Felner 1981
Felner 1981
Felner 1981
2, 1981
Because crisis events place demands on people that e x c e e d their normal resources
for dealing with t h e m (Murphy, 1961), they often produce rapid, significant
This research is based on a dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment for the PhD degree
at the University of Rochester. Appreciation is due to Alice Wilson, Raymond P. Lorion,
Ellis Gesten, Michael DeStefano, and Raymond Francis for their contributions to data
collection and analyses. Parts of this article were presented at the 46th annual meeting of
the Eastern Psychological Association, Boston, April 1978. This research was supported
by Grant MN 11820-04 from the National Institute of Mental Health, Experimental and
Special Training Branch and Graiat NIH 4-SOC7-RR07015 from the National Institute of
Health, Biomedical Support Division.
2All correspondence should be sent to Robert D. Felner, Department of Psychology, Box
1 la Yale Station, Yale University, New Haven Connecticut 06520.
181
0091-0562/81/0400-0181 $ 0 3 . 0 0 / 0 © 1981 Plenum Publishing Corporation
182 Felner, Ginter, Boike, and Cowen
changes in their coping styles (Caplan, 1964). If a person's typical coping strat-
egies are ineffective in crisis situations, new ones, sometimes effective, some-
times ineffective, must be developed (Caplan, 1961; Lindemann, 1956; Stein
1970; Morley, Note 1). Frequently, the way in which a crisis is resolved has
important long-term consequences for the individual. Whereas effective crisis-
management builds future strengths and adaptive resources in the person, poor
crisis-management makes for more serious maladjustment.
Although the loss of a significant other is widely thought to precipitate
crisis reactions and maladaption (Darbonne, 1967; Klein & Lindemann, 1961),
actual studies of its effects have yielded mixed results, i.e., some researchers
have found relationships between death and later maladaptive behavior, e.g.,
psychiatric problems, particularly depression (Barry, Barry, & Lindemann,
1965; Birtchnell, 1969; Bratfos, 1967; Brown, 1961; Sugar, 1970; Rutter,
1966), others (Blaine & Carmen, 1968; Gregory, 1966; Munro, 1966) have
not. For children, both parent death or divorce may be severe life events that
predispose to crisis reactions and maladaptation (Felner, Stolberg, & Cowen,
1975; Sugar, 1970). Illustratively, Koller and Castanos (1968) and Haim (1970)
report that a significant proportion of adolescents and adults who attempted or
committed suicide had childhood histories of parent separation, divorce, or
death.
Several investigators have studied the differential consequences of various
types of parent loss. Glueck and Glueck (1950) found that a significantly higher
proporation of delinquent boys came from homes broken by parent death than
by divorce. Brown and Epps (1966) found criminal behavior to be associated
with parental separation experiences, other than death, in childhood. Sugar
(1970), however, argues that the effects of death and divorce are basically
similar, with essentially identical mourning responses being triggered by both
events.
Thus, although some studies suggest serious early crisis has long-lasting ef-
fects, and that these effects may differ as a function of the particular crisis-
producing event, most of the work is (a) retrospective and (b) difficult to
replicate across studies.
Felner et al. (1975) studied the school adjustment problems associated
with specific crisis events. Among maladapting primary graders, children with
histories of parental death or divorce were found to be significantly more
maladjusted than referred, demographically matched noncrisis controls. More-
over, the two crisis groups had different types of problems, i.e., elevated acting-
out problems for separation/divorce children, and elevated shy-anxious problems
for death children.
Although the preceding findings are instructive, they raise the question
of whether similar findings would be obtained in a "normal" sample. More-
over, most prior studies of children in crisis focus on their symptoms or prob-
lems. Concern about the limits of pathology-oriented conceptualizations in
Parental Death or Divorce and School Adjustment 183
mental health (Cowen, Gardner, & Zax, 1967; Zax & Cowen, 1976) also applies
to the study of crisis, where there is a shifting focus to the positive aspects of
crisis resolution (Finket, 1974; 1975). Hence, it may also be useful to examine
strengths and resources in crisis children, including the possibility that different
crisis events are associated with different resource patterns.
The present article reports two separate studies that address the following
specific questions: (a) Among young maladapting referred children, do children
with and without crisis histories have different health resource patterns as well
as problem profiles? (b) Do referred children with separated or divorced parents
have different resources, as well as problems, than those who have lost a parent
through death?, and (c) Will similar problem/resource profiles be found among
"normal" nonreferred children who have experienced comparable crises?
Based on earlier findings (Felner et al., 1975), children with histories of
parental separation or divorce are expected to have serious acting-out overall
maladjustment problems, and children who have experienced parental death
more serious shy-anxious and overall problems than referred noncrisis peers.
Directionally similar, if less extreme, differences should be found among non-
referred children who have experienced comparable life crises. No prediction
about differential patterns of resources can be made due to lack of prior studies.
STUDY 1
Method
Subjects in this study were primary graders identified through the Primary
Mental Health Project (PMHP), a program for early detection and prevention of
school maladaptation (Cowen, Dorr, Clarfield, Kreling, McWilliams, Pokracki,
Pratt, Terrell, & Wilson, 1973; Cowen, Trost, Lorion, Dorr, Izzo, & Isaacson,
1975; Cowen, 1980). Typically, PMHP children are referred for classroom-
identified behavioral (e.g., acting-out, withdrawal) or educational problems.
The program uses trained nonprofessional child-aides, working under profes-
sional supervision, as direct help-agents for such children (Zax, Cowen, Izzo,
Madonia, Merenda, & Trost, 1966). During the school year 1974-75, 211 children
were seen in four PMHP project schools.
Subjects
referral packet dealt with family background problems that seemed to be con-
tributing to children's current difficulties (cf. below). Two such background
items "parental separation or divorce" and "death of a parent," were used to
select "crisis" children in this study. A total of 68 children with a history of
parental separation or divorce and 21 others with a history of parental death
were so identified by teachers. These two crisis subgroups were matched to
each other and a control group (i.e., 90 referred children without crisis histories)
for sex, school grade, socioeconomic status, school location, repeat-in-grade
status, and repeat in PMHP. Four children had to be dropped from the separation/
divorce group to obtain satisfactory matches on these demographic variables.
Measures
3In Study I, the control group may have included a very few children who had experienced
a parental death or divorce that, in the teacher's opinion, was not related to adjustment
problems. In Study II, however, teachers checked all family problems known to be present
without regard to relationship to adjustment problems.
Parental Death or Divorce and School Adjustment 185
Results
STUDY II
Method
Experimental Context
Primary grade teachers in six local schools (four parochial, two public) in
Ridgway and DuBois, Pennsylvania, submitted school adjustment information
for subjects in this study. The two communities are largely rural. Residents,
most of whom were from "upper-lower" or "lower-middle" socioeconomic
strata, are primarily employed in mining, foresting, and light industry.
4The factors are orthogonal; the correlation between the CARS factors and the HRI factors
is quite low.
186 Felner, Ginter, Boike, and Cowen
Subjects
Subjects were randomly selected from grades K - 3 in the six schools and in-
cluded 468 children who were rated on a school adjustment measure and half that
number (243) on a health index. Within the full sample (n = 468), 37 children
with histories of parental separation or divorce and 14 with histories of par-
ental death were identified, using the same procedure described for Study I
(see footnote 3). A control group of 51 children without crisis histories was
matched to these crisis groups for sex, school, grade, and repeat-in-grade status.
Further, chi-square analyses revealed the 14 children with parental death histories
to be matched to the 37 with parental separation or divorce background on the
previously listed demographic variables.
Measures
Results
Separate one way ANOVAs were performed comparing divorce, death, and
control subjects on each of the six TEF dimensions. Again, where significant
overall Fs were found Newman-Keuls tests of differences among specific group
means were performed. The latter are reported where p < .05. Because there
were too few (n = 5) children in the parental death group in the HRI sample to
permit comparisons with the other groups, the latter analyses were limited to
t tests comparing divorce children and controls on the seven HRI dimensions.
Significant group differences were found on TEF acting-out, F(2, 99) =
5.21, p < .01, socialization problems, F(2, 99) = 3.64, p < .05, and overall
Parental Death or Divorce and School Adjustment 187
DISCUSSION
Children with family histories of death and divorce were found to show
greater overall school maladaptation than children without such histories.
Specifically, those with parental separation/divorce have more serious acting-
out problems than both death and noncrisis controls, whereas those with histories
of parental death have more serious shy-anxious problems than separation/divorce
children or controls. These findings are consistent with earlier data (Felner et al.,
1975). That they were found, here, to generalize to a rural "normal" sample,
strengthens the support for the conclusion that different life and familial crises
have specific and differential effects on children's classroom behavior.
Teachers also made differential judgments about the competencies of these
groups. Although the findings are somewhat uneven, divorce children, in general,
were judged to have fewer competencies than death children or noncrisis controls,
a difference that showed up on three specific HRI factors and on the HRI
sum score. Teachers saw control children as having significantly greater resources
than children with histories of parental divorce. Of particular interest is that in
both the normative and referred samples, controls were judged to have signif-
icantly greater resources for coping with class rules than separation/divorce
children. The deficiencies of divorce children in following rules and in two other
competencies, frustration tolerance and peer sociability, may contribute to the
elevated acting-out problems of divorce children.
It must be noted that matching difficulties and limited sample sizes
precluded finer-grained analyses of such pertinent questions as the possible dif-
ferential impact of different crisis situations across sex and socioeconomic
groups. Nor could age of the child at the time of the event be considered due to
the lack of availability of such data. Another limit of the present findings is
that they depend heavily on teacher judgments. Further work should include
other types of outcome criteria (e.g., independent behavioral observation of
behavior, multiple measures of personality adjustment, and self-concept).
Caplan (1964) and others (Murphy, 1961; Felner et al., 1975) suggest
that certain crisis consequences result from a person's increased sensitivity to
188 Felner, Ginter, Boike, and Cowen
REFERENCE NOTES
1. Morley, W. Treatment of the patient in crisis. Unpublished manuscript, 1964.
2. Felner, R. D., Norton, P. C., & Cowen, E. L. The development of a school-based crisis
intervention program for young children. Manuscript in preparation, Yale University,
1978.
REFERENCES
Barry, H., Jr., Barry, H., III,& Lindemann, E. Dependency in adult patients following early
maternal bereavement. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 1965, 40, 196-206.
Birtchnell, J. The possible consequences of early parent death. British Journal of Medical
Psychiatry, 1969, 42, 1-12.
Blaine, G. B., & Carmen, C. R. Causal factors in suicide attempts .by male and female college
students. American Journal of Psychiatry, 1968, 125, 146-149.
190 Felner, Ginter, Boike, and Cowen
Bratfos, O. Parental deprivation in childhood and type of future mental disease. Acta
Psychiatriea Scandinavia, 1967, 43, 453-461.
Brown, F. Depression and childhood bereavement. Journal of Mental Science, 1961,107,
754-777.
Brown, R., & Epps, P. Childhood bereavement and subsequent crime. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 1966, 112, 1043-1048.
Caplan, G. (Ed.). Prevention of mental disorder in children. New York: Basic Books, 1961.
Caplan, G. Principles of preventive psychiatry. New York: Basic Books, 1964.
Clarfield, S. P. The development of a teacher referral form for identifying early school
maladaption. American Journal of Community Psychology, 1974, 2, 212-218.
Cowen, E. L. The primary mental health project: Yesterday, today and tomorrow. Journal
of Special Education, 1980, 14, in press.
Cowen, E. L., Dorr, D., Clarfield, S. P., Kreling, B., McWilliams, S. A., Pokracki, F., Pratt,
D. M., Terrell, D. C., & Wilson, A. B. The AML: A quick screening device for early
detection of school maladaptation. American Journal of Community Psychology,
1973, 1, 12-35.
Cowen, E. L., Gardner, E. A., & Zax, M. (Eds.). Emergent approaches to mental health
programs. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1967.
Cowen, E. L., Trost, M. A., Lorion, R. P., Dorr, D., Izzo, L. D., & Isaacson, R. V. New ways
in school mental health: Early detection and prevention of school maladaptation.
New York: Human Sciences Press, 1975.
Cummings, J., & Cummings, E. Ego and milieu: Theory and practice of environmental
therapy. New York, Atherton, 1966.
Darbonne, A. Crisis: A review of theory, practice and research. Psychotherapy: Theory,
Research andPraetiee, 1967, 4, 49-59.
Dohrenwend, B. S. Social stress and community psychology. American Journal of Com-
munity Psychology, 1978, 6, 1-14.
Dohrenwend, B. S., & Dohrenwend, B. P. (Eds.). Stressful life events. New York: Wiley,
1974.
Felner, R. D., Stolberg, A., & Cowen, E. L. Crisis events and school mental health referral
patterns of young children. Journal of Consulting and ClinicalPsychology, 1975,43,
305-310.
Finkel, N. J. Strens and traumas: An attempt at categorization. American Journal of Com-
munity Psychology, 1974, 2, 265-273.
Finkel, N. J. Strens, traumas, and trauma resolution. American Journal of Community
Psychology, 1975, 3, 173-178.
Gersten, J. C., Langner, R. S., Eisenberg, J. G., & Orzeck, L. Child behavior and life events:
Undeskable change or change per se? In B. S. Dohrenwend & B. P. Dohrenwend
(Eds.), Stressful life events. New York: Wiley, 1974.
Gesten, E. A health resources inventory: The development of a measure of the personal and
social competence of primary grade children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 1976, 44, 775-786.
Glueck, S., & Glueck, E. Unravelling fuvenile delinquency. Boston: Harvard University
Press, 1950.
Gregory, I. Retrospective data concerning childhood loss of a parent. I: Category of parental
loss by decade of bkth, diagnosis and MMPI. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1966,
15, 362-367.
Haim, A. Adolescent suicide. New York: International Universities Press, 1970.
Hetherington, E. M., Cox, M., & Cox, R. The aftermath of divorce. In J. A. Stevens, Jr.
& M. Mathews (Eds.), Mother-child, father-child relations. Washington, D.C.: NAEYL.
1977.
Hetherington, E. M., Cox, M., & Cox, R. Stress and coping in divorce: A focus on women.
In J. Gullahorn (Ed.), Psychology and women in transition. Washington, D.C.: B. H.
Winston & Sons, 1979.
Klein, D. C., & Lindemann, E. Preventive interventions in individual and family crisis
situations. In G. Caplan (Ed.), Prevention of mental disorders in children. New
York: Basic Books, 1961.
Parental Death or Divorce and School Adjustment 191
Koller, K. M., & Castanos, J. N. The influence of parental deprivation in attempted suicide.
Mental Journal of Australia, 1968, 1, 396-399.
Lindemann, E. The meaning of crisis in individual and family living. Teachers College
Record, 1956, 57, 310-315.
Lorion, R. P., Cowen, E. L., & Caldwell, R. A. Normative and parametric analysis of school
maladjustment. American Journal of Community Psychology, 1975, 3, 291-302.
Munro, A. Parental deprivation in depressive patients. British Journal of Psychiatry, 1966,
112, 443-457.
Murphy, L. B. Preventive implications of development in the preschool years. In G. Caplan
(Ed.), Prevention of mental disorders in children. New York: Basic Books, 1961.
Rutter, N. Children of sick parents. Maudsley Monograph, No. 16. New York: Oxford
University Press, 1966.
Stein, K. A. A challenge to the role of the crisis concept in emergency psychotherapy.
Dissertation Abstracts International, 1970, 30, 5045B.
Sugar, M. Divorce and children. Southern Medical Journal, 1970, 63, 1458-1461.
Tuckman, S., & Regan, R. A. Intactness of the home and behavioral problems in children.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 1967, 7, 225-233.
Zax, M., & Cowen, E. L. Abnormal psychology: Changing conceptions (2nd ed.). New
York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1976.
Zax, M., Cowen, E. L., Izzo, L. D., Madonia, A. J., Merenda, J., & Trost, M. A. A teacher-
aide program for preventing emotional disturbance in primary grade schoolchildren.
Mental Hygiene, 1966, 50, 406-414.