JCPPublication Wertz
JCPPublication Wertz
JCPPublication Wertz
net/publication/228384780
CITATIONS READS
469 2,297
1 author:
Frederick Wertz
Fordham University
65 PUBLICATIONS 1,848 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
All content following this page was uploaded by Frederick Wertz on 18 March 2020.
Frederick J. Wertz
Fordham University
1I would like to thank Andy Giorgi, Tom Cloonan, Connie Fischer, Steen Halling, Chris Mruk, Mufid Hannush,
Peter Ashworth, Niki Skoufalos, and Tracy Prout for their help. I would also like to express my gratitude to Joe
Ponterotto, Beth Haverkamp, and Sue Morrow for their vision, leadership, professionalism, sensitivity, and
meticulousness as editors.
2
Abstract
A brief history includes some of Edmund Husserl’s basic methods and concepts,
Edmund Husserl. In the tradition of Giambattista Vico, Franz Brentano, and William Dilthey,
Husserl broadened the concepts and methods of modern science to include the study of
consciousness, profoundly influencing philosophy, other humanities, and the social sciences
throughout the 20th century. Husserl formulated scientific methods which are uniquely fashioned
substantive contributions to psychology in the work of Karl Jaspers, Max Scheler, Martin
Heidegger, Jean-Paul Sartre, Maurice Merleau-Ponty, Alfred Schutz, Gaston Bachelard, Gabriel
Marcel, Emmanuel Levinas, and Paul Ricoeur (Spiegelberg, 1982). Although their works
imagination, emotions, behavior, language, and social processes, the greatest impact on
psychology has occurred in the area of mental health (Spiegelberg, 1972). This work was a
protest against dehumanization in psychology and offered original research and theory that
faithfully reflects the distinctive characteristics of human behavior and first person experience.
Halling and Nill’s (1995) excellent brief history of phenomenology in psychiatry and
psychotherapy highlights the multiple sources and influences which undergird, compliment, and
permeate the field’s traditional and mainstream approaches. In Europe, Ludwig Binswanger,
Eugene Minkowski, Erwin Straus, Medard Boss, F.J.J. Buytendijk, Viktor von Gebsattel, Igor
4
Caruso, Henri Ey, H.C. Rümke, J.H van den Berg, Viktor Frankl, and R.D. Laing assumed
leadership roles and produced an impressive volume and breadth of scholarship across diverse
topics in clinical psychology. Some themes unifying these works include the emphasis on
“experience, process, freedom, the importance of the client-therapist relationship, and viewing
the client’s problems from his or her perspective” (Halling and Nill, 1995, p.28).
This work began to come to the attention of American psychologists in the 1930s through
Robert McLeod and later Gordon Allport as well as through European-Americans Andreas
Angyl, Adrian Van Kaam, and Henri Ellenberger as well as more recent immigrants Kurt
Kaffka, Wolfgang Kohler, Paul Tillich and Erwin Straus. Halling and Nill (1995) cite Martin
Buber’s participation in the William Alanson White Memorial Lectures in 1957 and the
untranslated papers including those of many of the above mentioned authors, as the two pivotal
psychologists. American born mental health scholars and practitioners such as James Bugental,
Eugene Gendlin, and Irving Yalom have made original contributions to a broadening American
This section will briefly detail the methods that characterize phenomenological research
developed by Husserl and some basic concepts concerning human psychological life that the
knowledge begins with a fresh and unbiased description of its subject matter. Husserl
(1913/1962) employed two procedures called epochés, which are abstentions from influences
that could short-circuit or bias description. The first is the “epoché of the natural sciences”
(Husserl, 1939/1954, p. 135), and requires that the researcher abstain from utilizing (“brackets”)
natural scientific theories, explanations, hypotheses, and conceptualizations of the subject matter.
This epoché involves setting aside prior scientific assumptions in order to gain access, in
Husserl’s famous phrase, “to the things themselves (Sachen selbst)!” This epoché delivers the
scientific knowledge. This return to phenomena as they are lived in contrast to beginning with
scientific preconceptions is a methodological procedure and does not imply that such knowledge
is false; it simply suspends received science, puts it out of play, and makes no use of it for the
This first epoché delivers the investigator to the “natural attitude” in the prescientific life-
world (lebenswelt), that is, to the unreflective apprehension of the world as it is lived, precisely
the world, whose existence we assume. For the most part, we do not notice the conscious and
experiential processes through which the world is objectively given, do not reflect on its
meanings, and do not attend to the subjective performances that constitute the world’s meanings.
The natural attitude is appropriate for physical scientific research, which does not investigate
meaning or subjectivity; however, sciences that seek knowledge of human experience cannot
remain naïve about consciousness. They require a transformation of attitude, a new epoché, the
6
epoché of the natural attitude (Husserl, 1939/1954, p. 148-150). This second epoché is a
methodological abstention used to suspend or put out of play our “naive” belief in the existence
of what presents itself in the life-world in order to focus instead on its subjective manners of
appearance and givenness—the lived-through meanings and the subjective performances that
subtend human situations. Again, this is a purely methodological operation; it does not imply
that what presents itself in human life does not exist. The existence and validity of human
situations are “bracketed” only in order to allow the shift from naïve, straightforward encounter
to a reflection on how the life-world presents itself, that is, to its constitutive meanings and
This second epoché and the analyses that follow from it allow us to recollect our own
experiences and to empathically enter and reflect on the lived world of other persons in order to
apprehend the meanings of the world as they are given to the first person point of view. The
psychologist can investigate his or her own original sphere of experience and also has an
intersubjective horizon of experience that allows access to the experiences of others (Husserl,
1939/1954, p. 254). Husserl refers to the focus on experience (apart from issues concerning the
p. 236) because it “reduces” the investigative field to the psychological. This presence to the
psychological allows the investigator to reflectively describe the meanings of and psychological
performances in the situations lived through. In Husserl’s view, the scientific study of
subjectivity requires a more radical epoché beyond the scope of this article--the transcendental
fundamental to qualitative research because it grasps “what” something is: the intuition of
essence or the eidetic reduction. This method is neither inductive nor deductive; it descriptively
delineates the invariant characteristic(s) and clarifies the meaning and structure/organization of a
subject matter. Husserl (1913/1962) developed and formalized a special procedure that provides
rigor in knowing essences called free imaginative variation. One starts with a concrete example
of the phenomenon of which one wishes to grasp the essence and imaginatively varies it in every
possible way in order to distinguish essential features from those which are accidental or
incidental. This is the method par excellence for the acquisition of qualitative knowledge, for it
informs us of what something essentially is. Eidetic seeing or insight provides evidence of those
features that must be present in any instance and all possible instances of a subject matter.
According to Husserl, there are different kinds of essences such as the exact, formal
essences of mathematics (e.g., “three,” “triangle”) and material essences (e.g., “rock,” “lentil”)
that require inexact, morphological concepts to delineate them. The life sciences study vital
phenomena which have their own kind of essential being, and psychology must consider the
“schizophrenia” or “mental life”--in order to insure that its properly psychological features are
phenomenological critique and overcoming of reductionism. For instance, it has shown that
physicalistic concepts distort the essence of the body, which is not merely an object in the
physical world but an agent of action, and the essence of behavior, which is not merely muscular
movement but involves a meaningful goal structure. Eidetic science helps set the proper
8
epistemological and conceptual foundations for the empirical sciences. Through eidetic
analyses, phenomenological philosophy has offered “regional ontologies” which inform us of the
essential kinds of being investigated in empirical sciences such as physics, chemistry, biology,
psychology, sociology, and theology. A rigorously scientific empirical psychology requires the
foundation of eidetic psychology if its investigations are to respect the essential qualities of its
subject matter.
gain insight into their essence(s), Husserl reaffirmed and radically revised Brentano’s broadly
(independent of consciousness itself). I see a blackbird, think two and two is four, hallucinate a
pink elephant, speak my mother’s name, open a door, or resent an insult. In these examples, my
mental life involves a transcendence, that is, a relation to something (an “object”) beyond itself
that means something to me. The procedure developed by Husserl (1913/1962, 1939/1954)
situations, their meaning, and the processes that generate those meanings. Intentional analysis
begins with a situation just as it has been experienced--with all its various meanings--and
reflectively explicates the experiential processes through which the situation is lived.
analyses of mental life radically contextual and ecological. “The lived world” is a central theme.
The intentionality of human mental life is not an isolated ray, illuminating a single object;
9
intentionality includes its relational context as it illuminates a “world.” The life-world manifests
itself as a structural whole that is socially shared and yet apprehended by individuals through
their own perspectives. Spatial surroundings span through and beyond my immediate situation
in an order of mutual references including equipment, cultural objects, natural objects, aesthetics,
other people, and cultural institutions. While human situations are at every moment given
meanings through a living present, they unfold within a larger temporal process that includes
both the collective and individual person’s determinate past and yet-to-be-determined future.
Every human situation includes our bodies not only as things in the world but as subjective ways
of relating to our surroundings. The human body plays a key role in the constitution of the world
and the establishment of the meaning of our surroundings through its sensory-perceptive
and habits in situations whose meanings (e.g., “too hot,” “useful,” “comfortable,” “nearly within
reach”) are constituted by our bodily ways of being toward them. The life-world as a whole--
every situation in it and every moment of our psychological lives--entails various forms of
sociality as part of its essential structure. Language also pervades the meanings of our
surroundings and forms part of what makes the life-world more a collective place than the
levels—e.g., the ethnic, national, cultural, and religious--have historically shaped and
inextricably pervade the life-world and must therefore be acknowledged by any psychology that
seeks full knowledge of the human being. And yet the individual person experiences this world
in a way that is uniquely relevant and meaningful to the self in the course of their individual
histories that begin with their birth and end with their death, making the life-world a place
meaningfully apprehended from “one’s own” perspective (Eigenwelt). A person must not be
10
reduced to his or her facticity or actuality because as long as the person is alive, he/she holds
greater potential for activities and meaningful relations to the world which means that the future
Apart from research in which norms are borrowed from the physical sciences--research
where hypothetical deductive explanations have been tested by means of quantitative analysis,
psychologists of many stripes have used qualitative methods designed to investigate meaning and
subjectivity. However, these works, including Sigmund Freud’s The Interpretation of Dreams
(1900) and William James’ The Varieties of Religious Experience (1902/1982), lacked a
methodological norms. The Social Science Research Council began an initiative in the 1930s to
study such qualitative methods that were being used across the social sciences. Sponsored by the
Council, Gordon Allport (1942) cited extensive use of qualitative methods in psychology,
acknowledged their low status but high scientific value, compellingly challenged virtually every
criticism of these methods, and called for a formal account that would establish rigorous
methodological norms for their use. Only in the 1970s did American psychology begin to
benefit from the formalization and deliberate development of such methods and methodologies.
The advances made in counseling and clinical psychology within the phenomenological
movement were similar to those in psychoanalysis in that, although qualitative research was
conducted, there was almost no formal specification of its procedures or methodological norms.
Research based on scholarly reading and informal analyses of clinical experience was presented,
but systematic empirical research utilizing publicly available data collected primarily for
11
research purposes was extremely rare. One notable exception was Lang and Esterson’s (1963)
interview-based study of the relationship of schizophrenic symptoms to the family, but even this
study left implicit the analytic procedures through which they reached their conclusions
regarding the social intelligibility of symptoms. The emergence of rigorously specified and
deliberately employed research procedures and of attempts to make the research process
transparent in publications did not occur until the 1960s and 1970s.
psychology and credits Amedeo Giorgi with leadership. Giorgi, who is not a clinical
psychologist but received his PhD from Fordham University in Experimental Psychology
Phenomenological Psychology that had been founded by Van Kaam, in 1962. His primary task,
research method in which general psychology students as well as those researching clinical and
counseling topics could be trained. Influenced by Husserl and Merleau-Ponty, Giorgi worked
through the 1960s to develop such a method and in 1970 offered the first course in the Duquesne
the program quickly expanded into a multi-semester sequence, required of all doctoral students,
that culminated in an “Integration Seminar” taught by Giorgi and attended by other dissertation
advisors in which students formulated their dissertation research methods. In 1970, Giorgi
founded and edited the Journal of Phenomenolgical Psychology, the first psychology journal
with the explicit intent of providing a forum for qualitative research in psychology.
The emergence of phenomenological research was led by Giorgi and the Duquesne
Circle, including William Fischer, Rolf Von Eckartsberg, Anthony Barton, Constance Fischer,
12
Edward Murray, Frank Buckley, Charles Maes, and Paul Richer. From 1970 to the present,
Giorgi developed varied methods, described procedures used in various projects, refined his
understanding of their phenomenological core, and wrote on such general methodological issues
as reliability and validity. This focus on reflectively and deliberately developing and accounting
for research methods has given rise to an impressive production of research projects. In the past
psychology (Smith, 2002). These topics include, for instance, expectations prior to
resolution of adolescent suicide ideation, disclosing one’s HIV+/AIDS diagnosis, and caring for
a spouse with Alzheimer’s disease. Other graduate programs have trained students in the use of
phenomenological methods and the publication of a considerable body of empirical research has
appeared in the psychological literature (e.g., Valle and Halling, 1988; Giorgi et al, 1971, 1975,
Phenomenological research, largely influenced by this work at Duquesne, has led the
qualitative research movement in psychology. Rennie, Watson, and Monteiro (2000) conducted
a study of the rise of qualitative research in the 20th century that included a literature search using
terms currently associated with the field. Prior to 1980, “phenomenological (and existential
phenomenological) psychology” is the only term that yielded hits in psychology journals—a total
of 126 hits in contrast to 9 for “Qualitative Research”, “Grounded Theory”, and “Discourse
Analysis” combined across the social sciences. Of journals publishing articles involving the
search terms, the Journal of Phenomenolgical Psychology, consistent over the last three decades,
13
included by far the most (195), with the next closest journals being the Journal of Pragmatics (a
Language and Communications journal with 54 discourse analysis articles) and Canadian
“overwhelmingly appeared in psychology journals and dissertations rather than coming from
other fields (350 vs. 9)” and came mainly from North America (269 vs. 92) (2000, p. 185-186).
critical, general and potentially intersubjective. Like all good science, they require critical
thinking, creativity, and reflective decision making that give rise to many procedural variations
expression/description, analytic procedures, and ways of presenting findings have been used.
Giorgi (1989b) has indicated several core phenomenological characteristics that hold across the
variations of these psychological research methods: this research is descriptive, utilizes the
situations, and provides knowledge of psychological essences (that is, the structures of meaning
descriptions provided by research participants, Giorgi (1985; Giorgi & Giorgi, 2003), says that
four steps are involved: 1) reading the entire description in order to grasp the sense of the whole;
2) rereading the description and demarcating spontaneous shifts in meaning, or “meaning units”
in the text with a psychologically sensitive interest in the phenomenon under investigation; 3)
reflecting on each and every meaning unit in order to discern what it reveals about the
from it; 4) synthesizing these reflections and insights into a consistent statement that expresses
14
participants is accounted for and its analytic treatment is available for public scrutiny.
had been conducted at Duquesne in 1978, I reviewed all completed dissertations and attempted
to specify the procedures from start to finish of the research project. I was especially interested
reflection. I supplemented this with an effort to reflectively track my own operative procedures
efforts led to a more explicit specification of the phenomenological analytic operations involved
in psychological research (Wertz, 1983a & 1985). A subsequent review of informally conducted
psychological research in the broad phenomenological tradition found the same procedures to be
implicitly operative throughout the literature prior to 1970 (Wertz, 1983b). These same basic
constituents of descriptive psychological reflection have been found to be used by Freud and
subsequent psychoanalysts (Wertz, 1993), leading to the conclusion that the method Giorgi has
begun to specify characterizes any and every genuinely psychological qualitative research
method. The role of the phenomenological approach to psychology has merely been to clarify its
research method is what it intends to be, a method that is shaped according to the intrinsic
The research project begins with the identification of a psychological topic. This
identification involves locating and delineating its presence in the life-world. Counseling
15
phenomena might include problems or situations that lead clients to counseling; professional
counseling practices; the counseling process; relational issues between counselor and client;
outcomes of counseling. In defining the research problems and goals, the researcher reviews
established knowledge and critically identifies its limits; some gap between knowledge and
reality that requires qualitative knowledge, an understanding of what occurs. Research is then
designed to solve the problem, fill in the gap, overcome the flaw. All the choices the researcher
makes throughout the project are ideally determined after critically and reflectively weighing the
relative merits of the alternatives for making our knowledge a better description of reality.
Data Constitution
Participants. One of the researcher’s first choices involves the identification and
selection of human beings whose lives involve a revelatory relationship with the subject matter
witnesses, 4) a system or group of related persons. The basis of this decision is the judgment of
whose experience most fully and authentically manifests or makes accessible what the researcher
is interested in. In a study on “racism related stress,” participants may be recruited from among
working racial minorities, but the researcher may also identify a novel that would offer
would contribute relevant descriptions of the expressive responses of racially stressed workers.
If the gap in the literature or analyses of the experiences of work-stressed minorities suffering
stress indicates the need to better understand the “stressors,” persons who induce stress, then they
The question of “how many participants?” can only be answered properly by considering
the nature of the research problem and the potential yield of findings. If in depth knowledge of
one individual’s experience will fulfill the goal(s) of the research, one participant may be
sufficient. For instance, if a researcher seeks to know in depth the psychological process through
which a professional develops multicultural counseling competence, the choice of one master
counselor who is recognized as a superlative model in this area might provide sufficient access to
the phenomenon to fulfill important research aims. However, knowledge of differences in the
field may suggest the need for representatives of other approaches if the research aims at
generality across the field. It is not always possible to determine the required number of
participants before conducting the research and carrying out analyses. Particularly when the
research requires knowledge that addresses a broad range of the topic’s manifestations, the
“saturation,” redundancy of findings that fulfill the research goals, is achieved. The nature and
deliberation and critical reflection considering the research problem, the life-world position of
the participant(s), the quality of the data, and the value of emergent findings with regard to
research goals are required in a continuing assessment of adequacy. Participant selection always
limits the results, and reflective accounting of such limits is an important part of the research
process.
Situation(s). The situations studied further limit the knowledge achieved by research.
The researcher may select the situation(s) to be described, as one might in investigating a
particular short term vocational counseling protocol that is offered at several educational sites.
The researcher may also designate a “type” of experience and invite the participant to choose the
17
he/she kept a secret from his/her counselor. Situations researched can be naturally occurring, as
when Mexican American students are asked to describe a situation at college in which they
“became very distressed”. The situation can also be constructed specifically for the research, as
might occur if one solicited participants to learn a new relaxation intervention in the research
situation and provide descriptions of the learning process as they master the activity.
Procedures of description. Descriptions are usually verbal but some researchers have
used other expressive forms such as drawings (Wertz, 1997a). Descriptions may be generated
from the point of view of the “self,” the “other,” or both. Phenomenology does not privilege first
person description and acknowledges that others have (in some cases superior) access to
clients who do not return after the first visit by describing their expressive behavior during the
by a person who observes another person’s conduct. However, many studies, such as one on the
expectations of counseling prior to the first visit, require first person description.
Typically, descriptions use ordinary language and may be provided verbally or in writing
gives the participant(s) a descriptive task with instructions that specify a focus and yet remain
open to the particular content that the participant offers. Interviews are useful when the
phenomenon of interest is complex in structure, extensive in scope, and/or subtle in features that
participants are not likely to offer spontaneously in response to questions or instructions at the
outset. Interviews typically begin with open-ended instructions such as “Please describe a
situation in which you had an interaction with another person who was unsupportive of your
18
sexual orientation.” The researcher might have an interview schedule with a number of follow
up issues or questions that would be brought into the interview at appropriate moments or in
order to complete the description if they were not spontaneously addressed, such as “did that
unsupportive interaction change the other person in any way after the interaction occurred?.”
Researchers have sometimes used literary, even fictional texts or asked participants to provide
imaginative descriptions, which can be useful provided that they are not ideologically derived
and truly describe in detail a specific instance of the subject matter. The most outstanding
quality of data sought by the phenomenological researcher is concreteness, that the descriptions
reflect the details of lived situations rather than hypotheses or opinions about, explanations of,
participant starts out saying “In my view, sexual risk taking is primarily due to the failure of
parental responsibility,” the researcher might gently ask the participant to “describe an actual
situation in which you did something risky in a sexual encounter, starting beforehand and
walking through the experience from its beginning through what happened afterwards.” The
data collected should provide the researcher access to concretely described psychological life
beyond any previous knowledge or preconceptions. Descriptions are almost always surprising in
their concrete details. These descriptions of situations provide data that transcends even what the
Preparatory operations. In order to prepare the data for analysis, the phenomenological
researcher may listen to and transcribe verbal descriptions and interviews. Once in written form,
data are openly read first without the research focus in mind in order to grasp the participant’s
19
expression and meaning in the broadest context. Because the description may be lengthy and
complex, particularly when it involves time and multiple features and processes, the researcher
differentiates parts of the description, identifying “meaning units” which organize data for later
analysis of parts. For instance, in a description of career counseling, the researcher may
differentiate the following series of meaning units: 1) the participant’s surprise at a suggestion
(based on an inventory) that she has interest in business, 2) a spontaneous series of images of
unattractive business work situations, 3) voicing doubt to the counselor about the rightness of the
inventory, 4) the counselor asking the client to elaborate a series of positive responses to
In preparing the data, researchers may eliminate redundancy, for instance in interview
data, if it does not appear to contribute to the meaning of the description (sometimes redundancy
is meaningful). Other incidental and irrelevant expressions found in the description may be
eliminated. Some phenomenological researchers name themes found in the descriptions in order
identification of themes and any “coding” or categorization of data is merely preparatory in that
it organizes data conveniently for a more in depth, structural, eidetic analysis that follows.
Finally, descriptive material may be reordered so as to be maximally useful for the later analysis.
Researchers often use a narrative that temporally reflects the original experience, but material
may be ordered by themes if that better suits the research. These operations prepare an
organized written description of situation(s) in the first person language of the participant(s).
empathic. The researcher strives to leave his or her own world behind and to enter fully, through
the written description, into the situations of the participants. The researcher empathically joins
sharing of the experience is the basis for later reflection on meanings and experiential processes.
This attitude involves an extreme form of care that savors the situations described in a slow,
meditative way and attends to, even magnifies all the details. This attitude is free of value
judgments from an external frame of reference and instead focuses on the meaning of the
situation purely as it is given in the participant’s experience. This is the implementation of the
phenomenological epoché. The researcher attends not only to what is experienced but also
linguistic, social, behavioral, and so on that are involved in its constitution and in this way
number of reflective operations. The contours of the phenomenon of interest are distinguished
from its baseline--the lived experience prior to the subject matter of interest. For instance, the
researcher may note in a protocol describing “perfectionism,” the point at which the participant
begins becoming perfectionistic while working on a term paper, before which the work was not
perfectionistic. This enables the researcher to grasp what the matter being investigated is,
distinct from the rest of the participant’s experience in which it is embedded. The researcher
then goes on to distinguish its parts or constituents as preparation for discovering how the
various moments interrelate in their overall organization. For instance, in the protocol on
21
perfectionism, the participant’s imagination of the teacher citing numerous faults may be one
part of the description distinct from the student’s repetitive use of a grammar check tool. The
researcher reflects on the relevance of each part of the described situation and of the
psychological process involved, that is, what they freshly reveal for our knowledge about the
phenomenon of interest. The phenomenological researcher does not remain content to grasp the
obvious or explicit meanings but reads between the lines and deeply interrogates in order to gain
might grasp in a participant’s thought of suicide not only a challenge to an abandoning other but
an implicit trust that the other will show love though rescue. The phenomenological researcher
continually focuses on relations between different parts of the situation and the psychological
processes that subtend it while attempting to gain explicit knowledge of how each constituent
contributes to the organization of the experience as a whole. The researcher continually moves
from part to part and from part to whole in order to grasp the structural organization and
characteristic of analysis is that the researcher attempts to grasp the essence of the individual’s
Finally, in an advanced stage of the analysis, the researcher may deliberately abandon the
epoché and interrogate the situation in view of previously posited concepts and theories.
Preconceptions may be used as heuristic guides for knowledge. If they are phenomenologically
useful, they may reveal aspects of the material that were or were not yet previously evident.
the phenomenon can be instructive for a later discussion of the theory. The researcher may
synthesize his or her insights concerning the essence of this particular instance of the topic with
22
statementa in the voice and language of the psychologist. This amounts to a single case study, in
such descriptions might be titled “An Individual Psychological Structure of Being Sexually
Harrassed by a Mentor” or “An Idiographic Structure of Suicide Ideation” (see Wertz, [1983a
and 1985] for an individual analysis of Marlene’s experience of being criminally victimized).
a topic, and the research problem or goals require movement beyond particular individual
instances. Fortunately, this is possible because what is generally qualitatively true is also
necessarily evident in each individual instance encountered and analyzed in the study.
the individual empirical analyses for what seem to be general characteristics and features. Such
judgments remain relatively inconclusive inasmuch as individual analysis do not directly present
generality may be utilized as a starting point for the process of directly examining evidence that
The second procedure, which brings to bear a broader scope of empirical evidence of
generality, is to look in other cases for a feature that was identified as potentially general in the
first case in order to “verify” the broader applicability of the insight or knowledge. Anything
that can be “verified” in more than one instance is to some extent “general.” Questions
regarding “how general?” may carry us beyond qualitative research into the quantitative realm,
but generality in qualitative knowledge can be extended in other ways. Two or many more
individual cases that were analyzed individually can be compared, and commonalities can be
23
identified and delineated. Phenomenological researchers typically perform this operation with
all of the instances collected empirically in order to identify common meanings, general
constituents, themes, psychological processes, and organizational features. This operation often
yields the finding that some knowledge statements are evident in more than one case and are
therefore general but are not true of all cases. For instance, although the “dream of rescue” may
be found in some instances of suicide ideation, others may hold no such immanent meaning and
some may instead contain the implicit teleology of an end to worldly suffering in detached,
solitary peace. This gives rise to insights into typical variations in the subject matter or
knowledge of types.
what is universal is often trivial and of little use; variations (differences) that are not completely
idiosyncratic though not universally true are usually the most significant. Procedures using
systematically collected and freshly generated data may be supplemented by less formal but
invaluable use of instances generated from the researchers’ personal memory. In addition, non-
fictional publications and media (including professional literature offering genuinely concrete
descriptions), personal imaginative production, and fictional publication and media may be used
to provide broader ranging and more diverse data sources for reflection on levels of generality.
These can be essential in the researcher’s ability to extend knowledge from the idiosyncratic,
through the typical, to the highly general and finally even the universal. While remembering,
imagining, and collecting new instances of already discovered findings, the researcher stays on
the lookout for counter-instances of the phenomenon that throw into question and require
One distinctively phenomenological method is imaginative free variation, used for the
purpose of grasping general essences at various levels, for instance the essences of particular
context bound “types.” Essential knowledge in psychology does not imply freedom from context,
demonstrated by the imaginal exercise of removing that context and discovering the collapse of
meaningful coherence. Through imaginative variation, one determines what must be the case of
all (imaginable) instances for them to be considered members of the typical category of
phenomena. Giorgi (1982, pp. 332-338) clarifies the point that essential generality in
psychology tends not to be universal laws but what he calls “empirical generality,” “contingent
Presentation
does quantitative research and this is gladly provided by journals committed to publishing such
material. Beyond the framing of the research problem in light of previous knowledge, reports
researched, procedures of data collection, and methods used in organizing and analyzing the
data. The validity of these procedures is established by demonstrating their fidelity to the
phenomenon under investigation in its prescientific life-worldly presence. Because this research
emphasizes the importance of access “to the things themselves” and honors the most concrete
individual instance with the bedrock level of evidence they require, research reports may contain
raw data—verbatim descriptions provided by participants or interviews, either in the body of the
text or as an appendix. It is also common practice to include material that reflects the
25
researcher’s organization of the data and expresses lived experience quite directly, such as
succinct first person narratives distilled from long interview transcripts. Research reports may
also contain sample analyses that illustrate and account for procedures. These provide readers
with an opportunity to follow and judge the soundness and evidentiary basis of the conclusions.
Findings may be presented in various forms ranging from an abstract to a series of nutshell
propositions in bullet points or to long elaborative essays. Findings may be represented and
their capacity to maintain the groundedness of all knowledge claims. Finally, the findings may
be discussed with regard to 1) their impact on a knowledge field, e.g., bearing on previous
research and theory, 2) their practical applications, e.g., for professionals, policy makers, lay
public, and 3) their impact on participants in research which may have been problematic,
schizophrenia. Some distinctive variations present in this research are its program evaluative and
action-oriented goals, its inclusion of participants as active collaborators with the researchers, its
The research problem. The context of the study was a program in an academic medical
center where outpatients were closely monitored for symptoms and in order to prevent relapse
provided early intervention including education, individual assessment of relapse patterns, and
individualized action plans. Twice-weekly relapse prevention groups took place first during
26
hospitalization and then after discharge. Not one patient (of 36 eligible) returned to attend group
sessions after discharge. The readmission rate remained unchanged and the ineffectiveness of
the program became apparent. The researchers suspected that the program’s approach to the
problem of recidivism was limited by the clinical way the problem was defined, namely its focus
on the symptoms of disorder per se and its assumption that the return of the symptoms of
stakeholders, including family members and especially the patients themselves, had not been
taken into consideration, nor the larger social and material environment or the world and the
agency of the patient. The purpose of this research was to acquire knowledge of the problem of
recidivism by examining the actual discharge situation as lived through and experienced by the
patients themselves. The researchers were also interested in gaining knowledge of the meaning
action research, Davidson et al (2001) tracked twelve recidivist patients (defined as having had 2
rehospitalizations in the last year) “to elicit their experiences of rehospitalization, the
circumstances of this event, and the function it served in their lives” (p. 167). Data was collected
through open-ended interviews that encouraged participants to provide narratives of their life
experience leading up to, during, and following their most recent rehospitalization. Rather than
posing questions that the researchers presumed to be relevant, they allowed the descriptions to
provide access to meanings relevant to the participants. Interviewers also solicited descriptions
of the situations in which patients received the new relapse prevention interventions during
analyzed the protocols and then met together to establish a consensus about findings. The
analysis attempted to understand the participants’ experiences independent of any prior views of
the researchers, focusing on experiences from the point of view of the participants without
considering how well they conformed or did not conform to the researchers’ preconceptions.
The researchers also involved the participants themselves in the process of elaborating the
meanings found in the described situations. Transcripts were analyzed directly within each
individual and across individuals. The team identified the themes of the experience in each
individual case and unified them in an edited synthesis of their understanding of each case in
narrative form. Researchers then compared the individuals, put aside statements that were
limited to individual cases, and retained only statements that were confidently based on the data
and, even if implicit, were present in all cases, culminating in a general structural synthesis.
and integrated the strongly data-evident insights of the investigators within a single narrative. A
group of original research participants were then convened in order to read through and provide
feedback on the tentative findings. They were specifically asked to identify important areas that
were missed and to evaluate how faithful the narrative was to their experience. Finally, the
researchers collaborated with the participants to use the findings to design a new intervention.
Findings. The most striking finding was that the clinicians’ goal of preventing
rehospitalization was not found within the experience of participants. The hospital was
experienced as an attractive place of safety, food, respite, care, and privacy to which participants
appreciated being able to return as if “for a vacation,” a word used by several participants.
Perhaps the most important meaning of the hospital was “a place where people listen to you.”
28
For one participant, the positive value of the hospital grew steadily in the course of three
hospitalizations, the third being “the best I think.” This meaning of the hospital was structurally
dependent on the context of a relatively impoverished community life, for instance in a homeless
shelter or, as one participant reported, “broke, unemployed, the same harsh feeling everyday.” In
contrast to the hospital, life in the community was characterized as socially isolated, without
supportive and caring others. One participant characterized himself as “popular” in the hospital
in contrast to being alone and abandoned outside it. The self outside the hospital was
experienced as powerless and lacking in control, not just in relation to symptoms but more
importantly in relation to employment and financial well being. Even the distress associated
with symptoms gave way to a numbness and apathy that one participant described as “becoming
cold…don’t care no more.” Here the personal body is lived in the mode of powerlessness, as an
“I can’t (travel, get a job, make friends)” and temporality is one of becoming colder, more numb,
closing off to the world. As this context became increasingly established in their lives, the
participants saw mental health treatments and programs as useless, not worth any effort, and
idle exercises disconnected from their current lives. The only value of “the program,”
remembered within the hospital context, was the care shown by the “treaters” who were
experienced as the ones with power and competence. The participants saw no connection
between outpatient treatment, which was not worth the effort, and rehospitalization with its
plenitude of care.
The revised program no longer focused on teaching participants to recognize the symptoms of
schizophrenia and to act on them but on addressing participants’ isolation and loneliness by
29
helping them establish a sense of community of care in which they belong outside the hospital.
The new program also addressed the sense of powerlessness pervading participants’ self by
helping them achieve mastery over the conditions and problems that they themselves initially
identified as significant and worth the effort. The new program invited participants become
social agents—decision makers and care givers involved in something larger than themselves to
which they belonged. Finally, the researchers attempted in the new program to make the benefits
of treatment more salient from the point of view of former patients. The new program, which
was implemented by former patients who thereby became consumer-providers, included such
participants, who became agents in planning and activities that had tangible impact on their
“after care,” staying out of the hospital became a byproduct of a more meaningful, socially
satisfying, and free life. Compared to a matched control group three months after discharge, the
readmission rate of those who participated in the new program was reduced by 70% and total
Commentary
assumptions about the nature of the patients’ problems along with assumptions about the causes
interviews, to the life-world situations within and outside of the hospital as they were
experienced by former patients in the course of their own lives, 3) an analysis of the meanings of
situations inside and outside the hospital and the psychological processes that gave rise to them,
and 4) imaginative variation through which the essentials of the life-worldly experience was
30
grasped first at the level of each former patient and then at a more general level that held for all
former patients. There was no claim to universality in these findings but only to generality
within the context of the program investigated in this study. It is easy to imagine other former
patients for whom the hospital is a virtual “prison to be avoided at all cost” and is a place one
returns to only involuntarily, in the hands of more powerful others like the police. But this
different type of meaning of the hospital is not a part of the essence of recidivism as experienced
by those in the program under investigation; for them the hospital was an attractive and welcome
refuge. If a study aimed at greater generality beyond this type of recidivism, other persons
possibly from other hospitals, with other kinds of life experiences would have be selected as
participants. A study of different participants would most likely yield different context-bound
or empirically contingent essential characteristics. Perhaps there are universal truths in the
psychology of recidivism, but this was not the most fruitful level of analysis for the present study
and might be too high flown and consequently trivial for psychology as a discipline because its
subject matter is essentially quite variable and bound by its different contexts.
reflection on “the things themselves” and in its care not to impose order on its subject matter.
Phenomenology does not form theories, operationalize variables, deduce or test hypotheses, or
have essential features that can be intuited and described by the research scientist.
“Interpretation” may be employed, and may be called for in order to contextually grasp parts
can provide culturally critical and emancipatory knowledge, it is not ideologically driven and
does not subordinate its grasp of human experience to any ideology; phenomenology dwells with
and openly respects persons’ own points of view and honors the multiperspectivity found in the
the concreteness of person-world relations and accords lived experience, with all its
found in other approaches: writing down one’s preconceptions prior to carrying out the research,
keeping a research journal of reflections and insights, including participants and other
nonprofessionals in any and every phase as co-researchers, interviewing in depth, naming themes
in data, analyzing linguistic expressions, interpreting within broad contexts, deconstructing taken
for granted realities, studying individual cases, presenting narratives, critiquing culture, and
applying resolutions in action. Some of these procedures, such as in depth interviewing, are quite
generic. The value of these generic procedures is phenomenologically justified when demanded
by the nature of subject matter. Nevertheless, because they are not relevant for every research
method. Moreover, when utilized in phenomenological research, such procedures must be used
32
in distinctive ways, as for instance interviews must take place within the phenomenological
Other approaches may use procedures that are distinctively phenomenological and
essential to its approach. In some cases, other approaches to qualitative research have been
derived from or built on the work of phenomenologists, as is the case with some interpretive,
critical, collaborative, and action research. In other cases, “phenomenological” procedures have
been independently discovered and employed by other approaches. This is to be expected if the
principles and practices developed by phenomenologists are necessary and required by the
demands of scientific rigor as they interact with the nature of psychological subject matter. Any
researcher who 1) sets aside pervious theories (the epoché of sciences), 2) secures descriptive
access to the immanent meanings within psychological life as it occurs in natural contexts (the
complexities of these meanings utilizing reflection on the psychological processes that constitute
them (intentional analysis), and 4) gains insights about what is essential to the psychological
processes under study (intuition of essence, the eidetic reduction) is employing core
experience of birth by the French obstetrician Frederick Leboyer, who had formal training in
neither philosophy nor psychology, brilliantly describes and uses photographs to illustrate the
Probably the most outstanding example of such protophenomenology is William James’ (1902)
classic study of religious experiences. Although unaware of Husserl’s work, James deliberately
33
bracketed natural science, prior theories and naïve preconceptions of religion and employed the
any name other than simply “psychology.” James performed extensive and intensive intentional
analyses with rigorous imaginative variation and a consistently penetrating intuitive grasp of the
Had psychology not been dominated by the natural science approach and instead founded itself
procedures demanded by it, psychology would have been phenomenological from the beginning,
and no specifically “phenomenological” movement would have been needed. Had James’ turn
of the century investigation of religious experience been recognized as fulfilling the demands of
science, appropriated by the discipline of psychology as a good example of rigorous science, and
set the standard for research in the young discipline, psychology would have been an early leader
rather than a tardy follower in 20th Century qualitative research. If someday psychology accepts
the contributions of the phenomenological movement and appropriates them as a normative part
longer be called for and these practices could simply be called “psychological research.”
Until such a time when phenomenological contributions are incorporated into the
relatively distinct historical and ongoing movement. As such, there are eight distinctive features
that make this movement worthy of study and relevant for researchers in the field of counseling
psychology: 1) Its continuous and multiple lines of development over a 100 year period; 2) Its
34
sophisticated and still evolving philosophical foundation; 3) Its concepts and methods specially
designed for the discipline of psychology; 4) Its development across all basic disciplinary areas
such as learning, perception, language, cognition, personality, and social life; 5) Its formalization
of qualitative research methods and methodology with justification and norms concerning
reliability and validity (Giorgi, 1992, 1989a, 1989b, 1988, 1986a, 1986b & 1970; Wertz, 1999 &
1986b); 6) Its long standing and diverse contributions in specific areas of mental health and
clinical praxis, and training in qualitative research; 8) Its lively dialog with other disciplines,
with other schools of psychology, and with other approaches to research in psychology.
The phenomenological movement has expanded the conceptual foundation and practice
of science in order to include the descriptive study of subjectivity and the full human person. The
matter with an open attitude and evoking fresh, detailed descriptions that capture the richness
and complexity psychological life as it is concretely lived. This approach provides researchers
with well established methods capable of securing sensitive insights into the human meanings of
situations and the processes that engender them. This approach is especially suited for
counseling psychologists, whose work brings them close to the naturally occurring struggles and
triumphs of persons. Counseling psychologists require high fidelity knowledge of persons that
maximally respects the experience and situational contexts of those they serve. Informal
phenomenological inquiry can be seamlessly integrated with counseling practice, and formal
phenomenological research can compliment other scientific methods, both quantitative and
qualitative, as well as provide rigorous practices and knowledge in its own right. I hope that
35
approach that has developed steadily through the last century as the field enhances itself by
References
Allport, G.W. (1942). The use of personal documents in psychological science. Prepared for the
Committee on the Appraisal of Research. Bulletin #49, New York: Social Science
Council.
Cloonan, T.F. (1995). The Early History of Phenomenolocial Psychological Research Methods
Davidson, L., Stayner, D.A., Lambert, S, Smith, P., & Sledge, W.S. (2001). Phenomenological
handbook of interpretive and participatory methods (pp. 163-182). New York: New
Giorgi, A., et al. (Eds) (1971, 1975, 1979, 1983), Duquesne studies in phenomenological
University Press.
37
Giorgi, A. (1986a) Theoretical justification for the use of descriptions in psychological research.
Giorgi, A. (1986b). The role of observation and control in laboratory and field research settings.
Giorgi, A. (1988). Validity and reliability from a phenomenological perspective. In W.J. Baker,
L.P. Mos, H.V. Rappard, & H.J. Stam (Eds), Recent trends in theoretical psychology.
Giorgi, A. (1989a). One type of analysis of descriptive data: Procedures involved in following a
Giorgi, A. (1989b). Some theoretical and practical issues regarding the psychological
Giorgi, A. (1989a). An example of harmony between descriptive reports and behavior. Journal
Giorgi, A.P. & Giorgi, B.M. (2003). The descriptive phenomenolocical psychological method.
In P. Camic, J.E. Rhodes, & L. Yardley (Eds.), Qualitative research in psychology. (pp.
James, W. (1902/1982). The varieties of religious experience. New York: Penguin Books.
Laing, R.D. & Esterson, A. (1963). Sanity, madness, and the Family. New York: Penguin
Books.
Rennie, D.L., Watson, K.D., & Monteiro, A.M. (2001). The rise of qualitative research in
Smith, D.L. (2002). Fearfully and wonderfully made: The history of the Duquesne University’s
University Press.
Wertz, F.J. (1981). The birth if the infant: A developmental perspective. Journal of
Wertz, F.J. (1983a). From everyday to psychological description: Analyzing the moments of a
Wertz, F.J. (1983b). Some components of descriptive psychological reflection, Human Studies,
6(1), 35-51.
Wertz, F.J. (1985). Methods and findings in an empirical analysis of "being criminally
Wertz, F.J. (1987a). Abnormality from scientific and pre-scientific perspectives, Review of
Wertz, F.J. (1999). Multiple methods in psychology: Epistemological grounding and the
166.