Matthew Richard Currie Thesis PDF
Matthew Richard Currie Thesis PDF
Matthew Richard Currie Thesis PDF
EVALUATION
Matthew R. Currie
September, 2010
Abstract
aggressive youth. The results of several outcome studies from the US, UK and Europe indicate that
ART is effective in reducing aggressive behaviours and thoughts and recidivism among offenders.
This study evaluated the effectiveness of ART for aggressive juvenile offenders (aged 17 to 20 years)
at an Australian youth justice custody centre. The results of an initial pilot study (N= 5) indicated
overall support for the clinical utility of ART. Thus, with some methodological and programmatic
amendments a larger (N= 20) study was undertaken. A repeated measures design was used with
significant reductions in self-reported aggressive behaviours and thoughts, cognitive distortions, and
impulsivity and some improvement in social problem solving skills. The effect sizes were moderate to
large and the treatment effects were maintained at two year follow-up. Contrary to expectations,
custody worker ratings of participant’s aggressive/antisocial behaviours did not show significant
reduction at the six month follow-up. Additionally, these ratings were not clinically significant at any
time point. This measure had not previously been used in a correctional setting, however, the
workers may have been unreliable respondents due to their lack of knowledge of the ART
participants. Despite efforts, a waitlist comparison group could not be obtained. Results from this
study provide support, in terms of youth self-report measures, for the application of ART with
moderate to high risk aggressive offenders in an Australian youth justice custodial setting. Further
investigation with a control comparison and a reliable non-self-report informant is required to confirm
the findings. The results also support the cultural and age-related applicability of the learning
procedures inherent in ART for this group of moderate to high risk young offenders.
ii
DECLARATION
I declare that this thesis is my own account of my research and does not contain work that has been
previously submitted for a degree at any institution or for publication, without due acknodelgement.
Matthew R. Currie
iii
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
My eternal gratitude to my supervisor Katie Wood who saw to it that I actually completed this thesis.
There were a few occasions where I seriously doubted that I would. In addition to her role as my
coordinating research supervisor, Katie has provided invaluable support as a professional practice
supervisor. Working with mostly involuntary, violent young offenders has been, and continues to be,
highly rewarding and at times really, really challenging. Katie has been a wonderful mentor in helping
me to reflect on my personal journey in the work I do, as well as build on my strengths to do it better.
I would also like to thank my colleagues Cath Powell and Charlene Pereira, both, particularly Cath,
helped facilitate all the ART programs that were part of this thesis. Most importantly, they also helped
me to navigate the complexities of the youth justice system. I also wish to thank Michael Crewdson,
Felicity Dunn, and Tania Nahum all of whom, a various stages of my professional development have
acted as clinical supervisors. Their contribution to my personal and professional development has
been highly rewarding. I would like to thank each of the young men who participated in this research
and freely gave of their time at each of the follow-up assessments. In most cases they did this long
after exiting the criminal justice system, which stands as testimony to the effectiveness of ART.
Finally, big respect to Arnold Goldstein and his colleagues for developing ART and making sure it is
iv
CONTENTS
Abstract .............................................................................................................................................ii
Table of Figures................................................................................................................................ix
Summary ............................................................................................................................... 16
Summary ............................................................................................................................... 32
Skillstreaming ........................................................................................................................ 33
v
Defining Social Competence, Social Skills and Social Problem Solving................................ 36
Summary ............................................................................................................................... 46
Evaluations of ACT................................................................................................................ 60
Summary ............................................................................................................................... 62
Summary ............................................................................................................................... 83
Chapter 5 Previous ART Evaluations and Rationale for the Present Study................................... 86
Method .................................................................................................................................. 92
Participants................................................................................................................... 92
Measures...................................................................................................................... 92
Procedure..................................................................................................................... 95
vi
Results .................................................................................................................................. 97
Participants................................................................................................................. 110
Measures.................................................................................................................... 112
Procedure................................................................................................................... 115
vii
Impulsivity ................................................................................................................... 162
Appendix B: Ethics………………………………………………………………………………………...230
Appendix C: Measures……………………………………………………………………………………243
viii
Table of Figures
ix
List of Tables
Table 5 Pre- and post- treatment raw scores on the AQ, SSRS, and HIT Questionnaires ............ 97
Table 6 Pilot study pre- and post- treatment descriptives and Wilcoxon T-values for all outcome
measures ................................................................................................................................. 99
Table 7 Methodological changes in the main study: Summary and rationale ............................... 108
Table 8 ART group completers, non-completers, waitlist controls, and refusals for main study .. 112
Table 9 Reliability coefficients for all dependent measures at T1, T2, T3 and T4......................... 121
Table 10 Inter-correlations for all dependent measures in the main study at T1……………………123
Table 11 Descriptive statistics and main effect ANOVA results from T1 to T4 on the AQ ........... 125
Table 12 Descriptive statistics and main effect ANOVA results from T1 to T4 on the HIT........... 131
Table 13 Descriptive statistics and main effect ANOVA results from T1 to T4 on the SPSI ........ 142
Table 14 T-score descriptive statistics and main effect ANOVAS from T1 to T3 for the ABCL.... 146
Table E1 Inter-correlations for all dependent measures in the main study at T2…………………...317
Table E3 Descriptives and main effect ANOVA results from T1 to T4 on the AQ (LOCF) ........... 318
Table E3 Descriptives and main effect ANOVA results from T1 to T4 on the HIT (LOCF)........... 319
Table E4 Descriptives and main effect ANOVA results from T1 to T4 on the SPSI (LOCF) ....... 320
Table E5 Descriptives and main effect ANOVA results from T1 to T4 for Impulsiveness (LOCF).321
Table E6 T-score descriptivs and main effect ANOVAS from T1 to T3 for the ABCL (LOCF) ..... 322
x
xi
1
Chapter 1
The World Health Organisation (WHO) has reported that violence is one of the leading global
public health concerns today (Krug, Dahlberg, Mercy, Zwi, & Lazano, 2002). Aggressive behaviour
and violent offending represents one of the most prevalent, stable, and costly problems in society
(Guerra & Slaby, 1990; Kazdin, 1987). The individual and financial burden of violent offending
includes increased public expenditure associated with policing, probable recurring imprisonment of
offenders and victim costs (Polaschek & Dixon, 2001; Serin, Gobeil, & Preston, 2009). Recent
Australian statistics indicate that offences defined as “acts intended to cause injury” account for the
highest proportion (18%) of the total adult (median age of 33 years) prison population (Australian
Bureau of Statistics [ABS], 2008; p.21). According to the ABS, the proportion of violent offenders
among younger prisoners (18-25 years) is even greater, accounting for 25% of the total prison
population. Moreover, ABS crime statistics indicate an average yearly increase in assaults by 5% for
each year between 1995 and 2007 (Australian Institute of Criminology [AIC], 2009). Although most
correctional jurisdictions in Australia offer some form of anger management program for violent
offenders, there are few published evaluations of such interventions (Howells & Day, 2002). Indeed,
Serin et al. (2009) noted that despite the considerable financial, social and psychological costs
associated with aggressive offending there is relative lack of accumulated knowledge in the
According to Goldstein and Glick (1987) interventions aimed at remediating antisocial and
aggressive behaviours in young people need to address the cognitive, behavioural and affective
behavioural interventions have been shown to be most effective in treating the complex set of
behaviours associated with conduct related disorders once they have become an entrenched
behavioural repertoire among antisocial youth (Fonagy & Kurtz, 2002; Kazdin, 1997; Lochman et al.,
2000). Aggression Replacement Training (ART; Goldstein, Glick, & Gibbs, 1998) represents an
exemplar of this type of intervention. Several researchers (e.g., Barnoski, 2004; Fonagy & Kurtz,
2002; Palmer, 2007; Polaschek, 2006) have indicated that ART is one of the most efficacious
interventions in working with moderate to high risk violent young offenders. This thesis reports the
findings of a two-year longitudinal evaluation of ART in an Australian, custodial youth justice setting
with males aged 17 to 20 years. The results of two studies (i.e., a small pilot study and a larger multi-
Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder, Oppositional Defiant Disorder, and Conduct Disorder) and their
relationship to youth aggression and offending behaviour. In Chapter 2 the “what works” research
relevant to offending behaviour (Andrews, Bonta, & Hoge, 1990) is examined, emphasising the risk-
need-responsivity model of offender treatment and meta-analytic research that shows cognitive-
behavioral interventions as the most effective in treating offender populations. Chapter 3 discusses
the current theoretical perspectives that inform ART, including operant and social learning theories
which represent the theoretical basis of CBT interventions with aggressive youth and offender
components of ART (i.e., Skillstreaming, Anger Control Training, and Moral Reasoning Training) in
terms of their respective practice content and procedures, theory, and empirical evidence. In Chapter
5 the published evaluations of ART to date are reviewed which leads into the rationale informing the
present study.
3
Chapter 6 reports the pre- to post- treatment results of the initial pilot study of ART in an
Australian youth justice custodial setting. Firstly, the aims, hypotheses and methodology of the pilot
study are described. Then the results of the pilot study will be reported and briefly discussed, with an
emphasis on the key learnings and how they translated into programmatic and methodological
refinements for the Main study. Chapter 7 begins with a detailed description of the methodology
applied in the Main study followed by details of data preparation and analysis procedures.
Subsequently the results for the Main study are reported specific to the targets of change assessed
(i.e., youth self-report -aggression, -cognitive distortions, -social problem solving and -impulsiveness,
and custody worker-reports of youth aggression and antisocial behaviour). Chapter 8, the final
chapter, discusses the results of the main study. Again this chapter is structured according to the
specific targets of change. The maintenance of treatment gains at two-year follow-up is then
examined. Group process variables, such as the therapeutic alliance and group cohesion, is then
discussed as they relate to the effectiveness of ART and indeed any intervention with offenders. The
final sections of the Discussion review the clinical and policy and planning implications of this thesis,
followed by the methodological strengths and limitations. This thesis will conclude with
recommendations for future research and final comments summarising how this study has
contributed to offender treatment literature broadly and the ART literature specifically. Figure 1
provides an overview of the research plan and changes implemented from Studies One (pilot study)
Note. NS = non-significant; AQ = Aggression Questionnaire (Buss & Warren, 2000); HIT = How I Think Questionnaire (Barriga,
Gibbs, Potter, & Liau, 2001); SSRS = Social Skills Rating System (Gresham & Elliot, 1990); SPSI = Social Problem Solving
Inventory- Revised (D'Zurilla, Nezu, & Maydeu-Olivares, 2002); I7 = Impulsiveness Scale (Eysenck, Pearson, Easting, &
Allsopp, 1985); ABCL = Adult Behavior Checklist (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2003).
5
The two most commonly used systems of classification for psychiatric disorders- the
International Classification of Diseases- Tenth Edition (ICD-10; WHO, 1992) and the Diagnostic and
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Fourth Edition- Text Revised (DSM-IV-TR; American
externalising behaviours (e.g., lying, stealing, oppositionality, and aggression) throughout childhood
and adolescence. These disorders are commonly referred to as the disruptive behaviour disorders
(DBD’s) which comprise Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) (or Hyperkinetic Disorders
{HKD] in the ICD-10) Oppositional Defiant Disorder (ODD) and Conduct Disorder (CD) (Cowling et
al., 2005). In Australia, the DSM is the most widely used system of classification for clinical disorders.
Thus, for the purpose of the present review, discussion of the diagnostic categories relevant to
aggressive and antisocial behaviour will focus primarily upon the criteria described in the DSM-IV-TR
(APA, 2000).
interpersonal actions that consist of verbal or physical behaviours that are destructive or injurious to
others or to objects” (p.31). Throughout the course of “normal” psychosocial development, evidence
for aggressive behaviour can be seen in the social interactions of almost all children and adolescents
to varying degrees (Hawley, 1999; Tremblay, 2000). In cases where aggression becomes a more
entrenched pattern of social interaction, however, a disruptive behaviour disorder (DBD) diagnosis
might be indicated (Lochman et al.). The externalising behaviours associated with the DBD’s violate
the rights of others and common social rules and standards (APA, 2000). Epidemiological studies
indicate that conduct related disorders are increasing in clinical and non-clinical populations with
6
DBD’s being the most common reason for referral to child and adolescent psychiatric services
(Kazdin, 2002; Phelps & McClintock, 1994; Webster-Stratton & Dahl, 1995). In Australia, a national
survey of 4,500 young people aged between 4-17 years reported a prevalence rate of 14.2% for
Kazdin (1987) reported that an early onset of a DBD can be related to continuing aggressive
and anti-social behaviours into adolescence and adulthood (e.g., school drop out, alcoholism, drug
abuse, juvenile delinquency, adult crime, marital/interpersonal disruption, and poor physical and
mental health outcomes). Given the reported stability of aggressive and antisocial behaviour across
time and setting, and that the complexity of behaviours vary qualitatively and quantitatively, the task
Prevention Research Group, 1999a; 1999b; Kazdin, 1987; 1997). In this section, the DBD’s are
offender populations. It is important to note that the participants in the present study were not
assessed for DBD’s. Rather, the aim of this section is to provide a context for the nosology of violent
offending behaviour.
of inattentive behaviour and/or hyperactivity-impulsivity that is more frequent and severe than is
typically observed in individuals of the same developmental level (APA, 2000). Children and
adolescents with inattentive behaviour often make careless mistakes with schoolwork, find it hard to
persist with tasks and are easily distracted. Those with problems in the area of
hyperactivity/impulsivity often fidget and talk excessively, interrupt others, and are constantly “on the
go” (APA, 2000). The DSM-IV-TR (APA, 2000) distinguishes between three subtypes of ADHD based
on the predominant symptom pattern for the past six months. Children and adolescents with
7
Type; those with primarily inattentive symptoms are diagnosed with ADHD- Predominantly Inattentive
Type; and those with primarily hyperactivity-impulsivity symptoms are diagnosed with ADHD-
age of symptom onset before seven years and the presence of symptoms across two or more
settings (e.g., school and home). In contrast, the ICD-10 hyperkinetic disorder does not distinguish
between subtypes, rather a single disorder is described relative to the presence of both inattentive
Type. The ICD-10, unlike the DSM-IV-TR, does not allow for comorbid diagnoses, with the exception
of the presence of conduct related problems warranting a diagnosis of hyperkinetic conduct disorder.
These differences in diagnostic classification have implications for the reported prevalence rates of
HKD and ADHD. ADHD has consistently been shown to have a significantly higher prevalence rate
The prevalence rate for ADHD among school-aged children has been estimated to
approximate 3-7% (APA, 2000). Polanczyk et al. (2007) reported a global prevalence rate of 5%.
Given that epidemiological studies represent approximations of clinical diagnosis, the accuracy of
prevalence rates is dependent on having reliable diagnostic tools (Rowland, Lesesne, & Abramowitz,
2002). In the case of ADHD, diagnosis is primarily based on teacher and parent ratings of child
behaviour, hence the rigors of clinical assessment are often lacking in the assessment of this
condition (Rowland et al.). Moreover, Rowland et al. argued that the presence of comorbid conditions
(e.g., ODD, CD, anxiety and learning disorders) might confound accurate diagnosis, which is likely to
contribute to inflated prevalence rates. Population estimates of ADHD vary greatly with upper
prevalence rate for ADHD of 15% for boys and 7% for girls between the ages 4-17 years. The
disorder was more prevalent among primary school aged boys (19%) than adolescent males (10%)
(Sawyer et al., 2000). However, this survey was not based on the full diagnostic criteria, which might
have artificially inflated the prevalence rates. The gender differences reported by Sawyer et al.
approximate other epidemiology research in which the diagnosis of ADHD among males is estimated
to be greater than females by ratios ranging from 2:1 to 9:1 (APA, 2001). Several explanations have
been proposed to help explain these gender differences, including biased sampling (i.e., differences
in gender ratios tend to decrease among community as opposed to clinical samples) and possibly
biased diagnostic criteria and tools which favour detection in males (Barkley, 1995; Goldman et al.,
The early symptoms of inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity are often detected in the first
years of school (Goldstein & Rider, 2005). The effects of ADHD cause significant psychosocial
impairment across multiple domains including academic failure, poor interpersonal relations, poor
self-esteem, and self-regulatory deficits (Barkley, Fischer, Edelbrock, & Smallish, 1990). High rates of
psychosocial impairment across the lifespan have been reported for those diagnosed with the
disorder. For example, 32 to 40% are estimated to drop out of school, 50-70% reported having few
friends, 70-80% reported underachievement in the work place, 40-50% engaged in antisocial
behaviour, and 18-25% have been shown to develop personality disorders in adulthood (International
Oppositional Defiant Disorder (ODD) and Conduct Disorder (CD). The DSM-IV-TR (APA,
2000) defines ODD as a persistent pattern of angry, antagonistic, hostile, vindictive, and irritable
behaviour toward authority figures and/or peers for at least six months. The problem behaviours
occur more often than is typical of similarly matched peers in terms of age and developmental level.
9
The onset of oppositional behaviours usually occurs in the home and later transfers to other settings,
such as school, usually before eight years of age. In contrast, CD is characterised by “a repetitive
and persistent pattern of behaviour in which the basic rights of others or major age-appropriate
societal norms or rules are violated” (APA, 2000; p.93). These behaviours fall into four categories: (1)
actual or threatened physical aggression toward people or animals; (2) deliberate destruction or
damage to property; (3) acts of wilful deception and theft; and (4) serious violation of societal rules.
These diagnostic categories are typically manifested through antisocial behaviours such as recurrent
instances of bullying, frequent physical fights, deliberate destruction of other people’s property,
breaking into houses or cars, staying out late at night despite parental prohibitions, running away
from home, and/or frequent truancy from school. To meet DSM-IV criteria for CD, children and
adolescents must exhibit three or more of these behaviours during the past 12 months, with at least
Unlike ADHD, a diagnosis of ODD or CD does not require that the behaviours are present in
more than one setting. Yet, as with ADHD, a multi-informant (i.e., teacher, parent, self-report and
clinical assessment) approach to diagnosis is usually employed and it is not uncommon for
disagreement between informants, which can complicate diagnosis and influence prevalence rates
(Bird, Gould, & Staghezza, 1992; Werry, 1997). Prevalence rates for ODD have been estimated to be
between 2 to 15% (Nock, Kazdin, Hiripi, & Kessler, 2007). According to Goldstein and Rider (2005)
these variations in estimates are likely to be attributable to small sample sizes and unreliable
diagnostic methods. Australian Epidemiological data indicate a prevalence rate for CD of 3% among
4-17 year olds (Sawyer et al., 2000). The prevalence rate was higher for males (4.4%) than females
(1.6%) which is consistent with other reports of gender differences for CD (APA, 2000). In a review of
the existing literature Kazdin (1987) estimated a US population prevalence rate of CD ranging from 4
10
to 10%. Among juvenile offenders, however, the prevalence of CD is far greater. For example, Fazel,
Doll, and Langstrom (2008) reported a prevalence rate of 52.8% for CD for both males and females
in juvenile detention. For males, this represented 5 to 10 times greater risk for the development of CD
compared to community samples. CD has consistently been shown to be the most prevalent
psychiatric disorder among young offender populations (Abrantes, Hoffmann, & Anton, 2005). Yet,
even after excluding CD, Teplin, Abram, McClelland, Dulcan, and Mericle (2002) reported that almost
60% of males in juvenile detention met criteria for one or more psychiatric disorders.
There is debate among researchers as to whether ODD merely represents a milder, earlier
developmental manifestation of CD (Maughan, Rowe, Messer, Goodman, & Meltzer, 2004; Nock et
al., 2007). The diagnostic criteria for ODD in both the DSM-IV-TR (APA, 2000) and ICD-10 (WHO,
1992) allude to this purported developmental progression from ODD to CD in precluding a diagnosis
of ODD in the presence of CD. Certainly, there is much research showing that the early onset of
behavioural problems in childhood, consistent with a diagnosis of ODD, represents a key risk factor in
the development of CD (Broidy, et al. 2003; Dodge & Pettit, 2003; Loeber, 1991).
Adolescents who are most likely to be chronically antisocial are those who first evidenced
symptoms of ODD in the preschool years. Thus, the primary developmental pathway for
serious conduct disorders in adolescents and adulthood appears to be set in the preschool
period. ODD is a sensitive predictor of subsequent CD, in that nearly all youths have shown
While some youth with early onset ODD go on to develop CD, many do not (Loeber, Burke, Lahey,
Winters, & Zera, 2000; Nock et al., 2007). Nevertheless, comorbidity between the two conditions is
more the norm than the exception (Loeber, 1991; Webster-Stratton & Dahl, 1995).
11
Comorbidity and disruptive behaviour disorders. Kazdin (1997) suggested that comorbidity
among the DBD’s is very common among cases referred for treatment. He reported that as many as
70% of the cases treated in clinical settings meet DSM criteria for at least two disorders. Sawyer et
al. (2000) reported that 23% of all children and adolescents in their sample who met criteria for one
disorder (ADHD, CD or Depressive Disorder) also reported symptoms consistent with diagnosis of a
second. Overall, males had a higher rate of comorbidity (27%) than females (15%). Less than 1% of
children and adolescents met criteria for all three disorders. The literature abounds with reports of
comorbid relationships between ADHD, ODD, CD, learning difficulties, mood disorders, anxiety
disorders, communication disorders, and Tourette's Disorder (APA, 2000; Biederman, Newcorn, &
Sprich, 1991). In an eight year follow-up study, Barkley et al. (1990) found that 80% of the children
with ADHD in their study were still hyperactive as adolescents and that 60% of them had developed
ODD or CD. Numerous studies (e.g., Barkley et al., 1990; Biederman, Petty, et al., 2008; Bird et al.,
1994) have found that young people with comorbid ADHD, ODD or CD, as opposed to one or the
other, are more likely to have greater cognitive impairment, increased family dysfunction, poorer
psychosocial outcomes (e.g., peer rejection, educational and employment failure, substance abuse,
While a diagnosis of one DBD does not in, and of itself, indicate an increased likelihood of
(i.e., a persistent pattern of impulsive, hyperactive, defiant and aggressive behaviours across various
settings) have consistently been shown to predict adverse psychosocial outcomes throughout
12
childhood, adolescence and into adulthood (Goldstein & Rider, 2005; Kazdin, 1987). Hence, current
aim to integrate multiple risk factors in a cumulative explanatory model (Dodge & Pettit, 2003; Frick,
2004). According to Dandreaux and Frick (2009) the large number of common risk factors associated
with CD can be grouped as either dispositional (e.g., difficult child temperament, impulsivity, deficits
association with a deviant peer group). Similarly, Nietzel, Hasemann and Lynam (1999) proposed an
etiological sequence comprising multiple risk factors in the development of violent offending
behaviour. They suggested four main contributing pathways: (1) Distal antecedents, including
(e.g., family functioning) factors; (2) Early indicators- for CD such as poor parenting and early
aggression; (3) Developmental processes- school failure, cognitive style (e.g., hostile attributions)
and substance abuse; and (4) Maintenance variables- antisocial peer associations, opportunism, and
socioeconomic deprivation. In assessing individual level of risk for severity and stability of aggressive
and antisocial behaviours, a cumulative approach has been proposed that takes account of multiple
risk factors from pre-birth through childhood and adolescent development (Frick, 2004; Nietzel,
A robust predictor of increasing antisocial behaviour in terms of frequency and type (e.g.,
violent offending) has been the subtyping of CD according to age of onset- childhood or adolescent
onset type (Moffit et al., 2008). Additionally, several researchers (e.g., Broidy, et al. 2003; Dodge &
Pettit, 2003; Frick, 2004; Webster-Stratton & Dahl, 1995) have identified early physical aggression as
a precursor to the development of CD. Bor, Najman, O’Callaghan, Williams, and Anstey (2001)
conducted a longitudinal study of 3,792 Australian mothers and their children at age five with follow-
13
up at age 14 years. The strongest predictor of adolescent delinquency was aggressive behaviour at
age five. Bor et al. concluded that aggression in the early years is a reliable and powerful predictor of
continuing adolescent aggressive behaviour. The stability of aggressive and antisocial behaviour
across time suggests that the prognosis is likely to be poor for some children. Indeed, Webster-
Stratton and Dahl (1995) suggested that almost 50% of severely conduct disordered children
continue to be antisocial into adulthood. Gelhorn, Sakai, Price and Crowley (2007) reported an even
higher rate of antisocial behaviour persistence. Seventy-five percent of their large (N=43,093)
retrospective, American, stratified sample who met criteria for CD also met criteria for adult Antisocial
Personality Disorder (APD). Moreover, persistent antisocial behaviour was most strongly predicted by
victim and violence orientated offences, including “Stealing with Confrontation”, “Cruelty to People”,
The findings of Gelhorn et al. (2007) and Bor et al. (2001) fit within the developmental
pathway (e.g., Fergusson & Horwood, 2002; Frick, 2004; Moffitt, Capsi, Harrington, & Milne, 2002;
Odgers et al., 2008). Drawing on data from the Dunedin Longitudinal Study, Moffitt and her
colleagues (1993; 2001; 2002) provided a detailed examination of a relatively uncommon group of
group of antisocial offenders. LCP is characterised by early childhood cognitive deficits, difficult
temperament, and hyperactivity, which are exacerbated by environmental risks including poor
parenting, economic impoverishment and family discord. In later childhood and throughout
adolescence, environmental risk factors broaden to include problematic social relationships such as
peer isolation and conflict with teachers, and a later tendency toward antisocial peer associations and
poor relations with employers and partners. By contrast, AL antisocial behaviour is thought to co-
14
occur with the onset of puberty and is consistent with the normative task of individuation and
separation from parents and the greater influence of peers. The early environmental and
neurobiological risk factors associated with LCP are not typically seen in the AL group. However, in
cases where AL youth become involved in criminal activities, they are less likely to be convicted of a
violent offence or to spend time in prisons compared to youth in the LCP group (Odgers et al., 2008).
Research has consistently shown that childhood onset of conduct related problems, compared to
adolescent onset, is predictive of poorer outcomes throughout adolescence and into adulthood, such
as increased impulsivity, criminality and aggression (Frick, Cornell, Barry, Bodin, & Dane, 2003;
Moffitt, 1993).
temperament in the development of antisocial behaviour (Frick & Morris, 2004). Specifically,
emotional regulation, particularly regulation of negative emotions combined with a lack of fearful
behavioural inhibitions, and callous-unemotional (CU) traits (i.e., a lack of empathy and guilt) have
consistently been shown to be associated with the development and maintenance of severe conduct
problems (Frick, 2004; Frick & Morris, 2004). This combination of emotional dysregulation and CU
traits are more commonly associated with the early onset LCP group of conduct disordered youth
(Moffitt, 1993). Moreover, consistent with the construct of adult psychopathy, the presence of CU
traits in youth has been shown to predict heightened levels of aggressive offending, increased
severity and number of conduct problems, more police contact, and a greater likelihood of family
histories of APD compared to antisocial youth without CU traits (Frick et al., 2003; Frick & Loney,
1999).
Frick and his colleagues (2005) investigated the predictive utility of CU traits relative to
severity and stability of conduct problems among a community sample of school children in Grades 3,
15
4, 6, and 7 over a four year period. Parent and teacher ratings on the Callous-Unemotional subscale
of the Antisocial Process Screening Device (APSD; Frick & Hare, 2001) were used to classify
participants as high or low on CU traits at each of the three follow-up assessments, each one year
apart. Parents and teachers also completed measures related to DSM-IV (APA, 2000) criteria for
ODD, CD and ADHD. Children completed a self-report measure of delinquency, which enabled the
researchers to determine the number and type of offences committed over the four year period. Four
groups (Control; CU-only; conduct problems [CP]- only; and CU+CP) of approximate equal size (n =
25) were compared on repeated measures over the four year period. As predicted, children with
conduct problems and CU traits showed a more severe and chronic pattern of antisocial behaviour
compared to the control, or CU-only or CP-only groups. Specifically, the CU+CP group showed
significantly higher levels of conduct problems, self-reported delinquency, and parent-reported police
contacts. Despite the limitation of a small sample size, Frick and colleagues demonstrated that the
presence of CU traits can predict conduct problem severity among a subgroup of at risk youth, in
Comorbid psychiatric illness among youth in juvenile detention has been widely reported
(Abram, Teplin, McClelland & Dulcan, 2003; Atkins et al., 1999; Fazel et al., 2008; Teplin, Abram,
McClelland, Dulcan & Mericle, 2002). Fazel et al. (2008) conducted a meta-analysis of 25 surveys
conducted in various locations around the world, including Australia, in which estimates of psychiatric
disorders for male and female youth in juvenile detention (mean age 15.6 years, range 10-19 years).
They found that the incidence of ADHD among incarcerated youth was two to four times greater
compared to the general adolescent population and adolescents in juvenile detention were
16
approximately 10 times more likely to suffer a psychiatric illness than their non-offender peers. Eme
(2008) reported similar high levels of ADHD among juvenile offenders compared to age-related
peers. In a small Australian study, Bickel and Campbell (2002) reported that 98% of the juvenile
detainees (aged 12 – 18 years, 43 males and 7 females) in their study met criteria for CD, 46% met
criteria for ADHD, 46% were suffering a mood disorder, and 32% suffering an anxiety disorder.
Comobidity with CD was high, 52% were found to have three or more mental health problems in
addition to CD. These results were based on the Adolescent Psychopathology Scale (Reynolds,
1998) a semi-structured psychometric tool designed to assess the potential presence of DSM-IV
diagnostic categories. The absence of a full diagnostic clinical interview in this study may have
Summary
In this chapter, the DBD’s were reviewed in order to place adolescent and early-adult
criminality, in particular violent offending, within the context of current clinical nosology. As noted, a
diagnosis of one of the DBD’s does not necessarily suggest a heightened risk of criminal behaviour.
However, numerous studies have reported a high rate of comorbidity between the DBD’s (Barkley et
al., 1990; Kazdin, 1997; Sawyer et al., 2000). There is evidence that early behavioural problems in
childhood represent a key risk factor in the development of CD. There is also evidence to support the
has been linked to a life-course persistent (LCP) group of antisocial offenders who are unlikely to just
grow out of aggressive and antisocial behaviours (Frick, 2004; Moffitt et al., 2002; Odgers et al.,
2008). Additionally, LCP offenders have been shown to be at greater risk of long-term negative
psychosocial outcomes including increased impulsivity, criminality, and aggression and early-adult
17
development of APD. The presence of CU traits in combination with conduct related problems has
also been identified as a major risk factor in the developmental trajectory of increasing aggressive
and antisocial behaviours (Frick et al., 2005). CD is the most common psychiatric disorder among
young offenders, although high rates of comorbidity with other psychiatric disorders have been
reported (Bickel & Campbell, 2002; Fazel et al., 2008). Of particular relevance to the present study is
the finding that early physical aggression is a reliable predictor for the development of CD and
ongoing antisocial aggressive behaviours (Broidy, et al. 2003; Dodge & Pettit, 2003; Frick, 2004;
The next chapter provides an overview of the key principles that constitute the “what works’
literature relevant to offenders. The empirical evidence with respect to the effectiveness of specific
Chapter 2
Since the mid-1970’s, there has been a pervasive belief that lasting behaviour change
among criminal offenders is at best unlikely, if not impossible. This pessimistic view was primarily
informed by Martinson’s (1974) influential publication- “What works? Questions and answers about
prison reform”. Here, Martinson summarised the results of several hundred studies according to
negative versus positive outcomes. Hollin, Browne and Palmer (2004) criticised this approach
arguing that it failed to account for differences in the types of intervention employed, setting of
intervention, and the outcome measures used. They argue that more recent advances in statistical
procedures, such as meta-analysis, provide a more accurate picture in aggregating the results of
multiple empirical outcome studies. Indeed, according to Hollin and his colleagues, meta-analytic
procedures have significantly altered the pessimistic view that “nothing works” to a more empirically
validated position that behaviour change and transfer of gain can be achieved through certain
offender treatment programs. In this chapter, an overview of the core principles of “what works” in
treating adult and juvenile offenders, that is the criminogenic risk-need-responsivity model (Andrews,
Bonta, & Hoge, 1990) is reviewed. Then the empirical literature relevant to the general CBT approach
Andrews and his colleagues (Andrews, Bonta, & Hoge, 1990; Andrews et al., 1990; Bonta &
Andrews, 1996) were the first to examine the specific variables associated with offender rehabilitation
and reductions in recidivism (Hornsveld, Nijman, Hollin, & Kraaimaat, 2008). Andrews et al. (1990)
19
described five core principles that constitute effective practice in treating offenders with the aim of
reducing rates of re-offending: (1) Risk- level of risk of reoffending should be determined in order to
match intensity of service provision (i.e., higher risk = more intensive). Risk factors are categorised as
either static- that is unchangeable (e.g., family history, age of first offence) or dynamic- that is
potentially changeable (e.g., criminal associates and cognitions, impulsivity, education, employment,
social skill and problem solving deficits); (2) Need- criminogenic needs include antisocial attitudes
and beliefs, antisocial peer associations, substance abuse, unstable accommodation and social
supports, lack of employment and education, hostility and anger and poor social problem solving
distress, group cohesion, and community participation (Ogloff & Davis, 2004). With respect to
reducing recidivism, the criminogenic needs should be the primary targets of change; (3)
Responsivity- interventions should be matched to meet risk and need principles in addition to
developmental needs, motivation, cognitive capacity, and learning styles; (4) Program Integrity-
interventions should have built in processes of monitoring treatment fidelity. That is, practice should
be firmly placed within a body of evidence-based theory and program design and delivered within
clearly defined parameters (i.e., a practice manual); and (5) Professional Discretion- notwithstanding
judgement should provide for scope in adapting to situational and individual client need (Andrews &
Andrews and Dowden (2006) conducted a meta-analysis that included 374 primary studies to
test the empirical validity of the risk principle (i.e., the extent to which assessment of level of risk
enhances correctional treatment outcomes). The inclusion criteria required the primary studies to
20
have reported a follow-up assessment period, a treated group of offenders compared to a non-
treated or non-equivalent treatment group, a measure of recidivism, and a data-set that could provide
for the calculation of an effect size based on recidivist data. Additionally, the primary studies were
coded for the presence of appropriate treatment factors (i.e., adherence to the core principles of need
and responsivity). Overall, moderate support for the risk principle (eta squared = .17, p>.00) was
found. This finding was the same irrespective of treatment setting (i.e., community or institutional).
However, there were age effects showing that the risk principle did not reach statistical significance
with older offenders (20-years or older) although the treatment effects were significant for the
principles of need and general responsivity irrespective of age. Andrews and Dowden suggested that
this finding might be partly explained by poor reporting practices of risk for the older participants and
a method of coding risk that was based upon procedures outlined in earlier research with younger
offenders. In other words, the classification methods for determining high versus lower risk offenders
might have been an inaccurate test of the risk principle with older offenders. Notably, the risk
principle only enhanced treatment effectiveness in studies where the principles of need and general
responsivity were adhered to. General responsivity refers to the use of theoretically informed
principles of offender treatment such as social learning theory (see Chapter 3) and cognitive–
behavioural treatment approaches. This finding suggested the importance of the interaction effects of
the risk-need-responsivity model as a holistic approach to offender treatment, which is consistent with
Ward and Stewart (2003) criticised the risk-need-responsivity model for its over-emphasis on
criminogenic needs as targets of change, to the exclusion of more psychological and interpersonal
orientated needs that serve to improve offenders quality of life. According to these authors, the
21
dynamic targets of change (i.e., criminogenic needs) in the risk-need-responsivity model represent an
“primarily concerned with risk reduction” (p.125). Ward and Stewart argued that reductions in
recidivism in offender treatment have become the benchmark of correctional program design and
effectiveness to the exclusion of other more subjective human, and at least equally relevant, clinical
targets of change. They proposed a broader conceptualisation of need that encompasses a holistic
model of human well-being, including the “psychological needs for relatedness, autonomy, and
competence” (p.134). Some support for Ward and Stewart’s contention is indicated by the use of
Several researchers (Losel, 2001; Serin et al., 2009; Tate, Reppucci, & Mulvey, 1995) have indicated
that the sole use of recidivism, while an important measure of outcome, limits our understanding of
program effectiveness and fails to account for other important clinical outcomes. In essence, Ward
and Stewart argue for a strengths-based model of intervention in offender treatment that builds upon
universal psychological and humanistic needs, thereby reducing the inclination toward antisocial and
criminal behaviour. In reply, Bonta and Andrews (2003) suggested that Ward and Stewart had over-
simplified the risk-need-responsivity model by suggesting that the model does not take account of
offender strengths or broader universal needs. The Level of Service Inventory- Revised (LSI-R)
developed by Andrews and Bonta (1995) is a widely used measure of offender risk and needs, which
according to Ogloff and Davis (2004) addresses offender strengths in the absence of a risk (e.g.,
antisocial peer associations) domain. Moreover, Ogloff and Davis suggested that implicit to the
responsivity principle is the notion of working with individual offenders with an understanding of their
Several meta-analytic studies (e.g., Andrews et al., 1990; Izzo & Ross, 1990; Landenberger
& Lipsey, 2005; Lipsey, Chapman, & Landenberger, 2001; Lipsey & Wilson, 1998) have reported
favourable results in treating offender populations, specifically reduced rates of recidivism, where
structured CBT programs have been employed. Izzo and Ross (1990) conducted a meta-analysis of
46 studies of behaviour change programs with juvenile offenders ranging in age from 11 to 18 years
with a mean age of 13.2 years. The majority of studies analysed involved male offenders or mixed
gender groups of participants. Studies were initially classified according to their theoretical
systems theory, reality therapy, and other). Consistent with findings from the vast majority of
psychotherapy outcome studies (see Hubble, Duncan & Miller, 1999 for review) Izzo and Ross found
no significant difference between theoretical models in terms of reducing recidivism. However, their
component analysis showed that programs that included a cognitive component (i.e, social skills
training, social problem solving, modelling, role-playing, and cognitive-behaviour modification) were
more than twice as effective as programs that did not. Further, regression analysis showed that two
variables significantly accounted for the variance in effect size: (1) the presence of a cognitive
component; and (2) a community treatment setting rather than an institutional setting. These two
factors have consistently been shown to produce greater effect sizes in treating both adult and
juvenile offenders (Andrews et al., 1990; Goldstein et al., 1998; Wilson, Bouffard, & MacKenzie,
2005). However, this is not to suggest that programs delivered in institutional settings cannot produce
significant change, despite limitations such as limited opportunity for rehearsal of newly acquired
behavioural group treatments were employed with juvenile (aged 15-18 years) or adult (aged 20-30
years) offender populations. In nine of the studies reviewed, the intervention took place in a custodial
setting, whereas the remaining five were administered in a community setting while participants were
either on a parole or probation order. The results showed an overall reduction in recidivism of about
one third for CBT treatment participants compared to the treatment-as-usual (i.e., standard parole,
probation or prison case-management) control group. Specifically, those who received the CBT
treatment reoffended at a rate of 26 percent, compared to 38 percent for the control group.
Importantly, Lipsey and colleagues noted that the largest reductions in recidivism were associated
high level training in CBT and an emphasis upon treatment integrity. The smaller effect sizes were
associated with programs conducted by lesser trained staff and in institutional settings.
To explore these and other variations in treatment effects, Landenberger and Lipsey (2005)
extended their 2001 research by examining potential moderating effects of CBT interventions with 58
juvenile and adult offenders. Three moderator variables were identified that explained a significant
proportion of the variance in recidivism rates: (a) the level (i.e., low, moderate or high) of re-offence
risk of participants, with higher levels of risk being associated with larger effect sizes; (b)
maintenance of treatment integrity; and (c) the inclusion or exclusion of specific treatment
skill development in the treatment were associated with larger effect sizes. In contrast, smaller effect
sizes were found to be associated with the inclusion of victim impact and behaviour modification
treatment components. Additionally, results of the meta-analysis were consistent with previous
findings in showing the general CBT approach to produce a 50% greater reduction in recidivism over
24
and above that of the control group. Once the effects of the moderator variables were controlled for,
there were no differences in the effectiveness between specific, “brand name” CBT interventions
compared to a general CBT intervention. The authors concluded that the general CBT approach
appeared to have a positive effect upon adult and juvenile offender recidivism rates.
having provided a clear and empirically supported framework for effective intervention with offenders
(Ogloff & Davis, 2004). Further refinement of this model has been recommended (Andrews &
Dowden, 2007; Bonta & Andrews, 2003) including increased recognition of the role of broader
universal and psychological needs of offenders (Ward & Stewart, 2003). With regard to specific
modes of intervention in working with adult and juvenile offenders, there is now a substantial body of
research demonstrating that the CBT approach is effective in reducing reoffending (Lipsey, 2009).
Importantly, the meta-analytic research indicates several key treatment components that contribute to
integrity and the inclusion of anger control/self-regulation training and interpersonal problem solving
have indicated the need for offender treatment evaluation studies to include other relevant measures
In the next chapter, the relationship between operant and social learning theories are
Chapter 3
This chapter examines the theoretical context of aggressive behaviour relative to cognitive-
aimed at treating aggression. Firstly, a brief overview of Operant (Skinner, 1938, 1953) and Social
(Bandura, 1977) learning theories will be presented. These theoretical perspectives are described to
The behaviourist theory of operant learning (Skinner, 1938, 1953) proposed that behaviour is
contingent upon environmental consequences. Skinner (1938, p.138) formulated this theory in terms
turn produces a Consequence (i.e., ABC). Operant conditioning or learning was further elaborated by
Skinner with reference to two behavioural consequences that either increase (i.e., reinforcement) or
decrease (i.e., punishment) the frequency of a given behaviour. Two reinforcement contingencies
were proposed: (1) Positive reinforcement increases behaviour when the consequence of that
behaviour is experienced as rewarding (e.g., an aggressive youth threatens another youth with
violence if he does not hand over money. The money is promptly handed over and the aggressive
behaviour is positively reinforced); and (2) Negative reinforcement increases the likely repetition of a
becomes verbally abusive toward a parent when told she is grounded. The parent gives in and allows
the child to go out. The aggressive behaviour has been negatively reinforced by the removal of the
unwanted consequence (i.e., the grounding). In contrast, Skinner described two forms of punishment
that decrease the likelihood of a given behaviour: (1) Positive punishment occurs when a behaviour is
followed by an unwanted consequence (e.g., a youth commits a violent offence and is given a
custodial sentence by the courts); and (2) Negative punishment represents the removal of something
desirable following a behaviour (e.g., loss of privileges such as a parent grounding an adolescent). A
major criticism of operant learning theory has been its failure to provide detail of the role of internal,
cognitive processes and how they contribute to the learning and repetition of behaviour (Palmer,
2003). Bandura’s (1977) theory of social learning attempted to address the socio-cognitive aspects of
learning.
Social learning theory (Bandura, 1977) was developed in response to the perceived
behaviour was strictly viewed in terms of environmental consequences (Palmer, 2003). Bandura
attempted to explain behaviour by building on operant learning theories by including the internal,
cognitive process of observational learning. In essence, Bandura proposed that learning could occur
through observing the actions of another (i.e., modelling). The likelihood of the modelled behaviour
punishment). According to social learning theory, two main principles of motivation also play an
important role in maintaining behaviour: Vicarious learning operates through observing the
consequences of behaviour for others (optimally an esteemed other); and Self-reinforcement, which
27
refers to intrinsic motivation for behaviour implicit in self-validating thoughts and feelings (e.g., feeling
pride after completing a task and self-talk like “I did well”). Vicarious learning and self-reinforcement
serve to increase or decrease the repetition of a given behaviour (Bandura, 1977, 1986; Palmer,
2003).
Several researchers (Akers, 1999; Bandura, 1973; Nietzel, 1979) have applied social
learning principles to antisocial behaviour, arguing that salient models for aggressive and offending
behaviour include: family members; antisocial associates; and cultural influences (e.g., television,
film, videogames). In defining the interaction between the individual and environment, Goldstein
(1994) proposed a three-tier model of analysis for the geographical ecology of aggression: the
macrolevel provided an analysis of aggression at the international, national and regional level; the
mesolevel examined violence at the level of communities; and the microlevel analyses examined
violence as it occurs in the home. Goldstein et al. (1998) clearly view the development of aggressive
behaviour in terms of social learning theory, they wrote “Aggression is primarily learned behaviour,
learned by observation, imitation, direct experience and rehearsal” (p.3). While social learning theory
endeavored to take greater account of internal cognitive processes than earlier behaviourist learning
principles (Skinner, 1938; 1953) critics of the model argued that it did not go far enough in explaining
behavioural acquisition with respect to cognition (Palmer, 2003; Sestir & Bartholow, 2007). The next
section will examine cognitive-behavioural therapy (CBT) and its underlying theory which has
CBT draws heavily from the learning theories of behaviourism (Skinner, 1938, 1953) and the
increasing interest in the role of cognition pre-empted in social learning theory (Bandurra, 1977). In
28
particular, CBT with aggressive youth emphasises the importance of understanding the thoughts and
perceptions that lead to aggressive behaviour when a young person is faced with a perceived threat
or aggravation, actual or illusory (Lochman et al., 2000). According to Goldstein and Glick (1987)
interventions for antisocial and aggressive behaviour in young people need to address the cognitive,
behavioural and affective components of aggression to show even moderate treatment gains. CBT
interventions aim to do this through training in self-instruction and self-regulation techniques, social
problem-solving, affect labelling, social perspective taking (or moral reasoning), relaxation training,
and anger management skills. Figure 2 provides a useful representation of where various CBT
interventions sit within a continuum from purely cognitive to purely behaviourally oriented methods of
intervention.
29
representation of the cognitive-behavioural theory and methods that underlie ART. Typically CBT
based treatments employ an integrated multimodal approach in addressing the complexities of child,
adolescent and adult aggression (Fonagy & Kurtz, 2002; Kazdin, 1997; Lochman et al., 2000). The
theoretical basis of the CBT approach to understanding aggressive youth is based upon Novaco’s
(1979, 1994) conceptualisation of anger in adults, in which the relationship between precipitating
events and anger arousal is considered to be indirect. Rather, it is the individual’s cognitive
expectations and appraisals that serve to mediate an angry or aggressive response to a given
30
including maladaptive and antisocial behaviour, are framed in terms of the interaction between
(SIP; Crick & Dodge, 1994; Dodge, Laird, Lochman, & Zelli, 2002) models. SIP models provide a
theoretical framework that specifies the mental steps involved in social-cognition. The model
proposes that in responding to social stimuli, an individual initially encodes cues, then makes mental
representations of those cues, which are associated with emotional states and goals, and
subsequently generates possible behavioural responses, evaluates potential responses, and finally
makes a decision and enacts the selected behaviour. SIP theorists propose that through social
learning processes including situational specific reinforcement, modelling, and efficacy expectancies,
children develop stable patterns of social-cognitive processing, personality-like traits that serve as
Crick and Dodge (1994) proposed a non-linear, six-stage cognitive process involved in
children’s competent performance in social situations. In step 1, children encode aspects of the
external (i.e., an event) and internal (i.e., self-talk) cues. It is hypothesised that not all cues are
attended to but that social cues are selectively attended to. Step 2 involves interpretation of the
social cues, which may call upon mental representations stored in long-term memory. Other
interpretative processes include causal attributions, attribution of intent, and evaluation of goal
attainment. Self-evaluation occurs with respect to self-efficacy and outcome expectancies based
upon similar past experiences. All of these processes are influenced by social schemata, scripts and
social knowledge stored in long-term memory, which in turn can be altered or revised based upon
new experiences. Following encoding and interpretation of cues, Crick and Dodge proposed that in
Step 3, children enter into a process of goal clarification by way of selecting a desired outcome (e.g.,
31
keeping out of trouble or getting even). At Step 4, the child will extract from memory possible
responses to immediate social cues. Faced with a novel situation, the child may generate new
behaviour alternatives. Step 5 involves the evaluation of possible responses based on outcome
6, the selected response is behaviourally enacted. The entire mental process will begin again given
the external response of other players in the situation, which will feedback into the model at different
steps. The SIP model of behavioural acquisition imbues the individual with agency in actively
interpreting social information through cognitive processes (Sestir & Bartholow, 2007).
Dodge et al. (2002) reported strong evidence for the construct validity of the SIP model.
Using a sample comprising 387 children, from kindergarten to third grade, they analysed responses
to hypothetical vignettes that described social interactions involving either peer provocation or peer
group entry dilemmas. Children’s open ended responses designed to assess a hostile attribution
bias (i.e., a tendency to attribute hostile intent on the part of others) were rated in terms of intent-
“benign”, “hostile” or “don’t know”. A measure of social problem solving was coded for type of
response: aggressive (i.e., physical, verbal or threat); competent (i.e., socially appropriate); authority-
punish (i.e., appeals to have the provocateur punished); authority-intervene; passive-inept (i.e., a
non-assertive response); irrelevant other (i.e., a nonsense response); and unable to provide further
response. Children’s goal orientation- instrumental or pro-social was rated and emotional
understanding (personal and other) was also assessed. Several measures of parent and teacher
ratings of aggressive behaviour were also administered. Confirmatory factor analysis revealed
support for a partial mediation model of teacher rated aggression consistent with the SIP model.
Children’s emotional understanding (self and other) and specific social information processing
patterns (i.e., hostile attributions, hostile response generation, positive aggression evaluation, and
32
instrumental goal orientation) predicted aggressive behaviour in the classroom. Dodge and his
colleagues concluded that the support they found for a multi-dimensional information processing
model (i.e., SIP) as opposed to a uni-dimensional perceptive (e.g., social intelligence), has
behaviour should include components that address the multiple cognitive processing deficits and
Summary
Social information processing theory, unlike its behavioural and social learning predecessors,
was specifically developed in order to address issues of prevention and/or intervention among
aggressive children and youth (Sestir & Bartholow, 2007). Cognitive-behavioural theory has drawn
intervention aimed at altering the behaviours of persistently aggressive youth (Hollin, 2004; Palmer,
2003). In the next chapter the content and procedures and theory and evidence pertaining to each of
Chapter 4
framework, comprising three CBT components that are delivered in three weekly group training
sessions (one meeting per week for each of the three components) over a 10-week period. The
sessions typically last for about one hour and are co-facilitated. (1) Skillstreaming (the behavioural
component) is designed to teach the young person effective social skills aimed at displacing
aggressive behaviour; (2) Anger Control Training (the emotional component) aims to reduce the
frequency of anger arousal and to teach techniques of self-control when anger is aroused; and (3)
Moral Reasoning Training (the cognitive/values component) is designed to “facilitate progress along
the natural stage-sequential trajectory of moral-cognitive development so that youths will make more
Skillstreaming
that time, Goldstein (1973) referred to the intervention as Structured Learning Therapy- a prescriptive
behavioural psychotherapy designed for working with adult psychiatric patients prior to
social skills training (SST) curriculum aimed at increasing the repertoire of prosocial behaviours of
aggressive children and youth through a psychological skills training approach (Goldstein et al.,
1998). Learning to resolve problematic or conflictual social situations through the development of
social and problem-solving skills represents a cornerstone of CBT interventions with aggressive
34
children and youth (Fonagy & Kurtz, 2002). In this section, the theory, evidence and practice of SST
and social problem solving will be examined with specific reference to the research pertaining to
treatment integrity (Goldstein et al., 1998). The Skillstreaming curriculum comprises nine sequential
steps: (1) Define the skill- initially this is undertaken by group participants as a brainstorming session
(e.g., “What does ‘keeping out of fights’ mean for the group”?), the group responses are then
reframed by the trainers into a succinct definition of the given skill. Additionally the meaning and
reasoning behind each step is discussed and defined as a thinking (i.e., cognitive) or action (i.e.,
behavioural) or both (e.g., listening) step; (2) Model the skill- trainers model successful use of the skill
for trainees; (3) Establish trainee skill need- here the trainers ask group members to identify when,
where and with whom they could use the skill (e.g., “When was the last time you could have used the
skill… “Keeping out of fights”?); (4) Select a role-player- the trainer selects a group member to role-
play their scenario for the group; (5) Set up the role-play- the main actor selects a co-actor to role-
play their scenario with them, the role-plays are determined by group members responses at Step 3;
(6) Conduct the role-play; (7) Provide performance feedback in a prescribed order (i.e., co-actor,
group members, co-trainer, main-trainer, and main-actor); (8) Select next role-player and repeat
steps 5-7; and (9) Assign skill homework- Skillstreaming practice sheet (see Appendix A). Table 1
shows the social skill- “keeping out of fights”, trainer notes, suggested content for trainer models, and
Table 1
Skillstreaming skill- "keeping out of fights"
Definition: Avoiding unnecessary conflicts by controlling angry feelings (conflicts can be physical and/or
verbal).
1. Stop and think about what triggered you to Stay calm. What event or action are you becoming
want to fight. (Think) angry about?
2. Decide what you want to happen in your What will be the long-range outcome? Do you want to
future. (Think) be treated with respect? What will increase your
personal power?
3. Think about other ways to handle the You might negotiate, stand up for your rights, ask for
situation besides fighting. (Think) help, use humour, or pacify the person.
4. Decide on the best way to handle the Which of your options from step four is least likely to
situation and do it. (Think & Act) escalate the situation?
School or neighbourhood: Main actor tells classmate that he/she wants to talk out their differences
instead of being pressured to fight. Main actor asks a neighbour to speak with their kids about throwing
rocks at him/her rather than beating them up.
Home: Main actor resolves potential fight with older sibling by asking parent to intervene. Main actor
resolves an argument with a parent by agreeing to complete a task as requested, thus pacifying the
parent.
Peer group: Main actor goes for help when he/she sees peers fighting on school steps. Main actor
decides to ignore the insulting remarks made by a peer who feels it is necessary to fight to “prove
yourself.”
Comments: Defining what a fight entails is generally useful when doing this skill. Many trainees see fights
as merely physical, which is not the case. Prior to teaching or reviewing this skill, it is often useful to
review or teach Using Self-Control. These are both excellent skills to use as reinforcement with the Self-
Control Chain.
36
Ten core social skills are delivered over the 10-week Skillstreaming curriculum form the
standard ART curriculum (see Appendix A). While the skills and skill steps change on a weekly basis,
the procedures do not. It is important to note that the overarching goal of Skillstreaming is to prepare
participants for real-life pro-social social interaction. As such, the content of each session should
parallel interpersonal relationships that are relevant to youth. The models provided by trainers
therefore entail scenarios that depict social competence in youth-relevant relationships including
peers, family members, teachers and other significant agents of change in the young peoples lives,
this maximises the potential for transfer and maintenance of treatment gains (Goldstein, 2004).
behavioural, cognitive, and affective skills that when well integrated enable an individual to apply
those combined skills adaptively in multiple social situations. Bierman and Welsh also make
reference to the notion that social competence is a subjective measure of an individual’s ability to
perform social tasks successfully within a given social milieu. The performance of a given social skill
and the capacity to socially problem solve are likely to be part of that assessment. Similarly, Cook et
al. (2008) argued that social competence represents the ability to successfully negotiate a wide array
of interpersonal interactions in a variety of social settings. They argued that social skills are learned
behaviours that are associated with the subjective measure of social competence.
One of the earliest definitions of the social skills construct was provided by Argyle and Kendon
(1967). They proposed three interrelated components that constitute socially skilled performance: (1)
social perception- refers to the ability to perceive and correctly interpret verbal and nonverbal social
cues. In the previously described social information processing (SIP; Crick & Dodge, 1994) model,
37
social perception represents the combination of Step 1- encoding and Step 2- interpretation; (2)
social cognition- following perception and interpretation of social cues, the individual makes a
decision regarding a suitable response. Here an amalgamation of SIP Steps 3- goal clarification, and
Step 4- Response access or construction drawn from memory schemas, and Step 5- response
decisions are performed; and (3) social performance- refers to directly observable social behaviour,
In a comprehensive review, Gresham, Cook, Crews, and Kern (2004) attempted to resolve
disparities in defining the social skill construct. They examined and synthesised the SST literature
published from 1980 to 2004, in part to assess the construct validity of SST interventions. Based on
their findings, they concluded that social skills involved three main areas: (a) social interaction; (b)
prosocial behaviour; and (c) social-cognitive skills, all three of which form the basis of a SST
intervention. Furthermore, these domains are consistent with the construct validity reported for
behaviour rating scales such as the Social Skills Rating System (Gresham & Elliot, 1990) and the
School Social Behaviour Scales (Merrel, 2002) which are widely used measures in assessing child
Social problem solving was defined by D’Zurilla and Nezu (2001) as “the self-directed
solutions for specific problems encountered in everyday living” (p.112). The ability to problem solve
within a social context, like the effective use of social skills, plays a key role in the construct of social
competence (McGuire, 2001). D’Zurilla and Goldfried (1971) conceptualised problem solving in terms
of a five-stage, sequential cognitive process: (1) problem orientation; (2) defining and formulating the
problem; (3) generating alternative solutions; (4) decision making; and (5) implementation of a
solution and verification of effectiveness. The incorporation of the cognitive steps delineated in
38
D’Zurilla and Goldfried’s model is paralleled in the SIP model later proposed by Crick and Dodge
Social problem solving training approaches, like social skills training, specify a series of
sequential steps in teaching social problem solving skills (McMurran, Egan, & Duggan, 2005). Those
steps broadly follow the problem solving model developed by D’Zurilla and Goldfried (1971). For
example, Spivack and Shure’s (1982) interpersonal cognitive problem-solving (ICPS) training teaches
six key ICPS skills to aggressive children and youth: (a) generation of alternative solutions; (b)
consideration of consequences of behaviour to self and other; (c) development of means-ends (i.e.,
sequential) thinking; (d) promotion of social-causal thinking (i.e., the impact of behaviour upon
others); (e) sensitivity to problems (i.e., knowledge of situational specific problems that can occur);
and (f) dynamic orientation (i.e., understanding that behavioural motivations may not always be
clear). While social problem solving training is not an explicit component of ART (Goldstein, Glick, &
Gibbs, 1998) the concepts and indeed some of the procedures are intrinsic to all components of the
program.
Social skills training, as the behavioural enactment of social competence, is more closely
aligned to a behavioural theoretical approach (see Figure 2, p.30). Whereas, social problem solving
training has greater association with cognitively oriented modes of behaviour change. While these
therapeutic methods are presented as distinct modalities of intervention, it is important to note that all
are variants of CBT and that in practical terms there is considerable overlap in the processes and
procedures of each of these modes of change (Gundersen & Svartdal, 2006; Longmore & Worrell,
2007). Moreover, as already indicated, both social skills and social problem-solving represent
interdependent components of the social competence construct with measurable improvement in one
Social skill deficits and poor social competence are evident in the aetiology and maintenance
1994; Reinecke & Ginsburg, 2008; Segrin, 2000); social anxiety (Beidel, Turner, Young, & Paulson,
2005); social phobia (Spence, 2000); Autism Spectrum Disorders (Bellini & Peters, 2008; White,
Keonig, & Scahill, 2007); ADHD (Fenstermacher, 2006; Pfiffner & McBurnett, 1997); and conduct
problems (Bullis, Walker, & Sprague, 2001; van Manen, Prins, & Emmelkamp, 2004). Similarly,
social problem-solving deficits among aggressive and antisocial youth have also been reported
(Cunliffe, 1992; Goldstein, 1999; Lochman et al., 2000). Compared to their socially competent peers,
antisocial youth have deficits in social competence that are a significant risk factor in the
maintenance of aggressive behaviours and poor psychosocial outcomes into adulthood (Moffitt et al.,
2002; Patterson, Reid, & Dishion, 1992). This is not to suggest, however, that all young offenders
consistently show deficits in social competence, or that the construct can be used to differentiate
Gresham (1997) distinguished between social skill acquisition deficits (i.e., the child does not
possess the skill) and social skill performance deficits (i.e., the skill is known but poorly enacted).
SST programs work from the premise that aggressive children and youth show evidence of both
(Goldstein, 1999). Spence (2003) suggested that performance deficits among aggressive youth
“For example, the conduct disordered adolescent may find it more effective, and may receive
more positive reinforcement from the deviant peer group, if they engage in physical violence
Hence SST interventions such as Skillstreaming generally employ principles of operant learning
(Skinner,1953) for contingency management (e.g., reward systems) and social learning (Bandura,
1977) in which three specific behaviour change strategies are used: (a) an exemplar models the
desired behaviour (modelling); (b) frequent opportunity for guided practice and rehearsal of the
desired behaviour (role-play) preferably across various settings (in-vivo); and (c) the modelling and
transfer and maintenance of skill acquisition. Cook and his colleagues (2008) summarised four
common objectives of SST programs, including Skillstreaming: (1) promotion of prosocial skill
acquisition; (2) improving social competence through accurate skill performance; (3) reducing or
replacing antisocial behaviours; and (4) ensuring transfer and maintenance of social skills beyond the
treatment setting.
Support for the effectiveness of SST as stand alone intervention in treating various emotional
and behavioural disorders of childhood and adolescence is mixed (Cook et al., 2008). In particular,
the effectiveness of SST with secondary school aged youth has been questioned on the basis of the
widely held belief that once antisocial behaviour has become entrenched in adolescence, the
effectiveness of behaviour change programs is at best limited (Cook et al.). Contrary to this view,
Gresham, Cook, Crews, and Kern (2004) reported that 64% of students with emotional and/or
included 338 studies with more than 25,000 children and youth between the ages of 3-18 years.
41
In contrast, Quinn, Kavale, Mathur, Rutherford and Forness (1999) conducted a meta-
analysis of 35 studies published between 1981 and 1994. Studies were selected for inclusion on the
basis of three criteria. First, participants in each of the studies had to meet eligibility for special
education under the US Federal Government classification of emotionally disturbed. Second, the
focus of the research had to be an investigation of the effectiveness of an SST program. Third, the
data within each study had to allow for the calculation of an effect size. The average age of
participants was 11.53 years. The analysis of the pooled data revealed a small mean effect size of
.199. According to Gresham and his colleagues (2004) of the 35 studies included in the Quinn et al.
meta-analysis only two met the first of their inclusion criteria. Additionally, the Quinn et al. analysis
did not require the primary studies to have a control group, or specific research design (i.e.,
experimental or quasi-experimental). A further criticism was the inclusion of studies that employed
measure of improvement in social skills. Gresham et al. argued that these variables are not targets of
change within the SST construct, and thus the Quinn et al. meta-analysis is “tainted theoretically by
Drawing on the meta-analysis of Gresham et al. (2004) Cook et al. (2008) conducted a
“mega-analysis” (p.134) to examine the effectiveness of SST for secondary school aged students
either suffering or at risk of EBD (N=5000). The results were similar to that of the earlier study by
Gresham et al. Approximately two thirds of secondary students with EBD, compared to one third of
controls, showed evidence of improved social skills following SST. However, the inclusion of only
assessment of SST effectiveness under “real world” conditions. Cook and his colleagues suggested
42
that future researchers should turn their attention to the evaluation of SST in school and community
van Manen, Prins and Emmelkamp (2004) conducted an effectiveness study in which a
social-cognitive intervention program (SCIP) based on Crick and Dodge’s (1994) SIP model (n= 42)
and a stand alone SST intervention (n= 40), and a waitlist control group (n= 15) were compared on
several behavioural, affective, and cognitive outcome measures. Participants included 97 aggressive
boys, between 9- to 13- years, attending outpatient mental health clinics throughout the Netherlands.
All participants met DSM-IV (APA, 1994) criteria for CD or ODD, or Disruptive Behaviour Disorder-
Not Otherwise Specified. The SCIP included Spivack and Shure’s (1982) ICPS training and a social-
cognitive component designed to increase self-reflection and perspective taking abilities and reduce
self-centred cognitive distortions (drawing on the work of Selman, 1971, 1980) and an affective self-
control training component. Both the SST and social-cognitive treatment conditions were delivered
over 11-weeks in a group treatment format. Various parent, teacher and child self-report measures of
aggressive behaviour, self-control, social-cognitive skills, and social skills were administered at pre-
and post- treatment and one -year follow-up. Results showed a significant reduction in aggressive
behaviour and significant improvement across all other measures for both treatment conditions,
compared to no change for the waitlist control group. Additionally, there was a significant difference
between the SCIP and SST treatment conditions with the SCIP condition showing improvement on
more of the outcome measures at one-year follow-up. At one-year follow-up effect sizes of 0.56 for
the SST condition and 0.76 for the SCIP condition were reported. The greater effect size for the SCIP
treatment is not surprising given that it is in line with current treatment recommendations for conduct
disordered children and youth (Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group, 1999a, 1999b). The
SCIP represents a multi-modal treatment package similar to ART. Importantly, and in contrast to the
43
findings of Quinn et al. (1999) SST also showed a significant, moderate strength effect when
considered as a stand alone treatment. It was concluded that interventions with aggressive children
and youth that address both cognitive deficiencies and cognitive distortions such as the SCIP (and
indeed ART) show greater improvement after treatment than interventions that only address cognitive
deficiencies as with stand alone SST programs (van Manen et al., 2004).
In a review of the SST theory and research literature, Bullis, Walker and Sprague (2001)
identified several areas of weakness in the implementation and evaluation of SST programs. In
particular, they noted the relative lack of longitudinal research that might provide empirical support for
the maintenance and transfer of post treatment gains. In the absence of such data, it is not possible
to determine the extent to which SST programs provide a long-term preventative effect in reducing
aggressive and antisocial behaviour and the associated negative life outcomes (Cook et al., 2008).
Moreover, Cook etal. indicated that SST interventions needed to be provided within a
“comprehensive intervention approach” (p.82) for high risk antisocial youth. Again this
research findings clearly support over that of a single method approach (Conduct Problems
As previously stated, ART does not include a specific social problem-solving component.
However, the cognitive-behavioural training procedures that are common to social problem-solving
treatment models, such as that previously described in Spivack and Shure’s (1982) interpersonal
cognitive problem-solving (ICPS) program, are consistent with, explicit and/or implicit, aspects of
program content across all three components of ART (i.e., Skillstreaming, Anger Control Training,
44
and Moral Reasoning Training). The social problem-solving evaluation research is, therefore, not
reviewed here. However, as this study was interested in evaluating potential changes in social
problem solving abilities following treatment, a social problem solving measure was included. Hence,
this section will briefly review the evidence pertaining to social problem-solving deficits among
Numerous studies have identified various social-cognitive skill deficits and distortions
among aggressive children and youth (Dodge & Coie, 1987; Dodge & Pettit, 2003; Hollin, 2003;
Järvinen, 2001; Lansford et al., 2006). Specifically, compared to their non-aggressive age-related
peers, aggressive youth have been shown to: misattribute hostile intentions to others and fail to take
into account non-hostile cues, usually referred to as a hostile attribution bias (Crick & Dodge, 1994;
Dodge & Coie, 1987; Järvinen, 2001); select instrumental goals- such as winning a game, rather than
interpersonal goals- like the development or maintenance of friendship (Lansford et al., 2006; Slaby &
Guerra, 1988); generate fewer alternative solutions to social problems (Palmer & Hollin, 1999) and
rely on aggressive responses and a tendency to evaluate the outcomes of those responses in
Slaby and Guerra (1988) examined the cognitive mediators of aggression among 144
participants (equal number of males and females) ranging in age from 15-18 years, with a mean age
of 17-years. They compared three groups of adolescents: (a) antisocial/violent juvenile offenders; (b)
high-aggressive secondary school students; and (c) low-aggressive secondary school students.
Three categories of cognitive content were assessed: (a) beliefs legitimising aggression; (b) beliefs
about the expected outcome for the aggressor; and (c) beliefs about the expected outcome for the
victim. It was predicted that, compared to the low-aggressive students, the aggressive offenders and
high-aggressive students would be more likely to hold beliefs that aggression increases self-esteem
45
and avoids a negative image among peers and that victims deserve what they get and don’t really
suffer anyway.
With respect to social-cognitive processes, Slaby and Guerra (1988) made hypotheses
relating to six information-processing components delineated in Crick and Dodge’s (1994) model.
Specifically, it was hypothesised that compared to low-aggressors, the aggressive offenders and
high-aggressors would: (a) seek less information; (b) attribute hostility to others and respond with
aggression; (c) generate fewer social solutions; (d) anticipate fewer consequences for an aggressive
response; and (e) select fewer “best” and “second best” solutions (i.e., “a solution judged by others to
be nonviolent and goal-directed”; Slaby & Guerra; p.581), which would less likely to be rated as
effective. The results indicated support for all of the hypotheses. A significant positive relationship
was also found between higher levels of aggression and cognitive content that endorsed aggressive
Järvinen and Pakaslahti (1999) reported similar findings in a study comparing the social
problem solving strategies employed by aggressive versus sociable children and youth in a seven-
year longitudinal, repeated measures study. A total of 47 aggressive children, 26 girls and 21 boys,
were assessed at baseline (10-11 years of age) via peer nominations for level of aggressiveness
(e.g., “Who starts fighting in a conflict situation in the school yard”?) versus non-aggressiveness (e.g.,
“Who never fights”?). The same participants were followed-up seven years later (17-18 years of age)
and again peer nominations were used to determine levels of aggression and a vignette based
measure of social problem solving was administered at both assessment points. Results revealed
overall stability of aggressive behaviour, although 11 participants moved from the non-aggressive to
the aggressive group and five female participants originally deemed aggressive moved into the non-
aggressive group. Moreover, stability of aggressive behaviour for both male and female participants
46
was predicted by aggressive childhood problem solving strategies and a lack of non-aggressive
solutions. A strength of the Järvinen and Pakaslahti study was the length of interval between initial
and follow-up assessments, which provided reliable, long-term evidence in support of the reported
association between social problem solving deficits and aggressive behaviour. However, a high
attrition rate from the original cohort (N=120) due to problems tracking original participants, made it
difficult to determine if the lack of males in the childhood aggressive group who subsequently might
have moved into the late-adolescent non-aggressive group is attributable to a real-world phenomena
or a biased sample. Järvinen and Pakaslahti concluded that their results were consistent with the
findings of other research (Crick & Dodge, 1994; Dodge & Coie, 1987; Slaby & Guerra, 1988) in
which teaching problem solving strategies to aggressive children and youth can mitigate aggressive
Summary
The research discussed in the previous sections clearly shows an association between
cognitive deficiencies (e.g., social skills and social problem solving) and cognitive distortions (e.g.,
hostile attribution bias and cognitive content advocating aggression) which have been implicated in
the onset and maintenance of aggressive behaviour (Gibbs, 2003; Goldstein et al., 1998; Lansford et
al., 2006; van Manen et al., 2004). As already noted, ART attempts to address both deficiencies and
distortions through the cognitive, behavioural and affective components of the program. In particular,
the aim of Skillstreaming is to provide frequent opportunity, through role-plays, for youth to practice
pro-social skill acquisition and non-aggressive social problem solving strategies. Cook et al. (2008)
indicated that the evidence in support of SST interventions has been mixed, particularly with respect
to youth in the mid- to late- adolescent age range. Indeed none of studies reported in this section
47
included participants above the age of 18-years. Given the age range of participants in the present
studies (i.e., 17 to 19.5 years for both pilot and main studies) there is an opportunity to address this
gap in the research literature relative to the effects of SST interventions with aggressive young
adults. In the next section, the theory and evidence pertaining to Anger Control Training, the affective
According to Goldstein et al. (1998) the intention of Anger Control Training (ACT) is twofold:
(a) to reduce the frequency of anger arousal and (b) to teach techniques of self-control when anger is
aroused. ACT, the affective component of ART, was originally developed by Feindler and Ecton
(1986) and later adapted by Goldstein and colleagues to fit within the multi-component intervention of
advances in approaches to treatment, the predominant approach in treating aggressive youth relied
on the provision of contingencies within a behaviour modification framework (Feindler & Baker,
2004). The use of contingencies within the controlled treatment setting usually saw initial reduction in
aggressive behaviour, however these gains frequently failed to transfer when the youth returned to
their natural environment. A natural environment which according to Goldstein et al. provided
frequent reinforcement of the notion that “might make’s right”. This is the earliest, most immature
stage of sociomoral development in which physical dominance is the basis for moral decision making
(this concept is further elaborated in the next section- Moral Reasoning). This coupled with a
behaviourist approach that failed to take full account of the cognitive and sociomoral components of
adolescent aggression was largely responsible for the apparent failure of transfer. In their approach
to treating adolescent anger problems, Feindler and Ecton drew upon Novaco’s (1975) adaption of
48
stress inoculation training (SIT) which Meichenbaum (1977) developed for the treatment of anxiety
disorders and impulsive children. In this section, the theory underpinning cognitive-behavioural
approaches to anger management interventions is outlined. The research concerned with evaluating
these interventions is reviewed, with specific reference to evaluations of ACT as a stand alone
intervention.
Anger control training: Content and procedure. Anger control techniques provide
instruction in self-regulatory skills that can serve to inhibit emotional and physiological arousal, in
anger provoking situations. This is achieved through a didactic, psycho-educational approach that
trains young people to become increasingly aware of the interaction between the cognitive,
physiological and behavioural components of their anger. ACT is typically presented in weekly,
group sessions. A minimum 10-week program is optimal, although programmes can be adapted to
individual treatment and extended time periods with chronically aggressive youth (Feindler &
Guttman, 1994).
modelled for participants. Then participants perform role-plays of real-life anger provoking situations
in which anger reduction techniques, such as deep breathing, counting backwards and use of
pleasant imagery, are used. Participants learn to identify physiological cues (e.g., clenched fists,
shallow/rapid breathing) that represent the early indicators of anger arousal, as well as self-
statements that are likely to increase or maintain arousal (e.g., “He meant that”. “I’ll show him”). ACT
homework tasks include variations of Feindler and Guttman’s (1994) “Hassle Log”. The Hassle Log
It consists of a “what”’, “when”’, “where” and “‘with whom” format for monitoring anger, in addition to a
49
self-evaluation of behavioural responses and an anger rating scale. The content of the hassle log is
Although the content differs, the procedure in ACT is similar to that of Skillstreaming.
Goldstein et al. (1998) outlined nine sequential steps involved in ACT comprising the Anger Control
Chain. As shown in Figure 3, a new strategy is introduced into the Anger Control Chain each week,
As shown in Figure 3, the Anger Control Chain begins with teaching youth to identify two
types of Triggers- (a) external triggers- the anger arousing event; and (b) internal triggers- the
negative self-statements that increase and perpetuate anger arousal (e.g., “Who does he think he is?
I’ll show him”); Cues/Body Signs- physiological cues of anger; Anger Reducers- strategies aimed at
reducing anger arousal through the use of cognitive distracters, relaxation techniques, and buying
time to think through a non-aggressive response (i.e., deep breathing, backward counting, and
pleasant imagery); Reminders- short positive self-statements that are self-instructional, increase self-
50
control, and are designed to counter the negative self-statements at the stage of internal triggers
(e.g., “I’m in control”, “its not worth it”); Thinking Ahead- designed to teach consequential thinking in
broad terms including short-term, long-term, internal (i.e., “how will I feel”?), external (i.e.,
consequences imposed by an external agent), and social (e.g., loss of friends) consequences; Angry
Behaviour Cycle- up until this point the focus of ACT has been upon how participants should respond
to perceived provocation by others (i.e., external triggers). In week six, the Angry Behaviour Cycle
explores what participants do to initiate anger in others and how one person’s angry response can
often trigger a similar response in another and keep angry feelings going back and forth without
resolution; Social Skills- here Skillstreaming merges with ACT, in that participants select an
appropriate social skill to incorporate into the role-plays of the anger control chain; Self-evaluation-
teaches participants to realistically self-reflect on: (a) how well they have handled a conflict; (b) self-
reward for handling it well; and (c) critical self-evaluation of what could have done better (i.e., self-
coaching). The final two weeks of ACT are dedicated to the role-play rehearsal of the full sequence of
Kassinove and Tafrate (2006) argued that confusion in distinguishing between the emotional
state of anger and related cognitive and behavioural constructs such as aggression, hostility and
violence has led to confusion among clinicians and researchers regarding the goals of treatment for
anger-related disorders. This, in turn, has contributed to ineffective treatment models that have
tended toward a hegemonic approach in which a specific aspect of the construct has been
emphasised (e.g., behavioural) over other important aspects (e.g., physiological or cognitive arousal).
comprising affective, behavioural, cognitive and physiological aspects that affect both experience and
expression (Spielberger, Reheiser, & Sydeman, 1995). The definition of anger provided by Novaco
(1998) held significance for the interpersonal aspect of the emotional state- “a negatively toned
perceived to be the source of an aversive event” (p.13). Navaco’s (1975) model of anger as a
frequent, although not necessary, precursor to interpersonal violence, has been the predominant
dysfunctional manifestations of this common emotional state (Hollin & Bloxsom, 2007). Figure 4
According to Novaco (1975, 1997) the subjective experience of anger represents a complex
interplay between environmental activation (i.e., perceived threat or verbal or physical provocation)
physiological arousal (e.g., increased heart rate, increased body temperature, muscular tension)
cognitive structures and processes (e.g., attributions, affect labelling, information processing,
schemas) subjective affective experience (i.e., intensity and frequency) and behavioural enactment
(e.g., physical and emotional withdrawal, physical or verbal aggression). Novaco’s multi-component
model indicates the potential for bi-directional change between the behavioural, cognitive and
physiological aspects of anger arousal, so that change in one aspect of the model can produce
53
reciprocal change in another. This is consistent with the aforementioned social information
In addition to the influence of Novaco’s (1975) model of anger, ACT has drawn extensively
on the work of early developmental theorists (Luria, 1961; Vygotsky, 1962) in which the role of self-
talk has been implicated in self-regulation and behavioural disinhibition. Self-talk, also known as
private speech, inner dialogue, self-statements, and inner speech, refers to the experience of talking
to oneself, initially through external/overt verbalisations and later internal/covert speech (Brinthaupt,
Hein, & Kramer, 2009). Both Luria and Vygotsky provided evidence that in early cognitive
development, children are reliant upon the verbal commands of adult caregivers for regulation of
behaviour. Subsequently, the child’s overt self-verbalisations increasingly provide opportunity for self-
regulation, usurping the need for external, usually parental, controls. Eventually the child’s use of
overt self-verbalisations gives way to covert, inner speech representing a critical developmental step
toward self-directed and self-motivated regulation of behaviour. Several studies (see Harris, 1990 for
review) have provided support for Vygotsky’s early developmental theories. A series of investigations
conducted by Luria showed some support for the effectiveness of interventions aimed at remediating
the consequences of failures in the development of internalised private speech among children
showing signs of hyperactivity and poor impulse control. However, Luria’s research conclusions have
been questioned on the basis of small samples and overreliance upon observational design, which
was poorly described and lacking in the detail required for replication (Harris, 1990).
The intervention procedures employed by Luria were later adapted by Meichenbaum and
developing increased self-control among behaviourally disordered second grade children (N=15). The
54
training included a sequence of procedures similar to those employed in ACT: (a) the trainer models
task performance and self-instructs aloud while the child observes; (b) the child performs the same
task while the trainer instructs the child aloud; (c) the child performs the task again while self-
instructing aloud; (d) the child again performs the task while whispering self-instructional statements;
(e) and finally the child performs the task with covert, inner self-instruction (without whispering). The
results of their study revealed a significant improvement for the treatment group on measures of
cognitive performance and impulsivity compared to an attentional control group (i.e., cognitive
modelling without self-instructional training) and a no-treatment control group. The improvements
were maintained at one month follow-up. In a further study, Meichenbaum and Goodman reported
compared to children assessed as reflective. According to their observations, the impulsive children
tended to employ immature and self-stimulatory self-statements, whereas the private speech of the
reflective children was observed to be significantly more self-guiding and instrumental in content with
greater use of inaudible verbalisations (i.e., private speech). With reference to the development of
internalised private speech and its relationship to anger control, Novaco wrote: “A basic premise is
that anger is fomented, maintained, and influenced by the self-statements that are made in
provocation situations” (p.17). Similarly, Bandura (2007) noted the significance of self-talk in the
acquisition of socio-cognitive skills among children, suggesting that “models verbalise aloud their
reasoning strategies as they engage in problem-solving activities. The thoughts guiding their
decisions and actions are thus made observable and acquirable” (p.57). Given that impulsivity is a
criminogenic risk factor (Andrews et al., 1990; Andrews & Dowden, 2007) the relationship between
impoverished private speech and impulsivity reported by Meichenbaum and Goodman has important
The role of poor impulse control (i.e., impulsivity) has been well established as an important
risk factor in the development and maintenance of antisocial and aggressive behaviour (Barratt,
1994; Eysenck & Eysenck, 1977; Eysenck & McGurk, 1980; Moffitt, 1993). Moreover, the longitudinal
research conducted by Moffitt and her colleagues (1993; 2001; 2002) has implicated deficits in
impulse control in the development of the previously defined life course-persistent (LCP) offending.
As noted, impulsivity has also been identified as a dynamic risk factor (i.e., changeable) among
offender populations (Andrews, 1996). According to Andrews and his associates (Andrews, Bonta &
Hoge, 1990; Andrews et al., 1990) the dynamic risk factors should be the target of any treatment
where the overarching goal is to reduce recidivism (i.e, Need Principle). In defining impulsivity,
Eysenck and Eysenck (1977) described both pro-social and antisocial aspects of the construct. They
found that impulsiveness was related to the higher order personality factors of Psychoticism (i.e,
lesser extent Extroversion (i.e., outgoing sociability, with a tendency to seek out novel situations).
Eysenck et al. (1985) later defined socially acceptable impulsivity as Venturesomeness, not unlike
was labeled Impulsiveness. Similarly, Dickman (1990) reported a two-factor structure for
Functional impulsiveness was more closely aligned with extroversion, while dysfunctional
Another line of research supporting the relationship between impulsivity and aggression has
been the typological study of reactive versus proactive aggression. Numerous studies (Crick &
56
Dodge, 1991; Dodge & Coie, 1987; Marcus & Kramer, 2001; Raine et al., 2006) have found support
for the broad distinction made between reactive and proactive aggressive types, each with differing
etiological and developmental trajectories. Reactive aggression has been defined as a “hot blooded”,
defensive or retaliatory response to a perceived threat and is associated with autonomic arousal and
emotional reactivity. In contrast, proactive aggression has been defined in terms of goal directed or
instrumental aggression with the intent to gain power over others in the interests of material gain
and/or dominance (Dodge et al., 1990; Dodge, 1991). There is some research to suggest that
physiological underarousal is associated with proactive aggression, whereas overarousal has been
associated with reactive aggression (Raine et al.; Scarpa & Raine, 1997). Although impulsivity has
been found to be significantly associated with both reactive and proactive aggressive subtypes, the
relationship has generally been found to be stronger with the reactive type (Felthous & Barratt, 2003;
Raine et al.).
Several studies (Carroll et al., 2006; Luengo, Carrillo-de-la-Peña, Otero, & Romero, 1994;
Smith & Waterman, 2006) have investigated differences on measures of impulsivity among offender
They assessed a large (N=1,226) mixed gender sample of school aged adolescents- 12 to 18 years
(M=14.28 years; SD= 1.91) at two time points, with one year between assessments. Participants
history. According to scores on the Esyenck Impulsivity Scale for children (Eysenck, Easting, &
Pearson, 1984) both the delinquent groups reported higher impulsivity at both time points and higher
impulsivity was associated with future risk of antisocial behaviour. The strongest correlations between
57
impulsivity and types of antisocial behaviour were associated with rule breaking, vandalism, and
aggression.
among 129 (114 males, 15 females) adolescents (M age = 15.52 years). Three equal sized (n = 43)
groups of early-onset, late-onset and non-offenders were compared on an abbreviated version of the
Impulsiveness Questionnaire (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1977) self-reported offence history, and several
tests of cognitive function. Results showed that all seven of the offence history variables (i.e., school
misdemeanors, stealing, soft- and hard- drug use; vehicle-related offenses, abuse of property, and
physical aggression) were positively associated with the two offender groups. Consistent with
Moffitt’s (1993, 2001, 2002) conceptualisation of LCP offenders, Carol et al. found that the early-
onset offender group reported significantly higher incidences of offending behaviours than the late-
onset group, except for soft-drug use and abuse of property. The offender groups also reported
significantly higher levels of delinquency than the non-offender group. Impulsivity also differed
between the offender and non-offender groups. Although no significant difference on impulsiveness
was found between the late and early onset offenders, both showed greater impulsivity than their
Smith and Waterman (2006) examined self-reported aggressive behaviour and impulsivity
among a group of incarcerated violent and non-violent male (n=115) and female (n=133) offenders,
compared to male (n=114) and female (n=122) non-offenders in the UK. The age range of female
participants was 15-49 years and for males 18-49 years. The violent male and female offenders did
not differ significantly with respect to self-reported aggression and impulsivity scores. Additionally,
Smith and Waterman found a significant increasing linear trend in aggression and impulsivity from
significantly higher levels of impulsivity than the other groups. Smith and Waterman concluded their
results showed further support for the contention that increased impulsivity can reliably distinguish
offender from non-offender populations and violent compared to non-violent offenders, although not
Many of the impulsivity treatment outcome studies have been clinical trials of the effects of
outcomes with respect to psychological interventions aimed at reducing impulsivity have tended to
report findings relative to school-aged children (Baer & Nietzel, 1991; Kendall & Finch, 1978;
Meichenbaum & Goodman, 1971) with or without a diagnosis of ADHD. Baer and Nietzel conducted
age = 9.62 years) impulsivity was investigated. They reported that CBT interventions for impulsivity
for the treated children were close to the normative group average both before and after treatment.
Baer and Nietzel concluded that this finding may have been due to confounding the definitions of
impulsivity with externalizing behaviours in the primary studies, arguing that the two constructs may
or may not be correlated. In addition, they questioned the validity of the main measure of impulsivity
(i.e., Matching Familiar Figures Test; Kagan, 1966 as cited in Baer & Nietzel) and the reliability of the
normative data provided. Overall, the findings of this meta-analysis were inconclusive with respect to
determining the effectiveness of CBT interventions with impulsive children. Moreover the
In a study of young-adult university students, McMurran, Blair and Egan (2002) investigated
the relationship between aggression, impulsiveness, social problem solving, and alcohol use among
59
70 non-offender, British males (M age = 27 years). Contrary to expectation alcohol use did not
correlate with any of the other measures in the study. However, impulsivity was found to be
negatively related to social problem solving, and social problem solving was negatively related to
aggression. There was no direct relationship between impulsivity and aggression, rather the
relationship was found to be mediated by social problem solving. That is, heightened impulsivity
predicted social problem solving deficits, which in turn predicted increased aggression in this student
sample. McMurran et al. suggested that impulsivity in early childhood may act as a learning obstacle
with respect to the acquisition of social problem solving abilities. Later, social problem solving deficits
social difficulties. With respect to reducing aggression, McMurran et al. concluded that treatments
that teach social problem solving skills to aggressive youth are likely to prove beneficial. Equally,
(Meichenbaum & Goodman, 1971) might prevent social problem solving deficits thereby reducing the
One study that did examine impulsivity as a treatment outcome with aggressive offenders
was the first evaluation of ART conducted by Goldstein and Glick (1987). They reported a significant
reduction for worker-reported impulsivity in the treatment group (n=24) compared to a “brief
instruction control group” (n=24) and a no-treatment control (n=12). The ART-group, compared to
both control groups, was also found to have significantly fewer incidences of acting out behaviours,
as indicated by institutional incidence reports. These results were replicated for the 36 control group
participants who later completed the ART program. Despite the well established link between
outcome among offenders has been relatively lacking. Some evidence in support of the CBT
60
treatment procedures included in ART (e.g., self-instructional-, social skills-, and anger control-
training) has been provided in studies showing reduced impulsivity with children (Baer & Nietzel,
1991; Kendall & Finch, 1978; Meichenbaum & Goodman, 1971). However, there is a need for further
investigation of the effectiveness of these methods among older, aggressive offenders and the
Evaluations of ACT
There are few published evaluations of ACT as a stand alone intervention. The two studies
reviewed here provide some evidence for effectiveness in treating aggressive adolescents.
Additionally, these studies provide some insight into the difficulties faced by program
treatment integrity and facilitator training and competency are important considerations in program
outcomes studies and can have significant implications in regard to outcomes. This was certainly the
case in the evaluation of ACT conducted by Nugent, Champlin and Wiinimaki (1997).
Nugent et al. (1997) evaluated ACT with 102 males aged between 12 and 18 years (M age =
14.7 years) living in a residential group home for adolescent males with chronic behavioural
problems. Of the total sample, nine living in one group home received ACT, while four in another
group home did not, and the remaining 89 participants formed a no-treatment comparison group
drawn from a previous study conducted by the researchers. Variations in length of custody meant
that participants in the treatment group received varying lengths of ACT. After controlling for the
covariate- length of time in custody, the results showed that the length of time in ACT was
Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach, 1991) and case note reports of acting out behaviours. The behavioural
61
improvement for ACT participants was significantly greater compared to the adolescents in the non-
treatment group home and the matched control group of typical delinquent adolescents in custody.
The decrease in externalising behaviours for ACT participants paralleled the commencement of the
intervention in the group home. At the cessation of the intervention there was a significant increase in
acting-out behaviours.
According to Nugent et al. (1997) the ACT program finished at a time when one of the group
leaders became angry and verbally aggressive toward a participant in the presence of other
residents, which resulted in the termination of the employee and the ACT program. They noted that
the potential effect of adverse modelling by the group leader may have influenced the failure of
maintenance of treatment gains. Other threats to the validity of the study included the small sample
size in the ACT treatment group. In addition, only two participants were assessed at treatment-end
for incidence of acting-out behaviours, necessitating a single-case study design for the post-ACT
results. Additionally, it would seem that five ACT participants did not receive the full 10-week
treatment due to an open group format such that each time a new group member entered the group,
the program started again from week one. The use of a convenience sample within a residential
facility necessitated an open group format. Given the structured and sequential format of ART, this
represented a significant departure from the program structure described by Goldstein et al. (1998)
and a threat to treatment integrity. As previously discussed there is a necessary balance required
between the “what works” principles of program integrity and professional discretion. Inevitably the
demands of a given treatment setting require some flexibility in program structure and available
staffing in terms of facilitator training and experience. However, as already discussed, the meta-
analytic studies have shown that greater effect sizes have been associated with programs that
62
maintained treatment integrity and facilitators with high level training in CBT (Lipsey et al., 2001;
A more robust evaluation of ACT was conducted by Sukhodolsky et al. (2009) in which 26
young people between 11 and 16 years of age (24-boys, 2-girls, mean age 12.7years) with Tourette’s
Syndrome and comorbid disruptive behaviour problems were randomly and equally assigned to ACT
or treatment-as-usual (TAU). Several clinician, parent and child report measures of anger and
externalising behaviours were administered to all participants at baseline and treatment-end (i.e., 10-
weeks later). At base-line, all participants showed high levels of non-compliance and just over half
showed explosive anger outbursts. At post-treatment follow-up, ACT participants showed significantly
greater reduction on these measures than the TAU, with improvement evident on both parent and
blinded clinician ratings. A large effect size (1.8) was reported for ACT on parent ratings of disruptive
behaviour. Similarly, the blind clinician ratings showed a 69% behavioural improvement for ACT
participants compared to 15% for the TAU control condition. Sukhodolsky et al. suggested caution in
generalising their findings given the small and homogenous sample in relation to ethnicity and socio-
economic status. Moreover only three participants met criteria for CD and none were outside the
normal range of scores on the CBCL (Achenbach, 1991) subscale delinquent behaviour, suggesting
that their results may not generalise to adolescents with more serious forms of conduct related
problems. A further limitation of Sukhodolsky et al’s. study was the lack of longitudinal follow-up
making it difficult to know the extent to which treatment gains were maintained over time and setting.
Summary
ACT has been developed within a sound developmental (Luria, 1961; Vygotsky, 1962) and
previously discussed, there is a substantial body of research that has evaluated the broad CBT
approach in treating offenders (Andrews, et al., 1990; Izzo & Ross, 1990; Landenberger & Lipsey,
2005; Lipsey et al., 2001; Lipsey & Wilson, 1998). However, despite the often reported association
between impulsivity and offending behaviour (Andrews, 1996; Carroll et al., 2006; Luengo et al.,
1994; Smith & Waterman, 2006) there is a paucity of research in which impulsivity has been
investigated as a treatment outcome among offender populations. Thus, it is unclear if the treatment
procedures employed in ACT can effect changes in impulsivity. There are notably few outcome
studies in which ACT as a stand alone intervention has been specifically investigated. Moreover, the
methodological limitations including small sample sizes, simple pre- and post- treatment research
design without longitudinal follow-up, and in the case of Nugent et al. (1997) apparent threats to
treatment integrity. Notwithstanding, Hollin (2004) argued that anger management programs such as
ACT should be viewed as a component, as opposed to a stand alone intervention, in working toward
reducing aggressive behaviours and cognitions among violent offenders. The next section reviews
the theory and evidence for the third, and final, component of ART- Moral Reasoning Training.
Explanations of antisocial and aggressive behaviour, like any human behaviour, are framed
within the context of a complex interplay between internal and external and individual and social
phenomena (Palmer, 2003). The research relevant to effective behaviour change with violent
offenders provides clear support for treatment programs that employ multiple modes of change (i.e.,
behavioural, affective, and cognitive) in targeting the complex set of variables associated with
aggressive behaviour (Andrews, 1995; Hollin, 2004; Hollin & Palmer, 2006). Potter, Gibbs and
64
Goldstein (2001) argued that effective treatment programs for antisocial youth must incorporate a
moral learning component. Proponents of this view contend that the frequently reported failure of
offender treatment programs to generalise beyond the treatment setting can in part be attributed to
the omission of program content aimed at increasing social perspective taking and social problem
solving abilities and reducing self-centred cognitive distortions that serve to maintain antisocial
behaviour (Gibbs, 2003; Goldstein, et al., 1998). In the previous sections of this chapter the
behavioural (Skillstreaming) and affective (Anger Control Training) components of ART were
explained in terms of their applied procedure and theoretical underpinnings. In this section the
Moral reasoning training: Content and procedure. Moral Reasoning Training aims to
that youths will make more mature decisions in social situations” (Goldstein et al., 1998; p. 107). Prior
to the moral reasoning session (i.e., at the end of Skillstreaming or Anger Control Training) group
participants read a short story or problem situation in which a protagonist is faced with a moral
dilemma (see Appendix A for example) followed by a series of closed-questions to which group
members provide responses. The problem situations are portrayed in various settings (e.g., home,
school, work, custodial facility) and are designed to facilitate a peer group discussion of moral values
such as honesty, individual and community responsibility, and prosocial decision making within peer
and family relationships and broader community and social interactions. The problem situations
provide role-taking opportunities including social perspective-taking while also stimulating discussion
on the role of cognitive distortions in morally immature decision making and subsequent adverse
Goldstein and his colleagues (1998) described four phases of moral reasoning facilitation:
Phase 1: Introducing the problem situation- here the aim is to ensure that all group members clearly
understand the essence of the problem situation (e.g., “Who can tell the group what the main themes
in this problem are?” and “Why is that a problem?”). Also the relevance of the problem situation to the
everyday lives of group members needs to be made explicit (e.g., “Do problems like this really
happen?” or “Who has been in a situation like this?”); Phase 2: Cultivating mature morality- the aim of
this phase is to establish moral maturity as the group norm by initially attending to morally mature
reasoning among group members. This is achieved by listing prosocial decisions on a whiteboard
and relabelling the “should” as strong (e.g., “It takes courage to stand up for, and do, the right thing”);
Phase 3: Remediating moral developmental delay- having established moral maturity as the group
norm in the previous phase, the task of the facilitator turns to directly addressing the problem of moral
developmental delay. Group members who have responded in ways indicative of moral immaturity
are asked to explain their reasoning to the group. Morally mature group members are encouraged to
respectfully challenge antisocial decision making by indentifying implied thinking errors and creating
only thinks about there own needs or wants?”); and finally Phase 4: Consolidating mature morality- at
this stage of the moral reasoning session, the morally immature group members, having heard the
mature group decisions, are invited to change their responses in order to come to a morally mature,
prosocial group consensus or at least a morally mature majority (e.g., “Does the group agree that not
stealing the car is the majority position?”). Finally the group facilitator reviews the morally mature
decision making of the group emphasising the strength and maturity in prosocial behaviour (e.g., “I’m
impressed with the groups ability to make strong decisions based upon sound and mature reasons).
establishing a positive peer culture as the norm. It is not intended as opportunity for the group
facilitators to impose their own moral, religious or political beliefs upon young people. This is
achieved through the use of a Socratic dialogue, circular questioning and respectful challenging of
perspective can be traced to the work of Piaget (1932) who was interested in the cognitive processes
and structures of children’s intelligence, not just the content (Palmer, 2003). According to Piaget
(1952) children employ distinct reasoning structures that inform their thinking at four stages of
cognitive development: the sensorimotor stage (birth– 18-months)- the child relies on the physical
senses (e.g., taste, touch) to make sense of the world and his/her understanding of objects and
persons is limited to the physical attributes, the child is totally egocentric; the preoperational stage
(18-months– six years)- symbolic thought processes are evident through the emerging ability to
classify objects according to similarities and differences and hold mental representations of objects in
mind without needing to see or act upon the object. Perspective taking skills are not yet fully
developed, the child believes that his/her perspective is the same for others; the concrete operational
stage (six years– early adolescence) - here the child begins to develop the capacity for abstract
thinking and inductive reasoning. Beginning to demonstrate the capacity to generalise from a specific
circumstance or concept to broader rules of understanding, however the capacity for abstraction is
limited to concrete examples (Palmer, 2003). At this stage of cognitive development the pervasive
egocentricism characteristic of the earlier two stages shifts toward a developing sense of self as
being distinct from other (Gibbs, 2003); and the formal operational stage (early adolescence
67
onwards) - at this stage an individual develops abstract reasoning skills that allows for complex
problem solving in terms of possible consequences of a given action. The attainment of formal
operational thinking sees the individual develop the ability to plan into the future and consider multiple
perspectives, leaving behind the egocentric biases of earlier cognitive stages (Gibbs, 2003; Palmer,
2003).
Piaget (1932) proposed that children’s moral development is a corollary of their cognitive
development that can be seen in two stages of moral reciprocity: reciprocity as a fact- a “you scratch
my back, I’ll scratch yours” mentality (p.323); and reciprocity as an ideal- a more mature moral
perspective in which the early-adolescent begins to take account of psychological and circumstantial
motives for behaviour and moves toward a moral reasoning perspective more akin to “do unto others
as you would have them do unto you” (p.272). The major advance of Piaget’s conceptualisation of
moral development was his proposition that children actively construct their capacity for moral
reasoning, that they are not just passive recipients of social and parental moral values. According to
Piaget, it is through social interaction particularly age-related peers that moral development
progresses beyond egocentricism toward an increasing capacity to deduce the perspective of others
Kohlberg (1969) expanded upon Piaget’s model of moral development by initially attempting
to replicate his findings and then expand them beyond adolescence into adulthood. Kohlberg, like
Piaget (1932, 1952) was interested in the development of moral reasoning in terms of the cognitive
structures and processes that lead to higher stages of development. Kohlberg (1969) proposed that
progression from one stage of moral development to the next was sequential and invariant. As
outlined in Table 2, Kohlberg’s (1969) proposed a six-stage, trichotomous system for understanding
moral development, each stage represents increasing complexity in terms of reasoning and
68
abstraction abilities. The stages in Kohlberg’s (1969) model are defined with respect to the structures
of social interaction at each level. These structures operate within the individual as a model or
schema for making moral decisions (Palmer, 2007). An important extension of Piaget’s work by
Kohlberg was the incorporation of social perspective-taking, at the individual and collective levels.
Table 2
At the preconventional stage, rules and social norms are viewed as external to the self and
the individual has no perceived agency in the development of a moral code. Moral decision making is
based upon the individual’s relationship to authority figures such as parents, teachers and police and
the avoidance of punishment. With respect to criminal behaviour, offending is justified in terms of
“might makes right”, an entirely egocentric position in which the decision to commit a crime is based
upon perceived differences in power (i.e., Stage 1). At stage 2, the complete egocentricism of Stage
70
1 has progressed toward a greater capacity to take account of the motives and perspectives of
others. However, social perspective taking is limited to a pragmatic exchange- “you scratch my back,
I’ll scratch yours”. Offending behaviour is justified in terms of weighing up risk versus gain (e.g., “Will I
get caught?”) and pro-social decision making is primarily based upon deals- “if I do this, what’s in it
for me”. The conventional level of reasoning see’s the individual move toward greater comprehension
of the need for social norms and rules in order to maintain interpersonal and community relationships.
At Stage 3 reasoning, there is recognition of the need for mutuality in relationships based upon
building reciprocal trust and care, a “treat others as you would hope they would treat you”. Offending
behaviour at this stage can be justified with respect to maintaining a relationship. Gibbs (2004)
suggested that the risk at this stage of moral development can be an overemphasis on the opinions
of others to the extent that the individual becomes a “moral marshmallow” (p.54) whereby peer
influence leads to antisocial behaviour. While at Stage 3 there is evidence of mutuality in personal
relationships, Stage 4 represents these same principles applied for the betterment of the broader
community. At Stage 4, the individual begins to ask “How am I contributing to society?” and moral
decision making is characterised by a sense of fairness and equity for the good of the social whole.
Criminal behaviour is justified with respect to the greater interest of society, such as when a workers
union takes strike action in support of the rights of workers as a collective (Gibbs, 2004; Palmer,
2003).
The post conventional level (Stages 5 and 6) represents a transition from the internalisation
of societal expectations and rules (i.e., Conventional Reasoning) to a greater capacity for abstraction
and differentiation between the self and other (Colby, Kohlberg, Gibbs, & Lieberman, 1983). Here the
individual formulates a set of personal values, that are consistent with universal morals, however the
self-formulated values take precedence over societies. Thus the principles of the social contract
71
implied in Stages 3 and 4 are upheld in Stages 5 and 6 only where there is consistency between the
universal principles of justice, equality and dignity for all human beings. At the post conventional
level, offending behaviour can be justified where these basic principles of human rights are in conflict
with the law such as when a peaceful protester is charged with a civil order offense for participating in
a human rights protest (Palmer, 2003; 2005). It is noteworthy that in the results reported for
Kohlberg’s (1963) original child and adolescent sample, only 20% of the oldest age cohort (16-years)
showed evidence of Stage 5 moral reasoning and even fewer (5%) Stage 6. These higher stages of
moral development, particularly Stage 6, have been found to be so rare, outside of academic
philosophers, that many researchers have questioned their actual existence (Gibbs, 2003). Indeed in
later investigations of the model Colby et al. removed Stage 6 from the study design.
Colby et al. (1983) reported the findings of a 20-year, cross-sectional, longitudinal study of
moral development in which several of the assumptions underlying Kohlberg’s theory were tested.
Specifically, the invariant order of the stages was examined, as well as the relationship between
moral development and age, socio-economic status, and education. The sample comprised 58 boys
aged 10-, 13-, and 16- years of age at time-1, with five follow-up interviews conducted at 3-4 year
intervals. Participants were asked to provide justifications for their responses to nine different
hypothetical moral dilemmas. Their responses were then blind scored by researchers in terms of
Kohlberg’s moral stages. As predicted, participants progressed through the developmental stages in
the hypothesised, sequential order. Consistent with the theoretical tenets of a cognitive-
developmental model, the moral development of participants was also found to be significantly and
positively associated with age, socio-economic status, IQ and level of education. Four percent of
participants showed evidence of reversal to an earlier stage. This finding stands in contrast to earlier
results in which approximately 20% of the sample showed reversals (Kohlberg & Kramer, 1969). The
72
substantial reduction in the frequency of reversals was largely due to what Colby et al. (1983)
referred to as refinement in the scoring systems that provided for re-scoring of the participants
responses. Gibbs (2003) suggested that more recent advances in the measurement of moral
reasoning, where the potential for biases in scoring have been reduced, indicate that reversals are
common. According to Gibbs, people are likely to reason across various stages depending upon
situational variables, this represents a major departure from Kohlberg’s (1969) original invariance
proposition.
Kohlberg (1969) like Piaget (1932) viewed moral development to be closely linked to
cognitive development and that higher stages of moral thought required the capacity to take the role
of another or social perspective taking (Palmer, 2007). Social perspective taking has been defined in
terms of the increasing capacity of children to “recognise, articulate, and coordinate the internal
states of others” (Marsh, Serafica, & Barenboim, 1980, p.140). There is general support for the
purported developmental sequence of social perspective taking that see’s a child develop beyond
early stages of egocentrism toward an increasing capacity to take account of differing thoughts and
feelings between self and other, although they frequently confuse the two, by about the age of six-
years (i.e., simple perspective taking). By the age of eight- or nine- years, the preadolescent is
capable of placing her/himself in the shoes of the other as distinct from the self and there is also
recognition that others can do the same. Sometimes referred to as third-party perspective taking, by
approximately 10-years of age a child is able to take into account multiple perspectives (i.e.,
simultaneous perspective taking) (Marsh et al., 1980; Selman, 1980). Table 3 shows Selman’s (1980)
73
stages of social perspective taking which like Kohlberg’s stages of moral development consists of
Table 3
The first two stages of Selman’s model (preconventional reasoning) are characterised by an
egocentric bias. At Stage 1, the child is incapable of imagining the perspective of the other, their
74
worldview is entirely self-centred. Stage 2 sees the early development of social perspective taking in
which the child can begin to appreciate that others have their own feelings and thoughts, however
there is a tendency toward minimisation of the other-perspective in favour of self- needs and wants.
By Stage 3, the all consuming egocentric biases give way partially to a greater concern for the
perspectives of others, although this decentration is limited to concern for the perspectives of
significant others such as family and friends. Stage 4 represents the development of a broader
understanding of social perspectives that encompasses the capacity to take account of differences
between individualistic and collective (social) perspectives. At the post-conventional level (Stages 5
and 6) the development of complex systems of perspective taking, whereby a consistent set of moral
values form the basis of perspective taking, is consolidated. Kohlberg (1976) suggested that Piaget’s
cognitive-developmental stages were integral to Selman’s (1971) stages of social perspective taking,
which were in turn a necessary, although not sufficient, precursor to his moral stages.
A study conducted by Walker (1980) provided some support for this proposition. He
hypothesised that the attainment of stage 3- conventional moral reasoning would require the
(1971) Stage 3 of social perspective taking. Participants included 146 fourth- (M age = 9.9 years)
through seventh- (M age =13.0 years) grade children. Girls (n= 80) and boys (n= 66) were
approximately evenly distributed throughout the four grades. Several measures were administered to
assess Piaget’s cognitive developmental level, as well as a measure of social perspective taking and
Kohlberg’s moral judgement interview at pre- and post- test, and a six-week follow-up assessment. A
treatment and control group was compared on all measures. The treatment-group received a single,
45-minute session designed to stimulate moral development up to stage 3 through the use of moral
dilemma role-plays and discussion of potential stage 3 problem solutions. Findings supported the
75
hypothesis, and Kohlberg’s (1976) original supposition, that both cognitive and perspective-taking
development were prerequisites for moral development. Additionally, results showed that the moral
education program based on Kohlberg’s approach produced stable and generalised treatment gains
at six week follow-up. The treatment group showed an average increase of approximately one-half
stage in moral development compared to the no-treatment control group. Walker concluded that a
moral education curriculum that incorporated techniques aimed at increasing problem-solving and
perspective-taking skills of participants (as does ART) can affect commensurate improvements in
Gibbs (2003) revised and expanded upon the work of Kohlberg by placing greater emphasis on
the role of deficits in social perspective taking and empathy and the use of self-serving cognitive
distortions in the development and maintenance of antisocial behaviour among youth. Gibbs, Potter,
Barriga, and Liau (1996) used the “three-D’s” to refer to the difficulties seen in antisocial youth: (a)
developmental delay in moral judgement; (b) self-serving cognitive distortions; (c) and social skill
deficiencies. According to Gibbs (2003) prosocial behaviour can in part be attributed to mature moral
perception and subsequent decision making relative to actual behaviour. Conversely, antisocial and
aggressive behaviour can be partly understood with respect to developmentally delayed or immature
morality.
Kohlberg’s (1969) model of moral development applied to antisocial and chronically aggressive
youth. Gibbs (2003) distinguished between immature versus mature sociomoral reasoning. He
employed the term sociomoral reasoning, as opposed to moral reasoning, in order to avoid pejorative
76
and simplistic connotations of right and wrong. The first two stages presented in Table 4 represent
immature reasoning that is characterised by superficial and egocentric moral judgements. Gibbs
argued that the developmentally delayed youth makes moral decisions based upon observable
features of the situation including physical strength and physical consequences (stage 1) or tit for tat,
Table 4
A brief case example of immature reasoning is provided by Gibbs when he asked Joey a 15-
“Why is it so important to obey the law?” I asked Joey. “Because, [pause], like in a store, you
may think no one sees you, but they could have cameras!” he replied. His other explanations
were generally similar: Keeping promises to other is important because if you don’t, they
might find out and get even; helping others is important in case you need a favour from them
The egocentric and overly pragmatic moral justifications made by Joey are consistent with the moral
reasoning of a child rather than those of mid-adolescence, by which time normal moral development
would predict egocentric decentration and an increasing capacity to take the perspective of others
(Gibbs, 2003; Palmer, 2007). There is a substantial body of research (Blasi, 1980; Nelson, Smith, &
Dodd, 1990; Palmer, 2003, 2005; Stams et al., 2006) supporting the relationship between
developmental delay in moral reasoning and offending behaviour. Specific to violent offending, the
ability to perspective take and undertake moral decision making has been shown to be arrested
among chronically aggressive youth (Gibbs, 2003; Goldstein, et al., 1998; Lochman, et al., 2000;
Palmer, 2005).
In an early narrative review of the literature, Blasi (1980) examined the relationship between
moral reasoning and several theoretically associated cognitive and behavioural outcome measures.
Blasi reviewed 15 studies in which delinquent and non-delinquent participants were compared, 12
studies comparing moral reasoning and moral action, 17 studies where the relationship between
moral reasoning and degree of honesty was assessed, and 11 studies where the relationship
between moral reasoning and resistance to social conformity was examined. Overall, the research
78
showed a significant relationship between moral thinking and moral behaviour, which was particularly
so in studies where delinquent and non-delinquent youth were compared. Of the 15 delinquent
versus non-delinquent studies compared, 10 showed the moral reasoning stage of the delinquent
youth to be significantly lower then their non-delinquent age-matched peers. Support was also found
for the hypothesis that higher stages of moral reasoning were associated with greater levels of
honesty and altruism. However, contrary to expectation, higher levels (i.e., postconventional) of moral
reasoning were not strongly associated with social non-conformity. Blasi identified discrepancies in
scoring procedures of moral reasoning measures and poor operationalisation of moral behaviour as
limiting the generalisability of the studies included in his review. Also, although extensive in terms of
the number of available studies reviewed and comprehensive within the narrative approach, Blasi’s
investigation did not provide quantitative analysis. Meta-analytic procedures conducted since have
provided substantial statistical support (i.e., aggregated effect sizes) for Blasi’ conclusions regarding
antisocial youth. For example, Nelson et al.’s (1990) meta-analysis included 15 published and
unpublished studies, which compared moral immaturity of delinquent and non-delinquent youth. The
total combined sample comprised 673 male and female participants, with an average age of 15.34-
years (age range 11-17 years). Results showed that the moral reasoning of the juvenile delinquents
was significantly lower (immature) than non-delinquent age-related peers. Nelson et al. were unable
to distinguish type of offence (violent, nonviolent) because the studies did not consistently provide
this information.
In an attempt to address several of the limitations identified in previous studies, Stams et al.
(2006) conducted a large scale (N = 4,814) meta-analysis comprising 50 studies in which groups of
delinquent (n = 2,316) and non-delinquent (n = 2,498) youth (age range 10-20 years) were compared
on moral reasoning and numerous moderator variables (e.g., type of offence, gender, diagnosed
79
psychopathic disorder, IQ, incarcerated versus non-incarcerated, length of incarcerated). Stams and
colleagues partially replicated the study conducted by Nelson et al. (1990) and also examined the
potential impact of the moderator variables on the relationship between moral immaturity and juvenile
delinquency. Consistent with the findings of Nelson et al., it was found that delinquent youth scored
significantly lower on moral reasoning compared to non- delinquent youth. Additionally, several of the
moderator variables were found to significantly influence moral reasoning scores, including: gender
(males were lower on moral reasoning than females); later adolescence was associated with larger
effect sizes than early or middle adolescence; delinquents with psychopathic personality traits
showed significantly lower moral judgement scores than non-psychopathic delinquents and even
greater moral immaturity when compared to non-delinquents. An institutionalisation effect was also
delinquents and the longer the period of incarceration (18-months or more) the greater the
discrepancy in moral reasoning ability. Given the large sample size, Stams et al. concluded that the
statistical support provided for the relationship between delayed moral judgment and delinquency
should be considered robust. However, the results did not imply causation (i.e., low moral reasoning
causes offending behaviour) as all the studies included in the analysis were cross-sectional. Notably,
the relationship between stages of moral reasoning and moral behaviour has been reported to vary
depending on social context and purpose (Hains, 1984; Krebs & Denton, 2005).
Delays in moral development and social perspective taking are not the only schemas
associated with antisocial and aggressive behaviour (Gibbs, 2003). Cognitive distortions have been
80
defined as “inaccurate or rationalising attitudes, thoughts, or beliefs concerning one’s own or others
social behaviour” (Liua, Barriga, & Gibbs, 1998, p. 337). The use of cognitive distortions has been
widely described in relation to various internalising problems such as depression and anxiety (Beck,
1996, 1967; Ellis, 1962) and externalising problems such as the criminal personality (Yochelson &
Samenow, 1976) and mechanisms for prosocial disengagement (Bandura, 1973) and avoidance of
cognitive dissonance (Gibbs, 2003) and cognitive processes that lead to the misinterpretation of
social information processing cues leading to aggressive acts (Dodge & Coie, 1987; Dodge,
According to Gibbs (2003) and Goldstein et al. (1998) the developmental delays in moral
reasoning among antisocial youth are also associated with two cognitive distortions or thinking errors:
Primary Cognitive Distortion- this thinking error is characterised by a tendency among egocentric
young people to attribute far greater importance to one’s own views, thoughts feelings, needs and
wants at the expense of others legitimate views; and Secondary Cognitive Distortions which are
represented by three self-centred thinking errors that serve to reinforce the primary distortion: (1)
Blaming Others- the misattribution of responsibility for one’s own antisocial behaviours to sources
external to the self, including blaming the victim (e.g., “he asked for it”), blaming intoxication or a bad
mood, or blaming ones own history of victimisation; (2) Minimising/ Mislabelling- attempting to make
one’s antisocial behaviour seem less then, or different to what it really is, such as arguing that it did
not cause any real harm or even distorting antisocial actions as being admirable (e.g., “it was just a
joke”, “everybody lies its no big deal”) or labelling victims in derogatory, dehumanising terms; (3)
rationalisation for ones own aggressive behaviour or alluding to the misnomer that behaviour change
(self or others) is impossible (e.g., “I’ve always been this way”). The primary and secondary thinking
81
errors are thought to maintain antisocial and aggressive behaviour through rationalisations that serve
to prevent cognitive dissonance (i.e., feelings of guilt, shame, remorse) (Barriga, Gibbs, Potter, &
Liau, 2001). There is a substantial body of research in which the relationship between self-serving
cognitive distortions and antisocial behaviour among youth has been investigated (Palmer, 2007).
Barriga, Landau, Stinson, Liau and Gibbs (2000) conducted a study designed to investigate
the discriminative power of types of cognitive distortions (i.e., self-serving versus self-debasing)
relative to type of behavioural problems (i.e., internalising versus externalising) among female and
male youth. A group of incarcerated delinquents (M age= 15.8 years) was compared to a group of
non-delinquent High School students (M age = 16.4 years). Both groups were approximately matched
for socio-economic and ethnic similarity. Several cognitive and adolescent problem, self-report
measures were administered and misconduct reports for the delinquent youth were also examined
two months after initial assessment. The results showed that both self-serving and self-debasing
cognitive distortions were more prevalent among delinquent youth compared to non- delinquent
youth. The self-serving cognitive distortions were more often associated with externalising disorders
and self-debasing distortions were more associated with internalising disorders. Support for the
specificity of type of cognitive distortion by type of disorder was also found even after controlling for
comorbidity. Barriga and his collegues recommended that youth who miminise/mislabel their
antisocial behaviour require interventions aimed at increasing perspective taking skills (i.e., victim
empathy). While internalising youth who distort their behavioural consequences in terms of self-
blame and self-debasing distortions (e.g., catastrophising) are more likely to benefit from cognitive
restructuring methods that objectively examine the evidence and challenging irrational beliefs.
Liua, et al. (1998) investigated the relationship between self-serving cognitive distortions and
overt versus covert behavioural referents among a group of 52 male juvenile delinquents compared
82
to 51 High School, non-delinquents aged between 14- and 18- years. The How I Think (HIT)
Questionnaire (Barriga, et al., 2001 see Measures section for description) makes the broad
distinction between overt cognitive distortions involving direct confrontational antisocial behaviour
such as physical aggression (e.g., “people need to be roughed up once in while”) versus covert
cognitive distortions involving non-confrontational antisocial behaviours such as lying and stealing
(e.g., “If someone is careless enough to lose a wallet they deserve to have it stolen”). Similarly,
several researchers (Loeber, Lahey, & Thomas, 1991; Loeber & Schmaling, 1985; Patterson, 1982)
have found support for distinct dimensions of antisocial behaviour comprising overt (e.g., fighting,
arguing) versus covert (stealing, lying, fire setting) antisocial acts. Liua et al. found strong support for
their main hypothesis that overt versus covert cognitive distortions would be directly associated with
their respective behavioural (overt vs covert) referents. Additionally, there was a strong, positive
relationship between self-serving cognitive distortions and antisocial behaviour for both the
delinquent and non-delinquent groups. Both self-serving cognitive distortions and antisocial
behaviour were higher among the delinquent compared to the non-delinquent group. Path-analysis
revealed that overt cognitive distortions predicted overt behavioural referents (i.e., physical
aggression). By contrast, covert cognitive distortions predicted covert antisocial behaviour (e.g.,
stealing). It was concluded that the inclusion of cognitive restructuring techniques aimed at reducing
cognitive distortions among antisocial youth can serve to reduce both overt and covert antisocial
behaviour.
Barriga, Hawkins and Camelia (2008) extended the earlier research of Liua et al. (1998) by
partial replication and examining the cross-cultural validity of the earlier findings among a group of
239 males aged 10-19 years (mean age = 14.22 years) from schools on the Caribbean Island of
Curacao. The results were consistent with the findings of Liua et al. Self-serving cognitive distortions
83
were significantly associated with externalising behaviours and self-debasing distortions were
behaviours revealed specific associations between endorsement of cognitive distortion types and
type of aggression (i.e., physical or verbal aggression). Physically aggressive behaviour was
significantly related to cognitive distortions theoretically underpinning such behaviour, whereas verbal
to the argument that there is a generic criminal mind set (see Yochelson & Samenow, 1976) Barriga
et al. suggested that there is a tendency on the part of antisocial male youth to employ rationalising
or guilt neutralising cognitive distortions relative to specific behaviours. That is, there appears to be
Summary
In the previous section, the theories of moral development proposed by Piaget (1932) and
Kohlberg (1969) were reviewed in terms their influence on the development of the procedures and
content inherent in the Moral Reasoning component of ART. Several researchers (Blasi, 1980;
Nelson et al., 1990; Palmer, 2003, 2005; Stams et al., 2006) have reported evidence of sociomoral
developmental delay as a correlate of offending behaviour. Gibbs (2003) has emphasised the role of
deficits in Selman’s (1971) stages of social perspective taking and the use of self-serving cognitive
distortions among antisocial and aggressive youth. Persistently aggressive youth tend to lack
perspective taking and moral decision making abilities (Gibbs, 2003; Goldstein et al., 1998; Lochman
et al., 2000; Palmer, 2005). Consistent with developmental delay in moral reasoning, Goldstein et al.
(1998) described four cognitive distortions (Self-centred, Blaming Others, Assuming the Worst, and
Minimising/Mislabeling) that antisocial and aggressive youth frequently employ as way of rationalising
their harmful behaviour and avoiding cognitive dissonance. The procedures employed in Moral
84
Reasoning Training are designed to challenge these self-serving cognitive distortions and provide
frequent opportunity for social perspective taking. In the next section, previous evaluations of ART
will be reviewed.
85
86
Chapter 5
There are numerous published descriptions of ART and adaptations of the program in
various settings and among diverse populations (Goldstein, 2004). These include: school settings
(Amendola & Oliver, 2003; Gundersen & Svartdal, 2006; Roth & Striepling-Goldstein, 2003; Salmon,
2003) children and youth with Autism Spectrum Disorders (Moynahan, 2003) youth in residential care
(Barnoski, 2004; Nugent, Bruley, & Allem, 1999) young offenders (Goldstein & Glick, 1994;
Holmqvist, Hill, & Lang, 2009) adult offenders (Hatcher et al., 2008; McGuire & Clark, 2004) and
violent forensic psychiatric patients (Hornsveld, 2005; Hornsveld, Nijman, Hollin, & Kraaimaat, 2008).
Although some of these studies have included evaluation data, there are relatively few published
outcome studies of ART (Hornsveld et al.). Notwithstanding, several researchers (Fonagy & Kurtz,
2002; Goldstein, 2004; Palmer, 2007; Polaschek, 2006) have indicated that ART has demonstrated
itself to be one of the most efficacious interventions in working with aggressive youth. Improvement
behaviours, increases in prosocial behaviours and significant reduction in reconviction rates have all
Goldstein and Glick (1994) reported the findings of one of the earliest effectiveness studies
of ART in the treatment of a community based sample of post-release delinquents. Three groups
(i.e., “ART for youths and parents/family members”, “ART for youths only”, and “no-ART control
group”) were compared on a measure of “total skill change”. While there were no significant
87
differences between the ART groups, there were differences between these groups and the controls.
Compared to the no-ART control group, the two treatment groups showed an overall increase in
situations. The ART for youths and parents/family members group were the least likely to be re-
arrested (15%) followed by the ART for youth only group (30%) while the no-ART control group were
In order to assess transfer of treatment effects beyond the institutional setting, Goldstein and
Glick (1994) evaluated the carry-over of ART treatment gains post release back into the community.
Of the 54 youths released from a juvenile detention centre in New York, 17 had received ART while
37 had not. Parole Officers completed a global assessment measure on all 54 participants. On four of
the six psychosocial domains assessed (i.e., home & family, peer, legal, and overall, but not school
or work) ART recipients rated better than the non-ART group. However no statistical significance data
were reported.
Studies conducted with juvenile offenders have reported decreased recidivism rates for ART
program participants compared to no-ART or waitlist control groups. Barnoski (2004) conducted a
two-year longitudinal evaluation of ART by assigning 1,500 juvenile offenders to either an ART group
or a wait-list control group. Results from the 18-month follow-up showed a marked reduction in
recidivism (24%) for the ART participants compared to the control group. More recently, McGuire and
Clark (2004) adapted the ART youth program for adult inmates in the United Kingdom. ART is an
accredited program within the UK justice system. Reconviction rates for ART participants at a one
year follow-up showed a clear reduction (i.e., 20.4% compared to 34.5% for non-ART participants).
Hatcher and her colleagues (2008) extended the original study conducted by McGuire and
Clark (2004) by examining reconviction rates for ART completers compared to non-completers and a
88
no ART control group. Participants included 106 adult male, mostly violent, offenders placed on a
community-based order. Using a national offender database, matched pairs of research participants
were extracted on the basis of offence type, age, criminogenic risk scores, and number of previous
convictions. Participants were aged between 18 to 53 years, with a mean age of 27.42 years. Exactly
half were allocated to ART while the other half were allocated to the control group. At the 10-month
follow-up period, 47 of 106 research participants (44.34%) had been reconvicted, of those 38.3%
were reconvicted of violent offences while the remaining comprised various property offences. Just
over half (50.9%) of the no-ART control group were reconvicted, compared to 39.2% of the treatment
group. At 10-month follow-up, there was a 13.3% decrease in reconviction for the treatment group
compared to the control comparison. Comparison of those participants who completed ART
compared to matched non-completers revealed a 7.8% increase in reconviction rates for the non-
completer group. Hatcher et al. noted that the reconviction rates were favourable in comparison to
meta-analytic studies in which an average reduction of 10% in reconviction rates for offender
treatment groups compared to controls was reported. Hatcher et al. noted their small sample size
compared to other similar studies. It was further noted that as a violence offence-specific program,
ART cannot attract the numbers of participants that broad-based offending programs (e.g., cognitive
Gundersen and Svartdal (2006) reported the outcomes of a 24-session intervention based on
ART, delivered in school settings by teachers. Participants were 16 girls (mean age = 14.1 years) and
49 boys (mean age = 12.6 years). From pre- to post- treatment, there was significant improvement in
social skills for parent and teacher report but not for youth self-report. Parent and teacher ratings of
problem behaviours showed a significant reduction for the ART treatment group compared to the no-
ART control group. For the youth self-report of problem behaviours and cognitive distortions
89
associated with aggression, the treatment and control groups showed significant improvement. The
authors suggested that this result could partly be explained by a possible dilution effect between
treatment conditions as some participants were drawn from the same classrooms resulting in a
of ART for working with violent forensic inpatients or outpatients. The program involves 15, 90-
minute, weekly sessions, with eight participants per group and is designed for adult forensic patients
with a diagnosis of either APD and/or a comorbid psychotic disorder (in remission) or for young adult
forensic patients with a diagnosis of CD or APD. The program consisted of all ART components, with
a self-regulation module added. Hornsveld (2005) reported the outcomes of two groups of male
forensic patients- 109 inpatients with a mean age of 32.5-years and 44 younger outpatients (mean
age = 23.4 years) all with a history of serious violent offending. Participants completed a battery of
self-report measures designed to assess personality traits, hostility, aggressive behaviour, social
skills, and social anxiety at commencement of the treatment, treatment-end, and 15-week post-
treatment follow-up. Results showed a significant reduction in self-reported hostility and aggressive
behaviour at treatment-end with those gains maintained at post-treatment follow-up. There was no
significant difference on scores for social anxiety or social skills. In explaining these non-significant
findings, Hornsveld noted that compared to Dutch norms, the program participants reported above
average scores on aggressive behaviour and below average social anxiety and greater social
competence at intake assessment. This apparent inflated sense of social competence and lack of
social anxiety might be also partly explained by elevated narcissism that some researchers have
identified as typical of the Cluster B personality disorders including APD (Hare, Hart, & Harpur, 1991)
and violent offending (Baumeister, Smart, & Boden, 1996; Bushman & Baumeister, 2002).
90
The ART evaluation literature to date has shown considerable promise in terms of
generalisability across time and setting in working with aggressive youth (Hollin, 2003). However,
none of the previous evaluations of ART have included a separate analysis of the different
components of the intervention, making it difficult to determine the relative impact of each component
(Palmer, 2007). Furthermore, a limitation of the ART evaluation studies among offender groups has
been the overreliance upon recidivism as the sole or main measure of outcome. As previously
discussed, several researchers have voiced criticism of offender outcome studies for this reason
(Serin et al., 2009; Tate et al., 1995). It is argued that while recidivism represents an important
treatment outcome among offender populations, other more psychologically orientated variables
provide important information in regard to clinically relevant outcomes. The present study aimed to
address these potential limitations by including psychological measures that are designed to assess
the cognitive, behavioural and affective targets of change in ART (i.e., aggressive behaviours and
Notably, the majority of ART evaluation research has consisted of participants in mid-
adolescence or older offenders. To date, no known study has investigated the effectiveness of ART
among young adults (e.g., 18-20 years). The present study aimed to assess the effectiveness of ART
in treating young-adult, Australian, male offenders. Further, to date, there has been no published
evaluation ART in Australia. Indeed, there is a relative absence of published research in which
treatment programs for aggressive youth, particularly violent juvenile offender programs, have been
empirically investigated in Australia (Boni, 1999; Howells & Day, 2002). Ogloff (2002) commented
that “…there is a very real need for Australian-based research in offender rehabilitation. Very real
91
Australian society” (p.246). As such, another important aim of the studies comprising this thesis was
to determine the applicability of an American developed program in terms of its cultural relevance to
Australian youth.
Several researchers have noted the unique difficulties that both researchers and clinicians
institutional resistance to outsiders (Fleck, Thompson, & Narroway, 2001; Hollin, 2001b; Tate et al.,
1995; Wakai, Shelton, Trestman, & Kesten, 2009). These authors have noted the importance of
piloting a program in order to ascertain and find solutions to potential obstacles before entering into a
major research project. Hence, an initial pilot study (see Chapter 5, next) aimed to identify potential
research, institutional and programmatic obstacles before undertaking the main study. Furthermore,
the pilot study provided an opportunity to determine the appropriateness of the measures employed
in a youth justice custodial setting. Although the selected measures have well established
psychometric properties in community settings, there is relatively little, if anything known about their
Chapter 6
Pilot Study
An extended version of this pilot study has been published previously (Currie, Wood,
Williams, & Bates, 2009). As previously, discussed the main aims of the pilot study were: (a) to
determine the appropriateness of the measures employed in a youth justice custodial setting; and (b)
investigate the overall effectiveness of ART in simple pre- and post- treatment research design
before undertaking a larger outcome evaluation. To this end, three specific hypotheses were made,
Method
Participants
Six young men (17-18 years) initially agreed to participate in the pilot study. All had
committed violence-related offences and were serving a custodial sentence at a Victorian state
Juvenile Justice Centre. In week three of the 10-week program, one participant self-selected out of
the group and the research. The remaining five participants completed the 10-week ART program.
Measures
Aggressive behaviours and thoughts were assessed using the Aggression Questionnaire
(AQ; Buss & Warren, 2000) which is designed to assess aggressive tendencies among children and
93
adults. The instrument is written at a third-grade reading level, and comprises 34-items providing
scores on five subscales: Physical Aggression (e.g., “I may hit someone if he or she provokes me”);
Verbal Aggression (e.g., “My friends say that I argue a lot”); Anger (e.g., “At times I get very angry for
no good reason”); Hostility (e.g., “At times I fell like I’ve gotten a raw deal out of life”); and Indirect
Aggression (e.g., “I sometimes spread gossip about people I don’t like”). Respondents rate each
item on a 5-point scale from “Not at all like me” to “Completely like me”. Higher scores indicate
higher levels of aggression. Scores can be classified in terms of severity based on their percentile
rank according to standardized norms. Buss and Warren provided seven classifications- “low” (2nd–
14th percentile), “low average” (15th–27th percentile), “average” (28th–71st percentile), “high
average” (72nd–81st percentile), “high” (82nd–97th percentile), and “very high” (98th percentile and
above). The AQ was standardized in a large (N = 2,038) community based sample and reported
reliability (Cronbach’s alpha) is moderate to high for the subscales, with alphas ranging from .71 to
.88. Internal consistency for the total scale score was very high (alpha = .94).
The Social Skills Rating System (SSRS; Gresham & Elliot, 1990) was used to measure
social skills. In the present study, the self-report, Secondary Student Form was used, comprising 34
items that are rated on a 3-point scale (“Never”, “Sometimes” and “Very Often”) across four
subscales: Cooperation (e.g., “I use my free time in a good way”) Assertion (e.g., “I make friends
easily”) Empathy (e.g., “I say nice things to others when they have done well”) and Self-control (e.g.,
“I ignore other children when they tease me or call me names”). Gresham and Elliot reported
reliability coefficients ranging from adequate to high (i.e., alpha coefficients from .67 to .77 for the four
Cognitive distortions were assessed using the How I Think Questionnaire (HIT: Barriga, et
al., 2001). This is a 54-item self-report measure designed to assess four categories of self-serving
94
cognitive distortions (i.e., Thinking Errors): Self-Centered (“When I get mad, I don’t care who gets
hurt”) Blaming Others (“If I made a mistake its because I got mixed up with the wrong crowd”)
Minimizing/Mislabelling (Everybody lies, its no big deal”) and Assuming the Worst (“Its no use trying
to stay out of fights”). These four cognitive distortions represent inaccurate or biased ways of
perceiving and interpreting environmental stimulus, which have been shown to play a central role in
antisocial behaviour and criminogenic thinking (Gibbs, 2003). Items comprising first-person
statements are rated on a 6-point scale ranging from “Agree Strongly” to “Disagree Strongly”. The
HIT can be administered in groups or with individuals. It is typically completed in 5 to 15 minutes and
requires only a fourth-grade reading level. Percentile rankings based on the normative sample
provide for three qualitative severity descriptors- non-clinical (<50th – 72nd percentile), borderline-
clinical (74th – 82nd percentile), and clinical (84th – 100th percentile). Moderate to high internal
consistency was reported, with Cronbach’s alphas ranging from .63 to .92 for the cognitive distortion
subscales and the behavioural referent subscales (i.e., Lying, Stealing, Oppositional Defiant, and
Physical Aggression). Estimates of internal consistency were very high for the total scale score
(alpha = .95).
Treatment integrity was assessed on an ongoing basis via video footage using the
Washington State Aggression Replacement Training Quality Assurance (QA) Form (Hayes,
2001). This tool assesses adherence to the ART treatment model across multiple process (e.g., “Was
a positive climate established through welcoming students?”) and content (e.g., “Did most youth
complete the [homework]?”) domains, relative to each of the program components (i.e.,
Skillstreaming, Anger Control Training, and Moral Reasoning Training). A categorical (yes/no)
checklist format is used, with different weightings for each of the items assessed. For each of the
three program components, treatment adherence scores range from 0-100 for the main-trainer and 0-
95
9 for the co-trainer, higher scores indicate greater treatment adherence. The role of the main trainer
is emphasised in the QA assessment, which is reflected in the allocation of scores for each trainer.
Detailed qualitative corrective feedback is also provided. All scores are combined to provide an
and 3 = “Highly Competent”) for each of the three ART program components (see Appendix C for
Procedure
Prior to commencing this pilot study, approval was sought and granted from relevant Human
Research Ethics committees (i.e., Department of Community Services Victoria and Swinburne
University of Technology see Appendix B). Initial referral to ART was made by the health workers
(i.e., psychologists or social workers) who undertook a case-management role of the young people
for the duration of their custodial sentence. The health workers determined level of criminogenic risk
(moderate to high) for inclusion in the ART program. Risk was assessed using the Victorian
Offending Needs Indicator for Youth (VONIY; Department of Human Services Victoria- Youth Justice,
2004). The VONIY provides scores across several domains of ciminogenic risk and need in addition
to a score (low-moderate- high-intensive) relative to level of intervention required. To date, there has
been no published psychometric evaluation of this assessment tool. The Principal Researcher then
met with each young person individually to explain ART and the expectations and requirements for
participation in the program. The plain language statement and consent form (see Appendix B) was
read to potential participants and explanation offered for any terms or content requiring clarification.
Voluntary participation was emphasized and it was explained that participation in ART was not
96
conditional upon participation in the research. After a “cooling off” period of two days, all of the initial
Screening for history of substance use and previous psychiatric diagnoses was conducted
during the initial intake assessment. Exclusion criteria included current or recent (previous 6-months)
history of psychotic symptoms. None of the participants reported a history of psychosis. The self-
report measures were administered one-on-one, which also provided an opportunity to assess the
reading capability of each young person before entry into the program. All participants appeared to
have at least adequate reading, writing and comprehension skills required for participation in ART.
The program was facilitated by the present author (provisional psychologist) and a female
colleague (Master of Social Work). Only the Principal Researcher had received accredited training in
ART, so he main-trained for the duration of the 10-week program, while the co-trainer assisted. Both
to note that in the interests of maintaining treatment integrity and providing a forum for clinical
supervision relative to ART both facilitators participated in regular supervision. For all groups, the
participants gave informed consent for the ART sessions to be videotaped in weeks three and eight.
These videotapes were then sent to an experienced ART Master Trainer in Washington State, USA.
After viewing the sessions written feedback was provided using the ART QA Form (see Appendix C).
Additionally, the co-facilitators attended fortnightly, in person, supervision with a clinical supervisor.
97
Results
Statistical analysis was conducted using SPSS version-14. Several researchers (e.g., Cohen,
1994; Kazdin, 1999; Kendall, Marrs-Garcia, Nath, & Sheldrick, 1999) have been critical of the
arbitrary cut off of p<.05 in determining statistical significance, particularly in the case of
psychotherapy outcome studies. Hence, as this pilot study was exploratory we set a liberal p-value of
Table 5 shows case by case, pre- to post- treatment raw scores and indication of clinical
versus non-clinical scores for all participants on the three total scale score outcome measures.
Table 5
Pre- and post- treatment raw scores on the AQ, SSRS, and HIT Questionnaires (N=5)
AQ SSRS HIT
Case Pre Post Pre Post Pre Post
A 105* 108* 50.0 50.0 3.64* 4.33*
Note. AQ= Aggression Questionnaire; SSRS= Social Skills Rating System; HIT= How I Think
Questionnaire; * = Score within clinical range.
98
As shown in Table 5, participants raw scores for the total scale score on the AQ showed a downward
trend from pre- to post- treatment in all but one case, A, who reported a slight increase in overall
aggression at post- treatment. An improvement in social skills was evident at post-treatment, with
scores on the SSRS slightly higher at post-treatment, except for case A, who reported no change.
For all cases, except “E”, participants remained within the clinical range for cognitive distortions at
post-treatment on the HIT. The HIT scores for cases A and B showed an increase in cognitive
distortions at post-treatment, while the remaining three cases reported a decrease at post-treatment.
Given the small sample size (N=5) a one-tailed, Wilcoxon signed-ranks test was employed to
test for significant differences between pre-and post- treatment scores on the outcome measures.
Table 6 shows means, standard deviations, and Wilcoxon signed-ranks T-values for the AQ, SSRS,
and HIT. As predicted, a comparison of mean aggression scores showed a significant overall
reduction from pre- to post- treatment (p = .06). The AQ subscales of Anger (p = .06) and Hostility (p
revealed a significant increase in scores on the Self-Control subscale (p = .03) and the total scale
score (p = .06) on the SSRS from pre- to post- treatment. Contrary to expectation, there was no
significant difference between pre- and post- treatment scores on the total scale score for the HIT (p
Table 6
Pilot study pre- and post- treatment descriptives and Wilcoxon T-values for all outcome measures
(N=5)
Pre-Treat Post-Treat
Wilcoxon
Variable M (SD) M SD
T value
AQ Total 106.60 (15.27) 90.60 (12.97) 1.0*
- Physical 31.00 (4.85) 26.60 (6.80) 1.0
- Anger 22.20 (2..05) 19.60 (2..07) 0.0*
- Hostility 22.00 (8.15) 16.00 (3.94) 0.0*
- Indirect 17.00 (5.29) 14.80 (3.90) 2..0
- Verbal 14.40 (2.30) 13.60 (1.34) 1.0
SSRS Total 53.20 9.98 57.00 11.77 0.0*
- Cooperation 13.20 1.48 13.20 4.66 2..0
- Assertion 15.80 1.92 16.40 1.82 1.0
- Self-Control 9.20 4.32 11.80 4.15 0.0*
- Empathy 15.00 3.54 15.60 3.13 2..0
HIT Total 3.39 .69 3.33 .91 2..0
- Assuming the Worst 3.16 .79 3.22 .85 2..0
- Blaming Others 3.46 .87 3.46 1.03 2..0
- Minimizing/Mislabeling 3.69 .75 3.56 .90 2..0
- Self-Centered 3.27 .62 3.13 .92 1.0
Note. *p<.10; AQ= Aggression Questionnaire; SSRS= Social Skills Rating System; HIT= How I
Think Questionnaire
In order to illustrate qualitative change, a single case study- The case of “C” is presented in
Appendix D.
100
Discussion
The results supported the predicted reduction in overall aggression and increased pro-social
skills from pre- to post- treatment. However, contrary to expectation there was no change in cognitive
With respect to the predicted change in aggressive thoughts and behaviours, there was a
significant reduction in scores on the Anger (i.e., irritability, easily frustrated, affective lability) and
Hostility (i.e., feelings of bitterness, paranoia, social isolation) subscales of the AQ, which are thought
to represent an “internalising approach to anger” (Buss & Warren, 2000, p.15). According to Buss
and Warren, high scorers on the Anger subscale are most likely to respond to CBT interventions
aimed at identifying common external triggers, internal physiological cues, and techniques in arousal
reduction such as cognitive distraction and relaxation techniques. These modes of change are
consistent with the Anger Control Training component of ART. High scorers on the Hostility scale are
thought to be responsive to treatments aimed at challenging the consistent and erroneous hostile
attributions made toward others (Buss & Warren, 2000). As previously discussed, improving social
problem solving skills represents an implicit aspect of the social skills training component of ART (i.e.,
Skillstreaming). By teaching the substitution of social skills in place of aggression and the breaking
down of those skills into sequential skill steps, ART participants learn to become aware of the
perspectives of others, increase their capacity to interpret social cues, and rehearse different social
solutions to potentially anger arousing situations. Overall, the reduction in Anger and Hostility scores
suggested a change in cognitive appraisals of anger provoking situations and increased capacity to
Results also revealed a significant increase in Self-Control on the SSRS (Gesham & Elliot,
101
1990). According to Gresham and Elliot, this subscale provides a measure of non-aggressive
behavioural responses employed when faced with conflict situations, such as being teased, and in
situations requiring prosocial skills such as taking turns or compromising. This finding is consistent
with an increase in participants repertoire of pro-social skills taught in Skillstreaming and the self-
regulatory techniques acquired through ACT. While these positive effects are promising, the lack of
change in participants cognitive distortions from pre- to post- treatment needs to be considered in the
context of the theoretical underpinnings of CBT, and specifically the rationale for including the moral
Several researchers (e.g., Hayes, Strosahl, Bunting, Twohig, & Wilson, 2004; Longmore &
Worrell, 2007; Maruna & Mann, 2006) have questioned the value of cognitive interventions in
ameliorating symptoms associated with psychopathology. Hayes and his colleagues argued that
symptom reduction in CBT is often evident “before procedures thought to be central to its success
have been implemented” (p.15). They further argued that behaviour change is not necessarily
dependent upon cognitive change. In a review of CBT component analysis studies, Longmore and
Worrell found no significant difference in effectiveness between the cognitive and behavioral
elements of CBT in all but one of the 13 studies reviewed. Consistent with Hayes (2004) it was
concluded that cognitive interventions appear to add nothing over and above that of the behavioural
components of CBT. However, as Longmore and Worrell pointed out, it may be the combined multi-
modal packaging of CBT that makes it effective. In other words, an intervention employing any CBT
component may have implications for change in other components. However, the component
analysis research reviewed by Longmore and Worrell was limited to studies of depression and
anxiety. There are differences in the cognitions associated with these disorders and those thought to
be associated with the conduct related disorders and indeed differences in treatment approach.
102
Moreover, as noted earlier, the results from several meta-analytic studies (Andrews et al., 1990; Izzo
& Ross, 1990; Landenberger & Lipsey, 2005; Wilson et al., 2005) have found that inclusion of a
cognitive component has consistently been shown to produce greater effect sizes in treating both
As described previously, the moral reasoning component of ART employs these techniques
in challenging cognitive distortions specifically relevant to aggressive youth. Palmer (2007) noted that
while ART outcome research indicates overall effectiveness, the exact contribution of each module
cannot be determined until component analysis is undertaken. Specifically, she questioned if the
moral reasoning component provides any added value over and above that of typical anger
management programs that do not include this cognitive/values component. Gundersen and Svartdal
(2006) attempted to analyse the relative effectiveness of the different components of ART, and found
by the HIT (Barriga et al., 2001) for both the treatment and control conditions. They explained the
finding in terms of a potential dilution effect between the two conditions as both groups of children
continued to interact in the school/classroom setting. Notably, Gundersen and Svartdal only delivered
four to five sessions of moral reasoning as part of the overall ART treatment and still found a positive
effect. Consistent with Longmore and Worrell (2007) they suggested that training youth in any one of
the components (Skillstreaming- behavioural; Anger Control Training- affective; and Moral
Some methodological and sample differences between the present study and Gundersen
and Svartdal (2006) also offer some explanation for the different outcomes on the HIT (Barriga et al.,
2001). The Gundersen and Svartdal study was conducted in a school setting, with male (n = 49) and
female (n = 16) participants, with a combined average age of 13.4 years. In contrast, this pilot study
103
was conducted in a custodial setting, with five males between the ages of 17-18 years, who had
committed serious violent offenses. The differences in the degree and persistence of behavioural
disorder between the two samples are also evident in their respective pre to post mean scores on the
HIT. The treatment group in the Gundersen and Svartdal study was just within the clinical range of
scores at pre-treatment (M=3.03) while in the present study participants were well within the clinical
range (M=3.39). Given their age, history of aggressive behaviour and early incarceration it is
reasonable to hypothesise that the ART participants would fit within the childhood-onset or LCP
developmental pathway for antisocial behaviour (Frick, 2004; Moffitt et al., 2002). Certainly, they
would seem to be further along in the developmental trajectory of the LCP subgroup than the
participants described in the Gundersen and Svartdal study and more entrenched in the cognitive
distortions associated with criminogenic thinking. As already noted, the HIT (Barriga, et al., 2001) is a
specific measure of cognitive distortions associated with antisocial and criminal behaviour. Hence the
present results might reflect a lack of cognitive change specific to these distortions (i.e., Self-centred,
Blaming Others, Assuming the Worst, and Minimising/Mislabelling) rather than cognition broadly. The
SSRS (Gresham & Elliot, 1990) and the AQ (Buss & Warren, 2000) are not only measures of
behavioural activation, both include items designed to assess cognition, and pre- to post- change
With respect to the exploratory aim of the pilot study to evaluate the cultural relevance of
ART in an Australian cultural context, the Moral Reasoning problem situations were written in a
language specific to American youth. Feedback given by participants at the conclusion of the pilot
program indicated the need to re-write the problems in a vernacular more familiar to Australian youth.
104
Therefore, expressions such as “Midtown Bar and Grill” became “local pub”, “AWOL” became
“escaped”, “going steady” became “going out with”, “nark” became “dob”, “play ball” became “play
footy” etc. Other changes to language were also made to some of the Skillstreaming and ACT
participant handouts. Apart from these minor adaptations, the feedback from participants in this pilot
study suggested that the ART program content was culturally relevant and applicable to aggressive
Australian youth.
At the time of commencing the pilot study, the Victorian Juvenile Justice Review (Day,
Howells, & Rickwood, 2003) had made recommendations regarding the implementation of evidence-
based programs, including ART, across both community and custodial youth justice services in the
State of Victoria. Several researchers have noted that the implementation of a new program and its
evaluation in a correctional setting can be fraught with difficulty (e.g., Fleck et al., 2001; Hollin, 2001b;
Tate et al., 1995; Wakai et al., 2009). Thus, another aim of the pilot study was to provide an
opportunity for the early identification of potential institutional and programmatic obstacles in
preparation for the main study. Apart from program/research participants, the stakeholders in a
custodial setting are numerous and their investment, or lack thereof, can have major implications to
the success of a program (Fleck et al., 2001; Wakai et al., 2009). Several initial hurdles to
implementation had to be overcome throughout the pilot period, including logistical problems such as
timetabling the three ART sessions per week within existing therapeutic, educational, and vocational
programming, and finding a suitable and available group-work space. Clarification of the research
design, particularly the inclusion/exclusion criteria, with the Health Services team who were
responsible for the overall case management of each youth and hence the initial referrals to ART was
also required. By far the most challenging and ongoing problems were related to communication
between the Centre staff and the program/research facilitators. Although the facilitators ensured
105
regular communication with the Health Services team, it became clear that on a daily basis the unit
staff, who undertook an individual key-worker role with the youth, had the most frequent contact with
them. As such, by week five, a written outline of the weekly program content was distributed on a
weekly basis and regular verbal progress reports were also given to the unit staff. These procedures
are consistent with recommendations made by Goldstein et al. (1998) relative to ART implementation
and enhancement of transfer of treatment gains. Goldstein et al. referred to these potential agents of
As discussed previously, Andrews et al. (1990) described Program Integrity (i.e., the in built
processes for monitoring treatment fidelity) as one of the five core principles in effective offender
treatment. Several meta-analyses (Andrews & Dowden, 2006; Landenberger & Lipsey, 2005; Lipsey
et al., 2001) provided strong evidence for the importance of the integrity principle. Similarly, in the
largest evaluation of ART to date, Barnoski (2004) found that treatment integrity and the related
concept of facilitator competency significantly influenced ART outcomes. Where an ART trainer was
ranked as “not competent” on the Washington State ART QA measure (see Measures section above)
recidivism data at the 18-month follow up was found to be slightly higher, although not significantly
different to that of the no-treatment control group. In contrast, “competent” or “highly competent”
delivery of ART was associated with a 24% reduction in recidivism over and above that of the control
group this difference was significant. Treatment integrity for the pilot study was assessed in sessions
3 and 8 by sending the video recordings of these sessions to the Washington State QA Specialist.
The facilitators were ranked as “highly competent” on the QA measure (see Appendix C).
106
A limitation of the pilot study was the sole use of self-report outcome measures. A number of
researchers (e.g., Kazdin & Weisz, 1998; Moffitt et al., 2008; Posavac & Carey, 2003) have indicated
the need for a multi-informant approach in evaluation research as important criteria for identifying
effective treatments. Moreover, subsequent to the pilot study, it was concluded that the
aforementioned key-worker role undertaken by the unit staff provided an opportunity to assess
potential behavioural and cognitive change of ART participants through a suitable non-self-report
measure. The addition of such a measure in the main study offered a broader, multi-informant
approach to evaluating the effectiveness of ART. Several other methodological changes were
implemented at the conclusion of the pilot study. Table 7 provides a summary and rationale for those
changes relevant to the key learning’s from the pilot study. Each of these changes are elaborated
upon in the next chapter which describes the research design and results of the main study, which
The present results provide some support for the efficacy of ART in reducing self-reported
aggression and increasing social skills among Australian youth justice clients serving a custodial
sentence for violence or violence related offences. However, the findings also revealed a lack of
change specific to cognitive distortions associated with aggressive and antisocial behaviour. Of
particular relevance to the findings of this study and the subsequent main study of ART was the
conclusions made by several researchers (Beck, 1996; Longmore & Worrell, 2007; Maruna & Mann,
2006) that the overall effectiveness of CBT interventions might lie in the interaction effects of the
components or as is the case with ART, a multimodal approach. Moreover, the distinction between
cognitive, behavioural and affective components in CBT might represent more of an arbitrary, theory
107
driven construct rather than the actual practice of the model in an applied setting. In conclusion, the
findings of this small, pilot study warrant further investigation with a larger sample, over an extended
period so that generalisability and maintenance of treatment gains can be assessed beyond the
Table 7
Change Rationale
Inclusion of waitlist control group Allow for statistical comparison between treatment group
and waitlist control at T2
Upper age range increased to 22-years of As recommended by Health Services staff at the
age custody centre increase in upper age limit in line with
centre’s upper age limit and increased potential subject
pool
Note. T2 = Time-2 assessment; SSRS = Social Skills Rating System; ABCL = Adult Behaviour Checklist;
SPSI-R:S = Social Problem Solving Inventory- Revised: Short Form
109
Chapter 7
Main Study
This chapter presents the method and results for the main study, which included the key
learning’s from the pilot study (see Table 7). In addition to extending the research done in the pilot
study, there were several other aims specific to the main study. As previously discussed, most of the
evaluations of ART to date have evaluated its effectiveness with adolescent populations (i.e., up to
18 years of age). A few published studies (Hatcher et al., 2008; Hornsveld, 2005; McGuire & Clarke,
2004) have reported outcomes for older, adult offenders. To date, there are no known published
outcome studies of ART among the age cohort that sits between adolescence and adulthood (i.e.,
early adulthood 18-24 years). Thus, one the aims of the main study was to investigate the
effectiveness of ART in an Australian custodial youth justice (i.e., 18-22 years) setting with a group of
young-adult male offenders. Consistent with Palmer’s (2007) recommendation regarding the need to
assess the relative contribution of each of the respective components of ART (i.e., cognitive,
behavioural, and affective) the measures employed in the main study were selected on the basis of
their theoretical relationship to each of the programs targets of change. Palmer argued that compared
to the anger control and social skills training procedures in ART, that there was relatively less
evidence for improvements in moral decision making and perspective-taking associated with the
moral reasoning component. Thus, the intention here was to provide evidence of change relative to
the specific targets of change implied within the three components of ART, particularly moral
reasoning.
110
In line with reommnedations made by Serin et al. (2009) and Tate et al. (1995) another aim
of the main study was to examine the clinical significance of potential post-treatment changes not just
criminogenic risk reduction. To this end, four specific hypotheses and two research questions were
posed. From pre-treatment (intake) to post-treatment (24-month follow-up) it was predicted that ART
group?
II. If there is evidence of post-treatment gain/s are they maintained over time (i.e., from
Method
Participants
Conistent with the pilot, participants in the main study (N=20) were serving at least a three
month custodial sentence at a youth custody centre located in Victoria, Australia for violent or
violence-related offences. The age range of participants was 18.7 to 20.5 years of age (M=19.6;
111
SD=.60). On average participants had completed 9.4 years of education. Most (74%) came from
intact two-parent families. With respect to family criminal history, 50% indicated that their father had a
previous criminal conviction and 5% stated that their mother had a previous conviction. Ten percent
of participants stated that their father or mother suffered from either an Axis I or II psychiatric
condition, while a quarter (25%) of participants indicated that they did not know. Nine young people
(45%) stated that they had previously been diagnosed with an Axis I disorder, including depression,
Recruitment
Recruitment into the main study was according to the same procedures described in the
pilot. All participants referred to ART met eligibility for the program in terms of clinically judged
cognitive ability, type of offence (i.e., violence-related) level of ciminogenic risk (moderate to high)
length of sentence (i.e., at least three months) and no self-reported history of psychotic symptoms in
the last six months. Table 8 shows completers, non-completers, waitlist, and refusals for each of the
Table 8
ART group completers, non-completers, waitlist controls, and refusals for main study
Thirty-two young men were referred to ART by the Health Services team. Of those, 20 completed the
treatment and their data were analysed for the main study. The inclusion of a waitlist control group
was intended to provide an examination of the potential contribution of extra-therapeutic factors (i.e.,
changes on outcome measures irrespective of treatment) at treatment end for the treatment group
compared to the waitlist group (i.e., T2). However, institutional factors beyond experimental control
did not allow for a waitlist control group sizable enough for statistical comparison. These factors
included the length of custody before parole, participation in other co-occurring vocational (e.g.,
forklift driving) and educational (e.g., literacy) programs, and other criminongenic need (e.g., drug
Measures
The Aggression Questionnaire (Buss & Warren, 2000) and the How I Think Questionnaire
(Barrriga et al., 2001) were retained. The Social Problem Solving Inventory- Revised: Short Form
(SPSI-R:S; (D'Zurilla, Nezu, & Maydeu-Olivares, 2002) was added to the battery of youth self-report
113
measures in order to assess potential changes in problem solving abilities and the Impulsiveness
subscale of the I7 questionnaire (Eysenck, Pearson, Easting, & Allsopp, 1985) was included to assess
impulsivity. As previously discussed, several researchers (Kazdin & Weisz, 1998; Moffitt, et al., 2008;
Posavac & Carey, 2003) have indicated the need for a multi-informant approach in outcome research
as important criteria for identifying effective treatments. Thus, the Adult Behaviour Checklist (ABCL;
Achenbach & Rescorla, 2003) was added in the main study to provide for other informants (i.e.,
custodial key-workers).
The Social Problem Solving Inventory- Revised: Short Form (SPSI-R:S; (D'Zurilla et al.,
2002) is a 25-item, self-report instrument designed to assess the ability to resolve problems faced in
everyday life for ages 13 and older. Respondents rate their responses on a 4-point Likert scale from
0 (“Not at all true of me”) to 4 (“Extremely true of me”). The SPSI-R:S comprises two adaptive
problem solving dimensions- Positive Problem Orientation (PPO; e.g., “Whenever I have a problem I
believe that it can be solved”) and Rational Problem Solving (RPS; e.g., “When I am trying to solve a
problem, I think of as many options as possible until I cannot come up with any more ideas”) and
three dysfunctional dimensions- Negative Problem Orientation (NPO; e.g., “I feel threatened and
afraid when I have an important problem to solve”) Impulsivity/Careless Style (ICS; e.g., “When I am
trying to solve a problem I go with the first good idea that comes to mind”) and Avoidance Style (AS;
e.g., “When a problem occurs in my life I put off trying to solve it for as long as possible”). The
dysfunctional dimensions were reverse scored for calculation of the total scale score, as described by
D’Zurila et al. High total scale scores indicate greater overall social problem solving abilities. The
SPSI-R:S provides seven qualitative severity descriptors based on standard scores, which have a
mean of 100 and standard deviation of 15. For standard scores of 55 and below a respondent is
categorised as “extremely below the norm group average”; 56-70 “very much below the norm group
114
average”; 71-85 “below norm group average”; 86-114 “norm group average”; 115-129 “above norm
group average”; 130-144 “very much above norm group average”; and 145 and above “extremely
above the norm group average”. D'Zurilla et al. indicated that a standard score of one or more
standard deviations above or below the norm group average should be considered a substantial
departure from the average. The normative sample of young adults (age 17-39 years) consisted of
950 American college students. D'Zurilla et al. reported adequate reliability estimates for the SPSI-
R:S subscales with alpha’s ranging from .74 for ICS up to .83 for NPO and AS, and a high of .89 for
Impulsivity was assessed using the I7 Questionnaire (Eysenck et al., 1985) which is a self-
report, 54-item, forced choice format (yes/no) questionnaire that includes three scales:
Impulsiveness, Venturesomeness, and Empathy. The present study employed the 19-item, uni-
dimensional I7Impulsiveness scale (e.g., “Do you often do things on the spur of the moment?”)
designed to assess poor impulse control. Higher scores on the I7 indicate greater impulsivity. The
scale was validated with a mixed gender sample, ranging in age from 16-89 years. Eysenck et al.
reported good internal consistency for males (alpha = .84) on the I7.
The Adult Behavior Checklist (ABCL; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2003) represents a revision
and increase in aged norms of the earlier Achenbach (1997 cited in Achenbach & Rescorla) Young
Adult Behavior Checklist (YABCL) which was normed for ages 18 to 30. The ABCL is completed by
“people who know the adult well, such as spouses, partners, friends, roommates, therapists, parents,
adult children, and other relatives” (p.6). In this study, the 118 problem behaviour items of the ABCL,
which are rated on a three point rating scale (“0=Not True”, “1=Somewhat or Sometimes True”,
“2=Very True or Often True”) and constitute the total scale score. The ABCL comprises eight
Anxious/Depressed (14-items; e.g., “feels worthless, nervous tense”) Withdrawn (nine items; e.g.,
“trouble making friends”) Somatic Complaints (nine items; e.g., “tired without good reason”) Thought
Problems (12-items; e.g., “strange behaviour, strange ideas”) Attention Problems (17-items; e.g.,
Breaking Behaviour (13-items; e.g., “trouble with the law”) and Intrusive (six items; e.g., “brags a lot;
shows off”). The ABCL provides scores for two global second order factors: Internalizing- assesses
problems within the self, and Externalizing- items associated with conflicts with other people and
social mores. The ABCL raw scores are converted to T scores (M= 50; SD= 10). A T score of 65 to
69 is considered to be in the “borderline clinical range” and T scores of 70 and above are considered
to be within the “clinical” range. According to Achenbach and Rescorla, both the borderline and
clinical range of scores significantly differentiate between people referred to mental health services
for behavioural, emotional, and social problems and non-referred demographically matched
individuals. The normative sample for ABCL was based on a large (N= 1,660) American national
probability sample. Mean alpha coefficients of .85 for the Empirically Based Scales were reported
(Achenbach & Rescorla). Internal consistency was very high for the Internalizing (alpha = .92) and
Externalizing (alpha = .93) factors, and the Total Problems scale (alpha = .97). Relevant to the
purposes of the present study only the Aggressive and Rule-Breaking syndrome scales were
analysed. The Internalizing and Externalising and total scale scores were also included in the present
analysis.
Procedure
The ART co-trainers were the same in the main study as the pilot study. However,
subsequent to the “highly competent” QA assessment in the pilot study (see Appendix C) the roles of
main- and co- trainer were alternated between the co-facilitators from week to week for the five
116
groups comprising the main study. Several procedures aimed at ensuring treatment integrity were
employed in the main study. These included the use of self-assessment session checklists for each
of the three ART components (see Appendix C). Additionally, the co-trainers attended fortnightly
supervision with a clinical psychologist experienced in adolescent group therapy. Also, the present
author maintained regular email contact with the Washington State ART Master Trainer in which
The research procedure for the main study was the same as for the pilot with the exception
of additional post-treatment assessment points at six month post-treatment follow-up (T3) and two
year post-treatment follow-up (T4). The key-worker report measure (i.e., ABCL; Achenbach &
Rescorla, 2003) was administered at T1, T2 and T3 but not T4. The ABCL was not administered at
T4 because at this point participants would have either exited from the justice system altogether or if
convicted of another offence would have most likely entered the adult justice system. Even for those
who might have re-entered the youth justice system, it was likely that they would not have been
allocated the same key-worker, making comparison of the measure at T4 problematic. This
procedure was consistent with a repeated- measures, multi-informant research design (Posavac &
Carey, 2003).
117
Results
Preparation of Data
All data screening and analysis was performed using SPSS version 17. Prior to analysis, all
variables were screened for data entry errors, outliers and violations of the assumptions for
multivariate analysis across all data collection points (i.e., T1 to T4). Potential outliers were assessed
using histograms, box plots, and descriptive statistics. The extent to which extreme values influenced
the distribution was determined by comparing the mean values with the 5% trimmed mean (Pallant,
2007). Where a substantial difference was identified, the outlier was replaced with the next closest
mean score at that time point which was not an outlier (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). This procedure
was implemented for one outlier on each of the SPSI-R subscales “Positive Problem Orientation”
(PPO) and “Negative Problem Orientation” (NPO) both at T2. These scores were causing significant
To assess significant departures from normality, skewness, kurtosis and standard error (SE)
scores were calculated. Tabachnick and Fidell (1996) outlined procedures for determining statistical
significance of non-normality in small to moderate sample sizes based upon the calculation of a z–
score for the skewness and kurtosis values. The critical z–score of 2.33 for one-tailed tests (p =.01)
was used as the cut-off for significant departure from normality for all dependent variables at each of
the data collection points (T1 to T4). There were no significant departures from normality except on
several of the subscales for the worker-report ABCL. Transformations were not performed on the
ABCL as it was scored using computer scoring software and all scores were entered as T–scores.
Moreover, potential problems with the validity of this scale with respect to its use in a correctional
118
setting were identified and will be discussed later in the General Discussion (see section- Social
Missing data, or missing cases as in the present study, is common in most studies,
particularly in research involving longitudinal follow-up (Pallant, 2007; Streiner, 2002). In the present
study, there were three missing cases at the six month follow-up (T3) and six missing cases at the
two year follow-up (T4) for the youth self-report data. None of the missing cases were the same at T3
as T4. For the worker-report ABCL, five cases were missing at T3. Although there are several
suggested methods, each with various strengths and weaknesses, there is no agreed gold standard
regarding the best approach to use with missing data. All have important implications relative to the
Streiner (2002) reviewed several of the most common approaches to dealing with missing
cases, including multiple regression imputation methods to predict missing values. However, this
procedure requires a large sample size and thus was not feasible in the present study. The two
methods, reviewed by Streiner, that were possible in the present study were replacement of missing
values with the group mean at that data collection point or the “last observation carried forward”
(LOCF) for specific missing cases. Replacing missing cases with the group mean increases the risk
of a Type I error (i.e., concluding a significant difference, when in fact there is not one). In contrast,
the LOCF approach is more conservative with respect to the risk of a Type I error, but increases the
risk of failing to detect significant improvement at post-treatment or the degree of improvement (i.e.,
Type II error). In line with Streiner’s recommendation, the present study initially ran the statistical
analysis for both the group mean replacement and LOCF methods to assess for differences. The
119
results of this comparison revealed no substantial differences in the results for each the respective
methods (see Appendix D for SPSS output for mean replacement and LOCF methods). Therefore, as
SPSS provides a function for this procedure, the present study employed the group mean
Statistical Analysis
main effect differences across each of the four data collection time points (i.e., the independent
variable) for all dependent measures. The univariate main effect was reported where the sphericity
assumption was met. Where the assumption was violated, the multivariate main effect (Wilks Lamda)
was reported. For significant main effects, a linear trend effect (i.e., consistent decrease or increase
on a given variable) was calculated using SPSS polynomial function to test for significant linear
change from T1 to T4. In cases where the main effect was significant but the linear effect was not, a
increase/decrease on a given variable) between T1 and T4 was calculated. In cases where the main
effect was significant but neither the linear nor quadratic effects were, a planned contrast using
SPSS- “difference” function, was performed (Rosenthal, 1985). To guard against Type I familywise
error, a conservative alpha (p < .01) for all significance testing was employed (Tabachnick & Fidell,
2001).
Effect sizes (partial eta squared) were calculated for all significant main and interaction
effects. Consistent with Cohen’s (1988) interpretive guidelines, the strength of an effect size was
reported as small (.01), medium (.06), or large (.14). Additionally, several researchers (Kazdin, 1999;
Kendall, Marrs-Garcia, Nath & Sheldrick, 1999; Wise, 2004) have recommended that treatment
120
effectiveness studies report changes based on normative data for the outcome measures used.
Thus, the group means for participants in the present study were compared to the available
normative data (i.e., percentiles or standard scores or T scores) for each of the dependent measures,
Table 9 shows the reliability coefficients (Cronbach’s alpha) for all dependent variables used in
the present study at each data collection point. Reliability estimates for the ABCL (Achenbach &
Rescorla, 2003) were not calculated as the scores reported for this scale were T-scores based on the
Table 9
Reliability coefficients for all dependent measures at pre-treatment (T1), treatment-end (T2), 6-month
follow-up (T3), and 24-month follow-up (T4)
Cronbach’s alpha
Pre Post 6-month 24-month
(n = 20) (n = 20) (n = 17) (n = 14) N of Items
AQ Total .94 .93 .92 .95 34
- Physical .92 .94 .86 .94 8
- Anger .76 .82 .69 .88 7
- Hostility .82 .77 .65 .76 8
- Indirect .77 .66 .62 .74 7
- Verbal .62 .87 .61 .75 5
HIT Total .93 .95 .96 .95 54
- Self-Centred .80 .85 .88 .74 9
- Blaming Others .75 .84 .88 .81 10
- Minimizing/Mislabeling .80 .89 .93 .85 9
- Assuming the Worst .72 .83 .89 .81 11
- Lying .77 .85 .86 .72 8
- Stealing .91 .91 .96 .89 11
- Oppositional Defiant .70 .83 .88 .74 10
- Physical Aggression .78 .92 .91 .85 10
SPSI- R:S Total .87 .80 .68 .89 25
- Positive Problem Orientation .83 .86 .65 .92 5
- Negative Problem Orientation .82 .62 .41 .79 5
- Rational Problem Style .77 .85 .83 .95 5
- Impulsive/Carelessness Style .60 .54 .86 .79 5
- Avoidance Style .88 .93 .79 .86 5
I7 Impulsiveness Scale .80 .82 .74 .85 19
Note. AQ = Aggression Questionnaire; HIT = How I Think Questionnaire; SPSI- R:S = Social Problem
Solving Inventory-Revised: Short Form
122
As indicated in Table 9, most measures showed moderate to very high internal consistency at each
of the data collection points. There were some instances where alpha coefficients fell below the
recommended cut-off of .70 for scale reliability for research purposes (DeVellis, 2003). However,
these instances of low reliability need to be considered in light of the small sample size in the present
study. It is not uncommon for low Cronbach values to be found in such cases (DeVellis, 2003;
Pallant, 2007).
Table 10 shows the intercorrelations between all dependent measures at T1. A similar table has
been included for T2 assessments (see Appendix E). Due to sample attrition at T3 and T4, no
correlation matrices were included for these data collection points. The direction of the correlations
(Pearson’s r) within scales was as expected relative to the construct validity of each measure. For the
AQ scale the strength of significant intercorrelations ranged from moderate (r=.57) for anger and
verbal aggression and strong (r=.89) for the AQ total scale and physical and indirect aggression
respectively. The strength of association between the HIT and it subscales ranged between moderate
(r=.39) for physical aggression and lying to strong (r=.91) for the HIT total and minimizing/mislabeling
and oppositional defiant and self-centered. As expected the dysfunctional problem solving
approaches- NPO, ICS, and AS were all showed moderate to high, negative associations with the
SPSI-R:S total scale. While the positive problem solving approaches- PPO and RPS showed
Table 10
Inter-correlations for all dependent measures in the main study at T1 (N=20)
AQ PHY ANG HOS IND VER HIT SC BO MM AW L S OD PA SPSI PPO NPO RPS ICS AS I7
AQ -
PHY .89** -
ANG .77** .82** -
HOS .71** .39*. .34 -
IND .89** .73** .60** .65** -
VER .74** .63** .57** .32 .63** -
HIT .63** .49* .33 .71** .56** .28 -
SC .62** .53** .29 .62** .50* .33 .90** -
BO .39* .14 .16 .61** .35 .18 .86** .73** -
MM .67** .61** .40 .62** .64** .33 .91** .76** .66** -
AW .59** .46* .31 .69** .51* .17 .94** .79** .76** .86** -
L .46* .25 .28 .74** .52** .10 .70** .51* .60** .68** .70** -
S .40* .23 .13 .53** .40* .19 .90** .78** .90** .80** .81** .57** -
OD .61* .49* .27 .68** .43* .26 .89** .91** .73** .76** .85** .45* .73** -
PA .70** .72** .48* .47* .56** .39* .84** .81** .60** .81** .82** .39* .62** .83** -
SPSI -.21 .07 -.01 -.68** -.13 .06 -.45* -.31 -.51* -.32 -.48* -.50* -.40* -.45* -.16 -
PPO .18 .21 -.08 .01 .31 .20 -.06 .10 -.20 .01 -.16 -.08 -.17 .10 .07 .54** -
NPO .34 .02 .18 .71** .22 .16 .33 .27 .43 .15 .35 .39* .25 .41* .12 -.84** -.25 -
RPS .10 .00 .01 .13 .05 .19 .05 .18 .12 -.09 -.06 .11 -.01 .02 .05 .62** .51* .20 -
ICS .46* .19 .03 .65** .51* .29 .54** .52** .56** .43* .42* .45* .42 .54** .41* -.54** .24 .57** .30 -
AS .15 -.11 -.18 .71** .11 -.15 .44* .38* .43* .32 .43* .59** .36 .43* .14 -.81** -.09 -.75** .26 .60** -
I7 .36 .24 .15 .30 .44* .26 .41* .34 .44* .41* .28 .05 .40* .37 .48* -.30 .08 -.30 .08 .56** .25 -
Note. All 1-tailed correlations; *p<.05, **p<.01
AQ = Aggression Questionnaire Total; PHY = Physical Aggression, ANG = Anger, HOS = Hostility, IND = Indirect Aggression, VER = Verbal Aggression; HIT = How I Think Questionnaire Total:
SC = Self-Centred, BO = Blaming Others, MM = Minimising/Mislabelling, AW = assuming the Worst, L = Lying, S = Stealing, OD = Oppositional Defiant, PA = Physical Aggression; SPSI = Social
Problem Solving Inventory-Revised: Short Form Total: PPO = Positive Problem Orientation, NPO = Negative Problem Orientation, RPS = Rational Problem Solving, ICS = Impulsive/Careless
Style, AS = Avoidance Style; I7 = Eysenck Impulsiveness Scale.
124
With regard to the intercorrelations between measures, Table 10 shows that for the HIT and AQ Total
Score and subscales most were significantly and positively related. As cognitive distortions increased
so too did aggression. The exception was for the AQ subscales Anger and Verbal Aggression, which
were not significantly related to any of the HIT subscales except Physical Aggression. Surprisingly,
there were few significant associations between the AQ and the SPSI-R:S. Overall, the only AQ
subscale that was significantly related to social problem solving was Hostility. As hostility increased
general social problem abilities (i.e., SPSI-R:S total) tended to decrease and dysfunctional problem
solving approaches (i.e., NPO, ICS and AS) tended to increase. Not surprisingly, the HIT and the
SPSI-R;S total scores showed mostly significant and inverse relationships, as cognitive distortions
increased social problem solving abilities tended to decrease. As expected, Impulsiveness showed a
significant, positive association with ICS on the SPSI-R:S. Except for Indirect Aggression,
Iimpulsiveness was not significantly related to the AQ. Overall, the strength of intercorrelations at T2
(see Appendix E) tended to increase slightly, which might have been due to possible practice effects.
Table 11 shows means, standard deviations, and main effect ANOVA results from T1 to T4
Table 11
Descriptive statistics and main effect ANOVA results from T1 to T4 on the AQ (N=20)
Pre (T1) Post (T2) 6-month (T3) 24-month (T4) Main Effect
AQ Total 102.25 (26.00) 89.35 (24.51) 83.26 (20.73) 85.14 (22.01) 4.91 .004 .32
Physical 26.85 (8.86) 21.40 (9.00) 20.94 (6.56) 20.14 (7.49) 6.07 .001 .31
Anger 19.41 (5.04) 18.23 (5.79) 17.47 (4.64) 15.93 (5.58) 2.62 .06 Ns
Hostility 21.60 (7.69) 18.10 (5.76) 17.23 (4.70) 20.00 (5.16) 4.40 .007 .19
Indirect 16.55 (4.97) 14.45 (4.48) 15.00 (3.89) 14.43 (3.83) 4.82* .01 .46
Verbal 16.40 (3.80) 16.70 (4.84) 15.23 (3.36) 14.64 (3.41) 1.99 .13 Ns
As shown in Table 11, there was a significant main effect for time on the AQ Total scale score,
Physical Aggression, Hostility, and Indirect Aggression. However, there was no significant change for
Anger and Verbal Aggression. The effect sizes reported for the significant main effects were large,
ranging from .19 for Hostility to .46 for Indirect Aggression (Cohen, 1988).
Figures 5 to 8 show the direction of change on the AQ from T1 to T4 for each of the
significant main effects. To test linear or quadratic change from T1 to T4, a series of polynomial trend
As shown in Figure 5, the linear trend for the AQ total score was significant (F (1,19)= 7.88, p= .01,
partial η2 = .29). There was a significant linear reduction in overall self-reported aggression from T1
to T2 (F (1,19)= 12.65, p= .002, partial η2 = .40) with a large effect size. There was no significant
change between T2 and T3 (F (1,19)= 5.25, p= .03) or between T3 and T4 (F (1,19)= 1.73, p= .20).
According to the clinical cut-offs for the AQ Total Score, the average score at T1 fell at the 76th
percentile (“High Average”) with all subsequent mean scores falling between the 50th and 60th
The linear trend effect shown in Figure 6 for Physical Aggression was significant (F (1,19)= 12.10, p=
.003, partial η2 = .39). There was a significant reduction in self-reported physical aggression from T1
to T2 (F (1,19)= 16.43, p= .001, partial η2 = .46). The effect sizes were large. From T2 to T3 (F
(1,19)= 3.21, p= .09) and T3 to T4 F (1,19)= 3.98, p= .06) there was no significant change in self-
reported physical aggression. According to the percentile rankings and qualitative descriptors for the
AQ, the mean scores on this subscale at T1 fell at the 90th percentile (well within the “clinical” range).
All subsequent mean scores fell between the 50th and 55th percentile (“normal” range) (Buss &
Warren, 2000).
128
While the main effect for Hostility was significant, the linear trend was not (F (1,19)= 1.46, p = .24).
However the quadratic effect was significant (F (1,19)= 9.11, p= .007, partial η2 = .32). Planned
contrasts revealed a significant reduction in hostility from T1 to T2 (F (1,19)= 8.70, p= .008, partial
η2 = .31). Again these effect sizes were large according to Cohen’s (1988) criteria. There was no
significant change between T2 and T3 (F (1,19)= 3.83, p= .07) or between T3 and T4 (F (1,19)= 1.10,
p= .31). The group means on Hostility were within the “normal” range of scores at all time points.
129
Both the linear (F (1,19)= 2.33, p = .14) and quadratic (F (1,19)= 1.16, p = .30) trend effects for
Indirect Aggression were non-significant. As the main effect was significant, planned contrasts were
conducted. The tests showed a significant (F (1,19)= 11.04, p= .004, partial η2 = .37) decrease in
indirect aggression between T1 and T2. There was no significant change between T2 and T3 (F
(1,19)= .46, p= .51) or between T3 and T4 (F (1,19)= .77, p= .39). The group means for Indirect
Summary for aggression results. The AQ Total and Physical Aggression scores showed a
significant linear reduction between T1 and T4. The linear trend for Hostility was not significant but
130
the quadratic effect was. For Indirect Aggression both the linear and quadratic effects were
significant. For each of these significant changes planned contrasts revealed that the reductions were
evident at post-treatment (T2). Given that there was no significant increase or decrease on these
measures at T3 or T4, the results suggest that the reductions in self-reported aggressive behaviours
and thoughts were maintained at two-year (T4) follow-up.There was no significant change on the AQ
subscales Anger and Verbal Aggression. All scores on the AQ were either clinical or borderline
clinical at T1 and T2 but had fallen within the “normal” range by T3 and remained there at T4.
Table 12 shows the descriptive statistics and main effect ANOVA results for the HIT Total
Scale, the four cognitive distortions (i.e., Self-centred, Blaming Others, Minimizing/Mislabeling,
Assuming the Worst) and the four behavioural referents (i., Lying, Stealing, Oppositional Defiant, and
Table 12
Descriptive statistics and main effect ANOVA results from T1 to T4 on the HIT (N=20)
Pre (T1) Post (T2) 6-month (T3) 24-month (T4) Main Effect
HIT Total 3.17 (.70) 2.78 (.76) 2.58 (.88) 2.13 (.50) 11.66 .000 .38
Self-Centered 3.18 (.83) 2.79 (.77) 2.67 (.87) 2.21 (.53) 8.54 .000 .31
Blaming Others 3.04 (.76) 2.73 (.80) 2.56 (.87) 2.02 (.55) 9.70 .000 .34
Minimizing/ Mislabel 3.22 (.89) 2.80 (.90) 2.50 (1.04) 2.02 (.59) 11.26 .000 .37
Assuming the Worst 3.21 (.68) 2.78 (.73) 2.55 (.85) 2.39 (.56) 6.65 .000 ..29
Lying 3.28 (.79) 2.82 (.84) 2.74 (.89) 2.46 (.56) 7.48 .000 .28
Stealing 2.76 (1.04) 2.50 (.84) 2.32 (.98) 1.68 (.48) 8.91 .000 .32
Oppositional Defiant 3.47 (.72) 3.04 (.79) 2.71 (.93) 2.31 (.60) 12.98 .000 .41
Physical Aggression 3.20 (.77) 2.78 (.89) 2.57 (.91) 1.97 (.59) 13.02 .000 .40
The main effects for all HIT subscales and the total scale score were highly significant (p < .001). The
effect sizes were large ranging from .28 (Lying) to .41 (Oppositional Defiant and Physical
Aggression).
Figures 9 to 13 plot the direction of change for the group means between T1 and T4 on the
HIT Total Score, cognitive distortion and behavioural referent subscales. To test linear change from
132
one time point to the next, a series of polynomial trend analyses were conducted. Clinical significance
based on the normative data provided for the HIT was also reported.
The linear trend effect displayed in Figure 9 was significant with a large effect size (F (1,19)= 11.65,
p= .000, partial η2 = .38). There was no significant change between T1 and T2 (F (1,19)= 4.52, p=
.05) and T2 and T3 (F (1,19)= 5.21, p= .03). However, there was a significant reduction on the HIT
total score between T3 and T4 (F (1,19)= 34.11, p= .000, partial η2 = .64). The group mean at T1
(88th percentile) was clinically significant while at T2 it had dropped to within the borderline clinical
133
range (74th percentile). At T3 (62nd percentile) and T4 (well below the 50th percentile) scores were
The trend effect for Self-centered Thinking (see Figure 10) was significant (F (1,19)= 36.50, p= .000,
partial η2 = .66). While there was no significant difference between T1 to T2 (F (1,19)= 2.84, p= .11)
or between T2 to T3 (F (1,19)= 3.71, p= .07) there was a significant reduction in self-centred thinking
between T3 to T4 (F (1,19)= 26.87, p= .000, partial η2 = .57). The effect size was large. The mean
score for Self-Centered Thinking at T1 reached clinical significance (83rd percentile) while all post-
The linear trend effect shown in Figure 11 for Blaming Others was also significant (F (1,19)= 30.19,
p= .000, partial η2 = .61). Between T1 and T2 (F (1,19)= 2.14, p= .16) and between T2 to T3 (F
(1,19)= 3.41, p= .08) there was no significant change in participants tendency to blame others.
However, there was a significant reduction between T3 and T4 (F (1,19)= 29.14, p= .000, partial η2 =
.61). The effect sizes were large. At T1, participants scores were “borderline clinical” (81st percentile)
and thereafter fell within the “normal” range (Barriga et al., 2001).
135
Figure 12 shows a highly significant linear trend effect for Minimizing/Mislabeling (F (1,19)= 51.92, p=
.000, partial η2 = .73) with a significant reduction between T3 and T4 (F (1,19)= 38.40, p= .000,
partial η2 = .67). The effect sizes for the linear trend and planned contrast were large. Planned
contrasts showed that there was no significant difference between T1 and T2 (F (1,19)= 4.13, p=
.06,) or T2 and T3 (F (1,19)= 5.20, p= .03). Normative data indicated that at T1 participants scores
fell within the “clinical” range (88th percentile). At post-treatment assessment (T2) mean scores were
136
“borderline” clinical (77th percentile) and subsequently dropped within the “normal” range. By T4
The linear trend effect for Assuming the Worst was significant (F (1,19)= 24.21, p= .001, partial η2 =
.56). Participants tendency to assume the worst showed a significant reduction between T3 and T4 (F
(1,19)= 12.86, p= .002, partial η2 = .40) but not between T1 and T2 (F (1,19)= 6.75, p= .02) or T2 and
T3 (F (1,19)= 5.55, p= .03). The group mean at T1 was clinically significant (89th percentile) while at
137
T2, it fell within the “borderline clinical” range (78th percentile). By T3 (66th percentile) and T4 (56th
percentile) the group average had fallen within the “normal” range.
Figure 14 shows the linear trend effect for Lying, which was significant (F (1,19)= 23.99, p= .001,
partial η2 = .56) and the effect was large. There was a significant reduction in participants self-
reported lying between T1 and T2 (F (1,19)= 9.22, p= .007, partial η2 = .33) and between T3 and T4
(F (1,19)= 14.53, p= .001, partial η2 = .43) but not T2 to T3 (F (1,19)= 2.80, p= .11). The group mean
138
at T1 fell within the “borderline clinical” range (79th percentile). All subsequent scores fell within the
The declining linear trend for Stealing was also significant (F (1,19)= 29.00, p= .001, partial η2 = .60)
with a significant reduction between T3 and T4 (F (1,19)= 38.07, p= .000, partial η2 = .67) but not
between T1 and T2 (F (1,19)= 1.12, p= .30) or T2 and T3 (F (1,19)= 2.36, p= .14). At T1, scores fell
within the “clinical” range (88th percentile) while at T2 they were classified as “borderline clinical” (80th
percentile). By T3 and T4 scores had fallen within the “normal” range (72nd and below the 50th
percentiles, respectively).
139
The linear trend for the Oppositional Defiant subscale shown in Figure 16 was significant and large (F
(1,19)= 54.94, p= .001, partial η2 = .74). Although the reduced scores between T1 and T2 were non-
significant (F (1,19)= 5.04, p= .04) there was a significant reduction between T2 and T3 (F (1,19)=
8.92, p= .008, partial η2 = .32) and between T3 and T4 (F (1,19)= 28.33, p= .000, partial η2 = .60).
Group means were well within the “clinical” range at T1 (89th percentile) but had fallen within the
“borderline clinical” range (77th percentile) at T2. By T3 and T4, the group mean fell within the
The linear trend effect for the HIT subscale- Physical Aggression (Figure 17) was significant and
large (F (1,19)= 41.78, p= .001, partial η2 = .69). There was a significant reduction between T3 and
T4 (F (1,19)= 38.39, p= .000, partial η2 = .67) but not between T1 and T2 (F (1,19)= 4.90, p= .04) or
T2 and T3 (F (1,19)= 4.31, p= .05). The group mean at T1 was high and clinically significant (88th
percentile). By T2, the group mean was just within the “borderline clinical” range (73rd percentile). By
T3 and T4, scores were within the “normal” range (65th and less than the 50th percentiles,
respectively).
141
Summary of cognitive distortions and behavioural referents. The linear trend analysis
for the HIT Total Score and all of its subscales showed significant reductions between T1 and T4.
Group means for the HIT fell within the “clinical” or “borderline clinical” range at T1 but fell within the
“normal” range by the six month follow-up (T3). The reductions in cognitive distortions were
Table 13 shows descriptive statistics and main effect ANOVA results from T1 to T4 on the
Table 13
Descriptive statistics and main effect ANOVA results from T1 to T4 on the SPSI (N=20)
Pre (T1) Post (T2) 6-month (T3) 24-month (T4) Main Effect
M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) F(3,57) P Partial
η2
SPSI Total 11.28 (2.80) 12.72 (2.94) 12.72 (2.51) 13.40 (2.35) 4.92 .004 .21
PPO 12.45 (4.62) 13.95 (4.44) 14.82 (2.92) 15.64 (3.62) 3.82 .02 ns
NPO 7.50 (4.74) 6.45 (3.49) 6.41 (3.04) 6.07 (3.19) 0.83 .48
ns
RPS 8.65 (3.77) 10.35 (4.77) 10.47 (3.87) 12.07 (4.90) 3.48 .02
ns
ICS 8.70 (3.37) 7.90 (3.18) 8.32 (4.47) 8.79 (3.62) 0.33 .80
ns
AS 8.50 (6.24) 6.35 (5.57) 6.94 (3.93) 5.86 (3.73) 2.03 .12
ns
Note. SPSI = Social Problem Solving Inventory; PPO = Positive Problem Orientation; NPO = Negative Problem
Orientation; RPS = Rational Problem Solving; ICS = Impulsive/Careless Style; AS = Avoidance Style; ns = non-
significant.
The main effect for the SPSI total scale score was significant. The increasing mean scores between
T1 and T4 was consistent with significant improvement in overall social problem solving. However,
the main effects for the SPSI subscales did not reach significance at the conservative p < .01. Figure
18 shows the trend effect for the SPSI total scale from T1 to T4.
143
The linear trend effect shown in Figure 18 was significant and large (F (1,19)= 8.69, p= .008, partial
η2 = .31). There was significant improvement in overall social problem solving between T1 and T2 (F
(1,19)= 9.61, p= .006, partial η2 = .34) but not between T2 and T3 (F (1,19)= 3.65, p= .07) or T3 and
T4 (F (1,19)= 3.55, p= .08). The interpretive guidelines for the SPSI-R:S are based on standard
scores, which have a mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 15 (D’Zurilla et al., 2002). According to
these guidelines, the group means for the current participants fell within the “norm group average” at
all time points (i.e., a standard score between 86-114) for the Total Score and subscale scores.
Summary of social problem solving. The youth self-report scores for social problem
solving showed significant improvement from T1 to T4 on the SPSI total scale. However, there was
144
no significant change on the five SPSI subscales. All scores on the SPSI were within the normal
The main effect for Impulsiveness was significant with a large effect size (F(3,57)= 4.91, p=
.004, partial η2 = .21). Figure 19 shows the mean scores from T1 to T4 for the youth self-report
scores on Impulsiveness.
The linear trend effect displayed in Figure 19 was significant and large (F (1,19)= 14.58, p= .001,
between T2 (M = 11.10, SD = 4.29) and T3 (M = 9.82, SD = 3.56) (F (1,19)= 8.34, p= .009, partial
η2 = .31). The reductions between T1 (M = 12.50, SD = 3.91) and T2 (F (1,19)= 1.89, p= .19) and T3
and T4 (M = 8.93, SD = 4.08) (F (1,19)= 5.68, p= .03) were non-significant. In the normative sample,
a mean score of 9.84 (SD= 4.13) was reported for males between 16 and 19 years of age (Eysenck
et al., 1985). In the present study, the group mean for impulsiveness at T1 fell at the 74th percentile,
above the normative average. By T3, the significantly reduced group mean (M= 9.82) was
comparable to the normative average for similar aged males (Eysenck et al).
Table 14 shows the T-score group means, standard deviations and ANOVA main effect
results for the ABCL from T1 to T3, as reported by custodial workers. None of the main effects for the
worker-report ABCL were significant at the conservative p < .01. Moreover, the T- score group means
and standard deviations show a relatively flat distribution between T1 and T3.
146
Table 14
T-score descriptive statistics and main effect ANOVAS from T1 to T3 for the ABCL (N=20)
ABCL Total 55.80 (8.14) 52.50 (9.50) 52.33 (7.35) 3.64* .05
Ns
Aggression 56.95 (6.83) 57.25 (8.01) 56.33 (7.59) .17* .86
Ns
Rule Breaking 64.25 (10.82) 61.75 (6.57) 60.93 (7.49) 1.43 .25
Ns
Internalizing 55.15 (10.12) 51.25 (8.68) 51.80 (9.22) 1.75 .19
Ns
Externalizing 58.85 (7.19) 57.38 (10.29) 56.00 (9.70) 2.08* .15
Ns
Note. T-scores have a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10; * Multivariate test- Wilks’
Lambda was reported due to lack of sphericity- df = (2,18); ns = non-significant.
According to the clinical cut-off scores for the ABCL Total Problem, Internalising and
Externalizing scales, a T-score of 60-63 (approximately the 84th through to the 90th percentiles) is
classified as “borderline clinical”. T-scores greater or equal to 64 are considered to fall within the
“clinical” range (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000). For the Syndrome scales (e.g., Aggression, Rule-
breaking Behaviour) a slightly higher cut-off is indicated with borderline T-scores ranging from 65 to
69, and T scores needing to be greater than 70 to be categorised as “clinical” . At no time in the
present study did any of the worker-reported ABCL scores reach borderline or clinical cut-offs.
In the next chapter, the results for the main study are discussed relative to the specific
Discussion
The aims of the main study were firstly to evaluate the effectiveness of ART in Australia with
a group of young adult, violent male offenders. Consistent with Palmer’s (2007) recommendation, the
second aim was to provide further evidence for improved morel decision-making and social
perspective taking abilities thought to be associated with the moral reasoning training procedures in
ART. To this end, the measures used in the main study were selected on the basis of their theoretical
relationship to the specific cognitive, affective and behavioural targets of change implicit within each
of three components of ART. The third aim of the main study was to add to the existing “what works”
literature by examining treatment outcomes relative to behavioural and psychological change (i.e.,
aggressive thoughts and behaviours, self-serving cognitive distortions, social problem solving, and
impulsivity) not just criminogenic risk reduction as assessed by recidivism (Serin et al., 2009; Tate et
al., 1995).
Several hypotheses were made relative the aims of the main study. From pre-treatment (T1)
to 24-month (T4) follow-up, it was predicted that ART participants would show evidence of significant:
(1) reductions in self-reported and worker-reported aggressive behaviours and thoughts; (2)
reductions in self-reported aggressive and antisocial cognitive distortions; (3) improvement in self-
reported social problem solving abilities; and (4) reductions in self-reported impulsivity. In this
chapter, the results of each of the main study hypotheses are discussed relative to the targets of
change implicit in ART (i.e., Aggressive Behaviour and Thoughts, Cognitive Distortions and Moral
The present study also included two broad research questions: (1) Does participation in ART
reduce aggressive symptoms compared to a waitlist control group? and (2) If there is evidence of
148
post-treatment gain/s will they be maintained over time (i.e., from treatment-end [T2] to 24-month
follow-up [T4])? As previously mentioned, it was not possible to address the first research question
because of insufficant numbers in the waitlist group. This issue will be further discussed in the section
on Limitations. The evidence in support of the second research question will be discussed in the
section- Maintenance of Treatment Gains. A notable absence in the offender rehabilitation literature
is discussion of therapeutic process variables such as the therapeutic alliance or group cohesion, and
how these factors contribute to treatment effectiveness (Dowden & Andrews, 2004; Holmqvist, Hill, &
Lang, 2007; Ross, Polaschek, & Ward, 2007). The importance of attending to group processes in
ART was a key learning from this study, and will be discussed in the section- Group Process versus
Content. The final sections of this chapter will discuss the Limitations, Clinical Implications and
recommendations for Future Research, and provide a Summary of Findings and some Concluding
behaviours and thoughts following participation in ART. The AQ Total score showed a significant
reduction at treatment-end (T2) with a large effect size (Cohen, 1988). At the six-month (T3) and two-
year (T4) follow-up, there was no further change (increase or decrease) on the AQ Total. This finding
suggests that there was an immediate post-treatment gain and that those gains were maintained at
two-year follow-up with no further increases or decreases. According to Buss and Warren (2000) the
AQ Total score provides a measure of aggression relative to both behavioural frequency (e.g., “I get
into fights more than most people”) and cognitive intensity (e.g., “At times I feel like a bomb ready to
explode”). The measurement of frequency and intensity is consistent with Novaco’s (1975, 1997)
149
component model has been operationalised by Feindler and her colleagues (Feindler, 2003; Feindler
& Guttman, 1994; Feindler & Ecton, 1986) in the Anger Control Training (ACT) component of ART.
The multi-component targets of change in ACT include: environmental activation (i.e., External
processes (i.e., verbalization of self-talk; Internal Triggers; Reminders; Thinking Ahead; Self-
Evaluation); subjective affective experience (i.e., Internal Triggers; Reminders); and behavioural
enactment (i.e., Social Skills). The significant change on the AQ Total score, the maintenance of
those changes at two year follow-up, and the large effect size (partial η2 = .32) indicated strong
In addition, the AQ subscales- Physical Aggression, Hostility and Indirect Aggression all
showed significant reductions at treatment-end. As was the case with the AQ Total score, the change
on these measures was evident at T2 and there was no further change (increase or decrease) at
subsequent follow-ups. Again, these results indicate support for maintenance of the initial treatment
gains at two year follow-up and, according to Cohen’s (1988) criteria, the effects sizes were large.
High scorers on the Physical Aggression subscale typically lack impulse control and knowledge of
non-aggressive responses (i.e., pro-social skill deficits) when faced with perceived provocation or
frustration (Buss & Warren, 2000). In addition, high scorers on this subscale are likely to employ
cognitive distortions to justify their aggressive acts such as “they asked for it” (i.e., Blaming Others
and Mislabeling). The presence of poor impulse control and the use of cognitive rationalisations are
consistent with the dynamic risk factors which Andrews et al. (1990) argued should be the primary
targets of change in treatment aimed at ameliorating criminal behaviour. Poor impulse control, lack of
150
pro-social skills, and the use cognitive distortions to justify and maintain aggressive behaviour were
key characteristics of the participants in the present study prior to completing ART.
While the Physical Aggression subscale represents a tendency toward the externalisation of
aqgression, the Hostility and Indirect Aggression subscales reflect a propensity toward angry
rumination (i.e., internalisation of anger). High scores on Hostility are consistent with feelings of
paranoia (e.g., “I sometimes feel that people are laughing at me behind my back”) and the cognitive
distortion Assuming the Worst (e.g., “I can’t help losing my temper a lot”). Buss and Warren (2000)
noted that individuals scoring high on Hostility are likely to demonstrate hostile attributions toward
others. As previously discussed, several researchers (e.g., Barriga et al., 2001; Dodge et al., 2002;
Dodge & Pettit, 2003) have identified hostile attributions as being pervasive in the social-cognitive
appraisals of aggressive children and youth. The reduced Hostility scores for participants in the
present study might reflect an increased capacity to attend to non-hostile social cues and respond
accordingly through the use of one of the prosocial skills taught in Skillstreaming.
Indirect Aggression (e.g., “If I’m angry enough I may mess up someone else’s work”)
measures the tendency toward passive or non-confrontational aggression. High scores on this scale
are associated with oppositional and avoidant behaviours among children and antisocial personality
characteristics among adults, with concomitant chronic levels of frustration and angry cognitions
(Buss & Warren, 2000). Both Berkowitz (1990) and Novaco (2007) discussed the relationship
between chronic negative affect (e.g., frustration, annoyance, irritability) and the propensity to
experience anger-related feelings and thoughts, which in turn is associated with a “hair-pin” trigger for
aggressive behaviour. Further, Novaco noted the role of accumulated negative affect and angry
rumination in generating aggressive social interactions. Similarly, Gilbert and Daffern (in press)
151
cognitions and their association with schematic biases that interpret benign social situations as
threatening and requiring an aggressive response. This is consistent with anecdotal reports from
participants in the present study. They indicated a tendency to find themselves in anger provoking
situations that easily escalated into physical and/or verbal aggression. The reduced tendency toward
angry rumination (i.e., less Hostility and Indirect Aggression) for the ART participants, and the
maintenance of those gains at two year follow-up, may well be associated with a reduction in the
In addition to testing for statistically significant change, this study was also interested in
examining “real-life” or clinically significant change. Several researchers (e.g., Kazdin, 1999; Kendall
et al., 1999; Wise, 2004) have described various procedures for determining clinical significance.
Consistent with Kazdin (1999) the present study determined clinical significance by comparing the
group means at each data collection point with the clinical cut-offs provided for each of the measures
used. Prior to participation in ART (T1) the group means on the AQ Total and the Physical
Aggression, Anger and Hostility subscales were clinically significant, while the group means for
Verbal and Indirect Aggression fell within the borderline clinical range. By post-treatment (T2) scores
on the AQ total, Physical Aggression, Hostility and Indirect Aggression fell within the “normal” range,
and by six month follow-up (T3) all scores were within “normal” range, where they remained at 24-
month follow-up (T4). These results indicated that the post-treatment reductions in participant’s self-
reported aggression were associated with clinically relevant symptom reduction. While these
improvements in aggressive related symptoms might transfer into very real improvements in the daily
social functioning of these youth, information from other informants known to the participants (e.g.,
152
family members, peers, intimate partners) would be needed to support this. Additionally, Kazdin
(1999) noted that post-treatment clinical change may or may not be associated with improved
The youth self-report reductions in aggressive behaviours and thoughts were consistent with
previous evaluations of ACT as a stand alone intervention (Nugent et al., 1997; Sukhodolsky et al.,
2009) and as part of the multi-component ART program (Gunderson & Savardtal, 2006; Hornsveld,
2005). Although there were limitations to the Nugent et al. study (i.e., unequal sample sizes between
treatment and control groups and compromised treatment integrity) their results showed significant
behavioural improvement on the CBCL (Achenbach, 1991) for ACT participants compared to the
comparison groups. However, in contrast to the present study, Nugent et al. reported that these
treatment gains were not maintained beyond completion of ACT. They suggested that this might have
been due to the adverse effects of the program ending prematurely as a result of the aggressive
behavior of the group leader. Sukhodolsky et al. compared 26 young people (M age = 12.7years) with
comorbid disruptive behaviour disorders and Tourette’s Syndrome. They reported a significant
reduction in parent, clinician and child report measures of anger and externalizing behaviours
compared to the treatment as usual group. Given that only two participants in this study met criteria
for CD and none were outside the normal range of scores on the CBCL, Sukhodolsky et al. cautioned
against generalising their findings to adolescents with more serious forms of conduct related
problems. Given that particpants in the present study were moderate to high risk, young adult, violent
offenders, the results provide further evidence that the treatment procedures contained within the
ACT component of ART, are effective in treating more serious forms of conduct related problems.
However, several researchers (e.g., Beck, 1996; Longmore & Worrell, 2007; Maruna & Mann, 2006)
have noted that the overall effectiveness of CBT interventions appears to lie in the interaction effects
153
of the affective, cognitive and behavioural treatment components implicit in the CBT approach. This is
consistent with the theoretical underpinnings of ART in which a multimodal approach to altering
entrenched and difficult to change aggressive behaviour is recommended (Goldstein et al., 1998).
support the findings of two previous evaluations of ART. Gunderson and Savardtal (2006) reported
significant reductions in child, parent and teacher ratings of problem behaviours at post-treatment
follow-up for ART participants in a school setting (M age = 13.4 years). There were substantial
differences in the age, treatment setting, and degree of aggressive cognitions and behaviours
between the Gunderson and Savardtal study and the present study. A more valid comparison of ART
outcomes relative to these factors was reported by Hornsveld (2005). He reported the post-treatment
findings for an adaptation of ART (i.e., Aggression Control Therapy) which, in addition to the three
standard components of ART, included a component designed to improve goal setting abilities and
positive self-reinforcement for goal attainment. Participants included 109 older (M age = 32.5 years)
forensic inpatients and 44 younger (M age = 23.4 years) outpatients, all with a history of violent
offending and a diagnosis of APD and/or CD. Similar to the findings of the present study, results
and these gains were maintained at follow-up assessments (i.e., 5-15 weeks after treatment-end).
Consistent with the principle of criminogenic risk (Andrews et al., 1990) Hornsveld indicated that the
intervention was most successful with participants who reported entrenched and higher levels of
anger.
Subsequent to the pilot study the worker-report ABCL (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2003) was
added to allow for a multi-informant research design and thereby increase the external validity of the
study (Kazdin & Weisz, 1998; Posavac & Carey, 2003). The ABCL was administered to custodial
154
workers from pre-treatment (T1) to six month follow-up (T3). Contrary to expectation, there was no
significant change at any time point on this measure. Also contrary to expectation, the group T-score
means for participants in the main study fell within the “normal” range at all assessment points. Given
that the ABCL is designed to assess both externalising and internalising symtomatology these results
were particularly surprising and go against the high prevalence rates reported for comorbid
psychiatric symtoms among offender populations (Abram et al., 2003; Atkins et al., 1999; Teplin et
al., 2002). For example, one large international meta-analysis reported that male and female youth in
juvenile detention were approximately 10 times more likely to suffer a psychiatric illness than their
non-offender peers (Fazel et al., 2008). It is possible that the reliability of the ABCL in this setting was
Achenbach and Rescorla (2003) indicated that the ABCL can be completed by a person who
knows the individual well (e.g., partners, friends, roommates, therapists, parents, etc). Respondents
are required to rate each item based on the preceding six months. Given that participants were
serving a custodial sentence at the time of this study, access to informants from their personal lives
was impractical. Moreover, the present study was interested in the observations of the same
informant from pre-treatment to post-treatment to six month follow-up. Although the custodial
workers had an individual key worker role with detainees for the duration of their custody, there was
considerable variation between workers in the extent to which they engaged with the youth relative to
this role and more specifically their actual knowledge of the youth. Additionally, as noted by several
researchers, the selection of a suitable non-self-report measure was hindered by the lack of
psychometrically evaluated measures that have been assessed for use in correctional settings
(Shelton, Sampl, Kesten, Zhang, & Trestman, 2009; Wakai, Shelton, Trestman, Kesten, 2009). With
hindsight, and similar to the methodology employed by Nugent et al. (1997) a more reliable measure
155
of worker-reported changes in detainees aggressive and antisocial behaviour might have been
sourced through examination of worker-reported incident reports during the custody period or direct
The prediction that ART participants would report fewer cognitive distortions following
treatment was supported. The main and trend effects for the HIT total score, and the cogntive
Worst were all significant and the effect sizes were large (Cohen, 1988). Similarly, the HIT
behavioural referents- Lying, Stealing, Oppositional Defiant, and Physical Aggression showed
The reduction in cognitive distortions is generally consistent with findings reported by Nas,
Brugman and Koops (2005) who evaluated the effectiveness of the EQUIP program (Gibbs, Potter, &
Goldstein, 1995) with 108 males (12-21 years) in the Netherlands. The treatment and control groups
were recruited from high-security juvenile correctional facilities and all particpants had committed
serious violent offences (e.g., murder, violent robbery, rape). The EQUIP program represents an
extension of ART. Nas et al. reported significant reductions in cognitive distortions for the treatment
group but not the control group. At post-treatment the HIT Total score and the subscales- Self-
centred, Minimizing/Mislabeling, Blaming Others, Lying, and Stealing, showed significant reductions.
However, in contrast to the present study, Nas et al. did find significant reductions on Assuming the
Worst, Oppositional Defiant, or Physical Aggression. This inconsistency might be accounted for by
the offenders being higher risk than those in the current study. None of the participants in the present
study had committed rape or murder. Given that Nas et al. did not report longitudinal follow-up, it is
156
not known if the treatment gains were maintained. In the present study, the follow-up data indicated
The present study did not include a direct measure of moral reasoning, instead, the HIT was
used as a measure of primary (i.e., self-centered) and secondary (i.e., blaming others,
minimising/mislabeling, and assuming the worst) cognitive distortions. As previously discussed, these
have been researched extensively with respect to their role in preventing cognitive dissonance and
thereby maintaining antisocial attitudes and behaviour (Barriga et al., 2000; Gibbs, 2003; Liua,
Barriga & Gibbs, 1998). In addition, there is a substantial body of research in which the moral
reasoning of offenders has been shown to be arrested at the early egocentric stages of moral
development such as Stage 1- “might makes right” and the pragmaticism of Stage 2- “you scratch my
back…” (Blasi, 1980; Nelson et al., 1990; Stams et al., 2006). Moreover, Selman’s (1971) stages of
social perspective taking were recognised by Kohlberg (1976) as a necessary precursor to the
development of more mature moral reasoning. Without having assessed moral reasoning
specifically, the present study cannot know the extent to which, if any, participation in ART has
progressed the socio-moral development of participants. However, according to Barriga et al. (2001)
and Gibbs (2003) the cognitive distortions assessed by the HIT represent a corollary of
pervasive egocentric bias. Thus, the reductions in cognitive distortions reported by the youth in this
study suggest evidence of what Gibbs (2003) referred to as movement from immature moral
superficiality toward more “social decentration or social construction” (p.32) or what Kohlberg (1969)
called higher levels of moral reasoning. Goldstein et al. (1998) defined the aims of moral reasoning
training in exactly these terms. Furthermore, there was evidence that the reductions in cognitive
distortions were clinically significant. Participants scores on the HIT at T1 placed them within the
157
subclinical range, and more often in the clinical range, whereas by T3 (6-month follow-up) all scores
had fallen to within the “normal” range of scores (Barriga et al., 2001).
Numerous researchers (e.g., Barriga et al., 2000; Day & Howells, 2008; Goldstein et al.,
1998; Palmer, 2007; Yochelson & Samenow, 1976) have indicated that a priority for offender
treatment programs should be the correction of cognitive distortions and the promotion of social
Palmer (2007) few studies have undertaken component analysis in order to determine the additive
value of moral reasoning training procedures to standard anger management interventions for violent
offenders. The substantial reductions in self-reported aggression, impulsivity and cognitive distortions
for partcipants in the present study provide evidence to support moral reasoning as part of a multi-
component CBT treatment approach. Further support is also evident for the argument that the
perspective taking opportunities provided through the Moral Reasoning component of ART should be
Subsequent to the pilot study, the SSRS (Gresham & Elliot, 1990) was replaced with the
SPSI-R:S (D'Zurilla et al., 2002) to account for the increased age range (i.e., 17-20 years) of
participants (the SSRS is not designed for use with individuals older than 18-years). Use of the SPSI-
R:S was also consistent with the broader theoretical conceptualisation and measurement of social
competence, which has been variously defined as comprising a combination of behavioural, cognitive
and affective skills (Bierman & Welsh, 2000; Cook et al., 2008). McGuire (2001) indicated that on a
continuum of cognitive to behavioural CBT interventions, social skills training is more behaviourally
oriented whereas social problem solving interventions are more closely aligned to cognitive theory
158
and methods of behaviour change (see Figure 2, p.29). However, both social skills and social
problem solving represent interdependent components of the social competence construct, with
improvement in one suggesting improvement in the other (Cunliffe, 1992; McGuire, 2001). Given the
multiple targets of cognitive and behavioural change in ART, the SPSI-R:S was considered to be a
suitable measure of the cognitive change aspects of social competence (i.e., social problem solving).
In addition, it was not possible to source an actuarial measure of social skills suitable for use in a
The results of the main study show partial support for the predicted increase in social
problem solving abilities among participants. There was a significant increase in general social
problem solving abilities (i.e., SPSI-R:S Total scale) at post-treatment (T2). However, none of the
other main effects for the SPSI-R:S subscales showed significant change at the conservative p-value
of .01. D'Zurilla et al. (2002) defined the Total scale as a global indicator of problem solving ability.
The higher scores at post-treatment for participants in this study indicated an increased capacity for
constructive problem solving. The improvement in global social problem solving and the maintenance
of those gains at six and 24-months follow-ups was consistent with Sukhodolsky et al.’s (2005) study
which showed improvements in parent reported problem behaviors, including aggression, and child-
reported anger and social problem solving at post-treatment and three month follow-up assessments.
This was for both treatment groups, which involved a modified version of ACT (Feindler & Ecton,
1986) in which SST components from ART or Social Problem Solving Training (SPST) components
were added.
As previously discussed, in order to reduce the potential for Type I, familywise error (i.e.,
false positive) a conservative p-value of .01 was set for significance testing in the main study.
However, the main effects for the adaptive social problem solving subscales PPO and RPS were
159
close to significance at the less conservative p = .02. In contrast, the dysfunctional problem solving
subscales NPO, ICS, and AS were not even close to reaching significance. These results suggest
that the significant improvement in global social problem solving was largely attributable to increased
adaptive social problem solving abilities, not reduced dysfunctional social problem solving. However,
these results need to be considered in light of the lack of clinical significance on the SPSI-R:S at any
time point, including at intake (T1). The interpretive guidelines provided by D'Zurilla et al. (2002)
indicate that the group means from T1 to T4 on all of the domains assessed by the SPSI-R:S were
within the “norm group average”. Thus, while there was statistically significant improvement in global
social problem solving, the group means for participants in the present study indicate that on average
their social problem solving abilities were comparable to “normal” age-related peers. These results
stand in contrast to previous research indicating that violent offending is associated with clinically
significant social problem solving deficits among juvenile (Cunliffe, 1992; Dodge et al., 1990;
Goldstein, 1999; McMurran, et al., 2005; Slaby & Guerra, 1988) and adult (McMurran, 2005;
McGuire, 2001) offenders. There are several possible theoretical and methodological explanations for
Similar to the findings of the present study, Hornsveld (2005) reported that participants in his
evaluation of ART (adapted for forensic patients) showed slightly above average social competency
scores, based on Dutch norms. Hornsveld noted that this finding was inconsistent with behavioural
observations made by program facilitators and research evidence in which deficits in social
competency among violent offenders has often been reported. An association between aggressive
behaviour and unrealistic positive self-evaluations has been reported by several researchers (e.g.,
Baumeister et al., 1996; Bushman & Baumeister, 2002; Orobio de Castro, Brendgen, Boxtel, Vitaro,
& Schaepers, 2007). Contrary to the popular belief that low self-esteem underlies aggression,
160
Bushman and Baumeister found that violent offenders scored significantly higher on measures of
narcissism than non-offenders, and that there was no significant difference in self-esteem scores
between the two groups. They argued that narcissism, not self-esteem, is related to violent offending
and that such offenders are likely to self-evaluate more positively than evaluations relating to social
to peer and teacher reports, has been reported relative to the social-information processes of
aggressive children (Dodge, 1980; Lochman & Dodge, 1998; Orobio de Castro et al., 2007).
Consistent with previous research that has demonstrated a link between violent offending
and increased narcissism, participants in this study might have rated themselves as more socially
competent than they actually are Overall, the behavioural observations of participants in the present
study indicated support for the contention that violent offenders are likely to evidence deficits in age
competencies, participants frequently bemoaned the repetitive (i.e., over-learning) and simplified pro-
social skill steps employed in ART. These complaints were often voiced within aggressive/antisocial
language and behaviour and initial rehearsal of pro-social skills again served to reinforce the
perception of the facilitators that these youth lacked social competence despite their claims to the
contrary.
Another possible explanation for the lack of clinically significant scores on the SPSI-R:S
might have been that participants in the present study were deliberately “faking good”. As with any
self-report measure, D'Zurilla et al. (2002) discussed the potential for respondents to report falsely
inflated social problem solving abilities in the interests of social desirability. Given there was no
indication of socially desirable response sets with the other measures employed in this study, it
161
seems reasonable to assume that impression management was not systematically employed by
participants when they responded to the SPSI-R:S. However, Nas et al. (2005) reported a significant
positive relationship between social desirability and social skills in their evaluation of ART.
Participants in their study with higher scores on social desirability also tended to self-report better
social skills. Future research would benefit from the inclusion of a specific social desirability measure
as a way of determining the extent to which, if any, participants employ impression management in
their response sets. Also, a non-self-report measure of social problem solving completed by workers
It is also possible that the scores on the SPSI-R:S for participants in the present study
represented an accurate self-appraisal of social problem solving abilities and that on average, in this
small sample, their aggressive behaviour was not directly related to social problem solving deficits.
D'Zurilla and Maydue-Olivares (1995) distinguished between process versus outcome measures of
social problem solving. They indicated that process measures, such as the SPSI-R:S, are designed
contrast, outcome measures assess the actual implementation of those solutions. D'Zurilla and
Maydue-Olivares indicated that the cognitive steps involved in social problem solving processes may
or may not be correlated with solution implementation depending upon situational factors, individual
expectancies, and affective responses to the perceived social problem. This aligns with social
information processing (SIP) models of aggressive behaviour that specify a series of complex mental
steps before an individual reaches the final stage of solution implementation (Dodge, 1994; Dodge,
1986; Dodge et al., 2008). Future evaluations of ART would benefit from the inclusion of an outcome
social problem solving measure in order to assess actual solution implementation. The Interpersonal
Problem-Solving Assessment Technique (IPSAT; Getter & Nowinski, 1981)) which is a semi-
162
structured paper-pencil measure, might be an appropriate measure of this sort, given the sample
characteristics of the present study. The IPSAT assess both social problem solving processes (i.e.,
generation of alternative solutions and decision making) and outcomes (i.e., respondents are
required to indicate which solution they would implement given a series of socially problematic
Impulsivity
The linear reduction for impulsivity from pre-treatment to 24-month follow-up was significant
and the effect was large. The reduction was also clinically significant. At intake (T1) the mean of
impulsiveness for participants in the present study was above that of the normative average for
similar aged males reported by Eysenck et al. (1985). This finding is consistent with the results of
previous research in which impulsivity has been related to the development and maintenance of
aggressive behaviour (Barratt, 1994; Eysenck & Eysenck, 1977; Eysenck & McGurk, 1980; Moffitt,
1993; White et al., 1994) and as a dynamic risk factor (i.e., changeable) among offender populations
(Andrews, 1996). At six month follow-up (T3) the average level of impulsiveness for participants in
this study was comparable to the normative average, with a further, though non-significant, reduction
The initially elevated scores on impulsivity for participants in this study are consistent with
other research showing heightened impulsivity among delinquent youth in general (Carroll et al.,
2006; Luengo et al., 1994) and violent offenders, in particular (Smith & Waterman, 2006). Luengo et
al. compared scores for three (non-delinquent, minor-delinquent, and major-delinquent) groups of
adolescents (12-18 years) on the children’s version of I7 Impulsiveness scale (Eysenck et al., 1985).
Results showed that both delinquent groups were higher on impulsivity than the non- delinquent
163
group. In addition, higher impulsivity was associated with increased risk of future antisocial
behaviour. Carroll et al. reported similar findings in comparing groups of early-onset offenders, late-
onset offenders, and non-offenders among Australian adolescents. Although there were no significant
differences between the offender groups, offenders were significantly higher on impulsivity than non-
offenders. In assessing impulsivity among violent versus non-violent adult offenders, Smith and
offenders, to violent offenders. Moreover, the violent offenders were significantly more impulsive than
the other two groups. Smith and Waterman concluded that heightened impulsivity can reliable
distinguish offenders from non-offenders, and violent offenders from non-violent offenders. This
supported the argument that impulsivity, as a dynamic crimnogenic risk factor, is an appropriate and
From a theoretical perspective, the present results indicate further support for the
effectiveness of the CBT training procedures (e.g., self-instructional training, think-out-loud, problem
solving, consequential thinking) that have been shown to reduce impulsiveness and aggression with
children and adolescents (Baer & Nietzel, 1991; Luria, 1961; Kendall & Finch, 1978; Meichenbaum &
Goodman, 1971). Moreover, the present results extend this earlier research by demonstrating that
these training strategies can be effective in reducing impulsivity and aggression among older violent,
young-adult offenders. According to Eysenck (2004) the I7 impulsiveness scale represents a measure
of dysfunctional impulsiveness associated with cognitive deficits such as lack of planning and
consequential thinking, and antisocial behavior, including aggression. Ramirez and Andreu (2006)
suggested that the lack of consequential thinking among impulsive children and youth is associated
with a tendency to select small, immediate rewards over larger, delayed rewards. Similarly, in
describing the inability of aggressive and antisocial youth to delay gratification from within a self-
164
centered worldview, Carducci (1980) described such youth “as fixated at a level of getting their own
throbbing needs met, regardless of the effects on others” (p.157). Here Carducci alluded to the
possible that the three components of ART can, as Goldstein et al. (1998) suggested, replicate the
early developmental processes that lead to emotional self-regulation and mature socio-moral
reasoning. These replication procedures might serve as the basis for the remediation of aggression-
related cognitive and behavioural deficits, even among older offenders, as in the present study.
As previously noted, despite impulsivity having been identified as a significant risk factor for
non-violent and violent offending (Andrews, 1996; Carroll et al., 2006; Luengo et al., 1994; Waterman
& Smith, 2006) there are few published studies in which it has been investigated as a treatment
outcome among offender populations. There is limited research, therefore, with which to compare the
present findings. The reduction in self-reported impulsivity for the current youth was similar to the
findings reported by Goldstein and Glick (1987). In their pre/post evaluation of ART, they found that
worker-rated (not self-report as in the present study) impulsivity scores showed significant reduction
at post-treatment for ART participants (n=24) compared to a no-treatment (n=12) and a “brief
instruction” (n=24) control groups. These results were replicated with the 36 control participants who
later completed the ART program. The present study extends the findings of Goldstein and Glick by
providing follow-up assessments at six- and 24- month follow-up in which the reductions in impulsivity
were maintained and by demonstrating that ART can reduce impulsivity among an older, young-adult
The present study posed the research question- are ART-treatment gain/s maintained over
time (i.e., from treatment-end [T2] to 24-month follow-up [T4])? Support for therapeutic maintenance
was indicated by the significant linear trend effects showing reductions in self-reported aggressive
behaviour and thoughts, cognitive distortions, impulsivity, and a significant increasing trend for
general social problem solving abilities. Furthermore, from pre-treatment to two-year follow-up all
significant effects were in the predicted direction; at no time was there a marked deterioration on
cognitive or behavioural outcomes. Consistent with these results, Hornsveld (2005) found that ART
and that those gains were maintained at 15-week follow-up. More often, maintenance of treatment
gains among offender groups has been reported in terms of reconviction rates for ART participants
compared to untreated controls. For example, in the largest evaluation of ART to date (N=1,500)
Barnoski (2004) reported a significant reduction in recidivism for juvenile offenders who completed
ART (18.8%) compared to a no-treatment control group (24.8%) at 18-month follow-up. Similarly,
McGuire and Clarke (2004) reported a reconviction rate of 20.4% for ART participants, compared to
34.5% for controls at one year follow-up of adult offenders in the UK. At 10-month follow-up, Hatcher
et al. (2008) reported on overall decrease of 13.3% in reconviction rates for an ART treatment group
compared to the no-treatment control. A few researchers (Goldstein et al., 1998; Goldstein &
Martens, 2000; Hollin, 1999) have noted that the investigation of treatment maintenance, or
longitudinal follow-up, has been relatively lacking in the offender “what works” literature.
Several procedures have been described to enhance the transfer and maintenance of
therapeutic gains (Goldstein et al., 1998; Goldstein & Martens, 2000). With respect to transfer, there
166
are five core techniques that have been incorporated into the training procedures of ART: (1) General
Principles- the trainee is provided with rules, strategies, and organising principles that lead to
successful performance of the skill in real life situations and the ability to adapt these principles to
various settings and players; (2) Overlearning- here the trainee is given repeated opportunities to
practice a given skill set, initially after observing an exemplar, then in his own role-play’s and again
through observation or participation as a co-actor in the role-plays of other group members. Goldstein
et al. stated that “to maximize transfer through overlearning, the guiding rule is not “practice makes
perfect’”(implying that one simply practices until one gets it right and then moves on) but “practice of
perfect makes perfect’ (implying that numerous trials of correct responses are needed after the initial
success) (p.160); (3) Identical Elements- the closer the practice environment resembles the actual
environment the higher the probability the skill will be transferred; (4) Stimulus Variability- transfer is
enhanced by practicing the skill in a variety of situations; and (5) Mediated Reinforcement (or real life
reinforcement) - transfer of a newly acquired skill is enhanced when the skill is repeatedly reinforced
in real life. According to Goldstein et al. such reinforcement is mediated by the trainee, not by others.
This is achieved through teaching the trainee procedures including self-reinforcement, self-
instruction, self-correction, and self-rewarding. Each of these transfer enhancers are built into the
ART curriculum.
Goldstein and his colleagues (1998; 2000) have also emphasised the importance of
engaging significant others (e.g., family members, pro-social peers, and support workers) in the
treatment process. They referred to these agents of change as Transfer Coaches, who have the
primary role of providing support, encouragement and reinforcement outside the treatment setting.
Evidence suggests that the greater the number of external support systems recruited for these
purposes, the greater the chance of transfer and maintenance of pro-social gains (Goldstein &
167
Martens, 2000; McGuire, 2003). Recruitment of significant others in the treatment process has also
been found to support the use of relapse prevention strategies for several problem behaviours,
including substance abuse (Marlett & Gordon, 1985; Peters, 1993) sex offences (Launay, 2001;
Polaschek, 2003; Price, 1999) and violence (Bush, 1995). While Bush argued that the onus of
maintenance of treatment gains is the responsibility of the offender, staff should be trained in roles
similar to that of the transfer coach. In his research, the role of the treatment team, which included
parole and correctional officers, was to ensure that instruction in “self-risk management” strategies
As already noted, a key learning from the pilot study was the importance of ensuring ongoing
communication, in the form of progress reports and program updates, between the custodial workers
and the ART facilitators. This exchange of information was instrumental in facilitating the workers role
as Transfer Coach, which undoubtedly contributed to skill transfer and maintenance. Goldstein et al.
(1998) argued that the frequently reported failure of behaviour change programs to show
maintenance of gains beyond the treatment setting can in part be explained by the ultimate return to
a family, peer group and/or community that is unfamiliar, unable and/or unwilling to engage the youth
in the newly acquired pro-social skills and attitudes. Thus, the well learned aggressive and antisocial
cognitions and behaviours are rapidly reinstated. The present study did not engage participants
significant others in the role of Transfer Coaches post-release. This represents a limitation in the
present study with respect to ensuring relapse prevention strategies were in place prior to release
back into the community. Despite this, there was evidence of treatment maintenance at two year
follow-up for these youth, which stands as testimony to the multimodal training procedures in ART. In
addition, the evidence for treatment maintenance provides further support for the criminogenic risk
168
principle which indicates that higher risk offending behaviour (such as violent offending) requires
Interest in the therapeutic process has only recently started to receive attention in the CBT
literature and much less in the offender treatment literature (Marshall, 2009; Marshall & Burton,
2010). To date, the overarching goal of offender treatment programs has been the prevention or
reduction of recidivism (Marshall & Serran, 2004; Tate et al., 1995). The emphasis on criminogenic
risk and need has resulted in more attention been given to program content and technique rather
than to process variables, including the impact of the therapeutic relationship on treatment outcome
(Holmqvist et al., 2007; Marshall & Burton, 2009; Ross, Polaschek, & Ward, 2007). However, the
importance of the therapeutic alliance in predicting successful therapeutic outcome has been well
established in individual psychotherapy (Horvath & Luborsky, 1993; Horvath & Symonds, 1991;
Raue, Goldfried, & Barkham, 1997). Large meta-analyses have repeatedly shown that the quality of
the therapeutic relationship accounts for up to 30% of the variance in psychotherapy outcome studies
Ross et al. (2007) defined the therapeutic alliance (TA) in terms of “a collaborative
relationship between therapist and client that can facilitate positive change for the client” (p.463).
They further argued that the operationalisation of the TA in the treatment of offender populations
does not differ significantly from other client groups. Bordin (1979) conceptualised the TA as a
working alliance between the therapist and client, comprising three core features that are negotiated
between therapist and client: (1) an agreed set of therapeutic goals; (2) agreement in undertaking
specific tasks aimed at attainment of those goals; and (3) a bond between therapist and client that is
169
function in part, if not entirely, of the strength of the working alliance” (Bordin, 1979, p.253).
According to Andrews and his colleagues (Andrews & Bonta, 2003; Andrews et al., 1990; Bonta &
Andrews, 2003) non-criminogenic needs, including the therapeutic alliance, fit within the principle of
responsivity. This aspect of the “what works” principle speaks largely to the relational processes that
underpin effective intervention with offenders. The importance of relational/process variables in the
delivery of ART became increasingly evident throughout the duration of each program, and had
important implications for improving treatment effectiveness throughout the three year intervention
The concept of cohesiveness is also a core relational component in the effective outcome of
group interventions (Hornsey, Dwyer, & Oei, 2007; Marmarosh, Holz, & Scottenbauer, 2005; Yalom,
1995). According to Yalom (1995) the complexities of the relational aspects of therapy are increased
in the group setting by multiple intra- and inter- personal relationships and roles (i.e., therapist, co-
therapist, and several clients). This issue is further complicated in working with involuntary clients, as
was the case in the present study (Goldstein & Martens, 2000; Trotter, 2006). Yalom distinguished
between the process and content of group therapy in terms of the explicit material presented,
including the task at hand (i.e., content) compared to the meaning/s underlying that material relative
to the interpersonal relationships in the “here and now” of the group (i.e., process). In the early stages
of delivering ART, in particular the first pilot program, there was an emphasis upon program content
over process.
In Bordin’s (1979) conceptualisation of the working alliance, the first two core features- goals
and tasks are for the most part predetermined by the principal aim (i.e., aggression replacement) and
content of ART. However, Goldstein and his colleagues (1998) were clear that the prescriptive
170
programming within ART does not preclude ensuring participants are given an opportunity for
programmatic “buy in”. While a standard 10-week ART curriculum was employed in the present
study, amendments were made to the social skills taught and the moral reasoning dilemmas selected
for discussion within a given group depending upon the needs of the group. Goldstein et al. indicated
that such amendments have proven to be an effective method in increasing trainee motivation.
Additionally, establishing the need for a given skill set represents part of the “buy in” process. For
every new skill (cognitive, behavioural and affective) taught throughout the ART curriculum, the
therapist poses the question “why is it important to be able to… [e.g., identify triggers and body
signs]”. With experience in delivering ART, the co-therapists became increasingly aware that such
open ended questions all too often were met with a glassy-eyed silence from group members. Hence
the “why” question was reframed as “when was the last time being able to… [e.g., think ahead] would
have been important for you to know”. The nuance in the later wording of this question reflected our
assumption that the group members needed this particular cognitive, behavioural and/or affective skill
and also ensured that each participant identified a personally relevant scenario for the role-plays. The
opportunity to negotiate with the co-trainers regarding the content of the program provided in-vivo
practice opportunities for several targets of change within ART. These included pro-social modeling,
social skills training, social problem solving and exercising affective self-control within the group
L. was serving a custodial sentence for a serious assault against another young man whom
L. claimed had bullied him at school (i.e., Blaming Others; MinimsingMislableing). L. had
no use for and was above the program content of ART. By approximately mid-way through
the 10-week curriculum, he attended group and described an altercation he had had with a
171
staff member regarding a request to have a protein drink made available to detainees at the
centre. The staff member had indicated that this would not be possible as it was against
Centre policy. In response L. became verbally abusive. After initial angry complaining by
group members, a discussion led the group, and L, to concede that his aggressive approach
was ineffective and perhaps an attempt at negotiation might prove more fruitful. [The
reserved for the later stages of an ART program, or in some instances is beyond the grasp of
less socially skilled participants]. Hence it was agreed by the group that they would use this
session for the practice of the skill “negotiation”. After L. had practiced his scenario in a role-
play he indicated that he felt confident to attempt using the skill with Centre staff. At the next
session, L reported that staff had been responsive to his non-aggressive approach and that
they would put his request to the Centre manager for consideration. By the following week, L
reported to the group that his request had been approved, with some conditions.
This case example highlights the need for flexibility on the part of the therapist in delivering
interventions in a correctional setting. Andrews et al. (1990) referred to this in the “what works”
principles of Responsivity and Professional Discretion. The value of such flexibility for the client was
further evident in his subsequent behaviour and attitude within ART. While his resistance continued
to be evident at other times in the group, there was a notable shift in his willingness to participate and
Andrews and Kiessling (1980) identified five core characteristics of correctional staff that
were thought to increase therapeutic effectiveness with offenders. They defined the first component
in terms of effective use of authority, characterised by a “firm but fair” approach in interacting with
172
offenders. Secondly, service providers should model and reinforce pro-social values, attitudes and
behaviours. The third aspect involves directly teaching problem solving skills to offenders, such as
consequential thinking and alternative solutions. The fourth characteristics involved demonstrating
effective use of community resources through brokerage, advocacy and referral. The fifth component,
described as the most important, was summarised as relationship factors comprising open, warm,
and enthusiastic communication by service providers. These relational qualities foster mutual respect
which directly and positively influences treatment outcomes (i.e., therapeutic alliance or cohesiveness
in the group setting). As shown in the study by Nugent et al. (1997) the failure of a service provider to
model these prosocial relational qualities can seriously compromise treatment integrity. In this study,
the aggressive behaviour toward clients by a group leader was likely to have contributed to the failure
The few studies in which the relationship between TA and treatment outcomes has been
investigated specifically among young offenders have reported mixed findings. Chassin, Young and
Light (1980) found that practical support, such as task and goal orientated approaches, was indicated
by young offenders as more important to them than insight or interpersonal orientated therapies.
Similarly, Gold and Osgood (1992) found that the most effective treatment outcomes for juvenile
detainees were associated with a staff approach that was authoritative, like Andrews and Kiessling’s
(1980) “firm but fair” approach, rather than an approach that focused upon the emotional problems of
young offenders. Florsheim, Shotorbani, Guest-Warnick, Barratt and Hwang (2000) examined the
relationship between working alliance in the treatment of 121 delinquent boys (M age = 15.6 years)
placed in community-based residential programs. Measures of the working alliance between a key
staff member (as nominated by the individual youth) and each boy were administered along with the
CBCL (Achenbach, 1991) parent, teacher and youth self-report versions. Florsheim et al. reported
173
that a positive working alliance at three months into treatment predicted significant improvement on
the CBCL and recidivist measure at one year follow-up. They also reported a positive relationship
The findings of Florsheim et al. (2000) are consistent with the view that the working alliance
(Bordin, 1979) or group cohesion (Yalom, 1995) is as much of a pragmatic concern in successful
treatment outcome as is the explicit goal/s or task/s therein. This was the certainly the experience of
the present researcher in the delivery of ART. The importance of attending to both process (i.e.,
group cohesion, working alliance) and content (i.e., the explicit manualised tasks) in the service of
treatment integrity was indicated in the ART QA assessment feedback provided by the Washington
State Master Trainer (see Appendix C). Moreover, the ongoing use of the ART self-assessment
session checklists by the co-trainers in this study not only ensured adherence to treatment integrity
but also provided a framework for reflective practice relative to the group processes. These practice
reflections provided the material for much of the discussion in the fortnightly supervision attended by
the ART co-trainers, which in turn ensured role and task clarity and treatment integrity.
As noted in the introduction, violent offenders account for 25% of the current young-adult
(18-25 years) prison population in Australia (ABS, 2009) and recent Australian crime data suggested
that assault-related crime is increasing at a rate of 5% for each year between 1995 and 2007 (AIC,
2009). These crime statistics highlight the need for effective, evidence-based interventions aimed at
among Australian populations (Ogloff, 2002). The results of this study provide preliminary evidence
that the American developed ART program is effective in treating Australian violent young-adult
174
offenders in a custodial setting. This in turn suggests at least some reduction in the burden of
financial, psychological and social costs associated with aggressive and antisocial behaviours
(Werry, 1997).
Perhaps the most important clinical implication of the present study was that the delivery of
ART in an institutional setting, as opposed to a community setting, produced generally positive and
thinking errors, level of impulsivity, and social problem solving abilities. Numerous researchers (e.g.,
Andrews et al., 1990; Izzo and Ross, 1990; Lipsey et al., 2001; Wilson et al., 2005) have reported
that offender treatment programs delivered in a community setting are likely to produce better
outcomes in terms of recidivism compared to those delivered in custodial settings. Although the
present study did not compare ART outcomes with respect to treatment setting (i.e, community
versus custodial) it is clear that ART can be highly effective in a custodial setting despite the limited
opportunity to practice the newly acquired prosocial skills and a ubiquitous antisocial peer influence.
together for extended periods in institutional settings has been well documented (Dodge, Lansford, &
Dishion, 2006; Greenwood, 2006; Lipsey, 2006; Osgood & Briddell, 2006). The results of the present
study have implications with respect to the timing of cognitive-behavioural interventions within these
settings. Most of the youth involved in this study were released shortly after completion of ART and
subsequently returned to their respective communities, families, peers and intimate relationships. The
previously discussed maintenance of treatment gains at two year follow-up, suggests that the youth
did apply at least some of the newly acquired pro-social skills, attitudes and behaviours in their real-
life environment at the two year follow-up (i.e., transfer and maintainence). This provides further
175
support for the cognitive-behavioural content and procedures in ART, even in the case of later
intervention, as in the present study, with violent young-adult offenders in an institutional setting.
adolescence (Goldstein & Glick, 1994; Gundersen & Svartdal, 2006; Barnoski, 2004) or older
offenders (Hatcher et al., 2008; McGuire & Clark, 2004). Given the age range of participants in the
present studies (17- 20.5 years) there is now evidence that ART and its modes of change (i.e., social
learning principles and procedures) are effective for early-adult male offenders. The positive
individual effects of participation in ART and the maintenance of those gains at two year follow-up
suggest potential improvements in at least some of the multiple individual, familial, and social costs of
aggressive and antisocial behavior described by Werry (1997). At two year follow-up one participant
commented that he had on several occasions used the ACT technique of “If… Then… Thinking
Ahead” (i.e., consequential thinking) to avoid escalating interpersonal conflict with his partner. In
terms of reoffending, of the 14 participants who could be contacted at two year follow-up, only two
self-reported that they had reoffended, and only one of those reported an assault-related offence.
Twelve of the 14 participants indicated that they were currently employed at two year follow-up.
As well as clinical implications, the results of this study have important implications relative to
social policy and planning. The three year ART intervention and evaluation period comprising this
study was funded by the Victorian Department of Human Services - Youth Justice Division.. The
funding followed from the recommendation made in the Victorian Juvenile Justice Rehabilitation
Review (Day et al., 2003) that ART, as an evidence-based program for violent offenders, be
implemented across the State’s youth justice system. At the time of writing these concluding
comments, ART is no longer being delivered in the State of Victoria. Hollin and Palmer (2006) noted
the difficulty for researchers in conducting longitudinal outcome studies in that the length of the
176
research can outlast a government’s term in office or sometimes rapidly changing policy directions.
They suggested that these circumstances can lead to “policy-led evidence” as apposed to “evidence-
led policy” (p.267). The two-year evaluation of ART in Washington State juvenile justice (Barnoski,
2004) was the culmination of a 10 year implementation and dissemination process. That process
included a policy and legislative commitment to evidence-based programs, including ART, and the
training of multiple ART facilitators at every juvenile justice county court (i.e., “evidence-led policy”). It
is difficult to determine the extent to which the results of this study can influence social policy and
planning in Australia. However, Hollin and Palmer indicated that research such as this can, and
should play an important role in contributing to evidence-led policy and that there is a responsibility
and need for clinician-researchers to take up that role. Simiarly, Goldstein (1998; 1999; 2004)
strongly advocated the role of the ART trainer as a “scientific practitioner”. A role that he argued
required a commitment to empirical evaluation of the program and dissemination of those findings to
There were a number of limitations to the present study that may limit the generalisability of
the results. The original research design included the recruitement of participants via a staged
process and depending upon length of sentence allocation to either a waitlist control group or an
immediate (i.e., most immediate start date for next ART program) treatment group. However, several
institutional and sentencing factors meant that the number of waitlist participants was too small to
form a meaningful comparison group. The length of stay at the custody centre was the most frequent
impediment. Some explanation of the Victorian juvenile justice system is necessary to elaborate on
this limitation.
177
In the State of Victoria, where this study was conducted, there exists a diversionary approach
to youth justice known as the “dual-track” system. This system allows magistrates to request a “Youth
Training Centre (YTC) suitability” report, completed by a youth justice worker, before sentencing.
Where a young person aged 18-21 years is deemed suitable, the court can order that a custodial
sentence be served in a youth justice centre (separate from offenders aged under 18-years) as an
alternative to imprisonment in an adult correctional facility. This ensures that 50% of all custodial
sentences for youth (aged 18 to 21 years) are served at a YTC rather than adult correctional facility.
The dual-track system is built on the principles of diversion and offender rehabilitation and there is an
expectation that the young person will participate in offence specific rehabilitation programs during
the period of YTC custody. This emphasis on rehabilitation also means that the length of stay in
custody at a YTC is usually approximately half of the actual sentence, the remaining period is served
on a community based order. Hence, a young person who is given a 12-month custodial sentence
will only usually serve six months in a YTC (Hanson, 2009). This meant that a young person who was
allocated to the ART waitlist control group was often due for release before completion of the
program, or he had commenced another program during the wait period that clashed with the
timetabling of ART.
Another limitation of the present study was the lack of recidivist data. This occurred despite
exhaustive efforts on the part of the researcher to gain access to the appropriate police records
database. Informed consent was obtained in the initial plain language and consent form for the
researcher to access the youth justice electronic records. However, given the age of participants in
this study, it became evident that if they reoffended after completion of ART that most likely they
would be dealt with through the adult courts and correctional system and these offences would not be
recorded in the youth justice data. Despite having obtained additional signed consent from the 14
178
participants that could be found for the final data collection at T4, and approval from the university
and police ethics committees, numerous administrative delays from within the department
responsible for releasing the requested data meant that the recidivist data was not obtained in time
for inclusion in this thesis. Similar difficulties of program implementation, administration and research
encountered in this study have been discussed by other researchers (Hollin, 1995, 2001; Tate et al.,
A number of researchers (e.g., Cotter, Burke, Stouthhamer-Loeber, & Loeber, 2005; Hollin &
Palmer, 2006; Posavac & Carey, 2003) have discussed the problem of sample attrition in longitudinal
research designs. At intake all participants agreed to provide a contact address and telephone
number of a significant other who would be most likely to know the whereabouts of the participant at
follow-up assessment points. Despite several attempts and alternate methods (i.e., cross-checking
last known contact details through youth justice data records and online telephone listings) three
participants were non contactable at the six month follow-up, and six different participants were non
contactable at the two year follow-up. It was unclear whether this attrition represented a random or
systematic (e.g., more likely to have reoffended) sample effect. However, given that different
participants were missing at each follow-up, and other than one case at the two-year follow-up, no
participant refused further participation, it seems reasonable to assume that the attrition was not due
to a systematic sample bias. Irrespective, participant losses can bias research outcomes (Posavac &
Carey).
Cotter et al. (2005) noted that there is general agreement among researchers that a capture
rate of 80% is sufficient in longitudinal research with respect to maintaining the validity of a study. At
the two year follow-up, there was a 70% capture rate for the present study. Given the small sample
size, there were limited options for dealing with the missing cases. Thus, as suggested by Streiner
179
(2002) the statistical analyses were conducted for the group mean replacement and “last observation
carried forward” (LOCF) methods. Both methods produced similar results, so the mean replacement
method was used as SPSS provides a function for this method. This method was preferential to the
loss of statistical power associated with not including the cases at T3 and T4 (Streiner, 2002;
The present study would have benefited from a diagnostic interview that provided a clinical
diagnosis relative to the DBD’s and APD and potential comorbidities. The inclusion of such an
assessment in future research could provide an index of problem severity enabling a determination of
ART effectiveness relative to clearly defined DSM-IV-TR (APA, 2000) diagnostic criteria. Such an
approach would be consistent with an assessment of the dynamic risk factors described by Andrews
et al. (1990) that in principle are changeable and have been shown to be predicative of recidivism.
Furthermore, this type of assessment could also provide information relative to the age of onset of a
discussed, early onset CD and CU traits have been shown to be associated with the life-course
persistent (LCP) group of antisocial offenders who are at greater risk of long-term negative
APD (Frick, 2004; Moffitt et al., 2002). To date, there has been no known published evaluation of
ART with respect to a formal assessment of problem severity and clinical presentation.
There are several lines of research enquiry indicated by the strengths and limitations of this
study. Replication with a larger sample, including some form of comparison control group, is needed.
A particular criticism of offender outcome studies has been the over-reliance on recidivism as the
180
sole determinant of outcome or self-report measures to the exclusion of comparative data provided
by other respondents and clinical relevant measures (Hollin & Palmer, 2006; Serin et al., 2009; Tate
et al., 1995). The present study did attempt to address these concerns by including a worker-report
measure of aggressive behaviours and thoughts (i.e., ABCL; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2003) and
multiple self-report measures relevant to ART targets of change and criminongenic needs. However,
given that the questionable reliability of the worker reports, future evaluations of ART in correctional
settings should attempt to compare self- and other- report measures. As previously discussed,
recidivist data, worker-reported incident reports, and semi-structured interviews with case workers
The advantage of assessing TA or group cohesion in future ART evaluations has already
been discussed. It may be, consistent with the findings of psychotherapy outcome studies (see Miller
et al., 1997 for review) that the effectiveness ART is highly dependent upon these relational, process
factors. Certainly, several researchers with interest in the area of offender treatment (Andrews &
Dowden, 2004; Marshall & Serran, 2004; Polaschek, 2006; Trotter, 2006) have noted that further
to non-indigenous, youth in detention across Australia. In 2006, the national average indicated that
indigenous young people were 21 times more likely to be in detention than non-indigenous age-
related peers (Taylor, 2007). The findings of one Australian state report (Harding et al., 1995 as cited
in Howells & Day, 2002) indicted that 45% of non-sexual violent assault charges were recorded
against indigenous adults. This finding is staggering when considered in light of population estimates
showing that indigenous Australians make up only 2.5% of the total population. Participants in the
present study did not include indigenous youth. However, given their over representation among
181
young offenders in Australia, it is important that the application of ART, with respect to the program
content and processes, be evaluated among this group (Howells & Day, 2002). In a qualitative review
of appropriate intervention strategies with violent aboriginal offenders, Mals, Howells, Day and Hall
(2000) noted that while there are areas of overlap with respect to ciminogenic risk and need between
indigenous and non-indigenous violent offenders, there are important differences relative to the
principle of responsivity. In particular, they suggested that differences in literacy, increased incidence
of alcohol fueled and family violence, and cultural norms- particularly with rural compared to urban
aborigines, are likely to require amendments to program implementation and processes. At the time
of writing, a 12-month pilot program of ART has been completed in the Australian State of
Queensland. Here the prevalence of indigenous youth (10-17 years) in detention is higher than non-
indigenous youth. Initial anecdotal reports indicate that the content of ART is applicable, with some
amendments to language and variations in cultural norms portrayed in the moral reasoning problems,
with young indigenous offenders in this State. However, a systematic research protocol is yet to be
undertaken.
The results of this study provided support for the predicted reductions in aggressive
behaviour and thoughts, antisocial and aggressive cognitive distortions and impulsivity. Some
support was also found for improved social problem solving abilities on the youth self-report
measures following ART participation. The changes on these measures were evident at treatment-
end (T2) and were maintained at the six month (T3) and two year (T4) follow-ups. Hollin and Palmer
(2006) noted that assessment of the relationship between statistical and clinical outcomes in offender
research is particularly lacking. Consistent with others (e.g., Kazdin, 1999; Kendall et al., 1999; Wise,
182
2004) they argued that the assessment of clinical significance is necessary to determine treatment
effects with respect to meaningful long-term outcomes. The present study has contributed to the
existing “what works” literature, and more specifically the ART outcome evaluation research, by
including clinically relevant measures of outcome and comparing the group means of participants in
the present study with the normative data available for each measure. These comparisons showed
that at each data collection point, the reductions in aggressive behaviours and thoughts, cognitive
distortions, and impulsivity were clinically significant. At the six month, post-treatment follow-up, at
the latest, all group means on these self-report measures had fallen to within the “normal” range.
The group means on social problem solving for participants in this study did not reach clinical
significance at any time. Similarly, the workers ratings of participants aggressive behaviours and
thoughts at pre-treatement, post-treatment, and six month follow-up remained within the “normal”
range at each data collection point. This finding in combination with the lack of statically significant
change on the ABCL (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2003) was discussed with respect to potentially
unreliable respondents in a correctional setting. One way of addressing this issue might be through
involving custody workers in the delivery of ART. This approach is used in Washinton State where
community and custodial workers (i.e., parole and probation officers and youth workers) are trained in
ART and participate in delivery of the program. Involving workers in the actual delivery of ART is
likely to increase their understanding of the program content and procedures, as well as increasing
programtic “buy in”, which in turn is likely to increase particpants skill transfer and maintenance of
treatment gains.
The results of this study also indicate support for the training procedures (i.e., modeling, role-
plays, “thinking out loud”, perspective taking opportunities, identification of cognitive distortions,
cognitive restructuring) employed in each of the three components of ART. In particular, Palmer
183
(2003) noted that until component analysis of ART was undertaken it was difficult to determine the
relative contribution of each of the modes of behavioural and cognitive change included in the
intervention, particularly what, if anything, the moral reasoning component adds to standard anger
management procedures. The clinically and statistical significant reductions on the measure of
cognitive distortions (i.e., HIT; Barigga et al., 2001) supported the added value of the moral reasoning
training component. This result also supported the argument that effective interventions with
chronically aggressive youth must include a component that addresses the underlying self-serving
cognitive distortions that serve to rationalise and maintain aggressive and antisocial behaviour (Day
& Howells, 2008; Barriga et al., 2000; Gibbs, 2003 Goldstein et al., 1998; Yochelson & Samenow,
1976). The present research has contributed to the clinical/forensic research by providing evidence of
the effectiveness of ART and its training procedures with a group of young –adult, moderate to high
risk offenders. Most of the previous evaluations of ART have investigated outcomes among
adolescent or adult participants. This was the first known study to provide evidence of the programs
While the overall effectiveness of anger management programs has been reasonably well
established, evidence supporting these interventions with violent offenders is relatively lacking (Day,
Howells, Mohr, Schall, & Gerace, 2008) Broadly in Australia, while “evidence-based practice” has
become the catch-cry of government and non-government community service agencies, including
youth justice, there are few published studies in which systematic evaluation of human service
programming has been undertaken (Ainsworth, 2001; Boni, 1999; Brann, Coleman, & Luk, 2001).
With respect to offender rehabilitation research in Australia, Ogloff (2002) noted that much of our
evidence-based practice is based upon programs that have been developed in America and that
“very real questions exist concerning the applicability of international offender rehabilitation research
184
to Australian society” (p.246). The present study has provided initial evidence to support the
effectiveness of the American developed ART program among violent, young-adult offeders in
Australia. Other than minor amendments to the language used in some components of the program
(mainly moral reasoning problems) the present research has demonstrated that ART is culturally
The present results also indicated support for each of the multi-modal components (i.e.,
social skill training, anger control, and moral reasoning) that comprise ART. Palmer (2007) noted the
lack of research evidence pertaining to the effectiveness of moral reasoning training procedures
among offender populations. Through the selection of measures that assessed the specific cognitive,
affective and behavioural targets of change in ART, this study provided evidence that moral
reasoning training can reduce ciminogenic cognitive distortions, which suggested an increased
capacity for moral descion making and social perspective taking. Notably, much of the previous
research evidence for the treatment procedures included in ART has involved child or adolescent
samples. This study has provided evidence of clinical effectiveness with young adults, who present
with well practiced and chronic aggression-related behaviours and cognitions. Moreover, the reported
treatment gains were maintained at the two-year follow-up. These findings have added to the
international “what works” literature by providing further evidence in support of social learning theory
(Bandura, 1977) and its operationalisation in CBT interventions for moderate to high risk, young-
References
Abram, K. M., Teplin, L. A., McClelland, G. M., & Dulcan, M. K. (2003). Comorbid psychiatric
Abrantes, A. M., Hoffmann, N. G., & Anton, R. (2005). Prevalence of co-occurring disorders among
Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/ 4-18 and 1991 profile. Burlington,
Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2003). Manual for the ASEBA adult forms and profiles.
Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth & Families.
Ainsworth, F. (2001). After ideology: the effectiveness of residential programs for ‘at-risk’
Akers, R. L. (1999). Criminological theories: introduction and evaluation (2nd ed.). Chicago, IL:
Amendola, A. M., & Oliver, R. W. (2003). LSCI and aggression replacement training: a mutli-modal
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). The diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders-
Andrews, D. A., & Bonta, J. (1995). The level of service inventory- revised (LSI-R). Toronto, ON:
Multi-health Systems.
Andrews, D. A., & Bonta, A. (2003). The Psychology of Criminal Conduct (3rd ed.). Cincinnati, OH:
Anderson Publishing.
186
Andrews, D. A., & Dowden, C. (2007). The risk-need-responsivity model of assessment and human
Andrews, D. A., & Dowden, C. C. (2006). Risk principle of case classification in correctional
Andrews, D. A., & Kiessling, J. J. (1980). Program structure and effective correctional practices: a
summary of the CaVIC research. In R. R. Ross & P. Gendreau (Eds.), Effective correctional
Andrews, D. A., Bonta, J., & Hoge, R. D. (1990). Classification for effective rehabilitation:
Andrews, D. A., Zinger, I., Hoge, R. D., Bonta, A., Gendreau, P., & Cullen, F. T. (1990). Does
Ang, R. P. (2003). Social problem-solving skills training: does it really work? Child Care in Practice, 9,
5- 13.
Argyle, M., & Kendon, A. (1967). The experimental analysis of social performance In L. Berkowitz
(Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 55-98). New York: Academic
Press.
Atkins, D. L., Pumariega, A. J., Rogers, K., Montgomery, L., Nybro, C., Jeffers, G., et al. (1999).
Mental health and incarcerated youth I: prevalence and nature of psychopathology. Journal
Australian Bureau of Statistics [ABS]. (2006). Population distribution, aboriginal and Torres Strait
Australian Bureau of Statistics [ABS]. (2008). Prisoners in Australia. Canberra: Australia: Author.
Australian Institute of Criminology [AIC]. (2009). Australian crime: facts & figures 2008. Canberra,
Australia: Author.
Baer, R. A., & Nietzel, M. T. (1991). Cognitive and behavioral treatment of impulsivity in children: a
meta-analytic review of the outcome literature. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 20, 400-
412.
Bandura, A. (1973). Aggression: a social learning analysis. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: a social cognitive theory. Englewood
Bandura, A. (2007). Albert Bandura. In G. Lindzey & W. M. Runyan (Eds.), A history of psychology in
Bandura, A., Ross, D., & Ross, S. A. (1961). Transmission of aggression through imitation of
Barkley, R. A., Fischer, M., Edelbrock, C. S., & Smallish, L. (1990). The adolescent outcome of
hyperactive children diagnosed by research criteria I: an eight year follow-up study. Journal
juvenile offenders. Washington State: Washington State Institute for Public Policy.
188
Violence and mental disorder: developments in risk assessment (pp. 61–79). Chicago, IL:
Barriga, A. Q., & Gibbs, J. C. (1996). Measuring cognitive distortion in antisocial youth: development
and preliminary validation of the "how I think" questionnaire. Aggressive Behavior, 22, 333-
343.
Barriga, A. Q., Gibbs, J. C., Potter, G. B., & Liau, A. K. (2001). How i think (HIT) questionnaire
Barriga, A. Q., Hawkins, M. A., & Camelia, C. R. T. (2008). Specificity of cognitive distortions to
Barriga, A. Q., Landau, J. R., Stinson, B. L., Liau, A. K., & Gibbs, J. C. (2000). Cognitive distortions
and problem behaviors in adolescents. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 27, 35-56.
Bates, G. W., & Levery, B. J. (2003). Social problem solving and vulnerability to depression in a
Beck, A.T. (1996). Beyond belief: a theory of modes, personality, and psychopathology. In P.M.
Salkovskis (Ed.), Frontiers of Cognitive Therapy (pp. 1-25). New York: Guilford Press.
Beidel, D. C., Turner, S. M., Young, B., & Paulson, A. (2005). Social effectiveness therapy for
children: three-year follow-up. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 721-725.
Bellini, S., & Peters, J. K. (2008). Social skills training for youth with autism spectrum disorders. Child
Berkowitz, L. (1990). On the formation and regulation of anger and aggression: a cogntive-
Berkowitz, M. W., & Gibbs, J. C. (1985). The process of moral conflict resolution and moral
development. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 29, 71-84.
Bickel, R., & Campbell, A. (2002). Mental health of adolescents in custody: the use of the ˜adolescent
Biederman, J., Newcorn, J., & Sprich, S. (1991). Comorbidity of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
with conduct, depressive, anxiety, and other disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 148,
564-577.
Biederman, J., Petty, C. R., Dolan, C., Hughes, S., Mick, E., Monuteaux, M. C., et al. (2008). The
long-term longitudinal course of oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder in ADHD
Bierman, K. L., & Welsh, J. A. (2000). Assessing social dysfunction: the contributions of laboratory
Bird, H. R., Gould, M. S., & Staghezza, B. (1992). Aggregating data from multiple informants in child
Bird, H. R., Gould, M. S., & Staghezza-Jaramillo, B. M. (1994). The comorbidity of ADHD in a
community sample of children aged 6 through 16 years. Journal of Child and Family Studies,
3, 365-378.
190
Blasi, A. (1980). Bridging moral cognition and moral action: a critical review of the literature.
Boni, N. (1999). Youth and serious crime: directions for Australasian researchers into the new
millennium, Children and Crime: Victims and Offenders. Melbourne, Aust: Australian Institute
of Criminology.
Bonta, J., & Andrews, D. A. (2003). A commentary on Ward and Stewart's model of human needs.
Bor, W. (2004). Prevention and treatment of childhood and adolescent aggression and antisocial
behaviour: a selective review. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 38, 373-380.
Bor, W., Najman, J.M., O’Callaghan, M., Williams, G.M., & Anstey, K. (2001). Aggression and the
Bordin, E. S. (1979). The generalizability of the psychoanalytic concept of the working alliance.
Brad, J. B., Roy, F. B., Sander, T., Ehri, R., Sander, B., & Stephen, G. W. (2009). Looking again, and
harder for a link between low self-esteem and aggression. Journal of Personality, 77, 427-
446.
Brann, P., Coleman, G., & Luk, E. (2001). Routine outcome measurement in child and adolescent
mental health service: an evaluation of HoNOSCA. Australian and New Zealand Journal of
Brinthaupt, T. M., Hein, M. B., & Kramer, T. E. (2009). The self-talk scale: development, factor
Brodbeck, C., & Michelson, L. (1987). Problem-solving skills and attributional styles of agoraphobics.
Broidy, L. M., Nagin, D. S., Tremblay, R. E., Bates, J. E., Brame, B., Dodge, K. A., et al. (2003).
Bullis, M., Walker, H. M., & Sprague, J. R. (2001). A promise unfulfilled: social skills training with at-
Bush, J. (1995). Teaching self-risk management to violent offenders. In J. McGuire (Ed.), What
works: reducing re-offending: guidelines from research and practice (pp. 139-154).
Buss, A. H., & Warren, W. L. (2000). Aggression questionnaire: manual. Los Angeles, CA: Western
Psychological Services.
Caldwell, M. F., & Van Rybroek, G. J. (2005). Reducing violence in serious juvenile offenders using
Carducci, D. J. (1980). Postive peer culture and assertiveness training: complementary modalities for
dealing with disturbed and disturbing adolescents in the class room. Behavioral Disorders, 5,
156-162.
Carroll, A., Hemingway, F., Bower, J., Ashman, A., Houghton, S., & Durkin, K. (2006). Impulsivity in
Catalano, R. F., Loeber, R., & McKinney, K. C. (1999). School and community interventions to
prevent serious and violent offending. . Washington D.C.: Office of Juvenile Justice and
Delinquent Prevention.
192
Chambers, J. C., Eccleston, L., Day, A., Ward, T., & Howells, K. (2008). Treatment readiness in
Chassin, L., Young, R. D., & Light, R. (1980). Evaluations of treatment techniques by delinquent and
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ:
Cohen, J. (1994). The earth is round (p< .05). American Psychologist, 49, 997-1003.
Colby, A., Kohlberg, L., Gibbs, J. C., & Lieberman, M. (1983). A longitudinal study of moral judgment.
Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. (1999a). Initial impact of the fast track prevention
trial for conduct problems 1: the high-risk sample. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. (1999b). Initial impact of the fast track prevention
trial for conduct problems II: classroom effects. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Cook, C. R., Gresham, F. M., Kern, L., Barreras, R. n. B., Thornton, S., & Crews, S. D. (2008). Social
skills training for secondary students with emotional and/or behavioral disorders: a review
and analysis of the meta-analytic literature. Journal of Emotional & Behavioral Disorders, 16,
131-144.
Corder, G. W., & Foreman, D. I. (2009). Nonparametric statistics for non-statisticians: a step-by-step
Cotter, R. B., Burke, J. D., Loeber, R., & Mutchka, J. (2005). Predictors of contact difficulty and
refusal in a longitudinal study. Criminal Behavior and Mental Health, 15, 126-137.
Cotter, R. B., Burke, J. D., Stouthamer-Loeber, M., & Loeber, R. (2005). Contacting participants for
Cowling, V., Costin, J., Davidson-tuck, R., Esler, J., Chapman, A., & Niessen, J. (2005). Responding
to disruptive behaviour in schools: collaboration and capacity building for early intervention
Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1996). A review and reformulation of social information-processing
Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1996). Social information-processing mechanisms in reactive and
Cunliffe, T. (1992). Arresting youth crime: a review of social skills training with young offenders.
Currie, M. R., Wood, C. E., Williams, B., & Bates, G. W. (2009). Aggression replacement training in
Australia: youth justice pilot study. Psychiatry, Psychology & Law, 16, 413-426.
Dandreaux, D. M., & Frick, P. J. (2009). Developmental pathways to conduct problems: a further test
of the childhood and adolescent-onset distinction. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37,
375-385.
Day, A., & Howells, K. (2002). Psychological treatments for rehabilitating offenders: evidence-based
Day, A., Howells, K., & Rickwood, D. (2003). The Victorian juvenile justice rehabilitation review.
Day, A., Howells, K., Mohr, P., Schall, E., & Gerace, A. (2008). The development of CBT
de Castro, B., Brendgen, M., Van Boxtel, H., Vitaro, F., & Schaepers, L. (2007). “Accept me, or
Aggression: Psychiatric Assessment and Treatment (pp. 293-312). New York: Marcel Dekker
Inc.
DeRubeis, R.J., Tang, T.Z., & Beck, A.T. (2001). Cognitive therapy. In K.S. Dobson (Ed.), Handbook
DeVellis, R. F. (2003). Scale development: theory and applications (2nd ed.). Newbury Park, CA:
Sage.
Dickman, S. J. (1990). Functional and dysfunctional impulsivity: personality and cognitive correlates.
Dodge, K. A. (1980). Social cognition and children's aggressive behavior. Child Development, 51,
162-170.
Perlmutter (Ed.), Minnesota Symposium on Child Psychology (Vol. 18, pp. 77-125). Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Dodge, K. A. (1991). Reactive and proactive aggression. In D. J. Pepler & K. H. Rubin (Eds.), The
development and treatment of childhood aggression (pp. 201-218). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
195
Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S. (1990). Mechanisms in the cycle of violence. Science, 250,
1678-1683.
Dodge, K. A., & Coie, J. D. (1987). Social-information-processing factors in reactive and proactive
aggression in children's peer groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53,
1146-1158.
Dodge, K. A., Greenberg, M. T., & Malone, P. S. (2008). Testing an idealized dynamic cascade
model of the development of serious violence in adolescence. Child Development, 79, 1907-
1927.
Dodge, K.A., Laird, R., Lochman, J.E., & Zelli, A. (2002). Multidimensional latent-construct analysis of
Dodge, K. A., Lansford, J. E., & Dishion, T. J. (2006). The problem of deviant peer influences in
influences in programs for youth: problems and solutions (pp. 3-13). New York, NY US:
Guilford Press.
Dodge, K. A., & Pettit, G. S. (2003). A biopsychosocial model of the development of chronic conduct
Dodge, K.A., Pettit, G.S., Bates, J.E., & Valente, E. (1995). Social information-processing patterns
partially mediate the effect of early physical abuse or later conduct problems. Journal of
Dowden, C., Antonowicz, D., & Andrews, D. A. (2003). The effectiveness of relapse prevention with
D'Zurilla, T. J., & Goldfried, M. R. (1971). Problem-solving and behavior modification. Journal of
D'Zurilla, T. J., & Maydeu-Olivares, A. (1995). Conceptual and methodological issues in social
D'Zurilla, T. J., Nezu, A. M., & Maydeu-Olivares, A. (2002). Social problem solving inventory- revised
Eme, R. F. (2008). Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and the juvenile justice system. Journal of
Enticott, P. G., & Ogloff, J. R. P. (2006). Elucidation of impulsivity. Australian Psychologist, 41, 3-14.
Eysenck, S. B. G. (2004). How the impulsiveness and venturesomeness factors evolved after the
essays in honour of Marvin Zuckerman (pp. 107-112). Oxford, UK: Elsevier Inc.
Eysenck, S. B. G., & Eysenck, H. J. (1977). The place of impulsiveness in a dimensional system of
personality description. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 16, 57-68.
Eysenck, S. B. G., Easting, G., & Pearson, P. R. (1984). Age norms for impulsiveness, venturism,
Eysenck, S. B. G., Pearson, P. R., Easting, G., & Allsopp, J. F. (1985). Age norms for impulsiveness,
Fazel, S., Doll, H., & Langstrom, N. (2008). Mental disorders among adolescents in juvenile detention
Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 47, 1010-1019.
Feindler, E. L. (2003). Anger control training. In Research Press (Ed.). Aggression replacment
training (ART): a comprehensive intervention for aggressive youth [video] Champaign, Ill:
Research Press.
Feindler, E. L., & Baker, K. (2004). Current issues in anger management interventions with youth. In
Replacement Training: practice, research, and application (pp. 31-50). Chichester, UK: John
Feindler, E. L., & Ecton, R. B. (1986). Adolescent anger control: cognitive-behavioural techniques.
Feindler, E. L., & Guttman, J. (1994). Cognitive-behavioural anger control training In C. W. LeCroy
(Ed.), Handbook of child and adolescent treatment manuals (pp. 170-199). New York:
Lexington Books.
Feindler, E. L., & Starr, K. E. (2003). From steaming mad to staying cool: a constructive approach to
Felthous, A. R., & Barratt, E. S. (2003). Impulsive aggression In E. F. Coccaro (Ed.), Aggression:
Psychiatric assessment and treatment (pp. 123-148). New York: Marcel Dekker.
program for boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. School Psychology Quarterly,
21, 197-224.
198
Fergusson, D. M., & Horwood, L. J. (2002). Male and female offending trajectories. Development and
Fleck, D., Thompson, C. L., & Narroway, L. (2001). Implementation of the problem solving skills
training programme in a medium secure unit. Criminal Behaviour & Mental Health, 11, 262-
273.
Florsheim, P., Shotorbani, S., Guest-Warnick, G., Barratt, T., & Hwang, W.C. (2000). Role of the
Fonagy, P., & Kurtz, Z. (2002). Disturbances of conduct. In P. Fonagy, M. Target, D. Cottrell, J.
Phillips, & Z. Kurtz. What works for whom? a critical review of treatments for children and
Frick, P. J. (2004). Developmental pathways to conduct disorder: Implications for serving youth who
show severe aggressive and antisocial behaviour. Psychology in the Schools, 41, 823-834.
Frick, P. J., Cornell, A. H., Barry, C. T., Bodin, S. D., & Dane, H. E. (2003). Callous-unemotional traits
and conduct problems in the prediction of conduct problem severity, aggression, and self-
Frick, P., & Hare, R. (2001). Antisocial process screening device (APSD): technical manual. Toronto,
Frick, P. J., & Loney, B. R. (1999). Outcomes of children and adolescents with conduct disorder and
Frick, P., & Morris, A. S. (2004). Temperament and developmental pathways to conduct problems
Frick, P. J., Stickle, T. R., Dandreaux, D. M., Farrell, J. M., & Kimonis, E. R. (2005). Callous–
unemotional traits in predicting the severity and stability of conduct problems and
Gannon, T. A., & Wood, J. L. (2010). Introduction: Special issue on group processes and aggression.
Gelhorn, H. L., Sakai, J. T., Price, R. K., & Crowley, T. J. (2007). DSM-IV conduct disorder criteria as
Gendreau, P. (1996). The principles of effective interventions with offenders. In A. Harland (Ed.),
Choosing correctional options that work (pp.117-130). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Getter, H., & Nowinski, J. K. (1981). A free response test of interpersonal effectiveness Journal of
Gibbs, J. C. (2004). Moral reasoning training: the values component. In A. P. Goldstein, R. Nensén,
practice, research and application (pp. 51-72). Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
Gibbs, J.C. (2003). Moral development and reality: beyond the theories of Kohlberg and Hoffman.
Gilbert, F., & Daffern, M. (in press). Integrating contemporary aggression theory with violent offender
treatment: How thoroughly do interventions target violent behavior? Aggression and Violent
Gold, M., & Osgood, D. W. (1992). Personality and peer influence in juvenile corrections.
Goldman, L. S., Genel, M., Bezman, R. J., Slanetz, P. J., Karlan, M. S., Davis, R. M., et al. (1998).
Goldstein, A. P. (1973). Structured learning therapy: toward a psychotherapy for the poor. New York:
Academic Press.
Goldstein, A. P. (1999). The prepare curriculum: teaching prosocial competencies (revised edn.).
Goldstein, A. P., Sprafkin, R. P., & Gershaw, N. J. (1979). Structured learning therapy: training for
Goldstein, A.P (2004). Evaluations of effectiveness. In A.P. Goldstein, R. Nensén, B. Daleflod, & M.
Kalt, (Eds). New perspectives on aggression replacement training: practice, research and
Goldstein, A. P., & Glick, B. (1987). Aggression replacement training: a comprehensive intervention
Goldstein, A.P., & Glick, B. (1994). The prosocial gang: implementing aggression replacement
Goldstein, A.P., Glick, B., & Gibbs, J.C. (1998). Aggression replacement training: a comprehensive
Intervention for aggressive youth (Revised Edition). Illinois, USA: Research Press.
201
Goldstein, S., & Rider, R. (2005). Resilience and the disruptive disorders of childhood. In R. Brooks &
S. Goldstein (Eds.), Handbook of resilience in childhood (pp. 203-222). New York: Kluwer
Academic/Plenum Publishers.
Lansford (Eds.), Deviant peer influences in programs for youth: problems and solutions. (pp.
Gresham, F. M. (1997). Social competence and students with behavior disorders: where we've been,
where we are, and where we should go. Education and Treatment of Children, 20, 233-249.
Gresham, F. M. (2002). Best practices in social skills training. In A. Thomas & J. Grimes (Eds.), Best
School Psychologists
Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (1990). Social skills rating system manual. Circle Pines, MN:
Gresham, F. M., Cook, C. R., Crews, S. D., & Kern, L. (2004). Social skills training for children and
youth with emotional and behavioral disorders: validity considerations and future directions.
Guerra, N. G., & Slaby, R. G. (1990). Cognitive mediators of aggression in adolescent offenders: II.
Gundersen, K., & Svartdal, F. (2006). Aggression replacement training in Norway: outcome
Hains, A.A. (1984). Variables in social cognitive development: moral judgment, role-taking, cognitive
Adolescence, 4, 65-74.
Hanson, S. (2009). Youth Justice in Victoria: the benefits of Victoria’s youth justice system and the
challenges ahead. Melbourne, Aust.: YMCA World Alliance Youth Justice Campaign.
Hare, R. D., Hart, S. D., & Harpur, T. J. (1991). Psychopathy and the DSM—IV criteria for antisocial
Harris, K. R. (1990). Developing self-regulated learners: the role of private speech and self-
Hatcher, R. M., Palmer, E. J., McGuire, J., Hounsome, J. C., Bilby, C. A. L., & Hollin, C. R. (2008).
Aggression replacement training with adult male offenders within community settings: a
reconviction analysis. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology, 19, 517 - 532.
Hayes, C. (2001). The Washington State ART Qaulity Assurance form. Washington State Institute for
Hayes, S.C. (2004). Acceptance and commitment therapy and the new behaviour therapies. In S.C.
Hayes, V.M. Follette, & M.M. Linehan (Eds.), Mindfulness and acceptance: expanding the
Hayes, S.C., Strosahl, K.D., Bunting, K., Twohig, M., & Wilson, K.G. (2004). What is acceptance and
commitment therapy? In S.C. Hayes and K.D. Strosahl (Ed’s.). A practical guide to
Hemphill, S. A., Smith, R., Toumbourou, J. W., Herrenkohl, T. I., Catalano, R. F., McMorris, B. J., et
al. (2009). Modifiable determinants of youth violence in Australia and the United States: a
longitudinal study. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Criminology, 42, 289-309.
Hoge, R. D., & Andrews, D. A. (1996). Assessing the youthful offender: issues and techniques. New
Hollin, C. R. (1999). Treatment programs for offenders: meta-analysis, "what works," and beyond.
Hollin, C. R. (2001a). Editorial: Social problem solving and offenders. Criminal Behaviour and Mental
Hollin, C. R. (2001b). The role of the consultant in developing effective practice. In G. A. Bernfeld, D.
Hollin, C.R. (2003). Aggression replacement training: putting theory and research to work. Reclaiming
Replacement Training: practice, research and application (pp. 51-72). Chichester, UK: John
Hollin, C. R. (2010). Commentary: directions for group process work. Aggression and Violent
Hollin, C. R., & Bloxsom, C. A. J. (2007). Treatments for angry aggression In T. A. Gannon, T. Ward,
A. R. Beech & D. Fisher (Eds.), Aggressive offender’s cognition: theory, research, and
practice (pp. 215-229). Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
204
Hollin, C. R., Browne, D., & Palmer, E. J. (2004). Delinquency and adolescent offenders.
Hollin, C. R., & Palmer, E. J. (2006). Offending behaviour programmes: controversies and resolutions
application, and controversies (pp. 247-278). Chichester, England: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
Holmqvist, R., Hill, T., & Lang, A. (2007). Treatment alliance in residential treatment of criminal
Holmqvist, R., Hill, T., & Lang, A. (2009). Effects of aggression replacement training in young
53, 74-92.
Hornsveld, R. H. J. (2004). Aggression control therapy for forensic psychiatric patients: Development
and preliminary results In A. P. Goldstein, R. Nensén, B. Daleflod & M. Kalt (Eds.), New
Hornsveld, R. H. J. (2005). Evaluation of aggression control therapy for violent forensic psychiatric
Hornsveld, R. H. J., Nijman, H. L. I., Hollin, C. R., & Kraaimaat, F. W. (2008). Aggression control
therapy for violent forensic psychiatric patients. International Journal of Offender Therapy
Horvath, A. O., & Luborsky, L. (1993). The role of the therapeutic alliance in psychotherapy. Journal
Horvath, A. O., & Symonds, B. D. (1991). Relation between working alliance and outcome in
Howells, K., & Day, A. (2002). Grasping the nettle: treating and rehabilitating the violent offender.
Howells, K., Day, A., Bubner, S., Jauncey, S., Parker, A., Williamson, P. & Heseltine, K. (2001). An
evaluation of anger management programs with violent offenders in two Australian states.
Hubble, M. A., Duncan, B. L., & Miller, S. D. (Eds.). (1999). The heart & soul of change: what works
International consensus statement on ADHD. (2002). Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review,
5, 89-111.
Izzo, R. L., & Ross, R. R. (1990). Meta-analysis of rehabilitation programs for juvenile delinquents: a
Jaffee, S., & Hyde, J. S. (2000). Gender Differences in Moral Orientation: a Meta-Analysis.
Kassinove, H., & Tafrate, R. C. (2006). Anger-related disorders: basic issues, models, and diagnostic
comparative treatments (pp. 1-28). New York: Springer Publishing Company Inc.
Kazdin, A.E. (1987). Treatment of antisocial behaviour in children: Current status and future
Kazdin, A.E. (1997). Practitioner review: psychosocial treatments for conduct disorder in children.
Kazdin, A.E. (1999). The meanings and measurement of clinical significance. Journal of Consulting
Kazdin, A. E. (2002). Psychosocial treatments for conduct disorder in children and adolescents. In P.
E. Nathan & J. M. Gorman (Eds.), A guide to treatments that work (2nd ed., pp. 57-86). New
Kazdin, A. E., & Weisz, J. R. (1998). Identifying and developing empirically supported child and
review of the findings of a seven-year follow-up from childhood to late adolescence. Criminal
and changes in aggressive behavior: a 7-year follow-up from childhood to late adolescence.
Kendall, P. C., & Finch, A. J. (1978). A cognitive-behavioral treatment for impulsivity: a group
Kendall, P.C., Marrs-Garcia, A., Nath, S.R., & Sheldrick, R.C. (1999). Normative comparisons for the
evaluation of clinical significance. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67, 285-
299.
D. A. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and research (pp. 347-480). Chicago:
Rand McNally.
T.Lickona (Ed.), Moral development and behavior (pp. 31-53). New York: Holt, Rinehart &
Winston.
207
Kohlberg, L., & Krammer, R. (1969). Continuities and discontinuities in childhood and adult moral
Krebs, D. L., & Denton, K. (2005). Toward a more pragmatic approach to morality: a critical
Krug, E. G., Dahlberg, L. L., Mercy, J. A., Zwi, A. B., & Lozano, R. (2002). World report on violence
Landenberger, N.A., & Lipsey, M.W. (2005). The positive effects of cognitive-behavioural programs
Lansford, J. E., Malone, P. S., Dodge, K. A., Crozier, J. C., Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E. (2006). A 12-
Launay, G. (2001). Relapse prevention with sex offenders: practice, theory and research. Criminal
Lee, S. I., Schachar, R. J., Chen, S. X., Ornstein, T. J., Charach, A., Barr, C., et al. (2008). Predictive
validity of DSM-IV and ICD-10 criteria for ADHD and hyperkinetic disorder. Child Psychology
Liau, A. K., Barriga, A. Q., & J.C, G. (1998). Relations between self-serving cognitive distortions and
overt vs. covert antisocial behavior in adolescents. Aggressive Behavior, 24, 335-346.
Lipsey, M. W. (2006). The effects of community-based group treatment for delinquency: a meta-
(Eds.), Deviant peer influences in programs for youth: Problems and solutions. (pp. 162-184).
Lipsey, M. W. (2009). The primary factors that characterize effective interventions with juvenile
Lipsey, M.W., Chapman, G.L., & Landenberger, N.A. (2001). Cognitive-behavioural programs for
offenders. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 578, 144-157.
Lipsey, M.W., & Wilson, D.B. (1998). Effective intervention for serious juvenile offenders: A synthesis
of research. In R. Loeber and D.P. Farrington (Eds). Serious and Violent Juvenile Offenders:
Risk factors and successful interventions (pp.313-344).. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Lochman, J.E., Whidby, J.M., & FitzGerald, D.P. (2000). Cognitive-behavioral assessment and
treatment with aggressive youth. In P.C. Kendall (Ed.). Child & adolescent therapy:
Loeber, R. (1991). Diagnostic conundrum of oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder.
Loeber, R. (1991). More enduring than changeable? Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Loeber, R., Burke, J. D., Lahey, B. B., Winters, A., & Zera, M. (2000). Oppositional defiant and
conduct disorder: a review of the past 10 years, Part I. Journal of the American Academy of
Loeber, R., Burke, J., & Pardini, D. A. (2009). Perspectives on oppositional defiant disorder, conduct
disorder, and psychopathic features. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied
Longmore, R.J., & Worrell, M. (2007). Do we need to challenge thoughts in cognitive behaviour
Losel, F. (2001). Evaluating the effectiveness of correctional programs: bridging the gap between
Luengo, M. A., Carrillo-de-la-Peña, M. T., Otero, J. M., & Romero, E. (1994). A short-term
longitudinal study of impulsivity and antisocial behavior. Journal of Personality and Social
Luria, A. R. (1961). The role of speech in the regulation of normal and abnormal behaviour. New
York: Liveright.
Maag, J. W. (2006). Social skills training for students with emotional and behavioral disorders: a
Mals, P., Howells, K., Day, A., & Hall, G. (2000). Adapting violence rehabilitation programs for the
Marsh, D. T., Serafica, F. C., & Barenboim, C. (1980). Effect of perspective-taking training on
Marshall, W. L., & Burton, D. L. (2010). The importance of group processes in offender treatment.
Marshall, W. L., & Serran, G. A. (2004). The role of the therapist in offender treatment. Psychology,
Martinson, R. (1974). What works? questions about prison reform. The Public Interest, 35, 22-54.
Maruna, S., & Mann, R.E., (2006). A fundamental attribution error? Rethinking cognitive distortions.
Maughan, B., Rowe, R., Messer, J., Goodman, R., & Meltzer, H. (2004). Conduct disorder and
McGuire, J. (2001). What is problem solving? a review of theory, research and applications. Criminal
McGuire, J., & Clark, D. (2004). A national dissemination program. In A.P. Goldstein, R. Nensén, B.
practice, research and application (pp.139-150). Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
McMurran, M. (2005). Social problem solving and offenders: reflections and directions. In M.
McMurran & J. McGuire (Eds.), Social problem solving and offending: evidence, evaluation
and evolution (pp. 297-308). Chichester: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
McMurran, M., Egan, V., & Duggan, C. (2005). Stop & think! social problem-solving therapy with
solving and offending: evidence, evaluation and evolution (pp. 207-220). Chichester: John
Plenum.
Meichenbaum, D. H., & Goodman, J. (1971). Training impulsive children to talk to themselves: a
Merrel, K. (2002). School social behavior scales (2nd ed.). Eugene, OR: Assessment Intervention
Services.
211
Miller, S. D., Duncan, B. L., & Hubble, M. A. (1997). Escape from Babel: toward a unifying language
Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Jaffee, S. R., Kim-Cohen, J., Koenen, K. C., Odgers, C. L., et al. (2008).
Research review: DSM-V conduct disorder: research needs for an evidence base. Journal of
Moffitt, T. E., & Caspi, A. (2001). Childhood predictors differentiate life-course persistent and
Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Harrington, H., & Milne, B. J. (2002). Males on the life-course-persistent and
Moynahan, L. (2003). Enhanced aggression replacement training with children and youth with autism
Muñoz, L., Frick, P., Kimonis, E., & Aucoin, K. (2008). Types of aggression, responsiveness to
Nas, C. N., Brugman, D., & Koops, W. (2005). Effects of the EQUIP programme on the moral
judgement, cognitive distortions, and social skills of juvenile delinquents. Psychology, Crime
Nelson, W. J., & Birkimer, J. C. (1978). Role of self-instruction and self-reinforcement in the
Nelson, J. R., Smith, D. J., & Dodd, J. (1990). The moral reasoning of juvenile delinquents: a meta-
Nezu, A. M. (1985). Difference in psychological distress between effective and ineffective problem-
Nietzel, M. T. (1979). Crime and its modifications: a social learning perspective. Oxford: Pergamon.
Nietzel, M. T., Hasemann, D. M., & Lynam, D. R. (1999). Behavioral perspectives on violent behavior.
offenders: contemporary strategies and issues. New York: Kluwer Acadamic/Plenum Press.
Nock, M. K., Kazdin, A. E., Hiripi, E., & Kessler, R. C. (2007). Lifetime prevalence, correlates, and
persistence of oppositional defiant disorder: results from the national comorbidity survey
Novaco, R. W. (1975). Anger control: the development and evaluation of an experimental treatment
Novaco, R. W. (1979). The cognitive regulation of anger and stress. In P. C. Kendell & S. C. Hollon
Novaco, R. W. (1997). Remediating anger and aggression with violent offenders. Legal and
Novaco, R.W. (2007). Anger dysregulation. In T. A. Cavell & K. T. Malcolm (Eds.), Anger, aggression
and interventions for interpersonal violence (pp. 3-54). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
213
Novaco, R. W., & Welsh, W. N. (1989). Anger disturbances: cognitive mediation and clinical
Nugent, W. R., Bruley, C., & Allen, P. (1999). The effects of aggression replacement training on male
and female antisocial behavior in a runaway shelter. Research on Social Work Practice, 9,
466-482.
Nugent, W. R., Champlin, D., & Wiinimaki, L. (1997). The effects of anger control training on
Odgers, C. L., Moffitt, T. E., Broadbent, J. M., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H., et al.
(2008). Female and male antisocial trajectories: from childhood origins to adult outcomes.
Ogloff, J. (2002). Offender rehabilitation: from "nothing works" to what next? Australian Psychologist,
37, 245-252.
Ogloff, J., & Davis, M. R. (2004). Advances in offender assessment and rehabilitation: contributions
Osgood, D. W., & Briddell, L. O. (2006). Peer effects in juvenile justice. In K. A. Dodge, T. J. Dishion
& J. E. Lansford (Eds.), Deviant peer influences in programs for youth: problems and
Palmer, E. J. (2003). Offending behaviour: moral reasoning, criminal conduct, and the rehabilitation
Palmer, E. J. (2005). The relationship between moral reasoning and aggression, and the implications
Palmer, E.J. (2007). Moral cognition and aggression. In T.A. Gannon, T. Ward, A.R. Beech, & D.
Fisher (Eds.), Aggressive offenders cognition: theory, research, and practice (pp.199-214).
Palmer, E. J., & Hollin, C. R. (1999). Social competence and sociomoral reasoning in young
Parker, J. S. (2009). Distinguishing Between Early and Late Onset Delinquents: Race, Income,
Verbal Intelligence and Impulsivity. North American Journal of Psychology, 11, 273-284.
Patterson, G. R., Reid, J. B., & Dishion, T. J. (1992). A social interactional approach: antisocial boys
Pfiffner, L. J., & McBurnett, K. (1997). Social skills training with parent generalization: treatment
effects for children with attention deficit disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Phelps, L., & McClintock, K. (1994). Papa and peers: a biosocial approach to conduct disorder.
Piaget, J. (1932). The moral judgment of the child. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International University Press.
Polanczyk, G., de Lima, M. S., Horta, B. L., Biederman, J., & Rhode, L. A. (2007). The worldwide
Polaschek, D. L. L. (2003). Relapse prevention, offense process models, and the treatment of sexual
Polaschek, D. L. L. (2006). Violent offender programmes: concept, theory and practice In C. R. Hollin
controversies (pp. 113-154). Chichester, England: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
Polaschek, D. L. L., & Dixon, B. G. (2001). The violence prevention project: The development and
evaluation of a treatment programme for violent offenders Psychology, Crime & Law, 7, 1-23.
Posavac, E. J., & Carey, R. G. (2003). Program evaluation: methods and case studies (6th ed.).
Potter, G. B., Gibbs, J. C., & Goldstein, A. P. (2001). The equip program implementation guide:
teaching youth to think and act responsibly through a peer-helping approach. Champaign, IL:
Research Press.
Price, D. M. (1999). Relapse prevention and risk reduction: results of client identification of high risk
Quinn, M. M., Kavale, K. A., Mathur, S. R., Rutherford, J. R. B., & Forness, S. R. (1999). A meta-
analysis of social skill interventions for students with emotional or behavioral disorders.
Rahill, S. A., & Teglasi, H. (2003). Processes and outcomes of story-based and skill-based social
competency programs for children with emotional disabilities. Journal of School Psychology,
46, 413-429.
Raine, A., Dodge, K., Loeber, R., Gatzke-Kopp, L., Lynam, D., Reynolds, C., et al. (2006). The
Ramírez, J. M., & Andreu, J. M. (2006). Aggression, and some related psychological constructs
(anger, hostility, and impulsivity): some comments from a research project. Neuroscience
Raue, P. J., Goldfried, M. R., & Barkham, M. (1997). The therapeutic alliance in psychodynamic-
Reed, M. K. (1994). Social skills training to reduce depression in adolescents. Adolescence, 29, 293-
303.
Ross, E. C., Polaschek, D. L. L., & Ward, T. (2008). The therapeutic alliance: a theoretical revision for
Rowland, A. S., Lesesne, C. A., & Abramowitz, A. J. (2002). The epidemiology of attention-
Salmon, S. (2003). Teaching empathy: the PEACE curriculum. Reclaiming Children and Youth, 12,
167-173.
Sawyer, M. G., Arney, F. M., Baghurst, P. A., Clark, J. J., Graetz, B. W., & Kosky, R. J. (2000).
Mental health of young people in Australia: child and adolescent component of the national
survey of metal health and well-being. Canberra: Commonwealth Dept. of Health and Aged
Care.
Scarpa, A., & Raine, A. (1997). Phychophysiology of anger and violent behavior. Psychiatric Clinics
Segrin, C. (2000). Social skills deficits associated with depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 20,
379-403.
Selman, R. L. (1971). The relation of role taking to the development of moral judgment in children.
Serin, R. C., Gobeil, R., & Preston, D. L. (2009). Evaluation of the persistently violent offender
53, 57-73.
Serin, R. C., & Preston, D. L. (2001). Managing and treating violent offenders. In J. B. Ashford, B. D.
Sales & W. H. Reid (Eds.), Treating adult and juvenile offenders with special needs. (pp. 249-
Sestir, M. A., & Bartholow, B. (2007). Theoretical explanations of aggression and violence In T. A.
Gannon, T. Ward, A. R. Beech & D. Fisher (Eds.), Aggressive offenders' cognition: theory,
reasearch and practice. West Sussex, EN: John Wiley & Sons.
218
Shelton, D., Sampl, S., Kesten, K. L., Zhang, W., & Trestman, R. (2009). Treatment of impulsive
aggression in correctional settings. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 27, 787-800.
Slaby, R. G., & Guerra, N. G. (1988). Cognitive mediators of aggression in adolescent offenders: 1.
Smith, P., & Waterman, M. (2006). Self-reported aggression and impulsivity in forensic and non-
forensic populations: the role of gender and experience. Journal of Family Violence, 21, 425-
437.
Spence, S. H. (2000). The treatment of childhood social phobia: the effectiveness of a social skills
Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 41, 713-726.
Spence, S. H. (2003). Social skills training with children and young people: theory, evidence and
Spielberger, C. D., Reheiser, E. C., & Sydeman, S. J. (1995). Measuring the experience, expression,
Spivack, G., & Shure, M. B. (1982). Interpersonal cognitive problem solving and clinical theory. In B.
Lahey & A. E. Kazdin (Eds.), Advances in Clinical Psychology (Vol. 5, pp. 323-372). New
York: Plenum.
Stams, G. J., Brugman, D., Dekovic, M., van Rosmalen, L., van der Laan, P., & Gibbs, J. C. (2006).
Stouthamer-Loeber, M., & Loeber, R. (2002). Lost opportunities for intervention: undetected markers
for the development of serious juvenile delinquency. Criminal Behaviour & Mental Health, 12,
69-82.
Streiner, D. L. (2002). The case of missing data: methods of dealing with dropouts and other
Sukhodolsky, D. G., Golub, A., Stone, E. C., & Orban, L. (2005). Dismantling anger control training
for children: a randomized pilot study of social problem-solving versus social skills training
Sukhodolsky, D. G., Vitulano, L. A., Carroll, D. H., McGuire, J., Leckman, J. F., & Scahill, L. (2009).
Randomized trial of anger control training for adolescents with tourette's syndrome and
disruptive behavior. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 48,
413-421.
Sullivan, C. J., Veysey, B. M., & Dorangrichia, L. (2003). Examining the relationship between problem
history and violent offending in high-risk youth. Journal of Offender Rehabilitation, 38, 17 -
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics (4th ed.). Boston, Mass: Allyn &
Bacon.
Tate, D. C., Reppucci, N. D., & Mulvey, E. P. (1995). Violent juvenile delinquents: treatment
effectiveness and implications for future action. Amercian Psychologist, 50, 777-781.
Taylor, N. (2007). Juveniles in detention in Australia, 1981-2006. Canberra, Aust: Australian Institute
of Criminology
Teplin, L. A., Abram, K. M., McClelland, G. M., Dulcan, M. K., & Mericle, A. A. (2002). Psychiatric
Tremblay, R. E. (2000). The development of aggressive behaviour during childhood: what have we
learned in the past century? International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24, 129-141.
van Manen, T., Prins, P., & Emmelkamp, P. (2004). Reducing aggressive behavior in boys with a
social cognitive group treatment: results of a randomized, controlled trial. Journal of the
Vorrath, H. H., & Brendtro, L. K. (1985). Postive peer culture (2nd ed.). Hawthorne, NY: Aldine.
Wakai, S., Shelton, D., Trestman, R. L., & Kesten, K. (2009). Conducting research in corrections:
challenges and solutions. Behavioral Sciences & The Law, 27, 743-752.
Walker, L. J. (1980). Cognitive and perspective-taking prerequisites for moral development. Child
Ward, T., & Stewart, C. (2003a). Criminogenic needs and human needs: a theoretical model.
Ward, T., & Stewart, C. (2003b). The relationship between human needs and ciminogenic needs.
Waschbusch, D. A., & King, S. (2006). Should sex-specific norms be used to assess attention-
Webster-Stratton, C., & Dahl, R. W. (1995). Conduct disorder. In M. Hersen & R. T. Ammerman
(Eds.), Advanced Abnormal Child Psychology (pp. 333-352). Hillsdale, New Jersey:
Werry, J. S. (1997). Severe conduct disorder: some key issues. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 42,
577-583.
221
White, J. L., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Bartusch, D. J., Needles, D. J., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1994).
White, S. W., Keonig, K., & Scahill, L. (2007). Social skills development in children with autism
Wilson, D. B., Bouffard, L. A., & MacKenzie, D. L. (2005). A quantitative review of structured, group-
oriented, cognitive-behavioral programs for offenders. Criminal Justice and Behaviour, 32,
172-204.
World Health Organisation [WHO]. (1992). The international classification of disease (10th ed.).
Geneva: Author.
Yalom, I. D. (1995). The theory and practice of group psychotherapy (4th ed.). New York: Basic
Books.
Yochelson, S., & Samenow, S. E. (1976). The Criminal Personality: a profile for change (Vol. 1). New
Zuckerman, M. (1979). Sensation seeking: beyond the optimal level of arousal. Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
222
Anger Control
Week Skillstreaming Moral Reasoning
Training
Rehearse Full-
9 Understanding the Feelings of Others Shaun’s Problem
Sequence
10
Expressing Affection or Appreciation Full-Sequence Johnny’s Problem
224
225
226
Name: ___________________________________
Mark has been going out with a girl named Maria for about two months. It used to be a lot of fun to
be with her, but lately it’s been sort of a drag. There are some other girls Mark would like to go out
with. Now Mark sees Maria coming down the school hallway. What should Mark say or do?
1. Should Mark avoid the subject with Maria so Maria’s feelings aren’t hurt? a. Should avoid subject
b. Should bring it up
c. Can’t decide
2. Should Mark make up an excuse, like being too busy to see Maria, as a a. Excuse
way of breaking up? b. No excuse
c. Can’t decide
3. Should Mark simply start going out with other girls so that Maria will get a. Yes
the message? b. No
c. Can’t decide
4. Let’s change the situation a bit. What if Mark and Maria have been living a. Should break up
together for several years and have two small children? Then should b. No, shouldn’t break up
Mark still break up with Maria? c. Can’t decide
5. Let’s go back to the original situation. This is what happens: Mark does a. Yes, should get even
break up with Maria. He let her know how he feels and starts dating b. No, shouldn’t get even
another girl. Maria feels hurt and jealous and thinks about getting even c. Can’t decide
somehow. Should Maria get even?
a. Yes, should get even
6. What if the tables were turned, and Maria broke up with Mark. Should b. No, shouldn’t get even
Mark get even somehow? c. Can’t decide
Mark's Problem Situation continues the theme of mature, caring relationships but focuses on the
problem of ending a dating relationship that is going nowhere. The main value of this problem
situation for moral judgment development arrives with discussion of the last questions, which concern
vengeance.
As with most of the problem situations, many group members choose positive responses. The
majority position tends to be that Mark should discuss breaking up (Question 1) rather than making
up an excuse (Question 2) or simply starting to date other girls (Question 3). Accordingly most of the
open-ended suggestions (in response to Question 4) are positive: "Just tell her you'd like to date
other girls"; "Be considerate and remember she's human, too"; "Explain how you feel, that you don't
want to settle down'; "Listen to what she has to say about it'. Of the responses we have heard our
favourite is "I think we should see other people. What do you think?" The group member who gave
this response also indicated that he would first try to, "Work things out", before breaking up with
Maria. As to the reasons for bringing the subject up (Question 1), one group member pointed out that
Mark "should be man enough to tell her"; if he doesn't, another suggested, "Maria might lose a
chance to get another boyfriend," and "would be hurt more in the long run", than by just being told.
Speaking more pragmatically (against the idea of simply starting to date other girls), another group
member suggested that then those girls could find out how Mark treated Maria and dump him for
being a two-timer.
Of course, not all of these responses are positive. On the open-ended question, one group member
wrote, "Do things to make Maria drop him." Another wrote that he would say, "I'm dumping you,
bag!" These group members may also advocate avoiding Maria or making up an excuse. After
discussion, however, they are often willing to acquiesce to the majority position and thereby make a
positive group discussion possible.
An abrupt turnabout occurs on Question 5, in which Mark and Maria are live-in partners with two
small children. Then the majority usually favours not breaking up, on the grounds that Mark has a
responsibility to the children (e.g. "The kids should have both a dad and a mom."). If he left, "it would
hurt the kids, because they would feel it was their fault." One group member suggested, "He loved
her once. Why should one argument make him not love her again?" A pragmatic group member
pointed out that he might have to pay child support if he leaves.
The majority position continues to be positive on the vengeance questions, 6 and 7. The majority is
against either Maria's getting even if Mark breaks up (Question 6) or Mark's getting even if the tables
are turned (Question 7). Suggestions are that, "Mark should just tell himself that it's her loss"; "Let
bygones be bygones." Mark, "wouldn't want her to get even with him [so he shouldn't do that to her;
Stage 3] and if one of them retaliated, "there would just be more trouble." One group member
suggested, somewhat ominously that Mark shouldn't get even because he "might do something really
bad and wind up in here."
Count on several group members advocating retaliation, however - especially by Mark against Maria.
Reasons included, "Give her a taste of her own medicine" and suggested that Mark should get even
because, "He'd be mad" and, as further justification, disclosed that he himself had gotten mad and
228
beaten up several girls who had left him for other guys. He remained silent when a peer asked,
"Does that make it right?" and asked why he nonetheless thought it was wrong if Maria got even with
Mark. Nor would he acquiesce to a group decision against getting even. At least he felt peer group
opposition and perhaps this is the reason he was more accommodating to positive majority positions
on subsequent occasions.
It is sometimes helpful to ask the group exactly what is meant by "getting even." Responses range
from, "Showing off [ to Maria] with a new girlfriend;' to, "Telling him [the new boyfriend] that she was
a good lay for you," to "Slashing their tyres,' -- or faces! The majority will often brand these
responses, once stated for group consideration, as immature or destructive. Nonetheless, many
group members will comment that although Mark and/or Maria shouldn't get even, they probably
would. If the group is still developing, the group leader may need to model re-labelling -- that is ,
comment on how much strength and courage it takes not to, "Give in to childish desires to get even."
The degree of positive content may be surprising and should be encouraged. The group leader
should comment on the great potential the group has shown for becoming a positive group. Using re-
labelling, the group leader should emphasize that a strong group is one where members care about
another's feelings. Bear in mind, however that the group members expressing more negative
sentiments may be speaking more candidly; their words may be consistent with the actual behaviour
of the majority. After all, consider how common "payback" or vengeance is in the daily life of the
troubled adolescent! Similarity, in social skills exercises, initial absence of caring about another's
feelings can be striking. Clearly the group challenge is to accomplish the translation of responsible
words into responsible actions.
229
230
Appendix B: Ethics
B2. Swinburne University Human Research Ethics Committee (HREC)- Approval (2004)
B5. Partcipant Information and Consent form for the Release of Information from
1. Your Consent
You are invited to take part in this research project.
This Participant Information contains detailed information about the research project. Its purpose is to
explain to you as openly and clearly as possible all the procedures involved in this project before you
decide whether or not to take part in it.
Please read this Participant Information carefully. Feel free to ask questions about any information in
the document. You may also wish to discuss the project with a relative or friend or your health
worker. Feel free to do this.
Once you understand what the project is about and if you agree to take part in it, you will be asked to
sign the Consent Form. By signing the Consent Form, you indicate that you understand the
information and that you give your consent to participate in the research project.
You will be given a copy of the Participant Information and Consent Form to keep as a record.
2. Purpose and Background
The purpose of this project is to evaluate the effectiveness of Aggression Replacement Training
(ART) with Australian youth.
Up to 30 young men from Malmsbury Youth Training Centre will participate in this project.
We know that some young people have problems managing their anger, which can damage personal
relationships and cause trouble with the law. We are interested in seeing if we can help you find
ways to deal with problems other than using aggression. To do this we want to run a program called
Aggression Replacement Training or A.R.T. at Malmsbury. ART was originally developed in America
and there have been good results for young people with it there. However, we need to know how
and if it will work with Australian youth, that what this study is for.
You are invited to participate in this research project because your health and key workers at
Malmsbury thought that this program could provide you with some useful skills. That is, you might
learn some new skills to help you control your anger and get along better in your personal
relationships.
The results of this research will be used to help the principal researcher. Matthew Currie, to obtain a
degree.
232
3. Procedures
Participation in this project will involve
• Attendance at three ART group sessions per week for 10-weeks. Each session lasts about one
hour, so that’s a total of about three hours per week. We will run ART at Malmsbury on three
different days of the week and you will be in the group with about seven other guys.
• We will get about 16 guys to sign up for the program. The participants that are due for release
earlier will go into the first group and those who have a longer stay at Malmsbury will go into the
second group- which will start as soon as the first group finishes. So half the guys will start the
program immediately and the other half will start 10-weeks later when the first group has finished.
We do this so that we can see if there is any difference between a group of guys who have done
ART compared to a group who have not, sometimes called a waitlist-control group.
• We will ask you some questions about your mental health, your drug use and your family
background. We only do this part of the testing once- just before the program begins. This will
take about 2-hours. We will ask everyone participating in the study to complete the same
paper/pencil questionnaires just before the program begins, then again 10-weeks later (end of the
program), and six months later, 12-months later and 2-years later. This is so that we can see what
effect ART has over time. Each time you do the questionnaires it will take about 30 to 45-minutes.
• We will also ask your key-worker or probation officer (post-release) to complete some
questionnaires about how they see you relate to others.
3.1 Consent for the Release of Information
We are seeking your consent for the release of information from The Department of Human Service-
Juvenile Justice regarding your convictions just before the program begins, then six months, 12-
months and 2-years after the program ends. DHS-JJ already has these records. Again we do this so
that we can see if participation in ART can reduce or prevent re-offending.
3.2 Video Taping of ART Sessions
Because ART is new to Australia the ART-Trainer must go through an accreditation process. We
have to videotape some of the ART sessions and send it to our teacher in the US, Chris Hayes (ART
Master Trainer). It’s like a project we have to pass, Chris is the only person who will see the tapes,
after he has watched them they are destroyed. The tapes will never be shown on TV, or sold by
anyone to make money. If you sign the consent form, it means you are giving your permission
for your ART program to be taped, and for the people listed above to see the tapes.
4. Possible Benefits
Possible benefits from ART might include an increase in your ability to control your anger, which in
turn might help you to get along better with others and keep out of trouble, as well overall better self-
esteem. We cannot guarantee that this will happen for you but there is evidence from the USA that
some of these benefits have occurred for many of the people who have done the program.
233
5. Possible Risks
We do not believe that there are any serious risks associated with your participation in the program.
However, the program will challenge you to think and do things differently and sometimes that might
cause you some anxiety. If this happens for you we will provide lots of opportunities for you discuss
this with the ART-Trainers. If that doesn’t help you could talk to your key-worker or health-worker at
Malmsbury.
6. Alternatives to Participation
Alternative procedures/alternative treatments include individual meetings with your key-worker and
health-worker at Malmsbury.
7. Privacy, Confidentiality and Disclosure of Information
We are serious about keeping your details confidential. You need to be aware that there are some
situations where we would need to change that. You don’t have to tell us anything you don’t want to,
but if you choose to tell us about illegal behaviour or a situation that puts yourself or others at risk of
harm, then we are required to report that information to the appropriate authorities. This might mean
contacting the police or your health-worker. We would let you know if this was going to happen. If you
have any questions, or do not understand what is happening, make sure you say something. You can
tell your key-worker, health-worker, or ask us.
You will need to tell The Chair the name of one or both of the researchers given in section 10 above.
12. Participation is Voluntary
Participation in any research project is voluntary. If you do not wish to take part you are not obliged
to. If you decide to take part and later change your mind, you are free to withdraw from the project at
any stage.
Your decision whether to take part or not to take part, or to take part and then withdraw, will not affect
your routine treatment, your relationship with those treating you or your relationship with anyone at
Malmsbury.
Before you make your decision, a member of the research team will be available to answer any
questions you have about the research project. You can ask for any information you want. Sign the
Consent Form only after you have had a chance to ask your questions and have received satisfactory
answers.
13. Ethical Guidelines
This project will be carried out according to the National Statement on Ethical Conduct in Research
Involving Humans (June 1999) produced by the National Health and Medical Research Council of
Australia. This statement has been developed to protect the interests of people who agree to
participate in human research studies.
The ethical aspects of this research project have been approved by the Human Research Ethics
Committee of Swinburne University and the Department of Human Services Victoria.
14. Reimbursement for your costs
You will not be paid for your participation in this project.
235
Note: All parties signing the Consent Form must date their own signature.
236
237
238
August 9, 2008
Dear Keith,
Re: Amendment- HREC No. 03/27 Aggression Replacement Training (ART): An evaluation
study
The above mentioned Doctoral research project is due to undertake final data collection (i.e., 2-year
follow-up) from participants (N=20). In the original Participant Information and Consent Form (dated
16/01/2006) signed consent was obtained for the following clause-
We are seeking your consent for the release of information from The Department of Human Services-
Juvenile Justice regarding your convictions just before the program begins, then six months, 12-
months and 2-years after the program ends. DHS-JJ already has these records. Again we do this so
that we can see if participation in ART can reduce or prevent re-offending.
This de-identified data is needed in order to track recidivism, which is an important outcome measure
in determining clinical significance subsequent to treatment. However it has become apparent that
those individuals who may have re-offend subsequent to leaving Malmsbury Youth Justice Centre
would be likely to enter the adult, penal system and therefore would not be listed on the Juvenile
Justice data collection system. It is for this reason that the Principal Researcher has sought
permission from Victoria Police- Research & Project Governance to access the police LEAP system.
Victoria Police has given ‘in principle’ support for this de-identified data to be provided, however they
have indicated (see attached letter) that specific consent must be sought from research participants.
In order for this written consent to be obtained we are seeking an amendment to above mentioned
HREC application to mail out an amended consent form (see attachment) to be signed by
239
In addition we are seeking ethics approval to provide participants with a $50 gift certificate to be
provided upon receipt of the signed consent form and completion of the final set of questionnaires
relating to the 2-year follow-up (final) data collection. This is intended as an incentive to return the
signed consent form promptly and remuneration in recognition of participant’s ongoing willingness to
give of their time over the duration of this longitudinal evaluation study.
Your timely response to this request for amendment would be greatly appreciated.
Yours sincerely,
Dr Katie Wood Mr Matthew Currie
(Research Supervisor) (Principal Researcher)
240
Consent for the Release of Information from Victorian Police Data (LEAP) Records
The purpose of this additional consent form is to give Matthew Currie (Principal Researcher) your
consent for the release of information from Victoria Police regarding your convictions 6-months and
24-months after your completion of the ART program. Matthew Currie agrees to de-identify the
information by applying a code that only he can match to your name, so the information will be
confidential. The purpose of this information is so that we can see if participation in ART can reduce
or prevent re-offending. On return of this signed consent form in the pre-paid, self addressed
envelope you will receive a gift voucher to the value of $50 in recognition of my time and effort in
agreeing to participate in this study.
Participation is Voluntary
Participation in any research project is voluntary. If you do not wish to take part you are not obliged
to. If you decide to take part and later change your mind, you are free to withdraw from the project at
any stage.
Signature
:……………………………………………………Date:………………………………………………
……
Signature
:……………………………………………………Date:………………………………………………
Note: All parties signing the Consent Form must date their own signature.
241
SUHREC Project 03/27 Aggression Replacement Training (ART): An evaluation study Dr Catherine
Wood, FLSS; Mr Matthew Currie Project Duration Extended to 31/12/2009 [Approved Modifications:
June 2005, Aug/Sep 2008, Sep 2009 (Victoria Police Request VPHREC Project 82/09)]
I refer to your email of 30 September 2009 attaching evidence of approval from the Victoria Police
Human Research Ethics Committee to involve Victoria Police in the way outlined in prevous
communication. In light of this information, which was put to the Chair of SUHREC for endorsement, I
confirm on-going Swinburne ethics clearance in line with standard conditions (reprinted below).
- All human research activity undertaken under Swinburne auspices must conform to Swinburne and
external regulatory standards, including the National Statement on Ethical Conduct in Human
Research and with respect to secure data use, retention and disposal.
- The named Swinburne Chief Investigator/Supervisor remains responsible for any personnel
appointed to or associated with the project being made aware of ethics clearance conditions,
including research and consent procedures or instruments approved. Any change in chief
investigator/supervisor requires timely notification and SUHREC endorsement.
- The above project has been approved as submitted for ethical review by or on behalf of SUHREC.
Amendments to approved procedures or instruments ordinarily require prior ethical appraisal/
clearance. SUHREC must be notified immediately or as soon as possible thereafter of (a) any serious
or unexpected adverse effects on participants and any redress measures; (b) proposed changes in
protocols; and (c) unforeseen events which might affect continued ethical acceptability of the project.
- At a minimum, an annual report on the progress of the project is required as well as at the
conclusion (or abandonment) of the project.
- A duly authorised external or internal audit of the project may be undertaken at any time.
Please contact me if you have any queries about on-going ethics clearance, citing the SUHREC
project number. Copies of clearance emails should be retained as part of project record-keeping.
29 September 2009
Dear Mr Currie,
Your response provided 26 September 2009 has been considered out of sessions.
I am now pleased to advise you that your application has received formal approval.
I draw your attention to the terms of the ‘Declaration by researcher(s)’ in your application, including
the following requirements:
• To provide a final report and a copy of any published material at the end of the research project,
and
• To notify VPHREC in writing immediately if any change to the project is proposed and await
approval before proceeding with the proposed change.
If you have any queries or require further clarification please contact Georgina Lee.
Please note that you also require approval from the Research Coordinating Committee (RCC) before
commencing your research.
Yours sincerely,
Appendix C: Measures
10. Was the new skill correctly introduced, defined, and briefly explained in understandable language? .............................
11. Was the new skill perfectly modelled by the trainer and co-trainer using a relevant adolescent situation?.....................
12. Did someone point to the skill steps during the modelling and role plays? ................................................................
13. Did each youth express how the skill could be personally useful? .....................................................................................
14. Did each youth correctly role-play the skill as the main actor?............................................................................................
15. Did each youth pick their own role play partner?..................................................................................................................
16. Did each youth provide performance feedback? ..................................................................................................................
17. Was the order of performance feedback correct (co-actor, group members, co-trainer, trainer, main actor)?................
18. Were new Skillstreaming Homework Reports given to each youth as homework and the top half filled out?.................
19. Was behaviour appropriately managed?...............................................................................................................................
20. Did the session pace keep the students interested and active?..........................................................................................
21. Did the students appear to understand the skill being taught in this session?................................................................
22. Does the primary Trainer interact with the youth in a positive manner?.............................................................................
23. Does the Co-trainer interact with the youth in a positive manner?......................................................................................
24. Does the Co-trainer aid the Trainer in delivering the curriculum? .......................................................................................
261
YES NO
Please place a check in the box that best describes what happened in this session.
▼ ▼
1. Was a positive climate established through welcoming the students? ................................................................
2. Were issues since the last Anger Control Training session resolved? ................................................................
3. Were group norms reviewed, emphasizing positive participation? ................................................................
4. Were all ACT concepts covered to this point reviewed? (e.g. ABC’s, Personal Power,
etc)................................................................................................................................................
5. Did most youth complete the hassle log(s)? ................................................................................................
6. Were the hassle logs used to review the Anger Control Chain? ................................................................
7. Were efforts appropriately and genuinely acknowledged? ................................................................................................
8. Were achievements rewarded?................................................................................................................................
9. Were the hassle logs kept in the students’ folders? ................................................................................................
10. Was the new ACT concept correctly introduced, defined, and briefly explained in understandable
language? ................................................................................................................................................................
11. Was the Anger Control Chain correctly reviewed? ................................................................................................
12. Were visual aids used (poster of the Anger Control Chain or other visual aids)?................................
13. Was the Anger Control Chain perfectly modelled by the trainer and co-trainer using a relevant
adolescent situation? ................................................................................................................................
14. Did someone point to the Anger Control Chain links during the modelling and role plays? ................................
15. Did each youth express how the concept could be personally useful?................................................................
16. Did each youth correctly role-play the Anger Control Chain as the main actor?...............................................................
17. Did each youth pick their own role-play partner?................................................................................................
18. Did each youth provide performance feedback during the class?................................................................
19. Was the order of performance feedback correct (co-actor, group members, co-trainer, trainer,
main actor)? ................................................................................................................................................................
20. Were new hassle logs given to each youth as homework? ................................................................................................
21. Was behaviour appropriately managed during the session?..............................................................................................
22. Did the session pace keep the students interested and active?................................................................
23. Did the students appear to understand the ACT lesson? ................................................................................................
24. Does the primary Trainer interact with the youth in a positive manner?................................................................
25. Does the Co-trainer interact with the youth in a positive manner?................................................................
26. Does the Co-trainer aid the Trainer in delivering the intervention? ................................................................
262
Please place a check in the box that best describes what happened in this session. YES NO
▼ ▼
1. Was the Problem Situation Chart made before the group? ................................................................................................
2. Did the Trainer study the Problem Situation Chart for patterns of thinking and decide the order to
discuss the questions?.............................................................................................................................................................
3. Was a positive climate established through welcoming the students? ................................................................
4. Were issues since the last moral reasoning session resolved? ...........................................................................................
5. Were Group Norms for the discussion session reviewed (page 111 A.R.T. Revised Ed.)? ................................
6. Were the four Thought Traps reviewed as established by the Washington State A.R.T. Quality
Assurance process?................................................................................................................................................................
7. Was the Problem Situation read to the class? .......................................................................................................................
8. Was the real problem correctly defined? ................................................................................................................................
9. Was the real problem related to the lives of the group members?.......................................................................................
10. Was moral maturity established through: ..............................................................................................................................
a) Eliciting mature responses first? ................................................................................................................................
b) Reconstructing less mature responses, and.....................................................................................................................
c) Listing them on an easel pad, chalk board, or white board? ...........................................................................................
11. Were more mature group members used to challenge the less mature reasoning and Thought Traps
expressed by some group members? ................................................................................................................................
12. Was the list of mature reasons used to challenge the less mature reasoning and Thought Traps
expressed by some group members? ................................................................................................................................
13. Were role-taking opportunities created for the youth? (e.g. "What would the world be like if everyone
behaved that way?" "What would you do if you were _____?") .........................................................................................
14. Was there an attempt made to make positive decisions and mature reasons unanimous or a group
decision? ................................................................................................................................................................
15. Were comments by each group member acknowledged?................................................................................................
16. Were individuals within the group praised for positive decisions and mature reasons?................................
17. Was the whole group praised for positive decisions and mature reasons?................................................................
18. Were all group members involved in the discussion? ................................................................................................
19. Was behaviour appropriately managed during the session?...............................................................................................
20. Did the session pace keep the students interested and active?..........................................................................................
21. Did the Trainer remain objective during the session? ................................................................................................
22. Does the Trainer interact with the youth in a positive manner?...........................................................................................
23. Does the Co-trainer interact with the youth in a positive manner?......................................................................................
263
Item YES=1
Please place a "1" in the box that best describes what happened in this session. Weight NO=0
1. Was a positive climate established through welcoming students? 2 1
2. Were any issues since the last anger control training session dealt with? 2 1
3. Were group norms reviewed, emphasizing positive participation? 2 1
4. Were all relevant ACT concepts covered to this point reviewed? 3 1
5. Did most youth complete the hassle log(s)? 11 1
6. Were the hassle logs used to review the anger control chain? 3 1
7. Were efforts honestly and genuinely acknowledged? 2 1
8. Were achievements rewarded? 2 1
9. Were the hassle logs kept in the students’ folders? 2 1
10. Was the new ACT concept correctly introduced, defined, and briefly explained in understandable language? 11 1
11. Was the Anger Control Chain correctly reviewed? 3 1
12. Were visual aids used (poster of the Anger Control Chain or other visual aids)? 2 1
13. Was the Anger Control Chain perfectly demonstrated by the trainer (as Main Actor) and co-trainer using a
relevant adolescent situation? 11 1
14. Did someone point to the Anger Control Chain concepts during the demonstration and practice sessions? 2 1
15. Did each youth express how today’s information could be personally useful? 2 1
16. Did each youth correctly practice the Anger Control Chain as the main actor? 11 1
17. Did each youth pick their own practice session partner? 2 1
18. Did each youth provide performance feedback during the class? 2 1
19. Was the order of performance feedback correct (co-actor, group members, co-trainer, trainer, main actor)? 2 1
20.Were new hassle logs given to each youth as homework? 3 1
21. Was behavior appropriately managed during the session? 3 1
22. Did the session pace keep the students interested and active? 3 1
23. Did the students appear to understand the ACT lesson? 3 1
24. Does the primary instructor interact with the youth in a positive manner? 11 1
25. Does the co-instructor interact with the youth in a positive manner? 3 1
26. Does the co-instructor aid the trainer in delivering the intervention? 3 1
Item YES=1
Please place a "1" in the box that best describes what happened in this session. Weight NO=0
1. Was a positive climate established through welcoming students? 3 1
2. Were any issues since the last social skills training session dealt with? 1 1
3. Were group norms reviewed, emphasizing positive participation? 2 1
4. Did most youth complete the Social Skills Homework Report? 11 1
5. Were the Social Skills Homework Reports used to review last week’s social skill? 3 1
6. Were homework efforts appropriately and genuinely acknowledged? 3 1
7. Were homework achievements rewarded? 3 1
8. Were the Social Skills Homework Reports collected or kept in the students’ folders? 3 1
9. Were visual aids used (skill cards distributed and social skill title and steps displayed)? 3 1
10. Was the new social skill correctly introduced, defined, and briefly explained in understandable language? 11 1
11. Was the new social skill perfectly demonstrated by the trainer (as Main Actor) and co-trainer using a
relevant adolescent situation? 11 1
12. Did the someone point to the social skill steps during the demonstration and practice session? 2 1
13. Did each youth express how the social skill could be personally useful? 3 1
14. Did each youth correctly practice the social skill as the main actor? 11 1
15. Did each youth pick their own practice session partner? 1 1
16. Did each youth provide performance feedback? 3 1
17. Skill steps read to the class by the assigned youth during performance feedback? 1 1
18. Was the order of performance feedback correct (co-actor, group members, co-trainer, trainer, main
actor)? 2 1
19. Were new Social Skills Homework Reports given to each youth as homework and the top section filled
out? 3 1
20. Was behavior appropriately managed? 3 1
21. Did the session pace keep the students interested and active? 3 1
22. Did the students appear to understand the skill being taught in this session? 3 1
23. Does the primary instructor interact with the youth in a positive manner ? 11 1
24. Does the co-instructor interact with the youth in a positive manner? 3 1
25. Does the co-instructor aid the instructor in delivering the curriculum? 3 1
Jurisdiction: Melbourne Aust. Date: July 2005 Instructor: Matt Currie Co-Instructor: Cath Powell
Week Number: 6
YES=1
Item NO=0
Please place a "1" in the box that best describes what happened in this session. Weight ▼
1. Was the Problem Situation Chart made before the group? 3 1
2. Did the instructor study the Problem Situation Chart for patterns of thinking and decide in what order to
discuss the questions? 11 1
3. Was a positive climate established through welcoming students? 3 1
4. Were issues since the last moral reasoning session dealt with?. 3 1
5. Were Group Norms for the discussion session reviewed (page 111 in the book)? 3 1
6. Were the four thinking errors reviewed? 3 1
7. Was the Problem Situation read to the class? 3 1
8. Was the real problem correctly defined and related to the lives of the group members? 11 1
9. Was moral maturity established through:
a) Eliciting mature responses first? 5 1
b) Reconstructing less mature responses, and 5 1
c) Listing them on an easel pad, chalk board, or white board? 5 1
10. Was there an attempt made to make positive decision and mature reasons unanimous or a group
decision? 2 1
11. Were comments by each group member acknowledged? 3 1
12. Were individuals within the group who evidenced more mature reasoning ecouraged to explain their
reasoning? 3 1
13. Was the whole group praised for positive decisions and mature reasons? 3 1
14. Were all group members involved in the discussion? 11 1
15. Did the instructor encourage clear explanation of each person's answer? 3 1
16. Was behavior appropriately managed during the session? 3 1
17. Did the session pace keep the students interested and active? 3 1
18. Did the instructor remain objective during the session? 3 1
19. Does the primary instructor interact with the youth in a positive manner? 11 1
20. Does the co-instructor interact with the youth in a positive manner? 3 1
21. Does the co-instructor aid the trainer in delivering the intervention? 3 1
Washington State
Delivery Skills
Adherence Clarity: Simplicity: Without Objectivity: Pacing: Not Engagement: Total
Understandable by unnecessary Presented as too fast, not too Interesting and
youth complication or factual content, slow involving
explanation without moralizing
or debate
Social Skills 3 3 3 3 3 3 18
Training
Anger Control 3 3 3 3 3 3 18
Training
Moral Reasoning 3 3 2 3 2 3 16
Total 9 9 8 9 8 9 52
Scoring:
0 = Not Competent
1 = Borderline Competent
2 = Competent
3 = Highly Competent
267
Matt Currie
Comments: The Trainer and Co-trainer are a very effective team. They balance each other well. It is clear who is the
main Trainer. The group norms were effectively used by the Trainers to manage group behavior in an assertive but not
demeaning manner which avoided power struggles. Number 5 - The Trainer conducted a good review of the Skill
Homework. At least one youth had used the skill (Dealing with Group Pressure) poorly. The Trainer could have analyzed
the failure of the skill by having the youth look at each step and praising the youth for success up to the point that he gave
into the group. One could phase it as a failure to complete the last step successfully. The Trainer did a particularly nice
job of dealing with one youth who said the skill was "Stupid". In spite of the youth's initial resistance the Trainer was able
to obtain a commitment from the youth to try the skill in the future. Number 10 - There are official definitions of each skill
on page 25 of the Washington State A.R.T. Manual, Revision 16. There are language issues with our different dialects of
English. The Trainer defined the new skill so the youth appeared to understand it. I encourage the Trainer to emphasize
that this skill is about deciding on our role in the problem, since most aggressive youth use the thinking error of Blaming
Others and see problems as primarily caused by others. Number 11 - The Trainer could read each step of the skill during
the model if the youth do not appear to understand what the Trainer is doing. These youth appeared to understand what
the trainer was doing.
Background
In 2005, C was serving a six month custodial sentence at a Victorian Youth Justice Centre
for assault and assault related offences, reportedly while substance affected and in the company of
peers. At the time of participation in ART, C was 18 years old. He was the second oldest child in a
sibship of five. Prior to incarceration, he resided with his mother, three brothers and sister. The
family immigrated to Australia in 2002, fleeing civil war in their homeland. Client records indicated
that the family was well educated and of some standing in their community prior to the civil unrest.
Family relationships were reported to be close and loving. C’s father was assumed to be deceased
after 10 years of no contact. However, some time after the family arrived in Australia, news was
received that the father had survived and entered into a new marriage and begun a new family. The
client reported no ill-feeling toward his father, as his religion allowed for multiple wives. C conveyed
an eagerness to initiate contact with his father again, but in 2004 he received news that his father had
died. According to C, this news precipitated his violent and antisocial behaviour leading up to the
offence.
Participation in ART
C initially presented as angry and mistrustful within the group. In the first session, he
commented that “it’s a jungle out there” referring to life in and outside of the institution and the
apparent need to be hyper-vigilant for potential, real or imagined, threat. Within the first few sessions,
C disclosed information regarding his experiences while living through the Civil War, stating that he
had witnessed friends and relatives beaten and killed. The ART trainers had some concern regarding
269
C’s ability to follow the program material. Initially, he appeared to struggle with understanding the
program content and performing the sequential steps in the role-plays. The trainers discussed the
extent to which cultural differences (e.g., variations in social norms) might affect his ability to fully
By midway through the program (week 5), the trainers’ concerns regarding C’s ability to follow
the material appeared unfounded, the following note was written by the trainers at that time-
In week 8 we wrote-
“[the client] presented calm and focused. Challenged other group members appropriately.
At that time, C was challenged to respond to a series of incidents involving another young
person making ongoing threats and racial taunts. During the break of the session in week 8, the ART
trainers witnessed such an incident. Later the client stated that he had drawn upon some of the skills
he learned in the program to control his anger. C’s Health Worker and other Centre staff noted that
he demonstrated a high level of self-control and mature problem solving skills by avoiding aggressive
confrontation and seeking staff support to manage what was a volatile situation. Information from C’s
[The client’s] behaviour since his last Incident Report has improved dramatically. He is
learning that instead of dealing with issues through physical violence, mediation is the best
possible way to sort through issues. [The client] has requested mediation on a number of
occasions. Centre Management have acknowledged the improved behaviour and approach
that [the client] adopts when dealing with challenging situations. He has consistently
270
demonstrated his ability to remove himself from negative peers and obtains advice and
On the AQ total score C’s pre-treatment raw score was 113 placing him at the 85th percentile
and within the clinical range of scores. Whereas at post-treatment follow-up his self-reported level of
aggression was 77, at the 40th percentile and within the normal range of scores on the AQ. A similar
pattern of clinically significant reductions in aggressive behaviours and thoughts was evident for C
across all of the AQ subscales: Physical, pre-treatment score placed him, at the 92nd percentile,
compared to post-treatment score at the 60th percentile; Verbal, pre- 81st percentile, at post-treatment
his 55th percentile; Anger, pre-treatment 70th percentile, compared to a post-treatment 50th percentile;
Hostility, pre- 93rd percentile, compared to post-treatment 33rd percentile; and Indirect, pre- 23rd
percentile, while at post-treatment assessment his score placed him at 5th percentile. Apart from
Indirect aggression, which was within the normal range of scores at both pre- and post- treatment
assessment, all of C’s subscale scores on the AQ were within the clinical or subclinical range at pre-
treatment. At post-treatment follow-up C’s self-reported aggressive behaviours and thoughts were
within the normal range of scores compared to the normative sample provided by Buss and Warren
(2000). These results show clear evidence of clinically meaningful change for C subsequent to
participation in ART, which is consistent with the case note entries cited above.
C’s scores on the SSRS showed a slight increase in total social skills from pre-treatment with
a raw score of 47 (42nd percentile), compared to post-treatment with a raw score of 49 (50th
percentile). Both pre- and post- scores were within the normal range. Scores on the HIT total
remained within the clinical range at both pre-treatment (raw score= 3.38; 92nd percentile) and post-
271
treatment (raw score= 3.11; 86th percentile) assessment, indicating a slight, though non-significant,
C completed ART in mid- July 2005 and made a valuable contribution to the group. In
particular, he provided his peers with real-life modelling of ART in practice. These changes are partly
reflected in C’s pre- to post- scores on the outcome measures. Although scores on the HIT remained
stable and clinically significant, C demonstrated effective application of pro-social skills, emotional
self-regulation, and consequential thinking when he did not react to ongoing provocation from a peer
Table E1: Inter-correlations for all dependent measures in the main study at T2
Table E2: Descriptives and main effect ANOVA results from T1 to T4 on the AQ (LOCF method)
Table E3: Descriptives and main effect ANOVA results from T1 to T4 on the HIT (LOCF method)
Table E4: Descriptives and main effect ANOVA results from T1 to T4 on the SPSI (LOCF method)
Table E5: Descriptives and main effect ANOVA results from T1 to T4 for Impulsiveness (LOCF)
Table E6: T-score means and main effect ANOVAS from T1 to T3 for the ABCL (LOCF method)
273
AQ Total (Mean replacement method)
Descriptive Statistics
Mean Std. Deviation N
AQtotal 102.2500 25.99570 20
AQtotalT2 89.3500 24.51052 20
AQtotalT3 83.2600 20.72772 20
AQtotalT4 85.1420 22.01162 20
Descriptive Statistics
Mean Std. Deviation N
AQphy 26.8500 8.86314 20
AQphyT2 21.4000 8.99942 20
AQphyT3 20.9410 6.55720 20
AQphyT4 20.1420 7.48934 20
Descriptive Statistics
Mean Std. Deviation N
AQanger 19.4115 5.03732 20
AQangerT2 18.2345 5.79046 20
AQangerT3 17.4705 4.63531 20
AQangerT4 15.9290 5.57687 20
Descriptive Statistics
Mean Std. Deviation N
AQhos 21.6000 7.69415 20
AQhosT2 18.1000 5.75738 20
AQhosT3 17.2345 4.69635 20
AQhosT4 20.0000 5.16058 20
Descriptive Statistics
Mean Std. Deviation N
AQind 16.5500 4.96806 20
AQindT2 14.4500 4.47772 20
AQindT3 15.0000 3.87977 20
AQindT4 14.4290 3.83494 20
Multivariate Testsb
Effect Partial Eta
Value F Hypothesis df Error df Sig. Squared
factor1 Pillai's Trace .460 4.821a 3.000 17.000 .013 .460
Wilks' Lambda .540 4.821a 3.000 17.000 .013 .460
Hotelling's Trace .851 4.821a 3.000 17.000 .013 .460
Roy's Largest Root .851 4.821a 3.000 17.000 .013 .460
281
Descriptive Statistics
Mean Std. Deviation N
AQverbal 16.4000 3.80305 20
AQverbalT2 16.7000 4.83518 20
AQverbalT3 15.2345 3.36436 20
AQverbalT4 14.6420 3.41216 20
Descriptive Statistics
Mean Std. Deviation N
HITtotal 3.1687 .70479 20
HITtotalT2 2.7806 .76124 20
HITtotalT3 2.5769 .88224 20
HITtotalT4 2.1317 .50313 20
Mauchly's Test of Sphericityb
Measure:MEASURE_1
Within Subjects Effect Epsilona
Approx. Chi- Greenhouse-
Mauchly's W Square df Sig. Geisser Huynh-Feldt Lower-bound
dimension1 factor1 .809 3.751 5 .586 .878 1.000 .333
Tests of Within-Subjects Effects
Source Type III Sum Partial Eta
of Squares df Mean Square F Sig. Squared
factor1 Sphericity Assumed 11.184 3 3.728 11.659 .000 .380
Greenhouse-Geisser 11.184 2.633 4.247 11.659 .000 .380
Huynh-Feldt 11.184 3.000 3.728 11.659 .000 .380
Lower-bound 11.184 1.000 11.184 11.659 .003 .380
Error(factor1) Sphericity Assumed 18.227 57 .320
Greenhouse-Geisser 18.227 50.033 .364
Huynh-Feldt 18.227 57.000 .320
Lower-bound 18.227 19.000 .959
284
Descriptive Statistics
Mean Std. Deviation N
HITsc 3.1778 .82945 20
HITscT2 2.7944 .76919 20
HITscT3 2.6727 .86629 20
HITscT4 2.2074 .53087 20
Descriptive Statistics
Mean Std. Deviation N
HITbo 3.0350 .75622 20
HITboT2 2.7300 .80269 20
HITboT3 2.5640 .87054 20
HITboT4 2.0160 .55428 20
Descriptive Statistics
Mean Std. Deviation N
HITmm 3.2222 .88816 20
HITmmT2 2.8000 .89660 20
HITmmT3 2.4972 1.03864 20
HITmmT4 2.0171 .58668 20
Mauchly's Test of Sphericityb
Within Subjects Effect Epsilona
Approx. Chi- Greenhouse-
Mauchly's W Square df Sig. Geisser Huynh-Feldt Lower-bound
dimension1 factor1 .660 7.354 5 .196 .797 .920 .333
Tests of Within-Subjects Effects
Source Type III Sum Partial Eta
of Squares df Mean Square F Sig. Squared
factor1 Sphericity Assumed 15.456 3 5.152 11.255 .000 .372
Greenhouse-Geisser 15.456 2.391 6.465 11.255 .000 .372
Huynh-Feldt 15.456 2.759 5.603 11.255 .000 .372
Lower-bound 15.456 1.000 15.456 11.255 .003 .372
Error(factor1) Sphericity Assumed 26.092 57 .458
Greenhouse-Geisser 26.092 45.428 .574
Huynh-Feldt 26.092 52.415 .498
Lower-bound 26.092 19.000 1.373
290
Descriptive Statistics
Mean Std. Deviation N
HITaw 3.2091 .68419 20
HITawT2 2.7818 .72632 20
HITawT3 2.5461 .84718 20
HITawT4 2.3897 .55684 20
Descriptive Statistics
Mean Std. Deviation N
HITlying 3.2813 .78836 20
HITlyingT2 2.8188 .84339 20
HITlyingT3 2.7423 .88711 20
HITlyingT4 2.4630 .55965 20
Mauchly's Test of Sphericityb
Within Subjects Effect Epsilona
Approx. Chi- Greenhouse-
Mauchly's W Square df Sig. Geisser Huynh-Feldt Lower-bound
dimension1 factor1 .777 4.468 5 .485 .844 .984 .333
Tests of Within-Subjects Effects
Source Type III Sum Partial Eta
of Squares df Mean Square F Sig. Squared
factor1 Sphericity Assumed 6.922 3 2.307 7.483 .000 .283
Greenhouse-Geisser 6.922 2.531 2.735 7.483 .001 .283
Huynh-Feldt 6.922 2.952 2.345 7.483 .000 .283
Lower-bound 6.922 1.000 6.922 7.483 .013 .283
Error(factor1) Sphericity Assumed 17.576 57 .308
Greenhouse-Geisser 17.576 48.087 .366
Huynh-Feldt 17.576 56.089 .313
Lower-bound 17.576 19.000 .925
294
Descriptive Statistics
Mean Std. Deviation N
HITsteal 2.7591 1.04333 20
HITstealT2 2.5045 .84124 20
HITstealT3 2.3162 .97784 20
HITstealT4 1.6813 .47991 20
Descriptive Statistics
Mean Std. Deviation N
HITod 3.4650 .72277 20
HITodT2 3.0350 .78558 20
HITodT3 2.7065 .92820 20
HITodT4 2.3080 .59865 20
Mauchly's Test of Sphericityb
Within Subjects Effect Epsilona
Approx. Chi- Greenhouse-
Mauchly's W Square df Sig. Geisser Huynh-Feldt Lower-bound
dimension1 factor1 .888 2.102 5 .835 .928 1.000 .333
Tests of Within-Subjects Effects
Source Type III Sum Partial Eta
of Squares df Mean Square F Sig. Squared
factor1 Sphericity Assumed 14.471 3 4.824 12.981 .000 .406
Greenhouse-Geisser 14.471 2.784 5.197 12.981 .000 .406
Huynh-Feldt 14.471 3.000 4.824 12.981 .000 .406
Lower-bound 14.471 1.000 14.471 12.981 .002 .406
Error(factor1) Sphericity Assumed 21.180 57 .372
Greenhouse-Geisser 21.180 52.901 .400
Huynh-Feldt 21.180 57.000 .372
Lower-bound 21.180 19.000 1.115
298
Descriptive Statistics
Mean Std. Deviation N
HITpa 3.2000 .76914 20
HITpaT2 2.7800 .89183 20
HITpaT3 2.5705 .91408 20
HITpaT4 1.9710 .58887 20
Descriptive Statistics
Mean Std. Deviation N
SPSItotal 11.2800 2.79729 20
T2SPSItotal 12.7200 2.94039 20
T3SPSItotal 12.7176 2.51401 20
T4SPSItotal 13.4000 2.34498 20
Mauchly's Test of Sphericityb
Within Subjects Effect Epsilona
Approx. Chi- Greenhouse-
Mauchly's W Square df Sig. Geisser Huynh-Feldt Lower-bound
dimension1 factor1 .642 7.859 5 .165 .764 .874 .333
Tests of Within-Subjects Effects
Source Type III Sum Partial Eta
of Squares df Mean Square F Sig. Squared
factor1 Sphericity Assumed 47.814 3 15.938 4.915 .004 .206
Greenhouse-Geisser 47.814 2.291 20.868 4.915 .009 .206
Huynh-Feldt 47.814 2.623 18.230 4.915 .006 .206
Lower-bound 47.814 1.000 47.814 4.915 .039 .206
Error(factor1) Sphericity Assumed 184.825 57 3.243
Greenhouse-Geisser 184.825 43.533 4.246
Huynh-Feldt 184.825 49.834 3.709
Lower-bound 184.825 19.000 9.728
302
Descriptive Statistics
Mean Std. Deviation N
PPO 2.4900 .92332 20
PPOT2 2.8400 .76667 20
SMEAN(sumPPOT3) 14.8235 2.92422 20
SMEAN(sumPPOT4) 15.6429 3.62168 20
Descriptive Statistics
Mean Std. Deviation N
sumNPO1 7.5000 4.74064 20
sumT2NPO1 6.4500 3.48644 20
SMEAN(sumT3NPO1) 6.4118 3.04456 20
SMEAN(sumT4NPO1) 6.0714 3.18656 20
Descriptive Statistics
Mean Std. Deviation N
sumRPS 8.6500 3.77352 20
sumRPST2 10.3500 4.77135 20
SMEAN(sumRPST3) 10.4706 3.86778 20
SMEAN(sumRPST4) 12.0714 4.90396 20
Descriptive Statistics
Mean Std. Deviation N
sumICS1 8.7000 3.37327 20
sumT2ICS1 7.9000 3.17722 20
SMEAN(sumT3ICS1) 8.3529 4.47144 20
SMEAN(sumT4ICS1) 8.7857 3.61544 20
Descriptive Statistics
Mean Std. Deviation N
sumAS1 8.5000 6.23656 20
sumT2AS1 6.3500 5.56564 20
SMEAN(sumT3AS1) 6.9412 3.92657 20
SMEAN(sumT4AS1) 5.8571 3.72555 20
Mauchly's Test of Sphericityb
Measure:MEASURE_1
Within Subjects Effect Epsilona
Approx. Chi- Greenhouse-
Mauchly's W Square df Sig. Geisser Huynh-Feldt Lower-bound
dimension1 factor1 .750 5.095 5 .405 .837 .975 .333
Tests of Within-Subjects Effects
Measure:MEASURE_1
Source Type III Sum Partial Eta
of Squares df Mean Square F Sig. Squared
factor1 Sphericity Assumed 79.023 3 26.341 2.033 .119 .097
Greenhouse-Geisser 79.023 2.511 31.469 2.033 .131 .097
Huynh-Feldt 79.023 2.925 27.020 2.033 .121 .097
Lower-bound 79.023 1.000 79.023 2.033 .170 .097
Error(factor1) Sphericity Assumed 738.450 57 12.955
Greenhouse-Geisser 738.450 47.712 15.477
Huynh-Feldt 738.450 55.567 13.289
Lower-bound 738.450 19.000 38.866
310
I7 Impulsiveness (Mean replacement method)
Descriptive Statistics
Mean Std. Deviation N
EIStotal 12.5000 3.91354 20
EIStotalT2 11.1000 4.29075 20
EIStotalT3 9.8230 3.55758 20
EIStotalT4 8.9290 4.08417 20
Descriptive Statistics
Mean Std. Deviation N
ABCL Full Scale 55.80 8.141 20
Worker
T2ABCLtot 52.50 9.498 20
T3ABCLtot 52.3325 7.34608 20
Multivariate Testsb
Effect Partial Eta
Value F Hypothesis df Error df Sig. Squared
factor1 Pillai's Trace .288 3.644a 2.000 18.000 .047 .288
Wilks' Lambda .712 3.644a 2.000 18.000 .047 .288
Hotelling's Trace .405 3.644a 2.000 18.000 .047 .288
Roy's Largest Root .405 3.644a 2.000 18.000 .047 .288
313
ABCL- Internalizing (Mean replacement method)
Descriptive Statistics
Mean Std. Deviation N
ABCL Internalizing 55.15 10.122 20
T2ABCLint 51.25 8.675 20
T3ABCLint 51.8000 9.21783 20
Descriptive Statistics
Mean Std. Deviation N
ABCL Externalizing 58.85 7.191 20
T2ABCLext 57.38 10.285 20
T3ABCLext 56.0000 9.69536 20
Mauchly's Test of Sphericityb
Measure:MEASURE_1
Within Subjects Effect Epsilona
Approx. Chi- Greenhouse-
Mauchly's W Square df Sig. Geisser Huynh-Feldt Lower-bound
dimension1 factor1 .721 5.891 2 .053 .782 .839 .500
Descriptive Statistics
Mean Std. Deviation N
ABCL Aggression 56.95 6.832 20
T2ABCLagg 57.25 8.006 20
T3ABCLagg 56.3325 7.59270 20
Multivariate Testsb
Effect Partial Eta
Value F Hypothesis df Error df Sig. Squared
factor1 Pillai's Trace .017 .156a 2.000 18.000 .857 .017
Wilks' Lambda .983 .156a 2.000 18.000 .857 .017
Hotelling's Trace .017 .156a 2.000 18.000 .857 .017
Roy's Largest Root .017 .156a 2.000 18.000 .857 .017
316
Descriptive Statistics
Mean Std. Deviation N
ABCL Rule Breaking 64.25 10.818 20
T2ABCLrule 61.75 6.568 20
T3ABCLrule 60.9325 7.48660 20
Mauchly's Test of Sphericityb
Measure:MEASURE_1
Within Subjects Effect Epsilona
Approx. Chi- Greenhouse-
Mauchly's W Square df Sig. Geisser Huynh-Feldt Lower-bound
dimension1 factor1 .951 .907 2 .635 .953 1.000 .500
Tests of Within-Subjects Effects
Measure:MEASURE_1
Source Type III Sum Partial Eta
of Squares df Mean Square F Sig. Squared
factor1 Sphericity Assumed 119.494 2 59.747 1.425 .253 .070
Greenhouse-Geisser 119.494 1.906 62.683 1.425 .254 .070
Huynh-Feldt 119.494 2.000 59.747 1.425 .253 .070
Lower-bound 119.494 1.000 119.494 1.425 .247 .070
Error(factor1) Sphericity Assumed 1593.502 38 41.934
Greenhouse-Geisser 1593.502 36.220 43.995
Huynh-Feldt 1593.502 38.000 41.934
317
Table E1
Intercorrelations among all dependent measures at T2 (N=20)
AQ -phy -ang -hos -ind -ver HIT -sc -bo -mm -aw -l -s -od -pa SPSI -ppo -npo -rps -ics -as I7
AQ -
- phy .88** -
- ang .87** .84** -
- hos .64** .34. .43* -
- ind .82** .58** .52* .59** -
- ver .81** .61** .67** .31 .74** -
HIT .62** .65** .65** .49* .37 .26 -
- sc .50* .53** .53** .47** .29 .16 .95** -
- bo .54** .60** .62** .42* .30 .17 .96** .91** -
- mm .67** .70** .68** .50* .45* .31 .95** .85** .87** -
- aw .63** .65** .66** .44* .35 .34 .97** .90** .90** .91** -
-l .56** .48* .54** .70** .40* .18 .56** .80** .75** .87** .80** -
-s .48* .50* .54** .42* .34 .12 .90** .89** .93** .90** .89** .82** -
- od .56** .60** .63** .40* .25 .29 .93** .89** .92** .81** .93** .67** .82** -
- pa .63** .76** .66** .28 .33 .36 .90** .86** .85** .87** .87** .60** .76** .85** -
SPSI -.22 -.04 -.24 -.55** -.17 .09 -.38 -.33 -.39* -.31 -.40* -.45* -.49* -.38* -.07 -
- ppo .18 .14 -.07 -.06 .34 .43* -.30 -.26 -.32 -.23 -.33 -.26 -.44* -.29 -.09 .68** -
- npo .40* .09 .33 .72** .40* .23 .16 .12 .11 .19 .17 .40* .15 .11 -.03 -.68** -.18 -
- rps -.05 -.14 -.12 -.14 .02 .25 -.52** -.44* -.50* -.51* -.56* -.47* -.66** -.48* -.29 .70** .66** -.10 -
- ics .52* .32 .43* .44* .53** .46* .27 .25 .32 .21 .27 .23 .31 .34 .12 -.54** -.04 .33 -.25 -
- as .16 -.11 .05 .60** .25 -.06 .07 .10 .12 -.02 .08 .21 .13 .10 -.15 -.82** -.31 .80** -.26 .47* -
I7 .25 .18 .12 .36 .32 .02 .49* .43* .53** .45* .45* .39* .60** .46* .31 -.47* -.28 .10 -.48* .51* .28 -
Note. All 1-tailed correlations; *p<.05, **p<.01
AQ = Aggression Questionnaire; phy = Physical Aggression, ang = Anger, hos = Hostility, ind = Indirect Aggression, ver = Verbal Aggression; HIT = How I Think
Questionnaire: sc = Self-Centred, bo = Blaming Others, mm = Minimising/Mislabelling, aw = assuming the Worst, l = Lying, S = Stealing, od = Oppositional Defiant, pa =
Physical Aggression; SPSI = Social Problem Solving Inventory-Revised: Short Form: ppo = Positive Problem Orientation, npo = Negative Problem Orientation, rps =
Rational Problem Solving, ics = Impulsive/Careless Style, as = Avoidance Style; I7 = Impulsiveness Scale
318
Table E2
Descriptives and main effect ANOVA results from T1 to T4 on the AQ (LOCF method)
Pre (T1) Post (T2) 6-month (T3) 24-month (T4) Main Effect
AQ Total 102.25 (26.00) 89.35 (24.51) 88.45 (20.97) 86.30 (25.13) 4.71 .005 .20
Physical 26.85 (8.86) 21.40 (9.00) 21.05 (7.36) 20.14 (7.49) 6.16 .001 .25
Anger 19.41 (5.04) 18.23 (5.79) 18.05 (4.86) 16.90 (6.32) 2.12* .13 ns
Hostility 21.60 (7.69) 18.10 (5.76) 17.95 (5.40) 19.40 (6.22) 3.71 .01 .16
Indirect 16.55 (4.97) 14.45 (4.48) 15.35 (3.99) 14.60 (4.50) 5.49* .008 .49
Verbal 16.40 (3.80) 16.70 (4.84) 16.05 (4.05) 14.75 (4.25) 1.74* .20 ns
Note. AQ = Aggression Questionnaire; Physical = Physical Aggression; Indirect = Indirect Aggression; Verbal = Verbal Aggression; * = Multivariate
Wilks’ Lambda (df = 3,17) reported due to lack of sphericity; ns = non-significant
319
Table E3
Descriptives and main effect ANOVA results from T1 to T4 on the HIT (LOCF method)
Pre (T1) Post (T2) 6-month (T3) 24-month (T4) Main Effect
HIT Total 3.17 (.70) 2.78 (.76) 2.48 (.93) 2.31 (.76) 8.07 .000 .30
Self-Centered 3.18 (.83) 2.79 (.77) 2.63 (.88) 2.42 (.83) 5.04 .004 .21
Blaming Others 3.04 (.76) 2.73 (.80) 2.54 (.93) 2.26 (.76) 5.56 .002 .23
Minimizing/ Mislabel 3.22 (.89) 2.80 (.90) 2.44 (1.07) 2.24 (.76) 8.40 .000 .31
Assuming the Worst 3.21 (.68) 2.78 (.73) 2.56 (.86) 2.49 (.76) 6.15 .001 .24
Lying 3.28 (.79) 2.82 (.84) 2.68 (.91) 2.61 (.81) 6.58 .001 .26
Stealing 2.76 (1.04) 2.50 (.84) 2.26 (.99) 1.97 (.85) 4.85 .005 .20
Oppositional Defiant 3.47 (.72) 3.04 (.79) 2.72 (.95) 2.50 (.80) 9.06 .000 .32
Physical Aggression 3.20 (.77) 2.78 (.89) 2.55 (1.00) 2.15 (.72) 9.41 .000 .33
320
Table E4
Descriptives and main effect ANOVA results from T1 to T4 on the SPSI (LOCF method)
Pre (T1) Post (T2) 6-month (T3) 24-month (T4) Main Effect
M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) F(3,57) p Partial
SPSI Total 11.28 (2.80) 12.72 (2.94) 12.48 (2..60) 12.89 (2.87) 3.80 .01 .17
PPO 12.45 (4.62) 13.95 (4.44) 14.95 (3.12) 15.05 (4.12) 3.36 .03 ns
NPO 12.50 (4.74) 13.55 (3.49) 12.90 (3.96) 13.60 (3.84) .50 .68
ns
RPS 8.65 (3.77) 10.35 (4.77) 10.65 (3.90) 11.40 (5.28) 2.39 .07
ns
ICS 11.30 (3.37) 12.10 (3.18) 11.50 (4.55) 11.20 (4.25) .34 .79
ns
AS 11.50 (6.24) 13.65 (5.57) 12.40 (4.91) 13.20 (4.21) 1.57 .21
ns
Note. SPSI = Social Problem Solving Inventory; PPO = Positive Problem Orientation; NPO = Negative Problem Orientation; RPS = Rational
Problem Solving; ICS = Impulsive/Careless Style; AS = Avoidance Style; ns = non-significant
321
Table E5
Descriptives and main effect ANOVA results from T1 to T4 for Impulsiveness (LOCF method)
Pre (T1) Post (T2) 6-month (T3) 24-month (T4) Main Effect
M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) F(3,57) p Partial
Impulsiveness 12.50 (3.91) 11.10 (4.29) 10.10 (3.68) 9.35 (4.88) 4.05 .01 .18
322
Table E6
T-score descriptivs and main effect ANOVAS from T1 to T3 for the ABCL (LOCF method)
ABCL Total 55.80 (8.14) 52.50 (9.50) 53.15 (8.86) 1.66 .20
ns
Aggression 56.95 (6.83) 57.25 (8.01) 57.60 (8.80) .10 .91
ns
Rule Breaking 64.25 (10.82) 61.75 (6.57) 61.60 (8.64) 1.03 .39
ns
Internalizing 55.15 (10.82) 51.25 (8.68) 51.80 (10.07) 1.66 .20
ns
Externalizing 58.85 (7.19) 57.38 (10.29) 57.35 (11.56) .39 .40
ns