Keller Et Al - 2008 - Prevention-Focused Self-Regulation and Aggressiveness

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 47

Kent Academic Repository

Keller, Johannes, Hurst, Melanie and Uskul, Ayse K. (2008) Prevention-focused


self-regulation and aggressiveness. Journal of Research in Personality,
42 (4). pp. 800-820. ISSN 0092-6566.

Downloaded from
https://kar.kent.ac.uk/32384/ The University of Kent's Academic Repository KAR

The version of record is available from


https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2007.10.005

This document version


UNSPECIFIED

DOI for this version

Licence for this version


UNSPECIFIED

Additional information

Versions of research works

Versions of Record
If this version is the version of record, it is the same as the published version available on the publisher's web site.
Cite as the published version.

Author Accepted Manuscripts


If this document is identified as the Author Accepted Manuscript it is the version after peer review but before type
setting, copy editing or publisher branding. Cite as Surname, Initial. (Year) 'Title of article'. To be published in Title
of Journal , Volume and issue numbers [peer-reviewed accepted version]. Available at: DOI or URL (Accessed: date).

Enquiries
If you have questions about this document contact [email protected]. Please include the URL of the record
in KAR. If you believe that your, or a third party's rights have been compromised through this document please see
our Take Down policy (available from https://www.kent.ac.uk/guides/kar-the-kent-academic-repository#policies).
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 1

Running Head: SELF-REGULATION AND AGGRESSIVENESS

Prevention-Focused Self-Regulation and Aggressiveness

Johannes Keller,1, 2 Melanie Hurst,2 and Ayse Uskul 3

1) Université Catholique de Louvain; 2) Universität Mannheim; 3) University of Essex

Address correspondence to

Johannes Keller,
Universität Mannheim,
Lehrstuhl für Mikrosoziologie & Sozialpsychologie,
68131 Mannheim, Germany;
[email protected]
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 2

Abstract

The present research examined the relationship between individual differences in self-regulatory

mechanisms as outlined in regulatory focus theory (promotion- and prevention-focused self-

regulation) and aggressiveness. Two studies revealed that the more individuals’ habitual self-

regulatory orientation is dominated by a prevention-focus, the more likely they are to score high

on measures of cynical hostility, reciprocity norm endorsement, and aggressiveness. An

additional study involving the manipulation of perceived violation of a reciprocity norm showed

that predominantly prevention-focused participants were particularly sensitive to the experience

of a norm violation and reacted in a hostile and aggressive manner following the norm violation

experience. Findings indicate that a prevention-focused style of self-regulation is associated with

aggressiveness and suggest that endorsement of (negative) reciprocity norms and sensitivity to

norm violations are relevant factors that help explain the differences in aggressiveness observed

among individuals with a predominantly prevention-focused style of self-regulation.

Word count: 143

Keywords: aggressiveness, differential sensitivity, reciprocity norm, regulatory focus, self-

regulation
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 3

Prevention-Focused Self-Regulation and Aggressiveness

Hostility, aggression, and violence are the source of a great deal of physical pain and

psychological damage in human societies across the globe. The frequency with which humans

hurt one another is reflected in the fact that it is almost impossible to read through a daily

newspaper or to tune into the evening news without being confronted with instances of hostility,

aggression or violence. It is evident that the consequences of hostility and aggressiveness

continue to be are a burden on not only the perpetrators and victims but also the development of

human societies at large. Accordingly, the question of when and why people become aggressive

and motivated to hurt each other can be understood as one of the most important topics in the

social sciences. Understanding the causes of hostility and aggressiveness is therefore an

important item on the social scientific research agenda. The current paper is devoted to this item

on the agenda. Specifically, we examine the relationship between individuals’ style of self-

regulation and (a) trait level aggressiveness as well as (b) aggressive behavioral tendencies. In

our analysis, we focus on one particular mode of self-regulation that reflects a commitment to

oughts and duties and that can be described as vigilant and prevention-oriented in character.

Recent social psychological research has documented that many fundamental social

cognitive as well as social interactive mechanisms are heavily influenced by the situationally or

habitually activated type of self-regulatory orientation, which affects individuals’ thought

processes and behavioral tendencies (for an overview on self-regulation research, see Baumeister

and Vohs, 2004). One specific theoretical perspective figures prominently in the field of research

on self-regulatory orientations: regulatory focus theory (RFT) introduced by Higgins (Higgins,

1997; 1998). RFT holds that two basic modes of self-regulation can be differentiated. In the

prevention mode of self-regulation, individuals strive to achieve safety and security, fulfill their

duties and obligations, and avoid losses. In the promotion mode of self-regulation, individuals are
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 4

guided by the need for nurturance and growth, the desire to reach their ideal goals and

aspirations, and the motivation to achieve gains.

RFT has been applied to an impressively wide spectrum of topics and psychological

phenomena across a diverse array of domains (for an overview, see Higgins and Spiegel, 2004).

The existing evidence shows that a vast number of social cognitive as well as social interactive

phenomena can be related to the basic self-regulatory orientations outlined in RFT. The present

contribution aims to explore the role of RFT with respect to an important social interactive

phenomenon, aggressiveness, by testing the hypothesis that a prevention-focused self-regulatory

orientation is associated with aggressive tendencies. Somewhat surprisingly, there is very little

research on the role of self-regulatory mechanisms in aggressiveness. In fact, Bettencourt,

Talley, Benjamin, and Valentine (2006) recently noted in their meta-analytic review on

personality and aggressive behavior that theories of aggression have largely ignored the role of

self-regulation in aggressive behavior. The current research aims to address this theoretical and

empirical gap.

The present contribution also attempts to integrate ideas proposed in RFT and the General

Aggression Model (GAM) (Anderson & Bushman, 2002). Specifically, we start from the basic

assumption that several of the personal and situational input factors proposed in the GAM (i.e.,

variables that increase the probability of aggressive behavior in a given situation) can be

associated with constructs and mechanisms that are relevant for prevention-focused self-

regulation. The three studies reported in the current paper present empirical analyses focusing on

(a) the relationship between distinct personal input factors that share a conceptual association

with aggressive tendencies and prevention-focused self-regulation, and (b) the interaction of one

specific situational input factor, namely experience of a norm violation, with prevention-focused

self-regulation. In the following sections we outline in detail the theoretical underpinnings of the
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 5

proposed relationship between prevention-focused self-regulation and aggressiveness, starting

with a short introduction to RFT.

Core Assumptions of Regulatory Focus Theory

Extending the basic hedonic principle that people approach pleasure and avoid pain, RFT

holds that it is necessary to differentiate among distinct types of pleasures and distinct types of

pain, and to assess the specific strategic orientations and types of goal pursuit that reflect self-

regulation guided by two distinct motivational systems – promotion focus and prevention focus.

Self-regulation with a promotion focus is characterized as the motivation to attain growth and

nurturance, bring one’s actual self into alignment with one’s ideal self, and reach gains (and

avoid non-gains). In contrast, self-regulation with a prevention focus entails the motivation to

attain security, bring one’s actual self into alignment with one’s ought self (i.e., fulfilling one’s

duties and obligations), and avoid losses (and attain non-losses).

Research on RFT (see Higgins, 1998; Higgins & Spiegel, 2004 for reviews) shows that

both types of regulatory orientations are related to specific consequences. The psychological

consequences of promotion-focused self-regulation are: (a) a special sensitivity to the presence or

absence of positive outcomes, (b) application of eager strategic means, (c) ambitious and keen

striving to reach ones aspirations as reflected in tenacious goal pursuit that is focused on maximal

goals, and (d) cheerfulness-dejection emotions in response to positive and negative events. In

contrast, the psychological consequences of prevention-focused self-regulation are: (a) a special

sensitivity to the presence or absence of negative outcomes, (b) application of vigilant strategic

means, (c) a risk-averse and defensive orientation in the pursuit of minimal goals, and (d)

quiescence-agitation emotions (i.e., relaxation versus worry and anxiety) in response to positive

and negative events. In our theoretical considerations, we focus on the consequences regarding

differential sensitivity, specifically on the sensitivity to negative outcomes and cues associated

with prevention-focused self-regulation.


Self-regulation and aggressiveness 6

According to RFT, the two self-regulatory systems can be situationally induced (e.g.,

Freitas, Liberman, & Higgins, 2002; Friedman & Förster, 2001; Shah, Higgins, & Friedman,

1998). However, and most important in the present context, RFT posits that individuals may

differ in their predominant chronic or habitual self-regulatory orientations. Several measures to

assess individual differences in self-regulatory orientation have been developed (e.g., regulatory

focus questionnaires, cf. Higgins et al; 2001; Keller, 2007; Lockwood, Jordan, & Kunda, 2002;

regulatory strength measures, cf. Shah, Higgins, & Friedman, 1998). It is important to note that

the two modes of self-regulation have been conceptualized as independent constructs. In line

with this proposition, measures of the two chronic regulatory foci have been found to be largely

uncorrelated or slightly positively correlated (cf. Higgins et al., 2001; Lockwood, et al., 2002;

Keller, 2007). Thus, prevention-focused self-regulation does not represent the opposite pole of

promotion-focused self-regulation. This implies that it is possible that one of the two modes is

associated with a certain psychological phenomenon, while the other mode is not. The present

studies examine the relationship between prevention-focused self-regulation and aggressiveness

by integrating ideas of RFT and the GAM, which we will describe next.

Input Factors in the General Aggression Model (GAM)

The theoretical framework underlying the GAM (Anderson, 1997; Anderson & Bushman,

2002; Anderson, Deuser, & DeNeve, 1995) is based on a distinction of person and situational

variables as input factors contributing to aggressive tendencies. According to the model, these

input factors exert their impact on appraisal and decision processes (i.e., the outcomes of episodic

aggression processes) via cognitive, affective, and arousal routes. The person factors discussed in

the GAM include traits (e.g., susceptibility towards hostile attributions, unstable/labile self-

esteem), sex, beliefs that are related to a preparedness to aggress (e.g., self-efficacy beliefs),

attitudes (towards violence), values (e.g. normative convictions about what one should or ought

to do; e.g., reciprocity beliefs), long-term goals, and the availability and accessibility of
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 7

behavioral scripts. The situation factors discussed in the GAM include aggressive cues (e.g.,

weapons), provocation and frustration (insults, goal interference, perceived injustice), pain and

discomfort (e.g., heat, noise, unpleasant odors), drugs (e.g., alcohol, caffeine), and incentives

(perceived cost/benefit ratio). The present research was designed to assess how several of these

personal and situational input variables discussed in the GAM relate to the distinct self-

regulatory orientations proposed in RFT.

Note that most of the recent research in the context of the GAM focused on the routes to

aggression and how cognitive mechanisms (i.e., the role of knowledge structures and knowledge

accessibility) contribute to aggressive behaviors (cf. Anderson, 1997; Anderson & Dill, 2000;

Bushman, 1995). The present approach contributes to existing research on the GAM by focusing

on how motivational (self-regulatory) mechanisms relate to specific input factors proposed in the

GAM.

Self-Regulation and Aggressiveness

There is good reason to expect that self-regulatory mechanisms can help us understand the

underlying factors and processes of human aggression. In the section below, we describe the

general rationale underlying the hypothesized link between prevention-focused self-regulation

and aggressiveness.

Prevention-Focused Self-Regulation and Aggressiveness

As outlined above, the GAM proposes distinct personal and situational variables as input

factors contributing to aggressive tendencies. The core assumption underlying the present

research holds that self-regulatory orientations are meaningfully related to several of these input

factors. Specifically, we propose that a prevention-focused style of self-regulation is related (a)

to personal factors that have been shown to be associated with increased aggressive tendencies

(e.g., hostile attribution bias, hostility, labile-self-esteem, neuroticism, rumination, distrust1), and

(b) to situational factors that have been documented to trigger aggressive reactions in individuals
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 8

(e.g., observing other people’s norm violation; experiencing injustice). Accordingly, we propose

that prevention-focused self-regulation is positively related to aggressiveness because this style

of self-regulation involves several aspects that are known input factors of aggressive tendencies -

specifically, sensitivity to rule violations, reciprocity norm endorsement, hostile attribution

tendency, and a general cynical and misanthropic perspective on the world.

At first sight, this assumption may appear paradoxical given that prevention-focused self-

regulation is typically characterized by a fear of doing the wrong thing associated with a risk-

averse and careful behavioral orientation. Note, however, that the prevention-focus is not

necessarily related to risk-averse avoidance and flight tendencies (cf. Scholer, Stroessner, &

Higgins, in press). Specifically, under circumstances of threat and negative experiences (such as

rule violations) an active response strategy reflecting a fighting spirit can serve the basic

prevention-focused goal of reaching safety and security. That is, protecting and maintaining

safety and security sometimes can require offensive acts reflecting a fight (rather than flight)

response as well as retribution and reciprocation of negative treatments. Moreover, a concern

with ought standards (such as rules and normative standards, e.g., the reciprocity norm) is most

likely associated with a tendency to punish those who violate relevant normative standards in the

attempt to maintain the normative status quo. In combination, several aspects of prevention-

focused self-regulation suggest that this style of self-regulation may be related to aggressive

tendencies.2 In the sections below we outline the relation between prevention-focused self-

regulation and prominent personal and situational input factors proposed in the GAM which

support the assumed positive relation between the prevention-focus and aggressiveness.

Prevention-focused self-regulation and personal input factors in the GAM. Based on

conceptual and empirical considerations it can be assumed that prevention-focused individuals

are likely to (1) score high on measures of neuroticism, (2) show hostile attribution biases, (3)

have labile self-esteem, (4) engage in ruminative thinking, (5) endorse (negative) reciprocity
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 9

norms, and (6) be chronically skeptical, cynical (distrustful), and hostile. One underlying reason

for these assumptions is the relationship between punitive parenting styles and a chronic

prevention-focus which has been empirically supported by recent research findings (Keller, in

press; Manian, Strauman, & Denny, 1998). Given that punitive parenting styles have also been

shown to be related to several critical personal factors that render individuals prone to aggressive

tendencies (cynical hostility, cf. Sarason, Ganzer, & Granger, 1965; hostile attribution bias, cf.

Gomez & Gomez, 2000; Gomez, Gomez, DeMello, & Talent, 2001; labile self-esteem, cf.

Kernis, Brown, & Brody, 2000), it seems plausible to assume a relationship between these

personal factors and the prevention-focus. Moreover, neuroticism (a known factor in the

development and expression of aggressive tendencies; cf. Bettencourt et al., 2006) is

conceptually closely related to the emotional component of the prevention-focus (worry and

anxiety; cf. Higgins, 1998), which suggests a close association between prevention-focused self-

regulation and this particular personality trait.

Recent findings obtained in our lab indicate a reliable association between the prevention-

focus and the distinct personality traits mentioned above. Specifically, it emerged that the

habitual tendency to engage in prevention-focused self-regulation was positively correlated with

neuroticism, labile self-esteem, ruminative thinking, cynicism, interpersonal distrust, and

hostility (Keller, 2007; Keller et al., 2007). These empirically documented associations between

several of the proposed personal input factors of aggression and the prevention-focus suggest a

positive relationship between this style of regulatory focus and aggressive tendencies.

Prevention-focused self-regulation and situational factors in the GAM. Regarding the

relationship between prevention-focused self-regulation and distinct situational input factors, it

has been proposed – based on the differential sensitivity assumption outlined earlier – that the

special sensitivity of prevention-focused individuals to negative events and cues may make those

individuals particularly prone to act and react aggressively. That is, higher perceptual sensitivity
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 10

and sensibility towards negative events and cues may render predominantly prevention-focused

individuals particularly “vulnerable” to such cues, making them more likely to display aggressive

reactions and tendencies. Note that it seems plausible to assume that the particular sensitivity

and attentional focus of prevention-focused individuals to negative events and cues may - in the

long run - result in a cynical and skeptical perspective on the social world in general, which may

be described as a misanthropic and hostile attitude. And since hostility represents one crucial

aspect underlying aggression and aggressiveness, the degree to which an individual endorses

such a hostile and misanthropic attitude is most probably related to the individual’s level of

aggressiveness.

Overall, the analysis outlined above leads to the hypothesis that there is a significant

positive relationship between the prevention-focus and aggressive tendencies. As a starting

point, we tested this basic assumption in Studies 1 and 2, where we assessed the relationship

between self-report measures of regulatory focus and self-report measures of (a) aggression

(Buss & Perry, 1992) and (b) cynical hostility (Cook & Medley, 1954), as well as measures of

other constructs that represent established correlates of aggressive tendencies (rumination and

neuroticism). Study 2 also included measures of reciprocity norm endorsement to test the

assumption that beliefs reflecting reciprocity norm endorsement (particularly negative reciprocity

norm endorsement) are related to aggressiveness. Previous research has shown that negative

reciprocity norm endorsement is significantly related to anger, vengeance, and a cynical

perspective regarding other people (Eisenberger et al., 2004). These findings suggest a

meaningful association between reciprocity norm endorsement and aggressiveness. Moreover,

because a prevention-focus implies a concern with duties and responsibilities reflecting a concern

with norms and normative standards, it seems plausible to assume that reciprocity norm

endorsement is a distinct characteristic of predominantly prevention-focused individuals (a more

elaborate discussion of this aspect is presented in the introduction to Study 2 below). Finally,
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 11

Study 3 examined the prevention-focus-aggressiveness link by testing the effect of the

manipulation of the experience of a norm violation on aggressive tendencies.

Study 1

The goal of Study 1 was to assess the relationship between chronic regulatory focus

orientations and related constructs (such as rumination) and individual differences in

aggressiveness and related constructs (specifically, cynical hostility).

Participants and Procedure

Two independent samples of students at the University of Mannheim (Study 1a: N = 90;

Mage = 23.4; 39 women) and the University of Michigan (Study 1b: N = 38; Mage = 23.0; 27

women) completed measures designed to assess habitual levels of aggressiveness and regulatory

focus, as well as an aggressiveness-related trait, cynical hostility. Rumination was assessed in

Study 1a only. In both studies, responses were assessed using 7-point response scales; the only

exception was the Buss and Perry (1992) aggression questionnaire used in Study 1b, where 5-

point response scales were used (in Study 1a we used 7-point scales for this instrument as well).

Unless indicated otherwise, all scale endpoints were labeled not at all true and completely true.

Measures

Agressiveness. We used the Buss and Perry (1992) aggression questionnaire to assess trait

aggressiveness. Sample items of the three subscales used in the present studies read: “When

frustrated, I let my irritation show” (anger subscale); “When people annoy me, I may tell them

what I think of them” (verbal aggression subscale); and “When people are especially nice, I

wonder what they want” (hostility subscale). We used the overall aggressiveness scores in the

subsequent analyses (overall αs = .85 in both Studies 1a and 1b).

Regulatory focus. We assessed habitual self-regulatory orientations using the regulatory

focus scale developed by Lockwood et al. (2002) which consists of a prevention focus subscale

(9-items, α = .76 [Study 1a] and α =.86 [Study 1b], sample item: “In general, I am focused on
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 12

preventing negative events in my life”) and a promotion focus subscale (9-items, α = .81 [Study

1a] and α =.84 [Study 1b], sample item: “I frequently imagine how I will achieve my hopes and

aspirations”).

Cynical hostility. We also assessed cynical hostility (Cook & Medley, 1954) as an

aggressiveness-related measure relevant to the present context. Sample items read: “Most people

will use somewhat unfair means to gain profit or an advantage rather than to lose it,” and “No

one cares much what happens to you” (assessed with dichotomous true / false response options; α

= .69 [Study 1a] and α = .73 [Study 1b]).

Ruminative thinking. In Study 1a we also assessed participants’ tendency to engage in

ruminative thinking, which reflects an attentional focus on negative events and negative aspects

in life in general and, with a short version of the rumination scale used by Fresco, Frankel,

Mennin, Turk, and Heimberg (2002; adapted from Nolen-Hoeksema, Morrow, & Fredrickson,

1991). Participants completed this 9-item measure with response scales ranging from 1 (almost

never) to 7 (almost always) after reading the following lead-in statement: “Please indicate how

often you think and act the way described in the respective statement.” Sample items are: “Think

about all your shortcomings, failings, faults, and mistakes,” and “Think about how sad you feel”

(α = .91). This measure was included as an indicator of the differential sensitivity mechanism

according to which prevention-focused self-regulation is related to a heightened focus on

negative aspects in life. Moreover, since previous research has revealed that ruminative thinking

is related to aggressiveness (e.g., Bushman, 2002; Bushman, Bonacci, Pedersen, Vasquesz, &

Miller, 2005; Verona, 2005), this construct was expected to function as an underlying factor in

the proposed association between prevention-focused self-regulation and aggressiveness.

Results and Discussion

To test the relationship between regulatory orientation and aggressiveness we computed

zero-order correlations (see Table 1) as well as regression analyses. In Studies 1a and 1b, the
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 13

prevention focus scale was significantly positively correlated with the trait aggressiveness scale,

a finding supportive of the proposed association between these constructs. The promotion focus

scale did not correlate with aggressiveness. In a finding supportive of the differential sensitivity

assumption, the prevention-focus scale was positively and robustly correlated with rumination.

Regression analyses with aggressiveness as the criterion variable controlling for promotion

(Model 1 in Table 2) and cynical hostility scores (Model 2 in Table 2) reveal that controlling for

promotion scores has no meaningful impact, whereas the prevention – aggressiveness

relationship drops substantially, but remains significant, when cynical hostility is controlled for.

This suggests that it is not only the tendency to perceive the social world from a cynical

perspective that contributes to the relationship between prevention-focus and aggressiveness and

that there must be some other processes or mechanisms that contribute to this link.

To test the potential role of ruminative thinking in the link between the prevention-focus

and aggressiveness, we entered ruminative thinking scores into the regression model (for Study

1a) as an additional predictor. As shown in Model 3 in Table 1, prevention-focus ceases to be a

significant predictor of aggressiveness when ruminative thinking scores are controlled for. Thus,

negative cynical thinking and rumination appear to be important underlying mechanisms in the

link between the prevention-focus and aggressiveness, with rumination being a more powerful

factor accouting for this link.

Overall, these results support the reasoning that a prevention-focused self-regulatory

orientation is positively related to aggressiveness. We observed this association in two samples

(in the USA and Germany) with different cultural backgrounds. Moreover, we found that

negative thinking (as reflected in cynical hostility and rumination) seems to be a process

underlying the relationship between prevention-focus and aggressiveness. To advance the

validity and reliability of the obtained findings, we conducted Study 2 to gather further empirical

evidence.
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 14

Study 2

Study 2 was designed to test the robustness of the link between the prevention-focus and

aggressiveness by employing an alternative measure of self-regulatory orientation. Moreover,

Study 2 examined the role of neuroticism and reciprocity norm beliefs in the link between the

prevention-focus and aggressiveness.

Neuroticism reflects the emotional component of prevention-focus (cf. Higgins, 1998) and

has been found to relate to aggressiveness (cf. Sharpe & Desai, 2001; von Collani & Werner,

2005). Thus, neuroticism represents a construct with the potential to explain the link between the

prevention-focus and aggressiveness. Moreover, ruminative thinking, which we found to

underlie the relationship between prevention-focus and aggressiveness in Study 1, has been

established as a close correlate of neuroticism and anxiety (cf. Kuyken, Watkins, Holden, &

Cook, 2006; Nolen-Hoeksema, 2000; Roberts, Gilboa, & Gotlib, 1998; Trapnell & Campbell,

1999). Thus, existing evidence suggests that neuroticism may contribute to the link between the

prevention-focus and aggressiveness, an assumption we tested in Study 2.

In addition to exploring the role of neuroticism in the link between the prevention-focus

and aggressiveness, Study 2 also examined the role of reciprocity norm beliefs in this link with a

particular focus on negative reciprocity. The reciprocity norm prescribes that one should return

favors and retaliate against infringements (Gouldner, 1960). In other words, it refers to the

obligation to help those who helped us in the past and to retaliate against those who have been

detrimental to our interests and well-being (Perugini et al., 2003). This conceptualization reveals

that reciprocity can be positive or negative. The positive reciprocity norm deals with the

obligation to repay favorable treatment; the negative reciprocity norm deals with retribution as a

response to unfavorable treatment (Goudner, 1960). Reciprocity norm beliefs refer to the

internalization of these reciprocity norms, with negative reciprocity beliefs referring to the

internalization of the negative reciprocity norm and positive reciprocity beliefs referring to the
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 15

internalization of the positive reciprocity norm. As with any norm endorsement, individuals

differ in the extent to which they hold these beliefs (Eisenberger et al., 2004; Perugini et al.,

2003).

Examining the role of reciprocity norm beliefs seems to be promising in the present context

for several reasons. From a theoretical perspective, prevention-focused self-regulation implies a

preoccupation with duties and responsibilities, reflecting a concern with norms and normative

standards. Thus, prevention-focused self-regulation should increase sensitivity to the normative

appropriateness of others’ responses (i.e., whether one’s actions are reciprocated in line with a

general reciprocity norm). In the most general terms, reciprocity norm endorsement should be

observed among predominantly prevention-focused individuals, regardless of whether the focus

is on negative or positive forms of reciprocity. Given the focus of the current research on

aggressiveness and its relationship to the prevention-focus and the existing evidence on the link

between negative reciprocity beliefs and aggressiveness (and related constructs; cf. Eisenberger,

et al., 2004; Eder et al., 2006), we are particularly interested in the role of negative reciprocity.

Reciprocating negative behavior displayed by others can be seen as reflecting the motivation to

attain security and to avoid (or minimize) losses - which is particularly relevant to prevention-

focused individuals. Specifically, retaliating against the harmful behavior of others may help

individuals to protect themselves from further hurtful behavior in the future, and may be

experienced and construed as a fair and normatively appropriate compensation for losses or

infringements experienced in previous interactions. Moreover, Perugini et al. (2003) suggested

that negative reciprocity behavior involves sensitivity to negative interpersonal events, which

represents a defining feature of prevention-focused self-regulation. Thus, we hypothesize that

the endorsement of reciprocity beliefs in general (and negative reciprocity in particular) is related

to prevention-focused self-regulation.
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 16

In addition, there is reason to assume that the endorsement of negative reciprocity beliefs

could be related to aggressiveness. From a conceptual perspective, it seems evident that negative

reciprocity should be closely related to reactive aggression – which can be defined as aggressive

acts committed in response to provocation or frustration (Dodge, 1991; Dodge, Lochman,

Harnish, Bates, & Pettit, 1997; Miller & Lynam, 2006). Thus, reactive aggression can in some

sense be understood as the behavioral expression of negative reciprocity. Hence, it seems likely

that individuals who strongly endorse negative reciprocity beliefs are more likely to engage in

acts reflecting reactive aggression. From an empirical perspective, previous research has shown

that negative reciprocity norm endorsement is significantly related to anger, vengeance, and a

cynical perspective regarding other people. For example, using self-report and behavioral

measures, Eisenberger et al. (2004) showed that negative reciprocity norm beliefs are strongly

related to anger in everyday life and anger following mistreatment by others. Accordingly, it

seems plausible to assume that negative reciprocity norm endorsement is meaningfully related to

aggressiveness.

Research by Perugini and colleagues (2003) revealed that endorsement of negative

reciprocity norms in the behavioral domain is positively associated with reciprocation wariness,

that is, cautiousness in reciprocating help, resulting from a fear of being taken advantage of, and

negatively associated with proneness to forgive and with emotional stability (the opposite of

neuroticism). Perugini et al. (2003) further showed that individuals who strongly endorse

negative reciprocity beliefs are less generous and more likely to engage in punishments in the

face of negative behavior. Moreover, Eder and colleagues (2006) found that individuals who

endorse the negative reciprocity norm are particularly likely to see retaliation as a proper

response to wrongdoing.

The correlates of negative reciprocity norm endorsement that reflect its relationship with

anger, retaliation, and aggressiveness suggest that this construct may be another important
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 17

potential candidate to explain the link between prevention-focused self-regulation and

aggressiveness. Study 2 was designed to test the potential role of this construct in addition to the

explanatory power of neuroticism - which represents an established risk factor regarding

aggression (Bettencourt et al., 2006; Sharpe & Desai, 2001; von Collani & Werner, 2005).

Method

Participants and Procedure

University of Mannheim students (N = 88, Mage = 23.2; 46 women) filled out a

questionnaire that consisted of two self-regulatory measures and scales that assessed

aggressiveness, cynical hostility, neuroticism, and reciprocity norm beliefs.

Measures

Regulatory focus. Participants completed the same regulatory focus measure used in Study

1 (Lockwood et al., 2002) (αPromotion= .83; αPrevention = .78), as well as an alternative instrument

recently developed and validated in our lab to assess self-regulatory focus (Keller, 2007) that

comprises a prevention subscale (α = .83, sample item: “My life is often shaped by fear of failure

and negative events”), and a promotion subscale (α = .76, sample item: “If I know that my

performance is being evaluated by other people, that spurs me on and increases my ambition to

do well”). Replicating previous findings (reported in Keller, 2007), the congruent subscales of

the two regulatory focus measures (i.e., the Lockwood et al. and the Keller measure) were

significantly correlated (prevention subscales: r = .65, p < .001; promotion subscales: r = .43, p <

.001), whereas the correlations involving incongruent dimensions of self-regulation were not

significant (r = .15, n.s., and -.10, n.s., respectively).

Aggressiveness. As in Study 1, we used the Buss and Perry (1992) aggression measure to

assess trait aggressiveness (in this case, we assessed the physical aggression subscale as well).

Parallel to Study 1, the overall aggressiveness score (α = .90) was used in the analyses reported

below.
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 18

Cynical hostility. The same measure as in Study 1 (Cook & Medley, 1954) was used to

assess cynical hostility (α = .64).

Neuroticism. We used a German version (Eggert, 1983) of Eysenck’s personality inventory

to assess neuroticism (α = .86).

Reciprocity norm beliefs. We used two measures to assess reciprocity norm beliefs. One of

these measures was developed by Eisenberger et al. (2004) and consists of two subscales that

assess positive reciprocity norm beliefs (10 items, sample item: “If someone does me a favor, I

feel obligated to repay them in some way”) and negative reciprocity norm beliefs (14 items,

sample item: “If someone dislikes you, you should dislike them”). We used short versions of

both subscales (each scale comprising 7-items; αnegative reciprocity =. 87, αpositive reciprocity =. 81). (Note:

negative reciprocity norm as assessed by this measure refers to advisability of retribution for

unfavorable treatment).

The second measure of reciprocity norm beliefs was developed by Perugini and colleagues

(2003) and consists of three subscales with each subscale comprising 9 items. A sample item of

the beliefs in reciprocity subscale reads: “If I work hard, I expect it will be repaid” (α = .77). A

sample item of the positive reciprocity subscale reads: “I’m ready to do a boring job to return

someone’s previous help” (α = .74). A sample item of the negative reciprocity subscale reads: “If

somebody is rude to me, I become rude” (α = .86). (Note: negative reciprocity as assessed by

this measure refers to negative reciprocity as expressed in behaviors). In the analyses reported

below, we used a composite score based on the two negative reciprocity norm subscales in order

to have both the cognitive and behavioral aspects of negative reciprocity represented in the

measure (α = .93). Since both scales were highly correlated (r = .78), use of a composite score

was also appropriate in order to avoid analyses that could potentially suffer from problems of

multicollinearity.

Results and Discussion


Self-regulation and aggressiveness 19

In line with our theoretical analysis and the findings obtained in Study 1, data revealed

significant zero-order correlations between both prevention scales and aggressiveness scores (see

Table 3). Both prevention scale scores were also significantly positively correlated with scores of

cynical hostility, replicating the pattern observed in Study 1. As in Study 1, neither promotion

focus scale was meaningfully correlated with aggressiveness or cynical hostility.

Reciprocity norm endorsement scores as measured by two different scales were positively

associated with prevention focus scores, with the exception that prevention focus scores on the

Lockwood et al. scale were not reliably associated with negative reciprocity as measured by

Eisenberger et al. (2004). Replicating previous research, reciprocity beliefs were also positively

related to scores on aggressiveness and cynical hostility, with the exception of positive

reciprocity beliefs measured with Perugini et al.’s (2003) scale.

Next, we computed regression analyses in order to test whether the correlates of

prevention-focused self-regulation (negative reciprocity, neuroticism, and cynical hostility) help

explain the relationship between habitual prevention-focused self-regulation and aggressiveness.

In keeping with the analysis conducted in Study 1, we ran initial analyses including promotion

and prevention scale scores as predictors (see Table 4; analyses are reported separately for the

Lockwood et al. and the Keller scales). Parallel to Study 1 we found that prevention but not

promotion scale scores were significantly related to aggressiveness (Model 1). Next, we tested

whether this relation could be explained by negative reciprocity norm beliefs. The analysis

revealed that controlling for negative reciprocity norm beliefs (composite measure) markedly

reduced the strength of the relationship between prevention and aggressiveness, indicating that

negative reciprocity norm beliefs are in part involved in the relationship between prevention-

focused self-regulation and aggressiveness (Model 2).3 We then added cynical hostility into the

regression model. As depicted in Table 4 (Model 3), including cynical hostility scores resulted in

a substantial drop in the relationship between the prevention-focus and aggressiveness,


Self-regulation and aggressiveness 20

replicating the pattern observed in Study 1. Finally, we added neuroticism (Model 4) and found

that prevention focus scores are no longer a significant predictor in the regression models when

negative reciprocity beliefs, cynical hostility, and neuroticism are controlled for. That is, our

findings show that negative reciprocity beliefs, cynical hostility, and neuroticism contribute to

understanding the link between prevention-focused self-regulation and aggressiveness.

A critical reader may come to the conclusion that the relation between prevention-focused

self-regulation and aggressiveness may be reducible to the correlation between neuroticism and

aggressiveness. We argue against this interpretation referring to several reasons. First,

neuroticism is only a marginally significant predictor in one of the two regression models in

Study 2 (see Model 4, right panel in Table 4). It seems not adequate to argue that the association

between prevention–focused self-regulation and aggressiveness is reducible to the impact of

neuroticism when the latter factor is not even a reliable predictor in the model. Second, partial

correlations between the two prevention focus scales and aggressiveness remain marginally

significant when controlling for neuroticism scores (r = .19, p < .09 and r = .18, p < .10,

respectively). Third, neuroticism is only one among several distinct elements in the

conceptualization of the prevention-focus (reflecting the affective component of this style of self-

regulation; cf. Higgins, 1998) and it would be misleading to argue that prevention-focused self-

regulation can be reduced to neuroticism. Supporting the distinction between neuroticism and

prevention-focused self-regulation, other lines of research in our lab revealed that the relations

between prevention-focused self-regulation and other phenomena – for example interpersonal

trust (Keller et al. 2007) and value orientations (Keller, 2007) – remain robust and hardly differ

from zero-order correlations when neuroticism is controlled for. Finally, it is important to note

that - in contrast to the prevention focus scales - neuroticism was not significantly related to

negative reciprocity norm endorsement (see Table 3), a factor that is of critical relevance with
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 21

respect to aggressiveness. Accordingly, we think it is fair to conclude that prevention-focused

self-regulation and neuroticism represent two distinct (although related) constructs.

Summarizing the results of Studies 1 and 2, one can say that the obtained findings concur

with the proposition that there is a significant positive relationship between prevention-focused

self-regulation and aggressiveness. Importantly, findings are robust across different samples and

across measures of regulatory focus. Several aspects are particularly noteworthy with respect to

the findings obtained in Study 2. First, replicating the pattern observed in Study 1, we observed

that cynical hostility partially contributes to the link between prevention focus and

aggressiveness; however, it does not fully mediate this link. Second, endorsement of negative

reciprocity norm beliefs is found to be a partial mediator of the relationship between prevention-

focused self-regulation and aggressiveness. Thus, sensitivity to normative standards and the

expectation to reciprocate others’ negative behavior in part explains the relationship between

prevention focus and aggressiveness. Third, the neuroticism findings indicate that the emotional

aspect of prevention-focused self-regulation that involves worry and anxiety also contributes to

the relationship between prevention-focused self-regulation and aggressiveness. This finding is

in line with previous results (cf. Bettencourt et al. 2006; Sharpe & Desai, 2001; von Collani &

Werner, 2005) documenting a prominent role of neuroticism in the development and expression

of aggression. In sum, these findings add to our understanding of the underlying personality

mechanisms contributing to the observed relationship between prevention-focused self-regulation

and aggressiveness.

The observed results documenting a positive association between aggressiveness and

prevention-focused self-regulation appear particularly noteworthy in view of the available

evidence pointing to the fact that personality characteristics related to prevention-focused self-

regulation - such as cynicism, distrust as well as anxiety and neuroticism - shifted to higher levels

during recent decades (Kanter & Mirvis, 1989, Twenge, 2000). From our perspective, the upward
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 22

shift in these critical personality characteristics across birth cohorts is particularly problematic in

light of the fact that these characteristics are correlated with hostility and aggressiveness.

The Interplay of Personal and Situational Input Factors

Up to this point, our empirical analyses have focused on personality factors and the role

they play in the association between prevention-focused self-regulation and aggressiveness.

However, as outlined in the theoretical introduction, we suppose that several situational input

factors as discussed in the GAM may also be related to prevention-focused self-regulation. We

assume – based on the differential sensitivity mechanism proposed in RFT – that prevention-

focused self-regulation is related to a special sensitivity to negative aversive cues in general, and

to one aversive cue that is known to increase aggressiveness, in particular: the experience or

observation of norm violations. Specifically, we propose that rendering reciprocity norm

violation salient may trigger aggressive tendencies particularly strongly in prevention-focused

individuals. This assumption is based on two insights. First, according to RFT, prevention-

focused self-regulation is characterized by a special sensitivity to negative cues. Since

experiencing a violation of the reciprocity norm represents an aversive and hence negative cue, it

stands to reason that prevention-focused individuals may notice and react particularly strongly

and aggressively to this cue. Second, the results of Study 2 revealed that (a) prevention-focused

self-regulation is associated with the endorsement of (negative) reciprocity beliefs, and (b) the

endorsement of negative reciprocity beliefs is positively associated with aggressiveness. In

combination, these findings lead to the hypothesis that prevention-focused individuals should be

particularly likely to react with negative reciprocity to a norm violation. .

Thus, we propose that the comparatively high level of aggressiveness expressed by

predominantly prevention-focused individuals’ can be explained (at least in part) by prevention-

focused participants’ specific sensitivity to negative events such as norm violations. In

particular, prevention-focus is likely to be associated with a specific sensitivity to the detection of


Self-regulation and aggressiveness 23

norm violations and with a particularly strong tendency to interpret norm violations as the

outcome of intentional actions (reflecting a hostile attribution bias). Moreover, we assume that

as a consequence of these tendencies, prevention-focused individuals are particularly likely to

react in a hostile and aggressive manner in situations where they are confronted with a norm

violation

To test our assumptions, we designed an experimental study examining the violation of a

normative standard as a critical situational input factor triggering aggressiveness specifically

among prevention-focused individuals. Thus, the study was designed to answer the question

whether the relation between prevention-focused self-regulation and aggressiveness reflects a

particular sensitivity to norm violations in prevention-focused individuals. By manipulating the

experience of a norm violation, we followed a methodological strategy designed to establish a

causal chain as recently discussed by Spencer, Zanna, and Fong (2005) who noted that a

reasonable strategy to document a causal chain in the attempt to understand the underlying

mechanisms involved in a given psychological phenomenon is to experimentally vary the

proposed underlying factor (as a complementary strategy to the often applied mediational

analysis based on the measurement of mediating variables).

Study 3

The considerations outlined above were tested in an experimental study involving the

manipulation of a violation of the reciprocity norm and the assessment of participants’ perception

of norm violation, hostile attribution tendency, and level of aggressiveness as a function of

chronic self-regulatory orientation. Aggressiveness was measured using both direct and displaced

aggression, the latter representing a measure of actual behavioral aggression.

Method

Participants and Procedure


Self-regulation and aggressiveness 24

Seventy-seven students at the University of Mannheim (Mage = 22.4; 37 women)

participated in this study and received monetary compensation (2 Euro plus extra money

depending on their response in the experimental setting, see description below). They were

randomly assigned to one of two experimental conditions. Four participants were excluded

because they did not follow the experimental procedures adequately, leaving a sample size of 73

for subsequent analyses.

When participants arrived at the lab, a confederate of the experimenter appeared and served

as the ostensible other participant so that participants were made to believe that there was another

participant present with whom they would later be interacting. They were told that the other

participant would be sitting in an adjacent room and completing the same material. Once seated

in their cubicle, participants received a questionnaire that contained the measure of chronic

regulatory focus used in Study 1 (Lockwood et al.’s scales; αPromotion= .81; αPrevention = .76)

Manipulation of norm violation. After completing the regulatory focus measure,

participants received new materials from the experimenter and first read the descriptions

involving the cover story for the manipulation of norm violation. The cover story explained that

the researchers were interested in individuals’ reactions in social interactions involving decision-

making regarding money exchange.

We chose the trust game interaction paradigm that was developed by Berg, Dickhaut, and

McCabe (1995) which has been frequently used in game theoretical analyses of trust (e.g.,

Bohnet & Zeckhauser, 2004; Buchan & Croson, 2004; Kosfeld, Heinrichs, Zak, Fischbacher, &

Fehr, 2005). In this experimental game, participants initially receive a certain amount of money

and learn that they can transfer any amount of this money to a second player and that this amount

will be tripled before the other player actually receives the transfer amount. At this point, the

other player (who receives the money) is free to decide whether or not to reciprocate by sending

back a certain amount of money. In an absolute trust relationship, participants who are in the
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 25

position to initiate the money transfer would decide to send the full amount to the interaction

partner, and the person receiving the money would send back half of the amount he or she

received, resulting in the best possible joint outcome for the players.

In order to manipulate norm violation, we varied the level of reciprocation participants

experienced in the paradigm (with a low level of reciprocity reflecting a violation of the

reciprocity norm). Each participant initially received a payment of 2 Euros (in form of ten 20

Cent coins) for showing up. Participants then learned that they were randomly assigned either to

the role of a money transfer initiator or the role of a transfer receiver (in reality, all participants

were assigned to the role of money transfer initiator). Participants in the role of a money transfer

initiator had the option to transfer any amount between 0.20 and 2 Euro to another person (the

receiver), who would then decide how much money he or she wanted to send back. It was

further explained that the amount of money the initiator was willing to transfer (between 0.20 and

2 Euro) would be tripled. That is, if the initiator decided to transfer 2 Euro, the receiver would

get 6 Euro and could then decide how much of this amount he or she was willing to send back to

the initiator. Participants also learned that the transfer receiver could select an amount varying in

steps of 0.05 Euro (this information was necessary given that the amount of money they received

in the back transfer could represent 160%, - see below - which required that the interaction

partners have the option of using coins of lesser value). Moreover, participants were told that the

individuals involved in the interaction would remain strictly separated and had no chance of

communicating with each other.

Following the detailed description of the scenario, participants were asked to decide on the

amount of money they would be willing to transfer to the receiver and to put the respective

amount in an envelope provided by the experimenter, who then ostensibly brought the envelope

to the transfer receiver in the adjacent room. In reality, the experimenter went to the other room,

checked the amount of money the participant had put in the envelope, and selected one of several
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 26

previously prepared envelopes containing the amount of money that either reflected a repayment

of 100% (for participants randomly assigned to the norm violation condition) or a repayment of

160% (for participants randomly assigned to the control condition). The amount of repayment

was based on the findings of a pre-test which revealed that among students at the University of

Mannheim, a repayment of 155% or higher in the trust game paradigm is perceived as fair and

appropriate in terms of the reciprocity norm. The experimenter then returned to the participant

with the selected envelope and an additional questionnaire containing the measures of dependent

variables.

Materials

Manipulation check. First, in order to make sure that all participants had opened the

envelope before responding to the dependent measures, we asked them to indicate the amount of

money they had received as repayment from their interaction partner. All participants indicated

the correct amount of repayment. Next, participants responded to three items designed to assess

the perceived fairness of the repayment they had received, which served as a manipulation check

of the norm violation manipulation. The fairness items read: “I felt that the return transfer from

my interaction partner was fair;” “The return transfer from my interaction partner deviated

strongly from what one could expect as a fair return transfer in terms of reciprocity;” “I feel that

the behavior of my interaction partner was unfair” (the latter two items were reverse coded, 1

[not at all true] to 7 [completely true]). A fairness index was computed averaging across the

items (α = .94). After responding to the manipulation check questions, participants proceeded to

complete the dependent measures. All measures described below were assessed on 7-point scales

ranging from 1 (not at all true) to 7 (completely true).

State Anger. To assess affective reactions to the money exchange interaction, we included

four items designed to assess anger. Sample items read: “The behavior of my interaction partner

on this task made me angry;” “My interaction partner made me angry because he did not behave
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 27

fairly in terms of reciprocity.” We computed an anger index averaging across the four items (α =

.92).

Hostile attributions. Participants’ tendency to attribute their (ostensible) interaction

partners’ behavior to mean intentions was assessed using three items that read: “I am certain that

my interaction partner deliberately behaved this way so as to provoke me;” “My interaction

partner deliberately behaved this way to harm me;” and, “I think that my interaction partner

transferred this amount without intending me any ill will” (the latter item is reverse coded). We

computed a hostile attribution index averaging across the three items (α = .76).

State aggression. In line with previous work (Farrar & Krcmar, 2006), we used a state

version of the Buss and Perry (1992) aggression questionnaire to assess state aggression.

Specifically, we modified the original trait items in such a way that the statements referred to

participants’ ostensible interaction partner and the current situation in the experimental session.

As a lead-in-statement to the state aggression items, participants read: “Now imagine that upon

leaving the room you happen to run into your interaction partner, who is holding the money he

received in his hand and is laughing. Please read the following statements carefully and indicate

to what extent these statements apply to your personally in this situation.“ Participants

responded to 20 state aggression items that measured state verbal aggression (sample item: “I

would tell this person directly what I thought of his or her behavior”), state physical aggression

(sample item: “This person could drive me to the point where we would get into a physical

confrontation”), state anger (sample item: “In this situation I would feel like a powder keg about

to explode”), and state hostility (sample item: “In this situation I would definitely think that life

is treating me unfairly”). We computed a mean state aggression index averaging across the 20

items (α = .95).

Displaced aggression. Following a procedure used by Mussweiler and Förster (2000) to

assess displaced aggressive tendencies, we asked participants in the concluding part of the study
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 28

to select 10 pictures out of a set of 30 that would ostensibly be given later to participants in an

unrelated future study conducted by the same research group. The 30 pictures were selected

from the International Affective Picture System (Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention,

1995). These pictures have been pre-tested for their valence using rating scales ranging from 1

(negative) to 9 (positive). The set of 30 pictures used in the present study comprised 10 negative

pictures with ratings below 4 (e.g., a picture of a rotting animal corpse), 10 positive pictures with

ratings above 6 (e.g., a picture of three puppies), and 10 neutral pictures with ratings between 4

and 6 (e.g., a picture of a book). Participants received a poster where all 30 pictures could be

seen simultaneously, as well as a pile of the 30 pictures as separate photographs. They were

instructed to put the 10 selected photographs from the pile in an envelope that was provided by

the experimenter. As cover story, participants learned that the research group was conducting

studies in another field testing mechanisms of visual perception in social context, and that the

participants in the respective studies were to receive visual stimulus materials. Moreover,

participants were told that it was of great importance to ensure that the materials used in these

studies represented a wide variety of stimuli and that it was important that they select the stimuli

rather than the researchers, because researchers who were familiar with the study goals could

introduce bias in the selection of the pictures. In other words, participants were lead to believe

that their help was required to develop an unbiased set of stimuli for future studies. We used the

mean valence of the 10 selected pictures as a measure of displaced aggression (lower scores

reflecting higher aggression). This measure represents an indicator of actual behavioral

aggressive tendencies.

Results and Discussion

In preparing the regression analyses designed to test the proposed moderation hypothesis,

we first centered the prevention and promotion focus scores, standardized the norm violation

variable by coding it as +1 (for the norm violation condition) and -1 (for the control condition),
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 29

and computed the interaction term by multiplying the centered prevention as well as the centered

promotion scores with the norm violation variable. Results of regression analyses are reported in

Table 5 separately for the manipulation check and each criterion variable.

Manipulation Check

In an initial analysis we checked the effectiveness of the norm violation manipulation and

regressed responses to the perceived fairness items onto prevention and promotion focus scores,

the categorical variable of norm violation and their interaction terms. The analysis revealed the

expected norm violation main effect (perceived fairness was significantly higher in the control

condition compared to the norm violation condition), as well as a main effect of habitual

prevention focus (stronger endorsement of prevention focus significantly predicted lower

perceived fairness). The interaction term did not reach the conventional level of significance;

however the negative interactive effect at trend level is in line with the hypothesis that the norm

violation is perceived as more unfair, the more prevention-focused participants are in their self-

regulatory orientation.4

Main Dependent Measures

Next, we separately analyzed the main dependent measures by regressing them on the same

predictors as above. As expected, regression results revealed the hypothesized significant

interaction effect between prevention focus and norm violation for all criterion variables (anger,

hostile attribution, state aggression, and displaced aggression; significant main effects for norm

violation and for prevention focus scores emerged in the analyses focusing on anger, hostile

attributions, and state aggression). The findings are graphically displayed in Figure 1 (based on a

median split on the prevention scale scores). As is evident, scores on measures of anger, hostile

attributions, and state aggression are significantly higher in the norm violation condition

compared to the control condition, and, most importantly, this effect is particularly strong in

individuals reporting a strong prevention-focused orientation of self-regulation. A parallel effect


Self-regulation and aggressiveness 30

emerged on the displaced aggression measure (which is reverse scored: higher scores reflect

selection of pictures with more positive valence) – that is, the norm violation resulted in a

stronger tendency to engage in displaced aggression as reflected in the selection of more

negative/aversive pictures. This finding is particularly noteworthy, because the picture selection

measure represents a measure of actual behavioral aggression.

The findings obtained in this experimental study suggest that prevention-focused self-

regulation is associated with a particularly strong tendency to react angrily and aggressively in

situations involving a norm violation, indicating that prevention-focused individuals seem to be

most sensitive regarding norm violations. Moreover, such individuals are particularly likely to

attribute hostile intentions and to react with displaced aggressive tendencies following the

experience of a violation of the reciprocity norm. In combination, the results suggest that

sensitivity to norm violation can be understood as a critical factor that contributes to prevention-

focused individuals’ higher level of aggressiveness. The fact that individuals who engage in

extreme acts of violence – for example, individuals involved in homicidal rampages (such as the

Virgina Tech massacre or the Columbine high school shooting) – are often found to see

themselves as victims of unfair treatment (i.e., norm violations) suggests that the findings

observed in our experimental study reflect one aspect of the mechanisms that are most likely

involved in real world incidents of drastic acts of aggression.

General Discussion

In relating aggressiveness to RFT (Higgins, 1998), we proposed that a prevention-focused

mode of self-regulation is associated with trait aggressiveness and to behave aggressively. The

obtained findings strongly support this hypothesis. Across several studies, participants

consistently scored higher on measures of aggressiveness, the more they endorsed a prevention-

focused orientation of self-regulation. In the reported studies, we applied different measures to

assess individual differences in regulatory focus and also different measures to assess trait
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 31

aggressiveness and the tendencies to act and react aggressively. The proposed relationship was

observed irrespective of the type of measurement applied to assess the relevant constructs (i.e.,

chronic regulatory focus and aggressiveness), and it emerged in different cultural milieus (USA

and Germany).

Going beyond the first generation of research (Zanna & Fazio, 1982) concerning the

question whether a relationship between (prevention-focused) self-regulatory mechanisms and

aggressiveness can be observed (which can be answered affirmatively based on the results

obtained in Study 1 and 2), we also addressed the second-generation question focusing on

underlying mechanisms. In this respect, we found evidence supporting the notion that specific

aspects of prevention-focused self-regulation contribute to the observed relationship between

prevention-focused self-regulation and aggressiveness. Specifically, we found that cynical

hostility and rumination (reflecting a general tendency of negative thinking) as well as

neuroticism and endorsement of negative reciprocity norm beliefs play an important role. We

also obtained experimental evidence supporting the notion that differential sensitivity to norm

violation is a crucial factor underlying the association between prevention-focused self-regulation

and aggressiveness. Overall, findings suggest that negative thinking and sensitivity to norm

violations play a crucial role and contribute to individual differences between prevention- and

promotion-focused individuals in aggressive tendencies.

We acknowledge the fact that we are not in a position to draw conclusions regarding the

causality of the observed relations and the mechanisms underlying the relation between

prevention-focused self-regulation and aggressiveness given the correlational nature of the

reported findings. Additional (experimental) research is clearly necessary (and currently

underway in our lab) to test the causality and the underlying mechanisms of the relation between

prevention-focused self-regulation and aggressiveness in a systematic fashion.


Self-regulation and aggressiveness 32

The reported research findings are innovative in several respects. First, in relating RFT to

aggressiveness we document the crucial impact of self-regulatory mechanisms with regard to an

important social interactive phenomenon. So far, the role of self-regulatory mechanisms has

been largely neglected in the analysis of aggressiveness (cf. Bettencourt et al., 2006), and the

present studies help to fill in this gap. Second, we specifically put the differential sensitivity

assumption entailed in RFT to a test in the context of aggressiveness and obtained strong

empirical support. We found that predominantly prevention-focused participants were

particularly sensitive to the violation of a norm, and this suggests that prevention-focused

individuals are particularly sensitive to and vigilant about normative standards. Since this aspect

of prevention-focused self-regulation has not been documented before, the current findings

extend our knowledge on the specific mechanisms characteristic of this distinct self-regulatory

mode.

Third, the observation that prevention-focused participants responded with increased

displaced aggression in a context where a norm violation was rendered salient (Study 3) is a

novel and intriguing finding, which provides evidence for the role of self-regulatory mechanisms

in the field of displaced (as compared to direct reactive) aggressive behavior. Our findings

indicate that prevention-focused individuals are likely to show displaced aggression the more

they feel that relevant norms are violated in a social context. This might contribute to our

understanding of displaced aggression observed in individuals who are victims of behaviors

reflecting norm violations (e.g., students who suffer from bullying; individuals who feel deprived

of basic rights or who experience injustice).

Finally, the finding that the violation of the reciprocity norm triggers aggression reflects an

innovative aspect of the current research, because this specific aversive input factor has not been

systematically assessed in the context of RFT or the context of aggression. The findings provide

evidence supporting the notion that experiencing a violation of the reciprocity norm is a powerful
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 33

situational input factor triggering aggressive responses, and that this is particularly true for

individuals who are prevention-focused in their self-regulatory orientation. The observed

relationship between prevention-focused self-regulatory orientation and sensitivity to norm

violations (as well as the endorsement of the negative reciprocity norm) suggest that further

analyses of prevention-focused self-regulatory mechanisms in the field of aggression focusing on

these constructs seem promising.

In sum, the present work represents a new and promising approach to the study of

aggressiveness and its underlying forces and mechanisms that takes into account that social

interactive processes, in general, and aggression, in particular, are heavily influenced by the self-

regulatory mechanisms that are guiding individuals’ thoughts and behavior.

A cautionary note regarding our theoretical perspective emphasizing prevention-focused

mechanisms of self-regulation seems appropriate at this point: Our focus on prevention-focused

self-regulation in the present work does not reflect the (implicit) assumption that promotion-

focused self-regulation is not related to aggressiveness. On the contrary, in the context of the

GAM we suppose that certain situational and personal input factors that contribute to aggressive

tendencies are likely to be related to promotion-focused mechanisms. For example, theoretical

considerations suggest that frustration through interfering disruptions in the strive to reach an

ideal or maximal goal – that is, a goal standard related to the promotion focus – is most likely to

trigger aggression in promotion-focused individuals. Thus, it is important to note that our

position is not that only prevention-focused self-regulation is related to aggressiveness, and that

promotion-focused self-regulation is irrelevant in the discussion of aggressiveness. Our position

is that both modes of self-regulation can contribute to the development and expression of

aggressiveness, though they do so through very different routes and mechanisms. The present

work focused on mechanisms related to the prevention focus. Additional studies focusing on

mechanism related to the promotion focus are currently underway in our lab, and we posit that
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 34

the analysis of both orientations of self-regulation can contribute substantially to our

understanding of the underlying mechanisms of aggressiveness.

In sum, the theoretical perspective and the empirical findings reported in the present paper

contributes to the understanding of the role self-regulatory mechanisms play in trait

aggressiveness and aggressive tendencies and thereby enhance our knowledge of the factors that

contribute to aggressive behavior. As such, the current studies open a new avenue of research for

studying aggression that incorporates the crucial impact of self-regulatory mechanisms.

Implications

Given the great relevance of aggression in everyday life, a discussion of several practical

implications of the present work with respect to potential means for decreasing aggressiveness

(particularly in prevention-focused individuals) seems appropriate. In the most general terms,

our findings suggest that it would be important to counteract a misanthropic, cynical perspective

(e.g., making hostile attributions) and negative thinking (e.g., rumination) in individuals. With

respect to the misanthropic, cynical perspective that contributes to prevention-focused

individuals’ aggressiveness, it seems worthwhile to try and counteract hostile dispositional

attributions by highlighting the importance of social context as a critical factor in driving

individuals’ behavior, and to emphasize the fact that individuals’ characteristics and behaviors

are malleable and subject to change. With respect to the tendency toward ruminating and

negative thinking, it seems appropriate to try and counteract prevention-focused individuals’

tendency to focus on negative environmental cues and related emotional experiences as well as

their tendency to brood and dwell on bad and negative things. Of course, this seems a rather

difficult undertaking, particularly if individuals live in a context where they are consistently

confronted with negative cues. Nonetheless, recent approaches in positive psychology (Snyder &

Lopez, 2002) indicate that there are possible strategies for changing individuals’ focus and

inducing a more hopeful and positive perspective.


Self-regulation and aggressiveness 35

Another possible strategy to counter prevention-focused individuals’ aggressive tendency

could be to make use of prevention–focused individuals’ respect for normative standards and to

emphasize that non-aggressive and non-retaliatory behavior are normatively appropriate. Based

on the notion that prevention-focused individuals’ are particularly concerned with fulfillment of

oughts and responsibilities, such a strategy seems theoretically particularly meaningful.

Finally, given the empirical evidence documenting that being raised under an

authoritarian parenting style contributes not only to the development of a prevention-focused

self-regulatory orientation (Keller, in press), but also to established risk factors in the context of

aggression (labile self-esteem, neuroticism, cynical hostility, hostile attribution bias), it seems

that having parents consider the consequences of authoritarian childrearing practices might also

be helpful. In combination, such efforts to counteract the mechanisms documented as underlying

factors of aggression in the current research might prove useful in limiting and inhibiting the

development and expression of aggressiveness.


Self-regulation and aggressiveness 36

References

Anderson, C. A. (1997). Effects of violent movies and trait hostility on hostile feelings and

aggressive thoughts. Aggressive Behavior, 23, 161-178.

Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2002). Human aggression. Annual Review of Psychology,

53, 27-51.

Anderson, C. A., Deuser, W. E., & DeNeve, K. (1995). Hot temperatures, hostile affect, hostile

cognition, and arousal: Test of a general model of affective aggression. Personality and

Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 434-448.

Anderson, C. A., & Dill, K. E. (2000). Video games and aggressive thoughts, feelings, and

behavior in the laboratory and in life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 772-

790.

Baumeister, R. F., Bushman, B. J., & Campbell, K. (2000). Self-esteem, narcissism, and

aggression: Does violence result from low self-esteem or from threatened egotism? Current

Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 26-29.

Baumeister, R. F., Smart, L., & Boden, J. M. (1996). Relation of threatened egotism to violence

and aggression: The dark side of high self-esteem. Psychological Review, 103, 5-33.

Baumeister, R. F. & Vohs, K. D. (Eds.) (2004). Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory,

and applications. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Berg, J., Dickhaut, J., & McCabe, K. (1995). Trust, reciprocity, and social history. Games and

Economic Behavior, 10, 122-142.

Bohnet, I., & Zeckhauser, R. (2004). Trust, risk, and betrayal. Journal of Economic Behavior

and Organization, 55, 467-484.

Bettencourt, B. A., Talley, A., Benjamin, A. J., & Valentine, J. (2006). Personality and

aggressive behavior under provoking and neutral conditions: A meta-analytic review.

Psychological Bulletin, 132, 751-777.


Self-regulation and aggressiveness 37

Buchan, N., & Croson, R. (2004). The boundaries of trust: Own and others’ actions in the US and

China. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 55, 485-504.

Bushman, B. J. (1995). Moderating role of trait aggressiveness in the effects of violent media on

aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 950-960.

Bushman, B. J. (2002). Does venting anger feed or extinguish the flame? Catharsis, rumination,

distraction, anger, and aggressive responding. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,

28, 724-731.

Bushman, B. J., & Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Threatened egotism, narcissism, and direct and

displaced aggression: Does self-love or self-hate lead to violence? Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 75, 219-229.

Bushman, B. J., Bonacci, A. M., Pedersen, W. C., Vasquez, E. A., & Miller, N. (2005). Chewing

on it can chew you up: Effects of ruminatio on triggered displaced aggression. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 969-983.

Buss, A. H., & Perry, M. (1992). The aggression questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 63, 452-459.

Caprara, G. V. (1986). Indicators of aggression: The dissipation-rumination scale. Personality

and Individual Differences, 7, 763-769.

Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention (1995). The international affective picture system:

Digitized photographs. Gainesville, FL: University of Florida, Center for Research in

Psychophysiology.

Collins, K., & Bell, R. (1997). Personality and aggression: the dissipation-rumination scale.

Personality and Individual Differences, 22, 751-755.

Cook, W. W., & Medley, D. M. (1954). Proposed hostility and pharisaic-virtue scales for the

MMPI. Journal of Applied Psychology, 38, 414-418.


Self-regulation and aggressiveness 38

Denson, T. F., Pedersen, W. C., & Miller, N. (2006). The displaced aggression questionnaire.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 1032-1051.

Dodge, K. A. (1991). The structure and function of reactive and proactive aggression. In D. J.

Pepler, & K. H. Rubin (Eds.), The development and treatment of childhood aggression (pp.

201-218). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Dodge, K. A., Lochman, J. E., Harnish, J. D., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S. (1997). Reactive and

proactive aggression in school children and psychiatrically impaired chronically assaultive

youth. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 37-51.

Eder, P., Aquino, K., Turner, C., & Reed, A. (2006). Punishing those responsible for the prison

abuses at Abu Ghraib: The influence of the negative reciprocity norm (NRN). Political

Psychology, 27, 807-821.

Eggert, D. (1983). Eysenck-Persönlichkeits-Inventar: E-P-I; 2. Auflage. [Eysenck Personality

Inventory: E-P-I; 2nd edition]. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

Eisenberger, R., Lynch, P., Aselage, J., & Rohdieck, S. (2004). Who takes the most revenge?

Individual differences in negative reciprocity norm endorsement. Personality and Social

Psychology Bulletin, 30, 787-799.

Farrar, K., & Krcmar, M. (2006). Measuring state and trait aggression: A short, cautionary tale.

Media Psychology, 8, 127-138.

Fresco, D. M., Frankel, A. N., Mennin, D. S., Turk, C. L., Heimberg, R. G. (2002). Distinct and

Overlapping Features of Rumination and Worry: The Relationship of Cognitive Production

to Negative Affective States. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 26, 179-188.

Freitas, A. L., Liberman, N., & Higgins, E. T. (2002). Regulatory fit and resisting temptation

during goal pursuit. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 291-298.

Friedman, R. S., & Förster, J. (2001). The effects of promotion and prevention cues on creativity.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 1001-1013.


Self-regulation and aggressiveness 39

Gomez, R., & Gomez, A. (2000). Perceived maternal control and support as predictors of hostile-

biased attribution of intent and response selection in aggressive boys. Aggressive Behavior,

26, 155-168.

Gomez, R., Gomez, A., DeMello, L., & Tallent, R. (2001). Perceived maternal control and

support: Effects on hostile biased social information processing and aggression among

clinic-referred children with high aggression. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,

42, 513-522.

Gouldner, A. W. (1960). The norm of reciprocity: a preliminary statement. American

Sociological Review, 25, 161-178.

Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52, 1280-1300.

Higgins, E. T. (1998). Promotion and prevention: Regulatory focus as a motivational principle. In

M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 30, pp. 1-46). San

Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Higgins, E. T., Bond, R. N., Klein, R., Strauman, T. (1986). Self-discrepancies and emotional

vulnerability: How magnitude, accessibility, and type of discrepancy influence affect.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 5-15.

Higgins, E. T., Friedman, R. S., Harlow, R. E., Idson, L. C., Ayduk, O. N., & Taylor, A. (2001).

Achievement orientations from subjective experiences of success: promotion pride versus

prevention pride. European Journal of Social Psychology, 31, 3-23.

Higgins, E. T. & Spiegel, S. (2004). Promotion and prevention strategies for self-regulation: A

motivated cognition perspective. In R. F. Baumeister & C. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self-

regulation (pp. 171-187). New York: Guilford.

Kanter, D. L., & Mirvis, P. H. (1989). The cynical Americans. Living and working in an age of

discontent and disillusion. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.


Self-regulation and aggressiveness 40

Keller, J. (2007). Development and validation of a self-report scale assessing performance-related

chronic self-regulatory concerns: The regulatory concerns questionnaire. Manuscript in

preparation. University of Mannheim.

Keller, J. (in press). On the development of regulatory focus: The role of parenting styles. European

Journal of Social Psychology.

Kernis, M. H., Brown, A. C., & Brody, G. H. (2000). Fragile self-esteem in children and its

associations with perceived patterns of parent-child communication. Journal of Personality,

68, 225-252.

Kosfeld, M., Heinrichs, M., Zak, Paul J., Fischbacher, U., & Fehr, E. (2005). Oxytocin increases trust

in humans. Nature, 435, 673-676.

Kuyken, W., Watkins, E., Holden, E., & Cook, W. (2006). Rumination in adolescents at risk for

depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 96, 39-47.

Lockwood, P., Jordan, C. H., & Kunda, Z. (2002). Motivation by positive and negative role models:

Regulatory focus determines who will best inspire us. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 83, 854-864.

Manian, N., Strauman, T. J., & Denney, N. (1998). Temperament, recalled parenting styles, and

self-regulation: Testing the developmental postulates of self-discrepancy theory. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1321-1332.

Miller, J. D., & Lynam, D. (2006). Reactive and proactive aggression: Similarities and

differences. Personality and Individual Differences, 41, 1469-1480.

Miller, J. D., Lynam, D., & Leukefeld, C. (2003). Examining antisocial behavior through the lens

of the five factor model of personality. Aggressive Behavior, 29, 497-514.

Mussweiler, T., & Förster, J. (2000). The sex→aggression link: A perception-behavior

dissociation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 507-520.


Self-regulation and aggressiveness 41

Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). The role of rumination in depressive disorders and mixed

anxiety/depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 504-511.

Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Morrow, J., & Fredrickson, B. L. (1991). Response Styles and the Duration

of Episodes of Depressed Mood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 20-28.

Perugini, M., Gallucci, M., Presaghi, F., & Ercolani, A. P. (2003). The personal norm of

reciprocity. European Journal of Personality, 17, 251-283.

Roberts, J. E., Gilboa, E., & Gotlib, I. H. (1998). Ruminative response style and vulnerability to

episodes of dysphoria: Gender, neuroticism, and episode duration. Cognitive Therapy and

Research, 22, 401-423.

Roney, C. J., Higgins, E. T., & Shah, J. Y. (1995). Goals and framing: How outcome focus

influences motivation and emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 1151-

1160.

Sarason, I. G., Ganzer, V. J., & Granger, J. W. (1965). Self-description of hostility and its

correlates. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1, 361-365.

Scholer, A. A., Stroessner, S. J., & Higins, E. T. (in press). Responding to negativity: How a

risky tactic can serve a vigilant strategy. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology.

Schroeder, M. L., Wormworth, J. A., & Livesley, W. J. (1994). Dimensions of personality

disorder and the five-factor model of personality. In P. T. Costa & T. A. Widiger (Eds.),

Personality disorders and the five-factor model of personality (pp. 117-127). Washington,

DC: American Psychological Association.

Shah, J., Higgins, E. T., & Friedman, R. S. (1998). Performance incentives and means: How

regulatory focus influences goal attainment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,

74, 285-293.
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 42

Sharpe, J. P., & Desai, S. (2001). The revised NEO personality inventory and the MMPI-2

psychopathology five in the prediction of aggression. Personality and Individual Differences,

31, 505-518.

Snyder, C. R., & Lopez, S. J. (Eds.) (2002). Handbook of positive psychology. Oxford: Oxford

University Press.

Spencer, S. J., Zanna, M. P., & Fong, G. T. (2005). Establishing a causal chain: Why experiments

are often more effective than mediational analyses in examining psychological processes.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 845-851.

Sukhodolsky, D. G., Golub, A., & Cromwell, E. N. (2001). Development and validation of the

anger rumination scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 31, 689-700.

Tangri, S. S., Burt, M. R., & Johnson, L. B. (1982). Sexual harassment at work: Three

explanatory models. Journal of Social Issues, 38, 33-54.

Trapnell, P. D., & Campbell, J. D. (1999). Private self-consciousness and the five-factor model of

personality: Distinguishing rumination from reflection. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 76, 284-304.

Twenge, J. M. (2000). The age of anxiety? Birth cohort change in anxiety and neuroticism, 1952-

1993. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 1007-1021.

Verona, E. (2005). Moderating effects of rumination and gender on context-specific aggression.

Aggressive Behavior, 31, 420-436.

von Collani, G., & Werner, R. (2005). Self-related and motivational constructs as determinants of

aggression. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 1631-1643.

Zanna, M. P., & Fazio, R. H. (1982). The attitude-behavior relation: Moving toward a third

generation of research. In M. P. Zanna, E. T. Higgins, & C. P. Herman (Eds.), Consistency in

social behavior. The Ontario Symposium on Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 2 (pp.

283-301). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.


Self-regulation and aggressiveness 43

Author note

This research was supported by a post-doctoral grant from the German Science Foundation to

Johannes Keller. Ayse K. Uskul was supported by a post-doctoral fellowship from the Social

Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada during the conduct of Study 1.
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 44

Footnotes
1
. Note that the recent meta-analysis on personality and aggressive behavior conducted by

Bettencourt and colleagues (2006) revealed that irritability (reflecting trait anger), rumination,

emotional susceptibility (reflecting neuroticism), and narcissism (reflecting labile self-esteem)

are among those traits that are reliably related to aggressive behavior. Several lines of research

have found that neuroticism is related to aggressiveness (Sharpe & Desai, 2001; von Collani &

Werner, 2005), that (angry) rumination is a close correlate of aggression (Denson, Pedersen, &

Miller, 2006); Bushman, 2002; Bushman, Bonacci, Pedersen, Vasquesz, & Miller, 2005;

Caprara, 1986; Collins & Bell, 1997; Sukhodolsky, Golub, & Cromwell, 2001; Verona, 2005),

that labile self-esteem (narcissism) is positively associated with aggression (Baumeister,

Bushman, & Campbell, 2000; Baumeister, Smart, & Boden, 1996; Bushman & Baumeister,

1998; note that labile self-esteem/narcissism is also robustly related to neuroticism, cf.

Schroeder, Wormworth, & Livesley, 1994), and that interpersonal trust is inversely related to

aggressiveness (Miller, Lynam, & Leukefeld, 2003).


2
. We want to emphasize that we are not making the claim that aggressiveness is exclusively

related to prevention-focused self-regulatory mechanisms and that promotion-focused self-

regulation is irrelevant in the context of aggressiveness. As outlined in the general discussion, we

discuss that certain input factors in the GAM are conceptually linked to promotion-focused self-

regulation (e.g., when individuals experience and interference in their efforts to reach ideal or

maximal goals). Thus, there is reason to assume that the analysis of promotion-focused self-

regulation may also contribute to our understanding of aggressiveness. However, our focus in the

present article is on the role of prevention-focused self-regulatory mechanisms.


3
. Note that additional analyses including positive reciprocity norm beliefs and general

reciprocity beliefs as predictors revealed that these variables are not relevant, all coefficients t <

1.10, n.s.
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 45

4
. A critical reader may be tempted to argue that an interaction effect on the manipulation check

at trend level significance is a critical limitation. However, it should be noted that we are not

making the claim that perceived fairness is the only relevant aspect that contributes to

prevention-focused participants’ increased tendency to react aggressively. As outlined in the

introduction and documented in Study 1 and 2, it is much more meaningful to assume that

several aspects related to prevention-focused self-regulation (not only the enhanced sensitivity to

rule violations but also a tendency to reciprocate, a hostile attribution tendency, and the

inclination to react with anger and hostility) contribute to an enhanced aggressive tendency in

predominantly prevention-focused individuals. Accordingly, we think it is fair to conclude that

finding an interaction effect on the manipulation check (reflecting only one of the discussed

aspects) at a marginal level of significance is not a major limitation in the present context.
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 46

Table 1

Zero-order Correlations between Variables Included in Studies 1a and 1b

Prevention Promotion Aggression Cynical Rumination


Index Hostility
Prevention - .05 .41*** .30** .50***

Promotion .08 - -.06 .14 -.18+

Aggression Index .58*** -.04 - .49*** .53***

Cynical Hostility .47*** .07 .65*** - .18+

Note: Results obtained in Study 1a are shown above the diagonal; results of Study 1b are

depicted below the diagonal. + p < .10; * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001. Rumination was

assessed only in Study 1a.

You might also like