Linguistic Studies of Text and Discourse - Volume 2 (Collected Works M A Halliday)
Linguistic Studies of Text and Discourse - Volume 2 (Collected Works M A Halliday)
Halliday
Linguistic Studies of
Text and Discourse
M. A. K. Halliday
Edited by Jonathan Webster
Continuum
The Tower Building, 11 York Road, London, SE1 7NX
370 Lexington Avenue, New York, NY 10017-6503
Preface Vll
Acknowledgements lX
Editor's introduction 3
Editor's introduction 85
IX
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
X
PART ONE
3
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
literature and allow for 'the comparison of each text with others, by
the same and by different authors, in the same and in different genres.'
Halliday analyses W. B. Yeats' poem Leda and the Swan in terms of
'the "dine ofverbality", and the structure of the nominal groups', and
compares the findings with those from another Yeats poem His Phoenix,
Tennyson's Morte d'Arthur, and a passage of prose from the New
Scientist. Halliday also examines three short passages of modem English
prose, by John Braine, Dylan Thomas, and Angus Wilson, in terms of
nominal groups, lexical sets and cohesion. The concept of cohesion,
described as an alignment or grouping of various descriptive categories,
was developed particularly for literary textual analysis.
On the role of linguistics in literary analysis, Halliday concludes,
'Linguistics is not and never will be the whole of literary analysis, and
only the literary analyst - not the linguist - can determine the place of
linguistics in literary studies. But if a text is to be described at all, then
it should be described properly; and this means by the theories and
methods developed in linguistics, the subject whose task is precisely to
show how language works. The literary analyst is not content with
amateur psychology, armchair philosophy, or fictitious social history;
yet the linguistics that is applied in some accounts of literature, and the
statements about language that are used as evidence, are no less amateur,
armchair and fictitious.'
First published in 1977, the second chapter in this part, 'Text as
semantic choice in social contexts', begins by describing in detail the
semantic system, with particular attention to the textual (or text-
forming) component, and its corresponding structure-generating sys-
tems and cohesive relations. Halliday defines a text as 'an instance of
social meaning in a particular context of situation.' Text is the means
of exchange of meanings among societal members in the context of
situation. Halliday's notion of text encompasses the traditional and the
spontaneous, both literary and conversational. By way of illustration,
Halliday focuses on a narrative entitled Fables for Our Time, presenting
a situational interpretation of the text, and concluding with a system.ic
description of a single sentence from the same text. A sentence, after
all, is the product of 'numerous micro-acts of semantic choice'. It is a
lexicogramrnatical structure, which has 'its origin in the functional
organization of the semantic system.' In so doing, Halliday illustrates
the semiotic cycle: 'the network that extends from the social system, as
its upper bound, through the linguistic system on the one hand and the
social context on the other, down to the "wording", which is _the text
in its lexicogrammatical realization.'
4
Chapter One
5
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
phonetic theory is already valid and relevant for the purpose. At the
same time the descriptive statements made about a literary text are
meaningful only in relation to the total "pure" description of the
language concerned: if the linguist hopes to contribute to the analysis
of English literature he must first have made a comprehensive descrip-
tion of the English of the period at all levels. (It can presumably be
taken for granted that the categories of such a description will be
formally defined, and that the description will not be restricted to
below the rank of the sentence.) If for example all clauses of a particular
poem are shown to have the same structure, it is essential to know
whether or not this is the only permitted clause structure in the
language; and if not, what its relative frequency is in a large sample
representative of "the language in general".
Moreover, a text is meaningful not only in virtue of what it is but
also in virtue of what it might have been. The most relevant exponent
of the "might have been" of a work of literature is another work of
literature. Linguistic stylistics is thus essentially a comparative study. To
pursue the example above, we also need to know the relative frequency
of this clause structure in other works of the same period and the same
genre. The more texts are studied, the more anything said about any
one text becomes interesting and relevant.
We can therefore define linguistic stylistics as the description of
literary texts, by methods derived from general linguistic theory, using
the categories of the description of the language as a whole; and the
comparison of each text with others, by the same and by different
authors, in the same and in different genres.
While insisting that stylistic studies use the same methods and
categories as nonliterary descriptions, we must make the proviso that
such studies may require new alignments or groupings of descriptive
categories, through which the special properties of a text may be
recognized. This may include the bringing together of categories and
items described at different levels as well as those scattered throughout
the description of any one level. An example of such a grouping, in
which various grammatical and lexical features are brought together, is
cohesion.
The principal categories subsumed under cohesion are:
A. Grammatical
1. Structural (clauses in sentence structure)
(a) Dependence
(b) Linking
6
THE LINGUISTIC STUDY OF LITERARY TEXTS
2. Non-structural
(a) Anaphora
(i) deictics and submodifiers
(ii) pronouns
(b) Substitution
(i) verbal
(ii) nominal
B. Lexical
1. Repetition of item
2. Occurrence of item from same lexical set
7
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
8
THE LINGUISTIC STUDY OF LITERARY TEXTS
"set toward the message" that detennines the particular type of linguis-
tic patterning that is characteristic of literature.
If we keep the word "pattern" as a general, non-technical name for
all the organization, at all levels, that is a crucial property of language
as such, then the special property of literary language is the patterning
of the variability of these patterns. In other words, the creative writer
finds and exploits the irregularity that the patterns allow, and in
doing so superimposes a further regularity. It is this "regularity", as
we may reasonably call it provided we avoid giving the term an
arithmetical interpretation, that marks the "focus on the message".
This is clearly displayed by any good linguistic description of a text -
provided there exists already a good description, textual or otherwise,
of that language.
All illustrations in linguistics are misleading. Language does not
operate except in the context of other events; even where these are, as
with written texts, other language events, any one point made about a
piece of text which is under focus raises many further points extending
way beyond it into the context. This does not mean that no linguistic
statements can be self-sufficient, but that the only ultimately valid unit
for textual analysis is the whole text. It takes many hours of talking to
describe exhaustively even the language of one sonnet.
However, if students can be asked to comment on the language of
literary texts within the time limits of an examination, it should be
possible to give selective illustrations of what would be regarded as a
good answer to a question on the language of particular short texts. I
propose here to refer to W. B. Yeats' poem Leda and the Swan, and to
three short passages of modern English prose by John Braine, Dylan
Thomas, and Angus Wilson.
9
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
10
THE LINGUISTIC STUDY OF LITERARY TEXTS
For example:
Cataphoric:
The absence efguides
the only impracticable route
Anaphoric:
turned to the ascent of the peak. The climb
Homophoric:
the truth
In two instances, the more agreeable and all the more satisfaction, the is not
a deictic at all but a distinct formal item which operates as submodifier
in the nominal group.
The complete statement of the formal properties of these relations,
such that they can be recognized as distinct structures, is complex and
involves lexis as well as grammar - though in spoken English, since
tonicity (the placing of the tonic in the tone group) can be observed, it
is possible to make a purely grammatical statement that accounts for
most occurrences. In written English the general picture is as follows:
there is a high probability that
(a) if there is a modifier (other than the) or qualifier in the nominal
group, the is cataphoric,
(b) if there is no modifier or qualifier, then
(i) if in the preceding context there has occurred a lexical item
which is either the same item as, or from the same lexical set
as, the head of the nominal group, the is anaphoric,
(ii) if not, the is homophoric.
Table 1 (p. 20) shows all the nominal groups, other than those
consisting only of pronoun or personal name, in Leda and the Swan.
Out of a total of 25, no less than 15 have both a specific deictic (10
the, 5 others) and a modifier (other than the deictic) or qualifier or
both. This contrasts, for example, with Yeats' poem His Phoenix,
which contains 81 nominal groups of which only 17 are of this type.
In nominal groups with modifier or qualifier, if the, or other specific
deictic, is present it is usually cataphoric; moreover, samples of mod-
ern English prose writing show that the most frequent use of the is
in fact cataphoric reference to modifier or qualifier, not anaphoric
reference ("second mention") as often supposed. In Leda, however,
out of ten nominal groups having the and a modifier or qualifier,
only one, the brute blood <!.f the air, had the in cataphoric use. The
remainder, althou~h they have both (a) items whose place in structure
11
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
(at M or Q) makes them potentially defining, and (b) the item the
whose function is usually to show that such potentially defining items
are in fact defining, yet have non-cataphoric the. That is to say, in
spite of the the, the dark webs are not identified by their being dark -
like the loins, they are to be identified anaphorically, in fact by ana-
phoric reference to the title of the poem. The only other type of
writing I can call to mind in which this feature is found at such a
high density is in tourist guides and, sometimes, exhibition catalogues.
(I hope I need not add that this is in no sense intended as an adverse
criticism of the poem.)
The second example is the distribution of verbal items in Leda and
the Swan. Most of this poem, especially the first ten and a half lines, is
organized in nominal groups; they account for 69 of the 83 words in
this first part. There are 14 verbal groups in the poem, and in addition
four words of the class "verb" operating directly in the structure of (as
opposed to being rankshifted into) nominal groups (staggering, loosening,
broken, burning).
The table below represents a sort of scale of "verbness" in the use of
verbal items - the "dine of verbality", to give it a jargonistic label. On
the extreme left, most "verbish" of all, is the finite verbal group in free
clause; the further over to the right, the more the status of verb is
attenuated, until finally it is subordinated altogether to the nominal
element without even the formality of a rankshift. In Leda, with its
preponderance of nominal groups, the verbal items are considerably
deverbalized: contrast again His Phoenix, and also the 16 lines from
Tennyson's Morte d'Arthur beginning Then quickly rose Sir Bedivere, and
ran (columns as in Table 2 on p. 21):
1 2 3 4 5 6
Leda 5 2 3 4 4
His Phoenix 30 12 2 6 2 2
Marte d'Arthur 17 3 2
(extract)
12
THE LINGUISTIC STUDY OF LITERARY TEXTS
13
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
Every inch of the room was covered with furniture. Chairs stood on
couches that lay on tables; mirrors nearly the height of the door were
propped, back to back, against the walls, reflecting and making endless
the hills of desks and chairs with their legs in the air, sideboards,
dressing-tables, chests-of-drawers, more mirrors, empty bookcases,
wash-basins, clothes cupboards. There was a double bed, carefully made
with the ends of the sheets turned back; lying on top of a dining table
on top of another table there were electric lamps and lampshades, trays
and vases, lavatory bowls and basins, heaped in the armchairs that stood
on cupboards and tables and beds, touching the ceiling. The one
window, looking out on the road, could just be seen through the
curved legs of sideboards on their backs. The walls behind the standing
mirrors were thick with pictures and picture frames.
Dylan Thomas
Her little bedroom at the hotel was ugly - the more hideous for
having been recently redecorated with a standard 'contemporary' wall-
paper. All over the walls floated gay little blue and pink cafe tables,
around them a few Vermouth and Pernod bottles and the word
'Montmartre' in pretty childish script. The design was no doubt carefully
chosen to enchant cross-Channel travellers; it had no message for Meg.
In the first weeks she had sought every excuse to be away from the
room; but now suddenly the wallpaper, the pink, bevel-edged, modern-
istic mirror, and the furniture of shaded pink and silver began to give
her a sense of anonymity. They were so remote from anything she
knew or cared for that she felt free, safe, and hidden.
Angus Wilson
14
THE LINGUISTIC STUDY OF LITERARY TEXTS
JB DT AW
Nominal groups 36 49 37
Nominal groups with lexical head 32 49 28
with M/Q (lexical) 16 11 12
with D 22 19 15
Head from 'room' set 19 40 9
from 'furniture and decor' 14 34 5
from 'constructional' 5 6 4
15
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
16
THE LINGUISTIC STUDY OF LITERARY TEXTS
17
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
same way. All literary analysis, if one is at all interested in the special
properties of the language of literary texts or of a particular genre, is
essentially comparative. This makes it all the more essential to be
consistent, accurate and explicit: to base the analysis firmly on a sound,
existing description of the language. While this means restraining oneself
from inventing new categories, a temptation to which the literary
analyst must be especially exposed, it does not of course preclude new
alignments of established categories, such as Professor McIntosh's use of
"involvement" in studying Shakespearean dialogue. 6 These may be
required in any linguistic study, but they are perhaps especially fruitful
in comparative literary studies. Another example is the relation of
cohesion referred to above, which is very valuable in comparing long
texts. The concept of cohesion has been developed especially for literary
textual analysis; but every category brought together under this heading
is drawn from the total description of English and has exactly the same
range of application whatever the text to which it is applied.
Of course no amount of faithful adherence to the same description
will be of any use if the description is not a good one in the first place.
We are still plagued with steam grammars, with their ragged categories,
their jumbled criteria and their fictions; descriptions of English which
give little insight into the way the language works or indeed the way
any language works, except perhaps the Latin they were originally
modelled on. This kind of pre-linguistic linguistics is no use for literary
studies. It is no paradox that it is modem scientific, including statistical,
linguistics that proves really illuminating when applied to the study of
literature - no paradox at least to anyone who studies language
seriously, since the study of language perhaps more than anything else
shows up the artificial nature of the dichotomy between arts and
sciences. Not only do we need to be able to state accurately the role of
a particular pattern or item in the language, what it contrasts with,
what it may and may not combine with and so on; we may want to
know its probability of occurrence under various definable conditions.
It is of no interest to show that nine-tenths of all clauses in a certain
poem are, say, of the class "interrogative" unless we know how this
relates to the probabilities of occurrence of this and the other terms in
the mood system. The originality of a person's use of his language
consists in his selecting a feature not where it is impossible (has not
been previously selected) but where another would be more probable
- and even more in his balanced combination of the improbable with
the probable, as in the lexis of Leda and the Swan, which is an interesting
blend of old and new collocations. ·
18
THE LINGUISTIC STUDY OF LITERARY TEXTS
I have stressed grammar, but this is of course only one of the levels
involved, and the usefulness oflinguistic theory in application to literary
studies depends on its ability both to comprehend and to integrate all
the levels of language. That is another reason for insisting on the need
for up-to-date linguistics: not only must the literary analyst have access
to theories for the description of all levels of linguistic patterning -
grammar, lexis, phonology and phonetics, and their graphic parallels -
but he must be able to see them in interaction as they must always
interact in any language event. He may want to analyse, for example,
her loosening thighs as a grammatical item, with a certain structure - is it
the same as the staggering girl, or not?; as a collocation in lexis - is loosen
the same lexical item as loose?; as a piece of English phonology - how
does it exploit the patterns of English rhythm and intonation?; and in
terms of its phonetic properties. He may feel he needs to do all this so
that he can see what it is doing in the poem. I have not, for lack of
time, given illustrations of phonological and phonetic analysis; but this
is a good place to point out that when we speak of "linguistics" in
literary studies, this is really a shorthand for "the linguistic sciences",
an~ is to be taken to include them both - linguistics and phonetics.
If it is considered that the meaning of a piece of literature lies
between rather than within the lines, it seems likely that linguistics has
no message. This is not to say that the literary allusion is outside the
scope of linguistic analysis; on the contrary, all use of language is
allusion, and textual allusion is only one endpoint on a scale the other
end of which is the context of our whole previous experience of the
use of an item or pattern: hence the insistence that a work ofliterature,
like any other piece of language activity, is meaningful only in the
perspective of the whole range of uses of the language. Similarly it is
not to say that literary "figures of speech" cannot be analysed; it is true,
however, that they do need rigorous linguistic definition. Again there
is no sharp line to be drawn between metaphor and non-metaphor;
but if linguistics cannot describe certain parts of language it is likely to
be of little use for any application. Linguistics is not and will never be
the whole of literary analysis, and only the literary analyst - not the
linguist - can determine the place of linguistics in literary studies. But
if a text is to be described at all, then it should be described properly;
and this means by the theories and methods developed in linguistics,
the subject whose task is precisely to show how language works. The
literary analyst is not content with amateur psychology, armchair
philosophy, or fictitious social history; yet the linguistics that is applied
in some accounts of literature, and the statements about language that
19
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
~
+M/Q - MIQ
D M ',Q MQ
the staggering the thighs the great the loins
girl caressed by wings the air
the dark webs the dark beating his bill
the feathered webs above the his breast
glory her nape staggering her nape
the broken wall caught in girl his knowledge
the burning his bill the strange his power
roof and heart beating
tower the brute
+D
blood
specific the indifferent
of the air
beak
those terrified
vague fingers
that white rush
her helpless
breast
her loosening
thighs
+D non- a sudden blow a shudder in
specific the loins
-D body
20
THE 'LINGUISTIC STUDY OF LITERARY TEXTS
Table 2 Verbal items in Leda and the Swan and the fifteen lines from
Tennyson's Morte d'Arthur
Items in
Iterns in verbal group (i.e. operating nominal group
at 'predicator' in clause structure) (i.e. not
operating at P.)
l 2 3 4 5 6
Clause class
Independent Dependent Rankshifted
(system: status)
(irrelevant)
Group class
Finite Finite Non-finite Finite Non-finite
(system: finiteness)
5 2 3 - 5 4
15 2 3 - 2 -
Note: Compare the narrative style in Tennyson (the most 'active' verbs in the most 'verbal'
setting) with the tension set up in Leda (the most 'active' verbs in the least 'verbal'
setting, as if describing a picture rather than recounting an event).
21
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
Notes
1. See Thomas A. Sebeok (ed.): Style in Language (Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press, 1960), p. 352.
2. Word, 17 December 1961 (see Volume 1, Chapter 2).
3. A more correct theoretical statement of structural cohesion is that it is
presupposition at the rank of the sentence. Presupposition is the special
relation between elements of a non-chain-exhausting structure that have as
their exponents terms in a non-choice-exhausting system. Thus in I'll come
if you want me the structural relation of "conditioning" clause and "condi-
tioned" clau~, which is a type of dependence, is one of presupposition.
4. I omit here phonological cohesion: that with grammatical categories
expounded directly by phonology, for example (British English) tone 4 in
anaphoric use. ,.
5. Op. cit., p. 356.
6. See Angus McIntosh, '"As You Like It": a grammatical clue to character',
Review ef English Literature, 4.2, April 1963.
22
Chapter Two
Experiential Logical
First published in Grammars and Descriptio11s, edited by Teun A. van Dijk and Janos S.
Petofi. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter, 1977, pp. 176-226.
23
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
WORD:
I
Verb Noun
I
Adverb
I
Preposition ~ WORD
COMPLEX
MORPHEME ..,_
_______..,._ MORPHEME
COMPLEX
24
TEXT AS SEMANTIC CHOICE IN SOCIAL CONTEXTS
1. 2 Structural con.figurations
It follows from the above that each type of unit - clause, verbal group,
nominal group, etc. - is in itself a structural composite, a combination
of structures each of which derives from one or other component of
the semantics.
A clause, for example, has a structure formed out of elements such
as Agent, Process, Extent; this structure derives from the system of
transitivity, which is part of the experiential component. Simul-
taneously it has a structure formed out of the elements Modal and
Propositional: this derives from the system of mood, which is part of
the interpersonal component. It also has a third structure composed of
the elements Theme and Rheme, deriving from the theme system,
which is part of the textual component.
For example:
Ithe Grays Iretired Ito their beds I
Experiential TRANSITIVITY Medium Process Location:
locative
Interpersonal MOOD Modal Propositional
Textual THEME Theme Rheme
It is not the case that the same constituent structure (same bracketing)
holds throughout, with only the labels differing. This is already clear
from this example: the thematic and modal structures are simple binary
ones, whereas the transitivity structure is not. In any case, the represen-
tation just given is oversimplified; the Modal constituent includes the
finite element in the verb, and consists of Subject plus Finiteness,
yielding an analysis as follows:
25
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
26
TEXT AS SEMANTIC CHOICE IN SOCIAL CONTEXTS
(extrinsic) (enabling)
Logical Experiential
(univariate) (multivariate)
27
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
e (paratactic
C: nominal group: nominal group: -~ nominal group: conjunction
and
';; hypotactic)
types of participant;
class, quality,
person ('role')
....ii
0
deixis
l;i
!'.)
quantity, etc.
.!19 lexical cohesion:
"
.!!
Q. adverbial group: adverbial group: 0 adverbial group: reiteration,
e
u
0 prepositional group:
types of circumstance
prepositional group
comment
§ prepositional group:
conjunction
collocation
28
TEXT AS SEMANTIC CHOICE IN SOCIAL CONTEXTS
29
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
30
TEXT AS SEMANTIC CHOICE IN SOCIAL CONTEXTS
31
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
32
TEXT AS SEMANTIC CHOICE IN SOCIAL CONTEXTS
(1) (a) If the information focus falls on an element that is other than
the last accented element in the information unit (an "accented"
element being any element that is not "inherently Given"), the focus is
marked.
Semantically this means that the information structure is environ-
mentally specific; there is no indetemiinacy, and all else in the
information unit is Given. Example (s. 11):
(But they decided instead to ... ) // 1 gossip a/bout the / shameless /
pair//
(1) (b) If the focus falls on the last accented element (last element
other than any that are "inherently Given"), it is unmarked. Semanti-
cally this means that the information structure is not environmentally
specific; and indetenninacy results. In this case, the whole information
unit may be New; or the New may begin at any structural boundary.
The only restriction is that the New cannot be discontinuous: G+ N,
N+G, G+N+G are all possible structures, but N+G+N is not - the
33
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
(2) The focal element, the element that is defined by the tonic
prominence, is the highest-ranking sub-constituent for which the
prominence is culminative. That is to say, if the information unit is
one clause (this being, as a matter of fact, the unmarked form of the
mapping), the focal constituent will be a group - the group being the
next-ranking unit below the clause - provided the tonic promi-
nence falls on the last accented syllable in the entire group.
Example (s. 15):
where the tonic prominence on shears marks the entire group a pair ef
unoiled garden shears as being the focal constituent (since the information
unit is a clause). If the tonic prominence had been assigned to unoiled,
the focal constituent would be only the word unoiled itself, since the
prominence would not be culminative for the whole nominal group,
but only for the word. In the former instance, with the reading we
have assumed, the information structure is unmarked. In the latter, the
information structure would be marked, and would therefore presup-
pose a specific semantic environment, in which the point at issue was
'what kind of garden shears would you as soon live with?'
The information system, in contrast to the thematic system, is
hearer-oriented. That is to say, the meaning that is encoded in the
Given-New structure is that of 'recoverable, or not recoverable, to the
hearer'. This in tum depends on the environment, both verbal and
non-verbal; if a meaning is recoverable, it is in some '\Nay or other (but
there are many possible ways) present in the environment. Since the
environment includes the preceding text, the information structure
often serves to relate a piece to what has gone before it. But recovera-
bility is not a simple matter of previous mention; and in any case it is
the speaker's decision what he is going to treat as recoverable. He is
free to use the system as he pleases, and frequently uses it to great effect
as a means of constructing the environment it is designed to reflect.
The speaker has total discretion; he is constructing all the meanings
at once. The reader of a written text is in the peculiar position of
having discretion at just those points where the written medium
happens to be most ambiguous; of which the information system is
one. He can, if he chooses, read sentences 2 and 3 as ·
34
TEXT AS SEMANTIC CHOICE IN SOCIAL CONTEXTS
which imposes the interpretation 'Do you believe that's her name? -
that's what he calls her'. The only grounds for rejecting this are that it
doesn't make sense (of the text; it makes perfectly good sense of the
sentence). There could, of course, be no better grounds than that. But
it illustrates the kind of decision that a reader is making all the time,
whereas a hearer has the solution presented to him readymade.
We tend to think of the information unit as being unlike the
lexicogrammatical units of clause, group and so on, in that while
the latter are specified simultaneously by all semantic components,
the former is defined solely by the textual component. A clause is the
domain of systems of all kinds, experiential (e.g. transitivity), interper-
sonal (e.g. mood), and textual (e.g. theme); whereas the information
unit is the domain only of the information systems. But this is not
wholly true. The information unit is also exploited by the interper-
sonal component as the carrier of the systems of KEY: those, related to
mood, whereby the speaker selects the key signature that attaches to
the particular role assignment he is making for himself and for the
hearer. The choice of key is expressed by the tone contour; and the
point of origin for this choice is the information unit - since the
information unit is encoded as a tone group, and one tone group is
one complete tone contour, the two naturally coincide. So the
information structure operates as a kind of distinct but simultaneous
constituent hierarchy, or "rank scale"; one that is mapped on to the
conventional grammatical hierarchy of clause, group, word and mor-
pheme, but realizes a different set of semantic systems. The rationale
for this is very simple: the information structure is simply the phono-
logical system doing extra work. It is the phonological hierarchy that
is being "borrowed" for the occasion.
The various types of phonological contrast, those of intonation, of
rhythm or "pulse", and of articulation, are organized as a distinct
constituent hierarchy, or "phonological rank scale"; in English, tone
group, foot (or stress group), syllable, phoneme. This hierarchy, and the
"phonotactics" that is based on it, functions as a whole in the realization
of the lexicogrammatical system. But once it has come into being, so
to speak, particular parts of it function on their own in the representation
of particular semantic systems, which can thus be regarded as "bypassing"
the grammar - as "meanings encoded directly in sounds" - although
for theoretical reasons it is useful to include them in the systematic
35
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
36
TEXT AS SEMANTIC CHOICE IN SOCIAL CONTEXTS
37
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
and so on. And the contrast between tone 3 and tone 4, indicating
different degrees of dependence, is available to almost any information
unit that is contingent on another one having tone 1.
If now we consider a sentence such as the last one of the main
paragraph (s. 11), we find a large number of possible interpretations,
including but by no means limited to the following:
But they decided to phone their friends and gossip about the
instead and neighbors shameless pair
I I 3 ... instead II 3 ... neighbors II 1 . gossip ... II
II 3 ... instead II 1 ... neighbors II 1 . gossip ... II
II 4 ... instead II 3 ... neighbors II 1 . gossip ... II
II 4 ... instead II 1 ... neighbors // 1 . gossip ... II
II 1 ... neighbors II 1 . gossip ... II
II 4 ... instead II 1 .... gossip ... II
2. 5 Referential cohesion
Certain elements in the linguistic system have the property that they
are interpretable only by reference to something other than themselves.
These are the personals, demonstratives (including the) and compara-
tives; for example she, this, earlier as in She's shy. This is what I meant.
You should have come earlier. These are perfectly intelligible on their
own; but they are interpretable only when we know who "she" is,
what "this" is, and "earlier" than what.
The reference may be exophoric, to some phenomenon located out-
side the text and in the context of situation; or endophoric, to an element
within the text, typically something that has preceded (anaphoric) but
sometimes to something that follows (cataphoric). So she may refer to
someone present and identifiable, or to someone previously mentioned;
this likewise to some object present and identifiable, or to something -
an object or other phenomenon, or a fact - just mentioned or about to
be mentioned; earlier may involve comparison with the present moment
("than now") or with some time previously mentioned. In a written
narrative, all reference can be assumed to be endophoric; 'but the
38
TEXT AS SEMANTIC CHOICE IN SOCIAL CONTEXTS
39
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
2. 7 Conjunctive cohesion
Any pair of adjacent sentences may be related by one of a small set of
semantic relations, which may be described in most general terms
under the four headings of 'and', 'yet', 'so', and 'then' (additive,
adversative, causal, temporal). Each of these covers a wide range of
more specific meanings.
Like other cohesive relations, these have their structural counterparts
in the form of relations within the sentence, for example in the hypotactic
structures with besides, although, because, after. But the systems of options
are different under these two conditions, the cohesive and the structural.
Each one of the types of conjunctive relation has in principle two
interpretations, according to the functional-semantic component from
which it is derived. Either the conjunctive meaning resides in the
ideational component, as a relation within the thesis; or it resides in the
interpersonal component, as a relation within the speech process. These
have been referred to respectively as "external" and "internal", taking
the communication process as the point of departure_: a relation
between things - between phenomena that constitute the ideational
40
TEXT AS SEMANTIC CHOICE IN SOCIAL CONTEXTS
(external) First of all the machine broke down. Next it started to make
alarming noises inside.
(internal) First of all the machine has broken down. Next it doesn't
belong to me anyway.
The tender things they said to each other ... sounded as lyric to them
as flowers in bud ... To the Grays, however, the bumbling romp of
the lover and his lass was hard to comprehend and even harder to
tolerate ...
where the meaning is 'by contrast', 'on the other hand', and the
adversative relation resides not so much in the phenomenon of there
being the two attitudes ('seemed good to the hippos but bad to the
parrots') as in the narrator's juxtaposing of them as a step in the
narrative, brought out by the foregrounded status of the Grays as a
marked Theme in the second sentence.
2. 8 Lexical cohesion
This is the special kind of texture that is achieved by the use of
vocabulary, (a) by reiteration and (b) by collocation. Both of these can
be exemplified from the text (numerals refer to sentences):
41
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
:s:_
same word
.
same
referent
lover 1, 8
including excluding unrelated
same referent same referent referent
lovemaking 10 love 18
synonym inamoratus 15 courtship 9 mate 1
endeannent 12
super- male 5 qffection 5 female 5, 16
ordinate male 16
general creatures 10
word pair 11
The display reflects the fact that the word lover is semantically complex;
it contains the two components of affection and mating, and both of
these elements are reiterated throughout the text. There is a certain
arbitrariness in both dimensions of the table, but each is motivated by
general considerations, the vertical dimension representing the organ-
ization of the system and the horizontal the patterning of the text. The
vertical dimension is really a continuity, ranging from (1) repetitions
(of the same lexical item - which is itself by no means a determinate
concept; here we take the morpheme love as criterial), through (2)
synonyms at more or less the same level of generality, to (3) related
items of greater generality, those which are higher in the lexical
taxonomy, up to (4) the class of "general words" that figure at the top,
which have very little specific content and occur mainly as cohesive
agents. The horizontal dimension shows the referential relationship
between the reiterated item and the base word: co-referential, inclusive,
exclusive or unrelated.
(b) Collocation Qexical). Keyword: lover (in title). Cohesive chain
formed of items related by collocation:
lass (title) - lover (1) - lass (1) -
affection (4) - spring (8) - lover (8) -
lass (8) - young (8) - oblivious (8) -
happily (8) - tender (9) - lyric (9) -
flowers (9) - bud (9)
These are pairs or sets of items that have a strong tendency in the
system to co-occurrence; hence when they do co-occur in a text, the
effect is cohesive. The two concepts of reiteration and collocation are
42
TEXT AS SEMANTIC CHOICE IN SOCIAL CONTEXTS
overlapping: lexical items that typically collocate with one another (i.e.
are related in the lexical system) are often those which are partially
synonymous (i.e. are related in the semantic system). But they are not
identical concepts; the relationships are on two different levels. There
is no, or only a tenuous, semantic link between lover and spring: but
these are regularly collocated in English writing from Shakespeare to
the present day. Conversely, there is no strong collocational bond
between courtship and endearment, or between lover and inamoratus - it
must be rare to find the latter pair in the same text under any
circumstances! The lexical structure of a text depends on both types of
relationship, and on the interplay that occurs between the two.
The sequences illustrated are probably the most pervasive in the
present text, but there are other important strands of lexical cohesion,
for example the motifs of derision (arrogant - scornful - sharp-tongued -
mocking - derision) and of monstrosity. For a detailed study and
interpretation of collocational patterning in English, see Sinclair et al.
(1970).
Cohesion can be thought of as a process in the text, the linking of
some element - often but not always an element that is inherently
presupposing - to something that has gone before, or in certain
instances to something that is to follow. It would be wrong, however,
to conceive of it as having no place in the semantic system, as what
some linguists call a "surface" phenomenon. Cohesion is also a relation
in the system. As such it is not directional, though it is ordered in the
case of inherently presupposing elements (reference items and substi-
tutes). The meaning of this relation in its most general terms is that of
"co-interpretation". This in tum takes on a number of more specific
meanings according to the type of cohesion: co-referentiality (identity
of reference) is one of these more specific meanings. Co-interpretation
refers to the fact that the elements that are "tied" by the cohesive
relation are interpreted (not identically but) as a whole, with mutual
dependence or "solidarity" between them.
A general treatment of cohesion, covering reference, substitution,
ellipsis, conjunction and lexical cohesion, will be found in Halliday and
Hasan (1976). This book also contains a scheme of analysis and notation
for describing the cohesive properties of a text. For a treatment of
cohesion in English literary texts see Gutwinski (1974).
43
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
44
TEXT AS SEMANTIC CHOICE IN SOCIAL CONTEXTS
45
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
46
TEXT AS SEMANTIC CHOICE IN SOCIAL CONTEXTS
47
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
48
TEXT AS SEMANTIC CHOICE IN SOCIAL CONTEXTS
in much the same way as the linguistic system itself. An example of this
"layering" from the present text is the use of the generic form of the
fable as the vehicle of a humorous essay, already referred to above. The
"level of literary execution" is part of the total realizational chain; see
Hasan (1971).
When there is foregrounding of lexicogrammatical ot phonological
features in a literary text, particular forms of linguistic prominence that
relate directly to some facet of its literary interpretation, this is closely
analogous to the "bypassing" phenomenon that is found within the
linguistic system when some element in the semantics is realized directly
in phonological terms (cf. 2.3 above). At this point there is isomorphism
between two adjacent strata, and the phenomenon can be represented
as a straight pass through one of the strata! systems. We have referred
to this already in relation to the information and "key" systems in the
semantics of English: an example is the bandwidth of a falling tone
expressing the degree of "newness" or semantic contrast involved in a
statement. It is possible in such a case to set up a grammatical system as
an interface between the semantics and the phonology; and there are
strong reasons for doing so, since there is a systematic interrelationship
between this and other grammatical systems, although strictly in its own
terms the grammatical representation is redundant because there is
neither neutralization nor diversification at this point.
The point is a significant one because a great deal of stylistic
foregrounding depends on an analogous process, by which some aspect
of the underlying meaning is represented linguistically at more than one
level: not only through the semantics of the text - the ideational and
interpersonal meanings, as embodied in the content and in the writer's
choice of his role - but also by direct reflection in the lexicogrammar or
the phonology. For an example of this from a study ofWilliam Golding's
novel The Inheritors see Chapter 3 of this volume, where it is suggested
that the particular impact of this novel on reader and critic may be
explained by the fact that the underlying semiotic is projected simul-
taneously on to the semantics, in the content of narrative and dialogue,
and on to the grammar in the highly untypical transitivity patterns that
characterize, not so much individual clauses (none of which is in itself
deviant), but the distribution of clause types in the writing as a whole.
The present text does not display this feature of multi-level fore-
grounding to any great extent because it is both short and prose. A
verse text, however short, provides scope by virtue of its generic form
for the sort of patterned variability of patterns which is involved in this
kind of multiple projection; whereas in a prose text it is likely to appear
49
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
50
TEXT AS SEMANTIC CHOICE IN SOCIAL CONTEXTS
kind of truth about it: see Pike (1959). Linguistic theory has remained
at a stage at which particulateness is treated as the norm, and a number
of different and not very clearly related concepts are invoked to handle
the non-particulate aspects of language. As far as text studies, and text
meaning, is concerned, however, we cannot relegate the indeterminacy
to an appendix. The text is a continuous process. There is a constantly
shifting relation between a text and its environment, both paradigmatic
and syntagmatic: the syntagmatic environment, the "context of situ-
ation" (which includes the semantic context - and which for this
reason we interpret as a semiotic construct), can be treated as a constant
for the text as a whole, but is in fact constantly changing, each part
serving in tum as environment for the next. And the ongoing text-
creating process continually modifies the system that engenders it,
which is the paradigmatic environment of the text. Hence the dynamic,
indeterminate nature of meaning, which can be idealized out to the
margins if one is considering only the system, or only the text, emerges
as the dominant mode of thought as soon as one comes to consider the
two together, and to focus on text as actualized meaning potential.
The essential feature of text, therefore, is that it is interaction. The
exchange of meanings is an interactive process, and text is the means
of exchange: in order for the meanings which constitute the social
system to be exchanged between members they must first be repres-
ented in some exchangeable symbolic form, and the most accessible of
the available forms is language. So the meanings are encoded in (and
through) the semantic system, and given the form of text. And so text
functions as it were as potlatch: it is perhaps the most highly coded
form of the gift. The contests in meaning that are a feature of so many
human groups - cultures and sub-cultures - are from this point of view
contests in giving, in a re-encoded form in which the gift, itself an
element in the social semiotic (a "meaning") but one that in the typical
or at least the classic instance is realized as a thing, is realized instead as
a special kind of abstract symbol, as meanings in the specifically
linguistic sense. Such a gift has the property that, however great its
symbolic value (and however much it may enrich the recipient), it
does not in the slightest degree impoverish the giver.
We can see this aspect of text, its function as exchange, most clearly
in the phenomenon of semantic contest: in competitive story-telling,
exchange of insults, "capping" another's jokes and other forms of
verbal exploit. Oral verse fom1s such as ballads, lyrics, and epigrammatic
and allusive couplets figure in many cultures as modes of competing,
and even written composition may be predominantly a competitive
51
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
52
TEXT AS SEMANTIC CHOICE IN SOCIAL CONTEXTS
form of text. The meanings so created are not, of course, isolates; they
are integrated systems of meaning potential. It is in this sense that we
can say that the meanings are the social system: the social system is
itself interpretable as a semiotic system.
Persistence and change in the social system are both reflected in text
and brought about by means of text. Text is the primary channel of
the transmission of culture; and it is this aspect - text as the semantic
process of social dynamics - that more than anything else has shaped
the semantic system. Language has evolved as the primary mode of
meaning in a social environment. It provides the means of acting on
and reflecting on the environment, to be sure - but in a broader
context, in which acting and. reflecting on the environment are in tum
the means of creating the environment and transmitting it from one
generation to the next. That this is so is because the environment is a
social construct. If things enter into it, they do so as bearers of social
values.
Let us follow this line of reasoning through. The linguistic system
has evolved in social contexts, as (one form of) the expression of the
social semiotic. We see this clearly in the organization of the semantic
system, where the ideational component has evolved as the mode of
reflection on the environment and the interpersonal component as the
mode of action on the environment. The system is a meaning potential,
which is actualized in the form of text; a text is an instance of social
meaning in a particular context of situation. We shall therefore expect
to find the situation embodied or enshrined in the text not piecemeal,
but in a way which reflects the systematic relation between the
semantic structure and the social environment. In other words, the
"situation" will appear, as envisaged by Hymes (1971), as constitutive
of the text; provided, that is, we can characterize it so as to take
cognizance of the ecological properties of language, the features which
relate it to its environment in the social system.
A text is, as we have stressed, an indeterminate concept. It may be
very long, or very short; and it may have no very clear boundaries.
Many things about language can be learnt only from the study of very
long texts. But there is much to be found out also from little texts; not
only texts in the conventional forms of lyric poetry, proverbs and the
like, but also brief transactions, casual encounters, and all kinds of
verbal micro-operations. And among these there is a special value to
the linguist in children's texts, since these tend to display their environ-
mental links more directly and with less metaphorical mediation. (For
a description of a short piece of child language, showing its relationship
53
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
54
TEXT AS SEMANTIC CHOICE IN SOCIAL CONTEXTS
55
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
56
TEXT AS SEMANTIC CHOICE IN SOCIAL CONTEXTS
that of respondent, and not the other way round (c( Five to Nine
(1972), Sinclair et al. (1972)) - despite our concept of education, it is
not the learner who asks the questions.
The selection of options in the textual systems, such as those of
theme, information and voice, and also the selection of cohesive patterns,
those of reference, substitution and ellipsis, and cortjunction, tend to be
determined by the symbolic forms taken by the interaction, in particular
the place that is assigned to the text in the total situation. This includes
the distinction of medium, written or spoken, and the complex sub-
varieties derived from these (written to be read aloud, and so on); we
have already noted ways in which the organization of text-forming
resources is dependent on the medium of the text. But it extends to
much more than this, to the particular semiotic function or range of
functions that the text is serving in the environment in question. The
rhetorical concepts of expository, didactic, persuasive, descriptive and
the like are examples of such semiotic functions. All the categories
under this third heading are second-order categories, in that they are
defined by reference to language and depend for their existence on the
prior phenomenon of text. It is in this sense that the textual component
in the semantic system was said to have an "enabling" function vis-a-vis
the other two: it is only through the encoding of semiotic interaction
as text that the ideational and interpersonal components of meaning
can become operational in an environment.
The concept of genre discussed in Section 3 (p. 44) is an aspect of
what we are calling the "mode". The various genres of discourse,
including literary genres, are the specific semiotic functions of text that
have social value in the culture. A genre may have implications for other
components of meaning: there are often associations between a particular
genre and particular semantic features of an ideational or interpersonal
kind, for example between the genre of prayer and certain selections in
the mood system. Hence labels for generic categories are often function-
ally complex: a concept such as "ballad" implies not only a certain text
structure with typical patterns of cohesion but also a certain range of
content expressed through highly favoured options in transitivity and
other experiential systems - the types of process and classes of person
and object that are expected to figure in association with the situational
role of a ballad text. The "fable" is a category of a similar kind.
The patterns of determination that we find between the context of
situation and the text are a general characteristic of the whole complex
that is formed by a text and its environment. We shall not expect to be
able to show that the options embodied in one or another particular
57
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
sentence are determined by the field, tenor and mode of the situation.
The principle is that each of these elements in the semiotic structure of
the situation activates the corresponding component in the semantic
system, creating in the process a semantic configuration, a grouping of
favoured and foregrounded options from the total meaning potential,
that is typically associated with the situation type in question. This
semantic configuration is what we understand by the register: it defines
the variety ("diatypic variety" in the sense of Gregory (1967)) that the
particular text is an instance 0£ The concept of "register" is the
necessary mediating concept that enables us to establish the continuity
between a text and its sociosemiotic environment.
The tenor is also on two levels, since two distinct sets of role
relationships are embodied in the text: one between the narrator and
his readership, which is embodied in the narrative, and one among the
participants in the narrative, which is embodied in the dialogue:
58
TEXT AS SEMANTIC CHOICE IN SOCIAL CONTEXTS
Since under each of the headings of "field" and "tenor" the text has
appeared as a complex of two distinct levels, we might be tempted to
conclude that a fictional narrative of this kind was really two separate
"texts" woven together. As a purely abstract model this could be made
to stand; but it is really misleading, not only because it fails to account
for the integration of the text - and in any sensible interpretation this
is one text and not two - but also because the relation between the
two levels is quite different in respect of the tenor from what it is in
respect of the field. As regards the tenor, the text does fall into two
distinct segments, the narrative, and the dialogue; each is characterized
by its own set of role relationships, and the two combine to form a
whole. As regards the field, however, there is no division in the text
corresponding to the two levels of social action: the whole text is at
one and the same time an act of malicious gossip and an act of verbal
art, the one being the realization of the other. We could not, in other
words, begin by separating out the two levels and then go on to
describe the field and the tenor of each; we have to describe the field
of the text, and then the tenor of the text, and both in different ways
then reveal its two-level semiotic organization.
The oneness of the text also appears in the characterization of the
mode, the symbolic structure of the situation and the specific role
assigned to the text within it:
59
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
60
TEXT AS SEMANTIC CHOICE IN SOCIAL CONTEXTS
61
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
deriving from the "tenor" are all assigned to the interpersonal component;
and those deriving from the "mode", to the textual component.
The logical component enters in to the picture in a dual perspective
which we shall not attempt to discuss in detail here. The meanings
that make up this component are generalized ideational relations such
as co-ordination, apposition, reported speech, modification and sub-
modification; as such they form a part of the ideational component. But
once in being, as it were, they may also serve to relate elements of the
other components, interpersonal and textual. To take a simplest example,
the meaning 'and' is itself an ideational one, but the 'and' relation can as
well serve to link interpersonal as ideational meanings: hell and damnation!
as well as snakes and ladders. Compare, in the present text, the "and-ing"
of alliterative (textual) features in disdain and derision, mocking and monstrous.
It should perhaps be stressed in this connection that the interpreta-
tion of the semantic system in terms of these components of ideational
(experiential, logical), interpersonal and textual is prior to and indepen-
dent of any consideration of field, tenor and mode. Such an interpreta-
tion is imposed by the form of internal organization of the linguistic
system. Hence we can reasonably speak of the determination of the
text by the situation, in the sense that the various semantic systems are
seen to be activated by particular environmental factors that stand in a
generalized functional relationship to them.
This picture emerges from a description of the properties of the text,
especially one in terms of the relative frequency of options in the
different systems. Much of the meaning of a text resides in the sort of
foregrounding that is achieved by this kind of environmentally moti-
vated prominence, in which certain sets of options are favoured
(selected with greater frequency than expected on the assumption of
unconditioned probability), as a realization of particular elements in the
social context. The inspection of these sets of options one by one, each
in its situational environment, is of course an analytical procedure; their
selection by the speaker, and apprehension by the hearer, is a process
of dynamic simultaneity, in which at any moment that we stop the
tape, as it were, a whole lot of meaning selections are going on at
once, all of which then become part of the environment in which
further choices are made. If we lift out any one piece of the text, such
as a single sentence, we will find the environment reflected not in the
individual options (since these become significant only through their
relative frequency of occurrence in the text), but in the particular
combination of options that characterizes this sentence taken as a
whole. As an example, consider the sentence:
62
TEXT AS SEMANTIC CHOICE IN SOCIAL CONTEXTS
"I would as soon live with a pair ef unoiled ,{!arden shears", said her
inamoratus.
63
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
by the register in question. The field, tenor and mode of the situation
collectively detennine the register and in this way function as consti-
tutive of the text.
What is revealed in a single sentence, or other unit of lexicogram-
matical structure, is its origin in the functional organization of the
semantic system. Each of the semantic components, ideational (experi-
ential and logical), interpersonal and textual, has contributed to its
makeup. The final section offers a detailed interpretation of one
sentence of the present text, in terms of systems and structures; taking
the same sentence as before, and presenting it as the product of
numerous micro-acts of semantic choice. This will complete the
semiotic cycle, the network that extends from the social system, as its
upper bound, through the linguistic system on the one hand and the
social context on the other, down to the "wording", which is the text
in its lexicogrammatical realization.
5 Analysis of a sentence
"I would as soon live with a pair ef unoiled garden shears", said her
inamoratus.
5. 1 Systemic descriptions
(All system networks are simplified so as to show only those portions chat are relevant to the sentence
under description. Systems in which only unmarked selections are made in this sentence, such as
polarity and voice, are omitted entirely.)
A. Logical
a) Clause complex (= sentence)
(This sentence is a clause complex consisting of two clauses in "quoting" relation. For the notion
of the "complex" (clause complex, group complex, etc.) see 1.1 above. All complexes, and only
complexes, display a recursive option of the form
]-----
... [re-enter)
(any complex)-----' stop
64
TEXT AS SEMANTIC CHOICE IN SOCIAL CONTEXTS
clause
complex expansion (addition/condition}
TYPE
LOGIC identity ('equals'}
RELATI
projection ('says'}
!STRUCTURAL
~ESCRIPTION
B: Quoted +-A: Quoting I
Note:_The intersection of these two systems yields the following paradigm of clause complex types:
tactic h tactic
expansion co-ordinate conditional, causal, concessive
identity appositive non-defining relative
projection quoting (direct speech) reporting (indirect speech)
65
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
Reporting (s. S)
a p
ldontsee how any male in his right mind could entertain q/fectlon for
afemale that has no more charm than a capsized bathtub.
nominal group I
verbal group would live
adverbial group as soon
prepositional group with a pair of unolled garden shears
nominal group a pair of 1D1oiled garden shears
verbal group said
nominal group her lnamoratus
These groups are all "simplexes"; they contain no logical (paratactic or hypotactic) structures. Examples of
group complexes would be:
as soon, or sooner
her lnamoratus, the male hippopotamus
one African afternoon at halfpast Jorn:
c) Word complexes
Note: Verbal, nominal and adverbial groups consisting of more than one element are simultaneously
structured both as word constructions (multivariate) and as word complexes (univariate). For example,
Experiential:
-
y
Epithet +
p
Modifier
Classifier +
a
Head
Thing
word complex:
univariate structure
multivariate structure
System network
paratactic
'TAXIS'--+ [
hypotactic
{
word complex
TYPE OF expansion
LOGICAL--+
RELATION Ldentity
66
TEXT AS SEMANTIC CHOICE IN SOCIAL CONTEXTS
Note: The intersection of these two systems yields the following paradigm of word complex types:
aratactic h tactic
expansion co-ordinate modifying
identity appositive defining appositive
!STRUCTURAL
DESCRIPTrON
p:Modifier +-a: Head I
Adverbial group as soon
SYSTEMIC adverbial group: (hypotactic/expansion)
DESCRIPTION
67
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
Note: The nominal group/ can be regarded as having a logical structure consisting of a: Head only. A
prepositional group resembles a clause in having no logical structure; sec C. lntctpersonal, below.
B. Experiential
a) Clauses
System network
l
material
il TRANSITIVITY
~ (+ Process; equative
0 +Medium) [
imperfective (Process:
'stay')
perfective
l
end;+
Attribute)
rclational extent
intensive location
~ manner
relational ____., cin;;wn.. --+- cause
(Process: [ stantial accompaniment (Process:
relation) matter 'be with')
possessive
existential
In addition, the Process may be accompanied by a Modulation, realized through (by pre-selection in) verbal
group, adverbial group, or both:
willing (active) (adv. gp.: 'willingly')
MMODULATION
- [
other
(passiv
(vbl. gp.: 'probable')
permitted
[ neutral
-+ oblique (hypothetical) (vbl. gp.: oblique)
68
TEXT AS SEMANTIC CHOICE IN SOCIAL CONTEXTS
b) Verbal groups
System network
PROCES~ taxonomies of processes (Thing: lexical verb), inclu-
TYPE ding: process: action: verbal: statement (:say, state,
announce, etc.) process: relation: ascriptive:
imperfective(: live, keep, stay, etc.)
verbal
group
(+Thing:
I past (Tense: past)
69
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
c) Nominal groups
System network
PART!- taxonomies of participants (Thing: lexical now,),
CIPANT-+ incl.: human/ social role: sex (:inamoratus)
TYPE object I artefact: tool (:shear..)
---+ associated taxonomies, including:
common
(Thing:
common) quantity:
l NUMfil!4[cow,t
mass
-+[one
more
1
w,measured
MEASURE•[ collecti.ve/
measured partitive/
nominal (+ Num- quantltative
group erative
(+Thing) = Head) descriptive:
bifurcation
_Jself (Thing: I) (Numerative:
proper pair of)
(Thing: , lnon-self (other persons/ objects/ places/
proper) institutions)
Nominal group/
SYSTEMIC nominal group: proper: self
DESCRIPTION
ISTRUCTURAL
DESCRIPTION
Thing: self
70
TEXT AS SEMANTIC CHOICE IN SOCIAL CONTEXTS
d) Adverbial group
System network
extent
TYPE
OF
STANCE
--+
CIRCUM-
t location
manner
(Thing: manner)
cause
accompaniment
means
- + Lquality: modulation: 'willingly'
(Thing: soon)
similarity
adverbial matter
group
(+Thing:
circum-
stance) equal (Comparative: as)
[
compared -
COMPAR:...._. [ (+ Comparative) unequal
ISON uncompared
e) Prepositional group
System network
extent
location
TYPE OF ~ manner
__...., cause
PROCESS accompaniment (Process: with)
preposi- matter
tional
group
(+ Process:
circum-
stantial; - , . (to 'nominal group' network
+Medium) (Medium: nominal group))
71
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
C. Interpersonal
a) Information unit
System network
l~m,
mild
TONIC _[neutral
[ strong (tone I+)
information neutral
Unit PRE- - - o , [
TONIC insistent (pre-tone- I)
marked
(tone 2, 3, 4, or 5)
Information unit / would as soon live with a pair of unoi/ed garden shears said her lnamoratus
b) Clauses
System network
---. [declarative (Subject· Finite)
Interrogative
indicative {
(Mood: Subject ---+ [attitudinal(+ Attitude= Modulation)
clause MOOD--+[ + Finite)
(+Modal;+ non-attitudinal
Proposition imperative
= Predicator ( ... ))
Note: A clause having the features 'quoting/ given' does not enter into the mood system. It is structured as
Predicator· Subject (optionally Subject· Predicator if Subject is personal pronoun); the Predicator is
a 'quoting' verb, finite, in simple past or present tense.
72
TEXT AS SEMANTIC CHOICE IN SOCIAL CONTEXTS
c) Verbal groups
System network
[fmite(+ Finite)
verbal - [possible
group
modalmcl
other -
certain -
[virtually
(Finite [neutral absolutely
MODALITY • [ =Modal) -
oblique (Modal: would/could/should/
might)
non-modalmcl
(Finite= Tense)
Nominal group/
SYSTEMIC
DESCRIPTION
nominal group: speech role: speaker I.
'
STRUCTURAL Person: speaker
DESCRIPTION
I
.
'
73
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
D.Textual
a) Information unit
System network
clause com-
unmarked
(B-New·
~lause
B
r[ unmarked (Given· New:
culminative focus)
Information unit B: / would as soon live with a pair of unoiled garden shears
A: said her inamoratus
b)Clauses
System network
extended
(modulation/time/modality etc.)
[
unmarked ----+ (Theme=(Theme • Extension))
- [ (Theme=Subject)
unextended
clause eclarativ marked
(+Theme,
+ • Rheme)
single theme
[
- multiple theme
74
TEXT AS SEMANTIC CHOICE IN SOCIAL CONTEXTS
specific-
[
1-[ personal-+
who, etc.)
[possessive(mylherlwhose,
etc.)
demonstrative (this, etc.)
selective
referential (//my I this, etc.)
[
--+ interrogative (who I whose, etc.)
DEIXIS, singular (a I one)
(+ Deic· [
tic) { - - non-singular (some I some)
partial
unmarked (a I some)
[
nominal restricted---+ -+[
group marked (one I some)
non-specific - [ total (each I every I all I both)
unrestricted (any I either I no I neither)
Nominal group /
SYSTEMIC nominal group: ((specific: selective: ((personal:
DESCRIPTION non- ssessive / referential / ronoun
!STRUCTURAL
DESCRIPTION
Deictic = Head: pronoun
75
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
a) Clause complex "/ would as soon live with a pair said her inamoratus
of unoiled garden shears"
Experiential:
transitivity;
Participant ! Process
I
Participant
76
TEXT AS SEMANTIC CHOICE IN SOCIAL CONTEXTS
d) Nominal group I
Logical: modiftcation u: Head
Experiential: taxonomy of participants ing:self
Interpersonal: person Pcnon: speaker
Textual: deixis Deictic: oun
77
B. Generalized structure showing all ranks
LXGR: 1111 I would I as soon I live I with [a pair ofunoiledgarden shears] II said I her inamoratus Ill
<did say>
PHON: II - I +, I would as I soon I live with a pair of/ unoi/ed I garden Is he a rs said her i I namo I ratus II
Clause:
<combined>
II Subject I Predicator IAdjunct (I} I !Adjunct (2) II Predicator ISubject
+
n.gp.
I
V.
would
gp.
live
adv. gp.
as soon
I prep. gp.
with
n.gp.
[a pair of
V.gp.
'did say'
n. gp.
her inamoratus
unoiled garden
shears]
Group: LOO
(word
Ia: Head IP: Modifier
+-a: Head
IP +-a I [P+-(aa-+aP
(a13Y+-aPP+-aPn))]
IP +-a
Ip
+-(l
comp!.)
Group: EXP. Thing Tense Thing Comp. Thing I Process Medium Tense Thing Thing
[Numerative
Epithet Classifier
Thing]
Group: INT. IPerson IFinite Predic., [Connotation
Person]
IFinite Predic. Connotation=
Person
I
Group:
TEXT.
IDeictic [Deictic] IDeictic
TEXT AS SEMANTIC CHOICE IN SOCIAL CONTEXTS
79
LINGUISTIC ANALYSIS AND TEXTUAL MEANING
80
TEXT AS SEMANTIC CHOICE IN SOCIAL CONTEXTS
8. But it was spring, and the lover and his lass were young, and they
were oblivious of the scornful comments of their sharp-tongued
neighbors, and they continued to bump each other around in the
water, happily pushing and pulling, backing and filling, and snort-
ing and snaffling.
9. The tender things they said to each other during the monolithic
give-and-take of their courtship sounded as lyric to them as flowers
in bud or green things opening.
10. To the Grays, however, the bumbling romp of the lover and his
lass was hard to comprehend and even harder to tolerate, and for a
time they thought of calling the A.B.I., or African Bureau of
Investigation, on the ground that monolithic lovemaking by enor-
mous creatures who should have become decent fossils long ago
was probably a threat to the security of the jungle.
11. But they decided instead to phone their friends and neighbors and
gossip about the shameless pair, and describe them in mocking and
monstrous metaphors involving skidding buses on icy streets and
overturned moving vans.
12. Late that evening, the hippopotamus and the hippopotami were
surprised and shocked to hear the Grays exchanging terms of
endearment.
13. "Listen to those squawks," wuffied the male hippopotamus.
14. "What in the world can they see in each other?" gurbled the
female hippopotamus.
15. "I would as soon live with a pair of unoiled garden shears," said
her inamoratus.
16. They called up their friends and neighbors and discussed the
incredible fact that a male gray parrot and a female gray parrot
could possibly have any sex appeal.
17. It was long after midnight before the hippopotamuses stopped
criticizing the Grays and fell asleep, and the Grays stopped malign-
ing the hippopotamuses and retired to their beds.
18. Moral: Laugh and the world laughs with you, love and you love alone.
81
PART TWO
85
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
of the processes of the external world, and of the internal world of his
own consciousness, together with the participants in these processes
and their attendant circumstances; and it embodies a very basic distinc-
tion of processes into two types, those that are regarded as due to an
external cause, an agency other than the person or object involved, and
those that are not . . . Transitivity is really the cornerstone of the
semantic organization of experience; and it is at one level what The
Inheritors is about.'
J. B. Priestley is described by Halliday in 'The de-automatization of
grammar: from Priestley's An Inspector Calls' (1982) as 'at once a
novelist, dramatist, essayist, autobiographer, critic, and social historian.'
The predominant themes in his plays include interdependence and
social responsibility: 'no man is an island'; and time: 'time is not ticking
our lives away'. Halliday acknowledges the much criticized 'flat and
naturalistic' language spoken by Priestley's characters, and the declarat-
ive manner in which the themes are sometimes enunciated. However,
in An Inspector Calls, the time theme is 'a motif that permeates the
interaction of the participants, and is more or less covertly woven in to
the dialogue.' Once the dramatist stops 'defining the nature of the
world' and starts 'implying his world rather than presenting it', the
language is likely to become 'de-automatized'. Halliday prefers Muka-
fovsky's term 'de-automatization' to 'foregrounding' 'since what is in
question is not simply prominence but rather the partial freeing of the
lower-level systems from the control of the semantics so that they
become domains of choice in their own right.' To 'de-automatize'
means to 'try to interpret the grammar in terms that go beyond its
direct realizational function'; 'to focus out the background, and let the
words and structures speak for themselves'. In An Inspector Calls the
complex interrelationship of themes related .to obligation, personality
and time are built from semantic systems ohime, mood and polarity.
The last two papers in this part, 'Poetry as scientific discourse: the
nuclear sections of Tennyson's In Memoriam' (1987) and 'The construc-
tion of knowledge and value in the grammar of scientific discourse:
with reference to Charles Darwin's The Origin ef Species' (1990) explore
works that share a common birthright, both being 'constituted out of
the impact between scientific and poetic forces of meaning.' While
Tennyson's In Memoriam is science in poetry, Darwin's The Origin of
Species is poetry in science. Halliday employs the same 'grammatics' to
interpret and understand both. Halliday looks at the central passage in
Tennyson's In Memoriam in terms of its logical structure, n:iood, and
transitivity patterns, revealing a rhetorical and logical dynamic that
86
EDITOR'S INTRODUCTION
87
Chapter Three
First published in Literary Style: A Symposium, edited by Seymour Chatman .. London and
New York: Oxford University Press, 1971, pp. 330-68.
88
LINGUISTIC FUNCTION AND LITERARY STYLE: WILLIAM GOLDING
89
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
into the nature and use of language, but particularly so in the context
of stylistic studies.
The demands that we make on language, as speakers and writers,
listeners and readers, are indefinitely many and varied. They can be
derived, ultimately, from a small number of very general headings; but
what these headings are will depend on what questions we are asking.
For example, if we were to take a broadly psychological viewpoint and
consider the functions that language serves in the life of the individual,
we might arrive at some such scheme as Biihler's, referred to above. If
on the other hand we asked a more sociological type of question,
concerning the functions that language serves in the life of the
community, we should probably elaborate some framework such as
Malinowski's (1935) distinction into a pragmatic and a magical func-
tion. Many others could be suggested besides.
These questions are extrinsic to language; and the categorizations of
language function that depend on them are of interest because, and to
the extent that, the questions themselves are of interest. Such categori-
zations therefore imply a strictly instrumental view oflinguistic theory.
Some would perhaps reject this on the grounds that it does not admit
the autonomy of linguistics and linguistic investigations. I am not
myself impressed by that argument, although I would stress that any
one particular instrumental view is by itself inadequate as a general
characterization of language. But a purely extrinsic theory of language
functions does fail to take into account one thing, namely the fact that
the multiplicity of function, if the idea is valid at all, is likely to be
reflected somewhere in the internal organization of language itsel£ If
language is, as it were, programmed to serve a variety of needs, then
this should show up in some way in an investigation of linguistic
structure.
In fact this functional plurality is very clearly built into the structure
of language, and forms the basis of its semantic and "syntactic" (i.e.
grammatical and lexical) organization. If we set up a functional frame-
work that is neutral as to external emphasis, but designed to take into
account the nature of the internal semantic and syntactic patterns of
language, we arrive at something that is very suggestive for literary
studies, because it represents a general characterization of semantic
functions - of the meaning potential of the language system. Let me
suggest here the framework that seems to me most helpful. It is a rather
simple catalogue of three basic functions, one of which has two sub-
headings.
In the first place, language serves for the expression of content: it
90
LINGUISTIC FUNCTION AND LITERARY STYLE: WILLIAM GOLDING
91
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
92
LINGUISTIC FUNCTION AND LITERARY STYLE: WILLIAM GOLDING
93
Table 1 Functions and ranks in the grammar of Modern English
MODIFICATION
r:i -I; %8 ATTITUDE DEIXIS
"' -g a. fl3:
epithet function Sil
classification la· 5· attitudinal modifiersdeterminers
enumeration ~ sub-modification ;;· ~- \:1 intensifiers "phoric" elements
C
Nominal GROUP ( noun classes)
"' " g !:l (qualifiers)
l(adiective classes) ·"'0--------- ~ 1-----------t'(~1d_efi_m_i_te_artt_·_c_le.,_)_ _ _---;
!3
"MINOR PROCESSES" !;; narrowing ,.,.~ COMMENT CONJUNCTION
Adverbial prepositional relations ~ sub-modification ~ (classes of comment (classes of discourse
(incl. prepositional) (classes of circum- 0 adjunct) adjunct)
GROUP stantial adjunct) :E
~--------
WORD LEXICAL "CONTENT" compounding LEXICAL "REGISTER" COLLOCATION
(incl. lexical (taxonomic organi- (expressive words) (collocational
item) zation of vocab- derivation (stylistic organization organization of
ulary) ofvocabularv\ vocabulary)
95
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
nature of experience, in which things that happened the week after next
turns out to be an acceptable sentence. (I am not suggesting it is
serious, or offering a deep literary interpretation; I am merely using it
to illustrate the nature of language.) On the interpersonal level the
language expresses, through a pattern of question (or exclamation) and
response, a basic relationship of seeker and guide, in interplay with
various other paired functions such as yours and mine, for and against,
child and adult, wonderment and judgement. The texture is that of
dialogue in narrative, within which the Queen's complex thematic
structures (e.g. there's one great advantage to it, that . .. ) contrast with the
much simpler (i.e. linguistically unmarked) message patterns used by
Alice.
A functional theory of language is a theory about meanings, not
about words or constructions; we shall not attempt to assign a word or
a construction directly to one function or another. Where then do we
find the functions differentiated in language? They are differentiated
semantically, as different areas of what I called the "meaning potential".
Language is itself a potential: it is the totality of what the speaker can
do. (By "speaker" I mean always the language user, whether as speaker,
listener, writer, or reader: homo grammaticus, in fact.) We are consider-
ing, as it were, the dynamics of the semantic strategies that are available
to him. If we represent the language system in this way, as networks of
interrelated options which define, as a whole, the resources for what
the speaker wants to say, we find empirically that these options fall into
a small number of fairly distinct sets. In the last resort, every option in
language is related to every other; there are no completely independent
choices. But the total network of meaning potential is actually com-
posed of a number of smaller networks, each one highly complex in
itself but related to the others in a way that is relatively simple: rather
like an elaborate piece of circuitry made up of two or three complex
blocks of wiring with fairly simple interconnections. Each of these
blocks corresponds to one of the functions of language.
In Table 1, where the columns represent our linguistic functions,
each column is one "block" of options. These blocks are to be thought
of as wired "in parallel". That is to say, the speaker does not first think
of the content of what he wants to say and then go on to decide what
kind of a message it is and where he himself comes into it - whether it
will be statement or question, what modalities are involved and the
like. 3 All these functions, the ideational, the interpersonal and the
textual, are simultaneously embodied in his planning procedures. (If we
pursue the metaphor, it is the rows of the table that are w1red "in
96
LINGUISTIC FUNCTION AND LITERARY STYLE: WILLIAM GOLDING
97
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
98
LINGUISTIC FUNCTION AND LITERARY STYLE: WILLIAM GOLDING
99
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
100
LINGUISTIC FUNCTION AND LITERARY STYLE: WILLIAM GOLDING
C C
-~
0
ACTION .,
:! g
cl .!3 0 ::I -
·g
B ~ !
J §. e.
intransitive transitive 0
!'.l .&J
A Process:
movement other movement other ~
C. .
·13
0 F,
human {people 9 12 24
tribe 2 I 4
part of body 2 1 3 2 8
inanimate 4 12 3 20
17 3 14 16 s S6
B(i)
!people 4 3• 2 11
human tribe 5 I 2 9
part of bod
inanimate 13 2 5 2 23
22 4 4 9 2
43
B (ii)
h {people 13 2 2 4 22
uman tribe
part of body 3 I 2 6
inanimate 3 I 2 4 6 2 19
19 3 2 2 7 4 8 2 47
C
I 1 2 4 8
human ~=le 3 2 s II 3 II 3 2 40
part of bod 2 I 5 8
inanimate 2 I 3 4 II
8 4 6 13 6 IS 12 3 67
• including two passives, which are also negative and in which the actor is not explicit: The tree
would not be cajoled or persuaded.
101
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
102
LINGUISTIC FUNCTION AND LITERARY STYLE: WILLIAM GOLDING
selected by the writer and their relation to the total meaning of the
work. If in the selections he has made there is an unexpected pattern of
frequency distributions, and this turns out to be motivated, it seems
pointless to argue that such a phenomenon could not possibly be
significant.
What cannot be expressed statistically is foregrounding: figures do
not tell us whether a particular pattern has or has not "value in the
game". 14 For this we need to know the rules. A distinctive frequency
distribution is in itself no guarantee of stylistic relevance, as can be seen
from authorship studies, where the diagnostic features are often, from a
literary stand point, very trivial ones. 15 Conversely, a linguistic feature
that is stylistically very relevant may display a much less striking
frequency pattern. But there is likely to be some quantitative turbu-
lence, if a particular feature is felt to be prominent; and a few figures
may be very suggestive. Counting, as Miller remarked, has many
positive virtues. Ullmann offers a balanced view of these when he
writes "Yet even those who feel that detailed statistics are both
unnecessary and unreliable [in a sphere where quality and context,
aesthetic effects and suggestive overtones are of supreme importance]
would probably agree that a rough indication of frequencies would
often be helpful" (1965, p. 22). A rough indication of frequencies is
often just what is needed: enough to suggest why we should accept the
analyst's assertion that some feature is prominent in the text, and to
allow us to check his statements. The figures, obviously, do not alone
constitute an analysis, interpretation, or evaluation of the style.
But this is not, be it noted, a limitation on quantitative patterns as
such; it is a limitation on the significance of prominence of any kind.
Deviation is no more fundamental a phenomenon than statistical
deflection: in fact there is no very clear line between the two, and in
any given instance the most qualitatively deviant items may be among
the least relevant. Thus if style cannot be reduced to counting, this is
because it cannot be reduced to a simple question of prominence. An
adequate characterization of an author's style is much more than an
inventory of linguistic highlights. This is why linguists were so often
reluctant to take up questions of criticism and evaluation, and tended
to disclaim any contribution to the appraisal of what they were
describing: they were very aware that statements about linguistic
prominence by themselves offer no criterion of literary value. Never-
theless some values, or some aspects of value, must be expressed in
linguistic terms. This is true, for example, of metrical patterns, which
linguists have always considered their proper concern. The question is
103
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
how far it is also true of patterns that are more directly related to
meaning: what factors govern the relevance of "effects" in grammar
and vocabulary? The significance of rhythmic regularity has to be
formulated linguistically, since it is a phonological phenomenon,
although the ultimate value to which it relates is not "given" by the
language - that the sonnet is a highly valued pattern is not a linguistic
fact, but the sonnet itself is (Levin 1971). The sonnet form defines the
relevance of certain types of phonological pattern. There may likewise
be some linguistic factor involved in determining whether a syntactic
or a lexical pattern is stylistically relevant or not.
Certainly there is no magic in unexpectedness; and one line of
approach has been to attempt to state conditions under which the
unexpected is not relevant - namely when it is not really unexpected.
Prominence, in this view, is not significant if the linguistically unpre-
dicted configuration is predictable on other grounds; specifically, by
reference to subject-matter, the implication being that it would have
been predicted if we had known beforehand what the passage was
about. So, for example, Ullmann warns of the danger in the search for
statistically defined key-words: "One must carefully avoid what have
been called contextual words whose frequency is due to the subject-
matter rather than to any deep-seated stylistic or psychological tend-
ency" (1965, p. 27). Ullmann's concern here is with words that serve
as indices of a particular author, and he goes on to discuss the
significance of recurrent imagery for style and personality, citing as an
example the prominence of insect vocabulary in the writings of Sartre
(Ullmann 1965, p. 29; see also 1964, pp. 186-8); in this context we
can see that, by contrast, the prevalence of such words in a treatise on
entomology would be irrelevant. But it is less easy to see how this can
be generalized, even in the realm of vocabulary; is lexical foregrounding
entirely dependent on imagery?
Can we in fact dismiss, as irrelevant, prominence that is due to
subject-matter? Can we even claim to identify it? This was the third
and final question I asked earlier, and it is one which relates very
closely to an interpretation of the style of The Inheritors. In The Inheritors,
the features that come to our attention are largely syntactic, and we are
in the realm of syntactic imagery, where the syntax, in Ohmann's
words, "serves [a] vision of things .... since there are innumerable
kinds of deviance, we should expect that the ones elected by a poem
or poet spring from particular semantic impulses, particular ways of
looking at experience" (1967, p. 237). Ohmann is concern~d primarily
with "syntactic irregularities", but syntax need not be deviant in order
104
LINGUISTIC FUNCTION AND LITERARY STYLE: WILLIAM GOLDING
105
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
expression, one that is congruent with the total meaning, and so the
verb forms that are characteristically associated with it are motivated at
every level. Similarly, it is not irrelevant to the style of an entomolog-
ical monograph (although we may not be very interested in its style)
that it contains a lot of words for insects, if in fact it does. In stylistics
we are concerned with language in relation to all the various levels of
meaning that a work may have.
But while a given instance of syntactic or lexical prominence may be
said to be "motivated" either by the subject-matter or by some other
level of the meaning, in the sense of deriving its relevance therefrom, it
cannot really be said to be "due to" it. Neither thesis nor theme imposes
linguistic patterns. They may set up local expectancies, but these are by
no means always fulfilled; there might actually be very few insect words
in the work on entomology - and there are very few in Kafka. 16 There
is always choice. In The Inheritors, Golding is offering a "particular way
of looking at experience", a vision of things which he ascribes to
Neanderthal man; and he conveys this by syntactic prominence, by the
frequency with which he selects certain key syntactic options. It is their
frequency which establishes the clause types in question as prominent;
but, as Ullmann has remarked, in stylistics we have both to count things
and to look at them, one by one, and when we do this we find that
the foregrounding effect is the product of two apparently opposed
conditions of use. The foregrounded elements are certain clause types
which display particular patterns of transitivity, as described in the next
section; and in some instances the syntactic pattern is "expected" in that
it is the typical form of expression for the subject-matter - for the
process, participants, and circumstances that make up the thesis of the
clause. Elsewhere, however, the same syntactic elements are found
precisely where they would not be expected, there being other, more
likely ways of "saying the same thing".
Here we might be inclined to talk of semantic choice and syntactic
choice: what the author chooses to say, and how he chooses to say it.
But this is a misleading distinction; not only because it is unrealistic in
application (most distinctions in language leave indeterminate instances,
although here there would be suspiciously many) but more because the
combined effect is cumulative: the one does not weaken or cut across
the other but reinforces it. We have to do here with an interaction,
not of meaning and form, but of two levels of meaning, both of which
find expression in form, and through the same syntactic features. The
immediate thesis and the underlying theme come toget~er in the
syntax; the choice of subject-matter is motivated by the deeper mean-
106
LINGUISTIC FUNCTION AND LITERARY STYLE: WILLIAM GOLDING
ing, and the transitivity patterns realize both. This is the explanation of
their powerful impact.
The foregrounding of certain patterns in syntax as the expression of
an underlying theme is what we understand by "syntactic imagery",
and we assume that its effect will be striking. But in The Inheritors these
same syntactic patterns also figure prominently in their "literal" sense,
as the expression of subject-matter; and their prominence here is doubly
relevant, since the literal use not only is motivated in itself but also
provides a context for the metaphorical - we accept the syntactic vision
of things more readily because we can see that it coincides with, and is
an extension of, the reality. The Inheritors provides a remarkable
illustration of how grammar can convey levels of meaning in literature;
and this relates closely to the notion of linguistic functions which I
discussed at the beginning. The foregrounded patterns, in this instance,
are ideational ones, whose meaning resides in the representation of
experience; as such they express not only the content of the narrative
but also the abstract structure of the reality through which that content
is interpreted. Sometimes the interpretation matches our own, and at
other times, as in the drawing of the bow in Passage A below, it
conflicts with it; these are the "opposed conditions of use" referred to
earlier. Yet each tells a part of the story. Language, because of the
multiplicity of its functions, has a fugue-like quality in which a number
of themes unfold simultaneously; each of these themes is apprehended
in various settings, or perspectives, and each melodic line in the
syntactic sequence has more than one value in the whole.
107
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
108
LINGUISTIC FUNCTION AND LITERARY STYLE: WILLIAM GOLDING
specify location, e.g. the man turned sideways in the bushes, he rushed to
the edge of the water; and on the other hand, in addition to what is
clearly movement, as in a stick rose upright, and what is clearly location,
as in there were hooks in the bone, there is an intermediate type
exemplified by [the bushes] waded out, where the verb is of the
movement type but the subject is immobile.
The picture is one in which people act, but they do not act on
things; they move, but they move only themselves, not other objects.
Even such normally transitive verbs as grab occur intransitively: he
grabbed at the branches is just another clause of movement (c( he smelled
along the shaft of the twig). Moreover a high proportion [exactly half] of
the subjects are not people; they are either parts of the body [8] or
inanimate objects [20], and of the human subjects half again [14] are
found in clauses which are not clauses of action. Even among the four
transitive action clauses, cited above, one has an inanimate subject and
one is reflexive. There is a stress set up, a kind of syntactic counter-
point, between verbs of movement in their most active and dynamic
form, that of finite verb in independent clause, 20 in the simple past
tense characteristic of the direct narrative of events in a time sequence,
on the one hand, and on the other hand the preference for non-human
subjects and the almost total absence of transitive clauses. It is particu-
larly the lack of transitive clauses of action with human subjects (there
are only two clauses in which a person acts on an external object) that
creates an atmosphere of ineffectual activity: the scene is one of constant
movement, but movement which is as much inanimate as human and
in which only the mover is affected - nothing else changes. The
syntactic tension expresses this combination of ae:tivity and helplessness.
No doubt this is a fair summary of the life of Neanderthal man. But
Passage A is not a description of the people. The section from which it
is taken is one in which Lok is observing and, to a certain extent,
interacting with the tribe; they have captured one of the people, and it
is for the most part their doings that are being described. And the tribe
are not helpless. The transitivity patterns are not imposed by the
subject-matter; they are the reflection of the underlying theme, or
rather of one of the underlying themes - the inherent limitations of
understanding, whether cultural or biological, of Lok and his people,
and their consequent inability to survive when confronted with beings
at a higher stage of development. In terms of the processes and events
as we would interpret them, and encode them in our grammar, there
is no immediate justification for the predominance of intransitives; this
is the result of their being expressed through the medium of the
109
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
110
LINGUISTIC FUNCTION AND LITERARY STYLE: WILLIAM GOLDING
that they are part of a general syntactic and semantic scheme. That this
passage is representative in its transitivity patterns can be seen from
comparison with other extracts. 21 It also exemplifies certain other
relevant features of the language of this part of the book. We have seen
that there is a strong preference for processes having only one partici-
pant: in general there is only one nominal element in the structure of
the clause, which is therefore the Subject. But while there are very few
Complements, 22 there is an abundance of Adjuncts [44]; and most of
these [40] have some spatial reference. Specifically, they are (a) static
[25], of which most [21] are place adjuncts consisting of preposition
plus noun, the noun being either an inanimate object of the immediate
natural environment (e.g. bush) or a part of the body, the remainder [4]
being localizers (at their farthest, at the end, etc.); and (b) dynamic [15],
of which the majority [10] are of direction or non-terminal motion
(sideways, [rose] upright, at the branches, towards the island, etc.) and the
remainder [5] perception, or at least circumstantial to some process that
is not a physical one (e.g. [looked at Lok] along his shoulder, [shouted] at
the green drifts). Thus with the dynamic type, either the movement is
purely perceptual or, if physical, it never reaches a goal: the nearest
thing to terminal motion is he rushed to the edge ef the water (which is
followed by and came back!).
The restriction to a single participant also applies to mental process
clauses [16]. This category includes perception, cognition, and reaction,
as well as the rather distinct sub-category of verbalization; and such
clauses in English typically contain a Phenomenon, that which is seen,
understood, liked, etc. Here, however, the Phenomenon is often [8]
either not expressed at all (e.g. [Lok] gazed) or expressed indirectly
through a preposition, as in he smelled along the shaft ef the twig; and
sometimes [3] the subject is not a human being but a sense organ (his
nose examined this stuff and did not like it). There is the same reluctance
to envisage the "whole man" (as distinct from a part of his body)
participating in a process in which other entities are involved.
There is very little modification of nouns [10, out of about 100]; and
all modifiers are non-defining (e.g. green drifts, glittering water) except
where [2] the Modifier is the only semantically significant element in
the nominal, the Head noun being a mere carrier demanded by the rules
of English grammar (white bone things, sticky brown stuff). In terms of the
immediate situation, things have defining attributes only if these attributes
are their sole properties; at the more abstract level, in Lok's understanding
the complex taxonomic ordering of natural phenomena that is implied
by the use of defining modifiers is lacking, or is only rudimentary.
111
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
112
LINGUISTIC FUNCTION AND LITERARY STYLE: WILLIAM GOLDING
113
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
114
LINGUISTIC FUNCTION AND LITERARY STYLE: WILLIAM GOLDING
giant. There are abstract nominals: demoniac activity, its weight of branches.
And there are perhaps more modifiers and complex verb forms than
usual. None of these features is occurring for the first time; we have
had forward-looking flashes throughout, e.g. (p. 191) He had a picture of
Liku looking up with soft and adoring eyes at Tanakil, guessed how Ha had
gone with a kind of eager fearfulness to meet his sudden death and
(pp. 212-13) "Why did you not snatch the new one?" and "We will take
Tanakil. Then they will give back the new one", both spoken by the more
intelligent Fa (when transitive action clauses do occur in Language A,
they are often in the dialogue). But there is a greater concentration of
them in B (i), a linguistic complexity that is also in harmony with the
increased complexity of the events, which has been being built up ever
since the tribe first impinged on the people with the mysterious
disappearance of Ha (p. 65). The syntax expresses the climax of the
gradual overwhelming of Lok's understanding by new things and
events; and this coincides with the climax in the events themselves as,
with the remainder of the people all killed or captured, Lok's last
companion, Fa, is carried over the edge of the waterfall. Lok is alone;
there are no more people, and the last trace of his humanity, his
membership of a society, has gone. In that moment he belongs to the
past.
Lok does not speak again, because there is no one to speak to. But
for a while we follow him, as the tribe might have followed him,
although they did not - or rather we follow it; there can be no him
where there is no you and me. The language is now Language C, and
the story is that of homo sapiens; but for a few paragraphs, beginning at
B (ii), as we remain with Lok, the syntax harks back to the world of
the people, just as in B (i) it was beginning to look forward. The
transition has taken place; it was a strange creature, smallish, and bowed
that we had come to know so well. But it is still the final, darkening
traces of this creature's world that we are seeing, fleetingly as if in an
escaping dream.
A brief sketch of B (ii): There are very few transitive clauses of
action [4]; in only one of these is Lok the agent - and here the
"affected" entity is a part of his own body: it put up a hand. The others
have the water and the river as agent. Yet nearly half [22] the total
number of clauses [47] have Lok as subject; here, apart from a few [4]
mental process clauses, the verb is again one of simple movement or
posture, and intransitive (turn, move, crouch, etc.; but including for the
first time some with a connotation of attitude like sidle and trot; c£
broke into a queer, loping run). The remaining subjects are inanimate
115
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
objects [19] and parts of the body [6]. But there are differences in these
subjects. The horizons have widened; in addition to water and river we
now have sun and green sky - a reminder that the new people walk
upright; c£ (p. 143) they did not look at the earth but straight ahead; and
there are now also human evidences and artifacts: path, rollers, ropes.
And the parts of the body no longer see or feel; they are subjects only
of intransitive verbs of movement (e.g. its long arms swinging), and
mainly in non-finite clauses, expressing the dependent nature of the
processes in which they participate. A majority [32] of the finite verbs
are still in simple past tense; but there is more variation in the
remainder, as well as more non-finite verbs [8], reflecting a slightly
increased proportion of dependent clauses that is also a characteristic of
Language C. And while in many clauses [21) we still find spatial
adjuncts, these tend to be more varied and more complex (e.g. down
the rocks beyond the terracefrom the melting ice in the mountains).
This is the world of the tribe; but it is still inhabited, for a brief
moment of time, by Lok. Once again the theme is enunciated by the
syntax. Nature is no longer totally impenetrable; yet Lok remains
powerless, master of nothing but his own body. In Passages A and B
taken together, there are more than 50 clauses in which the subject is
Lok; but only one of these has Lok as an agent acting on something
external to himself, one that has already been mentioned: Lok picked up
Tanakil. There is a double irony here. Of all the positive actions on his
environment that Lok might have taken, the one he does take is the
utterly improbable one of capturing a girl of the tribe - improbable in
the event, at the level of subject-matter (let us call this "level one"),
and improbable also in the deeper context ("level two"), since Lok's
newly awakened power manifests itself as power over the one element
in the environment that is "superior" to himself. It is at a still deeper
"level three" that the meaning becomes clear. The action gets him
nowhere; but it is a syntactic hint that his people have played their part'
in the long trek towards the human condition.
By the time we reach Passage C, the transition is complete. Here,
for the first time, the majority of the clauses [48 out of 67] have a
human subject; of these, more than half [25] are clauses of action, and
most of these [19] are transitive. Leaving aside two in which the thing
'affected' is a part of the body, there is still a significant increase in the
number of instances [17, contrasting with 5 in the whole of A and B
together] in which a human agent is acting on an external object. The
world of the inheritors is organized as ours is; or at least ~n a way that
we can recognize. Among these are two clauses in which the subject is
116
LINGUISTIC FUNCTION AND LITERARY STYLE: WILLIAM GOLDING
they, referring to the people ("the devils": e.g. they have given me back a
changeling); in the tribe's scheme of things, the people are by no means
powerless. There is a parallel here with the earlier part. In Passage A
the actions of the tribe are encoded in terms of the world-view of the
people, so that the predominance of intransitive clauses is interpreted
at what we called "level two", although there is a partial reflection of
"level one" in the fact that they are marginally less predominant when
the subject-matter concerns the tribe. Similarly, in Passage C references
to the people are encoded in terms of the world-view of the tribe, and
transitive structures predominate; yet the only member of the people
who is present - the only one to survive - is the captured baby, whose
infant behaviour is described in largely intransitive terms (pp. 230-31).
And the references to the people, in the dialogue, include such
formulations as "They cannot follow us, I tell you. They cannot pass over
water", which is a "level-one" reassurance that, in a "level-two" world
of cause and effect whose causes are often unseen and unknown, there
are at least limits to the devils' power.
We can now see the full complementarity between the two
"languages", but it is not easy to state. In Language A there is a level-
two theme, that of powerlessness. The momentary hints of potency
that we are given at level one represent an antithetic variation which,
however, has a significance at level three: the power is ascribed to the
tribe but signifies Lok's own incipient awareness, the people's nascent
understanding of the human potential. This has become a level-two
theme in Language C; and in like fashion the level-two theme of
Language A becomes in Language C a level-one variation, but again
with a level-three significance. The people may be powerless, but the
tribe's demand for explanations of things, born of their own more
advanced state, leads them, while still fearfully insisting on the people's
weakness in action, to ascribe to them supernatural powers.
While there are still inanimate subjects in the clause [11], as there
always are in English, there is no single instance in Passage C of an
inanimate agent. In A and B we had the echo ef Liku 's voice in his head
sent him trembling . . ., the branches took her, the water had scooped a bowl
out ef the rock; in C we have only the sail glowed, the sun was sitting in
it, the hills grow less. Likewise all clauses with parts of the body as sub-
ject [8] are now intransitive, and none of them is a clause of mental
process. Parts of the body no longer feel or perceive; they have
attributes ascribed to them (e.g. his teeth were wolf's teeth) or they move
(the lips parted, the mouth was opening and shutting). The limbs may
move and posture, but only the whole man perceives and reacts to his
117
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
118
LINGUISTIC FUNCTION AND LITERARY STYLE: WILLIAM GOLDING
119
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
120
LINGUISTIC FUNCTION AND LITERARY STYLE: WILLIAM GOLDING
mentioned here that stand out in the same way. Every work achieves a
unique balance among the types and components of meaning, and
embodies the writer's individual exploration of the functional diversity
of language.
APPENDIX
Extracts from The Inheritors
A. (pp. 106-7)
The bushes twitched again. Lok steadied by the tree and gazed. A head
and a chest faced him, half-hidden. There were white bone things
behind the leaves and hair. The man had white bone things above his
eyes and under the mouth so that his face was longer than a face should
be. The man turned sideways in the bushes and looked at Lok along
his shoulder. A stick rose upright and there was a lump of bone in the
middle. Lok peered at the stick and the lump of bone and the small
eyes in the bone things over the face. Suddenly Lok understood that
the man was holding the stick out to him but neither he nor Lok could
reach across the river. He would have laughed if it were not for the
echo of the screaming in his head. The stick began to grow shorter at
both ends. Then it shot out to full length again.
The dead tree by Lok's ear acquired a voice.
"Clop!"
His ears twitched and he turned to the tree. By his face there had
grown a twig: a twig that smelt of other, and of goose, and of the
bitter berries that Lok' s stomach told him he must not eat. This twig
had a white bone at the end. There were hooks in the bone and sticky
brown stuff hung in the crooks. His nose examined this stuff and did
not like it. He smelled along the shaft of the twig. The leaves on the
twig were red feathers and reminded him of goose. He was lost in a
generalized astonishment and excitement. He shouted at the green
drifts across the glittering water and heard Liku crying out in answer
but could not catch the words. They were cut off suddenly as though
someone had clapped a hand over her mouth. He rushed to the edge
of the water and came back. On either side of the open bank the
bushes grew thickly in the flood; they waded out until at their farthest
some of the leaves were opening under water; and these bushes leaned
over.
The echo of Liku's voice in his head sent him trembling at this
perilous way of bushes towards the island. He dashed at them where
121
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
normally they would have been rooted on dry land and his feet
splashed. He threw himself forward and grabbed at the branches with
hands and feet. He shouted:
"I am coming!"
B. (pp. 215-17)
(i) Lok staggered to his feet, picked up Tanakil and ran after Fa along
the terrace. There came a screaming from the figures by the hollow
log and a loud bang from the jam. The tree began to move forward
and the logs were lumbering about like the legs of a giant. The
crumplefaced woman was struggling with Tuami on the rock by the
hollow log; she burst free and came running towards Lok. There was
movement everywhere, screaming, demoniac activity; the old man was
coming across the tumbling logs. He threw something at Fa. Hunters
were holding the hollow log against the terrace and the head of the
tree with all its weight of branches and wet leaves was drawing along
them. The fat woman was lying in the log, the crumpled woman was
in it with Tanakil, the old man was tumbling into the back. The
boughs crashed and drew along the rock with an agonized squealing.
Fa was sitting by the water holding her head. The branches took her.
She was moving with them out into the water and the hollow log was
free of the rock and drawing away. The tree swung into the current
with Fa sitting limply among the branches. Lok began to gibber again.
He ran up and down on the terrace. The tree would not be cajoled or
persuaded. It moved to the edge of the fall, it swung until it was lying
along the lip. The water reared up over the trunk, pushing, the roots
were over. The tree hung for a while with the head facing upstream.
Slowly the root end sank and the head rose. Then it slid forward
soundlessly and dropped over the fall.
(ii) The red creature stood on the edge of the terrace and did nothing.
The hollow log was a dark spot on the water towards the place where
the sun had gone down. The air in the gap was clear and blue and
calm. There was no noise at all now except for the fall, for there was
no wind and the green sky was clear. The red creature turned to the
right and trotted slowly towards the far end of the terrace. Water was
cascading down the rocks beyond the terrace from the melting ice in
the mountains. The river was high and flat and drowned the edge of
the terrace. There were long scars in the earth and rock 'Yhere the
branches of a tree had been dragged past by the water. The red creature
122
LINGUISTIC FUNCTION AND LITERARY STYLE: WILLIAM GOLDING
came trotting back to a dark hollow in the side of the cliff where there
was evidence of occupation. It looked at the other figure, dark now,
that grinned down at it from the back of the hollow. Then it turned
away and ran through the little passage that joined the terrace to the
slope. It halted, peering down at the scars, the abandoned rollers and
broken ropes. It turned again, sidled round a shoulder of rock and
stood on an almost imperceptible path that ran along the sheer rocks.
It began to sidle along the path, crouch, its long arms swinging,
touching, almost as firm a support as the legs. It was peering down into
the thunderous waters but there was nothing to be seen but the
columns of glimmering haze where the water had scooped a bowl out
of the rock. It moved faster, broke into a queer loping run that made
the head bob up and down and the forearms alternate like the legs of a
horse. It stopped at the end of the path and looked down at the long
streamers of weed that were moving backwards and forwards under the
water. It put up a hand and scratched under its chinless mouth.
C. (pp. 228-9)
The sail glowed red-brown. Tuami glanced back at the gap through
the mountain and saw that it was full of golden light and the sun was
sitting in it. As if they were obeying some signal the people began to
stir, to sit up and look across the water at the green hills. Twal bent
over Tanakil and kissed her and murmured to her. Tanakil's lips parted.
Her voice was harsh and came from far away in the night.
"Liku!"
Tuami heard Marian whisper to him from by the mast.
"That is the devil's name. Only she may speak it."
Now Vivani was really waking. They heard her huge, luxurious
yawn and the bear skin was thrown off. She sat up, shook back her
loose hair and looked first at Madan then at Tuami. At once he was
filled again with lust and hate. If she had been what she was, if Marian,
if her man, if she had saved her baby in the storm on the salt water -
"My breasts are paining me."
If she had not wanted the child as a plaything, if I had not saved the
other as a joke -
He began to talk high and fast.
"There are plains beyond those hills, Marian, for they grow less; and
there will be herds for hunting. Let us steer in towards the shore. Have
we water - but of course we have water! Did the women bring the
food? Did you bring the food, Twal?''
123
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
Twal lifted her face towards him and it was twisted with grief and
hate.
"What have I to do with food, master? You and he gave my child
to the devils and they have given me back a changeling who does not
see or speak."
The sand was swirling in Tuami's brain. He thought in panic: they
have given me back a changed Tuami; what shall I do? Only Madan is
the same - smaller, weaker but the same. He peered forward to find
the changeless one as something he could hold on to. The sun was
blazing on the red sail and Madan was red. His arms and legs were
contracted, his hair stood out and his beard, his teeth were wolfs teeth
and his eyes like blind stones. The mouth was opening and shutting.
"They cannot follow us, I tell you. They cannot pass over water."
Notes
1. The results were presented in a paper read to the Conference of
University Teachers ofEnglish, London (Bedford College), April 1965.
2. Paul Zumthor suggests (private communication) that a particular literary
tradition may be characterized by the emphasis and value placed on one
particular function, a shift in emphasis being associated with a major break
in the tradition. Cf. Zumthor (1971).
3. Nor the other way round, at least in the typical instances. There are
certain linguistic activities in which one or other function is prescribed
and the speaker required to supply the remainder: "language exercises"
such as "Now ask your neighbour a question" (in foreign language
classes) and "Write a sonnet" (in school).
4. Cf. the discussion by Tzvetan Todorov (1971).
5. Including those which specify types of communication role, or illocution-
ary force, which Richard Ohmann proposes to use in a definition of
literature. See Ohmann (1971).
6. Rene W ellek, "Closing statement (retrospects and prospects from the
viewpoint ofliterary criticism)", in T. A. Sebeok (1960, pp. 417-18).
7. Dell H. Hymes, "Phonological aspects of style: some English sonnets", in
Seymour Chatman and Samuel Levin (1967, pp. 33-53, see pp. 33-4).
8. On diatypic variation see Michael Gregory (1967, pp. 177-98).
9. Charles E. Osgood, "Some effects of motivation on style of encoding",
in Sebeok (1960, p. 293).
10. As n. 7.
11. Angus McIntosh, "Saying", Review of English Literature, 6 (April 1965),
19. It is worth quoting further from the same paragraph: "It is at least
clear that any approach to this kind of problem which looks _at anything
less than the whole text as the ultimate unit has very little to contribute.
124
LINGUISTIC FUNCTION AND LITERARY STYLE: WILLIAM GOLDING
125
Chapter Four
THE DE-AUTOMATIZATION
OF GRAMMAR: FROM PRIESTLEY'S
AN INSPECTOR CALLS
(1982)
First published in LAnguage Form and Linguistic Variation: Papers Dedicated to A,wus McIntosh,
edited by John M. Anderson. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 129-59.
126
THE DE-AUTOMATIZATION OF GRAMMAR: J. B. PRIESTLEY
127
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
128
THE DE-AUTOMATIZATION OF GRAMMAR: J. B. PRIESTLEY
men will not learn that lesson, then they will be taught it in fire and
blood and anguish.
Moreover this "lesson" is further thrown into relief by repeated
references to the "every man for himself" burden of Mr. Birling's self-
important sermonizing. Compare the various lines in I Have Been Here
Before which are explicitly concerned with time, e.g. the following
spoken by Dr. Gorder in Act III:
We do not go round in a circle. That is an illusion, just as the circling
of the planets and stars is an illusion. We move along a spiral track.
Characters such as Dr. Gorder and Inspector Goole, in their different
roles as guru, the 'Great Detective', and the like, serve as natural
spokesmen for the issues that are being raised.
But the themes are by no means always treated in this declarative
way. The time theme in An Inspector Calls, while clearly present, is
subdued and largely implicit. In this connection it seems worth stressing
once again that it is wrong to treat Priestley's dramatic time theme as if
it was some kind of a scientific theory. Priestley used to be criticized
for a naive acceptance of currently popular time theories such as those
ofJ. W. Dunne (1934) and P. D. Ouspensky (1931), and especially for
the "recurrence and intervention" concept that he used in I Have Been
Here Before, with its obvious contradictions if taken literally. But this
was never meant by him to be held up as a theory and evaluated as
true or false. It is, first, something to be explored for its dramatic
potential (and many of his plays make use of it in one form or another);
and secondly, something that gives a new dimension to human
relationships. Hence it is significant not for its intellectual content, as
something to be presented in the text, but as a motif that permeates
the interaction of the participants, and is more or less covertly woven
in to the dialogue. Thus in An Inspector Calls (Act III):
(1) We hardly ever told him anything he didn't know; did you notice
that?
(2) I didn't notice you told him that it's every man for himsel£ - Is that
when the Inspector came, just after Father had said that?
- and of course the very last lines of the play. Such manifestations are
"more or less" covert; they are hardly buried deep, but they do not
constitute a direct presentation of the theme, and unlike the examples
cited earlier they are likely to be taken in by the audience subcon-
sciously rather than consciously.
It is a general assumption of structuralism that the meanings of a
129
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
work of literature are dispersed throughout the text not just at different
places in the syntagm but also, and more significantly, at different levels
within the code. This dispersal into what Firth called the various
"modes of meaning", for which he used the analogy of the dispersal of
light throughout the spectrum, is typically an unconscious feature of
the writing process (cf Mukafovsky's (1977) observation that "aesthetic
intentionality does not necessarily presuppose a conscious intent"), and
is hidden from the reader/listener unless he digs for it. Eco (1976)
compares a work of art to a language, saying that "there must be an
underlying system of mutual correlations, and thus a semiotic design
which cunningly gives the impression of non-semiosis". It is this that
accounts for the paradox of "poetic language": there is no such thing,
but we can all recognize it when we see it. To quote Mukafovsky
again: "poetic language is permanently characterized only by its func-
tion: however, function is not a property but a mode of utilizing the
properties of a given phenomenon" (pp. 3-4). How much the more is
this true of the language of prose works, where we do not even pretend
there is such a thing as "prosaic language" (as is clear from the meaning
this expression has acquired). McIntosh (1965) observed of prose fiction
that
... quite often where the impact of an entire work may be enormous,
yet word by word, phrase by phrase, clause by clause, sentence by
sentence, there may seem to be nothing very unusual or arresting, in
grammar or in vocabulary ...
130
THE DE-AUTOMATIZATION OF GRAMMAR: J. B. PRIESTLEY
131
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
MRS BIRLING: I think we've just about come to an end of this wretched
business - (1)
GERALD: I don't think so (2). Excuse me (3).
(He goes out. They watch him go in silence. We hear
the front door slam.)
SHEILA: (to Inspector) You know, you never showed him that
photograph of her (4).
INSPECTOR: No (5). It wasn't necessary (6). And I thought it better not to
(7).
MRS BIRLING: You have a photograph of this girl (8)?
INSPECTOR: Yes (9). I think you'd better look at it (10).
MRS BIRLING: I don't see any particular reason why I should - (11).
INSPECTOR: Probably not (12). But you'd betterlook at it (13).
MRS BIRLING: Very well (14).
(He produces the photograph and she looks hard at it.)
INSPECTOR: (taking back the photograph) You recognize her (15)?
MRS BIRLING; No (16). Why should I (17)?
INSPECTOR: Of course she might have changed lately, but I can't believe
she could have changed so much (18).
MRS BIRLING: I don't understand you, Inspector (19).
INSPECTOR: You mean you don't choose to do, Mrs Biding (20).
MRS BIRLING: (angrily) I meant what I said (21).
INSPECTOR: You're not telling me the truth (22).
MRS BIRLING: I beg your pardon (23)!
BIRLING: (angrily, to Inspector) Look here, I'm not going to have this,
Inspector (24). You'll apologize at once (25).
INSPECTOR: Apologize for what - doing my duty (26)?
BIRLING: No, for being so cffensive about it (27). I'm a public man
- (28).
INSPECTOR: (massively) Public men, Mr Biding, have responsibilities as
well as privileges (29).
BIRLING: Possibly (30). But you weren't asked to come here to talk
to me about my responsibilities (31).
SHEILA: Let's hope not (32). Though I'm beginning to wonder (33).
MRS BIRLING: Does that mean anything, Sheila (34)?
SHEILA: It means that we've no excuse now for putting on airs and
132
THE DE-AUTOMATIZATION OF GRAMMAR: J. B. PRIESTLEY
that if we've any sense we won't try (35). Father threw this
girl out because she asked for decent wages (36). I went
and pushed her further out, right into the street, just
because I was angry and she was pretty (37). Gerald set her
up as his mistress and then dropped her when it suited him
(38). And now you're pretending you don't recognize her
from that photograph (39). I admit I don't know why you
should, but I know jolly well you did in fact recognize her,
from the way you looked (40). And if you're not telling the
truth, why should the Inspector apologize (41)? And can't you
see, both of you, you're making it worse (42)?
(She turns away. We hear the front door slam again.)
This exchange, which occurs at what is more or less the middle point
of the play, constitutes a small, self-contained enactment that revolves
around a photograph. In the simplest terms it has a structure as shown
in Figure 1:
'you look at it'
[Inspector]
---
'you're lying' [Birlings senior]
~
'you're insulting'
I think
I don't think } 'this business has ended'
I thought
I think
I don't see
probably not
might have
l 'the photograph should be looked at'
can't believe
could have l 'has she changed?'
133
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
it wasn't necessary
it was better not to
you'd better
'(to) look at the photograph'
[any ... reason why] I should
you'd better
[why] should I?
I thought it better
I think you'd better
I don't see ... why I should
134
THE DE-AUTOMATIZATION OF GRAMMAR: J. B. PRIESTLEY
'objective' probability:
Modulation
Objectified Subjective
it wasn't necessary
I thou t it better not to Gerald
I think you'd better MrsBirling
you'd better
Modulation:
( i ) objectified ( ii ) qualified by subjective
modality
135
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
understand
mean
mean
tell the truth
136
THE DE-AUTOMATIZATION OF GRAMMAR: J. B. PRIESTLEY
1-18
19-22
23-35
probabilities obligations
36-38 narrative
39 - 42 categoricalities
[setting]
0) Mrs. Biding and Gerald: uncertainty building up (about ongoing
process)
137
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
SLAM (i)
[challenge]
1) Inspector: obligations (a) Gerald need not see photograph (he will
confess anyway)
(b) Mrs. Biding must see photograph
(she is a tougher nut: modalized, then
demodalized)
2) Mrs. Birling: rejects obligations (a) to look
(b) to recognize
3) Inspector: rejects her rejection, by rejecting probability of
consequences
(conflict]
4) Mrs. Biding: presents as conflict of understanding
5) Inspector: rejects her interpretation ('you are lying')
[confrontation]
6) Bidings: take offence ('you are not discharging your obligation to
our status')
7) Inspector: rejects (a) ('I am discharging my obligation to society')
(b) ('you are not discharging your obligation to
society')
8) Biding: rejects ('it is not your obligation to tell me my obligations')
9) Sheila: questions categoricality of this and affirms her
understanding
[explanation]
10) Sheila: narrates events revealed so far
(summation]
11) Sheila: evaluates (a) ('you are categorical')
(b) ('I am uncertain')
(c) ('I am categorical that you are wrong')
(d) ('your understanding is at fault')
SLAM (ii)
138
THE DE-AUTOMATIZATION OF GRAMMAR: J. B. PRIESTLEY
139
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
Indicative Imperative
positive
CATEGORICAL
CATEGORICAL
negative
140
THE DE-AUTOMATIZATION OF GRAMMAR: J. B. PRIESTLEY
141
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
142
THE DE-AUTOMATIZATION OF GRAMMAR: J. B. PRIESTLEY
143
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
past
=====present present
past in
present
past
present
in past
past (3)
present---========-
-----------------------present
past (5) ~ .
past m
present~
------------------------~present
past
-=========
Figure 6
(sequent future)
Movement of tense in Sheila's longest tum
ending up with "We'd have to start all over again" - perhaps hardly
surprising in view of this somewhat disorientating oscillation of tem-
poral focus.
Once again let us try and distance ourselves from the immediate
textual environment in order to see the extent to which the expression
of time has been de-automatized. To do this we need to refer to the
144
THE DE-AUTOMATIZATION OF GRAMMAR: J. B. PRIESTLEY
past:
present:
145
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
Indicative
categorical
PROBABILITY
median will
I
usually
low may
I
categorical it isn't
146
THE DE-AUTOMATIZATION OF GRAMMAR: J. B. PRIESTLEY
Scientists once put a very accurate clock inside a space ship. Then they
fired it into space. When the ship came back to earth they looked at the
clock and gasped. For the clock on the space ship was showing a
different time to the clocks on earth. The clock on the space ship had
somehow lost time while it was out in space! But how can you lose
time?
Scientists say that when a space ship travels very fast, close to the
speed of light, then time slows down in the space ship. It seems
unbelievable, but it's true. Maybe it is a bit like reading a book. If you
were one of the people in the book then time would be the same for
everyone in the book. But if you were outside the book it would be
different. You would be just someone reading that hook. Then your
time would be different. You could read about someone's whole lifetime
in just a few hours. That's one way oflooking at it Maybe we are like
characters in a book, trapped on earth. (Mark Oakey 1978)
147
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
148
Chapter Five
1 Background
Linguists concerned with the study of texts, and particularly the study
of highly-valued texts, the central domain of linguistic stylistics, have
had to contend with two opposing traditions in the study of meaning.
According to one persuasion, the meaning of a text is its referential
content; meaning is displayed in the words and their construction; and
if two texts have the same truth value they are held to be synonymous.
According to the other, the meaning of a text is everything the
particular critic can think of to say about it; meaning lurks in the void
that is set "between the lines"; and no text is ever synonymous even
with itself. The task for a linguistics of style has been to develop the
positive features of both these images of language, both the leaner,
classical one and the more fleshy, romantic one; to demonstrate that
the meaning of a text is accessible - it is not some private figment of
the critic's psychic powers - but that it encompasses a great deal more
than the manifest propositional content.
Significant contributions to this task came from some of the great
figures of the middle years of this century. Mukafovsky made it clear
that if a text was highly valued this was not because it was written in
any special language evolved for the purpose (there is no such thing as
a "literary register"), but because of the functions to which the language
was addressed (1977). Mukafovsky offered a rich multifunctional view
of language, and from this he derived both a general theory ~f literary
First published in Functions of Style, edited by David Birch and Michael O'Toole. London:
Pinter, 1987, pp. 31-44.
149
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
2 General considerations
In the 1960s some important pioneering work was done in the
linguistic study of literary texts based on this stratal-functional model of
language (e.g. Gregory 1967, 1974; Hasan 1964, 1967; Leech 1965,
1969; McIntosh 1963, 1966; Sinclair, 1966). At the same time we were
working towards an interpretation of the grammar of English that
would serve the needs (inter alia) of stylistic research: that was itself
multifunctional, multistratal, and abstract or "cryptotypic" in the
Whorfian sense. Systemic grammar with its framework of levels and its
metafunctional hypothesis was an attempt to meet this need (Halliday
1985a; cf. Kennedy 1982). The Hjelmslevian perspective has been
sharpened by Martin's (1985) complementarity of synoptic and
dynamic, and Lemke's work (1985) has greatly strengthened the
theoretical foundations.
In the past five to seven years the field has been immensely enriched
by combining the linguistic interpretation of texts in terms o(grammar
150
POETRY AS SCIENTIFIC DISCOURSE: ALFRED TENNYSON
151
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
152
POETRY AS SCIENTIFIC DISCOURSE: ALFRED TENNYSON
LIV.
THE wish, that of the living whole
No life may fail beyond the grave;
Derives it not from what we have
The likest God within the soul?
Are God and Nature then at strife,
That Nature lends such evil dreams?
So careful of the type she seems,
So careless of the single life;
That I, considering everywhere
Her secret meaning in her deeds,
And finding that of fifty seeds
She often brings but one to bear;
I falter where I firmly trod,
And falling with my weight of cares
Upon the great world's altar-stairs
That slope thro' darkness up to God;
I stretch lame hands of faith, and grope,
And gather dust and chaff, and call
To what I feel is Lord of all,
And faintly trust the larger hope.
153
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
LV.
"SO careful of the type?" but no.
From scarped cliff and quarried stone
She cries "a thousand types are gone:
I care for nothing, all shall go.
Thou makest thine appeal to me:
I bring to life, I bring to death:
The spirit does but mean the breath:
I know no more." And he, shall he,
Man, her last work, who seem'd so fair,
Such splendid purpose in his eyes,
Who roll'd the psalm to wintry skies,
Who built him fanes of fruitless prayer,
Who trusted God was love indeed
And love Creation's final law -
Tho' Nature, red in tooth and claw
With ravine, shriek'd against his creed -
Who loved, who suffer'd countless ills,
Who battled for the True, the Just,
Be blown about the desert dust,
Or seal'd within the iron hills?
No more? A monster then, a dream,
A discord. Dragons of the prime,
That tare each other in their slime,
Were mellow music match'd with him.
0 life as futile, then, as frail!
0 for thy voice to soothe and bless!
What hope of answer, or redress?
Behind the veil, behind the veil.
The remainder of the paper will be on the interpretation of this text.
154
POETRY AS SCIENTIFIC DISCOURSE: ALFRED TENNYSON
a 'PI P+2
Pa2a+4 (3a2a+S
al p+3 114«'PI
134•pa f34P=P p+S 11+6 p+7» a+2
155
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
156
POETRY AS SCIENTIFIC DISCOURSE: ALFRED TENNYSON
verbal: love, trust, suffer, the Medium being man; elsewhere the Process
is material but there is a semiotic Range or circumstance: roll the psalm,
build Janes cf prayer, battle for the True. There is a minor motif of
attribution: be careful/careless, with Medium Nature; be at strife, Medium
God and Nature; and the contrasting pair man seemed fair [but] dragons
were mellow music match'd with him. The transitivity patterns are in phase
with the logical ones, so we can identify a second dimension of
structure having an ideationally based phasal organization:
Table 2 Patterns of transitivity in the 17 stanzas
Ideational Transitivity Clause Passage Medium
phase complex
phase A material: projection: § 53- I, we
destructive report/ § 54 st. 1.
idea
phase B material: expansion: § 54 sts. [Nature-,i.)
dispositive enhancing 2-5. I
phase C mental; projection: § 55 st. 1 Nature
verbal quote/ - st. 2,
locution 1.4.
phase D mental; expansion; § 55 st. man
verbal elaborating 2, 1.4 -
st. 5.
phase E (relational: § 55 sts.
attributive) 6, 7.
A comparison of the two will show that this is slightly, but not
randomly, out of phase with the pattern set up by the interpersonal
structure.
It may be helpful to note the major lexical chains deriving from the
transitivity selections:
know faith trust doubt hope wish dream
goal purpose final fruitless vam rubbish futile
good ill evil careful careless
soul spirit breath life death grave
falter fall lame grope secret veil
In addition to the foregrounded antonymies we may notice the range
. of certainty to uncertainty traversed by the first set, from know to dream.
157
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
158
Table 3 Text movement shown by dominant selections in interpersonal
and ideational systems
PHASES l-5: PHASESA-E:
INTERPERSONAL IDEATIONAL
1 1 A
2 Transitional
54 3 (l ) declarative 3 B
(a) material: dispositive
Medium==I
4 (2) present (b) expansion : enhancing
1 (a)
mental ; verbal C
Medium = Nature
2 (b) projection :
auote/locution
3 ( 1)interrogative; 4 (a) mental ; verbal D
moodless Medium= man
55 4 (2) past (b) expansion: elaborating
( 1) = mood ( a ) transitivity
(2) primary (deictic) ( b) clause complex
tense
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
160
POETRY AS SCIENTIFIC DISCOURSE: ALFRED TENNYSON
In other words: the soul is dead; you have no more need of that
hypothesis.
Nature, interpreted by science, has shown how man arose from the
rest of creation. At first sight, this seems threatening; we are no longer
protected by being uniquely the children of God. Individuals perish;
and Tennyson's personal grief at the loss of a loved friend provokes
what at level one is a rage against nature's indifference: her carelessness
of the single life, casually abandoned and destroyed. But at level two
161
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
the scientific enquiry takes over and the interest is in the type. Species
perish too; many are there in the fossil library, that no longer exist.
And the human species will also become extinct: mankind is not
privileged - there is no immortality, even of the soul.
You appeal to me, says Nature, and your soul goes puffi This
"spirit", the fiction of ancient Greek and Hebrew philosophy, is simply
a puff of breath: a dressed-up pneuma and psyche - the breathing in
and out, the painted deities of Heng and Ha that guard a Taoist temple.
This is how the whole fancy started: - I know no more. McKellar (1985)
provides an illuminating, fully documented account of the history of
the concept of psycho-physical duality in western thinking.
It is clearly threatening to find one is no longer immortal. But, while
what we know from science may be disturbing, the fact that we
have come to know it is exhilarating; and it is the liberating power
of knowledge that Tennyson is celebrating here. As Gibson points out
(1958, p. 67), despite the protestation of ignorance in the final stanza
of§ 55 (Behind the veil, behind the vei0, "[Tennyson's] dramatic stance
has actually taken him a long way from any such humble position". He
has read Sir Charles Lyell's Principles of Geology; and while there is no
suggestion that he had read any work of Darwin (and he could not
have read The Origin of Species, which appeared nearly ten years after In
Memoriam), he was clearly and indeed explicitly aware of the current of
evolutionary theory that was gathering momentum in biology. The
entire dynamic of the text - the rhetorical structure (Phases 1-5), the
dialogue with Nature, the movement created by the clause complexes
(§ 4 above), the grammar's construction of blind trust as a lost cause -
is both a presentation of what Gibson calls "the disquieting new
world of science" and a celebration of it as something that increases
rather than diminishes the stature of mankind.
These stanzas thus fall within the tradition of the scientific imagin-
ation: the works of the great poet-scientists from Lucretius onwards
whose poetic text displays in a unique form the grammatical construc-
tion of reality. And this leads to one final question: what is the role of
texts of this kind in the long-term dialogue with Nature, that which
Prigogine and Stengers refer to (and use as sub-title) in their book
Order out of Chaos? (Prigogine and Stengers 1985: esp. the Preface,
pp. xxvii-xxxi). Is there a wider context, a further level of the social
semiotic, in which this passage has to be interpreted?
162
POETRY AS SCIENTIFIC DISCOURSE: ALFRED TENNYSON
163
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
could not lead; for the comb of the hive-bee, as far as we can see, is
absolutely perfect in economising wax.
Thus, as I believe, the most wonderful of all known instincts, that of
the hive-bee, can be explained by natural selection having taken
advantage of numerous, successive, slight modifications of simpler
instincts; natural selection having by slow degrees, more and more
perfectly, led the bees to sweep equal spheres at a given distance from
each other in a double layer, and to build up and excavate the wax
along the planes of intersection. The bees, of course, no more knowing
that they swept their spheres at one particular distance from each other,
than they know what are the several angles of the hexagonal prisms and
of the basal rhombic plates. The motive power of the process of natural
selection having been economy of wax; that individual swarm which
wasted least honey in the secretion of wax, having succeeded best, and
having transmitted by inheritance its newly acquired economical instinct
to new swarms, which in their turn will have had the best chance of
succeeding in the struggle for existence.
Darwin is writing in the language of science. Behind this lie some four
centuries of scientific English, from Chaucer onwards; and 150 years of
continuous development from the English texts of Isaac Newton's works.
It is, obviously, very different from the language of Tennyson.
Darwin's text is characterized by very simple clause complexes: the
favoured structural pattern is one of embedding rather than taxis
(hypotaxis or parataxis). There is a higher lexical density (lexical words
per ranking clause), much more nominalization and a greater use of
grammatical metaphor. (For these concepts, see Halliday 1985a; for
these features as characteristic of written English, see Halliday 1987; for
the relation between lexical density and grammatical metaphor, see
Ravelli 1985.)
Where did this language evolve? It evolved as the language of
physics - physicists led the way in creating the discourse of science.
The model for
Hence the saving of wax by largely saving honey must be a most
important element of success in any family of bees.
164
POETRY AS SCIENTIFIC DISCOURSE: ALFRED TENNYSON
(Prigogine and Stengers 1985, p. 83). This was the major preoccupation
of the French Enlightenment; McKellar has described (1985: Chapter
VII, esp. pp. 542 ff.) the rejection of this psycho-physical dualism,
especially La Mettrie's attempt to demystify the psyche through the
concept of the semiotic man-machine. Somehow, mankind had to re-
enter the world of nature.
This was the significance of the new evolutionary theories, first in
geology and then in the biological sciences: man once again became
part of nature. But there was a price: he had to give up his godhead.
Evolution could restore the "vital principle" - but only if man was first
dethroned. Darwin finds it necessary to defend his theory by urging
"that there is grandeur in this view oflife" (459) (cf. Chapter 6 below):
When I view all beings not as special creations, but as the lineal descendants
of some few beings which lived long before the first bed of the Silurian
system was deposited, they seem to me to become ennobled. (458)
165
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
this understanding
raises man still
higher
man's place in
nature is restored
nature destroys
species - including man
nature destroys
individuals -
including humans
my loved friend
has perished
166
POETRY AS SCIENTIFIC DISCOURSE: ALFRED TENNYSON
Obviously Tennyson was not describing, and could not describe, issues
that were not yet formulated or apprehended by the scientists them-
selves. But that is irrelevant; in any case he was not describing anything
- that is not how the grammar was being put into service. What he
was doing with the grammar was something considerably more com-
plex: constructing a semiotic universe at the intersection of science and
poetry. The point is that he was doing this with the grammar (as
always, in its systemic sense of "lexicogrammar"); so it is reasonable
that we should use the study of grammar (our "grammatics", so to
speak) as a means of trying to understand it.
167
Chapter Six
First published in I.A rappresentazione verba/e e iconica: valori estetid e funziona/i. Atti de/ XI
Congresso Nazionale de/l'Assodazzione ltaliana di Anglistica, Bergamo, 24 e 25 Ottobre 1988,
edited by Clotilde de Stasio, Maurizio Gotti and Rossana Bonadei. Milan: ,Guerini Studio,
1990, pp 57-80.
168
THE CONSTRUCTION OF KNOWLEDGE AND VALUE: CHARLES DARWIN
169
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
In their most general form, these clauses represent the two related
motifs of 'a causes/is caused by x', 'b proves/is proved by y'. Let me
cite another pair of examples taken from a different text:
These results cannot be handled by purely structural models oflaterality
effects . . . [b + prove + y]
(if ... ) both word recognition and concurrent verbal memory produce
more left than right hemisphere activation. [a + cause + x)
170
THE CONSTRUCTION OF KNOWLEDGE AND VALUE: CHARLES DARWIN
Note in particular the sequence [are] constantly [refracted] efter the unusual
manner ... The unusual Refraction is therefore pe,form'd by ... Fonnulai-
cally: "a happens . . . The happening of a is caused by ... ". The
::~ationl l
r :-~.-e~_n_e____~_h_e_m_e_ _ _ _ _ _ _N_e_w--------~
::;:tion l
lr:-~.-e~_n_e____~_h_e_m_e_ _ _ _ _ _ _N_e_w
________ ~
Figure 1 Transitivity (ideational), mood (interpersonal), and theme and
information (textual) structures in the "favourite" clause type
171
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
172
THE CONSTRUCTION OF KNOWLEDGE AND VALUE: CHARLES DARWIN
173
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
relations: relations such as 'i.e.', 'e.g.', 'and', 'or', 'but', 'then', 'thus',
'so'. These are grammaticalized in various ways, typically (in English)
by conjunctions and prepositions. There are many possible ways of
categorizing these relations, depending on the criteria adopted; one
schema that I find useful in applying the model of the grammar to
discourse analysis is that shown in Table 2. 8
In the type of clause that we are considering here, however, these
relationships come to be lexicalized, as verbs; for example, the verbs
produce, arise from, depend on, lead to as expressions of the causal
relationship. Furthermore, this logical-semantic space is then crosscut
along another dimension, according to whether the relationship is
being set up in rebus or in verbis; 9 thus the causal relationship may be
either (in rebus) 'a causes x' or (in verbis) 'b proves (= causes one to
say) y'. Not all the logical-semantic relationships are lexicalized to
the same extent; nor is this last distinction between relations in the
events and relations in the discourse equally applicable to all. But the
general pattern is as shown, with the experiential content entirely
located within the two nominal groups and the verbal group setting
up the relation between them. Table 3 lists some of the common
verbs by which these logical-semantic relations are construed in lexical
form.
Only a handful of these verbs occur in Newton's writings. The
number has noticeably increased half a century later, in Joseph Priest-
ley's History and Present State of Electricity; and by the time of James
174
THE CONSTRUCTION OF KNOWLEDGE AND VALUE: CHARLES DARWIN
175
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
other worlds; and it does so, in this case, by this complex of semogenic
strategies: "packaging" into extended nominal groups, nominalizing
processes and properties, lexicalizing logical-semantic relations first as
verbs and then as nouns, and constructing the whole into the sort of
clause we meet with everywhere - not just in academic writing but in
the newspapers, in the bureaucracy, and in our school textbooks -
typified by the following from a primary school science text: lung cancer
death rates are clearly associated with increased smoking. The grammar of a
natural language is a theory of experience, a metalanguage of daily life;
and the forms of verbal representation that evolved as part of modem
science have penetrated into almost every domain of our semiotic
practice.
*********
176
THE CONSTRUCTION OF KNOWLEDGE AND VALUE: CHARLES DARWIN
177
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
178
THE CONSTRUCTION OF KNOWLEDGE AND VALUE: CHARLES DARWIN
179
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
-.., others
~~,
authors of the highest eminence I
I authority:
to my mind it accords bener with what we know of
the laws impressed on matter by the
Creator
'>-
the production and extinction our view
of the past and present opinion -+ inference
inhabitants of the world .... prophecy -+ confidence
[modal metaphors)
(seem tome)
when I they
II
II species, life forms;
differentiation
judging from the past (may safely infer) [static)
not one living species
of the species now living
for the manner in which all organic
beings are grouped
the greater number of species (can so far take a species
of each genus prophetic glance succession
all the species of many genera into filrurity as to groups
we the common & widely-spread species foretell) no cataclysm
belonging 10 the larger and dominant
selection
groups
all the living forms of life
as (may feel certain organic
we the ordinary succession by generation that)
no cataclysm forms oflife
hence We (may look with and idowments
and as natural selection some confidence)
all corporeal and mental endowments
the world
from New
this planet
180
THE CONSTRUCTION OF KNOWLEDGE AND VALUE: CHARLES DARWIN
grandeur
law of gravity
C
few
life beallllful and evolved life originally breathed in to a few
breathed in endless wonderful fonns or one
fonns
fixed laws of gravity
endless fonns most beautiful and
wonderful being evolved
[Now of final sentence]
181
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
These are developed side by side in the form of fairly long nominal
groups which bring out, through their lexicogrammar, the number and
diversity of species, the collocation of 'species' with 'life', and the
steady, irreversible forward progression through time; the three sub-
motifs are then united in a specific reference to birds, various insects and
worms (these elaborately constructed forms), which is the final appearance of
this motif as Theme. The effect is one of a massive and powerful life-
engendering process - which is, however, presented synoptically as an
objectified 'state of affairs', since words representing processes are in
fact nominalized: production, extinction, succession, (no) cataclysm, selection.
This, as we saw earlier, is a feature of the grammar of the most highly
favoured clause type in scientific writing: the nominalization picks up
the preceding argument and presents it in this "objectified" form as
something now to be taken for granted. Here it also contrasts with the
more dynamic presentation of the motifs figuring as New (see B
below).
The third motif (III in Figure 3) is that of the sources leading to
speciation: these laws; from the war of nature, famine and death; .from so
simple a beginning. This comes in almost at the end; and Darwin leads
into it by taking over laws into the Theme from the previous Rheme
(... produced by laws acting around us. These laws . .. ; c[ C below). The
effect is to juxtapose, both within the Theme (and hence, being also
"given", both to be construed as something already established), the
two conflicting principles in nature - its lawfulness, and its lawlessness
- which together by their dialectic interaction account for the origin
of species.
I shall return below to the extraordinary final sentence of the text.
Meanwhile let us consider the motifs that constitute the "main point"
of the argument, as these appear clause by clause with the grammatical
function of the New. 18 These are showi:i in Text 3 (see Appendix) and
set out diagrammatically in Figure 4.
The first such motif (lettered A in Figure 4) is that (?f alternative
explanations: specifically, creation versus evolution. It may be helpful
to set the wordings out in a list:
182
THE CONSTRUCTION OF KNOWLEDGE AND VALUE: CHARLES DARWIN
The final one of these is the last appearance of this motif until the very
last words of the text (... have been, and are being, evolved'); meanwhile,
via the two semantic features of generation (lineal descendants) and
antiquity (long before the first bed . .. ), itJeads us in to the second of the
"new" motifs, that of transmission - or better, transmitting, since the
way the grammar constructs it is at least as much clausal as nominal. 19
Like the second of the thematic motifs, this second motif within the
New (B in Figure 4) is also constructed out of three sub-motifs:
183
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
I authorities A explanation
II the phenomenon of species: B the process of evolution:
(i) inhabitants & groups (i) transmission & procreation
(ii) life form & endowments (ii) time: past - future
(iii) variation (iii) ennoblement
III sources (law/war of nature) C law (natural selection)
phrases and/or clauses in them and all functioning as the final element in
the one ranking clause - a clause which is (anomalousJ;r} non-finite,
despite being the main and only clause in the sentence.2
Up to this point, then (that is, up to the final sentence of the final
paragraph), the clauses are rather clearly organized, through their
textual functions of Theme (in Theme-Rheme) and New (in Given-
New), around a small number of distinct but interlocking motifs. We
could summarize this pattern as in Table 4 above. Then, in the final
sentence, the motifs ofll, III, Band Care all brought together: and in
an extremely complex pattern. The sentence begins with There is
grandeur in this view of life ... Here grandeur, which relates to B(iii), is
unusual in being at the same time both Theme and New; hence it is
doubly prominent. 21 On a first reading, in this view of life seems to
complete the clause; and since it is anaphorically cohesive (by reference
this, and by lexical repetition of life) it is read as not only Rheme but
also Given. It then turns out that Darwin has misled us with a
grammatical pun, and that life actually begins a new clause, life ...
having been originally breathed into a few forms or into one, all of which is a
projected Qualifier to view ('the view that life was originally breathed
.. .'). This makes life thematic, and (since no longer anaphoric) a
continuation of the motif of life forms in II(ii); this motif is then
carried on into the New, in (breathed) into a Jew forms or into one. The
next clause turns out to be another projected Qualifier to view,
paratactically related to the last yet finite where the other was non-
finite; furthermore it is a hypotactic clause complex in which the
dependent clause comes first. The dependent clause has as Theme this
planet, relating cohesively to the world at the very beginning of II(i): and
as New (has gone cycling on) according to the fixed law ofgravity, where law
of gravity derives from motif C (natural laws) but shifts the attention
184
THE CONSTRUCTION OF KNOWLEDGE AND VALUE: CHARLES DARWIN
[by the laws of natural [life] forms most beautiful have been and are
selection] and most wonderful being evolved
life having been originally breathed into a few forms or into one
185
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
are, evolving). Yet all this load of work is hardly worth mentioning
beside the major responsibility the word evolved has to bear, along with
the verbal group of which it is a part: that of sustaining the climax of
450 pages of intense scientific argument. This is the culmination
towards which the entire text has been building up. It would be hard
to find anywhere in English a sentence, or a clause, or a group, or a
word that has been made to carry such an awesome semiotic'load.
I do not know how long it took Darwin to compose these two
paragraphs, or whether he reflected consciously on their construction
as he was doing it - I imagine not. I certainly had no idea, when
starting the analysis, of what I was going to find. I had the sense of a
remarkable and powerful piece of writing, as the climax to a remarkable
and powerful book; and it struck me that something of the effect of
these two paragraphs might lie in the patterning of the Theme and
of the New - that is, in the textual component within the grammar of
the clause. It is important to stress that that is in fact all that I have
been looking at in this paper; I have said almost nothing about cohesion
or transitivity or mood or the clause complex or any of the other
lexicogrammatical systems / processes that go into the makeup of a
text. Some, at least, of these other features would undoubtedly show
interesting and significant patterns if we were to analyse them with this
or some comparable kind of functional grammatics.
It is pointless to try and classify a text such as this - to ask whether
it 'is' a scientific treatise or a declaration of faith or an entertaining
work of literature. It is a product of the impact between an intellectual
giant and a moment in the space-time continuum of our culture, with
all the complexity of meaning that that implies. With this very partial
analysis - a fragment of the grammar of a fragment of the text - I have
tried to suggest something of how this text takes its place in semo-
history. Some of the thematic patterning here is like that which I
described in the first part of the paper, which evolved primarily (I
think) in the context of scientific endeavour; we can recognize
instances where Darwin is backgrounding some point already covered,
so getting it taken for granted, and moving on from it, by a logical-
semantic "process", to a foregrounded next stage; e.g. from the war ef
nature ... the production ef the higher animals directly follows. (There are
more of this type in the more strictly "scientific" passages; for example
the account of the honeycomb in Chapter 7, pp. 255-6.) But the
pattern has rather a different value here from that which it typically has
in the context in which it evolved; Darwin's strategy i~ that of
accumulating masses of evidence rather than moving forward logically
186
THE CONSTRUCTION OF KNOWLEDGE AND VALUE: CHARLES DARWIN
Notes
1. That is, there are many "disjunctions"; see J. L. Lemke, Semiotics and
Education, (1984) Toronto: Victoria University (Toronto Semiotic Circle,
Monographs, Working Papers and Prepublications), 1984; especially
pp. 132 ff. Dialectal disjunctions are mainly phonetic; cf. the Prague
School's concept of functional equilibrium in phonology.
2. The term "diatypic" is taken from Michael Gregory, "Aspects of varieties
differentiation", Journal ef Linguistics 3, 1967. The term "register" was first
used in this sense by T. B. W. Reid in "Linguistics, structuralism,
philology", Archivum Linguisticum 8, 1956; cf. M. A. K. Halliday,
Angus McIntosh and Peter Strevens, 1964.
3. See Ruqaiya Hasan, "Semantic variation and sociolinguistics", Australian
Journal of Linguistics (1989).
4. The concept ofregister should therefore be defined so as to make explicit
the dimension of power, as pointed out by Gunther Kress, "Textual
matters: the social effectiveness of style", and Norman Fairclough, "Reg-
ister, power and socio-semantic change", both in David Birch and
Michael O'Toole (1988).
5. See M. A. K. Halliday, "On the language of physical science", in Mohsen
Ghadessy (1999).
6. For this and other aspects of the systemic-functional grammar referred to
throughout this paper see M. A. K. Halliday (1985a).
7. The Given + New structure is not, in fact, a structure of the clause; it
constructs a separate unit (the "information unit") realized by intonation
as a tone group. In spoken English the typical (unmarked) discourse
pattern is that where one information unit is mapped on to one clause;
further semantic contrasts are then created by departure from this
unmarked mapping. In written English there are of course no direct
signals of the information unit; while the unmarked mapping may be
187
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
188
THE CONSTRUCTION OF KNOWLEDGE AND VALUE: CHARLES DARWIN
APPENDIX
Text 1
From Charles Darwin, The Origin ef Species by Means ef Natural Selection,
London:J. Murray, 1859
Authors of the highest eminence seem to be fully satisfied with the
view that each species has been independently created. To my mind it
accords better with what we know of the laws impressed on matter by
the Creator, that the production and extinction of the past and present
inhabitants of the world should have been due to secondary causes, like
those determining the birth and death of the individual. When I view
all beings not as special creations, but as the lineal descendants of some
few beings which lived long before the first bed of the Silurian system
was deposited, they seem to me to become ennobled. Judging from
the past, we may safely infer that not one living species will transmit its
unaltered likeness to a distant futurity. And of the species now living
very few will transmit progeny of any kind to a far distant futurity; for
the manner in which all organic beings are grouped, shows that the
greater number of species of each genus, and all the species of many
genera, have left no descendants, but have become utterly extinct. We
can so far take a prophetic glance into futurity as to fortell that it will
be the common and widely-spread species, belonging to the larger and
dominant groups, which will ultimately prevail and procreate new and
dominant species. As all the living forms of life are the lineal descend-
ants of those which lived long before the Silurian epoch, we may feel
certain. that the ordinary succession by generation has never once been
broken, and that no cataclysm has desolated the whole world. Hence
we may look with some confidence to a secure future of equally
inappreciable length. And as natural selection works solely by and for
189
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
the good of each being, all corporeal and mental endowments will tend
to progress towards perfection.
It is interesting to contemplate an entangled bank, clothed with
many plants of many kinds, with birds singing on the bushes, with
various insects flitting about, and with worms crawling through the
damp earth, and to reflect that these elaborately constructed forms, so
different from each other, and dependent on each other in so complex
a manner, have all been produced by laws acting around us. These
laws, taken in the largest sense, being Growth with Reproduction;
Inheritance which is almost implied by reproduction; Variability from
the indirect and direct action of the external conditions of life, and
from use and disuse; a Ratio of Increase so high as to lead to a Struggle
for Life, and as a consequence to Natural Selection, entailing Diver-
gence of Character and the Extinction of less-improved forms. Thus,
from the war of nature, from famine and death, the most exalted object
which we are capable of conceiving, namely, the production of the
higher animals, directly follows. There is grandeur in this view of life,
with its several powers, having been originally breathed into a few
forms or into one; and that, whilst this planet has gone cycling on
according to the fixed law of gravity, from so simple a beginning
endless forms most beautiful and most wonderful have been, and are
being, evolved.
Text2
The text showing Theme-Rheme structure (Theme underlined)
Authors of the highest eminence seem to be fully satisfied with the
view that each species has been independently created. To my mind it
accords better with what we know of the laws impressed on matter by
the Creator, that the production and extinction of the past and present
inhabitants of the world should have been due to secondary causes, like
those determining the birth and death of the individual. When I view
all beings not as special creations, but as the lineal descendants of some
few beings which lived long before the first bed of the Silurian system
was deposited, they seem to me to become ennobled. Judging from
the past, ~ may safely infer that not one living species will transmit its
unaltered likeness to a distant futurity. And of the species now living
very few will transmit progeny of any kind to a far distant futurity; for
the manner in which all organic beings are grouped, sho~s that the
greater number of species of each genus, and all the species of many
190
THE CONSTRUCTION OF KNOWLEDGE AND VALUE: CHARLES DARWIN
Text3
The text showing Given-New structure (New underlined)
191
HIGHLY VALUED TEXTS
those determining the birth and death of the individual. When I view
all beings not as special creations, but as the lineal descendants of some
few beings which lived long before the first bed of the Silurian system
was deposited, they seem to me to become ennobled. Judging from
the past, we may safely infer that not one living species will transmit its
unaltered likeness to a distant futurity. And of the species now living
very few will transmit progeny of any kind to a far distant futurity; for
the manner in which all organic beings are grouped, shows that the
greater number of species of each genus, and all the species of many
genera, have left no descendants, but have become utterly extinct. We
can so far take a prophetic glance into futurity as to fortell that it will
be the common and widely-spread species, belonging to the larger and
dominant groups, which will ultimately prevail and procreate new and
dominant species. As all the living forms of life are the lineal descend-
ants of those which lived long before the Silurian epoch, we may feel
certain that the ordinary succession by generation has never once been
broken, and that no cataclysm has desolated the whole world. Hence
we may look with some confidence to a secure future of equally
inappreciable length. And as natural selection works solely by and for
the good of each being, all corporeal and mental endowments will tend
to progress towards perfection.
It is interesting to contemplate an entangled bank, clothed with
many plants of many kinds, with birds singing on the bushes, with
various insects flitting about, and with worms crawling through the
damp earth, and to reflect that these elaborately constructed forms, so
different from each other, and dependent on each other in so complex
a manner, have all been produced by laws acting around us. These
laws, taken in the largest sense, being IGrowth with Reproduction;
Inheritance which is almost implied by reproduction; Variability from
the indirect and direct action of the external conditions of life, and
from use and disuse; a Ratio of Increase so high as to lead to a Struggle
for Life, and as a consequence to Natural Selection, entailing Diver-
gence of Character and the Extinction of less-improved forms.j Thus,
from the war of nature, from famine and death, the most exalted object
which we are capable of conceiving, namely, the production of the
higher animals, directly follows. There is grandeur in this view of life,
with its several powers, having been originally breathed into a few
forms or into one, and that, whilst this planet has gone cycling on
according to the fixed law of gravity, from so simple a beginning
endless forms most beautiful and most wonderful have be.en, and are
being, evolved.
192
------1l PART T H R E E ! - - - -
EVERYDAY TEXTS
(WRITTEN, SPOKEN)
EDITOR'S INTRODUCTION
195
EVERYDAY TEXTS
196
Chapter Seven
SOME LEXICOGRAMMATICAL
FEATURES OF THE
ZERO POPUIA TION GROWI'H TEXT
(1992)
Introduction
This paper presents a partial interpretation of the ZPG text (see
Appendix 3, p. 287) using systemic grammar. It consists of seven
sections: (1) Theme; (2) Information structure; (3) Mood and modality;
(4) Transitivity; (5) Clause complexes; (6) Lexical cohesion; (7) Nom-
inalization and grammatical metaphor. 1
I have used the suggested numbering of the portions of the text, in
30 segments. Of these 30, Segments 1-3 and 24-28 (a total of 8),
while they have distinct functions in the structure and format of a
letter, have almost none of the features of a clause; they are '.'claus-
ettes" in the terminology of the Nigel grammar (Matthiessen 1985,
pp. 1_02-3).
197
EVERYDAY TEXTS
xj3 a
1r----~-------~=2
1~~-~=2
1 Theme
Table 1 shows the Theme of each ranking clause, including textual,
interpersonal and topical (experiential) Theme. Whatever is not shown
198
LEXICOGRAMMATICAL FEATURES: ZERO POPULATION GROWTH
199
EVERYDAY TEXTS
200
LEXICOGRAMMATICAL FEATURES: ZERO POPULATION GROWTH
before with your contribution, and now in § 29 just before the final
segment. In § 30, for the only time in the letter, the writer uses I; and
the thematic move from 'I' to 'you' in I hope you'll lzelp gives the
required personal flavour to the carefully contrived little postscript.
The thematic movement of the text clearly bears out Fries' interpre-
tation of clause Theme in terms of the rhetorical concept of "method
of development" (Fries 1981, 1994, 1996, 1997). We could diagram
this movement as in Figure 2.
201
EVERYDAY TEXTS
time
public
us
public
U.S.T.<us us
your help
U.S.T.
your money
national government
us
elected officials
----------public
you (give)
I
your money
U.S.T.
I
~
you
Figure 2 Thematic progression
202
LEXICOGRAMMATICAL FEATURES: ZERO POPULATION GROWTH
& both e/lected of/ficials and the A//3 merican / public //1
need the popu/lation stresses / data re/vealed by our / study //
22 II 3 please make a / special contri/bution to /1 zero popu/
lation growth to/day//
23 //3 what/ever you / give //3 twenty five dollars/ fifty dollars
&
203
EVERYDAY TEXTS
The transcription shows what in one possible reading are the prominent
motifs within the New.
In this reading, most ranking clauses required more than one tone
group; this is predictable, since the text as a whole is clearly more like
writing than like speech. Where this effect was simply due to high
lexical density the pattern was the one mentioned above: a sequence
of tone 3 (low rising) followed by a final tone 1 (falling). Since tone
3, among its other functions, is also the listing tone in speech this
pattern also occurred where the clause incorporated a list. (e.g. § 6).
In other contexts, where a segment consisted of more than one tone
204
LEXICOGRAMMATICAL FEATURES: ZERO POPULATION GROWTH
group the pre-final tone was usually tone 4 (falling-rising); this is the
characteristic tone where the Theme carries a separate tone group (as
it often does, especially when a marked Theme), and also where there
is a rising dependency in the clause complex (that is, j3 followed by
a; these two are actually the same phenomenon, since where the ~-
clause does come first it is thereby marked as thematic in the clause
complex).
So if we want to establish what it is that emerges as the overall
information structure of this text, the motifs that make up the "main
point" as it unfolds, we can examine two sets of instances: (a) the
unmarked tonics (i.e. those which are clause-final - here mainly also
final in the segment), which select tone 1; and (b) the tone 4 tonics,
which are either marked (not clause-final) or on clauses that are not
final in the segment. Those of type (a) will directly reveal the main
point; while those of type (b) will express highlighted Themes, some-
times involving a feature of contrast (e.g. national vs. local in § 19),
against which the main point is explicitly foregrounded. We can ignore
tonics on tone 3. (Tone 13, which occurs rarely here, can be treated as
a variant of tone 1.)
The story told by the tone 1 tonics is that the text begins by taking
the 'wow!' of publicity as its main point(§ 4-9). Note that the relevant
unit is the whole element of clause structure on which the tonic falls;
hence, in § 7, such an ovenvhelming response, and in § 8, nothing short of
incredible. It then moves on to public health and the cities{§ 9-12), and
from there to the sense of crisis and appeal for help (§ 14-18). The
next five segments re-enact the two previous points with slightly
different emphases: the quality of life on a more local level(§ 19-21),
and the urgency of the appeal (§ 22-23; note tonics on today,
immediately, most). Finally the postscript returns to publicity (§ 29) and
then requests a reply (§ 30).
The tone 4 tonics, on the other hand, mainly highlight motifs that
we have already identified as Themes: us, our data, our meagre
resources, and our Urban Stress Test. Interestingly, the other Themes
- you, and the general public - turned out not to be highlighted in
this way. But, as already stressed, too much should not be derived from
one reading; especially a reading by someone who is an outsider to this
sub-culture.
205
EVERYDAY TEXTS
206
LEXICOGRAMMATICAL FEATURES: ZERO POPULATION GROWTH
207
EVERYDAY TEXTS
w e ~ public 4-9
we
public
II you
we~
14-23
we officials
we
you
m 1
30
----you
- with the Urban Stress Test ("our" test) appearing from time to time
as before. But the significance of Subject is rather different from that of
Theme; the Subject is the element in which is vested "modal responsi-
bility" - on which the validity of the writer's argument is made to rest,
in.each clause. This appears clearly when we a~d a response:
Staffers stayed late into the night. - Did they?
Media and public reaction has been incredible. - Has it?
We urgently need your help. - Do you?
It is even clearer in the imperative, where "modal responsibility" means
responsibility for providing the goods-&-services in question:
Please make a special contribution. - Must I?
Note finally that all ranking clauses have pos1t1ve polarity. The
message is a positive one; the only negatives are either ensconced in
the lexicon (ignore, uncontrolled) or else used in nominal groups to
heighten the positive effect (we had no idea we'd get such an overwhelming
response; reaction has been nothing short of incredible).
208
LEXICOGRAMMATICAL FEATURES: ZERO POPULATION GROWTH
4 Transitivity
Table 3 presents the analysis of transitivity, showing the process type
and the items occupying the various transitivity functions: the process
itself, and any participants and circumstances. These are shown for all
clauses, including those that are embedded, since embedded clauses
select freely in the transitivity systems. Embedded clauses are numbered
.1, .2, .3, etc. following the clause in (a nominal group within) which
they are embedded.
There are thus 55 clauses: the 36 ranking clauses already identified,
plus 19 that are embedded. The distribution of process types is given
in Table 4.
The clear preponderance of material processes (33 out of 55, constant
across all clause statuses) would suggest that the text is largely concerned
with actions and events; and so at one level it is. However, with the
doubtful exception of hold local offidals accountable, all these material
processes, whether middle or effective, represent actions which are
either abstract or, if concrete, then of a very general kind. We could
categorize these as in Table 5.
These portray a realm where the action is abstract, bland and
formulaic. Where there is agency, the Actor/ Agent is typically some
abstract object or process: calls or requests, a test or a study, pressures,
decisions and problems. The only actions performed by human Agents
are giving money, tackling problems, releasing results or completing
forms {there is also arming activists - with data). Where there is no
agency, and the Actor is just the Medium for the action or event,
human Actors preponderate; and their actions are vague generalities,
like planning, taking action, or making decisions and contributions.
Of the remaining 22 clauses, most (17) are either verbal or attribu-
tive. Thus one motif is that of verbal action, with seven instances of
answer, tell, hear, talk, report or warn. The ten attributive include the
four with need (see notes to Table 4), three with be (furious, critical,
incredible) and three others (have an idea, get a response, and criteria
rangingfrom ... to ...). Thus the motifs brought out by the transitivity
patterns are those of abstract or generalized actions, verbal events, and
attributes; the attributes being mainly prerequisites to such actions and
events, or their consequences.
All the four most frequent clause types - material effective, material
middle, verbal, and relational attributive - are distributed evenly
throughout the text; there is no clear pattern of movement from one
kind of process to another. This is perhaps partly why I felt that the
209
Table 3 Process types and transitivity functions
Clause Process Process Medium Other participants and circumstances
no. type
a-clauses 7 6 1 2 2 5 23
~-clauses 6 3 1 2 1 13
[[-clauses 8 3 3 4 19
21 12 3 7 2 10
Total 33 10 12 55
Notes:
1 The three clauses with make + Range (make decisions, make choices, make contributions)
are all interpreted as material: (i) make contributions, even if "unpacked" as contribute
(money), would still be material; (ii) make decisions, make choices could not be
unpacked here as decide, choose because they could not project: the attached clauses
are clearly expansions (x~ in planningfor people; = [[that could drastically affect the quality
ef our lives), whereas if the congruent fonns were mental they would be able to
project - as they could if the nominal groups in question were singular, e.g. make
the (sound) decision/ choice not to pursue the matter.further.
2 The four clauses with need (we need your help, the materials they need, the public need
the data, put the test in the hands ef those who need it) are all interpreted as relational:
attributive/possessive, 'must have'. Of these, the first could also be interpreted as
mental + projection, congruently we need you to help us; but given that help is
followed by a cohesive lexical chain support - contribution - contribution - give - $25
$50 or $100, it seems more appropriate to interpret this too as a possessive.
3 The clause to hold local qfficials accountable ... might by itself be taken as mental,
agnate to hold that local qfficials are accountable (' consider that ... '); but this is
precluded here by the preceding clause what they can do. In this context the
interpretation must be as material process, with accountable . . . as resultative
Attribute.
5 Clause complexes
As noted in the introduction above, eight of the thirty segments(§ 1-3
and 24-28) consist of clausettes; we shall leave these out of consider-
ation here, and consider the remaining 22 segments. These contain 36
ranking clauses; 13 standing alone (as clause "simplex"), 8 ~n 2-clause
complexes and 15 in 3-clause complexes.
212
Table 5 Material processes
abstract general
'make/be busy' 'do research' 'create/ affect/ do' 'give/use'
effective callsjam we monitor media test is created you give (money)
(Medium/ Goal; switchboard coverage
+Actor/ Agent) pressures affect cities we arm activists with
staff is swamped we release results data
with requests decisions affect lives
stress data are revealed (money) puts test in
resources are by study problems threaten hands of
stretched health
test ranks urban areas you can (give money)
you complete
form test translates data (money) is used
officials tackle
problems
they do what
EVERYDAY TEXTS
para tactic 2
There are thus 14 tactic pairs - since a complex of two clauses has
just one such pair, while a complex of three clauses has two. Table 6
shows the distribution by taxis and interdependency (logical-semantic
relation). The favoured type is hypotactic enhancement; within these,
four are clauses of Cause: Purpose, all non-finite with to: to use it well,
to take positive action, to make sound choices, to put the Urban Stress Test in
the hands of those who need it most. Three others are also non-finite: one
Manner, by completing the enclosed reply form, one Matter, in planning for
people, and one which could be interpreted as Time: Simultaneity
('meanwhile', answering questions . .. ) or alternatively as not enhancing
but extending ('and'). The other two are finite: one Time and one
Concession.
Three of the four purpose clauses are associated with need: to use
it well we need your help, to make sound choices . , . elected officials ...
need the population-stress data, [your money] will be used immediately to put
the Urban Stress Test in the hands of those who need it most. This motif
also turns up in one of the three embedded clause complexes (see
below):
the materials [[they need '11 to warn community leaders ... ]]
This co-occurrence of need (required possession) with purpose lends
some sense of urgency to the discourse; and this is perhaps reinforced
by the other two purpose clauses, which co-occur with can do, can act.
Again, one of these sequences is in an embedded clause complex:
citizens ... [[who want to know '11 what they can do x 11 to hold local
officials accountable ... ]]
while the other is the quaintly worded we can act to take positive
action . ... But although purpose is clearly the single most favoured
fonn of interdependency (six out of the seventeen tactic pairs), it is
never highlighted: all six instances have the simple non-finite form
with to, instead of a finite clause (e.g. if we are to ... ), or ~n explicit
conjunction (e.g. in order to . .. ).
214
LEXICOGRAMMATICAL FEATURES: ZERO POPULATION GROWTH
Clause no. Symbol Boundaries (first and last word, with conjunction(s) if any;
{seg.) = clause takes u~ whole ofse8!!!enQ
4 (seg.)
5 (seg.)
6A a staffers ... night
B 'Pl answering questions
C p+2 and talking ... country
7A 'P and we ... test
B au we ... idea
C a'P we'd ... response
8 (seg.)
9 (seg.)
10 (seg.)
llA a ZPG's .. « ... » ... cities
B «-P>> created ... research
12 (seg.)
13 (seg.)
14A 'P but to use ... well
B a we ... help
15A 1 our ... infonnation
B +2 and our ... limit
16 (seg.)
17 (seg.)
18 (seg.)
19A 'P even though our ... growth
B ua we ... act
C a'P to take ... level
20 (seg.)
21A 'j>a to make ... choices
B P'P in planning ... people
C a both ... study
22 (seg.)
23A (l whatever ... immediately
B 'P to put ... most
29 (seg.)
30A a I hope
B •pa you '11 ... coverage
C ~·~ b~ comeleting ... form
215
EVERYDAY TEXTS
6 Lexical cohesion
The main lexical bonding in the text, as is to be expected, is provided
by reference to ZPG itself and to the Urban Stress Test. Either ZPG
or one of its reference items we, our occurs in almost all segments in
the substance of the letter (16 out of 22, and also in the Address); the
U.S. T., or it, is mentioned eight times.
The remaining lexical items cluster mainly into seven sets, which
we could label "action, research, publicity, concerns, stress· factors,
216
LEXICOGRAMMATICAL FEATURES: ZERO POPULATION GROWTH
people, and places". The items entering into these sets are all nominals
(nouns or other constituents of nominal groups), and the chains are
purely lexical, not lexico-referential: the only reference item is such
in§ 7.
Taken together, the items in these nine sets (the seven above, plus
ZPG and the Urban Stress Test) account for 65% of all occurrences of
lexical words in the text (162 out of 250). The remaining 35% include
all the verbs and adverbs; high frequency and general lexical items like
idea, limit, trends, day, night; and items such as ovenuhelming, eager,furious,
drastic, outraged, remarkable, spedal, incredible where the linking motif is
interpersonal rather than experiential.
Figure 4 shows the pattern of occurrence of items from the major
sets in ·each of the 22 segments. Either ZPG or the U.S.T. (or both)
occurs in all but three (§ 8, 16, 20 - the our in § 20 is not referring to
ZPG); these connections are not shown. All other co-occurrences are
shown by lines labelled with the serial numbers of the segments in
question; thus, items from both "action" and "research" motifs will be
found in § 13, 18, 21. It will be seen that six out of the seven motifs
are strongly bonded: apart from "publicity - concerns", all possible
cluster pairs are represented in at least one segment, and the mean
number of occurrences of each such pair is three (that is, given six
clusters, there are fifteen possible cluster pairs, and the number of pair
occurrences in the text, by segment, is 45).
This strongly centripetal bonding, with each motif co-occurring
with all the others and with ZPG and the Urban Stress Test, tends to
keep the text in one place. There is a slight drift away from 'publicity'
in the first part towards 'action' in the second, with a brief return to
'publicity' at the end - the two being then cortjoined for the first time
in the prosaic appeal to help us monitor this remarkable media coverage by
completing the enclosed reply form. But there is little other movement. The
motifs of research, concerns, places and people occur throughout the
text, and there are no less than five segments in which three out of
these four occur together (§ 9, 10, 11, 18, 19; ZPG also figures in all
five).
The exception is the stress factors. These are mentioned only once,
in § 12; and they co-occur here only with the motif of place (184
urban areas), and with the Urban Stress Test, referred to here as it (the
only time it is not spelt out in full). ZPG does not occur in this
segment. This insulation of the stress factors from the rest of the
discourse keeps technical matters firmly at a distance; the effect is
somewhat paternalistic, with some such message as 'keep the funds
217
CONCERNS
stresses pressures
problems consequences
uncontrolled population
growth
population stress crisis
public health well being
quality oflife
1011181920
10
18 IO
19 II
19
20
891013 181920
rolling, but don't bother your heads with the difficult stuff - we'll look
after all that'. (Compare translates complex technical data into an easy-to-use
action tool.)
219
EVERYDAY TEXTS
we've made the test (the results?) easy to understand so that citizens
who are concerned about these things, and elected officials, and
people whose opinions count, can use it (them?) to get things
done.
14 But to use it well we urgently need you to help us.
15 We have only a small staff, and they've been swamped by being
asked so many questions. We haven't enough resources to deal
with any more.
16 You have to support us now so that we can survive.
17 The Urban Stress Test may be our best opportunity ever to get
people to listen to what we are saying about population.
18 We can use the money you contribute for sending out to the more
and more people who are working for us in different places all the
information (?) they need in order to warn important members of
the community that people are already beginning to suffer (?)
because of overpopulation, before this becomes a crisis.
19 Even though our national government still ignores what happens if
the population goes on growing without being controlled, we can
try to do something about it in our own neighbourhoods.
20 Every day local officials in our communities are deciding things
which will make a difference to the way we live.
21 So that they can choose properly when they plan for people (plan
for the population?), both elected officials and the American public
need to know what we found out when we studied stresses coming
from overpopulation.
22 Please contribute extra money (?) to ZPG today.
23 Whatever you give - $25, $50, $100 or as much as you can - will
be used immediately to tell the results to the people who most
need to know them (give the test to the people who most need to
. ?)
use 1t ..
29 The results of the test were reported as a top news story by
hundreds of newspapers and TV and radio stations from coast to
coast.
30 I hope you'll help us to keep a record of all this remarkable number
of reports, by filling in the form I'm enclosing for you to reply on.
When the text is approached from this angle three features seem to
stand out: cliches, ambiguities, and grammatical metaphors. As a final
step, we will discuss each of these in turn.
220
LEXICOGRAMMATICAL FEATURES: ZERO POPULATION GROWTH
(i) 'we are under pressure', e.g. calls jammed our switchboard, the
deluge of calls, swamped with requests, resources stretched to the limit;
(ii) 'we amaze people', e.g. met with an overwhelming response, reaction
nothing short of incredible, remarkable media coverage;
(iii) 'crisis threatens', e.g. threaten public health and well-being, your
support is critical, reach crisis stage, ciffect the quality of our lives;
(iv) 'arm and act', e.g. arm our growing network of local activists, act to
take positive action at the local level.
It looks as though all these cliches serve a function: that of easing
the somewhat awkward disjunction (which appears in the theme and
information structures; c£ Sections 1 and 2 above) between two
apparently contradictory messages: 'look what we've done - we're
great!' and 'we're puny and weak - so help us!' The use of wordings
which are so hackneyed that they have lost much of their experiential
meaning may help to paper over this contradiction.
(2) Ambiguities. Ambiguity and grammatical metaphor are closely
interconnected: grammatical metaphors tend to leave many semantic
relationships implicit and by the same token to neutralize certain
distinctions. Ambiguities arose in this text particularly in relation to
three issues: the nature of the problem, the status of the test, and the
kind of assistance requested:
.(i) (§ 10, 18) population-related problems that threaten public health and
well-being; emerging population-linked stresses ... reach crisis stage: is
it the size of the population that is becoming threatening and
critical, or other factors that arise from this?
(ii) (§ 13, 23) translates complex data into an easy-to-use action tool; to
put the Urban Stress Test in the hands of those who need it most: is it
the test itself, or the results of the test, that are to be made
available?
(iii) (§ 22) make a special contribution to ZPG today: are we being
asked to do something to further the aims of zero population
growth, or to give money to a particular organization?
There are other, less salient ambiguities; for example in § 8 and
elsewhere referring to the media response (is it the quantity of the
reactions that is remarkable, or their nature and quality - many people
responded, or people responded favourably?), and in § 21 in planning
for people (does this mean planning on behalf of the public, or planning
what to do as regards the growth of population?).
The word population in fact occurs seven times; and every occurrence
221
EVERYDAY TEXTS
from the vagueness of the last two, where the sense is 'having some
(unspecified) connection with (the) population', each one of these
instances is problematic. Population-stress data might be data about
the stress caused by the population, or undergone by the population;
the population message might be a message about how matters are, or
about how they should be, and either concerning or addressed to the
population; and even population growth, which presumably must
mean people becoming more in number rather than becoming taller
and fatter, is introduced in the contradictory context of zero.
Nowhere is the concept of 'too many people', overpopulation, made
explicit.
It could fairly be said that those to whom the letter is addressed will
"get the message" in all these instances and reject interpretations that
are inappropriate; and that it is of no consequence if they are misinter-
preted or not understood by others. But, apart from the fact that this
seems rather shortsighted - surely the message is one that is intended
to be spread? - there is a deeper problem with these rather oblique
forms of discourse: that they become so bland as to lose all impact. If
the grammar creates a universe of pseudo-objects, like easy-to-use
action tools, population-related problems, emerging population-linked
stresses approaching crisis stage and positive action at the local level,
then all of reality is held at a safe distance; and when these objects are
further disneyfied by the use of pseudo-intimacies like dear friend of
ZPG, our phones started to ring, our small staff is being swamped, with
222
LEXICOGRAMMATICAL FEATURES: ZERO POPULATION GROWTH
your contribution, and I hope you'll help us, the underlying message is
that life can go on beingjust one long Happy Hour.
(i) With your contributions, ZPG can arm our network ...
(a) Who are your and our? 'You' is obviously the recipient of the letter.
Assuming that 'we' are the writers, is this our anaphoric to ZPG or
not? [2] (It is, although the structure is unusual; it would be more
usual to write We at ZPG can ann our. ... )
(b) With your contributions: does this with mean "giving", as in with your
guns we can ann our militia, or does it mean "using"? [2] (It means
"by using your contributions".)
(c) Your contributions: is 'you' here a possessor, as in your property, or is
it a participant in some transitivity relation, like your role "the part
you play"? (The second; "you" is Actor in a process of giving, i.e.
"giver"). In that case, has the action been performed or not - that
is, is the giving in the past or in the future? [3 options altogether]
(The action is in the hoped-for future.)
Rewording: By using the money we hope you are going to give us,
we at ZPG can arm our network ...
223
EVERYDAY TEXTS
224
LEXICOGRAMMATICAL FEATURES: ZERO POPULATION GROWTH
225
EVERYDAY TEXTS
8 Summary
In this paper I have adopted a grammatical perspective on discourse,
interpreting the text simultaneously as object and as instance. By
bringing a text under attention we assign to it value as an object; this is
the activity which turns a text into "literature" (not thereby implying
'good' literature, of course). By bringing it under grammatical attention
we display its properties as an instance; this shows how it means, since
every feature it displays is located in the context of its alternatives.
These agnate forms, representing what might have been meant but was
not, constitute an infinity of shadow texts against which the one in
focus achieves its reality.
Among the grammatical systems instantiated in any text we can
identify some where the choices made are likely to assume significance:
very general categories of theme, transitivity and so on, and also
syndromes of features like grammatical metaphor. Selections made
within these systems will give the text its distinctive flavour and
determine how we evaluate it relative to its sociosemiotic context. We
could have examined other features, or explored these ones further -
indefinitely further - in delicacy; in stylistic studies we may be treating
of choices that are very delicate indeed. But we also have to make
choices of our own, especially that between depth and breadth; and in
the absence of obvious foregrounding of any one grammatical system
it seemed preferable to aim for breadth.
I have not offered more than brief evaluative comments, although
no doubt the wording of my own discourse construes an attitude
towards its object. What I have tried to do rather is to s~ow the
grammar at work creating meaning in the form of written text. We
226
LEXICOGRAMMATICAL FEATURES: ZERO POPULATION GROWTH
tend to think of grammar as simply taking its orders from above; our
own discourse has to make it strange, to show it de-automatized in the
text under review. The patterns we bring out create meaning both at
their own level and at each "higher" level that they realize - with
which they are related by metaredundancy, in the terms of Lemke's
explanatory model (Lemke 1984). I have concentrated on the meaning
of the grammar at its own level, trusting others to explore the various
realms of meaning that lie beyond.
Notes
1. For the grammar and its application to discourse analysis, see Halliday
1985a, 1985b.
2. The following symbols are used:
interdependency: logical-semantic relations:
parataxis 1 2 3 .. . ~ elaborating
hypotaxis a ~ y .. . + extending expansion
x enhancing
'idea
projection
"locution
3. The Subject is realized as that element· which, together with the Finite
verbal operator, constitutes the mood-marking element of the clause;
hence, in a declarative clause, the element that is recapitulated in the tag,
e.g. there in there's no danger ef that, is there? The unmarked pattern in a
declarative clause is that in which the Subject is also thematic.
227
Chapter Eight
First published in What's Going on Here? Complementary Studies of Professional Talk, edited
by Allen D. Grimshaw. Norwood: Ablex, 1994, pp. 175-229.
228
"so YOU SA y 'PASS'
229
EVERYDAY TEXTS
230
"so YOU SAY 'PASS'
2 Orientation
I decided to behave as a grammarian, since that is what I am, and I
have been studying the grammar of conversation for a reasonably long
time. The text thus became for me an instance of a particular register:
that is, typical not just of "text in English" but of text in a particular
functional variety of English. This is a semantic characterization:
functional variation, as embodied in the concept of register, refers to
the orientation towards certain areas of meaning, which can be inter-
preted theoretically as a setting of the probabilities in the content-plane
systems of the language. The register, in tum, is associated with a
particular situation type, particular settings of the parameters of field,
tenor, and mode, which in the present instance could be summarized
as follows:
231
EVERYDAY TEXTS
232
"SO YOU SAY 'PASS' ... "
233
EVERYDAY TEXTS
both the process and its outcome are critical to the effective functioning
of the text in the defined context of situation.
The subtext, in turn, has its own micro-structure deriving from this
culminative function that it performs in relation to the text as a whole.
The most general structure that we could postulate for such a culmi-
native subtext would be that of
(Problem • Resolution J Decision
- that is, a decision component either alone or preceded by the raising
and disposing of one or more problems. In this instance all these
elements are present, with perhaps three cycles of problem plus
resolution; but there is an additional element following the decision,
which would not have been predicted and which may or may not
figure as an optional element within this text type (the alternative being
that it falls outside it and indicates a shift of register). In the absence of
evidence either way we shall adopt the simpler solution and treat it as
an optional element that is part of the same text.
Within this there is a further layer of micro-structure which is still
reasonably constituent-like, though as the subtext proceeds it becomes
more a structure of interpersonal motif and hence more prosodic in its
realization. The "problem and resolution" sequence begins with an
enunciation of the problem, which is followed by proposals for its
solution; then there arises another problem, in the form of a challenge
followed by a defence. Focusing more delicately still, we can recognize
two proposals for the solution of the initial problem, and then two
cycles of challenging and defending. The "decision" component turns
out to consist of a decision proper (itself comprising enunciation plus
symbolic enactment - the enunciation incorporating a motif of "comic
relief'), followed by a summing-up. At this point the subtext proceeds
beyond the decision to a "post-mortem"; the post-mortem, in tum,
engenders another problem, which is, however, shelved, not resolved.
This post-mortem imparts to the subtext an incipient cydicity which
presumably could become severely dysfunctional; but it is inhibited by
an ex machina closure, and it is this that allows us to treat the entire
subtext as an element of the "dissertation defence", at least for purposes
of the present exercise.
The movement of the subtext is set out in Figure 1.
234
"so YOU SAY 'PASS' ••. "
235
I (upswing)
Problem • Resolution:
1 Enunciation of problem
2 Proposals for resolution:
J - proposal
S - proposal
3 Complication (challenge and defence):
time factor:
challenge
A- defence
scope factor:
challenge
A-defence
----=--
Decision
4 Decision proper:
enunciation of decision (incl. comic relief)
enactment
5 Summing-up
--=-========~
III (return upswing)
Post-mortem:
6 Problem
7 Closure
Figure 1 Movement of the subtext
"so YOU SAY 'PASS' . . . "
turns 18 and 20; Pat, turns 22-31). Each of these calls forth a response
from Adam in defence of his supervisee. The choreography is as
follows:
2. James: recommends 'pass'
but qualifies 'judgement defective'
Adam: counters 'has learnt something'
Shenn: recommends'pass'
but qualifies 'vision too narrow'
Adam: counters 'has to produce something'
3. James: challenges 'timing too slow?'
Adam: defends 'not really slow'
Pat: challenges 'coverage too restricted?'
Adam: defends 'not really restricted'
Adam has now countered all reservations and challenges, aided by
some temporary alliances (e.g. with Sherm on the question of scope).
He now comes to the hinge of the subtext, clause 109 in tum 32: So I
can tell her that - you know, I . . . , where the introductory so functions
as internal causal conjunctive: "as a result of (causal) the preceding
discourse (conjunctive) I conclude (internal) .... " The conventions of
this particular register require that the sponsoring magus should not
actually pronounce the magic spell. He can set it up, create the context
237
Tums Clauses
32 A- defence 100-108
4 decision proper
for it; and this Adam does, as far as the formulation of a projecting
clause I can tell her that . .. ; but he must not complete the projection.
What Adam does is to keep the formula open with a holding expression
you know and then wait for the visiting druids to come in with the
words themselves. This they do, in turns 33 and 34 - Sherm: she's won;
James: she's overcome.
Adam's role now becomes that of the jester; he provides some comic
relief Phases 1-3 have involved a certain amount of tension: not in
relation to what would be decided - the decision has already been
arrived at; but in relation to what processes would be gone through on
the way to its being adopted. Each of the interactants has to mould the
discourse in a certain patterning, to use it to achieve certain prescribed
ends; and this has entailed a modest degree of conflict - or at least of
incompatibility, such that they have had to manoeuvre, take up
positions, and monitor the direction in which they were going. The
resolution of the problem is at the same time a resolution of the
tension, an audible sigh in the discourse; and Adam adopts the textbook
strategy for such occasions - he tells a funny story, one that has himself
as foil and that is relevant to the business in hand. This role naturally
falls to the local druid: it is his novice who is being initiated, so it is
he who plays the clown at this critical juncture in the proceedings.
There remains the final symbolic act: the execution of the charter.
This is performed in a series of self-consciously casual little rituals that
are strung throughout the remainder of the subtext (turns 46-47, 76,
79-81, and 106-113). I have interpreted these as a continuation of
Phase 4 because they constitute the formal enactment of the decision;
but they do not in fact begin to appear until after the completion of
Phase 5, when Adam has gone out and then returned accompanied by
the candidate.
Meanwhile, in Adam's absence, the three visiting magi have proceeded
239
EVERYDAY TEXTS
to talk about the novice; and they have been doing so in noticeably
different terms - seeing her now as an initiate, "one of us", who has
duly performed the rites. They have discussed her future in the order,
the important work she has already been doing, and how to ensure her
continued participation. This has contributed further to relaxing the
tension, and might well have been the final sequence in the discourse.
But - perhaps partly prompted by the need to keep the text alive for
the symbolic enactment of the decision - one of the interactants,
Sherm, has by now become personally involved. His own uprightness,
and that of the institution itself, has been called into question by this
newcomer to the order; and he probes the issue in a lengthy exchange
extending over 57 turns, a total of 102 clauses. The debate rests
inconclusive; partly because it is extraneous to the matter in hand - it
cannot affect the main business of the text - and partly because in any
case it cannot be resolved without going and looking up the records.
Once again it falls to the sponsor of the candidate to de-fuse the
tension and bring the business to a close. This he does by again
providing comic relief; again, in fact, referring to the past, but this time
in very general terms: all those changes depended on how some mothers
behaved in 1937. Then, before anyone can come back to the topic
anew, he signs off, smartly, with an uncompromisable closing formula:
O.K. Thank you three muchly. This has the appropriate "coefficient of
weirdness" recognized long ago by Malinowski (1923/ 1959) as a
necessary feature of the spell.
In the next three sections I shall examine certain features of the
grammar of the subtext, interpreting these in terms of a systemic-
functional grammar. For a summary of the relevant aspects of the
grammar of English see Halliday (1985a). For an example of other
discourse studies embodying the same approach see Hasan (1985) and
Martin (1992).
240
"so YOU SAY 'PASS' •.. "
241
EVERYDAY TEXTS
242
"so YOU SAY 'PASS' "
but again allowing for the very short length of many of the turns; and
higher than the three other interactants, whose contributions are almost
entirely one-clause sentences. It is the roles in the development of the
discourse, not the individual qualities of the speaker, that are being
realized through the lexicogrammatical organization of the sentences.
When Adam's role shifts to that of light relief, in Phase 4, his clause-
to-sentence ratio goes up again to 2 (15:7): not as high as when he is
defending the candidate, but higher than when he is largely uninvolved.
At the same time the dominant form of interdependency becomes
parataxis, instead of the more structured hypotaxis of the defensive
passages.
The number of instances is not large, and these figures could arise
by chance. But it is a general principle of spoken discourse that the
more spontaneous, un-self-monitored the speech, the more intricate
the tactic structure of the sentences; and it seems likely that this is what
is being reflected here, as summarized in Table 5.
Thus, as expected, if we simply measure the overall intricacy of the
sentences - their mean length in terms of the number of ranking
clauses - this is primarily determined by the mode: where turns are
short (that is, the mode orientation is dialogic) the sentences will
average out as less intricate, because many turns will consist of one
clause only. But if we break the text up according to mode orientation
into its predominantly dialogic and predominantly monologic portions,
and consider these separately, then within each mode the intricacy of
243
EVERYDAY TEXTS
244
"so YOU SAY 'PASS'
it will be
probably
ositive
ositioll P
proP ative
·tiOll r,eQ
propos1
probably not
it won't be
(= not probable)
'probable that ... not' = 'not probable that', 'supposed not to .. .' =
'not supposed to ... ') and two outer values where it is skew: high
('certainly'/'required to': 'certain that ... not'= 'not possible that .. .',
'required not to .. .' = 'not allowed to .. .') and low ('possibly'/
'allowed to'; 'possible that ... not' = 'not certain that .. .', 'allowed
not to .. .' = 'not required to .. .'). There are other, more delicate
distinctions within these three, and there are also other dimensions
joining positive to negative; but these can be ignored for purposes of
the present analysis.
In Phase 2 of the subtext the distribution of modalized clauses is as
shown in Table 6.
The numbers are small; but this does represent a rather high
concentration of modalities in relation to the subtext as a whole (and
apparently also in relation to the remainder of the text). No distinction
is being made here among the various sub-systems of modality: thus
they're probably away, it's probable/likely they're away, they're likely to be
away, I think they're away, are all being taken together as instances of
"median probability"; and so on. These all embody more delicate
semantic distinctions; but the text is too short to warrant our treating
them apart.
245
EVERYDAY TEXTS
you should do
supposed to
required not to
you mustn't do
(= not allowed to)
supposed not to
you shouldn't do
(= not suppos~ to)
246
"so YOU SAY 'PASS' .•. "
247
EVERYDAY TEXTS
248
"so YOU SA y 'PASS' . . . "
249
EVERYDAY TEXTS
250
"so YOU SAY 'PASS' .•. "
251
EVERYDAY TEXTS
252
"so YOU SAY 'PASS' ••• "
the Theme: I and you become the primary motifs in the development
of this new phase of the discourse. Then follows a passage in which
other persons come in as topical Themes: Ann, Jennifer, and so on,
serve as examples of the topic under discussion. For the final movement
of this phase, the Themes are once again I and you, but now
interspersed with impersonal it, that, there, as Sherm and Lee manoeuvre
towards an acceptable compromise: one which involves an elaborate
thematic interplay between themselves and sections of their own
ongoing discourse - for example, it referring to what you're implying,
that referring to Ann eventually taught too, and so on. As a coda, Adam
comes in once again with a nominalized Theme what's really critical; this
puts all their Is and yous and its and thats to shame - and so has to be
softened with the aid of a Rheme that is yet another piece of light
relie£ Then, before Sherm and Lee can continue - or else both turn
on him -Adam closes the session.
Adam began this closing Theme by means of the continuative well -
a form of textual Theme, as noted earlier. This typically introduces a
new direction, either predicted (e.g. 'I'm now answering the question
you've just asked me') or unpredicted; here, it is what enables Adam
to get his overriding move accepted by the others. An interesting
feature of this subtext is how small a range of nontopical Themes
occur. A number of the modalizations in Phase 2 were thematic (6 out
of 9 of James', 5 out of 13 of Sherm's; c£ Section 6 above), showing
James and Sherm using 'in my opinion' as a method of development;
but other than these, interpersonal Themes were extremely rare. As far
as textual Themes are concerned, there were some continuatives
(mainly well and oh) at critical points, and the usual paratactic and
hypotactic conjunctions such as and, so, then, but, if, because, and that,
which have to be thematic if they are there at all (and hence appear in
every clause complex); but hardly any conjunctive expressions - in that
sense, along those lines, that is to say were almost the only ones to occur.
And within the topical Themes there are scarcely any circumstantials
(time, place, manner, matter, cause), such as are very frequently found
in many other varieties of text. The subtext studied here is developed
almost entirely around two motifs: first, the (then absent) candidate and
her work, and second, the interlocutors themselves and their own
discourse on the issue that concerns them. The exception throughout
is Adam, who uses the potential of thematic variation to bring about
the resolutions, the transitions, and finally a conclusion to the discourse.
253
EVERYDAY TEXTS
9 Summary
So to my own conclusion, which must be (like Adam's) brie£ My aim
has been, in intention at least, a modest one: that of showing the
language at work in the creation of meaning. A text is created by its
context, the semiotic environment of people and their activities that
we have construed via the concepts of field, tenor, and mode; it also
creates that context. The relationship that we refer to as realization
between levels of semiosis - situation (doing) realized in semantics
(meaning), semantics realized in lexicogrammar (wording), and so on -
is a dialectic one involving what Lemke (1984) interprets as n-order
metaredundancies. A semiotic event is an event on many levels.
In this chapter, the focus has been on wordings: on patterns of
selection at the lexicogrammatical level. It is important not to think
of the lexicogrammar as a kind of outer clothing, as the "expression" of
meanings that somehow pre-exist the text. There is no prior meaning
that is then encoded into wording: there can be no dissertation defence
(and, needless to say, no dissertation) except as lexicogrammatical
events.
In looking at patterns in the wording I have of course been highly
selective; I have said nothing about cohesion, mood, transitivity,
information structure, tense, grammatical metaphor, the verbal and
nominal groups, or derivational morphology, nor about the lexical
choices that represent the more "delicate" regions of the lexicogram-
mar. All these and other systems would have repaid equally detailed
analysis, and more: a rich interpretation of the grammar of any text
would itself constitute a text of vastly greater length than the original.
I chose to discuss one or two systems that related rather clearly, as it
seemed to me, to the functions of the text in its particular context of
culture. The text before us - and the subtext picked out for closer
attention - exemplified the power of discourse to change the environ-
ment that engendered it. In particular, Lee emerges from the text as a
changed persona; and in the process, her relation with all the others has
undergone a change. We might want to think of the entire text as a
kind of expanded performative: 'We dub thee PhD.' But this would
obscure a more fundamental point, which is that every text is perfor-
mative in this sense. There can be no semiotic act that leaves the world
exactly as it was before.
254
APPENDIX 1
TRANSCRIPTION OF "SUBTEXT"*
Legend
// tone group boundary (always also foot boundary)
/ foot boundary
" silent beat
bold location of tonic prominence (focus of information)
pause (sufficient to disrupt rhythm)
incomplete clause
255
APPENDIX I
256
TRANSCRIPTION OF "SUBTEXT"
257
APPENDIX I
258
TRANSCRIPTION OF "SUBTEXT"
33 s. II 1 • she's I won //
34]. II 1 . she's / over/come II
35 A. II 1 yeah // 1 • I / told her the I story· of when I / took my /
orals // 1 • it was / big Jack / Bismark and // 1 Jedley /
Still/well and // 1 they came / out of the / oral and / Still/well
says / you're going to / have to / tell him / Jack and // 1
Bismark says oh / no he says / go on I you tell him / Jedley //
1 • it was / this whole I business // 1 • she / already / knows
so there's // 1 no I point //1 • let me - / • let me / go / get
her and // 1 bring her / in II
36 P. II 1 yeah it was an I excellent I job I / think II
37 A. II 1 + got to/ figure out I how to get/ out of here II
38 S. II 1 someone/ help/ Adam//
39 P. II 1 + she's been/ teaching new / courses // 1 + too / every se/
mester // 2 hasn't she II
40 S. II l yeah// 1 • she/ has// 3 • they/ teach I/ think nine/ hours
II
41 ]. II 1 . for a / while she was in/ danger of/ losing her ... /slot//
2 wasn't she //
42 P. II 2 • well / wasn't the po/sition o/riginally just a / one year po/
sition //
43 S. II 4 • yeah it / wasn't ...• it I wasn't that / she was in / danger
of/ losing the po/sition it was that the po// 1 + sition was in//
1 + danger of being/ lost//
44 ]. II 1 yeah //
45 S. II 4 . and in / point of/ fact I // 1 think it was / opened on the
II 13 basis of an ap/peal which / says // 1 this is just too /
valuable a / person to / lose she's an // 1 + absolutely / first
rate // 4 teacher // 1 + really / very very / good // 4 • I /
must say that her II 4 nineteen sixty/ eight story// 1 + shocks
me//
46 A. II 1 • well we I have something for you to / sign //
47 L. II 1 couple of/ somt;things //
48 S. II 1 Adam there/ had to be I female A I l's pre/ nineteen sixty/
nine I // 1 just don't be/lieve ... // • I spent the/ first -
49 P. II 2. you were in I shock/ there for a/ while II
50 A. II 4. there were ...• there were I certain I people who were .. ./
TI A's II
51 S. II 1. but the/ A / I's //
52 A. II 3 Carol/ Collins// 3 uh I Debbie/ what•s her name II
53 ]. II 1 Sue I Phillips II
259
APPENDIX I
260
TRANSCRIPTION OF "SUBTEXT"
** *** *
83 S. II 1 that's not I true because // 1 I did // 3 • when I Mary went /
out-
84 L. II 4. when/ you came/ in// 1 they went /out//. but you see
/ she was / looking about -
85 S. II 2 • be/fore that / time //
86 L. II 1 yeah // 1 • be/fore you were / in there II
87 A. II Ann-
88 ]. II 1 Wilder //
89 L. II 1 Lowell//
90 S. II 1 Wilder //
91 A. II 2 • did / she / teach // • I / thought she -
92 S. II 1 no there's / this - / • there's a / further as//1 sertion //
93 L. II 1 Ann e/ventually / taught // 1 too // 1 yes // 1 • but / that
just is / not des/ criptive of the I situ/ ation any / more and ...
94 S. II 1 • I / know but I ... / frankly I don't / like what's being // 1
said // 1 it / bothers me it dis// 1 turbs me // 1 • in the sense
that I pre/fer not - ...• to be/lieve it's not / really / true //
95 L. II 3 o I k // 1 fine //
96 S. II 1 + therefore I shall / look for I evidence that it's / not true //
97 A. II 5. and/ he'll/ find it god/dammit// 4 he knows/ how to be
a / soci/ologist //
98 S. II 13 • and uh /. it / may tum I out that I won't / find that /
evidence and// 1 then I'll / really be up/set II
99 L. II 4 well you / may / find it // 4 • and / if you / do // 1 look for
/ marriage as the //1 • differ/entiating / factor//
100 S. II 4 • but I if I / do find it I -
101 P. II 1 + have an un/usual ef/fect then if/ that's the/ case //
102 L. II 1 + • well / I've never been / able to / sort out the dis//4
crimination I've re/ceived as a / woman from the dis// 1
crimination I've re/ ceived as a / single / person // 1 • they get
/ inter/twined and they // 1 inter/act with each / other /
sometimes / /
103 A. II 1 well and there / may be / others / too // 1 other / kinds of
dis/crimination as/ well// 3. you I mentioned I age and// 1
that's that's / clearly a -
261
APPENDIX I
104 L. II 1 oh yeah//
105 S. II 1 yeah but // 1 that would / not have been / • / relevant //
106 L. II 1 this is / not for I signature // 1 wait a minute // 1 wait a
minute/.// 1 this was/ just a/ double/ copy// 1. I/ wanted
a / copy of I what I /did//
107 S. II 2 • then it's / this and // 2 this just the // 2 two things //
108 L. II 1 yeah//
109 A. II 2 .just/ two things//
110 L. II 1 one is the / abstract / 1 yeah // 3 one is the / abstract and
// 1 one's the ac/ceptance //
111 S. II 1 that will be /. pre/sumably / duplicated // 1 • you'll need/
112 L. // 1 yes // 1 • and they / will ac/ cept the / xeroxing / of it //
113 S. II 1 + good they// 1 +save/ signatures//
114 L. // 4 • well / that / may have been a / personal / thing but the //
4 reason/ given wasn't/ based on/ personal-
115 S. II 4 • in / that sense it's // 1 + interesting how the / • / climate
and / circumstances have / changed / that is to say //4 if in/
deed there were ... a/ personal reaction/ negative the// 1 last
thing in the / world they would be / rationalized in / terms of
would be ...• you know the //1 • fact that it was a/ female or
/ whatnot at// 4 this point//
116 A. II 1 • well / what's really / critical is that// 4 all those/ changes
de/pended on// 1 how some I mothers behaved in/ nineteen
thirty / seven // ... // 1 o / k // 1 thank you / three // 1
muchly//
262
APPENDIX 2
ANALYSIS OF "SUBTEXT"
Legend
CLAUSE
[n} clause broken off and not counted
[[ rankshifi:ed (embedded) clause
n- clause uncompleted but counted
CLAUSE COMPLEX
a /3 r ... hypotactic
1 2 3 ... paratactic
= elaborating
+ extending
l expansion
' idea
"locution
}
projection
Xenhancing
MOOD/POLARITY
declarative, interrogative: WH-, interrogative:yes/no, imperative, minor,
non-finite
positive, negative tagged('+ tag'), untagged (others)
MODALITY
modalization: e.g. modulation: e.g.
I think it is subjective, explicit I want you to
it may be subjective, implicit you should
it probably is objective, implicit you're supposed to
it's likely it is objective, explicit it's desirable for you to
263
APPENDIX 2
264
ANALYSIS OF "SUBTEXT"
11 JI
2.. '.
fl 11 fUftk.r1o,/ lt~WI(
S'tnFT
'" C&Allr. (EMllmltla f~l'IMI. I
1l ~"' Q>Mh.C.)(.
T·,_,E° M E
'"TOPICAi.
MOO.I) I
I A , I ; so you... ~
.1. J" l. ~r
' A
s
7
b
g
} o(
'fi
w&U.
~
IJLUUC I
1
d.,.d.,-
~
'
MAAor
I~ }
I
=II
J.""~
~
""'t,;..r
a. .... ~
dt.,J.r
l
Ito "l. I\O~I\A., ~
I l ~ ....
.• lo.. \ ~
17 '[ 1.11.J-
• tN':lo)ff
IS-- +.,i b..r """"""
11..d'" clu.l.r
i'I l.~ ~ duw
q s ).j)-
~rvt.l-.~. ~
k..tq-
·1F1 "l. ~
A..,
3
c!ul.r-
ro---... il&J.r-
l).1)4.1.. uJ::rjn
10 T LI .....,..or
.u.. J,...,.. t 'j1..;J.. ~ ~
II s '-,,3 .......,or
lt. j' l't ._;,._.,.
265
APPENDIX 2
floUIRITr I_ _ ..,...,
....
••
~ .D
•"TAAAI
• l
..._•. -- -
I T V
- -....WS'ITIVITY
pos vl,L
~DS
~OS
~o,
~
~-, \o..:) s-~/ittJ
r<J.
~
-cJtr/,oSs hl6'.t':~j
"bt
i,or MuU-.1\
~I"
r-e.l iAA./~ twlW':toJ
~oS 111\0.1:'
½,k. objjir+
-~
~OI
re.l oJtrjv.t
r 0$
oS
)'os
)caos
~
~k
a,.1.j/Ulf'
~-
o',/•'"1' ........i;..,.,__
t..J.J/~ rJ..
rd
~/i-J, "b'-/ef
-'~/i"'f, v1,.l/cf'"
v1.l
•Jv.r
oJtr/iJ
~ ~k rvJj/~ -ar
~OS .rel. CtL/u.r
~ ~
J,,os vl,L
~SIS .... ~ s..J.jJL,+
.......,..
~OJ
266
ANALYSIS OF "SUBTEXT"
al
~ 'lA!flC
IAIOJt1JS€
o»tl'I.Ck
at1PT
~aw
1HE'M £
ioPIUIL.
.-.ool) I
2S'} )
I
(JI
ske;
"'-'
cu.J.r
ll'IOII•
II u. i
2.7 ;
>'.2,..
-t3
i;.,ui,o
~ t 11..iJ,.
~
K,.J-
d.a.cl.r
<:J.w.r-
i
l 13
:
! A :w-; II ~ l HwJ.. jw,r+~U-- ,:L.J..,-
r. .
I
'i ""II K...cr d..&,J,,.r
'"( "'-.a>- ~
.LC} =.i ti,.&., c:u...l.r
: JO; +J,.. ~t- t~ ~ c1.t.J..r.
' :ff i? CL8 S\....L
~
d,ad..,-
d,.a.,J..r
31 11-w\.or
!, } ,,,,
lct
;;i_, ~
l.J.·s
~
i~
t+O ~t al\Al
~}
i""ter-
41 +3 CI\Al l"i'V
~ k.aY d.ulJ-
'f 3 +.t.( o.,.; a,, d&c.1.r
44 . ~ ~ iJ- ~
=,r 1 clul..r
..a 5 .;.;;WH
~S'
wt.w
-··
a.:.hw.iw- ~II.I,,. cLw.r-
....t..(.
4Jo ~ ~ d.AAJ,,,r-
't7 ~ if cla.J.r-
"[ t).a.J..r-
WIG) """"fi...h.
""'"1,.;.y...
4-8"• d.a.J.r
4'f U':tk.bdt ;.. .......
.S'"o :
i
* d.e,..w-
[&"1] : ? 1·01t.J-
~I. I
~ ~
267
APPENDIX 2
M ~!IA &..l"T' Y
1bUIAllY
Mo- . ~""' ft&J• a'f', ,.a.r Tilt.A-"7'1 TPl•TY
__..._._ ,.,,,,,.L
~
*"-"" \/ilLIJC f~OU')S.Ytl£"
ttw1.r:oJ
J,•s b·e)wul
,-
~·'A ~
~/•"+ ~k r~-,..._ fftat
~
~I~ !'NIT
~OS
Po'
~- ~~ rJ..
vl.l
iJ../i#.t
ubl-
"'ji,o.s°s J.~--
por ~O.I\
~/fJf !MM":"!,
~°' vt:.l
~OS .,t(,
Joos v1,.t.,
;. , r...t. i.d../'v.r
~, _,y
_..
~liS
~OS ""-41"
r,J._ °""/iAk-
"°J
~&J n( clw/iAJ'
~ lfl>l/O>.&.L.J
,.,..s ,-t-
,.,9$ ,L,c.) s--:-t,' ;.-..., pu.Y' ~.~
>·· kf,1'. tfr>f/-r ~;~
~
ro, 1-..->
~I'"+ rd.
,.,.aJ'
id./.:,..;
vl..l
rs fo~
1-4jf---+ ......,..:~
.,...,.
"G/C'
tQS
l""':j/ito./' __...
,-...,,Y:~
t: SW:i/4¥f'
li,,..J,:-
,-J,:~J
~ ~ 61d/"'+ Ill......
268
ANALYSIS OF "suBTEXT"
-e if
-;_ :,.,..~ I 'Ii Rl1'C1'101f ~ T E" M Tu MOOI) e- I
~--- 'i>-
IIICLA\Jf£
C0"1'1.£X (<,,uDDI"") 78:rv& ~··~-~~ ioPIUU•.
I
SJ u.:e..U.
I ; 5'2-, ..... _6 cl.i.cl,,r
~I "'-·- 7 ~
fJ
a( iJ-- d..tJ,,.r
'g W-t'h.Yc.
'fl IA.,o,d..J-1;; IU-t"f..iJ''
:;r'~J
ri. l. M...:...&., 'J,- d.4J.r
I : 'I fw ku- ro,,-J,-..;J'e.
;I <l'o
N>"F
'"y j....t~ fl<>•• t'),.rt.
Lll ~ Pk.- <t.J;..r-
!~9 iJ c( l ~
,, ]
60. ~ ~ l ~ ~ do.J...r-
>fi ~
~ cL,<l.,.,--
62-.' o( ~ d.a.ct.cu
:ll IU'At~(.
·a ""'"~
I'S A 63 1
"'t ·'.f.u"<;1' wi. o.Y
d.o.J,.r
~:l.lH
{,~
<.'.?.l\.d. ·~ c.t.....t..,-
bb ".U I c4J.r
q 1(2.
"3
C!""-'"jrt l d.ul.r'
' 68' l ~
If::, s. 69
°"""
b...t' l J..r....l.,-
19 A 7i.. ~~ ~ GLu.t.r
73 I l~ K..L .•. 8 (U~
7't t:.t W\.lllff-a,...
·t3 sk.- d.o...lcv-
7{ CNA. 1'kt., c:lt.J..r
Jlo t4o< C>A).. l~"-' .:.r ~
77 jdt S"'--~ a-..., J.uw-'
' 7s- +b{_
,CU\4l cl.&J.r
' 7~ I jS a.I ~ W-~
&-o' =s- l ~ t~n..:JL "--- ~
269
APPENDIX 2
f:bLAIU"T'/ M o J) A- l. I T V 1A..Allf1"1T1V1Ty
.. .,..._..,., >a,,o,,/ iMdllUL.A 101'1
y,- •,~ 11.1£1mt'10H VALUE" -- ,A 9-..ocE""rs Tyf'c ,. ~.
~a.Y
~/t,<p rel.
~
oJH/~ ....,.J',"'j
rel_ ~1..~
.,.,.j' hJ...w
-*
rcJ.
r,,.,:>r
~
r-e1.
rJ.
r-l ~,~,. ,
i.d../ ,:,..r
c.JHjdtT..._
~~if~
roJ
},o~
res
~ r..l :all,.,/ 'v..r
~DS ~ o»:,-/WT--.
~D\ ~
r".s ~
~ dw/i.N'
~
~=:f"l
~
270
ANALYSIS OF "SUBTEXT"
ii~
t .,./~,,~
; I I1ifC. A»tc:.11orl S\t1F'T
Utltt,.
WNtll.Sf
•raM··- 'E,s,(.,,.,~ Tt:·Xfl.JAL
-r IJ E"'M
r
f:'
' inlllcAI-
Moqi) I
I
l..t
A:~}
;JN
'83:
I
+l,
bl,...t'
)eo1-.
c:.,vJ,.
oNl
t tL.:,J..
t it,..J<.- ~
I
~
»L.
__9
~
JaJ..,--
J..3 A I W
!l-l ~J.,
~
d,..,.1-r
~-
1J p q, oh.
~ claw-
<-7 A ll:i_
~ c:u.1.r--
li" r <t3
~ cl.uJ.ar
3o A <ts-- bu.i\.or
31 p qq- So
~
J-.c.l..,-
271
APPENDIX 2
lbc.*1'! .....
, ~
M t,j ,A- L I -r 'I
..... --, ......... _., "7 • 4..a/ .fl"7'1\/ITY
J,os reA I
GM.( i,J:;"
~
~t. . r-1
,..t.
~1,~ -.lo:~
t;"°' s-~,
~ ial. ~,..,.
loi.> ~ \tltt/i
IMdi- AJ.J/4ilf' r..L t:M, /:-.X
),01 rt..l o.ll,,/a
,.,_,y
Jiu~ M i"-/.;.J-
ros v...L c:J./i,.Y
~or re.A.. cliw-F...r
pos.
~.s rel. Cd../;,.;
rel oh-/i,¥
~
)to.s rel e.ec.i:W-
),o.s r..t. ~ff'
~ --.t':c..oj
J,o,,
~
to..:.
~I-+ ,.J..
~'"'..:)
IUc•*
"j n-1--
~ ~:'°-'
272
ANALYSIS OF "SUBTEXT"
i
~
Mold
Q2. d..ut.r
c( d...cL.,-
: fot.
''l03
loi,.
;1o<
~·=.t
IA.a
yov...
~--~1
cleJ-r-
d.tJar-
ciR.J.or
; 10S'
:10(..
;l°7
torr ..l
"Jo(
,
;aot
OI\A.~
W\,.__...
C,AJ}.
I,.
~
I.)
..... cl.c.w
Lnt: WH
l'loll~
da..W
:1 (14;•) ~ ,J.uJ..r
'"'t- So .r c:l..J.a,-
33 5· 110 ~ ck.lar
31f- J 111 st-,... ~
3:f" A 112 ~uaL r d...J...r-
I13 I ~ d.4.J.r-
-t-.2, 0-NJ.. d.ut,r
"l" +Jt 0-JVJ. ½
;:,h- ct.cw-
llfo '~ !:fO"'- c;wt.r
/'7 -+ 4, c1Jv!._ 8- ruct.r-
118' okn.o
~
(~
l~ ~
Ito} I ~J.r
Ill "..t. ~
:; } I
~ ~
I v
u-
Jlo p llft- a-- cletL.r--
37 4 12.(1 o( d.t.J.r-
IU ;e kow flOllr~
38' s '17 !oM&OA«.-- ~<tr
3~ p 12.i"" ~ ~
273
APPENDIX 2
'pos ~ ,-J.
},os
~/'* rel.
id.f,'J-
oJ/w,/,;J,
),o, vi,l,
~
t,~ i~ ~:oj
~oS ~k. J'~ ..-e! iA/;J puY
l_u,.s ~!'!j
ros ~
~ ~l. ~ ~
~
~
rel. cllr/d
'."!1
~o.s ~ rJ~J vJ.l
foS ~
Joos ~
ros ~bl
M..Y"
~
~
J vbl-
k"j~ ~~· vbl
vbt
~J
vbl
os r-el iJ../v,¥
/oos NJ"~~
nl t-,l iri-
ios n-.a>--
os ('f\D,Y
}ws ~QA
~~M rel oJJ., /v¥
),o.s '2Jk ~
]Po.s ,,_,....
,-J-:~
},os ~
rr·~ MOY
J,o, ~
~DJ ~t:11\.
r~fcr ~
t
~
+2. ~ l.uc.tM"
. 4-~ 5 o(
-.-.
,,........... r~ ,Jr ~
11, ]
117 ~ ~~ ~
i?i· ~ ~ d.e.J.r
13~
lif-0
l I
=-L
"I
~
~ :r
cl.uV
11+1 t,uul'ray ~ul<jl,I rk1...r-
j°"'-
~
"°"~
'1-7 L l1t3 ~
~1t s IS.2.. ~
j5"3 I ~
IS¼ ! cLut..r
.
SS'" L 1SS-
275
APPENDIX 2
fouliTJ .. _. ~. ,...
.,.... ...,.
A
-
-1\ .ii .f IT' Y
..-.. ••• ',-r.,,.,J
1t.... ,vS1T1'/tTY
,. - .
·"" Pll..«tiss 'T"ffl!-
"!:, rel ~1~
ros
~- rv-6,J/~ ,..,.y
E Vbl.
re.l
NJ--
t:
)'oS' ~ ~/41'
rJ.,,
rJ.
rtwll-:of
oAw/:iJ
cJ/w/;.,J-
t,03
0~
rJ.
l'\..Y
ros __...
poc tf ~/~ r.J. ~iw-
~ --.)";~
~=
f°'
~
rJ..
v-eJ;~
aJJr/J..r ,-.J-:":J
276
ANALYSIS OF "SUBTEXT"
Moo~ I
ss- I i~
t.2..( o.Nl. t~ d.uJor-
'"JOC...
J!, ~ ~
:n. s I d..eJ..r
i ~
"I lloA-~
'57 L
n',,o°1 ~ t~ ~4N.. dit.J..r
lb I
. l&1..
/C.3
I
:b(
~,(
~
~k.
~
~
cLcJc..,
~
tt.
d.4c.t.r-
fa, s ~
sq L 1
,o s I"-,. 1 J.ut.r
bl L /IS '
l tt..:J... '1- ~
l,f:, l ""
IJo
·1
~
67 'j t71 ~
=( ~
'i' L 171.
. 173 } c(
~
4-
d&cl..r
clAcA.ar-
l7«t ~ ~ l ,J..uJ.r-
277
APPENDIX 2
~:~
rd. cJJ,,/.;,.r
wvu>::,
r..L oJI.//~ -.,.):""J
,,.._;,~
Joos fM.\J';c...j
~ ~OJ\.
~..,,, re.l i.J./~
~+f-~ ~-OJ\.
1.Jd~ r..l it1./vk
pos (~ i'fv.k
Yo-' ~
p~J rd.
Jc-as rel.
rJ.. cJl1 ·
E
~
tNJ""
p,,.,aY
,-.r
278
ANALYSIS OF "SUBTEXT"
,..c4
~
·1 i Ii PM/;ft«I C..AWtc.
ll'IQ.Hf' S)l1FT
CoMl\.0. 1 ~ ) TetT"llc..
I ._. C ,;_, E""
1/r..... Toflf<.tt-
Mooj) I
70 s 17"' I
i mM,O{"
177 : I. cte..kr
,7~ !} I ~°"3""" I dul..r-
/"fl ; "l.. a ~
:,
73 .J lfr'J huM>('""
fir s lit,- ~
JS L Ii;(° ~or
/Bf, cLJt. •:-t.r
19 ~ ~-- _12 ~
''l i ,--.-- ~ ciu.lAr
'j 18'&"'
l" 161
'4J\ U1!..-- f1't:y/n
clo WQ...' i..t-:yn
77 L t•t~
78- A 1,0 ttw..or
~]
82., S . Ii!' I ~".,._~... i.si....r. - - rkJ.Ar
"[ w#..sJ,' ~
'f'l,.~ bw-9 c.Y d...utM
<tt7 ri I d.. ~
l't8' •1., t ~
279
APPENDIX 2
fot,AIIT'I N ,,, D A I I T ·y
Ma8M I,J i'TIOI/ l'tQJ>CA.A'TION
T ~ R N" r, 'Tt "' 7't
...,,.
-. ,,,_
--··- ('to,css T"IPe l,_.faa• .,.J.
~
~:~
~~
~ ~~- tN>,)--
,-.y-
~OJ tWJY'
~ n.Ai"
~~
re.l oJl..,/""
,.J., oJl.rjpQS
~OS ~
~
11J.A.,
rel cJl,,/v,N
~=1
(NJ,: °I
"
280
ANALYSIS OF "SUBTEXT"
(! f ii !I °''"'"',r.,r
FIJllct1olll AA,h.
ll{(""'1t' (. SW•fT
UIIOAl(t:
,ME"MC
rrar'*- ,.:- · , -ro,,~
Mo«u> I
81 s l«i«t
,l.00
J ,o( o( h..J-
I
~
d.uLv-
Sit L
. 2J>t
;2..oz.. 1
; 2.0.3
,
lo,
11 wLu..
WkV\.
b
M~ d-&J.r
de.Jar
~
; 2'>'f .'
.
'tb( ~ YO(/..,.__ sl,i. db1..r-
\
~ s ·2r,( I ~
I
L lc>6
ff, b.
1 ~ul.~ I.JQCA. ~
'ii'/ A u,7 ~
tf 1 A 21,
.ti,<-
~ k..-
l ::~n
qi.. s 2.,3 I\C ~ ~
CJ3 L .11'f-
.t1S":
l \
ti. ~
M~
~
cAAJ..r-
~dar
s 2.u-. J l
""' 'tq '
•..t lJ""
~ t.
cJ,ulv--
chJ..r
1 ~ d.AJ.ar-
,1,,
..Ut
. .22.o
l r/. ,o(
'j4
i11l..,-.Jc.,i l
a,
\k'
\¥
~
~
clcJ..r
<4Ja,-
' .22.t
"~ s :~ 1 o(
;1 tr I
~
cl.t.J..r-
,!..Jar-
281
APPENDIX 2
.-.-v
Pou11t.11Y
, ... _.. __
lt.1. . ,.C
-
_., ··~--"',.
~1\~,
~- L..
... -·-
•T,.~
.
1A...1Wlr'Tn/lTV
;-~·"'ll "TY,£"
.. , .
T
I rel oJt,.,/~
~
~o.t ,-r
~
i
E J ~
t,J....,
~OS ~
~QI -"":~
Joos rt-l. Q.ftiW"' uliL
J-o.s ..........
A!j ~ o).r/i.f\J' o11w/wc.
;o.J 41,u)-:~
,_...:.sJ.
~ ~I
"J..l.
N,.k:oJ-
lo-,k:;.
~~ .........: ~
rd. ~,.,,;.;
~
ros
~ eour ~:,...J
IUj ,.J. dk/,;..,-
282
ANALYSIS OF "SUBTEXT"
t
~ !I F.ltl0f1 o,J
IN Q.411,t'
Uwl(..
OftFT
C.OMf\.Cf°L (flff/£Jll>III(
M E"
MO'LO I
97 A vs 0-'\D k J.a.i..r--
2.lL } c( LA.. ~
21.7 h,ot,,)
J1 NII\·~
',Ir s i~}
.Zl..~
lo(
l
OJI...<{.
ff.-or
Ct
l.. ckJ..r
uo ~~ l ~J..r
~ l 231 } I ...,.d.l. ~
l.12 +1,_~ aA-.<tj ,,..,..,
,J.ac.L,,r
233 o(_
to, p
ZJS" J ol.. cl....L.r
l3b ;1 ~ Jh..,y <kdw-
lo3 A-
2JjYJ
l..3'1
21,o
I
~~
o.rvj.
~
s
~
clb.t.r-
~
c:1-a.J.,.r
.2..,, }
l4l-
I
·i.. OAd, ~
~- clot.i.sr
d-..~
283
APPENDIX 2
A.at,_ftALl,-V
A.IMl11 --A._t ,,&"'&.-J u .. auuT,o,J "T«.MS1i1\/rry
t;.,.,,.1,
"--~~ I
,o.r ,_,...
t=:
,,,,_.r;~
NLl o11,r/.;..;
ros ~ l~""t rel 1-d./i.J"
~ tkA1"
;os rJ. olH/.:..i ,._Jr=i
t..,..,
J.Jj/~
~
~
.,_r
~
f°'
,,.,J rJ.. c.Jlw/,-.u n,uJ'"
~ ~ _...:~
t= ,_,.,.
~
oJw/~
~: re.l
,._x
f°' .~
~h rd. e.,...iK
tcJ., 11!,L
,-.l
}oo.s
~ ~- ~i"'f ~ e.Jw/ia-
"°'
J,.0.1 rt.l iL/:J-
284
ANALYSIS OF "SUBTEXT"
-~
" fUlfCl\0111 R,Ja,Jt(
[ b,/t.lJW
Coll,a"-ft
SW1FT
ToP1c.t1.
~oqJ)
lot ~
Ill L lSB"
~
1/3 s is, ~
~o
~ dad.,-
,,,.,. L 261 } I ~ µ, ct.d.r-
. ~2:- t.,2. w " - - 14 dMw-
'"ll I\Otl;..a-..
,,r s
w}
u.,,.
1..€.S"
"t~
ri.
'I
lilL.t
~
lw-:.r.r:,j ......&ac.cl
~ tA.cJ.,.r-
-~_,15 dM.1.,-
....,...16 cJ..A.r
14-c. ... ~
"I
~
cl.uw-
c.g ~
._._ ..t..r
285
APPENDIX 2
Pol.AQny r...nOALJTV
,... _ .. _ , .. 1-r,,...J 1--.ft1U ..__ 1-""' IR. 9N f>TI /11Y
- t..£.,,.C
Iv.a. ,1.-.:- !311.nff,..,,o,,J , . , . ,,J{ ••·-'-,.- .I f to c.£U 7Y/ € -·
rs.r
~:
r' OJ
rel
,..J_
r-eJ...
,'d..j~
L',J_I~
1'd../v.I
~~
~QJ\ o~.v·"f ~
MW";-} W-: rest
~o! tDw 5~("" j,-.4,.Y)~j
~:
),os
n,,..w-
re.I..
reJ.
~i&t
iet./.:....r
rre,j:"'
pot ~ IN.J':tDJ
~~ rJ. o»v/;,.Jr
r~ re.l
re)_
i o/../.;...r
oJH/.:J,
...,.,.;:o;oj
~:""
~: <"d.
.',e4,./
c.Jw/wc. ~
286
APPENDIX3
287
APPENDIX 3
Your supprt now is critical. ZPG's 1985 Urban Stress Test may be
our best opportunity ever to get the population message heard.
With your contribution, ZPG can arm our growing network of
local activists with the materials they need to warn community leaders
about emerging population-linked stresses before they reach the crisis
stage.
Even though our national government continues to ignore the
consequences of uncontrolled population growth, we am ad to take
positive action at the local level.
Every day decisions are being made by local officials in our com-
munities that could drastically affect the quality of our lives. To make
sound choices in planning for people, both elected officials and the
American public need the population-stress data revealed by our study.
Please make a special contribution to Zero Population Growth today.
Whatever you give - $25, $50, $100 or as much as you can - will be
used immediately to put the Urban Stress Test in the hands of those
who need it most.
Sincerely,
Susan Webster
Executive Director
P .S. The results of ZPG's 1985 Urban Stress Test were reported as a top
news story by hundreds of newspapers and TV and radio stations
from coast to coast. I hope you'll help us monitor this remarkable
media coverage by completing the enclosed reply form.
288
BIBLIOGRAPHY
289
BIBLIOGRAPHY
290
BIBLIOGRAPHY
Fowler, R. (ed.) (1966) Essays on Style and Language: Linguistic and Critical
Approaches to Literary Style. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Fowler, R. (1981) Literature as Social Discourse: The Practice ef Linguistic Criticism.
London: Batsford Academic and Educational.
Fries, P. H. (1981) 'On the status of Theme in English: arguments from
discourse'. Fomm Linguisticum 6.1. Reprinted in J. S. Petofi and E. Sozer
(eds), Micro and Macro Connexity ef Texts. Hamburg: Buske, 1983.
Fries, P. H. (1994) 'On theme, rheme and discourse goals', in M. Coulthard
(ed.), Advances in Written Text Analysis. London: Routledge.
Fries, P. H. (1995) 'Patterns of information in initial position in English', in
P. H. Fries and M. Gregory (eds).
Fries, P. H. ( 1996) 'Themes, methods of development, and texts', in R. Hasan
and P. H. Fries (eds).
Fries, P. H. (1997) 'Toward a discussion of the flow of information in a
written English text', in M. J. Cummings and M. Gregory (eds), Relations
and Functions in Language. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Fries, P.H. and Gregory, M. (eds) (1995) Discourse in Society: Systemic Functional
Perspectives. Norwood, NJ: Ablex (Advances in Discourse Processes).
Garvin, P. L. (ed. and trans.) (1964) A Prague School Reader on Esthetics, Literary
Stmcture and Style. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.
Genette, G. (1975-76) 'Structuralism and literary criticism', in H. S. Gill (ed.).
Ghadessy, M. (ed.) (1995) Thematic Development in English Texts. London:
Pinter.
Gibson, W. (1958) 'Behind the veil: a distinction between poetic and scientific
language in Tennyson, Lyell and Darwin', Victorian Studies 2.
Gill, H. S. (ed.) (1975-76) Parole and Langue: Studies in French Structuralism.
Patiala, India: University of Patiala (Pakha Sanjam 8).
Golding, W. (1955) The Inheritors. London: Faber.
Gregory, M. (1967) 'Aspects of varieties differentiation', journal ef Linguistics
3.2.
Gregory, M. (1974) 'A theory for stylistics exemplified: Donne's Holy Sonnet
XIV', Language and Style 7 .2.
Gregory, M. (1978) 'Marvell's "To his Coy Mistress": the poem as a linguistic
and social event', Poetics 7.4.
Gregory, M. (1985) 'Towards "communication" linguistics: a framework', in
J. D. Benson and W. S. Greaves (eds).
Gutwinski, W. (1974) Cohesion in Literary Texts: A Study ef Some Grammatical
and Lexical Features of English Discourse. The Hague: Mouton.
Halliday, M.A. K. (1961) 'Categories of the theory of grammar', Word 17.3:
242-92. In Halliday 2002.
Halliday M. A. K. (1967a) Grammar, Society and the Noun. London: H. K.
Lewis (for University College London).
291
BIBLIOGRAPHY
292
BIBLIOGRAPHY
293
BIBLIOGRAPHY
294
BIBLIOGRAPHY
295
BIBLIOGRAPHY
296
INDEX
abstract 29,51,59, 107,111,114,150,209 choice 16, 23-4, 95-7, 99-100, 105-6, 127,
action 131, 167
social 52-9 ofkey 35-7
clauses 108-18, 173 semantic 48, 64, 80
actor 89, 110, 112-14, 156, 223, 228, 250 of theme 31-2, 80, 251
additive 40, 174 unmarked 31, 37, 80
adjective 172 class(es) 10, 12, 18, 42, 54-7, 63, 89, 95,
adjunct 111-14, 116 169-70
adverb 7, 24, 80, 112, 217 variable 247, 250
adverbial group 7, 24, 79 clause 6-7, 10-13, 16, 30-7, 46, 61-4,
adversative 40, 41, 174 79-81, 109-14, 117,158, 170-86,
aesthetic 103, 131, 187 197-216, 235-53
Agent 25, 31, 115-17, 228 clause-final 205
ambiguity 28, 220-1, 225 clause-internal 63
ambiguous 34, 60,158,223 clausette-complex 197
anaphora 7-8 complex(es) 24, 26, 64, 154-5, 160-4,
anaphoric 7-8, 10-12, 16,30,33,38,95, 112, 184-6, 197-8,205,212-16,232-3,240,
184-5, 223 244,253
apposition 7, 24, 62, 91 Theme 178-80, 201,252
art 50, 130 type(s) 49, 106, 170, 209
verbal 50, 58-9, 151, 177 code 63, 130
artistic 50,120,131 cognitive 91, 97
attributive 108, 113, 212 coherent 95, 151, 178
auxiliary 134, 146 cohesion 6-8, 15-18, 38-44, 57, 61, 186
axis 147 lexical 8, 16, 33, 41-3, 178, 197, 216
cohesive 7-8, 16, 30, 33, 39-43, 47, 56,
Beneficiary 228 184
Berger, P. L. 130, 148 co-interpretation 43
Bernstein, B. 52 collocation(al) 8, 13, 16, 19, 41-3, 182, 225
Braine, J. 9, 13-14, 126 communication 40, 91-2, 95, 151
Buhler, K. 89-92 communicative 63
comparative 6, 12, 18, 39, 63, 100, 118, 174
cataphoric 8, 10-11, 38 component
categories 6-8, 16-18, 57, 93, 119-20, 230 ideational 40, 53, 61-2, 119
causal 40, 174, 237 interpersonal 25, 29, 31, 35, 40, 53, 62
causative 113, 175 logical 26, 61, 80, 91
Cause 214 textual 25-30, 35, 45-7, 57, 61, 173, 186,
cluin 42, 49 250
chaos 177 conative 89, 91-2
Chaucer, Geoffrey 164, 170 conjunction{s) 43, 57, 152, 174, 215, 250, 253
297
INDEX
298
INDEX
299
INDEX
metaphor 19, 96, 244 past 47, 60, 105, 108-9, 112, 115-16, 145-7,
grammatical 160, 164, 196, 219-23, 226, 150-1, 158,160,165,181,183,206,223
233,242,254 periodicity 48, 50, 208
metaphorical 53-4, 107, 118, 206 penon 30,37,39, 56-7, 63,135,229,251
metaredundancy 227, 254 phonetic 6, 19,150
microsemiotic 52, 148 phonological 5, 19, 23, 27-8, 32, 35, 44,
micro-structure 233-4 45-6,49-50,88,98, 102,104,120
modal 25, 31, 36, 40, 134, 139 phonology 19, 23,27, 46,49-50,99, 169
modalities 96, 134, 229, 245, 247-8 Pike, K. 50-1
modality 32, 56, 79, 134-6, 139, 145, 151, poem 6,9, 11-12, 18-19, 52,88,98, 100,
197,206,232-3, 244-50 104, 152, 155, 160, 167, 178
modalization 206, 232-3, 244, 247-9, 253 poetic 8,162, 166-7, 177-8
mode poetry 53, 88,102,149,163,167,177
field, tenor and mode 54, 58, 60, 62, 64, polarity 31-2, 64, 79, 139-40, 145-6, 208,
152, 232-3, 254 233, 244
model 59, 63,150,164,169,174,227 possessive 7, 112, 208
modifier 8, 10-11, 14-15, 95, 111, 113, 115, pragmatic 90
222 Prague School 29, 92, 150
modulation 37, 61, 79, 134, 139-40, 232-3, predicate 102
244, 246-9 predicator 200
monologic 229, 241, 243 preposition 24, 32, 111, 113, 174
mood 18, 25, 29,31,35, 56-7,61, 63, 79, prepositional 32, 112, 114
139, 145, 151, 155, 156, 158, 171, 186, Priestley,J. B. 126-9, 131, 141-2, 146-7, 174
197,206, 232-3, 250,254 Prigogine, I. 162, 165, 167
morpheme 35, 42 probability 134-6, 139, 142, 146, 206, 244
most delicate grammar 79 process 25-6, 40-1, 43, 46-9, 51, 57-8,
motif 43, 50, 52, 63, 128-9, 139, 142, 150, 60-3, 106, 108-12, 130,135,140,145,
156-7, 161, 170, 177-85, 205, 209, 214, 151-2, 156, 163-4, 170,182,184,206,
217,233-5,247,249, 253 209,212,223,228,233-4,250
movement 13, 48, 108-9, 111-12, 115-16, mental 60, 95, 108, 111-12, 113-14, 115,
127, 137, 141, 144, 158-9, 162, 173, 175, 117, 135, 206
201,208,217,234,251 relational 63
Mukafovsky,J. 130-1, 147, 149 semantic 48, 53, 63
pronoun 7-8, 11, 14, 16, 30, 31
narrative 13, 36, 38-9, 41, 44, 47, 49, 52, prose 5, 9, 11-13, 15, 17, 49, 52, 88, 98, 107,
58-61, 96, 107-8, 136-7, 147, 151, 177, 118, 126, 130
200, 206, 251 prosodic 233-4
network 23-4, 30, 45, 64, 79-80, 96-7, 221, protolanguage 161
223-5
New 32-3, 61, 173-6, 178-86, 204
question 31-3, 36-7, 39, 61, 92, 95-6
Newton, Isaac 152, 164, 170, 172, 174
nominal 12-13, 111, 115, 175, 183
group(s) 7-8, 10-12, 14-16, 24-5, 30-2, Range 157
34, 79,80, 170,174,176,183,198, 208-9, rank 6-7, 10,24,27, 35, 46, 79
217, 222 scale 28, 46, 79
nominalization 30, 160, 164, 170, 172, 182, rankshift(ed) 7, 10, 12, 198
197 realization 26, 35, 45-6, 49, 50, 52, 59,
non-finite 116, 184, 206, 214 62-4, 79, 139, 142, 152, 160, 174, 234,
noun 15, 24, 80, 101, 111, 114, 172, 175-6, 244, 254
217, 222 recunion 91
novel 89,100,110, 118-19 recunive 24, 26, 64
referential 30, 38-40, 42, 149, 178
paradigmatic 24, 48, 51 relational 63, 170, 212
paragraph 8,38, 108,115,169,179, 184-5 representational, conative and 89
paratactic 24, 160, 184, 214, 242, 250, 253 Rheme 25, 30-1, 61, 89, 173, 178-9, 182,
parataxis 155, 160, 161, 164, 243-4 184-5, 200,204,250,251,253
see also hypotaxis rhetorical 55, 57, 92, 161-2, 178, 201, 204,
participant 44, 55-6, 61, 106, 108, 111-14, 250
119,129, 208-9,223,228, 250-1 rhythm 19, 35, 88, 185, 232-3
passive 113, 156, 185 rhythmic 104
300
INDEX
301