Memory Strategies
Memory Strategies
Published
Spring 2015
Abstract
The current study examines how teachers use memory strategies to present their lessons. Two
research questions are asked. First, what memory strategies do teachers use to teach their lessons?
Second, how do teachers use memory strategies in their instruction? Eleven teachers complete an
open-ended questionnaire to identify the memory strategies they use and give examples how they
use these strategies to teach. Rehearsal is found to be the most frequently used strategy, followed
activities of these memory strategies are given to show how teachers use these strategies in their
classrooms.
and rehearsal, are helpful in remembering information. Most studies on memory strategies in
classrooms focus on how students use these strategies, e.g., elaboration (Willoughby, Porter,
Belsito, & Yearsley, 1999), mental imagery (Bozorgmanesh & Mohmmad, 2012), mnemonics
(Scruggs, Mastropieri, Berkeley, & Marshak, 2010), organization (Pang, 1991), and rehearsal
Only a few studies on memory strategies in classrooms focus on how teachers use these
strategies. Pressley, Allington, Wharton-McDonald, Block, and Morrow (2001) noted that
teachers’ instruction of memory strategy is lacking in the intensity necessary for students to learn
how to use memory strategies effectively. In addition, Ornstein, Coffman, and Grammer (2009)
found that teachers vary considerably in how much they use memory-relevant language, such as
strategies and metacognitive questions (questions related to students’ knowledge of how memory
Memory Strategies
The memory strategies recommended to teachers and students include elaboration, mental
imagery, mnemonics, organization, and rehearsal (Santrock, 2011; Schunk, 2012; Woolfolk,
2013). Discussion of each of these strategies is given as follows for teachers and students to
adopt.
Running head: MEMORY STRATEGIES 4
elaboration. Woolfolk (2013) explained that elaboration assists encoding and retrieval of new
information because it links new information to older information. Ways to elaborate include
remember how to spell the word, rendezvous, students can use elaboration to make a meaningful
sentence (e.g., the restaurant will be our rendezvous). Another way to elaborate is to answer
questions about new content. For example, to remember the fact that the Western Spotted Skunk
lives in a hole in the ground, students can ask, “Why would that fact be true?” When students
generate answers to why questions, they relate what they already know to the new information.
After examining elaboration strategy use as a function of prior knowledge with students in
elementary schools, Willoughby, Porter, Belsito and Yearsley (1999) got positive findings for
elaboration for all grade levels when learners have access to an extensive network of information.
In fact, elaboration is an effective memory strategy for both children and adults across a variety of
learning tasks (Pressley, Wood, Woloshyn, Mmartin, King, & Menke, 1992).
mental imagery. The dual coding theory (Paivio, 1971) states that memory for linguistic
both verbal and nonverbal systems results in the dual coding of information. Schunk (2012)
suggested different ways to elicit students’ mental imagery, such as having students close their
eyes and think about a story scene, steps of an experiment, or dance movement, etc. For
example, to remember a historic incident, students can use mental imagery to visualize a
battlefield with eyes closed. After reviewing the use of mental imagery in classrooms, Douville
Running head: MEMORY STRATEGIES 5
(2004) concluded that mental imagery is best used in facilitating deep-level engagement in
concepts.
There are various types of mnemonic strategies: rhymes (e.g., “righty tighty, lefty loosey”),
spelling rules (e.g., “i before e except after c”), song lyrics (e.g., “head and shoulders, knees and
toes”), phrases (e.g., use “never eat soggy waffles” to remember the compass directions “north,
east, south, and west”), acronyms (e.g., use “HOMES” to remember the five Great Lakes –
Huron, Ontario, Michigan, Erie, and Superior), method of loci (e.g., use the logical movement
after supper to remember “a tiger” on the countertop, “a monkey” in the sink, “a hippo” in the
dishwasher, and “a bald eagle” on the sofa), keyword method (e.g., connect vivid images of two
apples getting married to remember “Annapolis is the capital of Maryland”). After reviewing
educationally-relevant mnemonic strategy research, Levin (1994) stated that mnemonic strategies
equip students with skills to acquire straightforward factual information, but fail to promote
Organization. Connecting items to one another can organize information in such a way
that recalling one item also recalls other items linked to it. Santrock (2011) explained that
organization makes large amounts of information more manageable and more meaningful.
Woolfolk (2013) recommended the use of hierarchy to integrate pieces of information, the use of
chunking to group information into higher-order units to be remembered as single units, or the use
of outline to organize information. For example, to remember the 50 states, students can organize
states by region (West, South, Midwest, Northeast, East, etc). To remember vocabulary, students
Running head: MEMORY STRATEGIES 6
can group semantically related words by meanings (categories, instances, associates, synonyms,
etc.). Banerjee and White (2015) found that organizational strategy is highly specific to the
demands and goals of individual tasks even when tasks share commonalities such as involving the
same cognitive domain. It means that organizing words on the basis of semantic category
enhances memory of a word-list learning task, but organizing words on the basis of nonsemantic,
organization strategy is one of the most efficient means of improving free recall in the absence of
cognitive support.
Rehearsal. Consciously rehearsing information over and over can somewhat slightly
extend the length of time it stays in memory. Santrock (2011) stated that rehearsal works best
when encoding and remembering of a list of items for a brief period of time, but it does not work
well when for retaining information over the long term. Woolfolk (2013) also mentioned that
rehearsal works well with highly overlearned material, such as multiplication facts, spelling
words, or a play script, but it does not work well for remembering more complex and meaningful
information. For example, to remember multiplication facts, students can repeat them over and
over verbally or in written form. However, Harris and Qualls (2000) stated that rehearsal is
primarily used for disposable memory traces, such as a single-use telephone number that would
There are only a few studies on memory strategies in classrooms which focus on the types
of memory strategy teachers teach their students and the types of memory-relevant language
teachers use in the class. To observe the memory strategies teachers taught to support children’s
Running head: MEMORY STRATEGIES 7
learning, Moely, Hart, Leal, Santulli, Rao, Johnson, and Hamilton (1992) conducted a cross-
sectional study of classrooms from kindergarten through sixth grade. They observed how teachers
structured classroom learning activities in a range of subjects and how they monitored and
directed children’s study. To do so, they developed a coding system to record many aspects of the
teaching process, and used factor analyses to identify four factors: teachers’ responses to error;
and strategies. Among these factors, cognitive processes and strategies included instances in
which teachers gave suggestions about studying or learning, such as offering rationales for
strategy use, providing information about appropriate cognitive processes for task performance;
advising of the need for memory activity, telling children not to engage in certain study strategies,
The authors found that the most commonly coded teacher behaviors included requests for
answers to questions. Instruction that called for children to engage in cognitive processing and
strategy use occurred rather infrequently. Teachers of grade 4 and above provided rationales for
the use of strategies more often than teachers of younger children. Teachers of lessons involving
mathematics activity and language arts suggested students use such strategies more than those
teaching lessons involving other activities. Teachers instructing language arts suggested deduction
strategies most often whereas teachers instructing mixed subject matter (including math) most
often promoted the use of specific aids for problem solving. Rehearsal was the memory strategy
taught most often by all teachers, followed by elaboration, and mental imagery. Subsequent
training in the use of the memory strategy of organization was also given to children. Among
average and low achievers of the group, those whose teachers were relatively high in strategy
Running head: MEMORY STRATEGIES 8
suggestions showed improved memory performance and more deliberate use of the organization
strategy than did children whose teachers rarely made strategy suggestions.
In contrast to the study of Moely, et al. (1992), Ornstein and his colleagues (Coffman et.
al., 2008; Ornstein, Coffman, & Grammer, 2009; Ornstein, Coffman, Grammer, San Souci, &
McCall 2010) undertook a longitudinal study to examine the memory-relevant language teachers
use during the course of instruction and the mnemonic goals expressed in their lessons. Based on
Moely, et al.’s cognitive processes and strategies factor, Ornstein et. al. developed a coding
instrument, the Taxonomy of Teacher Behaviors. The four categories of teacher memory-relevant
language in the Taxonomy concerned the nature and extent of various instructional memory-
cognitive structuring activities (encouraging children to engage with the materials in ways that
facilitate the encoding and retrieval of information), memory requests (asking students to retrieve
soliciting metacognitive information with the goal of facilitating children’s performance). The
authors then followed students throughout their elementary school years and made several visits
to observe them in their classrooms. During each visit, one observer employed the Taxonomy and
The authors found that instructional activity was the most frequent activity and memory
request was the second-most frequent activity. The narratives showed that most of these memory-
related requests were implied deliberate, in which the demand for the use of memory was implicit
without an expressed prompt to remember or not to forget, and a small percentage of these
requests were expressed deliberate, in which memory demands were explicitly stated. In addition,
Running head: MEMORY STRATEGIES 9
they also found that the achievement of low-achieving students increased when they were placed
in classrooms in which teachers were categorized as “high- mnemonic teachers” who frequently
Previous studies on how teachers use memory strategies focused on the types of memory
strategy teachers teach their students and the types of memory-relevant language they use in the
class. Moely, Hart, Leal, Santulli, Rao, Johnson, and Hamilton (1992) investigated how and when
teachers ask students to use study and memory strategies in the elementary school classroom.
Ornstein and his colleagues (Coffman et. al., 2008; Ornstein, Coffman, & Grammer, 2009;
Ornstein, Coffman, Grammer, San Souci, & McCall 2010) examined the memory-relevant
language teachers use to encourage students to use memory strategies during the course of
The current study further examines how teachers use memory strategies but the focus is on
the types of memory strategies teachers use to present their lessons. Two research questions are
asked. First, what memory strategies do teachers use to teach their lessons? Second, how do
teachers use memory strategies in their instruction? Getting to know the memory strategies other
teachers use help teachers make informed decisions how and when to use these strategies in their
own instruction.
Method
Participants
program in education at a Midwestern state university. There were 3 males and 8 females with a
Running head: MEMORY STRATEGIES 10
mean age of 29.6 years, ranging from 22 to 44 years old. The average length of teaching
experience was 6.15 years, ranging from 1 to 21 years. One participant taught first grade, two
taught second-grade, four taught fourth-grade, and four taught fifth- grade.
Procedure
graduate course in education. They were asked to write responses to two questions: “What
information did your students need to remember in the subject you teach?” and “What strategies
did you use to help your students to remember this information? ” The first question served as a
lead-in to help participants answer the second question. The responses shared by participants in
the second question were coded if they fitted as rehearsal, mental imagery, elaboration,
Results
Table 1 presents the memory strategies teachers use to teach their lessons. Out of the 238
examples of memory strategies used, 111 (46.64%) were rehearsal, 54 (22.69%) were mental
imagery, 46 (19.33%) were elaboration, 17 (7.14%) were mnemonic, and 10 (4.2%) were
organization.
Table 1
Representative examples and activities of these memory strategies explain how teachers
use these strategies in their classrooms. For example, teachers use drill and practice, and a variety
of games to help students rehearse what they have learned (see Table 2). One teacher wrote, “We
created flashcards to practice memorizing common conversions that were the building blocks for
more complex conversion.” Rehearsal was used widely from first to fifth grade no matter how
Table 2
The Examples and Activities of the Memory Strategy of Rehearsal Used by Teachers
To generate mental imagery, teachers use realia and visual aids, or engage students in
mental math or visualization (see Table 3). One teacher wrote, “As I described unit to unit
conversion, students imagined what this looked like in both units.” Mental imagery was used
widely in different elementary grade levels by teachers with less than five years of teaching
experiences.
Table 3
The Examples and Activities of the Memory Strategy of Mental Imagery Used by Teachers
Not only can teachers elaborate the information by using analogy and making meaningful
connections to real life, they may also ask students to generate their own personalized examples,
complete their own projects, and teach their peers (see Table 4). One teacher wrote, “I used what
Running head: MEMORY STRATEGIES 13
the kids already knew and built on it.” Elaboration was used mainly by fourth and fifth-grade
Table 4
The Examples and Activities of the Memory Strategy of Elaboration Used by Teachers
Acronyms, phrases, and songs are mnemonics teachers use in different subjects (see Table
5). One teacher wrote, “Tablespoon and teaspoon both start with the letter T, T rhymes with 3, so
there are 3 teaspoons in a tablespoon.” Mnemonics were used mainly by fourth and fifth-grade
Table 5
The Examples and Activities of the Memory Strategy of Mnemonics Used by Teachers
Teachers also use anchor charts, concept maps, check sheets, and bookmarks, or engage in
chunking to organize the information for students to remember (see Table 6). One teacher wrote,
“We made a web of words that often gave clues as to whether a person should add or deduct.”
Organization was used mainly in fourth and fifth grades by teachers with less than five years of
teaching experiences.
Table 6
The Examples and Activities of the Memory Strategy of Organization Used by Teachers
General Discussion
The current study examines the types of memory strategies teachers use to present their
lessons. Rehearsal and mental imagery were used widely by first to fifth- grade teachers whereas
elaboration, mnemonics and organization were used mainly by fourth and fifth-grade teachers.
Rehearsal and elaboration were used by teachers regardless of their teaching experiences whereas
mental imagery, mnemonics and organization were used by teachers with less than five years of
teaching experiences.
Rehearsal is found to be the most frequently used strategy, followed by mental imagery,
elaboration, mnemonics, and organization. Previous study also found that rehearsal is the memory
strategy taught most often by teachers to their students (Moely et al., 1992). The high percentage
of teachers using rehearsal in the classroom (46.64%) indicates that teachers frequently use drill
and practice, as well as games, to repeat foundational information for students to remember. As
reviewed, rehearsal works well in remembering a list of items, arithmetic facts, spelling words,
and scripts but is less effective for remembering more complex and meaningful information
and/or information over the long term (Santrock, 2011; Woolfolk, 2013). From the examples
given by teachers, rehearsal is used appropriately for students to remember foundational matter
that is continuously revisited and built upon. In fact, rehearsal is the strategy to encode
complete a task.
When the information is deemed important, other memory strategies are needed to move it
to long-term memory. Memory strategies which move information from working memory to
long-term memory are elaboration, mental imagery, mnemonics and organization. However, the
Running head: MEMORY STRATEGIES 17
much higher than the percentage of teachers using any of those strategies which move
adopt these strategies more often and more widely. In planning lessons individually or as a team,
these memory strategies can be incorporated into instructional activities of a variety of topics.
The memory strategies discussed in the current study may benefit other teachers or
educators who would like to apply memory strategies in their own educational settings. With
exposure to how other teachers use memory strategies, teachers or educators can equip themselves
with a number of strategies before developing their own versions. However, caution has to be
taken when reading these strategies because they are ascertained from teachers’ self-reported
responses to an open-ended question. In the real classroom, teachers who participated in this
study may not use these memory strategies or may not implement these strategies in the ways
they describe. Nevertheless, exposure to a teacher who teaches memory strategies would lead
students toward more effective learning (Moely, et al., 1992). Further studies might compare
students’ academic performance under teachers who use more memory strategies and those who
use fewer memory strategies in presenting the lessons, or examine memory strategies by content
Conclusions
Memory strategies are critical in promoting learning at schools. The types of memory
strategies teachers use to present their lessons determine how students would remember the
whereas elaboration, mental imagery, mnemonics and organization are helpful in moving
Running head: MEMORY STRATEGIES 18
information from working memory to long-term memory. Therefore, teachers are encouraged to
learn from other teachers how to incorporate these memory strategies in presenting lessons to
their students.
Running head: MEMORY STRATEGIES 19
References
Banerjee, P. & White, D. A. (2015, January 5). Clinical assessment of organization strategy: An
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pas0000077
Bozorgmanesh, A. A., & Mohmmad, H. (2012). The effect of mental imagery on visual and verbal
Coffman, J. L., Ornstein, P. A., McCall, L. E., & Curran, P. J. (2008). Linking teachers’ memory-
Douville, P. (2004). Use mental imagery across the curriculum. Preventing School Failure, 49,
36-39.
Levin, J. R. (1994). Memory strategies and classroom learning: A twenty-year report card. The
Moely, B. E., Hart, S. S., Leal, L., Santulli, K. A., Rao, N., Johnson, T., & Hamilton, L. B. (1992).
The teacher’s role in facilitating memory and study strategy development in the
Harris, J. L., & Qualls, C. D. (2000). The association of elaborative or maintenance rehearsal with
age, reading comprehension, and verbal working memory performance. Aphasiology, 14,
515-526.
Ornstein, P. A., Coffman, J. L., & Grammer, J. K. (2009). Learning to remember. In O.A. Barbarin
& B. H. Wasil (Eds.), Handbook of child development and early education. New York:
Guilford.
Running head: MEMORY STRATEGIES 20
Ornstein, P. A., Coffman, J., Grammer, J., San Souci, P., & McCall, L. (2010). Linking the
classroom context and the development of children’s memory skills. In J. Meece & J.
Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
Pang, H. (1991). The development of children’s knowledge of organization strategy and memory
monitoring and their relation to memory behavior. Psychological Science (China), 6, 23-27.
Pressley, M., Wood, E., Woloshyn, V. E., Martin, V., King, A., & Menke, D. (1992). Encouraging
Pressley, M., Allington, R., Wharton-McDonald, R., Block, C. C., & Morrow, L. M. (2001).
Learning to read: Lessons from exemplary first grades. New York: Guilford.
Schunk, D. H. (2012). Learning theories: An educational perspective (6th ed.). Boston, MA:
Pearson.
Scruggs, T. E., Mastropieri, M. A., Berkeley, S. L., & Marshak, L. (2010). Mnemonic strategies:
46, 79-86.
Tam, H., Jarrold, C., Baddeley, A., & Sabatos-DeVito (2010). The development of memory
Willoughby, T., Porter, L., Belsito, L. & Yearsley, T. (1999). Use of elaboration strategies by
students in grades two, four, and six. The Elementary School Journal, 99, 221-231.