Social Categorization and Identity in The Philippines
Social Categorization and Identity in The Philippines
Social Categorization and Identity in The Philippines
MA CECILIA GASTAROO-CONACO
Department ofPsychology
University ofthe Philippines
Diliman, Quezon City
ABSTRACT
INTRODUCTION
Bruner, Goodnow and Austin ( 1956) said that "to categorize is to render
discriminably different things equivalent, to group the objects and events and
people around us into classes, and to respond to them in terms of their class
membership rather than their uniqueness." By grouping objects and events to
gether that, to our minds, have commonalities on certain dimensions, we are able to
think about and respond to them in familiar learned ways. Categorization thus
serves as an anchor in a complex and potentially overwhelming environment (Lingle,
Altom and Medin, 1984).
Nowhere is the value of categorization more apparent than in dealing with
our social world. Without categorization, every person and every social situation
would have to be processed as new, leaving the social perceiver confused and
overwhelmed by the volume of information that needs to be processed prior to
engaging in any action.
269
270 Transactions National Academy of Science
MEIRODOI..OGY
Subjects
At the outset, this researcher was interested in obtaining a sample that would
give a fair distribution of the different eUmic groups in the country. Since a large
national sample was not possible for the research, it was decided to pilot the
research with the Social Science classes at the College of Social Sciences and
Philosophy of tl1e Univesity of the Philippines in Diliman. The Social Science courses
are general education courses required of all students at the university in their first
two years of college. It was thus reasonable to expect the students to be fairly
varied in their backgrounds. Three Social Science classes were approached with a
total of 106 respondents ultimately participating in the study.
The respondents' ages ranged from 16 to 26 years with 46.2% falling in the
18-ycar-old bracket. Majority (79.2%) were Catholics. More than half of the
respondents (65. 1 %) graduated from a private lligh school and only 34.9% came
from public high schools.
Majority (59.4%) of the respondents listed a home address situated in Metro
Manila with the rest listing provincial addresses from all over t11e country. Only
35.8% of the respondents, however, mentioned the National Capital Region as their
Conaco: Social Categorization and Identity 271
region o f origin. The rest mentioned various regions all over the country, although
regions in Luzon (Central Luzon and Southern Tagalog) were over-represented
(1 3.2% and 15. 1%, respectively).
Respondents categorized themselves as belonging to different ethnic groups.
Table I shows the distribution of the self-ascribed ethnicity of the respondents It
should be noted that although Manileno is not, strictly speaking, an ethnic group,
a small percentage of the respondents gave that as their etlmic affi liation.
Parental ethnicity of the respondents was also varied. Approximately one
third of the respondents (33.96%) had parents who came from different ethnic
groups. The rest had parents belonging to t11e same ethnic group.
Across the different ethnic groups, 65.09% claimed Tagalog or Pilipino as the
dominant language used. Of the remaining 34. 9 1 %, 1 3 . 2 1 % said their dominant
language was English. Thus, only a total of 2 1 .7% actually used the language of
the ethnic group they belonged to as a major means of conununication.
Questions about other languages used by the respondent showed that 22%
of those who did not mention Tagalog or Pilipino as their dominant language
mentioned it later as another language also used.
Procedure
The .first question of interest in the research was the matrix of social categories
in which both eUmic and national group memberships may be embedded. To tap
into group memberships viewed as relevant and important, respondents were simply
asked to list down all the social categories or groups which they felt they belonged
to and to rate each one in terms of it personal importance. Order of listing was
u ed as an indicator of category information accessibility and group salience.
The maximum number of social categories listed was l 3 with a mean of 7.58
groups mentioned. Among Ute mentioned categories, family, gender and religion, in
272 Transactions National Academy of Science
that order, were mentioned first by 91 .9% of the respondents. Family was particularly
salient with 27.4% mentioning it first. This finding is not smprising and is consistent
with previous research on the marked significance of the family to tl1e Filipino
(Doronila, 1989, 1992; Medina, 199 1 ; Torres, n.d.).
National group came in a poor fourth among the first mentions with only four
respondents mentioning it before all other groups named. Across mentions,
however, national group was identified as a relevant social category by 67.92% of
the respondents. It would appear then that nationality, though viewed as a relevant
social category, is not as salient (i.e., not the first tlting t11at come� to mind) as
apparently more basic group memberships.
How did the ethnicity-based social category fare? Many of those who criticize
the Filipino's lack of nationalist sentiment often blame heightened sense of region
alism or strong sense of ethnicity as the social cognitive approach that would trace
to an overly salient and highly accessible ethnic category. Doronila ( 1989), citing
the literature on Pltilippine ethnic groups, reported that "ethnic boundaries have
not yet been transcended, for which reason it is also reported that Philippine society
remains primarily familistic and secondarily regionalistic in orientation."
Data collected showed that ethnicity was mentioned in second place, at tl1e
earliest, and this by only 2 out of 106 respondents. Taken all together, however,
ethnicity was mentioned as relevant by only 38.68% of the respondents. It appears
that more respondents were mindful of t11e national compared to the ethnic group
category. The mean salience rating (based on the category list position) for
nationality was 3 .24 compared to 2.68 for ethnicity. Though caution in interpreta
tion is advised given the skewness of the sample, the results appear to be consist
ent with a nationalist mind-shift tl1at Doronila ( 1 992) noted in her research.
Consistent with the data on category salience, the importance ratings given
to the national group were also higher compared to those for the ethnic group
(mean rating for nationality is 3 .08, whereas that for ethnicity is 2.68, given a 4-
point rating scale where 4 = very important).
Five types of respondents may be identified based on the category salience
data: ( l ) those who seem to find no relevance to either nationality or etlmicity as
evidenced by their failure to mention eitl1er national or ethnic social categories; (2)
those for whom ethnic group seems dominant, mentioning only this category but
not the national group· (3) those for whom ethnicity is primary but do not forget
the national group; ( 4) those who mention or consider the national group before
tltinking of tl1eir ethnicity; and (5) those who consider only the national group.
Further research should be done to look into the impact of these five cognitive sets
on the definition of national identity.
Conaco: Social Categorization and Identity 273
CONCLUSIONS
In summary, three major findings are underscored. First, national and ethnic
groups are identified as relevant social categories but they are not as salient nor as
easily accessible as categories like family, gender or religious groups. Second,
being Filipino w-as more salient and rated as more important than being a member of
one's eUmic group. Third, despite the cognitive awareness of the importance of the
national category, category content was less positive compared to the ethnic group
category.
What are the implications of the findings? It would appear that, on the cogni
tive level, we acknowledge our Filipino-ness and the importance of being a member
of tllis particular social category. Yet, it seems apparent that we have a less clear
picture of what the Filipino is and how being Filipino relates to our other social
identities. We see little overlap between our definition of our own group and the
Filipino.
On the affective level, we still favor our own group over the national group.
We ascribe more positive features to our ethnic group than we do to U1e Filipino
seemingly ignoring the fact tl1at one identity is subordinate to t11e other.
This research certainly raises more questions with its results than it had
i ntended to answer. Clearly, more research needs to be undertaken if we are to more
fully understand the Filipinos' processing of social categories and their social
identity.
ACKNOWLEDGMENT
Percentage
Tagalog 41.51
Ilocano 13.21
Manileno 10.38
Bicolano 9.43
Cebuano Visayan 8.50
Chinese/mestizo 4.72
Ilonggo 4.72
Pampango 3.n
Davaoeno 2.83
Muslim .94
Table 3. Mean number of overlapping features for ethnic and national groups
Ilocano 2.357
Tagalog 3.227
Bicolano 2.400
Pampango 2.500
Manileno 2.455
Cebuano Visayan 3.444
Davaoeno 2.000
Muslim l .OOO
Chinese/mestizo 2.200
Ilonggo 2.800
Combined groups 2.821
REFERENCES
Bruner, J.S., J.J. Goodnow and G.A. Austin. 1 95 6 . A Study of Thinking. New York:
Wiley.
Constantino, R. 1974. Identity and Consciousness: The Philippine Experience. Quezon City.
Corpuz, O.D. 1990. Roots of the Filipino Nation. Quezon City: Aklahi Foundation.
DoronUa, M.L.C. 1989. The Limits of Educational Change: National Identity Formation in a
Philippine Public Elementary School. Quezon City: University of the Philippines Press.
-------- 1992. National Identity and Social Change. Quezon City: University of
the Philippines Press and U.P. Center for Integrative and Development Studies.
Giles, H. and P. Johnson. 1 9 8 1 . The role of language in ethnic group relations. In: J.
Turner and H. Giles (eds.) Intergroup Behavior. Chicago: University of Chicago
Press.
Llncle, J.H., M.W. Altom and D.L. Medin. 1984. Of cabbages and kings: assessing the
extendability of natural object concept models to social things. In: R.S. Wyer Jr. and
T.K. Srull (eds.) Handbook of Social Cognition, vol. I. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum.
Medina, B.T.G. 1 99 1 . The Filipino Family. Quezon City: University of the Philippines Press.
Oakes, P. 1 987. The salience of social categories. In: J. Turner Rediscovering the Social
Group. New York: Basil Blackwell.
Torres, A.T. n.d. A Portrait of Filipino Culture. Module No. I of the Psychological Association
of the Philippines Module Series.
Tajfel, H. 1 98 1 . Human Groups and Social Categories. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Turner, J.C. 1 9 8 1 . The experimental social psychology of intergroup behavior. In: J. Turner
and H. Giles (eds.) Intergroup Behavior. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Turner, C. 1987. Rediscovering the social group: A Selfcategorization Theory. New York:
Basil Blackwell.
Tversky. A. 1977. Features of Similarity. Psychological Review, 84, 327-352.