Unknown - 2005 - Birkbeck Eprints An Open Acce
Unknown - 2005 - Birkbeck Eprints An Open Acce
Unknown - 2005 - Birkbeck Eprints An Open Acce
http://eprints.bbk.ac.uk
Contact Birkbeck ePrints at [email protected]
1
Investigating the Psychological and Emotional
Dimensions in Instructed Language Learning: Obstacles
and Possibilities
JEAN–MARC DEWAELE School of Languages, Linguistics, and Culture,Birkbeck College, University of London
Email: [email protected]
Abstract
In this article I put forth the core argument that Second Language Acquisition (SLA) needs to account for the
psychological and emotional dimensions of second language (L2) learning, but that a number of epistemological
and methodological difficulties must be surmounted before this new research program can be a reality. To
illustrate my arguments, I examine in depth 2 research programs developed by my colleagues and me over the last
decade: research on extraversion as a psychological variable investigated within the tradition of individual
differences in SLA, and research on the expression of emotion in the L2. Throughout the article, I argue against
research isolationism and for more interdisciplinarity in the field of instructed SLA. I contend that research on
instructed SLA would benefit from an increased methodological and epistemological diversity and that a focus on
affect and emotion among researchers might inspire authors of teaching materials and foreign language teachers to
pay increased attention to the communication of emotion and the development of sociocultural competence in a
L2.
In this article, I argue in favour of a general broadening of the theoretical and methodological horizons in the field
of instructed Second Language Acquisition (SLA) by including the psychological and emotional dimensions of
second language (L2) learning in our research programs. I am convinced that the field needs not only more breadth
but also more depth and detail. With increasing breadth, we would see studies that take into account a larger
number of relevant independent variables—quantitative or qualitative—than are usually included in studies on
instructed SLA. More depth and detail would be attained in the field of instructed SLA if we saw studies featuring
samples that were sufficiently large with complete reports of means, standard deviations of relevant variables, and
a detailed description of the population, the experimental procedure, and the variables (cf. Grosjean, 1998),
including relevant psychological variables not usually reported in the SLA literature. This broadening implies that
findings in specific subfields should be communicated to the wider SLA community through presentations at
general conferences and publications in journals with a wide focus. SLA researchers need to be aware of
developments in contiguous areas that might benefit their own research. An awareness of the psychological
research on individual differences, for example, may help to get rid of the monolithic view of the prototypical
faceless learner, whose identity often disappears in gross group averages. This article is organized as follows. The
first part deals with a number of methodological and epistemological issues surrounding instructed SLA research. I
defend the view that the best way forward in SLA is through a mixed focus on individual learners as well as on
groups using appropriate research designs. I argue that research in L2 learning would benefit from broadening the
cultural and sociodemographic pool of participants and from combining quantitative and qualitative research. I
also warn against research isolationism and call for more interdisciplinarity in the field. The second and third parts
of the article provide illustrations of the previous points by referring to two growing areas of research in SLA in
which my colleagues and I have been involved for some time now, namely the study of extraversion and other
psychological variables as sources of individual differences in L2 learning and the study of the expression of
emotion in a L2. In the final part of the article, I point out that research in the language of emotion and the ability
to express affect and interpersonal language functions in general has important implications for L2 teaching. I also
raise some ethical questions about the inclusion of highly emotional vocabulary in the curriculum and the L2
users’ freedom to deviate from sociopragmatic norms in the L2. Before venturing into my discussion, however,
two important points need to be made about definitional issues of the research domain of affect and emotion in
SLA and the personal and professional affiliations and incentives I bring to this article.
With respect to the first point, it is important to realize that the domain of affect and emotion in SLA is situated at
the crossroads of sociolinguistics, sociopragmatics, intercultural pragmatics, cognitive linguistics, social
psychology, cognitive psychology, and cultural psychology. There is inevitably a certain overlap between key
concepts in these disciplines. For example, for Barron (2003), pragmatic competence is based on three types of
knowledge: “knowledge of the linguistic resources available in a given language for realizing particular
illocutions, knowledge of the sequential aspects of speech acts and finally, knowledge of the appropriate
contextual use of the particular languages’ linguistic resources” (p. 10). Kasper and Rose (2001) emphasized the
latter type of knowledge in their definition of sociopragmatic competence: “the social perceptions underlying
participants’ interpretation and performance of communicative action” (p. 2), as did Lyster (1994) in his definition
of sociolinguistic competence: “the capacity to recognize and produce socially appropriate speech in context” (p.
263). Dewaele and Pavlenko (2002) added a focus on nonverbal communication in their definition of sociocultural
2
competence, that is, the ability “to identify, categorize, perceive and engage in verbal and non-verbal behaviors
similarly to other members of a particular speech community” (p. 268). The slightly overlapping concepts reflect
the fact that many SLA researchers combine the different approaches, while also adopting aspects of interlanguage
pragmatics or intercultural communication.
Second, because a writer’s and scholar’s views are shaped by professional and personal experience, it is important
for readers to know the background of this writer. I would like to define myself professionally as a European SLA
researcher and a foreign language teacher who is concerned by both dimensions equally. I studied, taught, and did
research in universities in Brussels and London. I am aware that my educational setting has influenced my views
on teaching and research. I consider them as two sides of the same coin. Therefore, in the last part of this article I
will argue that good research into SLA is crucial for the development of good teaching practices. If not much
research is available, then not only do we not have enough knowledge to go on in terms of designing materials or
curricula, but also the legitimacy of such language areas as worthy of teaching remains problematic.
The learner is not only an object of scientific curiosity, but also a crucial witness of his or her own learning
process. Interviews with students after their study abroad (Evans, 1988; Kinginger & Farrell, 2004) highlight
important differences in self-reported social behavior of language students during their stay abroad. Although
some students use every opportunity to engage in conversations with native speakers of the target language, others
avoid contact outside their own linguistic community. This difference in behavior might account for the wide
interindividual variation in the amount of linguistic progress after such a stay abroad (cf. Regan, 2004; Towell &
Dewaele, 2005) and in the amount of metapragmatic awareness the students gained through interactions with
native speakers of the target language (Kinginger & Farrell, 2004). In other words, by carrying out detailed
qualitative research on a limited number of participants, patterns can be uncovered that could help in the
interpretation of quantitative research findings on larger population samples.
3
I believe a good alternative to a forced choice between quantitative and qualitative instructed SLA research is
triangulation, that is, the use of a combination of different research methodologies in order to answer common
research questions. In this approach, empirical and quantitative accounts can be combined with an emic
perspective, or participant-relevant view, “as a result of which the L2 learners’ and users’ voices and opinions are
heard on a par with those of the researchers” (Pavlenko, 2002, p. 297). Pavlenko also defended the idea of
triangulation:
I argue that poststructuralist studies, which see L2 learning as a process of socialisation rather than creative construction or
interlanguage development, provide new ways of framing the interaction between social contexts and learning processes,
which can productively be combined with more linguistically and cognitively oriented interactionist approaches in SLA. (p.
291)
Indeed, there is growing acceptance within the SLA community that learners’ feelings and reflections on their
learning process, language use, and changing identity offer valuable insights in aspects traditionally overlooked in
SLA. It would be impossible for every study to combine the different approaches. However, editors of books or
special issues on a specific theme could ensure that a variety of perspectives are represented.
4
condition. I am certainly not claiming that such an approach does not provide rich insights, only that by
considering groups that may be heterogeneous in many other ways, individual differences tend to be ironed out.2
Finally, difficulties regarding technical expertise in quantitative SLA research ought to be acknowledged. Certain
common research techniques, such as statistics, have become widely used. However, given that SLA researchers
(myself included) are often autodidacts in statistics, a great deal of the research lacks methodological rigor, is
based on small samples (cf. Lazaraton, 2000; Norris & Ortega, 2000) or relies on extremely 371 simple designs,
where one single independent variable is correlated with one single dependent variable (cf. Dewaele, 1993).3
Complex research designs require more advanced statistical techniques. Multivariate analyses and regression
analyses are increasingly used more widely. They provide SLA researchers with not only significance values (p)
for specific relationships but also effect sizes (e.g., eta squared) for multiple variables. More sophisticated
statistical techniques, such as path analysis,4 have been used successfully in SLA to determine the effect of
multiple independent variables on a single dependent variable (MacIntyre & Charos, 1996). The debate on the use
of statistics within SLA is situated within a wider discussion on the merits of various methodologies that belong to
specific paradigms. Indeed, some researchers remain skeptical about the usefulness of statistics and quantitative
research methods for the study of language. This view is reflected by the French linguist Milner (1989) who
argued that “aucune proposition importante de la théorie linguistique n’est fondée sur le décompte (not a single
important proposition in linguistic theory is based on counting)” (p. 586).
5
SLA researchers interested in individual differences can learn substantially from psychologists.5 Several journals in
psychology include the collocation individual differences in their titles, while to the best of my knowledge such a
journal does not exist in the field of SLA. As I started my doctoral research in the early 1990s, I became aware of a
relatively strong skepticism among applied linguists towards psychological variables. I was particularly surprised
because psychological research into individual differences in the performance of complex (nonverbal) cognitive
tasks seemed to be focusing on phenomena that were equally relevant for L2 production. The number of linguistic
studies that included psychological variables was relatively limited and most of these dealt with motivation (cf.
Skehan, 1989). I set up a research design that combined concepts from personality psychology, applied linguistics,
and bilingualism and focused on interpersonal communicative processes in French interlanguage. One personality
variable, namely extraversion, was found to correlate significantly with temporal variables in participants’ speech
recorded in a stressful situation. A bibliographical search showed that very few SLA researchers had investigated
the effect of extraversion on L2 production.
In my search for possible reasons for this lack of attention, I found the widely cited study of the “good language
learner” by Naiman, Fröhlich, Stern, and Todesco (1978), who included extraversion in their research design but
later dismissed the instrument to measure it, the Eysenck Personality Inventory (EPI). This exclusion led to a
rejection in SLA circles of a perfectly valid and robust psychological dimension as an independent variable.
Applied linguists have been interested in psychological dimensions insofar as they are predictors of success in
language learning (i.e., error-free production and reception). Only one of the so-called “Big Five” personality
dimensions6 has a strong prima facie link with language learning, namely the extraversion/introversion dimension.
Naiman et al. (1978) collected written data through a questionnaire on French as a Foreign Language developed by
the International Association for the Evaluation of Educational Achievement (IEA). The questionnaire included a
Listening Test of French Achievement and an Imitation Test. Samples were culturally homogeneous groups of 72
Canadian high school students from grades 8, 10, and 12 learning French as a L2. The authors claimed that these
three levels were representative of three levels of proficiency: beginner, intermediate, and advanced. The aim of
the researchers was to establish the psychological profile of the good language learner, in other words, the
participants with the highest test scores. One of their hypotheses was that the good language learner would be more
extraverted. This hypothesis was based on self-reports from good language learners reported by Naiman, Fröhlich,
and Stern (1975) where nearly a third of the participants mentioned that being more extraverted was an asset in
foreign language learning. When this hypothesis was disconfirmed by the data in the 1978 study (Pearson
correlation values ranging from –.11 to –.13; see p. 67), the authors questioned the construct validity of the
independent variable (the score on the introversion/ extraversion dimension) rather than their dependent variables
or their research design: “the constructs these tests were supposed to be measuring were in fact not being
adequately measured by these tests” (Naiman et al., 1978, p. 67). In other words, they refused to believe that a high
score on the extraversion scale indicated that a student was in fact an “extravert” in the language classroom (p. 67).
This particular remark has been widely cited in applied linguistic research and has had a devastating effect on the
reputation of the extraversion variable (Dewaele & Furnham, 1999).
Indeed, in psychology, the psychometric validity of the EPI has hardly ever been questioned since it was first
developed. Some applied linguists have expressed their doubts about Naiman et al.’s (1978) conclusions, but these
doubts seem to have passed largely unnoticed. McDonough (1986), for example, observed:
It may have been the case that their criterion measures were simply not sensitive to the relevant kinds of performance. . . . It
seems plausible that extroverts are more likely to perform better on tests of oral proficiency than on tests of imitation or
listening comprehension. (p. 139)
Larsen-Freeman and Long (1991) also questioned Naiman et al.’s (1978) conclusions and the choice of dependent
variables: “While all of these [measures] may be valid in their own right, none of them may provide a global
measure of language proficiency” (p. 185). In our study (Dewaele & Furnham, 1999), we reviewed 33 SLA studies
that had included the extraversion variable, and we discovered that no systematic relationships were ever found
between extraversion scores and linguistic variables or test results based on written material. Significant
correlations were found in studies that used linguistic variables extracted from oral language. The strength of the
relationship was found to depend on the task that the speaker was asked to perform. Correlations between
extraversion and linguistic measures were much higher for complex verbal tasks than for simple ones, especially
when the complex tasks were executed in stressful situations such as oral exams. No significant differences in
linguistic accuracy existed between extraverts and introverts. It is very likely that if Naiman et al. (1978) had used
a wider variety of more sophisticated linguistic variables, covering not only written language but also
communicative oral language, they might have found that the construct validity of the EPI was not to blame for the
lack of expected correlations.
Dewaele and Furnham (2000) carried out an empirical analysis of oral fluency indicators in French interlanguage
and explored possible underlying causes of the better performance of extraverts than that of introverts. One such
potential cause is the extraverts’ superior capacity in short-term memory, allowing them to maintain automatic
speech production in stressful situations. Lieberman and Rosenthal (2001) suggested that extraverts’ superior
short-term memory capacity makes them more able to engage in multitasking and thus able to engage in nonverbal
decoding when it is a secondary task. This ability to gauge the reactions of interlocutors contributes to the
extraverts’ superior social skills. Referring to this study by Lieberman and Rosenthal (2001), I speculated
6
(Dewaele, 2002b) that levels of dopamine and norepinephrine,7 which are vital in attentional and working memory
processes, might exceed optimal levels more easily in introvert than in extravert L2 users. Such excess could cause
an overload and a breakdown in fluency. These dopamine levels seem to remain under the maximum threshold in
extravert L2 users. As a consequence, extravert L2 users suffer little disruption in the functioning of the short-term
memory and minimal reduction in fluency. Another crucial aspect of the problem is that introverts tend to suffer
more from communicative anxiety than extraverts, especially in stressful situations. This anxiety is linked to
catecholamine activity,8 and norepinephrine in particular, which also seems to affect the capacity or efficiency, or
both, of the short-term memory. The combination of high anxiety and high introversion thus reinforces the effects
on speech production in the L2, especially in stressful interpersonal situations.
In sum, I wanted to demonstrate that this particular psychological variable, extraversion, has been unloved in the
SLA community for the wrong reasons. Researchers who developed appropriate research designs found that
extraversion is a good predictor of fluency in oral L2 production (cf. Berry, 2004; Wakamoto, 2002). Moreover,
this particular psychological variable allows the researcher to dig deeper into the neurological substrate of oral L2
production, opening up unexplored avenues of investigation in SLA.
7
high proportion of female participants (71%) and that the participants were generally highly educated (31% with a
MA, 30% with a Ph.D.). We could not therefore claim that our sample was representative of the general
population, but it did allow us to uncover some interesting trends.
The first studies based on this database have been published recently. In my first study (Dewaele, 2004a), I
investigated language choice for swearing and found that the multilingual’s dominant language (often the L1) was
used most frequently. An early start in the learning process and high frequency of use of a language were found to
be strong predictors for swearing in that language. In a second study, I analyzed variation in the perceived
emotional force of swear words and taboo words in the multilinguals’ different languages (Dewaele, 2004b). A
comparison of mean scores in the different languages showed that, on average, the emotional force is highest in the
L1 and gradually lower in languages learned later. Perception of emotional force of swear words and taboo words
was also positively correlated with self-rated proficiency and frequency of use of a language. Participants who had
started learning a L2 at a younger age tended to rate the emotional force of swear words and taboo words more
highly in that L2 than learners who started language learning later. Strong correlations appeared between
perception of emotional force of swear words and frequency of use of swear words in the different languages,9
suggesting that “As a rule, language users seem to avoid use of linguistic ‘nuclear’ devices if they are unsure about
their yield” (Dewaele, 2004a, p. 102). The general preference for swearing in the L1 and the stronger emotional
resonance of swear words in that language did not prevent participants from occasionally using their other
languages, depending on the intended perlocutionary effects and the identity of the interlocutor. In another study
(Dewaele, 2005), I focused on the effect of context of acquisition on language choice for swearing and perception
of emotional force of swear words and taboo words in languages learned later in life. The results showed that
instructed language learners used the target language less frequently and gave lower ratings on emotional force of
swear words and taboo words in that language than did mixed learners (i.e., who had a combination of classroom
instruction and naturalistic contact) and naturalistic learners (i.e., who had not benefited from any classroom
instruction).
Pavlenko (2004) used the database to investigate the role of emotion-related factors in language choice in bi- and
multilingual families. She found that “most of the time, factors other than emotions govern language choice and
use in such families, among them language dominance, social context and linguistic competence of the
interlocutors” (p. 179). Quantitative and qualitative analyses of the responses of 141 bi- and multilingual parents
showed that “perceived language emotionality and affective repertoires offered by particular languages also play a
role in language choice and use in parent/child communication, in particular in emotional expression” (p. 185).
Although the quantitative analyses showed general trends, namely the superior emotionality of the L1 and the
preference for that language to express emotions, the comments made by participants to the open questions in the
online questionnaire alerted us to the fact that, for a minority of participants, adult L2 socialization had made other
languages seem equally, or even more, emotional than the L1. This finding suggests that the higher emotional
resonance of the L1 is not a law of nature but rather a reflection of averages. The multilingual’s dominant language
is usually the language of emotion, and, in our database, about 90% of the participants declared themselves to be
dominant in their L1. We published these findings in two journal special issues (Pavlenko & Dewaele, 2004a,
2004b) to which scholarly “border-crossers” contributed. The aim was, as Wierzbicka (2004) explained in the
preface to the second special issue,
to restore the balance between, on the one hand, “objective” and “scientific” study of language and cognition and on the
other, a study open to the “soft data” of human testimonies and subjective experience, including experiential knowledge of
bilingual persons. (pp. 103–104)
In sum, these two illustrations that I have discussed at length show that methodological and epistemological
diversity can greatly benefit SLA research. As Crago (in press) eloquently put it: “The aim is to avoid a
balkanization of thought by bringing ideas together that create transfer from one mindset and set of theories and
practices to other ones.” A wide knowledge of the SLA literature and a good understanding of relevant concepts in
contiguous research areas allow SLA researchers to “catch” all the relevant variables for their study and help them
avoid simplifications or overgeneralizations. It is equally important that their nuanced findings reach teachers and
those in charge of language teaching methods. I will point to a number of implications of the research on affect and
emotions for teaching in the following section.
8
emotion than do fellow learners from “closer” cultures with similar levels of proficiency. Being able to judge the
interlocutor’s emotional state is crucial for successful communication (Rintell, 1984).
Emotion-free course books do not prepare L2 learners to become proficient L2 users. I personally remember
feeling quite a fool when, at the end of a period of instructed learning of Spanish at the university in Brussels, I
went to Spain only to discover that I was unable to produce anything but bland talk with my interlocutors. I could
say something about the weather (que calor ‘it’s hot’), I could order tapas, and ask for directions, but I was unable
to impress Spanish girls with my sophistication and wit, which mattered a lot tome at the time. I felt like a terrible
bore, acutely aware of my lack of sociocultural and sociopragmatic competence. I tried in vain to recall anything
from my course books that could constitute the basis of an interesting conversation. There was nothing. I did,
however, discover some nice poetry when I was in Spain that helped me expand my emotion vocabulary. These
texts, the telenovelas, and the encounters with Spaniards helped me develop a basic understanding of emotions in
Spanish culture and how they can be displayed and verbalized. Should my Spanish teacher have warned me
against the use of swear words? Should my course book have contained a list of swear words with ratings of their
intensity? Should it have proposed a wider range of sentences to display one’s dis/satisfaction? These are difficult
ethical questions. Just how much emotion-laden vocabulary and expressions should be taught to the learners?
Should these words and expressions include the many synonyms referring to sexual anatomy and sexual behavior?
What about words with racist connotations?
One could argue that knowledge of these words and expressions constitutes an essential part of sociocultural
competence in the target language and that they should be taught with the necessary words of caution. However,
the press and parent associations would probably lambaste the author of any course book that ventured into the
dark underbelly of a target language. It is hard to imagine course books with the red warning sticker “offensive
texts, not appropriate for under-18s.” Do we have to leave learners unaware of emotion-laden words and
expressions and let them experience them through authentic interactions? Can we, as teachers, remain silent about
a vibrant and crucial part of the target language and culture? I would argue that it is possible to prepare learners, to
a certain extent, in the perception and use of emotional speech acts. One such method is through use of authentic
material such as film extracts that can help to illustrate the combination of words with body language, vocal cues,
gestures, common collocations, and so on (Planchenault, 2005). The other method is through a stay in the country
where the target language is used. Evans and Fisher (2005) showed that even short exchange visits (up to 11 days)
can lead to significant increases in the use of expressive language.
The argument about the teaching of emotion-laden vocabulary and expressions leads to a second ethical question
that merits increased attention in SLA, namely learner resistance towards cultural assimilation and problems of
cultural stereotyping on the part of both the learner and members of the host culture. In situations of instructed L2
learning, the teacher expects learners to acquire an interlanguage that will gradually approximate native speaker
norms. This acquisition can be difficult when it involves the learner’s presentation of self and face. Users of L2s
may be aware of the sociopragmatic and sociocultural norms of the target language, but they may decide that these
norms are in conflict with their own beliefs and hence consciously deviate from the native speaker norm when they
become L2 users. I am, for example, aware of the ambiguous nature of piropos in Spain. Some male Spanish
friends describe piropos as friendly banter in which males express their admiration for the looks of a female; other
friends, both male and female (depending on age, class, education, and political persuasion), find piropos
unacceptable and offensive. Coming from a culture where this type of speech act is considered inappropriate, I
would never utter a single piropo in Spain. Similarly, Ohara (2002) found that some of her participants, female
American L2 users of Japanese, refused to conform to gender variation patterns in pitch level. They felt that the
high-pitched voice Japanese women use in certain situations—because it is perceived to be more feminine and
polite—did not conform with their own gender identities (see also Siegal, 1996). Conversely, some L2 users may
overcome an initial unwillingness to conform to native speaker norms after realizing that an approach modeled on
their L1 was unsuccessful. Evans (1988) quoted a British student who had spent a semester in Italy and declared:
The Italians are so different, and if they want something they will go out and get it. I’ve been taught that you ask for it
politely. You realize that unless you do what they do, shout, nothing will come out of it. (p. 45)
In sum, more work is needed to improve our knowledge of how to promote the acquisition of the expression of
emotion and other essential parts of L2 sociocultural competence. Instructed L2 learning includes not only the
literal decoding of text and speech in the L2 but also the reading of the interlocutor’s face, an awareness of vocal
cues, body language, and so on. If not much research is available in these areas, however, we do not have enough
knowledge to go on in terms of designing materials or curricula, and the very legitimacy of such language areas as
worthy of teaching remains problematic. Good research into the psychological and emotional dimensions of SLA
is crucial for the development of good teaching practices.
CONCLUSION
I have argued in this article that to obtain a more complete picture of the language learner and user, we need to
expand our current theoretical horizon. We need to understand what makes instructed L2 learners and L2 users
behave the way they do, and we also need to accept that any answer can only be tentative. Communicative
behavior may be influenced by cultural, social, or psychological variables which can be factored in any analysis,
9
but it is also largely linked to unquantifiable factors such as the users’ free will and random events that might
affect the language learning process. I am not arguing in favor of tearing down all the borders separating the
different areas of SLA, or between SLA and its contiguous disciplines. That would be both utopian and
impractical. A better solution, in my view, would be a loose confederation of research areas with porous borders
that allow free circulation of research ideas and methodologies. There will always be enough space for academic
border-crossers to plant new flags in “unclaimed territories.” Debates among SLA researchers from different
perspectives are crucial and the best guarantee against stagnation.
Two interesting epistemological questions hover over the different issues raised in this article: the deceptively
simple question, Why is something there? and its more fiendish counterpart, Why is something absent? The second
question is harder to answer because there is a presupposition that something ought to be there but is not, for some
unknown reason. These questions can be asked about the synchronic variation in the learner’s interlanguage (i.e.,
What external factors affect the presence or absence of an element?), and about the diachronic variation in the
interlanguage (i.e., Why and when does an element appear?). The same question can also be asked about the
discipline as a whole. Why have certain questions been researched? Why have certain independent and dependent
variables been included in research designs? Does the absence or paucity of published work on certain variables
mean that they have not been researched? Or might it suggest that they have been wrongly researched? Or have the
researchers decided that the publication of inconclusive results was not worth the effort? These are fundamental
questions facing anyone contemplating a research project in SLA, especially those starting their doctoral research,
who naturally prefer not to come up with a null result after years of hard work.
My guess is that the answers to the two epistemological questions will vary according to the position of a
researcher within a particular network. I assume that researchers at the center of the network share a larger set of
common points of view and a longer common history than do those at the periphery. Those at the center tend to
encourage work on questions where they can safely guess the answer. They may not welcome challenges to the
very basis of their research area. The lack of an outside view means that researchers may remain unaware of their
own prejudices. This lack of awareness does not stop them from doing excellent work, of course, but the seed of
stagnation is present. My argument is that by stimulating interdisciplinarity and by encouraging cross-fertilization,
we can ensure a healthy future for SLA. However, interdisciplinarity is not without its own risks. Sokal and
Bricmont (1999) have demonstrated that scientists who import ideas and concepts from other disciplines into their
own are more likely than those who do not to misunderstand or misinterpret crucial concepts leading to erroneous
conclusions. An example of this type of erroneous conclusion would be, in my view, the use of the concept of
extraversion by Naiman et al. (1978) which I have discussed at some length in this article.
As instructed SLA researchers, we have a responsibility towards the foreign language teaching community. It is
therefore crucial that we ask ourselves: How could instructed SLA research be more responsive to the realities of
various learner populations and to the heterogeneity in language learning experiences across individuals? How can
we research L2 learning puzzles in ways that produce findings that truly serve learner-centered education? I
pinpointed some of the acquisitional challenges presented to L2 users, especially those aspects of sociopragmatic
and sociocultural competence that are difficult to learn within the confined space of the classroom. Although well-
informed teachers and appropriate material can contribute to a raising of the learners’ awareness of these aspects of
the target language, L2 learners need to be aware of the gap and then decide whether or not they feel inclined to
bridge it. The last leg of the learners’ journey can only be unsupported. Authentic heated interactions with other
users of the target language will provide ample opportunity to learn what could not be taught.
I fervently believe that a stronger focus on physiological, psychological, affective, and emotional issues in SLA
can provide crucial theoretical insights into L2 acquisition that are now missing. It can also be a new impetus for
authors of course books by assisting them with the design of better and richer materials and curricula. This
collaboration between SLA researchers and foreign language practitioners will ultimately benefit the instructed L2
learner.
ACKNOWLEDGMENT I would like to thank the guest editor of the special issue, Lourdes Ortega, Jennifer Sia, and the
anonymous reviewers for their helpful and stimulating suggestions on previous versions of this article.
NOTES
1 Students enrolled in language departments in British universities are expected to study abroad (cf. Coleman, 1996, Evans,
1988). 2 Regan (2004) and Bayley and Langman (2004) investigated this question for morphological variables in interlanguage
and found that individual results in their data largelymatched group patterns. They argued that it is therefore justified from an
empirical and a theoretical viewpoint to report group results in studies of SLA. 3 I had originally included two independent
variables (extraversion and neuroticism) and one dependent variable (lexical richness) in the design. A negative correlation
emerged between extraversion and lexical richness, but no relation appeared between neuroticism and lexical richness. Given
that there was no strong case to be made for a possible effect of neuroticism, I preferred to remove any reference to it. It is very
possible that many more researchers in individual differences in SLA behave in similar ways. Studies with null results may be
considered less exciting and are more likely to remain unpublished. However, if a case can be made for the potential effect of an
independent variable, it should be investigated and reported, even if no significant effect emerges. 4 Path analysis is a special use
of multiple regression to analyze the direct and indirect effects of variables hypothesized as causal (Cohen, 1992). 5 It is probably
not a coincidence that the most prominent researchers on individual differences in SLA (Mac- Intyre, Skehan, D¨ornyei, and
Segalowitz, to name but a few) have degrees in psychology. 6 The other personality dimensions have been identified as
10
neuroticism, openness, agreeableness, and conscientiousness (Furnham & Heaven, 1999). 7 Dopamine is a neurotransmitter
system ofmajor importance within the brain. It is involved in a range of apparently different brain mechanisms including central
control of locomotion and neural processes generating motivational behavior, mood, and emotion (Bradford, 1986). Dopamine is
the immediate biosynthetic precursor of norepinephrine (the latter is also known as noradrenaline). 8 Catecholaminergic neurons
control motor activity and mood (Bradford, 1986). 9 For the L2: r (901) = .32, for the L3: r (647) = .43; for the L4: r (414) = .45;
for the L5: r (218) = .50; for all: p < .0001 (Dewaele, 2004a, p. 101).
REFERENCES
Barron, A. (2003). Acquisition in interlanguage pragmatics. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: Benjamins.
Bayley, R., & Langman, J. (2004). Variation in the group and the individual: Evidence from second language acquisition.
International Review of Applied Linguistics, 42, 303–318.
Belz, J. A., & Kinginger, C. (2002). The cross-linguistic development of address form use in telecollaborative language
learning: Two case studies. Canadian Modern Language Review, 59, 189–214.
Belz, J. A., & Kinginger, C. (2003). Discourse options and the development of pragmatic competence by classroom learners
of German. Language Learning, 53, 591–647.
Berry, V. B. (2004). A study of the interaction between individual personality differences and oral performance test facets.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Kings College, University of London, England.
Bradford, H. F. (1986). Chemical neurobiology. An introduction to neurochemistry. New York: W.H. Freeman & Co.
Bruhn de Garavito, J., & White, L. (2002). L2 acquisition of Spanish DPs: The status of grammatical features. In A. T. Pérez-
Leroux & J. M. Liceras (Eds.), The acquisition of Spanish morphosyntax: The L1/L2 connection (pp. 151–176). Dordrecht,
The Netherlands: Kluwer.
Cohen, J. (1992). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: John Wiley.
Coleman, J. A. (1996). Studying languages: A survey of British and European students, the proficiency, background, attitudes
and motivations of students of foreign languages in the United Kingdom and Europe. London: Centre for Information on
Language Teaching and Research.
Cook, V. (2002). Introduction: Background of the L2 user. In V. Cook (Ed.), Portraits of the L2 user (pp. 1–28). Clevedon,
UK: Multilingual Matters.
Crago, M. (in press). Encountering the other in research on childhood bilingualism. The International Journal of Bilingualism.
Dewaele, J.-M. (1993). Extraversion et richesse lexicale dans deux styles d’interlangue française [Extraversion and lexical
richness in two styles of French interlanguage]. ITL Review of Applied Linguistics, 100, 87–105.
Dewaele, J.-M. (2002a). Psychological and sociodemographic correlates of communicative anxiety in L2 and L3 production.
The International Journal of Bilingualism, 6, 23–39.
Dewaele, J.-M. (2002b). Individual differences in L2 fluency: The effect of neurobiological correlates. In V. Cook (Ed.),
Portraits of the L2 user (pp. 219– 250). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.
Dewaele, J.-M. (2004a). Blistering barnacles! What language do multilinguals swear in? Estudios de Sociolingüistica, 5, 83–
106.
Dewaele, J.-M. (2004b). The emotional force of swear words and taboo words in the speech of multilinguals. Journal of
Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 25, 204–222.
Dewaele, J.-M. (2005). The effect of type of acquisition context on perception and self-reported use of swearwords in the L2,
L3, L4 and L5. In A. Housen & M. Pierrard (Eds.), Investigations in instructed second language acquisition (pp. 531–559).
Berlin, Germany: Mouton De Gruyter.
Dewaele, J.-M.,& Furnham, A. (1999). Extraversion: The unloved variable in applied linguistic research. Language Learning,
49, 509–544.
Dewaele, J.-M., & Furnham, A. (2000). Personality and speech production: A pilot study of second language learners.
Personality and Individual Differences, 28, 355–365.
Dewaele, J.-M., & Pavlenko, A. (2002). Emotion vocabulary in interlanguage. Language Learning, 52, 265– 324.
Dörnyei, Z. (2001). Motivational strategies: Creating and maintaining student motivation in the foreign language classroom.
New York: Cambridge University Press.
Evans, C. (1988). Language people. The experience of teaching and learning modern languages in British universities. Milton
Keynes/Philadelphia: Open University Press.
Evans, M., & Fisher, L. (2005). Measuring gains in pupils’ foreign language competence as a result of participation in a
school exchange visit: The case of Y9 pupils at three comprehensive schools in the UK. Language Teaching Research, 9,
173– 192.
Firth, A., & Wagner, J. (1997). On discourse, communication, and (some) fundamental concepts in SLA research. Modern
Language Journal, 81, 285–300.
Furnham, A., & Heaven, P. C. L. (1999). Personality and social behaviour. New York: Oxford University Press.
11
Grosjean, F. (1989).Neurolinguistics, beware! The bilingual is not two monolinguals in one person. Brain and Language, 36,
3–15.
Grosjean, F. (1998). Studying bilinguals: Methodological and conceptual issue. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1,
131–149.
Han, Z. (2003). Fossilization in adult second language acquisition. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.
Harris, C. L., Ayçiçegi A., & Gleason, J. B. (2003). Taboo words and reprimands elicit greater autonomic reactivity in a first
than in a second language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 561–579.
Harris, C. L., Gleason, J. B., & Ayçiçegi, A. (in press). When is a first language more emotional? Psychophysiological
evidence from bilingual speakers. In A. Pavlenko (Ed.), Languages and emotions of multilingual speakers. Clevedon, UK:
Multilingual Matters.
Herdina, P., & Jessner, U. (2002). A dynamic model of multilingualism. Perspectives of change in psycholinguistics.
Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.
Horwitz, E. K. (1986). Preliminary evidence for the reliability and validity of a foreign language anxiety scale. TESOL
Quarterly, 20, 559–562.
Kecskes, I., & Papp, T. (2000). Foreign language and mother tongue. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kinginger, C. (2004a). Alice doesn’t live here anymore: Foreign language learning and renegotiated identity. In A. Pavlenko
& A. Blackledge (Eds.), Negotiation of identities in multilingual contexts (pp. 219–242). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.
Kinginger, C. (2004b). Bilingualism and emotion in the autobiographical works of Nancy Huston. Journal of Multilingual and
Multicultural Development, 25, 159–178.
Kinginger, C., & Farrell, K. (2004). Assessing development of meta-pragmatic awareness in study abroad. Frontiers: The
Interdisciplinary Journal of Study Abroad, 10, 19–42.
Larsen-Freeman, D. (1997). Chaos/complexity science and second language acquisition. Applied Linguistics, 18, 141–165.
Larsen-Freeman, D., & Long, M. H. (1991). An introduction to second language research. New York: Longman.
Lazaraton, A. (2000). Current trends in research methodology and statistics in applied linguistics. TESOL Quarterly, 34, 175–
181.
Lieberman, M. D., & Rosenthal, R. (2001). Why introverts can’t always tell who likes them: Social multitasking and non-
verbal decoding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 294–310.
Lyster, R. (1994). The effect of functional-analytic teaching on aspects of French immersion students’ sociolinguistic
competence. Applied Linguistics, 15, 263–287.
MacIntyre, P. D., & Charos, C. (1996). Personality, attitudes, and affect as predictors of second language communication.
Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 15, 3–26.
McDonough, S. H. (1986). Psychology in foreign language teaching . London: George Allen & Unwin.
Milner, J.-C. (1989). Introduction à une science du langage [Introduction to a science of language]. Paris: Seuil.
Naiman, N., Fröhlich, M., & Stern, H. H. (1975). The good language learner: A report. Toronto, Canada: Ontario Institute for
Studies in Education.
Naiman, N., Fröhlich, M., Stern, H. H., & Todesco, A. (1978). The good language learner. Toronto, Canada: Ontario Institute
for Studies in Education.
Norris, J. M., & Ortega, L. (2000). Effectiveness of L2 instruction: A research synthesis and quantitative meta-analysis.
Language Learning, 50, 417–528.
Ohara, Y. (2002). Finding one’s voice in Japanese: A study of pitch levels of L2 users. In A. Pavlenko, A. Blackledge, I.
Piller, & M. Teutsch-Dwyer (Eds.), Multilingualism, second language learning, and gender (pp. 231–254). Berlin, Germany:
Mouton De Gruyter.
Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. New York: Oxford University
Press.
Pavlenko, A. (1998). Second language learning by adults: Testimonies of bilingual writers. Issues in Applied Linguistics, 9, 3–
19.
Pavlenko, A. (2002). Poststructuralist approaches to the study of social factors in L2. In V. Cook (Ed.), Portraits of the L2
user (pp. 277–302). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.
Pavlenko, A. (2004). “Stop doing that, la Komu Skazala!”: Language choice and emotion in parent-child communication.
Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 25, 179–203.
Pavlenko, A., & Dewaele, J.-M. (2004a). (Eds.). Bilingualism and emotions [Special issue]. Estudios de Sociolingüıstica, 5(1).
Pavlenko, A., & Dewaele, J.-M. (2004b). (Eds.). Languages and emotions: A crosslinguistic perspective [Special issue].
Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 25(2–3).
Planchenault, G. (2005). Utiliser des extraits des films en classe de français langue étrangère. Etude sur l’’enseignement et
l’acquisition des compétences socioculturelle et sociopragmatique [Using film sequences in the French classroom: A study on
12
sociopragmatic awareness and the expression of sociolinguistic variants in French interlanguage]. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, University of London, England.
Regan, V. (2004). The relationship between the group and the individual and the acquisition of native speaker variation
patterns: A preliminary study. International Review of Applied Linguistics, 42, 335– 348.
Rintell, E. (1984). But how did you feel about that? The learner’s perception of emotion in speech. Applied Linguistics, 5,
255–264.
Schrauf, R.W. (2000). Bilingual autobiographical memory: Experimental studies and clinical cases. Culture and Psychology,
6, 387–417.
Siegal, M. (1996). The role of learner subjectivity in second language sociolinguistic competency: Western women learning
Japanese. Applied Linguistics, 17, 356–382. Skehan, P. (1989). Individual differences in second language learning . London:
Arnold.
Sokal, A., & Bricmont, J. (1999). Impostures intellectuelles [Intellectual impostures]. Paris: Librairie générale française.
Towell, R., & Dewaele, J.-M. (2005). The role of psycholinguistic factors in the development of fluency amongst advanced
learners of French. In J.-M. Dewaele (Ed.), Focus on French as a foreign language: Multidisciplinary approaches (pp. 210–
239). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.
van Lier, L. (1996). Interaction in the language curriculum: Awareness, autonomy, and authenticity. New York: Longman.
Wakamoto, N. (2002). Language learning strategy and personality variables: Focusing on extroversion and introversion.
International Review of Applied Linguistics, 38, 71–81.
Wierzbicka, A. (2004). Preface: Bilingual lives, bilingual experience. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development,
25, 94–104.
13