Keller Et Al - 2008 - Prevention-Focused Self-Regulation and Aggressiveness
Keller Et Al - 2008 - Prevention-Focused Self-Regulation and Aggressiveness
Keller Et Al - 2008 - Prevention-Focused Self-Regulation and Aggressiveness
Downloaded from
https://kar.kent.ac.uk/32384/ The University of Kent's Academic Repository KAR
Additional information
Versions of Record
If this version is the version of record, it is the same as the published version available on the publisher's web site.
Cite as the published version.
Enquiries
If you have questions about this document contact [email protected]. Please include the URL of the record
in KAR. If you believe that your, or a third party's rights have been compromised through this document please see
our Take Down policy (available from https://www.kent.ac.uk/guides/kar-the-kent-academic-repository#policies).
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 1
Address correspondence to
Johannes Keller,
Universität Mannheim,
Lehrstuhl für Mikrosoziologie & Sozialpsychologie,
68131 Mannheim, Germany;
[email protected]
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 2
Abstract
The present research examined the relationship between individual differences in self-regulatory
regulation) and aggressiveness. Two studies revealed that the more individuals’ habitual self-
regulatory orientation is dominated by a prevention-focus, the more likely they are to score high
additional study involving the manipulation of perceived violation of a reciprocity norm showed
of a norm violation and reacted in a hostile and aggressive manner following the norm violation
aggressiveness and suggest that endorsement of (negative) reciprocity norms and sensitivity to
norm violations are relevant factors that help explain the differences in aggressiveness observed
regulation
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 3
Hostility, aggression, and violence are the source of a great deal of physical pain and
psychological damage in human societies across the globe. The frequency with which humans
hurt one another is reflected in the fact that it is almost impossible to read through a daily
newspaper or to tune into the evening news without being confronted with instances of hostility,
continue to be are a burden on not only the perpetrators and victims but also the development of
human societies at large. Accordingly, the question of when and why people become aggressive
and motivated to hurt each other can be understood as one of the most important topics in the
important item on the social scientific research agenda. The current paper is devoted to this item
on the agenda. Specifically, we examine the relationship between individuals’ style of self-
regulation and (a) trait level aggressiveness as well as (b) aggressive behavioral tendencies. In
our analysis, we focus on one particular mode of self-regulation that reflects a commitment to
oughts and duties and that can be described as vigilant and prevention-oriented in character.
Recent social psychological research has documented that many fundamental social
cognitive as well as social interactive mechanisms are heavily influenced by the situationally or
processes and behavioral tendencies (for an overview on self-regulation research, see Baumeister
and Vohs, 2004). One specific theoretical perspective figures prominently in the field of research
1997; 1998). RFT holds that two basic modes of self-regulation can be differentiated. In the
prevention mode of self-regulation, individuals strive to achieve safety and security, fulfill their
duties and obligations, and avoid losses. In the promotion mode of self-regulation, individuals are
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 4
guided by the need for nurturance and growth, the desire to reach their ideal goals and
RFT has been applied to an impressively wide spectrum of topics and psychological
phenomena across a diverse array of domains (for an overview, see Higgins and Spiegel, 2004).
The existing evidence shows that a vast number of social cognitive as well as social interactive
phenomena can be related to the basic self-regulatory orientations outlined in RFT. The present
contribution aims to explore the role of RFT with respect to an important social interactive
orientation is associated with aggressive tendencies. Somewhat surprisingly, there is very little
Talley, Benjamin, and Valentine (2006) recently noted in their meta-analytic review on
personality and aggressive behavior that theories of aggression have largely ignored the role of
self-regulation in aggressive behavior. The current research aims to address this theoretical and
empirical gap.
The present contribution also attempts to integrate ideas proposed in RFT and the General
Aggression Model (GAM) (Anderson & Bushman, 2002). Specifically, we start from the basic
assumption that several of the personal and situational input factors proposed in the GAM (i.e.,
variables that increase the probability of aggressive behavior in a given situation) can be
associated with constructs and mechanisms that are relevant for prevention-focused self-
regulation. The three studies reported in the current paper present empirical analyses focusing on
(a) the relationship between distinct personal input factors that share a conceptual association
with aggressive tendencies and prevention-focused self-regulation, and (b) the interaction of one
specific situational input factor, namely experience of a norm violation, with prevention-focused
self-regulation. In the following sections we outline in detail the theoretical underpinnings of the
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 5
Extending the basic hedonic principle that people approach pleasure and avoid pain, RFT
holds that it is necessary to differentiate among distinct types of pleasures and distinct types of
pain, and to assess the specific strategic orientations and types of goal pursuit that reflect self-
regulation guided by two distinct motivational systems – promotion focus and prevention focus.
Self-regulation with a promotion focus is characterized as the motivation to attain growth and
nurturance, bring one’s actual self into alignment with one’s ideal self, and reach gains (and
avoid non-gains). In contrast, self-regulation with a prevention focus entails the motivation to
attain security, bring one’s actual self into alignment with one’s ought self (i.e., fulfilling one’s
Research on RFT (see Higgins, 1998; Higgins & Spiegel, 2004 for reviews) shows that
both types of regulatory orientations are related to specific consequences. The psychological
absence of positive outcomes, (b) application of eager strategic means, (c) ambitious and keen
striving to reach ones aspirations as reflected in tenacious goal pursuit that is focused on maximal
goals, and (d) cheerfulness-dejection emotions in response to positive and negative events. In
sensitivity to the presence or absence of negative outcomes, (b) application of vigilant strategic
means, (c) a risk-averse and defensive orientation in the pursuit of minimal goals, and (d)
quiescence-agitation emotions (i.e., relaxation versus worry and anxiety) in response to positive
and negative events. In our theoretical considerations, we focus on the consequences regarding
differential sensitivity, specifically on the sensitivity to negative outcomes and cues associated
According to RFT, the two self-regulatory systems can be situationally induced (e.g.,
Freitas, Liberman, & Higgins, 2002; Friedman & Förster, 2001; Shah, Higgins, & Friedman,
1998). However, and most important in the present context, RFT posits that individuals may
assess individual differences in self-regulatory orientation have been developed (e.g., regulatory
focus questionnaires, cf. Higgins et al; 2001; Keller, 2007; Lockwood, Jordan, & Kunda, 2002;
regulatory strength measures, cf. Shah, Higgins, & Friedman, 1998). It is important to note that
the two modes of self-regulation have been conceptualized as independent constructs. In line
with this proposition, measures of the two chronic regulatory foci have been found to be largely
uncorrelated or slightly positively correlated (cf. Higgins et al., 2001; Lockwood, et al., 2002;
Keller, 2007). Thus, prevention-focused self-regulation does not represent the opposite pole of
promotion-focused self-regulation. This implies that it is possible that one of the two modes is
associated with a certain psychological phenomenon, while the other mode is not. The present
by integrating ideas of RFT and the GAM, which we will describe next.
The theoretical framework underlying the GAM (Anderson, 1997; Anderson & Bushman,
2002; Anderson, Deuser, & DeNeve, 1995) is based on a distinction of person and situational
variables as input factors contributing to aggressive tendencies. According to the model, these
input factors exert their impact on appraisal and decision processes (i.e., the outcomes of episodic
aggression processes) via cognitive, affective, and arousal routes. The person factors discussed in
the GAM include traits (e.g., susceptibility towards hostile attributions, unstable/labile self-
esteem), sex, beliefs that are related to a preparedness to aggress (e.g., self-efficacy beliefs),
attitudes (towards violence), values (e.g. normative convictions about what one should or ought
to do; e.g., reciprocity beliefs), long-term goals, and the availability and accessibility of
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 7
behavioral scripts. The situation factors discussed in the GAM include aggressive cues (e.g.,
weapons), provocation and frustration (insults, goal interference, perceived injustice), pain and
discomfort (e.g., heat, noise, unpleasant odors), drugs (e.g., alcohol, caffeine), and incentives
(perceived cost/benefit ratio). The present research was designed to assess how several of these
personal and situational input variables discussed in the GAM relate to the distinct self-
Note that most of the recent research in the context of the GAM focused on the routes to
aggression and how cognitive mechanisms (i.e., the role of knowledge structures and knowledge
accessibility) contribute to aggressive behaviors (cf. Anderson, 1997; Anderson & Dill, 2000;
Bushman, 1995). The present approach contributes to existing research on the GAM by focusing
on how motivational (self-regulatory) mechanisms relate to specific input factors proposed in the
GAM.
There is good reason to expect that self-regulatory mechanisms can help us understand the
underlying factors and processes of human aggression. In the section below, we describe the
and aggressiveness.
As outlined above, the GAM proposes distinct personal and situational variables as input
factors contributing to aggressive tendencies. The core assumption underlying the present
research holds that self-regulatory orientations are meaningfully related to several of these input
to personal factors that have been shown to be associated with increased aggressive tendencies
(e.g., hostile attribution bias, hostility, labile-self-esteem, neuroticism, rumination, distrust1), and
(b) to situational factors that have been documented to trigger aggressive reactions in individuals
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 8
(e.g., observing other people’s norm violation; experiencing injustice). Accordingly, we propose
of self-regulation involves several aspects that are known input factors of aggressive tendencies -
At first sight, this assumption may appear paradoxical given that prevention-focused self-
regulation is typically characterized by a fear of doing the wrong thing associated with a risk-
averse and careful behavioral orientation. Note, however, that the prevention-focus is not
necessarily related to risk-averse avoidance and flight tendencies (cf. Scholer, Stroessner, &
Higgins, in press). Specifically, under circumstances of threat and negative experiences (such as
rule violations) an active response strategy reflecting a fighting spirit can serve the basic
prevention-focused goal of reaching safety and security. That is, protecting and maintaining
safety and security sometimes can require offensive acts reflecting a fight (rather than flight)
with ought standards (such as rules and normative standards, e.g., the reciprocity norm) is most
likely associated with a tendency to punish those who violate relevant normative standards in the
attempt to maintain the normative status quo. In combination, several aspects of prevention-
focused self-regulation suggest that this style of self-regulation may be related to aggressive
tendencies.2 In the sections below we outline the relation between prevention-focused self-
regulation and prominent personal and situational input factors proposed in the GAM which
support the assumed positive relation between the prevention-focus and aggressiveness.
are likely to (1) score high on measures of neuroticism, (2) show hostile attribution biases, (3)
have labile self-esteem, (4) engage in ruminative thinking, (5) endorse (negative) reciprocity
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 9
norms, and (6) be chronically skeptical, cynical (distrustful), and hostile. One underlying reason
for these assumptions is the relationship between punitive parenting styles and a chronic
prevention-focus which has been empirically supported by recent research findings (Keller, in
press; Manian, Strauman, & Denny, 1998). Given that punitive parenting styles have also been
shown to be related to several critical personal factors that render individuals prone to aggressive
tendencies (cynical hostility, cf. Sarason, Ganzer, & Granger, 1965; hostile attribution bias, cf.
Gomez & Gomez, 2000; Gomez, Gomez, DeMello, & Talent, 2001; labile self-esteem, cf.
Kernis, Brown, & Brody, 2000), it seems plausible to assume a relationship between these
personal factors and the prevention-focus. Moreover, neuroticism (a known factor in the
conceptually closely related to the emotional component of the prevention-focus (worry and
anxiety; cf. Higgins, 1998), which suggests a close association between prevention-focused self-
Recent findings obtained in our lab indicate a reliable association between the prevention-
focus and the distinct personality traits mentioned above. Specifically, it emerged that the
hostility (Keller, 2007; Keller et al., 2007). These empirically documented associations between
several of the proposed personal input factors of aggression and the prevention-focus suggest a
positive relationship between this style of regulatory focus and aggressive tendencies.
has been proposed – based on the differential sensitivity assumption outlined earlier – that the
special sensitivity of prevention-focused individuals to negative events and cues may make those
individuals particularly prone to act and react aggressively. That is, higher perceptual sensitivity
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 10
and sensibility towards negative events and cues may render predominantly prevention-focused
individuals particularly “vulnerable” to such cues, making them more likely to display aggressive
reactions and tendencies. Note that it seems plausible to assume that the particular sensitivity
and attentional focus of prevention-focused individuals to negative events and cues may - in the
long run - result in a cynical and skeptical perspective on the social world in general, which may
be described as a misanthropic and hostile attitude. And since hostility represents one crucial
aspect underlying aggression and aggressiveness, the degree to which an individual endorses
such a hostile and misanthropic attitude is most probably related to the individual’s level of
aggressiveness.
Overall, the analysis outlined above leads to the hypothesis that there is a significant
point, we tested this basic assumption in Studies 1 and 2, where we assessed the relationship
between self-report measures of regulatory focus and self-report measures of (a) aggression
(Buss & Perry, 1992) and (b) cynical hostility (Cook & Medley, 1954), as well as measures of
other constructs that represent established correlates of aggressive tendencies (rumination and
neuroticism). Study 2 also included measures of reciprocity norm endorsement to test the
assumption that beliefs reflecting reciprocity norm endorsement (particularly negative reciprocity
norm endorsement) are related to aggressiveness. Previous research has shown that negative
perspective regarding other people (Eisenberger et al., 2004). These findings suggest a
because a prevention-focus implies a concern with duties and responsibilities reflecting a concern
with norms and normative standards, it seems plausible to assume that reciprocity norm
elaborate discussion of this aspect is presented in the introduction to Study 2 below). Finally,
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 11
Study 1
The goal of Study 1 was to assess the relationship between chronic regulatory focus
Two independent samples of students at the University of Mannheim (Study 1a: N = 90;
Mage = 23.4; 39 women) and the University of Michigan (Study 1b: N = 38; Mage = 23.0; 27
women) completed measures designed to assess habitual levels of aggressiveness and regulatory
Study 1a only. In both studies, responses were assessed using 7-point response scales; the only
exception was the Buss and Perry (1992) aggression questionnaire used in Study 1b, where 5-
point response scales were used (in Study 1a we used 7-point scales for this instrument as well).
Unless indicated otherwise, all scale endpoints were labeled not at all true and completely true.
Measures
Agressiveness. We used the Buss and Perry (1992) aggression questionnaire to assess trait
aggressiveness. Sample items of the three subscales used in the present studies read: “When
frustrated, I let my irritation show” (anger subscale); “When people annoy me, I may tell them
what I think of them” (verbal aggression subscale); and “When people are especially nice, I
wonder what they want” (hostility subscale). We used the overall aggressiveness scores in the
focus scale developed by Lockwood et al. (2002) which consists of a prevention focus subscale
(9-items, α = .76 [Study 1a] and α =.86 [Study 1b], sample item: “In general, I am focused on
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 12
preventing negative events in my life”) and a promotion focus subscale (9-items, α = .81 [Study
1a] and α =.84 [Study 1b], sample item: “I frequently imagine how I will achieve my hopes and
aspirations”).
Cynical hostility. We also assessed cynical hostility (Cook & Medley, 1954) as an
aggressiveness-related measure relevant to the present context. Sample items read: “Most people
will use somewhat unfair means to gain profit or an advantage rather than to lose it,” and “No
one cares much what happens to you” (assessed with dichotomous true / false response options; α
ruminative thinking, which reflects an attentional focus on negative events and negative aspects
in life in general and, with a short version of the rumination scale used by Fresco, Frankel,
Mennin, Turk, and Heimberg (2002; adapted from Nolen-Hoeksema, Morrow, & Fredrickson,
1991). Participants completed this 9-item measure with response scales ranging from 1 (almost
never) to 7 (almost always) after reading the following lead-in statement: “Please indicate how
often you think and act the way described in the respective statement.” Sample items are: “Think
about all your shortcomings, failings, faults, and mistakes,” and “Think about how sad you feel”
(α = .91). This measure was included as an indicator of the differential sensitivity mechanism
negative aspects in life. Moreover, since previous research has revealed that ruminative thinking
is related to aggressiveness (e.g., Bushman, 2002; Bushman, Bonacci, Pedersen, Vasquesz, &
Miller, 2005; Verona, 2005), this construct was expected to function as an underlying factor in
zero-order correlations (see Table 1) as well as regression analyses. In Studies 1a and 1b, the
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 13
prevention focus scale was significantly positively correlated with the trait aggressiveness scale,
a finding supportive of the proposed association between these constructs. The promotion focus
scale did not correlate with aggressiveness. In a finding supportive of the differential sensitivity
assumption, the prevention-focus scale was positively and robustly correlated with rumination.
Regression analyses with aggressiveness as the criterion variable controlling for promotion
(Model 1 in Table 2) and cynical hostility scores (Model 2 in Table 2) reveal that controlling for
relationship drops substantially, but remains significant, when cynical hostility is controlled for.
This suggests that it is not only the tendency to perceive the social world from a cynical
perspective that contributes to the relationship between prevention-focus and aggressiveness and
that there must be some other processes or mechanisms that contribute to this link.
To test the potential role of ruminative thinking in the link between the prevention-focus
and aggressiveness, we entered ruminative thinking scores into the regression model (for Study
significant predictor of aggressiveness when ruminative thinking scores are controlled for. Thus,
negative cynical thinking and rumination appear to be important underlying mechanisms in the
link between the prevention-focus and aggressiveness, with rumination being a more powerful
(in the USA and Germany) with different cultural backgrounds. Moreover, we found that
negative thinking (as reflected in cynical hostility and rumination) seems to be a process
validity and reliability of the obtained findings, we conducted Study 2 to gather further empirical
evidence.
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 14
Study 2
Study 2 was designed to test the robustness of the link between the prevention-focus and
Study 2 examined the role of neuroticism and reciprocity norm beliefs in the link between the
Neuroticism reflects the emotional component of prevention-focus (cf. Higgins, 1998) and
has been found to relate to aggressiveness (cf. Sharpe & Desai, 2001; von Collani & Werner,
2005). Thus, neuroticism represents a construct with the potential to explain the link between the
underlie the relationship between prevention-focus and aggressiveness in Study 1, has been
established as a close correlate of neuroticism and anxiety (cf. Kuyken, Watkins, Holden, &
Cook, 2006; Nolen-Hoeksema, 2000; Roberts, Gilboa, & Gotlib, 1998; Trapnell & Campbell,
1999). Thus, existing evidence suggests that neuroticism may contribute to the link between the
In addition to exploring the role of neuroticism in the link between the prevention-focus
and aggressiveness, Study 2 also examined the role of reciprocity norm beliefs in this link with a
particular focus on negative reciprocity. The reciprocity norm prescribes that one should return
favors and retaliate against infringements (Gouldner, 1960). In other words, it refers to the
obligation to help those who helped us in the past and to retaliate against those who have been
detrimental to our interests and well-being (Perugini et al., 2003). This conceptualization reveals
that reciprocity can be positive or negative. The positive reciprocity norm deals with the
obligation to repay favorable treatment; the negative reciprocity norm deals with retribution as a
response to unfavorable treatment (Goudner, 1960). Reciprocity norm beliefs refer to the
internalization of these reciprocity norms, with negative reciprocity beliefs referring to the
internalization of the negative reciprocity norm and positive reciprocity beliefs referring to the
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 15
internalization of the positive reciprocity norm. As with any norm endorsement, individuals
differ in the extent to which they hold these beliefs (Eisenberger et al., 2004; Perugini et al.,
2003).
Examining the role of reciprocity norm beliefs seems to be promising in the present context
preoccupation with duties and responsibilities, reflecting a concern with norms and normative
appropriateness of others’ responses (i.e., whether one’s actions are reciprocated in line with a
general reciprocity norm). In the most general terms, reciprocity norm endorsement should be
is on negative or positive forms of reciprocity. Given the focus of the current research on
aggressiveness and its relationship to the prevention-focus and the existing evidence on the link
between negative reciprocity beliefs and aggressiveness (and related constructs; cf. Eisenberger,
et al., 2004; Eder et al., 2006), we are particularly interested in the role of negative reciprocity.
Reciprocating negative behavior displayed by others can be seen as reflecting the motivation to
attain security and to avoid (or minimize) losses - which is particularly relevant to prevention-
focused individuals. Specifically, retaliating against the harmful behavior of others may help
individuals to protect themselves from further hurtful behavior in the future, and may be
experienced and construed as a fair and normatively appropriate compensation for losses or
that negative reciprocity behavior involves sensitivity to negative interpersonal events, which
the endorsement of reciprocity beliefs in general (and negative reciprocity in particular) is related
to prevention-focused self-regulation.
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 16
In addition, there is reason to assume that the endorsement of negative reciprocity beliefs
could be related to aggressiveness. From a conceptual perspective, it seems evident that negative
reciprocity should be closely related to reactive aggression – which can be defined as aggressive
Harnish, Bates, & Pettit, 1997; Miller & Lynam, 2006). Thus, reactive aggression can in some
sense be understood as the behavioral expression of negative reciprocity. Hence, it seems likely
that individuals who strongly endorse negative reciprocity beliefs are more likely to engage in
acts reflecting reactive aggression. From an empirical perspective, previous research has shown
that negative reciprocity norm endorsement is significantly related to anger, vengeance, and a
cynical perspective regarding other people. For example, using self-report and behavioral
measures, Eisenberger et al. (2004) showed that negative reciprocity norm beliefs are strongly
related to anger in everyday life and anger following mistreatment by others. Accordingly, it
seems plausible to assume that negative reciprocity norm endorsement is meaningfully related to
aggressiveness.
reciprocity norms in the behavioral domain is positively associated with reciprocation wariness,
that is, cautiousness in reciprocating help, resulting from a fear of being taken advantage of, and
negatively associated with proneness to forgive and with emotional stability (the opposite of
neuroticism). Perugini et al. (2003) further showed that individuals who strongly endorse
negative reciprocity beliefs are less generous and more likely to engage in punishments in the
face of negative behavior. Moreover, Eder and colleagues (2006) found that individuals who
endorse the negative reciprocity norm are particularly likely to see retaliation as a proper
response to wrongdoing.
The correlates of negative reciprocity norm endorsement that reflect its relationship with
anger, retaliation, and aggressiveness suggest that this construct may be another important
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 17
aggressiveness. Study 2 was designed to test the potential role of this construct in addition to the
aggression (Bettencourt et al., 2006; Sharpe & Desai, 2001; von Collani & Werner, 2005).
Method
questionnaire that consisted of two self-regulatory measures and scales that assessed
Measures
Regulatory focus. Participants completed the same regulatory focus measure used in Study
1 (Lockwood et al., 2002) (αPromotion= .83; αPrevention = .78), as well as an alternative instrument
recently developed and validated in our lab to assess self-regulatory focus (Keller, 2007) that
comprises a prevention subscale (α = .83, sample item: “My life is often shaped by fear of failure
and negative events”), and a promotion subscale (α = .76, sample item: “If I know that my
performance is being evaluated by other people, that spurs me on and increases my ambition to
do well”). Replicating previous findings (reported in Keller, 2007), the congruent subscales of
the two regulatory focus measures (i.e., the Lockwood et al. and the Keller measure) were
significantly correlated (prevention subscales: r = .65, p < .001; promotion subscales: r = .43, p <
.001), whereas the correlations involving incongruent dimensions of self-regulation were not
Aggressiveness. As in Study 1, we used the Buss and Perry (1992) aggression measure to
assess trait aggressiveness (in this case, we assessed the physical aggression subscale as well).
Parallel to Study 1, the overall aggressiveness score (α = .90) was used in the analyses reported
below.
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 18
Cynical hostility. The same measure as in Study 1 (Cook & Medley, 1954) was used to
Reciprocity norm beliefs. We used two measures to assess reciprocity norm beliefs. One of
these measures was developed by Eisenberger et al. (2004) and consists of two subscales that
assess positive reciprocity norm beliefs (10 items, sample item: “If someone does me a favor, I
feel obligated to repay them in some way”) and negative reciprocity norm beliefs (14 items,
sample item: “If someone dislikes you, you should dislike them”). We used short versions of
both subscales (each scale comprising 7-items; αnegative reciprocity =. 87, αpositive reciprocity =. 81). (Note:
negative reciprocity norm as assessed by this measure refers to advisability of retribution for
unfavorable treatment).
The second measure of reciprocity norm beliefs was developed by Perugini and colleagues
(2003) and consists of three subscales with each subscale comprising 9 items. A sample item of
the beliefs in reciprocity subscale reads: “If I work hard, I expect it will be repaid” (α = .77). A
sample item of the positive reciprocity subscale reads: “I’m ready to do a boring job to return
someone’s previous help” (α = .74). A sample item of the negative reciprocity subscale reads: “If
somebody is rude to me, I become rude” (α = .86). (Note: negative reciprocity as assessed by
this measure refers to negative reciprocity as expressed in behaviors). In the analyses reported
below, we used a composite score based on the two negative reciprocity norm subscales in order
to have both the cognitive and behavioral aspects of negative reciprocity represented in the
measure (α = .93). Since both scales were highly correlated (r = .78), use of a composite score
was also appropriate in order to avoid analyses that could potentially suffer from problems of
multicollinearity.
In line with our theoretical analysis and the findings obtained in Study 1, data revealed
significant zero-order correlations between both prevention scales and aggressiveness scores (see
Table 3). Both prevention scale scores were also significantly positively correlated with scores of
cynical hostility, replicating the pattern observed in Study 1. As in Study 1, neither promotion
Reciprocity norm endorsement scores as measured by two different scales were positively
associated with prevention focus scores, with the exception that prevention focus scores on the
Lockwood et al. scale were not reliably associated with negative reciprocity as measured by
Eisenberger et al. (2004). Replicating previous research, reciprocity beliefs were also positively
related to scores on aggressiveness and cynical hostility, with the exception of positive
In keeping with the analysis conducted in Study 1, we ran initial analyses including promotion
and prevention scale scores as predictors (see Table 4; analyses are reported separately for the
Lockwood et al. and the Keller scales). Parallel to Study 1 we found that prevention but not
promotion scale scores were significantly related to aggressiveness (Model 1). Next, we tested
whether this relation could be explained by negative reciprocity norm beliefs. The analysis
revealed that controlling for negative reciprocity norm beliefs (composite measure) markedly
reduced the strength of the relationship between prevention and aggressiveness, indicating that
negative reciprocity norm beliefs are in part involved in the relationship between prevention-
focused self-regulation and aggressiveness (Model 2).3 We then added cynical hostility into the
regression model. As depicted in Table 4 (Model 3), including cynical hostility scores resulted in
replicating the pattern observed in Study 1. Finally, we added neuroticism (Model 4) and found
that prevention focus scores are no longer a significant predictor in the regression models when
negative reciprocity beliefs, cynical hostility, and neuroticism are controlled for. That is, our
findings show that negative reciprocity beliefs, cynical hostility, and neuroticism contribute to
A critical reader may come to the conclusion that the relation between prevention-focused
self-regulation and aggressiveness may be reducible to the correlation between neuroticism and
neuroticism is only a marginally significant predictor in one of the two regression models in
Study 2 (see Model 4, right panel in Table 4). It seems not adequate to argue that the association
neuroticism when the latter factor is not even a reliable predictor in the model. Second, partial
correlations between the two prevention focus scales and aggressiveness remain marginally
significant when controlling for neuroticism scores (r = .19, p < .09 and r = .18, p < .10,
respectively). Third, neuroticism is only one among several distinct elements in the
conceptualization of the prevention-focus (reflecting the affective component of this style of self-
regulation; cf. Higgins, 1998) and it would be misleading to argue that prevention-focused self-
regulation can be reduced to neuroticism. Supporting the distinction between neuroticism and
prevention-focused self-regulation, other lines of research in our lab revealed that the relations
trust (Keller et al. 2007) and value orientations (Keller, 2007) – remain robust and hardly differ
from zero-order correlations when neuroticism is controlled for. Finally, it is important to note
that - in contrast to the prevention focus scales - neuroticism was not significantly related to
negative reciprocity norm endorsement (see Table 3), a factor that is of critical relevance with
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 21
Summarizing the results of Studies 1 and 2, one can say that the obtained findings concur
with the proposition that there is a significant positive relationship between prevention-focused
self-regulation and aggressiveness. Importantly, findings are robust across different samples and
across measures of regulatory focus. Several aspects are particularly noteworthy with respect to
the findings obtained in Study 2. First, replicating the pattern observed in Study 1, we observed
that cynical hostility partially contributes to the link between prevention focus and
aggressiveness; however, it does not fully mediate this link. Second, endorsement of negative
reciprocity norm beliefs is found to be a partial mediator of the relationship between prevention-
focused self-regulation and aggressiveness. Thus, sensitivity to normative standards and the
expectation to reciprocate others’ negative behavior in part explains the relationship between
prevention focus and aggressiveness. Third, the neuroticism findings indicate that the emotional
aspect of prevention-focused self-regulation that involves worry and anxiety also contributes to
in line with previous results (cf. Bettencourt et al. 2006; Sharpe & Desai, 2001; von Collani &
Werner, 2005) documenting a prominent role of neuroticism in the development and expression
of aggression. In sum, these findings add to our understanding of the underlying personality
and aggressiveness.
evidence pointing to the fact that personality characteristics related to prevention-focused self-
regulation - such as cynicism, distrust as well as anxiety and neuroticism - shifted to higher levels
during recent decades (Kanter & Mirvis, 1989, Twenge, 2000). From our perspective, the upward
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 22
shift in these critical personality characteristics across birth cohorts is particularly problematic in
light of the fact that these characteristics are correlated with hostility and aggressiveness.
Up to this point, our empirical analyses have focused on personality factors and the role
However, as outlined in the theoretical introduction, we suppose that several situational input
assume – based on the differential sensitivity mechanism proposed in RFT – that prevention-
focused self-regulation is related to a special sensitivity to negative aversive cues in general, and
to one aversive cue that is known to increase aggressiveness, in particular: the experience or
individuals. This assumption is based on two insights. First, according to RFT, prevention-
experiencing a violation of the reciprocity norm represents an aversive and hence negative cue, it
stands to reason that prevention-focused individuals may notice and react particularly strongly
and aggressively to this cue. Second, the results of Study 2 revealed that (a) prevention-focused
self-regulation is associated with the endorsement of (negative) reciprocity beliefs, and (b) the
combination, these findings lead to the hypothesis that prevention-focused individuals should be
norm violations and with a particularly strong tendency to interpret norm violations as the
outcome of intentional actions (reflecting a hostile attribution bias). Moreover, we assume that
react in a hostile and aggressive manner in situations where they are confronted with a norm
violation
among prevention-focused individuals. Thus, the study was designed to answer the question
causal chain as recently discussed by Spencer, Zanna, and Fong (2005) who noted that a
reasonable strategy to document a causal chain in the attempt to understand the underlying
proposed underlying factor (as a complementary strategy to the often applied mediational
Study 3
The considerations outlined above were tested in an experimental study involving the
manipulation of a violation of the reciprocity norm and the assessment of participants’ perception
chronic self-regulatory orientation. Aggressiveness was measured using both direct and displaced
Method
participated in this study and received monetary compensation (2 Euro plus extra money
depending on their response in the experimental setting, see description below). They were
randomly assigned to one of two experimental conditions. Four participants were excluded
because they did not follow the experimental procedures adequately, leaving a sample size of 73
When participants arrived at the lab, a confederate of the experimenter appeared and served
as the ostensible other participant so that participants were made to believe that there was another
participant present with whom they would later be interacting. They were told that the other
participant would be sitting in an adjacent room and completing the same material. Once seated
in their cubicle, participants received a questionnaire that contained the measure of chronic
regulatory focus used in Study 1 (Lockwood et al.’s scales; αPromotion= .81; αPrevention = .76)
participants received new materials from the experimenter and first read the descriptions
involving the cover story for the manipulation of norm violation. The cover story explained that
the researchers were interested in individuals’ reactions in social interactions involving decision-
We chose the trust game interaction paradigm that was developed by Berg, Dickhaut, and
McCabe (1995) which has been frequently used in game theoretical analyses of trust (e.g.,
Bohnet & Zeckhauser, 2004; Buchan & Croson, 2004; Kosfeld, Heinrichs, Zak, Fischbacher, &
Fehr, 2005). In this experimental game, participants initially receive a certain amount of money
and learn that they can transfer any amount of this money to a second player and that this amount
will be tripled before the other player actually receives the transfer amount. At this point, the
other player (who receives the money) is free to decide whether or not to reciprocate by sending
back a certain amount of money. In an absolute trust relationship, participants who are in the
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 25
position to initiate the money transfer would decide to send the full amount to the interaction
partner, and the person receiving the money would send back half of the amount he or she
received, resulting in the best possible joint outcome for the players.
experienced in the paradigm (with a low level of reciprocity reflecting a violation of the
reciprocity norm). Each participant initially received a payment of 2 Euros (in form of ten 20
Cent coins) for showing up. Participants then learned that they were randomly assigned either to
the role of a money transfer initiator or the role of a transfer receiver (in reality, all participants
were assigned to the role of money transfer initiator). Participants in the role of a money transfer
initiator had the option to transfer any amount between 0.20 and 2 Euro to another person (the
receiver), who would then decide how much money he or she wanted to send back. It was
further explained that the amount of money the initiator was willing to transfer (between 0.20 and
2 Euro) would be tripled. That is, if the initiator decided to transfer 2 Euro, the receiver would
get 6 Euro and could then decide how much of this amount he or she was willing to send back to
the initiator. Participants also learned that the transfer receiver could select an amount varying in
steps of 0.05 Euro (this information was necessary given that the amount of money they received
in the back transfer could represent 160%, - see below - which required that the interaction
partners have the option of using coins of lesser value). Moreover, participants were told that the
individuals involved in the interaction would remain strictly separated and had no chance of
Following the detailed description of the scenario, participants were asked to decide on the
amount of money they would be willing to transfer to the receiver and to put the respective
amount in an envelope provided by the experimenter, who then ostensibly brought the envelope
to the transfer receiver in the adjacent room. In reality, the experimenter went to the other room,
checked the amount of money the participant had put in the envelope, and selected one of several
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 26
previously prepared envelopes containing the amount of money that either reflected a repayment
of 100% (for participants randomly assigned to the norm violation condition) or a repayment of
160% (for participants randomly assigned to the control condition). The amount of repayment
was based on the findings of a pre-test which revealed that among students at the University of
Mannheim, a repayment of 155% or higher in the trust game paradigm is perceived as fair and
appropriate in terms of the reciprocity norm. The experimenter then returned to the participant
with the selected envelope and an additional questionnaire containing the measures of dependent
variables.
Materials
Manipulation check. First, in order to make sure that all participants had opened the
envelope before responding to the dependent measures, we asked them to indicate the amount of
money they had received as repayment from their interaction partner. All participants indicated
the correct amount of repayment. Next, participants responded to three items designed to assess
the perceived fairness of the repayment they had received, which served as a manipulation check
of the norm violation manipulation. The fairness items read: “I felt that the return transfer from
my interaction partner was fair;” “The return transfer from my interaction partner deviated
strongly from what one could expect as a fair return transfer in terms of reciprocity;” “I feel that
the behavior of my interaction partner was unfair” (the latter two items were reverse coded, 1
[not at all true] to 7 [completely true]). A fairness index was computed averaging across the
items (α = .94). After responding to the manipulation check questions, participants proceeded to
complete the dependent measures. All measures described below were assessed on 7-point scales
State Anger. To assess affective reactions to the money exchange interaction, we included
four items designed to assess anger. Sample items read: “The behavior of my interaction partner
on this task made me angry;” “My interaction partner made me angry because he did not behave
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 27
fairly in terms of reciprocity.” We computed an anger index averaging across the four items (α =
.92).
partners’ behavior to mean intentions was assessed using three items that read: “I am certain that
my interaction partner deliberately behaved this way so as to provoke me;” “My interaction
partner deliberately behaved this way to harm me;” and, “I think that my interaction partner
transferred this amount without intending me any ill will” (the latter item is reverse coded). We
computed a hostile attribution index averaging across the three items (α = .76).
State aggression. In line with previous work (Farrar & Krcmar, 2006), we used a state
version of the Buss and Perry (1992) aggression questionnaire to assess state aggression.
Specifically, we modified the original trait items in such a way that the statements referred to
participants’ ostensible interaction partner and the current situation in the experimental session.
As a lead-in-statement to the state aggression items, participants read: “Now imagine that upon
leaving the room you happen to run into your interaction partner, who is holding the money he
received in his hand and is laughing. Please read the following statements carefully and indicate
to what extent these statements apply to your personally in this situation.“ Participants
responded to 20 state aggression items that measured state verbal aggression (sample item: “I
would tell this person directly what I thought of his or her behavior”), state physical aggression
(sample item: “This person could drive me to the point where we would get into a physical
confrontation”), state anger (sample item: “In this situation I would feel like a powder keg about
to explode”), and state hostility (sample item: “In this situation I would definitely think that life
is treating me unfairly”). We computed a mean state aggression index averaging across the 20
items (α = .95).
assess displaced aggressive tendencies, we asked participants in the concluding part of the study
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 28
to select 10 pictures out of a set of 30 that would ostensibly be given later to participants in an
unrelated future study conducted by the same research group. The 30 pictures were selected
from the International Affective Picture System (Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention,
1995). These pictures have been pre-tested for their valence using rating scales ranging from 1
(negative) to 9 (positive). The set of 30 pictures used in the present study comprised 10 negative
pictures with ratings below 4 (e.g., a picture of a rotting animal corpse), 10 positive pictures with
ratings above 6 (e.g., a picture of three puppies), and 10 neutral pictures with ratings between 4
and 6 (e.g., a picture of a book). Participants received a poster where all 30 pictures could be
seen simultaneously, as well as a pile of the 30 pictures as separate photographs. They were
instructed to put the 10 selected photographs from the pile in an envelope that was provided by
the experimenter. As cover story, participants learned that the research group was conducting
studies in another field testing mechanisms of visual perception in social context, and that the
participants in the respective studies were to receive visual stimulus materials. Moreover,
participants were told that it was of great importance to ensure that the materials used in these
studies represented a wide variety of stimuli and that it was important that they select the stimuli
rather than the researchers, because researchers who were familiar with the study goals could
introduce bias in the selection of the pictures. In other words, participants were lead to believe
that their help was required to develop an unbiased set of stimuli for future studies. We used the
mean valence of the 10 selected pictures as a measure of displaced aggression (lower scores
aggressive tendencies.
In preparing the regression analyses designed to test the proposed moderation hypothesis,
we first centered the prevention and promotion focus scores, standardized the norm violation
variable by coding it as +1 (for the norm violation condition) and -1 (for the control condition),
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 29
and computed the interaction term by multiplying the centered prevention as well as the centered
promotion scores with the norm violation variable. Results of regression analyses are reported in
Table 5 separately for the manipulation check and each criterion variable.
Manipulation Check
In an initial analysis we checked the effectiveness of the norm violation manipulation and
regressed responses to the perceived fairness items onto prevention and promotion focus scores,
the categorical variable of norm violation and their interaction terms. The analysis revealed the
expected norm violation main effect (perceived fairness was significantly higher in the control
condition compared to the norm violation condition), as well as a main effect of habitual
perceived fairness). The interaction term did not reach the conventional level of significance;
however the negative interactive effect at trend level is in line with the hypothesis that the norm
violation is perceived as more unfair, the more prevention-focused participants are in their self-
regulatory orientation.4
Next, we separately analyzed the main dependent measures by regressing them on the same
interaction effect between prevention focus and norm violation for all criterion variables (anger,
hostile attribution, state aggression, and displaced aggression; significant main effects for norm
violation and for prevention focus scores emerged in the analyses focusing on anger, hostile
attributions, and state aggression). The findings are graphically displayed in Figure 1 (based on a
median split on the prevention scale scores). As is evident, scores on measures of anger, hostile
attributions, and state aggression are significantly higher in the norm violation condition
compared to the control condition, and, most importantly, this effect is particularly strong in
emerged on the displaced aggression measure (which is reverse scored: higher scores reflect
selection of pictures with more positive valence) – that is, the norm violation resulted in a
negative/aversive pictures. This finding is particularly noteworthy, because the picture selection
The findings obtained in this experimental study suggest that prevention-focused self-
regulation is associated with a particularly strong tendency to react angrily and aggressively in
most sensitive regarding norm violations. Moreover, such individuals are particularly likely to
attribute hostile intentions and to react with displaced aggressive tendencies following the
experience of a violation of the reciprocity norm. In combination, the results suggest that
sensitivity to norm violation can be understood as a critical factor that contributes to prevention-
focused individuals’ higher level of aggressiveness. The fact that individuals who engage in
extreme acts of violence – for example, individuals involved in homicidal rampages (such as the
Virgina Tech massacre or the Columbine high school shooting) – are often found to see
themselves as victims of unfair treatment (i.e., norm violations) suggests that the findings
observed in our experimental study reflect one aspect of the mechanisms that are most likely
General Discussion
mode of self-regulation is associated with trait aggressiveness and to behave aggressively. The
obtained findings strongly support this hypothesis. Across several studies, participants
consistently scored higher on measures of aggressiveness, the more they endorsed a prevention-
assess individual differences in regulatory focus and also different measures to assess trait
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 31
aggressiveness and the tendencies to act and react aggressively. The proposed relationship was
observed irrespective of the type of measurement applied to assess the relevant constructs (i.e.,
chronic regulatory focus and aggressiveness), and it emerged in different cultural milieus (USA
and Germany).
Going beyond the first generation of research (Zanna & Fazio, 1982) concerning the
aggressiveness can be observed (which can be answered affirmatively based on the results
obtained in Study 1 and 2), we also addressed the second-generation question focusing on
underlying mechanisms. In this respect, we found evidence supporting the notion that specific
neuroticism and endorsement of negative reciprocity norm beliefs play an important role. We
also obtained experimental evidence supporting the notion that differential sensitivity to norm
and aggressiveness. Overall, findings suggest that negative thinking and sensitivity to norm
violations play a crucial role and contribute to individual differences between prevention- and
We acknowledge the fact that we are not in a position to draw conclusions regarding the
causality of the observed relations and the mechanisms underlying the relation between
underway in our lab) to test the causality and the underlying mechanisms of the relation between
The reported research findings are innovative in several respects. First, in relating RFT to
important social interactive phenomenon. So far, the role of self-regulatory mechanisms has
been largely neglected in the analysis of aggressiveness (cf. Bettencourt et al., 2006), and the
present studies help to fill in this gap. Second, we specifically put the differential sensitivity
assumption entailed in RFT to a test in the context of aggressiveness and obtained strong
particularly sensitive to the violation of a norm, and this suggests that prevention-focused
individuals are particularly sensitive to and vigilant about normative standards. Since this aspect
of prevention-focused self-regulation has not been documented before, the current findings
extend our knowledge on the specific mechanisms characteristic of this distinct self-regulatory
mode.
displaced aggression in a context where a norm violation was rendered salient (Study 3) is a
novel and intriguing finding, which provides evidence for the role of self-regulatory mechanisms
in the field of displaced (as compared to direct reactive) aggressive behavior. Our findings
indicate that prevention-focused individuals are likely to show displaced aggression the more
they feel that relevant norms are violated in a social context. This might contribute to our
reflecting norm violations (e.g., students who suffer from bullying; individuals who feel deprived
Finally, the finding that the violation of the reciprocity norm triggers aggression reflects an
innovative aspect of the current research, because this specific aversive input factor has not been
systematically assessed in the context of RFT or the context of aggression. The findings provide
evidence supporting the notion that experiencing a violation of the reciprocity norm is a powerful
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 33
situational input factor triggering aggressive responses, and that this is particularly true for
violations (as well as the endorsement of the negative reciprocity norm) suggest that further
In sum, the present work represents a new and promising approach to the study of
aggressiveness and its underlying forces and mechanisms that takes into account that social
interactive processes, in general, and aggression, in particular, are heavily influenced by the self-
self-regulation in the present work does not reflect the (implicit) assumption that promotion-
focused self-regulation is not related to aggressiveness. On the contrary, in the context of the
GAM we suppose that certain situational and personal input factors that contribute to aggressive
considerations suggest that frustration through interfering disruptions in the strive to reach an
ideal or maximal goal – that is, a goal standard related to the promotion focus – is most likely to
position is not that only prevention-focused self-regulation is related to aggressiveness, and that
is that both modes of self-regulation can contribute to the development and expression of
aggressiveness, though they do so through very different routes and mechanisms. The present
work focused on mechanisms related to the prevention focus. Additional studies focusing on
mechanism related to the promotion focus are currently underway in our lab, and we posit that
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 34
In sum, the theoretical perspective and the empirical findings reported in the present paper
aggressiveness and aggressive tendencies and thereby enhance our knowledge of the factors that
contribute to aggressive behavior. As such, the current studies open a new avenue of research for
Implications
Given the great relevance of aggression in everyday life, a discussion of several practical
implications of the present work with respect to potential means for decreasing aggressiveness
our findings suggest that it would be important to counteract a misanthropic, cynical perspective
(e.g., making hostile attributions) and negative thinking (e.g., rumination) in individuals. With
individuals’ behavior, and to emphasize the fact that individuals’ characteristics and behaviors
are malleable and subject to change. With respect to the tendency toward ruminating and
tendency to focus on negative environmental cues and related emotional experiences as well as
their tendency to brood and dwell on bad and negative things. Of course, this seems a rather
difficult undertaking, particularly if individuals live in a context where they are consistently
confronted with negative cues. Nonetheless, recent approaches in positive psychology (Snyder &
Lopez, 2002) indicate that there are possible strategies for changing individuals’ focus and
could be to make use of prevention–focused individuals’ respect for normative standards and to
emphasize that non-aggressive and non-retaliatory behavior are normatively appropriate. Based
on the notion that prevention-focused individuals’ are particularly concerned with fulfillment of
Finally, given the empirical evidence documenting that being raised under an
self-regulatory orientation (Keller, in press), but also to established risk factors in the context of
aggression (labile self-esteem, neuroticism, cynical hostility, hostile attribution bias), it seems
that having parents consider the consequences of authoritarian childrearing practices might also
factors of aggression in the current research might prove useful in limiting and inhibiting the
References
Anderson, C. A. (1997). Effects of violent movies and trait hostility on hostile feelings and
Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2002). Human aggression. Annual Review of Psychology,
53, 27-51.
Anderson, C. A., Deuser, W. E., & DeNeve, K. (1995). Hot temperatures, hostile affect, hostile
cognition, and arousal: Test of a general model of affective aggression. Personality and
Anderson, C. A., & Dill, K. E. (2000). Video games and aggressive thoughts, feelings, and
behavior in the laboratory and in life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 772-
790.
Baumeister, R. F., Bushman, B. J., & Campbell, K. (2000). Self-esteem, narcissism, and
aggression: Does violence result from low self-esteem or from threatened egotism? Current
Baumeister, R. F., Smart, L., & Boden, J. M. (1996). Relation of threatened egotism to violence
and aggression: The dark side of high self-esteem. Psychological Review, 103, 5-33.
Berg, J., Dickhaut, J., & McCabe, K. (1995). Trust, reciprocity, and social history. Games and
Bohnet, I., & Zeckhauser, R. (2004). Trust, risk, and betrayal. Journal of Economic Behavior
Bettencourt, B. A., Talley, A., Benjamin, A. J., & Valentine, J. (2006). Personality and
Buchan, N., & Croson, R. (2004). The boundaries of trust: Own and others’ actions in the US and
Bushman, B. J. (1995). Moderating role of trait aggressiveness in the effects of violent media on
Bushman, B. J. (2002). Does venting anger feed or extinguish the flame? Catharsis, rumination,
distraction, anger, and aggressive responding. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
28, 724-731.
Bushman, B. J., & Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Threatened egotism, narcissism, and direct and
displaced aggression: Does self-love or self-hate lead to violence? Journal of Personality and
Bushman, B. J., Bonacci, A. M., Pedersen, W. C., Vasquez, E. A., & Miller, N. (2005). Chewing
on it can chew you up: Effects of ruminatio on triggered displaced aggression. Journal of
Buss, A. H., & Perry, M. (1992). The aggression questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social
Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention (1995). The international affective picture system:
Psychophysiology.
Collins, K., & Bell, R. (1997). Personality and aggression: the dissipation-rumination scale.
Cook, W. W., & Medley, D. M. (1954). Proposed hostility and pharisaic-virtue scales for the
Denson, T. F., Pedersen, W. C., & Miller, N. (2006). The displaced aggression questionnaire.
Dodge, K. A. (1991). The structure and function of reactive and proactive aggression. In D. J.
Pepler, & K. H. Rubin (Eds.), The development and treatment of childhood aggression (pp.
Dodge, K. A., Lochman, J. E., Harnish, J. D., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S. (1997). Reactive and
Eder, P., Aquino, K., Turner, C., & Reed, A. (2006). Punishing those responsible for the prison
abuses at Abu Ghraib: The influence of the negative reciprocity norm (NRN). Political
Eisenberger, R., Lynch, P., Aselage, J., & Rohdieck, S. (2004). Who takes the most revenge?
Farrar, K., & Krcmar, M. (2006). Measuring state and trait aggression: A short, cautionary tale.
Fresco, D. M., Frankel, A. N., Mennin, D. S., Turk, C. L., Heimberg, R. G. (2002). Distinct and
Freitas, A. L., Liberman, N., & Higgins, E. T. (2002). Regulatory fit and resisting temptation
Friedman, R. S., & Förster, J. (2001). The effects of promotion and prevention cues on creativity.
Gomez, R., & Gomez, A. (2000). Perceived maternal control and support as predictors of hostile-
biased attribution of intent and response selection in aggressive boys. Aggressive Behavior,
26, 155-168.
Gomez, R., Gomez, A., DeMello, L., & Tallent, R. (2001). Perceived maternal control and
support: Effects on hostile biased social information processing and aggression among
clinic-referred children with high aggression. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
42, 513-522.
Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52, 1280-1300.
M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 30, pp. 1-46). San
Higgins, E. T., Bond, R. N., Klein, R., Strauman, T. (1986). Self-discrepancies and emotional
Higgins, E. T., Friedman, R. S., Harlow, R. E., Idson, L. C., Ayduk, O. N., & Taylor, A. (2001).
Higgins, E. T. & Spiegel, S. (2004). Promotion and prevention strategies for self-regulation: A
Kanter, D. L., & Mirvis, P. H. (1989). The cynical Americans. Living and working in an age of
Keller, J. (in press). On the development of regulatory focus: The role of parenting styles. European
Kernis, M. H., Brown, A. C., & Brody, G. H. (2000). Fragile self-esteem in children and its
68, 225-252.
Kosfeld, M., Heinrichs, M., Zak, Paul J., Fischbacher, U., & Fehr, E. (2005). Oxytocin increases trust
Kuyken, W., Watkins, E., Holden, E., & Cook, W. (2006). Rumination in adolescents at risk for
Lockwood, P., Jordan, C. H., & Kunda, Z. (2002). Motivation by positive and negative role models:
Regulatory focus determines who will best inspire us. Journal of Personality and Social
Manian, N., Strauman, T. J., & Denney, N. (1998). Temperament, recalled parenting styles, and
Miller, J. D., & Lynam, D. (2006). Reactive and proactive aggression: Similarities and
Miller, J. D., Lynam, D., & Leukefeld, C. (2003). Examining antisocial behavior through the lens
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Morrow, J., & Fredrickson, B. L. (1991). Response Styles and the Duration
Perugini, M., Gallucci, M., Presaghi, F., & Ercolani, A. P. (2003). The personal norm of
Roberts, J. E., Gilboa, E., & Gotlib, I. H. (1998). Ruminative response style and vulnerability to
episodes of dysphoria: Gender, neuroticism, and episode duration. Cognitive Therapy and
Roney, C. J., Higgins, E. T., & Shah, J. Y. (1995). Goals and framing: How outcome focus
influences motivation and emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 1151-
1160.
Sarason, I. G., Ganzer, V. J., & Granger, J. W. (1965). Self-description of hostility and its
Scholer, A. A., Stroessner, S. J., & Higins, E. T. (in press). Responding to negativity: How a
risky tactic can serve a vigilant strategy. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology.
disorder and the five-factor model of personality. In P. T. Costa & T. A. Widiger (Eds.),
Personality disorders and the five-factor model of personality (pp. 117-127). Washington,
Shah, J., Higgins, E. T., & Friedman, R. S. (1998). Performance incentives and means: How
regulatory focus influences goal attainment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
74, 285-293.
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 42
Sharpe, J. P., & Desai, S. (2001). The revised NEO personality inventory and the MMPI-2
31, 505-518.
Snyder, C. R., & Lopez, S. J. (Eds.) (2002). Handbook of positive psychology. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Spencer, S. J., Zanna, M. P., & Fong, G. T. (2005). Establishing a causal chain: Why experiments
are often more effective than mediational analyses in examining psychological processes.
Sukhodolsky, D. G., Golub, A., & Cromwell, E. N. (2001). Development and validation of the
Tangri, S. S., Burt, M. R., & Johnson, L. B. (1982). Sexual harassment at work: Three
Trapnell, P. D., & Campbell, J. D. (1999). Private self-consciousness and the five-factor model of
Twenge, J. M. (2000). The age of anxiety? Birth cohort change in anxiety and neuroticism, 1952-
von Collani, G., & Werner, R. (2005). Self-related and motivational constructs as determinants of
Zanna, M. P., & Fazio, R. H. (1982). The attitude-behavior relation: Moving toward a third
social behavior. The Ontario Symposium on Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 2 (pp.
Author note
This research was supported by a post-doctoral grant from the German Science Foundation to
Johannes Keller. Ayse K. Uskul was supported by a post-doctoral fellowship from the Social
Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada during the conduct of Study 1.
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 44
Footnotes
1
. Note that the recent meta-analysis on personality and aggressive behavior conducted by
Bettencourt and colleagues (2006) revealed that irritability (reflecting trait anger), rumination,
are among those traits that are reliably related to aggressive behavior. Several lines of research
have found that neuroticism is related to aggressiveness (Sharpe & Desai, 2001; von Collani &
Werner, 2005), that (angry) rumination is a close correlate of aggression (Denson, Pedersen, &
Miller, 2006); Bushman, 2002; Bushman, Bonacci, Pedersen, Vasquesz, & Miller, 2005;
Caprara, 1986; Collins & Bell, 1997; Sukhodolsky, Golub, & Cromwell, 2001; Verona, 2005),
Bushman, & Campbell, 2000; Baumeister, Smart, & Boden, 1996; Bushman & Baumeister,
1998; note that labile self-esteem/narcissism is also robustly related to neuroticism, cf.
Schroeder, Wormworth, & Livesley, 1994), and that interpersonal trust is inversely related to
discuss that certain input factors in the GAM are conceptually linked to promotion-focused self-
regulation (e.g., when individuals experience and interference in their efforts to reach ideal or
maximal goals). Thus, there is reason to assume that the analysis of promotion-focused self-
regulation may also contribute to our understanding of aggressiveness. However, our focus in the
reciprocity beliefs as predictors revealed that these variables are not relevant, all coefficients t <
1.10, n.s.
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 45
4
. A critical reader may be tempted to argue that an interaction effect on the manipulation check
at trend level significance is a critical limitation. However, it should be noted that we are not
making the claim that perceived fairness is the only relevant aspect that contributes to
introduction and documented in Study 1 and 2, it is much more meaningful to assume that
several aspects related to prevention-focused self-regulation (not only the enhanced sensitivity to
rule violations but also a tendency to reciprocate, a hostile attribution tendency, and the
inclination to react with anger and hostility) contribute to an enhanced aggressive tendency in
finding an interaction effect on the manipulation check (reflecting only one of the discussed
aspects) at a marginal level of significance is not a major limitation in the present context.
Self-regulation and aggressiveness 46
Table 1
Note: Results obtained in Study 1a are shown above the diagonal; results of Study 1b are
depicted below the diagonal. + p < .10; * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001. Rumination was