Is Bad Stronger Than Good?
Is Bad Stronger Than Good?
www.emeraldinsight.com/0143-7739.htm
LODJ
37,6
Leadership: is bad stronger
than good?
Maria Fors Brandebo and Sofia Nilsson
690 Department of Security, Strategy and Leadership,
Swedish Defence University, Karlstad, Sweden, and
Received 24 September 2014 Gerry Larsson
Revised 17 April 2015
25 August 2015
Department of Security, Strategy and Leadership,
27 August 2015 Swedish Defence University, Karlstad, Sweden and
Accepted 31 August 2015
Department of Public Health, Hedmark University College,
Elverum, Norway
Abstract
Purpose – The purpose of this paper is to investigate if the thesis “bad is stronger than good” also
holds true for a number of leadership issues, more specifically: trust in the immediate leader, emotional
exhaustion, work atmosphere and propensity to leave.
Design/methodology/approach – Questionnaire responses were obtained from military personnel
in Estonia, Sweden, Switzerland and the Netherlands (n ¼ 625).
Findings – Multiple regression analyses revealed a certain pattern. Constructive leadership
behaviours showed stronger positive associations with trust in the immediate supervisor and work
atmosphere, than destructive leadership behaviours showed negative associations. On the other
hand, destructive leadership behaviours showed stronger positive associations with emotional
exhaustion and propensity to leave, than constructive leadership behaviours showed negative
associations. This suggests that constructive leadership behaviours possibly have a greater
impact on positive phenomenon and/or phenomenon associated with work-related relationships.
On the other hand, destructive leadership behaviours appear to have a greater impact on negative
phenomena with a stronger personal meaning. The results also show that the passive forms of
destructive leadership are the behaviours that had the strongest impact on the investigated
dependent variables.
Research limitations/implications – Limitations related to item construction, common method
variance, response set tendencies, translation of the instruments, and lack of response rate are
discussed.
Practical implications – The results emphasize the importance of focusing on both constructive and
destructive leadership at the selection stage, as well as during training of military leaders. Focusing on
them separately obstructs optimal leader development and prevents leaders from gaining authentic
self-knowledge. The results also point at the importance of including both aspects of leadership in
leader evaluation processes.
Originality/value – The use of both constructive and destructive leadership behaviours with
respondents from multiple nations in the same analysis.
Keywords Military, Trust, Emotional exhaustion, Constructive leadership, Destructive leadership,
Propensity to leave, Work atmosphere
Paper type Research paper
Work atmosphere
Soldiers and officers often work closely together under stressful and demanding
conditions. In order to maintain one’s spirits and be capable of functioning, a good work
LODJ atmosphere is of utmost importance. Work atmosphere has not been particularly
37,6 highlighted as an outcome of destructive leadership. However, research shows that
passive leadership may create a work climate characterized by frustration and stress,
resulting in anti-social behaviour in work groups (Einarsen, 1999; Leymann, 1996).
Laissez-faire leadership has also shown positive correlations with experiences of
workplace stressors such as role conflicts and role ambiguity, leading to conflicting
694 expectations. Further, laissez-faire leadership has been found to have a high correlation
with high conflict levels in working groups, which can be assumed to affect the work
atmosphere in the organization (Hinkin and Schriesheim, 2008; Skogstad et al., 2007).
Previous research also suggests that the relationship between laissez-faire leadership
and workplace stressors are mainly explained by the presence of laissez-faire
leadership, not by the lack of constructive leadership (Skogstad et al., 2007). Given this,
we assume that:
H2. Destructive leadership behaviours show stronger negative associations
with workplace atmosphere than constructive leadership behaviours show
positive associations.
H3. Passive destructive leadership behaviours show stronger negative associations
with workplace atmosphere than active destructive leadership behaviours.
Emotional exhaustion
In military contexts, mental endurance and strength are paramount to preserving
battle fitness, making this an important issue to investigate. The relationship between
destructive leadership and individual-related consequences such as stress
and well-being is well documented (Burris et al., 2008; Chen and Kao, 2009; Schyns
and Schilling, 2013; Tepper, 2000). To illustrate, destructive leadership has been
suggested to lead to subordinates expressing emotional exhaustion (Chi and Liang,
2013; Harvey et al., 2007; Tepper, 2000). This may have negative organization-related
consequences since individuals who feel emotionally exhausted tend to withdraw from
their job in order to prevent further resource depletion (Chi and Liang, 2013). Based on
the principle that bad is stronger than good, and the meta-analytical finding that
reveals higher correlations between destructive leadership and well-being compared to
constructive leadership and well-being (Schyns and Schilling, 2013), we therefore
suggest that:
H4. Destructive leadership behaviours show stronger positive associations with
subordinates’ emotional exhaustion than constructive leadership behaviours
show negative associations.
Method
Participants and procedure
Questionnaire responses were obtained from military personnel in Estonia, Sweden,
Switzerland and the Netherlands attending courses at their respective national defence
academies. Before giving informed consent, all participants received oral and/or written
information about the study. The Estonian and Dutch data were collected in two ways:
during classroom settings; and online (by e-mail). The Swedish and Swiss data were
collected during ordinary classroom settings. Participants responded anonymously.
The response rate was 100 per cent for the Estonian sample, 90 per cent for the Dutch
sample and 62 per cent for the Swedish sample. The response rate for the Swiss sample
is unknown. The number of participants was originally 625. Due to handling of missing
values, the final number of participants was 533. The handling of missing values was
conducted as follows. On the main instrument, the Destrudo-L, participants with two or
more missing values on each of the four-item factor scales were dropped. For those with
Outcome measures
Leadership Emotional
behaviour Trust Work atmosphere exhaustion Propensity to leave
Measures
The questionnaire was originally developed in Swedish and translated into English.
Contacts in each participating country translated the questionnaire from English into
their respective native language.
Emotional stability. Data were collected using the one item from the single-item
measure of personality (Woods and Hampson, 2005) designed to measure emotional
stability (neuroticism reversed) dimension/factor in the Big Five model of personality
(Costa and McCrae, 1992): how much does each description sound like you? Generally,
I come across as: someone who is sensitive and excitable, and can be tense. Someone
who is relaxed, unemotional, rarely gets irritated and seldom feels blue. The factor/item
is measured on a nine-point, bipolar graded line.
Knowledge of the leader. Knowledge of the leader was measured using the item
“I know my immediate supervisor/commander this well”. The scale ranged from 1,
“Not at all” to 6, “Very well”.
Destructive leadership behaviours. The Destrudo-L, a 20-item questionnaire developed
by Larsson et al. (2012) was used to measure destructive leadership behaviours.
The questionnaire consists of five factors with four items in each: arrogant, unfair (sample
item “Treats people differently”), Cronbach α: 0.75; threats, punishments, overdemands
(sample item “Uses threats to get his/her way”), Cronbach α: 0.69; ego oriented, false
(sample item “Does not keep promises”), Cronbach α: 0.78; passive, cowardly (sample item
“Does not show an active interest”), Cronbach α: 0.75; and uncertain, unclear, messy
(sample item “Is bad at structuring and planning”) Cronbach α: 0.78.
Constructive leadership behaviours. To measure constructive leadership behaviours,
21 items forming three factors (exemplary model, individualized consideration, and
inspiration and motivation) from the Developmental Leadership Questionnaire (DLQ)
(Larsson, 2006) were used. As no specific hypothesis was developed regarding the three
factors, a mean score based on all 21 items was used. The Cronbach α coefficient for
the scale was 0.96. Sample items: “Acts in accordance with the opinions he or she
expresses”, “Takes time to listen” and “Creates enthusiasm for a task”. The response
scale on the original questionnaire ranges from 1 to 9. However, in order to be able to
Estonia The Netherlandsb Switzerland Sweden
n ¼ 41 n ¼ 319 n ¼ 68 n ¼ 105
Background characteristics n % M SD n % M SD n % M SD n % M SD χ2/F pa
Gender
Women 5 12 14 4 3 4 15 14
Men 36 88 305 96 65 96 89 86 14.63 0.002
Age
p29 years 4 10 53 17 28 41 36 35
X30 years 37 90 266 83 40 59 68 65 32.25 0.000
Civilian education
Secondary school/high school 8 20 180 56 18 27 45 43
College/university 33 80 139 44 50 73 59 57 35.88 0.000
Branch of service
Army 30 73 52 77 43 42
Navy 1 2 1 2 30 29
Air force 9 22 11 16 29 28 47.22 0.000
Sex of rated leader
Male 47 90 314 98 68 100 97 92
Female 4 10 5 2 0 0 8 8 17.36 0.001
Emotional stability 5.37 1.51 6.45 1.71 5.55 1.69 6.39 1.89 0.000
Knowledge of rated leader 3.61 1.12 3.68 1.19 4.03 1.18 3.79 1.24 ns
Notes: aA Bonferroni correction was applied. All differences are at a 0.008 level of significance; bInformation on branch of service is lacking for the Dutch
sample
the participants
bad stronger
than good?
Basic description of
Table II.
697
Leadership: is
LODJ mix items from Destrudo-L and DLQ on the same scoring sheet, the DLQ response scale
37,6 was modified. Thus, the response scale on all items ranged from 1, “Never or almost
never” to 6, “Very often or always”.
Trust. Trust was measured with two items: “How high is your trust in your
superior/commander’s individual characteristics?” and “How high is your trust in
your superior/commander as a leader?” The items were formulated based on a study
698 on trust in military leaders (Fors Brandebo and Larsson, 2012). The scale ranged
from 1, “low” to 6, “high”. The Cronbach α coefficient for this scale was 0.91.
Work atmosphere. Work atmosphere was measured with four items based on a
previous study on military personnel’s motivation to continue in the armed forces
(Larsson et al., 2007). Sample items included: “I’m getting on well with my colleagues”
and “I feel that we have a nice atmosphere at work”. The scale ranged from 1, “Do not
agree” to 6, “Agree totally”. The Cronbach α coefficient for the scale was 0.78.
Emotional exhaustion. In order to measure emotional exhaustion, nine items from
the Maslach Burnout Inventory (Maslach et al., 1996) was used. Sample items included:
“I feel emotionally drained from my work” and “Working with people all day is really a
strain for me”. The respondents estimated the frequency on a scale from 1, “a few times
a year” to 6, “every day”. The Cronbach α coefficient for the scale was 0.88.
Propensity to leave the profession. Propensity to leave the armed forces was
measured with four items based on a previous study on military personnel’s motivation
to continue in the armed forces (Larsson et al., 2007). Sample items included: “I have
plans to change employer”, and “I will quit shortly”. The scale ranged from 1, “Do not
agree” to 6, “Agree totally”. The Cronbach α coefficient for the scale was 0.73.
Statistics
Dimensional analysis of the 20 destructive leadership behaviour items based on the
covariance matrix was performed using structural equation modelling (SEM) with
maximum likelihood estimates. The software Amos was used. The five-factor model
obtained in the original Destrudo-L (Larsson et al., 2012) was used as point of departure.
Acceptable model fit was determined at a root mean square error of approximation
(RMSEA) of 0.08.
Subgroup comparisons were performed using χ2 tests, t-tests and one-way analyses
of variance. Pearson correlation coefficients were computed to assess bivariate
associations between variables. Statistical significance was assumed at p o 0.05.
Hierarchical multiple regression analyses were used to test the five hypotheses.
Separate analyses were performed for each of the four dependent variables: trust, work
atmosphere, emotional exhaustion and propensity to leave. In order to reduce the
number of independent variables, each of the individual background variables and
rated leader background variables were individually tested against each of the four
dependent variables (t-tests and one-way analyses of variance, except for emotional
stability and knowledge of the leader, where bivariate correlations were computed).
No statistically significant differences were found on any of the t-tests or one-way
analyses of variance, while emotional stability and knowledge of the leader both
correlated statistically significantly with each of the four dependent variables.
Following from this, it was decided to keep these two independent variables and drop
the other individual background variables and rated leader background variables.
For each of the four regression analyses a few additional participants were omitted
as they were deemed as outliers ( W 3.0 SD). Table IV shows the final number of cases
for each of the dependent variables. The independent variables were entered in the Leadership: is
following order: emotional stability; knowledge of the leader; nationality (dummy bad stronger
variables: Swiss, Swedish, Estonia, Dutch); the five destructive leadership scales; and
the constructive leadership scale.
than good?
Ethics
All participants were treated in accordance with the ethical principles of human 699
research formulated by the Swedish Research Council (2002).
Results
Dimensionality analysis of the Destrudo-L
The goodness-of-fit between two different models and the empirical outcome was
tested. The first included the five factors obtained in the original development of the
Destrudo-L (see the Methods section above). The outcome was an RMSEA of 0.082.
A second test was run where the following specified covariances between factors were
added: arrogant, unfair and threats, punishments, overdemands; passive, cowardly and
uncertain, unclear, messy; and ego oriented, false and all the other four factors.
The goodness-of-fit of this model and the empirical outcome was acceptable, an
RMSEA of 0.074 was obtained (90 per cent confidence interval 0.068-0.080).
Other common goodness-of-fit indices included a normed fit index of 0.857, a
goodness-of-fit index of 0.903 and an adjusted goodness-of-fit index of 0.876. The factor
loadings of all items but three were 0.60 or higher. In summary, the confirmative factor
analysis yielded a result which was considered as good enough to proceed with the
remaining statistical analysis using the original five-factor structure. As this SEM
analysis was not the main aim of the present study, no tables will be provided in addition
to the reported findings. Detailed results can be obtained directly from the authors.
Descriptive statistics
Table III illustrates that the destructive leadership scales generally have low means,
while the constructive leadership scale have a high mean. Two of the dependent
variables – emotional exhaustion and propensity to leave the profession – have
particularly low mean scores.
The bivariate correlations within each of the two sets of leadership scales are high
and there are strong negative correlations between the two kinds of scales.
The bivariate correlations within the four dependent variables are modest to strong.
Between these variables and the two sets of leadership scales, the bivariate correlations
are modest to strong for trust in the supervisor and work atmosphere and low
to modest for emotional exhaustion and propensity to leave. All are in the
expected direction.
700
LODJ
Table III.
deviations and
Means, standard
observed variables
correlations for the
Scale 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 M SD
a
1. Arrogant, unfair 0.75 0.59 0.60 0.42 0.52 −0.46 −0.45 −0.36 0.28 0.26 −0.19 −0.19 2.31 1.02
2. Threats, punishments, over-demandsa 0.69 0.58 0.44 0.49 −0.37 −0.34 −0.29 0.28 0.20 −0.19 −0.08 1.99 0.86
3. Ego oriented, falsea 0.78 0.59 0.63 −0.59 −0.52 −0.37 0.32 0.22 −0.20 −0.18 2.20 1.10
4. Passive, cowardlya 0.75 0.65 −0.58 −0.53 −0.39 0.34 0.26 −0.19 −0.16 2.20 1.06
5. Uncertain, unclear, messya 0.78 −0.63 −0.58 −0.41 0.35 0.36 −0.12 −0.18 2.15 1.04
6. Constructive leadershipa 0.96 0.72 0.52 −0.29 −0.26 0.21 0.34 4.36 0.91
7. Trust in the supervisora 0.90 0.58 −0.31 −0.27 0.16 0.33 4.27 1.23
8. Work atmospherea 0.77 −0.45 −0.38 0.12 0.27 4.69 0.81
9. Emotional exhaustiona 0.87 0.36 −0.20 −0.08 1.93 0.77
10. Propensity to leave the professiona 0.72 0.06 −0.14 1.85 0.94
11. Emotional stabilityc,d − 0.01 6.24 1.77
12. Knowledge of rated leadera,d − 3.74 1.20
Notes: n ¼ 625. aThe scale ranged from 1 to 6; bThe numbers in italic refers to Cronbach α scores; cThe scale ranged from 1 to 9; dControl variables
Trust in the superior Work atmosphere
Leadership: is
β final model SE β final model SE bad stronger
n ¼ 525 n ¼ 525 than good?
Step 1
Emotional stability 0.001 0.018 −0.008 0.016
Adj R2 0.025 0.013
R2 change 0.022* 0.015* 701
Step 2
Knowledge of the leader 0.041 0.028 0.071 0.025
Adj R2 0.140 0.105
R2 change 0.116* 0.093*
Step 3
Nationality, Swiss 0.269* 0.100 0.195* 0.089
Nationality, Swedish 0.171* 0.081 −0.041 0.073
Nationality, Estonia 0.193 0.119 0.180 0.105
Nationality, Dutch Reference Reference Reference Reference
Adj R2 0.138 0.104
R2 change 0.003 0.005
Step 4
Arrogant, unfair −0.150* 0.045 −0.040 0.040
Threats, punishments overdemands 0.094 0.052 −0.064 0.045
Ego oriented, false −0.070 0.045 0.006 0.040
Passive, cowardly −0.080 0.042 −0.090* 0.037
Uncertain, unclear, messy −0.126* 0.047 −0.018 0.042
Adj R2 0.524 0.290
R2 change 0.387* 0.191*
Step 5
Constructive leadership 0.875* 0.054 0.323 0.050
Adj R2 0.686 0.342
R2 change 0.160* 0.053*
Total R2 0.693 0.356
Emotional exhaustion Propensity to leave
β final model SE β final model SE
n ¼ 520 n ¼ 519
Step 1
Emotional stability −0.056* 0.016 0.003 0.022
Adj R2 0.041 0.002
R2 change 0.043* 0.004
Step 2
Knowledge of the leader −0.032 0.024 −0.018 0.034
Adj R2 0.054 0.018
R2 change 0.014* 0.017*
Step 3
Nationality, Swiss −0.099 0.086 −0.119 0.124
Nationality, Swedish 0.283* 0.070 −0.141 0.100
Nationality, Estonia 0.047 0.101 0.336* 0.145
Nationality, Dutch Reference Reference Reference Reference Table IV.
Multiple regression
(continued ) analysis
LODJ Adj R2 0.069 0.037
37,6 R2 change 0.020* 0.025*
Step 4
Arrogant, unfair 0.021 0.039 0.122* 0.055
Threats, punishments overdemands 0.090* 0.044 −0.022 0.063
Ego oriented, false 0.027 0.039 −0.082 0.056
702 Passive, cowardly 0.092* 0.036 0.045 0.051
Uncertain, unclear, messy 0.103* 0.041 0.257* 0.057
Adj R2 0.191 0.155
R2 change 0.129* 0.125*
Step 5
Constructive leadership 0.040 0.045 −0.053 0.065
Adj R2 0.191 0.154
R2 change 0.001 0.001
Total R2 0.208 0.172
Table IV. Note: *Statistically significant at p o0.05
nationality and knowledge of the leader. The constructive leadership factor explained
an additional 16.0 per cent of trust in the immediate supervisor after controlling for
emotional stability, nationality, knowledge of the leader and destructive leadership
(R2 changes if entering constructive leadership before the destructive leadership behaviours:
51.7 and 3.0 per cent, respectively). In the final model, the following variables had
statistically significant β values: constructive leadership ( β ¼ 0.875, po0.001), arrogant,
unfair ( β ¼ −0.150, po0.001), nationality, Swiss ( β ¼ 0.269, po0.007), uncertain, unclear,
messy ( β ¼ 0.126, po0.008) and nationality, Swedish ( β ¼ 0.171, po0.036). H1 could
therefore be said to be supported.
Work atmosphere. The regression equation of the final model was statistically
significant (F ¼ 25.8, p o 0.001) and the adjusted R2 was 0.34. The destructive
leadership factors explained an additional 19.1 per cent of the variance in work
atmosphere, after controlling for emotional stability, nationality and knowledge of the
leader. Constructive leadership explained an additional 5.3 per cent of work atmosphere
after controlling for emotional stability, knowledge of the leader, nationality and
destructive leadership (R2 changes if entering constructive leadership behaviours
before destructive: 22.1 and 2.3 per cent, respectively). In the final model, the following
variables had statistically significant β values: constructive leadership ( β ¼ 0.323,
p o 0.001), knowledge of the leader ( β ¼ 0.071, p o 0.004), passive, cowardly
( β ¼ −0.090, p o 0.016) and nationality: Swiss ( β ¼ 0.195, p o 0.029). H2 was
therefore not supported. Since the only destructive leadership scale that gave a
significant contribution was passive, cowardly, H3 was supported.
Emotional exhaustion. The regression equation of the final model was statistically
significant (F ¼ 12.13, p o 0.001) and the adjusted R2 was 0.19. The destructive
leadership factors explained an additional 12.9 per cent of the variance in emotional
exhaustion, after controlling for emotional stability, knowledge of the leader and
nationality. Constructive leadership explained 0 per cent of emotional exhaustion after
controlling for emotional stability, knowledge of the leader, nationality and destructive
leadership (R2 changes if entering constructive leadership behaviours before
destructive: 4.9 and 8.2 per cent, respectively). In the final model, the following
variables had statistically significant β values: nationality, Swedish ( β ¼ 0.283, Leadership: is
p o 0.001), emotional stability ( β ¼ −0.056, p o 0.001), passive, cowardly ( β ¼ 0.092, bad stronger
p o 0.011), uncertain, unclear, messy ( β ¼ 0.103, p o 0.012) and threats, punishments, than good?
overdemands ( β ¼ 0.090, p o 0.041), supporting H4, that destructive leadership
behaviours show stronger positive associations with emotional exhaustion than
constructive leadership behaviours show negative associations, although the adjusted
R2 in the final model is only modestly high. 703
Propensity to leave. The regression equation of the final model was statistically
significant (F ¼ 9.59, p o 0.001) and the adjusted R2 was 0.15. The destructive
leadership factors explained an additional 12.5 per cent of the variance in work
motivation, after controlling for emotional stability, knowledge of the leader and
nationality. Constructive leadership explained an additional 0.00 per cent of work
motivation after controlling for emotional stability, knowledge of the leader, nationality
and destructive leadership (R2 changes if entering constructive leadership behaviours
before destructive: 6.5 and 6.0 per cent, respectively). In the final model, following
variables had statistically significant β values: Uncertain, unclear, messy ( β ¼ 0.257,
p o 0.001), nationality, Estonia ( β ¼ 0.336, p o 0.021) and arrogant, unfair ( β ¼ 0.122,
p o 0.029), contradicting H5.
Discussion
The aim of this study was to investigate if the hypothesis “bad is stronger than good”
also holds true for a number of leadership outcomes, more specifically trust in the
immediate leader, work atmosphere, emotional exhaustion and propensity to leave.
Based on previous research, five hypotheses were formulated. In line with the results
from Schyns and Schilling (2013), our findings are somewhat mixed. Below we discuss
our findings for each hypothesis. First, however, we conclude that the dimensional
analysis of the Destrudo-L using SEM confirmed the original five-factor structure and
justified the following analyses.
Regarding trust in the immediate supervisor, H1 was partly supported.
The correlation with trust in the immediate supervisor was higher for constructive
leadership compared to destructive leadership and it was also the constructive
leadership factor that contributed most to the final model. Concerning work
atmosphere, our results did not support H2. Passive, cowardly was the only destructive
leadership factor that gave a significant contribution to the model, supporting H3, that
the passive destructive leadership behaviours show stronger negative association with
work atmosphere than active destructive leadership behaviours. Moving on to
emotional exhaustion, besides nationality (Swedish) and emotional stability, the
destructive leadership factors passive, cowardly, uncertain, unclear, messy and threats,
punishments, overdemands gave significant contributions to the model leading to the
conclusion that H4 was supported. Finally, regarding propensity to leave
the profession, H5 was also contradicted, since the only significant contribution was
noted for nationality (Estonia) and the destructive leadership factors uncertain, unclear,
messy and arrogant, unfair. To summarize, the knowledge gained by previous research
contributed poorly to our hypotheses. In the following, we will discuss potential
explanations for our findings.
Obviously, one possible explanation for these findings may be statistical.
The factors emotional exhaustion and propensity to leave the profession had skewed
response distributions. A majority of the respondents estimated their emotional
LODJ exhaustion and their propensity to leave as low. In contrast, the factors trust in the
37,6 immediate supervisor and work atmosphere were also skewed, although not as much:
most individuals gave high ratings for trust in their leader and work atmosphere.
However, if the results are not just a statistical effect, we would also like to suggest
alternative explanations for our findings. First, a closer look at our four dependent
variables reveals a certain pattern. Constructive leadership behaviours covaried more
704 strongly with trust in the immediate supervisor and work atmosphere, while destructive
leadership behaviours were more strongly associated with emotional exhaustion and
propensity to leave. Trust in the immediate leader and work atmosphere can be regarded
as positive phenomena while emotional exhaustion and propensity to leave can be
considered as negative phenomena. The first two are positively formulated and the two
latter negatively. These two pairs of phenomena also have another common
denominator: trust in the immediate supervisor and work atmosphere have an
external focus and are work-related concepts, i.e. the items mainly focus on others who
are significant in one’s working life. Likewise, emotional exhaustion and propensity to
leave express a more personal meaning, i.e. they estimate the individual’s feelings and
intentions. This suggests that constructive leadership behaviours possibly have a greater
impact on positive phenomenon and/or phenomenon associated with work-related
relationships. On the other hand, destructive leadership behaviours appear to have a
greater impact on negative phenomena with a stronger personal meaning, which is in line
with previous studies. For example, Schyns and Schilling (2013) found that destructive
leadership showed higher negative correlations with commitment and well-being while
constructive leadership had higher positive correlations with attitude towards the leader
and job satisfaction.
The responses on the positive outcome variables were explained to a higher degree
by the predictor variables compared to the negatively formulated outcome variables.
Thus, 69 per cent of trust in the immediate supervisor and 34 per cent of experienced
work atmosphere was explained, compared to only 19 per cent on emotional exhaustion
and 15 per cent on propensity to leave the profession. This shows that more
person-oriented factors are mainly explained by other aspects. It also indicates the
leader’s limitations regarding influence on these aspects. They are mainly explained by
other phenomena that are not controlled for in this study. For instance, trust in leaders
have been suggested to be more determined by the leader’s behaviours than
demographics, personality and structural factors (Dirks, 2006), while research supports
that emotional exhaustion is not only influenced by leadership but also by other
aspects such as personal coping strategies, emotional culture and personal resources
(Grandey et al., 2005; Ito and Brotheridge, 2003).
Our results show that the passive forms of destructive leadership had a stronger
impact on our investigated dependent variables compared to the active forms, except
for trust in the immediate supervisor. One reason for why arrogant, unfair gave the
strongest contribution, of the destructive leadership factors, to trust in in the immediate
supervisor may be related to the fact that the factor can be said to express a lack of
both integrity and benevolence which has been suggested to be core components of
trust in leader (together with ability) (Mayer et al., 1995). Fairness (or lack of) has also
been shown to be associated with trust in leaders (Dirks and Ferrin, 2002).
Elaborating on why the passive forms appears to have a stronger impact on the
dependent variables, it can be argued that these behaviours most likely are intermittent
and therefore easier for the individual to deal with and have less effect on the daily
work. A leader who behaves arrogantly, is unfair and/or uses threats may have the
greatest negative effect on subordinates when he or she is present. In contrast, passive Leadership: is
behaviours can be considered as reflecting a more lasting attitude or having a more bad stronger
lasting effect on subordinates’ work climate. Passive behaviours have been found to be
more likely to cause frustration and problems (even when the leader is not present) in
than good?
terms of the leader creating a work climate characterized by uncertainty, role
ambiguity and conflicts (Einarsen, 1999; Leymann, 1996; Skogstad et al., 2007). These
behaviours are probably more subtle and difficult to detect. One could argue that 705
subordinates who score low on trust and high on emotional exhaustion and propensity
to leave are more inclined to rate the leader’s behaviour as destructive. However,
emotional stability is controlled for in step 1 of the regression analysis, indicating that
it is not emotionally instable individuals who account for these results.
Speculating on possible consequences of these findings, we would like to point to a
potential problem. The factors passive, cowardly and uncertain, unclear, messy, capture
behaviours related to the leader being absent, not showing active interest, giving unclear
instructions and behaving confused. These are behaviours that may be consequences of
leader-related antecedents such as work-related stress. Due to shortage of time, leaders
therefore may behave destructively and since one of our largest work life problems is
stress, this implies that many leaders who would normally not be prone to behaving
destructively, may fall into using passive destructive leader behaviours with
unfavourable consequences for subordinates’ health and performance.
Like previous results, our findings show modest to strong correlations between the
different destructive leadership factors and constructive leadership supporting the
finding that destructive and constructive leadership behaviours do not exist apart but
reflect integrated parts of leadership (Aasland et al., 2010). In order to understand leaders’
influence on different kinds of work- and organizational-related consequences, both
destructive and constructive leadership behaviours need to be taken into consideration.
Finally, regarding practical implications, our results emphasize the importance of
focusing on both constructive and destructive leadership at the selection stage, as well
as during training of military leaders. Focusing on them separately obstructs optimal
leader development and prevents leaders from gaining authentic self-knowledge. The
results also point at the importance of including both aspects of leadership in leader
evaluation processes.
Our results indicate further that destructive leadership has a stronger impact on
negative phenomena with a stronger personal meaning. Krasikova et al. (2013) suggest
that organizations are more prone to intervene when destructive leaders encourage
subordinates to pursue destructive goals than when they use destructive methods of
influence with followers. This can be valuable information for leaders as well as experts
who work in human resources. Destructive leaders can have severe negative impact on
individuals, leading to increased sick leave and/or turnover which, in turn, results in
negative consequences for the organization. Passive destructive behaviours may
perhaps be more difficult to discover compared to active forms which may lead to a less
chance of someone detecting and intervening. In this respect, it is important to create
structures for how the organization can detect and handle destructive leaders.
References
Aasland, M., Skogstad, A., Notelaers, G., Birkeland Nielsen, M. and Einarsen, S. (2010),
“The prevalence of destructive leadership behaviour”, British Journal of Management,
Vol. 21 No. 2, pp. 438-452.
Ashforth, B.E. (1994), “Petty tyranny in organizations”, Human Relations, Vol. 47 No. 7,
pp. 755-778.
Ashton, M.C., Jackson, D.N., Paunonen, S.V., Helmes, E. and Rothstein, M.G. (1995), “The criterion Leadership: is
validity of broad factor scales versus specific facet scales”, Journal of Research in
Personality, Vol. 29 No. 4, pp. 432-442.
bad stronger
than good?
Baumeister, R.F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C. and Vohs, K.E. (2001), “Bad is stronger than
good”, Review of General Psychology, Vol. 5 No. 4, pp. 323-370.
Belenky, G.L., Noy, S.L. and Solomon, Z. (1985), “Battle stress: the Israeli experience”,
Military Review, Vol. 65 No. 7, pp. 28-37. 707
Bilgiç, R. and Sümer, H.C. (2009), “Predicting military performance from specific personality
measures: a validity study”, International Journal of Selection and Assessment, Vol. 17
No. 2, pp. 231-238.
Burris, E.R., Detert, J.R. and Chiaburu, D.S. (2008), “Quitting before leaving: the mediating effects
of psychological attachment and detachment on voice”, Journal of Applied Psychology,
Vol. 93 No. 4, pp. 912-922.
Bycio, P., Hackett, R.D. and Allen, J.S. (1995), “Further assessments of Bass’s (1985)
conceptualization of transactional and transformational leadership”, Journal of Applied
Psychology, Vol. 80 No. 4, pp. 468-478.
Campbell, D.J. (2012), “Leadership in dangerous contexts”, in Laurence, J.H. and Matthews, M.D.
(Eds), The Oxford Handbook of Military Psychology, Oxford University Press, New York, NY,
pp. 158-175.
Chen, H.C. and Kao, H.S.R. (2009), “Chinese paternalistic leadership and non-Chinese
subordinates’ psychological health”, International Journal of Human Resource
Management, Vol. 20 No. 12, pp. 2533-2546.
Chi, S.-C.S. and Liang, S.-G. (2013), “When do subordinates’ emotion-regulation strategies matter?
Abusive supervision, subordinates’ emotional exhaustion, and work withdrawal”,
The Leadership Quarterly, Vol. 24 No. 4, pp. 125-137.
Clements, C. and Washbush, J.B. (1999), “The two faces of leadership: considering the dark side of
leader-follower dynamics”, Journal of Workplace Learning, Vol. 11 No. 5, pp. 170-175.
Collins, J.J. and Jacobs, T.O. (2002), “Trust in the profession of arms”, in Snider, D.M. and
Watkins, G.L. (Eds), The Future of the Army Profession, The McGraw-Hill, Boston, MA,
pp. 39-58.
Costa, P.T. Jr and McCrae, R.R. (1992), NEO PI-R: Professional Manual, Psychological
Assessment Resources, Odessa, FL.
Craig, B. and Kaiser, R.B. (2012), “Destructive leadership”, in Rumsey, M.D. (Ed.), The Oxford
Handbook of Leadership, Oxford University Press, Oxford, NY, pp. 439-454.
Dirks, K.T. (2006), “Three fundamental questions regarding trust in leaders”, in Bachman, R.
and Zaheer, A. (Eds.), Handbook of Trust Research, MPG Books Group, pp. 15-28.
Dirks, K.T. and Ferrin, D.L. (2002), “Trust in leadership: meta-analytic findings and implications
for research and practice”, Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 87 No. 4, pp. 611-628.
Duffy, M.K., Ganster, D.C. and Pagon, M. (2002), “Social undermining in the workplace”, Academy
of Management Journal, Vol. 45 No. 2, pp. 331-351.
Einarsen, S. (1999), “The nature and causes of bullying at work”, International Journal of
Manpower, Vol. 20 Nos 1/2, pp. 16-27.
Einarsen, S., Aasland, M. and Skogstad, A. (2007), “Destructive leadership behaviour: a definition
and conceptual model”, The Leadership Quarterly, Vol. 18 No. 3, pp. 207-216.
Fors Brandebo, M. and Larsson, G. (2012), “Influence of IED attacks on leadership: dealing with
the invisible enemy”, Res Militaris, Vol. 2 No. 3.
LODJ Frischer, J. and Larsson, K. (2000), “Laissez-faire in research education – an inquiry into a
Swedish doctoral program”, Higher Education Policy, Vol. 13 No. 2, pp. 132-155.
37,6
Glasø, L., Einarsen, S., Matthiesen, S.B. and Skogstad, A. (2010), “The dark side of leaders:
a representative study of interpersonal problems among leaders”, Scandinavian Journal of
Organizational Psychology, Vol. 2 No. 2, pp. 3-14.
Grandey, A.A., Fisk, G.M. and Steiner, D.D. (2005), “Must ‘service with a smile’ be stressful?
708 The moderating role of personal control for American and French employees”, The Journal
of Applied Psychology, Vol. 90 No. 5, pp. 893-904.
Hannah, S.T., Campbell, D.J. and Matthews, M.D. (2010), “Advancing a research agenda for
leadership in dangerous contexts”, Military Psychology, Vol. 22 No. S1, pp. 157-189.
Harvey, P., Stoner, J., Hochwarter, W. and Kacmar, C. (2007), “Coping with abusive supervision:
the neutralizing effects of ingratiation and positive affect on negative employee outcomes”,
The leadership Quarterly, Vol. 18 No. 3, pp. 264-280.
Hinkin, T.R. and Schriesheim, C.A. (2008), “An examination of ‘nonleadership’: from laissez-faire
leadership to leader reward omission and punishment omission”, Journal of Applied
Psychology, Vol. 93 No. 6, pp. 1234-1248.
Ito, J.K. and Brotheridge, C.M. (2003), “Resources, coping strategies, and emotional exhaustion:
a conservation of resources perspective”, Journal of Vocational Behavior, Vol. 63 No. 3,
pp. 490-509.
Jones, E., Massey Kantak, D., Futrell, C.M. and Johnston, M.W. (1996), “Leader behavior,
work-attitudes, and turnover of salespeople: an integrative study”, Journal of Personal
Selling & Sales Management, Vol. 16 No. 2, pp. 13-23.
Kant, L., Skogstad, A., Torsheim, T. and Einarsen, S. (2013), “Beware of the angry leader: trait
anger and trait anxiety as predictors of petty tyranny”, The Leadership Quarterly, Vol. 24
No. 1, pp. 106-124.
Kelloway, E.K., Sivanathan, N., Francis, L. and Barling, J. (2005), “Poor leadership”, in Barling, J.,
Kelloway, E.K. and Frone, M.R. (Eds), Handbook of Work Stress, Sage Publications,
Thousand Oaks, CA, pp. 89-112.
Kolditz, T.A. (2007), In Extremis Leadership: Leading as if Your Life Depended on it, Jossey-Bass,
San Francisco, CA.
Krasikova, D.V., Green, S.G. and LeBreton, J.M. (2013), “Destructive leadership: a theoretical
review, integration, and future research agenda”, Journal of Management, Vol. 39 No. 5,
pp. 1308-1338.
Larsson, G. (2006), “The developmental leadership questionnaire (DLQ): some psychometric
properties”, Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, Vol. 47 No. 4, pp. 253-262.
Larsson, G., Fors Brandebo, M. and Nilsson, S. (2012), “Destrudo-L: development of a short scale
designed to measure destructive leadership behaviours in a military context”, Leadership
& Organization Development Journal, Vol. 33 No. 4, pp. 383-400.
Larsson, G., Fors, M., Levin, A.-L. and Thuresson, A. (2007), Ledarskapets betydelse för värnpliktigas
och unga officerares motivation att fortsätta inom Försvarsmakten (The Importance of
Leadership to the Motivation of Conscripts and Young Officers to Continue in the Armed
Forces) (ILM Serie I:38), Försvarshögskolan, Institutionen för ledarskap och management,
Karlstad.
Larsson, G., Carlstedt, L., Andersson, J., Andersson, L., Danielsson, E., Johansson, A., Johansson, E.,
Robertsson, I. and Michel, P.-O. (2003), “A comprehensive system for leader evaluation and
development”, Leadership & Organization Development Journal, Vol. 24 No. 1, pp. 16-25.
Leymann, H. (1996), “The content and development of mobbing at work”, European Journal of
Work and Organizational Psychology, Vol. 5 No. 2, pp. 165-184.
Maslach, C., Jackson, S.E. and Leiter, M.P. (1996), Maslach Burnout Inventory Manual, 3rd ed., Leadership: is
Consulting Psychologists Press, Palo Alto, CA.
bad stronger
Mayer, R.C., Davis, J.H. and Schoorman, F.D. (1995), “An integrative model of organizational than good?
trust”, Academy of Management Review, Vol. 20 No. 3, pp. 709-734.
Mullen, J., Kelloway, E.K. and Teed, M. (2011), “Inconsistent style of leadership as a predictor of
safety behaviour”, Work & Stress, Vol. 25 No. 1, pp. 41-54.
Ness, J., Jablonski-Kaye, D., Obigt, I. and Lam, D.M. (2011), “Understanding and managing stress”, 709
in Sweeney, P.J., Matthews, M.D. and Lester, P.B. (Eds), Leadership in Dangerous
Situations. A Handbook for the Armed Forces, Emergency Services, and First Responders,
Naval Institute Press, Annapolis, MD, pp. 40-59.
Neuman, J.H. and Baron, R.M. (2005), “Aggression in the workplace: a social-psychological
perspective”, in Fox, S. and Spector, P.E. (Eds), Counterproductive Work
Behavior: Investigations of Actors and Targets, American Psychological Association,
Washington, DC, pp. 13-40.
Nilsson, S., Jonsson, E., Fors Brandebo, M. and Larsson, G. (2012), “Military professionalism of the
Swedish armed forces in the 21st century”, in Stouffer, J. and Lindsay, D. (Eds), Threats to
Military Professionalism: International Perspectives, Canadian Defence Academy Press,
Kingston, pp. 169-194.
Osborn, R.N., Hunt, J.G. and Jauch, L.R. (2002), “Toward a contextual theory of leadership”,
The Leadership Quarterly, Vol. 13 No. 6, pp. 797-837.
Podsakoff, P.M., MacKenzie, S.B., Lee, J.Y. and Podsakoff, N.P. (2003), “Common method biases in
behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies”, Journal
of Applied Psychology, Vol. 88 No. 5, pp. 879-903.
Reed, G.E. and Bullis, R.C. (2009), “The impact of destructive leadership on senior military officers
and civilian employees”, Armed Forces and Society, Vol. 36 No. 1, pp. 5-18.
Schyns, B. and Schilling, J. (2013), “How bad are the effects of bad leaders? A meta-analysis
of destructive leadership and its outcomes”, The Leadership Quarterly, Vol. 24 No. 1,
pp. 138-158.
Skogstad, A., Hetland, J., Glasø, L. and Einarsen, S. (2014), “Is avoidant leadership a root cause of
subordinate stress? Longitudinal relationships between laissez-faire leadership and role
ambiguity”, Work & Stress, Vol. 28 No. 4, pp. 323-341.
Skogstad, A., Einarsen, S., Torsheim, T., Aasland, M.S. and Hetland, H. (2007), “The destructiveness
of laissez-faire leadership behavior”, Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, Vol. 12 No. 1,
pp. 80-92.
Swedish Research Council (2002), “Research ethical principles in humanistic-social research”,
Vetenskapsrådet, Stockholm.
Sweeney, P.J. (2010), “Do soldiers re-evaluate trust in their leaders prior to combat operations?”,
Military Psychology, Vol. 22 No. 1, pp. 70-88.
Sweeney, P.J., Matthews, M.D. and Lester, P.B. (2011), “Leading in dangerous situations:
an overview of the unique challenges”, in Sweeney, P.J., Matthews, M.D. and Lester, P.B.
(Eds), Leadership in Dangerous Situations: A Handbook for the Armed Forces,
Emergency Services, and First Responders, Naval Institute Press, Annapolis, MD,
pp. 3-18.
Sweeney, P.J., Thompson, V. and Blanton, H. (2009), “Trust and influence in combat:
a interdependence model”, Journal of Applied Social Psychology, Vol. 39 No. 1, pp. 235-264.
Tepper, B.J. (2000), “Consequences of abusive supervision”, Academy of Management Journal,
Vol. 43 No. 2, pp. 178-190.
LODJ Thoroughgood, C.N., Tate, B.W., Sawyer, K.B. and Jacobs, R. (2012), “Bad to the bone: empirically
defining and measuring destructive leader behavior”, Journal of Leadership &
37,6 Organizational Studies, Vol. 19 No. 2, pp. 230-255.
Woods, S.A. and Hampson, S.E. (2005), “Measuring the Big Five with single items using a bipolar
response scale”, European Journal of Personality, Vol. 19 No. 5, pp. 373-390.
Yukl, G. (2006), Leadership in Organizations, Pearson Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle River, NJ.
710
Further reading
Tepper, B.J. (2007), “Abusive supervision in work organizations: review, synthesis and research
agenda”, Journal of Management, Vol. 33 No. 3, pp. 261-289.
For instructions on how to order reprints of this article, please visit our website:
www.emeraldgrouppublishing.com/licensing/reprints.htm
Or contact us for further details: [email protected]