Erard Viglione (2014) The Rorschach Performance Assessment System (R-PAS) in Child Custody Evaluations
Erard Viglione (2014) The Rorschach Performance Assessment System (R-PAS) in Child Custody Evaluations
Erard Viglione (2014) The Rorschach Performance Assessment System (R-PAS) in Child Custody Evaluations
To cite this article: Robert E. Erard & Donald J. Viglione (2014) The Rorschach Performance
Assessment System (R-PAS) in Child Custody Evaluations, Journal of Child Custody, 11:3, 159-180
Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the
“Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis,
our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to
the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions
and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,
and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content
should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources
of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims,
proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or
howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising
out of the use of the Content.
This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any
substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,
systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &
Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-
and-conditions
Journal of Child Custody, 11:159–180, 2014
Copyright # Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
ISSN: 1537-9418 print=1537-940X online
DOI: 10.1080/15379418.2014.943449
ROBERT E. ERARD
Psychological Institutes of Michigan, P.C., Bloomfield, Michigan
DONALD J. VIGLIONE
California School of Professional Psychology, Alliant International University,
San Diego, California
Downloaded by [Robert Erard] at 08:07 10 September 2014
159
160 R. E. Erard and D. J. Viglione
research has shown that respondents in the context of a CCE tend to elevate
on validity scales on these instruments, which raises some question about
the accuracy of self-report inventories in such high-stakes contexts
(Bagby, Nicholson, Buis, Radovanovic, & Fidler, 1999; Bathurst, Gottfried,
& Gottfried, 1997). Furthermore, even when several such instruments are
used, they are likely to have spurious correlations with each other as a result
of shared mono-method variance (Meyer, Riethmiller, Brooks, Benoit, &
Handler, 2000). Such shared variance applies to the findings from personal
interviews as well. Restricting the evidence base to personality assessment
methods that rely on a single methodology (e.g., self-report) may lead to
faulty conclusions (Meyer et al., 2001).
With these limitations concerning self-report methods in mind, this article
focuses on the Rorschach as the prototypical, non-self-report assessment instru-
ment for CCEs. First we address the advantages of including performance-based
assessment techniques, particularly the Rorschach. Next we consider the types
of questions answered by the Rorschach and how they can contribute to CCE
findings. Then, criticisms and doubts about the Rorschach are examined. Finally,
we describe how a recently introduced, evidence-focused Rorschach system
addresses previous problems with the test.
The APA guidelines for child custody evaluations in family law proceedings
(2010) emphasize the importance of using multiple methods in conducting
CCEs. Because performance-based methods1 do not share method variance
with personal interviews or self-report tests, they can be valuable resources
for such multiple method assessments. Minimizing the covariance or redun-
dancy among multiple measures or predictors provides the best opportunity
R-PAS in Child Custody 161
for incremental validity, that is, adding unique knowledge about critical
evaluation targets. Indeed, Erdberg (2008) has cogently argued that
such multimethod assessment should be considered a standard of forensic
personality assessment.
Performance-based measures address how people actually behave, not
just what they say about themselves. As with tests of intelligence or ability,
performance-based personality tests challenge the respondent to perform a
task, solve a problem, or demonstrate a process to make personality pro-
cesses and behavioral patterns amenable to observation and quantification.
Thus, they expose the personality in action, and its performance is observed
and captured for quantification and examination.
An additional advantage of many performance-based methods is that
because they involve performing in front of a live observer, they typically
require the participant to manage the associated performance anxiety. On
Downloaded by [Robert Erard] at 08:07 10 September 2014
The Rorschach stimulus situation involves very little guidance for the respon-
dent beyond presenting an inkblot and posing the question, ‘‘What might this
be?’’ In answering the question, contrary to what is popularly imagined, the
respondent does not simply project his or her imagination onto a perfectly
ambiguous inkblot and see whatever he or she wants or needs to see. It is
more accurate to conceive of the Rorschach as an interpretive problem-
solving task (Exner, 1993). Hermann Rorschach carefully structured the
inkblot images to be visually contradictory and inconsistent, so that any
response will be imperfect and flawed. Thus, with minimal support from
the examiner, the respondent must solve the problem of how to offer an
162 R. E. Erard and D. J. Viglione
involved parenting; child abuse; role reversals between parents and children;
and alienating co-parenting behavior (Johnston et al., 2005); albeit, it should
never be used on its own to ‘‘diagnose’’ such problems.
It is generally best not to use the Rorschach or any other psychological
test in a mechanical fashion to hunt for pathology as represented by simple
discrepancies from nonpatient norms. Many personality characteristics discov-
ered in this fashion may have little to do with the important referral questions
(or allegations) on which the case hinges. Moreover, one often cannot readily
determine the extent to which a particular trait (e.g., narcissism or depen-
dency) necessarily translates into a parenting problem. A preferable approach
is first to consider what is at issue in the particular case. Are there concerns
about parental violence or self-destructive behavior? Is there reason to be
concerned that a parent has serious difficulty with self-other boundaries and
cannot distinguish her own needs from the child’s needs? Does a parent show
signs of severe instability of mood? Does a parent easily become confused
about what is real vs. what he imagines or thinks in a way that is confusing
to both himself and others? If one starts with the important psychological
questions presented by the pleadings, the referral questions, the parental alle-
gations, or the family history (i.e., from the ‘‘story’’ of the case; Erard, 2005),
one is much more likely to use the Rorschach effectively in efforts to answer
such questions and to provide a richer understanding of the problems at hand.
Although the Rorschach has been widely used in testing adults and children
in child custody contexts (Ackerman & Ackerman, 1997; Quinnell & Bow,
2001), many forensic psychologists avoid it for a number of reasons,
including lack of confidence in or knowledge of the reliability and validity
164 R. E. Erard and D. J. Viglione
of inferences drawn while using the instrument, concerns about the admissi-
bility of Rorschach-based testimony as evidence, and worries about being
able to explain the Rorschach in court. Each of these will be briefly addressed
in the following discussion, along with references for exploring these matters
in greater depth.
levels of inter-coder reliability is typically achieved for nearly all of the coded
variables with adequate base rates (Meyer et al., 2002; Parker, Hanson, &
Hunsley, 1998; Viglione & Meyer, 2008). The overall reliability for the
Rorschach CS and Rorschach Oral Dependency scale (recast as Oral Depen-
dency Language in R-PAS) are excellent, with chance-corrected summary
score coefficients of about .90 and response-level judgments between .80
and .85 (Viglione & Meyer, 2008). Similar inter-coder reliability results have
been found using the Rorschach Performance Assessment System (R-PAS;
Viglione, Blume-Marcovici, et al., 2012). Indeed, the reliability found among
most Rorschach variables in current use is comparable to that of simple,
physical measurements in medicine and much better than judgments such
as surgeons’ diagnoses of breast abnormalities and employees’ supervisors’
evaluations of job performance (Meyer et al., 2002).
The Rorschach has been in continuous clinical use for over 90 years and
has been studied in thousands of peer-reviewed publications throughout the
world. Nevertheless, several Rorschach critics have persistently claimed that
‘‘the Rorschach is invalid’’ (e.g., Garb, 1999). Indeed this point has been
argued so repetitiously that one sometimes hears clinicians, researchers,
and even journalists echoing this catchphrase as though it were an estab-
lished scientific fact (e.g., Herbert, 2009). The arguments for invalidity
include observations that some past Rorschach research data sets have been
difficult to obtain, that some Rorschach research has shown poor method-
ology, that some has relied on illusory correlations, that many results have
not been replicated or that validity studies often show negligible correlations
with various self-report instruments (with the latter treated as the ‘‘gold stan-
dard’’ criterion for the participants’ ‘‘actual’’ personality characteristics), that
several studies suggest that Rorschach CS norms may overpathologize rela-
tively normal individuals, and that some Rorschach scores have fared poorly
in predicting particular (self-report based) diagnostic classifications with
which they could be theoretically aligned. Most of these arguments were
R-PAS in Child Custody 165
Bow, 2001). Meyer and colleagues (2001) found that the results from such
Rorschach meta-analyses compare favorably to those from other widely
respected psychological and medical tests.
Rorschach critics (e.g., Garb, Wood, Lilienfeld, & Nezworski, 2005) have
responded to this meta-analytic evidence with the observation that such
research on ‘‘global’’ test efficacy cannot be used to establish the validity
of individual Rorschach variables, which is a fair point. As Messick (1995)
has emphasized, it is not particularly meaningful to speak of the validity of
a personality test. Rather, validity applies to particular test variables and their
interpretations and must be evaluated in the context of the purposes for
which the instrument is used.
Addressing the criticism of Garb et al. (2005), Mihura et al. (2013)
recently reviewed the enormous CS Rorschach literature and meta-
analytically evaluated the specific evidence concerning the validity of each
of 53 individual CS Rorschach variables.3 This achievement has made it poss-
ible to identify with considerable specificity which variables are
well-supported, poorly supported or unsupported, and understudied. It turns
out (as is also true with any of the major self-report personality tests) that
there are many strongly supported variables and a number of weakly sup-
ported ones. Its findings provided the most important contribution to decid-
ing which CS variables would be included in the R-PAS system. All but one4
of 30 variables that were found to have good or excellent support are
included in R-PAS.
Additional meta-analytic studies of non-CS variables, such as the
Rorschach Oral Dependency Scale (ROD) by Bornstein (1999) and Walsh,
Mihura, and Meyer (2012); the Ego Impairment Index (EII) by Diener,
Hilsenroth, Shaffer, and Sexton (2011); the Mutuality of Autonomy scale
(MOA) by Graceffo, Mihura, and Meyer (2014); and Aggressive Content
(AGC) by Kiss, Mihura, and Meyer (2012) have all demonstrated medium
effect size relationships with nonself-report criteria. Again, taking into
166 R. E. Erard and D. J. Viglione
account the research evidence, R-PAS adopted all of these variables, with
some adjustments or modifications.
One of the most important conclusions from the Mihura et al. (2013)
meta-analysis and other reviews (e.g., Greenwald, Poehlman, Uhlmann, &
Banaji, 2009; Hiller et al., 1999; Viglione & Meyer, 2008; Viglione, 1999) is
that many Rorschach variables do better at predicting particular ‘‘real world’’
or ‘‘life outcome’’ criteria (such as behavioral ratings by expert observers,
personal history, and treatment status) than they do at predicting diagnostic
or other self-report based classifications. Thus, the fact that a particular
Rorschach score correlates poorly with a closely related MMPI-2 score cannot
be regarded as evidence of the invalidity of the Rorschach score (cf.,
Ganellen, 1996; Meyer et al., 2001), any more than the generally weak
correlations between self-report and informant reports of personality traits
invalidate the latter. The absence of strong associations between the
Downloaded by [Robert Erard] at 08:07 10 September 2014
mentioning the Rorschach in federal, state, and military courts of appeal prior
to 1987. Criticisms doubting the scientific validity of the test were raised in
only 10.5% of these cases. Perhaps this should not be surprising. As the
second most commonly used personality instrument among clinical psychol-
ogists and neuropsychologists (Camara, Nathan, & Puente, 2000), the second
or third most commonly used personality test in child custody evaluations
(approximately 45% of evaluators use it; Ackerman & Ackerman, 1997;
Hagen & Castagna, 2001; Quinnell & Bow, 2001), and with nearly 100 new
Rorschach articles being published per year (Butcher & Rouse, 1996), the
Rorschach can hardly be rejected under the Frye v. United States (1923)
standard (the most commonly used standard for admissibility in those years)
as lacking general acceptance. But importantly, the majority of these courts
also found the Rorschach to be reliable (i.e., valid).
Contrary to the expectations of some forensic psychologists, the
Rorschach did not suffer more admissibility problems after the introduction
of the Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals, Inc.6 standard in 1993.
Weiner, Exner, and Sciara (1996) surveyed psychologists who had presented
Rorschach-based testimony in court. Out of nearly 8,000 reported cases, only
one case emerged in which Rorschach-based testimony was excluded. In an
update of his prior research, Meloy (2008) reviewed all published appellate
citations between 1996 and 2005, a period that coincided with an upsurge in
journal articles critical of the Rorschach. He found that Rorschach citations
were three times as frequent during this period. Nevertheless, the test itself
was only actually criticized in three cases (2% of the total).
But even if Rorschach-based testimony is almost always admitted, what
about the danger of devastating cross-examinations by opposing counsel and
challenges by other experts? Returning to the Weiner et al. (1996) survey of
forensic psychologists offering Rorschach-based testimony at the trial court
level, out of nearly 8,000 cases, the use of the Rorschach as a basis for expert
opinions was only challenged in six, usually unsuccessfully. Although it is
168 R. E. Erard and D. J. Viglione
possible that in the most recent seven years such challenges at the trial
level have become more frequent, such a sudden shift has not come to
our attention. Indeed, the absence of any corresponding uptick in criticism
of the test at the appellate level implies that they are either no more frequent
or even less effective.
‘‘What might this be?’’) and the processes and behaviors involved in solving that
task.7 The inkblot designs present incomplete, evocative, yet contradictory stim-
uli, for which no single answer fits the blot perfectly. Assessment research shows
that we resolve, accept, account for, or ignore these inconsistencies in character-
istic ways that reflect how we resolve conflicts in everyday life. For example,
a detail-oriented person may focus on individual small areas of the blot and
attribute importance to them in organizing responses. One who glosses over
such details and inconsistencies might provide a more impressionistic, approxi-
mate answer, using the entire blot. Sad or depressed people who experience
themselves as damaged or who feel the world is a gloomy place are likely to
express this outlook by giving responses that attribute broken, injured, or sad
features to the inkblots. Those who personalize their interpretation of reality
and have poor judgment will often see things on the card that most people have
trouble seeing the same way. Thus, the Rorschach elicits a sample of behavior
that reflects how people look at the world, the kind of persons they are,
and how they are likely to approach things that happen in their lives. By
explaining the test process in this manner, the expert can demonstrate to
the court that the processes involved in the Rorschach are not so mysterious,
but are actually quite consonant with our everyday dealings in the world.
Finally, one can discuss research findings showing that the Rorschach
variables with the most validity are those whose scored test behavior most
closely resembles the clinical content of interest or interpretation (e.g.,
cognitive codes and the communication and thinking slips of a person
with schizophrenia; Mihura et al., 2013).
Based on the authors’ own extensive experience in offering Rorschach-
based testimony in court, we believe that most often this kind of approach
will suffice to persuade the trier of fact that the Rorschach is an understand-
able procedure that makes sense. As Weiner (2008) wisely points out, ‘‘The
courtroom is not the place to elaborate the multiple intervariable interactions
that shape a sophisticated interpretation of Rorschach data’’ (p. 123). For the
R-PAS in Child Custody 169
most part, judges and juries quickly lose interest in richly detailed and highly
technical explanations that are not strictly necessary for understanding the
substance of the relevant opinions being offered.
For the unusual circumstance of facing an aggressive cross-examination
on Rorschach-based testimony, Weiner (2008) offers a number of helpful
examples of effective answers to likely gambits. Recognition that the
Rorschach is one of the most widely taught and utilized, best researched,
and most useful tools in personality assessment, that it has consistently
evolved and changed in response to new research and valid criticisms, and
that Rorschach inferences that are included in the final report have been
checked against and integrated with findings from multiple sources can serve
as a basis for a persuasive defense of one’s methods.
Of course some familiarity with professional and popular literature that
is critical of the Rorschach, along with the literature that effectively responds
Downloaded by [Robert Erard] at 08:07 10 September 2014
Variable Selection
Much as Exner reviewed the Rorschach literature prior to the mid-70 s as
a foundation for selecting CS variables in 1974, the R-PAS authors reviewed
the contemporary empirical literature to decide which variables to select for
R-PAS (Meyer et al., 2011, Chapter 18). Those variables with the greatest empiri-
cal and response process behavioral support are emphasized on Page 1 of the
R-PAS Summary Score and Profile and those with less support are listed on
Page 2. The conceptual interpretive domains, which consist of groups of
inter-related variables found on these pages, have obvious implications for par-
enting and custody evaluations. Cognitive Processing and Engagement vari-
ables inform us about the complexity of cognitive processes, sophistication
of decision-making, and motivation and aspirations. Perception and Thinking
Problems reveal information on severity of psychological disturbance, prob-
lems in judgment, lapses in reasoning, cognitive confusion, and at worst, psy-
chosis and thought disorder. Stress and Distress address depression, anxiety,
emotional vulnerability, and trauma reactions versus hardiness and resilience.
Self and Other Representation variables reveal information about interpersonal
relatedness, understanding of others and relationships, and relational themes
such as aggression, mutuality, neediness, passivity, and dependency.
Advances in Administration
Since 1949, the variable number of responses (R) in Rorschach administration
has been considered a psychometric problem and a threat to research pro-
gress and interpretive validity (Cronbach, 1949), especially because many
R-PAS in Child Custody 171
variables are correlated with R (Viglione & Meyer, 2008). With its introduction
of R-Optimized administration, R-PAS has greatly reduced nuisance and error
variability introduced by virtually eliminating overly short and sharply reduc-
ing overly long records (Dean, Viglione, Perry, & Meyer, 2007, 2008; Meyer
et al., 2011; Reese, Viglione, & Giromini, 2014). With the added R-Optimized
instruction (‘‘please give two, or maybe three responses,’’ followed by
prompts when only one response is offered and a request to return the card
after a maximum of four), examiner variability as expressed through tenden-
cies to elicit long or short records is also greatly reduced.8 Research summar-
ized in the R-PAS manual indicates that central tendencies of variables are not
influenced by this change in administration, so that normative targets should
be as expected with R-PAS.
With R-PAS, examiner variability in coding is also potentially minimized
by the more detailed guidelines, although this is yet to be empirically demon-
Downloaded by [Robert Erard] at 08:07 10 September 2014
strated. For the first time in the literature, extensive instructions are provided
on how to clarify responses in the service of accurate coding, so as to elicit rel-
evant information but not to provoke unwanted elaborations that obscure the
individual’s natural behavior and cognitive and affective processing patterns.
CONCLUSION
itations with previous systems, but these have been addressed by the recently
introduced Rorschach system, R-PAS. Its standard score profiles make R-PAS
findings more amenable to meaningful interpretation and presentation in
court. In addition, interpretations themselves have been sharpened to align
with the available research and covert processes involved in test behaviors.
Major R-PAS interpretive domains, supported by the personality assessment
literature, such as engagement and cognitive processing; perception and
thinking problems; stress and distress; and understanding of self, others,
and relationships are pertinent and often central to CCEs. Accordingly, the
Rorschach, in its most modern form, is well-suited to CCEs and provides
an evidentiary basis for inferences about personality functioning that supple-
ments and helps to clarify and organize information from other sources.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
NOTES
1. The term, ‘‘projective test,’’ with its misleading and pejorative connotations, has been replaced in
favor of designations for these tests as ‘‘performance’’ or ‘‘performance-based’’ tests of personality (Meyer
& Kurtz, 2006).
2. In an intriguing recent development, mirror neurons, which neuroscience research has implicated
in empathy and theory of mind, have been shown to be implicated in the formation of Human Movement
responses on the Rorschach (Giromini, Porcelli, Viglione, Parolin, & Pineda, 2010; Pineda, Porcelli,
Parolin, & Viglione, 2011; Porcelli, Giromini, Parolin, Pineda, & Viglione, 2013.
3. For narrative summaries of validity of individual variables see Bornstein and Masling (2005), Exner
and Erdberg (2005), and Viglione (1999).
174 R. E. Erard and D. J. Viglione
4. Although Frequency of Organizational Activity (Zf) validly measures sophistication and effort in
processing, it was omitted based on considerations of parsimony and lack of incremental validity over other
R-PAS variables, especially the variable Complexity (Meyer, Viglione, Mihura, Erard, & Erdberg, 2011).
5. This should not necessarily be an unexpected finding. People who are honest with themselves
may be able to respond with reasonable accuracy to questions about their degree of intelligence, level
of artistic talent, or degree of attractiveness to the opposite sex, but we know that such reports are by
no means so highly correlated with the results from I.Q. tests or independent expert judgments as to
be used interchangeably with them (Mabe & West, 1982). In the absence of ‘‘gold standard’’ criteria,
self-report and performance-based tests often stand on a roughly equal footing, with each reflecting valid
representations of different aspects of the same construct (Bornstein, 2002).
6. For more detailed discussions of the applicability of the Frye v. United States (1923) and Daubert v.
Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals, Inc. (1993) standards to the Rorschach, see Erard (2012); Erard, Meyer, and
Viglione (2014); McCann and Evans (2008); and Ritzler, Erard, and Pettigrew (2002a, 2002b); but see Grove
and Barden (1999) and Grove, Barden, Garb, and Lilienfeld (2002).
7. This is the behavioral representational or response process point of view.
8. R-Optimization is particularly helpful in forensic contexts, such as child custody, where defensive
responding may lead to litigants producing brief and uninformative records.
9. Currently available non-patient data for children and adolescents are less coherent and more
Downloaded by [Robert Erard] at 08:07 10 September 2014
incomplete than those for adults (Meyer, Erdberg, & Shaffer, 2007; Hamel, Shaffer, & Erdberg, 2000).
Accordingly, R-PAS interpretation of non-adult records considers only those variables for which adequate
data are available and takes a very conservative approach before considering child and adolescent scores
pathological (Rorschach Performance Assessment System, 2012). Examiners around the world are being
trained and certified to collect new R-PAS normative data for both adults and children.
10. For more detailed discussions of admissibility issues surrounding R-PAS in particular, see Erard,
2012, and Erard, Meyer, and Viglione, 2014; but see Gurley, Piechowski, Sheehan, and Gray, 2014, and
Kvisto, Gacono, and Medoff, 2013.
REFERENCES
Butcher, J. N., Graham, J. R., Ben-Porath, Y. S., Tellegen, A., Dahlstrom, W. G., &
Kaemmer, B. (2001). Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2 (rev.
ed.). Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.
Butcher, J. N., & Rouse, S. V. (1996). Personality: Individual differences and clinical
assessment. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 87–111.
Calloway, G. C. (2005). The Rorschach: Its use in child custody evaluations. Journal
of Child Custody, 2, 143–158.
Camara, W. J., Nathan, J. S., & Puente, A. E. (2000). Psychological test usage: Implica-
tions in professional psychology. Professional Psychology: Research and
Practice, 31, 141–154.
Campbell, D. T., & Fiske, D. W. (1959). Convergent and discriminant validation by
the multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 56, 81–105.
Cronbach, L. J. (1949). Statistical methods applied to Rorschach scores: A review.
Psychological Bulletin, 46, 393–429.
Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals, Inc. 509, US 579 (1993).
Downloaded by [Robert Erard] at 08:07 10 September 2014
Dean, K. L., Viglione, D. J., Perry, W., & Meyer, G. J. (2007). A method to optimize
the response range while maintaining Rorschach Comprehensive System
validity. Journal of Personality Assessment, 89, 149–161.
Dean, K. L., Viglione, D. J., Perry, W., & Meyer, G. J. (2008). Correction to: ‘‘A method
to optimize the response range while maintaining Rorschach Comprehensive
System validity’’. Journal of Personality Assessment, 90, 204.
Diener, M. J., Hilsenroth, M. J., Shaffer, S. A., & Sexton, J. A. (2011). A meta-analysis
of the relationships between the Rorschach Ego Impairment Index (EII) and
psychiatric severity. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 18, 464–485.
Erard, R. E. (2005). What the Rorschach can contribute to child custody and
parenting time evaluations. Journal of Child Custody, 2, 119–142.
Erard, R. E. (2007). Picking cherries with blinders on: A comment on Erickson
et al. (2007) regarding the use of tests in family court. Family Court Review,
45(2), 175–184.
Erard, R. E. (2012). Expert testimony using the Rorschach Performance Assessment
System in psychological injury cases. Psychological Injury and Law, 5(2),
122–134.
Erard, R. E., Meyer, G. J., & Viglione, D. J. (2014). Setting the record straight:
Comment on Gurley, Piechowski, Sheehan, & Gray (2014) on the admissibility
of the Rorschach Performance Assessment System (R-PAS) in court. Psychological
Injury and Law, 7, 165–177. doi:10.1007=s12207-014-9195-x
Erdberg, P. (2008). Multimethod assessment as a forensic standard. In C. B. Gacono
& F. B. Evans (Eds. with N. Kaser-Boyd & L. A. Gacono), The handbook of
forensic Rorschach assessment (pp. 561–566). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Erickson, S. K., Lilienfeld, S. O., & Vitacco, M. J. (2007). A critical examination of suit-
ability and limitation of psychological tests in family court. Family Court Review,
45, 185–192.
Exner, J. (2007). A new U.S. adult nonpatient sample. Journal of Personality
Assessment, 89(S1), S154–S158.
Exner, J. E. (1993). The Rorschach: A Comprehensive System, Vol. 1: Basic
foundations (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Wiley.
176 R. E. Erard and D. J. Viglione
Exner, J. E., Jr. (1974). The Rorschach: A comprehensive system (Vol. 1). New York,
NY: Wiley.
Exner, J. E, Jr., & Erdberg, P. (2005). The Rorschach: A comprehensive system, Vol. 2:
Advanced interpretation (3rd ed.). Oxford: Wiley.
Foster, S. L., & Cone, J. D. (1995). Validity issues in clinical assessment. Psychological
Assessment, 7, 248–260.
Frye v. United States, 392, F. 1013 (D.C. Cir.) (1923).
Gacono, C. B., Evans, F. B., & Viglione, D. J. (2002). The Rorschach in forensic
practice. Journal of Forensic Psychology Practice, 2, 33–54.
Ganellen, R. J. (1996). Integrating the Rorschach and the MMPI-2 in personality
assessment. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Garb, H. N. (1999). Call for a moratorium on the use of the Rorschach inkblot test in
clinical and forensic settings. Assessment, 6(4), 313–317.
Garb, H. N., Wood, J. M., Lilienfeld, S. O., & Nezworski, M. T. (2005). Roots of the
Rorschach controversy. Clinical Psychology Review, 25, 97–118.
Downloaded by [Robert Erard] at 08:07 10 September 2014
Giromini, L., Porcelli, P., Viglione, D. J., Parolin, L., & Pineda, J. A. (2010). The
feeling of movement: EEG evidence for mirroring activity during the observa-
tions of static, ambiguous stimuli in the Rorschach cards. Biological Psychology,
85, 233–241.
Graceffo, R. A., Mihura, J. L., & Meyer, G. J. (2014). Mutuality of Autonomy Scale:
A systematic review of the validity literature. Journal of Personality Assessment.
doi:10.1080=00223891.2014.919299
Graham, J. R. (2000). MMPI-2: Assessing personality and psychopathology (3rd ed.).
New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Greene, R. L. (2000). The MMPI-2: An interpretive manual (2nd ed.). Needham
Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Greenwald, A. G., Poehlman, T. A., Uhlmann, E., & Banaji, M. R. (2009). Understand-
ing and using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive
validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 17–41.
Grove, W. M., & Barden, R. C. (1999). Protecting the integrity of the legal system:
The admissibility of testimony from mental health experts under Daubert=
Kumho analyses. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 5(1), 224–242.
Grove, W. M., Barden, R. C., Garb, H. N., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2002). Failure of Rorschach-
comprehensive-system-based testimony to be admissible under the Daubert-Joiner-
Kumho standard. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 8(2), 216–234.
Gurley, J. R., Piechowski, B. L., Sheehan, L. D., & Gray, J. (2014). The admissibility of
the R-PAS in court. Psychological Injury and Law, 7, 9–17.
Hagen, M. A., & Castagna, N. (2001). The real numbers: Psychological testing in custody
evaluations. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 32(3), 269–271.
Hamel, M., Shaffer, T. W., & Erdberg, P. (2000). A study of nonpatient preadolescent
Rorschach protocols. Journal of Personality Assessment, 73, 280–294.
Herbert, W. (2009, July 30). Analyze this. The real problem with the Rorschach:
It doesn’t work. [Newsweek Web log message]. Retrieved from http://content.
knowledgeplex.org/ksg/ cache=assets=2801=3235= 3235154.html
Hiller, J. B., Rosenthal, R, Bornstein, R. F., Berry, D. T. R., & Brunell-Neuleib, S. (1999).
A comparative meta-analysis of Rorschach and MMPI validity. Psychological
Assessment, 11, 278–296.
R-PAS in Child Custody 177
Millon, T., Millon, C., & Davis, R. (1994). Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory-III.
Minneapolis, MN: Pearson Assessments.
Moore, R. C., Viglione, D. J., Rosenfarb, I. S., Patterson, T. L., & Mausbach, B. T. (2013).
Rorschach measures of cognition relate to everyday and social functioning in
schizophrenia. Psychological Assessment, 25, 253–263. doi:10.1037=a0030546
Morey, L. C. (2007). Personality Assessment Inventory professional manual (2nd ed.).
Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Parker, K. C., Hanson, R. K., & Hunsley, J. (1988). MMPI, Rorschach, and WAIS:
A meta-analytic comparison of reliability, stability, and validity. Psychological
Bulletin, 103, 367–373.
Pineda, J., Porcelli, P., Parolin, L., & Viglione, D. J. (2011). Mu suppression and
human movement responses to the Rorschach test. NeuroReport, 5, 223–226.
Porcelli, P., Giromini, L., Parolin, L., Pineda, J. A., & Viglione, D. J. (2013). Mirroring
activity in the brain and movement determinant in the Rorschach test. Journal of
Personality Assessment, 95, 444–456. doi:10.1080=00223891.2013.775136
Quinnell, F. A., & Bow, J. N. (2001). Psychological tests used in child custody
evaluations. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 19, 491–501.
Reese, J. B., Viglione, D. J., & Giromini, L. (2014). A comparison between Compre-
hensive System and an early version of the Rorschach Performance Assessment
System administration with outpatient children and adolescents. Journal of
Personality Assessment. doi:10.1080=00223891.2014.889700
Ritzler, B., Erard, R., & Pettigrew, G. (2002a). Protecting the integrity of Rorschach
expert witnesses: A reply to Grove and Barden (1999) re: The admissibility of
testimony under Daubert=Kumho analyses. Psychology, Public Policy, and
Law, 8, 201–215.
Ritzler, B., Erard, R., & Pettigrew, G. (2002b). A final reply to Grove and Barden:
The relevance of the Rorschach Comprehensive System for expert testimony.
8, 235–246.
Rorschach, H. (1921). Psychodiagnostik. Bern, Switzerland: Bircher.
Rorschach Performance Assessment System. (2012). Problems with child and ado-
lescent normative reference data for the Rorschach and the interim solution
adopted by R-PAS. Retrieved from http://www.r-pas.org/AboutStatement.aspx
R-PAS in Child Custody 179