.
28. Sandler AD, Sutton KA, DeWeese J, Girardi MA, Sheppard V, Bodfish JW. Lack of
benefit of a single dose of synthetic human secretin in the treatment of autism and
pervasive developmental disorder. N Engl J Med 1999; 341:1801 – 1806.
29. Carey T, Ratliff-Schaub K, Funk J, Weinle C, Myers M, Jenks J. Double-blind
placebo-controlled trial of secretin: effects on aberrant behavior in children with
autism. J Autism Dev Disord 2002; 32:161– 167.
30. Molloy CA, Manning-Courtney P, Swayne S, Bean J, Brown JM, Murray DS,
Kinsman AM, Brasington M, Ulrich CD 2nd. Lack of benefit of intravenous
synthetic human secretin in the treatment of autism. J Autism Dev Disord 2002;
32:545 – 551.
31. Sponheim E, Oftedal G, Helverschou SB. Multiple doses of secretin in the treatment
of autism: a controlled study. Acta Paediatr 2002; 91:540 – 545.
32. Owley T, Steele E, Corsello C, Risi S, McKaig K, Lord C, Leventhal BL, Cook Jr
EH. Double-blind, placebo-controlled trial of secretin for the treatment of autistic
disorder. MedGenMed 1999; Oct 6:E2.
33. Owley T, McMahon W, Cook EH, Laulhere T, South M, Mays LZ, Shernoff ES,
Lainhart J, Modahl CB, Corsello C, Ozonoff S, Risi S, Lord C, Leventhal BL, Filipek
PA. Multisite, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial of porcine secretin in autism.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2001; 40:1293– 1299.
34. Corbett B, Khan K, Czapansky-Beilman D, Brady N, Dropik P, Goldman DZ,
Delaney K, Sharp H, Muller I, Shapiro E, Ziegler R. A double-blind, placebo-
controlled crossover study investigating the effect of porcine secretin in children
with autism. Clin Pediatr (Phila) 2001; 40:327– 331.
35. http://www.autism.com/ari/secretin2.html, accessed on 2.26.03.
36. Crook WG. The Yeast Connection Handbook. Jackson, TN: Professional Books/
Future Health, Inc., 1996.
37. Horvath K, Papadimitriou JC, Rabsztyn A, et al. Gastrointestinal abnormalities in
children with autistic disorder. J Pediatr 1999; 135:559 – 563.
38. Gupta S. Treatment of children with autism with intravenous immunoglobulin.
J Child Neurol 1999; 14:203 – 205.
39. Plioplys AV. Intravenous immunoglobulin treatment of children with autism. J Child
Neurol 1998; 13:79 – 82.
40. Gupta S. Immunological treatments for autism. J Autism Dev Disord 2000; 30:475–
479.
41. Seidel S, Kreutzer R, Smith D, McNeel S, Gilliss D. Assessment of commercial
laboratories performing hair testing. JAMA 2001; 285:67 –72.
42. Agency for Toxic Substance and Disease Registry. Mercury toxicity. Am Fam
Physician 1992; 46:1731 – 1741.
43. Forman J, Moline J, Cernichiari E, et al. A cluster of pediatric metallic mercury
exposure cases treated with meso-2,3-dimercaptosuccinic acid (DMSA). Environ
Health Perspect 2000; 108:575 – 577.
44. Kales SN, Goldman RH. Mercury exposure: current concepts, controversies, and a
clinic’s experience. J Occup Environ Med 2002; 44:143 – 154.
45. Laidler JR. DAN! Mercury Detoxification Consensus Group. Position Paper. San
Diego, CA: Autism Research Institute, 2001.
46. Kidd PM. Autism, an extreme challenge to integrative medicine. Part II. Medical
management. Altern Med Rev 2002; 7:472– 499.
47. Lindsay LA, Tsiouris JA, Cohen IL, Shindledecker R, DeCresce R. Famotidine
treatment of children with autistic spectrum disorders: pilot research using single
subject research design. J Neural Transm 2001; 108:593– 611.
48. Zissermann L. The effects of deep pressure on self-stimulating behaviors in a child
with autism and other disabilities. Am J Occup Ther 1992; 46:547 – 551.
49. Krauss KE. The effects of deep pressure touch on anxiety. Am J Occup Ther 1987;
41:366– 373.
50. Edelson SM, Edelson MG, Kerr DC, Grandin T. Behavioral and physiological effects
of deep pressure on children with autism: a pilot study evaluating the efficacy of
Grandin’s Hug Machine. Am J Occup Ther 1999; 53:145 – 152.
51. Thompson BM. An historical commentary on the physiological effects of music:
Tomatis, Mozart and neurophysiology. Integr Physiol Behav Sci 2000; 35:174 – 188.
52. Committee on Children with Disabilities, American Academy of Pediatrics.
Auditory integration training and facilitated communication for autism (RE9752).
Pediatrics 1998; 102:431 – 433.
53. Gravel JS. Auditory integrative training: placing the burden of proof. Am J Speech
Lang Pathol 1994; 3:25 – 29.
54. Rankovic CM, Rabinowitz WM, Lof GL. Maximum output intensity of the
Audiokinetron. Am J Speech Lang Pathol 1996; 5:68– 72.
55. Bettison S,. Long-term effects of auditory training on children with autism. J Autism
Dev Disord 1996; 26:361 – 367.
56. Jacobson JW, Mulick JA, Schwartz AA. A history of facilitated communication:
science, pseudoscience, and antiscience. Am Psychol 1995; 50:750 – 765.
57. Smith MD, Haas PJ, Belcher RG. Facilitated communication: the effects of facilitator
knowledge and level of assistance on output. J Autism Dev Disord 1994; 24:357–367.
58. Cardinal DA, Hanson D, Wakeham J. Investigation of authorship in facilitated
communication. Ment Retard 1996; 34:231 – 242.
59. Eberlin M, McConnachie G, Ibel S, Volpe L. Facilitated communication: a failure to
replicate the phenomenon. J Autism Dev Disord 1993; 23:507 –530.
60. Regal RA, Rooney JR, Wandas T. Facilitated communication: an experimental
evaluation. J Autism Dev Disord 1994; 24:345– 355.
61. Konstantareas MM. Allegations of sexual abuse by nonverbal autistic people via
facilitated communication: testing of validity. Child Abuse Negl 1998; 22:1027 –
1041.
62. Mostert MP. Facilitated communication since 1995: a review of published studies.
J Autism Dev Disord 2001; 31:287– 313.
I. INTRODUCTION
Behavior problems are prevalent among persons with autism. In early childhood
tantrums and disruptive behavior are displayed, whereas adolescents and young
adults often exhibit aggressive, stereotypic, and self-injurious behaviors (18).
Often parents, teachers, and other caregivers inadvertently enable children to
manipulate their environment by displaying inappropriate behaviors, thus
reinforcing and maintaining them. Persistent problem behaviors may, over time,
interfere with participating in school, developing functional skills, and engaging
in community activities. Additionally, placement options for young adults are
often dictated by the presence of aberrant behavior (19). Once established,
problem behaviors require direct intensive intervention before they are
diminished. Consequently, behavioral intervention is optimal in early childhood
reinforcer. Likewise, Fisher et al. (34) employed a strategy for assessing effective
punishers. The punisher that produced the greatest reduction in target behaviors is
selected for inclusion in the treatment protocol (34).
Currently, many schools employ positive behavior interventions that do not
utilize punishers. However, some behaviors may require the inclusion of a
punisher in an intervention to rapidly produce behavior change (35).
Consistent with the variability present within the continuum of ASD, the
population displays a wide array of language and communication deficits. Some
children’s language skills display idiosyncratic language or echolalia (39).
Individuals who do acquire complex language encounter difficulties similar to
those of their counterparts with learning disabilities, regarding conversational
turn taking, shifting language levels to fit the situation and skills of their
conversation partner, and following the social protocols of pragmatic language
(40). Similarly, nonverbal language skills are also problematic for children with
ASD (41).
Problems in joint attention and use of symbols have been isolated as core
deficits in communication in this population (42). Joint attention refers to
problems in coordinating attention between people and objects. It relates to skills
in orienting and attending to a social partner, shifting gaze, sharing affect and
experience, and following the gaze and point of another person. Symbol use
refers to learning conventional or shared meanings for symbols including
gestures and words. Many individuals with ASD are limited in learning symbols
and in using conventional gestures such as showing, waving, pointing, and head
nodding (43). These are target skills for intervention (44). A relationship seems to
exist between competence in communication and behavior and overall level of
outcome. Thus, language is a critical area for intervention (45).
A. Intervention
Language and communication skill interventions have utilized developmental
and behavioral approaches. Although behavioral approaches to intervention are
more widely represented in the research, it is one area of intervention where the
two paradigms intertwine. Interventions have centered on developing functional
communication, as well as verbal and nonverbal communication (46).
2. Verbal Communication
A vast literature documents the effects of speech and language intervention for
children with autism. Most of these studies have utilized discrete trial training or
more contemporary approaches of applied behavior analysis to develop verbal
language skills, which have ranged from developing single-word vocabulary to
describing objects and pictures and responding to questions. Perhaps the most
widely known techniques are the discrete trial training protocols addressing
labeling and naming skills designed by Lovaas. Limitations of discrete trial
training are failure to develop spontaneous use of language and to establish
generalization in natural contexts (50).
Recent discrete trial training approaches have successfully taught
responses to “wh” questions and elicited descriptions of objects and pictures
(51) and comprehension of prepositions (52). Other contemporary approaches
include, but are not limited to, incidental teaching (53) and pivotal response
training (54). Incidental teaching techniques utilize naturally occurring adult-
child interactions to teach the language skills and incorporated behavioral
techniques such as prompting, imitating, and requesting (55). Pivotal response
training centers on initiation of communication that is considered to be a pivotal
behavior. Child-initiated communication will be a catalyst for responses from
others, thus providing reinforcers for communication and facilitate the
development of language skills (56). It has been documented that self-initiated
communications have been taught to children with autism who possessed poor
spontaneous communication (57).
3. Nonverbal Communication
The use of augmentative and alternative communication systems enables persons
with severe communication disorders to compensate for their disabilities by
supporting existent speech or developing nonspeech symbol systems such as sign
language, visual symbols displayed on communication boards, and voice output
devices. The goal of these systems is to empower individuals to independently
communicate their wants and needs (58).
Sign language has been used with many special education populations. It is
widely documented that total communication (use of both sign language and oral
speech) is an effective method of teaching receptive and expressive vocabulary to
individuals with autism (59). Signing does not hinder the development of speech,
but research findings suggest that it may enhance the use of speech. Acquiring
skills in signing is especially important for students with limited communication
who may never acquire speech (60).
It is essential to provide independent functional communication systems to
students with autism. Visual symbols have been used successfully to facilitate
compliance, initiate communication, and minimize dependence on verbal cues
(61). Nonetheless, the efficacy of a system of visual symbols that is most widely
used in public schools (the picture exchange communication system) is not
empirically documented (62).
V. SOCIAL SKILLS
The general literature in social competence suggests that the use of prosocial
behaviors may contribute to the social adjustment of individuals and influence
their interpersonal relationships. Positive as well as negative social behavior
tends to be reciprocal (80). For example, observations of nursery school
children’s social behavior have revealed that positive social behaviors, including
giving attention and affection, expressing approval and personal acceptance, and
giving objects to another, are related to peer acceptance. Conversely, negative
social behaviors, including noncompliance, interference, and attack, relate to
rejection by others (81). Socially competent individuals posses the components of
social behaviors (i.e., facial expressions, gestures, greetings), monitor those
behaviors through a system of rules, and utilize these skills to obtain desired goals
(82).
There are critical social skills that appear to foster friendship such as
demonstrating sensitivity to the needs and feelings of others, expressing warmth
and affection, and sharing goals and activities. Moreover, attitudes that
precipitate friendship include finding people to be sources of satisfaction and
enjoying the exchange of affection (83). Obtaining and using the skills required
for reciprocal social exchanges and making friends are challenges experienced by
individuals on the autism spectrum (84).
Deficits in social competence were included in the first descriptions of
autism (85) and remain salient features of the disorder. Children evidence
delayed or aberrant social behavior beginning in early childhood (86). Compared
to same-age peers, children with autism exhibit more sterotypies and self-
injurious behaviors and spend more time engaged in meaningless activity (87).
Research findings suggest that social interaction is not reinforcing to many
students diagnosed with ASD (88). Proximity to peers is avoided or the quality of
interactions is lacking when these students do engage with others (89). This is an
issue of concern as reciprocal peer exchanges are salient to the development of
social relationships (90).
People with autism have difficulty talking to others (91) and coordinating
joint attention (92). At higher skill levels interpreting nonverbal language (93)
and comprehending humor are difficult for students with ASD (94). Additionally,
demonstrating the capacity to extend beyond self is problematic in this
population. A section of the Autism Diagnostic Interview Schedule – Revised
(ADI-R) (95) focusing on the assessment of social development and play
investigates the ability to offer comfort, among other behaviors needed to
formulate relationships. Developing empathy is important, as it is a core
requirement for establishing social relationships with others (96). Analyzing the
parent responses in ADI-Rs can assist in formulating and sequencing appropriate
goals for social development.
A. Intervention
Bandura’s work in social learning theory (97) posited that specific identifiable
skills form the basis for socially competent behaviors and that interpersonal
difficulties may arise as a function of a faulty and/or incomplete behavioral
repertoire (98).
Interventions have focused predominantly on developing social skills
in young children with ASD using a variety of techniques. Social skills training
has addressed initiating social exchanges (99) using picture schedules to
initiate social exchanges (100) and prompting reciprocal interaction through
incidental teaching (101). Other studies aimed at improving social interactions
have used social scripts (102), and substantiated the power of peer-mediated
interventions (103). Social behaviors have been increased by using peers in
pivotal response training (104) and involving children in integrated play groups
(105).
While there are numerous models for social skills training, no one model
fits all needs. Social skills training models share common elements: social skills
change to correspond to the social setting (106); verbal and nonverbal learned
responses guide interpersonal skills (107); social competence is dependent upon
the receipt of reinforcers (108); and models are designed with assumption that
skills can be taught (109).
Some students with ASD possess abilities that enable participation in a traditional
academic curriculum, while others experience cognitive delays that compromise
participation in a functional curriculum. Irrespective of ability level, generalizing
what has been learned across materials, settings, and times is challenging. To
teach toward generalization, a curriculum should plan for practicing skills in
natural settings with naturally occurring reinforcers from multiple sources such
as parents, teachers, and peers (112).
Consistent with other special education populations, comprehensive
assessment must drive the design of the “what, how, and where” of educational
programming. Consideration of issues presented in previous sections of the
chapter must be reflected in the selection of the curriculum and the design of an
instructional schedule. Similarly, other attributes affecting learning, such as
attention, impulsivity, motivation, and cue dependence, must also be considered
(113). Irrespective of the level of ability, the goal of an educational program
should be to assist students in developing independence and obtaining quality of
life to the greatest extent possible (114).
The infinite number of interventions aimed at improving outcomes for
students with ASD is overwhelming leaving parents and professionals vulner-
able to the latest fads in treatment. After completing a comprehensive critical
review of instructional treatment programs for students with autism, the state
of New York concluded that instruction utilizing ABA was the only one that had
empirical merit and substance (115).
Although the term “ABA” is frequently used, many parents and
professionals are uncertain about what ABA really is and its approach to
intervention. Green (116) provides a succinct explanation in the following
passage from her article on behavior analytic instruction.
Behavior analytic instruction begins with a comprehensive assessment of each
learner’s current skills and needs, accomplished by observing the learner
directly in a variety of situations and recording what she or he does and does
not do. Every skill that is selected for instruction (often called a target) is
defined in clear, observable terms and broken down into its component parts.
Each component response is taught by presenting or arranging one or more
specific antecedent stimuli, such as cues or instructions from another person,
and/or items of interest to the learner. (116)
A. Intervention
Olley notes that the emphasis of the literature is on the method rather than the
content of instruction (118). Consequently, it is easy to select a strategy of “how”
to teach, but often difficult to isolate and sequence “what” should be taught.
Appropriate educational programs for students with ASD require utilizing the
technology of instruction in designing errorless learning and structuring
environments that facilitate learning (119).
1. Instructional Techniques
In ABA, the instructional unit is the learning trial, which is defined as a structured
opportunity for a response, in the presence of an antecedent, followed by a
consequence (SRS) (120). Instructional methodologies address the manner in
which lessons are organized and delivered and responses are evoked. Instruction
can be organized in massed, spaced, and collective teaching trials. Massed trials
elicit the same response from students in rapid succession. Spaced trials require
students to complete a task with intervening intervals of time. Collective trials are
presented sequentially to students in group instructional settings (121).
The demonstration of learning can be solicited by employing systematic
antecedent and response-prompting strategies. Response prompting includes
strategies such as time delay and least-to-most prompts. Time delay techniques
present prompts and fade them in incremental time segments between the
direction and the response. The system of least-to-most prompts utilizes a
prompting hierarchy. Following the instructional cue, response prompts of
gradually increasing levels of assistance are provided until the student
demonstrates the correct response (122). The use of preference and reinforcer
assessments (as described earlier) can assist in motivating students to learn and
empowering them with choices (123).
2. Learning Environments
Students with ASD rely on external organizers embedded within the environment
to anticipate tasks and expectations for performance. Thus, organizing the
setting, the timing, and the sequencing of tasks and presenting materials that
motivate the child to learn are essential. Dedicated space for specific learning
tasks should be defined within the classroom (124). Ideally, student schedules
should be organized by referring to the student assessment. Tasks that are difficult
should be presented at optimal times for learning for the individual (125).
Similarly, preferred activities can be used as reinforcers alternating the
presentation of them with difficult, less preferred lessons.
The use of visual supports represents best practice for many students with
ASD (126). Visual activity schedules using photographs or symbols provide
students with advanced organizers for the daily schedule (127). Picture schedules
can assist students in anticipating transitions, and in comprehending the demands
of activities and social interactions (128). Recent work by Stromer et al.
successfully incorporates computer technology in constructing activity
schedules. Computerized activity schedules enable students to independently
execute the visual and auditory prompts embedded in the program. This area has
promising implications for individuals with ASD who are cue dependent (129).
VII. SUMMARY
Across the nation parents with children challenged by ASD seek the services of
professionals who are knowledgeable and empathic to their needs. This quest is
as frustrating as the deficits that their children present, often depleting the
emotional and financial resources of the family. At this time there are vast
numbers of children in need of appropriate, effective intervention. Many
interventions are available that produce positive outcomes. It is important for
parents and professionals to be vigilant about obtaining assessments and
interventions that address skills identified as priority areas of development as
discussed in this chapter. It is equally important to investigate the efficacy of
those interventions so that critical time is not wasted in fruitless endeavors.
Behavioral and educational gains should be well documented and changes should
be made in instructional strategies if progress is not demonstrated within a
reasonable time. Developing self-help and leisure time activities are also critical
skills for students with ASD. Obtaining assistance in securing appropriate service
and developing these skills will potentially optimize future placements and
enhance the quality of life for students with ASD and their families (132).
REFERENCES
1. Filipek PA, Accaerdo PJ, Ashwal MD, Baranek GT, Cook EH, Dawson G, Gordon
B, Gravel JS, Johnson CP, Kallen RJ, Levy SE, Minshew NJ, Ozonoff S, Prizant,
BM, Rapin R, Rogers SJ, Stone WL, Teplin SW, Tuchman RF, Volkmar FR.
Practice parameter: screening and diagnosis of autism. Neurology 2000; 55:468 –
479.
2. Lord C, Rutter M, LeCouteur A. The Autism Diagnostic Interview Schedule
Revised. J Autism Dev Disord 1994; 24(5):659– 685.
3. Lord C, Storoschuk S, Rutter M, Pickles, A. Using the ADI-R to diagnose autism in
preschool children. Infant Mental Health J 1993; 14(3):234 –252.
4. Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (IDEA), 20 U.S.C.A. 1400-1485,
originally the Education of All Handicapped Children Act (EAHCA, signed on
Nov. 29, 1975 by President Ford, amended in 1978 and 1986 and incorporated into
a new law in 1990.
5. Trapani C. Transition Goals for Adolescents with Learning Disabilities. Boston:
Little, Brown, 1990.
6. Free Appropriate Education (FAPE) Board of Education (Hendrick Hudson Central
School District v. Rowley, 458 U.S. 176 (1982).
7. Mandlawitz MR. The impact of the legal system on educational programming for
young children with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 2002;
32(5):495– 505.
8. Turnbull HR, Wilcox BL, Stowe MJ. A brief overview of special education law
with focus on autism. J Autism Dev Disord 2002; 32(5):479– 493.
9. Zirkel PA. The autism case law: administrative and judicial rulings. Focus Autism
Other Dev Disord 2002; 17(2):84– 93.
10. Katsiyannis A, Reid R. Autism and section 504: rights and responsibilities. Focus
Autism Other Dev Disord 1999; 14(2):66– 72.
11. T.H. v Board of Education of Palatine Community Consol. Sch. District, No. 98 C.
4633, 98 C 4632 (N.D. Illinois 1999).
12. Herr S. Special education law and children with reading and other disabilities.
J Law Educ 1999; 28(3):337 –389.
13. Krantz PJ, McClannahan LE. Strategies for integration: Building repertoires that
support transitions to public schools. In: Ghezzi PM, Williams WL, Carr JE,
eds. Autism: Behavior Analytic Perspectives. Reno, NV: Context Press, 1999:221 –
231.
14. Lord C, McGee JP, eds. Educating children with autism. National Research
Council. Washington, DC: National Academy Press, 2001.
15. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Mental Health: A Report of the
Surgeon General. Rockville, MD: USDHHS, 1999.
16. Lord C, McGee JP, eds. Educating Children with Autism. National Research
Council, Washington, DC: National Academy Press, 2001.
17. Lord C, McGee JP, eds. Educating Children with Autism. National Research
Council, Washington, DC: National Academy Press, 2001.
18. Horner RH, Carr EG, Strain PS, Todd AW, Reed HK. Problem behavior
interventions for young children with autism: a research synthesis. Focus Autism
Other Dev Disord 2002; 16:205– 214.
19. Horner RH, Diemer SM, Brazeau KC. Educational support for students with severe
problem behaviors in Oregon: a descriptive analysis from the 1987 –88 school year.
J Assoc Persons Severe Handicaps 1992; 17:154 – 169.
20. Horner RH, Carr EG, Strain PS, Todd AW, Reed HK. Problem behavior
interventions for young children with autism: a research synthesis. Focus Autism
Other Dev Disord 2002; 16:205– 214.
21. Risley TR, Wolf M. Establishing functional speech in echolalic children. Behav
Res Ther 1967; 5:73 – 88.
22. Lovaas OI, Simmons JQ. Manipulation of self-destruction in three retarded
children. J Appl Behav Anal 1969; 2:143 –157.
23. Horner RH, Carr EG, Strain PS, Todd AW, Reed HK. Problem behavior
interventions for young children with autism: a research synthesis. Focus Autism
Other Dev Disord 2002; 16:205– 214.
24. Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (IDEA), 20 U.S.C.A. 1400-1485,
originally the Education of All Handicapped Children Act (EAHCA, signed on
Nov. 29, 1975 by President Ford, amended in 1978 and 1986 and incorporated into
a new law in 1990.
25. Dunlop G, Kern L, Worchester J. ABA and academic instruction. Focus Autism
Other Dev Disord 2001; 16:129 – 136.
26. Iwata B, Vollmer T, Zarcone JR. The experimental (functional) analysis of
behavior disorders: methodology, applications and limitations. In: Repp AC, Singh
NN, eds. Perspectives on the Use of Nonaversive and Aversive Interventions for
Persons with Developmental Disabilities. Sycamore, IL: Sycamore 1990:301 – 330.
27. Didden R, Duker PC, Korzilius H. Meta-analytic study on treatment effectiveness
for problem behaviors with individuals who have mental retardation. Am J Ment
Retard 1997; 101:387– 399.
28. Fisher WW, Piazza CC, Alterson CJ, Kuhn DE. Interresponse relations among
aberrant behaviors displayed by persons with autism and developmental
disabilities. In: Ghezzi PM, Williams WL, Carr JE, eds. Autism: Behavior
Analytic Perspectives. Reno, NV: Context Press, 1999:113 – 135.
29. Neef NA, Iwata BA. Current research on functional analysis methodologies: an
introduction. J Appl Behav Anal 1994; 27:211 –214.
30. Fisher WW, Piazza CC, Alterson CJ, Kuhn DE. Interresponse relations among
aberrant behaviors displayed by persons with autism and developmental
disabilities. In: Ghezzi PM, Williams WL, Carr JE, eds. Autism: Behavior
Analytic Perspectives. Reno, NV: Context Press, 1999:113 – 135.
31. Iwata BA, Dorsey MF, Slifer KJ, Bauman KE, Richman GS. Toward a functional
analysis of self-injury. Anal Intervent Dev Disord 1982; 2:3 – 20.
32. Fisher WW, Piazza CC, Bowman LG, Hagopian LP, Owens JC, Slevin I. A
comparison of two approaches for identifying reinforcers for persons with severe
and profound disabilities. J Appl Behav Anal 1992; 25:491 – 498.
33. Pace GM, Ivancic MT, Edwards GL, Iwata B, Page TJ. Assessment of stimulus
preference and reinforcer value with profoundly retarded individuals. J Appl Behav
Anal 1985; 18:249 – 255.
34. Fisher WW, Piazza CC, Bowman LG, Kurtz PF, Sherer MR, Lachman SR. A
preliminary evaluation of empirically derived consequences for the treatment of
pica. J Appl Behav Anal 1994; 27:447 – 457.
35. Pfiffner LJ, O’Leary SG. The efficacy of all-positive management as a function of
the prior use of negative consequences. J Appl Behav Anal 1987; 20:265 – 271.
36. Lord C, McGee JP, eds. Educating Children with Autism. National Research
Council, Washington, DC: National Academy Press, 2001:37.
37. Scotti JR, Ujcich KJ, Weigle KL, Holland CM, Kirk KS. Intervention with
challenging behavior of persons with developmental disabilities: a review of
current research practices. J Assoc Persons Severe Handicaps 1996; 21:123 –134.
38. Fisher WW, Piazza CC, Bowman LG, Hagopian LP, Owens JC, Slevin I. A
comparison of two approaches for identifying reinforcers for persons with severe
and profound disabilities. J Appl Behav Anal 1992; 25:491 – 498.
39. Lord C, Paul R. Language and communication in autism. In: Cohen D, Volkmar F,
eds. Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders. New York: John
Wiley & Sons, 1997:195 – 225.
40. Tager Flusberg H. Brief Report: Current theory and research on language and
communication in autism. J Autism Dev Disord 1996; 26:169 – 178.
41. Lord C, Paul R. Language and communication in autism. In: Cohen D, Volkmar F,
eds. Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders. New York: John
Wiley & Sons, 1997:195– 225.
42. Sigman M, Ruskin E. Continuity and change in the social competence of children
with autism, down syndrome and developmental delays. Monogr Soc Res Child
Dev 1999; 64(1):114.
43. Stone W, Ousley O, Yoder P, Hogan K, Hepburn S. Nonverbal communication in 2-
and 3-year old children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord 1997; 27:677 – 696.
44. Mundy P, Crowson M. Joint attention and early social communication: implications
for research on intervention with autism. J Autism Dev Disord 1997; 27:653– 676.
45. Garfin D, Lord C. Communication as a social problem in autism. In: Schopler E,
Mesibov G, eds. Social Behavior in Autism. New York: Plenum Press, 1986:237 –
261.
46. Lord C, McGee JP, eds. Educating Children with Autism. National Research
Council. Washington, DC: National Academy Press, 2001.
47. Fisher WW, Thompson R, Kuhn D. Establishing discrimination control of
responding using functional and alternative reinforcers. J Appl Behav Anal 1998;
31:543 – 560.
48. Horner RH, Carr EG, Strain PS, Todd AW, Reed HK. Problem behavior
interventions for young children with autism: a research synthesis. Focus Autism
Other Dev Disord 2002; 16:205– 214.
49. Goldstein H. Communication intervention for children with autism: a review of
treatment efficacy. J Autism Dev Disord 2002; 32:373– 396.
50. Lovaas OI, Koegel R, Simmons JQ, Stevens-Long J. Some generalization and
follow-up measures on autistic children in behavior therapy. J Appl Behav Anal
1973; 6:131 – 166.
51. Krantz PJ, Zalewski L, Hall E, Fenski E, McClannahan L. Teaching complex
language skills to autistic children. Anal Intervent Dev Disord 1981; 1:259– 297.
52. Egel AL, Shafer MS, Neef NA. Receptive acquisition and generalization of
prepositional responding in autistic children: a comparison of two procedures. Anal
Intervent Dev Disord 1984; 4:285 – 298.
53. Hart BM, Risley TR. Incidental teaching of language in preschool. J Appl Behav
Anal 1975; 8:411– 420.
54. Koegel LK, Koegel RL, Carter CM. Pivotal responses and the natural language
treatment paradigm. Semin Speech Lang 1998; 19:355 – 372.
55. Hart BM, Risley TR. Incidental teaching of language in preschool. J Appl Behav
Anal 1975; 8:411– 420.
56. Koegel L. Communication and language intervention. In: Koegel R, Koegel L, eds.
Teaching Children with Autism. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing,
1995:17 – 32.
57. Charlop M, Trasowech J. Increasing autistic children’s daily spontaneous speech.
J Appl Behav Anal 1991; 24:379– 386.
58. Beukelman D, Mirenda P. Augmentative and Alternative Communication:
Management of Severe Communication Disorders in Children and Adults 2d ed.
Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes, 1998.
79. Lord C. Early social development in autism. In: Schopler E, Van Bourgondien ME,
Bristol MM, eds. Preschool Issues in Autism. New York: Plenum Press, 1993:61 –
94.
80. Staub EL. The Development of Prosocial Behavior in Children. Morristown, NJ:
General Learning Press, 1975.
81. Hartup WW, Glazer JA, Charlesworth R. Peer reinforcement and sociometric
status. Child Dev 1967; 38:1017 – 1024.
82. Trower P. Toward a generative model of social skills: a critique and synthesis. In:
Curran JP, Monti PM, eds. Social skills training. New York: Plenum Press,
1982:339 – 427.
83. Herbert M. Social skills training with children. In: Hollin CR, Trower P, eds.
Handbook of Social Skills Training. Vol. 1. New York: Pergamon Press, 1986:11 –
32.
84. Rutter M, Mahwood L, Howlin P. Language delay and social development. In:
Fletcher P, Hale D, eds. Specific Speech and Language Disorders in Children.
London: Whurr, 1992.
85. Kanner L. Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nerv Child 1943; 2:217– 250.
86. Sigman M, Ruskin E. Continuity and change in the social competence of children
with autism, down syndrome and developmental delays. Monogr Soc Res Child
Dev 1999; 64(1):1 – 113.
87. Lord C. Early social development in autism. In: Schopler E, Van Bourgondien ME,
Bristol MM, eds. Preschool Issues in Autism. New York: Plenum Press, 1993:61 –
94.
88. McConnell SR. Interventions to facilitate social interaction for young children with
autism: review of available research and recommendations for educational
intervention and future research. J Autism Dev Disord 2002; 32(5):351– 372.
89. McGee GG, Feldman RS, Morrier MJ. Benchmarks of social treatment for children
with autism. J Autism Dev Disord 1997; 27:353 – 364.
90. Dunn J, McGuire S. Sibling and peer relationships in childhood. J Child Psychol
Psychiatry 1992; 33:67– 105.
91. McGee GG, Feldman RS, Morrier MJ. Benchmarks of social treatment for children
with autism. J Autism Dev Disord 1997; 27:353 – 364.
92. McArthur D, Adamson LB. Joint attention in preverbal children: autism and
developmental language disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 1996; 26:481 – 496.
93. Celani G, Battachi MW, Arcidiacono L. The understanding of emotional meaning
of facial expressions in people with autism. J Autism Dev Disord 1999; 29:57– 66.
94. St James PJ, Tager Flusberg H. An observational study of humor in autism and
Down syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord 1994; 24:603– 617.
95. Lord C, Rutter M, LeCouteur. The Autism Diagnostic Interview Revised. Western
Psych, 1999.
96. Herbert M. Social skills training with children. In: Hollin CR, Trower P, eds.
Handbook of Social Skills Training. Vol. 1. New York: Pergamon Press, 1986:11 –
32.
97. Bandura A. Social Learning Theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall,
1977.
98. Hops H. Children’s social competence and skill: current research practices and
future directions. Behav Ther 1983; 14:3 –18.
99. Taylor B, Leven L. Teaching a student with autism to make verbal initiations:
effects of a tactile prompt. J Appl Behav Anal 1998; 31:651 – 654.
100. Krantz PJ, MacDuff MT, McClannahan LE. Programming participation in family
activities for children with autism: parents’ use of photographic activity schedules.
J Appl Behav Anal 1993; 26:137 – 138.
101. McGee GC, Almeida MC, Sulzer-Azaroff B, Feldman RS. Prompting recip-
rocal interaction via peer incidental teaching. J Appl Behav Anal 1992; 25:117 –
126.
102. Krantz PJ, McClannahan LE. Social interaction skills with children with autism: a
script-fading procedure for beginning readers. J Appl Behav Anal 1998; 31:191 –
202.
103. Odom SL, Strain PS. Peer-mediated approaches to promoting children’s
interaction: a review. Am J Orthopsychiatry 1984; 54:544 –557.
104. Pierce K, Schreibman L. Multiple peer use of pivotal response training social
behaviors in children with autism: effects of peer-implemented pivotal response
training. J Appl Behav Anal 1997; 30:157 – 160.
105. Wolfberg PJ, Sculer AL. Integrated play groups: promoting the social and cognitive
dimensions of play in children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord 1993; 23:467 –
489.
106. Rinn RC, Markley A. Modification of skill deficits in children. In: Bellack AS,
Hersen M, eds. Research and Practice in Social Skills Training. New York: Plenum
Press, 1981:107 – 129.
107. Trower P. Skills training for adolescents’ social problems: a viable alternative.
J Adolesc 1978; 1:319– 329.
108. Asher SR, Oden SL, Gottman JH. Children’s friendships in school settings. In: Katz
LG, ed. Current Topics in Early Childhood Education. Norwood, NJ: Ablex, 1977;
1:33– 57.
109. Gresham F. Social skills training with handicapped children: a review. Rev Educ
Res 1981; 51:139– 176.
110. McConnell SR. Interventions to facilitate social interaction for young children
with autism: review of available research and recommendations for educa-
tional intervention and future research. J Autism Dev Disord 2002; 32(5):351–
372.
111. Odom SL, Strain PS. Peer-mediated approaches to promoting children’s
interaction: a review. Am J Orthopsychiatry 1984; 54:544 –557.
112. Matson J. Behavioral treatment of autistic persons: a review of research from 1980
to the present. Res Dev Disord 1996; 17(6):433– 465.
113. Trapani C. Psychoeducational assessment of children and adolescents with
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. In: Accardo PJ, Blondis TA, Whitman BY,
Stein MA, eds. Attention Deficits and Hyperactivity in Children and Adults. 2d ed.
New York: Marcel Dekker, Inc., 2000:197 – 214.
114. Lord C, McGee JP, eds. Educating Children with Autism. National Research
Council. Washington, DC: National Academy Press, 2001.
I. INTRODUCTION
The judicious mix of these two modes of thought is crucial to the success of
science in the pursuit of truth. Good scientists do both. They churn up many new
ideas and winnow the wheat from the chaff by critical experiment and analysis to
try to distinguish the promising ideas from the worthless ones.
Earlier chapters of this book have demonstrated that treatment of autism,
like diagnosing it and understanding its cause(s), is still in flux. With a sense of
urgency created by a growing prevalence of autism, the health care and
educational communities have risen to the task of creative thinking and continue
to produce a plethora of new and appealing ideas about interventions that may
benefit children with autism.
4. Environmental Factors
Influencing social policies, reducing architectural barriers, and making low-cost
drugs available represent interventions in this dimension. Parent counseling or
training to enhance their parenting or coping skills improves the microenviron-
ment of the child and belongs in this dimension.
children with autism were no more or less pathological than the general
population. Autism was not a form of social withdrawal but a developmental
disability characterized by impairment in relationships and communication skills
from the beginning of life and by repetitive preoccupations and movements.
In the ensuing years, Division TEACCH became foremost in the field,
publishing over 50 books, chapters, and articles to promote its precepts, and
editing the Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders to keep up-to-date on
other work in the field. One of its main ongoing contributions has been the
diagnostic instruments and many special curricula developed by its research unit
in collaboration with its clinical units with subsequent field trials in TEACCH
classrooms and centers before publication. The curricula include social skills,
communication skills, preschool programs, teacher training, family training,
behavior management, prevocational training, job advocacy and placement, and
family advocacy (6,7). The concepts introduced by TEACCH that were so novel
at its inception have become “best practice,” and often standard practice, in
special education, and even mandated by law. Individualized education plans
(IEPs), for example, are now required by federal law and are based on
individualized assessment with parents as integral members of IEP teams.
More recently, a new principle has evolved at Division TEACCH. This
posits that people with autism should be understood and worked with, not in the
context of “normalization,” but in the context of a “culture of autism” (7). This
holds that people with autism are part of a distinctive group with common
characteristics that are different from, but not necessarily inferior to “normal”
people. Indeed, the differences between people with autism and others may
sometimes favor people with autism. Their relative strengths in visual skills,
recognizing details, and memory, among other areas, may become the basis of
successful adult functioning, if properly promoted and nurtured. Therefore, the
goal for helping people with autism is not to make them more normal but to
cultivate and emphasize their strengths and interests. This is in direct contrast to
most programs for developmental disabilities; they emphasize remediation of
deficits with exclusive focus on that goal. Moreover, capitalizing on individual’s
interests, however unusual they may seem to the outside observer, increases
motivation of people with autism for engagement and learning. Focus on
strengths and interests can enhance efforts to work positively and productively
with people with autism, rather than coercing and forcing them in directions that
do not interest them and that they may not comprehend.
TEACCH was, initially, an approach to management of autism
characterized by a new interpretation of autism that vindicated families,
established the need for structured teaching for children, established a
multifaceted professional-parent relationship, and provided an organizational
structure with collaborative policies that provided service and support to families
across multiple aspects of life and across the life span. The professional-parent
e.g., finger tapping, does not interfere with the child’s learning, this behavior is
accepted in the belief that this will advance the accomplishment of educational
objectives.
Most people will need special classrooms (or social or job settings) for part
or all of the time where their physical environment, curriculum, and personnel
can be structured to support individual needs. Some higher-functioning children
need less structure and can work effectively and benefit from regular educational
or other programs.
(continued )
(continued )
These 30 results reflect comparisons of two groups, the same group under two conditions, status of a group before and after TEACCH structured teaching, or
simply describe a group without any comparison.
LOE, level of evidence; IBF, impairment of body function; A&P, activity and participation; EF, environmental factors; þ , improved; —, worse; ns, not
statistically significant; Vineland, Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales-Survey Form (communication, socialization and self-help care); PEP-R,
PsychoEducational Profile, Revised.
Improved Worse
Improved result result Unchanged or result
(statistically (not statistically ns ( p ¼ . .05) (statistically
Outcomes by dimensions of disability significant) evaluated) result significant)
Impairment of body structures
Impairment of body functions †† †† †† †† † †† †
†† †† ††
†
Developmental status (cognitive, motor, II-S (14,14,14,14,14,14,14) II-S (14)
perceptual domains)
IV-S (13,13,13,13,13)
Behavior (autistic, bizarre, aggressive, IV-S (13) IV-W (11) II-S (9)
problem) V (8,13) V (13)
Activity and participation † †† †† †† ††
Engaging in work and play activities IV-S (10) V (13) II-S (9)
(on-task, attending)
Engaging with people (affective, relating, V (13) II-S (9,9)
complying, cooperating)
Using language/communicating V (13) II-S (9)
Family and community participation (living III-W (12)
at home)
Environmental factors † †
Parent influence on child (effective IV-S (10) IV-W (11)
teaching/involvement)
The dots reflect 30 results from Table 4. Each dot is elucidated by its level of evidence (coded I –V for type of research design þ S, M, or W for strong,
moderate, or weak control to threats of validity in conducting the study) followed by the citation number for the study that produced this result.
There were also three results, positive, about engaging with other people,
but two were not statistically significant and the other was only an anecdotal
report.
Did TEACCH intervention produce more meaningful use of language?
There were three, positive, results about increased communication, but, again,
two were not statistically significant and the other was anecdotal.
Finally, one measure of participation limitation showed that even older
adolescents who were moderately to severely autistic were able to remain living
at home when supported by the TEACCH support system of structured teaching
at home and school plus training of and collaboration with parents. This result
was not statistically evaluated.
Environmental Factors. Parent influence on the child improved (with
statistical significance) in two measures that explored effect of structured
teaching training for parents. Parents became more effective in teaching their
own children and being involved with them.
accompany family in the car safely and take advantage of community dental care
rather than requiring expensive, specialty dental care necessary before.
Environmental Factors. An anecdotal report (29) for the same child
indicated that caregiving in general was made easier as a result of SI therapy.
(continued )
Part A. Average-of-Group Results: Experimental versus control groups, same group under experimental versus control conditions, before
versus after status of treated group, or description of posttreatment only
Study and Dim. of
research design Outcome of interest disability Measure Result Clin. imp. Statistics LOE
1983 Ayres Self-stimulatory behaviors IBF Time samples þa Yes V
(16):
prospective
case study
1983 Reilly Variety of speech A&P SVB of ASIEPb — Sign. IIc
(23):
single subject Mean length of utterance A&P SBV of ASIEPb — Sign. IIc
alternating Autistic speech A&P SVB of ASIEPb — Sign. IIc
treatments Speech function A&P SVB of ASIEPb ns IIc
ABAB design Articulation A&P SVB of ASIEPb ns IIc
Rate of vocalizations A&P SVB of ASIEPb ns IIc
1999 Larrington Physical posture IBF Photographs þ Yes IV-W
(29): single- Destructive behavior IBF Home/school charts/notes þ Yes IV-W
subject AB Engaging/playing A&P Home/school charts/notes þ Yes IV-W
design Toileting A&P Home/school charts/notes þ Yes IV-W
Family activity participant A&P Parent report/anecdote þ Yes V
Going to dentist A&P Parent report/anecdote þ Yes V
Easier caregiving EF Group home/family anecdote þ Yes V
1999 Stagnitti Tactile tolerance IBF Anecdote þd Yes V
(24): Affect A&P Anecdote þd Yes V
(continued )
LOE, level of evidence; IBF, impairment of body function; A&P, activity and participation; EF, environmental factors; þ , improved; —, worse; U,
unchanged; ns, not statistically significant.
a
Effective at 20 weeks, then deteriorated during subsequent 26 weeks while in body case following scoliosis surgery and after subsequent menarch.
b
Sample of Vocal Behavior of the Autism Screening Instrument for Educational Planning.
c
Inadequate information to rate conduct of study.
d
Improvement faded by 5 months posttreatment but 6- and 9-month assessments showed “sensory defensiveness cured.”
e
Carryover also present; ABC, Autism Behavior Checklist; ESCS, Early Social Communication Scales.
f
Based on items from Conners Patent Rating Scale.
1980 Ayres (26): IBF Behavior (language, awareness Observations (qualitative and 6/10a IV-W
single-subject of environment, purposeful different for each participant)
ABA design; pre activities, self-stimulation,
and post social and emotional behavior)
measures
1999 Case-Smith A&P Mastery play Engagement check/videotapes 3/5 IVb
(27): single- A&P Nonengagement Engagement check/videotapes 4/5 IVb
subject ABA A&P Interactions Engagement check/videotapes 1/5 IVb
design: 3 wk
baseline, 10
week treatment
1999 Linderman A&P Social interaction FBA for CSIDc 2/2 d — IV-W
(28): single- A&P Response to affection FBA for CSIDc 1/1 d — IV-W
subject ABA A&P Response to movement FBA for CSIDc 1/1 d — IV-W
design: 2 wk A&P Approach new activities FBA for CSIDc 1/1 d 0/1 IV-W
baseline, 7 or A&P Functional communication FBA for CSIDc — V
11 wk treatment IBF Disruptive behaviors Anecdote 2/2
Indicates a statistically significant (p , .05) result.
a
All improved but 6 were rated as good responders and 4 as poor responders.
b
Inadequate information to rate conduct of study.
c
Modified Functional Behavior Assessment for Children with Sensory Integrative Dysfunction.
d
Visual analysis of trend, slope, level, and serial dependency calculations.
not predict the good responders. Edelson et al. found that their best responders
were those who presented with higher anxiety levels compared with those with
lower levels.
Ayres and Mailloux (16) reported a tendency for regression in adolescent
years associated with the onset of puberty. The authors offered this as possible
support for the success of SI therapy being based, at least in part, “on the capacity
to enter into the neurobiological development of the child during the early critical
period for maturation of sensory integrative mechanisms.”
Part A. Average-of-Group Results. Experimental vs. control groups, same group under experimental vs. control conditions, before vs. after
status of treated group, or posttreatment-only description
Improved result Improved Worse result
Outcomes by dimensions of (statistically (but not statistically Unchanged or ns (statistically
disability significant) evaluated) (p ¼ , .05) results significant)
Impairment of body structures
Impairment of body functions †† †† †† †† †† †† †† †† †† †† †† ††
Sensory response (tactile I-S (33,33) V (24) I-S (33)
tolerance, aversion,
defensiveness, orienting to
sound)
Behavior (self-stimulation, I-S (33,33,33,34) III-W (31,31) I-S (34,34,34,34,34)
destructive, tantrums, IV-W (29)
restlessness, activity level, IV-M (32,32)
self-injury, anxiety, V (16,24,24,31,31)
stereotypie, behavior
regulation)
Physical posture IV-W (29)
Activity and participation †† †† †† †† †† †† † †† †† †† † †† †
Engaging in work/play I-S (33,33) IV-W (29) I-S (33,33)
activities (off-task, using
objects, attending,
initiating)
Self-care (toileting) IV-W (29)
Engaging with people I-S (33,33) III-W (31) I-S (33)
(relating, socialization, V (24,31)
affect)
studies in this evidence report), Barthelemy, Jonas, and Lelord (each first author
for one study). One study (50) is known to have used participants in another study
(45). Given the time frame and place in which these studies were conducted and
the participant number in some of the publications, it is highly likely that there
were overlaps of participants between some or all of the studies published by the
French group. Thus, the total number of individuals from whom evidence is
available is almost certainly much fewer than the apparent 182 and may be as few
as 86 people shown in six of the studies (45,49,14).
Daily
megavitamin Comparison
Study treatment treatment Duration Population Na Age
1978 Rimland 2.4 mg/kg to 3 Phases of no Variable 12 Boys, 4 girls with autistic symptoms 16 4 – 19 yr
(49) 94.3 mg/kg treatment and from earlier megavitamin/cluster
B6 (mode 1 phase of analysis study who were B6-
6 mg/kg) placebo responsive and relapsed on
withdrawal; 6 had score indicating
definite presence of autism; living at
home
1980 30 mg/kg Placebo phase 14 da Autism diagnosed with DSM criteria 37
Barthelemy B6 þ 10 mg/
(47)a kg Mg
(continued )
Daily
megavitamin Comparison
Study treatment treatment Duration Population Na Age
1981 Lelord 30 mg/kg Part 1: 2 phases Variable 4 wk Part 1: 26 boys, 18 girls with autistic 44 3.5– 16 yr
(45) B6 þ 10– of no symptoms: social withdrawal, (mean age
15 mg/kg Mg treatment stereotypies, tantrums, 9.3 yr)
hypersensitivity to stimuli,
aggressive toward self and others,
disordered eating and sleeping
behavior; 28 in long-term
institutional care; 7 severe MR; 33 no
speech. Heterogenous group: 16 most
clearly resembled Kanner’s original
description of autism, 28 had other
development/neurological disorders
with autistic signs (11 mild signs,
9 moderate, 8 severe)
Part 2: placebo Part 2: 13 responders (hospitalized in 21
phase child psychiatric service) and 8
nonresponders from Part 1
(continued )
Daily
megavitamin Comparison
Study treatment treatment Duration Population Na Age
1989 Martineau 30 mg/kg 2 Phases of no 14 wk Boys with autism diagnosed with DSM- 6 4 y 7 mo –
(53) pyridoxine þ treatment III criteria: most disturbed in 8 yr
10 mg/kg Mg impaired communication, lack of
socially appropriate facial
expressions/gestures, resistance to
change, frustration, abnormal
aggression; global DQ 30– 70
1997 Findling 1 g/kg No treatment 10 wk 11 Boys, 1 girl diagnosed with autism 10 3– 17 yr
(54) B6 þ 10 mg/ and placebo using DSM-IIIR criteria; no medical
kg Mg phases or neurological disorders; no
psychotropic agents within 3 mo; all
lived at home
CPRS, Children’s Psychiatric Rating Scale; B6, vitamin B6 or pyridoxine; Mg, magnesium.
a
All participants crossed between treatment and control conditions.
b
Data taken from reviews (39, 41).
(continued )
LOE, level of evidence; IBF, impairment of body function; A&P, activity and participation; þ , improved result with therapy; U, unchanged result; uHVA,
urinary homovanillic acid; AER, conditioned auditory evoked responses at occipital (O) and central (C) sites; EP, evoked potentials; ns, not statistically
significant; Signif., significant; CPRS, Children’s Psychiatric Rating Scale; CGIS, Clinical Global Impression Scale; OCS, NIMH Global Obsessive
Compulsive Scale.
a
Also contained an index healthy group.
b
Also contained an index group comparing magnesium and placebo.
c
Improvement after 1 mo followed by a shading off.
d
Data plots.
to do” (57). That is, teachers are currently trained to give basic directions and
wait for a response because too much verbalizing has been understood to be too
distracting. In contrast, Soma Mukhopadhyay talks constantly—repetitively
urging, prodding, and directing. Thus, her technique is being called “rapid-
prompt method.” Instead of being distracting, this rapid-prompt method seems to
keep the children’s attention focused long enough for them to communicate. She
also does not try to redirect students when they engage in stereotypic movements
and fail to make eye contact. Instead, she ignores their erratic movements and
wandering eyes because, according to insights gleaned from her son’s writings,
these behaviors appear to serve other important functions that do not interfere
with learning. Tito has written that he can either see or hear, but not both
simultaneously, so that he must choose one. He says that he rocks and spins
because he cannot feel his body unless it is in motion.
via msn.com on February 2, 2003 yielded 114 results for websites that contained
the words “Tito Mukhopadhyay”; these included a link to the Cure Autism Now
website (58).
Examination of these Internet results showed, however, that all reflect the
same information that originated in a New York Times News Service story (57)
that subsequently appeared in multiple versions and venues: other newspapers,
People magazine, and three television broadcasts. In addition, one website shows
a book entitled Beyond the Silence, written by Tito Mukhopadhyay and published
by the National Autistic Society in London (19).
REFERENCES
23. Reilly C, Nelson D, Bundy A. Sensorimotor versus fine motor activities in eliciting
vocalizations in autistic children. Occup Ther J Res 1983; 8:187– 190.
24. Stagnitti K, Raison P, Ryan P. Sensory defensiveness syndrome: a paediatric
perspective and case study. Aust Occup Ther J 1999; 46:175– 187.
25. Baranek G. Efficacy of sensory and motor interventions for children with autism.
J Autism Dev Disord 2002; 32(5):397– 422.
26. Ayres A, Tickle L. Hyper-responsivity to touch and vestibular stimuli as a predictor
of positive response to sensory integration procedures by autistic children. Am J
Occup Ther 1980; 34(6):375 –381.
27. Case-Smith J, Bryan T. The effects of occupational therapy with sensory integration
emphasis on preschool-age children with autism. Am J Occup Ther 1999; 49:645 –
652.
28. Linderman T, Stewart K. Sensory integrative-based occupational therapy and
functional outcomes in young children with pervasive developmental disorders: a
single-subject study. Am J Occup Ther 1999; 52(2):207– 213.
29. Larrington G. A sensory integration based program with a severely retarded/
autistic teenager: an occupational therapy case report. Occup Ther Health Care
1987; 4(2):101– 107.
30. Ray T, King L, Grandin T. The effectiveness of self-initiated vestibular stimulation
in producing speech sounds in an autistic child. Occup Ther J Res 1988; 8:186– 190.
31. McClure M, Holtz-Yotz M. The effects of sensory stimulatory treatment on an
autistic child. Am J Occup Ther 1990; 45(12):1138 –1142.
32. Zissermann L. The effects of deep pressure on self-stimulating behaviors in a child
with autism and other disabilities. Am J Occup Ther 1991; 46(6):547– 551.
33. Field T, Lasko P, Henteleff T, Kabat S, Talpins S, Dowling M. Brief report: autistic
children’s attentiveness and responsivity improve after touch therapy. J Autism Dev
Disord 1997; 27:333– 339.
34. Edelson S, Goldberg M, Edelson M, Kerr D, Grandin T. Behavioral and
physiological effects of deep pressure on children with autism: a pilot study
evaluating the efficacy of Grandin’s Hug Machine. Am J Occup Ther 1999;
53:145 – 152.
35. Watling R. Selected literature exploring the effectiveness of a sensory-
based approach to the treatment of autism. Phys Occup Ther Pediatr 1998;
18(2):77– 85.
36. Dawson G, Watling R. Interventions to facilitate auditory, visual, and motor
integration in autism: a review of the evidence. J Autism Dev Disord 2000;
30(5):415– 421.
37. Rimland B. High dosage levels of certain vitamins in the treatment of children with
severe mental disorders. In: Hawkins D, Pauling L, eds. Orthomolecular Psychiatry.
New York: WH Freeeman, 1973.
38. Rimland B. An orthomolecular study of psychotic children. J Orthomolec
Psychiatry 1974; 3(371– 377).
39. Pfeiffer S, Norton J, Nelson L, Shott S. Efficacy of vitamin B6 and magnesium in
the treatment of autism: a methodology review and summary of outcomes. J Autism
Dev Disord 1995; 25(5):481– 493.
40. Sankar DV. Do they have vitamin deficiencies? J Autism Dev Disord 1979;
9(1):73 – 82.
41. Kleijnen J, Knipschild P. Niacin and vitamin B6 in mental functioning: a review of
controlled trials in humans. Biol Psychiatry 1991; 29:931 – 941.
42. Tolbert L, Haigler T, Waits M, Dennis T. Brief report: lack of response in an
autistic population to a low dose clinical trial of pyridoxine plus magnesium.
J Autism Dev Disord 1993; 23(1):193– 199.
43. Lelord G, Callaway E, Muh J, et al. Modifications in urinary homovanillic acid after
ingestion of vitamin B6; functional study in autistic children. Rev Neurol (Paris)
1978; 134:797 – 801.
44. Barthelemy C, Garreau B, Leddet I, Ernouf D, Muh J, Lelord G. Behavioral
and biological effects of oral magnesium, vitamin B6, and combined mag-
nesium –vitamin B6 administration in autistic children. Magnes Bull 1981; 2:150 –
153.
45. Lelord G, Muh J, Barthelemy C, Martineau J, Garreau B, Callaway E. Effects of
pyridoxine and magnesium on autistic symptoms—initial observations. J Autism
Dev Disord 1981; 11(2):219– 230.
46. Martineau J, Barthelemy C, Cheliakine C, Lelord G. Brief report: an open middle-
term study of combined vitamin B6 – magnesium in a subgroup of autistic children
selected on their sensitivity to treatment. J Autism Dev Disord 1988; 18:435 – 478.
47. Barthelemy C, Garreau B, Leddet I, et al. Biological and clinical effects of oral
magnesium and associated magnesium/vitamin B6 administration of certain
disorders in infantile autism. Therapie 1980; 35:627– 632.
48. Jonas C, Etienne T, Jouve J, Mariotte N. Interet clinique et biochimique de
l’association vitamine B6 þ magnesium dans le traitement de l’autisme residuel a
l’age adulte. Therapie 1984; 39:661 – 669.
49. Rimland B, Callaway E, Dreyfus P. The effect of high doses of vitamin B6 on
autistic children: a double-blind crossover study. Am J Psychiatry 1978;
135(4):472– 475.
50. Martineau J, Garreau B, Barthelemy C, Callaway E, Lelord G. Effects of vitamin B6
on averaged evoked potentials in infantile autism. Biol Psychiatry 1981;
16(7):627– 640.
51. Martineau J, Barthelemy C, Garreau B, Lelord G. Vitamin B6, magnesium, and
combined B6 – Mg: therapeutic effects in childhood autism. Biol Psychiatry 1985;
20(5-467-468).
52. Martineau J, Barthelemy C, Lelord G. Long-term effects of combined vitamin B6 –
magnesium administration in an autistic child: case report. Biol Psychiatry 1986;
21:511– 518.
53. Martineau J, Barthelemy C, Roux S, Garreau B, Lelord G. Electrophysiological
effects of fenfluramine or combined vitamin B6 and magnesium on children with
autistic behavior. Dev Med Child Neurol 1989; 31:721– 727.
54. Findling R, Maxwell K, Scotese-Wojtila L, Huang J, Yamashita T, Wiznitzer M.
High-dose pyridoxine and magnesium administration in children with autistic
disorder: an absence of salutary effects in a double-blind, placebo-controlled study.
J Autism Dev Disord 1997; 27(4):467– 478.
55. CBS Television. 60 Minutes: Beyond the silence. Cure Autism Now [website].
Available at: http://www.cureautismnow.org.
56. ABC Television. Good Morning America. Cure Autism Now [website]. Available
at: http://www.cureautismnow.org.
57. Blakeslee S. A rare map of autism’s world. In New York Times. Cure Autism Now.
Available at: http://www.cureautismnow.org.
58. Cure Autism Now. Beyond the Silence (Accessed February 2, 2003). Available at:
http://www.cureautismnow.org.
59. Sackett DL, Richardson WS, Rosenberg W, Haynes RB. Evidence-Based
Medicine: How to Practice and Teach EBM. New York: Churchill Livingstone,
1997.
The prognosis for ASD should be considered in terms of change for the better,
rather than cure. Cure is rare in ASD. Although some children, particularly those
with normal intelligence, functional language, and absence of stereotypies, may
not be readily recognized as autistic later in life, most continue to have at least
subtle deficits in social skills. Prognosis has been shown to depend on two
variables: the degree of abnormal autistic behaviors and the level of adaptive
functioning in the community. Adaptive functioning is dependent on intelligence
as well as on daily living abilities. Throughout the life span these two variables
interact. The prognosis for any given child depends upon his/her place in the
spectrum for each of these interacting trajectories. It appears that prognosis for
independence as an adult may correlate better with level of adaptive functioning
than with the severity of autistic behaviors (1,2,3).
REFERENCES