Attitudes Towards Mental Illness: A Comparative Sample Study of Sweden Contra India
Attitudes Towards Mental Illness: A Comparative Sample Study of Sweden Contra India
Attitudes Towards Mental Illness: A Comparative Sample Study of Sweden Contra India
Sandra Frykman
Julia Angbrant
Bachelor in Philosophy
Psychology
Luleå University of Technology
Department of Business Administration, Technology and Social Sciences
Engineering Psychology
Abstract
This study seeks to investigate the attitudes towards mental illness in Sweden in comparison
with India. To measure attitudes towards people with mental illness the questionnaire
Community Attitudes Towards Mental Illness (CAMI) was used containing positive and
negative statements regarding mental illness. These statements are divided into the four attitude
categories of Authoritarianism, Benevolence, Social Restrictiveness and Community Mental
Health Ideology. The questionnaire study consisted of a total of 410 students; 272 from
Sweden and 138 from India. Sweden and India were chosen because of their differences based
on factors that recent studies have shown affect a community’s attitudes towards mental illness;
socio-economic, religion and culture. The results show that there is a difference in attitudes
between the two countries in three out of the four attitude categories, where India showed a
more positive attitude than Sweden towards mental illness meaning they have a greater sense of
authoritative approach, goodwill, and social inclusion. This result was expected in the sense
that there was a significant difference between the countries but not in the sense that India was
to be the country to have a more positive attitude than Sweden since it goes against what
previous research indicated.
Introduction
In 2001, the World Health Organisation (WHO) estimated that one in four people would
experience a mental health condition during their lifetime and that approximately 450 million
people worldwide are disabled as a consequence (Janet, 2001). In recent years, mental illness is
becoming increasingly acknowledged as a global issue (Eaton, Kakuma, Wright, & Minas,
2014). At the same time, depending on which part of the world a person is living at the
situation for the ones suffering from mental illness can vary a lot. For example, access to proper
health care may vary considerably, depending on where you are located, which in turn often
depends on the country's economic growth. Even factors like culture and belief systems can
affect the situation of the people that are suffering from mental illness.
Culture
In this report, we will refer to culture in the terms of individualism and collectivism.
Attitudes
The term attitudes refer to a person’s underlying psychosocial processes only being revealed
when aggravated by specific factors (Antonak & Livneh, 2000).
Stigma
The term stigma refers to a social devaluation of a person due to an “attribute that is deeply
discrediting” and can be conceptualised as consisting of “problems of ignorance, prejudice and
2
Selected countries
In the following study, we intend to investigate two contradictions based on the factors that
recent studies have shown affect a community’s attitudes towards mental illness; socio-
economic, religion and culture. These factors are further explained in the next section. The
two selected countries are India and Sweden. The countries are chosen based on factors that
may affect the situation of the ones suffering of mental illness and the factors are opposites in
the two countries, meaning that Sweden is on one side of the scale while India is on the other.
By doing this comparing study about attitudes towards mental illness in each country, we
intend to investigate if the previous research about what factors that may affect stigma and
people's attitudes in this area are correct or not. We intend to investigate the two countries’
attitudes towards mental illness in terms of their openness meaning how accepting the
population are towards mental illness as a whole, towards people being mentally ill and how
willing people are to be exposed to mental illness and talk about it.
Swedes, being generally non-religious (Global Index of Religiosity and Atheism, 2012), of a
horizontal individualistic culture (Shavitt, Torelli & Riemer, 2011) and a high income country
(The World Bank, 2017), should according to previous research be more open to mental
illness in the sense of being more accepting towards it, susceptible to talk and learn about it
(Ghanean, Jacobsson & Törnkvist, 2013; Stuart, 2016).
India, being religious (Global Index of Religiosity and Atheism, 2012), of a vertical
collectivistic culture (Triandis & Bhawuk, 1997), and a lower-middle income country (The
World Bank, 2017), should according to previous research be less open, god willing and
accepting to mental illness and through that have a more negative attitude towards it (McDaid,
Knapp & Raja, 2008).
Socio-economic factors
Socio-economic factors, e.g. poverty and access to healthcare, have been found to be
associated with outcomes of mental illness (Lund et al., 2011) and are determining the context
in which stigma is enacted and experienced. In low- and middle-income countries, including
India, where the main part of the population do not have access to benefits of the social
3
welfare, the negative economic consequences of stigma can express itself by discrimination in
work. This may threaten the economic survival of an entire family (Koschorke et al., 2014).
In high-income countries, access to mental health care is more developed and thereby people
are more exposed to it (WHO, 2014). By it being more available to the population, the stigma
lowers and will therefore not affect the individual and its family as negatively as it might in
lower-income countries. A functioning, well-known, existing social welfare system, e.g. a
suicide prevention one, might lower the stigma in the country since people are more exposed
and aware to the psychological welfare in their society (WHO, 2014).
Religious factors
Being the birthplace of Hinduism and Buddhism, religion has always been an important part of
India’s everyday life and culture (Jyoti & Kour, 2017). The three biggest religions in India are
Christianity, Islam and Hinduism (Heitzman & Worden, 1996) while the two biggest ones in
Sweden are Christianity and Islam (www.myndighetensst.se). People will have different
attitudes and views of illness, their symptoms and its meaning depending on their religious
beliefs.
According to the Islamic belief and the Qur’an, God is omega and alpha meaning that
everything begins and ends with the all-knowing Allah. Therefore, if someone were to be
mentally ill, it is part of God’s plan and is either a punishment deserved or a trial well needed
in order to absolve the individual in question (Koenig, 2016). By believing that it is part of a
plan, one might try to endure the symptoms instead of seeking professional help or even
acknowledge it as a mental illness.
In Hinduism, the common Hindu belief is that everything is karma, meaning that all you do
has consequence and at the same time many Hindus stigmatize mental illnesses
(www.health.qld.gov.au). By believing that mental illness is caused by karma, it is easy to
assume that Hindus generally will have the attitude that individuals suffering from mental
illness are deserving of doing so because of a misdeed in a previous life.
In regards for Christianity, studies have shown that a large number of Christians suffering from
mental illness, believe their symptoms are caused by demons (Koenig, 2016). By believing this,
people will tend to see practitioners of exorcisms to a greater degree than a professional
psychologist leading to not acknowledging the symptoms as mental ones (Mercer, 2013). By
not thinking of one’s symptoms as a psychological illness but of a demon taking over one’s
body, e.g., one might not understands one’s need in seeking psychological help and thereby
not forming a sound understanding nor attitude towards mental illness.
According to reports from Win-Gallup International Association (2012), 81% of the Indian
population call themselves religious while only 19% of the Swedish population gave the same
answer.
Quack (2013) describes what affects people’s help-seeking behavior in the north of India and
how this in turn affects their attitude. He found that even though the patients’ symptoms were
4
identified by their relatives and family as psychological ones, they were taken to see a religious
healer (Quack, 2013).
Religion, tradition and different ideological beliefs might cause a certain behavior not being as
inviting to different approaches such as professional psychological or psychiatric help. When
being asked, people in India answered that both medicine and prayer are important but when
being asked to explain and assess their problems, they were not able to but relied fully on the
healers and doctors they had visited earlier. If a society has a tradition of only going to healers,
it is natural that that’s where they will keep going and they will keep on believing that it is
ghosts or demons causing their illness, and not understand that it is a psychological issue
(Quack, 2013). This shows that tradition, and religion along with it, has strong impact on the
societies and its people. By not accepting mental illnesses for what it is but seeing the
symptoms as demons, ghosts or a sign of God, it is hard to expect people to have a positive
attitude towards it.
Culture
Individualism and collectivism are attitudes and beliefs linked the society’s culture reflecting
social norms and values (Hampton, 2017). Collectivism is a perception that sets the common
interests over the individual interests (www.merriam-webster.com). Individualism on the other
hand is a perception that sees the individual's freedom and independence as the highest value
(www.merriam-webster.com). There are four types of individualism-collectivism; horizontal
collectivism, horizontal individualism, vertical collectivism and vertical individualism
(Hampton, 2017) where the horizontal orientation focuses on equality among people and the
vertical orientation focuses on a hierarchy within society and among all individuals (Singelis,
Triandis, Bhawuk, & Gelfand, 1995).
Sweden, being a horizontal individualistic society (Shavitt, Torelli, & Riemer, 2011) indicates
that its population should have a less stigmatizing and more positive attitude towards
individuals with mental illness. India, being a vertical collectivistic society (Triandis & Bhawuk,
1997) indicates that its population should have a more stigmatizing and negative attitude
towards individuals with mental illness.
According to Lee et al. (2005) a collectivist culture does not protect psychiatric patients against
familial stigma. An entire family will take up the shame and burden of having a mentally ill
member throughout the course of the illness (Lam, Tsang, Chan, & Corrigan, 2006).
However, the interfamilial and social connections of moral favor and relationship can break
down in the face of the powerful forces of stigma. Ultimately, families might abandon a
5
member with a mental disorder. Such abandonment is especially distressing because unmarried
adults are generally expected to live with their parents and siblings (Lee et al., 2005).
Earlier research has found that it is common around the world to have negative attitudes
towards mental illness and that it is even more so when a person has not been in contact with
it through a relative, friend or by being sick oneself (Siqueira, Abelha, Lovisi, Sarução, &
Yang, 2017). Studies show that the stigma surrounding mental illness affects people in a way
causing them to not seek help, this increasing the stigma even more (Siqueira, Abelha, Lovisi,
Sarução, & Yang, 2017).
Benevolence investigated claims like "more tax money should be used for care of mentally ill"
and "mentally ill have long been victims of mockery". A high mean value in this category
would indicate a good will towards people with mental illnesses.
Social Restrictiveness investigates if the mentally ill are perceived as dangerous, e.g. through
the allegation: "Mentally ill are a danger to themselves and to others". A high mean value in
this category indicates a social openness towards people with mental illness.
The last category of Community Mental Health Ideology investigates claims such as: "It is
scary to think that mentally ill people live in common neighbourhoods". A high mean value in
this category indicates community inclusiveness (Taylor & Dear, 1981) and thereby a more
positive attitude towards people with mental illness.
We intend to investigate this by taking a sample and have therefore selected two countries that
fall into each income-category; Sweden and India. In order to correspond to the time- and
amplitude frame of our bachelor thesis, we have decided to limit the comparison to the
attitudes of students in the two chosen countries.
Research questions:
- Does the attitude towards mental illness differ between students in India and Sweden?
- How does this result live up to what previous suggestions/theories in research indicate?
Hypothesis
Our hypothesis is that there will be a difference in the attitudes towards mental illness, both
among the students but more distinctly between the two countries of Sweden and India. We
believe that the students in Sweden will have a more positive attitude while the students in
India will have a more negative one. This hypothesis is based on the previous research that
indicates that factors such as socio-economic belonging, religion and culture have been found
to have impact on the life situation with mental illness and in determining the context in
which stigma is enacted and experienced.
Method
Design
The study had a between-group design with two groups; participants living in Sweden (Group
1) and participants living in India (Group 2). The dependent variable is in this case the attitude
of the participants towards mental health.
Participants
We got in contact with our participants in India at several different universities throughout the
country through professors in different departments giving us access to their students and by
the schools official Facebook pages. In Sweden, the contact has been made through the contact
registry available at Luleå University of Technology.
Since we were not primarily interested in examining differences between gender and age,
selection of participants was not stratified with respect to these variables. The only selections
made were that the participants were born in either Sweden or India, currently being
university students and that they had not studied any form of psychology at university level.
Group 1, with Swedish students, consisted of 272 participants and group 2 with Indian student
consisted of 138 participants, which makes a total of 410 participants in the questionnaire
study. This number of participants allowed for valid conclusions and did fit the project’s
timeframe.
Questionnaire
In order to collect information about the attitudes the participants have towards mental health,
we used “The Community Attitudes towards Mental Illness scale (CAMI)”. CAMI is designed
7
by Taylor and Dear (1981). CAMI contains 40 claims about mental illness that are rated on a
five-degree Likert-scale (CAMI Scale, 2017b ). The questionnaire is in English and the same
questionnaire was used in Sweden and India. A test version of the questionnaire in digital form
was sent to five people in a pilot study to see if there were any errors or problems with the
questionnaire. The questionnaire was sent to the participants digitally by email and via
Facebook communities. The emails and Facebook posts contained a link to the survey and
information about the study. Appendix 2 and Appendix 3 contain the information that was
sent along with the link to the survey. Appendix 4 and Appendix 5 contain the questions sent
to the participants via the link to the online survey. The emails to the Swedish participants
were sent by us personally in February 2018. We were given access to the Swedish students'
email addresses via Luleå University of Technology. In India, the email was sent through the
students' teachers, which we had contact with both in person and by email. The emails to
Indian students and the posts in the universities’ Facebook communities were sent out in
March and April 2018. Due to the fast responses and high number of participants in Sweden,
we did not need to send a reminding email to the Swedish participants. In India we had to
remind the teachers to send out the questionnaire and to encourage their students to
participate.
Data analysis
When compiling the data from the questionnaire and comparing the two sample groups,
several independent t-tests have been conducted to investigate if there were any differences
between the two groups. We investigated the test's properties with Cronbach’s Alpha for the
attitude categories in the test. The statistical software SPSS was used for analysis.
The answers were converted based on the instructions in the original study on CAMI (Taylor
& Dear, 1981). Five of the 10 statements in each category expressed a positive sentiment with
reference to the underlying concept, and the other five were negatively worded. For example,
for the authoritarianism category, five statements expressed a pro-authoritarian sentiment, and
five were anti-authoritarian. The response format for each statement was the standard Likert
five-point labelled scale: Strongly Agree/Agree/Neutral/Disagree/Strongly Disagree. All
responses were converted from words to numbers. Pro-questions were converted as follows:
Strongly Agree = 5 and Strongly Disagree = 1. Negative Anti-questions were converted the
opposite way: Strongly Agree = 1 and Strongly Disagree = 5. Thus, high value indicates a
more positive attitude towards mental illness for all questions.
Ethical considerations
As the study was based on people's attitudes and opinions about mental illness, which in some
contexts can be perceived as a private and sensitive subject, it was of high importance that we
related to the research ethical principles developed by the Swedish Research Council and the
Indian Research Council as well as the National Association of Psychological Science in India,
this in order to respect the participants.
We used anonymous questionnaires. The participants received information about the purpose
of the study and were able to give their consent as they completed the questionnaire. In terms
of use, content and empirical data were used exclusively for this study.
8
Results
To investigate whether there are any differences in attitudes between Sweden and India, a
comparison was made with the two countries’ responses to the questions from the
questionnaire CAMI. Several independent samples t-tests were performed, and the result show
a statistically significant difference in three out of a total of four attitude categories (Table 1).
Table 1
Swedish and Indian students’ mean scores (M) and mean differences (diff) in the four attitude
categories with t-tests.
Figure 1. Mean scores for Swedish and Indian students in each attitude category.
9
Authoritarianism
The Swedish participants’ (M=21.9632) responses to the questions in this category differed
from the Indian participants’ (M=24.2174) responses, t(408) = -3.985, p < .001. This means
the Indian participants have a more authoritative approach towards people with mental illness.
Benevolence
The Swedish participants’ (M=38.2316) responses to the questions in this category differed
from the Indian participants’ (M=40.8288) responses, t(408) = -4.069, p < .001. This means
the Indian participants have a higher sense of goodwill towards people with mental illnesses.
Social restrictiveness
As can be seen in Table 1, the countries did not differ in the category of Social restrictiveness.
Table 2
Swedish and Indian students’ mean scores (M) and mean differences (diff), in questions
differing the least and most, with t-tests.
6. The mentally ill are a burden on Sweden 272 3.50 -.967 -8.408 .000*
society. (B) India 138 4.47
10. The mentally ill have for too Sweden 272 3.72 -.102 -1.008 .934
long been the subject of ridicule. India 138 3.82
(B)
11. A woman would be foolish to Sweden 272 3.72 -.102 -1.008 .934
marry a man who has suffered from India 138 3.82
mental illness, even though he
seems fully recovered. (SR)
20. Residents should accept the Sweden 272 3.56 -.546 -6.196 .000*
location of mental health facilities India 138 4.11
in their neighbourhood to serve the
needs of the local community. (CI)
29. Mental hospitals are an outdated Sweden 272 2.95 .304 3.296 .000*
means of treating the mentally ill. India 138 2.64
(A)
10
30. The mentally ill do not deserve Sweden 272 4.28 .189 1.895 .936
our sympathy. (B) India 138 4.09
(A) = Authoritarianism
(B) = Benevolence
(SR) =Social Restrictiveness
(CI) = Community Mental Health Ideology
In question 6: “The mentally ill are a burden on society”, the Swedish participants had a lower
mean (M=3.50) than the Indian participants (M=4.47). This result indicates that Indian
participants show a higher sense of goodwill.
In question 20: “Residents should accept the location of mental health facilities in their
neighbourhood to serve the needs of the local community” the Indian participants had a
higher mean (M=4.11) than the Swedish participants (M=3.56). This indicates that the Indian
participants are more including, in their community, towards people with mental illness.
In question 29: “Mental hospitals are an outdated means of treating the mentally ill”, an Anti-
Authoritarianism question, the Indian participants had a lower mean (M=2.64) than Swedish
participants (M=2.95) indicating that the Swedish participants have a more authoritative
approach.
In question 10: “The mentally ill have for too long been the subject of ridicule”, Swedish
students (M=3.72) and Indian students (M=3.82) have similar means. Both countries show a
neutral attitude with a tendency of agreeing with the statement in question 10.
In question 11: “A woman would be foolish to marry a man who has suffered from mental
illness, even though he seems fully recovered”, Swedish participants (M=3.72) and Indian
participants (M=3.82) have similar means. Both countries show a neutral position with a
tendency of agreeing with the statement in question 11.
In question 30: “The mentally ill do not deserve our sympathy”, Swedish participants
(M=4.28) and Indian participants (M=4.09) have similar means. Both countries have a
tendency to disagree with the statement in question 30.
Table 3
Correlations between attitude categories.
Social
Authoritarianism Benevolence Restrictiveness
Authoritarianism ⎯
Benevolence -.713 ⎯
Community Mental
-.645 .722 -.723
Health Ideology
(N=410)
The correlations in Table 3 suggest that the attitude factors are not independent of each other.
The correlations in our study are similar to the correlations in the Taylor and Dear (1981)
study. The highest inter-dimension-correlation (r =.723) being between the two categories of
Social Restrictiveness and Community Mental Health Ideology (Table 3).
Table 4
Scale reliabilities (Cronbach’s Alpha) in this study and in the Taylor and Dear (1981) study.
To investigate the CAMI test’s properties, internal consistency was assessed with Cronbach’s
Alpha for each of the four attitude categories. The alpha coefficients in our study were
compared with the alpha coefficients in Taylor and Dear (1981) study. As can be seen in Table
4, the reliabilities in our study are similar to those reported by Taylor and Dear (1981). In our
study, reliability is higher in the attitude categories of Authoritarianism as well as in
Benevolence but slightly lower in the attitude categories of Social Restrictiveness and
Community Mental Health Ideology (Table 4). Overall, these alpha analyses indicate that our
test had about the same reliability as the original test in 1981.
12
Discussion
Results discussion
The purpose of this study was to investigate whether there are any differences in attitudes
towards mental health in the countries of Sweden and India and how this result lives up to
what previous suggestions/theories indicates. The results show differences in attitudes towards
mental illness between the countries. However, these differences do not correspond with our
hypothesis or with what previous research suggests. Based on previous research, the hypothesis
was that the Swedish population would have a more positive attitude towards mental illness in
comparison with the Indian population due to the general differences in religion,
socioeconomic status and culture. The results show the opposite. The Indian participants had a
more positive and accepting attitude in the attitude categories of Authoritarianism,
Benevolence, and Community Mental Health Ideology. This means that India has a greater
sense of authoritative approach, goodwill and social inclusion towards people with mental
illness.
Lund et al. (2011) claim that low- and lower middle-income countries should have a higher
level of stigma due to a lower exposure to mental health care which, according to our results,
proved to be wrong. The Swedish participants all live in a small city while the Indian
participants live in one of the world's biggest cities. A big city is more developed than a small
one leading to more exposure to several things, possibly including mental illness and mental
health care. So, perhaps residents in the big cities of India have a higher exposure to mental
health care and thereby a greater awareness of mental illness than residents in the small cities of
Sweden have? Lund et al. (2011) found that low exposure to mental health care leads to high
levels of stigma, which therefore might explain our results. According to Shavitt et al. (2011), a
country with a horizontal individualistic culture like Sweden should have a less stigmatizing
and more positive attitude towards individuals with mental illness than a country with a
vertical collectivistic culture like India. This theory is, according to our results, not supported
based on the assumption that the cultural belongings of the sample groups represent a
horizontal individualistic and a vertical collectivistic culture.
Previous research and theories suggest that the outside world and context affect a person's view
on mental health and we believe this can be related to our result. We believe that one
explanation to our result is related to the selection of Indian participants; students. They might
not represent the typical resident in a low- or lower-middle-income country with collectivistic
culture and strong religious beliefs. Young academics from bigger cities of India might not
have a strong religious belief or act according to a collectivistic culture and definitely do not
come from a low income background. The Indian students might be more individualistic than
we believed, come from a high income background and lack strong religious belief. With the
Internet making the world a smaller place, cultures are being mixed and information is more
accessible than ever before, and culture is not as divided as it once was. Therefore, the Indian
students may have been affected by the western, more individualistic culture and the Swedish
students may have been affected by the eastern, more collectivistic culture.
The results could also be an effect of deteriorated validity of the CAMI test. The test might
have been valid a decade or two ago, but with the world changing, developing and in a sense
13
becoming smaller, it may have lost some of its capability to discriminate between different
cultures attitudes towards mental illness in a proper way. The fact that the greatest difference
between the two countries was in the attitude category of Benevolence might make it
necessary to reappraise the belief that a collectivistic society would induce negative attitudes
towards mental illness. It is, according to the results, more probable that the people of a
collectivistic culture are more caring towards the people around them leading to a higher
willingness of showing goodwill towards people with mental illness, a higher eagerness to be
kind and help out.
The results from the questions that differed the most and least should be interpreted with care.
Many questions and comparisons may produce a few big differences by chance. For example
question 29 indicates a more authoritative approach among the Swedish participants, but this is
contrary to the global result for the category Authoritarianism where the Indian participants
show a more authoritative approach.
Method discussion
This study only contains data on university students’ attitudes. Young adults with high
education, in India where education at universities cost money, must have good financial
background. This may have affected the outcome. Older people or people from lower social
classes, in both countries, might have generated other results. We were aware of the risks with
delimiting the study to university students. The rationale for this was the availability of
comparable groups.
Since the samples were not representative for the populations in Sweden and India it is
inadequate to generalize the results to the populations in the countries. Any generalization
must be delimited to university students. For valid generalization to the populations large and
representative samples must be used. This study used rather large, but far from population
representative samples.
The benefits of using an electronic questionnaire are low cost and freedom for the participants
to fill it out whenever they want to. A downside is loss of control of who actually fills it out.
In a study of a subject of sensitive nature, like this study, the result can become systematically
partial to the participants' perceptions of what are correct and socially accepted response
options. This phenomenon is called social desirability and occurs in the vast majority of self-
reports and significantly increases the risk of misleading results (King & Bruner, 2000). This
may have had an effect on our results. The fact that the questionnaire was in English, and we
assume that English was not the native language for the majority of the participants, may have
influenced the results. The statements we consider containing more difficult, formal or
uncommon English words had more neutral answers. Possibly, if the participant did not
understand the assertion completely influenced how the person responded to the question.
Translating the original CAMI into Swedish and Hindi might have been preferable, but our
lack of translation resources forced us to use an English questionnaire for all participants, both
Swedish and Indian.
14
Because of our preconception that studying psychology was rare among students in India, only
students, Swedish as well as Indian, who never had taken any psychology courses were allowed
to participate. This was a precaution to make the samples as similar as possible. However, this
proved to be a mistake since a third (33.2%) of the contacted students in India indeed had
studied psychology at university level. Since a big number of students in India, as well as in
Sweden, seem to have studied psychology, they should have been included to make the
samples larger and more representative.
In the results we found a few outliers who had responded in extreme ways on all or most of
the questions, but they did not affect the results as a whole and were therefore not excluded in
the analyses.
Does the study measure what it intends to measure, in this case people's attitudes? Since we
chose to use a well-developed and well-known attitude questionnaire where focus is on
attitudes towards mental illness, the concept validity of the study becomes high. By choosing a
questionnaire that experienced researchers have designed and the fact that the questionnaire has
been tested in several previous studies, the concept validity of the study was confirmed. That
level of validity would have been impossible to achieve with a questionnaire developed by us.
All attitude categories had an internal consistency well above α = .70. This, in combination
with the fact that the values were similar, and in two attitude categories somewhat higher, in
our study compared to the Taylor and Dear (1981) study, indicates high reliability (Table 4).
Conclusions
The results show differences in attitudes towards mental illness between the countries of
Sweden and India in the attitude categories Authoritarianism, Benevolence, and Community
Mental Health Ideology. In these categories, the Indian students have greater sense of
goodwill, social openness and social inclusion towards people with mental illness. These
conclusions are contrary to our hypotheses.
Since our hypothesis was made on the case that the three factors of religion, socio-economics
and culture should affect the attitude towards mental illness, it would also be interesting to
conduct a bigger study where one aims to find out which, if any, of the factors of religion,
socio-economics and culture that actually do affect the attitude towards mental illness and in
what way.
16
References
CAMI Scale. (2017a). Scaling Community Attitudes Toward the Mentally Ill. Retrieved 29
April, 2018, from https://camiscale.com/
CAMI Scale. (2017b). CAMI Questionnaire & Key. Retrieved 24 January, 2018, from
http://purenyx.com/camiscale/wp-content/uploads/sites/47/2017/03/CAMI-
Questionnaire.pdf
Eaton. J., Kakuma, R., Wright, A., & Minas, H. (2014). A position statement on mental health
in the post-2015 development agenda. International Journal of Mental Health Systems, 8(28).
doi: 10.1186/1752-4458-8-28
Hampton, N. Z. (2017). Cultural Orientations and Attitude towards Mental Illness . Journal Of
Rehabilitation, 83(4), 25–33.
Heitzman, J., & Worden, R. L. (2016). India: A Country Study. Library of Congress, Federal
Research Division. Retrieved 7 February 2018 from
https://www.loc.gov/resource/frdcstdy.indiacountrystud00heit/?st=gallery
Ghanean, H., Jacobsson, & L., Törnkvist, B. (2013). Internalized stigma of mental illness in
Sweden and Iran: a comparative study. Open Journal of Psychiatry, 3(4), 370-374.
doi:10.4236/ojpsych.2013.34039
Indian Council of Medical Research. (2017). National Ethical Guidelines for Biomedical and
Health Research Involving Human Participants. Retrieved 13 February, 2018 from
http://www.icmr.nic.in/guidelines/National_Ethical_Guidelines_for_BioMedical_Research_I
nvolving_Children.pdf
Jyoti, J., & Kour, S. (2017). Factors affecting cultural intelligence and its impact on job
performance. Personnel Review, 46(4), 767-791. doi:10.1108/PR-12-2015-0313
King, M., & Bruner, G. (2000). Social Desirability Bias: A Neglected Aspect of Validity
Testing. Psychology and Marketing, 17(2), 73-163. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1520-
6793(200002)17:2<79::AID-MAR2>3.0.CO;2-0
17
Koschorke, M., Padmavati, R., Kumar, S., Cohen, A., Weiss, H. A., Chatterjee, S., . . . Patel,
V. (2014). Experiences of stigma and discrimination of people with schizophrenia in India.
Social Science & Medicine, 123, 149–159. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.10.035
Lam, C. S, Tsang, H., Chan, F., & Corrigan, P. (2006). Chinese and American perspectives on
stigma. Rehabilitation Education, 20(4), 269–279. doi: 10.1891/088970106805065368
Lee, S., Lee, M. T., Chiu, M. Y., & Kleinman, A. (2005). Experience of social stigma by
people with schizophrenia in Hong Kong. British Journal of Psychiatry, 186, 153–157.
doi:10.1192/bjp.186.2.153
Lund, C., De Silva, M., Plagerson, S., Cooper, S., Chisholm, D., Das, J., . . . Patel, V. (2011).
Poverty and mental disorders: breaking the cycle in low-income and middle-income countries.
The Lancet, 378(9801), 1502–1514. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60754-X
Mathers, C. D., & Loncar, D. (2006). Projections of global mortality and burden of disease
from 2002 to 2030. PLoS Medicine, 3(11), e442. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.0030442
McDaid, D., Knapp, M., & Raja, S. (2008). Barriers in the mind: promoting an economic case
for mental health in low- and middle-income countries. World Psychiatry, 7(2), 79–86.
McQueen, R. A., & Knussen, C. (2013). Introduction to research methods and statistics in
psychology: a practical guide for the undergraduate researcher. (2. ed.) Harlow, England:
Pearson.
Mercer, J. (2013). Deliverance, demonic possession, and mental illness: some considerations for
mental health professionals. Mental Health, Religion & Culture, 16(6), 595–611.
doi:10.1080/13674676.2012.706272
Myndigheten för Stöd till Trossamfund. (2004). Religiös förändring i norra Europa: En
studie av Sverige. Retrieved 7 February, 2018 from
http://www.myndighetensst.se/download/18.3db44c2e1556f5611c3a03a4/1466681703733/F
ULLTEXT02.pd
Quack, J. (2013). “What do I know?” Scholastic fallacies and pragmatic religiosity in mental
health-seeking behaviour in India. Mental Health, Religion & Culture, 16(4), 403–418.
doi:10.1080/13674676.2012.679358
Queensland Health. (2011). Health Care Providers’ Handbook on Hindu Patients. Division of
the Chief Health Officer, Queensland Health. Brisbane 2011. Retrieved 31 January, 2018,
from https://www.health.qld.gov.au/__data/assets/pdf_file/0024/156255/hbook-hindu.pdf.
Rathod, S., Pinninti, N., Irfan, M., Gorczynski, P., Rathod, P., Gega, L., & Naeem, F.
(2017). Mental health service provision in low- and middle-income countries. Health
Services Insights,10,1-7. doi:10.1177/1178632917694350
18
Semrau, M., Evans-Lacko, S., Koschorke, M., Ashenafi, L., & Thornicroft, G. (2015). Stigma
and discrimination related to mental illness in low- and middle-income countries.
Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 24(5), 382–394. doi:10.1017/S2045796015000359
Siqueira, S. R. G., Abelha, L., Lovisi, G. M., Sarução, K. R., & Yang, L. (2017). Attitudes
towards the mentally ill: A study with health workers at a university hospital in Rio de Janeiro.
Psychiatric Quarterly, 88(1), 25–38. doi:10.1007/s11126-016-9431-5
Shavitt, S., Torelli, C. J., & Riemer, H. (2011). Horizontal and vertical individualism and
collectivism: Implications for understanding psychological processes. In M. J. Gelfand, C.
Chiu, Y. Hong, M. J. Gelfand, C. Chiu, Y. Hong (Eds.), Advances in culture and psychology,
1, 309-350. New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press.
Singelis, T. M., Triandis, H. C., Bhawuk, D. P. S., & Gelfand, M. J. (1995). Horizontal and
vertical dimensions of individualism and collectivism: A theoretical and measurement
refinement. Cross-Cultural Research, 29(3), 240–275. doi: 10.1177/106939719502900302
Stuart, H. (2016). Reducing the stigma of mental illness. Global Mental Health, 3, e17. doi:
10.1017/gmh.2016.11
Taylor, S. M., & Dear, M. J. (1981). Scaling Community Attitudes Toward the Mentally Ill.
Schizophrenia Bulletin, 7(2), 225-240.
The World Bank. (2016). Gross domestic product 2016. Retrieved 6 February, 2017 from
http://databank.worldbank.org/data/download/GDP.pdf
The World Bank. (2017). World Bank Country and Lending Groups. Retrieved 6 February,
2017 from https://datahelpdesk.worldbank.org/knowledgebase/articles/906519-world-bank-
country-and-lending-groups
Triandis, H. C., & Bhawuk, D. P. S. (1997). Culture theory and the meaning of relatedness. In
P. Christopher Earley & M. Erez (Eds.), New perspectives on international industrial/
organizational psychology, (pp. 13–52). San Francisco: The New Lexington Press.
WHO. (2014). Mental Health Atlas: Country Profiles. Retrieved 29 January, 2018, from
http://www.who.int/mental_health/evidence/atlas/profiles-2014/ind.pdf?ua=1
WHO. (2001). The World Health Report 2001. Mental Health: New Understanding, New
Hope. Retrieved 24 March, 2018 from
http://www.who.int/whr/2001/en/whr01_en.pdf?ua=1
Appendices
Thank you for taking your time to answer this questionnaire aiming to find what the attitudes
are among students towards mental illness.
The following statements express various opinions about mental illness and the mentally ill.
The mentally ill refers to people needing treatment for mental disorders but who are capable of
independent living outside a hospital. Please pick the response which most accurately describes
your reaction to each statement. It's your first reaction which is important. Don't be concerned
if some statements seem similar to ones you have previously answered. Please be sure to answer
all statements.
The responses will be anonymous and it should take you no more than 10 minutes.
Hej!
Vi är två svenska psykologstudenter som gör en studie om attityder gentemot psykisk ohälsa i
Sverige och Indien och skulle uppskatta din medverkan. Enkäten är på engelska och kommer
ta er ungefär 5-10 minuter att fylla i.
https://goo.gl/forms/PzeLNynfuAAZnHl73
Tack på förhand!
Vänliga hälsningar,
Sandra & Julia
20
Hello!
We are two Swedish psychology students, who are are studying attitudes towards mental illness
in Sweden and India and would appreciate your participation. The survey is in English and
will take you about 5-10 minutes to fill in.
https://goo.gl/forms/Sc8NAWnj4CeAq25A3
Sincerely,
Julia Angbrant & Sandra Frykman
Appendix 4 - The opening question in the Questionnaire for Swedish students (Group 1)
Appendix 5 - The opening questions in the Questionnaire for Indian students (Group 2)
2. More tax money should be spent on the care and treatment of the mentally ill.
SA A N D SD
3. The mentally ill should be isolated from the rest of the community
SA A N D SD
4. The best therapy for many mental patients is to be part of a normal community.
SA A N D SD
7. The mentally ill are far less of a danger than most people suppose.
SA A N D SD
9. There is something about the mentally ill that makes it easy to tell them from normal
people.
SA A N D SD
10. The mentally ill have for too long been the subject of ridicule.
SA A N D SD
11. A woman would be foolish to marry a man who has suffered from mental illness, even
though he seems fully recovered.
SA A N D SD
12. As far as possible mental health services should be provided through community based
facilities.
SA A N D SD
22
13. Less emphasis should be placed on protecting the public from the mentally ill.
SA A N D SD
15. No one has the right to exclude the mentally ill from their neighbourhood.
SA A N D SD
16. Having mental patients living within residential neighbourhoods might be good therapy,
but the risks to residents are too great.
SA A N D SD
17. Mental patients need the same kind of control and discipline as a young child.
SA A N D SD
18. We need to adopt a far more tolerant attitude toward the mentally ill in our society.
SA A N D SD
19. I would not want to live next door to someone who has been mentally ill.
SA A N D SD
20. Residents should accept the location of mental health facilities in their neighbourhood to
serve the needs of the local community.
SA A N D SD
22. There are sufficient existing services for the mentally ill.
SA A N D SD
23. Mental patients should be encouraged to assume the responsibilities of normal life.
SA A N D SD
24. Local residents have good reason to resist the location of mental health services in their
neighbourhood.
SA A N D SD
23
25. The best way to handle the mentally ill is to keep them behind locked doors.
SA A N D SD
26. Our mental hospitals seem more like prisons than like places where the mentally ill can be
cared for.
SA A N D SD
27. Anyone with a history of mental problems should be excluded from taking public office.
SA A N D SD
28. Locating mental health services in residential neighbourhoods does not endanger local
residents.
SA A N D SD
29. Mental hospitals are an outdated means of treating the mentally ill.
SA A N D SD
31. The mentally ill should not be denied their individual rights.
SA A N D SD
33. One of the main causes of mental illness is a lack of self-discipline and willpower
SA A N D SD
34. We have the responsibility to provide the best possible care for the mentally ill.
SA A N D SD
36. Residents have nothing to fear from people coming into their neighbourhood to obtain
mental health services.
SA A N D SD
24
39. Most women who were once patients in a mental hospital can be trusted as
babysitters.
SA A N D SD
Appendix 7 - Table 5
Swedish and Indian students’ mean scores (M) and mean differences (diff) in all questions with
t-tests.
1. As soon as a person shows signs of Sweden 272 2.01 -.297 -2.900 .001*
mental disturbance, he should be India 138 2.31
hospitalized. (A)
2. More tax money should be spent Sweden 272 3.94 .231 2.280 .307
on the care and treatment of the India 138 3.71
mentally ill. (B)
3. The mentally ill should be isolated Sweden 272 1.64 -.031 -.353 .469
from the rest of the community. (SR) India 138 1.67
4. The best therapy for many mental Sweden 272 3.65 -.280 -2.965 .037*
patients is to be part of a normal India 138 3.93
community. (CI)
5. Mental illness is an illness like any Sweden 272 2.32 -.079 -.588 -.278
other. (A) India 138 2.40
6. The mentally ill are a burden on Sweden 272 3.50 -.967 -8.408 .000*
society. (B) India 138 4.47
25
7. The mentally ill are far less of a Sweden 272 2.54 -.014 -.134 .074
danger than most people suppose. India 138 2.56
(SR)
8. Locating mental health facilities in Sweden 272 3.37 -.687 -6.221 .001*
a residential area downgrades the India 138 4.06
neighbourhood. (CI)
9. There is something about the Sweden 272 2.09 -.770 -7.273 .246
mentally ill that makes it easy to tell India 138 2.86
them from normal people. (A)
10. The mentally ill have for too long Sweden 272 3.72 -.102 -1.008 .934
been the subject of ridicule. (B) India 138 3.82
11. A woman would be foolish to Sweden 272 3.72 -.102 -1.008 .934
marry a man who has suffered from India 138 3.82
mental illness, even though he seems
fully recovered. (SR)
12. As far as possible mental health Sweden 272 3.70 -.229 -2.434 .013*
services should be provided through India 138 3.93
community-based facilities. (CI)
13. Less emphasis should be Sweden 272 2.80 -.101 -.961 .141
placed on protecting the public India 138 2.90
from the mentally ill. (A)
14. Increased spending on mental Sweden 272 4.17 -.175 -1.891 .042*
health services is a waste of tax India 138 4.35
dollars. (B)
15. No one has the right to exclude Sweden 272 2.10 .411 4.257 .019*
the mentally ill from their India 138 1.69
neighbourhood. (SR)
16. Having mental patients living Sweden 272 3.55 .349 3.573 .888
within residential neighbourhoods India 138 3.20
might be good therapy, but the risks
to residents are too great. (CI)
17. Mental patients need the same Sweden 272 2.64 -.607 -5.639 .033*
kind of control and discipline as a India 138 3.25
young child. (A)
18. We need to adopt a far more Sweden 272 3.94 -.385 -4.176 .051
tolerant attitude toward the mentally India 138 4.33
ill in our society. (B)
19. I would not want to live next Sweden 272 2.23 .054 .521 .042*
door to someone who has been India 138 2.17
mentally ill. (SR)
20. Residents should accept the Sweden 272 3.56 -.546 -6.196 .000*
location of mental health facilities in India 138 4.11
26
31. The mentally ill should not be Sweden 272 1.93 .071 .611 .111
denied their individual rights. (SR) India 138 1.86
(CI) 32. Mental health facilities Sweden 272 3.46 -.276 -2.866 .270
should be kept out of residential India 138 3.74
neighbourhoods.
33. One of the main causes of mental Sweden 272 1.90 -.943 -7.940 .075
illness is a lack of self-discipline and India 138 2.85
willpower. (A)
34. We have the responsibility to Sweden 272 4.03 -.047 -.477 .012*
provide the best possible care for the India 138 4.07
mentally ill. (B)
35. The mentally ill should not be Sweden 272 2.06 -.271 -3.138 .020*
given any responsibility. (SR) India 138 2.33
36. Residents have nothing to fear Sweden 272 3.43 -.298 -3.315 .001*
27