Success in Reading - Four Characteristics of Strategic Readers
Success in Reading - Four Characteristics of Strategic Readers
Success in Reading - Four Characteristics of Strategic Readers
Language Arts
Volume 30 Article 4
Issue 1 September/October 1989
10-1-1989
L.D. Briggs
East Texas State University
Recommended Citation
Brown, D. L., & Briggs, L. (1989). Success in Reading: Four Characteristics of Strategic Readers. Reading
Horizons: A Journal of Literacy and Language Arts, 30 (1). Retrieved from
https://scholarworks.wmich.edu/reading_horizons/vol30/iss1/4
Effective Readers
Brown (1982) referred to effective readers as those stu-
dents who have some awareness of and control over their
cognitive reading skills. Interviews with children concerning
their reading knowledge revealed vague and often inaccurate
conceptions of reading (Clay, 1979). The less-skilled readers
showed little awareness of the need to use different strate-
gies for variations in reading purposes and texts. On the other
READING HORIZONS, Fall, 1989 page 31
Monitoring Comprehension
Recently there has been growing interest by cognitive
psychologists in readers' metacognitive knowledge. Meta-
cognition is a knowledge of and a conscious attempt to control
one's own cognitive processes (Flavell, 1979). During the
reading process, the pupil, the task, and the strategy used are
key factors to be evaluated according to the metacognitive
model (Saker, 1979). In analyzing reading comprehension,
teachers must give consideration to what readers know about
the gaining of meaning, how readers self-regulate the search
for meaning, and what strategies to employ if the reader fails
to understand. This process is known as comprehension
monitoring.
Summary
What are four characteristics of strategic readers? Strate-
gic readers establish goals, select appropriate strategies,
monitor comprehension, and display a positive attitude to-
ward reading. Reading is a major academic skill that is
introduced to children during the early school years. There-
fore reading instruction should prepare students to interact in
a meaningful manner with a variety of texts. Good readers
focus their attention on the major ideas as they incorporate
the metacognitive skills needed to accomplish the task of
understanding text. Consequently, good readers are strate-
gic readers who have developed the necessary skills to profit
fully from the decoding process of reading.
READING HORIZONS, Fall, 1989 page 37
References
Allington, R. L. (1980). Teacher interruption behaviors during primary-grade
oral reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, 371-377.
Anderson, R. H., & Armbruster, B. B. (1982). Reader and text studying
strategies. In W. Otto & S. White (Eds.), Reading expository material.
NY: Academic Press.
Baker, L. (1979). Comprehension monitoring: Identifying and coping with
text confusions. Journal of Reading Behavior, 11, 365-374.
Briggs, L. (1987). A poor attitude: A deterrent to reading improvement.
Reading Horizons, 27,202-208.
Brown, A. {1982}. Learning how to leam from reading. In J. Langer & M. T.
Smith-Burke (Eds.),Reader meets author: Bridging the gap. Newark, DE:
International Reading Association.
Brown, D., & Briggs, L. (1987). Collaborative learning: Bridging the gap
between reading and writing. Reading Improvement... 24,278-281.
Clay, M. (1979). Reading: The patterning of complex behavior. Auckland,
New Zealand: Heinemann.
Durkin, D. (1978-1979). What classroom observations reveal about reading
comprehension instruction. Reading Research Quarterly, 14,481-533.
Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring. American
Psychologist, 34, 906-911 .
Johnston, P. (1983). Reading comprehension assessment: A cognitive
basis. Newark, DE: International Reading Association.
Kennedy, L. D., & Halinski, R. S. (1978). Measuring attitudes: An extra
dimension. Journal of Reading, 18,518-522.
Kleiman, G. (1981). Comparing good and poor readers: A critique of
research. Center for the Study of Reading, Technical Report No. 246.
Markman, E. M. (1979). Realizing that you don't understand: Elementary
school children's awareness of inconsistencies. Child Development, 50,
643-655.
Myers, M., & Paris, S. G. (1978). Children's metacognitive knowledge
about reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 70,680-690.
Paris, S., & Myers, M. (1981). Comprehension monitoring, memory and
study strategies of good and poor readers. Journal of Reading Behavior,
13,5-22.
Schofield, H. (1980). Reading attitude and achievement: Teacher-pupil
relationships. Journal of Educational Research, 74,111-119.
Stauffer, R. (1969). Directing reading maturity as a cognitive process.
New York: Harper & Row.
Swanson, B. (1988). Strategic preferences of good and poor beginning
readers. Reading Horizons, 28, 255-262.
Thorndike, E. L. (1917). Reading as reasoning: A study of mistakes in
paragraph reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 8, 323-332.
READING HORIZONS, Fall, 1989 page 38
After our reading was completed, I set up the following hypothetical situation:
As a group we will go to Lansing to watch a basketball game in which MagiC
Johnson is going to be playing. The weather is favorable and we plan to hike on
the back roads from Hudsonville to Lansing. We are going to camp along the way
and in order to make the trip more fun, we will be telling stories along the way as
well as around the campfire.
Monday arrived. I had slipped out between classes and had built a "campfire"
in a vacant inner room, from crumpled red tissue paper piled overflashlights. We
left our classroom, destination unknown to the students, walked down the hall
and entered the darkened room to a whispered chorus of "Oooh, neat!" We read
our various tales by the light of a flashlight which was passed around, and found
that, as the pilgrims, we had many and diverse tales to tell.
The experience was worthwhile and enjoyable for us all. Each student had
a chance to shine, and the class had an opportunity to transcend the "here and
now" through their writing.