SHAMEANDITSFEATURES NedaSedighimornani PDF
SHAMEANDITSFEATURES NedaSedighimornani PDF
SHAMEANDITSFEATURES NedaSedighimornani PDF
net/publication/336776533
CITATIONS READS
8 19,598
1 author:
Neda Sedighimornani
University of Bath
8 PUBLICATIONS 73 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
All content following this page was uploaded by Neda Sedighimornani on 24 October 2019.
Neda Sedighimornanii
University of Bath,
United Kingdom
Abstract:
Shame is a complex emotion and often discussed with reluctance; these feelings are
usually incapacitating and unbearable. In this paper, the aim is to review our
understanding of shame. The paper highlights recent empirical findings in order to
define shame and explore its different aspects and characteristics such as its
development, its occurrence and its different forms and shapes. Furthermore, it
identifies differences between shame and similar affective experiences such as guilt and
embarrassment and takes a closer look at shame in different cultures and contexts.
Introduction
Shame is notoriously unpleasant emotion that almost all of us have experienced at least
once in our lifetimes. Almost everyone knows what the feeling of shame is like.
However, we are reluctant to disclose our shameful experiences. It is often mentioned
that shame is a taboo subject (Brown, 2010). Ironically, we are ashamed of our shame. It
seems to be much easier to say that ‚I am angry/sad/nervous‛ than to say ‚I am ashamed.”
Rather than finding it liberating, there is an assumption that talking about shame is
demeaning, or is a sign of weakness. Therefore, until recently, there has been a dearth of
research, understanding, and knowledge about shame, to the extent that shame and
similar emotions, such as guilt, are used interchangeably in the literature.
Since shame appears to be present in a wide variety of psychological disorders
and is associated with mental health problems such as depression, social phobia, and
eating disorders, it is necessary to extend our knowledge in this area, and to become
well-equipped to deal with shame.
The overarching purpose of this paper is to present an overview of shame, and
explores the nature and main features thereof. This paper has been divided into three
1. Understanding of Shame
‚The feeling you get as a result of doing or thinking something you believe to be bad or
immoral. It can also come from a fear of others finding out what you’ve done.”
‚When you feel as though you have done something bad to someone else you know. When
you can’t look at someone in the eye.”
These are a few examples of how students at our university, largely unfamiliar
with psychology, described shame. Like these students, I also find it very difficult to
define shame. From my personal experience, I can say that shame is an overwhelmingly
negative emotion. Feelings of shame make me feel insignificant and inferior. When I
feel shame, I wish the ground would swallow me up. I wish I could be invisible and not
be noticed. According to the Oxford English Dictionary, shame is: ‚The painful emotion
arising from the consciousness of something dishonouring, ridiculous, or indecorous in one’s
own conduct or circumstances (or in those of others whose honour or disgrace one regards as
one’s own), or of being in a situation which offends one’s sense of modesty or decency‛ (OED
online, December, 2014).
The current shame theorists and the empirical research suggest that shame is one
of the so-called self-conscious emotions (M. Lewis, 1992), because it mainly involves an
evaluation of the self. Shame is believed to be an incapacitating emotion that is
accompanied by the feeling of being small, inferior, and of shrinking. The self, as a
whole, is devalued and considered to be inadequate, incompetent, and worthless.
Shame might also involve the feeling of being exposed, condemned, and ridiculed
(Tangney, Stuewig, & Mashek, 2007; Vikan, Hassel, Rugset, Johansen, & Moen, 2010).
Gilbert (2002) stated that shame can be considered as a ‚multifaceted experience‛
with different features and components including: a ‚social or external cognitive
component, internal self-evaluative component, emotional component, behavioural component,”
and ‚physiological component‛ (p. 5). A social or external cognitive component refers to the
idea that shame often occurs in social contexts and evokes thoughts such as ‚others see
me as worthless and inferior‛. An internal self-evaluative component refers to the notion
that shame can include negative evaluations of the self and negative thoughts about the
self, such as ‚I am a failure, inadequate, ugly, and worthless.” An emotional component refers
to the idea that feelings and emotions such as self-disgust, anger, and anxiety may occur
or exist when individuals feel shame. A behavioural component includes defensive
responses, such as a desire to hide, avoid eye contact, engage in submissive behaviour,
express anger, or a desire to take revenge that is often associated with shame. A
physiological component suggests that shame is associated with a stress response, or that
it may increase parasympathetic activity.
negative emotions, they seem unable to describe a situation in which they have felt
shame or guilt.
Furthermore, Griffin (1995) contended that children do not experience self-
conscious emotions such as shame and pride in an adult form before the age of seven to
eight years. For example, when a group of five-year old children were asked to indicate
how a child who had violated a social standard and had been judged negatively in front
of his classmates felt, the majority of the children stated that the child felt sad, mad or
bad, but not ashamed or embarrassed. According to Griffin (1995), understanding social
standards, recognising violations, and being aware of a judgmental audience are
essential for experiencing shame and guilt, and children do not fully develop these
abilities until the age of eight.
Similarly, Leary (2007a) stated that self-conscious emotions are essentially social
emotions, and are evoked when an individual is able to imagine him- or herself in
others’ minds, and to recognise that others form opinions, or judge and evaluate the
self. In this regard, Heerey, Keltner, and Capps (2003) demonstrated that children with
autism, who had difficulty recognising and understanding that others form mental
states different from their own perspectives, minds, and beliefs (impairment in theory
of mind), also had a problem identifying non-verbal expressions of embarrassment and
shame in comparison to children without autism. However, their ability to identify non-
self-conscious emotions, such as anger, disgust, and contempt, was not significantly
different from that of children without autism.
It is perhaps reasonable to conclude that three-year old children show signs of
shame, but that sophisticated reasoning about shame and a complete understanding of
this complex emotion does not occur until the age of seven or eight. For instance,
Olthof, Ferguson, Bloemers, and Deij (2004) found that children seven-years old and
older attributed a greater degree of shame to a protagonist who did something wrong
that consequently led to negative identity and self-evaluation (such as lying about
taking necessary medicine and getting sick as a result), but attributed more guilt to a
protagonist who did something wrong that did not lead to unwanted identity, such as
sending a family pet away because of the protagonist’s allergy. This study showed that
seven-year old children are able to appreciate the sophisticated and subtle differences
between shame and guilt, which are usually seen in adults (Lagattuta & Thompson,
2007).
will interact with others in the future. For example, the infant’s experience of sensitive
care leads to the development of secure attachment, which in turn encourages the child
to see the self positively and to be more agreeable in social encounters. On the other
hand, insecure attachment (insensitive and poor primary care) leads to the formation of
a negative self-image, which in turn contributes to a lack of confidence and self-doubt
(see Thompson, 2006, for a review).
Colman and Thompson (2002) found that, while engaging in problem solving
tasks, insecure children seek their mother’s help more quickly and often in unnecessary
situations, and they express more frustration than do securely attached children. More
interestingly, when experiencing failure, securely attached children were confident
enough to see and accept their limitations and imperfections effortlessly, while insecure
children struggled to acknowledge their weaknesses (Clark & Symons, 2000).
Furthermore, Kelley, Brownell, and Campbell (2000) showed that critical and negative
maternal attitudes during a challenging task at 24 months were related to the
experience of shame and avoidance at 36 months. On the contrary, positive maternal
evaluations predicted higher determination and motivation in solving a challenging
task.
In general, it seems that children tend to internalise their parents’ attitudes
towards themselves (Ferguson & Stegge, 1995), which implies that if parents are hostile
and critical, children will view themselves in a negative light and criticise themselves
harshly (Lagattuta & Thompson, 2007). Bennett, Sullivan, and M. Lewis (2005) argued
that harsh parenting, criticism, and physical abuse lead children to believe that they are
unwanted and undesirable, which ultimately induces shame. These authors found that
physical abuse is related to shame, and that shame partially mediates the relationship
between abuse and behavioural maladjustments. Similarly, Gilbert, Allan, and Goss
(1996) found that the memory of being put-down, non-favoured, and belittled by
parents during childhood is related to shame-proneness in adulthood.
Moreover, Andrews’ research (1995, 1998, 2002) indicated that shame-proneness
is likely to stem from the experience of abuse, especially when the abuse lasts for a long
time. It seems likely that a multi-dimensional relationship exists between shame and
childhood abuse. Abuse (physical, sexual, or verbal) can make individuals feel inferior
and small. In other words, abuse of any kind is likely to put victims in a subordinate
position and to trigger submissive reactions (Andrews, 2002). It is not uncommon to see
that the abused child is stigmatised and blamed by the perpetrator and others in these
circumstances, which perhaps adds to the feeling of shame. Abused children may think
that there is something wrong with them that attracted the abuser. Even when the
abuse is discovered, the child is not relieved. In fact, after the discovery, he or she may
experience a higher level of shame (Feiring, Taska, & M. Lewis, 2002). The child may
fear how he or she will be perceived or treated in the future. There is also a possibility
that the involuntary physiological responses that are experienced during sexual abuse
cause further shame. Under these circumstances, victims of sexual abuse blame
themselves for unwanted reactions and feel disgusted with themselves (Pettersen,
2009).
disappointment because one’s desires or wishes have not been fulfilled, while the
difference between the actual self and the ought self generates agitation-related
emotions such as fear, threat, and restlessness because one has failed to meet
obligations and expectations.
It is relevant here that self-discrepancy theory predicts that failure to meet
others’ expectations (a discrepancy between actual/own and ideal/other) induces
feelings of shame, embarrassment, and despondency because these emotions are related
to beliefs that someone has lost her or his value or worth in the eyes of others. In
contrast, when there is a discrepancy between actual/own versus ought/own, feelings of
guilt, self-contempt, and uneasiness will be elicited because one has failed to live up to
one’s own standards and obligations.
There is some empirical evidence that suggests that self-discrepancies are related
to psychological problems. For example, Higgins, Klein, and Strauman (1985) found
that differences between actual and ideal representations were related to dejection-
related emotions, such as depression, and that discrepancies between actual and ought
self-representations were associated with agitation-related emotions, such as anxiety.
Similarly, when individuals were asked to write about actual/ideal differences, they
reported more sadness, and when they were asked to write about actual/ought
discrepancies, they felt more agitated (Higgins, Bond, Klein, & Strauman, 1986).
Nonetheless, the extent to which this theory is accurate in terms of predicting
emotional vulnerabilities, such as shame and guilt, is unclear. For instance, Tangney,
Niedenthal, Covert, and Barlow (1998) tested Higgins’s (1987) hypotheses, and
questioned their credibility. Specifically, they asked participants to complete a series of
questionnaires, such as the Selves Questionnaire that measures self-discrepancy, and
the Test of Self-Conscious Affect (TOSCA), which measures shame and guilt-proneness.
Their analyses indicated that not only was the discrepancy between actual/own and
ideal/other related to shame-proneness, but also all forms of self-discrepancies. In
addition, the association among all types of self-discrepancies were relatively high,
which brings the validity of the Selves Questionnaire and the theory into question.
According to the personal accounts of ashamed individuals, Lindsay-Hartz
(1984) found that failing to achieve an ideal image is not essential for experiencing
shame. Following these interviews, Lindsay-Hartz concluded that the feeling of shame
was more closely related to the recognition of a negative ideal (who we would not like
to be) rather than to the discrepancy between the actual self and the ideal self (Tangney
& Dearing, 2002). Lindsay-Hartz (1984) explained that ‚what we realize about ourselves
when ashamed is that we are who we do not want to be‛ (p.697). For example, participants
mentioned things like ‚I am fat and ugly‛ rather than ‚I failed to be pretty‛ and ‚I am bad
and evil‛ rather than ‚I am not as good as I want to be‛ (Lindsay-Hartz, de Rivera, &
Mascolo, 1995, p.227); or ‚I realized that I was a crook and a thief, and I didn’t want to be‛
(Lindsay-Hartz, 1984, p.697). This difference is not merely semantic. In fact, the
participants claimed that the difference was critical for understanding their feelings
(Gilbert, 1998).
emotions. However, if the circumstance is not relevant to survival goals, it will not
evoke any basic emotions.
In the next step, the model suggests that individuals will consider whether the
event is relevant for the self. If a situation is meaningful for the self and focuses on the
self, related self-representations might be activated explicitly or implicitly, which in
turn leads to self-evaluation. Self-representations may include the actual self, the ideal
self or the ‘ought’ self. They may also include private aspects of the self or of the public
self. As indicated by this theory, only after the activation of self-representations are self-
conscious emotions generated (Tracy & Robins, 2007b).
Once self-representations are activated, events can be evaluated to see whether
they are important for identity goals, such as: ‚Does it matter for who I am or would like to
be?” (Tracy & Robins, 2007b, p.10). According to the model, only when self-
representations are considered important for identity goals can self-conscious emotions
be evoked. However, if there are no identity concerns, no self-conscious emotions will
be generated.
The next aspect involves identity-goal congruence. This step governs the valence
of emotions. If the event is consistent with one’s goals, it elicits positive emotions,
whereas if the event is not consistent with one’s identity goals, it elicits negative
emotions.
Individuals are then motivated to identify the cause of the event. By using a
series of evaluations, individuals determine whether the event has an internal or
external cause. If an individual makes an internal attribution for the event, self-
conscious emotions result. For example, if someone makes a negative internal
attribution regarding failure in an exam, he or she is likely to feel shame or guilt.
However, if he or she makes a positive attribution, feelings of pride will be elicited.
External attributions evoke basic emotions such as anger.
Furthermore, according to this model, in order to distinguish between self-
conscious emotions (shame, guilt, embarrassment, and pride), it is essential to consider
other causal attributions such as stability (stable and invariant), controllability
(controllable vs. uncontrollable) and globality (the individual as a whole or some aspect
of the person). The model predicts that shame is evoked by internal, uncontrollable,
stable, and global attributions, while guilt is elicited by internal, controllable, unstable,
and specific (not global) attributions. Embarrassment can occur only when individuals
pay attention to the public self and when public self-representations are activated.
Internal attributions are sufficient for embarrassment to occur. No complex cognitive
ability or further attributions are needed to evoke embarrassment.
In support of this theory, Tracy and Robins (2006) conducted a series of studies.
In one of these studies, they asked participants to indicate how they felt about their
current grade point average. They then asked a trained analyst to code the contents of
the participants’ responses and determine whether the participants thought the grades
that they had received depended on internal causes, such as ability, or on external
causes, such as effort. In addition, the level of controllability that the participants
thought they had over a situation was determined. It was found that internal
attributions were associated with feelings of guilt and shame. Furthermore, students
who attributed their low grades to their abilities (an internal, stable, and uncontrollable
cause) were more likely to express shame, while those students who attributed their
low grades to their effort (an internal, unstable, controllable cause) were more likely to
feel guilt.
Considering attributions in shame from a different perspective, Yi and
Baumgartner (2011) asked participants to recall a recent experience of impulse buying,
and then indicate how they felt after buying that item. It was found that impulsive
shoppers who attributed the outcome to stable and uncontrollable aspects (for example,
‚my impulse buying reflects my weak self‛, p.459) were more likely to feel shame than
those who attributed the outcome to unstable and controllable aspects (for example, ‚I
got temporarily carried away by a discount”, p.459). According to the authors, encouraging
impulse buyers to attribute their impulse shopping to situational and short-term
factors, instead of to fixed and global elements, may induce guilt rather than feelings of
shame which, in turn, may help individuals to use problem-focused strategies rather
than avoidance-based methods to cope with their feelings.
image. Even in cognitive-attributional theories of shame, if one does not have a negative
self-image, why does a person attribute a negative event to the self? Negative self-image
seems to fertilise and make it possible for shame to occur and grow or vice versa. In
Table 1.1, we see how the conceptualisation of shame has evolved or changed over the
years. The notion that shame is a self-related emotion and highly negative is evident in
most of these theories.
Becoming exposed and self-conscious, related to For example, Erik Erikson (1950)
physical/body performances such as toilet training
Differences between the ego and the ego-ideal For example, Piers and Singer (1953)
A primitive innate affect. It occurs when there is For example, Tomkin (1963); Kaufman (1996);
disturbance/interruption or an abrupt end to Nathanson (1994)
excitement or joy
The self is considered bad; For example, H.B. Lewis (1971); Higgins (1987)
a discrepancy between actual/own and ideal/other
Making internal, stable, and uncontrollable For example, M. Lewis (1992); Tracy & Robins
attribution, attributing a negative incident to the (2004)
self
Realisation of low status/rank, feeling inferior and For example, Gilbert (2000)
submissive
frequently and have severe impacts on life functions and adaptability (Ferguson &
Stegge, 1995).
Correspondingly, state shame refers to a transient feeling of shame in response to
a particular situation (Goss, Gilbert, & Allan, 1994). At the state level, shame may
regulate social interaction, and increase humility or conformity (Ferguson & Stegge,
1995). In retrospective studies of shame, participants are often asked to recall or
describe a personal episode of the state of shame versus the state of guilt (Tangney,
1992). The experience of shame is often accompanied by a feeling of being self-
conscious, exposed or inferior, lacking power, feeling inhibited, and results in a faster
heartbeat, muscle tensing, and being hot or flushed (Turner, 2014; Wallbott & Scherer,
1989; Wicker, Payne, & Morgan, 1983). Although the experience of shame is unpleasant
and people often try to avoid it, state shame is not an indicator of an emotional
disorder. On the contrary, inability to experience shame is often unacceptable and, to
some extent, a sign of immorality (Tantam, 1998).
Trait shame (chronic) shame refers to a frequent or constant feeling of shame.
Trait shame may be a state of being rather than an emotional state (Tantam, 1998). At
the trait level, an individual’s identity is pervaded by a sense of worthlessness,
inadequacy, and helplessness (Ferguson & Stegge, 1995). Some shame theorists, such as
Tangney and her colleagues, believe that a corresponding trait or disposition for state
shame is shame-proneness, which is defined as a propensity to experience shame in
response to a range of negative situations (Tangney, 1996). In particular, Tangney (1996)
contended that shame-prone individuals (as opposed to guilt-prone individuals)
experience shame more easily when they are in a specific negative situation.
Harper (2011) suggested that trait shame may arise because one is frequently
suppressed or put down. In this situation, the said person may internalise the feeling of
shame, and shame becomes part of his/her identity and disposition. Similarly, Tantam
(1998) stated that frequent reminders of one’s faults and awareness of such faults may
lead to the development of trait shame.
Unsurprisingly, trait shame and shame-proneness are associated with a wide
variety of psychological symptoms. For example, trait shame or shame-proneness often
have stronger associations with psychological problems such as depression and anxiety
than does state shame (Allan, Gilbert, & Goss, 1994; Rusch et al., 2007).
Literature regarding shame focuses heavily on dispositional shame (Leeming &
Boyle, 2002). Some researchers concentrate on shame-proneness (being susceptible to
the experience of shame in response to particular situations), some pay more attention
to global shame (feeling shame frequently, irrespective of particular situations), and
some focus on trait shame in a specific domain such as feeling shame about physical
appearance, ethnicity, and education (Andrews, 1998).
State shame is usually examined when shame is induced in experimental studies
or when participants are asked to recall a personal shame-inducing episode. However,
it should be noted that there is no strict boundary between state shame and trait shame.
A person who is prone to the experience of shame and high in trait shame is also more
People in the general population often have difficulty in distinguishing shame from
similar emotions and constructs, such as guilt and embarrassment (Tangney & Dearing,
2002). It is the case that the terms shame and guilt are used interchangeably. However,
research has documented that feeling shame is very different from feeling guilt,
embarrassment, or shyness, and has different consequences. In order to understand
what feelings of shame entail, it is necessary to examine the difference between shame
and other similar constructs.
Pursuing H. B. Lewis’ line of work, Tangney and colleagues (see Tangney & Dearing,
2002) have provided evidence for the dissimilarities between shame and guilt over the
past three decades. Some of the strongest support for this notion comes from a series of
studies conducted by Niedenthal, Tangney, and Gavanski (1994). In these studies,
participants were asked to recall a personal experience of shame/guilt (Study 1c), or to
put themselves in situations that are shame/guilt-inducing (Studies 1a and 1b). They
were then asked to produce counterfactuals in order to change the problematic
situations. It is interesting that, regarding experiences of shame, participants were
inclined to undo aspects of themselves (‚if only I weren’t‛), while in guilt experiences
they tended to undo specific behaviour (‚if only I hadn’t‛). This is one of the main
reasons why the feeling of guilt is regarded as a less critical and harmful emotion than
that of shame.
Furthermore, it is a common belief that there is a difference between the action
tendencies of shame and guilt. Shame seems to be associated with avoidance-related
tendencies, such as avoiding shame-eliciting situations, withdrawing from others or
hiding. Guilt, on the other hand, is related to approach tendencies, such as approaching
others and trying to repair the damage done, for example by apologising. Guilt may
encourage a change in actions (Sheikh & Janoff-Bulman, 2010; Yi & Baumgartner, 2011).
As noted, in shame, the focus is on the self, whereas in guilt, individuals are able to
focus on the wellbeing of others (Joireman, 2004). This is why guilt has been associated
with perspective-taking and empathy towards others, while in shame the
preoccupation with the self is at odds with the other-oriented nature of empathy
(Parker & Thomas, 2009). Shame-prone individuals tend to be self-absorbed and
oblivious to others’ needs or requests (Tangney & Dearing, 2002). It could be assumed
that shame, especially chronic shame and shame-proneness, makes interpersonal
encounters difficult not because ashamed individuals do not feel empathy, but because
they are too self-conscious to show their true emotions and vulnerabilities. They find
social situations threatening and potentially shaming; hence, they avoid them as much
as possible.
Conventionally, shame is viewed as a public emotion and guilt as a private
emotion. The experience of shame results from public exposure. In other words,
individuals feel shame when their transgression has been seen and revealed publicly. In
this line of thought, Smith, Webster, Parrott, and Eyre (2002) found that when
participants’ failings were seen by others, they felt a stronger sense of shame than they
did of guilt. More specifically, this research (Studies 1 and 2) demonstrated that, in
hypothetical shame and guilt-related situations, when a transgression occurred in
public and an antagonist was exposed, participants assumed that the antagonist would
feel more shame than when a transgression occurred in private. However, with regard
to guilt, the public versus private situation did not matter. Participants assumed that
the antagonist would feel the same level of guilt in public as well as in private.
The notion that shame is a public emotion is to some extent in harmony with
Gilbert’s (1998, 2000) view of shame, which suggests that shame is related to social
ranking. When there is no audience, social ranking and status are essentially
meaningless (Kim, Thibodeau, & Jorgensen, 2011). Nevertheless, support for the public
versus private nature of shame/guilt is contradictory. For instance, Tangney, Miller,
Flicker, and Barlow (1996) asked participants to recall personal experiences of shame,
guilt, and embarrassment. Their findings indicated that shame is not a more public
emotion than is guilt. According to their analyses, both shame and guilt occurred
mainly and equally in public. However, 10.4% of the participants’ guilt experiences and
18.2% of their shame experiences happened in private. The literature suggests that
shame is a relatively individualised emotion and does not have unique triggers. The
common view is that the same situation can induce shame in one person and guilt in
another. It depends on how the role of self is interpreted, not whether the situation
takes place publicly or privately (Parker & Thomas, 2009). Nevertheless, it should be
mentioned that shamed individuals feel exposed. Although shame probably does not
require an actual audience or witness, often the thought of how one’s shortcomings will
appear to others is salient in the experience of shame (Tangney & Dearing, 2002). The
fear of negative evaluation is present in the experience of shame. This is perhaps why
shame is often assumed to be a more public/social emotion than is guilt.
between self-esteem and shame. Tangney and Dearing also postulated that individuals
who are shame-prone do not necessarily have low self-esteem, or vice versa. According
to their argument, it is possible to have high self-esteem but also to be shame-prone. For
instance, one may have a positive image of the self (as worthy and likable), while still
being shame-prone. One may easily experience shame when there is failure or a
negative incident. Similarly, these authors suggested that it is possible to have low self-
esteem but not to feel shame in response to transgressions or failures.
This section will look at how shame is experienced in different cultures, whether it can
be vicarious, and consider the effect of demographic variables such as age and gender
on shame.
children than are American parents, because they believe that shaming methods can be
rehabilitating (Wong & Tsai, 2007).
Furthermore, Fischer, Manstead, and Rodriguez Mosquera (1999) demonstrated
that individuals from an honour-based culture such as Spanish viewed shame more
positively than did participants from an individualist culture (Dutch). For instance,
when describing their experiences of shame, Spanish participants focused more on
other people and their relationship with them, whereas Dutch participants focused
more on their own personal experiences and feelings of self-failure (Rodriguez
Mosquera, Manstead, & Fischer, 2000).
Culture also affects how individuals respond to the experience of shame. For
example, the experience of shame for Filipino salespeople, who come from an
interdependent-oriented culture, is associated with social involvement and an attempt
to rebuild social contacts with customers, while Dutch salespersons, who belong to an
independent-oriented culture, tend to use defensive mechanisms such as avoiding
conversations with consumers in order to protect their self-image after experiencing
shame (Bagozzi, Verbeke, & Gavino, 2003).
In addition, studies suggest that, after vicarious shame, some participants are
motivated to distance themselves from the shameful events (Lickel et al., 2005), while
others may engage in activities in order to restore the damaged group’s image (Lickel et
al., 2007). It is interesting that, as Gunn and Wilson (2011) demonstrated, group
affirmation assists individuals to express shame over the mistreatment of out-groups,
which may in turn facilitate reparatory attitudes and actions. For example, in their
study, Canadians who were asked to choose the most important value for Canadians
and to indicate why this value was important to them and why they had selected this
particular value (group affirmation condition), expressed greater shame over the
mistreatment of and injustice towards Aboriginals, and they showed a greater tendency
towards compensatory actions.
appearance (Roberts & Goldenberg, 2007). In media and culture, women’s appearance
has a far greater value than other characteristics (Sanftner & Tantillo, 2011), and since
the idealised appearance and body are impossible to attain, and standards are
extremely narrow and rigorous (young, slim, white and so on), women are more prone
to experience bodily shame than men, and often tend to be judged and treated
negatively in social situations, such as at school and at work, merely because of their
appearance, even though this is irrelevant to their qualifications, experience, and
performance (Roberts & Goldenberg, 2007).
Thus, it can be said that culture and society put a lot of pressure on women with
regard to their appearance, age, and body. As a result, it is not unreasonable to assume
that these extra pressures contribute to the development of poor self-image, shame, and
self-esteem. For example, a recent report in England indicated that 18% of girls aged 10
to 13 were unhappy with their appearance, in comparison to 9% of boys (Lusher, 2014).
More importantly, this survey found that the way in which girls think about their
appearance and looks was perhaps a main contributor to the reduced wellbeing and
lower life satisfaction that was seen in girls.
4. Concluding remarks
This paper explored the concept of shame and its characteristics and nature. The first
section argued that shame is one of the self-conscious emotions; it starts to emerge
around 18-24 months. In shame, there is a feeling of inadequacy, unworthiness, and
inferiority. Shame can occur when someone makes internal, stable, uncontrollable, and
global attributions for a negative incident or when they feel they have a lower status in
relation to others. In the second section, there was analysis of the differences between
shame and similar constructs and how shame can be distinguished from guilt,
embarrassment, self-esteem, shyness, and humiliation. In the last section, I described
how shame is perceived in collectivist and individualist cultures. Previous research
indicates that shame can be vicarious and elicited as a result of particular group
membership. Women and people from underprivileged backgrounds, minorities, and
working class people are more prone to experience shame. It is necessary to further
study shame and expand our knowledge in this area in order to deal with it better and
manage it more appropriately.
References
Allan, S., Gilbert, P., & Goss, K. (1994). An exploration of shame measures: II:
Psychopathology. Personality and Individual Differences, 17, 719–722.
Andrews, B. (1995). Bodily shame as a mediator between abusive experiences and
depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104, 277–285.
Crystal, D. S., Parrot, W. G., Okazaki, Y., & Watanabe, H. (2001). Examining relations
between shame and personality among university students in the United States
and Japan: A developmental perspective. International Journal of Behavioral
Development, 25, 113-123.
Dickerson, S. S., Gable, S. L., Irwin, M. R., Aziz, N., & Kemeny, M. E. (2009). Social-
evaluative threat and proinflammatory cytokine regulation: An experimental
laboratory investigation. Psychological Science, 20, 1237-44.
Dickerson, S. S., Gruenewald, T. L., & Kemeny, M. E. (2004). When the social self is
threatened: Shame, physiology, and health. Journal of Personality, 72, 1191-1216.
Dickerson, S. S., Kemeny, M. E., Aziz, N., Kim, K. H., & Fahey, J. L. (2004).
Immunological effects of induced shame and guilt. Psychosomatic Medicine, 66,
124-131.
Dresler-Hawke, E., & Liu, J. H. (2006). Collective shame and the positioning of German
national identity. Psicologia Politica, 32, 131-153.
Edelstein, R. S., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). A cross-cultural examination of lexical studies of
self-conscious emotions. In J. L. Tracy, R. W. Robins, & J. P. Tangney (Eds), The
self-conscious emotions: Theory and research (pp. 194-208). New York, NY: Guilford
Press.
Feiring, C., Taska, L., & Lewis, M. (2002). Adjustment following sexual abuse discovery:
The role of shame and attributional style. Developmental Psychology, 38, 79-92.
Ferguson, T. J., & Stegge, H. (1995). Emotional states and traits in children: The case of
guilt and shame. In J. P. Tangney & K. W. Fischer (Eds.), Self-conscious emotions:
The psychology of shame, guilt, embarrassment, and pride (pp. 174–197). New York:
Guilford.
Fischer, A. H., Manstead, A. S. R., & Rodriguez Mosquera, P. M. (1999). The role of
honor-related versus individualistic values in conceptualizing pride, shame and
anger: Spanish and Dutch cultural prototypes. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 149-179.
Gilbert, P. (1998). What is shame? Some core issues and controversies. In P. Gilbert & B.
Andrews (Eds.), Shame: Interpersonal behavior, psychopathology, and culture (pp. 3–
38). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Gilbert, P. (2000). The relationship of shame, social anxiety and depression: The role of
the evaluation of social rank. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 7, 174 –189.
Gilbert, P. (2002). Body shame: A biopsychosocial conceptualisation and overview with
treatment implication. In P. Gilbert & J. N. V. Miles (Eds.). Body shame:
Conceptualisation, research and treatment (pp. 3-54). Hove: Brunner-Routledge.
Gilbert, P. (2011). Shame in psychotherapy and the role of compassion focused therapy.
In R.L. Dearing & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Shame in the therapy hour (pp. 325-354).
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Gilbert, P., & McGuire, M. T. (1998). Shame, status, and social roles: Psychobiology and
evolution. In P. Gilbert & B. Andrews (Eds.), Shame: Interpersonal behavior,
psychopathology, and culture (pp. 99 –125). New York, NY: Oxford University
Press.
Gilbert, P., & Miles, J. N. V. (2000). Sensitivity to social put-down: Its relationship to
perceptions of social rank, shame, social anxiety, depression, anger and self–
other blame. Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 757–774.
Gilbert, P., Allan, S., & Goss, K. (1996). Parental representations, shame, interpersonal
problems, and vulnerability to psychopathology. Clinical Psychology and
Psychotherapy, 3, 23-34.
Goss, K., Gilbert, P., & Allan, S. (1994). An exploration of shame measures: I. The Other
as Shamer Scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 17, 713–717.
Grabe, S., Hyde, J. S., & Lindberg, S. M. (2007). Body objectification and depression in
adolescents: The role of gender, shame, and rumination. Psychology of Women
Quarterly, 31, 164-175.
Greenwald, D. F., & Harder, D. W. (1998). Domains of shame: Evolutionary, cultural
and psychotherapeutic aspects. In P. Gilbert & B. Andrews (Eds.), Shame:
Interpersonal behavior, psychopathology, and culture (pp. 225–245). New York:
Oxford University Press.
Greenwald, D. F., & Harder, D. W. (1998). Domains of shame: Evolutionary, cultural
and psychotherapeutic aspects. In P. Gilbert & B. Andrews (Eds.), Shame:
Interpersonal behavior, psychopathology, and culture (pp. 225–245). New York:
Oxford University Press.
Griffin, S. (1995). A cognitive-developmental analysis of pride, shame, and
embarrassment in middle childhood. In J. P. Tangney & K. W. Fischer (Eds.), Self-
conscious emotions: The psychology of shame, guilt, embarrassment, and pride (pp. 219–
236). New York: Guilford.
Gross, C. A., & Hansen, N. E. (2000). Clarifying the experience of shame: The role of
attachment style, gender, and investment in relatedness. Personality and Individual
Differences, 28, 897-907.
Gruenewald, T. L., Dickerson, S. S., & Kemeny, M. E. (2007). A social function for the
self-conscious emotions: Social self-preservation theory. In J. Tracy, R. Robins, &
J. Tangney (Eds.), The self-conscious emotions: Theory and research (pp. 68-87). New
York: Guilford Press.
Gunn, G. R., & Wilson, A. E. (2011). Acknowledging the skeletons in our closet: The
effect of group affirmation on collective guilt, collective shame, and reparatory
attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 1474-1487.
Harper, J. M. (2011). Regulating and coping with shame. In R. Trnka, K. Balcar, & M.
Kuska (Eds.), Re-constructing emotional spaces: From experience to regulation (pp.
189–206). Prague, CZ: Prague College of Psychosocial Studies Press.
Heerey, E. A., Keltner, D., & Capps, L. M. (2003). Making sense of self-conscious
emotion: Linking theory of mind and emotion in children with autism. Emotion,
3, 394–400.
Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy: A theory relating self and affect. Psychological
Review, 94, 319-340.
Higgins, E. T., Bond, R. N., Klein, R., & Strauman, T. (1986). Self-discrepancies and
emotional vulnerability: How magnitude, accessibility, and type of discrepancy
influence affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 5–15.
Higgins, E. T., Klein, R., & Strauman, T. (1985). Self-concept discrepancy theory: A
psychological model for distinguishing among different aspects of depression
and anxiety. Social Cognition, 3, 51-76.
Hogg, M. A. & Vaughan, G. M. (2008). Social psychology. Essex, England: Pearson
Education Limited.
Johns, M., Schmader, T., & Lickel, B. (2005). Ashamed to be an American? The role of
identification in predicting vicarious shame for Anti-Arab prejudice after 9-
11. Self and Identity, 4, 331-348.
Joireman, J. (2004). Empathy and the self-absorption paradox II: Self-rumination and
self-reflection as mediators between shame, guilt, and empathy. Self and Identity,
3, 225-238.
Kelley, S. A., Brownell, C. A., & Campbell, S. B. (2000). Mastery motivation and self-
evaluative affect in toddlers: Longitudinal relations with maternal behavior.
Child Development, 71, 1061–1071.
Keltner, D., & Buswell, B. N. (1996). Evidence for the distinctness of embarrassment,
shame, and guilt: A study of recalled antecedents and facial expressions of
emotion. Cognition & Emotion, 10, 155–172.
Keltner, D., & Harker, L. (1998). The forms and functions of the nonverbal signal of
shame. In P. Gilbert & B. Andrews (Eds.), Shame: Interpersonal behavior,
psychopathology, and culture (pp. 78–98). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Kemeny, M. E., Gruenewald, T. L., & Dickerson, S. S. (2004). Shame as the emotional
response to threat to the social self: Implications for behaviour, physiology, and
health. Psychological Inquiry, 15, 153-160.
Kim, S., Thibodeau, R., & Jorgensen, R. S. (2011). Shame, guilt, and depressive
symptoms: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 68-96.
Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., & Matsumoto, H. (1995). A cultural perspective on self-
conscious emotions. In J. P. Tangney & K. W. Fischer (Eds.), Self-conscious
emotions: The psychology of shame, guilt, embarrassment, and pride (pp.439–464). New
York: Guilford.
Klein, D. C. (1991). The humiliation dynamic: An overview. The Journal of Primary
Prevention, 12, 93–121.
Lagattuta, K. H., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). The development of self-conscious
emotions: Cognitive processes and social influences. In J. L. Tracy, R. W. Robins,
& J. P. Tangney (Eds.), The self-conscious emotions: Theory and research (pp. 91-113).
New York: Guilford.
Leary, M. R. (2007a). How the self-became involved in affective experience: Three
sources of self-reflective emotions. In J. Tracy., R. Robins, & J. Tangney
(Eds.), The self-conscious emotions: Theory and research (pp. 38-52). New York:
Guilford.
Leeming, D., & Boyle, M. (2004). Shame as a social phenomenon: A critical analysis of
the concept of dispositional shame. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research
& Practice, 77, 375-396.
Lewis, M. (1995). Embarrassment: The emotion of self-exposure and evaluation. In J. P.
Tangney & K. W. Fischer (Eds.), Self-conscious emotions: The psychology of shame,
guilt, embarrassment, and pride (pp. 198–218). New York: Guilford.
Lewis, M. (1998). Shame and stigma. In P. Gilbert & B. Andrews (Eds.), Shame:
Interpersonal behavior, psychopathology, and culture (pp. 126–140). New York:
Oxford University Press.
Lewis, M. (2000). Self-conscious emotions: Embarrassment, pride, shame, and guilt. In
M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 623–
636). New York: Guilford.
Lewis, M. (2000). Self-conscious emotions: Embarrassment, pride, shame, and guilt. In
M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 623–
636). New York: Guilford.
Lewis, M. (2003). The role of the self in shame. Social Research, 70, 1181-1204.
Lewis, M. (2003). The role of the self in shame. Social Research, 70, 1181-1204.
Lewis, M. (2007). Self-conscious emotional development. In J. L. Tracy, R.W. Robins, &
J. P. Tangney (Eds.), The self-conscious emotions: Theory and research (pp. 134-149).
New York: Guilford.
Lewis, M., & Ramsay, D. (2002). Cortisol response to embarrassment and shame. Child
Development, 73, 1034-1045.
Lewis, M., Alessandri, S., & Sullivan, M. W. (1992). Differences in shame and pride as a
function of children’s gender and task difficulty. Child Development, 63, 630–638.
Lewis, H. B. (1971). Shame and guilt in neurosis. New York: International Universities Press.
Lewis, M. (1992). Shame: The exposed self. New York: Free Press.
Lickel, B., Schmader, T., & Spanovic, M. (2007). Group-conscious emotions: The
implications of others’ wrongdoings for identity and relationships. In J. L. Tracy,
R.W. Robins, & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), The self-conscious emotions: Theory and
Research (pp. 351-370). New York: NY: Guilford Press.
Lickel, B., Schmader, T., Curtis, M., Scarnier, M., & Ames, D. R. (2005). Vicarious shame
and guilt. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 8, 145-157.
Lickel, B., Schmader, T., Curtis, M., Scarnier, M., & Ames, D. R. (2005). Vicarious shame
and guilt. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 8, 145-157.
Lindisfarne, N. (1998). Gender, shame, and culture: An anthropological perspective. In
P. Gilbert & B. Andrews (Eds), Shame: Interpersonal behavior, psychopathology, and
culture (pp. 246-260). New York: Oxford University Press.
Lindsay-Hartz, J. (1984). Contrasting experiences of shame and guilt. American
Behavioral Scientist, 27, 689 –704.
Lindsay-Hartz, J., de Rivera, J. H., Mascolo, M. F. (1995). Differentiating guilt and shame
and their effects on motivation. In J. P. Tangney & K. W. Fischer (Eds.), Self-
conscious emotions: The psychology of shame, guilt, embarrassment, and pride (pp.274–
300). New York: Guilford.
Lusher, A. (2014). Report finds English children among the unhappiest in world. The
Independent. Retrieved from http://www.independent.co.uk/.
Mills, R. (2005). Taking stock of the developmental literature on shame. Developmental
Review, 25, 26–63.
Mosher, D. L., & White, B. B. (1981). On differentiating shame and shyness. Motivation
and Emotion, 5, 61-74.
Niedenthal, P., Tangney, J. P., & Gavanski, I. (1994). "If only I weren't" versus "If only I
hadn't": Distinguishing shame and guilt in counterfactual thinking. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 585-595.
Olthof, T., Ferguson, T. J., Bloemers, E., & Deij, M. (2004). Morality- and identity-related
antecedents of children’s guilt and shame attributions in events involving
physical illness. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 383–404.
Orth, U., Robins, R. W., & Soto, C. J. (2010). Tracking the trajectory of shame, guilt, and
pride across the life span. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 1061-
1071.
Ortony, A., & Turner, T. J. (1990). What's basic about basic emotions? Psychological
Review, 97, 315-331.
Parker, S., & Thomas, R. (2009). Psychological differences in shame vs. guilt:
Implications for mental health counsellors. Journal of Mental Health Counselling,
31, 213-223.
Pettersen, K. T. (2009). An exploration into the concept and phenomenon of shame within the
context of child sexual abuse. (Unpublished doctoral thesis), Norwegian University
of Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway.
Power, C.A., Cole, E. R., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2010). Poor women and the expression of
shame and anger: The price of breaking social class feeling rules. Feminism &
Psychology, 21, 179-197.
Roberts, T. A., & Goldenberg, J. L. (2007). Wrestling with nature: An existential
perspective on the body and gender in self-conscious emotions. In J. Tracy, R.
Robins, & J. Tangney (Eds.), The self-conscious emotions: theory and research (pp.
389-406). New York: Guilford Press.
Rodriguez Mosquera, P. M., Manstead, A. S. R., & Fischer, A. H. (2000). The role of
honor-related values in the elicitation, experience and communication of pride,
shame and anger: Spain and the Netherlands compared. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 26, 833-844.
Rozin, P. (2003). Five potential principles for understanding cultural differences in
relation to individual differences. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 273–283.
Rusch, N., Lieb, K., Gottler, I., Hermann, C., Schramm, E., Richter, H., . . . Bohus, M.
(2007). Shame and implicit self-concept in women with borderline personality
disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 500-8.
Sabini, J., Garvey, B., & Hall, A. L. (2001). Shame and embarrassment revisited.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 104–117.
Sanftner, J. L., & Tantillo, M. (2011). Body image and eating disorders: A compelling
source of shame for women. In R. Dearing & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Shame in the