Eye-Tracking Research in Autism Spectrum Disorder

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Current Developmental Disorders Reports (2019) 6:37–44

https://doi.org/10.1007/s40474-019-00158-w

AUTISM SPECTRUM (A RICHDALE AND L LAWSON, SECTION EDITORS)

Eye-Tracking Research in Autism Spectrum Disorder: What Are We


Measuring and for What Purposes?
Taralee Hamner 1,2 & Giacomo Vivanti 1,2

Published online: 18 April 2019


# Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019

Abstract
Purpose of Review The introduction of eye-tracking technology has enabled researchers in the field of autism spectrum disorder
(ASD) to “look through the eyes” of individuals with ASD. In the article, we review research in the area with a focus on (1) the
constructs that have been measured through eye-tracking paradigms, and (2) the application of this technology across different
purposes, including addressing basic science questions, facilitating diagnosis, and measuring intervention mechanisms and outcomes.
Recent Findings Most eye-tracking research to date has focused on quantifying differences in social attention between samples
with ASD and typical development. Other social constructs such as goal prediction, theory of mind, and joint attention have also
been investigated, but to a lesser degree. Similarly, cognitive constructs in non-social domains, such as working memory, visual
search, attentional disengagement, and habituation have been investigated through eye-tracking paradigms, but less frequently
than social attention. A modest but growing body of literature is focusing on the usefulness of eye-tracking to identify ASD
diagnostic markers and measure intervention outcomes, with promising yet inconclusive results.
Summary Eye-tracking technology is a feasible measurement tool to capture a range of psychological constructs and can be used
for a range of purposes relevant to both research and practice. However, the potential of eye-tracking for capturing processes
other than social attention and its relevance to diagnosis and intervention is still to be fully explored.

Keywords Autism . Eye-tracking . Visual attention . Social attention . Autism diagnosis . Autism intervention

Introduction “looking through the eyes” of those with ASD. Eye-tracking


technology uses the contrast between the pupil and the iris to
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental locate the center of the pupil and infrared light to create a
disorder characterized by impairments in social communica- corneal reflection. The vector between these two features is
tion and the presence of restricted, repetitive, or stereotyped then used to compute gaze intersection with a surface, thus
behaviors [1], occurring in 1 in 59 individuals in the USA [2]. measuring the focus of attention, or where a person is looking.
For decades, scholars in the field have argued that in order to The measurement of what individuals attend to can reveal
answer questions about psychological processes and experi- important information on how stimuli are prioritized, experi-
ence of individuals with ASD, one should aim to understand enced, and processed.
the world from their point of view [3]. Since the turn of the Compared with behavioral paradigms, the use of eye-
century, the introduction of eye-tracking technology has tracking in ASD research has two main advantages. First, it
allowed researchers to gain insight on ASD as if they were provides a more objective and accurate measurement of atten-
tional patterns (e.g., measuring duration or latency of atten-
tional engagement with a stimulus with millisecond-level pre-
This article is part of the Topical Collection on Autism Spectrum cision). Second, given its non-invasive nature, and the possi-
bility to acquire valid data via passive viewing of visual dis-
* Giacomo Vivanti
plays, eye-tracking is optimally suited for individuals who
[email protected]
experience difficulties with following verbal instructions and
1
A.J. Drexel Autism Institute, Drexel University, 3020 Market Street, handling complex social and cognitive demands, such as those
Suite 560, Philadelphia, PA 19104-3734, USA with ASD [4, 5].
2
Department of Psychology, Drexel University, Following the pioneering works of Klin et al., Pelphrey
Philadelphia, PA 19104-3734, USA et al., and van der Geest et al., eye-tracking has been used

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.


38 Curr Dev Disord Rep (2019) 6:37–44

extensively in the field of ASD to gain insight on atypical sensitive to the presence of non-social physical contingen-
information processing characteristic to the syndrome [6•, cies in the same social stimuli (such as audiovisual syn-
7•, 8•]. More recently, its application has been extended to chronies involving movement and non-social sounds)
the search for diagnostic markers and the measurement of [18]. Subsequent research using similar stimuli, however,
intervention outcomes [4, 9••]. In the reminder of the article, documented that reduced rather than increased attention to
we will provide a narrative overview of these different areas of audiovisual synchrony in infants at 10 months was pre-
inquiry in ASD eye-tracking research, discussing lessons dictive of an ASD diagnosis at 3 years of age [19]. As
learned and directions for future research. suggested by the study authors, atypical developmental
trajectories in ASD may contribute to these discrepant
Eye-Tracking Research Investigating the Nature findings, as infants with ASD may process multisensory
of Social Differences in ASD information differently early in development. In addition,
it has been suggested the earlier study introduced the con-
Most eye-tracking research in the field of ASD has attempted found of highly repetitive motion (i.e., clapping to “pat-a-
to quantify and explain social atypicalities of those with ASD cake”), which may cause a preferential bias in ASD.
focusing on the construct of social attention, i.e., allocation of Eye-tracking research has also been used to examine
attention to social features of a given scene. The construct of various constructs related to social cognition in ASD,
social attention, which has also been defined as “social including the ability to predict the behavior of others
orienting” [10] and “social visual engagement” [11], in eye- and understanding the mental states of others. For exam-
tracking research is commonly operationalized as the duration ple, Falck-Ytter, Krogh-Jespersen et al., and Vivanti
of attention to social versus non-social content of a stimulus et al. have used eye-tracking in predictive gaze para-
(for example, relative amount of attention to someone’s eyes digms that measured whether participants predictively
versus objects in the background). Two recent meta-analyses gazed towards an area or object that the agent in the
examining social attention in ASD [12•, 13] showed a medi- video was likely to act upon (e.g., an object that the
um effect in the magnitude of social attention difference be- agent was looking at and moving her hand towards, as
tween participants with and without ASD, with the ASD opposed to one that she was ignoring) [14, 20, 21, 22••,
groups exhibiting reduced attention to social stimuli. This 23]. Such predictive gaze paradigms are used to index
phenomenon was reported to increase in magnitude as the goal prediction, i.e., the ability to anticipate the behavior
social complexity of the stimuli increased, with differences of others. This research yielded mixed findings, with
remaining stable across development. Additional research Falck-Ytter reporting normative performance in toddlers
has shown that attention to faces in preschoolers with ASD with ASD, whereas Vivanti et al. and Krogh-Jespersen
is less modulated by social cues that increase social attention et al. documented reduced ability to anticipate the target
in individuals without ASD, including the presence of a direct of others’ actions in toddlers and preschoolers, which
versus averted gaze [14] and emotional versus neutral facial was related to reduced attention to social cues such as
expressions [15]. head turning and gaze direction.
Another line of eye-tracking research has examined Eye-tracking paradigms based on predictive gaze have
ASD-specific differences in visual attention to different also been used to study the theory of mind in young chil-
components within social stimuli. Initial research has sug- dren with ASD. In an eye-tracking version of the classic
gested that adults with ASD showed increased attention to false-belief task, Senju et al. documented that toddlers
the human mouth and decreased attention to human eyes with ASD failed to anticipate an agent’s behavior when
[6•]. However, subsequent research did not replicate this such behavior could only have been predicted if they had
finding [16] and provided indications that atypical face attributed a false belief to her (see also [25]), a pattern
scanning features are related to age. For example, consistent with the notion of a theory of mind impairment
Chawarska et al. documented that children with ASD [24]. However, more recently Cole et al. failed to replicate
showed increased attention to “outer” facial features this finding using a similar eye-tracking paradigm in
(i.e., hair, cheeks, chin) compared with typical peers at adults [26].
both 2 and 4 years of age. However, attention to “inner” Eye-tracking paradigms have also been used to examine
features (i.e., eyes, nose, mouth) was similar to typical joint attention in ASD, with research documenting ASD-
peers at age two, but significantly reduced at age four, specific impairments in response to joint attention as well
suggesting atypical developmental patterns [17•]. as initiations of joint attention in toddlers and preschoolers
Another eye-tracking study reported that toddlers (2 years [27, 28•]. Additionally, recent work by Bedford et al. has
of age) with ASD were less sensitive to animations documented early emerging differences in attentional en-
depicting social actions (e.g., an adult engaging in peek- gagement with objects looked at by others in at-risk infants
a-boo) compared with typical peers but abnormally who would later receive an ASD diagnosis [29].

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.


Curr Dev Disord Rep (2019) 6:37–44 39

Eye-Tracking Research Investigating the Nature Eye-tracking has also been used to examine repetitive be-
of Non-social Differences in ASD haviors in ASD, with research documenting increased atten-
tion to specific, non-social stimuli in children with ASD ver-
Eye-tracking studies have also sought to examine attentional sus typical controls, and an association between heightened
and cognitive processes that are not related to the social do- visual interest in objects and severity of repetitive behaviors in
main. For example, Joseph and colleagues used eye-tracking children (but not adults) with ASD [34, 42].
to document superior visual search (enhanced ability to dis- A recent area of eye-tracking research is the examination of
criminate between targets and distractors) in school-age chil- gender differences within the ASD population, with prelimi-
dren and adolescents with ASD, pointing to the usefulness of nary research suggesting that social attention might be less
eye-tracking research to understand strengths in addition to atypical in school-age females versus males with ASD [43•];
challenges in ASD [30•]. Interestingly, Gliga et al. document- a similar pattern was detected in infants at risk for ASD [44].
ed that advantages in visual search at 9 months predicted ASD Finally, eye-tracking technology has also been used to mea-
severity in toddlerhood [31]. Other eye-tracking research has sure physiological arousal through pupillometry (the measure-
documented superior performance in the embedded figures ment of pupil diameter and its dilation in response to different
test in ASD, indicating that school-aged children with ASD, stimuli). Preliminary evidence from this area of research in-
compared with typically developing peers, were faster at per- cludes atypical emotional reactivity in response to different
ceiving target figures that were embedded in larger shapes social stimuli in preschoolers ASD, such as emotional faces
[32] (see also [33] for additional eye-tracking evidence of [45, 46, 47••]. While this area of research is in its infancy. The
atypical global processing in ASD). relevance of eye-tracking pupillometry to index arousal re-
Eye-tracking research has also examined attention sponse as well as various brain processes is subject to increas-
shifting and disengagement in ASD, with recent research ing investigation [48, 49].
[34] documenting fewer shifts and longer disengagement Summarizing, eye-tracking research investigating the na-
time in response to pictures considered of high interest ture of ASD atypicalities has generally documented atypical-
(e.g., trains, cars) in adolescents and adults with ASD—a ities in how social information is processed, including differ-
finding consistent with earlier work linking atypical ences in how attention to social stimuli is allocated and mod-
shifting and disengagement to circumscribed interests in ulated by various cues. However, conflicting findings also
children with ASD [35]. Impairments in attentional disen- exist. Research on information processing in non-social do-
gagement have also been documented in eye-tracking re- mains is more limited and inconclusive. As outlined in the
search using the gap-overlap paradigm [36], although two discussion, the field would benefit from standardization and
recent eye-tracking studies found normative disengage- validation of eye-tracking paradigms to ensure precision of
ment in ASD [37, 38]. These different results might be in constructs measured and comparability of results between
part attributable to differences in the eye-tracking indexes studies.
of engagement/disengagement across studies (central
disengagement/peripheral saccade accuracy in [36] versus Early Identification and Diagnostic Markers
central disengagement peripheral saccade in [37, 38]).
Additional eye-tracking paradigms have been used to ex- In the past decade, eye-tracking has been used in longitudinal
amine habituation and response to invariant versus variable studies of infants at familial risk for ASD to investigate
stimuli. Recent research [39] has documented that whereas markers of early emerging atypicalities that might be informa-
preschoolers without ASD decreased their attention towards tive for early identification. A recent study in this area found
an invariant stimulus (repeated presentation of a square) and that newborns at high-risk for ASD showed atypical looking
increased their attention towards a variable stimulus (shapes) profiles evidenced by more time spent looking at inverted
over multiple trials, those with ASD showed a similar atten- faces and random motion compared to children not at risk,
tional decline in response to both the novel and repeating who showed a preference for non-inverted faces and biologi-
stimuli. Vernetti et al. also documented atypical fixation pat- cal motion [50]. However, a study by Jones and Klin using a
terns in response to invariant versus variable stimuli using similar design failed to identify differences in newborns at risk
social stimuli in toddlers with ASD [40]. for ASD. Rather, they documented that infants who went on to
Furthermore, Fanning et al. used eye-tracking to mea- receive an ASD diagnosis at 36 months showed normative
sure spatial working memory in preschoolers with ASD in attention to the eyes of a caregiver at 2 months of age, which
an adaptation of the classic A not B paradigm in which then declined through 6 months of age. Conversely, children
participants’ visual fixations to locations associated with not at risk for ASD showed an increase in eye looking over
previously presented stimuli were used as an index of this same period [51••].
working memory [41]. Results revealed normative perfor- Other studies found that at 6 months of age, high-risk in-
mance in the ASD group. fants who later received an ASD diagnosis showed reduced

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.


40 Curr Dev Disord Rep (2019) 6:37–44

spontaneous attention to social scenes [52••] and were less examined in the ASD field. For example, Murias et al. docu-
focused on the inner features of a face when the face was mented that eye-tracking-derived measures of social attention
speaking compared with infants who did not develop ASD were correlated with social outcome measures that are typical-
[53]. Additionally, as previously mentioned, eye-tracking re- ly used in ASD intervention trials, including caregiver reports
search by Falck-Ytter et al. documented reduced orienting to of adaptive behavior and ASD symptoms [62]. Recent work
audiovisual synchrony during the observation of biological by Dawson and colleagues documented changes in eye-
motion in 10-month-old infants was predictive of receiving tracking measures of social attention in a group of children
an ASD diagnosis [19]. Interestingly, an association between receiving a novel therapy based on autologous cord blood
preference for synchrony in toddlerhood and language abili- infusion [9••]. These changes were associated with changes
ties has been recently documented in children with and with- in parent-reported socialization measures, pointing to the use-
out ASD who were matched on language abilities [54], sug- fulness of eye-tracking markers as endpoints in clinical trials.
gesting that attentional responsivity to audiovisual synchrony A recent trial on bumetanide (a diuretic acting on GABAergic
might be an important factor for communication development inhibition) has used eye-tracking to document increase dura-
in typical and atypical social development. tion of attention to eyes during viewing of dynamic emotional
Further eye-tracking research has reported similar gaze fol- faces as well as improved face perception in ASD following
lowing patterns in 10-month old high- and low-risk infants in intervention [63•, 64].
response to an agent turning both her head and eyes to direct Additionally, Vivanti et al. documented the utility of eye-
attention, whereas the high-risk group was under-responsive tracking measures in predicting intervention response, with
to an “eyes-only condition” and relied more on collecting findings showing that an eye-tracking measure of goal antic-
information from head movements [55•] (see also [14, 23] ipation was associated with communication changes follow-
for similar results in older samples). Gaze following abnor- ing 1 year of early intervention in young children with ASD
malities during infancy detected through eye-tracking was [22••]. Finally, eye-tracking has been used to test hypotheses
shown to predict later ASD symptomatology [29, 56]. relevant to early intervention theories. For example, Trembath
This preliminary research highlights the potential for eye- et al. tested the long-held assumption that children with ASD
tracking to capture early deviations in social processes in ASD learn better in response to visually presented instructions
that are difficult to measure through behavioral observation, (such as pictures) compared with verbal instruction, finding
with recent findings showing robust concordance between little evidence for a “visual learning style” in ASD [65].
eye-tracking and clinical judgment of ASD risk, symptoms, While the use of eye-tracking technology in this area is in
and attentional patterns [13, 57]. Importantly, however, some its infancy, this preliminary work holds promise in testing
eye-tracking research reported no differences during infancy treatment theories and providing an objective measurement
in children later diagnosed with ASD compared with children of intervention mechanisms and outcomes (e.g., changes in
who do not develop ASD [58, 59]. Variability in eye-tracking social engagement and social cognition).
stimuli and indexes might contribute to such differences,
pointing to the need for validation of paradigms before the
usefulness of eye-tracking for early identification of ASD Discussion
can be fully ascertained.
Challenges and Future Directions
Intervention Science
In summary, the introduction of eye-tracking in the field of
Whereas most literature on ASD early intervention focuses on ASD has contributed critical advances in the characterization
broad measures of cognitive, language, and adaptive function- of social (and, to a lesser degree, non-social) information pro-
ing to establish treatment effects, there is an increasing em- cessing in children with ASD. Progress enabled by eye-
phasis in the field on the need for a better measurement of the tracking includes increased measurement precision as well
social processes that are engaged and shaped by intervention as the possibility to test attentional and cognitive processes
[60]. Eye-tracking technology affords a high degree of preci- in younger and more impaired children with ASD, who have
sion in the measurement of constructs relevant to early inter- been historically under-represented in experimental behavior-
vention mechanisms and outcomes, such as social attention/ al paradigms requiring compliance with verbal instructions
orienting and social cognitive constructs (e.g., goal prediction, (e.g., the classic theory of mind tests).
theory of mind), thus holding the potential to provide an ob- Importantly, however, it appears that the field has yet to
jective marker of treatment change. fully exploit the potential of eye-tracking to address questions
The feasibility of eye-tracking technology as a sensitive on the nature of ASD and implications for diagnosis and treat-
measure to treatment response has been recently shown in ment. First, eye-tracking research reviewed in this article sug-
the typically developing literature [61] and is currently gests that social attention is a frequently measured construct,

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.


Curr Dev Disord Rep (2019) 6:37–44 41

and findings of social attention differences in ASD appears to understood, leaving open the question of the degree to which
be robust. However, eye-tracking research focused on other biological wiring drives social visual engagement and social
constructs is less abundant and has resulted in more inconsis- visual engagement shapes biological connections, and how
tent findings, pointing to the need for more examination of the two processes interact in early development. Recent re-
constructs other than social attention. Research reviewed in search has started to shed light on the hardwired constraints
this article suggests that a range of psychological processes and neonatal transitions of visual social attention, but more
(e.g., theory of mind, response to joint attention, working research is needed to clarify the relative contribution of bio-
memory) can be measured through eye-tracking adaptations logical programming and experience (e.g., social attention
of behavioral paradigms, most commonly using anticipatory history) on ASD atypicalities detected through eye-tracking
gaze to stimuli/locations to examine what children expect to [68•, 69•].
see based on how previous information is processed. This Our review has also highlighted how the relevance of
window into information processing could be exploited fur- eye-tracking to the diagnosis of ASD is still unclear.
ther, through replication of existing studies and development Although initial eye-tracking findings of reduced social
of eye-tracking tasks tapping into cognitive constructs that are attention in ASD had sparked optimism on the possibility
difficult to measure with precision through standard cognitive that atypical eye-tracking patterns could provide an early
tests, especially in younger and non-verbal children with diagnostic signature for ASD, many studies could not
ASD. identify a link between eye-tracking patterns in infants
Furthermore, this line of research would benefit from an and a later diagnosis of ASD [58, 59]. The few studies
increased focus on factors that modulate differences in eye- that documented eye-tracking abnormalities in infants lat-
tracking measures between ASD and other groups (e.g., anx- er diagnosed with ASD have not yet identified a universal
iety disorders, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, other and specific eye-tracking marker to support the early di-
neurodevelopmental disorders), and within the ASD group, agnosis of ASD [51••, 52••]. For example, Pierce et al.
i.e., the circumstances/conditions under which eye-tracking (2016) documented abnormal visual preference for geo-
patterns in ASD are most abnormal, and how these differ metric shapes compared with social stimuli in young chil-
among different subgroups within the ASD population (for dren with ASD, but such atypical eye-tracking pattern had
example, individuals showing optimal versus suboptimal re- very low sensitivity for ASD, although it demonstrated
sponse to specific interventions). Knowledge in this area high specificity [70]. These results led the authors to pro-
would provide critical insight on how the allocation of atten- pose that performance on the task could be a potential
tion in ASD is impacted by different stimuli in different indi- biomarker for a specific ASD subtype.
viduals and how such abnormalities might be minimized It is possible that our ability to identify eye-tracking
through treatment. markers of ASD might be hindered by poor ecological
Additionally, more research is needed on the behavioral validity of stimuli presented through eye-tracking devices,
and neuropsychological correlates of eye-tracking measures. as visual displays might fail to enable bodily engagement
For example, recent research has used EEG and MRI to mea- with the child, possibly resulting in the failure to fully
sure neural responses to social stimuli believed to be disrupted capture the core social engagement impairments that char-
in ASD and targeted by treatment [66, 67]. Eye-tracking pat- acterize ASD [71]. This limitation has been recently
terns in response to social stimuli, including both fixation time targeted by eye-tracking research measuring viewing pat-
and pupil dilation response, could provide a sensitive and less terns in live interactions with caregivers or confederates
invasive and costly measure of social processing changes [55•]; studies using eye-wear technology (e.g., glasses)
targeted in the intervention. Additionally, eye-tracking pat- that records participants’ viewing pattern as they go about
terns might capture intervention-related changes (for example, their day or are worn by an examiner to detect gaze pat-
increased engagement with social stimuli) before those chang- terns during social interactions [72•, 73]. Future research
es are reflected in structure-functioning reorganization detect- should capitalize on such ecologically valid eye-tracking
able through neuroimaging. However, the use of eye-tracking approaches to identify markers relevant to the diagnosis
as an “intermediate” marker to measure social processing re- of ASD.
quires further investigation on the association between brain Another challenge in the field is that much research on
imaging-derived and eye-tracking-derived responses to social eye-tracking signatures of ASD in infancy has focused on
stimuli. In particular, the study of behavioral and infants who are at high risk to develop ASD by virtue of
neurofunctional correlates of eye-tracking measures would having an older sibling with ASD—a population that might
benefit from the adoption of a developmental perspective. differ in terms of underlying genetic or biological risk fac-
For example, the developmental interplay between visual en- tors compared with other populations at high risk for ASD
gagement with social information and neural activation/ (e.g., children who are born premature), and compared
processing in response to social stimuli remains poorly with children with idiopathic ASD and ASD associated

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.


42 Curr Dev Disord Rep (2019) 6:37–44

with specific syndromes (e.g., Fragile X). Therefore, more References


research is needed to identify early eye-tracking signatures
of ASD across different populations with, or at risk for Papers of particular interest, published recently, have been
ASD. Additionally, cross-syndrome eye-tracking research highlighted as:
comparing different diagnostic groups might be more in- • Of importance
formative than comparisons between ASD and typical de- •• Of major importance
velopment to understand which features are ASD-specific
versus shared across neurodevelopmental conditions. For 1. Association AP. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disor-
example, recent research shows that Williams syndrome ders, fifth edition. Arlington: American Psychiatric Publishing;
2013.
shares some of the eye-tracking atypicalities observed in
2. Baio J, Wiggins L, Christensen DL, Maenner MJ, Daniels J, Warren
ASD (e.g., reduced visual engagement with the target of Z, et al. Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children
others’ attention and reduced sustained attention) [28•], aged 8 years—autism and developmental disabilities monitoring
thus raising the question of whether abnormal eye- network, 11 sites, United States, 2014. MMWR Surveill Summ.
tracking patterns documented in previous research compar- 2018;67(6):1–23.
3. Volkmar FR, Stier DM, Cohen DJ. Age of recognition of pervasive
ing ASD to typical development are due to ASD or non- developmental disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 1985.
syndrome specific factors. Additionally, knowledge on 4. Klin A. Biomarkers in autism spectrum disorder: challenges, ad-
syndrome-specific versus shared eye-tracking atypicalities vances, and the need for biomarkers of relevance to public health.
detected through eye-tracking could provide crucial insight Focus. 2018;16(2):135–42.
5. Venker CE, Kover ST. An open conversation on using eye-gaze
on the adoption of specific treatment targets and strategies methods in studies of neurodevelopmental disorders. J Speech
across diagnostic boundaries [74]. Lang Hear Res. 2015;58(6):1719–32. https://doi.org/10.1044/
Finally, the potential relevance of eye-tracking in ASD 2015_JSLHR-L-14-0304.
treatment research is yet to be fully explored. Whereas 6.• Klin A, Jones W, Schultz R, Volkmar F, Cohen D. Visual fixation
patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors
many interventions for ASD are designed to promote so- of social competence in individuals with autism. Arch Gen
cial engagement and eye-tracking provides a fine-grained Psychiatry. 2002;59(9):809–16 Pioneering study using eye-
measurement of social engagement, very little research has tracking to document atypical processes in ASD.
capitalized on eye-tracking to examine outcomes and 7.• Pelphrey KA, Sasson NJ, Reznick JS, Paul G, Goldman BD, Piven
J. Visual scanning of faces in autism. J Autism Dev Disord.
mechanisms of change in response to intervention. Initial 2002;32(4):249–61 An early study documenting atypical face
promising work in this area points to the importance of this scanning.
direction for future research [9]. 8.• Van Der Geest JN, Kemner C, Verbaten MN, Van Engeland H.
Gaze behavior of children with pervasive developmental disorder
toward human faces: a fixation time study. J Child Psychol
Psychiatry. 2002;43(5):669–78 An early study using eye-
tracking to document atypical processes in ASD.
Conclusions 9.•• Dawson G, Sun JM, Davlantis KS, Murias M, Franz L, Troy J, et al.
Autologous cord blood infusions are safe and feasible in young
children with autism spectrum disorder: results of a single-center
In conclusion, the introduction of eye-tracking research in the phase I open-label trial. Stem Cells Transl Med. 2017;6(5):1332–9
study of individuals with ASD has improved our ability to This study establishes the feasibility of eye-tracking as a bio-
“see the world” from their point of view and offered novel marker of treatment response.
insight into processes that are otherwise difficult to capture— 10. Dawson G, Meltzoff AN, Osterling J, Rinaldi J, Brown E. Children
with autism fail to orient to naturally occurring social stimuli. J
especially in individuals on the severe end of the spectrum, Autism Dev Disord. 1998;28(6):479–85.
including those who have been historically considered to be 11. Klin A, Shultz S, Jones W. Social visual engagement in infants and
“untestable”. Critical challenges for future research include toddlers with autism: early developmental transitions and a model
expanding the range of psychological constructs measured of pathogenesis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015;50:189–203.
12.• Chita-Tegmark M. Attention allocation in ASD: a review and meta-
through eye-tracking paradigms and translating the potential analysis of eye-tracking studies. Rev J Autism Dev Disorders.
of this technology into practical benefits for the diagnosis and 2016;3(3):209–23 Important meta-analysis of social attention
intervention of individuals with ASD. in ASD.
13. Frazier TW, Strauss M, Klingemier EW, Zetzer EE, Hardan AY,
Eng C, et al. A meta-analysis of gaze differences to social and
Compliance with Ethical Standards nonsocial information between individuals with and without au-
tism. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2017;56(7):546–55.
Conflict of Interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of 14. Vivanti G, Dissanayake C. Propensity to imitate in autism is not
interest. modulated by the model's gaze direction: an eye-tracking study.
Autism Res. 2014;7(3):392–9.
Human and Animal Rights and Informed Consent This article does not 15. Vivanti G, Hocking DR, Fanning P, Dissanayake C. Social affilia-
contain any studies with human or animal subjects performed by any of tion motives modulate spontaneous learning in Williams syndrome
the authors. but not in autism. Mol Autism. 2016;7(1):40.

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.


Curr Dev Disord Rep (2019) 6:37–44 43

16. Falck-Ytter T, von Hofsten C. How special is social looking in 33. Nayar K, Voyles AC, Kiorpes L, Di Martino A. Global and local
ASD: a review. Progress in brain research. Elsevier; 2011. p. 209– visual processing in autism: an objective assessment approach.
22. Autism Res. 2017;10(8):1392–404.
17.• Chawarska K, Shic F. Looking but not seeing: atypical visual scan- 34. Manyakov NV, Bangerter A, Chatterjee M, Mason L, Ness S,
ning and recognition of faces in 2 and 4-year-old children with Lewin D, et al. Visual exploration in autism spectrum disorder:
autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2009;39(12): exploring age differences and dynamic features using recurrence
1663. An important study documenting development and quantification analysis. Autism Res. 2018;11(11):1554–66.
change in attention to facial features by age in ASD.–72. 35. Sasson NJ, Elison JT, Turner-Brown LM, Dichter GS, Bodfish JW.
18. Klin A, Lin DJ, Gorrindo P, Ramsay G, Jones W. Two-year-olds Brief report: circumscribed attention in young children with autism.
with autism orient to non-social contingencies rather than biological J Autism Dev Disord. 2011;41(2):242–7.
motion. Nature. 2009;459(7244):257–61. 36. Sabatos-DeVito M, Schipul SE, Bulluck JC, Belger A, Baranek GT.
19. Falck-Ytter T, Nyström P, Gredebäck G, Gliga T, Bölte S, Team E, Eye tracking reveals impaired attentional disengagement associated
et al. Reduced orienting to audiovisual synchrony in infancy pre- with sensory response patterns in children with autism. J Autism
dicts autism diagnosis at 3 years of age. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. Dev Disord. 2016;46(4):1319–33.
2018;59:872–80. 37. Fischer J, Koldewyn K, Jiang YV, Kanwisher N. Unimpaired atten-
20. Falck-Ytter T. Young children with autism spectrum disorder use tional disengagement and social orienting in children with autism.
predictive eye movements in action observation. Biol Lett. Clin Psychol Sci. 2014;2(2):214–23.
2010;6(3):375–8. 38. Fischer J, Smith H, Martinez-Pedraza F, Carter AS, Kanwisher N,
21. Krogh-Jespersen S, Kaldy Z, Valadez AG, Carter AS, Woodward Kaldy Z. Unimpaired attentional disengagement in toddlers with
AL. Goal prediction in 2-year-old children with and without autism autism spectrum disorder. Dev Sci. 2016;19(6):1095–103.
spectrum disorder: an eye-tracking study. Autism Res. 2018;11: 39. Vivanti G, Hocking DR, Fanning PA, Uljarevic M, Postorino V,
870–82. Mazzone L, et al. Attention to novelty versus repetition: contrasting
22.•• Vivanti G, Dissanayake C, Zierhut C, Rogers SJ, Team VA. Brief habituation profiles in autism and Williams syndrome. Dev Cogn
report: predictors of outcomes in the early start Denver model de- Neurosci. 2018;29:54–60.
livered in a group setting. J Autism Dev Disord. 2013;43(7):1717– 40. Vernetti A, Senju A, Charman T, Johnson MH, Gliga T, Team B.
24 This study establishes the utility of eye-tracking in predicting Simulating interaction: using gaze-contingent eye-tracking to mea-
intervention response. sure the reward value of social signals in toddlers with and without
23. Vivanti G, McCormick C, Young GS, Abucayan F, Hatt N, Nadig autism. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2018;29:21–9.
A, et al. Intact and impaired mechanisms of action understanding in 41. Fanning PA, Hocking DR, Dissanayake C, Vivanti G. Delineation
autism. Dev Psychol. 2011;47(3):841–56. of a spatial working memory profile using a non-verbal eye-track-
24. Senju A, Southgate V, Miura Y, Matsui T, Hasegawa T, Tojo Y, et al. ing paradigm in young children with autism and Williams syn-
Absence of spontaneous action anticipation by false belief attribu- drome. Child Neuropsychol. 2018;24(4):469–89.
tion in children with autism spectrum disorder. Dev Psychopathol. 42. Sasson NJ, Turner-Brown LM, Holtzclaw TN, Lam KS, Bodfish
2010;22(2):353–60. JW. Children with autism demonstrate circumscribed attention dur-
25. Senju A. Spontaneous theory of mind and its absence in autism ing passive viewing of complex social and nonsocial picture arrays.
spectrum disorders. Neuroscientist. 2012;18(2):108–13. Autism Res. 2008;1(1):31–42.
26. Cole EJ, Slocombe KE, Barraclough NE. Abilities to explicitly and 43.• Harrop C, Jones D, Zheng S, Nowell SW, Boyd BA, Sasson N. Sex
implicitly infer intentions from actions in adults with autism spec- differences in social attention in autism spectrum disorder. Autism
trum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2018;48(5):1712–26. Res. 2018;11(9):1264–75 This study documents sex differences
in ASD utilizing eye-tracking.
27. Billeci L, Narzisi A, Tonacci A, Sbriscia-Fioretti B, Serasini L,
44. Kleberg JL, Nyström P, Bölte S, Falck-Ytter T. Sex differences in
Fulceri F, et al. An integrated EEG and eye-tracking approach for
social attention in infants at risk for autism. J Autism Dev Disord.
the study of responding and initiating joint attention in autism spec-
2018;1–10.
trum disorders. Sci Rep. 2017;7(1):13560.
45. Nuske HJ, Vivanti G, Dissanayake C. Reactivity to fearful expres-
28.• Vivanti G, Fanning PA, Hocking DR, Sievers S, Dissanayake C.
sions of familiar and unfamiliar people in children with autism: an
Social attention, joint attention and sustained attention in autism
eye-tracking pupillometry study. J Neurodev Disord. 2014;6(1):14.
spectrum disorder and Williams syndrome: convergences and di-
46. Nuske HJ, Vivanti G, Dissanayake C. Others’ emotions teach, but
vergences. J Autism Dev Disord. 2017;47(6):1866–77 This study
not in autism: an eye-tracking pupillometry study. Mol Autism.
uses eye-tracking across neurodevelopmental disorders to doc-
2016;7(1):36.
ument shared and syndrome-specific atypicalities.
47.•• Nuske HJ, Vivanti G, Hudry K, Dissanayake C. Pupillometry re-
29. Bedford R, Pickles A, Gliga T, Elsabbagh M, Charman T, Johnson
veals reduced unconscious emotional reactivity in autism. Biol
MH, et al. Additive effects of social and non-social attention during
Psychol. 2014;101:24–35 This study uses pupilometry to docu-
infancy relate to later autism spectrum disorder. Dev Sci.
ment atypical arousal responses in ASD.
2014;17(4):612–20.
48. Bast N, Poustka L, Freitag CM. The locus coeruleus–
30.• Joseph RM, Keehn B, Connolly C, Wolfe JM, Horowitz TS. Why is
norepinephrine system as pacemaker of attention–a developmental
visual search superior in autism spectrum disorder? Dev Sci.
mechanism of derailed attentional function in autism spectrum dis-
2009;12(6):1083–96 This study uses eye-tracking to measure
order. Eur J Neurosci. 2018;47(2):115–25.
strengths in non-social processing in ASD.
49. Lynch GT, James SM, VanDam M. Pupillary response and pheno-
31. Gliga T, Bedford R, Charman T, Johnson MH, Baron-Cohen S,
type in ASD: latency to constriction discriminates ASD from typi-
Bolton P, et al. Enhanced visual search in infancy predicts emerging
cally developing adolescents. Autism Res. 2018;11(2):364–75.
autism symptoms. Curr Biol. 2015;25(13):1727–30.
50. Di Giorgio E, Frasnelli E, Salva OR, Scattoni ML, Puopolo M,
32. Keehn B, Brenner LA, Ramos AI, Lincoln AJ, Marshall SP, Müller Tosoni D, et al. Difference in visual social predispositions between
R-A. Brief report: eye-movement patterns during an embedded fig- newborns at low-and high-risk for autism. Sci Rep. 2016;6:26395.
ures test in children with ASD. J Autism Dev Disord. 2009;39(2):
51.•• Jones W, Klin A. Attention to eyes is present but in decline in 2–6-
383–7.
month-old infants later diagnosed with autism. Nature.

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.


44 Curr Dev Disord Rep (2019) 6:37–44

2013;504(7480):427. This study documents markers of atypical diuretic bumetanide: a proof-of-concept behavioral and functional
processing in early infancy related to later ASD diagnosis.–31. brain imaging pilot study. Autism. 2015;19(2):149–57.
52.•• Chawarska K, Macari S, Shic F. Decreased spontaneous attention to 65. Trembath D, Vivanti G, Iacono T, Dissanayake C. Accurate or
social scenes in 6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism assumed: visual learning in children with ASD. J Autism Dev
spectrum disorders. Biol Psychiatry. 2013;74(3):195–203 This Disord. 2015;45(10):3276–87.
study documents atypical processes in infancy related to later 66. Dawson G, Jones EJ, Merkle K, Venema K, Lowy R, Faja S, et al.
ASD diagnosis. Early behavioral intervention is associated with normalized brain
53. Shic F, Macari S, Chawarska K. Speech disturbs face scanning in 6- activity in young children with autism. J Am Acad Child Adolesc
month-old infants who develop autism spectrum disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2012;51(11):1150–9.
Psychiatry. 2014;75(3):231–7. 67. Ventola P, Yang DY, Friedman HE, Oosting D, Wolf J,
54. Righi G, Tenenbaum EJ, McCormick C, Blossom M, Amso D, Sukhodolsky DG, et al. Heterogeneity of neural mechanisms of
Sheinkopf SJ. Sensitivity to audio-visual synchrony and its relation response to pivotal response treatment. Brain Imaging Behav.
to language abilities in children with and without ASD. Autism 2015;9(1):74–88.
Res. 2018;11(4):645–53. 68.• Constantino JN, Kennon-McGill S, Weichselbaum C, Marrus N,
55.• Thorup E, Nyström P, Gredebäck G, Bölte S, Falck-Ytter T. Altered Haider A, Glowinski AL, et al. Infant viewing of social scenes is
gaze following during live interaction in infants at risk for autism: under genetic control and is atypical in autism. Nature.
an eye tracking study. Molecular autism. 2016;7(1):12 This study 2017;547(7663):340. This study documents social viewing atyp-
uses a novel eye-tracking technology to capture naturalistic so- icalities related to both biological and environmental
cial interactions. influences.–4.
56. Thorup E, Nyström P, Gredebäck G, Bölte S, Falck-Ytter T, Team
69.• Shultz S, Klin A, Jones W. Neonatal transitions in social behavior
E. Reduced alternating gaze during social interaction in infancy is
and their implications for autism. Trends Cogn Sci. 2018; This
associated with elevated symptoms of autism in toddlerhood. J
study reviews early emerging brain-behavior relationships crit-
Abnorm Child Psychol. 2018;1–15.
ical during early development and describes a model of brain-
57. Vargas-Cuentas NI, Roman-Gonzalez A, Gilman RH, Barrientos F,
behavior pathogenesis.
Ting J, Hidalgo D, et al. Developing an eye-tracking algorithm as a
potential tool for early diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder in 70. Pierce K, Marinero S, Hazin R, McKenna B, Barnes CC, Malige A.
children. PLoS One. 2017;12(11):e0188826. Eye tracking reveals abnormal visual preference for geometric im-
58. Elsabbagh M, Johnson MH. Autism and the social brain: the first- ages as an early biomarker of an autism spectrum disorder subtype
year puzzle. Biol Psychiatry. 2016;80(2):94–9. associated with increased symptom severity. Biol Psychiatry.
59. Ozonoff S, Iosif A-M, Baguio F, Cook IC, Hill MM, Hutman T, 2016;79(8):657–66.
et al. A prospective study of the emergence of early behavioral signs 71. Boraston Z, Blakemore SJ. The application of eye-tracking tech-
of autism. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49(3):256– nology in the study of autism. J Physiol. 2007;581(3):893–8.
66. e2. 72.• Edmunds SR, Rozga A, Li Y, Karp EA, Ibanez LV, Rehg JM, et al.
60. Vivanti G, Kasari C, Green J, Mandell D, Maye M, Hudry K. Brief report: using a point-of-view camera to measure eye gaze in
Implementing and evaluating early intervention for children with young children with autism spectrum disorder during naturalistic
autism: where are the gaps and what should we do? Autism Res. social interactions: a pilot study. J Autism Dev Disord. 2017;47(3):
2018;11(1):16–23. 898–904 This study uses novel eye-tracking technology to cap-
61. Fletcher-Watson S, Hampton S. The potential of eye-tracking as a ture gaze behavior during naturalistic social interactions.
sensitive measure of behavioural change in response to interven- 73. Ye Z, Li Y, Liu Y, Bridges C, Rozga A, Rehg JM, editors. Detecting
tion: processed data set. 2018. bids for eye contact using a wearable camera. Automatic face and
62. Murias M, Major S, Davlantis K, Franz L, Harris A, Rardin B, et al. gesture recognition (FG), 2015 11th IEEE International Conference
Validation of eye-tracking measures of social attention as a potential and Workshops on. IEEE; 2015.
biomarker for autism clinical trials. Autism Res. 2018;11(1):166– 74. Vivanti G, Hamner T, Lee NR. Neurodevelopmental disorders af-
74. fecting sociability: recent research advances and future directions in
63.• Hadjikhani N, Johnels JÅ, Lassalle A, Zürcher NR, Hippolyte L, autism spectrum disorder and Williams syndrome. Curr Neurol
Gillberg C, et al. Bumetanide for autism: more eye contact, less Neurosci Rep. 2018;18(12):94.
amygdala activation. Sci Rep. 2018;8(1):3602 This study uses
eye-tracking as measure of response to pharmacological inter- Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdic-
vention in ASD. tional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
64. Hadjikhani N, Zürcher NR, Rogier O, Ruest T, Hippolyte L, Ben-
Ari Y, et al. Improving emotional face perception in autism with

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.


Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:

1. use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
2. use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
3. falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
4. use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
5. override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
6. share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at

[email protected]

You might also like