A Correlational Study of Hardiness Health and Burnout Among Tea PDF

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 106

East Tennessee State University

Digital Commons @ East


Tennessee State University
Electronic Theses and Dissertations Student Works

May 1994

A Correlational Study of Hardiness, Health, and


Burnout among Teachers in the Sullivan County
School System
Deborah F. Morelock
East Tennessee State University

Follow this and additional works at: https://dc.etsu.edu/etd


Part of the Educational Administration and Supervision Commons, and the Other Psychology
Commons

Recommended Citation
Morelock, Deborah F., "A Correlational Study of Hardiness, Health, and Burnout among Teachers in the Sullivan County School
System" (1994). Electronic Theses and Dissertations. Paper 2753. https://dc.etsu.edu/etd/2753

This Dissertation - Open Access is brought to you for free and open access by the Student Works at Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State
University. It has been accepted for inclusion in Electronic Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Digital Commons @ East
Tennessee State University. For more information, please contact [email protected].
INFORMATION TO USERS

This manuscript has been reproduced from the microfilm master. UMI
films the text directly from the original or copy submitted. Thus, some
thesis and dissertation copies are in typewriter face, while others may
be from any type of computer printer.

The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the


copy submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality
illustrations and photographs, print bleedthrougb, substandard margins,
and improper alignment can adversely afreet reproduction.

In the unlikely, event that the author did not send UMI a complete
manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if
unauthorized copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate
the deletion.

Oversize materials (e.g., maps, drawings, charts) are reproduced by


sectioning the original, beginning at the upper left-hand comer and
continuing from left to right in equal sections with small overlaps. Each
original is also photographed in one exposure and is included in
reduced form at the back of the book.

Photographs included in the original manuscript have been reproduced


xerographically in this copy. Higher quality 6" x 9" black and white
photographic prints are available for any photographs or illustrations
appearing in this copy for an additional charge. Contact UMI directly
to order.

UMI
University Microfilms International
A Bell & Howell Information Company
300 North Z eeb Road. Ann Arbor, Ml 40106*1346 USA
313/761-4700 600/521*0600
Order Number 94241T0

A correlational study of hardiness, health, and burnout among


teachers in the Sullivan County School System

Morelock, Deborah Falin, Ed.D.


B u t Tennessee State University, 1994

UMI
300 N, Zceb Rd.
Ann Arbor, MI 48106
A CORRELATIONAL STUDY OF

HARDINESS, HEALTH, AND BURNOUT

AMONG TEACHERS

IN THE SULLIVAN COUNTY

SCHOOL SYSTEM

A Dissertation

Presented to

the Faculty of the Department of

Educational Leadership and Policy Analysis

East Tennessee State University

In Partial Fulfillment

of the Requirements for the Degree

Doctor of Education

by

Deborah Falin Morelock

May 1994
APPROVAL

This is to certify that the Graduate Committee of

DEBORAH FALIN MORELOCK

met on the

tWgntY-geCQnti day of March. 1994.


The committee read and examined her dissertation,

supervised her defense of it in an oral examination, and

decided to recommend that her study be submitted to the

Graduate Council Associate Vice-President for Research and

Dean, School of Graduate Studies, in partial fulfillment of

the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Education.

Chairman, Graduate Committee

Signed on behalf of
the Graduate Council
Associate Vice-President for
Research and Dean, school of
Graduate studies

ii
ABSTRACT

A Correlational Study of
Hardiness, Health, and Burnout
Among Teachers
In The Sullivan County
School System

by

Deborah F. Morelock

The purpose of this study vas to explore the


relationships among hardiness, health and burnout and to
investigate the relationships of the subscales of hardiness
to health and to the subscales of burnout among teachers in
the Sullivan County School System. The Hardiness Test was
used to measure hardiness and the Maslach Burnout Inventory
Form Ed to measure burnout. Results of the Health
Evaluation and Risk Test was reported on the Educators
Demographic Data Survey. The study sample consisted of 50X
teachers.

A statistically significant relationship was revealed


between hardiness and health; among the hardiness subscales,
commitment accounted for the largest amount of variance in
health. Multiple regression was used to analyze the
relationships among the subscales of hardiness and the
subscales of burnout. Of the hardiness subscales,
commitment and control equally accounted for the greatest
amount of variance in depersonalization and personal
accomplishment.

To determine the extent to which the subjects'


demographic variables were related to any study variables,
correlation coefficients were computed between demographic
variables and each of the study variables. While age, years
teaching experience, and level taught were significantly
related to the study variables, the extremely low
correlations indicate that only 1% of the variance in these
three variables were explained by the study variables,
preventing meaningful interpretation.

Kobasa's health and hardiness theory is supported by


the results of this study. These results also support
previous research findings which suggest that hardiness
buffers against burnout.

iii
IRB Number 01
Assurance Number M1194

IRB FORM 108 PROTOCOL NO. 93-048s

BAST TENNESSEE STATE UNIVERSITY


INSTITUTIONAL REVIEW BOARD

PROJECT TITLE: Hardiness: Its Relationship to Health and Burnout


Among Teachers in the Sullivan County School
System.

PRINCIPAL INVESTIGATOR: Deborah F. Morelock

The Institutional Review Board has reviewed the above-titled

project on November 12. 1993 w ith respect to the rights and

safety of human subjects, including matters of informed consent

and protection of subject confidentiality, and finds the project

acceptable to the Board.

Anthony J. DeLucia
Chairman, IRB

iv
DEDICATION

To my mother who taught me very early in my life the

value of an education. To my father who taught me later in

my life the difference between that education and wisdom.

v
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

I would like to publicly acknowledge the words of

encouragement and support that the members of Cohort III

have given me during the past three years; without them I

would have never completed this endeavor.

I am grateful to the Educational Leadership and Policy

Analysis Department at East Tennessee state University for

creating a doctoral program for working school

administrators and for admitting me to it.

A special word of thanks goes to Dr. Robert Me Elrath,

my advisor, teacher, and Chair of my Committee. Not only

did I have the best chairman I could hope for, I was blessed

with the ideal committee members: Dr. Charles Burkett, Dr.

Hal Knight, Dr. A. Keith Turkett, and Dr. Russ West who

contributed ideas, edits and positive encouragement.

To my children, Erin and Brooks, I express gratitude

for behaving as though Mom's schooling was a natural part of

life, not a source of inconvenience. Hopefully, you were

both too young to remember the amount of time I spent away

from you. To my husband, Chris, for his love and support,

as he endured much over the past three years to help me make

this dream a reality. I have yet to decide if the view is

worth the climb.

Finally, I appreciate the participation of the five

hundred one teachers in Sullivan County, Tennessee who

helped provide data for this study.

vi
CONTENTS

Chapter Page

1. INTRODUCTION ................................... 1

Statement of the Problem ..................... 3

Significance of the Study ............... 3

Purpose .................................. 4

Limitations of the Study .................... 4

Assumptions of the Study .................... 5

Research Questions .......................... 5

Definition of Terms ...»*.................... 6

Organization of the Study .............. :... 8

2. LITERATURE R E V I E W .............................. 10

Kobasa's Hardiness Theory ................. 10

Kobasa's Orginial Hardiness Research ...... 12

Other Hardiness Research ................... 14

Teachers and Hardiness Research ........... 17

Health's Evolution ........................... 19

Cardiovascular Disease and Health ......... 19

Relationship of Health to Hardiness ....... 23

Burnout ...................................... 24

Maslach's Study of Burnout ................. 25

Burnout and Hardiness ...................... 27

Burnout Research and Teachers ............. 29

s u m m a r y ..................... 32

3. REASEARCH DESIGN........................ 34

Population ................................. 34

vii
Chapter Page

Instruments ...................................... 35

The Hardiness T e s t ........................... 35

The Haslach Burnout Inventory Form Ed ....... 37

H E A R T ......................................... 39

Educators DemographicData Sheet .............. 40

Data Collections Procedures ..... 41

Data A n a l y s i s ............... 41

Hypotheses ..... 42

Summary of Methodology ........................... 45

4. RESULTS .......................................... 48

Sample Demographics ........................... 48

Description of Hardiness, Health,


and Burnout Scores ............................ 51

Hardiness T e s t ................................. 52

Health ......................................... 53

Burnout ........................................ 54

Research Questions ............................... 57

Research Question 1 ................. 57

Research Question 2 ......... *........ 58

Research Question 3 ....... 59

Research Question 4 ............................ 60

Research Question 5 ........................... 63

Age ...... 65

Gender ...................................... 66

Years Teaching Experience .................. 66

Highest Level of Education ................. 67

viii
Chapter Page

Level T a u g h t .............................. 67

Teaching Assignment ....................... 68

5. DISCUSSION, CONCLUSIONS AND


RECOMMENDATIONS ............................... 70

Discussion........................................ 70

Hardiness and H e a l t h .......................... 70

Hardiness and B u r n o u t .. ............ 72

Hardiness ........... *.......... 75

Conclusions..................................... 76

Recommendations ................................. 77

S u m m a r y ......................................... 78

BIBLIOGRAPHY ........................................... 81

APPENDIXES

A. The Hardiness T e s t .............................. 90

B. Maslach Burnout InventoryForm E d ............... 95

C. Educators Demographic DataSurvey ................ 98

V I T A .................................................... 99

ix
LIST OF TABLES

Table Page

1. Gender, Age, and Years Teaching


Experience ..................................... 49

2. Level of Education, School Level Assigned,


Class Type ..................................... 50

3. Descriptive Statistics for Hardiness and


Subscales, Health, and Burnout Subscales
(Raw Scores) .................................. 53

4. Frequency and Percentages of Total


Risk Points .......................... 54

5. Frequency and Percentages of


Experienced Burnout of Respondents as
Measured by the Maslach Burnout Inventory .... 56

6. Regression Analysis for Health Using


Hardiness Subscales ........................... 60

7. Regression Analysis for Emotional Exhaustion


Using Hardiness Subscales ..................... 62

8. Results of Regression Analysis for


Depersonalization Using Hardiness Subscales ... 63

9. Results of Regression Analysis for Personal


Accomplishment Using Hardiness Subscales ..... 63

10. correlation Coefficients of


Study V a r i a b l e s ............................... 69

11. Correlation Coefficients for Burnout


Subscales and Hardiness Subscales............. 74

x
CHAPTER 1

Introduction

Stress and contemporary life styles have become

inextricably linked (Selye, I960). Stress has been shown to

be related to poor health, chronic illness, exhaustion,

fatigue and burnout (Selye, 1978; sutterly, 1986; Maslach,

1986). Hardiness may be described as an aspect of the

personality which reduces the effects of stress.

Individuals who possess personality hardiness may actually

negate the harmful effects of stress and enjoy an enhanced

health status (Kobasa, 1977).

It is generally recognized that the increasingly higher

rate of turnover among public school teachers is due to

stress. Some of the sources of stress for teachers include;

(a) task overload (heavy work load, inadequate time for

preparation); (b) lack of control over activities and

outcomes (lack of teaching aids, inadequate resources,

incompetent administration); (c) insufficient satisfactions

from work (frequent negative and infrequent positive

feedback); (d) role conflicts (career development issues,

inadequate time for individual remedial work); (e) rapid or

unpredictable change; (f) interpersonal conflicts

(disruptive students, pupil misbehavior, difficult social

relations); (g) unrealistic expectations; and (h) feelings

of inadequacy (Dewe, 1986).

1
Although the nature and origin of stress experienced by

public school teachers nay vary, it is certain that stress

is part of the typical teacher's life. Haslach (1976) and

Cunningham (1983) have described the subsequent effects of

chronic stress on teachers in terns of burnout. They noted

that when the nultitude of stressors found in the teaching

situation continue without relief, the pattern of reactions

described by burnout seriously damages the teacher's ability

to perform: feelings become negative, attitudes become

cynical, concern for students is lost, frequency of physical

illness and absenteeism increases, and use of drugs and

alcohol frequently goes up. Thus, burnout, as the

subsequent outcome of continued stress, reflects the

cumulative reactions to this stress.

The role of hardiness (composed of control, commitment

and challenge dimensions) as a mediator of stress has been

presented as a personality orientation that helps people

cope with stressors and stress in ways that minimize the

potentially debilitating effects of life change (Kobasa,

1977).

Burnout is a "syndrome of emotional exhaustion,

depersonalization, and reduced personal accomplishment that

can occur" in individuals who work with people (Haslach,

1986, p.3). In this study, burnout will be measured by the

separate scores of the subscales for the Maslach Burnout

Inventory: Emotional Exhaustion, Depersonalization,


Personal Accomplishment (Maslach & Jackson, 1986).

Excessive stress over a long period of time can lead to

burnout and even threaten health. Two-thirds of office

visits to family doctors are prompted by stress-related

symptoms (Wallis, 1983).

As a result of this concern, it is essential that

teachers assume self-responsibility for their health through

wellness programs similar to that of the Health Evaluation

and Risk Test (HEART), sponsored through Blue Cross and Blue

Shield of Tennessee and used in the Sullivan County School

System.

For this study of hardiness, health, and burnout, the

theory of hardiness will provide the theoretical framework

because hardiness is connected with both health and burnout.

This study differs from previous health and hardiness

research in that health will not be measured through self-

reported instruments but through medical analysis.

Statement of the Problem

The problem is to determine the relationships of health

and burnout among teachers in the Sullivan County school

system and hardiness as a personality characteristic.

Significance of the Study

The investigation of stress-resistant personality


factors in educators has been advocated by Holt, Fine, and

Tollefson (1987) with an ultimate view toward increasing the

educator's potential for success in teaching. The value of

hardiness, a stress-resistant factor, to the educator has

application for deterring teacher burnout and entrapment.

Purpose

Concern with teacher stress is threefold. First, it is

quite probable that stress negatively and substantially

affects the classroom environment, the teaching-learning

process, and the attainment of educational goals and

objectives. Secondly, in the past 30 years, physicians and

health officials have come to realize how heavy a toll

stress is taking on the well-being of individuals. Finally,

a combination of stress and health factors can lead to

teacher burnout, which in turn does not lead to teacher

turnover; rather to teacher "entrapment". It is the purpose

of this study to explore those relationships.

Limitations of the. Study

The scope of the study will be limited to the teachers

in the Sullivan County School System who participate in the

Health Evaluation and Risk Test conducted by Blue Cross and

Blue Shield of Tennessee and volunteer to share that

information in this study. Non-participants may differ in

motivation level from participants (Borg & Gall, 1989).

A second limitation of the present study is a


statistical one. Correlational studies are appropriately

used in providing the degree of the relationship between

variables but cannot establish cause-and-effect

relationships between correlated variables (Borg & Gall,

1989). Therefore, causation cannot be concluded from the

results of this study.

Assumptions of the study

The study includes the following assumptions:

1. All self-reports of subjects, as noted on the

Educators Demographic Data Survey (EDDS) and as recorded on

the Maslach Burnout Inventory Form Ed (MBI), the Hardiness

Test, and the Health Evaluation and Risk Testing (HEART),

were true and accurate statements.

2. The MBI can accurately measure burnout.

3. The Hardiness Test can accurately measure

psychological hardiness.

4. The HEART can accurately measure health.

Research Questions

1. What is the relationship between hardiness and health,

that is, the fourteen factors as identified by the

American Heart Association which make it possible to

determine the extent of risk of cardiovascular disease?

2. What are the relationships among total hardiness and the

subscales of burnout (Emotional Exhaustion,


Depersonalization, and Personal Exhaustion) in public

school teachers in the Sullivan County School System?

3. What is the relationship of each subscale of hardiness

(commitment, control and challenge) to health in public

school teachers in the Sullivan County School System?

4. What are the relationships among the hardiness subscales

(commitment, control, and challenge) and each of the

subscales of burnout (Emotional Exhaustion,

Depersonalization, and Personal Accomplishment) in

public school teachers in the Sullivan County School

System?

5. What is the relationship, if any, between selected

demographicc variables as reported by the Educators

Demographic Data Survey and the three subscales of

burnout and hardiness?

Definitions of Terms

The following definitions apply to this study:

Hardiness: Hardiness is a personality constellation of

commitment, control, and challenge that mediates the effects

of stress to promote physical and psychological health

(Kobasa, 1977).

Commitment: Commitment is the tendency to involve

oneself fully in life and recognize one's distinctive goals

and priorities and the appreciation of one's ability to make

decisions and hold values (Kobasa, 1982).


Control: Control la the tendency to believe and act as

if one can influence the course of events (Kobasa, 1982).

Challenge: Challenge is the capacity to perceive

stressful life events as an opportunity and incentive for

personal growth, rather than a simple threat to security

(Kobosa, 1982).

Burnout: Burnout is a syndrome of emotional

exhaustion, depersonalization, and reduced personal

accomplishment that can occur in individuals who work with

people (Haslach, 1986).

Emotional Exhaustion: Emotional exhaustion consists of

a low energy level, sense of depletion, and feeling of being

overwhelmed by the emotional demands imposed by other people

(Haslach, 1986).

Depersonalization! Depersonalization is the attempt at

emotional self“protection manifested by detached, callous,

and even dehumanized response toward others (Haslach, 1986).

Reduced Personal Accomplishment: Reduced personal

accomplishment is the sense of inadequacy and failure that

arises subsequent to feelings of guilt for the negative

response toward others (Haslach, 1986).

HEART! Health Evaluation and Risk Test includes:

complete blood profile, body composition testing and

determination of target body weight, graded exercise

cardiovascular fitness test on stationary bicycle, blood

pressure and heart rate reading at rest and during exercise,


8

evaluation of personal and family traits of cardiovascular

disease, stress evaluation and tobacco use evaluation

(Wellness I).

Teachers; Teachers included regular and special

education classroom teachers, remedial teachers (such as

resource math, reading), as well as special areas (such as

physical education, art, music, and library). The term

"teacher" as used in this research project did not include

administrators or pupil professionals (such as guidance

counselors, social workers or school psychologists).

organization of the Study

This study was organized and presented in five

chapters. Chapter 1 contains the introduction of the study

and the statement of the problem including its purpose,

significance, limitations, and assumptions. Five research

questions, ten definitions of terms, and a discussion of the

study are also included in the chapter.

Chapter 2 provides the review of literature regarding

hardiness, health, and burnout. The literature review

begins with a description and an explanation of hardiness.

The chapter then presents research studies of hardiness in

teachers. The concept of health is discussed and presented

in relation to hardiness. Finally, burnout is described and

then related to teaching.

Chapter 3 is comprised of research design, procedures,


and methodology. It Includes a description of the target

population and the selection of the study sample, the

instruments, the treatment of data, the hypotheses, and a

summary of methodology.

Chapter 4 contains the presentation of demographic

characteristics of research subjects, analysis of data, and

results of hypotheses testing.

Chapter 5 includes a summary of the findings,

conclusions, recommendations, and implications.


CHAPTER 2

Literature Review

This literature review will address hardiness, health,

and burnout beginning with a description of hardiness and

explained by Kobasa and co-authors (Kobasa, 1977, 1979,

1982; Kobasa, Maddi, 8 Courington, 1981; Kobasa & Puccetti,

1983). Research studies of hardiness in teachers will be

presented (Hammond, 1987; Langemo, 1987; Holt, Fine &

Tollefson, 1987; Goor, 1990; Pierce & Malloy, 1990). The

concept of health as measured by cardiovascular risk factors

will be presented from the perspectives of several authors

(Lenfante, stone, & Castelli, 1987; Stoto, 1991; Gunby,

1992; Brownson, 1992), and its relativeness to hardiness

will be discussed. Burnout will be presented from the

perspective of Maslach (1986), and will also be related to

teaching (Byrme, 1992; Dworkin, 1985; Cadavid & Lunenburg,

1991; Lutz 6 Maddiralta, 1987).

Kobasa's Hardiness Theory

The term "personality hardiness" has been used to

describe persons who have a kind of personal and world view

that underlies the positive capacity to cope with and

mediate stress (Kobasa, 1979). In her originating research,

Suzanne C. Kobasa (1979) stated that "persons who experience

high degrees of stress, without falling ill, have a

10
11

personality structure differentiating then from persons who

become sick under stress” (p.3)* This personality structure

"hardiness” was defined as a constellation of commitment,

control, and challenge that serves as a "resistance

resource” in encounters with stress (Kobasa, Maddi, & Kahn,

1982, p. 169). The resistance resource theory formed the

foundation of Kobasa's proposed concept that individuals who

are exposed to high levels of stress who do not become ill,

may have characteristics that come under the rubric

"personal hardiness” (Kobasa, 1979). Kobasa explained that

these hardy individuals choose commitment rather than

alienation, control rather than powerlessness, and challenge

rather than threat. These three personality characteristics

remain the basis of her continuous research on personal

hardiness.

Commitment involves activity and curiosity, not

passiveness and alienation. It entails belief in the value

for "what one is and what one is doing, as well as a

tendency to involve oneself fully and vigorously in life."

The committed individual finds life in general, and work in

particular, meaningful and worth engaging, thereby lessening

the threat perceived in situations and circumstances

(Kobasa, 1982).

Control is the tendency to believe and act as if one

is influential (rather than helpless) in the course of

events in one's life. Individuals who have control strive


12

to understand the reasons for things that occur with

particular reference to their own sphere of responsibility

(Kobasa, 1982). Control involves developing a repertoire of

options and actions that transforms events into a continuing

life plan (Holt, Fine, & Tollefson, 1987).

Challenge, the third dimension of the hardiness

constellation, involves the belief that one should expect

and accept change, not stability, as the normal pattern of

life (Kobasa, 1982; Kobasa, Maddi, & Kahn, 1982). The

anticipation of change is positive, rather than threatening

and change is viewed as an incentive to growth. The

individual with this characteristic emphasizes growing and

changing, rather than conserving and protecting the status

quo (Holt, Fine, & Tollefson, 1987).

Kobasa's Original Hardiness Research

Kobasa's original research on hardiness (1979) was a

retrospective, single observation, correlational study (& =

200) of middle and upper level male executives of a major

utility company who worked in an environment of unusually

high stressors, as measured by stressful life events.

Demographically, the pool was quite homogeneous.

"The model characteristics of the subjects were (a)

male gender; (b) 40 to 49 years of age; (c) married,

with two children; (d) on the third or middle

management level, and having been there for 6 years or


13

more; (e) possessing at least a college degree; (£)

wife not working outside the home; and (g) usually

Protestant, and attending religious services very or

fairly often" (Kobasa, 1979, p.5).

Kobasa found that high stress/low illness individuals

could be distinguished from high stress/high illness

individuals. The high stress/low illness executives showed

a commitment to self, vigor, and an awareness of meaning in

their lives. Also, these individuals had a greater sense of

internal locus of control and were more oriented to

challenge (Kobasa, 1979).

Following the original study, Kobasa, Maddi, and Kahn

(1982) and Kobasa, Maddi, and Courington (1981) conducted

hardiness research in two longitudinal studies using

subjects with similar demographic characteristics as the

utility company executives. After completion of factor

analysis (Kobasa, Maddi, & Kahn, 1982) the cognitive scale

was dropped and the idea of a composite hardiness score was

developed.

The contributions from this follow-up research were:

(1) hardiness serves as a buffer to the effects of stress,

(2) hardiness is most operative when stressful life events

mount over a period of time, and (3) increased hardiness

increased the likelihood of maintaining health (Kobasa,

Maddi, & Courington, 1981; Kobasa, Maddi, & Kahn, 1982).

Hardiness was linked to Type A behaviors (Kobasa,


14

Maddi, & Zola, 1983) and to social supports (Kobasa &

Puccetti, 1983) in subsequent research. The two studies

continue to confirm the association of hardiness to stress -

induced illness. The study on Type A behavior supports the

contention that Type A's who are low in hardiness while

being exposed to high-stress conditions are even sicker than

was formerly indicated (Kobasa, Maddi, & Zola, 1983).

The study on social support concluded that family

support functions as a resistance resource only if hardiness

is high. Low-hardy executives, those who feel alienated,

lacking in control and threatened by change, may not resist

stress as actively if they are given strong cohesiveness and

expressiveness at home (Kobasa & Puccetti, 1983). It is the

hardy who tend to better utilize the positive aspects of

social support and assets.

Other Hardiness Research

Through 1983, Kobasa's published research used a pool

of subjects essentially identical to the sample in her

original study. During the next four years other

researchers began to submit additional subjects to a

hardiness examination; e.g., college student resident

assistants (Nowack & Hanson, 1983), female college students

(Ganellen & Blaney, 1984), female secretaries (Schmied &

Lawler, 1986), New York state school superintendents

(Falinski, 1985), faculty researchers, researchers, and


15

administrators at a southwestern university (Hammond, 1987),

and athletic trainers (Shapiro, 1987).

Nowack and Hanson (1983) found that hardiness

correlated significantly and accounted for 35% of the

variance in frequency and severity of illness. The authors

concluded that hardiness buffers burnout.

Ganellen and Blaney (1984) found that commitment and

challenge, but not control, were significantly correlated

with social support. Therefore, the researchers conclude

that the higher the level of social support subjects report,

the higher their level of hardiness. Also found was that

powerlessness and internal locus of control measure

perceptions of control, not control itself.

Schmied and Lawler (1986) set out to determine if the

original all male hardiness research could be generalized to

females. The authors stated:

"Hardiness may not generalize to females, especially

when using physical illness as the dependent measure;

hardiness in men may not be the same as hardiness in

women; because hardiness was associated in this study

with age, education and marital status, hardiness may

be a developmental trait; and hardiness may not

manifest itself in the occupational role of a secretary

where there is little opportunity for control or

challenge and Type A behaviors are unlikely to be

rewarded" (Williams, 1988, p.50).


16

Falinski (1985) found that personal hardiness

contributed to the ability of New York state school

superintendents to handle role stress and therefore feel job

satisfaction. In another similar study by Hammond (1987),

faculty, researchers, and administrators at a southwestern

university responded to a questionnaire revealing that

satisfaction could be predicted by hardiness, social

support, and coping strategies.

Shapiro (1987) in a later study of athletic trainers

confirmed that personal hardiness was a significant

contributor to job satisfaction of these individuals.

Up to this point, the majority of hardiness studies

involved executives and high-level managers. Since then

several studies of hardiness have included two other groups

of professionals who experience high daily stress, nurses

and teachers.

Simoni (1987) conducted a study of nurses in Kentucky.

He found that personal hardiness factors helped to reduce

the symptoms of burnout as well as to improve nurses'

perceptions of career satisfaction. In a similar study of a

subgroup of nurses working in oncology, D'Ambrosia (1982)

found that burnout could be predicted based on hardiness

factors, particularly the sense of control versus

powerlessness. Rich and Rich (1987) concluded that

hardiness is an important stress-resistance resource in

preventing or reducing burnout in female staff nurses.


17

Teachers and Hardiness Research

Recent studies of personal hardiness in teachers have

shown a relationship between hardiness factors and reduced

symptoms of burnout. In a study of university faculty,

Hammond (1987) and Langemo (1967) found that, indeed,

hardiness factors provided energy for coping with work-

related stress. Holt, Fine, and Tollefson (1987) surveyed

192 female regular and special education elementary teachers

to study the mediating effects of coping and hardiness on

the stress-burnout relationship. As in previous studies,

individuals with high occupational stress and low burnout

were found to choose active coping, feel less alienation,

and experience more internal locus of control. These

teachers were also likely to report both physical and mental

illness.

The findings in the Holt et al. (1987) study stimulated

interest in terms of teacher career stages and reported

levels of stress. In the group perceiving lower stress,

there was a higher percentage of older, more experienced

teachers and also younger, less experienced teachers. In

contrast, the high stress group contained more teachers with

five to ten years of experience, that is, the early,

teacher-career-stage group.

Goor (1990) in his investigation of teachers at three

career stages concluded that teachers who remain in the


18

educational profession despite their exposure to the stress

of frustrating job conditions and student failure had higher

personal hardiness than those vho left the profession. As a

group, more seasoned or later-career teachers had higher

personal hardiness characteristics than novice or early-

career teachers.

The study most significant to the present investigation

is that of Pierce and Molloy (1990). The subjects in this

research included a total of 750 teachers from 16

contrasting socio-economic status schools. The study was

designed to investigate the psychological and work patterns

of teachers experiencing high and low levels of burnout.

Higher levels of burnout were associated with poorer

physical health, higher rates of absenteeism, lower self-

confidence and more frequent use of regressive coping

strategies. Teachers classified as experiencing high levels

of burnout attributed most of the stress in their lives to

teaching and reported low levels of career commitment and

satisfaction. Further, teachers who recorded high levels of

burnout were characterized by lower levels of the

personality disposition of hardiness.

In summary, the three personality characteristics,

commitment, control and challenge, are the basis of ongoing

research on personal hardiness. While some researchers use

different ways to measure hardiness, research results on

hardiness in teacher samples are sufficient enough to


19

provide incentive for further investigation of Kabasa's

hardiness theory.

Health's Evolution

Humanity's earliest efforts at optimizing health

might be described as survival, avoidance of dangers. With

written records comes increased evidence related to health-

oriented practices. The Hebrew Old Testament and Greek

literature show that health practices were incorporated into

belief systems (Grasser & Craft, 1984). Hippocrates

supported the belief in the body's ability to heal. After

the Middle Ages, the use of the scientific method in health

practices was prevalent. Thus, the evolution of health

moved away from treatment of disease toward use of

information derived from interventions to determine risk

factors (Grasser & Craft, 1984).

cardiovascular Disease as a Measure of Health

The Framingham Heart study in 1948 pioneered the

concept that certain items, known eventually as risk

factors, were associated with the development of heart

disease (Lenfant, stone, & Castelli, 1987). From the 45-

year-old Framingham Heart study emerged the identification

of risk factors for cardiovascular disease such as high

blood cholesterol, elevated blood pressure, cigarette

smoking, overweight, elevated blood sugar, lack of physical


20

activity, and stress (Dawber, 1980).

The concept of risk factors spawned a new generation of

studies aimed at intervening to reduce the impact of risk

factors on health* The first conclusive evidence of the

value in lowering risk factors came in hypertension

intervention trials. A consensus then emerged that

identifying and treating high blood pressure would greatly

lower the stroke rate. This was followed*by a campaign to

encourage health professionals to measure blood pressure and

identify people with elevated blood pressure, and to bring

them into a treatment setting to have their pressures

controlled surfaced (Lenfante, Stone, & Castelli, 1987). By

the late 1970s, the stroke rate had fallen dramatically.

The scientific base provided by the 45-year-old Framingham

Heart Study and the risk factor concept associated with

lifestyle have provided the field with a base for health

promotion and disease prevention programs which include

HEART, the risk testing analysis used in the current study.

Cardiovascular disease is the leading cause of death

and disease in the United States, a somewhat alarming fact

since it is mainly a preventable illness. Cardiovascular

health is an important indicator of a person's overall

health. This is because persons who promote and protect

their own cardiovascular health have chosen lifestyles or

behaviors which lessen their risks, not only for

cardiovascular disease, but also for many other chronic


21

diseases.

chronic cardiovascular disease includes a large list of

diseases that involve the heart (hence, "cardio"), and

vascular system (arteries, veins, heart and lungs). These

include hypertension (also known as high blood pressure),

and atherosclerosis, also known as "hardening of the

arteries" (blocking of the arteries with plaque), i.e.,

coronary artery disease (angina and heart attacks), strokes,

peripheral vascular disease (involving the arteries of the

periphery of the body— neck and head, legs and arms). Also

included are some causes of chronic obstructive lung

diseases and some causes of chronic kidney diseases.

Chronic diseases, such as cardiovascular disease, "are all

related to a common set of risk factors (diet, smoking,

alcohol, exercise)" (stoto, 1991, p. 1460).

In 1989, nearly one million persons in this country

died of cardiovascular disease, "almost as many as those who

died of cancer, accidents, pneumonia, influenza, and all

other causes of death combined" (Gunby, 1992, p. 336).

Changes in health trends reflect a decline in mortality due

to heart disease, however, "heart disease remains the

leading cause of death among men and women" (Kritz-

Siverstein, 1992, p. 335). Progress in the treatment of

heart disease has been made yet "close to one quarter of the

US population, some 69,080,000 persons, has one or more

forms of cardiovascular disease today" (Gunby, 1992, p.


22

336) .

Cardiovascular disease as the leading cause of death is

’’greatly influenced by lifestyle and behavior."

"....Nutritional practices, exercise and stress management

are known to be influential in prevention and treatment of

cardiovascular disease" (Tanner, 1991, p. 845}. Behavioral

and attitudinal changes toward health prevention have

reduced certain risk factors. "Lifestyle has been defined

as all those behaviors over which an individual has control"

(Walker, 1987, p. 76). Risk factors are those tendencies

(inherited, developed or chosen), which increase one's odds

of developing a disease. Risk factors for development of

cardiovascular disease are (l) hypertension, (2) diabetes,

(3) smoking, (4) sedentary lifestyle, (5) diet high in fats,

(6) positive family history of heart disease, one of the

major risk factors in an individual's lifestyle is cigarette

smoking. "Cigarette smoking is now considered the leading

avoidable cause of mortality in the United States,

accounting for approximately 434,000 deaths in 1988"

(Brownson, 1992, p. 99).

Persons who choose lifestyles or behaviors that promote

and protect their overall health and their cardiovascular

health believe their "health outcomes are under their own

control" (Brownson, 1992, p.100.) By making conscious

choices about their lifestyles which promote health, they

have become participants in the health care process, as


23

opposed to those who are only recipients of health care.

They have chosen to prevent diseases and protect their

health. Their overall health, as well as their

cardiovascular health is more likely to remain free from

disease. "When people accept responsibility for their

health, changes in lifestyle practices could prevent and

reverse many of the health problems of today..." (Tanner,

1991, p.846). While health promotion is an imperative goal

for our nation. One needs only to look at the major causes

of cardiovascular disease to know that lifestyle factors are

major contributors. "A number of authors have voiced the

belief that health care is undergoing a revolution...with

the focus shifting specifically to lifestyle and wellness

behaviors..." (Grasser & Craft, 1984, p. 210).

Relationship of Health to Hardiness

Health was found to be an outcome of personality

hardiness (Kobasa, 1977, 1979; Kobasa & Pucetti, 1983).

Individuals high in hardiness are hypothesized to be better

able to withstand the negative effects of life stressors

and, consequently, are less likely than individuals in low

hardiness to become ill (Shepperd & Kashani, 1991). Their

resistance to illness presumably results from perceiving

life changes as less stressful (Kobasa, 1979) or from having

more resources at their disposal to cope with life changes

(Kobasa, 1982). In support of this hypothesis Kobasa found


that hardy executives were more likely to remain healthy

under conditions of high stress than were non-hardy

executives (Kobasa, Maddi, & Kahn, 1982).

An important issue in hardiness research in general

should be noted. Most investigations of hardiness have

relied on self-report instruments to assess health status

rather than using physiological measures or medical records

(Maddi, 1983). There is some evidence that self-report

measures of illness reflect negative affectivity and are

unrelated to actual, long-term health status (Watson &

Fennebaker, 1989). This evidence calls much of the

hardiness literature into question and adds importance to

the results of this study.

Burnout

One of the first uses of the term burnout was in 1974

by Fruedenberger. He used it to describe the conditions of

some volunteers in free clinics. He defined burnout as "a

situation in which one loses an idea - the incentive that

motivates the person. Something within that person dies and

does not return " (p. 160). Fruedenberger, a trained

psychoanalyst, based his model of burnout on emphasizing the

psychology of the individual. He used a case study approach

with focus on an individual's psychological capacities and

vulnerabilities when placed in a stressful work situation.

He was primarily concerned with the individual dynamics of


25

burnout and the psychological reasons why it occurs.

Maslach's Study of Burnout

Christina Maslach and her colleagues at the University

of California, Berkeley provided a complementary contrast to

the theory of burnout. Maslach and Pines, both trained

social psychologists, began to conduct extensive interviews

with over 200 social welfare workers (Pines & Kafry, 1978),

psychiatric nurses (Pines, Aronson & Kafry, 1981), poverty

lawyers (Maslach & Jackson, 1978), prison personnel (Maslach

& Jackson, 1981), and child-care workers (Pines & Maslach,

1978) .

From these studies Maslach arrived at a symptomatic

definition of burnout. She states burnout is a "syndrome of

physical and emotional exhaustion, involving the development

of negative self-concept, negative job attitudes, and loss

of concern and feeling for clients" (Pines & Maslach, 1978,

p.233). Maslach (1993) later expanded on this definition,

"Burnout is a syndrome of emotional exhaustion,

depersonalization, and reduced personal accomplishment that

can occur among individuals who do * people work' of some

kind" (p.l). According to Maslach, burnout is a response to

job-related stress, particularly of emotional overload and a

sense of being overwhelmed by the perceived demands of

others. It is a process; it does not happen spontaneously.

Defining the phenomenon of burnout has resulted in both


26

confusion and controversy. In an attempt to form a working

definition of burnout Maslach found consensus in three

areas. They include the occurrence of burnout at an

individual level; burnout as an internal psychological

experience involving feelings, attitudes, motives and

expectations; and burnout as a negative experience for the

individual, in that it involves distress, problems,

dysfunction and/or negative consequences (Holt, 1985).

Maslach (1984) addressed potential problems of an over-

expanded definition of burnout. These problems include: the

potential of inflating the incidence of the syndrome of

burnout and the risk of inappropriate causes and solutions.

As a result, burnout will become a catch-all phrase with

decreased diagnostic value.

Passivity, impatience, and self-doubt have been found

to have a close association with the experiences of burnout

(Maslach, 1986). For the most part, gender has little

effect on the overall incidence of burnout, however, slight

variations have been noted in depersonalization and

emotional exhaustion (Maslach, 1986). Depersonalization

tends to be demonstrated to a greater extent in men than in

women, while emotional exhaustion tends to occur more often

in women than men. Burnout tends to occur more frequently

in whites than in blacks; and its incidence is greater in

young rather than in older, more experienced people

(Maslach, 1986). Holt (1985), in a study of burnout and


27

hardiness among female elementary teachers ( H ^ l l ) , found

teachers with five to ten years experience were

significantly higher in the high stress-high burnout group.

Maslach clearly presented in her findings (1986) that

burnout does not afflict people indiscriminantly. The

conditions of burnout appear to be a product of a

combination of factors, both internal and external to the

individual, in varying combinations and levels of intensity.

Fruedenberger (1980) echoed this statement: "Not all people

are equally susceptible to burnout, such as the

underachiever and the happy-go-lucky individual with fairly

modest aspirations" (p.19).

Burnout and Hardiness

Not everyone is susceptible to burnout (Maslach, 1993).

Personality differences appear to be related to the

vulnerability of burnout (Maslach, 1982). Burnout, as

described by Maslach, parallels in many ways the

characteristics of low personality hardiness (Lambert &

Lambert, 1987).

Burnout-prone individuals tend not to be aware of their

limits (Maslach, 1982), which is consistent with the

commitment facet of hardiness (Kobasa, 1979). Maslach

(1982), emphasizing self-awareness, implies that a person

needs to know his/her strengths and weaknesses, assets and

liabilities, skills and talents as well as deficiencies in


28

those areas. Haslach (1982) stated that "The emotional

overload that precipitates burnout is more likely to occur

if you do not know when it is time to stop, to say no, or to

make changes" (p.65). Individuals who lack commitment tend

to "establish a sense of self-worth by winning approval and

acceptance of others. In so doing, the person may be so

accommodating that he or she is overextended too often"

(Maslach, 1985, p.63). Assuming responsibility for the

successes and failures of individuals is fertile ground for

experiencing a low sense of personal accomplishment

(Maslach, 1986).

Burnout is related to locus of control, another factor

that also contributes to low hardiness. Rotter defines

locus of control as the degree to which individuals feel

they have personal control over outcomes and suggests that

external locus of control individuals are more prone to

learned helplessness. These types of individuals believe

that they have little or no control over their environment

and tend to withdraw in faces of stress and frustration.

Kyriacou and Sutcliffe (1979) found a significant

correlation between self-reported teacher stress and

external locus of control. They noted that teachers with a

belief in external locus of control may be more likely to

appraise their environment as threatening and thereby may be

more prone to experience occupational stress, thus,

strengthening the conceptual link between burnout and


29

hardiness.

Characteristics of burnout-prone individuals (Maslach,

1982) that correspond to the challenge component of

hardiness include passiveness and powerlessness.

Individuals described like this tend to lack self-esteem or

confidence. This type of individual is at the mercy of his

environment instead of shaping and controlling it. This

behavioral pattern is consistent with that of low challenge

which is characterized by a strong sense of threat toward

changing life events (Kobasa, 1982).

Burnout Research and Teachers

In the education literature, the connection between

stress and burnout is fairly well established (Sarros &

Sarros, 1987; Rinke, 1989; Crump, 1991). Most of the

studies have identified specific work stressors such as role

ambiguity, work load, and overall work stress as

contributing to burnout. Few have looked at the degree to

which the individual teacher experiences the three aspects

of burnout (Schwab & Iwanicki, 1982), that is, emotional

exhaustion, depersonalization, and reduced personal

accomplishment. There is also no consistent estimate of the

extent of burnout among teachers.

Colasurdo (1987) in a study of San Diego teachers

indicated that 52% of the 215 respondents were burned out or

reported frequent feelings of burnout. A recent survey by


30

the American Federation of Teachers (Natale, 1993) yielded a

31% report of being burned out or "getting there".

Symptoms vary in both intensity and frequency. It is

not unusual for any teacher to experience occasional

feelings of burnout. It is when those feelings persist over

a long period of time that they manifest themselves in

chronic physical and emotional difficulties (Iwanicki,

1983).

Yet, there are teachers, despite their exposure to the

stress of teaching who do not exhibit symptoms of burnout at

the same high levels as other teachers in similar

situations. Previous research has attempted to examine the

relationships among teacher background, organizational

factors, individual personality, and various aspects of

teacher burnout. These studies have relied primarily on

teacher questionnaires and are subject to the limitations of

that type of instrument. Nevertheless, the findings provide

some interesting insights about the individual teacher and

burnout.

In a study of 469 randomly selected Massachusetts

teachers, Schwab and Iwanicki (1982) found age to be a

factor in feelings of emotional exhaustion and fatigue.

Younger teachers had more intense feelings than older

counterparts. Carlson (1992) found similar results in

studies with special education teachers. While these

findings have been replicated in other dissertations


31

(Tuettemann & Punch, 1992; Connoly & Sanders, 1986;

Harrison, 1983; Hooper, 1983; Raison, 1981), contradictory

findings about age have been determined in other studies

(Hipps & Haulpin, 1991; Clarke, 1991; Pierce & Molloy, 1990;

Colasurdo, 1981; Malanowski, 1982). A similar contradictory

pattern of findings has been seen when studies have included

teacher sex, years of teaching experience, level of

education, teaching level, marital status, regular or

special education, or size of school district. In a recent

study of 145 urban middle school teachers, Maynard (1992)

reported any attempt to predict teacher burnout should take

into consideration marital status, ethnicity, and sex.

There have been some personality characteristics which

have been shown to influence levels of experienced burnout.

Locus of control was found to be a significant mediator

between stress and burnout among 599 full-time elementary,

203 intermediate and 715 secondary teachers (Byrne, 1992).

Teachers with a more external locus of control evidenced

more feelings of burnout than those with a more internal

orientation. Similar results were found in a survey of 200

high school teachers (Mazur & Lynch, 1989) and in a study of

191 junior high teachers in an interracial cross-cultural

center (Cadavid & Lunenberg, 1991). significant

relationships have also been found between higher levels of

reported burnout an self criticism (Jackson, 1983), low self

regard (Hipps & Malpin, 1991), low sense of competence and


32

self-actualization (Pierce & Molloy, 1990), few coping

strategies (Cadavid & Lunenberg, 1991), and intolerance of

ambiguity (Fielding, 1982).

Cedoline (1982) feels that roost individuals who have

chosen teaching as a profession are disposed by their

personalities to certain attitudes and aptitudes. These

include a high dedication to their work, a sincere desire to

help others, and a willingness to accept certain

occupational disadvantages in order to do what they think

needs to be done. ''The personality characteristics of

altruism, sincere caring, and idealism— when met by lack of

feedback, limited appreciation, public apathy, and day-to-

day stress— make teachers vulnerable candidates for

occupational distress" (p.105).

Summary

Personality hardiness is a stress-resistance resource

(Kobasa, 1977), Health has been found to be an outcome of

hardiness (Kobasa, 1977, 1982; Kobasa, Maddi, & Courington,

1981; Rich & Rich, 1987). The literature is replete with

hardiness studies which measure health based on one's

perception rather than medical analysis (Kobasa, 1977, 1979,

1982; Kobasa, Maddi, & Courington, 1981; Kobasa & Pucetti,

1983; Hammond, 1987; Langeroo, 1987; Holt, Fine, & Tollefson,

1987; Goor, 1990? Pierce & Malloy, 1990).

The link between hardiness and burnout is essential in


33

the descriptions of low hardiness (Kobasa, 1977, 1979) and

burnout (Maslach, 1986), and in the similarities between

coping strategies of educators (Hammond, 1987) and teachers

who experience burnout (Lambert & Lambert 1987).

In the education literature, the connection between

stress and burnout is fairly well established (Sarros &

Sarros, 1987; Rinke, 1989; Crump, 1991). These findings, in

conjunction with Goor's (1990) study of hardiness at three

teacher career stages, corroborates the importance of

studying hardiness, health, and burnout among teachers in

the Sullivan County School System.


CHAPTER 3

Research Design

In this chapter, sample selection and data collection

procedures are presented; demographic information about the

subject population is discussed; instrumentation is

described; and research hypotheses are stated.

A descriptive correlational design was used to

investigate the relationships among hardiness, health and

burnout and to investigate the relationships of the

components of hardiness to health and to the components of

burnout in educators in the Sullivan County School System.

Population

The subjects used in this study were 804 full-time

teachers in the Sullivan County School System located in

Northeast Tennessee. Of these 804 subjects, 213 are high

school teachers, 201 middle school teachers and 390

elementary teachers. They represent four high schools,

nine middle schools, and seventeen elementary schools.

The term "teachers" included regular and special

education classroom teachers, remedial teachers (such as

resource math, reading), as well as special areas (such as

physical education, art, music, and library). The term

"teacher" as used in this research project did not include

administrators or pupil professionals (such as guidance

34
35

counselors, social workers or school psychologists).

Instruments

Two instruments, a personal demographic questionnaire

and a cardiovascular risk factor analysis, will be used to

measure the variables of this study. They include the

third generation version of the Hardiness Test, the Maslach

Burnout Inventory Form Ed, the Educators Demographic Data

Survey, the Health Evaluation and Risk Test (HEART).

The Hardiness Test

In this investigation, the third generation version of

the Hardiness Test, also known as the Personal Views survey,

was used to measure hardiness. This instrument was

developed by Suzanne Kobasa in 19B6. The current version is

a 50-item, four-point Likert scale estimated to take 10

minutes or less to take. It is the result of multiple itera-

and factor-analyses of data from several samples.

In her originating research in 1977, Suzanne C. Kobasa

used a 253-item questionnaire derived from four standardized

instruments and two personality measures she constructed.

The instruments chosen for their "theoretical relevance and

empirical reliability and validity" (Kobasa, 1979, p.5)

included: Jackson's Personality Research Form; Hahn's

California Life Goals Evaluation Schedules; Rotter's Test

for Internal vs. External Locus of Control; Maddi, Kobasa,


36

and Hoover's Alienation vs Commitment Test (Kobasa, 1977).

The Hardiness Test is comprised of three subscales:

challenge, commitment, and control. The challenge items are

numbers 2, 6, 9, 12, IS, 18, 21, 24, 27, 30, 33, 36, 37, 40,

43, 46, and 49. An example from this subscale is, (6) "I

feel uncomfortable if I have to make any changes in my

everyday schedule." The commitment items include numbers 1,

8, 11, 14, 17, 20, 23, 26, 29, 32, 38, 39, 41, 44, 47, and

50. An example from the commitment subscale is , (1) "I

often wake up eager to take up my life where it left off the

day before." The control items include numbers 3, 4, 5, 7,

10, 13, 16, 19, 22, 25, 28,31, 34, 35, 42, 45, and 48. An

example control item is, (4) "Planning ahead can help avoid

most future problems."

These subscales provide negative indicators of

hardiness. In other words, a low score indicates a high

level of hardiness. Each of these scales has adequate

reliability and validity. Factor analysis by Kobasa and

Haddi (1985) include "estimates of internal consistency

with Coefficient Alphas in the .90s for total hardiness

score, and in the .70s for commitment, control, and

challenge scores. Stability appears to be in the .60s over

periods of two weeks or more." The reported reliability

coefficients that were obtained are: .71 for commitment,

.67 for control, .59 for challenge, and .81 for total

hardiness. The validity of the hardiness construct as a


37

moderator of stress has been established through many

studies (Kobasa, 1979, 1980; Kobasa & Maddi, 1982; Kobasa,

Maddi, & Courington, 1981; Kobasa, Maddi & Puccetti, 1982;

Kobasa, Maddi, & Zola, 1983; Kobasa & Puccetti, 1983;

Kobasa, Maddi & Kahn, 1982).

The Maslach Burnout Inventory Form Ed

The Maslach Burnout Inventory Form Ed is a 22-item

Likert scale that contains three subscales, Emotional

Exhaustion, Depersonalization, and Personal Accomplishment;

it is estimated to take 10-15 minutes for completion

(Maslach & Jackson, 1986).

Burnout is a continuous variable with ranges of low to

high degrees of experienced feeling. Teachers are not

classified as "burned out" or "not burned out", instead they

are placed on a continuum of "more burned out" to "less

burned out". Those teachers who experience a higher degree

of burnout will score high on Depersonalization and

Emotional Exhaustion and low on Personal Accomplishment.

Scores of those teachers who experience a low degree of

burnout will be reversed, while teachers experiencing

moderate levels of burnout will have moderate scores on all

three subscales. Scores of each subscale are considered

separately and are not combined into a single, total score.

Scores are considered high if they are in the upper

third of the normative distribution, moderate if they are in


38

the middle third, and low if they are in the lower third.

Scores for groups of respondents can also be treated as

aggregate data with means and standard deviations computed

for each subscale, comparisons made to available normative

data or local norms, and correlations made with other

information obtained from respondents.

The Maslach Burnout Inventory norms and scale

development are based on a sample (H = 2000) human service

employees (police officers, teachers, counselors, social

workers, physicians, psychologists, and lawyers). The MBI

Form Ed is basically the same as the MBI. The only

modification of items in the MBI Form Ed has been to change

the word "recipient" to "student". In the teaching

profession, students are the teachers' recipients (Schwab,

1993). Validity and reliability are substantiated by two

studies conducted with these changes, Iwanicki and Schwab.

Factor analytic studies by Iwanicki and Schwab report

Cronbach alpha estimates of .90 for Emotional Exhaustion,

.76 for Depersonalization, and .76 for Personal

Accomplishment, while Gold reports estimates of .88, .74,

and .72, respectively. These reliabilities parallel those

of the MBI (Schwab, 1993).

Mean scores and standard deviations tend to be slightly

higher for teachers on Emotional Exhaustion (teachers mean «

21.25, overall sample mean “ 20.99); substantially higher on

Depersonalization (teachers mean = 11.00, overall sample


39

mean “ 8.73); and lower scores on Personal Accomplishment

(teachers mean « 33.54, overall sample mean = 34.58). Since

the mean scores vary from the overall sample, the cut-off

points for classifying teachers who are experiencing high,

average, and low levels of burnout vary from other subgroups

and the overall sample (Schwab, 1993).

HEART
Health will be measured through the analysis of HEART

(Health Evaluation and Risk Test) which is available to all

Sullivan County School System employees. This testing

occurred during the fall of 1993. Incentives to participate

include: no cost to employees, testing is done in a mobile

unit at each school on school time, and follow-up

conferences with a Blue Cross Blue Shield physician who

provides the participants with an explanation of their test


results.

The American Heart Association has identified the

factors which make it possible to determine the extent of

risk of cardiovascular disease. They include:

cardiovascular fitness, systolic blood pressure, diastolic

blood pressure, body fat percentage, total cholesterol,

H.D.L. cholesterol, triglycerides, glucose, resting

electocardiagraro, stress electrocardiogram, tobacco use,

tension and stress, personal history of heart disease,

family history of heart disease, and age factor. Tests


40

results from the cardiovascular disease testing and the

health evaluation questionnaire are used to develop a

composite cardiovascular disease risk factor score,

identifying the risk factors which affect individuals and to

what extent they may influence present and future risk of

cardiovascular disease. Included on each teacher's test

report is an entry labeled "Total Risk Points"; this is used

to determine the overall risk category and serves as a

measure of health. The risk categories include:

Total Risk Points Risk

5.9 or less Points Very Low


6.0 - 13.9 Points Low
14.0 - 22.9 Points Moderate
23.0 - 31.9 Points High
32.0 or more Points Very High

Each participant will be asked to report this risk

factor on the Educators Demographic Data Sheet distributed

with the Maslach Burnout Inventory Form Ed and the

Hardiness Test.

Educators Demographic Data Sheet

The Educators Demographic Data Sheet will be used to

collect descriptive information about the sample.

Demographic data will include sex, age, education level,

primary assignment, number of years in current assignment,

and number of years in education.


41

Data Collection Procedures

Data collection procedures followed in this study will

emphasize a personalized approach from the researcher to the

potential subjects. Prior to a first contact with the

teachers, a letter will be sent by John O'Dell,

Superintendent of the Sullivan County School System to all

regular classroom teachers in the system. He will request

their support in participating in this research study. The

researcher will attend all Wellness counseling sessions. At

that time each subject will be given a testing packet which

will include a letter explaining the study and asking for

their participation, instruments consisting of the Hardiness

Test, Maslach Burnout Inventory Form Ed, Educators

Demographic Data Sheet to include a request for the risk

factor from the Health Evaluation and Risk Testing, and an

envelope which is preaddressed to the researcher. The

answer sheets and return envelopes will be numerically coded

in order to protect the subjects' confidentiality and to

inform the researcher of nonresponse.

Data Analysis

This investigation used correlational techniques:

Pearson Product Moment correlations and multiple regression

analyses. Descriptive statistics and one-way analyses of

variance were computed as needed. Pearson product moment


42

correlation coefficients were used to evaluate the

correlations among all variables: the three MBI Form Ed

subscales; the three Hardiness subscales; and gender, age,

years teaching, teaching assignment, and level of education

from the Educators Demographic Data Survey. The level of

significance was 0.05.

Hypotheses

The twenty-two null hypotheses in this study are:

1. There is no statistically significant relationship

between levels of hardiness, as measured by the Hardiness

Test, and health, as reported on the Educators Demographic

Data Sheet (EDDS) and measured by the Health Evaluation and

Risk Test (HEART).

2. There is no statistically significant relationship

between levels of hardiness, asmeasured by the Hardiness

Test, and degrees of burnout asmeasured by the Emotional

Exhaustion subscale of the Maslach Burnout Inventory (MBI)

Form Ed.

3. There is no statistically significant relationship

between levels of hardiness, asmeasured by the Hardiness

Test, and degrees of burnout as measured by the

Depersonalization subscale of the MBI Form Ed.

4. There 1b no statistically significant relationship

between levels of hardiness, asmeasured by the Hardiness

Test, and degrees of burnout asmeasured by the Personal


43

Accomplishment subscale of the MBI Form Ed.

5. There is no statistically significant relationship

between levels of commitment, as measured by the Hardiness

Test, and health, as reported on the EDDS and measured by

HEART.

6. There is no statistically significant relationship

between levels of control, as measured bythe Hardiness

Test, and health, as reported on the EDDS and measured by

HEART.

7. There is no statistically significant relationship

between levels of challenge, as measured by the Hardiness

Test, and health, as reported on the EDDS and measured by

HEART.

8. There is no statistically significant relationship

between levels of commitment, as measured by the Hardiness

Test, and Emotional Exhaustion as measured by the MBI Form

Ed.

9. There is no statistically significant relationship

between levels of control, as measured by the Hardiness

Test, and Emotional Exhaustion as measured by the MBI Form

Ed.

10. There is no statistically significant relationship

between levels of challenge, as measured by the Hardiness

Test, and Emotional Exhaustion as measured by the MBI Form

Ed.

11. There is no statistically significant relatioinship


44

between levels of commitment, as measured by the Hardiness

Test, and Depersonalization as measured by the MBI Form Ed.

12. There is no statistically significant relationship

between levels of control, as measured by the Hardiness

Test, and Depersonalization as measured by the MBI Form Ed.

13. There is no statistically significant relationship

between levels of challenge, as measured by the Hardiness

Test, and Depersonalization as measured by the MBI Form Ed.

14. There is no statistically significant relationship

between levels of commitment, as measured by the Hardiness

Test, and Personal Accomplishment as measured by the MBI

Form Ed.

15. There is no statistically significant relationship

between levels of control, as measured by the Hardiness

Test, and Personal Accomplishment as measured by the MBI

Form Ed.

16. There is no statistically significant relatioinship

between levels of challenge, as measured by the Hardiness

Test, and Personal Accomplishment as measured by the MBI

Form Ed.

17. There is no statistically significant relationship

between age and the study variables (hardiness, commitment,

control, challenge, health, Emotional Exhaustion,

Depersonalization, and Personal Accomplishment).

18. There is no statistically significant relationship

between gender and the study variables (hardiness,


45

commitment, control, challenge, health, Emotional

Exhaustion, Depersonalization, and Personal Accomplishment).

19. There is no statistically significant relationship

between years teaching experience and the study variables

(hardiness, commitment, control, challenge, health,

Emotional Exhaustion, Depersonalization, and Personal

Accomplishment).

20. There is no statistically significant relationship

between highest level of education and the study variables

(hardiness, commitment, control, challenge, health,

Emotional Exhaustion, Depersonalization, and Personal

Accomplishment).

21. There is no statistically significant relationship

between level taught and the study variables (hardiness,

commitment, control, challenge, health, Emotional

Exhaustion, Depersonalization, and Personal Accomplishment).

22. There is no statistically significant relationship

between teaching assignment and the study variables

(hardiness, commitment, control, challenge, health,

Emotional Exhaustion, Depersonalization, and Personal

Accomplishment).

Summary of Methodology

A descriptive correlational design was used to study

the relationships among hardiness, health, and burnout among

teachers in the Sullivan County School System. A sample of


46

804 full time classroom teachers, representing four high

schools, nine middle schools, and seventeen elementary

schools was recruited.

Instrumentation included the Hardiness Test (a

composite score and three subscores for commitment, control,

and challenge), the Health Evaluation and Risk Test

(cardiovascular risk factor), and the Maslach Burnout

Inventory Form Ed (subscale scores for Emotional Exhaustion,

Depersonalization, and Personal Accomplishment) to measure

hardiness, health, and burnout, respectively. Demographic

data were collected from the Educators Demographic Data

Survey.

All regular classroom Sullivan County teachers were

contacted in writing by John O'Dell, superintendent of

Schools, to encourage them to volunteer to participate in

the study. Arrangements were made through Anthony Delucia,

Chairperson, Institutional Review Board, East Tennessee

State University to ensure that the Institutional Review

Board's criteria were met. Dates and locations at

participating schools were arranged for data collection.

An explanation was presented to participating teacher

subjects that included the purpose of the study,

instructions for completing the questionnaire, confirmation

of anonymity of data, procedure for requesting a copy of

research findings, and an expression of appreciation for the

teachers' participation in the study. Questions and


feedback were solicited from the teachers.
CHAPTER 4

Results

The purpose of this study was to explore the

relationships of hardiness and the three subscales of

hardiness: commitment, control, and challenge, to health

and to the subscales of burnout: Emotional Exhaustion (EE),

Depersonalization (DP), and Personal Accomplishment (PA) in

teachers. These relationships were investigated through a

descriptive correlational design.

The Hardiness Test and the Maslach Burnout Inventory

Form Ed were used to measure the personality traits. Health

was measured by reporting the total risk factor points

scored on the Health Evaluation and Risk Test (HEART). Data

collected from 501 teachers in the Sullivan County School

System were analyzed, utilizing Spearman rho and Eta

correlation coefficients and multiple regression analyses.

Descriptive statistics of the variables are presented,

followed by the results of the data analyses computed to

address each of the research hypotheses and questions.

Sample Demographics

Five hundred twenty-one (64.8%) of a total population

of eight hundred four teachers volunteered to participate in

this study. Five hundred one (96.16%) of the five hundred

twenty-one questionnaires were analyzed; twenty were

unusable because of missing risk factors.

48
49

Gender, age, years teaching experience, grade level

assigned, level of education, and regular or special

education assignment were the demographic variables selected

for analysis. Frequency distributions with percentages were

computed for each demographic variable and are presented

in Table 1.

Table 1

Gender. Ace, and Years Teaching Experience

Category Number Percent

Gender

Female 389 77.6


Male 112 22.4

501 100.0
Age
Under 26 18 3.6
26 - 35 72 14.4
36 - 45 204 40.7
46 - 55 163 32.5
56 and over 44 8.8

501 100.0

Years Teaching Experience


1-3 33 6.6
4 -9 55 11.0
10 - 14 76 15.1
15 - 19 126 25.4
20 - 24 118 23.3
25 - 29 72 14.4
30 and over 21 4.2

501 100.0
50

Table 2

Level of Education. School Level Assigned. Class Type

Category Number Percent

Level of Education

Bachelors 209 41.7


Masters 217 43.3
Masters Plus 68 13.6
Specialist 5 1.0
Doctorate 2 .4

501 100.0

School Level Assigned

Elementary 241 48.1


Middle 140 27.9
High 120 24.0

501 100.0

Class Type Assigned

Regular Ed 442 88.2


Special Ed 59 11.8

501 100.0

The majority of teachers participating in this study

were female (77.6%). Most respondents were between the ages

of 36-45 (40.7%) and 46-55 (32.5%) with the highest

percentage in the 36-45 age group (40.7%) and the lowest in

the under 26 age group (3.6%). Years of teaching experience

varied from 1-3 years (6.6%) to 30 and over (4.2%). Most

teachers had between 10-24 years teaching experience; 10-14


51

years (15.1%), 15-19 years (25,4%), and 20-24 years (23.3%).

The largest group of teachers were elementary (48.1%)

followed by middle (27.7%) then high school (24.0%). A

majority of teachers held Master's (43.3%) or Bachelor's

(41.7%) degrees. Four hundred forty -two (88.2%) were

regular education teachers. The sample included 59 (11.7%)

special education teachers.

Description of. Hardiness. Health, and Burnout Scores

Table 3 shows descriptive statistics for the

independent variables. The means, standard deviations,

ranges, and reliability coefficients are presented for the

raw scores for hardiness and its subscales. Descriptive

statistics and analyses of data for the health scale and

burnout subscales were computed on raw scores. Descriptive

statistics, including mean scores, standard deviations,

ranges of scores, and reliability coefficients were used to

report group performance on each instrument.

Reliability coefficients for the Hardiness Test subscales

ranged from .63 to .80. The coefficients on Table 3

indicate that commitment was the most reliable subscale

associated with the hardiness construct followed by control

and challenge. The Maslach Burnout Form Ed had higher

reliability coefficients. Reliability coefficients for the

MBI Form Ed ranged from .70 to .89, possibly this reflects

its standardization for commercial use.


An analysis of survey instrument data implied both the

Hardiness Test and Maslach Burnout Inventory Form Ed met

acceptable reliability requirements. An alpha reliability

coefficient of .86 indicated the Hardiness Test was a

reliable instrument to measure total hardiness in spite of

the fact that the subscale reliabilities were moderate,

ranging from .63 for challenge, .67 for control, and much

higher for commitment at .80. Reliability coefficients for

the Maslach Burnout Inventory Form Ed ranged from .70 for

Depersonalization, .79 for Personal Accomplishment, and .89

for Emotional Exhaustion. These moderate to high

reliabilities indicate that the three subscales for

measuring burnout are adequate.

Hardiness Test

The possible raw score range was 0 to 48 for the

commitment subscale, 0 to 51 for the control and challenge

subscales, and 0 to 150 for total hardiness. The mean raw

scores obtained in this study were 38.67 for commitment,

39.08 for control, 31.56 for challenge, and 109.44 for total

hardiness. The mean subscale scores are consistent with

those reported by Holt (1985) for a sample of 211 elementary

public school teachers.


53

Table 3

o v a v j.B V .L C B i.c r ^ n a ir u j.n B B B a n u _ o u w B c a j.


and Burnout Subscales fRaw Scores!

Factor Mean SD Range Reliability

Hardiness 109.44 12.97 69 - 137 .86

Commitment 38.67 5.20 23 - 48 .80

Control 39.08 4.84 27 - 50 .67

Challenge 31.56 5.69 15 - 49 .63

Health 13.30 5.97 .5 - 35.1

Burnout

EE 22.99 10.90 0-52 .89

DP 6.92 5.44 0-29 .70

PA 37.38 7.00 13 - 48 .79

Health
The possible scoring range for the Health Evaluation

and Risk Evaluation (HEART) was 0.5 to 40.3. The mean score

obtained in this study was 13.30 placing the teachers of the

Sullivan County School System in the low range for

cardiovascular disease. Comparisons of the health score to

other studies are ill-advised because of the variation in

instrumentation used by researchers to measure health.

Table 4 provides the frequency and percentages of total


54

risk points. Host teachers were in the low (48.3%) and

moderate (34.9%) categories of risk points with the highest

percentage in the 6.0 - 13.9 risk point group (48.3%) and

the lowest in the very high (.2%).

Table 4

Frequency and Percentages of Total Risk Points--------------

Total Risk Points Frequency Percentage

Very Low (5.9 or less) 46 9.2

Low ( 6.0 - 13.9) 242 48.3

Moderate (14.0-22.9) 175 34.9

High (23.0 - 31.9) 37 7.4

Very High (32.0 or more) 1 .2

501 100.0

Burnout

The possible scoring ranges for the Burnout subscales

were as follows: Emotional Exhaustion, 0 to 54;

Depersonalization, 0 to 30; Personal Accomplishment, 0 to 48

(Schwab, 1993). Means reported for subscale scores for a

sample of 469 Massachusetts teachers and 462 California

teachers were: 21.25 for Emotional Exhaustion (slightly

higher than the 20.99 overall sample mean); 11.00 for

Depersonalization (substantially higher than the 8.73

overall sample mean); and 33.54 for Personal Accomplishment

(slightly lower than the 34.58 overall sample mean) (Schwab,


55

1993) .

For this study sample, a mean of 22.99 was obtained for

Emotional Exhaustion which Indicates a moderate level of

experienced burnout, as measured by this subscale. For the

Depersonalization subscale, the sample mean of 6.92 Is

Indicative of a low level of Depersonalization. The sample

mean of 37.38 obtained for Personal Accomplishment is

indicative of a low level of this aspect of burnout. The

study sample means were consistent with the results reported

by.Schoenig (1986) for 188 public school teachers for the

three subscales: Emotional Exhaustion (H « 20.69),

Depersonalization (H = 6.72), and Personal Accomplishment

(M - 35.48).

Table 5 provides the frequency and percentages of

experienced burnout of respondents as measured by the

Maslach Burnout Inventory. The highest percentage of

teachers (60.4%) scored in the high category of personal

accomplishment with 22.4% in the moderate group and 17.2% in

the low group. Emotional exhaustion scores were evenly

distributed with 35.1% in the high and moderate group and

29.8% in the low group. The majority of teachers (68.3%)

were in the low depersonalization subscale.


56

Table 5

Frequency and Percentages o f Experienced Burnout of

Respondents as Measured bv the Maslach Burnout Inventory

Variables Frequency Percentage

Personal Accomplishment

High (0 - 30) 303 60.4

Moderate (31 -36) 112 22.4

Low (37 or over) 86 17.2

501 100.0

Emotional Exhaustion

High ( 27 or over) 175 35.1

Moderate ( 17 -26) 175 35.1

Low (0-16) 151 29.8

501 100.0

Depersonalization

High (14 or over) 68 13.6

Moderate (9-13) 90 18.0

Low (0-8) 343 68.4

501 100.0
57

Research Question 1

The first research question was: Hhat is the

relationship between hardiness and health among teachers in

the Sullivan County School System? A Spearman rho

correlation was used to address this question and null

hypotheses one:

Hoi: There is no statistically significant

relationship between levels of hardiness and health. A

correlation of .15 (p < .001) was obtained between total

hardiness and health among teachers in the Sullivan County

School System who participated in this study, indicating a

weak although statistically significant relationship and

resulting in the rejection of hypotheses one. Furthermore,

an r,3 of .023 indicates that hardiness accounts for only 2%

of the variance of health.

Further analyses, utilizing Spearman rho correlation

coefficients, were conducted with respect to the

relationships between the subscales of hardiness and health.

The coefficient obtained for the commitment subscale and

health was .086 (p < .001), with an r,3 of .007, indicating

that commitment accounts for 1% of the variance in health.

For the control subscale and health, the coefficient

obtained was . 0933 (p < .001), yielding an r,3 of .009,

which indicates that control explains 1% of the variance in


58

health. The coefficient obtained for the challenge subscale

and health was .1370 (g < .001), yielding an r,1 of .019

which indicates that challenge explains 2% of the variance

in health. These results further support the weak

relationship between total hardiness and health.

Research Question 2

The second research question was: What are the

relationships among total hardiness and the subscales of

burnout (Emotional Exhaustion, Depersonalization, and

Personal Accomplishment) in public school teachers in the

Sullivan County School System? Three null hypotheses were

stated in response to this question:

H02: There is no statistically significant

relationship between Hardiness and Emotional Exhaustion.

1^3: There is no statistically significant

relationship between Hardiness and Depersonalization.

Hq4: There is no statistically significant

relationship between Hardiness and Personal Accomplishment.

Spearman rho correlation coefficients were used to

analyze these relationships. The correlation coefficient

between total Hardiness and Emotional Exhaustion was -.53

(g < .001), indicating an inverse relationship between these

two variables. Null hypotheses two was rejected. An r,2 of

.2809 was obtained which indicates that Hardiness explains

28% of the variance in Emotional Exhaustion. Between total


Hardiness and Depersonalization, the coefficient was -.43

(E < .001), indicating an inverse relationship and the

rejection of hypotheses three. An r,3 of .1849 indicates that

Hardiness accounts for 18% of the variance in

Depersonalization. Between total Hardiness and Personal

Accomplishment the coefficient was .40 (e < *001),

indicating a positive relationship between these two

variables, thus hypotheses four was rejected. An r,a of .16

indicates that Hardiness accounts for 16% of the variance in

Personal Accomplishment. The moderate, inverse relationship

between total Hardiness and Emotional Exhaustion was the

strongest of the three; the other two were rather weak.

Research Question 3

The third research question was: What is the

relationship of each subscale of hardiness (commitment,

control and challenge) to health in public school teachers

in the Sullivan County School System? Stepwise multiple

regression analysis was used to address this question and

hypotheses five through seven. This technique allowed the

researcher to determine which independent variables remain

significant predictors of burnout when they are considered

in the presence of other previously significant predictors.

Stepwise regression also identifies which-independent*'

variables, together, explained the greatest amount of

variance.
60

H|)5: There is no correlation between levels of

commitment, as measured by the Hardiness Test, and health,

as reported on the Educators Demographic Data sheet (EDDS)

and measured by the Health Evaluation and Risk Test (HEART).

H„6: There is no statistically significant

relationships between levels of control and health.

Ho7: There is no statistically significant

relationship between challenge and health.

Commitment entered the regression equation at the first

step (£ » 16.58, p < .0001) and explained 4% of the

variance. Control and challenge did not enter the equation,

therefore, hypotheses five was rejected, while hypotheses

six and seven were retained. The results are presented in

Table 6.

Table 6

Regression Analysis for Health Using Hardiness subscales

Variables R Rl P

Commitment .193 .04 16.58

E < .0001

Research Question 4

The fourth research question was; What are the

relationships among the hardiness subscales (commitment,

control, and challenge) and each of the subscales of burnout


61

(Emotional Exhaustion, Depersonalization, and Personal

Accomplishment) in public school teachers in the Sullivan

County School System? To address this question and

hypotheses eight through sixteen three stepwise multiple

regression analyses were used.

Hull Hypotheses Eight through Sixteen

H(,8: There is no statistically significant

relationship between commitment and Emotional Exhaustion.

Hg9: There is no statistically significant

relationship between control and Emotional Exhaustion.

Hpio: There is no statistically significant

relationship between challenge and Emotional Exhaustion.

Holl: There is no statistically significant

relationship between commitment and Depersonalization.

Hq12: There is no statistically significant

relationship between control and Depersonalization.

Hol3: There is no statistically significant

relationship between challenge and Depersonalization.

H(,14: There is no statistically significant

relationship between commitment and Personal Accomplishment.

Hol5: There is no statistically significant

relationship between control and Personal Accomplishment.

1^16: There is no statistically significant

relationship between challenge and Personal Accomplishment.

Commitment entered the first regression equation at the


62

firBt step (£ « 219.04 s < .00005), accounting for 34% of

the variance in Emotional Exhaustion and the rejection of

hypotheses eight. At the second step, challenge entered the

equation (£ = 114.09 £ < .00005), explaining an additional

1% of the variance and the rejection of hypotheses nine.

Hypotheses ten was not rejected as control did not enter the

equation. In table 7 the results of the first regression

equation are presented.

Table 7

Regression Analysis for Emotional Exhaustion Using Hardiness

Subscales

Variable R R1 F

Commitment .58 .34 219.04

Challenge .59 .35 114.09

p < .00005

Commitment entered the second regression equation at

the first step (£ =• 43.44, p < .00005), explaining 24% of

the variance in depersonalization, challenge and control

did not enter the equation. For the second equation, in

which the subscales of hardiness were regressed on the

dependent variable Depersonalization, the results are

presented in Table 8. Hypothesis eleven was rejected while

hypotheses twelve and thirteen were retained.


63

Table 8

Results of Regression Analysis for Depersonalization Using

Hardiness SubBcales

Variable R R1 F

Commitment .49 .24 43.44

fi < .00005

Commitment entered the third equation at the first step

(E ° 24.05, p < .00005), explaining 15% of the variance in

Personal Accomplishment, and retaining hypothesis fourteen.

Challenge and control did not enter the equation, thus

hypotheses fifteen and sixteen were retained. The stepwise

multiple regression results are presented in Table 10.

Table 9

Results of Regression Analysis for Personal Accomplishment

Using Hardiness Subscales

Variable R R1 F

Commitment .14 .38 24.05

E> < .00005

Research Question 5

The fifth research question was: What is the

relationship, if any, between selected demographic variables


64

as reported by the Educators Demographic Data survey (EDDS)

and the three subscales of burnout and hardiness?

To determine the extent to which the subjects'

demographic variables were related to any study variables,

correlation coefficients were computed between demographic

variables and each of the study variables. Null hypotheses

seventeen through twenty-two were addressed.

Null Hypotheses Seventeen through Twenty-two

H(,17: There is no statistically significant

relationship between age and the study variables (hardiness,

commitment, control, challenge, health, Emotional

Exhaustion, Depersonalization, and Personal Accomplishment).

HqIS: There is no statistically significant relationship

between gender and the study variables (hardiness,

commitment, control, challenge, health, Emotional

Exhaustion, Depersonalization, and Personal Accomplishment).

Hq19: There is no statistically significant

relationship between years teaching experience and the study

variables (hardiness, commitment, control, challenge,

health, Emotional Exhaustion, Depersonalization, and

Personal Accomplishment).

Ho20: There is no statistically significant

relationship between highest level of education and the

study variables (hardiness, commitment, control, challenge,

health, Emotional Exhaustion, Depersonalization, and


65

Personal Accomplishment).

Ho21: There is no statistically significant

relationship between level taught and the study variables

(hardiness, commitment, control, challenge, health,

Emotional Exhaustion, Depersonalization, and Personal

Accomplishment).

Hq22: There is no statistically significant

relationship between teaching assignment and the study

variables (hardiness, commitment, control, challenge,

health, Emotional Exhaustion, Depersonalization, and

Personal Accomplishment).

Spearman rho correlation coefficients were computed

between the demographic variables age and years teaching

experience and total hardiness, hardiness subscales, health,

and burnout subscales. Eta correlation coefficients were

calculated between the demographic variables gender, highest

education level, level taught, type of teaching assignment

and total hardiness, hardiness subscales, health, and

burnout scales.

Ace

spearman rho correlation coefficients indicated a

statistically significant relationship between age and

Health (r,J = .36, p < .001). This relatively low

correlation indicates that age explains 13% of the variance

in the risk of cardiovascular disease. Only this portion of


66

hypothesis seventeen was rejected all other portions were

retained.

Gender

While Eta correlation coefficients revealed

statistically significant relationships between gender and

total hardiness (Eta «* ,1 3 , E < .005), commitment (Eta =

.16, e < .0005), control (Eta = .11, p <.01), and

Depersonalization (Eta ® .09, p < .05), these extremely low

correlations indicate that only 1% of the variance in these

four variables is explained by gender, preventing meaningful

interpretation. Hypothesis eighteen is rejected for these

four parts and retained for challenge, Emotional Exhaustion,

and Personal Accomplishment.

Years Teaching Experience

All variables (health, total hardiness and the

hardiness BUbscales and the burnout subscales) with the

exception of Depersonalization revealed statistically

significant relationships with years of teaching experience,

thus only one part (Depersonalization) of hypothesis

nineteen was retained. All other parts were rejected.

Spearman rho correlation coefficients between years teaching

experience and health (r, ■ .29, p < .05), years teaching

experience and Emotional Exhaustion (r, « .10, p < .05)

indicated 8% and 1% of the variance respectively. These


67

relatively low correlations indicate that older teachers

have greater cardiovascular disease risk and experience more

Emotional Exhaustion. Total hardiness (r( « -.15, £ < .01)

and the hardiness subscales, commitment (rt *» -.14, £ <

.001), challenge (r. -.12, £ < .01), control (rf = -.13, £

< .01), along with the burnout subscale Personal

Accomplishment (r, = -.10, £ < .05) were inversely related

to years teaching experience. This shows that younger

teachers were hardier and felt greater Personal

Accomplishment.

Highest Level of Education

To determine the extent to which the subjects' level of

education was related to any of the study variables, Eta

correlation coefficients were computed between level of

education and each of the study variables. Level of

education was significantly related to Personal

Accomplishment (Eta = .16, £ < .01), the extremely low

correlations indicate that only 2% of the variance in this

variable is explained by level of education, preventing

meaningful interpretations and the rejection of only the

Personal Accomplishment portion of hypothesis twenty and the

retainment of all other parts.

Level Taught

As indicated in Table 10, level taught (elementary,


middle or high) and Health ( Eta = .15, £ < .05)

Depersonalization (Eta » .16, p < .001) and Personal

Accomplishment (Eta » .14, p < .01) were found to be

statistically correlated through Eta coefficients. These

portions of hypothesis twenty-one were rejected. Hardiness,

commitment, challenge, control, and Emotional Exhaustion

were not statistically correlated, thus these portions of

hypothesis twenty-one were retained.

Teaching Assignment

To identify the depth of the relationships between

teaching assignment and the hardiness, health, and burnout

measures Eta coefficients were computed. The results

revealed no significant correlations, thus hypothesis

twenty-two was retained.


69

Table 10

Correlation Coefficients of Study Variables

Variable Risk H Commit Chall Control EE DP PA

Age .36* .08 .05 .09 .06 .07 .04 .09

Gender .01 .13* .16* .03 .11* .04 .09* .04

Experience .29* -.15* -.14* -.12* -.13* .10 .08 - .10*

Education .10 .10 .16 .09 .10 .03 .07 .14*

Level .15* .09 .09 .04 .04 .05 .16* .12

Assignment .07 .05 .06 .07 .10 .07 .10 .06

*E < .05

Risk «* HEART risk point


H « Total hardiness score
Commit ** Commitment
Chall *> Challenge
Control « Locus of Control
EE = Emotional Exhaustion
DP =* Depersonalization
PA = Personal Accomplishment
CHAPTER 5

Discussion, Conclusions, and

Recommendations

Discussion

Hardiness and Health

On the basis of hardiness theory it was anticipated

that the relationship between hardiness and health would

have been stronger than that obtained (r, “ .15, p < .001).

Correlations between the subscales of hardiness and health

revealed that commitment (r, “ .086, p < .001), control

(r, “ .093, p < .001), and challenge (r, = .137, p < .001)

were weakly correlated with health. The very low

correlation coefficient obtained between challenge and

health supports a growing sentiment that the challenge

component of hardiness is either not being measured

appropriately (Funk & Houston, 1987), or that it does not

contribute significantly to hardiness (Rich & Rich, 1987).

In view of the correlation coefficients obtained for

the subscales of hardiness in relation to health, the

results of the regression analysis were not unexpected.

Commitment entered the regression equation at the first

step, accounting for only 4% of the variance (£ = 16.58, p,

.0001). Control and Challenge failed to enter the equation.

It is possible that hardiness does not interact in a

70
71

protective way with health as proposed by Kobasa (1985). In

hardiness research, health has been viewed as the absence of

illness. The most common measures of health used in

hardiness research has been some form of symptom and disease

checklist (Kobasa, 1977, 1979, 1982; Kobasa, Maddi, & Kahn,

1982; Kobasa & Pucetti, 1983; Shepperd & Kashani, 1991).

Research findings reported for studies of hardiness and

health have varied in their support of a hardiness-health

link. The two strongest supporting studies that have been

reported is a positive relationship (p “ <48, p < .01)

between hardiness and physical illness by Nowack (1988) in a

study of professional employees and Tang (1989) in a study

of 167 police officers (p = .59, p < .001).

This study's departure from the customary use of

illness instrumentation to measure health resulted in a

significant correlation between hardiness and health, albeit

rather weak. Another pertinent issue in this regard is that

the mean health score of 13.27 (range = .5 - 35.1; SD =

5.97) for the study sample suggests a relatively low level

of cardiovascular disease risk among the subjects. It is

worth noting that the overall total risk factor for all

Sullivan County Educators was 16.24 (n = 942). It may be

that, for a sample with a broader range of health scores, a

higher correlation with hardiness would have been observed.

It is likely that a randomized sample, in contrast to the

volunteer participants in this study, would have elicited


72

less skewed health scores. It is conceivable that teachers

whose cardiovascular disease risk points were low were more

inclined to participate as volunteers in this study, thus

skewing the data. Because these findings are based on data

from a convenience sample, they cannot generalize to

teachers beyond this sample. The results do however,

support a theoretical link between hardiness and health.

Hardiness.and Burnout

The correlation coefficients obtained for total

hardiness and the burnout subscales are consistent with

previous research findings. For hardiness and Emotional

Exhaustion, a correlation coefficient of -.53 was obtained,

for Depersonalization, the correlation coefficient was -.43,

and for Personal Accomplishment, the correlation coefficient

was .40 (all at the .001 alpha level). These correlation

coefficients are shown in Table 11.

Clarke (1991) reported a correlation coefficient of .57

(S,.01) for total hardiness and burnout for 211 nursing

students. Schoenig (1986), in a study of 188 public school

teachers found a correlation coefficient of .48 (j> < .05)

between total hardiness and burnout. Clarke stated that

hardiness correlated with each of the subscales of burnout,

reporting correlation coefficients of -.49 for Emotional

Exhaustion, -.33 for Depersonalization, and .42 for Personal

Accomplishment (all at the .05 alpha level).


73

When relationships among the subscales of hardiness and

the subscales of burnout were analyzed with multiple

regression, it was found that commitment and control

accounted for most of the variation in the burnout scores.

Commitment and challenge explained a total of 35% of the

variance in emotional exhaustion. For Depersonalization

commitment accounted for 24% of the variance while control

and challenge failed to enter the equation. For Personal

Accomplishment, commitment accounted for 15%; neither

control nor challenge entered the equation. Although it is

difficult to compare these findings with previous research

findings because of instrumentation differences, they are

consistent with those reported by Williams (1968) and Holt

(1985).

Both Williams (1988) and Holt (1985) used the Locus of

Control Scale to measure hardiness in their respective

samples, both reported correlations with burnout for five

hardiness subscales that were being used to measure

hardiness. The correlation coefficients among the subscales

of hardiness and burnout reported by Holt (1985) were

consistent with those reported by Williams (1988). Neither

Holt nor Williams obtained statistically significant

relationships between the challenge subscale and burnout.

The limited extent to which the challenge subscale

interacted with the burnout variables is similar to its

interaction with health. The correlation of challenge with


74

the burnout variables was an expected finding based on

previous research reports (Holt, 1985; Williams, 1988;

Clarke, 1991).

Table 11

Correlation Coefficients for Burnout Subscales and Hardiness

Sttfrgsalee

EE DP PA

Commitment -.53* -.45* .34*

Control -.43* -.29* .26*

Challenge .40* -.25* .20*

*E < .001

The level of burnout for each of the subscales found in

the study sample is consistent with studies of public school

teachers (Schoenig, 1986) and female elementary teachers

(Holt, 1985). This finding suggests that burnout is

prevalent among educators. This concern is offset, at least

in part, by the interactions found among the commitment and

control components of hardiness and burnout. While not

strong, the relationships hold promise that hardiness could

provide some protection against the harmful effects of

stress on teachers.

As previously pointed out, these findings cannot be

generalized beyond this convenience sample. It is worth

noting; however, that despite the lack of randomization, the


75

study subjects represented 31 schools. Nevertheless, lack

of randomization clearly constitutes a study limitation.

The investigator found, when administering the

questionnaire, that teachers expressed considerable interest

upon learning that the purpose of the study was to

investigate the effects of stress and burnout on teachers.

All participants requested the results of the study. One

can speculate that teachers who either had an interest in

the subject of stress and burnout or those who believed

themselves to be experiencing stress and burnout might be

more inclined to volunteer to participate in the study. In

either event, participant selection would have an

undetermined influence on the results. Another observation

made by the investigator was that the number of teachers in

a school who volunteered appeared to be somewhat determined

by the interest and enthusiasm expressed by the school's

principal. To control for these and many other intervening

factors, a randomized or stratified sample would likely

provide more reliable results.

Hacflinegp
The results of this study support research findings on

hardiness in teachers (Holt, 1985; Williams, 1988) adding to

the applicability of hardiness to populations beyond the

initial sample of middle-age, male executives (Kobasa, 1977,

1979). These findings serve as encouragement to further


76

investigate hardiness in populations, such as educators.

As more is learned about how hardiness is experienced and

developed, it is possible that hardiness techniques can be

included in teacher preparation programs.

Findings in this study tend to support the premise that

the challenge component does not contribute to the composite

hardiness score. Hull, Van Treuren, and Virnelli (1987), in

particular, question whether hardiness is truly a unitary

concept, contending that commitment and control have

independent effects on health and disclaiming any

contribution from challenge. Other researchers' findings

have brought into question the validity of challenge as a

contributing factor to the explanation of hardiness (Jarvis,

1993; Tang, 1989; Nowack, 1991). Kobasa has indicated that

challenge has been somewhat elusive and that she is trying

to identify more definitive ways to conceptualize and

measure it (Kobasa, 1993); she maintains her stance that

challenge contributes to total hardiness.

Conclusions

For this sample, the following conclusions were drawn:

1. Hardiness is present and measurable in this sample

of subjects, most of whom were over 35 years of age.

2. Hardiness is significantly related to health,

albeit rather weakly.


77

3. Hardiness is correlated more strongly with

Emotional Exhaustion than with the other aspects of burnout.

4. Commitment is more strongly correlated with

Emotional Exhaustion than are the other components of

hardiness.

5. Contributions of the individual components of

hardiness to health and burnout are not equitable.

6. Results of this study tend to support the use of

total hardiness scores, rather than subscale scores.

7. Health is related to age, years of teaching

experience, and level taught, albeit very weak.

8. Female sample subjects are slightly more hardy than

males.

Recommendations

The findings in this study suggest the following

recommendations:

1. Further investigation of hardiness in randomized

sample of teachers.

2. Further investigation of hardiness through

longitudinal studies of burnout to establish causation

and determine if stress causes burnout or if teachers who

experience burnout are unable to cope with stressors.

3. Continued research on the relationship between

hardiness and health using true measures of health.

4. Determine whether challenge is a valid dimension of


78

hardiness, whether it is a component of the commitment

dimension, or whether it needs to be eliminated from the

hardiness construct.

5. Conduct research on how aspects of hardiness can be

taught and implement them into teacher preparation programs.

6. Implementation of other Wellness Programs into

school systems.

7. Share findings with the teachers of the Sullivan

County School System.

summary

The purpose of this study was to explore the

relationships among hardiness, health, and burnout among

teachers in the Sullivan County School System. Five

research questions and twenty-two null hypotheses were

formulated to guide this study. A descriptive correlational

design was used; statistical analyses included descriptive

statistics and simple and multiple correlations.

A statistically significant positive relationship was

revealed between hardiness and health; among the hardiness

subscales, commitment accounted for the largest amount of

variance in health. Significant relationships were obtained

between hardiness and each of the burnout subscales. Of the

hardiness subscales, commitment and control equally

accounted for the greatest amount of variance in Emotional


Exhaustion; commitment also accounted for the greatest

amount of variance in Depersonalization and Personal

Accomplishment.

To determine the extent to which the subjects'

demographic variables were related to any study variables,

correlation coefficients were computed between demographic

variables and each of the study variables. While age, years

teaching experience, and level taught were significantly

related to the study variables, the extremely low

correlations indicate that only 1% of the variance in these

three variables were explained by the study variables,

preventing meaningful interpretation.


BIBLIOGRAPHY

80
Bibliography

Antonovosky, A. (1979). Health._stress and coping. San


Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Borg, W.R. & Gall, M.D. (1989). Educational Research.


Longman: New York & London.

Brownson, A. (1992). Cardiovascular disease. American


Journal of Public Health, £2, 1, 99-102.

Byrne, B.M. (1992). Investigating causal links to burnout


for elementary, intermediate, and secondary teachers.
Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American
Educational Research Association, San Francisco, CA.
(ERIC Document Reproduction Services No. ED 344 886)

Cadavid, V. Lunenberg, F.C. (1991). Locus of control, pupil


control ideology, and dimensions of teacher burnout.
Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American
Educational Research Association, Chicago, IL. (ERIC
Document Reproduction Services No. ED 333 560)

Carlson, B.K. (1992). Effects of teacher and organization


related variables on the perceptions of burnout and
career direction of special education teachers in
Hawaii. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Hawaii,
1992). Dissertation Abstracts International. £2,
2615A.

Cedoline, A.J. (1982). Job burnout in public education.


New York: Teachers College Press.

Clarke, B.M. (1991). The burnout maze: An exploration of


the interrelationships of burnout, hardiness,
depression, response mode to burnout, and demographic
variables. (Doctoral dissertation, University of
Colorado, Denver, 1991).

Colasurdo, M.M. (1987). A descriptive study of professional


burnout amongst public school teachers in San Diego,
California. (Doctoral dissertation, United States
International University, 1987). Dissertation
Abstracts International. 42, 470A.

Connolly, C. & Sanders, W. (1986). Teacher stress— an


ongoing problem that needs attention. Paper presented
at the Annual Meeting of the Association of Teacher
Educators, Atlanta, GA. (ERIC Document Reproduction
Services No. ED 266 139)
82

Crump, D.R* (1991). Special and regular secondary education


teachers: Relationship of personality preference
stress hardiness and job satisfaction. (Doctoral
dissertation, Brigham Young University, 1991).

Cunningham, W. G, (1983). Teacher burnout-Solutions for the


1980's: A Review of the literature. The Urban Review.
IS. 37-51.

D'Ambrosia, S.J. (1987). A study to examine if there is a


relationship between burnout and hardiness of nurses
working with oncology patients. (Doctoral
dissertation, Temple University).

Dawber, T. W. (1980). The Farmlncham study: The


epidemiology of atherosclerotic disease. Cambridge,
MS, Harvard University Press.

Dewe, P. (1986). Stress: Causes, consequences and coping


strategies for teachers. Wellington, N.Z.: New
Zealand Education Institute.

Dworkin, A. G. (1985). When teachers give up: Teacher


burnout, teacher turnover and their impact on children.
Austin, TX: Hogg Foundation for Mental Health.

Falinski, J.M. (1987). Role-Stress, personal factors, and


work-related outcomes in the superintendency (conflict,
ambiguity, locus-of-control, satisfaction). (Doctoral
dissertation, University of Syracuse). Dissertation
Abstracts International. 42, 07A.

Fielding, M.A. (1982). Personality and situational


correlates of teacher stress and burnout. (Doctoral
dissertation, University of Oregon, 1982).

Freudenberg, H.J. (1974). Staff burn-out. Journal of


Social Issues. 30, 159-165.

Funk, S.D., & Houston, B.K. (1987). A critical analysis


of the Hardiness Scale's validity and utility.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. SI (3),
572-578,

Ganellen, R.J., & Blaney, P.H. (1984). Hardiness and social


support as moderators of the effects of life stress.
Journal of Personality, and Social Psychology. 42, 156-
163.
83

Goorf H. B. (1990). Self-efficacy beliefs and personal


hardiness characteristics of teachers at three career
stages. (Doctoral dissertation, University of
Virginia).

Gunby, R. (1992). Cardiovascular diseases remain nation's


leading cause of death. Journal, of the American
Medical Association. 267.(3). 335-336.

Hammond, L.A. (1987). Stress and role satisfaction: The


mediating effects of social support, hardiness, coping
strategies, and gender in academic multiple role
persons. (Doctoral dissertation, University of
Florida).

Harrison, S.N. (1983). The sources of teacher burnout among


elementary teachers of Mississippi (Doctoral
dissertation, University of Southern Mississippi,
1983).

Hipps, E. S. & Malpin, G. (1992). The differences in


teachers' and principals' general -lob stress and_stresg_
related to performance-based accreditation. Paper
presented at the Annual Meeting of the Mid-South
Educational Research Association, Knoxville, TN. (ERIC
Document Reporduction Services No. ED 353 345)

Hipps, E. S. & Malpin, G. (1991). The relationship of locus


of control, stress related to performance_based
accreditation, and lob stress to burnout in public
school teachers and principals. Paper presented at the
Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research
Association, Chicago, IL. (ERIC Reproduction Services
No. ED 333 557)

Holt, P. (1985). A study of the interaction of levels


of occupational stress, degree of burnout, and
personality hardiness in female elementary teachers.
(Doctoral dissertation, University of Kansas).

Holt, P., Fine, M.J. & Tollefson, N. (1987). Mediating


stress: Survival of the hardy. Psychology in the
Sshaals, 21 d), si-58.
Hooper, M. L. (1983). The relationship between selected
demographic variables, job satisfaction, and teacher
burnout (Doctoral dissertation, Mississippi State
University, 1983).
B4

Iwanicki, E. F. (1983). Toward understanding and


alleviating teacher burnout. Theory into Practice. 21
(1), 27-32.

Jackson, B.G. (1983). Burnout in teachers working with


educationally handicapped children. (Doctoral
dissertation, University of Southern California, 1983).
Dissertation Abstracts International. M , 387A.

Jenkins, S* & Calhoun, J.F. (1991). Teacher stress: Issues


and interventions. Psychology in the Schools. 28. CO-
70.

Kobasa, S.C. (1977). Stress, personality, and health: A


study of an overlooked possibility (Doctoral
dissertation, University of Chicago). Dissertation
Abstracts International. 2430B.

Kobasa, S.C. (1979). Stressful life events, personality,


and health: An inquiry into hardiness. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology. XL, 1-11.

Kobasa, S. C. (1982). The hardy personality: Toward a


social psychology of stress and health. In G.S.
Sanders & J.suls (Eds.), Social psychology of health
and illness (pp.3-321.

Kobasa, S.C.& Maddi, S.K. (1977). Existential personality


theory. In R. Corsini (Ed.), Current personality
theories. 243-276. Itasca, IL: Peacock.

Kobasa, S.C., Maddi, S.R., & Courington, S. (1981).


Personality and constitution as mediators in the
stress-illness relationship. Journal of Health and
Social Behavior. 21, 368-378.

Kobasa, S.C., Maddi, S.R., & Kahn, S. (1982). Hardiness and


health: A prospective study. Journal ofPersonality
and Social Psychology. Al, 168-177.

Kobasa, S. C., Maddi, S. R., Pucetti, M.C., & Zola, M. A.


(198S). Effectiveness of hardiness, exercise and social
support as resources against illness. Journal of
Psychosomatic Research. 21, 525-533.

Kritz-siverstein, D. (1992). Employment status and heart


disease risk factors in middle-aged women: The rancho
bernardo study. American Journal of Public Health. SI,
2, 215-219.
85

Langemo, D.K. (1990). Impact of work stress on female nurse


educators. Image: Journal of Nursing Scholarship. 22.,
159-162.

Lenfant, C,, Stone, E., & Castelli, W. (1987), Celebrating


40 years of the Framingham heart study. Journal.of
Schppl Health# Sir 7 , 279-281.
Luckner, J. L. (1990). Strategies for alleviating teacher
stress. ACEHI JOURNAL. 1£, 4-12,

Lutz, F.W. & Maddirala, J., (1988). Stress, burnout.and


reform mandated accountability. Paper presented at the
Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research
Association, New Orleans, LA. (ERIC Document
Reproduction Services No. ED 297 017)

Malanowski, J.R. (1982). A study of the relationships among


burnout, personal orientation, and demographic
variables of teachers. (Doctoral dissertation, Bowling
Green State University, 1981).

Martinez, J.G. (1989). Cooling off before burning out.


Academic Therapy. 2A, 3, 271-84.

Maslach, c. (1986). Burnout: The cost of caring. New York:


Prentice-Hall.

Maslach, C. & Jackson, S.E. (1986). Maslach burnout


inventory (2nd ed.). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting
Psychologists.

Maynard, J.A. (1992). An analysis of occupational burnout


among teachers in selected urban schools. (Doctoral
dissertation, Texas Southern University, 1992).

Mazur, P.J. & Lynch, M.D. (1989). Differential impact of


administrative, organizational, and personality factors
on teacher burnout. Teaching-and-Teacher-Education. 5 ,
4, 337-53.

Mechanic, D. (1976). Stress illness and illness behavior.


Journal of Human Stress. 2., 2-6.

Natale, J. A. (1993). Why teachers leave. The Executive


Educator. 22# 14-18.

Nowack, K.M. & Hanson, A.L. (1983). The relationship


between stress, job performance, and burnout in college
student resident assistants. Journal of College
Student Personnel. Nov., 545-550.
86

Pardine, P., Dytell, R., and Friedman, A. (April 1982). The


stress-illness relationship mediated bv negligent
health behavior. Paper presented at the annual
convention of the Eastern Psychological Association,
Baltimore. (ERIC Reproduction Services No. ED 273 575)

Pender, N.J. (1987). Health promotion in nursing practice


(2nd ed.). Norwalk, CT:Appleton & Lange.

Pierce, C.M., & Holloy, G.N. (1990). Psychological and


biographical differences between secondary school
teachers experiencing high and low levels of burnout.
British Journal of Educational Psychology. 60. 37-51.

Pines, A.M., Aronson, E., & Kafry, D. (1981). Burnout:


From tedium to personal growth. New York: The Free
Press.

Pines, A.M., & Kafry, D. (1978). Occupational tedium in the


social services. Social Work. 22* 499-507.

Pines, A.M., & Maslach, C. (1980). Characteristics of staff


burnout in mental health settings. Hospital and
communitv Psvchiatrv. 22, (4), 233-237.

Raison, J. c. (1981). Job burnout and stress in regular and


special education teachers: Its relation to contact
with handicapped students. (Doctoral dissertation,
University of Minnesota, 1981).

Rich, V.L., & Rich, A.R. (1985). Personality hardiness and


burnout in female staff nurses. Image: Journal of
Nursing Scholarship. X2, 2 , 63-66.

Rinke, J.M. (1989). The relationship between personality


type and burnout in Michigan community college
counselors. (Doctoral dissertation, Western Michigan
University, 1989).

Sarros, J.C., & Sarros, A.M. (1987). Predictors of teacher


burnout. The Journal of Educational Administration.
22* 2, 216-230.
Schmied, L.A., & Lawler, K.A. (1986). Hardiness, Type A
behavior, and the stress-illness relationship in
working women. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology. £1, 6 , 1218-1223.
87

Schoenig, T.M. (1987). An investigation of the


relationships among burnout, hardiness, stressful
teaching events, job and personal characteristics in
public school teachers. (Doctoral dissertation, Hofstra
University, 1987).

Schwab, R.L., Burnout in education. In Maslach burnout


inventory manual, 2nd ed, 1993.

Selye, H. (1978). The stress of life (rev. ed). Mew York:


Me Graw-Hill.

Shapiro, S. H. (1987). Determinants of job satisfaction


among athletic trainers: The relationship between
academic program factors and job factors, personal
factors, and hardiness personality. (Doctoral
dissertation, University of Virginia).

Shepperd, J.A., & Kashani, J.H. (1991). The relationship of


hardiness, gender, and stress to health outcomes in
adolescents. Journal of Personality. 59. 4, 747-768.

Smith, J.A. (1981). The idea of health: A philosophical


inquiry. Advances in Nursing Science. 2(3), 43-50.

Sotto, M. A. (1991), National health objectives for the year


2000: The demographic impact of health promotion and
disease prevention. American Journal of Public Health.
Si, 11, 1456-1465,

Sutterley, D. C. (1986). Stress management: Grazing the


clinical turf. Holistic Nursing Practice. 1 (1), 36-
53.

Tang, T.L. (1991). The effects of hardiness, iob-related


stress, and life stress on health and absence from
work. Paper presented at the Annual Convention of the
Psychological Association, Houston, TX. (ERIC Document
Reproduction Services No. Ed 299 508)

Tanner, E.K. (1991). Assessment of a health-promotive


lifestyle. Nursing Clinics of North America. 2£, 4,
845-853.

Tuettemann, E., & Punch, K.F. (1992). Psychological


distress in secondary teachers: Research findings and
their implications. Journal of Educational
Administration. 2fl, 1, 42-54.

Walker, S.N. (1987). The health-promoting lifestyle


profile: Development and psychometric characteristics.
Nursing Journal. 22, 2, 76-81.
88

Wallis, C. (1983, June 6). Stress: Can we cope? Time. 48-


54.

WilliantB, R.F. (1988). Hardiness, health care claims,


absenteeism and burnout: A prospective study of
direct and moderating effects. (Doctoral dissertation,
Case Western Reserve University).
Appendix A

The Hardiness Test

89
PLEASE NOTE

Copyrighted materials in this document have not been


filmed at the request o f the author. They are available
for consultation, however, in the author's library.

pages 90-93
pages 95-96

University Microfilms International


Appendix B

Maslach Burnout Inventory Form Ed

94
Appendix c

Educators Demographic Data Survey

97
98

EDUCATORS DEMOGRAPHIC DATA SHEET

Your sex:

_______ (1) Male (2) Female

Your age:

_______ Years

Please indicate the highest degree level you have achieved:

_______ (l) Bachelor's

_______ (2) Master's

_______ (3) Master's plus 30

_______ (4) Doctorate

How many years have you been in education?

_______ Years

Did you participate in the HEART analysis (Wellness Testing) during


the 1993-94 school year? ________ Yes No

If yes, what is your risk factor?

___________ Risk Factor Points

RETURN QUESTIONNAIRE TO DEBBIE M0REL0CK HOLSTON MIDDLE SCHOOL


THROUGH PONY EXPRESS.

THANKS POR YOUR PARTICIPATION

You might also like