Gregory M. Walton - Alia J. Crum - How Social Psychology Can Help People Change
Gregory M. Walton - Alia J. Crum - How Social Psychology Can Help People Change
Handbook of
Wise Interventions
HOW SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
CAN HELP PEOPLE CHANGE
edited by
Gregory M. Walton
Alia J. Crum
No part of this book may be reproduced, translated, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted,
in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, microfilming, recording,
or otherwise, without written permission from the publisher.
Gregory M. Walton, PhD, is Associate Professor of Psychology and the Michael Forman
University Fellow in Undergraduate Education at Stanford University. His research
focuses on how basic social-psychological processes contribute to major social problems,
such as how negative stereotypes and stigma change school settings for minority group
members in ways that can undermine these students’ feelings of belonging and achieve-
ment. Dr. Walton develops novel psychological interventions to address these processes,
including to increase student motivation, improve academic achievement, and reduce
achievement gaps between groups. He is a recipient of many awards for his research,
including the Cialdini Prize and the W
egner Theoretical Innovation Prize from the Soci-
ety for Personality and Social Psychology.
Alia J. Crum, PhD, is Assistant Professor of Psychology at Stanford University and Pri-
mary Investigator of the Stanford Mind and Body Lab. Her research focuses on mindsets;
how they affect important outcomes in such domains as exercise, diet, and stress; and
how they can be consciously and deliberately changed through intervention, to increase
physiological and psychological well-being. Dr. Crum is a recipient of awards, including
the Director’s New Innovator Award from the National Institutes of Health and the Ris-
ing Star Award from the Association for Psychological Science. She has worked as a clini-
cal psychologist for the VA health care system and has developed interventions focused
on mindset change for organizations, including LinkedIn, UBS, Stanford Health Care,
and the U.S. Navy.
v
Contributors
vii
viii Contributors
Introduction 1
Gregory M. Walton and Alia J. Crum
PA R T I . Education
xi
xii Contents
PA R T I V. Sustainability
18. The Social Norms Approach: A Wise Intervention for Solving 405
Social and Environmental Problems
Jessica M. Nolan, P. Wesley Schultz, Robert B. Cialdini,
and Noah J. Goldstein
Author Index 448
Gregory M. Walton and Alia J. Crum
The interventions shared in this volume address some of the hardest problems we face in
our personal lives and as a society. Long-standing educational inequalities lead to lost
potential, social conflict, and worse health and economic prospects. Too many people
are unhappy in their marriages and stressed in their jobs. Rates of obesity, diabetes, and
cardiovascular disease continue to creep upward, but even simple remedies, like motivat-
ing healthy eating, remain notoriously difficult. Conflicts between cultures and groups
appear intractable. Climate change is upon us, yet reversing widely practiced unsustain-
able behaviors can feel like a lost cause.
You might wonder: What in the world are such diverse problems all doing in the
same book? The answer is that we can make progress on each using a “psychologically
wise” intervention (Walton & Wilson, 2018). Each chapter describes the everyday inter-
pretations that feed into a problem and, in turn, shows how helping people to new ways
of thinking that are authentic, appropriate, and adaptive for their context can make a
difference, sometimes profound.
The “wise intervention” approach is distinct from other traditions and social reforms
because it does not rely on improving the objective qualities of situations, such as to
hire more tutors or to rearrange a cafeteria to make healthy options more accessible,
or of people, such as to increase students’ IQ. Instead, the power of wise interventions
comes from the rich intersection between psychological interpretations and the contexts
in which they arise. Wise interventions recognize that how we make sense of ourselves,
other people, and our circumstances does not exist in a vacuum but is a product of the
cultural and social contexts in which we live. They then rely on a simple observation:
that altering a maladaptive or pejorative interpretation, especially at a key time or in an
important context, can change how a person engages with the contexts and settings they
inhabit.
An enormous benefit of this approach is that, in the right circumstances, an initial
change in interpretation can become self-confirming. Then, even a brief exercise can
1
2 Introduction
transform people’s lives. For example, the intervention discussed by Slotter and Luchies
(Chapter 16) used three 7-minute exercises to help couples take a third-party perspective
on conflict in their marriage. This strengthened marital relationships over a year. Walton
and Brady (Chapter 2) discuss a social-belonging intervention, delivered in an hour-long
session in the first year of college. It improved grades over 3 years and life and career
satisfaction 7–11 years later.
How is that possible? How do these interventions work? What do they tell us about
people and social problems and efforts at reform?
To showcase this work, we invited the leading researchers who originally developed
diverse wise interventions to share their work. We gave each author a detailed 12-point
outline and asked them to address specific questions in a common order. Some chap-
ters present well-established interventions. For example, values affirmation (Sherman,
Lokhande, Müller, & Cohen, Chapter 3) and social norms interventions (Nolan, Schultz,
Cialdini, & Goldstein, Chapter 18) have been tested and replicated in numerous contexts
for more than a decade. Others share hot-off-the-press approaches that have only recently
been tested in real-world situations (e.g., Sparkman’s dynamic-norm approach, Chapter
19; Turnwald & Crum’s taste-focused labeling intervention, Chapter 12).
The 12 questions we asked authors are ones we believe are important for any inter-
vention to address—even (especially) if the answers are still being worked out. They are
as follows:
First, we asked for background: “What is the intellectual history, research tradi-
tion, and theorizing from which the intervention draws?” For instance, Sherman and
colleagues (Chapter 3) describe basic research on people’s need for self-integrity, the need
to see oneself as adequate, moral, and competent; how defensiveness and poor function-
ing can result when this need is threatened; and how this insight led to value-affirmation
interventions that bolster a sense of self-integrity to help students who face negative ste-
reotypes in school perform better, shrinking inequalities.
Second, we asked about psychological process: “Describe how past studies shed light
on a critical psychological process—the subjective meanings the intervention will aim to
affect.” Wise interventions address an array of fundamental interpretations:
• Is something good or bad? Are stress and arousal helpful or harmful (Crum,
Handley-M iner, & Smith, Chapter 9; Jamieson & Hangen, Chapter 10)? Are veg-
etable dishes appealing or unappealing (Turnwald & Crum, Chapter 12)?
• Can things change? Can challenges improve or are they fixed and permanent, such
as academic struggles (Dweck & Yeager, Chapter 1), feelings of nonbelonging in
school (Stephens, Hamedani, & Townsend, Chapter 5; Walton & Brady, Chap-
ter 2), experiences of bullying (Yeager & Lee, Chapter 13), or groups in conflict
(Goldenberg, Gross, & Halperin, Chapter 15)?
• Who am I and what can I become? Am I a good and decent person (Sherman et
al., Chapter 3)? What are my values and how can they be expressed in my behavior
(Bryan, Chapter 11)? Is schoolwork useful to me and relevant in my life (Hulleman
& Harackiewicz, Chapter 4)? How can I act efficiently to achieve my goals (Chen,
Chapter 7)? What am I grateful for (Layous, Chapter 8)? What kind of person
could I become if I work hard in school (Destin & Hernandez, Chapter 6)?
• How should I understand people and relationships? When my partner compli-
ments me, does that mean he or she loves me (Marigold, Chapter 17)? How should
Introduction 3
I interpret conflicts with a spouse (Slotter & Luchies, Chapter 16) or a student
who misbehaves in class (Okonofua & Ruiz, Chapter 14)? Can bullies change, or
will I always be a victim (Yeager & Lee, Chapter 13)? Can my relationship with a
place improve (Stephens et al., Chapter 5; Walton & Brady, Chapter 2)?
Third, we asked about evidence: “What is the evidence of the effectiveness of the
intervention in improving personally and/or socially important outcomes?” Each chap-
ter presents evidence from at least one randomized controlled trial that shows how an
intervention can improve important outcomes in a real-world setting, such as reducing
suspensions for middle school students (Okonofua & Ruiz, Chapter 14), improving atti-
tudes and behavior between Israelis and Palestinians (Goldenberg et al., Chapter 15),
increasing healthy eating (Bryan, Chapter 11; Turnwald & Crum, Chapter 12), or raising
academic performance over time (Dweck & Yeager, Chapter 1; Walton & Brady, Chapter
2; Sherman et al., Chapter 3; Stephens et al., Chapter 5; Destin & Hernandez, Chapter 6;
Chen, Chapter 7; Jamieson & Hangen, Chapter 10; Yeager & Lee, Chapter 13).
Fourth, we asked about mechanisms: “Describe evidence for how the intervention
achieves these effects.” For instance, Crum and colleagues (Chapter 9) show how an
intervention that represents stress as enhancing (as opposed to debilitating) can improve
health and work performance by changing what people pay attention to, what emotions
they experience, what they are motivated to do, and what their bodies are prepared to do,
as evidenced by their physiology.
Fifth, we asked about effects over time: “Does the intervention produce lasting ben-
efits? Why, how, and when?” For instance, Walton and Brady (Chapter 2) show that the
social-belonging intervention improves school performance and life success over years
by helping students engage more in the school social community (e.g., e-mailing profes-
sors, joining student groups) in ways that build peer and mentor relationships of lasting
benefit.
Sixth, we asked about heterogeneity: “In what kinds of social contexts and with
whom is the intervention more and less likely to be effective and why?” For instance,
Dweck and Yeager (Chapter 1) discuss how growth mindset of intelligence interventions
are more effective in high schools where peer norms support growth-oriented behaviors—
and less effective in schools where acting on a growth mindset might be uncool. Value-
affirmation interventions are more effective with people whose identities are under threat
in a context, such as negatively stereotyped racial-minority students, but less effective for
people not under threat (Sherman et al., Chapter 3).
Seventh, we asked about cousins: “What other interventions is this intervention
most closely related to? How is it similar and how is it different?” For instance, Jamieson
and Hangen (Chapter 10) and Crum and colleagues (Chapter 9) both aim to help people
see stress as an asset and not a negative. But they use distinct approaches that make one
more suitable for motivated performance contexts and the other for cases where the spe-
cific stressor is not easily identified or is more chronic and diffuse.
Eighth, we asked about content and implementation: “Describe the intervention
content or exercise in concrete terms and provide specific examples. Consider includ-
ing and annotating specific intervention materials.” Many chapters provide detailed
resources for implementing the intervention. For instance, Turnwald and Crum (Chapter
12) share a toolkit that teaches practitioners how to label foods more indulgently (https://
sparqtools.org/edgyveggies). Walton and Brady (Chapter 2) include annotated materials
4 Introduction
and share reflections from practitioners who implemented the social-belonging interven-
tion at diverse institutions through the College Transition Collaborative at Stanford Uni-
versity.
Ninth, we asked about nuances and misconceptions: “What are one to two nuances
people might miss about the intervention that are critical to its effectiveness? What do
these nuances tell us about the nature of the intervention and the psychological process it
targets?” Stephens and colleagues (Chapter 5) caution against ways a different education
intervention could be misunderstood or misused to perpetuate stereotypes.
Tenth, we asked about implications for practice: “Discuss how this intervention can
be implemented effectively in field settings to make a difference for people’s lives and/
or institutional goals.” For instance, Turnwald and Crum’s (Chapter 12) taste-focused
labeling approach flies in the face of the typical strategy for promoting healthy foods—
highlighting their healthy attributes—which can inadvertently make them less appealing.
Eleventh, we asked about implications for theory: “What does this intervention tell
us about who we are, about people, and psychological processes? What does this inter-
vention tell us about a given social problem?” Sparkman’s (Chapter 19) dynamic norms
approach shows that people conform to salient change in norms, even when this behavior
would violate current norms—that is, to behaviors that are not yet but are becoming
normative. And Yeager and Lee’s (Chapter 13) intervention to teach students an incre-
mental theory of personality highlights the important role played by implicit perceptions
of fixedness (“I’ll always be a victim; you’ll always be a bully”) in retaliatory processes
that contribute to school bullying.
Twelfth, we asked about future directions: “What are the high-priority next steps
for this intervention for research?” Many of our authors offer suggestions for testing par-
ticular theoretical questions, including for scaling interventions and for moving to new
contexts. For instance, Destin and Hernandez (Chapter 6) discuss how their pathways
intervention could be extended to peers and teachers to instill systemic change.
While all sections of each chapter will be of value to readers, some may be more
relevant for some readers than others. If you are a practitioner looking for a solution in a
specific area, you may be most interested in the “Content and Implementation” section,
which provides direction for applying a particular intervention; but we also encourage
you to be mindful of the “Nuances and Misconceptions,” as well as the “Heterogene-
ity” sections, which will help you apply the intervention skillfully and thoughtfully and
avoid misunderstandings. If you are a social psychologist conducting similar research,
you might be particularly interested in the “Psychological Process,” “Mechanisms,” and
“Effects over Time” sections. We encourage all readers to compare and contrast across
chapters, as noting the similarities and differences across approaches will help engender
a deeper and more integrated understanding of what wise interventions are, how they
work, and how they can move outcomes that matter.
How are the chapters organized? Somewhat artificially, by the primary outcome of
interest investigated in a line of research to date. However, important outcomes are often
deeply interrelated, as are academic motivation and achievement, health, well-being, and
personal relationships. Thus, many powerful psychological interventions affect multiple
kinds of outcomes, or could do so as explored in future work, even as a primary focus
has been in one area to date.
Together, these interventions illustrate the truth that every social problem is, in
part, a psychological problem (Walton & Dweck, 2009). Yet no social problem is only
Introduction 5
a psychological problem. Nor are wise interventions a silver bullet. The problems we
face are beastly, complex, and stubborn. And beastly, complex, and stubborn problems
require solutions from all angles, including a careful consideration of resources, incen-
tives, nudges, and the capacities of individuals for change. Wise interventions offer an
additional tool.
In some ways, this volume represents a back to the future for social psychology. The
field was forged out of the need in the mid-20th-century to understand the intergroup ani-
mosities and conflicts that defined World War II. Then followed a “cognitive revolution”
and an intense focus on controlled laboratory experimentation and deep understanding
of psychological processes. As the new century dawned, researchers began to fuse these
methodological and theoretical insights with a renewed commitment to real-world, eco-
logically valid contexts—a focus that has since exploded. It’s back to the future, but with
a new level of empirical sophistication and vigor for using social-psychological insights
to understand and address the pressing problems of today.
What wisdom you have in your hands! The interventions presented in this volume
were not easy or simple to construct. They are founded on hard-won psychological
insights, typically cultivated over years of research in the laboratory and the field, with an
intense commitment to understanding how interpretations play out in the real world and
how altering them might help people achieve their goals. Yet all of the interventions you
will read about are under construction and that construction is ongoing (Dweck, 2017).
In each case, there are important questions outstanding about why, how, for whom, and
in what contexts an intervention works or does not work. There is much we have yet to
learn. Above all else, we invite you to a journey of discovery about the role of psychology
in our everyday lives and how we can build this understanding to improve our lives, the
lives of others, and our societies.
Welcome!
REFERENCES
Dweck, C. (2017). Growth mindset is on a firm foundation, but we’re still building the house.
Mindset Scholars Network Blog. Retrieved from https://mindsetscholarsnetwork.org/
growth- mindset-firm-foundation-still-building-house.
Walton, G. M., & Dweck, C. S. (2009). Solving social problems like a psychologist. Perspectives
on Psychological Science, 4, 101–102.
Walton, G. M., & Wilson, T. D. (2018). Wise interventions: Psychological remedies for social and
personal problems. Psychological Review, 125, 617–655.
PA R T I
EDUCATION
C H A P TE R 1
Carol S. Dweck and David S. Yeager
Wise interventions can create a world of new meanings for people, and growth-mindset
interventions portray a world in which human abilities are not fixed but can be developed.
Growth-mindset-of-intelligence programs are grounded in decades of foundational work and
have undergone years of development and testing. These programs not only vividly convey
the idea of malleable ability to students but also make students eager to adopt the idea and
able to apply it in their schoolwork and in their lives. In this chapter, we discuss the roots
and the evolution of growth-mindset-of-intelligence interventions, recent large-scale experi-
ments that shed light on how and when they work best, and our vision for the future of these
interventions.
Wise interventions create a psychological world and invite people to enter it. It is
often a world that equips them to face potentially threatening challenges and to face them
with greater confidence and effectiveness because they have a new way to think about
those challenges. As a result, they are more likely to behave in ways that can bring better
results, be they emotional, social, or academic/professional results.
A growth mindset about intelligence is the belief that intellectual abilities are not
fixed but rather can be developed, for example, through hard work, good strategies, and
help and mentoring from others. In contrast, a fixed mindset is the belief that intellec-
tual abilities are fixed and cannot be improved. In this chapter, we show how growth-
mindset interventions have been developed and refined with the aim of creating a world
of meanings—one that encourages students to take on challenges, interpret setbacks or
struggles in adaptive ways, and persist in the face of failure (e.g., Aronson, Fried, &
Good, 2002; Blackwell, Trzesniewski, & Dweck, 2007; Paunesku et al., 2015; Yeager,
Romero, et al., 2016; Yeager et al., 2019; see Dweck & Yeager, 2019, for a review). Ulti-
mately, this can result in greater growth and achievement, potentially decreasing long-
standing grade disparities.
Yeager and Lee (Chapter 13, this volume) show how a growth mindset about
personality can create a similar psychological world in the social domain and how
9
10 I. Education
BACKGROUND
Mindset research grew out of two lines of previous research: learned helplessness (Selig-
man & Maier, 1967) and attribution theory (Weiner & Kukla, 1970). The research on
learned helplessness showed that animals exposed to uncontrollable negative events even-
tually stopped trying to prevent those events or escape from them. They remained passive
even when the environment later changed and would have rewarded their efforts. We saw
this behavior as perhaps similar to that of students who give up in the face of difficulty
when in fact they have the ability to learn and do well.
Attribution theory, particularly the groundbreaking work of Weiner and colleagues
(e.g., Weiner, 1985; Weiner & Kukla, 1970), revealed the importance of studying peo-
ple’s perceptions and interpretations—in this case, how people understood the causes
of their successes and failures (i.e., the “attributions” they made for their successes and
failures). In doing so, it provided a way to begin to understand the psychology of persis-
tence versus helplessness in humans—that is, it gave us a way to understand why, among
people who are equal in ability, some respond to challenging, threatening, or negative
events (such as failure) with persistence and some do not. The research growing out of
attribution theory suggested, for example, that viewing your failure as resulting from
your lack of effort was more likely to motivate persistence than viewing it as coming
from your lack of ability, since the latter is often seen as more stable and less controllable
(see Weiner, 1985; Weiner & Kukla, 1970).
Attribution theory’s great gift to the world was a well-defined variable (attributions)
that could be precisely manipulated and could change behavior. The whole idea that
motivation could be based on interpretations was tremendously exciting, especially since
the field was emerging from the long behaviorist era in which ascribing any thinking
to any organism, even humans, was seen as unscientific. However, whereas attribution
theorists viewed ability as stable and uncontrollable (Weiner, 1985), mindset research
took people’s interpretations a step further by proposing that people could view ability
itself as something that is fixed or, instead, as something that can be developed. More-
over, these different mindsets about ability could set the stage for the different attribu-
tions when a failure or setback occurred. This idea spawned a new research program
to understand the ramifications of these different perspectives on ability (see Dweck &
Leggett, 1988).
Mindsets can be seen as beliefs about the nature and workings of human attributes
(or attributes of the world): What is this attribute, what are its properties, how does it
work? In the case of mindsets about intelligence, the mindset is a belief about whether
A Growth Mindset about Intelligence 11
intellectual abilities are fixed or can be developed; it is a belief about whether people can
develop their abilities through their own agency in the context of the necessary guidance,
mentoring, and support from others. The research has shown that these mindsets—the
belief in ability as fixed or as malleable—are related to a host of meaningful variables
and it has established the effects of mindsets on academic outcomes. More specifically,
mindsets have been found to be at the center of a web of variables, which we have called
a “meaning system” (Blackwell et al., 2007; Dweck & Yeager, 2019; Robins & Pals,
2002). This means that the mindsets, when activated, may trigger and act through a host
of other psychological variables.
Generally speaking, a fixed-mindset meaning system is organized around people’s
concerns about their (fixed) abilities and how these abilities will appear or be judged.
Holding a fixed mindset has predicted people’s adoption or display of negative effort
beliefs (the belief that the need for high effort implies low ability or that effort will be
ineffective for those with low ability; Blackwell et al., 2007; Leggett, 1986; Miele &
Molden, 2010; Miele, Son, & Metcalfe, 2013); their performance goals or performance-
avoidance goals (the desire to validate or protect one’s ability, often by avoiding chal-
lenges that might reveal inadequacies; e.g., Hong, Chiu, Dweck, Lin, & Wan, 1999; Rob-
ins & Pals, 2002; see also Blackwell et al., 2007); their helpless attributions (the belief
that setbacks imply low ability; Blackwell et al., 2007; Dweck & Yeager, 2019; Hong et
al., 1999; Robins & Pals, 2002); and, as a result, fewer positive strategies in the face of
setbacks (persistence, remediating deficiencies, appropriate help seeking; Blackwell et al.,
2007; Moser, Schroder, Heeter, Moran, & Lee, 2011; Nussbaum & Dweck, 2008; Rob-
ins & Pals, 2002), which can lead to less than optimal learning or performance.
In contrast, the growth- mindset-
meaning system tends to be organized around
learning and developing one’s abilities. Holding a growth mindset has predicted people’s
adoption or display of more positive effort beliefs (the idea that hard work can build
abilities), learning goals (the desire to improve one’s ability by approaching challenging
learning tasks), resilient attributions (the belief that setbacks do not signify a lack of
ability but signify a need to change strategy), and, as a result, more positive, “mastery-
oriented” strategies in the face of setbacks (sticking with challenging assignments rather
than falling back to easier ones), which can lead to enhanced learning or performance.
These mindsets have particular resonance in a society that has been divided in its
beliefs about the nature of intelligence. On the one hand, there has been a belief, stem-
ming from the writings of Galton (Hereditary Genius; 1892), Terman (Genetic Studies
of Genius; 1926), and Jensen (The g Factor; 1998), that intelligence is hereditary and
unchangeable—and that it determines what people are capable of accomplishing. One of
us (Dweck) had a sixth-grade teacher who was so enamored with this view that she seated
the class around the room in IQ order and treated students accordingly.
However, there has also been a forceful opposing view, which was well articulated
by Alfred Binet, the co-inventor of the original IQ test. Binet (1909/1975) expressed his
alarm at the increasing use of his test by people who asserted that they were measur-
ing what they considered to be fixed intelligence. Binet did not see it that way at all:
“A few modern philosophers . . . assert that an individual’s intelligence is a fixed quan-
tity, a quantity which cannot be increased. We must protest and react against this brute
pessimism. . . . With practice, training, and above all, method, we manage to increase
our attention, our memory, our judgment and literally to become more intelligent than
we were before” (1909/1975, pp. 106–107). (See, e.g., Diamond, Barnett, Thomas, &
12 I. Education
Munro, 2007; Ramsden et al., 2011; Rueda, Posner, & Rothbart, 2005, for research on
the malleability of intellectual abilities and executive function; see Sauce & Matzel, 2018,
for a thoughtful perspective on the role of environment and experience in the develop-
ment of intellectual abilities). This view of intellectual ability as something that can grow
with experience and learning is in harmony with the strand of American culture that
reflects a can-do attitude and the idea that, no matter who they are, people can develop
themselves with the necessary support and opportunities. Our research on mindsets has
examined the impact of people’s own beliefs about intelligence and has asked: What is the
impact on people’s motivation and achievement of taking one view or the other?
The relationships between the fixed or growth mindsets and the variables that are
allied with them and give them their power have been studied in several ways. These
include moderate- to large-sample correlational studies in which mindsets predict the
other variables in the meaning system, which then go on to predict academic or social–
emotional outcomes (Blackwell et al., 2007, Study 1; Dweck & Yeager, 2019; Robins &
Pals, 2002; see Figure 1.1). They have also been investigated by demonstrating a correla-
tion between mindsets and specific variables in a given setting and then, in a separate
study, manipulating the mindsets to produce that effect experimentally to demonstrate
the causal effects of mindsets (e.g., Hong et al., 1999).
As an example, Hong et al. (1999, Study 1) initially showed that participants who
held more of a growth (vs. fixed) mindset were more likely to attribute their failure on a
laboratory task to effort, a factor that is typically considered highly controllable. They
FIGURE 1.1. The links between mindsets, the mindset meaning system, and academic outcomes
in three large studies. The mindset meaning system index is a composite of five items representing
the meaning system constructs. Mindset was measured with three fixed mindset-framed items.
Statistics are zero-order correlations estimated by meta-analytically aggregating three datasets:
The U.S. National Study of Learning Mindsets (NSLM) pilot (N = 3,306; Yeager, Romero, et
al., 2016); the U.S. NSLM (N = 14,894; Yeager et al, 2019); and the Norway U-say experiment
(N = 5,246; Rege et al., in press). All p’s < .001.
A Growth Mindset about Intelligence 13
also showed, in a field study (Study 2), that those holding more of a growth mindset were
more likely to take remedial action in the face of a deficiency—in line with their greater
emphasis on effort attributions. But were the mindsets a causal factor in the effort attri-
butions and remedial action?
To provide some background, in Study 2, freshmen at the University of Hong Kong
were offered the possibility of taking a remedial English course. Among those who were
deficient in English, this would be an excellent idea since all academic work and materials
were in English. Yet among the students who were deficient, those with more of a growth
mindset were the ones who showed far more enthusiasm for taking the course. It looked
as though those with a fixed mindset might possibly put their college performance in
jeopardy rather than admit to needing a remedial course.
Then, in an experimental study (Hong et al., 1999, Study 3), students were tem-
porarily taught a growth or fixed mindset (by means of a persuasive article) and then
experienced a failure on a task taken from an intelligence test. How did they understand
their failure and were they willing to take a tutorial that could improve their performance
on the test? After their poor performance, those in the growth-mindset group were more
likely than those in the fixed-mindset condition to attribute their poor performance to
a lack of effort, and were also far more likely to opt for the remedial tutorial. Finally,
follow-up analyses suggested that the difference in effort attributions mediated the dif-
ference in the tendency to take remedial action. In short, this experiment showed that the
mindsets can directly cause a difference in attributions and, in turn, a difference in the
willingness to address one’s deficiencies. (It is important to note that all students were
thoroughly debriefed at the end of the study.) A series of experiments by Nussbaum and
Dweck (2008) lent further support to the finding that a growth mindset can cause more
constructive, remedial actions in the face of failures than a fixed mindset.
We are often asked how mindset theory is related to attribution theory. As suggested
above, the answer is twofold. Mindsets grew out of attribution theory and mindsets can
set the stage for students to orient toward different attributions, as shown in our exper-
imental studies. When students are thinking in terms of fixed traits versus malleable
qualities, different reasons for their failures come to mind.
PSYCHOLOGICAL PROCESSES
We just spelled out the meaning systems that have been revealed by research on fixed and
growth mindsets. Here, we focus on how it all works when students are experiencing
those meanings.
When students enter challenging or uncertain academic situations—as they do when
they go to a new school or take a difficult class—they often worry whether their intel-
lectual abilities are up to the task. In a fixed mindset, where those intellectual abilities are
viewed as limited and unchangeable, they may wonder whether they will look and feel
dumb. As a result, a cascade of processes (the fixed-mindset-meaning system, Figure 1.1)
can be triggered. Students may then be afraid of the looming challenges, become reluc-
tant to exert effort (or become anxious when they do), see setbacks as confirming their
worst fears—and so on in a continuing cycle. We have seen firsthand that even students
who make it into the top universities in the United States can be caught in this fixed-
mindset cycle, worrying about saying something stupid, making a mistake, or getting a
14 I. Education
disappointing grade, and thereby revealing what they perceive to be their “permanently
deficient” intellectual capacity.
For one project we sought to understand what the fixed mindset sounded like in stu-
dents’ own voices. This project (sponsored by the Carnegie Foundation for the Advance-
ment of Teaching) focused on community college students who had failed a placement
test and were required to take developmental (remedial) math courses, an abyss from
which many never emerge. After failing the placement test, there was palpable despair in
many, expressed in fixed-mindset language, such as “I am embarrassed by how stupid
I am and suddenly feeling very discouraged . . . I can’t even tell which fraction is bigger
than another, or where they should fall on the number line. I feel like crying.”
Such fixed-ability attributions for their placement in remedial math were common:
“I don’t think I’m a math person”; “Me and math are like oil and water”; “When I got to
this [math] class, it was pretty much like a brick wall . . . I’m just not that kind of person”;
and “My whole family [was] never good at math. . . . ” They also spontaneously expressed
negative effort beliefs. Here is a student talking about how other students—those who
did not place into remedial math—do not need to apply effort to learn: “They’re the ones
who really catch on quickly—they can really advance their skills.”
The task of a growth-mindset intervention is to break this cycle and to shift stu-
dents into a growth-mindset-meaning system. In a growth mindset, students may, of
course, still be anxious when they enter a new school with as yet unknown standards
and requirements, but they may also be excited about being exposed to new subjects and
about what they will learn. And even if they have a disappointing initial experience, they
may be more likely to remain ready to exert effort, give more positive attributions for
setbacks, and continue to exhibit constructive strategies to make this learning happen—
particularly if they are in supportive environments.
EMPIRICAL EVIDENCE
Initial Interventions
Three early-stage randomized trials evaluated growth-mindset interventions in face-to-
face settings with relatively modest samples (Aronson et al., 2002; Blackwell et al., 2007;
Good, Aronson, & Inzlicht, 2003). Although the interventions varied in the format for
conveying a growth mindset, in the growth-mindset materials, and in the populations
of students who participated in the evaluation studies, they produced similar results.
Students who received the growth-mindset intervention tended to show greater academic
performance relative to controls, as measured by grades or achievement test scores.
More specifically, in their seminal study, Aronson et al. (2002) asked university stu-
dents to mentor younger students and to write “pen pal” letters to them over the course
of several sessions. This was the first use of the “saying-is-believing” exercise for growth-
mindset interventions—that is, the mentoring of the younger students was intended to
help instill the growth-mindset messages in the university students themselves. In this
exercise, the participants encouraged the younger students to adopt a growth mindset
by explaining to them that the “brain is like a muscle,” and that connections in the
brain grow stronger in response to rigorous learning experiences (messages that are now
core to the growth-mindset intervention). Over the next semester, the African American
A Growth Mindset about Intelligence 15
college students in the growth-mindset group earned higher grades (grade-point aver-
ages [GPAs]) overall compared to two control groups that received either no treatment or
information about multiple intelligences, as well as showing higher enjoyment and valu-
ing of academics. A second study, conducted by Good et al. (2003; N = 138 across four
conditions), found that an in-person growth-mindset program had a significant overall
effect on the achievement test scores of middle school students and was effective at nar-
rowing the gender gaps in achievement.
In another seminal study, Blackwell et al. (2007) implemented a growth-mindset
intervention via eight in-person workshops. The study was conducted with seventh-grade
students (N = 91 across two conditions) who attended a relatively low-achieving junior
high school where students’ math grades were declining over the seventh-grade school
year. Blackwell and colleagues embedded several sessions of training in growth mind-
set within a larger eight-session workshop on study skills and compared the effects to
a group that received eight sessions of study skills alone. The growth-mindset content
began with scientific content, or “news you can use.” (See Figure 1.2.) As in the Aron-
son et al. (2002) and Good et al. (2003) interventions, the Blackwell et al. intervention
led students to envision themselves growing stronger connections in their brains as they
FIGURE 1.2. The first page of the scientific article that was used in the Blackwell, Trzesniewski,
and Dweck (2007) intervention and that served as the starting point for many subsequent revised
interventions. Reprinted with permission from L. S. Blackwell.
16 I. Education
learned from new and challenging content. This was followed by activities to illustrate
how learning can make you smarter.
Blackwell and colleagues (2007) found a significant effect of the experimental condi-
tion on change in grades following the intervention. Specifically, students in the study-
skills control group continued to show declines in their math grades. However, for stu-
dents in the growth-mindset condition, the decline in grades was halted. The everyday
behaviors of students in the treatment group showed that they were more motivated
and engaged, as well—that is, math teachers, blind to students’ experimental condition
assignments, were more likely to nominate students for showing improvements in chal-
lenge seeking and hard work when they had been in the growth-mindset condition. Thus,
when students learned that the brain’s abilities can be developed over time, they seemed
more willing to seek out challenges and to stay engaged in their math classes, despite the
difficulty.
Preparing to Scale Up
These early studies, although small in scale, raised an intriguing question: Could the
growth-mindset message be communicated even more efficiently, in a way that could be
scaled up? If so, then it would be possible to replicate these growth-mindset intervention
effects in larger and more generalizable samples, and to begin to conduct studies that
shed light on mechanisms that explain the sustained effects from a short treatment.
Starting in the 2010s, a number of randomized trials did just that. Using a newly cre-
ated online platform, Paunesku and colleagues (2015) administered abbreviated versions
of the scientific content about the brain’s malleability from the Blackwell et al. (2007)
study, along with a “write a letter to a future student” exercise that drew on the Aron-
son et al. (2002) method. These short, self-administered, but still compelling, growth-
mindset sessions were delivered via a web survey, which made it easier to reach larger
samples and to randomly assign students to condition. Students’ grades were obtained
from school records several months after they completed the intervention.
The first time this shorter intervention was evaluated in a randomized trial was in a
pilot study with community college students taking developmental math—a population
with striking fixed mindsets, as suggested previously. In this pilot study (see Yeager &
Dweck, 2012, for a description), we learned that the growth mindset intervention signifi-
cantly reduced the rate at which students dropped out of their developmental math course
(from 20% to 9%).
It may seem surprising that we saw such promising results from a short online treat-
ment. But the vivid and emotionally persuasive message that your brain can grow when
you work hard on hard tasks and that you no longer had to feel “dumb at math” because
schoolwork was hard resonated with students deeply and, they told us, felt liberating.
The letters students wrote “to a struggling friend” suggest how much they related to the
growth–mindset message:
“Friend, I know you have been struggling in school for a little while and are becom-
ing discouraged, but there’s something I’d like to share with you. I read this interest-
ing article recently and it explained how we can grow our brains. They did studies
on animals and it showed that learning new skills to work your brain could help
A Growth Mindset about Intelligence 17
us become smarter and better at any subject. We can use better strategies to learn
things and practice, practice, practice. So maybe we can be study buddies and learn
new skills to work and grow our brains to make new and bigger and better connec-
tions. I hope this encourages you because you are not ‘dumb’ and could just use a
little help, and maybe we could do it together.”
“I thought I’d never do well and I felt so confused. . . . It was hard to see it at the time,
but all that confusion really paid off. It’s like something I read about the brain—
when you don’t understand something, that’s when your brain can grow the most. I
really believe that now. . . . After a lifetime of thinking ‘I can’t do math,’ I finally feel
like I can be good at this!!!”
The initial evidence, both the data and the testimonials, convinced us that even time-
limited exposures to the growth-mindset idea, when done well, could be inspiring and
actionable for students who needed to hear it.
The next crucial step involved randomized evaluation studies in grade 9–12 settings,
and across multiple schools. Paunesku and colleagues (2015; N = 1,594 students, K =
13 schools) evaluated the shorter intervention, somewhat revised for high school stu-
dents. They found that the growth-mindset intervention improved GPAs in “core” classes
(math, science, English, and social studies) for students who, prior to the intervention,
were earning lower grades than their peers.
Scaling Up Further
Yeager and colleagues next tested whether this “scalable” intervention could be improved,
adapted, and scaled up further, and whether doing so could improve outcomes for an
entire population of students. In a first case, the population was the entire incoming class
at a large, public 4-year university. Yeager, Walton, and colleagues (2016, Experiment 2;
N = 7,335 students) took the previous Paunesku et al. (2015) intervention and added tes-
timonies from more advanced students in which they described how they used a growth
mindset to make sense of the difficulties that accompanied the transition to college. This
updated growth-mindset intervention significantly improved full-time enrollment rates
among students who face disadvantages on the basis of their race or social class, thereby
potentially narrowing the achievement gap. Later, a different team of investigators repli-
cated the study and found treatment effects on first-year grades among Hispanic/Latinx
students at a different large, public 4-year college (Broda et al., 2018; N = 7,686).
In the next case (Yeager, Romero, et al., 2016), the population of interest for the
growth-mindset intervention was ninth-grade students making the transition to U.S.
public high schools. This study served as a pilot study for a larger intervention with a
nationally representative sample of ninth graders, discussed below. In this pilot study,
the intervention was put through an iterative design process to tailor the message for this
population. The result was a version that sharpened the elements that had been added
over the years: compelling scientific information, engaging writing exercises, and testimo-
nies from older students. This intervention was evaluated in a preregistered randomized
18 I. Education
Heterogeneity
Interestingly, although a growth-mindset intervention tends to influence mindsets and
the choice of challenge tasks (such as the choice of hard vs. easy problems) for students
across the achievement spectrum (Rege et al., in press; Yeager, Romero, et al., 2016; Yea-
ger et al. 2019), it has translated into higher grades chiefly among those students who are
not already earning high grades. It is possible that higher-achieving students may show
benefits on other outcomes, such as opting into more challenging courses over time, and
we have evidence from the National Study of Learning Mindsets (Yeager et al., 2019) and
a replication in Norway (Rege et al., in press) that this is the case. Thus, heterogeneity
A Growth Mindset about Intelligence 19
of intervention outcomes may depend on where the student has room to move—up the
grade ladder or into more challenging courses.
One important way to learn about what leads to heterogeneity in intervention effects
is to conduct your experiment in settings that have high and low levels of a factor. In the
National Study of Learning Mindsets we focused on the extent to which the peer and
teacher factors would influence the growth-mindset effect, and found that the growth-
mindset intervention was more effective at changing grades when the school or class-
room context was supportive of the growth-mindset message—that is, stronger effects
on grades appeared when peer norms favored challenge-seeking behaviors (Yeager et
al., 2019), or when the teacher held more of a growth mindset (Yeager et al., 2020). We
examine these findings in more detail below as we turn next to how intervention effects
are sustained over time.
In sum, it appears that a growth-mindset intervention is most effective among stu-
dents (1) who are facing ongoing academic challenges, and (2) who are in school or class-
room contexts that support the maintenance of growth-mindset beliefs and behaviors.
Effects over Time
What happens after a growth-mindset intervention? The previous section identified some
of the factors that can facilitate long-term growth mindset effects, but here we delve a bit
more deeply into the proposed sequence of processes that may unfold over time to bring
about those effects. This is a topic that has, of course, been of great interest to us.
We assume that after a growth-mindset intervention, students may try out growth-
mindset beliefs and behaviors, such as greater challenge seeking and harder work in the
classroom, and if this yields positive results—such as greater learning, higher grades,
approval from teachers, or support from peers—the greater challenge seeking and hard
work will continue and even increase.
We mentioned above that in the National Study of Learning Mindsets (Yeager et
al., 2019), the mindsets of students’ math teachers made a difference. Specifically, the
teachers who themselves held more of a growth mindset had students who showed larger
sustained effects of the growth-mindset treatment (Yeager et al., 2020). This could well
be because teachers with more of a growth mindset are more likely to notice changes in
students’ challenge seeking and effort, to believe that these changes can make a difference
in learning, and therefore to support these changes.
There were early hints of this in the Blackwell et al. (2007) intervention that an
environment that is supportive of growth-mindset behaviors could play a role. In that
study, as noted earlier, their seventh-grade math teacher did not know which students
had received a growth-mindset versus control (study-skills) intervention. Nonetheless,
the teachers singled out more of the students in the growth-mindset group as showing
changes in academic effort, in perseverance, in the seeking of challenges, and in request-
ing mentoring. Noting the changes, these perceptive teachers may have rewarded, sup-
ported, and helped sustain the students’ increased dedication to their work.
Another factor we mentioned earlier that made a difference in the longevity of the
mindset effects was peer norms (Yeager et al., 2019). Did peers in a student’s school show
a strong norm of seeking to work on challenging tasks? We found that when they did,
the overall advantage of the growth-mindset group over the control group was larger
than average (in fact, about double). However, when the peer norm of challenge seeking
20 I. Education
was weak, the students showed little or no effect over time of having received a growth-
mindset treatment—even though the intervention changed their mindsets and led them
to choose a challenging task within the intervention session to the same degree as stu-
dents in the schools with strong peer norms. This finding suggests that when students
considered putting their growth mindset into practice in their classrooms, perhaps they
felt reluctant to go up against established peer norms and risk disapproval, or they may
have actually tried out new behaviors in the classroom and were not met with support or
approval. Future research could study this process as it unfolds, as well as systematically
investigate ways to influence peer norms to allow and support growth mindset behavior.
Finally, a new growth-mindset intervention performed in Norway (Rege et al., in
press) showed how school policies could allow or prevent the effects of a growth-mindset
intervention. Some of the schools in the study required students to choose the level of
their math course before the school year began. Other schools, however, allowed students
to make their choice a couple of months later, after the start of the new school year. Since
the growth-mindset intervention was administered after the start of the new year, it was
chiefly students in the latter schools who were able to profit from the intervention in
terms of being motivated to choose advanced math courses at an appreciably higher rate
than the control group.
Clearly, there is still much more to learn about student, peer, teacher, and school fac-
tors that can help sustain growth-mindset effects over time and, in particular, about the
processes through which they do so. This learning will help make future interventions
more effective for more students and will inform educators how best to create cultures
and policies that are more conducive to challenge seeking, persistence, and achievement
in their students.
Effect Sizes
What effect sizes would be considered noteworthy for a growth-mindset intervention
(or any educational interventions)? Experts in evaluation studies (e.g., Cheung & Slavin,
2016; see also Hill, Bloom, Black, & Lipsey, 2008; Kraft, 2018) have argued that evalu-
ating effect sizes requires choosing the appropriate benchmarks. In the current case, this
means asking: What effects are typical for rigorous, high-quality educational interven-
tions, ones that seek to improve meaningful educational outcomes, such as grades? In
other words, what effect sizes are typical for large-sample interventions (be they ones
involving changes in curriculum, instruction, school organization, or student motiva-
tion) that use randomized trials and choose important, objective student outcomes as the
dependent variable?
Prominent educational economist Susan Dynarski (2017), addressing educational
outcomes, concluded that “a fifth of a standard deviation [0.20 SD] is a large effect.”
This statement is based on the “best-evidence synthesis” movement, which uses evidence
from large, rigorous evaluation studies to determine the effects of interventions that seek
to raise grades or test scores (Cheung & Slavin, 2016). It was recently confirmed by a
report on a large number of the newest randomized trials in education funded by the U.S.
federal government (Boulay et al., 2018). Among these promising interventions, the ones
that ended up having significant effects tended to top out at around 0.20 SD.
To give a better feel for the magnitude of this effect, around 0.20 SD is the effect of
a year of schooling for adolescents, a year with an excellent versus average teacher, or a
A Growth Mindset about Intelligence 21
smaller class size for adolescents (see Yeager et al., 2019, for a review). Given the many
variables affecting outcomes in educational settings, this effect is an achievement, even
for multifaceted, labor-intensive, and costly programs.1
There is a larger point, however. The point is that even a seemingly modest effect
propagated over large numbers of students, particularly at a very low cost, can produce
meaningful changes in the education and economic trajectories for many, many students.
The growth-mindset intervention with its online platform and very low cost per student
is capable of scaling to large populations, leading more students to enjoy challenges, pass
courses they might have failed, take harder courses than they would have, and contribute
to society in ways they might not have otherwise.
It is noteworthy, then, that growth-mindset interventions have shown much of what
is considered a “large” effect size in real-world educational experiments. Not surpris-
ingly, early studies that involved multiple sessions conducted by highly trained profes-
sionals showed larger effect sizes. However, these procedures were time-consuming and
costly, and therefore not really scalable.
One should not expect the same effect sizes for studies that delivered much more
minimal (under an hour) self-administered, online growth-mindset materials. Nonethe-
less, the effect size on core course GPA for lower-achieving students several months pos-
tintervention for studies that delivered these more minimal growth-mindset programs
was 0.10 SD–0.14 SD across three replication studies (Paunesku et al., 2015; Yeager,
Romero, et al., 2016; Yeager, Walton, et al., 2016; see also Yeager et al., 2019), and the
National Study of Learning Mindsets as well—that is, growth-mindset interventions can
provide roughly half of the largest effects typically seen for much more extensive and
expensive adolescent interventions.
Considerably larger effects were found in the National Study of Learning Mindsets
for schools that had peer norms that supported a growth mindset (and that were not
already among the highest achieving 25% of schools). Low- or medium-achieving schools
with supportive norms approached or exceeded treatment effects of 0.20 SD (Yeager et
al., 2019). Moreover, as we learn more about the factors that sustain the treatments and
turn them into higher grades and more advanced course taking, we hope to enhance the
effectiveness of the growth-mindset programs and obtain even higher effect sizes.
In sum, the effects of our growth-mindset interventions are noteworthy compared
to many of the most successful large and comprehensive educational interventions and
are found under the most rigorous of conditions. These effects are especially meaningful
given the very low per-person cost of the intervention, the ease of access, and the short
time involved in its administration.
COUSINS
1 Larger effects have sometimes been obtained when interventions are not rigorously evaluated (e.g., did
not use randomized controlled designs with large samples) or when researchers control the tests that
students take as the postintervention measure (so the intervention could “teach to their test”; Boulay et
al., 2018; see also Cheung & Slavin, 2016).
22 I. Education
international large-sample replications that have used our materials, such as Bettinger et
al. (2018) in Norway, and Outes et al.’s (2017) World Bank project in Peru. We also wish
to mention an intervention with parents (Andersen & Nielsen, 2016) and intervention-
relevant (although not yet intervention-based) findings with teachers (Canning, Muenks,
Green, & Murphy, 2019; Fuesting et al., 2019; see also Rattan, Good, & Dweck, 2012).
In the research by Canning et al. with 150 college science, technology, engineering, and
math (STEM) professors, those professors who held a more fixed mindset had double
the achievement gap among their students—that is, double the achievement gap between
White/Asian students and students from underrepresented minority groups. On average,
everyone did better with the growth-mindset professors but especially those from under-
represented groups. Further, students reported that the growth-mindset professors used
pedagogical practices that put more emphasis on learning and development, and this was
a mediator of the enhanced performance. These findings provide a foundation for future
interventions with teachers, and we continue to discuss this in “Future Directions” below.
Another line of work that is paving the way for future mindset interventions is a
program of research on organizational mindsets, spearheaded by Canning, Murphy, and
colleagues (2020). In a series of studies, spanning data from major business organizations
to data from laboratory experiments, people’s perceptions of an organization’s fixed or
growth mindsets about talent predicted such important things as their own commitment
to the organization, their report of immoral practices within the workplace, and their
perceptions of true support for innovation even when it might not succeed.
The growth-mindset-of-intelligence interventions have already given birth to a pro-
gram of research on growth-mindset-of-personality interventions noted earlier, spear-
headed by David Yeager (see Yeager & Lee, Chapter 13, this volume), in which students
were taught that personality was not simply fixed but consisted of qualities that could
be developed over time. Among other things, these interventions have led to meaningful
improvements in adolescents’ aggression (Yeager, Trzesniewski, & Dweck, 2013) and
stress responses (Yeager, Lee, & Jamieson, 2016), and decreases in the onset of symptoms
of clinical depression in students in their first year of high school (Miu & Yeager, 2015;
see also Schleider & Weisz, 2016).
Scholars have also taken growth-mindset interventions into the real world of conflict
resolution (Halperin, Russell, Trzesniewski, Gross, & Dweck, 2011; Goldenberg et al.,
2018; Goldenberg, Gross, & Halperin, Chapter 15, this volume), showing, for example,
that teaching the parties in the Middle East conflict that groups do not have fixed, inher-
ent qualities (are not inherently evil or aggressive) can lead to increases in hope for prog-
ress and in support for real concessions, increases that endured over a 6-month period
of unrest.
Mindset research has also begun to address issues of health (Burnette, Forsyth, Des-
marais, & Hoyt, 2019; Mueller, Rowe, & Zuckerman, 2017; Thomas, Burnette, & Hoyt,
2019). Moreover, growth mindset interventions in the area of health (beliefs about the
nature and causes of health and disease) pioneered by Alia Crum and her colleagues
(Crum, Leibowitz, & Verghese, 2017) have yielded many landmark findings, including
findings that illuminate the workings of placebo effects (see also Crum, Handley-M iner,
& Smith, Chapter 9, this volume).
Growth-mindset interventions that enhance empathy are also beginning to emerge
(Weisz, 2018; see Schumann, Zaki, & Dweck, 2014), as well as interventions that teach
a growth mindset about emotions (the idea that people can modulate and improve their
A Growth Mindset about Intelligence 23
emotions; Smith et al., 2018). There are also other lines of research that could easily
spawn interventions in the future, such as research on growth mindsets about prejudice
(beliefs about whether prejudice is fixed or malleable; Carr, Dweck, & Pauker, 2012;
Neel & Shapiro, 2012; see also Rattan & Dweck, 2010, on the confronting of bias) and
research on mindsets or theories about willpower (beliefs about whether the capacity for
willpower is fixed or malleable and limited or abundant; e.g., Job, Walton, Bernecker, &
Dweck, 2015; Mukhopadhyay & Yeung, 2010).
As we have seen, growth-mindset interventions address the issue of whether a human
attribute, such as intelligence or personality, can be developed—that is, whether it is con-
trollable through personal agency and environmental support or not. However, these are
not the only possible kinds of mindsets. Other mindsets can address beliefs about differ-
ent aspects of the nature and workings of attributes of humans or the world. For example,
in a fascinating line of work, Crum, Salovey, and Achor (2013; see also Crum, Handley-
Miner, & Smith, Chapter 9, this volume) have studied whether people believe stress is
good (facilitating) or bad (debilitating), the consequences of these beliefs, and the benefits
of an intervention offering people a view of the potential benefits of stress. Following
this lead, Haimovitz and Dweck (2016) have examined beliefs about whether failure is
facilitating or debilitating, finding that parents who believe failure is debilitating seem to
be transmitting more of a fixed mindset of intelligence to their children. These are just
some of the relatives in the large and growing growth-mindset family.
The Intervention Basics
The growth-mindset intervention has evolved over the years as it has been evaluated in
new studies and in different contexts (see Dweck & Yeager, 2019). Throughout its life,
the intervention has usually involved three elements. The first is scientific credibility. The
growth-mindset intervention conveys the new and emerging neuroscience of the brain’s
amazing potential to change and develop, and it explains that it is through intensive learn-
ing that denser and stronger neuronal connections are formed. The intervention goes on
to explain that, therefore, struggling to master something new or experiencing setbacks
24 I. Education
are not signs of inability, but rather are the means through which people strengthen the
connections in their brains and get smarter.
The second element is a sticky or memorable metaphor: that the brain is like a muscle
(Aronson et al., 2002). Intervention participants learn that the way that academic expe-
riences strengthen the brain’s connections can be seen as analogous to how strenuous
exercise strengthens our muscles. The goal of this framing is, again, to undermine nega-
tive effort beliefs and fixed-ability attributions and instead evoke the value of effort and
difficulty in a growth-mindset-meaning system.
The third critical element involves features that help bring these ideas to life. These
include testimonies from older students who were exposed to and used growth-mindset
messages to improve their learning in school. In more recent interventions, we have added
testimony from one or more admired figures who used a growth mindset and its prin-
ciples to achieve their goals in life.
There are also writing assignments for the participant. One of the most commonly
implemented exercises in the growth-mindset intervention, as we mentioned earlier, is
a “letter to a future student” writing assignment (originated by Aronson et al., 2002).
Students imagine a future student their age who is having a hard time in school and does
not yet know the growth-mindset message. They are then asked to write a compelling and
encouraging letter to the future student, using the ideas they just learned about a growth
mindset. These ideas are listed for them as a reminder of what they can include. Not
only might this exercise lead the participants to elaborate on the growth-mindset ideas
more fully (resulting in improved memory of the intervention messages), but it can also
encourage internalization of the message through the “saying-is-believing” effect (Aron-
son, 1999) discussed above—an effect that can enhance the likelihood that students will
apply the learning to their own schoolwork in the future.
Finally, in recent iterations, we have included an exercise to help students understand
why it’s important to have a stronger brain. Through the work of others (e.g., Fryberg,
Covarrubias, & Burack, 2018), we realized that not all groups of students may believe
that increasing their intellectual abilities is a valuable pursuit for its own sake. For this
reason, we now include an exercise that asks students what they hope to achieve in their
lives and then asks them to write about how a stronger brain could help them achieve it.
Despite all these elements, an intervention can easily be derailed if it does not convey
respect for the participants. This is particularly important with adolescents. Thus, we
begin the program by inviting students not only to participate in the program but also
to contribute to its development, since they are the experts on school and student life. In
this way, they are not simply research participants but are potential shapers of the future
program. In other words, we enlist them as collaborators who have important informa-
tion to offer, and we request this information throughout the intervention.
As suggested above, every growth-mindset-of-intelligence intervention must be tai-
lored to the target population in terms of, for example, age, school transition, and culture
or subgroup. In our own interventions, we have spent considerable time molding the
program to the psychology of the particular participants in that study. This is why in
our national study of the transition to high school we helped students understand why
a strong brain is important—we realized, as noted above, that this value is not univer-
sally understood or shared. This means that researchers or practitioners must address the
issues, needs, values, and norms of their population in tailoring a new growth-mindset
intervention.
A Growth Mindset about Intelligence 25
It is also worth clarifying what the growth-mindset intervention does not do. The
intervention does not promise students that they will reach any particular level of ability
in the future or that they will achieve any specific goal. Rather, the intervention empha-
sizes the capacity for continued development and improvement over time.
Current interventions also avoid focusing on effort alone as the vehicle for intellec-
tual growth, since sheer effort may be useless if a student does not have good strategies or
effective mentoring from others. For this reason, the intervention does not simply exhort
students to “try hard.” Moreover, effort exhortations alone are unlikely to be effective
even for promoting effort. Adults are always nagging students to try harder, with little
effect. But perhaps even more importantly, high effort has negative implications in a
fixed-mindset-meaning system: If you have to try hard, it means you are not smart (see,
e.g., Blackwell et al., 2007; see also Meyer, 1982). Thus, continual effort exhortations
could be insulting or threatening to students.
In summary, growth-mindset interventions begin by providing basic information
about the malleability of intellectual abilities— about the brain and its capacity for
growth. This is done in a compelling way, first scientifically and then in a personally
meaningful way—that is, in light of the scientific evidence, students are led to reinterpret
the meaning of effort and setbacks, not as negative things, but as vehicles for growth.
Along the way, they are given opportunities to internalize the growth mindset and they
are led to think about how a stronger brain could help them in their important future
endeavors.
NUANCES AND MISCONCEPTIONS
The concept of a growth mindset may seem quite simple: It is the belief that abilities (or
other personal attributes) can be developed. However, we have found that it can easily be
equated with other concepts that people are familiar with (such as being open-minded)
or with isolated aspects of the meaning system (such as effort).
Many educators have succeeded amazingly well in communicating and implement-
ing a growth mindset in their classrooms and schools (Berger, Woodfin, & Vilen, 2016;
Crow & Dabars, 2015). An elementary school principal whose school implemented
growth-mindset concepts reported:
“By embracing the productive struggle and helping our students understand that
through doing hard things they have the opportunity to grow their brains, we have
unleashed a wave that has contributed to our student growth landing in the top ten
percent of our state.”
And yet, there are many nuances to a growth mindset and its implementation that
must be kept in mind for implementations to be effective. Here are questions we have
been asked, along with our answers and their implications for interventions or educa-
tional practice.
• If a person has a growth (or fixed) mindset about intelligence, does he or she
have the same mindset about other human qualities, such as personality or athletic abil-
ity? There is only a very modest correlation among mindsets about different abilities or
26 I. Education
qualities, so people can most definitely have different mindsets about different attributes.
We have also observed that people can have different mindsets about themselves and oth-
ers. They may think, for example, that their own intelligence is more fixed than other
people’s, but their personality is more malleable than others’.
• Within a domain, like intelligence, do people have just one mindset and not the
other? In the past few years, we have come to understand that while people can have pre-
dominantly a fixed or predominantly a growth mindset about intelligence, everyone is a
mixture. For example, a person may be in a growth mindset most of the time, but certain
events, such as challenges, criticism, or failure, can trigger a fixed mindset. Indeed, any
event that makes people feel that their intelligence is being judged (by themselves or oth-
ers) can trigger a fixed mindset in them. This suggests that future interventions, rather
than just teaching a growth mindset, might also teach students how to recognize their
“fixed-mindset triggers” and how to work their way back to a growth mindset in the face
of triggering events (e.g., Orosz et al., 2020). Indeed, interventions in general can more
deliberately teach people strategies to maintain intervention effects over time.
• Is a growth mindset all about effort? As we noted, perhaps the most common
misconception is that a growth mindset is strictly about effort—that all a student needs
to do is to apply effort and all will be well. However, sheer effort, without good strate-
gies, resources, support, and mentorship, is often insufficient. And, if students are led to
believe that sheer effort should be sufficient, they may feel particularly inept when they
fail. Again, putting the onus entirely on the students and their effort denies the critical
importance of environmental support and resources in fostering success. Another prac-
tice that can result from putting an exclusive emphasis on effort is the offering of lavish
praise for students’ effort while neglecting their poor learning progress. This kind of
praise is meant to make students feel good even if they are failing to make strides in their
learning. This is ironic because implementing a growth mindset is precisely about helping
students make progress in developing their abilities, not about hiding their deficiencies
behind a veil of praise.
The bottom line is that educators or intervenors who simply focus on effort are most
likely not creating a full growth-mindset-meaning system or building an environment
that supports it, and their age-old exhortations to try hard (or their frequent praise for
effort) could end up being ineffective or undermining.
• Does a growth mindset mean that everyone is the same; that there are no differ-
ences in talent or ability; that anyone can achieve anything? While a growth mindset is
the belief in everyone’s potential to continually develop and improve, it does not mean
that everyone is the same at any given time, and it does not guarantee that even with great
effort everyone can end up at the same place. Although we believe that people’s potential
is typically great—and usually cannot be known in advance of good mentoring, good
strategy use, opportunities, and devotion to learning over long periods of time—educators
may mistakenly believe they are instilling a growth mindset when they tell their students,
“You can do or be anything!” However inspiring this may sound, it may often be unhelp-
ful, particularly when the educator is not paving the way by giving students knowledge
about the routes they must travel, a head start on the many skills and strategies they must
learn, and perhaps preliminary access to resources required to start them on their path. In
fact, students may later blame themselves when they fail to achieve their goals.
A Growth Mindset about Intelligence 27
• Are lectures on growth mindset enough? In this context, we might also mention
that some educators provide a lecture or two on growth mindset, show some TED Talks,
or put a growth-mindset chart on the walls, but do not change their practices or class-
room culture in ways that would really make students believe that they should take on
challenges, not worry about mistakes, ask for help, or take pride in their progress. Good
implementation requires that these growth-mindset supports be baked into teachers’ poli-
cies and practices. The key point is that those designing a growth-mindset intervention
need to develop a deep, nuanced understanding of the concepts it rests on and then design
the intervention to expertly convey the concepts, why they are important, and how to
apply them. Even with our decades of research on mindsets and our experience with
mindset interventions, it still took us 2 years to develop the intervention materials we
delivered in the National Study of Learning Mindsets (see Table 1.1). In designing a class-
room or school to support a growth mindset, educators also need to think through how
their practices foster and reward taking on challenges, sticking to them, using strategies,
interpreting failures, and, perhaps above all, valuing progress.
IMPLICATIONS FOR PRACTICE
There is growing evidence that educators’ mindsets play a role in students’ motivation
and achievement. Above we cited research by Canning, Muenks, and colleagues (Can-
ning et al., 2020) demonstrating that college students, especially those from underrep-
resented groups, earned lower grades in the STEM classes of fixed-mindset professors
and reported less motivation in those classes. Research is also beginning to document
the things educators do in their classes that have these effects, such as focusing on
TABLE 1.1. The R&D Process Used to Develop a Scalable Growth-Mindset Intervention
Phase Years References Step
1 2010–2012 Paunesku et al. (2015); 1.1. Test the “base” versions, adapted from in-person
Yeager, Walton, et al. interventions
(2016)
3 2014–2015 Yeager, Walton, et al. 3.1. Head-to-head test of “base” versus “new” with
(2016) proxy outcome (N ≈ 7,500)
3.2. Preregistered evaluation of “new” versus control,
GPA outcome, convenience sample (N ≈ 3,600)
4 2015–2016 Yeager et al. (2019) 4.1. Extensive rewriting to finalize “newer” version
4.2. Preregistered evaluation of “newer” versus
control, GPA and course-taking outcomes,
national sample
Note. R&D, research and development; GPA, grade-point average. Adapted from Dweck and Yeager (2019). Adapted
with permission from Sage Publications, Inc.
28 I. Education
and rewarding improvement or giving students chances to revise their work, as well as
resources for improving their work (Canning et al., 2019; Sun, 2015; see also Rattan et
al., 2012). And, as we noted above, similar findings are beginning to emerge from the
National Study of Learning Mindsets, which included data from math teachers and their
students.
These findings are important. It is our view that the full potential of growth-mindset
interventions to enhance students’ motivation and achievement will not be realized until
we can supplement direct-to-student interventions with teacher interventions and per-
haps with school reform (since it may be difficult to create growth-mindset climates
when teacher are themselves evaluated on their students’ short-term test scores). As we
saw earlier, many educators may not have learned the nuances of a growth mindset and,
even if they have, there are still many factors that may discourage teachers and school
administrators from experimenting with new school or classroom learning and reward
structures: (1) educators may not know how to turn their knowledge into classroom prac-
tices that communicate and support growth mindsets in their students and (2) the incen-
tive systems embedded in schools and institutions—such as teacher accountability or
school rankings based on test scores—may discourage teachers from experimenting with
new practices and (3) cultural models (Markus & Kitayama, 2010) of how teaching and
learning are supposed to proceed may not be optimal for many students (teachers lectur-
ing rather than students collaborating and mentoring one another; relatively permanent
ability groupings; grading strictly on outcomes and not factoring in improvement; short-
term classroom units rather than longer multifaceted projects that may involve work in
the community).
Prominent educators have grappled with many of these issues. For example, using a
growth-mindset perspective, Boaler (2015) has rethought mathematics teaching; Berger
and colleagues (2016), in the Expeditionary Learning (EL) schools, have rethought ele-
mentary school structure and curriculum; and Michael Crow, the president of Arizona
State University, has completely overhauled the conception, purpose, structure, curricu-
lum, and practices of his institution to provide a model for the modern U.S. university
(see Crow & Dabars, 2015). In each of these cases, students engage in more self-initiated,
deeper, more intrinsically motivated learning—the kind of learning that sustains a growth
mindset and the kind of learning that will help students think and thrive in the modern
world. It would be exciting to study these programs directly to determine whether growth
mindsets are indeed fostered and sustained in these settings and to examine the role that
the mindsets might play in enhancing the benefits of these programs.
One impediment to improving practice is that teachers do not have good ways to
gauge which of the teaching practices they try are working and which are not. For exam-
ple, if a teacher tries some growth-mindset practices meant to enhance student engage-
ment with challenging material, how can they tell whether they have succeeded? In an
exciting new undertaking, Dave Paunesku and Sarah Gripshover (research scientists at
the Project for Educational Research that Scales [PERTS]), and their colleagues (www.
perts.net/engage) have devised and are testing methods for quickly and anonymously
surveying students on a weekly basis to see whether there are changes in their reported
engagement that follow their teachers’ introduction of new methods.
Finally, in a major new initiative, Stephanie Fryberg and Mary Murphy (profes-
sors at the University of Michigan and Indiana University, respectively), working with
teachers in the Seattle, Washington, public school system, have been developing a new
A Growth Mindset about Intelligence 29
curriculum to teach educators a more deeply nuanced growth mindset and how to imple-
ment it in their classrooms. Once it is completed and rigorously tested, it will be made
available to educators online at no cost. At the same time, C. Y. Chiu (professor and Dean
of Social Science at the Chinese University of Hong Kong) and his colleagues, working
with educators in Hong Kong, are developing and testing online programs for teachers
and administrators. It will be interesting in the future to understand cultural differences
in how growth-mindset interventions are best designed and implemented.
In closing this section, we call attention to what a daunting task it is to create
growth-mindset programs that all teachers can use successfully. Some of the most skilled
psychologists, researchers, and educators are dedicated to this task, but it will surely take
considerable time before the task is complete. Indeed, we think that changing classroom
culture is the next “Mt. Everest” that the mindset research community will need to climb
together.
IMPLICATIONS FOR PSYCHOLOGICAL THEORY
Mindset research and interventions support the idea that people build powerful beliefs
about themselves, and that these beliefs can affect their motivation and ultimately their
accomplishments (Dweck, 2017; Walton & Wilson, 2018). In the case of fixed and
growth mindsets, based on their experiences in their settings and the messages embedded
in their culture, people develop beliefs about whether their attributes, such as intelligence,
are fixed at a certain level or can be developed. As we have seen, these mindsets—by sig-
naling to people what they can or cannot accomplish in a given environment—can have
cascading effects, influencing their goals, effort, reactions to setbacks, and longer-term
performance.
This viewpoint has important implications for understanding how people work—
that is, what makes them tick. It locates beliefs at the core of motivation and what is so
important is that beliefs can be changed (Dweck, 2017; Walton & Wilson, 2018). Thus,
mindsets (and the other key psychological variables explored in this volume) give psy-
chologists and practitioners a target for interventions. It does not mean that it is easy for
people to see things in a new way, but with helpful input they can often get there. When
beliefs that play important roles in people’s lives are targeted, the results can be greater
than one might expect, and this, we believe, is why short interventions done properly can
be effective. The emphasis on beliefs also leads us to try to understand the environments
people are in, how those environments support different beliefs, and how we might alter
those environments to support more fruitful ways of thinking—that is, ways of thinking
that help people reach their goals.
Mindset interventions show us that social problems often have a psychological
component (Walton & Dweck, 2009). Whether we are talking about recycling, energy
consumption, healthy eating, or school dropout, we are often trying to get people to
change their behavior by changing their minds. Yet, many or most interventions in the
“real world” are based on the idea that change, such as educational change, comes only
from large, costly restructuring of schools or curricula. Of course, structural change
will always be highly important, and mindset changes and structural changes can work
well together. Nonetheless, when structural changes are not yet forthcoming, “sufficient”
structural factors may be in place to make a change in “psychology” take root. In the
30 I. Education
National Study of Learning Mindsets, meaningful change took place among students in
lower-achieving schools. Many schools may not have been well resourced but there were
still opportunities for learning that could be better capitalized upon.
What other social problems can benefit from addressing beliefs to bring about
behavior change? The other chapters in this volume provide many compelling examples.
FUTURE DIRECTIONS
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Preparation of this chapter was supported in part by the William T. Grant Foundation, the
Yidan Prize Foundation/Optimus Foundation, the National Science Foundation (Grant No.
HRD 1761179), and the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (Grant
No. 10.13039/100000071 R01HD084772-01 and Grant No. P2C-HD042849 to the Population
Research Center at the University of Texas at Austin). The content is solely the responsibility of the
authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health
and the National Science Foundation.
REFERENCES
Allcott, H. (2015). Site selection bias in program evaluation. Quarterly Journal of Economics,
130(3), 1117–1165.
Andersen, S. C., & Nielsen, H. S. (2016). Reading intervention with a growth mindset approach
improves children’s skills. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA,
113(43), 12111–12113.
Aronson, E. (1999). The power of self-persuasion. American Psychologist, 54(11), 875–884.
Aronson, J. M., Fried, C. B., & Good, C. (2002). Reducing the effects of stereotype threat on Afri-
can American college students by shaping theories of intelligence. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology, 38(2), 113–125.
Berger, R., Woodfin, L., & Vilen, A. (2016). Learning that lasts: Challenging, engaging, and
empowering students with deeper instruction. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Bettinger, E. P., Ludvigsen, S., Rege, M., Solli, I. F., & Yeager, D. S. (2018). Increasing persever-
ance in math: Evidence from a field experiment in Norway. Journal of Economic Behavior
and Organization, 146, 1–15.
Binet, A. (1975). Modern ideas about children. (S. Heisler, Trans.). Menlo Park, CA: Suzanne
Heisler. (Original work published 1909)
32 I. Education
Blackwell, L. S., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Dweck, C. S. (2007). Implicit theories of intelligence pre-
dict achievement across an adolescent transition: A longitudinal study and an intervention.
Child Development, 78(1), 246–263.
Boaler, J. (2015). Mathematical mindsets: Unleashing students’ potential through creative math,
inspiring messages and innovative teaching. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Boulay, B., Goodson, B., Olsen, R., McCormick, R., Darrow, C., Frye, M., . . . Sarna, M. (2018). The
Investing in Innovation Fund: Summary of 67 evaluations (No. NCEE 2018-4013). Retrieved
from National Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance, Institute of Educa-
tion Sciences, U.S. Department of Education: https://ies.ed.gov/ncee/pubs/20184013.
Broda, M., Yun, J., Schneider, B., Yeager, D. S., Walton, G. M., & Diemer, M. (2018). Reducing
inequality in academic success for incoming college students: A randomized trial of growth
mindset and belonging interventions. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness,
11(3), 317–338.
Burnette, J. L., Forsyth, R. B., Desmarais, S. L., & Hoyt, C. L. (2019). Mindsets of addiction: Impli-
cations for treatment intentions. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 38(5), 367–394.
Canning, E. A., Muenks, K., Green, D. J., & Murphy, M. C. (2019). STEM faculty who believe
ability is fixed have larger racial achievement gaps and inspire less student motivation in their
classes. Science Advances, 5(2), eaau4734.
Canning, E. A., Murphy, M. C., Emerson, K. T. U., Chatman, J. A., Dweck, C. S., & Kray, L. J.
(2020). Culture of genius at work: Organizational mindsets predict cultural norms, trust, and
commitment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 46(4), 626–642.
Carr, P. B., Dweck, C. S., & Pauker, K. (2012). “Prejudiced” behavior without prejudice?: Beliefs
about the malleability of prejudice affect interracial interactions. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 103(3), 452–471.
Cheung, A. C. K., & Slavin, R. E. (2016). How methodological features affect effect sizes in edu-
cation. Educational Researcher, 45(5), 283–292.
Crow, M. M., & Dabars, W. B. (2015). Designing the new American university. Baltimore: John
Hopkins University Press.
Crum, A. J., Leibowitz, K. A., & Verghese, A. (2017). Making mindset matter. British Medical
Journal, 356, j674.
Crum, A. J., Salovey, P., & Achor, S. (2013). Rethinking stress: The role of mindsets in determin-
ing the stress response. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(4), 716–733.
Diamond, A., Barnett, W. S., Thomas, J., & Munro, S. (2007). Preschool program improves cogni-
tive control. Science, 318(5855), 1387.
Dweck, C. S. (2017). From needs to goals and representations: Foundations for a unified theory of
motivation, personality, and development. Psychological Review, 124(6), 689–719.
Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social–cognitive approach to motivation and personality.
Psychological Review, 95(2), 256–273.
Dweck, C. S., & Yeager, D. S. (2019). Mindsets: A view from two eras. Perspectives on Psycho-
logical Science, 13(3), 481–496.
Dynarski, S. M. (2017). For better learning in college lectures, lay down the laptop and pick up
a pen. Retrieved from www.brookings.edu/research/for-better-learning-in-college-lectures-
lay-down-the-laptop-and-pick-up-a-pen.
Fryberg, S. A., Covarrubias, R., & Burack, J. A. (2018). The ongoing psychological colonization
of North American indigenous people: Using social psychological theories to promote social
justice. In P. L. Hammack (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of social psychology and social jus-
tice. New York: Oxford University Press.
Fuesting, M. A., Diekman, A. B., Boucher, K. L., Murphy, M. C., Manson, D. L., & Safer, B. L.
(2019). Growing STEM: Perceived faculty mindset as an indicator of communal affordances
in STEM. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 117(2), 260–281.
A Growth Mindset about Intelligence 33
Galton, F. (1892). Hereditary genius: An inquiry into its laws and consequences. London: Mac-
millan.
Goldenberg, A., Cohen-Chen, A., Goyer, J. P., Dweck, C. S., Gross, J. J., & Halperin, E. (2018).
Testing the impact and durability of a group malleability intervention in the context of the
Israeli–Palestinian conflict. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA,
115(4) 696–701.
Good, C., Aronson, J., & Inzlicht, M. (2003). Improving adolescents’ standardized test perfor-
mance: An intervention to reduce the effects of stereotype threat. Journal of Applied Devel-
opmental Psychology, 24(6), 645–662.
Haimovitz, K., & Dweck, C. S. (2016). What predicts children’s fixed and growth intelligence
mind-sets?: Not their parents’ views of intelligence but their parents’ views of failure. Psycho-
logical Science, 27(6), 859–869.
Halperin, E., Russell, A. G., Trzesniewski, K. H., Gross, J. J., & Dweck, C. S. (2011). Promot-
ing the Middle East peace process by changing beliefs about group malleability. Science,
333(6050), 1767–1769.
Hill, C. J., Bloom, H. S., Black, A. R., & Lipsey, M. W. (2008). Empirical benchmarks for inter-
preting effect sizes in research. Child Development Perspectives, 2(3), 172–177.
Hong, Y., Chiu, C., Dweck, C. S., Lin, D. M.-S., & Wan, W. (1999). Implicit theories, attribu-
tions, and coping: A meaning system approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 77(3), 588–599.
Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g factor: The science of mental ability. Westport, CT: Praeger.
Job, V., Walton, G. M., Bernecker, K., & Dweck, C. S. (2015). Implicit theories about willpower
predict self-regulation and grades in everyday life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 108(4), 637–647.
Kraft, M. A. (2018). Interpreting effect sizes of education interventions. Retrieved from Brown
University: https://scholar.harvard.edu/files/mkraft/files/kraft_ 2018_interpreting_effect_
sizes.pdf.
Leggett, E. L. (1986). Individual differences in effort/ability inference rules and goals: Implications
for causal judgments. Dissertation Abstracts International, 47(6-B), 2662.
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (2010). Cultures and selves: A cycle of mutual constitution. Per-
spectives on Psychological Science, 5(4), 420–430.
Meyer, W. U. (1982). Indirect communications about perceived ability estimates. Journal of Edu-
cational Psychology, 74, 888–897.
Miele, D. B., & Molden, D. C. (2010). Naive theories of intelligence and the role of processing
fluency in perceived comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(3),
535–557.
Miele, D. B., Son, L. K., & Metcalfe, J. (2013). Children’s naive theories of intelligence influence
their metacognitive judgments. Child Development, 84(6), 1879–1886.
Miu, A. S., & Yeager, D. S. (2015). Preventing symptoms of depression by teaching adolescents
that people can change: Effects of a brief incremental theory of personality intervention at
9-month follow-up. Clinical Psychological Science, 3(5), 726–743.
Moser, J. S., Schroder, H. S., Heeter, C., Moran, T. P., & Lee, Y.-H. (2011). Mind your errors:
Evidence for a neural mechanism linking growth mind-set to adaptive posterror adjustments.
Psychological Science, 22(12), 1484–1489.
Mueller, C., Rowe, M. L., & Zuckerman, B. (2017). Mindset matters for parents and adolescents.
JAMA Pediatrics, 171(5), 415–416.
Mukhopadhyay, A., & Yeung, C. W. M. (2010). Building character: Effects of lay theories of
self-control on the selection of products for children. Journal of Marketing Research, 47,
240–250.
Neel, R., & Shapiro, J. R. (2012). Is racial bias malleable?: Whites’ lay theories of racial bias
34 I. Education
predict divergent strategies for interracial interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 103(1), 101–120.
Nussbaum, A. D., & Dweck, C. S. (2008). Defensiveness versus remediation: Self-theories and
modes of self-esteem maintenance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(5), 599–
612.
Orosz, G., Walton, G. M., Böthe, B., Tóth-Király, I., Henderson, A., & Dweck, C. S. (2020). Can
mindfulness help people implement a growth mindset?: Two field experiments in Hungary.
Unpublished manuscript, Stanford University, Stanford, CA.
Outes, I., Sánchez, A., & Vakis, R. (2017). Cambiando la mentalidad de los estudiantes: Evalu-
ación de impacto de ¡Expande tu Mente! Sobre el rendimiento académico en tres regiones del
Perú, 83, 1–106. Retrieved from www.grade.org.pe/wp-content/uploads/ddt83.pdf.
Paunesku, D., Walton, G. M., Romero, C., Smith, E. N., Yeager, D. S., & Dweck, C. S. (2015).
Mind-set interventions are a scalable treatment for academic underachievement. Psychologi-
cal Science, 26(6), 784–793.
Ramsden, S., Richardson, F. M., Josse, G., Thomas, M. S. C., Ellis, C., Shakeshaft, C., . . .
Price, C. J. (2011). Verbal and non-verbal intelligence changes in the teenage brain. Nature,
479(7371), 113–116.
Rattan, A., & Dweck, C. S. (2010). Who confronts prejudice?: The role of implicit theories in the
motivation to confront prejudice. Psychological Science, 21(7), 952–959.
Rattan, A., Good, C., & Dweck, C. S. (2012). “It’s ok—not everyone can be good at math”:
Instructors with an entity theory comfort (and demotivate) students. Journal of Experimen-
tal Social Psychology, 48(3), 731–737.
Rege, M., Hanselman, P., Solli, I. F., Dweck, C. S., Ludvigsen, S., Bettinger, E., . . . Yeager, D. S.
(in press). How can we inspire nations of learners?: An investigation of growth mindset and
challenge-seeking in two countries. American Psychologist.
Robins, R. W., & Pals, J. L. (2002). Implicit self-theories in the academic domain: Implications for
goal orientation, attributions, affect, and self-esteem change. Self and Identity, 1(4), 313–336.
Rueda, M. R., Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2005). The development of executive attention:
Contributions to the emergence of self-regulation. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(2),
573–594.
Sauce, B., & Matzel, L. D. (2018). The paradox of intelligence: Heritability and malleability coex-
ist in hidden gene–environment interplay. Psychological Bulletin, 144(1), 26–47.
Schleider, J. L., & Weisz, J. R. (2016). Reducing risk for anxiety and depression in adolescents:
Effects of a single-session intervention teaching that personality can change. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 87, 170–181.
Schumann, K., Zaki, J., & Dweck, C. S. (2014). Addressing the empathy deficit: Beliefs about the
malleability of empathy predict effortful responses when empathy is challenging. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 107(3), 475–493.
Seligman, M. E., & Maier, S. F. (1967). Failure to escape traumatic shock. Journal of Experimen-
tal Psychology, 74(1), 1–9.
Smith, E. N., Romero, C., Donovan, B., Herter, R., Paunesku, D., Cohen, G. L., . . . Gross, J. J.
(2018). Emotion theories and adolescent well-being: Results of an online intervention. Emo-
tion, 18(6), 781–788.
Sun, K. L. (2015). There’s no limit: Mathematics teaching for a growth mindset. Doctoral dis-
sertation, Stanford University, Stanford, CA. Retrieved from https://searchworks.stanford.
edu/view/11059824.
Terman, L. M. (1926). Genetic studies of genius (Vol. 1). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Thomas, F. N., Burnette, J. L., & Hoyt, C. L. (2019). Mindsets of health and healthy eating inten-
tions. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 49(6), 372–380.
Tipton, E. (2013). Improving generalizations from experiments using propensity score
A Growth Mindset about Intelligence 35
Gregory M. Walton and Shannon T. Brady
All students travel a social distance in coming to school. They leave behind families and
communities to enter new social spaces where they strive to build new friendships and
relationships with instructors while taking on challenging learning and achievement tasks.
In entering a new social space, people can readily wonder whether they will belong, but
particularly so when their social group is negatively stereotyped or underrepresented in
that space. In this state of belonging uncertainty, everyday challenges can seem to threaten
one’s belonging in the setting as a whole. To protect this sense of belonging, the social-
belonging intervention represents the general truth that common challenges and worries
about belonging are normal in an academic transition and improve with time. Across a
dozen randomized field trials, the intervention, delivered as an interactive exercise lasting an
hour or less, has improved students’ integration into secondary and postsecondary school
communities and raised academic performance years into the future. Typically the greatest
benefits are observed for students from groups that are negatively stereotyped or under-
represented in the context. Here we review the origins of the social-belonging intervention in
basic research; discuss its relationship to other psychologically “wise” interventions; review
outcome effects; discuss heterogeneity and processes over time; share materials to pro-
mote effective use; and discuss implications for psychological theory, education, and policy.
BACKGROUND
The roots of the social-belonging intervention lie in both the sociocultural history of the
United States and the intellectual tradition of social psychology, especially research on
the experience of students who contend with negative stereotypes in school.
For African Americans, other people of color, and people from lower social class
backgrounds, too often the history of the United States has been a fight for acceptance.
36
The Social‑Belonging Intervention 37
In school, this battle has been waged in court battles for desegregation (e.g., Brown v.
Board of Education), in national law (e.g., the Civil Rights Act of 1964), in school district
(e.g., school busing) and university policy (e.g., affirmative action), in civil protests, and
in individual students’ and families’ lives (see Figure 2.1).
Simultaneously, students of color have had to contend with stereotypes that allege
the intellectual inferiority of their group. The early psychologist Lewis Terman (1916),
a prominent eugenicist, introduced IQ to America as a fixed constraint that justified the
exclusion of children from mainstream education: “Children of [low IQ] should be seg-
regated in special classes and be given instruction which is concrete and practical. They
cannot master, but they can often be made efficient workers, able to look out for them-
selves” (p. 92). Moreover, Terman identified this inability with race: “It is interesting to
note that M. P. and C. P. [children with low IQ scores] represent the level of intelligence
which is very, very common among Spanish-I ndian and Mexican families of the South-
west and also among negroes. Their dullness seems to be racial, or at least inherent in the
family stocks from which they come. . . . These boys are uneducable beyond the merest
rudiments of training” (p. 91).
As legal, social, and political pressure to integrate schools mounted, the formal
rejection of children of color has, to some extent, receded. Yet the legacy of exclusion
and negative stereotypes remains (e.g., Herrnstein & Murray, 1994). What is it like
to enter a school setting in which your group has been excluded, is stereotyped as less
able and less worthy, and may still be underrepresented? Could concerns about belong-
ing that arise from this circumstance contribute to educational inequality today? By
understanding the psychological consequences wrought by this legacy of injustice, can
we develop theory-based ways to mitigate inequality and better support today’s diverse
students?
FIGURE 2.1. The 1957 desegregation of Little Rock Central High School. Reprinted from the
Will Counts Collection, Indiana University Archives.
38 I. Education
PSYCHOLOGICAL PROCESSES
Stereotype Threat
An important early observation in the development of the belonging intervention was that
inequalities in school achievement do not simply reflect differences in ability. Instead,
inequality reproduces at successive levels of education, even among students with the
same preparation (Steele, 1997; see also Walton & Spencer, 2009). For instance, even
with identical SAT scores and high school grades, Black college students earn worse
grades on average than White students, and women earn worse grades in math and sci-
ence than men. This “underperformance” is so common as to be termed “lawful” (Steele,
1997, p. 615). What in the college environment prevents equally well-prepared Black
students and women from achieving at the same rates as Whites and men?
Claude Steele and colleagues (Steele, Spencer, & Aronson, 2002; Steele, 1997, 2010)
pointed to a psychological burden imposed by negative stereotypes. Specifically, Steele
and colleagues theorized that when a negative stereotype is on the table people can worry
that others could view them through the lens of that stereotype: “If I do poorly, people
could think the stereotype is true.” For a student invested in school, this extra pres-
sure, termed stereotype threat, can cause distraction and anxiety that undermines perfor-
mance, ironically seeming to confirm the stereotype. In seminal studies, high-performing
Black college students scored worse than Whites on a test composed of items from the
Graduate Record Exam (GRE). But when the same items were represented as a laboratory
puzzle-solving exercise—and thus as nonevaluative and irrelevant to racial stereotypes—
Black students did as well as Whites, controlling for baseline SAT scores (Steele & Aron-
son, 1995). Similar patterns emerged among women in math (Spencer, Steele, & Quinn,
1999), and in hundreds of studies (Steele, 2010). Moreover, research has identified key
mechanisms of this process, including distraction and a drain on executive functioning
and cognitive resources, which lowers performance (Schmader, Johns, & Forbes, 2008).
Belonging Uncertainty
The fear in a testing situation that one could be seen in light of a negative stereotype may
be the tip of the iceberg. Students who face negative stereotypes or underrepresentation
may harbor a broader concern: “Can people like me belong here?” (Walton & Cohen,
2007; see also Goffman, 1963; Mendoza-Denton, Downey, Purdie, Davis, & Pietrzak,
2002). It is this broader concern that is addressed by the social-belonging intervention.
Worries about belonging in school are evident in stories told by many racial-minority
and first-generation college students. Former first lady Michelle Obama wrote in her
senior thesis, “I sometimes feel like a visitor on campus; as if I really don’t belong. . . . It
often seems as if . . . I will always be Black first and a student second” (Robinson, 1985,
p. 2). Supreme Court Justice Sonia Sotomayor said she felt like “a visitor landing in an
alien country [in college] . . . I have spent my years since, while at law school, and in my
various professional jobs, not feeling completely a part of the worlds I inhabit” (in Lud-
den & Weeks, 2009).
We define a sense of belonging as a person’s perception of the quality of his or her
relationship with a valued school, work, or community context as a whole. This relation-
ship goes beyond personal ties to individuals in the setting. Instead, it is fundamentally
symbolic. It arises from events and experiences that represent to the person his or her
relationship with the setting itself (Walton & Brady, 2017).
The Social‑Belonging Intervention 39
When I first arrived at school as a first-generation college student, I didn’t know anyone
on campus except my brother. I didn’t know how to pick the right classes or find the
right buildings. I didn’t even bring the right size sheets for my dorm room bed. I didn’t
realize those beds were so long. So I was a little overwhelmed and a little isolated.
Despite decades of professional success, Obama still recalls her reaction to bringing the
wrong sheets to college. To her at that time, they were more than just ill-fitting sheets, an
inconvenience to be remedied with a trip to the store. The event implied that the answer
to the implicit question she held—“Does a person like me belong here?”—might be “no.”
The need to belong is fundamental (Baumeister & Leary, 1995) and closely linked to
health, well-being, achievement, and identity (Walton & Brady, 2017). If a student infers
that he or she does not belong in school, it can be exceedingly difficult to stay motivated
and engaged and build relationships in school. Thus, inferences of nonbelonging can
become self-fulfilling. They can deprive students of the supports needed to succeed in a
40 I. Education
challenging academic environment. When doubts about belonging arise from disadvan-
tage, this process can perpetuate inequality.
EMPIRICAL EVIDENCE
Outcomes
The social-belonging intervention has been evaluated in at least a dozen rigorous, ran-
domized controlled field experiments testing effects on core academic outcomes. Improve-
ments are observed most often for students who face negative stereotypes and underrep-
resentation in a school setting.
The seminal study delivered the intervention or active control materials to a small
sample of Black and White students in the first year at a selective, predominately White
university (N = 92). Delivery took place in hour-long one-on-one sessions. The interven-
tion raised Black students’ grades over 3 years, through senior year, with no change for
White students, halving the racial achievement gap over this period. It also improved
Black students’ confidence in their belonging, well-being, and self-reported health at the
end of college (Walton & Cohen, 2007, 2011).
The Social‑Belonging Intervention 41
A second study adapted the intervention for women in engineering. Students in the
first semester of a selective engineering program (N = 228) took part in 45- to 60-minute
small-group sessions. In majors with a critical mass of female students, which averaged
33% women, there was no gender inequality in first-year grades and no treatment effect.
But in male-dominated majors, which averaged just 10% women, the belonging inter-
vention raised women’s first-year grades, eliminating a large gender inequality. It also
improved women’s reported experience and confidence in their prospects of success over
the first year (Walton et al., 2015).
Later studies have examined ways to reach larger populations, including online
through college and university systems that incoming students complete before beginning
college. Yeager and colleagues (2016) created intervention modules, lasting ≅ 30-minute
each, and tested these in three different institutionwide samples (i.e., ≥90% of each popu-
lation). In the first, students graduating from urban charter school networks (N = 584)—
almost all Black and the first in their families to attend college—completed online materi-
als in May of their senior year of high school. The social-belonging intervention, adapted
for this population, raised the percentage of students who stayed full-time enrolled in
college over the next year from 32 to 43%. In the second, students entering a large public
university (N = 7,418) completed online social-belonging, growth-mindset, or control
materials in the summer before matriculation. Receiving either intervention increased
the percentage of negatively stereotyped minority and first-generation college students
who completed the first year full-time enrolled from 69 to 73%, reducing the gap with
nonminority, continuing-generation students by 40%. In the third, students entering a
selective private university (N = 1,596) completed either the social-belonging interven-
tion, an intervention focused on critical feedback (see Yeager et al., 2014), one focused
on cultural fit, or control materials in the summer before matriculation. Receiving any of
the interventions raised first-year grades among negatively stereotyped minority students
and first-generation White students, reducing the achievement gap with more advantaged
students by 31%. (In the latter two cases, the various treatments were equally effective.)
Not all trials have been effective, however, as another online, prematriculation trial did
not affect first-year academic outcomes (Broda et al., 2018).
The belonging intervention has also been scaled through first-year classes. Murphy
and colleagues (2020) adapted the intervention for a highly diverse urban broad-access
institution and implemented it as a personal reading-and-writing exercise in an hour-long
session in first-year writing classes (N = 1,063). The intervention raised the percentage
of negatively stereotyped minority and first-generation students who stayed enrolled over
the next 2 years from 64 to 73%. Binning and colleagues (2020), having observed gender
disparities in an introductory physics course and racial disparities in a biology course,
adapted the intervention for these classes and implemented it in class through story shar-
ing, personal reflection, and small-group conversation. The intervention eliminated each
disparity, raising course grades for women in physics and for non-White students in biol-
ogy.
Another study adapted the intervention for the transition to middle school and deliv-
ered it to Black and White students early in sixth grade over two 30-minute class ses-
sions (N = 137; Goyer et al., 2019, Experiment 2). The intervention reduced disciplin-
ary citations among Black boys through the end of high school by 65%, reducing the
disparity with White boys by 75%, and supported higher levels of belonging and fewer
worries about negative stereotypes through middle school. Another study tested the same
42 I. Education
materials with diverse sixth-grade students in 11 public middle schools across a district
(N = 1,304). Here all students benefited in sixth grade, including reductions in disciplin-
ary referrals, higher grades, reduced absences, and improved school attitudes (Borman,
Rozek, Pyne, & Hanselman, 2019). A third study adapted the intervention for the tran-
sition to high school and found improvements in attendance, course passage rates, and
reduced discipline citations, especially for racial-minoritized students (Williams, Hirschi,
Sublett, Hulleman, & Wilson, 2020). A final study, conducted in a German university,
found improvements in first-semester grades for students with a migration background
(Marksteiner, Janke, & Dickhäuser, 2019).
Mechanisms
How does the belonging intervention foster lasting gains? Figure 2.2 depicts a process
model. Most proximally, the intervention aims to forestall global inferences of nonbe-
longing due to commonplace, everyday experiences in school (see Figure 2.2B). Consis-
tent with this, an early study found that, in the first week after delivery, the intervention
did not alter the kinds of challenges students reported on a daily basis. But it seemed to
prevent Black students from inferring on the basis of daily challenges that they did not
belong in school in general, thereby sustaining students’ sense of belonging on more
adverse days (Walton & Cohen, 2007). To illustrate, one day a Black student reported,
FIGURE 2.2. How beliefs about belonging can affect students’ behavior and academic outcomes
in the transition to college. Adapted from Yeager et al. (2016) with permission from the authors.
The Social‑Belonging Intervention 43
“Everyone is going out without me, and they didn’t consider me when making their plans.
At times like this I feel like I don’t belong here and that I’m alienated.” The intervention
did not prevent adversities (the first sentence) but it mitigated the global inference (the
second sentence). Over this week-long period, a tight relationship was found in the con-
trol condition between how much adversity Black students reported each day (how good
or bad their day was) and their level of belonging that night and the next afternoon: Fol-
lowing worse days Black students reported lower levels of belonging. That relationship
was not present for White students. And it was severed for Black students by the interven-
tion. This change in the construal of daily events statistically mediated the gain in grades
for Black students over the next 3 years, through graduation (Walton & Cohen, 2011; see
also Murphy et al., 2020; Walton et al., 2015).
Effects over Time
By protecting students’ sense of belonging in the face of adversity, the intervention helps
students stay engaged in school (see Figure 2.2C). Over the first week following treat-
ment in the original study, the intervention increased students’ reports of the extent to
which they e-mailed professors, attended office hours, met with study groups, partici-
pated in class, and studied (Walton & Cohen, 2007). Later studies found the intervention
increased students’ likelihood to live on campus and seek out academic support services
during their first year of college (Yeager et al., 2016, Experiments 1 and 3). These behav-
iors help students integrate into the campus community (see Figure 2.2D). Indeed, the
intervention has been shown to help students develop close friendships on campus, to
become more involved in student groups, and to develop mentor relationships (Walton et
al., 2015; Yeager et al., 2016, Experiment 3). Similarly, in middle school, it can improve
cycles of interaction between Black boys and their classroom teachers (Goyer et al., 2019).
A follow-up of the original belonging intervention illustrates the lasting benefits that
mentor relationships can confer to students. As noted earlier, students received the inter-
vention in the first year of college with no subsequent treatment. Seven to 11 years later,
at an average age of 27, Black participants who had received the intervention reported
greater life and career satisfaction (Brady, Cohen, Jarvis, & Walton, 2020). Statistically,
these effects were mediated by reports of greater mentorship during and after college.
Consider this report (Black, treatment condition):
“The first semester of my freshman year was very difficult for me. I was struggling
academically, didn’t feel like I fit in, and was unhappy with my major. . . . I began
to spend more time speaking with my freshman counselor. We really bonded, and
she helped me to realize that I did belong at [school]. Thanks to her, I was able to
connect better with my peers and perform better academically. We’ve kept in touch
ever since.”
These results are consistent with a model in which the intervention mitigated early wor-
ries about belonging to help students reach out and build mentor relationships, which
then empowered adult success (cf. Leitner, Ayduk, Boykin, & Mendoza-Denton, 2018).
Thus, the intervention unlocks the potential of both students and school contexts.
Notably, this role of the intervention can be opaque to recipients. In the same sample,
participants could not recall at the end of college what they had learned in the interven-
tion and credited none of their success in college to it (Walton & Cohen, 2011). The
44 I. Education
intervention does not work through the salience of its central message. Instead, it helps
students make sense of their ongoing stream of social experience in more productive
ways, helping students build relationships of lasting benefit.
Heterogeneity
The social-belonging intervention addresses a specific circumstance: People who want to
belong in a setting but who worry that they might not. The intervention is not designed
to promote a sense of belonging in general, for instance among people who have no desire
to belong in a setting. It will not help an artist with no interest in investment banking feel
that she fits in on Wall Street.
However, most people care about the school and work settings that define their lives
at least to some degree and want to belong and succeed in them. In such settings, the
intervention is generally most effective for people who face group disadvantage, such as
negative stereotypes or underrepresentation (e.g., Walton & Cohen, 2007, 2011; Yeager
et al., 2016; cf. Borman et al., 2019). Benefits may also be greatest in specific circum-
stances that give rise to greater levels of group-based threat, such as science, technology,
engineering, and math (STEM) settings in which women are grossly underrepresented
(Walton et al., 2015) or where performance disparities are large (Binning et al., 2020).
To achieve lasting gains, the intervention further depends on affordance of the
school environment in which it is delivered, especially the degree to which the environ-
ment affords genuine opportunities for belonging (Walton & Yeager, 2020). If, instead,
group-based threats make belonging unrealistic (see Figure 2.1), the intervention may be
ineffective and inappropriate. Further, if students do not have access to adequate learn-
ing opportunities, financial aid, or other objective supports, no psychological exercise is
likely to improve long-term outcomes.
Thus, the intervention should be most effective for students who have reason to ques-
tion their belonging in a setting they care about but which nonetheless offers opportuni-
ties for belonging for them, including opportunities to develop positive relationships that
support lasting gains. Additionally, the intervention will be effective only when its central
message is not already present in the setting in impactful, genuine ways.
COUSINS
grow with hard work, good strategies, and the help of others (Dweck & Yeager, Chapter
1, this volume), thus implying that, if you fail a math test, you are not “dumb at math”
but can improve with effort and a new approach. Like attributional retraining interven-
tions, growth-mindset interventions focus less on social-relational worries that arise from
negative stereotypes and thus on this source of group-based inequality.
Like belonging interventions, theory-of-personality interventions focus on students’
social experience in school and the possibility of improvement in the face of challeges.
However, they emphasize not belonging but the potential for individuals to change and,
in particular, the idea that bullies need not always be bullies and victims need not always
be victims (Yeager & Lee, Chapter 13, this volume). Among adolescents, this can reduce
aggression and increase prosocial behavior following ostracism, improve mental and
physical health, and raise academic achievement.
Value-affirmation interventions share a common origin with belonging interven-
tions in research on stereotype threat. But rather than providing an adaptive narrative
for understanding adversities, affirmation interventions aim to protect people’s sense of
inherent worth and value to help them cope (Sherman, Lokhande, Müller, & Cohen,
Chapter 3, this volume; see also Garcia & Cohen, 2013). They do so by asking stu-
dents to reflect on values of enduring personal importance, which serve as unconditional
sources of worth and helps people confront threats more effectively. The processes initi-
ated by affirmation interventions can be both similar to and different from those initiated
by belonging interventions (see Cook, Purdie-Vaughns, Garcia, & Cohen, 2012; Sher-
man et al., 2013; Walton & Brady, 2017). In the only direct empirical comparison, both
the belonging intervention and an affirmation-training intervention (inspired by classic
affirmation interventions) improved women’s psychological response to daily events (e.g.,
confidence in handling daily adversities) and raised first-year grades in male-dominated
engineering fields (Walton et al., 2015). Yet other processes differed. Whereas the belong-
ing intervention helped women develop greater friendships with male peers, affirmation
increased women’s identification with their gender group and promoted friendships with
other women. It was as though the former helped women experience a warmer climate in
engineering while the latter helped women develop personal resources to weather a chilly
climate.
Difference-education interventions also address social challenges faced by students
from marginalized backgrounds coming to college but whereas the belonging interven-
tion emphasizes everyday challenges experienced by all students, difference-education
interventions emphasize group-specific challenges, such as those faced uniquely by first-
generation college students (Stephens, Hamedani, & Townsend, Chapter 5, this volume).
However, both approaches convey that challenges are not specific to the self but normal
across a group of people and improve with time, and thus not evidence of a general lack
of belonging.
How Is the Intervention Represented?
Generally, the intervention is represented as an effort to learn from students about their
experience in the academic transition to benefit future students. Students are told they
will review conclusions and stories from older students and share their own experience
and expertise with future students to give them a better sense of what the transition will
be like. Thus, participants are represented as benefactors and co-creators of an exercise
to help others, not as beneficiaries of an intervention or recipients of a persuasive mes-
sage.
1. That it is normal to worry at first about whether you belong in a new school.
2. That these difficulties do not mean that you do not belong; rather, with time and
effort, most students typically come to feel at home in the school.
Stories
Most studies of college students have included six to nine stories (~100–150 words per
story); those of middle-school students, three stories in each of two sessions (~85 words
per story). The stories are represented as typical of diverse students’ experience in the
transition at hand. They are best understood as parables. Each describes an individual
student’s trajectory, drawing on past materials and students’ experiences gathered in
interviews and focus groups with careful editing to address critical themes effectively. As
parables, they do not give advice, which could feel prescriptive, presumptuous, or inappli-
cable (cf. Eskreis-Winkler, Fischbach, & Duckworth, 2018). Instead, they offer multiple
models for how a person’s experience could develop over time (Lockwood & Kunda,
1997). While they all convey the same theme, they also include some range to increase
the likelihood participating students find models who experienced challenges that relate
to their own circumstance and see diverse trajectories of growth from these challenges.
The Social‑Belonging Intervention 47
First, the stories articulate specific common worries and everyday negative experiences
many students have in an academic transition. Further, they voice the negative thoughts
and feelings a student can have in response to these challenges, including feelings of non-
belonging (e.g., “I thought professors were scary,” Appendix 2.1, B, Story 3). They thus
represent experiences, thoughts, and feelings of nonbelonging as normal. These chal-
lenges can be attributed explicitly to the difficulty of the transition (e.g., “Still, I think the
transition to college is difficult, and it was for me . . . ,” Appendix 2.1, B, Story 2). They
are not attributed to the school itself. Instead, negative content is placed within a positive
overall representation of the school appropriate for the setting (e.g., “I love [school name]
and I wouldn’t trade my experiences here for anything. . . . Still . . . ,” Appendix 2,1, B,
Story 2). Thus, the materials validate the pride students often feel in their school, even as
they acknowledge normative challenges and doubts.
The challenges described are appropriate to the context. In college, they may include
interactions with instructors (e.g., feeling intimidated, receiving critical feedback) and
peers in academic contexts (e.g., joining study groups), academic setbacks (a poor grade),
challenges making friends, and missing home or older friends. Importantly, the stories do
not raise negative experiences or doubts without resolving their implications for belong-
ing; the reader is not left to wonder whether the protagonist in fact does belong. They
also do not reify negative norms (e.g., “Everyone is depressed,” “Everyone gets drunk
all the time”) or attribute difficulties to enduring negative qualities of the school (e.g.,
“There’s a lot of pressure here”).
The stories are attributed to upper-year students whose seniority affords them a suffi-
ciently long vantage point on the transition, usually through a “tag” at the end of the story
that represents his or her identity (see Appendix 2.1). The students are diverse, including
both students who are more and less advantaged in the setting. To counter the stereotype
that only disadvantaged students question their belonging, the strongest characterization
of feelings of nonbelonging (e.g., feeling intimidated in class) are attributed to more advan-
taged students, such as to Whites in a predominately White college (Walton & Cohen,
2011) or to men in engineering (Walton et al., 2015). These “counterstereotypical” stories
render the intervention distinct from approaches that emphasize role models, in which
in-group members illustrate a path of growth for the self (cf. Lockwood & Kunda, 1997;
Stephens et al., Chapter 5, this volume). Instead, the emphasis is on feelings of nonbelong-
ing that arise from difficulties in the academic transition experienced by all students.
Second, the stories describe trajectories of improvement: How negative experiences and
doubts lessen with time, as students find communities and come to feel at home in the
school. This improvement is characterized by (1) behaviors that support belonging, such
as initiative a student takes to connect with others (e.g., attending office hours) or specific
activities or communities he or she joins (e.g., a student organization); (2) the development
of relationships that support belonging, such as with instructors or peers; and (3) psycho-
logical change that supports belonging. This psychological change includes both new, more
adaptive attributions for specific common challenges (e.g., “I had to remind myself that
making close friends takes time,” Appendix 2.1, B, Story 6; “And I saw that even when
48 I. Education
professors are critical, or their grading harsh, it didn’t mean they looked down on me or
that I didn’t belong. It was just their way of motivating high-achieving [school name] stu-
dents,” Appendix 2.1, B, Story 3) and the broad belief system targeted by the intervention
(“Everybody feels they are different freshman year from everybody else, when really in
at least some ways we are all pretty similar. Since I realized that, my experience has been
almost one-hundred percent positive,” Appendix 2.1, B, Story 1). Thus, the exemplars
model new behaviors, new relationships, and new ways of thinking that support belonging.
Improvement is attributed both to the passage of time and to students’ efforts,
resourcefulness, and resilience. The role of time is significant, as belonging can improve
with time as a person gets used to a new place and is receptive to efforts initiated by
others. Stories should not emphasize fixed qualities of individuals (e.g., “You have to be
outgoing to succeed here”) or atypical help, luck, or specific formal resources that may be
unavailable to or inappropriate for other students. For example, in a story about feeling
intimidated by professors, it would not be helpful to say, “Then I met Professor X and he
made all the difference.” The stories should not depict saviors but the patience, effective
strategies, and persistence all students can use to build belonging (e.g., “I began to take
more initiative in going to office hours and meeting with professors. When I made the
effort, I found that my professors became quite warm and were invested in me and in my
doing well,” Appendix 2.1, B, Story 4).
The timeline for improvement should be represented vaguely (e.g., “With time . . . ”);
otherwise students could worry that they have “missed the boat” if they lack a sense
of belonging at a specific future point in time. It could also be problematic to repre-
sent improvement in a specific distant future—then challenges may seem insurmountable
or not worth waiting out. The stories may also validate challenges or doubts that are
ongoing but represent these as not a fundamental threat to belonging (e.g., “Though I
still have doubts about myself sometimes they’re the kinds of things everybody feels on
occasion,” Appendix 2.1, B, Story 8).
Although the stories emphasize challenges experienced by all students, they may
also acknowledge group-specific challenges (Stephens et al., Chapter 5, this volume). For
instance, interviews with upper-year students in engineering revealed that women some-
times felt excluded from male peer groups. A corresponding intervention story validated
this experience but curtailed its negative implications for belonging (Walton et al., 2015,
Story 7):
“ . . . I remember once in my first term having lunch with some other civil engineers.
They spent 90% of the time talking about hockey, about which I know next to
nothing. I felt like I didn’t belong. It was discouraging. But over time I got to know
my classmates better, individually and as a group. Once I remember talking about
the TV show Monster Machines, which I have to admit I love. We had a great time
sharing stories about the different episodes. Even though I don’t share their love
of hockey, I realized that we do have a lot in common—an interest in how things
work—and that’s why we’re all engineers. . . . ”
DETAILS AND COHERENCE
As parables, the stories use specific details in symbolic or illustrative ways to high-
light both challenges (e.g., conversation about hockey) and improvement (e.g., Monster
The Social‑Belonging Intervention 49
Machines; “I realized that. . . . ”). These details should be ones that diverse participating
students can relate to, not individuating or specific to one person’s experience. Details
and language style should also be appropriate to the context at hand. And taken together,
each story should be internally consistent and coherent, and emphasize a clear theme.
The ordering of stories is intentional. For college students, the first story dispels a form
of “pluralistic ignorance”—the perception that worries about belonging are specific to
oneself (see Prentice & Miller, 1993). Attributed to a member of the group the materials
are most designed to reach, such as a Black student at a predominantly White college, the
story simply describes a student learning the core idea in the intervention: that early wor-
ries and challenges to belonging, which had seemed unique to oneself, are in fact common
for all students (Appendix 2.1, B, Story 1). The story does not describe these challenges
in any detail. It just implies that whatever difficulties a student experiences are likely to
be more common than it might seem.
The next story or two feature counterstereotypical exemplars: students from more
advantaged groups, who might seem least likely to worry about belonging, but who
describe strong, specific experiences of nonbelonging and how these improved with
time (Appendix 2.1, B, Stories 2 and 3). These stories counter the assumption that only
members of disadvantaged groups face significant worries about belonging. Later stories
address other comon themes in a context, such as teacher–student relationships, friend-
ships, imposter syndrome, and common goals (Appendix 2.1, B, Stories 4–9).
For middle school students, the themes are simplified and interwoven (Goyer et al.,
2019). Stories also may not be attributed to a student with a given social identity but pre-
sented alongside an array of images of students representing the diversity of the student
body, thus implying that the stories reflect the experiences of students in that community
as a whole.
Saying Is Believing
After reading the intervention summary and stories, students are asked to describe how
their own experience in the transition so far, and/or what they expect in the future, reflects
the experiences of the students they read about. These materials, students are told, may be
shared with future students to help them better understand what to expect in the transition.
In the original study, students wrote an essay describing their experiences of belonging,
rewrote this essay into a speech, and then delivered this speech to a video camera to cre-
ate footage that could be shared in first-year orientation the next year (Walton & Cohen,
2011). In other studies, students have written a personal letter to a future incoming student
(Walton et al., 2015; Yeager et al., 2016). As noted earlier, saying-is-believing exercises can
promote learning and personalization (see Aronson, 1999; Walton & Wilson, 2018).
Control Conditions
Multiple active control conditions have been tested. These feature the same structure of
activities—summary information, student stories, and saying-is-believing task—but lack
the critical focus on belonging. They have addressed instead how students (1) get used to
50 I. Education
the physical environment of a new school (e.g., generic information about the weather,
campus, and city; Walton & Cohen, 2011; Yeager et al., 2016), (2) develop study skills
(Murphy et al., 2020; Walton et al., 2015), or (3) become used to the school lunchroom
and interested in state politics (Goyer et al., 2019). Some studies have also included no-
treatment control conditions (Walton et al., 2015). Others supplement randomized con-
trol groups with nonrandomized campuswide comparisons (Murphy et al., 2020; Walton
& Cohen, 2011; Yeager et al., 2016).
NUANCES AND MISCONCEPTIONS
Not Mostly about Friendships
One misconception is that the intervention focuses on purely social experiences, such as
close friendships or feelings of homesickness. To the contrary, the emphasis is on experi-
ences of belonging and nonbelonging within the core academic context of school—in
classrooms, study groups, lab settings, and in interactions with classmates or instruc-
tors. The stories thus address worries about ability, about showing work to others, about
being evaluated, and about receiving critical feedback or poor grades. This focus arises
from the fact that it is the intellectual abilities and merit of racial-minority students and
women in math and science that is most directly impugned by negative stereotypes, not
their likability (e.g., Glick & Fiske, 2001). In so doing, the materials seek to convey that
worries about belonging and being valued and respected within the core academic con-
texts of school are normal (experienced by many students) and improve with time.
Acknowledgment of Difference
A third misconception is that the stories ignore group-based differences in students’ expe-
rience. They do not. They simply emphasize everyday worries about belonging that arise
from the difficulty of the transition for all students and how this improves with time.
Some variants also highlight group-specific challenges (e.g., the hockey story described
above). The saying-is-believing exercise also provides participating students an oppor-
tunity to describe challenges they have experienced, including experiences of difference
involving one or more of their social identities.
IMPLICATIONS FOR PRACTICE
There are many ways that institutions can convey adaptive ways of thinking about
belonging and build a culture that supports this. Here we focus on implementation in
school contexts, where most research has been conducted, but similar efforts can and
The Social‑Belonging Intervention 51
have been undertaken in work contexts. We describe ways schools have found to convey
and embody the specific themes of the belonging intervention. These efforts comple-
ment broader efforts to promote belonging and inclusion, such as to promote diversity
among students, staff, and faculty; to represent and value this diversity; to counter bias,
prejudice, and sexual harassment; and to structure communities so people have genuine
opportunities to build relationships of value (see Murphy, Kroeper, & Ozier, 2018).
As with any psychological exercise, no implementation of the belonging intervention
will be effective if the materials are not delivered in an impactful way, such that recipi-
ents pay attention, engage, and connect the material with their lives (see Weiss, Bloom,
& Brock, 2014). Psychological interventions are not “a worksheet to be handed out or
a lesson to ‘get through’ ” (Yeager & Walton, 2011, p. 289) but a tool to engage people
substantively in thinking about themselves or their experience in a new way.
It is also essential to maintain the integrity of the critical message (see Dweck,
2016). Simply handing out school swag or assuring students, “You belong!” does not
help students see that everyday challenges are normal and improve with time. Addi-
tionally, although it may be helpful to hear adaptive messages about belonging mul-
tiple times from multiple sources, the message should always feel authentic, not false or
manipulative.
In‑Person Experiences
Institutions can also implement in-person experiences to help students contend with chal-
lenges to belonging early on. This may involve structured reflections and small-group
discussion or specific assignments in first-year classes, each of which has been shown
to improve core academic outcomes (e.g., Binning et al., 2020; Murphy et al., 2020).
Residential programming offers another opportunity. Stanford created a credit-bearing,
discussion-based course, Frosh 101, to extend lessons introduced during new student
52 I. Education
orientation, including about belonging, into the first year. In a first session, facilitators
(more senior students) share challenges to and growth in belonging they experienced in
coming to college. First-year students then write anonymously about their own experi-
ence, describing why and how it is normal to worry at first about belonging in this transi-
tion and how this gets better with time. These writings are then collected and are read
aloud without identifying their authors. The group then discusses common themes. The
next week, students create mementos that reflect their discussion, such as a poem, a flier,
a painting, digital art, or a sculpture, to be displayed in the dorm.
Organizing such in-person experiences may require greater coordination and be
more costly per student. There can also be challenges to maintaining the fidelity of the
intervention message. Yet they may help shift a school culture.
Customization
Past research testing the social-belonging intervention has incorporated customized con-
tent among standard belonging stories in new contexts, under the assumption that dis-
tinct challenges to belonging arise in specific contexts.
The aim of the customization process is to understand the challenges to belonging
students experience in a particular environment and to identify adaptive and realistic ways
students can understand and respond to these challenges in that context. This process
generally involves user-centered pilot research, including surveys, interviews, and focus
groups with the target population. For women in engineering, it led to the incorporation
of new stories addressing feelings of exclusion from male peer groups (the hockey story
above) and experiences of sexist disrespect on campus (Walton et al., 2015). For gradu-
ates of urban charter schools, pilot research identified a passivity in building belonging
in college among students leaving highly structured urban charter school contexts. Story
revisions reemphasized the active steps students need to take to build relationships with
peers and faculty in college (Yeager et al., 2016). For middle school students, customiza-
tion led to stories addressing worries about getting lost in a larger school, forgetting a
locker combination, how one’s stomach hurt the first few months, and a fear of talking
to teachers (Goyer et al., 2019).
This customization has typically been carried out within the structure of the inter-
vention outlined above, including the first story emphasizing pluralistic ignorance fol-
lowed by counterstereotypical exemplar stories. The Belonging Guide mentioned earlier
discusses why and how to include customized content (Walton et al., 2017). Doing so
may make materials more relevant, authentic, and impactful for a context. However,
the customization and writing process requires significant expertise and should not be
carried out casually. Effective belonging stories are deceptively complex and difficult to
write well. Moreover, basic themes and threats to belonging are often common across
diverse contexts—thus, stories addressing core themes may be largely retained across set-
tings. Customizing some content does not imply that all content should be customized.
PRACTITIONER EXPERIENCES
Appendix 2.2 shares reflections from two practitioners who implemented the social-
belonging intervention at their institution through the College Transition Collaborative
(http://collegetransitioncollaborative.org).
The Social‑Belonging Intervention 53
IMPLICATIONS FOR THEORY
First, the belonging intervention illustrates how social-psychological factors can per-
petuate inequality (see Steele, 1997, 2010; Sherman et al., Chapter 3, this volume). The
intervention does not expand learning opportunities. Nor does it increase students’ basic
capability (e.g., intelligence, self-control) or motivation to learn, factors commonly cited
as causes of inequality (e.g., Herrnstein & Murray, 1994). Instead, it shows that potent
social-psychological processes follow from societal disadvantage in the form of pervasive
worries about belonging and how these reproduce inequality in school success.
Second, for theories of education, the belonging intervention highlights the untapped
capacity for better outcomes present in both many students from socially disadvantaged
backgrounds and many school contexts, yet how this potential can go hidden and unreal-
ized as a consequence of unaddressed feelings of nonbelonging (Walton & Spencer, 2009).
Third, the intervention highlights the causal role of social-psychological processes
for lifespan development. Social psychology is often identified with the “power of the
situation,” which can seem to imply that people simply bend with the wind of every new
context (Ross & Nisbett, 1991). If so, social-psychological processes would not affect
life outcomes. Yet worries about belonging can become embedded in the structure of
people’s lives, forestalling the development of social resources that pay dividends over
the life course.
Fourth, for “wise” interventions, the belonging intervention provides a paradigmatic
example of how a brief psychological exercise at a key point in time can produce recursive
and, thus, lasting change. Here, the intervention, delivered early in college or in middle
school improved major life and school outcomes 7–11 and 7 years later, respectively
(Brady et al., 2020; Goyer et al., 2019). In each case it did so, it seems, by helping students
make sense of and respond to daily experiences more adaptively, improving patterns of
social interaction and helping students develop stronger and more trusting relationships,
reinforcing feelings of belonging (see Walton & Wilson, 2018).
Fifth, for psychology, the intervention highlights the centrality of belonging for both
sustained motivation and achievement (Carr & Walton, 2014; Walton, Cohen, Cwir,
& Spencer, 2012) and functioning generally. Although we have focused on academic
outcomes, the belonging intervention has also been shown to improve health, happiness,
and daily functioning (e.g., higher and more stable self-esteem; Walton & Cohen, 2011;
Walton et al., 2015). Such findings suggest that belonging may serve as a psychological
“hub” for the self, essential to diverse outcomes (see Baumeister & Leary, 1995). Finally,
the intervention illustrates the close relationship between cognitive (attributional) and
affective (belonging) processes, and how an attributional approach can help people make
sense of their experiences in ways that support better affective outcomes (cf. Marigold,
Chapter 17, this volume).
FUTURE DIRECTIONS
opportunities for belonging that students could pursue? We speculate that benefits will be
limited in contexts that are too toxic for students to reasonably belong or where genuine
opportunity for learning is limited. Such research will inform and potentially integrate
psychological and sociological theories of societal inequality.
As we noted, the belonging intervention can also improve health (Walton & Cohen,
2011). Where, for whom, and how do health benefits arise? Future research should
include specific health-relevant physiological objective indicators, including measures of
both acute stress responses (e.g., cortisol) and functioning over time (e.g., the conserved
transcriptional response to adversity). Health and physiological outcomes may or may
not track with psychological and academic outcomes (cf. Miller, Cohen, Janicki-Deverts,
Brody, & Chen, 2016)—or with each other (see Destin, 2018).
Another important question involves the best way to think about customization and
new contexts, both in school contexts and elsewhere. We do not yet understand as well
as we need to the contexts in which more or less standard belonging materials will be
effective, the contexts in which customization is helpful or necessary, and how to deter-
mine this. In many cases, it may be most effective to design high-quality and engaging but
standard materials that can be presented to many people in a given kind of context (e.g.,
the transition to college), and to which recipients can flexibly respond by describing their
own experiences (through saying-is-believing prompts or group discussions; à la Walton
& Cohen, 2011; Walton et al., 2015; Yeager et al., 2016). In other cases, standard mate-
rials may be modified or even replaced by a bottom-up process in which participating
students begin by articulating, sharing, and discussing in safe ways their own challenges
to and growth in belonging (see Binning et al., 2020).
Finally, even as most research on the belonging intervention to date has focused on
school contexts, people also strive to belong in other achievement-related settings, such
as work contexts, where doubts about belonging may also undermine outcomes (e.g., Fas-
siotto et al., 2016). Future research should examine the role of belonging uncertainty and
belonging interventions in such settings and how the intervention can improve outcomes
and reduce inequalities there.
REFERENCES
Broda, M., Yun, J., Schneider, B., Yeager, D. S., Walton, G. M., & Diemer, M. (2018). Reducing
inequality in academic success for incoming college students: A randomized trial of growth
mindset and belonging interventions. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 11,
317–338.
Carr, P. B., & Walton, G. M. (2014). Cues of working together fuel intrinsic motivation. Journal
of Experimental Social Psychology, 53, 169–184.
Cook, J. E., Purdie-Vaughns, V., Garcia, J., & Cohen, G. L. (2012). Chronic threat and contingent
belonging: Protective benefits of values affirmation on identity development. Journal of Per-
sonality and Social Psychology, 102, 479–496.
Darley, J. M., & Gross, P. H. (1983). A hypothesis-confirming bias in labeling effects. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 20–33.
Destin, M. (2018). Socioeconomic mobility, identity, and health: Experiences that influence immu-
nology and implications for intervention. American Psychologist, 74(2), 207–217.
Devers, C., Daugherty, D., Steenbergh, T., Runyan, J., Oke, L., & Alayan, A. (2016). Enhanc-
ing student success: Disseminating a growth-mindset and social-belonging intervention with
smartphones. In Proceedings of EdMedia 2016—World Conference on Educational Media
and Technology (pp. 1674–1677). Vancouver, BC, Canada: Association for the Advancement
of Computing in Education (AACE). Retrieved August 28, 2018, from www.learntechlib.
org/primary/p/173171.
Dweck, C. (2016, January 11). Recognizing and overcoming false growth mindset. Retrieved from
www.edutopia.org/blog/recognizingovercoming-false-growth-mindset-c arol-dweck.
Eskreis-Winkler, L., Fishbach, A., & Duckworth, A. L. (2018). Dear Abby: Should I give advice or
receive it? Psychological Science, 29(11), 1797–1806.
Fassiotto, M., Hamel, E. O., Ku, M., Correll, S., Grewal, D., Lavori, P., . . . Valantine, H. (2016).
Women in academic medicine: Measuring stereotype threat among junior faculty. Journal of
Women’s Health, 25, 292–298.
Garcia, J., & Cohen, G. L. (2013). A social psychological perspective on educational intervention.
In E. Shafir (Ed.), Behavioral foundations of policy (pp. 329–350). New York: Russell Sage.
Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (2001). An ambivalent alliance: Hostile and benevolent sexism as comple-
mentary justifications for gender inequality. American Psychologist, 56, 109–118.
Goffman, E. (1963). Stigma: Notes on the management of spoiled identity. New York: Prentice
Hall.
Goyer, J. P., Cohen, G. L., Cook, J. E., Master, A., Apfel, N., Lee, W., . . . Walton, G. M. (2019).
Targeted identity-safety interventions cause lasting reductions in discipline citations among
negatively stereotyped boys. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 172(1), 229–259.
Herrnstein, R. J., & Murray, C. (1994). The bell curve: Intelligence and class structure in Ameri-
can life. New York: Free Press.
Leitner, J. B., Ayduk, O., Boykin, C. M., & Mendoza-Denton, R. (2018). Reducing negative affect
and increasing rapport improve interracial mentorship outcomes. PLOS ONE, 13, e0194123.
Lockwood, P., & Kunda, Z. (1997). Superstars and me: Predicting the impact of role models on
the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(1), 91–103.
Ludden, J., & Weeks, L. (2009, May 26). Sotomayor: “Always looking over my shoulder.” Wash-
ington, DC: National Public Radio. Retrieved from www.npr.org/templates/story/story.
php?storyId=104538436.
Mallett, R. K., Mello, Z. R., Wagner, D. E., Worrell, F., Burrow, R. N., & Andretta, J. R. (2011).
Do I belong?: It depends on when you ask. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychol-
ogy, 17(4), 432–436.
Marksteiner, T., Janke, S., & Dickhäuser, O. (2019). Effects of a brief psychological intervention
on students’ sense of belonging and educational outcomes: The role of students’ migration
and educational background. Journal of School Psychology, 75, 41–57.
Mendoza-Denton, R., Downey, G., Purdie, V. J., Davis, A., & Pietrzak, J. (2002). Sensitivity to
56 I. Education
status-based rejection: Implications for African American students’ college experience. Jour-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(4), 896–918.
Miller, G. E., Cohen, S., Janicki-Deverts, D., Brody, G. H., & Chen, E. (2016). Viral challenge
reveals further evidence of skin-deep resilience in African Americans from disadvantaged
backgrounds. Health Psychology, 35(11), 1225–1234.
Murphy, M. C., Gopalan, M., Carter, E. R., Emerson, T. U., Bottoms, B. L., & Walton, G. M.
(2020). A customized belonging intervention improves retention of socially disadvantaged
students at a broad-access university. Science Advances, 6, eaba4677.
Murphy, M. C., Kroeper, K. M., & Ozier, E. M. (2018). Prejudiced places: How contexts shape
inequality and how policy can change them. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain
Sciences, 5, 66–74.
Obama, M. (2014). Remarks by the president and first lady at College Opportunity Summit.
Retrieved January 5, 2017, from www.whitehouse.gov/the-pressoffice/2014/01/16/remarks-
president-and-first-lady-college-opportunity-summit.
Paunesku, D., Walton, G. M., Romero, C. L., Smith, E. N., Yeager, D. S., & Dweck, C. S. (2015).
Mindset interventions are a scalable treatment for academic underperformance. Psychologi-
cal Science, 26, 784–793.
Perry, R. P., & Hamm, J. M. (2017). An attribution perspective on competence and motivation:
Theory and treatment interventions. In A. Elliot, C. Dweck, & D. Yeager (Eds.), Handbook
of competence and motivation: Theory and applications (2nd ed., pp. 61–84). New York:
Guilford Press.
Prentice, D. A., & Miller, D. T. (1993). Pluralistic ignorance and alcohol use on campus: Some
consequences of misperceiving the social norm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 64(2), 243–256.
Robinson, M. L. (1985). Princeton-educated Blacks and the Black community (Undergraduate
thesis), Princeton University, Princeton, NJ. Retrieved from www.politico.com/pdf/080222_
MOPrincetonThesis_1-251.pdf.
Ross, L., & Nisbett, R. E. (1991). The person and the situation: Perspectives of social psychology.
New York: McGraw-H ill.
Schmader, T., Johns, M., & Forbes, C. (2008). An integrated process model of stereotype threat
effects on performance. Psychological Review, 115(2), 336–356.
Sherman, D. K., Hartson, K. A., Binning, K. R., Purdie-Vaughns, V., Garcia, J., Taborsky-Barba,
S., . . . Cohen, G. L. (2013). Deflecting the trajectory and changing the narrative: How self-
affirmation affects academic performance and motivation under identity threat. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 104(4), 591–618.
Spencer, S. J., Steele, C. M., & Quinn, D. M. (1999). Stereotype threat and women’s math perfor-
mance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35(1), 4–28.
Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and perfor-
mance. American Psychologist, 52, 613–629.
Steele, C. M. (2010). Whistling Vivaldi: How stereotypes affect us and what we can do. New York:
Norton.
Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of
African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(5), 797–811.
Steele, C. M., Spencer, S. J., & Aronson, J. (2002). Contending with group image: The psychol-
ogy of stereotype and social identity threat. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental
social psychology, Vol. 34 (p. 379–440). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Terman, L. M. (1916). The measurement of intelligence. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Walton, G. M., & Brady, S. T. (2017). The many questions of belonging. In A. Elliot, C. Dweck,
& D. Yeager (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation: Theory and application (2nd
ed., pp. 272–293). New York: Guilford Press.
The Social‑Belonging Intervention 57
Walton, G. M., & Cohen, G. L. (2007). A question of belonging: Race, social fit, and achievement.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 82–96.
Walton, G. M., & Cohen, G. L. (2011). A brief social-belonging intervention improves academic
and health outcomes of minority students. Science, 331, 1447–1451.
Walton, G. M., Cohen, G. L., Cwir, D., & Spencer, S. J. (2012). Mere belonging: The power of
social connections. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 513–532.
Walton, G. M., Logel, C., Peach, J., Spencer, S., & Zanna, M. P. (2015). Two brief interventions
to mitigate a “chilly climate” transform women’s experience, relationships, and achievement
in engineering. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107, 468–485.
Walton, G. M., Murphy, M. C., Logel, C., Yeager, D. S., & the College Transition Collaborative.
(2017). The social-belonging intervention: A guide for use and customization. Retrieved from
http://collegetransitioncollaborative.org/content/2017/belonging-custom-guide.
Walton, G. M., & Spencer, S. J. (2009). Latent ability: Grades and test scores systematically under-
estimate the intellectual ability of negatively stereotyped students. Psychological Science, 20,
1132–1139.
Walton, G. M., & Wilson, T. D. (2018). Wise interventions: Psychological remedies for social and
personal problems. Psychological Review, 125, 617–655.
Walton, G. M., & Yeager, D. S. (2020). Seed and soil: Psychological affordances in contexts help
to explain where wise interventions succeed or fail. Current Directions in Psychological Sci-
ence, 29, 219–226.
Weiss, M. J., Bloom, H., & Brock, T. (2014). A conceptual framework for studying the sources
of variation in program effects. Journal of Policy Analysis and Management, 33, 778–808.
Williams, C. L., Hirschi, Q., Sublett, K. V., Hulleman, C. S., & Wilson, T. D. (2020, March 2). A
brief social belonging intervention improves academic outcomes for minoritized high school
students. Motivation Science. [Epub ahead of print]
Wilson, T. D., Damiani, M., & Shelton, N. (2002). Improving the academic performance of col-
lege students with brief attributional interventions. In J. Aronson (Ed.), Improving academic
achievement: Impact of psychological factors on education (pp. 88–108). San Diego, CA:
Academic Press.
Yeager, D. S., Purdie-Vaughns, V., Garcia, J., Apfel, N., Brzustoski, P., Master, A., . . . Cohen,
G. L. (2014). Breaking the cycle of mistrust: Wise interventions to provide critical feedback
across the racial divide. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 804–824.
Yeager, D. S., & Walton, G. M. (2011). Social-psychological interventions in education: They’re
not magic. Review of Educational Research, 81, 267–301.
Yeager, D. S., Walton, G. M., Brady, S. T., Akcinar, E. N., Paunesku, D., Keane, L., . . . Dweck, C.
S. (2016). Teaching a lay theory before college narrows achievement gaps at scale. Proceed-
ings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 113, E3341–E3348.
58 I. EDUCATION
(A) Prelude and summary. (B) Annotated student stories. (C) Saying-is-believing essay prompt.
See Walton, Murphy, Logel, Yeager, and the College Transition Collaborative (2017) for experi-
menter script and more examples of materials.
(continued)
The Social‑Belonging Intervention 59
(continued)
60 I. EDUCATION
The Social‑Belonging Intervention 61
can succeed under the right conditions. A key tenet of the CTC approach is that student
success is a process and that academic struggle is a normal part of college life. This idea
has influenced the development of other student success initiatives beyond the intervention.
For example, our university promotes a campuswide Fail Forward Week in which students,
staff, and faculty are encouraged to share their failures publicly. The hope is that students
will see their academic struggles as typical and will seek the help they need.
Lewis & Clark was the first liberal arts college to implement the CTC’s belonging inter-
vention. When the CTC team approached our college with this opportunity, a focus on
students’ sense of “belonging” was not yet part of the institutional fabric. Yes, we were
eager to understand better what kinds of students succeed at Lewis & Clark College, and
yes, we were attentive to the importance of growing the diversity of our student body. Yet at
that time we hadn’t considered the role we needed to play in discussing and normalizing the
challenges all undergraduates face in order to help our students not only persist but thrive.
Presenting the CTC’s plan to the community required bringing together a large number
of stakeholders, ranging from the dean’s office to student life staff to faculty to the advising
office and more. Initially, there was support for the basic idea, but a great deal of skepticism
about using an experimental design for the intervention. “If something like this seems prom-
ising, why not provide it to all students right away?” What was harder to understand was
the fact that this intervention had never been tested at a primarily undergraduate institution,
and even if it had been, our students may be different from those at other schools. We needed
to tailor the intervention to our own students and assess its effectiveness before putting the
resources into providing it to everyone. These early conversations not only helped members
of our institution to understand the wisdom of assessment prior to widespread implementa-
tion but also to spread the concept of belongingness throughout our campus community.
Early data demonstrated that our entering first-year students were being affected by the
belonging intervention. As one student described, “Through reading about other students’
experiences of coming to Lewis & Clark and becoming more comfortable over time, I have
recognized that all students are experiencing the same stresses that I am now. There is no
easy way to get around the discomfort of starting all over. The best thing to do is embrace
the opportunities Lewis & Clark offers and be outgoing. By doing this, I will find myself at
home in no time.” As data collection progressed and results for the tailored belongingness
intervention continued to be promising, Lewis & Clark was able to make an informed choice
to implement the intervention for all entering students.
Although there is somewhat of a chicken- and-the-egg problem here, campuswide
changes began to be put in place on the heels of the CTC’s intervention. For example, a new,
required first-year experience called the “Pioneer Success Institute” was initiated the year
after the CTC intervention began. At the same time, faculty began to focus more explicitly
on inclusive pedagogy and the ways students’ struggles in those first few semesters could be
normalized in order to make their classrooms more inviting places to be. And perhaps most
striking, our institution recently unveiled a new strategic plan, where one of the six key goals
echoes the CTC’s intervention. As Lewis & Clark’s president, Wim Wiewel, said in his inau-
gural address (October 5, 2018), “The overarching goal is to create an institutional culture
of belonging, where all community members can fully participate.”
C H A P TE R 3
Self‑Affirmation Interventions
BACKGROUND
The motive to maintain a positive sense of self pervades social life. Dismissing evidence
that one is engaging in risky behavior, reacting defensively to good advice, feeling vigilant
and stressed in situations where one feels judged, and avoiding domains where one per-
ceives oneself to be failing seem like discrete phenomena. But they are similar at a psycho-
logical level. They all represent the mind’s attempt to deal with threats to the self. While
the different responses to threats in different arenas protect feelings of personal worth in
the short term, they can prove counterproductive in the long run. The attempts people
63
64 I. Education
make to protect themselves from threats to self-integrity are understandable and in some
cases even adaptive. Given the pervasiveness of threats to self in the various arenas of life,
people would lose confidence and grit if their sense of self were constantly under assault.
But over time self-protection can have costly effects in many domains, including health,
education, relationships, dispute resolution, and career success.
For over three decades, researchers and practitioners have applied self-affirmation
theory to understand and change behavior in a wide range of domains. The theory begins
with the premise that people are motivated to maintain an image of themselves as “mor-
ally and adaptively adequate,” as good people who are able to control important outcomes
in their lives (Steele, 1988). Claude Steele, the creator of self-affirmation theory, referred
to this need as a drive for “self-integrity.” Applied to social problems, self-affirmation
theory provides a lens for understanding why so many attempts at social and personal
change fail. It is because they can inadvertently threaten self-integrity, evoking psycho-
logical mechanisms that can shield self-integrity and also impede growth.
Self-affirmation interventions are situational opportunities, sometimes brief, for
people to affirm their global self-integrity. Most often they take the form of an oppor-
tunity for people to show their fidelity to a cherished value they hold. Because values
are central to people’s sense of self-integrity, expressing one’s fidelity to them is a simple
and effective way to affirm self-integrity. Self-affirmation interventions have received the
most attention in the domain of education. Indeed, affirmations have been applied in
schools around the world. The United States, Germany, and England are three countries
where thousands of students and dozens of public schools have participated—or are cur-
rently participating—in large field studies evaluating the impact of self-affirmation.
As Kurt Lewin (1951), the father of social psychology, stated, “There is nothing so
practical as a good theory.” Following his lead, we believe that the most useful interven-
tion of all is the theoretical wisdom that follows from self-affirmation theory. Armed
with this theory, educators and other practitioners can craft situations that meet the core
needs of the people they serve. For this reason, we first review self-affirmation theory.
Then we describe the successes and limitations of one of the most popular interventions
derived from this theory—brief writing activities that encourage people to identify and
reflect on their cherished values.
PSYCHOLOGICAL PROCESSES
Self‑Integrity Maintenance
The theoretical basis of self-affirmation rests on the insight that people are motivated to
maintain a global sense of personal adequacy. They strive to be morally and adaptively
adequate (Steele, 1988; Cohen & Sherman, 2014). How do people maintain self-integrity
in a world where it is continually under threat (Steele, 1988)?1 When Steele first proposed
self-affirmation theory, the prevailing notion in social psychology was that people were
motivated to directly neutralize threats to the self. A smoker might defensively dismiss the
dangers of smoking. An employee might attribute a bad job outcome to an unfair boss. A
1 Although we briefly highlight the key theoretical and conceptual points related to self-affirmation the-
ory, we recommend that any practitioner seeking to implement an affirmation intervention read a trio
of review papers to understand the intellectual history and empirical development of self-affirmation
theory (Steele, 1988; Sherman & Cohen, 2006; Cohen & Sherman, 2014).
Self‑Affirmation Interventions 65
teacher might attribute the underperformance of students to their laziness or lack of abil-
ity. Of course, people might also protect their self-integrity not only through cognitive
distortions but through behavioral change. The smoker quits. The employee or teacher
accepts a measure of personal responsibility and takes a more proactive role in improving
the situation. Yet people routinely engage in defensive distortions and biased judgments
in response to threats to the self. The “psychological immune system” gets activated and
shields people (Gilbert, Pinel, Wilson, Blumberg, & Wheatley, 1998). Motivated reason-
ing (Kunda, 1987) can lead people to feel good about themselves but to deny important
information and feedback that could lead to better outcomes in their lives. Yet, self-
affirmation theory suggests that denial and defense are not inevitable outcomes of threat.
People have flexibility in how they respond to threats to the self. They can do so
indirectly, and, when they do, this can provide them with the self-protection they need
to accept and act on threatening experiences. The cardinal motive of the self-system,
according to self-affirmation theory, is global self-integrity. If people feel reassured that,
on the whole, they are good, moral people, then they are better able to cope with threat-
ening situations without resorting to defensive justifications and other cognitive distor-
tions that protect self-integrity at the expense of learning. This process likely starts early
in life; infants can be consoled by touching and cuddling even when these do little to
remedy the source of the distress. Later in life, people are consoled by prayer, religion,
and social support—everyday “interventions” that reassure people they are “okay” even
while failing to resolve the provoking threat (Steele & Liu, 1983; Steele, 1988).
According to self-affirmation theory, people have a range of creative solutions to the
problem of sustaining self-integrity in any situation. A student who feels insecure about
her ability might act out in class in an effort to win approval from peers and thus reaffirm
self-integrity. The employee who feels alienated at work might decorate his desk with pic-
tures of family and friends. People can create cognitive worlds, tailor-made definitions of
success, that put their own qualities in a positive light. As research on the above-average
effect shows, most people on average see themselves as “above average” on a range of
desirable traits (Dunning, Meyerowitz, & Holzberg, 1989). They are able to do so, in
large measure, because they define what it means to be a good “leader,” “student,” or
“scientist” in a way that emphasizes their own idiosyncratic strengths and downplays
their weakness (Dunning, 2003; Dunning & Cohen, 1992), a tendency that is amplified
when their self-integrity comes under threat (Dunning, Leuenberger, & Sherman, 1995).
The major insight of self-affirmation theory is that people do not simply accept nega-
tive identities and stereotypes imposed on them in a situation but instead creatively find
ways to convey, in effect, “Even though it may not seem so in this situation, I am a person
of integrity.” The key practical insight of self-affirmation theory is this: Practitioners
should think about the raw materials they can introduce into everyday situations at every
level—face-to-face encounters, relationships, and institutions—that help people to main-
tain a sense of self-integrity in constructive ways. To the extent that people have a range
of possibilities for protecting the self in a situation, they will have less need to defensively
distort or deny threatening experiences from which they could otherwise learn.
Threat is not intrinsically a bad thing. It is the mind’s alert signal that there is a
threat in the situation. Indeed, sometimes, as noted earlier, threat can motivate positive
behavioral change (see Cohen & Sherman, 2014; Ehret, LaBrie, Santerre, & Sherman,
2015; Walton & Wilson, 2018). For example, when people are made to feel badly for
failing to live up to their own professed values, such as practicing healthy behavior, they
may subsequently seize an opportunity to redeem themselves—for instance, by making
66 I. Education
healthful choices (Stone & Focella, 2011). How people respond to threat, and whether
their response is adaptive or nonadaptive, depends on many factors but perhaps most of
all on the opportunities for course correction and self-affirmation available in the social
environment.
Self-affirmation theory offered a challenge to cognitive dissonance theory (Festinger,
1957) and its theoretical elaborations that posited self-consistency as a primary motive
(Aronson, 1969). The earliest research in self-affirmation theory showed that people
could absorb a psychological inconsistency, even when it implicated a valued self-concept,
when their self-integrity was bolstered in unrelated domains. For example, when people
affirmed their self-integrity by reflecting on values that were important to them, they no
longer rationalized their actions—for instance, by changing their attitudes to bring them
in line with regrettable behavior that they had subtly been pressured to engage in (Steele,
1988). This occurred even when the values were unrelated to the threatening action. A
person who asserts a love for art, for instance, might no longer need to rationalize smok-
ing behavior.
Research in self-affirmation theory went on to assimilate many of the findings that
had previously been ascribed to basic motives for self-consistency (Aronson, Cohen, &
Nail, 1999). One of the most heavily researched phenomena in cognitive dissonance was
the tendency to resist persuasive information contrary to long-held beliefs. In the health
domain, people often dismiss or rationalize away evidence that they are engaging in
behavior that puts their health at risk. For example, women who drank coffee were much
more critical of an article linking caffeine use with negative health outcomes than women
who did not drink coffee (Liberman & Chaiken, 1992). Such defensive processing or
“motivated reasoning” has long been a topic of study in psychology (Ditto & Lopez,
1992; Kunda, 1987) but the underlying motivation for it was unclear. Self-affirmation
theory suggested it arose from the threat such information poses to global self-integrity.
Thus, in one study, people were more open to threatening health information about their
unsafe sex practices when they had the opportunity to reflect on important values they
held in a different domain (Sherman, Nelson, & Steele, 2000). The opportunity to reflect
on the value of kindness or personal relationships, for example, led sexually active stu-
dents to acknowledge the risks of unsafe sex after viewing an acquired immunodeficiency
syndrome (AIDS) educational video, coffee drinkers to be more open to information link-
ing excessive caffeine intake to health risks (Sherman et al., 2000), and female alcohol
consumers to be more open to evidence linking alcohol consumption to breast cancer
(Harris & Napper, 2005). In the years since these original studies, there have been several
meta-analyses (e.g., Epton, Harris, Kane, van Koningsbruggen, & Sheeran, 2015; Ferrer
& Cohen, 2019) and narrative reviews of self-affirmation in the health domain (Ehret &
Sherman, 2014; Cohen & Sherman, 2014), as well as extensions into other domains, such
as intergroup conflict (Sherman, Brookfield, & Ortosky, 2017). Together the research
illustrates the impact of self-affirmation on increasing openness to threatening informa-
tion and promoting positive behavioral change in a wide range of life arenas.
Stress Reduction
The experience of self-threat is like a psychological alarm. It can be triggered by any
number of events. This includes exposure to counterattitudinal evidence, negotiations
with a political adversary, a mistake, an insult, and so on. There is, in other words, some-
thing vital at stake in many seemingly mundane social situations: one’s self (Goffman,
Self‑Affirmation Interventions 67
1978). Among the most important emotional symptoms of self-threat is stress. The stress
response is an adaptive mechanism designed to mobilize the body’s resources for an
environmental challenge (Sapolsky, 2004). However, when too severe or too chronic,
stress can impair performance and well-being. Research on self-affirmation demonstrates
that debilitating levels of stress can be forestalled by timely activities that reaffirm self-
integrity. For example, when college undergraduates were given self-affirmation activi-
ties to complete during winter break of the stressful first year of college, they reported
fewer visits to the health center (Keough & Markus, 1999). When people had the chance
to reflect on important values before having to give a stressful talk in front of a judg-
mental audience, they showed less of a spike in the stress hormone cortisol (Creswell et
al., 2005), and for those suffering from high levels of chronic stress, performed better
under the pressure (Creswell, Dutcher, Klein, Harris, & Levine, 2013). In a field study,
college undergraduates who engaged in a self-affirmation exercise the week before their
most stress-inducing midterm examination exhibited a less steep rise in urinary catechol-
amines, another biological marker of the stress response (Sherman, Bunyan, Creswell, &
Jaremka, 2009).
Defense of Social Identities
The research described so far shows the ubiquity of self-threat and the self-affirmation
process in social life. Another extension of self-affirmation research is to show how the
motive to protect the integrity of the self is directly tied to the motive to protect our social
or group identities. Affirming or threatening the self affects the way people judge and
treat groups, including their own. After completing a self-affirmation activity, people
were less likely to recoup lost self-esteem by stereotyping outgroups as inferior (Fein
& Spencer, 1997; Badea, Binning, Verlhiac, & Sherman, 2017). They were more chari-
table when explaining why their sports team won or lost (Sherman & Kim, 2005). They
were more willing to acknowledge “hard truths” about the injustices perpetrated by their
country against minorities and competing nations (Adams, Tormala, & O’Brien, 2006;
ehaji-Clancy, Effron, Halperin, Liberman, & Ross, 2011; see Badea & Sherman, 2019,
for review). They were less likely to denigrate the “other side” in political debates as
biased (Binning, Sherman, Cohen, & Heitland, 2010; Cohen, Aronson, & Steele, 2000;
see also Binning, Brick, Cohen, & Sherman, 2015; Cohen et al., 2007).
Self-affirmation works not by giving people something that they lack but by allowing
people to manifest what they already have—what they stand for, the psychological com-
mitments that ground their sense of self-integrity. An opportunity to assert one’s impor-
tant values, and to explain why they are important, is an opportunity to express com-
mitments that have been a lifetime in the making. A self-affirmation, in this sense, is
a situational channel that facilitates the link between inner assets and their outward
expression. (We describe implementation of the intervention in a section below and pro-
vide annotated materials in Appendixes 3.1 and 3.2, respectively.) A critical point is that
people cannot always go it alone in the self-affirmation process. They are constrained or
supported by the situation they are in. Thus, in virtually all of the studies described so
far, the self-affirmation takes the form of a question, introduced by the experimenters,
68 I. Education
that prompts people to reflect on their values and why they are important to them at a
moment of threat. Without the question, people may be more constrained in how they
can affirm the self, though people can be taught to self-affirm (Brady et al., 2016; Wal-
ton, Logel, Peach, Spencer, & Zanna, 2015). Naturally, there are individual differences
in how much a person can spontaneously self-affirm (Harris et al., 2019).
Laboratory Outcomes
The range of outcomes along which self- affirmation effects have been documented
include (1) acceptance of threatening information in health and politics (Sherman &
Cohen, 2002); (2) behavioral compliance with threatening information, such as increases
in healthful eating and exercise frequency, and reductions in alcohol consumption among
at-risk adults (Harris & Napper, 2005; Ehret & Sherman, 2018; Falk et al., 2015; see
Epton et al., 2015; Ferrer & Cohen, 2019, for reviews); (3) reductions in stress, such as
stress arising from social evaluation or performance situations (Creswell et al., 2005,
2013); (4) reductions in self-destructive coping behaviors, such as excessive eating (Logel
& Cohen, 2012; Logel, Kathmandu, & Cohen, 2019); (5) increases in intergroup under-
standing, as evidenced by less outgroup denigration (Badea et al., 2017), more compro-
mise in political conflict, and greater willingness to acknowledge wrongdoing on the part
of one’s group (ehaji-Clancy et al., 2011); (6) reductions in biased assimilation of new
evidence, as evidenced by evenhandedness in political partisans’ evaluation of presiden-
tial candidates’ performance in a debate (Binning et al., 2010); (7) acceptance of threaten-
ing changes at the workplace (Jiang, 2018; Wiesenfeld, Brockner, & Martin, 1999); (8)
reduction of ingroup favoritism in negotiation contexts (Sivanathan, Molden, Galinsky,
& Ku, 2008; Ward, Atkins, Lepper, & Ross, 2011); (9) reduction in self-handicapping in
athletic and academic domains (Finez & Sherman, 2012; Siegel, Scillitoe, & Parks-Yancy,
2005); and (10) better performance under stress and social identity threat (Creswell et
al., 2013; Martens, Johns, Greenberg, & Schimel, 2006; Shapiro, Williams, & Hambar-
chyan, 2013). The scope of domains where affirmations have been shown to have ame-
liorative effects suggests that a similar psychological process, to some extent, underlies
them all.
teachers administer affirmation writing exercises (or control exercises) using a random-
ized experimental procedure in which teachers were kept unaware of both students’ con-
dition assignment and the hypotheses motivating the study. The writing exercises were
tailored to be intelligible and engaging for this age group and for the students at this
school. Samples of student responses to the values affirmation prompts in various studies
are provided in Table 3.1 (Sherman et al., 2013; Ehret & Sherman, 2018). In the original
studies, roughly half of the students at the school were of African American descent and
the remainder of European American descent. Academic performance was assessed as
grade point average (GPA) in core courses (English, math, science, and social studies),
obtained through students’ transcripts.
“Athletic ability is my most important value. I love this value so much because sports
are my passion. I love football, baseball, and basketball. For all of these sports you need
athletic ability and be able to stay in shape. My second value is I have a sense of humor.
Humor is a great thing, it makes people laugh everyone has fun and there is nothing wrong
with humor. I love all different kinds of humor and that is why it is one of my values.”
“Creativity is important because I have to draw, sing, and, well, be creative! It’s fun to be
creative because you can see things in other ways as other people wouldn’t. I mean, it’s so
fun to be creative. My relationships with family and friends are EXTREMELY important.
Without them, who would I turn to? Who would make me smile and laugh and act how I
am today? My last value is a sense of humor because I love to laugh or make people laugh
and it just makes everything seem so much more fun.”
“For me it is very important to live in the moment and not take life so serious. Often times
when I try to plan something out things do not go the way I wanted and I am just let down
and bummed out. By living in the moment and enjoying each day as it coms I have become
a much happier person I feel overall. Also I feel that life has a lot of ups and downs and a
lot of what happens can not be controlled, by accepting that I have less worries and find
myself less stress. I feel that this is a very important value to have in order to make you a
more stress free person, which allows you to focus on the more important things in life.”
“Athletics are the reason I go to such an amazing school like (school). Without them, I do
not know how much life would of turned out. They have kept my head on straight and
also caused me to excel in school. They have also helped me grow as a young man because
they have taught me life lessons that would of been hard to learn without sports.”
Note. See Appendix 3.1 for annotated version of standard prompt for middle school students. Material
from Ehret and Sherman (2018). Reprinted with permission from Oxford University Press.
70 I. Education
MECHANISMS
people perceive social experience and, second, positive feedback loops between the self-
system and the social system. 2 Several psychological responses occur as a consequence
of a self-affirmation intervention (Cohen & Sherman, 2014; Sherman & Hartson, 2011).
First, affirmations evoke a more expansive self-conception (Critcher & Dunning, 2015).
Thinking about the value of religion, or the importance of relationships, for example,
helps people to realize that they have many sources of self-regard. From this perspective,
a threatening event seems more surmountable. People can persist in the face of challenge
and resist temptations to which they would otherwise cave because they have a greater
confidence in their ability to cope (Burson, Crocker, & Mischkowski, 2012; Logel &
Cohen, 2012; Schmeichel & Vohs, 2009).
Beyond increasing the psychological salience of self-resources, self-affirmation inter-
ventions also give people a more expansive frame for viewing a specific threat, help-
ing them to “put it in perspective” (Critcher & Dunning, 2015; Sherman et al., 2013;
Wakslak & Trope, 2009). When people experience a threatening situation, they tend to
fixate on it, a state of vigilance in which the threat commands their attention (Cohen &
Garcia, 2008; Kaiser & Major, 2006; Murphy, Steele, & Gross, 2007). In the short term,
this kind of vigilance can be adaptive. However, in the long term, it can consume mental
resources that could otherwise be expended on learning. It can also undermine psycho-
logical and physical health by focusing attention on adversity rather than the “bigger
picture.” Evidence that self-affirmation facilitates a more expansive view of situational
threats is provided by research on the process of “psychological untethering.” Under nor-
mal circumstances, people who face continual threat due to negative stereotypes, such
as African Americans in school, display a tethering between daily adversity on the one
hand and their sense of well-being and belonging on the other. They appear, on average,
to experience a bad grade or negative feedback not as an isolated incident but as a con-
firmation of the stereotype, and their well-being and belonging falter as a consequence.
However, when affirmed, this tethering is reduced and sometimes eliminated, such that
adversity no longer correlates with well-being and belonging (Cook, Purdie-Vaughns,
Garcia, & Cohen, 2012; see also Walton & Cohen, 2011). Against the backdrop of a
broadened self-view, daily assaults to self-worth loom less large.
This untethering mechanism helps to explain how the self-affirmation process prop-
agates through time, and for that matter how other psychological interventions do so as
well (Walton & Cohen, 2011). Although the intervention is objectively brief, its psycho-
logical effects are lived and relived in the encoding of social experience (Sherman et al.,
2013; Cook et al., 2012).
Longitudinal studies have assessed the untethering process as it plays out over time.
Cook and colleagues (2012) found that for African American students in the control
condition, feelings of belonging in school were tightly linked with academic performance.
They felt less belonging in school when they did poorly rather than well, whereas Euro-
pean Americans’ sense of belonging was relatively less conditional on their performance.
But for African American students who completed the affirmation, belonging was more
stable and relatively less tethered to their performance (Cook et al., 2012).
2 This section on mechanisms is not exhaustive, of course, and mechanistic questions can be addressed
at many other levels of analysis beyond what is discussed in this chapter. For example, research has
examined a neural reward-related mechanism underlying self-affirmation effects as evidenced by activa-
tion of the ventral striatum (Dutcher et al., 2016).
72 I. Education
Effects over Time
The field research shows that the self-affirmation process can perpetuate itself over time.
Brief self-affirmations can have effects that persist for days, weeks, months, and years.
How? One way is through recursive processes—that is, processes that recur, because they
feed off their own consequences (Cohen et al., 2009; Cohen & Sherman, 2014; Wilson
& Linville, 1985; Walton & Wilson, 2018). Feeling affirmed, a person performs better.
Performing better, the person feels more affirmed, in a repeating cycle (Cohen & Sher-
man, 2014; Cohen et al., 2009). The opportunity to initiate such a recursive cycle might
be especially great at key transitions, such as entry to middle school or college, when early
outcomes can have disproportionate effects on trajectories (Elder, 1998).
Another way that self-affirmation processes propagate through time is through its
interaction with the social system, which may also be recursive. Self-affirmed, a student
may perform better, and performing better, he or she may be recognized and rewarded
by teachers. The well-documented effects of teacher expectancies (Rosenthal, 1994) may
then carry forward the effects of the affirmation. The student may be given more atten-
tion and the benefit of the doubt, held to a higher standard, and deflected into higher-
expectation academic tracks, all of which may feed back to further affirm the student.
A formal model of this recursive and interactive change processes is presented in
Cohen and Sherman (2014; see also Cohen, Garcia, & Goyer, 2017; Ferrer & Cohen,
2019; Goyer et al., 2017). Figure 3.1 offers a diagrammatic model of a “cycle of adaptive
potential.” This model was developed to understand the process by which an interven-
tion, such as self-affirmation, or any formative experience, can lead to long-term change.
3 Such studies, of course, are correlational, so there is uncertainty in the direction of the causal arrow.
Perhaps adversity causes a decrement in psychological well-being, perhaps the reverse path applies, or
perhaps some third variable explains the link. We suspect the relationships are bidirectional and mul-
tiply determined. On the whole, however, the tethering effect shows that there is an intercorrelation
among variables for those under threat that does not occur for those not under threat and that can be
severed through timely affirmation.
Self‑Affirmation Interventions 73
Self System
g a b h
e Adaptive Outcomes f
c d
Social System
FIGURE 3.1. Cycle of adaptive potential embedded within cultural, legal, and economic con-
texts. Affirmation effects can propagate themselves over time by leading to changes in the envi-
ronment that cycle back and augment changes in the self. A student who is affirmed (path A) may
perform better initially, and that initial performance may then feed back to the self (path B) such
that the student has two sources of affirmation: the original affirming event and the improved
performance at school (and the sense of personal accomplishment and efficacy that would create).
Doing better at school may lead the teacher to notice, give positive feedback or more challenging
material (path C), which then may feed back to the student by leading to further improved per-
formance (path D). The student, performing better and feeling more supported by the teacher or
school as a result of this positive social feedback, may then challenge him- or herself further, opt-
ing for more difficult courses—for example, leading to a change in his or her social system (path
E), which could, in turn, serve as further affirmation that the student belongs within the system
(path F). Finally, the entire cycle of adaptive potential between the student, adaptive outcomes, and
the social system is embedded in a set of cultural, legal, and economic systems that interact with
the other systems (paths G and H, and the four other paths not yet specified).
It is the interaction between psychological processes and social processes, many of which
can be recursive in nature, that drives outcomes through time.
From this perspective, an intervention like self-affirmation is a “trigger,” the effects
of which are “channeled” by the social environment (Goyer et al., 2017; Pawson & Tilly,
1997; Ferrer & Cohen, 2019). Latino American middle school students who completed
a self-affirmation earned higher grades, the short-term effects, but then they were also
placed into more challenging courses over the long term (Goyer et al., 2017). They were
also more likely to be enrolled by their school in an advanced college preparation pro-
gram. Another study found that some of the lasting effect of affirmation on middle
school African Americans’ later outcomes was driven by the fact that it deflected them
from the remedial track in high school (Cohen et al., 2009; Goyer et al., 2017). Without
the institutional channeling process, long-term effects of self-affirmation may not have
been found.
74 I. Education
Heterogeneity
The evidence of positive affirmation effects seems to suggest the value of “scaling up” the
intervention to reach more students. One commentary urged “advancing values affirma-
tion as a scalable strategy for mitigating identity threats and narrowing national achieve-
ment gaps” (Borman, 2017). Indeed, this goal is the objective of the affirmation studies
in Germany, the United Kingdom, and the United States, as mentioned earlier. However,
we think that “scaling up” is not so much a matter of disseminating the same interven-
tion to as many students as possible. Rather it is a matter of targeting the intervention to
those contexts where it is most likely to be effective. As noted, several replication efforts
have turned up null results. As in medical science, interventions should ideally be tar-
geted to contexts where they are most likely to be effective. Indeed, in medical science, it
is increasingly clear that many interventions may have null results overall but still have
benefits for a small subgroup of superresponders (Mukherjee, 2015). How do we achieve
precise targeting? In short, through the identification of moderators, addressed in the
next section. Researchers and practitioners need to identify when, where, and for whom
wise interventions work best—an endeavor where much progress has been made and
still more, we suspect, awaits. The following section structures the review of moderators
around three factors: psychological threat, the presence of resources, and timeliness.
Psychological Threat
Most obviously, the effect of affirmation hinges on whether the target group in question
is under sufficient psychological threat to impede adaptive outcomes (Cohen & Sher-
man, 2014). The first affirmation studies were conducted with African American stu-
dents as the focal group. Virtually all their teachers were European American, creating
an awareness in these students that the stereotype could be used against them (Inzlicht &
Ben-Zeev, 2000; Sekaquaptewa & Thompson, 2003). African Americans in such an envi-
ronment were expected to be under consistent psychological threat due to the negative
stereotypes they contend with (i.e., stereotype threat; Steele, Spencer, & Aronson, 2002).
Thus, it was this group that was expected to benefit from the affirmation. Consistent with
theorizing, African Americans who were affirmed not only earned higher grades but also
exhibited significantly lower racial stereotype activation on a cognitive accessibility task.
Similar effects of affirmation on a negatively stereotyped ethnic group, with no effects
on European American students, were observed in field interventions conducted with
Latino Americans (Sherman et al., 2013). A study by Miyake et al. (2010) found that for
female students in an introductory physics course, self-affirmation improved exam scores
for those who subscribed relatively more to the stereotype that “men are better at science
than women.” In short, one key moderator of affirmation effects is whether students are
under consistent psychological threat. These consistent psychological threats often arise
from the groups to which people belong and the stereotypes that are targeted at them.
Threat may vary not only with students’ characteristics but with features of the
institution. Researchers conducted an affirmation intervention across 11 middle schools
in Madison, Wisconsin (Borman et al., 2018; Borman, Grigg, & Hanselman, 2016).
Using the procedures from the original studies, they found that the African American
and Latino American students showed an improvement in GPA, with no effect on the
European American and Asian participants. Although the overall effect size was smaller
Self‑Affirmation Interventions 75
than that observed in the previous studies, it varied by school. The researchers found that
the improvement in eighth-grade GPA among affirmed minority students was strongest
in schools that had more threatening contexts, with threat defined as the degree to which
minority students were underrepresented and the degree to which they underperformed
relative to their European American peers (see also Hanselman, Bruch, Gamoran, &
Borman, 2014).
Of course, there are limits to the level of threat an affirmation can mitigate. If the
environment is one of severe threat, where at every turn, racism or sexism is confronted,
it seems highly unlikely that affirmation will do much, if anything. Other steps will be
necessary. It is mainly where there is a low-level but consistent threat that is impeding
adaptive outcomes that affirmation is apt to be most effective. One potential benefit of
larger multisite studies is that they enable researchers to identify the ways institutional
settings vary in the degree of psychological threat present in them.
One strategy that practitioners and researchers can use to identify which individuals
are under consistent psychological threat in a given environment is to assess the degree
to which they underperform given their prior levels of achievement and preparation. Is
there an underperformance effect? A telltale sign of underperformance is the extent to
which a group of people performs worse than others even after prior indicators of success
are controlled (Steele, 1997). This suggests that “something else,” presumably something
in the environment of the school, may be suppressing students’ potential. Which group
underperforms may vary by context. In the United Kingdom, for example, low-income
students display a particularly large underperformance effect. Moreover, a recent study
found that low-income students who were in schools where they were mixed with high-
income students showed dramatic benefits in academic performance as a result of self-
affirmation (Hadden, Easterbrook, Nieuwenhuis, Fox, & Dolan, 2019). Another strategy
is to measure psychological threat using validated scales. For example, in Layous et al.
(2017), White men were found to have a relatively low level of belonging in school and
exhibited the greatest benefit of the affirmation.
Resources
Wise interventions can often be seen as the kick-start to a process that unfolds over
time and in a social context (Cohen & Sherman, 2014; Cohen et al., 2017; Goyer et al.,
2017). But for that process to unfold, there must be institutional supports to carry it
forward. For example, a self-affirmed student might perform better and be more will-
ing to seek out opportunities for learning. She might have the confidence to approach
a teacher for help, or to sign up for a more difficult course. What is critical here is the
availability of the social channels for the psychological currents, triggered by the affir-
mation, to keep moving forward. One study (Dee, 2015) found null effects of the affir-
mation intervention overall, but when examining classrooms that were more conducive
to cognitive growth (i.e., the ones that displayed large gains in test scores), affirmation
led to improved performance among minority students. What kinds of social resources
are needed to propagate the effects of self-affirmation and other wise interventions?
More research is needed on this question—we would categorize them into two types.
First, cognitive resources, in terms of objective opportunities for learning and continued
progress; and second, social resources, in the form of social reinforcement and valida-
tion (Cohen et al., 2017).
76 I. Education
Timeliness
A final key moderator of the benefits of affirmation is its timeliness. For maximal ben-
efit it must be timed to the emergence of threat and the availability of environmental
resources for change. In the health domain, Ferrer and Cohen (2019) demonstrate that
the timeliness of affirmation along these two dimensions predicts the degree of its ben-
efit. In school, affirmations should be timed to the emergence of threat, which, if unad-
dressed, might lead to deteriorating outcomes over time. Thus, in the original studies,
affirmations were timed to occur at the beginning of the school year and before exams
and at a key developmental milestone, the transition to middle school. One study found
that even a difference of 2 weeks in the timing of the affirmation—the first week of mid-
dle school versus the third week—had a large impact, with earlier timing yielding greater
benefit on student grades (Cook et al., 2012). Indeed, the effect of timing in this study
equaled the effect of providing the affirmation intervention at all obtained in previous
studies. For practitioners and researchers, what matters is not just “what” intervention is
used, but also and importantly, “where” and “when.”
In summary, the key moderators of any psychological intervention can be distilled
into what we refer to as the “three T’s” (Cohen et al., 2017; Ferrer & Cohen, 2019). It is
targeted at the right person (one experiencing threat). It is tailored to the need (an affir-
mation might be effective at addressing threats to self-integrity, but not a lack of skill).
And finally, the intervention is timely (given at a time and place where threat may hinder
access or use of institutional resources). It is the confluence of these three conditions that
predicts the positive impact of affirmation, as well, we suspect, of many interventions
(Ferrer & Cohen, 2019).
changed their actions to bring them in line with their values. The intellectual debt that
self-affirmation research owes to Rokeach is the notion that values are a powerful source
of self-integrity, and perhaps the basic unit of identity. Even in the harshest of circum-
stances, people can choose what they stand for.
Finally, self-affirmation interventions have an intellectual connection to other inter-
ventions that tap into identity and self- perception. “Foot-in-the-door” interventions
encourage people to take a small initial step on behalf of a cause, and, under some cir-
cumstances, this has been found to increase their willingness to make later larger sacri-
fices (Freedman & Fraser, 1966; see Burger, 1999, for review). These interventions paved
the path for our understanding of how change can persist through time. An initial action
causes deep changes in identity and self-perception, with the resulting changes carrying
the influence forward through time. A similar intellectual debt is owed to Wilson and
Linville’s (1985) classic research on attributional training, and their resurrection of the
notion of “exacerbation cycles” by Storms and McCaul (1976). The notion was that a
small initial change in psychology or behavior could propagate itself by interrupting a
negative feedback loop. The self-affirmation field studies complement and go beyond
these classic studies by extending the temporal window of observation. Rather than sim-
ply stopping the study with the first dependent measure (e.g., the first behavior after an
intervention, or grades in the first term after the intervention), several of the field studies
featured in self-affirmation research assess a chain of events that unfolds over a signifi-
cant portion of the life course (Borman et al., 2018; Cohen & Sherman, 2014; Goyer et
al., 2017; Tibbetts et al., 2016).
The implementation of any intervention should occur after careful pilot testing to deter-
mine the nature of the psychological threat, constraints in the context in which it is to
be administered (such as literacy level), and whether the materials are clear and impact-
ful for the target population. In our experience, this pilot testing period is also a time
to build trust with the teachers of the classrooms where the intervention is to be admin-
istered and with other key personnel within the school (principal, school psychologist).
The self-affirmation intervention that was used in the middle school context in Cohen
et al. (2006, 2009) and Sherman et al. (2013) was deployed after such pilot testing. It
can be found in Appendix 3.1—the materials are annotated to call out important details
and their intent. While there are many inductions of self-affirmation (see McQueen &
Klein, 2006, for review; see Napper, Harris, & Epton, 2009, for an alternative method),
the values writing exercise is the most commonly used manipulation of self-affirmation.
In this activity, participants first read over a list of values and choose the value or values
that are personally most important to them (in the affirmation condition) or values that
are unimportant to them (in the control condition; several different control conditions
have been used). When several affirmations are given over a school year, the content of
the activities is varied in order for them to stay fresh for the students. But each one asks
participants questions that tap into self-defining values. The writing activities typically
take about 10 minutes for students to complete.
There are several aspects to the implementation of the affirmation that, though
sometimes subtle, can make a difference. Here, we draw on the long tradition in social
psychology that emphasizes the importance of experimental manipulations that are
78 I. Education
immersive and impactful (Aronson, Ellsworth, Carlsmith, & Gonzales, 1990; Ross, Lep-
per, & Ward, 2010). One of the distinctive qualities of social psychology is the attention
to detail that goes into the creation and implementation of any manipulation, including
an intervention. What is critical—and what we are trying to create and duplicate—is not
a physical experience of writing about a value but a psychological experience of feeling
affirmed.
For one, the affirmation activities are described as regular classroom activities and
are never presented as an intervention to help students (see Yeager & Walton, 2011). No
student is made to feel that he or she is in need of help, a message that may increase psy-
chological threat. Indeed, research suggests that when people are made aware that the
affirmation is designed to help them, its impact is attenuated (Sherman, Cohen, et al.,
2009). This is not to say that people cannot use affirmation as a personal coping strategy
if they are aware of its benefits. As long as they feel they have choice in the decision to use
it, they still benefit even when aware of its salutary effects (Silverman, Logel, & Cohen,
2013). Indeed, the lessons of self-affirmation theory can be imparted to students with
positive results (Walton et al., 2015).
There are other implementation factors that are important. For example, we suspect
that some of the efficacy of the intervention comes from the fact that it appears to be an
activity that is from teachers or other institutional authorities. Students are thus led to
feel that teachers, or other institutional authorities, care about their values and how they
are important to them (cf. Bowen, Wegmann, & Webber, 2013). From the students’ point
of view, their values, and who they are and what they stand for, are welcome (see Walton,
Paunesku, & Dweck, 2012). This may help to explain why values affirmations bolster
students’ sense of belonging in school (Cook et al., 2012). Although speculative, one rea-
son replications may sometimes fail is that they are presented by outsiders rather than by
insiders or institutional representatives whose respect students care about (see Protzko &
Aronson, 2016). The key condition here is not at the literal or procedural level. It is at the
psychological level. Sometimes, it may not be possible to present the research activity as
a regular classroom activity (we describe one such instance below). What is important is
that students perceive that their values are respected by people who matter.
Another key detail related to an intervention’s implementation is the degree to which
it promotes student engagement. In the study in Wisconsin middle schools that featured
over 1,000 students, researchers coded the affirmation essays for student engagement,
which they operationalized as whether students selected an important value and then
discussed its personal importance (Borman et al., 2018). The vast majority (76%) of
the potentially threatened students in the affirmation condition were coded as having
engaged with the activity, and this group of students showed relatively larger gains. These
findings suggest that whether the implementation encourages student engagement, such
as by having the teachers convey its importance or by providing a quiet time for students
to complete it, is critical.
Implementation details may explain sometimes paradoxical results, as when the
affirmation works once but not a second time. When Borman and colleagues (Hansel-
man et al., 2017) sought to replicate their affirmation effects in the same 11 schools, they
found null effects. The authors consider a number of sources for this change, many of
them relevant to implementation. The teachers may have been less excited about partici-
pating in the study the second time around; the fact that they were on strike may have
played a role (see Borman, 2017, for discussion).
Self‑Affirmation Interventions 79
NUANCES AND MISCONCEPTIONS
We now review a case study that illuminates how many of the considerations laid out
above make a practical difference in the field. Whenever researchers and practitioners
apply a psychological theory to a novel context, they need to be aware of how the process
they seek to intervene on plays out in the specific setting they hope to change. The para-
graphs that follow address how two researchers, Mohini Lokhande and Tim Müller, took
on this challenge in the German educational system.
80 I. Education
Various steps were taken in order to (1) simplify the intervention materials so that
they would be understood by students with lower literacy skills; (2) make the affirma-
tion task more engaging so that students would put sufficient effort into thinking about
important values in their lives, such as embedding the value activity in an interactive
comic strip; (3) make the activity more concrete with specific references not to abstract
values but to specific activities (e.g., “spending time with family and friends” instead of
“valuing family and friends”); (4) include values that resonate in Germany; (5) break down
the essay-writing task into simple steps to make the writing process easier; (6) encour-
age students to focus on intrinsic values like feelings of belonging rather than extrinsic
ones like prestige; and (7) trigger a positive recursive cycle, while accommodating to data
protection regulations, by having the researchers provide growth-oriented feedback to
students after they completed the initial intervention.
It was necessary to consider who, in this context, was vulnerable to psychological
threat. Previous interventions featuring affirmation had been conducted in the United
States and were designed to alleviate the stereotype threat that racial minorities feel in
school (Cook et al., 2012; Walton & Cohen, 2007). By contrast, in contemporary Europe,
the primary stigma centers on immigration. Immigrant students may feel as though they
do not even belong in their country, let alone in the classroom, a state of “belonging uncer-
tainty” that has befallen immigrants worldwide (see Walton & Cohen, 2007). Moreover,
there exists a fairly pronounced achievement gap between immigrant students and their
native peers in Germany (Gebhardt, Rauch, Mang, Sälzer, & Stanat, 2012). The Expert
Council of German Foundations on Integration and Migration, a nonpartisan advisory
council on whose behalf the study was conducted, sought to address this gap. The purpose
of the study was to generate concrete lessons for future educational reforms in Germany.
As the self-affirmation intervention was translated into this new context, there were
several implementation details to consider. Attempts were made to retain many of the pro-
cedures used in the original interventions, but adjustments had to be made. Knowing the
key theoretical considerations, however, made adjustments possible without undermining
psychological impact. See Lokhande and Müller (2019) and Müller and Lokhande (2017)
for a detailed description of the procedure and results.
The study took place in lower secondary schools (age range 12–13 years) in the state
of Berlin. These schools are usually attended by students with lower academic ability and
from socially disadvantaged families. Moreover, most schools were ethnically diverse.
About two-thirds of the students spoke a language other than German at home. Many
had a Turkish, Arabic, or Eastern European background. The research was designed as a
large-scale replication study in 11 schools (N = 820). Because ethical guidelines and data
protection requirements are strict in Germany, it was not possible to present the study as a
regular classroom exercise. Instead, we designed a cover story that integrated the interven-
tion into the classroom as a supportive exercise for all students that, though initiated by
outside researchers, was supported by the school. This way, we increased the chances that
a key psychological element of the affirmation experience—the sense that “my values” are
being recognized and respected by institutional authorities—would be preserved.
The timing and context of the intervention were considered carefully. It was decided
to implement the intervention in a mathematics class where psychological threat was
expected to be most acute and debilitating (Borman et al., 2016). To interrupt the emer-
gence of recursive cycles, the intervention was administered at the beginning of the new
school year and, for seventh graders, right after the transition from primary to secondary
Self‑Affirmation Interventions 81
school. Also, an exam was administered immediately after the first affirmation, and then
8 weeks later. This way, the effect of the initial affirmation might be immediately chan-
neled into better performance, which might then facilitate better performance on subse-
quent exams (Cohen et al., 2009). To facilitate this recursive process, students across all
conditions received feedback on each of the two exams that emphasized their capacity
for growth (see Yeager & Dweck, 2012; all materials can be obtained from the authors
upon request).
The intervention was implemented by trained research administrators rather than
teachers. On the positive side, this facilitated treatment fidelity, ensuring maximal con-
trol over the implementation was obtained. On the negative side, this element of the
procedure might undercut certain key psychological elements from the experience. Stu-
dents might be less engaged by the activity because it was being delivered by outsiders.
Accordingly, innovative procedures were introduced to support student engagement. For
instance, one novel procedural element was the use of an appealing interactive comic strip
to increase students’ motivation to write an essay. In the story, an alien wants to learn
more about young people on Earth and asks students several questions related to their
values. (The annotated and translated materials can be found in Appendix 3.2.) As in the
original studies, students were told to write down their thoughts and feelings and told
that the assignment was nonevaluative.
Additionally, several prompts focused students on the intrinsically rewarding nature
of their values (Schimel, Arndt, Banko, & Cook, 2004). Students answered yes/no ques-
tions about different statements related to their two chosen values, such as “When I think
about [value x, value y], . . . I’m happy and content.” This element was designed to focus
students on the intrinsic rationale for their values (Schimel et al., 2004) and on emotional
experiences associated with these kinds of rationales (e.g., joy, contentment, lack of anxi-
ety; Rheinberg & Eser, 2018).
Replicating previous results, the affirmation interventions improved the perfor-
mance of students from an identity-threatened group (Lokhande & Müller, 2019). A
significant interaction effect between affirmation condition and ethnic background was
obtained. On the first exam, there was a positive effect of the affirmation on the math
achievement of immigrant students, though this was confined to Turkish immigrants and
did not occur for students of Arabic descent. Eight weeks later, however, both immigrant
groups performed significantly better on the second math test. Echoing results of previ-
ous studies, no significant effect of the affirmation was found for the nonimmigrant
group. Overall, the achievement gap between immigrant and nonimmigrant students was
reduced by approximately 40%.
The experience of adapting the value-affirmation procedure to the German context
highlights the importance of attention to psychological detail and meaning (see Lee &
Luykx, 2005). After the publication of the results (Müller & Lokhande, 2017), many
teachers showed an interest in using the intervention materials in their own classrooms.
But many thought that it would be a simple matter of handing out the comic strip and,
once completed, students would “magically” improve (cf. Yeager & Walton, 2011). As
the case study illustrates, implementation fidelity requires the creation of a positive psy-
chological experience, and this requires careful consideration of the meaning that every
procedural element will have for students. Implementation fidelity also requires setting
the stage so that any initial psychological effects of the intervention can be channeled,
sooner rather than later, into performance and the experience of progress.
82 I. Education
IMPLICATIONS FOR PSYCHOLOGICAL THEORY
The research reported in this chapter demonstrates the pervasive power of self-integrity
in mediating responses to many experiences in social life: confronting threats to one’s
social identity in social contexts, such as school, coping with regrettable actions, process-
ing medical and political information, and dealing with intergroup divides. When people
are able to affirm self-integrity by drawing on alternative self-resources, they are able to
tolerate, and even grow, from threatening experiences in their lives. The consistency of
affirmation effects across these diverse domains suggests a psychological unity.
Additionally, self-affirmation research has shown how “psyche and structure” inter-
act, propagating outcomes through time (Cohen et al., 2017; Cohen & Sherman, 2014;
Goyer et al., 2017). Psychological processes interact with processes in the social environ-
ment in recursive feedback loops. Rather than psychologize social problems—or sociolo-
gize them—the perspective offered in self-affirmation research suggests that the action is
in the interaction between these levels of analysis.
FUTURE DIRECTIONS
There are many questions to be answered. Three form the focus of our concluding sec-
tion:
First, self-affirmations can have negative effects. One type of situation in which this
is the case are those where threat has positive effects (Rokeach, 1973; Stone & Focella,
2011), and affirmation could short-circuit that process. In situations where self-protective
responses have proved adaptive, affirmation may have negative consequences (e.g., Jes-
sop, Ayers, Burn, & Ryda, 2018). Affirmation is not a panacea. For instance, when people
persist on a task because they are motivated to protect their self-integrity, an affirmation
could lead to disengagement. After repeated failure on a task where there is little oppor-
tunity for improvement, affirmation has been shown to lead people to disengage (Vohs,
Park, & Schmeichel, 2013). If a person is persisting on a task because of psychological
threat (e.g., “I don’t want to look dumb”), affirmation might lift this self-evaluative con-
cern and lead the person to give up.
Another situation where affirmations may backfire is in the absence of psychological
threat. For example, in educational contexts, for the most part affirmation intervention
studies have had null or negligible mean effects on the academic performance of non-
identity-threatened students. In a few studies, however, there appears to be some evidence
of negative trends for these groups, at least on the focal outcome (e.g., Brady et al., 2016;
Kizilcec, Saltarelli, Reich, & Cohen, 2017). Researchers do not yet know how robust
these effects occur or, to the extent that they occur, why (Binning & Browman, 2020).
One possibility, however, is that when an individual is not under consistent identity
Self‑Affirmation Interventions 83
threat, a self-affirmation may introduce alternative processes. For example, perhaps self-
affirmation leads people to feel more like an individual than a member of their group;
this may lead non-identity-threatened students to benefit less from “stereotype lift,” the
psychological boost they get from being on the upside of negative stereotypes (Walton &
Cohen, 2003). Additionally, insofar as some degree of threat or stress helps performance,
perhaps affirmation might lower it to suboptimal levels for some groups (Zajonc, 1965).
In research on persuasion, affirmation in nonthreatening contexts has increased self-
confidence, causing resistance rather than openness to change (Briñol, Petty, Gallardo,
& DeMarree, 2007).
A second question for future research is the spontaneity of the affirmation processes
(see Harris et al., 2019). An affirmation intervention—or any wise intervention—is but
a spark for a psychological process. That process can occur even without an intervention
to trigger it. Some people may engage the self-affirmation process spontaneously (Harris
et al., 2019). Indeed, it may be possible to train people to activate the self-affirmation
process on their own. Brady and colleagues (2016) examined the self-affirmation process
as it occurred spontaneously, by asking people to write down their thoughts after a stress
induction. They found that people do indeed differ in the extent to which they spontane-
ously self-affirm. A strong predictor of this tendency was high self-esteem.
Another study taught female engineering students how to self-affirm spontaneously
through a short slide show (Walton et al., 2015). Female students who worked in pre-
dominantly male fields of engineering earned higher grades as a result of this training.
Harris and colleagues (2019) have also examined individual differences in spontaneous
self-affirmation through the use of a self-report measure. Spontaneous self-affirmers
exhibit open-mindedness to threatening health information akin to those who complete
experimental affirmations (see also Cornil & Chandon, 2013; Ferrer et al., 2015). This
research highlights an important new frontier in self-affirmation research: How the pro-
cess of self-affirmation unfolds spontaneously and how differences in people’s ability to
spontaneously marshal the self-affirmation process may explain individual differences in
resilience, self-esteem, and perhaps clinical outcomes.
A third direction for affirmation research involves further exploring the interactions
between the self-affirmation process and the social environment. For example, some-
times a psychological process may have effects on the larger social environment. In one
study (Powers et al., 2016), classrooms with a higher proportion of minorities who had
been self-affirmed performed better on the whole. The benefits of the self-affirmation
intervention did not end with minority students but spilled over to affect the entire class-
room. How the environment changed was unclear, but one possibility is that the class-
room became more orderly and positive (Binning et al., 2019), enabling teachers to reach
a greater number of students, especially low-performing students who might have other-
wise been overlooked.
FINAL WORDS
Self-affirmation theory has had a long evolution. It began as a basic theory of how people
maintain the integrity of self. It has grown into a theory with applications to a wide range
of applied arenas, including education, health, and conflict. On the one hand, the effects
of affirmation interventions can be powerful and long lasting. On the other hand, these
84 I. Education
effects are conditional. They do not occur for all people and in all contexts. “Powerful
but conditional” is an apt way to describe them and many other wise interventions. A
large dose of humility is thus needed whenever scientists or practitioners apply them. To
adapt an intervention to a new context, it is important to understand whether and how
psychological threat plays out, for whom, and when. It is important to adapt the interven-
tion so that it is engaging and actually affirming. With these caveats acknowledged, we
can also be excited about the range of domains where self-affirmation can be applied—
many of which are still yet to be imagined.
REFERENCES
Adams, G., Tormala, T. T., & O’Brien, L. T. (2006). The effect of self-affirmation on perception
of racism. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 616–626.
Aronson, E. (1969). The theory of cognitive dissonance: A current perspective. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.),
Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 1–34). New York: Academic Press.
Aronson, E., Ellsworth, P. C., Carlsmith, J. M., & Gonzales, M. H. (1990). Methods of research
in social psychology. New York: McGraw-H ill.
Aronson, J., Cohen, G. L., & Nail, P. R. (1999). Self-affirmation theory: An update and appraisal.
In E. Harmon-Jones & J. Mills (Eds.), Cognitive dissonance theory: Revival with revisions
and controversies (pp. 127–147). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Badea, C., Binning, K. R., Verlhiac, J., & Sherman, D. K. (2017). In the aftermath of terrorism:
Effects of self- versus group-affirmation on support for discriminatory policies. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 76, 421–428.
Badea, C., & Sherman, D. K. (2019). Self-affirmation and prejudice reduction: When and why?
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28, 40–46.
Binning, K. R., Brick, C., Cohen, G. L., & Sherman, D. K. (2015). Going along versus getting it
right: The role of self-integrity in political conformity. Journal of Experimental Social Psy-
chology, 56, 73–88.
Binning, K. R., & Browman, A. S. (2020). Theoretical, ethical, and policy implications for con-
ducting social- psychological interventions to close educational achievement gaps. Social
Issues and Policy Review, 14, 182–216.
Binning, K. R., Cook, J. E., Purdie-Greenaway, V., Garcia, J., Chen, S. Apfel, N., . . . Cohen, G.
L. (2019). Bolstering trust and reducing discipline incidents at a diverse middle school: How
self-affirmation affects behavioral conduct during the transition to adolescence. Journal of
School Psychology, 75, 74–88.
Binning, K. R., Sherman, D. K., Cohen, G. L., & Heitland, K. (2010). Seeing the other side: Reduc-
ing political partisanship via self-affirmation in the 2008 presidential election. Analyses of
Social Issues and Public Policy, 10, 276–292.
Borman, G. D. (2017). Advancing values affirmation as a scalable strategy for mitigating identity
threats and narrowing national achievement gaps. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences of the USA, 114, 7486–7488.
Borman, G. D., Grigg, J., & Hanselman, P. (2016). An effort to close achievement gaps at scale
through self-affirmation. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 38, 21–42.
Borman, G. D., Grigg, J., Rozek, C. S., Hanselman, P., & Dewey, N. A. (2018). Self-affirmation
effects are produced by school context, student engagement with the intervention, and time:
Lessons from a district-wide implementation. Psychological Science, 29, 1773–1784.
Bowen, N. K., Wegmann, K. M., & Webber, K. C. (2013). Enhancing a brief writing intervention
to combat stereotype threat among middle-school students. Journal of Educational Psychol-
ogy, 105, 427–435.
Self‑Affirmation Interventions 85
Brady, S. T., Reeves, S. L., Garcia, J., Purdie-Vaughns, V., Cook, J. E., Taborsky-Barba, S., . . .
Cohen, G. L. (2016). The psychology of the affirmed learner: Spontaneous self-affirmation
in the face of stress. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108, 353–373.
Bratter, J. L., Rowley, K. J., & Chukhray, I. (2016). Does a self-affirmation intervention reduce
stereotype threat in Black and Hispanic high schools? Race and Social Problems, 8, 340–356.
Briñol, P., Petty, R. E., Gallardo, I., & DeMarree, K. G. (2007). The effect of self-affirmation in
nonthreatening persuasion domains: Timing affects the process. Personality and Social Psy-
chology Bulletin, 33, 1533–1546.
Burger, J. M. (1999). The foot-in-the-door compliance procedure: A multiple-process analysis and
review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 3, 303–325.
Burson, A., Crocker, J., & Mischkowski, D. (2012). Two types of value-affirmation: Implications
for self-control following social exclusion. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3,
510–516.
ehaji-Clancy, S., Effron, D. A., Halperin, E., Liberman, V., & Ross, L. D. (2011). Affirmation,
acknowledgment of in-group responsibility, group-based guilt, and support for reparative
measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 256–270.
Claro, S., Paunesku, D., & Dweck, C. S. (2016). Growth mindset tempers the effects of poverty on
academic achievement. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 113,
8664–8668.
Cohen, G. L., Aronson, J., & Steele, C. M. (2000). When beliefs yield to evidence: Reducing biased
evaluation by affirming the self. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1151–1164.
Cohen, G. L., & Garcia, J. (2008). Identity, belonging, and achievement: A model, interventions,
implications. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 365–369.
Cohen, G. L., Garcia, J., Apfel, N., & Master, A. (2006). Reducing the racial achievement gap: A
social-psychological intervention. Science, 313, 1307–1310.
Cohen, G. L., Garcia, J., & Goyer, J. P. (2017). Turning point: Targeted, tailored, and timely psy-
chological intervention. In A. J. Elliot, C. S. Dweck, & D. S. Yeager (Eds.), Handbook of com-
petence and motivation: Theory and application (pp. 657–686). New York: Guilford Press.
Cohen, G. L., Garcia, J., Purdie-Vaughns, V., Apfel, N., & Brzustoski, P. (2009). Recursive pro-
cesses in self-affirmation: Intervening to close the minority achievement gap. Science, 324,
400–403.
Cohen, G. L., & Sherman, D. K. (2014). The psychology of change: Self-affirmation and social
psychological intervention. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 333–371.
Cohen, G. L., Sherman, D. K., Bastardi, A., Hsu, L., McGoey, M., & Ross, L. (2007). Bridging
the partisan divide: Self-affirmation reduces ideological closed-mindedness and inflexibility
in negotiation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 415–430.
Cook, J. E., Purdie-Vaughns, V., Garcia, J., & Cohen, G. L. (2012). Chronic threat and contingent
belonging: Protective benefits of values affirmation on identity development. Journal of Per-
sonality and Social Psychology, 102, 479–496.
Cornil, Y., & Chandon, P. (2013). From fan to fat?: Vicarious losing increases unhealthy eating,
but self-affirmation is an effective remedy. Psychological Science, 24, 1936–1946.
Creswell, J. D., Dutcher, J. M., Klein, W. M. P., Harris, P. R., & Levine, J. M. (2013). Self-
affirmation improves problem-solving under stress. PLOS ONE, 8(5), e62593.
Creswell, J. D., Lam, S., Stanton, A. L., Taylor, S. E., Bower, J. E., & Sherman, D. K. (2007). Does
self-affirmation, cognitive processing, or discovery of meaning explain cancer-related health
benefits of expressive writing? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 238–250.
Creswell, J. D., Welch, W. T., Taylor, S. E., Sherman, D. K., Gruenewald, T. L., & Mann, T.
(2005). Affirmation of personal values buffers neuroendocrine and psychological stress
responses. Psychological Science, 16, 846–851.
Critcher, C. R., & Dunning, D. (2015). Self-affirmations provide a broader perspective on self-
threat. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41, 3–18.
86 I. Education
de Jong, E. M., Jellesma, F. C., Koomen, H. M. Y., & de Jong, P. F. (2016). A values-affirmation
intervention does not benefit negatively stereotyped immigrant students in the Netherlands.
Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 691.
Dee, T. S. (2015). Social identity and achievement gaps: Evidence from an affirmation interven-
tion. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 8, 149–168.
Ditto, P. H., & Lopez, D. F. (1992). Motivated skepticism: Use of differential decision criteria for
preferred and nonpreferred conclusions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63,
568–584.
Dunning, D. (2003). The zealous self-affirmer: How and why the self lurks so pervasively behind
social judgment. In S. J. Spencer, S. Fein, M. P. Zanna, & J. M. Olson (Eds.), Motivated
social perception: The Ontario symposium (Vol. 9, pp. 45–72). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Dunning, D., & Cohen, G. L. (1992). Egocentric definitions of traits and abilities in social judg-
ment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 341–355.
Dunning, D., Leuenberger, A., & Sherman, D. A. (1995). A new look at motivated inference: Are
self-serving theories of success a product of motivational forces? Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 69, 58–68.
Dunning, D., Meyerowitz, J. A., & Holzberg, A. D. (1989). Ambiguity and self-evaluation: The
role of idiosyncratic trait definitions in self-serving assessments of ability. Journal of Person-
ality and Social Psychology, 57, 1082–1090.
Dutcher, J. M., Creswell, J. D., Pacilio, L. E., Harris, P. R., Klein, W. M., Levine, J. M., . . . Eisen-
berger, N. I. (2016). Self-affirmation activates the ventral striatum: A possible reward-related
mechanism for self-affirmation. Psychological Science, 27, 455–466.
Eccles, J. S., Lord, S., & Midgley, C. (1991). What are we doing to early adolescents?: The impact
of educational contexts on early adolescents. American Journal of Education, 99, 521–542.
Ehret, P. J., LaBrie, J. W., Santerre, C., & Sherman, D. K. (2015). Self-affirmation and motiva-
tional interviewing (SAMI): Integrating perspectives to reduce resistance and increase effi-
cacy of alcohol interventions. Health Psychology Review, 9, 83–102.
Ehret, P. J., & Sherman, D. K. (2014). Public policy and health: A self-affirmation perspective.
Policy Insights from Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 222–230.
Ehret, P. J., & Sherman, D. K. (2018). Integrating self-affirmation and implementation intentions:
Effects on college student drinking. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 52, 633–644.
Elder, G. H. (1998). The life course as developmental theory. Child Development, 69, 1–12.
Epton, T., Harris, P. R., Kane, R., van Koningsbruggen, G. M., & Sheeran, P. (2015). The impact of
self-affirmation on health-behavior change: A meta-analysis. Health Psychology, 34, 187–196.
Falk, E. B., O’Donnell, M. B., Cascio, C. N., Tinney, F., Kang, Y., Lieberman, M. D., . . . Strecher,
V. J. (2015). Self-affirmation alters the brain’s response to health messages and subsequent
behavior change. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 112, 1977–
1982.
Fein, S., & Spencer, S. J. (1997). Prejudice as self-image maintenance: Affirming the self through
derogating others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 31–44.
Ferrer, R. A., & Cohen, G. L. (2019). Reconceptualizing self-affirmation with the “trigger-and-
channel” framework: Lessons from the health domain. Personality and Social Psychology
Review, 23(3), 285–304.
Ferrer, R. A., Taber, J. M., Klein, W. M., Harris, P. R., Lewis, K. L., & Biesecker, L. G. (2015).
The role of current affect, anticipated affect and spontaneous self-affirmation in decisions to
receive self-threatening genetic risk information. Cognition and Emotion, 29, 1456–1465.
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Finez, L., & Sherman, D. K. (2012). Train in vain: The role of the self in claimed self-handicapping
strategies. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 34, 600–620.
Freedman, J. L., & Fraser, S. C. (1966). Compliance without pressure: The foot-in-the-door tech-
nique. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4(2), 195–202.
Self‑Affirmation Interventions 87
Gandara, P., & Contreras, F. (2009). The Latino education crisis: The consequences of failed
social policies. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Gebhardt, M., Rauch, D., Mang, J., Sälzer, C., & Stanat, P. (2012). Mathematische Kompetenz
von Schülerinnen und Schülern mit Zuwanderungshintergrund. In M. Prenzel, C. Sälzer, E.
Klieme, & O. Köller (Eds.), PISA 2012: Fortschritte und herausforderungen in Deutschland
(pp. 275–308). Münster, Germany: Waxmann.
Gilbert, D. T., Pinel, E. C., Wilson, T. D., Blumberg, S. J., & Wheatley, T. P. (1998). Immune
neglect: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 75, 617–638.
Goffman, E. (1978). The presentation of self in everyday life. London: Harmondsworth.
Goyer, J. P., Cohen, G. L., Cook, J. E., Master, A., Apfel, N., Lee, W., . . . Walton, G. M. (2019).
Targeted identity-safety interventions cause lasting reductions in discipline citations among
negatively stereotyped boys. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 117, 229–259.
Goyer, J. P., Garcia, J., Purdie-Vaughns, V., Binning, K. R., Cook, J. E., Reeves, S. L., . . . Cohen,
G. L. (2017). Self-affirmation facilitates minority middle schoolers’ progress along college
trajectories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 114, 7594–7599.
Hadden, I. R., Easterbrook, M. J., Nieuwenhuis, M., Fox, K. J., & Dolan, P. (2019). Self-
affirmation reduces the socioeconomic attainment gap in schools in England. British Journal
of Educational Psychology.
Hanselman, P., Bruch, S. K., Gamoran, A., & Borman, G. D. (2014). Threat in context: School
moderation of the impact of social identity threat on racial/ethnic achievement gaps. Sociol-
ogy of Education, 87(2), 106–124.
Hanselman, P., Rozek, C. S., Grigg, J., & Borman, G. D. (2017). New evidence on self-affirmation
effects and theorized sources of heterogeneity from large-scale replications. Journal of Edu-
cational Psychology, 109, 405–424.
Harackiewicz, J. M., Canning, E. A., Tibbetts, Y., Giffen, C. J., Blair, S. S., Rouse, D. I., & Hyde,
J. S. (2014). Closing the social class achievement gap for first-generation students in under-
graduate biology. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106, 375–389.
Harackiewicz, J. M., Canning, E. A., Tibbetts, Y., Priniski, S. J., & Hyde, J. S. (2016). Closing
achievement gaps with a utility-value intervention: Disentangling race and social class. Jour-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111, 745–765.
Harris, P. R., Griffin, D. W., Napper, L. E., Bond, R., Schüz, B., Stride, C., & Brearley, I. (2019).
Individual differences in self-affirmation: Distinguishing self-affirmation from positive self-
regard. Self and Identity, 18(6), 589–630.
Harris, P. R., & Napper, L. (2005). Self-affirmation and the biased processing of threatening
health-risk information. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 1250–1263.
Ikizer, E. G., & Blanton, H. (2016). Media coverage of “wise” interventions can reduce concern
for the disadvantaged. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 22, 135–147.
Inzlicht, M., & Ben-Zeev, T. (2000). A threatening intellectual environment: Why females are
susceptible to experiencing problem-solving deficits in the presence of males. Psychological
Science, 11, 365–371.
Jencks, C., & Phillips, M. (1998). The Black–White test score gap. Washington, DC: Brookings
Institution Press.
Jessop, D. C., Ayers, S., Burn, F., & Ryda, C. (2018). Can self-affirmation exacerbate adverse reac-
tions to stress under certain conditions? Psychology and Health, 33, 827–845.
Jiang, L. (2018). Job insecurity and creativity: The buffering effect of self-affirmation and work-
affirmation. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 48, 388–397.
Kaiser, C. R., & Major, B. (2006). A social psychological perspective on perceiving and reporting
discrimination. Law and Social Inquiry, 31, 801–830.
Keough, K. A., & Markus, H. R. (1999). On being well: The role of the self in building the bridge
from philosophy to biology. Psychological Inquiry, 9, 49–53.
88 I. Education
Kinias, Z., & Sim, J. (2016). Facilitating women’s success in business: Interrupting the process
of stereotype threat through affirmation of personal values. Journal of Applied Psychology,
101(11), 1585–1597.
Kizilcec, R. F., Saltarelli, A. J., Reich, J., & Cohen, G. L. (2017). Closing global achievement gaps
in MOOCs. Science, 355, 251–252.
Kunda, Z. (1987). Motivated inference: Self-serving generation and evaluation of causal theories.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 636–647.
Layous, K., Davis, E. M., Garcia, J., Purdie-Vaughns, V., Cook, J. E., & Cohen, G. L. (2017). Feel-
ing left out, but affirmed: Protecting against the negative effects of low belonging in college.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 69, 227–231.
Lee, O., & Luykx, A. (2005). Dilemmas in scaling up innovations in elementary science instruc-
tion with nonmainstream students. American Educational Research Journal, 42, 411–438.
Lewin, K. (1951). Field theory in social science: Selected theoretical papers (D. Cartwright, Ed.).
New York: Harper & Row.
Liberman, A., & Chaiken, S. (1992). Defensive processing of personally relevant health messages.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 669–679.
Logel, C., & Cohen, G. L. (2012). The role of the self in physical health: Testing the effect of a
values-affirmation intervention on weight loss. Psychological Science, 23, 53–55.
Logel, C., Kathmandu, A., & Cohen, G. L. (2019). Affirmation prevents long-term weight gain.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 81, 70–75.
Lokhande, M., & Müller, T. (2019). Double jeopardy—double remedy?: The effectiveness of self-
affirmation for improving doubly disadvantaged students’ mathematical performance. Jour-
nal of School Psychology, 75, 58–73.
Lopez, M. H. (2009). Latinos and education: Explaining the attainment gap. Washington, DC:
Pew Research Center, Pew Hispanic Center Project. Retrieved from www.pewhispanic.
org/2009/10/07/latinos-and-education-explaining-the-attainment-gap.
Martens, A., Johns, M., Greenberg, J., & Schimel, J. (2006). Combating stereotype threat: The
effect of self-affirmation on women’s intellectual performance. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology, 42, 236–243.
McQueen, A., & Klein, W. M. (2006). Experimental manipulations of self-affirmation: A system-
atic review. Self and Identity, 5, 289–354.
Mitchell, D. E., Ream, R. K., Ryan, S., & Espinoza, J. (2008). Organizational strategies for
addressing California’s educational achievement gap (Policy brief prepared by California
Department of Education and the University of California). Retrieved from http://education.
ucdavis.edu/sites/main/files/Mitchell_PaperWEBR.pdf.
Miyake, A., Kost-Smith, L. E., Finkelstein, N. D., Pollock, S. J., Cohen, G. L., & Ito, T. A. (2010).
Reducing the gender achievement gap in college science: A classroom study of values affirma-
tion. Science, 330, 1234–1237.
Mukherjee, S. (2015). The laws of medicine: Field notes from an uncertain science. New York:
Simon & Schuster.
Müller, T., & Lokhande, M. (2017). Wider die Stereotypisierung: Bessere Schulleistung durch
Selbstbestätigung [Counteracting stereotype threat: Increasing achievement in school by self-
affirmation]. In Berlin Institute for Integration and Migration Research (BIM) and Research
Unit of the Expert Council of German Foundations on Integration and Migration (SVR—
Research Unit; Ed.), Vielfalt im Klassenzimmer. Wie Lehrkräfte gute Leistung fördern kön-
nen [Diversity in the classroom. How teachers can promote good student achievements]. Ber-
lin. Retrieved from www.svr- migration.de/publikationen/vielfalt-im-klassenzimmer.
Murphy, M. C., Steele, C. M., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Signaling threat: How situational cues affect
women in math, science, and engineering settings. Psychological Science, 18, 879–885.
Napper, L., Harris, P. R., & Epton, T. (2009). Developing and testing a self-affirmation manipula-
tion. Self and Identity, 8(1), 45–62.
Self‑Affirmation Interventions 89
Neal, D. (2006). Why has Black–White skill convergence stopped? Handbook of the Economics
of Education, 1, 511–576.
Pawson, R., & Tilley, N. (1997). Realistic evaluation. London: SAGE.
Pennebaker, J. W., & Chung, C. K. (2011). Expressive writing: Connections to physical and mental
health. In H. S. Friedman (Ed.), Oxford handbook of health psychology (pp. 417–437). New
York: Oxford University Press.
Pennebaker, J. W., & Evans, J. F. (2014). Expressive writing: Words that heal: Using expressive
writing to overcome traumas and emotional upheavals, resolve issues, improve health, and
build resilience. Enumclaw, WA: Idyll Arbor.
Powers, J., Cook, J. E., Purdie-Vaughns, V., Garcia, J., Apfel, N., & Cohen, G. L. (2016). Chang-
ing environments by changing individuals: The emergent effects of psychological interven-
tion. Psychological Science, 27, 150–160.
Protzko, J., & Aronson, J. (2016). Context moderates affirmation effects on the ethnic achieve-
ment gap. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7, 500–507.
Ready, D. D., & Wright, D. L. (2011). Accuracy and inaccuracy in teachers’ perceptions of young
children’s cognitive abilities: The role of child background and classroom context. American
Educational Research Journal, 48, 335–360.
Reardon, S. F. (2011). The widening academic achievement gap between the rich and the poor: New
evidence and possible explanations. In R. Murnane & G. Duncan (Eds.), Whither opportu-
nity?: Rising inequality and the uncertain life chances of low-income children (pp. 91–116).
New York: Russell Sage Foundation Press.
Rheinberg, F., & Engeser, S. (2018). Intrinsic motivation and flow. In J. Heckhausen & H. Heck-
hausen (Eds.), Motivation and action (pp. 579–622). Berlin: Springer.
Rheinschmidt, M. L., & Mendoza-Denton, R. (2014). Social class and academic achievement in
college: The interplay of rejection sensitivity and entity beliefs. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 107, 101–121.
Rokeach, M. (1973). The nature of human values. New York: Free Press.
Rosenthal, R. (1994). Interpersonal expectancy effects: A 30-year perspective. Current Directions
in Psychological Science, 3, 176–179.
Ross, L., Lepper, M., & Ward, A. (2010). History of social psychology: Insights, challenges, and
contributions to theory and application. In S. T. Fiske, D. T. Gilbert, & G. Lindzey (Eds.),
Handbook of social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 3–50). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Rothstein, R. (2005). Class and schools: Using social, economic, and education reform to close
the Black–White achievement gap. Washington, DC: Economic Policy Institute.
Sapolsky, R. M. (2004). Why zebras don’t get ulcers: The acclaimed guide to stress, stress-related
diseases, and coping—now revised and updated. New York: Holt Paperbacks.
Schimel, J., Arndt, J., Banko, K. M., & Cook, A. (2004). Not all self-affirmations were created
equal: The cognitive and social benefits of affirming the intrinsic (vs. extrinsic) self. Social
Cognition, 22, 75–99.
Schmeichel, B. J., & Vohs, K. D. (2009). Self-affirmation and self-control: Affirming core values
counteracts ego depletion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 770–782.
Schwalbe, M. C., Barnes, J., O’Reilly, F., Chande, R., Soon, Z. Sanders, M., & Cohen, G. L.
(2018). Improving literacy and numeracy in the UK: Evidence from a registered large-scale
values affirmation RCT. Poster presented at the annual conference of Society of Personality
and Social Psychology, Atlanta, GA.
Schwalbe, M., Chande, R., Barnes, J., O’Reilly, F., Sanders, M., Watson, J., & Cohen, G. (2019).
Values affirmation and UK further education college performance. Charlottesville, VA: Cen-
ter for Open Science. Retrieved from osf.io/qh9ep.
Schwarz, N., Bless, H., Strack, F., Klumpp, G., Rittenauer-S chatka, H., & Simons, A. (1991). Ease
of retrieval as information: Another look at the availability heuristic. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 61, 195–202.
90 I. Education
Sekaquaptewa, D., & Thompson, M. (2003). Solo status, stereotype threat, and performance
expectancies: Their effects on women’s performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psy-
chology, 39, 68–74.
Shapiro, J. R., Williams, A. M., & Hambarchyan, M. (2013). Are all interventions created equal?:
A multi-threat approach to tailoring stereotype threat interventions. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 104, 277–288.
Sherman, D. K., Brookfield, J., & Ortosky, L. (2017). Intergroup conflict and barriers to common
ground: A self-affirmation perspective. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 11(12).
Sherman, D. K., Bunyan, D. P., Creswell, J. D., & Jaremka, L. M. (2009). Psychological vulner-
ability and stress: The effects of self-affirmation on sympathetic nervous system responses to
naturalistic stressors. Health Psychology, 28, 554–562.
Sherman, D. K., & Cohen, G. L. (2002). Accepting threatening information: Self-affirmation and
the reduction of defensive biases. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 119–123.
Sherman, D. K., & Cohen, G. L. (2006). The psychology of self-defense: Self-affirmation theory.
In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 38, pp. 183–242).
San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Sherman, D. K., Cohen, G. L., Nelson, L. D., Nussbaum, A. D., Bunyan, D. P., & Garcia, J. (2009).
Affirmed yet unaware: Exploring the role of awareness in the process of self-affirmation.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 745–764.
Sherman, D. K., & Hartson, K. A. (2011). Reconciling self-protection with self-improvement: Self-
affirmation theory. In M. Alicke & C. Sedikides (Eds.), The handbook of self-e nhancement
and self-protection (pp. 128–151). New York: Guilford Press.
Sherman, D. K., Hartson, K. A., Binning, K. R., Purdie-Vaughns, V., Garcia, J., Taborsky-Barba,
S., . . . Cohen, G. L. (2013). Deflecting the trajectory and changing the narrative: How self-
affirmation affects academic performance and motivation under identity threat. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 104, 591–618.
Sherman, D. K., & Kim, H. S. (2005). Is there an “I” in “team”?: The role of the self in group-
serving judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 108–120.
Sherman, D. A. K., Nelson, L. D., & Steele, C. M. (2000). Do messages about health risks threaten
the self?: Increasing the acceptance of threatening health messages via self-affirmation. Per-
sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1046–1058.
Siegel, P. A., Scillitoe, J., & Parks-Yancy, R. (2005). Reducing the tendency to self-handicap: The
effect of self-affirmation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 589–597.
Silverman, A., Logel, C., & Cohen, G. L. (2013). Self-affirmation as a deliberate coping strategy:
The moderating role of choice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 93–98.
Sivanathan, N., Molden, D. C., Galinsky, A. D., & Ku, G. (2008). The promise and peril of self-
affirmation in de-escalation of commitment. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision
Processes, 107, 1–14.
Steele, C. M. (1988). The psychology of self-affirmation: Sustaining the integrity of the self. In
L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 21, pp. 261–302). San
Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and perfor-
mance. American Psychologist, 52, 613–629.
Steele, C. M., & Liu, T. J. (1983). Dissonance processes as self-affirmation. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 45, 5–19.
Steele, C. M., Spencer, S. J., & Aronson, J. (2002). Contending with group image: The psychol-
ogy of stereotype and social identity threat. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental
social psychology (Vol. 34, pp. 379–440). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Stone, J., & Focella, E. (2011). Hypocrisy, dissonance and the self-regulation processes that
improve health. Self and Identity, 10, 295–303.
Storms, M. D., & McCaul, K. D. (1976). Attribution processes and emotional exacerbation of
Self‑Affirmation Interventions 91
ÅThis
prompt conveys that it is not evaluative, and encourages a broad
WHAT ARE YOUR PERSONAL VALUES?
perspective.
In this assignment you will be answering several questions about your ideas, your ÅFor
younger or less literate students, prompt was simplified because
beliefs, and your life. It is important to remember while you are answering many did not know what the term value means: “Remember that there
these questions that there are no right or wrong answers. are no right or wrong answers to any of these questions. Please read
Please read carefully over this list of personal values and think about each of the
this list carefully. Then circle the TWO or THREE things that are MOST
values. Then circle the two or three values that are MOST important to you. We important to you.”
understand that all of these values may be important to you. Even if you feel that ÅPicking
two or three things gives students the opportunity to focus on
many of the values are important, please pick only TWO or THREE of them to circle. more than one value if they want, so that they don’t feel as though they
The most important values to me are: (circle two or three)
are leaving one out that is important to them.
Athletic Ability
Being Good at Art
92
Creativity
Independence
Living in the Moment
Membership in a Social Group
(such as your community, racial group, or school club)
Music ÅAlthough
not typically considered a “value,” calling music a “value”
imbues it with a higher meaning.
Politics
ÅThis
list was originally developed through research with U.S. college
Relationships with Friends or Family
students about what matters most to them and then adapted to the
Religious Values middle school context; studies done in different cultures should identify
Sense of Humor core values/topics that resonate.
ÅThe
list of values typically excludes the domain most relevant to the
threat, school, or academics, at least for the first intervention.
ÅClip
art chosen to appeal to seventh graders, not used for college
student affirmations.
(continued)
Used in Cohen, Garcia, Apfel, and Master (2006); Cohen, Garcia, Purdie-Vaughns, Apfel, and Brzustoski (2009); and Sherman et al. (2013).
Directions:
1. Look at the values you picked as most important to you.
2. Think about times when these values were or would be very important to you. ÅStudents
are encouraged to think about concrete instances when
3. In a few sentences, describe why these values are important to you. Focus on these values served as resources.
your thoughts and feelings, and don’t worry about spelling, grammar, or how
ÅInstructions
emphasize nonevaluative nature.
well written it is.
_______________________________________________________________________
ÅAmple
space provided for expressive writing, but not so much space
_______________________________________________________________________ that students feel intimidated.
_______________________________________________________________________
_______________________________________________________________________
_______________________________________________________________________
_______________________________________________________________________
93
_______________________________________________________________________
_______________________________________________________________________
_______________________________________________________________________
_______________________________________________________________________
_______________________________________________________________________
_______________________________________________________________________
_______________________________________________________________________
_______________________________________________________________________
_______________________________________________________________________
_______________________________________________________________________
_______________________________________________________________________ (continued)
Again, look at the values you picked as most important. List the top two reasons why
ÅBased
on theories of ease of retrieval, participants asked for a limited
these values are important to you: number of reasons why value was important (Schwarz et al., 1991).
ÅResearch
suggests that focusing on “why” rather than “how” a
value is important is more self-affirming (Schmeichel & Vohs, 2009).
1. This prompt provides another opportunity to focus students on the
rationales for their values.
2.
Make a check mark to show how much you agree with each of the following
statements:
94
ÅThese
statements were constructed to be easy for students to agree
2. In general, I try to live up to these values. with and to reinforce the writing that they did earlier.
Strongly Disagree Somewhat Somewhat Agree Strongly
Disagree Disagree Agree Agree
____ ____ ____ ____ ____ ____
Please read carefully over this list of personal values and think about each of the
values. Then circle the two or three values that are LEAST important to you. Even if you
feel that several of the values are not very important, please pick only TWO or THREE
of them to circle.
Athletic Ability
Being Good at Art
95
Creativity
Independence
Living in the Moment
Membership in a Social Group
(such as your community, racial group, or school club)
Music
Politics
Relationships with Friends or Family
Religious Values
Sense of Humor
(continued)
Directions:
1. Look at the values you picked as least important to you.
2. Think about times when these values would be important to someone else ÅSeveral
different control conditions have been used. The purpose of
(like another student at your school or a person you’ve heard about). this control condition is to have participants engage in values-related
writing that is not self-relevant.
3. In a few sentences, describe why these values would be important to someone
else (like another person at your school or a person you’ve heard about). Focus
on your thoughts and feelings, and don’t worry about spelling, grammar, or how
well written it is.
_______________________________________________________________________
_______________________________________________________________________
_______________________________________________________________________
_______________________________________________________________________
_______________________________________________________________________
96
_______________________________________________________________________
_______________________________________________________________________
_______________________________________________________________________
_______________________________________________________________________
_______________________________________________________________________
_______________________________________________________________________
_______________________________________________________________________
_______________________________________________________________________
_______________________________________________________________________
_______________________________________________________________________
_______________________________________________________________________ (continued)
Again, look at your least important values. List the top two reasons why someone
ÅThe
control condition is designed to take a similar amount of time and be
else (like another student at your school or a person you’ve heard about) would pick equally engaging, but to not make salient personally important values.
these as their most important value:
1.
2.
Make a check mark to show how much you agree with each of the following
statements:
97
2. In general, some people try to live up to these values.
ÅThe
character’s name is Nari. Nari is introduced as an
alien visiting Earth, whose task it is to interview the kids
for Nari’s (extraterrestrial) school newspaper. An alien
character was chosen in order to serve as an identity
figure for children from all ethnic or social backgrounds.
Nari is not human and therefore does not resemble any
existing ethnic/racial group, nationality, or religion. In
order for boys and girls to identify equally, Nari also lacks
any fixed gender.
ÅThe
value affirmation task was strongly simplified in order
to match a target group with high variation in literacy
skills. Pretests showed that a longer and more complex
task led to students being easily bored or unwilling to
write a substantial essay.
98
ÅThe
notion of “values” was not adopted here, since it
was regarded as a little too abstract for the age group
(12–13 years). Furthermore, writing essays about
personal values (as opposed to concrete experiences)
is a less common task in German schools. We aimed
to paraphrase values as concrete actions or things of
importance.
ÅThe
items were adopted to the German context. In
comparison to the U.S. original, we added “building or
fixing technical stuff” and “helping others” to the list.
Used in Müller and Lokhande (2017). Translated from German. Source: SVR Research Unit/Christian Müller. (continued)
ÅThe
writing task was broken down into several steps to
facilitate the writing process, enhance engagement with the
task, and support a deeper processing of the chosen items.
ÅThe
students are first asked to repeat their chosen items
and to generate seven words that describe why the two
things are important.
99
ÅIn
the essay task, the students are then asked to include as
many of the previously generated words as they can.
C H A P TE R 4
Chris S. Hulleman and Judith M. Harackiewicz
BACKGROUND
In most schools in the United States, educators decide what happens in the classroom.
They control the content, the learning activities, which students are in the classroom,
and how students interact with one another. Although there are many good reasons for
this, including making learning developmentally appropriate and sequenced, instructor-
centric environments can rob students of opportunities to engage in learning that is
meaningful and interesting to them. For example, nearly every student has wondered at
some point, “Why are we learning this?!” Decades of research in educational psychology
have revealed that students are more motivated to engage in learning, persist longer, and
learn more when they find some type of value in what they are learning (Eccles et al.,
1983; Wigfield, Rosenzweig, & Eccles, 2017). One study revealed that 90% of middle
school teachers reported that one of the top barriers for student motivation was a lack
of value for learning (Hulleman & Barron, 2013). This problem might be particularly
100
The Utility‑Value Intervention 101
PSYCHOLOGICAL PROCESSES
Utility Value
As outlined in the original (Eccles et al., 1983) expectancy-value framework, perceiving
any type of value in an activity is likely to increase motivation to perform the activity.
Reflecting on personal connections between course content and students’ lives is expected
to increase perceptions of utility value, which subsequently increases the likelihood that
students will engage in course-related tasks like studying for an exam. To instigate that
utility value, students’ responses to the intervention need to have three characteristics.
First, connections need to be personal. Intervention activities are most empowering
when students are able to create their own connections rather than being told about why
they should value material (Canning & Harackiewicz, 2015; Durik & Harackiewicz,
2007; Durik, Schechter, Noh, Rozek, & Harackiewicz, 2015; Hulleman, 2007). Second,
connections need to be specific. Just as specific goals (e.g., “I want to set a detailed bud-
get by June 1”) are more likely to lead to goal attainment than general goals (e.g., “I want
to spend less”; Locke & Latham, 2002), specific connections between course content
and everyday life is more likely to spur motivation and adaptive outcomes. Prior studies
suggest that specific examples in intervention essays partially explain intervention effects
(Harackiewicz, Canning, Tibbetts, Prinski, & Hyde, 2016; Rozek, Hyde, Svoboda, Hul-
leman, & Harackiewicz, 2014). Third, connections need to be content-relevant. If the
goal of utility-value interventions is to support learning in a specific class, then the con-
nections that students make to their lives need to be relevant to current course content.
Figure 4.1 outlines how an exemplar quote is personal, specific, and content-relevant;
these are the types of responses researchers or practitioners would ideally scaffold stu-
dents toward in utility-value interventions.
Intervention Prompt
I would like you to think about how what we have been learning about in this class is
important to your life in some way. What connection can you find between one of the topics
we have been studying and something that is important in your life? Write a few sentences
about that connection below:
Exemplary Quote
It is personal and specific and is content-relevant.
“Playing hockey and friction are connected because in hockey while passing or shooting
the puck it slides on the ice which causes friction, slowing down the puck. Friction could be
important to my life because I can better understand how much force I need to get the
puck to move with the friction moving against it.”
FIGURE 4.1. Exemplar student quote for utility-value-intervention mechanisms. Data from Hul-
leman, Hulleman, et al. (2018).
The Utility‑Value Intervention 103
Other Psychological Processes
As listed in Figure 4.2, these intervention processes instigate the cascade of psychological
processes, beginning with perceived utility value, that lead to the beneficial effects of the
utility-value intervention. Although utility value was originally hypothesized to be the
key psychological process instigated by utility-value interventions, our recent research
has revealed two related aspects of motivation from expectancy-value theory that utility-
value interventions also affect. First, when students become engaged in a learning activity
in a way that allows them to realize their knowledge of the content, their confidence in
their ability to learn in the course is likely to increase (i.e., success expectancies). Sec-
ond, as students perceive value in a course, particularly a content area that is difficult to
master, their perceptions of the negative consequences of being in the course are likely to
decrease (i.e., perceived costs). Decades of research based in the expectancy-value frame-
work of achievement motivation in education (i.e., Eccles et al., 1983) has revealed that
students’ success expectancies and perceived utility value are positively related to learn-
ing, whereas perceived cost is negatively related to learning (e.g., Barron & Hulleman,
2015).
MECHANISMS
Psychological Mechanisms
Motivation researchers have investigated several psychological mechanisms that could
explain the effects of utility value on motivation: identification, involvement, and inter-
est. For example, perceiving the relevance of an activity to one’s life or future goals may
lead an individual to identify with the activity (identification), become more actively
involved in learning (involvement), and develop an enduring interest in the topic (e.g.,
Dewey, 1913; Eccles & Wigfield, 2002; Hidi & Renninger, 2006). First, perceiving utility
value in a topic may lead to an increase in the perceived importance of an activity in gen-
eral, and eventually to the identification of the activity with the individual’s self-concept
(e.g., attainment value: Eccles et al., 1983; identified regulation: Deci & Ryan, 1985). For
example, finding an application for an activity (e.g., quadratic equations and engineer-
ing) opens up the possibility of making connections to things that are personally impor-
tant to the individual (e.g., a career as an engineer). Once these connections have been
made, repeated engagement in the activity can lead to the activity becoming incorporated
into the individual’s self-concept (i.e., identification). Second, perceiving utility value
in a topic may promote active task engagement. For example, perceiving a connection
between geometry and life may energize an individual to become more actively involved
in geometry class by seeking out learning opportunities, putting forth more effort, and
becoming more engaged. When students are active contributors to the learning process,
then they are more likely to feel in control (deCharms, 1968), self-determined (Deci &
Ryan, 1985), and efficacious (Bandura, 1997). Rather than being passive recipients of
education, students perceive that they are active participants and become absorbed in
the learning process (Harackieiwicz & Sansone, 1991). Third, students are more likely
to engage in activities that they find important, and perceiving utility value in a topic can
increase their willingness to seek out the activity over time. Repeated engagement facili-
tates the acquisition of activity-related skills and knowledge, and enhances the experience
Mechanisms
Psychological
Identification
Processes
Involvement
Interest
Intervention Fidelity Psychological Outcomes
Utility-Value Personal Perceived Utility Value Grades
104
Intervention Specific Success Expectancies Persistence
Content-Relevant Perceived Cost Career Intentions
Behavioral
Engagement
Performance
Behavioral Mechanisms
Co-occurring with the psychological mechanisms outlined above, students who per-
ceive increased utility value are more likely to demonstrate behavioral engagement and
increased performance in the activity. In the classroom, behavioral engagement includes
things like increased attendance, homework completion, and work quality. Increased
performance includes proximal measures of competence development, such as perfor-
mance on quizzes, tests, and oral presentations. These behavioral mechanisms work in
tandem with the psychological mechanisms. For example, increased attendance can lead
to increased learning, which results in increased positive affect toward school. When
repeated over time, this can lead to the development of interest. Conversely, experiencing
identification with the activity—such as when a student connects learning biology to his
or her interest in becoming a paramedic—can lead to an increased interest in classroom
activities, which enables the student to complete his or her homework at a higher level.
This then leads to better performance on quizzes and tests.
EMPIRICAL EVIDENCE
Outcomes
Since the original intervention studies (Hulleman et al., 2010), the utility-value inter-
vention has been replicated across of a variety of high school and college courses. In a
recent meta-analysis of utility-value interventions (Hulleman, Wormington, Tibbetts, &
Phillipoom, 2018), we found 33 field studies where 12,478 participants were random-
ized to a utility-value or control condition. Our meta-analytic results indicated that, on
average, the utility-value intervention boosted learning outcomes—such as exam scores,
end-of-semester course grades and pass rates, and interest in the topic—by a quarter
of a standard deviation (d = 0.24). Table 4.1 presents a representative sample of pub-
lished utility-value intervention studies. For most of these studies, intervention effects
were most pronounced for students most likely to experience adverse learning outcomes,
such as students with histories of lower achievement (e.g., Hulleman, Kosovich, Barron,
106 I. Education
Total 4,174
Note: For a comprehensive review, see a recent meta-analysis of utility-value interventions (Hulleman, Wormington,
Tibbetts, & Philipoom, 2018).
& Daniel, 2017), lower success expectancies (e.g., Hulleman & Harackiewicz, 2009), or
from traditionally underrepresented groups in higher education (e.g., Harackiewicz et
al., 2016).
Two sets of follow-up studies are important to highlight. In our first follow-up study,
we implemented the intervention in 30 high school science classrooms taught by 10 dif-
ferent teachers (N = 262; Hulleman & Harackiewicz, 2009). The writing activities were
shortened to make them developmentally appropriate and to fit into a 45-minute class
period. Students randomized to the utility-value condition were simply prompted to write
one to two paragraphs about how a topic they were studying in science related to their
lives. Students randomized to the control condition were prompted to write a one- to
two-paragraph summary of a topic they were studying in class. Students completed the
writing activities two to five times per semester depending on the teacher. We found that
the intervention enhanced both course grades and subsequent interest in science for low-
performing students in the utility-value condition compared to the control condition.
For example, less confident students increased their semester grade in the course by over
three-quarters of a grade point on a 4-point scale (d = 0.50).
In a second set of follow-up studies, Harackiewicz and colleagues (2016; Canning
et al., 2018; Priniski et al., 2019; Rosenzweig, Harackiewicz, et al., 2019) implemented
a utility-value intervention within a two-semester introductory biology sequence at a
research-intensive university. The basic paradigm was the same for all four studies. For
each of three units across the semester, students were randomly assigned to receive either
a utility-value writing assignment, in which they explained why course material was
The Utility‑Value Intervention 107
useful to them personally (or wrote a letter to a friend or family member about how the
course material was relevant to them), or a control assignment, in which they summa-
rized course material. These assignments were part of the course curriculum and were
graded for credit by biology graduate students blind to condition. In the first published
study, Harackiewicz et al. found that the utility-value intervention had an overall, small
positive effect on course grades for all students. They also found that the intervention
had a more positive effect for students with a history of lower success expectations in
the course (replicating the effects of Hulleman & Harackiewicz, 2009), and for under-
represented students, specifically students who were both first-generation and members
of underrepresented minority groups.
Effects over Time
Most of the utility-value intervention studies have examined intervention effects in a
single semester. This is because the intervention was designed to promote motivation
and engagement within a specific learning context; therefore, it seems unlikely that the
intervention should have long-lasting direct effects (Harackiewicz & Priniski, 2018).
However, it if promotes performance in foundational courses, it may influence broader
outcomes indirectly, by helping students succeed in these classes. It is also possible that
the intervention might promote identification processes that have implications for subse-
quent academic choices. Only two published studies have looked at results in subsequent
semesters, both in college biology. In the first study (N = 577; Canning et al., 2018), stu-
dents in the utility-value condition earned higher grades in the course, were more likely to
enroll in the second course of the biology sequence, and were less likely to abandon their
science, technology, engineering, or math (STEM) major, than students in the control
group. In the second study, Hecht, Harackiewicz, et al. (2019) followed students in the
original Harackiewicz et al. (2016) study for 2 years after the intervention and examined
whether they continued to the next course in the biology sequence and persisted in a bio-
science major. They found that the utility-value intervention promoted persistence in the
biomedical track for students who entered the introductory biology course with higher
levels of confidence in their ability to perform well in the course. In other words, these
researchers found a direct effect on long-term persistence, although the pattern of the
interaction was different from that observed many times on shorter-term outcomes, such
as interest (Hulleman et al., 2010) and course performance (Harackiewicz et al., 2016).
Moreover, this effect was mediated through different processes, showing the complexity
of understanding long-term effects of a task-specific intervention.
In addition to investigating the extent to which utility-value interventions last over
time, and which outcomes are impacted, it is essential to consider how the interven-
tion can have an effect over time. How do the short-term mechanisms translate into
broader effects? We can glean some insight from Harackiewicz and colleagues’ (2016)
college biology intervention. Hecht, Harackiewicz, et al. (2019) found that the process
through which the utility-value intervention influenced persistence was distinct from the
process through which it promoted course grades. Engagement with the course mate-
rial was related to course grades, whereas making personal connections to the mate-
rial was related to persistence in the biology major. These findings suggest that the
utility-value intervention may initiate two distinct processes that align with involvement
and identification, respectively: (1) helping underperforming students to engage with
108 I. Education
course material, thereby improving performance, which had a significant indirect effect
on persistence in the major; and (2) increasing reflection on the personal relevance of
course material, thereby helping confident students see why pursuing that domain may
be important.
Heterogeneity
At present, the majority of published utility-value intervention studies have found that
the intervention works best for students who are traditionally underrepresented, under-
served, or underprepared in the learning context (e.g., students with lower prior achieve-
ment, first-generation college students, racial/ethnic minorities; see Table 4.1). Our initial
hypothesis was that concerns about academic performance impeded students’ capacity
to perceive value through a narrowing of attention (Durik & Harackiewicz, 2007; Hul-
leman, 2007), and that prompting students to make those connections gave them the
opportunity they needed to make some initial connections. However, our more recent
research demonstrates that for some students, particularly those students who perform
poorly early in the course, the utility-value intervention boosts their success expectan-
cies (Hulleman, Kosovich, et al., 2017; Rosenzweig, Wigfield, & Hulleman, 2020). Stu-
dents who perform poorly initially tend to be more disengaged from the course. As a
result, nudging them to see the value in the course by completing utility-value interven-
tion activities may spark them to reengage with course material. Furthermore, students
whose values are not aligned with those of the learning context might feel disconnected
from the learning context from the beginning (Stephens, Hamedani, & Destin, 2014;
Harackiewicz et al., 2016). For example, students from groups traditionally underrep-
resented in higher education, such as students who belong to a racial/ethnic-minority
group, tend to value interdependent and communal goals more than independent and
agentic goals (Diekman, Brown, Johnston, & Clark, 2010). The utility-value intervention
provides these students an opportunity to identify their own values rather than being told
to connect to goals that are not their own (e.g., more independent than interdependent).
An important reason why moderator effects might vary across learning contexts
is that utility-value interventions provide an opportunity for students to articulate how
their personal goals and values might align with learning in a specific context, rather
than being told to connect to goals that are not their own (e.g., more independent than
interdependent). This aligns with the core aspects of the Eccles et al. (1983) expectancy-
value framework that explicates how student motivation is dependent upon the learning
context. Our current corpus of research makes it difficult to test this context-salient
hypothesis because interventions have been implemented in contexts with very few tra-
ditionally underrepresented students, which makes it difficult to fully examine students’
intersectional identities. For example, Harackiewicz and colleagues (2016) needed to col-
lect data across four semesters to have sufficient numbers of first-generation students
and racial/ethnic-minority students to conduct their analyses. Testing interventions in
new and diverse learning contexts will provide the opportunity to determine whether
the intervention logic model and our hypotheses about intervention moderators hold up.
Second, variation in the social context of learning could influence intervention
effects. One way of looking at this is by institutional context. Although the interven-
tions have been tested across different types of institutions (e.g., high school, community
The Utility‑Value Intervention 109
college, and research-intensive universities) and subject areas (e.g., math, psychology,
biology), our meta-analytic review of utility-value interventions did not show significant
heterogeneity in intervention effects by context (d’s from 0.19 to 0.31). Unfortunately,
large studies that include many different learning contexts in a single study have not
yet been published with the utility-value intervention. Such studies have the potential to
uncover important variations in learning context that could contribute to heterogeneity
in intervention impacts (cf. Yeager et al., 2019).
COUSINS
relevance affirmation, students first affirm their core values (by circling their top values
from a list), and then they write an essay about how their current educational pathway
connects to those core values. The relevance affirmation intervention does not target spe-
cific learning content like the utility-value intervention but instead activates more global
values that serve to affirm a student’s identity, which makes it similar to the prosocial
purpose intervention (Yeager et al., 2014).
Fifth, the utility-value intervention is similar to interventions that personalize the
learning context. In these context-personalization interventions (Cordova & Lepper,
1996), the learning content and tasks are customized for the individual student by using
personal details, such as the student’s name, or preferences, such as the student’s favorite
music group. Context-personalization interventions do not provide students the oppor-
tunity to actively make connections but instead makes the connection for the student by
imbuing the learning context with customized personal details and preferences.
Versions
Basic Writing Prompt
The original version of the intervention (see Figure 4.3) involved a simple prompt for
students to select a topic they are currently studying and write about how it connects
to something in their lives (Hulleman et al., 2010, Study 1; Hulleman & Harackiewicz,
2009). This version clearly puts the student perspective as the driver of making con-
nections that are personal and specific to that student. The challenge is that, for many
students, making strong connections is difficult without appropriate scaffolds, which is
why additional variants of the intervention have been developed. As presented in Figure
4.4, a modified version of the basic prompt was developed for college biology students
(Harackiewicz et al., 2016).
Letter
As described previously, the letter version prompts students to write a letter to a sig-
nificant other—parent, sibling, friend—about how what the student is learning in class
could be relevant to his or her significant other’s life (i.e., self–other overlap; Aron, Aron,
Tudor, & Nelson, 1991). This allows students to endorse a utility value for the material
without having to completely endorse it for themselves. It also offers the students an
opportunity to become a benefactor of a message intended to help others, instead of being
passive recipients of information.
Quotations
Gaspard and colleagues (2015) developed the first version of the utility-value inter-
vention that provided sample quotes from students who had already taken the course.
Instead of the instructor explicating the potential value of math, quotations from near-
age peers provide a scaffold for students to find their own value. Since their first version
The Utility‑Value Intervention 111
Now that we have reviewed the main topics and concepts from this unit, it is time to reflect on one
specific topic or concept.
Part A: Pick one of the topics or concepts that we have covered in this unit and briefly summarize the
main parts.
Part B: Apply this topic/concept to your life, or to the life of someone you know. How might the
information be useful to you, or a friend/relative, in daily life? How does learning about this topic apply to
your future plans?
You can either: 1) write about it in at least 5 sentences, 2) draw a concept map with a description, or 3)
draw a sketch with a description. If you do a concept map or a sketch, be sure to describe it well enough
so that the reader can understand it.
For example, if you were studying nutrition, you could choose a topic such as how food is digested.
Briefly summarize the digestive process – how foods are broken down in the mouth, stomach, and
intestines to make energy. Then you could write about how this applies to your own life. For example,
eating healthy foods helps your body produce energy to play your favorite sport or study for exams.
You could also draw a concept map of how your knowledge of digestion applies to your life. An example
is provided below. Remember that you would also need to add a brief written description with a concept
map or diagram.
Mouth
Sports
Healthy Digestive
Stomach Energy
Foods System
Studying
Intestines
FIGURE 4.3. The basic utility-value-intervention prompt for high school science. From Hulle-
man and Harackiewicz (2009). Reprinted with permission from the American Association for the
Advancement of Science.
Writing Assignment #1
Objective: Writing about scientific principles and phenomena is an increasingly important skill in
the 21st century. This assignment is designed to help you understand a major concept covered
in this unit while also helping you develop your science writing skills. One key to effective science
writing is explaining how science can be used in everyday life. You’ll do this in a 500–600 word
paper. You should:
1) Formulate and answer a question Sample questions
Select a concept that was covered in lecture • What is gene expression and how is it
and formulate a question. Use this question as applied in medical research?
the title of your essay. • What are mutagens and why are they
potentially harmful to DNA?
2) Explain how this applies to your life Examples of applications
Write a 500–600 word essay answering this • Medical researchers study gene expression
question, and discuss how the information patterns to diagnose and treat conditions
could be useful to you in your own life. Be caused by genetic variants. In your own
sure to include some concrete information that life, you will have access to more effective
was covered in this unit, explaining why the treatments for any serious illnesses you
information is relevant to your life and useful develop, thanks to these advances.
for you. Be sure to explain how the information • Farmers use artificial selection to produce
applies to you personally and give examples. plants and animals with the most desirable
traits, which can make crops more resilient.
Access to cheaper and more abundant crops
can save you money on groceries in your
own life.
• UV light is a mutagen that damages DNA
by causing thymine mutations, which can
lead to cancer. You can reduce the chances
of damaging your DNA in your own life, by
applying sunscreen when in the sun.
3) Structure your essay as suggested below
State your question in the title.
• 1st section: Give an overview of the answer to your question.
• 2nd section: Provide the scientific details of the answer to your question. Be sure to select the
relevant information from class notes and the textbook.
• 3rd section: Make it personal. Explain why this information is relevant to your life or useful for
you and give examples.
Since you will be writing about science from a personal perspective, you can use personal
pronouns (I, we, you, etc.).
FIGURE 4.4. The basic writing prompt for college biology. From Harackiewicz, Canning,
Tibbetts, Priniski, and Hyde (2016).
The Utility‑Value Intervention 113
Instructor Version: Build Connections
We designed the Build Connections activity so that teachers could have an activity that
they could lead and be engaged in that would instigate similar psychological processes as
the original utility-value intervention. This allows teachers to be the scaffold for learn-
ing, and to leverage the social dynamics of the classroom. The Build Connections mate-
rials and support guide are freely available online (https://characterlab.org/activities/
build-connections-for-classrooms) and Figure 4.6 presents an example completed Build
Connections worksheet. By putting student interests and goals as the first activity, the
Build Connections activity is driven by instructors developing deeper relationships with
students. Once they get to know student interests better, and how students are trying to
make connections to the material, then instructors can be better equipped to support
students in connection making as new material is introduced.
Implementation Factors
There are five important implementation factors for utility-value intervention that have
received some level of empirical support in the research literature: participant responsive-
ness, timing, dosage, length, and choice/variety. However, each of these factors is in need
of additional testing across different contexts and student groups.
FIGURE 4.6. The Build Connections intervention exercise for middle school and high school
teachers. From Character Lab (2019). Reprinted with permission.
The Utility‑Value Intervention 115
Participant Responsiveness
Several studies show that how responsive students are to the intervention prompts is
related to student outcomes. Hulleman and Cordray (2009) found that high school sci-
ence and college psychology students who made more specific, personal connections in
their writing reported more utility value in the course than either students in the utility-
value condition who made fewer specific and personal connections, or those in the control
group. Nagengast et al. (2018) replicated this finding in their sample of ninth-grade math
students in Germany who were randomized to a utility-value or control condition. Har-
ackiewicz et al. (2016) found similar results in their introductory biology sample, such
that students who wrote longer essays benefited more from the intervention. Not only did
they find that students who made more personal and specific connections were likely to
perform better in the course but they also found that students who were more engaged
in the activity (as measured by the number of words written) benefited more from the
intervention. In addition, two studies (Gaspard et al., 2019; Hulleman & Cordray, 2009)
found that teacher engagement in the intervention also influences intervention outcomes.
Timing
The original thinking was that the utility-value intervention is best delivered at the mid-
way point of the course and beyond. Because not all topics are equally relevant or easy
to connect to students lives, waiting until midsemester allows students to have learned
enough course content so that they can make personally meaningful connections. How-
ever, we have subsequently learned that waiting that long has drawbacks, as some stu-
dents begin to disengage from the learning context early in the semester. In college
biology, early assignments are particularly effective for students with a history of poor
performance (Canning, Priniski, & Harackiewicz, 2019). As a result, we now implement
the intervention as early as the second or third week of the semester.
Dosage
The utility-value intervention is not a one-shot intervention. Rather, it can take repeated
experiences for students to appreciate the utility value of course material. Hulleman and
Cordray (2009) found that high school science teachers who offered the intervention
more times during the semester had better intervention outcomes than teachers who
offered it fewer times in the semester. In college science courses, it can be difficult for stu-
dents to write personal essays because the norms of scientific writing discourage the use
of personal pronouns, and multiple assignments provide opportunities for feedback and
practice reflecting on connections. For example, Canning et al. (2018) found that three
utility-value assignments were more effective than a single assignment. Anecdotally, we
have seen teachers try to implement a brief utility-value reflection once a week, with the
result that students quickly grew tired of the exercise.
Length
The amount of time one intervention implementation takes students varies based on a
host of student factors, including age and reading and writing skills, as well as design
116 I. Education
constraints of the classroom context. Interventions have ranged from brief homework
assignments that take as little as 5 minutes (average of 7 minutes in an online math class-
room in Rosenzweig, Hulleman, Barron, Kosovich, & Wigfield, 2019), 15- to 20-min-
ute in-class assignments (e.g., Kosovich et al., 2019), 90-minute sessions that include
a 45-minute lesson by an instructor followed by completion of an individual activity
(Gaspard et al., 2015), or homework assignments that take several days or weeks to com-
plete (Harackiewicz et al., 2016; Hulleman et al., 2010; Hulleman, Thoman, Dicke, &
Harackiewicz, 2017).
Choice/Variety
Choice and variety are known to have powerful effects on motivation and performance
(Patall, Cooper, & Wynn, 2010), but these features have only recently been tested in the
utility-value intervention paradigm. Utility-value assignments have differed in terms of
assignment type (i.e., essay, letter, quotes), but they can also differ in terms of assignment
structure (e.g., whether students are exposed to a variety of assignments, whether stu-
dents are given choices about which assignments to complete). These issues are relevant
to consider when there are multiple assignments across a semester. Of course, the utility-
value interventions are imbued with choice—students decide what topic to write about
and what connections to draw—but the experience of choice can be further enhanced
by giving students choices among assignment types. Indeed, many early studies gave stu-
dents choices (e.g., Hulleman & Harackiewicz, 2009). In the college biology context, we
found that higher levels of choice had positive effects on perceived utility value, interest,
and performance (Priniski et al., 2019; Rosenzweig, Harackiewicz, et al., 2019), and that
having a variety of utility-value assignments enhanced course performance (Priniski et
al., 2019).
NUANCES AND MISCONCEPTIONS
Content Matters
If we want students to be motivated within a specific learning context, then the connec-
tions students make need to be specific to the topics they are learning in that context.
This means the intervention needs to activate content that is being learned. This usu-
ally requires work with instructors to decide on the proper scaffolds, such as sample
quotes, and timing the intervention so that there has been enough content learned to
The Utility‑Value Intervention 117
make informed connections. College students and students who are more advanced in
the content area may need different scaffolds—and even less scaffolding—than students
who are younger and less skilled.
“You need to come in with the attitude that this is not a superfluous activity. If you
don’t buy in, then your students won’t buy in. You have to value it and make it
important. At the beginning, I wasn’t as sure what the [utility-value intervention]
was. I administered it as extra credit in my classes at first. But with a better under-
standing of the activities, I made it more of a mandatory part of the course. Now
every year, it’s just built into my grading.”
IMPLICATIONS FOR PRACTICE
Instructor Support
Online Materials
If instructors want to change what they do to help students make more connections, then
we need to provide them with ideas and activities that can help. The Build Connections
activity is one such activity that is designed for instructors to use with their students, and
is freely available online (Character Lab, 2019). There is also supporting documentation,
including implementation dos and don’ts (see Figure 4.7). If instructors are interested in
providing sample quotations for students, where will they get the quotes? One important
resource would be to build a repository of quotations with the help of practitioners, and
share them widely.
118 I. Education
FIGURE 4.7. Implementation tips for helping students find utility value in learning.
Training
We can make utility-value intervention materials available to instructors, but how will
they know what kinds of scaffolds their students need? Should they use the writing version,
the quotes version, or a combination? We need to offer online and in-person training on
how to make the design decisions needed to develop effective interventions, and meaning-
fully integrating activities into the curriculum without overburdening either instructors
or students. Ultimately, instructors will need to change some of their practices—beyond
simply implementing a utility-value intervention—in order to best help their students find
personal value and meaning in learning. Figure 4.8 shares some reflections of middle
school teachers after implementing the Build Connections activity with their students.
Other Institutional Opportunities
Although the utility-value intervention was designed to be used by classroom teachers,
there are other educators who could make use of the intervention or its variants. For
example, academic advisors could use a version of the intervention to learn more about
how students are thinking about the connection between the courses they are taking in
a semester and their major or career interests. College access counselors could use a ver-
sion of the intervention to help students make connections between the colleges they are
interested in and their future goals. We already have evidence that the Build Connections
activity is being used in both of these ways in high schools and community colleges.
The Utility‑Value Intervention 119
FIGURE 4.8. Teacher feedback on using Build Connections. Data from Hulleman, Hulleman,
et al. (2018).
IMPLICATIONS FOR PSYCHOLOGICAL THEORY
For the field of psychology, the utility-value intervention serves as an example of the
importance of taking a theoretical construct—one found to be predictive of learning out-
comes in many longitudinal, correlational studies—and building an intervention based
on that construct that is then tested in an applied context. Despite decades of research
on the role of motivation in education (Bransford, Brown, & Cocking, 2000), our ini-
tial studies (Hulleman et al., 2010; Hulleman & Harackiewicz, 2009) provided the first
experimental tests of an educational intervention designed to leverage perceptions of util-
ity value to increase student learning outcomes. Although many educators have based
practice on the tenets of expectancy-value theory, the lack of rigorously tested applica-
tions of the central constructs is distressing, particularly given the press for researchers
to provide explicit recommendations for practitioners based on their findings. In the
case of utility-value interventions, one implication of the correlational research was that
120 I. Education
instructors needed to emphasize the relevance of the learning content to students’ lives.
However, our early research demonstrated that communicating utility value directly can
lead to unintended negative consequences for some students (Durik, Hulleman, & Har-
ackiewicz, 2015), and that allowing students to generate their own connections is likely
to be much more effective, particularly for students at risk of academic underperfor-
mance. If psychology is to achieve its potential to positively impact society, researchers
will need to increasingly conduct applied tests of their theories in the real-world contexts
that they desire to improve so that their recommendations are based on causal evidence
rather than identifying psychological phenomena that merely co-occur with desired out-
comes (Hulleman & Barron, 2016).
Second, the original insight of the utility-value intervention is that students need to
discover value in the learning material that matters to them. This led to structuring the
intervention so that individual students have a choice about what aspects of the course
content to connect to their lives, and which aspects of their lives to connect to the course
content. This autonomy-supportive approach to operationalizing the construct of utility
value has led to the development of variations of the intervention intended to provide
more and different scaffolding so that students can make the most personal and specific
connection possible. This work has deepened the definition of what it means to find
learning content useful, and how perceiving utility value can lead to increased learning
outcomes. The original conceptualization of utility value was considered to be closer to
extrinsic motivation, whereas the operationalization of utility value in our intervention
is more autonomous. In this way, this operationalization of the utility-value intervention
has instigated a theoretical question about whether or not utility value needs to be identi-
fied as internal to an individual’s self-definition, or if utility value can be related to more
external motivations, such as wanting to obtain rewards or avoid punishments.
FUTURE DIRECTIONS
First, although the original intent of the intervention was to boost learning outcomes
for students in a specific course, due to the demonstrated efficacy of the intervention
it is now imperative to consider how these effects might translate into broader changes
in students’ educational pathways. However, this leads to the question of how do we
get from the perception of value in a very specific context to broader change? It is not
clear that we would necessarily expect an intervention that is so context specific to have
continued effects beyond that context unless further prompting occurred in the future.
Instead, it is more likely that the broader effects of the intervention occur by changing
immediate course outcomes—such as performance and interest—which then translate
into additional course taking and better performance in those courses. Indeed, many of
the intervention studies to date have found significant effects on performance in gateway
courses. Improved performance in these foundational courses may initiate recursive pro-
cesses through which students develop confidence and/or identification with the domain,
and then continue to perform better in other courses in the discipline (Cohen & Sherman,
2014; Hecht, Harackiewicz, et al., 2019). In addition, improved course performance
may open up new possibilities, such as research experiences and admission to advanced
courses that continue to foster engagement, performance, and persistence in the field
The Utility‑Value Intervention 121
(Hecht, Priniski, & Harackiewicz, 2019; Goyer et al., 2017). Although we reviewed some
of our initial insight into the pathway of broader effects in the “Empirical Evidence” sec-
tion, much work remains to follow students in intervention studies as they move through
their education pathways. We may gain insights that help us design variants of the utility-
value intervention that leverage initial reflections on utility value so that they have the
potential to carry over beyond the initial learning context.
Second, as we continue to learn about the role that perceptions of value play in moti-
vating students, emerging findings demonstrate that some types of value are more moti-
vating for some students than others (Diekman et al., 2010; Thoman, Brown, Mason,
Harmsen, & Smith, 2015), and that different learning contexts differentially provide the
opportunity to find different types of value. For example, Thoman and colleagues have
found that research labs in the biomedical sciences tend to highlight individual achieve-
ment and accomplishment as the goals of the work, which represent more independent and
agentic types of values. In contrast, students from traditionally underrepresented groups
in higher education tend to be motivated by careers that highlight more interdependent
and prosocial values, like helping others. Thoman et al. have found that this value mis-
match partially explains the reasons why students from underrepresented groups choose
not to persist in STEM majors, even when controlling for academic performance. These
findings encourage us to consider whether utility-value interventions might be more effec-
tive for underrepresented students if they were structured in a way so that students were
encouraged to make connections between the course content and their interdependent
goals (e.g., Yeager et al., 2014; Tibbetts, Harackiewicz, Priniski, & Canning, 2016), or if
having the choice would be more effective. In our ongoing community college math work,
we have developed interventions prompts that provide sample quotations from students
that emphasize both independent and interdependent motives, and we are examining
whether students’ backgrounds influence the type of connections they make.
Third, there are a variety of implementation factors that we have identified as impact-
ing the effectiveness of the utility-value intervention, including timing, dosage, length,
choice, and variety, among others. We have also reviewed different types and structures
of utility-value interventions that have evidence of effectiveness, including writing- and
quotations-based versions. However, very few studies have been designed as experiments
where participants are randomly assigned to different implementation factors or design
features of the utility-value intervention (cf. Priniski et al., 2019). Future studies also
need to examine whether any of these factors vary as a function of level of education,
student demographic characteristics, or other aspects of the learning context. How much
customization is necessary in order to implement an effective utility-value intervention in
a new context?
Fourth, as we transition the implementation of the utility-value intervention from
researchers to instructors and other educational practitioners, how do we adapt and test
these utility-value practices while still instigating the targeted psychological processes?
For example, although Build Connections was designed based on the efficacy evidence
from the utility-value intervention, we still need to test whether this variation works,
and whether it works without direct researcher involvement. Clearly, collaborating with
practitioners from beginning to end of the translation process will be essential, both to
provide supports for implementation and to capture learnings on what does and does not
work.
122 I. Education
REFERENCES
Aron, A., Aron, E. N., Tudor, M., & Nelson, G. (1991). Close relationships as including other in
the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60(2), 241–253.
Aronson, E. (1999). The power of self-persuasion. American Psychologist, 54(11), 875–884.
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Freeman.
Barron, K. E., & Hulleman, C. S. (2015). Expectancy-value-cost model of motivation. In J. D.
Wright (Ed.), International encyclopedia of the social and behavioral sciences (2nd ed., Vol.
8, pp. 503–509). Oxford, UK: Elsevier.
Bransford, J. D., Brown, A. L., & Cocking, R. R. (2000). How people learn (Vol. 11). Washington,
DC: National Academy Press.
Brown, E. R., Smith, J. L., Thoman, D. B., Allen, J., & Muragishi, G. (2015). From bench to bed-
side: A communal utility-value intervention to enhance students’ science motivation. Journal
of Educational Psychology, 107(4), 1116–1135.
Canning, E. A., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2015). Teach it, don’t preach it: The differential effects
of directly-communicated and self-generated utility-value information. Motivation Science,
1(1), 47–71.
Canning, E. A., Harackiewicz, J. M., Priniski, S. J., Hecht, C. A., Tibbetts, Y., & Hyde, J. S.
(2018). Improving performance and retention in introductory biology with a utility-value
intervention. Journal of Educational Psychology, 110, 834–849.
Canning, E. A., Priniski, S. J., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2019). Unintended consequences of framing
a utility-value intervention in two-year colleges. Learning and Instruction, 62, 37–48.
Character Lab. (2019). Build Connections for classrooms. Retrieved from https://characterlab.
org/activities/build-connections-for-classrooms.
Cohen, G. L., & Sherman, D. K. (2014). The psychology of change: Self-affirmation and social
psychological intervention. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 333–371.
Cordova, D. I., & Lepper, M. R. (1996). Intrinsic motivation and the process of learning: Benefi-
cial effects of contextualization, personalization, and choice. Journal of Educational Psy-
chology, 88(4), 715–730.
deCharms, R. (1968). Personal causation. New York: Academic Press.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Self-determination. New York: Wiley.
Dewey, J. (1913). Interest and effort in education. Cambridge, MA: Riverside Press.
Diekman, A. B., Brown, E. R., Johnston, A. M., & Clark, E. K. (2010). Seeking congruity between
goals and roles: A new look at why women opt out of science, technology, engineering, and
mathematics careers. Psychological Science, 21, 1051–1057.
Durik, A. M., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2007). Different strokes for different folks: How personal
interest moderates the effects of situational factors on task interest. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 99, 597–610.
Durik, A. M., Hulleman, C. S., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2015). One size fits some: Instructional
enhancements to promote interest don’t work the same for everyone. In K. A. Renninger,
M. Nieswandt, & S. Hidi (Eds.), Interest in mathematics and science learning (pp. 49–62).
Washington, DC: American Educational Research Association.
Durik, A. M., Shechter, O., Noh, M. S., Rozek, C. S., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2015). What if I
can’t?: Perceived competence as a moderator of the effects of utility value information on situ-
ational interest and performance. Motivation and Emotion, 39, 104–118.
Eccles, J., Adler, T. F., Futterman, R., Goff, S. B., Kaczala, C. M., Meece, J. L., & Midgley, C.
(1983). Expectancies, values, and academic behaviors. In J. T. Spence (Ed.), Achievement and
achievement motives: Psychological and sociological approaches (pp. 75–146). San Fran-
cisco: Freeman.
Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, values, and goals. Annual Review of
Psychology, 53, 109–132.
The Utility‑Value Intervention 123
Gaspard, H., Dicke, A.-L., Flunger, B., Brisson, B. M., Häfner, I., Nagengast, B., & Trautwein, U.
(2015). Fostering adolescents’ value beliefs for mathematics with a relevance intervention in
the classroom. Developmental Psychology, 51(9), 1226–1240.
Gaspard, H., Parrisius, C., Kleinhansl, M., Piesch, H., Wille, E., Nagengast, B., . . . Hulleman,
C. S. (2019, June). Implementing utility-value interventions in mathematics classrooms: The
role of implementation fidelity. Presentation to the Curry School of Education, Charlottes-
ville, VA.
Goyer, J. P., Garcia, J., Purdie-Vaughns, V., Binning, K. R., Cook, J. E., Reeves, S. L., . . ., Cohen,
G. L. (2017). Self-affirmation facilitates minority middle schoolers’ progress along college
trajectories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 114(29), 7594–
7599.
Harackiewicz, J. M., Canning, E. A., Tibbetts, Y., Priniski, S. J., & Hyde, J. S. (2016). Closing
achievement gaps with a utility-value intervention: Disentangling race and social class. Jour-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111, 745–765.
Harackiewicz, J. M., Hulleman, C. S., & Pastor, D. A. (2009, August). Developmental trajectories
of interest within semester-long courses in high school science and introductory psychol-
ogy. Paper presented at the European Association for Research on Learning and Instruction
(EARLI) Biennial Conference, Munich, Germany.
Harackiewicz, J. M., & Priniski, S. J. (2018). Improving student outcomes in higher education:
The science of targeted intervention. Annual Review of Psychology, 69, 409–435.
Harackiewicz, J. M., Rozek, C. S., Hulleman, C. S., & Hyde, J. S. (2012). Helping parents to moti-
vate adolescents in mathematics and science: An experimental test of a utility-value interven-
tion. Psychological Science, 43, 899–906.
Harackiewicz, J. M., & Sansone, C. (1991). Goals and intrinsic motivation: You can get there from
here. In M. L. Maehr & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol.
7, pp. 21–49). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Hecht, C. A., Harackiewicz, J. M., Priniski, S. J., Canning, E. A., Tibbetts, Y., & Hyde, J. S.
(2019). Promoting persistence in the biological and medical sciences: An expectancy-value
approach to intervention. Journal of Educational Psychology, 111(8), 1462–1477.
Hecht, C. A., Priniski, S. J., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2019). Understanding long-term effects of
motivation interventions in a changing world. In E. Gonida & M. Lemos (Eds.), Advances
in motivation and achievement: Vol. 20. Motivation in education at a time of global change:
Theory, research, and implications for practice (pp. 81–98). Bingley, UK: Emerald Group.
Hidi, S., & Renninger, K. A. (2006). The four-phase model of interest development. Educational
Psychologist, 41, 111–127.
Hulleman, C. S. (2007). The role of utility value in the development of interest and achievement.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Wisconsin–Madison, Madison, WI.
Hulleman, C. S., & Barron, K. E. (2013, April). Teacher perceptions of student motivational
challenges and best strategies to enhance motivation. Paper presented in J. Turner (Chair),
Bridging the theory–practice divide: Teacher approaches to motivating students. Symposium
presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San
Francisco, CA.
Hulleman, C. S., & Barron, K. E. (2016). Motivation interventions in education: Bridging theory,
research, and practice. In L. Corno & E. M. Anderman (Eds.), Handbook of educational
psychology (3rd ed., pp. 160–171). New York: Routledge, Taylor & Francis.
Hulleman, C. S., & Cordray, D. S. (2009). Moving from the lab to the field: The role of fidelity
and achieved relative intervention strength. Journal of Research on Educational Effective-
ness, 2, 88–110.
Hulleman, C. S., Godes, O., Hendricks, B., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2010). Enhancing interest and
performance with a utility-value intervention. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(4),
880–895.
124 I. Education
Hulleman, C. S., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2009). Promoting interest and performance in high
school science classes. Science, 326, 1410–1412.
Hulleman, C. S., Hulleman, T. K., Wormington, S. V., Vines, E., Foran, M., & Clayton, K. (2018).
Build Connections: A collaboration to improve a teacher led utility-value intervention.
Charlottesville, VA: Motivate Lab.
Hulleman, C. S., Kosovich, J. J., Barron, K. E., & Daniel, D. (2017). Making connections: Repli-
cating and extending the utility-value intervention in the classroom. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 109(3), 387–404.
Hulleman, C. S., Thoman, D. B., Dicke, A., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2017). The promotion and
development of interest: The importance of perceived values. In P. O’Keefe & J. M. Harack-
iewicz (Eds.), The science of interest (pp. 189–208). New York: Springer.
Hulleman, C. S., Wormington, S. V., Tibbetts, C. Y., & Philipoom, M. (2018, August). A meta-
analytic synthesis of utility-value interventions in education. Paper presented at the bi-annual
meeting of the International Conference on Motivation, Aarhus, Denmark.
Kizilcec, R. F., & Cohen, G. L. (2017). Eight-minute self-regulation intervention raises educational
attainment at scale in individualist but not collectivist cultures. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences of the USA, 114(17), 4348–4353.
Kosovich, J. J., Hulleman, C. S., Phelps, J., & Lee, M. (2019). Improving algebra success with a
utility-value intervention. Journal of Developmental Education, 42(2).
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2002). Building a practically useful theory of goal setting and task
motivation: A 35-year odyssey. American Psychologist, 57(9), 705–717.
Nagengast, B., Brisson, B. M., Hulleman, C. S., Gaspard, H., Häfner, I., & Trautwein, U. (2018).
Learning more from educational interventions studies: Estimating complier average causal
effects in a relevance intervention. Journal of Experimental Education, 86, 105–123.
Patall, E. A., Cooper, H., & Wynn, S. R. (2010). The effectiveness and relative importance of
choice in the classroom. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(4), 896–915.
Priniski, S. J., Rosenzweig, E. Q., Canning, E. A., Hecht, C. A., Tibbetts, Y., Hyde, J. S., &
Harackiewicz, J. M. (2019). The benefits of combining value for the self and others in utility-
value interventions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 111(8), 1478–1497.
Rosenzweig, E. Q., Harackiewicz, J. M., Priniski, S. J., Hecht, C. A., Canning, E. A., Tibbetts, Y.,
& Hyde, J. S. (2019). Choose your own intervention: Using choice to enhance the effective-
ness of a utility-value intervention. Motivation Science, 5(3), 269–276.
Rosenzweig, E. Q., Hulleman, C. S., Barron, K. E., Kosovich, J. J., & Wigfield, A. (2019). Making
math matter: Adapting utility-value interventions for online math courses. Journal of Experi-
mental Education, 87(2), 332–352.
Rosenzweig, E. Q., Wigfield, A., & Hulleman, C. S. (2020). More useful or not so bad?: Examin-
ing the effects of utility value and cost reduction interventions in college physics. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 112(1), 166–182.
Rozek, C. S., Hyde, J. S., Svoboda, R. C., Hulleman, C. S., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2014). Gender
differences in the effects of a utility-value intervention to help parents motivate adolescents in
mathematics and science. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(1), 195–206.
Rozek, C. S., Svoboda, R. C., Harackiewicz, J. M., Hulleman, C. S., & Hyde, J. S. (2017). Utility-
value intervention with parents increases students’ STEM preparation and career pursuit.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 114, 909–914.
Stephens, N. M., Hamedani, M. G., & Destin, M. (2014). Closing the social-class achievement
gap: A difference-education intervention improves first-generation students’ academic perfor-
mance and all students’ college transition. Psychological Science, 25(4), 943–953.
Thoman, D. B., Brown, E. R., Mason, A. Z., Harmsen, A. G., & Smith, J. L. (2015). The role of
altruistic values in motivating underrepresented minority students for biomedicine. BioSci-
ence, 65, 183–188.
The Utility‑Value Intervention 125
Tibbetts, Y., Harackiewicz, J. M., Priniski, S. J., & Canning, E. A. (2016). Broadening participa-
tion in the life sciences with social–psychological interventions. CBE-Life Sciences Educa-
tion, 15(3), es4.
Walton, G. M., & Wilson, T. D. (2018). Wise interventions: Psychological remedies for social and
personal problems. Psychological Review, 125(5), 617.
Wigfield, A., Rosenzweig, E. Q., & Eccles, J. S. (2017). Achievement values. In A. Elliot, C.
Dweck, & D. Yeager (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation: Theory and applica-
tion (pp. 116–134). New York: Guilford Press.
Yeager, D. S., Hanselman, P., Walton, G. M., Murray, J., Crosnoe, R., Muller, C., . . . Dweck, C. S.
(2019). A national experiment reveals where a growth mindset improves adolescent achieve-
ment. Nature, 573(7774), 364–369.
Yeager, D. S., Henderson, M. D., Paunesku, D., Walton, G. M., D’Mello, S., Spitzer, B. J., & Duck-
worth, A. L. (2014). Boring but important: A self-transcendent purpose for learning fosters
academic self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(4), 559–580.
C H A P TE R 5
Difference‑Education
Improving Disadvantaged Students’ Academic
Outcomes by Changing Their Theory of Difference
Nicole M. Stephens, MarYam G. Hamedani,
and Sarah S. M. Townsend
126
Difference‑Education 127
stories, intervention participants learn that social group differences are contextual—that
is, they are a product of students’ diverse life experiences and backgrounds. This critical
insight can help students understand that their different backgrounds are not a deficiency
that holds them back but rather can be a source of strength that they can leverage to fit in
with the college community and find their own path to success. As described in this chap-
ter, we designed the first wave of difference-education interventions to examine whether
it was possible to use this contextual understanding of social group difference to improve
the academic outcomes of first-generation college students (Stephens, Hamedani, & Des-
tin, 2014; Townsend, Stephens, Smallets, & Hamedani, 2018).
BACKGROUND
Even after first-generation college students (i.e., students who do not have parents with
4-year college degrees) defy the odds to gain admission to college, they still confront
obstacles to success that continuing-generation students (i.e., students who have at least
one parent with a 4-year college degree) face less often. Beyond having fewer economic
resources and academic skills (e.g., Horn & Nuñez, 2000; Terenzini, Springer, Yaeger,
Pascarella, & Nora, 1996), first-generation students also confront an additional set of
psychological or cultural obstacles, such as a lack of fit or empowerment (Astin & Ose-
guera, 2004; Goudeau & Croizet, 2017; Sirin, 2005). We use the term fit to refer to the
feeling of being accepted, recognized, welcomed, and included within a setting, such as
the college community (e.g., Stephens, Brannon, Markus, & Nelson, 2015).1 We use the
term empowerment to refer to (1) the psychological experience of efficacy, ownership,
and control; and (2) the resulting willingness to enact the strategies needed to make the
most of one’s experience. Together these obstacles fuel a persistent social class achieve-
ment gap, such that first-generation students earn lower grades and more often drop out
compared to continuing-generation students (Astin & Oseguera, 2004; Sirin, 2005).
One especially important cultural obstacle is the mismatch between the largely
middle-class culture of higher education and the working-class beliefs, norms, and values
that many first-generation students bring with them to college (e.g., Stephens, Fryberg,
Markus, Johnson, & Covarrubias, 2012). For example, many colleges and universities
expect students to pave their own path and express their personal opinions (Fryberg
& Markus, 2007), while many working-class students are instead socialized to follow
the rules and adhere to socially accepted norms of behavior (e.g., Kohn, 1969). First-
generation students are guided by more interdependent norms and may not feel fully
comfortable expressing their ideas or opinions in class, or attending office hours to ask a
professor for clarification on an assignment or to explain a grade. This cultural mismatch
can lead first-generation students to feel excluded from these educational settings and
lead them to question whether they fit or belong in college (e.g., Covarrubias & Fryberg,
2015; Ostrove & Long, 2007; Walton & Cohen, 2007). First-generation students can
1 We use the term social fit, rather than belonging, to emphasize the relationship between a person’s
psychological experience and its congruence with a particular context, setting, or environment. This
term derives from decades of prior work on cultural fit, as well as person–environment, person–culture,
or person–organization fit (e.g., Chatman, 1991; Dawis, 1992; Edwards, Cable, Williamson, Lambert,
& Shipp, 2006; Fulmer et al., 2010).
128 I. Education
also be unfamiliar with the “rules of the game” that lead to success in higher education,
such as how to relate to professors or how to choose a major. This lack of familiarity
can undermine their sense of empowerment and efficacy (e.g., Housel & Harvey, 2010;
Reay, Crozier, & Clayton, 2009). These obstacles can systematically disadvantage first-
generation students and further fuel the social class achievement gap.
Intellectual History
In the first wave of studies, difference-education was designed to reduce the social class
achievement gap by helping first-generation students gain a sense of fit and empowerment
in college. Difference-education builds on the wise intervention literature in social psy-
chology by using the methodological strategy of changing students’ construal or under-
standing of their experience (Walton & Wilson, 2018)—in this case, their experience of
difference. This research area leverages the strategy of changing students’ lay theories
about ability and achievement to foster success in school (e.g., Wilson, 2011). A lay the-
ory is a set of fundamental assumptions about the nature of the self and social world that
shapes how people interpret and respond to their experiences (Molden & Dweck, 2006).
For example, one popular lay theory intervention provides an understanding that intel-
ligence or ability is malleable and can change over time (e.g., Blackwell, Trzesniewski, &
Dweck, 2007). Likewise, we theorize that changing students’ lay theories about social
group differences—in particular, teaching them a contextual theory of difference—can
improve their academic outcomes.
To explain why a contextual theory would improve disadvantaged2 students’ academic
outcomes, we drew from diverse literatures in psychology and education that typically do
not “talk” to one another. Research in social psychology, on the one hand, has identified
two prominent theories of difference—an essentialist theory and a contextual theory—and
examined their consequences for intergroup relations. This literature does not examine the
effects of a contextual theory on academic outcomes, but instead focuses on its effects on
intergroup outcomes. Research in education, on the other hand, theorizes about the aca-
demic benefits of teaching students about social group differences, but does not directly
isolate or test the causal effects of a contextual theory on these academic outcomes. As such,
both literatures provide complementary evidence for our theory, but do not formulate nor
test it directly. Below, drawing on the social psychology literature, we first provide an over-
view of these two lay theories and explain how they can shape the meaning of social group
difference and affect intergroup outcomes. Second, we draw on the education literature to
provide an overview of research in multicultural and social justice education, which sup-
ports the claim that a contextual theory can benefit disadvantaged students academically.
Research in social psychology identifies the meanings and consequences of essen-
tialist versus contextual theories of social group difference for intergroup outcomes. An
essentialist theory defines social group differences as biologically rooted and therefore
relatively fixed or unchanging essences or attributes of individual people or social groups
2 The term disadvantaged refers to students who are typically disadvantaged by the mainstream culture
of higher education—t ypically students who are first-generation, low-income, and/or underrepresented
racial or ethnic minorities. This term is meant to highlight how institutional environments can nega-
tively impact students’ learning experiences and chances for success, not that their backgrounds them-
selves are deficits.
Difference‑Education 129
(Gelman, 2004; Haslam, Rothschild, & Ernst, 2000; Prentice & Miller, 2007). In the
United States, the essentialist theory of social group differences is the most common way
of understanding difference (e.g., Markus, 2008).
Throughout history, an essentialist theory of social group difference has been used
to stigmatize social group differences and to downwardly constitute certain groups as
deficient or inferior (Markus, 2008; Markus & Moya, 2010). This essential view of dif-
ference means that a social group’s differences are likely to be interpreted as an indication
of their lower value in society, and this rationale can be used to justify their subjugation
and mistreatment. For example, if working-class Americans were to receive lower scores
than middle-class Americans on an intelligence test, an essentialist theory would suggest
that this difference is an indication of working-class people’s inherent inferiority, which
could be used to rationalize the act of withholding educational resources from them.
Indeed, throughout history, this logic has been used to justify prejudice and discrimina-
tion and to exclude, oppress, and withhold opportunities from a wide range of social
groups. It has been used to justify the genocide of Native Americans, the enslavement
of African Americans, and a wide range of discriminatory policies that excluded immi-
grants throughout U.S. history (e.g., Markus & Moya, 2010; Omi & Winant, 2015). As a
result, an essentialist theory of difference links social group differences with largely nega-
tive meanings and stigma, and shapes people’s experiences accordingly. When people use
this essentialist theory to make sense of social group differences, they often experience
differences as a threat, a source of deficiency, or a weakness.
An essentialist perspective contrasts sharply with another way of understanding
social group difference: a contextual theory. Well represented in the academy and among
the social sciences, this theory asserts that social group differences are not essential
but instead socially constructed (e.g., Markus, 2008; Markus & Moya, 2010; Omi &
Winant, 2015). According to this theory, social group differences come from the nor-
mal process of participating in, adapting, and responding to the diverse sociocultural
contexts that people inhabit throughout their lives.3 Further, considering the contextual
origins of difference, this theory implies that differences can change over time with new
contextual experiences (see Markus, 2008, for related arguments). The contextual view
means that social group differences are normal—that is, they stem from people’s different
life experiences, rather than some kind of inherent inferiority. As such, these differences
can no longer be used to justify the mistreatment of those who are different. For example,
if working-class Americans were to receive lower scores than middle-class Americans on
an intelligence test, a contextual theory suggests that this difference is an indication of
working-class students’ disparate levels of preparation or opportunities in schools. This
interpretation could then be used to rationalize the act of giving working-class students
more resources to develop their skills and abilities. As a result, a contextual theory des-
tigmatizes and normalizes the experience of difference, and shapes people’s experiences
accordingly. Therefore, when people use a contextual theory to make sense of social
group differences, they can experience differences positively, as an asset or strength.
Consistent with this idea, research in psychology suggests that exposure to a con-
textual theory can improve the quality of intergroup relations. Specifically, research has
3 The term sociocultural context refers to a socially and historically constructed environment that con-
tains a set of culture-specific ideas, practices, and institutions (Markus & Hamedani, 2019; Stephens,
Markus, & Fryberg, 2012).
130 I. Education
shown that a contextual theory compared with an essentialist theory leads to decreased
stereotyping and race-based categorization (Bastian & Haslam, 2006; Chao, Hong, &
Chiu, 2013; Keller, 2005); more identification and perceived similarity across different
groups (No et al., 2008); and higher levels of intergroup trust and desire for intergroup
contact (Kung et al., 2018; Williams & Eberhardt, 2008). For example, when confronted
with a social problem relevant to race, Williams and Eberhardt found that exposing
diverse participants to the idea that race is socially constructed led them to report greater
emotional engagement (based on a self-report measure of their mood) than those who
were exposed to the idea that race is essential.
Second, research in education—particularly work on multicultural and social justice
education—suggests that learning about the contextual nature of social group difference
has the potential to foster disadvantaged students’ academic engagement and improve
their performance in school (e.g., grades). Most of this interdisciplinary research in edu-
cation is not designed to isolate or test the causal effects of a contextual theory in the way
that experimental social psychologists would. Instead, the research tends to examine the
effects of multidimensional educational experiences, which typically include a contextual
theory as one part of many different ideas and practices. Although this research area does
not provide direct causal evidence of the benefits of a contextual theory, it suggests that
a contextual theory has the potential to benefit disadvantaged students.
Ethnic studies courses are one example of multicultural and social justice educa-
tion. They encourage students to view social groups, including their own, as socially and
historically situated, and highlight how contextual factors, such as institutions, policies,
and practices, can shape students’ experiences and life outcomes. Research examining
the effects of ethnic studies courses demonstrates that these learning experiences are
correlated with a variety of academic benefits (see Sleeter, 2011, for a review; see also
Bowman, 2010; Cole, Case, Rios, & Curtin, 2011; Denson, 2009; Nelson Laird, 2005).
These courses are often associated with improved academic performance and empower-
ment of disadvantaged students (e.g., Cabrera, Milem, Jaquette, & Marx, 2014; Sleeter,
2011). For example, one study of San Francisco high schools suggested that taking an
ethnic studies course in ninth grade led to an increase in ninth-grade attendance, grade-
point average (GPA), and credits earned (Dee & Penner, 2016).
Taken together, research from both psychology and education suggests that provid-
ing disadvantaged students with a contextual theory of difference could help destigmatize
social group differences and therefore help students understand their meanings in a new,
more positive way. That new understanding, in turn, could help improve their engagement
and performance in school (e.g., grades). In sum, research in psychology helps explain
why a contextual theory is likely to yield positive effects relative to an essentialist theory,
and education research provides initial evidence to support the idea that more positive
responses are likely to translate into improved academic outcomes.
PSYCHOLOGICAL PROCESSES
A contextual theory of difference has the potential to change how students make sense
of and respond to their own and others’ social group differences. Indeed, upon entering
college, students often experience challenges or obstacles associated with social group
differences. Research in psychology and education demonstrates the college environment
Difference‑Education 131
often fails to foster a sense of fit and empowerment for first-generation students (e.g.,
Ostrove & Long, 2007; Stephens, Brannon, et al., 2015). Illustrating a lack of fit, a first-
generation student at Vanderbilt University recounted, “Never before had I truly felt such
an extreme sense of estrangement and alienation. . . . I quickly realized that although I
may look the part, my cultural and socioeconomic backgrounds were vastly different
from those of my predominantly White, affluent peers. I wanted to leave” (Riggs, 2014,
para. 2). Describing the experience of feeling disempowered upon entering college, one
first-generation student at Cornell University said, “I had no road map for what I was
supposed to do once I made it to campus. . . . Aside from a check-in with my financial aid
officer . . . I was mostly keeping to myself to hide the fact that I was a very special kind
of lost” (Capó Crucet, 2015, para. 15).
A contextual theory of difference can help first-generation students understand their
social group differences in a new, more positive light—as a strength, resource, or asset,
rather than as a stigma, weakness, or deficiency. Specifically, as shown in Figure 5.1,
viewing differences as contextual should change the meaning of difference and help first-
generation students gain a sense of fit and empowerment.
Learning that difference is contextual can decrease the stigma attached to difference,
and therefore foster disadvantaged students’ sense of fit in the college setting. Provid-
ing a contextual theory of difference should increase students’ sense of fit by conveying
that their challenges are informed by their backgrounds and prior experiences, rather
than indicating their deficiency as individuals. In other words, the contextual theory
should convey the message that “differences are a normal part of the college experience.”
For example, a first-generation student should understand that having parents without
a college education means that it will likely be harder to get advice from one’s parents
about college (e.g., how to choose a major), and they may need to go to a trusted advisor
instead. Accordingly, students should then gain a sense of fit from learning that “my dif-
ferences will not prevent me from finding my path.”
Likewise, learning that difference is contextual can imply that one’s experiences of
difference are malleable rather than fixed, and therefore foster disadvantaged students’
sense of empowerment in college. Providing a contextual theory should help students
better understand that their challenges are due to experiences in different contexts (e.g.,
they attended a less rigorous high school), rather than some individual deficiency. In
other words, the contextual theory should convey that “differences are malleable” and
can be changed in the future. For example, when confronting background-specific chal-
lenges, a first-generation student should understand that struggling in one’s classes could
be a result of having less preparation in a low-quality high school, rather than less ability
to succeed. This should help students feel more efficacious and in control, and also foster
their willingness to take full advantage of the resources available to them. Accordingly,
students should then gain a sense of empowerment from learning that “I can overcome
my challenges and be successful.”
EMPIRICAL EVIDENCE
Outcomes
In an initial set of studies conducted at selective colleges, we delivered and tested difference-
education in an effort to improve the academic outcomes of incoming first-generation col-
lege students. These interventions exposed students to a contextual theory of social group
difference during the transition from high school to college by sharing contrasting stories
of senior college students from diverse social class backgrounds. The systematic varia-
tion in these stories showed intervention participants vivid examples of how their prior
social class contexts could inform their experiences in college. As of the publication of
this chapter, the intervention has been delivered in two ways: (1) through a student panel
that intervention participants attended in person (Stephens et al., 2014), and (2) through a
scalable version that intervention participants completed online (Townsend et al., 2018).
Both of these interventions were delivered early on in students’ first quarter or semes-
ter (see Yeager & Walton, 2011). We examined the outcomes of difference-education
by following intervention participants throughout their time in college, obtaining their
official grades from the registrar’s office, and conducting a series of surveys and an in-
person laboratory study (Stephens et al., 2014; Stephens, Townsend, Hamedani, Destin,
& Manzo, 2015; Townsend et al., 2018).
Across both the in-person and online interventions, the primary goal was to improve
first-generation college students’ academic outcomes. First, we examined the impact of
the intervention on students’ grades. For the in-person intervention, an analysis of stu-
dents’ official first-year grades revealed that first-generation students in the difference-
education condition earned higher GPAs during their first years in college than their
counterparts in the control condition (Stephens et al., 2014), thereby reducing the social
class achievement gap by 63%. In the scalable version in which students read older stu-
dents’ stories online, we examined students’ GPAs during the first 2 years of college. This
study showed that difference-education improved first-generation students’ cumulative
GPA throughout their first 2 years of college (Townsend et al., 2018).
We also examined whether students learned the contextual theory of difference that
the intervention sought to convey. If the intervention communicated the theory effec-
tively, we reasoned that participants should be able to articulate some of the insights they
gained. They should also be able to use the theory to make sense of and respond to social
group differences in a more positive way. We explored both of these possibilities.
Difference‑Education 133
First, we asked participants to write about what they learned from the student stories
that they had listened to in the panel or read about online. Based on our coding of themes
that emerged in participants’ open-ended responses, we found evidence consistent with
the idea that participants learned a contextual theory of difference. Specifically, both
first- and continuing-generation participants in the difference-education condition com-
pared to the control were far more likely to report that they had learned about how stu-
dents’ different backgrounds can matter in college (Stephens et al., 2014; Townsend et al.,
2018). For example, illustrating her understanding of how someone’s background shapes
the college experience, one intervention participant said, “People from different back-
grounds have different expectations of college.” Demonstrating a similar understanding,
another intervention participant said, “Everyone comes from such a different background
and has different motives for doing well.”
Second, we examined whether participants were able to use the theory to make sense
of and respond to social group differences in a more positive way. Consistent with the
idea that learning a contextual theory of difference conveys a relatively positive mean-
ing of difference, we found that participants in the difference-education condition com-
pared to the control condition reported greater appreciation of difference (e.g., valuing
diversity as part of college; Stephens et al., 2014). Furthermore, at the end of the second
year in college, we also examined participants’ comfort with social group difference in
the course of their interactions with other college students (see Stephens, Townsend, et
al., 2015, for detailed methods). Specifically, we asked participants from the in-person
intervention study to deliver a speech to a college student peer (i.e., a research assistant),
focusing on the topic of how their backgrounds could matter in college, and to complete
a series of stressful academic tasks. We then coded the content of the speech and also
obtained participants’ physiological responses. In the course of the speech, difference-
education (vs. control) participants showed a greater willingness to talk about the impact
of their different background contexts (e.g., family and friends from home), and also were
more comfortable while doing so, as indicated by their physiological responses (Stephens,
Townsend, et al., 2015). This finding suggests that difference-education encouraged par-
ticipants to understand and respond to difference in a new, more positive way compared
to participants in the control condition.
Mechanism
We theorized that the difference-education intervention improves students’ academic per-
formance (e.g., GPA) by helping them to understand that their social group differences
are contextual in nature, rather than essential features of people or social groups. Specifi-
cally, as explained previously, we theorize that learning a contextual theory of difference
should help students to experience not only a greater sense of fit as part of the college
community but also a greater sense of empowerment to more fully take advantage of
campus resources (see Figure 5.1).
Broadly consistent with our theorizing, we found that difference-education increased
both fit and empowerment across the in-person and online interventions. In the in-person
study, we found that both first- and continuing-generation students in the difference-
education condition reported greater fit than those in the control condition. In the online
study, however, we found that greater fit only emerged among first-generation students
in the difference-education versus control condition.
134 I. Education
Effects over Time
As described above, we observed benefits for students’ academic outcomes that persisted
throughout the first 2 years in college in the online difference-education intervention
(Townsend et al., 2018). We are currently examining whether the grades benefits persist
throughout the duration of college in both interventions. We theorize that these aca-
demic benefits will persist throughout college because of the ways in which a contextual
theory initiates a positive, self-reinforcing cycle of experiential and behavioral change
(e.g., Miller, Dannals, & Zlatev, 2017; Yeager & Walton, 2011). For example, when a
first-generation student confronts a background-specific challenge (e.g., difficulty choos-
ing a major), a contextual theory can help her change how she makes sense of her dif-
ferent experiences in college. Instead of interpreting this challenge as an indication of a
fixed deficiency, she can view it as a normal part of coming from her background (e.g.,
not having college-educated parents to advise her). This interpretation should help foster
a sense of fit with the college community. Moreover, using a contextual theory to make
sense of her experience should convey that her challenge is not fixed, and that there
are steps that she can take to overcome this background-specific challenge and improve
her outcomes in college. This interpretation should help foster a sense of empowerment.
In turn, these psychological experiences of fit and empowerment should foster positive
behavioral changes that have a number of downstream benefits. For example, this stu-
dent may be more likely to seek help or develop relationships with teachers, which can
amplify her psychological experiences of fit and empowerment and also improve her
academic performance.
Heterogeneity
Designing an effective psychological intervention requires considering a number of con-
textual and individual factors. For any psychological intervention to have a positive
Difference‑Education 135
impact, the people targeted must first have the opportunity (i.e., enough resources or
skills) to achieve better outcomes. For example, college students must have enough food
and reliable housing to be able to focus on academic pursuits, and they must also have
the necessary academic skills so that increased academic effort (e.g., spending more time
studying) will translate into better academic outcomes (e.g., Stephens, Markus, et al.,
2012). In the case of difference-education, we theorize that providing a contextual theory
improves students’ academic performance by fostering a greater sense of fit and empow-
erment. According to this logic, this intervention should be most effective when psycho-
logical obstacles play a role in undermining students’ performance. In other words, the
intervention should be most helpful when people already have a foundation of material
resources and academic skills, but are not performing up to their potential because of a
lack of fit or empowerment.
Beyond giving students the opportunity to succeed, it is also important to consider
the different academic contexts and groups with which the intervention can have the
most impact. There is nothing about difference-education that limits it to the context of
higher education, nor to the particular focus on first-generation college students. Though
future research should explore potential boundary conditions, we expect that difference-
education would be effective across different settings and with different social groups
(e.g., women or racial or ethnic minorities). The general methodological strategy of
difference-education should be the same: the intervention would seek to change people’s
experiences, behaviors, and outcomes by providing a contextual theory of difference.
However, as is the case in any intervention, context matters, and it is critically important
to take both the local context and the particular social group into account. In practice,
researchers and practitioners must work to understand the existing views and challenges
that a particular disadvantaged group faces in a setting, and tailor intervention methods
and content to address those particular views, challenges, and identity-based concerns.
In the case of different social groups, an intervention designed to address race or
gender (vs. social class) would need to take into account how people’s existing views
about race and gender may differ from those about social class. Because existing views
about race or gender (vs. social class) in the United States are likely more well-defined or
more essentialized, and thus more difficult to change (e.g., Destin, Rheinschmidt-Same,
& Richeson, 2017), an intervention that seeks to provide a contextual theory about racial
or gender differences would need to be tailored to counteract people’s existing narratives
about race or gender (e.g., as biological features of people). Moreover, since discussions
about race and gender (vs. social class) may also be more fraught, these discussions may
require additional steps to render them inclusive and empowering, rather than threaten-
ing. For example, ethnic studies courses rely on highly trained instructors to talk about
these difficult topics, and they typically engage students in these conversations over an
extended period of time (e.g., a semester).
Future research should also examine in what kinds of social contexts and with whom
the intervention is most likely to be effective. Because we theorize that a difference-
education intervention improves students’ outcomes by providing a contextual theory of
social group difference, we expect that the intervention could be effective in any setting
where a contextual theory would represent a significant change in how people would
otherwise make sense of their experiences. Specifically, we expect that a difference-
education intervention would be far more effective in contexts where essentialist theories
are the default understanding of social group difference than in contexts where people
136 I. Education
are already well versed in a contextual theory. For example, in a given college or univer-
sity, it is important to consider what diversity efforts are already being implemented. If
students have previously engaged in various discussions about the contextual nature of
difference (e.g., through an orientation program, a seminar, or an ethnic studies course),
they are less likely to benefit from a difference-education intervention.
COUSINS
Difference-education focuses on changing how students construe the meaning and nature
of social group differences, which in turn helps improve their academic experience and
performance. This focus on changing how people construe their experiences is similar to
many other approaches in the wise intervention literature (Dittmann & Stephens, 2017;
Walton & Wilson, 2018). Among these interventions, difference-education is perhaps
most similar to belongingness interventions in terms of the specific methods used to
deliver the message (e.g., Walton & Cohen, 2007). Belongingness interventions seek to
change construal by having intervention participants learn from the stories of other suc-
cessful students at their university. Similar to these methods, difference-education inter-
ventions also seek to change construal by exposing intervention participants to student
success stories. Belongingness and difference-education diverge, however, in the content
of the student stories and thus the nature of the particular lay theory that they provide.
Belongingness interventions focus on changing students’ lay theory of adversity by signal-
ing that these experiences are shared in common with other students and will improve
with time (e.g., Walton & Cohen, 2007). In contrast, difference-education focuses on
changing students’ lay theory of social group difference by helping them to understand it
as contextual. Compared to belongingness, difference-education therefore more explicitly
shows how students’ different backgrounds shape their different experiences in college.
Beyond the methodological similarity to other lay theory interventions in social psy-
chology, the key message in difference-education is most similar to Gurin’s work on inter-
group dialogue learning experiences (Gurin & Nagda, 2006; Gurin, Nagda, & Zúñiga,
2013).
Intergroup dialogues are weekly discussion sessions in which students from diverse
backgrounds meet in groups and talk about their commonalities and differences. They
are encouraged to critically reflect with other students about the source, nature, and
impact of social group differences (Gurin & Nagda, 2006; Gurin et al., 2013). They also
discuss topics such as power, inequality, and privilege. These discussions of social group
difference make visible how important sociocultural contexts systematically shape peo-
ple’s life experiences and outcomes—that is, the contextual nature of difference (Gurin
et al., 2013, p. 155). Indeed, Gurin and colleagues theorize that the dialogues “allow
a larger, social truth to emerge between and among individuals in which everyone rec-
ognizes themselves as social selves rather than autonomous beings” (p. 78). Participat-
ing in these dialogues has been shown to produce psychological tendencies associated
with intergroup skills, such as intergroup empathy, perspective taking, and intergroup
collaboration (see Gurin et al., 2013). Like intergroup dialogues, difference-education
also educates students about the contextual nature of social group differences. However,
unlike intergroup dialogues, difference-education provides a more focused message that
seeks to isolate the effects of a contextual theory without the more comprehensive learn-
ing experience.
Difference‑Education 137
college education) to a strength (i.e., having a broad perspective), emphasizing the contex-
tual nature of her positive experiences in college. Likewise, after describing her parents
as having obtained college degrees, one continuing-generation student said, “My choice
to attend [university name] really was supported by everyone in my family. There was no
sort of imposition by my parents [saying], ‘You need to go to the [school name]’ or any-
thing like that. It was like, ‘Wherever you want to go we’ll fully support you in any way
that we really can,’ and so they were very open with it.”
As in the first-generation story, the continuing-generation story links the student’s
social class background to a background-specific strength. In this case, having parents
who are both college educated and financially secure means that those parents are more
likely to be open and supportive about a student’s college choices.
Students in the control intervention, in contrast, were exposed to similar stories that
also included obstacles, strategies, and strengths. However, these stories did not commu-
nicate a contextual theory of difference—that is, the stories did not include background-
specific information about how students’ social class backgrounds shaped their college
experiences. Using the same intervention format, incoming students learned from suc-
cessful senior students’ personal stories about their experiences in college—in particular,
the challenges they faced and the strengths and strategies they leveraged to be successful.
One story mentioned an obstacle that a student faced in college (e.g., the coursework was
difficult) and then suggested a strategy for success, “Go to class, and pay attention. If you
don’t understand something or have a hard time with the material, meet with your teach-
ing assistant or professor during office hours.” As in the difference-education condition,
participants in the control condition learned about students’ different experiences in col-
lege, including the challenges they faced (e.g., a student found coursework to be difficult),
and the strengths and strategies that they learned to be successful (e.g., a student found
it helpful to meet with a professor). Notably, across both conditions, participants learned
the same types of strategies for success, such as seeking help from peers and professors.
The key difference between the conditions was that students in the control condition did
not learn how their own and others’ backgrounds could inform their college experiences
(i.e., their obstacles, strengths, or strategies for success).
NUANCES AND MISCONCEPTIONS
Avoid Stereotyping
It is critical to avoid stereotyping people based on their social group memberships when
discussing social group difference. Intentionally or unintentionally stereotyping students
Difference‑Education 139
will produce feelings of threat and exclusion, and ultimately undermine their engage-
ment, motivation, and performance (e.g., Major, Spencer, Schmader, Wolfe, & Crocker,
1998; Steele, 2010). Instead, research suggests the importance of recognizing the inter-
section of multiple identities that is unique to each person, as well as the importance of
intragroup variation and diversity (e.g., Cole, 2009; Crenshaw, 1991; Markus & Conner,
2014; Rosenthal, 2016).
In a difference-education intervention, multiple strategies can be used to avoid ste-
reotyping students or communicating limiting characterizations of their social groups.
First, it is important to demonstrate variation in students’ experiences both across and
within social groups; signal that these social differences are tendencies or patterns, rather
than one-to-one relationships; and show that social differences can evolve and change
with new experiences. We designed the difference-education stories to differ not only
across students’ social class backgrounds but also within their particular social class
background. For example, first-generation students’ stories included similar, but not
identical, content about their experiences in college. Another strategy is to maintain stu-
dents’ sense of individuality by showing them how the diverse sociocultural contexts of
their lives matter in ways that are unique to their particular backgrounds and identities.
For example, students’ social class, race, gender, and/or other backgrounds can shape
their experiences in distinct ways. Although the stories in difference-education focused
primarily on social class, they also acknowledged intersectional identities (e.g., being an
African American, male, first-generation college student).
et al., 2016; Sleeter, 2011). For disadvantaged students, seeing that difference is also rele-
vant to advantaged students can help normalize the experience of difference and reassure
them that they are not being singled out (e.g., Hummel & Steele, 1996). At the same time,
including advantaged students in discussions of social group difference can help ensure
that they will experience the topic of social group difference as self-relevant and impor-
tant for their own lives and experiences, while also reducing the likelihood that they will
perceive difference as a threat (e.g., Dover, Major, & Kaiser, 2016; Plaut, Garnett, Buf-
fardi, & Sanchez-Burks, 2011; Wilkins, Hirsch, Kaiser, & Inkles, 2016).
In a difference-education intervention, one strategy to help all people see differ-
ence as self-relevant is to include the contrasting stories of both advantaged and disad-
vantaged students. In the difference-education interventions described above, the stories
of the senior students, as well as the intervention participants, included both first- and
continuing-generation students. The contrast between these stories of students from dif-
ferent social class backgrounds can help all students learn how their backgrounds and
prior life experiences shape who they are. Including individuals from diverse backgrounds
also makes it possible to frame the intervention as a general (rather than group specific)
program that can help everyone adjust to a new setting.
IMPLICATIONS FOR PRACTICE
strategies are relevant to all interventions, they can be applied specifically to difference-
education interventions.
First, it is critical to adapt the stories to reflect students’ actual experiences in a
given college or university setting. To do so, we begin by talking to administrators and
interviewing students to better understand the financial, academic, and psychological
obstacles they face. We then adapt our intervention materials (i.e., the student stories) so
that they reflect the actual experiences of students in that setting. For example, if a vast
majority of students are Latinx, then it is critical to ensure that Latinx students’ perspec-
tives are well represented in the stories. Beyond representation, the stories should also
highlight the common experiences, challenges, activities, and campus resources available
there. For example, in a high-ranking university where first-generation college students
are frequently a very small minority, these students are likely to confront more questions
about belonging than they would if they were attending a lower-ranking university or
community college where first-generation students are more highly represented. Student
stories should be carefully adapted to include the most common and relevant issues.
Second, it is critical to conduct the intervention during a transitional moment. When-
ever possible, we deliver the difference-education intervention at the beginning of the aca-
demic year, typically within the first month of the fall semester. We believe that the inter-
vention content is likely to have the greatest impact in changing students’ construal when
they are still making sense of their experiences in this new setting. For example, when
they are still learning about what it means to come from a different background or to
have a different experience than other students. This meaning-making process could per-
sist for different lengths of time depending on the context. After students have adjusted
to a new setting for many months or years, their understandings of themselves and their
experiences may be more difficult to change.
Third, whenever possible, it is critical to ensure that participation in difference-
education intervention is voluntary, at least to some extent. As is the case in all interven-
tions, it is important for participants to feel like they have a choice. When students choose
to participate, particularly in middle-class U.S. contexts, they are more likely to be fully
engaged and motivated to learn (see Patall, Cooper, & Robinson, 2008). Research sug-
gests that forcing students to participate (e.g., in a mandatory program) is more likely to
produce reactance and decrease engagement (see Dobbin & Kalev, 2013; Dobbin, Kim,
& Kalev, 2011).
IMPLICATIONS FOR PSYCHOLOGICAL THEORY
At first glance, a review of research in mainstream social psychology might suggest that
helping disadvantaged students succeed requires avoiding discussions of social group dif-
ference altogether. Stemming from the essentialist view of difference, the topic of social
group differences is often represented as stigmatizing and a potential threat to avoid.
Markus (2008) argues that viewing difference in this negative light permeates much of
social psychology. For example, the literatures on intergroup relations, implicit bias,
social categorization, and social identity threat reflect this perspective on difference.
According to research in these areas, when social group differences become salient, they
have the potential to produce negative consequences, such as stereotyping, bias, and dis-
crimination (Brewer & Miller, 1984; Dovidio & Gaertner, 2000; see Gurin & Nagda,
142 I. Education
2006; Gurin et al., 2013, for discussion). For example, in the face of negative, essential-
izing stereotypes about one’s group, disadvantaged students are likely to underperform
on achievement tasks (e.g., Steele, 2010).
In contrast to this commonly held negative view of “difference as a threat,” research
in cultural psychology and the literatures on multicultural and social justice education
suggest that acknowledging social group difference need not create the experience of
threat. Instead, when viewed from a contextual perspective, social group differences have
the potential to serve as an important source of meaning, identity, and motivation (see
Markus, 2008; see also Brannon, Taylor, Higginbotham, & Henderson, 2017). Consis-
tent with these perspectives, the difference-education intervention presented here sug-
gests that consequences of social group difference hinge on how students construe the
meanings of difference and how it relates to their experiences as students. Indeed, as
we argued throughout this chapter, when difference is represented and understood as
contextual—that is, a product of people’s ongoing participation in particular sociocul-
tural contexts over time—it has the potential to be experienced positively and to help
students find a path to success that takes their differences into account.
FUTURE DIRECTIONS
This chapter explains what difference-education is, how to implement it effectively, and
why it can be an effective approach to reducing educational disparities. Yet, a number
of important questions remain unanswered. As is the case with other successful social
psychological interventions, future research on difference-education interventions should
further specify the necessary and sufficient components that drive the intervention’s ben-
efits. One important question is whether role models with similar backgrounds are neces-
sary for students to gain a contextual understanding of difference. For example, would
intervention participants need to listen to the stories of successful senior students, or
could they simply read a research article about the contextual sources of social inequal-
ity?
Future research should also investigate the specific ways in which interventions that
provide a contextual theory can catalyze psychological and behavioral changes in a recur-
sive cycle over time (see Miller et al., 2017; Yeager & Walton, 2011). Based on previous
work on multicultural and social justice education, as well as the difference-education
studies presented here, we suggest that fit and empowerment are two key processes that
explain how a contextual theory changes behavior (e.g., seeking resources) and improves
academic performance.
However, further work is needed to unpack precisely how these psychological
changes work in both directions to sustain long-term behavioral changes (and vice
versa). For example, in one direction, how might the psychological experiences of fit and
empowerment work together to initiate key behavioral changes, such as seeking campus
resources or developing a relationship with a mentor? And, in the other direction, how do
these behavioral changes impact and sustain students’ psychological changes over time as
they progress through college and encounter new experiences? Research is also needed to
better understand when the psychological benefits of fit and empowerment emerge only
for the disadvantaged group targeted by the intervention versus when they occur for all
students—both disadvantaged and advantaged.
Difference‑Education 143
Beyond fit and empowerment, research should consider other possible complemen-
tary processes through which learning a contextual theory of difference could catalyze
psychological or behavioral change. Although we have shown that difference-education
participants are more comfortable discussing their differences with a peer (Stephens,
Townsend, et al., 2015), future work might also consider whether students who learn a
contextual theory of difference become more culturally intelligent or competent—that
is, able to more effectively navigate across different cultural contexts (e.g., Ang & Van
Dyne, 2008; Earley & Ang, 2003; Leung, Ang, & Tan, 2014). Likewise, research could
ask whether students develop metacognitive skills about how cultures work and how
people’s thoughts, feelings, and actions can be shaped by their cultures (e.g., Leung, Lee,
& Chiu, 2013; Mor, Morris, & Joh, 2013).
In addition, future research should also consider how such a theory might be used to
drive cultural change at an institutional level. As in other social psychological interven-
tions (Yeager & Walton, 2011; Wilson, 2011), difference-education focuses on providing
a new lay theory (i.e., a contextual theory of social group difference) to change students’
psychological experiences (e.g., increase their fit and empowerment) and academic out-
comes. We believe that these individual-level changes are not a panacea and may fail to
support long-term change if they are not built into and reinforced by the larger college
culture. To maintain and support these individual-level changes over time, we must also
change institutional-level policies, practices, and resources (e.g., Markus & Conner, 2014;
Markus & Hamedani, 2019; Stephens, Markus, & Fryberg, 2012; Walton & Wilson,
2018). For example, universities could communicate a contextual theory of difference in
diversity courses; dorm programming or first-year programs or via shared cultural prod-
ucts, such as university websites or student guidebooks. At the same time, because people
can shape their cultures (Markus & Hamedani, 2019), providing a contextual theory at
an individual level also has the potential to change institutions. Increasing individual-level
awareness and understanding of difference has the potential to create a more inclusive and
accepting environment in which students are empowered to effect change.
In conclusion, even the most academically prepared first-generation students con-
front particular psychological obstacles that can fuel the social class achievement gap in
higher education. This chapter provided an overview of how difference-education can
help disadvantaged students overcome these psychological obstacles and find a path to
success that takes their different experiences into account. The research reviewed here
suggests that the difference-education approach has the potential to improve the aca-
demic outcomes of disadvantaged students by communicating a contextual theory of
social group difference. When students learn that their differences are a product of con-
textual experience, rather than essential features of people or groups, they should expe-
rience greater fit and empowerment in college. In turn, their improved experiences in
college can help them more fully engage in and take advantage of the resources available
to them and ultimately improve their opportunity to succeed.
REFERENCES
Adams, M., Bell, L. A., & Griffin, P. (2007). Teaching for diversity and social justice (2nd ed.).
New York: Routledge.
Ang, S., & Van Dyne, L. (2008). Handbook of cultural intelligence: Theory, measurement, and
applications. New York: Routledge.
144 I. Education
Astin, A. W., & Oseguera, L. (2004). The declining “equity” of American higher education.
Review of Higher Education, 27, 321–341.
Bastian, B., & Haslam, N. (2006). Psychological essentialism and stereotype endorsement. Jour-
nal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 228–235.
Ben-Zeev, A., Paluy, Y., Milless, K., Goldstein, E., Wallace, L., Marquez-Magana, L., . . . Estrada,
M. (2017). “Speaking truth” protects underrepresented minorities’ intellectual performance
and safety in STEM. Education Sciences, 7, 65.
Blackwell, L. S., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Dweck, C. S. (2007). Implicit theories of intelligence pre-
dict achievement across an adolescent transition: A longitudinal study and an intervention.
Child Development, 78, 246–263.
Bowman, N. A. (2010). Disequilibrium and resolution: The nonlinear effects of diversity courses
on wellbeing and orientations toward diversity. Review of Higher Education, 33, 543–568.
Brannon, T. N., Taylor, V. J., Higginbotham, G. D., & Henderson, K. (2017). Selves in con-
tact: How integrating perspectives on sociocultural selves and intergroup contact can inform
theory and application on reducing inequality. Social and Personality Psychology Compass,
11(7), e12326.
Brewer, M. B., & Miller, N. (1984). Beyond the contact hypothesis: Theoretical perspectives on
desegregation. In N. Miller & M. Brewer (Eds.), Groups in contact: The psychology of deseg-
regation (pp. 281–302). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Cabrera, N. L., Milem, J. F., Jaquette, O., & Marx, R. W. (2014). Missing the (student achieve-
ment) forest for all the (political) trees: Empiricism and the Mexican American studies con-
troversy in Tucson. American Educational Research Journal, 51, 1084–1118.
Capó Crucet, J. (2015, August 22). Taking my parents to college. New York Times. Retrieved from
www.nytimes.com.
Chao, M. M., Hong, Y. Y., & Chiu, C. Y. (2013). Essentializing race: Its implications on racial
categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104, 619–634.
Chatman, J. (1991). Matching people and organizations: Selection and socialization in public
accounting firms. Administrative Science Quarterly, 36, 459–484.
Cohen, G. L., Steele, C. M., & Ross, L. D. (1999). The mentor’s dilemma: Providing critical feed-
back across the racial divide. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 1302–1318.
Cole, E. R. (2009). Intersectionality and research in psychology. American Psychologist, 64, 170–
180.
Cole, E. R., Case, K. A., Rios, D., & Curtin, N. (2011). Understanding what students bring to
the classroom: Moderators of the effects of diversity courses on student attitudes. Cultural
Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 17, 397–405.
Covarrubias, R., & Fryberg, S. (2015). The impact of self-relevant representations on school
belonging for disadvantaged Native American students. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic
Minority Psychology, 21, 10–18.
Crenshaw, K. (1991). Mapping the margins: Intersectionality, identity politics, and violence
against women of color. Stanford Law Review, 43, 1241–1299.
Dawis, R. V. (1992). Person–environment fit and job satisfaction. In C. Cranny, P. Smith, & E.
Stone (Eds.), Job satisfaction (pp. 69–88). New York: Lexington Books.
Dee, T., & Penner, E. (2016). The causal effects of cultural relevance: Evidence from an eth-
nic studies curriculum (Working Paper No. 21865). Retrieved from www.nber.org/papers/
w21865.
Delgado, R., & Stefancic, J. (2000). Critical race theory: The cutting edge. Philadelphia: Temple
University Press.
Denson, N. (2009). Do curricular and cocurricular diversity activities influence racial bias?: A
meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 79, 805–838.
Destin, M., Rheinschmidt-Same, M., & Richeson, J. R. (2017). Status-based identity: A conceptual
Difference‑Education 145
framework integrating the social psychological study of socioeconomic status and identity.
Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12, 270–289.
Dittmann, A. G., & Stephens, N. M. (2017). Interventions aimed at closing the social class achieve-
ment gap: Changing individuals, structures, and construals. Current Opinion in Psychology,
18, 111–116.
Dobbin, F., & Kalev, A. (2013). The origins and effects of corporate diversity programs. In Q. M.
Roberson (Eds.), Oxford handbook of diversity and work (pp. 253–281). New York: Oxford
University Press.
Dobbin, F., Kim, S., & Kalev, A. (2011). You can’t always get what you need: Organizational
determinants of diversity programs. American Sociological Review, 76, 386–411.
Dover, T. L., Major, B., & Kaiser, C. R. (2016). Members of high-status groups are threatened
by pro-diversity organizational messages. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 62,
58–67.
Dovidio, J. F., & Gaertner, S. L. (2000). Aversive racism and selection decisions: 1989 and 1999.
Psychological Science, 11, 315–319.
Duncan-A ndrade, J. M. R., & Morrell, E. (2008). The art of critical pedagogy: Possibilities for
moving from theory to practice in urban schools (Vol. 285). New York: Lang.
Earley, P. C., & Ang, S. (2003). Cultural intelligence: Individual interactions across cultures.
Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Edwards, J. R., Cable, D. M., Williamson, I. O., Lambert, L. S., & Shipp, A. J. (2006). The
phenomenology of fit: Linking the person and environment to the subjective experience of
person– environment fit. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 802–827.
Fryberg, S. A., & Markus, H. R. (2007). Cultural models of education in American Indian, Asian
American and European American contexts. Social Psychology of Education, 10, 213–246.
Fulmer, C. A., Gelfand, M. J., Kruglanski, A. W., Kim-Prieto, C., Diener, E., Pierro, A., & Hig-
gins, E. T. (2010). On “feeling right” in cultural contexts: How person–culture match affects
self-esteem and subjective well-being. Psychological Science, 21, 1563–1569.
Gelman, S. A. (2004). Psychological essentialism in children. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8,
404–409.
Goudeau, S., & Croizet, J. C. (2017). Hidden advantages and disadvantages of social class: How
classroom settings reproduce social inequality by staging unfair comparison. Psychological
Science, 28, 162–170.
Gurin, P., & Nagda, B. (2006). Getting to the what, how, and why of diversity on campus. Edu-
cational Research, 35, 20–24.
Gurin, P., Nagda, B. A., & Zúñiga, X. (2013). Dialogue across difference: Practice, theory, and
research on intergroup dialogue. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
Haslam, N., Rothschild, L., & Ernst, D. (2000). Essentialist beliefs about social categories. British
Journal of Social Psychology, 39, 113–127.
Horn, L., & Nuñez, A.-M. (2000). Mapping the road to college: First-generation students’ math
track, planning strategies, and context of support (NCES 2000153). Washington, DC: U.S.
Government Printing Office.
Housel, T. H., & Harvey, V. L. (2010). The invisibility factor: Administrators and faculty reach
out to first-generation college students. Boca Raton, FL: Universal.
Hummel, M., & Steele, C. (1996). The learning community: A program to address issues of aca-
demic achievement and retention. Journal of Intergroup Relations, 23, 28–32.
Keller, J. (2005). In genes we trust: The biological component of psychological essentialism and its
relationship to mechanisms of motivated social cognition. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 88, 686–702.
Kohn, M. (1969). Class and conformity. Chicago: University Press.
Kung, F. Y., Chao, M. M., Yao, D. J., Adair, W. L., Fu, J. H., & Tasa, K. (2018). Bridging racial
146 I. Education
divides: Social constructionist (vs. essentialist) beliefs facilitate trust in intergroup contexts.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 74, 121–134.
Leung, A. K., Lee, S.-L., & Chiu, C. Y. (2013). Meta-knowledge of culture promotes cultural
competence. Journal of Cross-C ultural Psychology. 44, 992–1006.
Leung, K., Ang, S., & Tan, M. L. (2014). Intercultural competence. Annual Review of Organiza-
tional Psychology and Organizational Behavior, 1, 489–519.
Major, B., Spencer, S., Schmader, T., Wolfe, C., & Crocker, J. (1998). Coping with negative stereo-
types about intellectual performance: The role of psychological disengagement. Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 34–50.
Markus, H. R. (2008). Pride, prejudice, and ambivalence: Toward a unified theory of race and
ethnicity. American Psychologist, 63, 651–667.
Markus, H. R., & Conner, A. (2014). Clash!: How to thrive in a multicultural world. New York:
Hudson Street Press.
Markus, H. R., & Hamedani, M. G. (2019). People are culturally shaped shapers: The psychologi-
cal science of culture and culture change. In D. Cohen & S. Kitayama (Eds.), The handbook
of cultural psychology (2nd ed., pp. 11–52). New York: Guilford Press.
Markus, H. R., & Moya, P. M. (2010). Doing race: 21 essays for the 21st century. New York:
Norton.
Miller, D. T., Dannals, J. E., & Zlatev, J. J. (2017). Behavioral processes in long-lag intervention
studies. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12, 454–467.
Mitra, D. L. (2004). The significance of students: Can increasing “student voice” in schools lead
to gains in youth development? Teachers College Record, 106, 651–688.
Molden, D. C., & Dweck, C. S. (2006). Finding “meaning” in psychology: A lay theories approach
to self-regulation, social perception, and social development. American Psychologist, 61,
192–203.
Mor, S., Morris, M., & Joh, J. (2013). Identifying and training adaptive cross-cultural manage-
ment skills: The crucial role of cultural metacognition. Academy of Management Learning
and Education, 12, 453–475.
Moss-Racusin, C. A., van der Toorn, J., Dovidio, J. F., Brescoll, V. L., Graham, M. J., & Handels-
man, J. (2016). A “scientific diversity” intervention to reduce gender bias in a sample of life
scientists. Life Sciences Education, 15, 1–11.
Nelson Laird, T. F. (2005). College students’ experiences with diversity and their effects on aca-
demic self-confidence, social agency, and disposition toward critical thinking. Research in
Higher Education, 46, 365–388.
No, S., Hong, Y. Y., Liao, H. Y., Lee, K., Wood, D., & Chao, M. M. (2008). Lay theory of race
affects and moderates Asian Americans’ responses toward American culture. Journal of Per-
sonality and Social Psychology, 95, 991–1004.
Omi, M., & Winant, H. (2015). Racial formation in the United States (3rd ed.). New York: Rout-
ledge.
Ostrove, J. M., & Long, S. M. (2007). Social class and belonging: Implications for college adjust-
ment. Review of Higher Education, 30, 363–389.
Patall, E. A., Cooper, H., & Robinson, J. C. (2008). The effects of choice on intrinsic motiva-
tion and related outcomes: A meta-analysis of research findings. Psychological Bulletin, 134,
270–300.
Plaut, V. C., Garnett, F. G., Buffardi, L. E., & Sanchez-Burks, J. (2011). “What about me?”: Per-
ceptions of exclusion and Whites’ reactions to multiculturalism. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 101, 337–353.
Prentice, D. A., & Miller, D. T. (2007). Psychological essentialism of human categories. Current
Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 202–206.
Reay, D., Crozier, G., & Clayton, J. (2009). “Strangers in paradise?”: Working-class students in
elite universities. Sociology, 43, 1103–1121.
Difference‑Education 147
Riggs, L. (2014, January 14). What it’s like to be the first person in your family to go to college.
The Atlantic. Retrieved from www.theatlantic.com.
Rosenthal, L. (2016). Incorporating intersectionality into psychology: An opportunity to promote
social justice and equity. American Psychologist, 71, 474–485.
Sirin, S. R. (2005). Socioeconomic status and academic achievement: A meta-analytic review of
research. Review of Educational Research, 75, 417–453.
Sleeter, C. E. (2011). The academic and social value of ethnic studies: A research review. Retrieved
from www.files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED521869.pdf.
Steele, C. M. (2010). Whistling Vivaldi and other clues to how stereotypes affect us. New York:
Norton.
Stephens, N. M., Brannon, T. N., Markus, H. R., & Nelson, J. E. (2015). Feeling at home in col-
lege: Fortifying school-relevant selves to reduce social class disparities in higher education.
Social Issues and Policy Review, 9, 1–24.
Stephens, N. M., Fryberg, S. A., Markus, H. R., Johnson, C. S., & Covarrubias, R. (2012). Unseen
disadvantage: How American universities’ focus on independence undermines the academic
performance of first-generation college students. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 102(6), 1178–1197.
Stephens, N. M., Hamedani, M. G., & Destin, M. (2014). Closing the social-class achievement
gap: A difference-education intervention improves first-generation students’ academic perfor-
mance and all students’ college transition. Psychological Science, 25, 943–953.
Stephens, N. M., Markus, H. R., & Fryberg, S. A. (2012). Social class disparities in health and
education: Reducing inequality by applying a sociocultural self model of behavior. Psycho-
logical Review, 119, 723–744.
Stephens, N. M., Townsend, S. S. M., Hamedani, M. G., Destin, M., & Manzo, V. (2015). A
difference-education intervention equips first-generation college students to thrive in the face
of stressful college situations. Psychological Science, 26, 1556–1566.
Terenzini, P. T., Springer, L., Yaeger, P., Pascarella, E. T., & Nora, A. (1996). First-generation col-
lege students: Characteristics, experiences, and cognitive development. Research in Higher
Education, 37, 1–22.
Townsend, S. S. M., Stephens, N. M., Smallets, S., & Hamedani, M. G. (2018). Empowerment
through difference: An online difference- education intervention closes the social class
achievement gap. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 45(7), 1068–1083.
Walton, G. M., & Cohen, G. L. (2007). A question of belonging: Race, social fit, and achievement.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 82–96.
Walton, G. M., & Wilson, T. D. (2018). Wise interventions: Psychological remedies for social and
personal problems. Psychological Review, 125, 617.
Wilkins, C. L., Hirsch, A. A., Kaiser, C. R., & Inkles, M. P. (2016). The threat of racial progress
and the self-protective nature of perceiving anti-White bias. Group Processes and Intergroup
Relations, 20, 801–812.
Williams, M. J., & Eberhardt, J. L. (2008). Biological conceptions of race and the motivation to
cross racial boundaries. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 1033–1047.
Wilson, T. D. (2011). Redirect: The surprising new science of psychological change. New York:
Little, Brown and Company.
Yeager, D. S., & Walton, G. M. (2011). Social-psychological interventions in education: They’re
not magic. Review of Educational Research, 81, 267–301.
C H A P TE R 6
Mesmin Destin and Ivan A. Hernandez
The pathways intervention is built upon the images that young people have of the type of
people they might become in the future. Studies related to the pathways intervention dem-
onstrate the importance of guiding students to perceive the path to their desired futures as
open or within reach and to develop strategies that will help them progress toward desired
future identities. For students from lower-socioeconomic status (SES) backgrounds and
racial/ethnic-minority groups, the pathways intervention is particularly well suited to increase
the likelihood that they genuinely envision higher education as a part of their future, which
helps to support academic motivation through challenges. The pathways intervention and
related studies have led students to imagine future identities that are connected to educa-
tion and have increased students’ expected academic performance and planned effort on
school tasks. In its full implementation, the intervention has shown significant positive effects
on students’ academic outcomes and well-being that persisted through a 2-year follow-up
period. In this chapter, we describe the theory behind the pathways intervention and related
experiments, in addition to empirical evidence for its effects on a variety of outcomes and
implications for psychological theory, research, and policy.
BACKGROUND
Students in the United States have very high aspirations for educational and lifetime
success, in general. When asked how far they plan to go in school, the vast majority of
148
The Pathways Intervention 149
students express that they want to complete a college degree or beyond, regardless of
their socioeconomic background (Mickelson, 1990; Oyserman & Lewis, 2017; Schnei-
der & Stevenson, 1999). In reality, though, students from families with fewer financial
resources are less likely to reach college than students from wealthier families (Ma,
Pender, & Welch, 2016). This is in large part due to the many ways that having more
family financial assets affords students with better-resourced schools, more enriching
academic experiences, and increased access to future opportunities (Jackson, Johnson,
& Persico, 2016; Lafortune, Rothstein, & Schanzenbach, 2018; Lovenheim, 2011). For
example, a student who attends a high school in a wealthy neighborhood is more likely
to have regular access to guidance counselors and college preparatory curriculum than
a student who attends a school in a segregated, lower-income community (see Corwin,
Venegas, Oliverez, & Colyar, 2004; Gamoran & Hannigan, 2000; Kanno & Kangas,
2014). One reason why these resources matter for academic outcomes is that they signal
to students what is actually likely for their futures and either reinforce or weaken stu-
dents’ visions of their lives to come. These images of the future, which are sometimes
called possible selves or future identities, help to guide students’ everyday behaviors and
their motivation to engage and persist in school tasks that can lead to reaching high aca-
demic goals. The pathways intervention involves a series of activities that are designed
to bolster students’ developing future identities and their understanding of the path to
reach their goals. As a result, pathways helps to support students’ school motivation
even in contexts that do not otherwise consistently reinforce the images that they have
for their futures.
Before offering greater depth regarding the theory, evidence, and application related
to the pathways intervention, there are two important caveats or considerations to keep
in mind. First, the core issue that pathways aims to address originates at a level that is sit-
uated beyond the individual student or psychological processes. The access to resources
that students encounter in their schools and neighborhoods provides the foundation for
how they imagine their futures and pursue school goals. Ideally, acknowledging and
addressing the root manifestations of economic inequality that young people experience
should be part of any large-scale effort to improve academic outcomes through future
identities. At the very least, though, efforts at psychological intervention should leverage
the messages that students receive from their surrounding contexts (e.g., peers, parents,
teachers, schools, neighborhoods) in order to amplify the potential effects of individual,
student-targeted intervention.
Second, the idea of psychological intervention, itself, can carry certain assumptions
that do not always align with the broader goal of increasing educational equity. Psycho-
logical intervention can inherently situate the researcher—who is often from a dominant
racial/ethnic, socioeconomic, and/or cultural background—as the savior, and the student
as the target of the problem without expertise and agency. As described, the pertinent
target of intervention rests at a higher level than just the individual student. However,
psychological studies can provide strong evidence for how environments and experiences
shape people’s outlooks, and this evidence can be used to inform the development of sys-
temic interventions that go beyond the individual. Despite these considerations, the term
intervention is used to describe pathways research for simplicity and consistency with
other chapters in the book.
150 I. Education
PSYCHOLOGICAL PROCESSES
There are three primary psychological processes theorized to underlie the pathways inter-
vention. The foundation for the pathways intervention and the establishment of these
processes were derived from early work detailing the core postulates of identity-based
motivation theory, which builds from ideas including the working self-concept, situated
cognition, and possible selves (see Markus & Nurius, 1986; Markus & Wurf, 1987;
Miller, Brickman, & Bolen, 1975; Smith & Semin, 2007).
Dynamic Construction
Although people often tend to think about their identities as stable over time when
describing themselves, there are many important aspects of how individuals view and
express themselves that are shaped heavily by the environment in real time. People often
automatically bring to mind parts of themselves that are relevant to interacting with
the situation at hand (Markus & Wurf, 1987), and the basic ways that our minds pro-
cess information vary depending on aspects of the environment (Smith & Semin, 2004,
2007). These identities that are constructed and cued in context guide how it feels to
engage in particular behaviors, how situations are interpreted, and what behavior seems
relevant. The pathways intervention provides students with opportunities to elaborate
upon their current and possible future identities that are connected to education in a way
that increases their salience across environments and over time (Oyserman, 2015).
Action Readiness
Building upon dynamic construction, when a particular identity is brought to mind, it
accompanies a greater likelihood that a person engages in behaviors associated with that
identity (Oyserman & Destin, 2010). Specifically, people who articulate images of them-
selves reaching, or even failing to reach, a desired future (i.e., future identities) are more
effective in pursuing their educational and health goals than people who do not bring those
possible future identities to mind (Hoyle & Sherrill, 2006; Ruvolo & Markus, 1992). If a
student’s identity that is connected to education is not cued in context, then engaging in
behavior that is relevant to that identity (e.g., schoolwork, studying) does not feel fluent in
the moment. The pathways intervention enhances this connection between student identi-
ties and specific school behaviors that are relevant to reaching academic goals.
Interpretation of Difficulty
Finally, identities shape how people interpret and respond to experiences of difficulty
when pursuing identity-relevant goals (Fisher & Oyserman, 2017; Oyserman, Elmore,
Novin, Fisher, & Smith, 2018; Smith & Oyserman, 2015). More specifically, when a task
feels connected to a salient and valued identity, people are likely to interpret difficulty
during that task as a sign that they are working on something that has meaning and
importance. When important identities feel connected to academic achievement, then
everyday academic challenges are interpreted as meaning that school tasks are impor-
tant rather than impossible (Fisher & Oyserman, 2017; Smith & Oyserman, 2015). On
the other hand, when a task feels disconnected from any valued identity, encountering
The Pathways Intervention 151
difficulty is more likely to signal to a person that the task is impossible for them and that
they should relent. For example, lower-SES students who encounter evidence that there
are robust opportunities for socioeconomic mobility to reach their desired futures are less
likely to interpret academic difficulty as a sign that a school task is impossible (i.e., are
more inclined to persist when faced with academic difficulty) than those who encounter
evidence that there is not much opportunity to ascend the socioeconomic ladder (Brow-
man, Destin, Carswell, & Svoboda, 2017).
EMPIRICAL EVIDENCE
Outcomes
The pathways intervention and related randomized controlled field experiments have
shown positive effects on the academic motivation of students, with most studies focusing
on students from lower-SES backgrounds and racial/ethnic-minority groups. The seminal
implementation of the pathways intervention was delivered to 264 middle school students
from predominantly low-income backgrounds and racial/ethnic-minority groups (Oyser-
man et al., 2006). Students in this sample were randomly assigned to either a control con-
dition or an intervention condition where they participated in 11 different sessions that
were each completed during a single class period. Students in the intervention showed a
significantly better trajectory in their academic motivation, standardized test scores, and
grades compared with students in the control group. Further, the positive effects of the
intervention extended to outcomes including reductions in student absences, misbehav-
ior in school, and depression (Oyserman et al., 2006). These positive effects persisted
through a 2-year follow-up period. The pathways intervention effects persist because they
aim to shift the way that people view themselves in relation to academic contexts and
opportunities, congruent with a wise intervention approach (Walton & Wilson, 2018).
In addition to the main intervention study, smaller-scale field experiments have
tested specific aspects of the pathways approach. One series of studies, for example, dem-
onstrated the positive effects of need-based college financial aid information designed
to make the pathway to future opportunities feel open rather than closed. Ninety-six
seventh-grade students in urban middle schools from predominantly low-income, Black,
and Hispanic/Latinx backgrounds were randomly assigned to receive either the experi-
mental materials about financial aid or materials about the costs of college that might
act as a barrier or close the path to future goals. The experimental or control materials
were delivered during a single class session. The experimental treatment increased stu-
dents’ expected grades and planned effort on homework, studying, and reading (Destin
& Oyserman, 2009).
Another randomized controlled field experiment related to the pathways interven-
tion demonstrated the effects of thinking about desired future economic success as con-
nected to education. A sample of 295 seventh-grade students (59% African American or
Latinx; 61% of middle school students qualified for free or reduced lunch) were randomly
assigned to an experimental treatment condition where they received evidence of the con-
nection between higher education and higher income and a control condition that drew
attention to high-earning careers that were not connected to education (e.g., athletes,
entertainers, musicians). The experimental or control materials were delivered during a
single class session. Students in the experimental treatment condition planned to expend
more effort on homework and studying and were more likely to turn in an extra-credit
assignment the day after the experiment than students in the control condition (Destin
& Oyserman, 2010).
An additional field experiment related to the pathways intervention included a more
diverse sample of 156 seventh-grade students from varied socioeconomic backgrounds
and racial/ethnic groups. Once again, the materials about need-based financial aid that
were designed to make pathways to opportunities feel open had positive effects, this time
in comparison to a control group that did not receive any such materials. The materials
The Pathways Intervention 153
were delivered during a single class session, and students from lower-SES families who
were randomly assigned to the experimental treatment condition showed an increase in
their plans to engage in schoolwork compared with those in the control group (Destin,
2017). These studies offer evidence that the pathways intervention has positive effects on
students, and that they support academic motivation through the shared mechanism of
identity.
Mechanisms
The pathways intervention improves student outcomes by providing students with the
opportunity and support to elaborate upon their motivating identities, with a particular
emphasis on their developing future identities. In the main intervention study, future
identities are measured as possible selves, or written descriptions of who students hope to
become or avoid becoming in the future. Participating in pathways led students to articu-
late future identities that were connected to education and more detailed, plausible, and
linked to everyday strategies than control group students, which all activate the processes
of identity-based motivation and mediate positive effects on academic achievement. Simi-
larly, the field experiments related to the pathways intervention also activated students’
future identities in connection to education in order to support academic motivation.
Some experiments directly activated the mechanism by leading students to think about
desired successful futures as connected to education (Destin & Oyserman, 2010). Other
studies indirectly activated the same mechanism through an emphasis on resources that
open the pathway to desired future identities connected to education (Destin & Oyser-
man, 2009). In a direct test of this theoretical pathway among a sample of lower-SES
students, those who were randomly assigned to an experimental condition focused on
resources and an open path were more likely to envision future identities that were con-
nected to education than similar students who were randomly assigned to a comparison
condition (Destin, 2017). The increased salience of a future identity connected to edu-
cation mediated positive effects of the experimental treatment on student motivation.
Students planned to devote more effort toward their long-term academic goals if they
envisioned a salient future identity that included a college education. The experimental
manipulation shifted students’ perceptions of potential economic barriers and encour-
aged students to envision a desired future that was likely to include education. In all of
these studies, when students from lower-SES backgrounds engage with pathways materi-
als during early adolescence, they are more likely to imagine higher education as a part of
their future, which leads to increased academic motivation (see Figure 6.1).
Effects over Time
The majority of the field experiments related to the pathways intervention were not
designed to examine long-term effects. In most of the studies, students completed outcome
measures immediately after experimental manipulations materials were made salient.
However, the most robust evidence of the longitudinal impact of the pathways inter-
vention comes from the seminal implementation among students from predominantly
low-income backgrounds and racial/ethnic-minority groups (Oyserman et al., 2006).
These students participated in multiple sessions that reinforced key processes (see below
for more details). Students in the intervention displayed greater academic motivation,
154 I. Education
Identity-based
motivation
Pathways Academic
intervention outcomes
FIGURE 6.1. Identity-based motivation as the primary mechanism of the pathways intervention.
standardized test scores, grades, and also displayed reductions in absences, misbehavior
in school, and depression (Oyserman et al., 2006) compared with students in the control
group. These positive effects were mediated by shifts in identity and persisted through a
2-year follow-up period.
Heterogeneity
The processes related to identity-based motivation can apply across demographic groups,
but the pathways intervention and related experiments tend to be most relevant and effec-
tive for students from lower-SES backgrounds and racial/ethnic-minority groups. They
are designed to counteract contextual cues that students from these nondominant groups
encounter (e.g., financial barriers to higher education) that can weaken identities and
motivation. More generally, the pathways intervention matters most for students who
perceive barriers to their desired futures or who see a certain social identity as incompat-
ible with that desired future (see Debrosse, Destin, Rossignac-M ilon, Taylor, & Rogers,
2018; Debrosse, Rossignac-M ilon, Taylor, & Destin, 2018).
RELATED APPROACHES (“COUSINS”)
whereas affirmation interventions can sometimes disengage people from their goal pur-
suit (e.g., Vohs, Park, & Schmeichel, 2013).
Utility- value interventions may activate similar motivational processes to those
activated by the pathways intervention and identity-based motivation. Both perspectives
emphasize the ways that goal-related tasks, like schoolwork, are related to students’ per-
sonal lives. Utility-value experiments ask students to consider how course material or
knowledge is useful and relevant for goals or aspects of their life outside of school (Har-
ackiewicz, Canning, Tibbetts, Priniski, & Hyde, 2016). Although related, the pathways
intervention is more likely to target students’ construal of the social context (e.g., per-
ceived financial barriers to college) that influences the development of their identities,
whereas utility-value experiments focus more directly on the construal of the students’
schoolwork itself. After making more connections between the course material and their
lives, students display greater course achievement expectations, continuing interest in
the course, and value the course material more (Hulleman, Kosovich, Barron, & Daniel,
2017). One distinction between utility-value and pathways interventions is that the path-
ways intervention is more likely to start by invoking students’ broad images of their lives
in the future and then narrow to specific goal-related content. Utility-value approaches
are more likely to first invoke students’ specific goals or work on specific tasks that might
then broaden to be considered as part of a more complete image of their life in the future.
In other words, pathways often starts by asking, “Who do I want to become?,” while
utility value often asks, “How is this course content relevant to me?”
Growth-mindset-of-intelligence interventions, like the pathways intervention and a
number of other notable interventions (e.g., social-belonging interventions; see Walton
& Brady, Chapter 2, this volume), also target outcomes related to academic motivation
and achievement by providing students with new ways to make sense of and respond to
their academic experiences. Growth-mindset-of-intelligence interventions might address
the question “Am I smart enough to do the work in college?” They often impart in
students the belief that they can learn, change, and develop particular skills or traits,
such as intelligence. These beliefs alter the way students interpret and respond to aca-
demic difficulties, usually leading them to be more resilient and persistent in the face
of challenges (e.g., Blackwell, Trzesniewski, & Dweck, 2007; Yeager & Dweck, 2012).
Growth-mindset interventions directly convey that struggle is an opportunity for growth
and learning (see Paunesku et al., 2015). Similarly, social-belonging interventions help
students perceive challenges in their educational experiences as normal and evolving
over time. However, social-belonging interventions target the question “Can I come to
belong in college?”
The pathways intervention, on the other hand, conveys the meta-message that the
schoolwork they are currently engaging in is relevant to both who they are currently
and who they can become in the future, indirectly leading school challenges to be seen
as important to reaching their desired identity. Other related work directly disrupts stu-
dents’ misperception that experienced difficulties in an academic domain imply that they
will never be able to succeed in that domain or that they should pursue a different future
goal or identity (Lin-Siegler, Ahn, Chen, Fang, & Luna-Lucero, 2016). Much of this work
has been influenced by earlier attributional-retraining interventions (e.g., Wilson & Lin-
ville, 1982, 1985). The pathways intervention, instead, directly increases the likelihood
that students imagine education as a part of their future and guide students to perceive
the path toward their desired futures as open, expanding students’ perceptions of what is
156 I. Education
possible for them. When schoolwork feels connected to that salient and valued identity,
students interpret difficulty in school as something meaningful and important.
The pathways intervention is concerned with students’ successful pursuit of attain-
ing a desired future outcome. As such, pathways is also related to a family of other
goal-setting interventions. For example, proximal goal interventions guide students who
are disinterested and displaying significant challenges in math to set proximal goals (vs.
distal goals) to accomplish, which leads them to display greater math self-efficacy and
actual mastery of mathematical operations (Bandura & Schunk, 1981). Additionally,
implementation-intention interventions guide students to set “if–then” plans that tar-
get the execution of the preset goal intentions, which significantly increase the likeli-
hood that individuals successfully attain their goals (see Gollwitzer & Sheeran, 2006, for
meta-analyses and review). Similar to the pathways intervention, these interventions link
present behaviors to goal pursuit. However, pathways implements a combination of long-
term goals, but with an emphasis on an individual’s long-term identity. In the pathways
intervention, personal and unique goal setting is also very important. However, goal
proximity is a crucial distinction between these two approaches.
Whereas proximal goal interventions are designed to lead students to set short-term
small goals that build on one another, the pathways intervention starts by focusing on
distal futures (e.g., an adolescent thinking about an aspired adult career) and linking
them to more immediate action. Additionally, the pathways intervention shapes students’
perceptions about what is possible for them and people like them in the future, whereas
proximal goal interventions might more directly target current self-efficacy in a specific
domain (e.g., math). Further, whereas implementation intentions are intended to facilitate
goal pursuit by setting strategies to overcome obstacles, for example, pathways targets
the congruency between the current action and one’s future identity (e.g., “If I am to be
a college graduate, then completing this extra-credit assignment is congruent with that
identity, and therefore, is meaningful in the moment”). There are a number of other
related social-psychological interventions that space here does not allow for comparison
(see also Bryan, Walton, Rogers, & Dweck, 2011; Miller et al., 1975; Yeager et al., 2014).
The pathways intervention may be implemented over several weeks and consists of a
series of workshops. The seminal pathways intervention was delivered during 11 different
sessions that are each completed during a single class period. Each session had a specific
purpose, built on the previous session, and reinforced the primary aims over a 7-week
period (see Oyserman, 2015). The goals were to guide students to reflect on and identify
their desired adult identities and to describe strategies to attain them. The intervention
established a connection between students’ future identities and strategies to attain them,
shifted the way they interpreted difficulty to focus on the importance of their setbacks
in attaining their future identities, and created a link between their social identities and
possible futures.
The first two sessions include interactive activities leading students to identify their
adult possible selves and to increase their salience and congruence with their social iden-
tities. The third session focuses on identifying the role models and people who might
be negative forces in their lives. During the fourth session, students draw timelines
The Pathways Intervention 157
connecting their current life to their future, including potential obstacles and different
opportunities that might arise along the way. The fifth, sixth, and seventh sessions incor-
porate skill building as students generate strategies that would help them reach their
desired futures and avoid their undesired futures. This includes activities and opportu-
nities for students to internalize the idea that difficulty is a normative experience. The
final sessions allowed students to reflect on all that they learned, apply that knowledge
in small groups, and to discuss each session with a focus on reinforcing the main ideas.
The main ideas, or meaning-making meta-messages, include “We all care about school,”
“We all want a good future,” “Everyone faces obstacles and difficulties,” and “Failures
and setbacks are normal.” Throughout the intervention, facilitators establish a structure
where students can learn and consistently communicate the meta-messages and underly-
ing themes that permeate every activity that the students complete. The intervention is
also designed to convey messages in a manner that will be understood and that is devel-
opmentally appropriate. An implementation manual for the pathways intervention can be
found in Oyserman (2015).
In addition to the full pathways intervention implementation, several related field
experiments have tested specific aspects of the broader intervention during a single mid-
dle school class session. These experiments have taken place in homerooms and subject
classrooms during the academic term, as well as during summer programs. They differ
in content and length but target the same core processes as the main pathways inter-
vention. In the classroom, the teacher introduces the members of the research team as
visitors from a local university providing career-related information to young students,
and the materials are distributed on paper, including relevant text and figures. These
materials include real information, such as current figures of adult earnings without
using any form of deception. Students are then asked to complete a brief questionnaire
that is ostensibly a separate general survey, which contains the outcome measures and is
completed in class.
In one example of a field experiment related to the pathways intervention, students
randomly assigned to the treatment condition are introduced to the concept of need-
based financial aid and economic resources available to support the pursuit of higher
education (see Destin, 2017). The information included statements from sources like the
College Board indicating that “there is more financial aid available than ever before—
over $154 billion. . . . The financial aid system is based on the goal that anyone should
be able to attend college, regardless of financial circumstances. Here is how the system
works: Students and their families are expected to contribute to the cost of college to the
extent that they are able. If a family is unable to contribute the entire cost, financial aid is
available to bridge the gap.” In order to increase the likelihood that resources feel within
reach, students also learn that more information is available on the Free Application for
Federal Student Aid website (www.fafsa.gov). Students randomly assigned to a compari-
son condition, on the other hand, read information about the actual (at the time of the
study) costs of attending local universities. The statements include “At 4-year public insti-
tutions, tuition costs average $7,605 per year” and “At 4-year private institutions, tuition
costs average $27,293 per year.” Students then receive examples of what it would cost to
attend specific universities in the area. Some versions of the experiment also include a no-
information control condition. Students in all conditions complete short writing activities
that ask them questions to verify their comprehension of the material before completing
subsequent measures of identity and motivation in the moment.
158 I. Education
NUANCES AND MISCONCEPTIONS
Perhaps the greatest promise of the pathways intervention is in its potential implications
for practice across multiple levels of a student’s environment, rather than only as an
individual intervention. As shown in Figure 6.2, an individual student’s identity, psycho-
logical processes, and motivation are embedded within multiple systems of contextual
The Pathways Intervention 159
influence (see Bronfenbrenner, 1977, 1979). From the peer to teacher to parent to broader
institutional climate, these various levels of contextual influence provide different oppor-
tunities to translate pathways into a variety of other practical programs to support aca-
demic equity. Several experiments have begun to systematically show how specific types
of actions and programs at each of these levels can support positive student outcomes.
At the peer level, a recent experiment built from the pathways intervention to train
high school student near peers as mentors for eighth-grade student participants (Destin,
Castillo, & Meissner, 2018). In a cascading mentorship model, a random group of high
school student near-peer mentors completed five sessions of training by college student
research assistants. The randomly selected high school student mentors prepared to facili-
tate a series of discussions and activities with eighth-grade participants that were modi-
fied from the pathways intervention (Oyserman, 2015; Oyserman et al., 2006). After
the 5 days of training, the high school student mentors were each matched with a small
group of eighth-grade student mentees. For the next 5 days, they facilitated the pro-
gramming focused on topics including images for the future, timelines and obstacles,
and encountering and responding to difficulty, similar to the approach detailed in the
pathways “Intervention Content and Implementation” section. Before and after the 5-day
program, eighth-grade student mentees completed key measures of motivation. Eighth-
grade students who were randomly assigned to participate in the near-peer identity-based
mentoring showed increased academic persistence at the end of the sessions compared
with a control group that were instead randomly assigned to receive basic homework
help from high school tutors (Destin et al., 2018). The experiment demonstrated that
peers can influence identity-based processes that shape motivation and provided insight
for the development of programming that expands on the original pathways interven-
tion. Although not evaluated in this study, other studies have also demonstrated positive
effects for high school or college student mentors themselves (e.g., Schreier, Schonert-
Reichl, & Chen, 2013).
Institutional
climate
Faculty
interactions
Peer
interactions
Individual
student
FIGURE 6.2. Examples of levels of context with potential for programs that expand the path-
ways intervention and influence identity-based motivational processes.
160 I. Education
The key insight from the near-peer experiment is that taking the effort to connect
adolescents with those who are slightly older than them can be beneficial. The high
school students and eighth-grade students attended different schools in the same commu-
nity. This allowed the high school students to facilitate vivid and authentic conversations
about their lives and possible futures. It is important to note, however, that simply con-
necting students with one another without mentorship guidance was not as beneficial, as
shown by the homework-help tutoring condition. Instead, relevant, identity-based guid-
ance is necessary. Further, the experimental demonstration had immediate effects on
student motivation and indirect effects on their grades, but there were no direct effects on
their academic performance. It is likely that sustained interactions with near-peer men-
tors can lead to sustained positive effects on academic outcomes.
One level of contextual influence that does include more sustained interaction is
the teacher level. In another recent study that expanded on the pathways intervention,
researchers provided all participating teachers with a 2-day training in identity-based
motivation (Horowitz, Sorensen, Yoder, & Oyserman, 2018). This study included several
rigorous checks to evaluate the extent to which trained teachers delivered the content of
the intervention with fidelity. The content was well received by teachers and students, and
fidelity reached high levels. Importantly, surpassing fidelity thresholds in facilitation of
the content predicted important student outcomes, including higher grades and reduced
course failure. The study was not a randomized controlled trial designed to test causal
effects but rather demonstrated the potential of the pathways intervention to inform the
training and development of teachers as a way to shape this extremely influential aspect
of student context.
In addition to teachers, parents can also be a consistent level of contextual influence
for students. In a parent-focused experiment expanding on the pathways intervention,
researchers recruited parents of current eighth-grade students to participate in a study
where they would observe a panel of parents whose students had already progressed
beyond eighth grade in the same community (Destin & Svoboda, 2017). The parents who
served as panelists provided predetermined answers to a series of questions about their
own children’s journeys, how they helped their children think about different aspects of
their possible futures, and how they responded to their children’s experiences of academic
difficulty. The intervention was implemented toward the beginning of the school year.
Participants observed the rich discussion and completed an evaluation at the end of the
panel that included some key measures of attitudes and behavioral intentions. After the
panel, parents of current eighth graders planned to talk with their children sooner about
certain aspects of the future and they interpreted academic difficulty as a greater sign of a
task’s importance compared to parents in a control group. Further, the grades of students
whose parents were randomly assigned to observe the panel increased significantly over
the course of the school year compared with students whose parents were in the control
group. Similar to the teacher study, this parent study demonstrated the enormous poten-
tial in expanding the pathways intervention to strategically reaching adults who regularly
interact with students. The study also demonstrated the power of including the stories
and insights of actual community members into the pathways intervention to influence
student outcomes.
Finally, a higher and somewhat more abstract level of contextual influence where the
effects of the pathways intervention can be expanded comes from the school or institu-
tional climate that students encounter. Institutional climate can be especially influential
The Pathways Intervention 161
for students from groups that are nondominant in society or who are the minority within
a particular setting (Cheryan, Plaut, Davies, & Steele, 2009; Murphy, Steele, & Gross,
2007). One series of lab experiments demonstrated the effect of the institutional climate
in higher education on the identities and motivation of students from lower-SES family
backgrounds (Browman & Destin, 2016). College students from diverse backgrounds
were randomly assigned to encounter information that led them to perceive the institu-
tional climate as “warm” or “chilly” toward socioeconomic diversity. In the warm condi-
tion, information focused on how the institution is proud and supportive of its socioeco-
nomic diversity as exemplified through its commitment of various financial resources.
In the chilly condition, information focused on how the institution is generally content
to be a wealthy community as exemplified through its network of large donors and stu-
dents who are able to pay full financial aid. Lower-SES students felt more motivated and
more likely to succeed after encountering the warm climate compared with the chilly
climate (Browman & Destin, 2016). These types of cues within educational settings and
institutions (e.g., in their programming, resources, and everyday messaging) can comple-
ment the pathways intervention to influence the success of students from lower-SES back-
grounds and other nondominant groups in society.
The pathways intervention and processes related to identity-based motivation theory
are well situated to inform the development of many different types of programs focused
on student outcomes because of the inherent link between various levels of context and
individual identities. As described, existing evidence provides specific guidance at the
peer, teacher, parent, and institutional climate levels. These studies, however, provide just
a few examples of how the pathways intervention can inform the development of efforts
to influence various levels of an individual student’s context.
IMPLICATIONS FOR THEORY
The pathways intervention and related work demonstrate the importance of young peo-
ple’s views of themselves and their possible futures in shaping their current school engage-
ment and outcomes. The studies also show the inherent malleability of these influential
identities and the many different types of experiences and social forces that play a role
in how young people come to see themselves from moment to moment. Importantly, the
identities that students articulate in the studies remain self-generated because the explicit
imposition of a particular life path by researchers would likely weaken genuine student
motivation. The pathways intervention illustrates a need to provide the resources, oppor-
tunities, and supports for students to explore, express, and elaborate upon ideas about
themselves and their futures.
As mentioned at the outset, another implication is that studies related to the path-
ways intervention should not be considered interventions simply because they aim to
influence outcomes for participants from diverse and often underserved backgrounds
or because they are conducted in field settings. The idea of intervention can sometimes
inherently imply that researchers unlock the definitive answer to solving some persistent
social issue or fixing misguided behaviors of individuals or groups. Instead, the pathways
intervention and related studies contribute to theory building in a similar manner to stud-
ies including members of more dominant and privileged groups in society. They often,
however, also have a more externally valid conceptualization of context and the results
162 I. Education
can have more directly applicable implications. The results of these studies, though, have
the greatest implications in how they can inform broader efforts to reduce inequality
rather than as independent interventions that are not included in systematic initiatives to
influence multiple levels of context.
FUTURE DIRECTIONS
The study of other possible levels of context, such as the neighborhood level, in addition
to the potential effects of multiple coordinated efforts spanning different levels of con-
text, can continue to advance insight in this area of research. In other words, there may
be ways that approaches at certain levels can supplement approaches at other levels. For
example, parent or teacher programming might be coordinated with a direct-to-student
approach. Similarly, an attempt to shift institutional climates might be meaningfully
paired with a neighborhood-focused initiative. By increasing the likelihood that students
receive congruent messages that reinforce one another, their identities that support moti-
vation may develop in a more stable, cohesive, and integrated manner. Future work may
study how the effects of a traditional pathways intervention may expand when linked to
systemic changes and supports.
Another way to advance this area of work is to systematically test the specific types
of financial and learning resources that are necessary to maximize the potential positive
effects of interventions that target various levels of context. Further, a movement toward
intervention characterized by more systemic and coordinated efforts opens the possibili-
ties to more holistically evaluate the ways that contexts and identities influence a wider
range of outcomes for individuals, including health and well-being, across the lifespan
(Destin, 2019).
Finally, perhaps the most important area for continued advancement is greater inter-
face with social and public policy (Destin, 2018). Policies have the potential to shape
aspects of the broader overarching context in addition to specific aspects of the lives
of students and their families. On the positive end, researchers can provide tools and
frameworks to help policymakers think about the various ways that policies can influ-
ence broader contexts with implications for the identities and lives of individuals. On
the negative end, if researchers fail to interface with policy, individual programming and
intervention will remain inherently limited in its ability to reduce systemic and persistent
inequities. Insight from the pathways intervention can guide the development of a wide
range of policies, practices, and individual interactions that support students in drawing
deeper connections between their aspirations and real-life circumstances.
REFERENCES
Bandura, A., & Schunk, D. H. (1981). Cultivating competence, self-efficacy, and intrinsic inter-
est through proximal self-motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41(3),
586–598.
Blackwell, L. S., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Dweck, C. S. (2007). Implicit theories of intelligence pre-
dict achievement across an adolescent transition: A longitudinal study and an intervention.
Child Development, 78(1), 246–263.
The Pathways Intervention 163
Patricia Chen
Improving learning is not just about working harder, but also about how effectively learn-
ers direct their efforts. Many learners fall short of achieving their potential, not necessarily
because they are unable or unwilling to put in the effort but because they are not strategic
in the way they use their learning resources to build mastery. We showed that an online,
self-administered “strategic resource use” intervention, which guided learners to self-reflect
on how they would effectively use their resources to study for their exams, significantly
improved academic performance. The intervention guided them through the psychologi-
cal process of strategizing what to use and why (for what purpose), and planning when,
where, and how to learn effectively. Carrying out such reflection was related to more effective
resource use reported during learning, and in turn, to better academic performance. Our
research brings together principles from self-regulated learning theory, effective goal pursuit,
and wise intervention methods to demonstrate the important causal relationship between
strategic resource use and academic achievement. More consequentially, we show that this
psychology can be changed in a scalable way to empower many learners.
BACKGROUND
Every year, millions of dollars are spent by governments worldwide on improving educa-
tion through the provision of new or better-quality resources for learners (Organisation
for Economic Co-operation and Development, 2018; World Bank Group, 2018). These
might include, but are not limited to, textbooks, course pack readings, workbooks with
166
The Strategic Resource Use Intervention 167
practice problems, online encyclopedias, discussion forums, practice exam questions, sup-
plementary or remedial classes, workshops on study skills, and more. In schools, teachers
and school administrators never stop finding ways to create, evaluate, and choose educa-
tional resources to support student learning. Because of all these efforts, many learners
in our developed world today have excellent learning resources at their fingertips (or at
least attainable with some initiative on their parts). However, providing students with
resources does not directly improve mastery. Instead, students have to select and use the
resources wisely when learning to reap the pedagogical benefits that they bring.
The problem is that many students do not exercise such self-regulation in their learn-
ing (Pintrich, Smith, Garcia, & McKeachie, 1991; Zimmerman, 2011; Zimmerman &
Martinez-Pons, 1988). For example, when I asked two undergraduate students of mine to
describe the way that they generally studied for exams, one answered, “How do I prepare
for my exams? I just take out my textbook and start reading.” Another described the
process of studying as “You just take out your notes and the textbook and . . . start look-
ing through them.” The process of studying for exams, it seems, is often construed by
these students as a passive, unplanned activity that one begins without much forethought
or strategizing. This is in stark contrast to the way teachers and researchers conceive of
effective, self-regulated learning as an active process of trial and self-reflection (Bembe-
nutty, 2011; Chen, Ong, & Coppola, 2019).
When students do not approach their learning strategically—or in a self-reflective
and goal-directed manner—they may not learn or perform to their potential. As psy-
chologists, educators, school administrators, members of government, and parents, it
behooves us to do something to help maximize, rather than waste, our society’s human
potential.
PSYCHOLOGICAL PROCESS
Metacognition
How can we help our learners use the resources available to them to learn effectively?
A key is the psychological process of “metacognitive self-regulation,” or “metacogni-
tion” for short—defined as thinking about one’s approaches to learning (Pintrich, 2002;
Pintrich et al., 1991). Exercising metacognition goes beyond being aware of and know-
ing how to execute specific study skills (such as elaboration and organization)—it also
involves changing the way learners think about and approach their learning (Pintrich,
2002; Zimmerman, 1989). This involves strategizing what approaches might be appro-
priate to the task, monitoring how effective these approaches are, fine-tuning and chang-
ing approaches when unproductive, and reflecting on feedback (Chen, Chavez, Ong, &
Gunderson, 2017; Chen, Powers, Katragadda, Cohen, & Dweck, 2020; Pintrich, 2002;
Pintrich et al., 1991). Importantly, practicing metacognition during learning relates to
many important learning processes and outcomes, including greater self-efficacy, higher
levels of motivation, more effective learning strategies, higher exam performance, and
even better academic trajectories over time (Chen et al., 2017; Chen, Ong, et al., 2019;
Pintrich, Smith, Garcia, & McKeachie, 1993; Pintrich & De Groot, 1990).
To date, metacognition has been widely applied to many learning processes—
including how learners assess the extent of their learning, estimate the ease or difficulty
168 I. Education
of a new learning task, and switch cognitive strategies when in error (Dunlosky & Bjork,
2008; Dunlosky & Metcalfe, 2009; Fleming & Frith, 2014). However, a comparatively
understudied, albeit immensely important, part of the metacognitive process is how
learners manage their resource use in a self-reflective and purposeful manner. This has
been called “resource regulation” or “resource management”—and it is the psychological
process that I refer to when I describe people as being strategic in their resource use (Chen
et al., 2017; Chen, Ong, et al., 2019; Pintrich et al., 1991).
Resource Regulation
Resource regulation involves knowing what resources are available, but also the condi-
tional knowledge about when and how to use them effectively (Pintrich, 2002; Pintrich
& Garcia, 1994). Students who are not strategic in their resource use may be aware of
the various resources available to them, but they might not think about which specific
resources could be more or less conducive to learning for particular subjects and exam
formats; they might not deliberately think about the purpose for which they would use
each resource; or they might not be planful about how they could use each resource effec-
tively. Learners who practice strategic resource use, on the other hand, tend to consider
what they are expected to know when taking the exam, they strategize which resources
they should use to make their learning effective, they know the purpose for which they
intend to use each resource, and they are intentional and planful about how to use each
resource effectively. Although resources broadly encompass both internal (e.g., motiva-
tion levels, emotions, attention) and external resources (e.g., books, online websites,
classmates, discussion forum members, teachers, academic advisors), in this chapter
and in our strategic resource use intervention, I focus on students’ use of their external
resources during learning.
Prior work on students’ self-regulation of external resources has generally measured
resource regulation through retrospective self-reports of how they use their study time,
how strategically they select their study environments, how well they stick to their study
schedule, and whether they seek help appropriately (Bergin, Reilly, & Traynor, 2005;
Karabenick, 2003; Pintrich et al., 1991). For example, students respond to items such
as “I make good use of my study time for this course” and “I usually study in a place
where I can concentrate on my coursework” on scales from 1 (Not at all true of me) to
7 (Very true of me). Such research on resource regulation has mostly been correlational
in nature (e.g., Bergin et al., 2005; Chen, Ong, et al., 2019; Pintrich et al., 1991, 1993;
Valle et al., 2008). Importantly, the findings show that students who report being more
strategic in their resource use tend to be more intrinsically motivated, and they perform
better in their classes (Karabenick, 2003; Pintrich et al., 1991). However, they leave open
questions about causality—whether promoting more strategic resource use can causally
enhance learning and academic performance.
about how to make effective use of their resources when studying for an exam (Chen et
al., 2017). The intervention has been represented as a survey on student learning in the
class, and presented as a series of short messages and questions that students answer on
their own before they start studying for an exam. It prompts students to ask themselves—
and to answer for themselves: “What am I expected to master by the upcoming exam?”;
“What kinds of resources are available to me?”; “Which resources should I use to study
effectively, and which resources may not be as helpful?”; “Why will each resource I have
chosen to use be useful for my learning?”; and “How should I plan to use the resources
to study?”
In other words, the intervention does not teach students study strategies, tell them
how they should be studying, or prescribe what resources they should use—instead, it
guides students through the process of self-reflection by providing questions that encour-
age metacognition and model answers to scaffold their responses. This way, students are
empowered to take ownership of their strategies and plans (Janis & King, 1954; Walton
& Cohen, 2011; Wilson, 1990).
The strategic resource use intervention approach resonates with the “wise interven-
tion” approach in social psychology, because it targets how students make sense of their
learning process to improve how effectively they leverage their resources when learning
(Walton, 2014; Walton & Wilson, 2018). Our approach is also consistent with the spirit
of self-regulated learning, which involves a self-directed, agentic process of shaping one’s
own learning (Zimmerman, 2002).
EMPIRICAL EVIDENCE
Outcomes
So far, two classroom experiments have tested the strategic resource use intervention
and yielded promising results (Chen et al., 2017). Two double-blind, randomized, con-
trolled experiments were conducted in an introductory college statistics course of a large,
public Midwestern university. Each experiment recruited separate cohorts of students,
1 year apart. Students took the intervention (or a control) online as part of a survey
on student learning in the class, which took about 15 minutes to complete. They were
invited to do this about a week before each of their two class exams. Upon accessing the
online survey, students were randomly assigned to receive either the strategic resource
use intervention (treatment condition) or a control message (control condition). Those in
the control condition read a “business-as-usual” exam reminder, which informed them
about their upcoming exam in the way that their class instructor usually announced
exam reminders (and also consistent with the way that other instructors at that university
generally reminded their students about upcoming exams). Students randomly assigned
to the treatment condition also received that exam reminder and then completed a short
exercise that prompted them to strategize and plan out their resource use for their upcom-
ing exam (see the “Intervention Content and Implementation” section for more details).
Students in both conditions were comparable on their prior achievement levels (i.e., high
school grade-point average [GPA] and college GPA), their desired grades on each exam,
their levels of motivation to achieve their desired grades, how important these grades
were to them, and their confidence in attaining their desired grades.
170 I. Education
In both experiments, students who were randomly assigned to the treatment con-
dition outperformed their peers in the control condition on both class exams, and by
an average of one-third of a letter grade in the class. In Experiments 1 and 2, respec-
tively, there was an average difference of 3.64 and 4.21% in students’ final course grades
between those randomly assigned to the treatment and control conditions (Chen et al.,
2017). To illustrate this in practical terms, students who would have otherwise achieved
a B in the class instead performed at the level of a B+ if they were randomly assigned to
the intervention condition, and students who would otherwise have gotten a B+ instead
attained an A– in the intervention condition.
These intervention effects were not significantly moderated by students’ academic
achievement levels prior to starting the class, gender, race, or class standing. Although a
lack of moderation might seem surprising in light of other social-psychological interven-
tions that tend to target and benefit one group of students more than another, there was
no reason for us to believe a priori that different groups of students in that class were
already carrying out self-reflective resource use to different extents. Our findings (cor-
roborating observations of students in the class before the study) seem to suggest that the
self-reflective process our intervention targeted is one that many students were generally
unaware of or not practicing optimally on their own, at least with this student popula-
tion in this class context. Thus, the strategic resource use intervention had the potential
to benefit many learners in this class.
Mechanisms
What mechanisms might explain the intervention effects? The strategic resource use
intervention was designed to prompt students to self-reflect on how to approach their
learning effectively, so that they would direct their resource use in a more effective man-
ner when studying. We assessed these hypothesized mechanisms of self-reflection and
effective resource use. We measured self-reflection at the end of the class (to prevent prim-
ing these ideas among the control group midway through the class) with items such as
“As I studied for the class, I kept monitoring whether or not the way I was studying was
effective” and “I actively evaluated how well certain study techniques were working for
me in this class.” Through postexam surveys administered after each exam, we measured
students’ reports of how effective their resource use was when studying, and averaged this
across both exams to compute a proxy for how effectively they were using their resources
over the course of the class, on average.
Consistent with the psychological process described earlier, we found that students
in the treatment group reported being more self-reflective about their learning during
the class, compared to those in the control group. In other words, they reported actively
tailoring how they studied to the class context, monitoring how effectively they were
studying, changing the way they were studying when unproductive, and reflecting on
their performance during the class at a greater frequency, compared to controls. The
more students exercised such self-reflective learning, the higher the reported effectiveness
of their resource use, which in turn, related to better overall class performance (see Figure
7.1; Chen et al., 2017).
In addition to making students more self-reflective, we found that the strategic
resource use intervention also reduced negative feelings that students had about their
upcoming exams and increased their perceived control over their exam performance
The Strategic Resource Use Intervention 171
FIGURE 7.1. Modeling the process by which the intervention affects students’ final course per-
formance, mediated by their self-reflections on learning and the reported effectiveness of their
resource use when studying. Condition is coded as follows: control = 0, treatment = 1. CI repre-
sents the confidence interval. Residual error terms are not included in this figure. Reprinted with
permission from Chen, Chavez, Ong, and Gunderson (2017).
(Chen et al., 2017). Although not the main hypothesized mechanisms of the intervention,
these additional motivational effects of the intervention are worth noting, given that
exam-oriented negative affect (such as math anxiety) and perceptions of controllabil-
ity contribute to student achievement (Ashcraft, 2002; Ashcraft & Kirk, 2001; Skinner,
Wellborn, & Connell, 1990).
Importantly, the intervention did not just make students use more resources—in
fact, students in the treatment condition reported using fewer resources, on average, than
those in the control condition (Chen et al., 2017). This suggests that students in the
treatment condition were using their resources more effectively by reflecting on how
to approach their learning, rather than just using a greater number of resources. These
results are consistent with our goal of making learners more strategic (i.e., self-reflective
and goal directed)—not just getting them to study harder in a thoughtless, ineffective
manner.
follow-through mattered. Only the number of resources that students had planned to
use and reported using (“planned-and-used” resources) positively related to their exam
performance, but not the number of resources that students had planned to use but did
not follow through with (“planned-but-did-not-use” resources). The takeaway is that
students should make practical plans about their resource use in advance, and also make
it a point to follow through with their plans.
Taken together, our two field experiments demonstrated that promoting strategic
resource use can causally enhance learning and performance, and our findings provide
evidence for the psychological process by which it does so.
Effects over Time
Currently, the intervention design has focused on preparing students for their upcoming
exams within a particular class. At least within the same class context, the intervention
seems to work well. But can we create a version of the intervention that might enable
students to generalize the psychological process of resource regulation beyond the class
in which it was administered? This could involve asking students how they would apply
this psychological process to their other classes, or asking them how they might use this
in other domains of learning outside school (e.g., learning how to play a musical instru-
ment or a new sports skill). Our ongoing research is looking into how we might be able
to adapt the current intervention design to encourage transfer.
students who do not pay attention to or engage with the intervention in these meaningful
ways may not receive as much benefit as those who do. Future iterations of the interven-
tion could emphasize even more than we have previously done on how students might
include these psychological elements as they self-reflect on their resource use.
Because the intervention targets students’ self-reflection about their resource use, it
is plausible that the intervention confers more benefits to those who do not already carry
out this metacognitive process on their own. Although we did not measure this in our
previous studies, this could be an important moderator of the intervention effects that
future studies should assess.
Learning to proactively self-reflect on one’s resource use could be especially relevant
during developmental transitions that involve moving from learning in a more struc-
tured environment to an environment that requires greater independence and autonomy,
or changing from a familiar context to a new context that requires different strategies
to be effective. One such example is the transition from high school, where learning is
generally more structured and guided by the teacher, to college—a new learning environ-
ment where students are expected to navigate multiple, varied resources, to manage their
time on their own, and to figure out how to master the content independently. Another
example is the transition from school to the workforce, where many supervisors expect
employees to be resourceful and autonomous in picking up the necessary skills to get the
job done well. During these transitions, old ways of doing things may no longer work as
well in the new contexts. Hence, many people often feel overwhelmed by the experience
that their familiar methods are no longer as effective, by the number and variety of new
options to try, by the lack of structured guidance, and by the fact that there may not
always be a clear best way to learn. It is in these very moments that the strategic resource
use intervention might be especially useful.
Although we did not explicitly test this, I believe that students should want to invest
the effort to learn for the intervention to be effective. Without at least a basic amount
of motivation to try, neither strategizing nor planning how to use one’s resources for
learning might be fruitful. This is related to the idea of follow-through described earlier,
where the number of resources that students planned and followed through mattered
for performance, but not the number of resources that they planned but did not see
through. Specifically, this intervention is not targeted at motivating unmotivated stu-
dents to study—instead, it helps students who already have some motivation to learn to
channel their efforts productively. It is conceivable that pairing this intervention with one
that boosts motivation, such as a growth mindset intervention, a purpose intervention,
or a utility-value intervention (e.g., Blackwell, Trzesniewski, & Dweck, 2007; Hulleman
& Harackiewicz, 2009; Yeager et al., 2014), might better help students who lack self-
reflection and who also need some motivating.
COUSINS
The strategic resource use intervention differs in a number of ways from most traditional
study skills interventions that focus on telling students what learning strategies (e.g., high-
lighting, rehearsal, elaboration, organization, self-testing) are more or less effective for the
average learner (Hattie, 2009; Wingate, 2006). First, training cognitive learning strate-
gies, which increases learners’ awareness of various study strategies and how to execute
174 I. Education
them, is generally considered skill building, whereas the strategic resource use interven-
tion is a self-reflection exercise that walks learners through the metacognitive process of
strategizing and planning out how to maximize their resource use to learn (Chen et al.,
2017; Pintrich, 2002). The self-regulated learning literature clearly contrasts this meta-
cognitive process of “planning, monitoring, and regulating” learning (Pintrich, 2002,
p. 220) against just having the knowledge about various learning strategies. Second, while
many study skills interventions are often prescriptive in nature, the strategic resource
use intervention avoids telling students how they should study, but rather, guides them
through a process of effective strategizing and planning. Third, study skills interventions
in many schools are often taught in a general manner that is divorced from specific sub-
ject knowledge (Wingate, 2006), but the strategic resource use intervention is designed to
teach metacognitive principles through application within a specific class context.
The strategic resource use intervention belongs to a family of social-psychological
interventions that change the way people think about their goals and how they pursue them
(Walton & Wilson, 2018). Specifically, the intervention helps learners reflect on how they
might use the resources available to them in a purposeful, effective manner to achieve their
academic goals. Other interventions of this kind engage people in active reflection about
the goal pursuit process by asking them to commit to when, where, and how they would
take a desired action, or by having them anticipate obstacles that they might encounter
and describing what they would do to overcome these obstacles when working toward
their goals (Gollwitzer, 1999; Gollwitzer & Brandstätter, 1997; Oettingen & Gollwitzer,
2010). Such interventions have been effective at increasing the likelihood that people actu-
ally carry out the desirable behaviors, and also effective at producing significant gains in
goal achievement (Duckworth, Grant, Loew, Oettingen, & Gollwitzer, 2011; Duckworth,
Kirby, Gollwitzer, & Oettingen, 2013; Gollwitzer, 1999). The strategic resource use inter-
vention leverages this wisdom by getting people to commit to when, where, and how they
will take action on their goals. However, it is also unique in asking people to reflect on
which specific resources would be effective for their goal pursuit and why.
The strategic resource use intervention is similar to growth mindset of intelligence
interventions in that both focus on orienting people toward generating and employ-
ing effective strategies that are associated with internal and controllable attributions.
Growth mindset interventions focus on changing students’ beliefs about their intelligence
by teaching them that intelligence is controllable and malleable through hard work, effec-
tive strategies, and help from others (Aronson, Fried, & Good, 2002; Blackwell et al.,
2007; Paunesku et al., 2015; Yeager et al., 2019). These growth mindset interventions
have been very effective at changing learners’ beliefs about intelligence and improving
their academic achievement trajectories. The strategic resource use intervention and the
growth mindset interventions, however, differ in their main goals and mechanisms. By
changing people’s beliefs about the malleability of intelligence, growth mindset interven-
tions incline people toward a mastery orientation in their learning. This focus on mastery
orients people toward searching for effective strategies and greater persistence in times of
challenge or difficulty. The strategic resource use intervention, however, does not aim to
change people’s beliefs about their intelligence, but it targets their metacognition about
their resource use. Specifically, this intervention prompts people to reflect and plan what
strategies to employ, monitor their strategies as they learn, and switch strategies if they
are ineffective.
The Strategic Resource Use Intervention 175
Context and Timing
To date, we have published two randomized, controlled experiments on the strategic
resource use intervention that was conducted in an introductory statistics course at a
large, public Midwestern university (Chen et al., 2017). The statistics course was an
important prerequisite course to many majors in the natural sciences, social sciences,
business school, and premedicine track, and it additionally satisfied a quantitative skill
requirement at the undergraduate level. Therefore, students enrolled in the class generally
took their learning and performance seriously. All students in the class were taught by
the same instructor during the spring semesters when we conducted our studies, ensur-
ing that the instructional content and style were consistent across all our participants.
Students who consented to participate were randomized via the survey software to either
a treatment condition (where they would receive a business-as-usual exam reminder, the
strategic resource use intervention exercise, and survey questions) or a control condition
(where they would receive the same business-as-usual exam reminder and survey ques-
tions, but not the intervention).
The intervention was distributed online via “preexam surveys” to students in the
class about 10 days before each of their two class exams, with a participation deadline of
about 7 days prior to each exam. We chose this timing for this specific class context with
the instructor because students in the class during the spring semester typically started
studying about a week before their exams. Distributing the intervention at this time gave
students the opportunity to think about how they would approach their studying before
most of them actually started studying. It was also not too far in advance of their exams
so as to render the self-reflection exercise irrelevant to them.
Representation
We represented our intervention as a survey to understand student learning in the class.
The online intervention interface was modeled to look like a generic survey about stu-
dents’ goals, motivations, and feelings about the exam, along with an exam reminder and
open-ended questions about students’ study plans. It took about 15 minutes to complete
online.
Intervention Content
Overview
The survey began with an exam reminder that all students in the class typically received
from the instructor: “Your exam is coming up on [day and date]. You might want to start
preparing for it.” Students then completed our brief strategic resource use intervention
exercise where they were prompted to actively think about their upcoming exam format;
select which resources out of the many available to them they would use to study effec-
tively; explain why each resource would be useful to their learning; and plan out when,
where, and how they would use the resources. Next, I describe more details about each
of these components of the intervention exercise.
176 I. Education
Details
The strategic resource use exercise started with a message emphasizing scientific findings
that high-achieving students use their resources strategically when preparing for exams.
Students were then prompted to consider the kinds of questions that they expected on
their upcoming exam, and to think through what resources (including study materials
and people who can help them) would help them prepare for the exam most effectively.
These brief messages were meant to motivate them to engage in the intervention and to
initiate their self-reflection in an open-ended way, before the scaffolding that followed.
Following these general messages, students received more specific questions about
strategizing and planning out their resource use within their given class context. Students
were presented with a comprehensive checklist of all the class resources available to them,
and they were asked to select the resources out of this checklist that would help them
prepare for their upcoming exam effectively. For example, some resources that students
in the statistics course had were lecture notes, practice exam questions, past homework
problems, textbook readings, yellow formula card, online problem-solving applet, dis-
cussions with other students in the class (e.g., peer study groups), office hours held by the
lecture instructor, and office hours held by the graduate student instructor (i.e., teaching
assistant). This means that students had to reflect on which resources would be effec-
tive for developing mastery and which resources might not be so effective for their own
learning, thereby prioritizing their resource use. We generated this comprehensive list
of resources through discussions with the class instructor and teaching assistants, focus
groups with students who had previously taken the class, and student surveys.
After making their selections, students were asked to describe why they thought each
resource they selected would be useful for their exam preparation. Prompting students to
consider and make explicit the purpose for which they will use each of the resources they
had chosen is an important step in the self-reflective process, because it helps them bet-
ter channel their resource use more effectively toward those specific goals when they are
studying. This reflection may also make students aware of when they may not be using
a resource for its intended purpose. However, not all students may know how to give a
thoughtful answer to the question “Why do you think these resources would be useful for
your exam preparation?” without some guidance, even when they might be motivated to
self-reflect. Thus, immediately after students read the question, they were given a model
example to scaffold their responses:
The model example guided their responses by showing them how they might think
through the purposes for which they would use the various resources chosen (Collins,
Brown, & Holum, 1991; Schunk, 1981).
We observed from students’ responses that they indeed seemed to be using the model
examples provided to scaffold their own thought processes and responses to the prompt.
Here is an example of one student’s answer:
The Strategic Resource Use Intervention 177
“Lecture notes, past HW problems, practice exam questions: these will be helpful for
me because I can see the steps necessary for completing a problem and I can practice
doing the problems with a guide (my lecture notes) and follow the steps on those
until I can do them without using my lecture notes.
“Discussions, private tutoring: These will help me to verbally talk through my
problems so that I can make sure I know what I am talking about when I do the
problems on my own.”
The final step was planning when, where, and how they would use their resources.
The purpose of this planning component was to increase the likelihood that students
would actually follow through with their proposed strategies, rather than just building
castles in the air that they never implement (Leventhal, Singer, & Jones, 1965; Gollwit-
zer, 1999). As Ross and Nisbett (2011, p. 227) aptly put it, concrete planning helps to
“(facilitate) the link between positive intentions and constructive actions.” Students were
asked to plan out how they would use their resources to study, and again, they were given
a model example to guide their planning that showed specific, concrete plans of when,
where, and how a student might use their resources:
“Wednesday, 3/18, 3:00 P.M.–5:00 P.M., Library: Practice exam questions from 1 past
exam.”
“Friday, 3/20, 1:00 P.M.–3:00 P.M., Home: Read lecture notes for 2 topics.”
Here is an example of a student’s plan, which clearly shows in its structure and content
that they used the model example to guide their own response:
“Sunday 5/25—home: relistening to lectures I had trouble with and looking over
lecture notes.”
“Monday 5/26—home: look over old homework and listen to more lectures, start
HW 3.”
“Tuesday 5/27—library: finish HW 3 and do problem roulette.”
Importantly, this plan includes concrete descriptions of when, where, and how the stu-
dent intends to use the resources to study.
Control Condition
In each of the two published experiments (Chen et al., 2017), we compared the students
who were randomly assigned to the treatment condition to their peers who were ran-
domly assigned to the control condition. The control presented students with a business-
as-usual exam reminder that students typically received. After this, students filled out
survey questions. They were not exposed to any of the active ingredients of the strategic
resource use intervention in their surveys. We designed the control to be as close to busi-
ness as usual in the class, making sure that we did not take away anything from the class
that the instructor or students would normally do if the study had not been conducted. As
178 I. Education
mentioned earlier, students randomly assigned to the control condition were comparable
to those assigned to the treatment before the intervention.
NUANCES AND MISCONCEPTIONS
Not Just Planning
Although the strategic resource use intervention does involve a planning component, it
is not just another planning intervention. First, the mechanisms of the strategic resource
use intervention involve increasing students’ self-reflection on their learning and, in turn,
more effective resource use. Traditional planning interventions do not target these mech-
anisms (e.g., Gollwitzer, 1999; Oettingen & Gollwitzer, 2010). Second, we found that
the active psychological ingredients in students’ responses to the intervention, which were
positively related to their final course grades, went beyond planning: The more strategi-
cally students chose their resources (1) by taking the format of the upcoming exam into
consideration, and (2) by focusing on the goal of maximizing mastery, the better they
performed in the class. Our findings underscore that the psychology of our intervention
goes beyond committing to a plan of action.
The Strategic Resource Use Intervention 179
IMPLICATIONS FOR PRACTICE
The efficacy of the strategic resource use intervention is testimony to the wisdom that
learning is more effective when it is carried out in a strategic (rather than a passive or
an unregulated) manner. This is wisdom that many educators and psychologists already
have; yet it is also an idea that is sometimes challenging to convey to students in a way
that they can execute on their own. The strategic resource use intervention is one bite-
sized, online, and self-administered approach that educators and students can use to
empower learners.
Administration Format
Because of its brief, online, and self-administered nature, the strategic resource use
intervention can be conveniently disseminated to many students. Students access the
intervention via a survey link, or through an online learning platform (e.g., Electronic
Coach [ECoach] learning platform; Huberth, Chen, Tritz, & McKay, 2015). This online
administration makes the intervention easily scalable and it also caters to the preferences
of many learners among today’s Internet-savvy generation. Although we have not tried
implementing the intervention through pen-and-paper format, it is possible that adminis-
tration through this means could also be effective if the core components of the interven-
tion are retained.
Customization
When applying the intervention to a new class context, it is important to retain the key
psychological ingredients of strategic resource use, while also tailoring the intervention
content to be relevant to the class. Table 7.1 summarizes the key parts of the intervention
and provides examples of answers that exemplify the metacognitive process each part
targets. These are important parts of the psychological process that should be concep-
tually replicated in the new class context. At the same time, the details of the resource
checklist and model examples in the intervention should be tailored to make sure that
they are relevant to students’ learning within the specific class. This class-specific tailor-
ing also applies to the exam reminder that precedes the strategic resource use exercise.
For instance, the following is part of the planning model example that we tailored for a
different college chemistry class in ongoing, unpublished research (Cheng et al., 2019):
“Saturday, 10/7, 1:00 P.M.–3:30 P.M., Home: Redo tough problems in lab section and
write-ups, do extra practice problems on Canvas.”
“Sunday, 10/8, 3:00 P.M.–5:30 P.M., Library: Do timed practice exam, try to find
other examples of questions that I got stuck on.”
“Monday, 10/9, 10:00 A.M.–11:30 A.M., Chemistry Gazebo: Attend office hours with
the problems I had difficulty with.”
As this example illustrates, the dates, times, study locations, resources, and their use
are specific to the chemistry course and relevant to the timing of the students’ upcom-
ing exam. In short, designing the intervention involves integrating general principles of
180 I. Education
TABLE 7.1. Summary of Key Components of the Strategic Resource Use Intervention
Examples of answers that exemplify the
Components Key self-reflective questions psychological process
1. Clarify goals. “What grade am I aiming “I am aiming for an A on this exam, which
for?” means a score of 90 or higher.”
“For the exam, what am I “I am expected to master the topics from
expected to know and do Chapters 1 to 6.”
well?”
“How is the exam formatted “I need to explain the processes of
and what kinds of questions geological formations in open-ended essay
will I expect on the exam?” questions.”
4. Explain the purpose “Why would each resource “Trying the practice exam under timed
for which each be useful to my learning?” conditions will give me practice on problems
resource is used. similar to the kind that I will encounter on
my actual exam and also practice answering
them within the 75-minute time limit. As I
do the exam, I will look out for how I am
expected to apply my knowledge, so that I
know what content I should review later.”
“For what purpose do “I will do the extra practice assignments,
I intend to use each as well as the online practice problems,
resource?” especially for topics that I find difficult.
This will help me solidify my confidence
to handle the kinds of problems that I find
most challenging.”
5. Plan concretely how “When, where, and how “This Wednesday between 4 P.M. and 6
to use the resources. will I use each resource?” P.M. in the library, I plan to finish reading
Chapters 5 and 6 of my textbook. That same
day after dinner between 8 P.M. and 10 P.M.,
I will then meet with my study group in
the dorm study room to try practice exam
questions for Chapters 5 and 6. We will try
the questions independently and then discuss
those that each of us got wrong.”
The Strategic Resource Use Intervention 181
strategic resource use with the concrete, class-specific application of these principles to
make sure that the intervention is relevant.
own. Having said this, some mistakes are inevitable during the trial-and-error process of
designing and testing any new intervention. As we further develop the strategic resource
use intervention and test it in new class contexts, we can expect to make some mistakes
that we might not have foreseen; but importantly, we can all sensibly learn from such trial
and error along the way.
1 Thereal identities of students have been replaced by fictitious identities in brackets, to protect their
anonymity.
The Strategic Resource Use Intervention 183
He said this was the first time he ever really took a step back and thought seriously about
his study habits and why he’s using particular resources.” These preliminary feedback
seem to suggest to us that students do make use of the Exam Playbook, and that they find
it helpful. Such feedback has been encouraging to us, as we continue to seek teacher and
school partners to further test, refine, and distribute the intervention.
IMPLICATIONS FOR PSYCHOLOGICAL THEORY
Strategy: The Neglected Attribution
Our research places the spotlight back on an attribution that has been well studied
among learners in cognitive and educational psychology, but relatively neglected in social
psychology: strategy. The takeaway here is not about the importance of raw intellect or
sheer effort (e.g., how many hours a student studies or how many resources a student
uses)—although these are certainly important. Instead, our findings focus on the impor-
tance of self-reflective and goal-directed resource use during learning. Regardless of how
capable and effortful they may be, students may still not perform to their full potential
if they do not approach their learning strategically. The good news is that guiding the
average learner to become more strategic empowers them to learn more effectively and
to perform at a level higher than what they otherwise might be able to achieve (Chen et
al., 2017).
them to take control of and manage their learning. Given these complementary self-
regulatory processes, one might imagine that students might benefit all the more from
marrying a growth mindset message with guided prompts that encourage them to gener-
ate their own ways of investing effort strategically.
The Student’s Perspective
To date, education research has made giant strides in enhancing curriculum and peda-
gogy, especially from the perspective of the teacher or administrator (e.g., “How can
we teach better?”; Hattie, 2009). However, quite a significant portion of work on the
psychology of the student focuses on individual differences (e.g., personality, intelligence,
conscientiousness, working memory capacity), prior educational background, and demo-
graphic differences (Hattie, 2009). While these are valuable research directions, have we
been neglecting what learning means and how learning occurs from the perspective of
the student?
As students learn, develop, and encounter challenges as they grow, many of them
inevitably ask themselves questions such as, “Does this matter?” (e.g., Hulleman & Har-
ackiewicz, 2009; Yeager et al., 2014), “Am I dumb?” (e.g., Molden & Dweck, 2006;
Paunesku et al., 2015), and “Do I belong here?” (e.g., Walton & Cohen, 2011). Social-
psychological interventions address how frequently students are asking these questions,
and just as important, how they are answering them (Walton, 2014; Walton & Wilson,
2018). However, when it comes to managing their own learning, many students are not
asking themselves self-reflective questions about their learning enough—for example,
“How will I learn this subject?” (e.g., Zimmerman, 2002), “How should I plan my study
time?” (Gollwitzer, 1999), and “How will I study effectively for this upcoming exam?”
(Chen et al., 2017). The strategic resource use intervention aims to encourage more of
such self-reflection in students, and to scaffold their answers to important questions like
these.
FUTURE DIRECTIONS
Although we know that the strategic resource use intervention was effective for many
learners in one class context, there is still much to do to better understand the generaliz-
ability of its effects to different kinds of students and other learning contexts. For now,
we cannot yet conclude how well the effects of the intervention generalize to other types
of students (e.g., students of different ages, education levels, or cognitive abilities), sub-
jects (e.g., an English literature class vs. a statistics class), class structures (e.g., multiple
smaller tests rather than a few big exams, curved grading systems vs. noncurved grading
systems), exam formats (e.g., multiple-choice tests vs. open-ended essay questions), or
teaching styles (e.g., teaching students to study in a tightly structured manner vs. giving
students more autonomy over their own learning). Research in these areas will be valu-
able for building a better understanding of the possible heterogeneity of the intervention
effects and its boundary conditions. Importantly, we will also learn how to tailor the
current intervention design to better cater to different students and learning contexts.
Another exciting question for this area of research is: How can we adapt the interven-
tion to facilitate transfer of these self-regulatory principles? With the current class-specific
The Strategic Resource Use Intervention 185
design, we might not expect students to naturally generalize this metacognitive process
on their own to other learning contexts. The challenge is to teach resource regulation in a
way that concretely relates to students’ learning in their classes, but that at that same time
comes across as applicable to their learning more broadly. The good news is that we have
started thinking about ways to adapt the current intervention design to promote transfer.
As aforementioned, we are trying out methods such as asking students to apply the same
strategic planning process to another challenging class of theirs, asking students to teach
strategic resource use in a way that is applicable to another student who is learning a
different skill (e.g., music or sports), and instilling a mindset that orients people toward
being metacognitive more generally (Chen et al., 2020). This question about transfer is
important to pin down if we want to make scientifically informed judgments about the
dosage and design of the intervention to foster extensive, long-lasting benefits for learn-
ers.
Another future direction is to lower the barriers for teachers to customize the inter-
vention for their students. At present, customization involves a close partnership between
the researchers and instructors to produce a comprehensive resource checklist, to identify
how students are typically reminded to study for their exams in the class, and to edit the
model examples if they are not as relevant. The time and effort involved in this collabo-
ration might be a barrier to some teachers offering the intervention to their students. If
we can find ways to lower these barriers—such as by making the model examples more
generalizable, offering teachers a tool that allows them to easily self-customize the inter-
vention and distribute it to their classes, or creating a general version of the intervention
that is not specific to a subject or class—we might be able to make this more accessible
to teachers and students.
In our studies, we found that the intervention did not only boost self-reflection but
it also decreased students’ negative affect toward their upcoming exams, and increased
their perceived control over their exam grades. Might there be other effects of the inter-
vention that we have not measured or considered? It would be fruitful for future studies
to examine how the intervention might affect students’ classroom behaviors (Are they
engaging with their homework assignments, laboratory exercises, and class discussion
forums differently?), their motivation to learn (Are they more inclined toward a mas-
tery orientation in their learning?), their growth mindset (Does the intervention change
their fundamental beliefs about intelligence?), and their interactions with their peers and
teachers (Are they seeking help from others in a more effective manner?). These and other
factors are theoretically plausible, but unexplored, mechanisms of the intervention that
could contribute to students’ learning and academic performance across class contexts
and in an enduring manner.
CONCLUDING THOUGHTS
The fact that so much is being done by so many—including teachers, parents, schools,
governments—across the world is testimony to how much we all value learning. We can-
not afford to waste all these efforts to improve students’ learning on mindless, ineffective
resource use. My hope is that, with more research and interventions like the strategic
resource use intervention, our psychological science will keep finding new and better
ways to help our learners use their resources wisely and effectively to learn.
186 I. Education
REFERENCES
Aronson, J., Fried, C. B., & Good, C. (2002). Reducing the effects of stereotype threat on African
American college students by shaping theories of intelligence. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 38, 113–125.
Ashcraft, M. H. (2002). Math anxiety: Personal, educational, and cognitive consequences. Cur-
rent Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 181–185.
Ashcraft, M. H., & Kirk, E. P. (2001). The relationships among working memory, math anxiety,
and performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 224–237.
Bembenutty, H. (2011). Self-regulated learning. Danvers, MA: Wiley Periodicals.
Bergin, S., Reilly, R., & Traynor, D. (2005). Examining the role of self-regulated learning on
introductory programming performance. In Proceedings of the first international work-
shop on computing education research (pp. 81–86). New York: Association of Computing
Machinery.
Blackwell, L. A., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Dweck, C. S. (2007). Theories of intelligence and achieve-
ment across the junior high school transition: A longitudinal study and an intervention. Child
Development, 78, 246–263.
Chen, P., Chavez, O., Ong, D. C., & Gunderson, B. (2017). Strategic resource use for learning: A
self-administered intervention that guides self-reflection on effective resource use enhances
academic performance. Psychological Science, 28, 774–785.
Chen, P., Ong, D. C., & Coppola, B. C. (2019). Explore, exploit, and prune in the classroom: Mod-
eling the decision processes behind learners’ resource regulation. Manuscript under review.
Chen, P., Powers, J. T., Katragadda, K. R., Cohen, G. L., & Dweck, C. S. (2020). A strategic
mindset: An orientation toward strategic behavior during goal pursuit. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 117, 14066–14072.
Cheng, K. M., Chen, P., Schwartz-Poehlmann, J., Pfalzgraff, W., Ren, F., Katragadda, K. R., . . .
Schwartz, D. L. (2019). Getting a GRIP on resource regulation: Tailoring and testing the
strategic resource-use intervention in college chemistry classes. Manuscript in preparation.
Cohen, G. L., Garcia, J., Apfel, N., & Master, A. (2006). Reducing the racial achievement gap: A
social-psychological intervention. Science, 313, 1307–1310.
Collins, A., Brown, J. S., & Holum, A. (1991). Cognitive apprenticeship: Making thinking visible.
American Educator, 15, 6–11.
Duckworth, A. L., Grant, H., Loew, B., Oettingen, G., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2011). Self-regulation
strategies improve self-discipline in adolescents: Benefits of mental contrasting and imple-
mentation intentions. Educational Psychology, 31, 17–26.
Duckworth, A. L., Kirby, T., Gollwitzer, A., & Oettingen, G. (2013). From fantasy to action:
Mental contrasting with implementation intentions (MCII) improves academic performance
in children. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 4, 745–753.
Dunlosky, J., & Bjork, R. A. (2008). Handbook of metamemory and memory. New York: Psy-
chology Press Taylor & Francis Group.
Dunlosky, J., & Metcalfe, J. (2009). Metacognition. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.
Fleming, S. M., & Frith, C. D. (2014). The cognitive neuroscience of metacognition. Berlin:
Springer.
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American
Psychologist, 54, 493–503.
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Brandstätter, V. (1997). Implementation intentions and effective goal pursuit.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 186–199.
Hattie, J. (2009). Visible learning: A synthesis of over 800 meta-analyses relating to achievement.
New York: Routledge.
Huberth, M., Chen, P., Tritz, J., & McKay, T. A. (2015). E2Coach: Computer-tailored student
support in introductory physics. PLOS ONE, 10, e0137001.
The Strategic Resource Use Intervention 187
Hulleman, C. S., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2009). Promoting interest and performance in high
school science classes. Science, 326, 1410–1412.
Janis, I. L., & King, B. T. (1954). The influence of role playing on opinion change. Journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 49(2), 211–218.
Karabenick, S. A. (2003). Seeking help in large college classes: A person-centered approach. Con-
temporary Educational Psychology, 28, 37–58.
Kipling, R. (1902). The elephant’s child. London: Macmillan.
Leventhal, H., Singer, R., & Jones, S. (1965). Effects of fear and specificity of recommendation
upon attitudes and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2, 20–29.
Molden, D. C., & Dweck, C. S. (2006). Finding “meaning” in psychology: A lay theories approach
to self-regulation, social perception, and social development. American Psychologist, 61(3),
192–203.
Oettingen, G., & Gollwitzer, P. (2010). Strategies of setting and implementing goals: Mental con-
trasting and implementation intentions. In J. E. Maddux & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Social psy-
chological foundations of clinical psychology (pp. 114–135). New York: Guilford Press.
Organisation for Economic Co- operation and Development. (2018). Education spending.
Retrieved March 16, 2018, from https://data.oecd.org/eduresource/education-spending.
htm#indicator-chart.
Paunesku, D., Walton, G. M., Romero, C., Smith, E. N., Yeager, D. S., & Dweck, C. S. (2015).
Mind-set interventions are a scalable treatment for academic underachievement. Psychologi-
cal Science, 26, 784–793.
Pintrich, P. R. (2002). The role of metacognitive knowledge in learning, teaching, and assessing.
Theory into Practice, 41, 219–225.
Pintrich, P. R., & De Groot, E. V. (1990). Motivational and self-regulated learning components of
classroom academic performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 33–40.
Pintrich, P. R., & Garcia, T. (1994). Self-regulated learning in college stdents: Knowledge, strate-
gies, and motivation. In P. Pintrich, D. Brown, & C. Weinstein (Eds.), Student motivation,
cognition, and learning (pp. 113–133). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Pintrich, P. R., Smith, D. A. F., Garcia, T., & McKeachie, W. J. (1991). A manual for the use of
the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ). Retrieved from http://eric.
ed.gov/?id=ED338122.
Pintrich, P. R., Smith, D. A. F., Garcia, T., & McKeachie, W. J. (1993). Reliability and predictive
validity of the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ). Educational and
Psychological Measurement, 53, 801–813.
Plant, E. A., Ericsson, K. A., Hill, L., & Asberg, K. (2005). Why study time does not predict grade
point average across college students: Implications of deliberate practice for academic perfor-
mance. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 30, 96–116.
Ross, L., & Nisbett, R. E. (2011). The person and the situation: Perspectives of social psychology.
London: Pinter & Martin.
Schuman, H., Walsh, E., Olson, C., & Etheridge, B. (1985). Effort and reward: The assumption
that college grades are affected by quantity of study. Social Forces, 63, 945–966.
Schunk, D. H. (1981). Modeling and attribution effects on children’s development: A self-efficacy
analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 93–105.
Skinner, E. A., Wellborn, J. G., & Connell, J. P. (1990). What it takes to do well in school and
whether I’ve got it: A process model of perceived control and children’s engagement and
achievement in school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 22–32.
Valle, A., Núñez, J. C., Cabanach, R. G., González-Pienda, J. A., Rodríguez, S., Rosário, P., . . .
Münoz-Cadavid, M. A. (2008). Self-regulated profiles and academic achievement. Psico-
thema, 20(4), 724–731.
Walton, G. M. (2014). The new science of wise psychological interventions. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 23, 73–82.
188 I. Education
Walton, G. M., & Cohen, G. L. (2011). A brief social-belonging intervention improves academic
and health outcomes of minority students. Science, 331, 1447–1451.
Walton, G. M., & Wilson, T. D. (2018). Wise interventions: Psychological remedies for social and
personal problems. Psychological Review, 125, 617–655.
Wilson, T. D. (1990). Self-persuasion via self-reflection. In J. M. Olson & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), Self-
inference processes: The Ontario symposium (Vol. 6, pp. 43–68). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Wingate, U. (2006). Doing away with “study skills.” Teaching in Higher Education, 11, 457–469.
World Bank Group. (2018). Government expenditure on education, total (% of GDP). Retrieved
March 16, 2018, from https://data.worldbank.org/indicator/SE.XPD.TOTL.GD.ZS.
Yeager, D. S., Hanselman, P., Walton, G. M., Crosnoe, R., Muller, C., Tipton, E., . . . Dweck,
C. S. (2019). A national experiment reveals where a growth mindset improves achievement.
Nature: Human Behavior, 573, 364–369.
Yeager, D. S., Henderson, M. D., Paunesku, D., Walton, G. M., D’Mello, S., Spitzer, B. J., & Duck-
worth, A. L. (2014). Boring but important: A self-transcendent purpose for learning fosters
academic self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107, 559–580.
Zimmerman, B. J. (1989). A social cognitive view of self-regulated academic learning Journal of
Educational Psychology, 81(3), 329–339.
Zimmerman, B. J. (2002). Becoming a self-regulated learner: An overview. Theory into Practice,
41, 64–70.
Zimmerman, B. J. (2011). Motivational sources and outcomes of self-regulated learning and per-
formance. In B. J. Zimmerman & D. H. Schunk (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation of learn-
ing and performance (pp. 49–64). New York: Routledge.
Zimmerman, B. J., & Martinez-Pons, M. (1988). Construct validation of a strategy model of stu-
dent self-regulated learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 284–290.
PA R T II
Happiness Interventions
Kristin Layous
The content of happiness interventions ranges widely (e.g., expressing gratitude, performing
kind acts, focusing on one’s strengths), but all variations seek to address the same underly-
ing potential—that people can increase their own happiness. Across dozens of experiments,
happiness interventions have successfully boosted well-being, often via changes in how
people perceive themselves, their situations, and their relationships. Here I review empirical
evidence demonstrating the efficacy of happiness interventions, the conditions that make
happiness interventions more or less effective (e.g., person-activity fit), the instructions for
particular happiness interventions, and some of the nuances and misconceptions surround-
ing their implementation. In addition, I discuss the process by which happiness interventions
may affect long-term changes in happiness and other outcomes, as well as the potential for
happiness interventions to contribute to broader psychological theory.
BACKGROUND
Across the globe, people report happiness as an important goal for themselves (Diener,
2000) and their children (Diener & Lucas, 2004). Extensive research demonstrates that
191
192 II. Health and Well-Being
happiness is not only a pleasant emotional and cognitive experience, but is also predictive
of and causally related to positive outcomes in relationships, career, and physical health
(Lyubomirsky, King, & Diener, 2005). Thus, happiness aids success in important life
domains, which likely feeds back into one’s happiness in a virtuous recursive cycle.
Therapists have been attempting to relieve patients’ psychological suffering by modi-
fying maladaptive thoughts and behaviors since the late 1800s (see Smith & Glass, 1977,
for the efficacy of therapy), but a direct focus on increasing happiness, rather than sim-
ply alleviating distress, is relatively new (Seligman & Csiksentmihalyi, 2000; but see
Fordyce, 1977, 1983). Critics of happiness pursuit warned that “trying to be happier is
as futile as trying to be taller” (Lykken & Tellegen, 1996, p. 189), but, in their seminal
work, Lyubomirsky, Sheldon, and Schkade (2005) refuted this and other sources of pes-
simism regarding happiness pursuit. They argued that, despite relatively stable influences
on happiness like genetics and life circumstances (estimated to explain about 50 and 10%
of individual differences in happiness, respectively), a large proportion of happiness is left
unexplained by these factors and is likely subject to change by altering how one behaves
and how one perceives him- or herself and his or her social world. Indeed, during the
last two decades, research on happiness interventions has burgeoned, demonstrating that
increasing happiness is possible, even with brief and cost-effective strategies (Bolier et al.,
2013; Sin & Lyubomirsky, 2009).
To explore ways to promote happiness, Lyubomirsky, Sheldon, et al. (2005) analyzed
the thought patterns and behaviors of naturally happy people (see also Fordyce, 1977,
1983). For example, past research has demonstrated that happy people think gratefully,
perform kind acts, and strive for personally meaningful goals, so Lyubomirsky, Sheldon, et
al. (2005) reasoned that prompting people to engage in these types of thoughts or behaviors
might boost their well-being. They demonstrated that instructing people to think gratefully
or perform kind acts increased happiness relative to a control group (see also Emmons &
McCullough, 2003; Seligman, Steen, Park, & Peterson, 2005, for concurrent happiness
intervention studies). These findings provided proof of their concept that engaging in certain
types of thoughts and behaviors—which can be under one’s control—boosts happiness.
Thus, initial happiness interventions were empirically, not theoretically, driven—
researchers explored whether the correlates of happiness could be packaged and admin-
istered to cause increases in happiness. That said, Walton and Wilson (2018) offer a
helpful theoretical framework to understand why these happiness interventions work.
They propose that “wise interventions” are psychologically wise to the way people see
themselves, other people, or a particular situation, and use precise theory- and research-
informed strategies to alter these meanings. They reason that almost every situation is
open to some type of interpretation and it is the inferences that people draw that most
proximally drives behavior. Thus, if meanings can be changed, so can behaviors and,
ultimately, changes in objective life circumstances may follow.
Happiness interventions can readily be interpreted through this wise intervention
framework. First, happiness interventions are wise to the particular thoughts and behav-
iors that promote happiness. Second, via structured reflection exercises or the enactment
of new behaviors, happiness interventions help people make meaning of themselves and
their social world in ways that promote happiness. Third, although the initial change
within a person is subjective (i.e., they view themselves, their relationships, or their cir-
cumstances more positively), this altered state may lead to changes in behavior that builds
on itself in a self-enhancing recursive cycle.
Happiness Interventions 193
PSYCHOLOGICAL PROCESSES
Walton and Wilson (2018) argue that, in order to alter potentially problematic ways of
viewing the self and the social world, researchers need to consider three basic motives that
underlie people’s meaning-making processes: the need to understand oneself and one’s
surroundings (including their social relationships), the need for self-integrity, and the
need to belong. Promoting psychological need satisfaction may be particularly important
in the context of happiness interventions because psychological need fulfillment relates
directly to happiness, which is the primary desired outcome of happiness interventions
(Ryan & Deci, 2000).
a way that undermines their happiness, happiness interventions can alter these thought
processes and behaviors.
For example, happiness interventions focused on gratitude, such as listing what one
is grateful for (in a journal-t ype framework; Emmons & McCullough, 2003) or writing
a letter of gratitude to someone who has done something for you (e.g., Lyubomirsky,
Dickerhoof, Boehm, & Sheldon, 2011), amplify people’s recognition and appreciation of
the good things they already have, helping them construe their lives as more positive than
they did before.
Emmons and McCullough (2003) provided the following examples from people’s
gratitude lists: “waking up this morning,” “the generosity of friends,” “for wonderful
parents,” “to the Lord for just another day,” and “to the Rolling Stones.” None of these
blessings were new positive events happening to participants, but the gratitude activity
helped them actively appreciate aspects of their lives they likely take for granted on most
days. Although this initial change in perception is subjective, over time, this shift in per-
spective may help people take action that leads to more enduring changes in their objec-
tive circumstances (more on this in the “Effects over Time” section).
Similarly, when writing a gratitude letter, people actively appreciate the influential
relationships they already have (or have had in the past). Common themes include close
others giving letter writers unconditional love or social support in times of adversity, as
well as believing wholeheartedly in their ability to succeed. These letters also often reveal
a drive to work hard to prove that their benefactor’s efforts were not wasted, which may
point to ways in which gratitude can affect future behavior. Below are two excerpts from
letters that were representative of many written by college students (unpublished excerpts
from the Layous, Lee, Choi, and Lyubomirsky [2013] study).
Brother,
I wanted to write to you to let you know how grateful I am for everything you have done
for me. You are an amazing brother and mentor, someone who I look up to. I hope one
day I can come close to achieving the goals you have hoped I accomplish. You did not
have to be there for me like you were when we were younger. With mom and dad always
being gone, you raised me and [our other brother], something that would not be easy for
anyone to do, you only being 9 years old when I was born. I will never forget the nights
you stayed home to help me with my homework instead of being a typical teenager who
Happiness Interventions 195
enjoyed the life with friends, parties, and living carefree. . . . Every day I go to school
knowing I have to work hard, and then work harder, not to let you down. You have
never placed worry or pressure on me to succeed in life, but I know deep down, that that
is all you want. I look at you not only as a brother, but as a role model, a father figure,
and one of my best friends. I refuse to mess up my life, and make unforgettable mistakes,
knowing you sacrificed so much just for me to be able to do so. I am forever in debt to
you, in the most loving way possible. I want to do well in school, personal relationships,
work and in life, so I can make myself proud, and you proud. I don’t think I have ever
written out on paper just what exactly you have done for me, or how thankful deep
down I really am. So thank you [brother], thank you so much. If it weren’t for you, I
honestly don’t know what I would be doing. I promise I won’t let you down, [we’re] so
close to accomplishing our life dreams.
I love you . . . [you’re] an amazing brother and you will be an amazing father one day.
Your sister
One study clearly illustrated the ability of happiness interventions to change people’s
construal of their life events. Participants were randomly assigned to one of two happiness
interventions or a control condition (Dickerhoof, 2007). In all three conditions, people
were asked to write down personally satisfying experiences from the week (e.g., “I went
on a job interview, and I got the job on the spot!”) and then participants and independent
coders rated how satisfying the experiences were. Participants in the happiness interven-
tion conditions reported that their experiences were becoming marginally more satisfying
over time compared to participants in the control group. Alternatively, independent cod-
ers actually rated participant experiences as becoming less satisfying over time, indicat-
ing that participant perceptions of increased satisfaction were not objective—they were
due to improved construal of these situations. This perception of satisfaction mediated
the relationship between condition and well-being, indicating that an important way in
which happiness interventions boost well-being is by improving people’s perception of the
events in their lives. Importantly, this altered view of one’s experiences is likely to lead to
altered behavior—perhaps trying new activities or taking on new challenges—that feeds
back into one’s happiness is a recursive cycle. The altered understanding of one’s life is
just the first step to changing cycles of unhappiness.
Need for Self‑Integrity
People also have the need to view themselves as capable, competent agents in the world—
morally and adaptively adequate (Bandura, 1997; Cohen & Sherman, 2014). The grati-
tude letter examples demonstrate that people often write to close others who have believed
in them. Although the gratitude letter prompt does not explicitly ask participants to write
about others who have made them feel worthy, implicit in the activity is writing to some-
one who has invested in the letter writer, thus demonstrating the target’s view of the letter
writer as worthy.
Whereas gratitude interventions prompt reflection as a first step, kindness interven-
tions first prompt a behavior and then leave it up to the participants to reflect upon the
meaning of that behavior. One way in which performing acts of kindness boosts happi-
ness is likely by fostering a sense of accomplishment in the performer (e.g., he or she feels
like a good person). For example, across three experiments, participants prompted to help
196 II. Health and Well-Being
others increased in positive self-evaluations relative to those who did not help (William-
son & Clark, 1989). The self-evaluations included a variety of personal characteristics
that may make up one’s sense of self as morally and adaptively adequate (e.g., successful,
helpful, trustworthy, considerate, useful, and kind). In one specific type of kindness inter-
vention, participants were randomly assigned to “make someone else happy” throughout
the week for 4 weeks (excerpts from Layous, Kurtz, Margolis, Chancellor, and Lyu-
bomirsky [2018] included below). A consistent theme across these entries was the person
performing kind acts feeling proud of him- or herself for investing in others.
“I chose to make my boyfriend happier last week. I made him dinner and tried to be
nicer to him and not be so critical of him. I chose this person because he’s the per-
son I spend most of my free time with. I knew treating him nicely would make him
happy because a lot of times when I’m stressed I take it out on him. He was much
more appreciative toward me. I was very proud of myself that I could make him as
happy as I did.”
“This week I chose one of my friends and housemates. During the week we don’t have
much time to hang out, so this weekend I made sure we did. We went out together
Friday night and had a movie night the next night. I chose [friend] because we’ve
been friends for a long time, and I knew she was someone I really cared about. I
knew just spending time with her would make her happy. She was excited when she
realized I wanted to spend so much time with her. I felt proud of myself that I took
time away from doing things only for myself. I think I was successful in making her
happy.”
Need to Belong
People also have the fundamental need to belong—to feel close and connected with oth-
ers (Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Ryan & Deci, 2000). Importantly, people’s relationships
with others are also open for interpretation with people pondering questions such as
“Does my significant other truly love me?” or “Does this social group really care if I’m
here?” (Walton & Wilson, 2018).
Both gratitude and kindness interventions highlight positive relationships in one’s
life. Gratitude journaling is a private activity, but people often bring to mind their valued
social relationships. Similarly, although gratitude letters often go undelivered in experi-
ments to isolate the effect of simply writing the letter, the activity brings to mind impor-
tant others in one’s life and highlights the investment of others in the letter writer’s life.
Whereas gratitude activities typically simply bring to mind positive social relationships
(but see Walsh & Lyubomirsky, 2018, for the additional benefit of delivering the gratitude
letter), kindness interventions prompt people to behave in positive ways toward others:
strangers, acquaintances, or close others. Not surprisingly, both gratitude interventions
and kindness interventions boost people’s sense of connectedness to others (e.g., Kerr,
O’Donovan, & Pepping, 2015). Research suggests that even interactions with strangers,
like your coffee shop barista, promote belonging and happiness (Sandstrom & Dunn,
2014), and investing in close relationships provides even more of a boost in happiness
than investing in acquaintances (Aknin, Sandstrom, Dunn, & Norton, 2011), perhaps
because of the potential to trigger a positive feedback loop in your close relationships.
Happiness Interventions 197
Importantly, performing kind acts and expressing gratitude may work in tandem to
promote positivity in one’s relationships. In the above excerpt, the person who invested
time into making her boyfriend happier noted how appreciative he was of her efforts. Past
theory and research has demonstrated that those who feel grateful toward their partner
are more likely to engage in behaviors that strengthen the relationship (e.g., spending
quality time with one’s partner; Algoe, 2012). Thus, the initial kind act from Partner A
may trigger gratitude in Partner B, which stimulates Partner B to invest further in the
relationship, triggering an upward spiral of gratitude, positive relationship behaviors, and
relationship satisfaction in Partner A and Partner B (Algoe, 2012). Although a great deal
of gratitude research has taken place in the context of romantic relationships, research
has also demonstrated the positive effects of gratitude in close friendships (e.g., Lambert
& Fincham, 2011), indicating that gratitude has a role across relationship type, promot-
ing fulfilling relationships (Algoe, 2012). Given people’s inherent need to feel close to
others, it is no surprise that activities that promote positive relationships also contribute
to happiness.
EMPIRICAL EVIDENCE
Outcomes
The primary outcomes of happiness interventions are subjective well-being and depres-
sion. In the tradition of Diener (1984), subjective well-being includes three primary com-
ponents: positive affect, negative affect, and life satisfaction—sometimes reported sepa-
rately and sometimes reported as part of a composite with negative affect reverse-scored
(Busseri & Sadava, 2011). In the literature, subjective well-being and “happiness” are
used interchangeably, and subjective well-being is often used as an umbrella term that
subsumes measures that ask participants directly about their happiness (e.g., Lyubomir-
sky & Lepper, 1999). Sometimes researchers also measure participants’ eudaimonic well-
being—their sense of purpose and meaning in life—but research suggests that subjective
and eudaimonic well-being have so much overlap that differences between the two are
more conceptual than empirical (e.g., Disabato, Goodman, Kashdan, Short, & Jarden,
2016). Thus, for simplicity, I focus mainly on the subjective well-being and depression
results.
Two meta-analyses have investigated the efficacy of happiness interventions. The
first included 51 studies and found that the effect of PPIs is moderate on subjective well-
being and depression (Sin & Lyubomirsky, 2009). With stricter inclusion criteria (e.g.,
only peer-reviewed publications, only studies with randomization to condition), the sec-
ond meta-analysis included 39 studies and found that the effect of PPIs is small but
significant on subjective well-being and depression at postintervention, with the small
effects on subjective well-being persisting 3–6 months later (Bolier et al., 2013).
Recent papers also explored the effects of gratitude and kindness interventions spe-
cifically. A meta-analysis of gratitude interventions included 38 studies with 282 effect
sizes from various outcome variables and comparison conditions (Dickens, 2017; see also
Davis et al., 2016). Compared to a neutral control condition, people who engaged in a
gratitude intervention reported higher quality of relationships, grateful mood and dispo-
sition, life satisfaction, positive affect, happiness, well-being, and optimism, and lower
198 II. Health and Well-Being
depression at postintervention, with all effect sizes being small to moderate. The differ-
ences between the gratitude and control conditions remained at follow-up for well-being,
happiness, positive affect, and depression, with the follow-up period varying from 2
weeks to 6 months. A meta-analysis of kindness interventions included 27 studies with 52
effect sizes comparing the effect of performing kind acts versus various controls. Collaps-
ing across comparison condition types and various indicators of subjective well-being,
the effect of kindness interventions on the well-being of the actor was small to medium
(Curry et al., 2018).
In addition to boosting subjective well-being, happiness interventions also have posi-
tive effects on relationships, work, and physical health either directly or via increases
in happiness. As mentioned before, happiness interventions promote feelings of connec-
tion with others (Dickens, 2017; Kerr et al., 2015) and may trigger positive feedback
loops within one’s relationships (Algoe, 2012). People induced into a positive mood are
also better workers in a variety of ways—they set higher goals, persevere at challenging
tasks longer, complete a greater amount of work with no decline in quality, and come up
with more mutually beneficial solutions in negotiations (Walsh, Boehm, & Lyubomirsky,
2018). In addition, happiness is related to physical health (Pressman, Jenkins, & Mos-
kowitz, 2019), and people who engage in happiness interventions have reported fewer
symptoms of physical illness (Burton & King, 2008; Emmons & McCullough, 2003,
Study 1), and even fewer visits to the doctor (Burton & King, 2004; King, 2001) than
comparison groups.
Research on happiness interventions suggests potential pathways by which happiness
may affect physical health. Specifically, participants who engage in happiness interven-
tions (vs. various comparison conditions) have reported greater sleep duration (Emmons
& McCullough, 2003, Study 3) and sleep quality (Jackowska, Brown, Ronaldson, &
Steptoe, 2016), decreased diastolic blood pressure (Jackowska et al., 2016) and inflam-
mation (Redwine et al., 2016), and healthier eating (Fritz, Armenta, Walsh, & Lyubomir-
sky, 2019). Thus, happiness interventions may improve physical health by increasing the
healthy cardiovascular profile associated with positive emotions (Boehm & Kubzansky,
2012) and by promoting healthier behaviors (e.g., better eating and sleep habits).
Mechanisms
To understand how happiness interventions can affect long-term changes in well-being,
we must first explore the feelings, thoughts, and behaviors of people directly following
their engagement in a happiness intervention. In the “Psychological Processes” section
of this chapter, I focus on the potential for happiness interventions to change the way
people construe themselves, their relationships, and their circumstances in general, which
likely feed back into greater happiness over time in a recursive process. Here I focus on
some evidence of these proximal changes in the way people understand their current
circumstances and relationships (need to understand), feel about themselves (need for
self-efficacy), and feel connected to others (need for belonging).
Immediately following a gratitude expression activity, participants felt more grateful
(Lambert, Fincham, & Stillman, 2012; Layous et al., 2017), more positive overall (Lay-
ous et al., 2017; Watkins, Woodward, Stone, & Kolts, 2003), more connected to others
(Layous et al., 2017), and more emotionally and cognitively engaged at school (King
& Datu, 2018) than comparison groups. Directly after performing an act of kindness,
Happiness Interventions 199
participants felt more positive overall and evaluated themselves more positively on a vari-
ety of personal characteristics (e.g., successful, trustworthy) than participants who did
not perform an act of kindness (Williamson & Clark, 1989). Importantly, participants
reported these feelings directly after the activity—there had been no time for objective
life circumstances to change. This means that participants’ construal of themselves, their
relationships, and their circumstances had changed to view their life in a more positive
light. These immediate shifts in construal, positivity, and psychological need satisfaction
likely drive longer-term changes in well-being (Fredrickson, 2013; Layous & Lyubomir-
sky, 2014; Lyubomirsky & Layous, 2013).
Another possibility ripe for future exploration is that happiness interventions drive
immediate changes in behavior (i.e., not just construal), which then feed back into happi-
ness. For example, people induced into a grateful state are more likely to perform acts of
kindness (Bartlett & DeSteno, 2006), a behavior that also leads to higher happiness (see
Miller, Dannals, & Zlatev, 2017). More research needs to explore the proximal changes
in feelings, thoughts, and behaviors that promote happiness change well after the initial
intervention.
Effects over Time
Theoretically, happiness interventions may affect long-term well-being and related out-
comes (e.g., relationship closeness, workplace success, and physical health) via two non-
mutually exclusive pathways: recursive processes and continued practice. Regarding
recursive processes, the broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions states that posi-
tive emotions broaden people’s attention and prompt engagement in approach-oriented
behaviors (e.g., playing, exploring, learning, engaging with new activities or people;
Fredrickson, 2013). Although the boost in positive emotions is temporary, this broad-
ened, exploratory state allows people to take actions that may lead to durable changes in
well-being. For example, if a woman engages in a happiness intervention and feels more
positive as a result, she may feel energized to attend a gym class. While at this gym class,
she may meet someone with common interests who becomes a friend and an enduring
source of social support. Thus, the initial positive state promoted actions that created an
objective and stable happiness-promoting change—a new friend (see Fredrickson, Cohn,
Coffey, Pek, & Finkel, 2008, for empirical evidence).
Fredrickson (2013) focuses on positive emotions as the fuel that drives these
happiness-promoting actions, but based on the psychological processes and mechanisms
discussed earlier, I posit that other mediators are also at play (e.g., construal, psychologi-
cal need satisfaction, or positive behaviors). Regardless of the precise mechanism, a hap-
piness intervention may trigger a series of events that promotes sustained happiness (see
also Cohen & Sherman, 2014; Yeager & Walton, 2011; Walton & Wilson, 2018, for a
discussion of recursive processes). Although possible, the above scenario relies on a chain
of fortuitous events that may not happen—perhaps no one is particularly friendly at the
gym that day and therefore the upward spiral of positivity (i.e., the recursive process) is
not triggered. Indeed, Walton and Wilson (2018) note the importance of a receptive envi-
ronment for recursive processes to unfold.
This is where the second pathway, continued practice, comes into play. If, instead
of engaging in a happiness intervention only once—or only during a prescribed inter-
vention period (e.g., over 6 weeks)—a person decides to continue his gratitude practice
200 II. Health and Well-Being
indefinitely, he increases the chance that this gratitude practice will set off a chain of
positive events in his life, thus sustaining his happiness. Whereas at first, the gratitude
intervention may only have changed his subjective construal of his life, eventually, this
subjective construal leads to positive actions that will improve objective circumstances
(e.g., more high-quality social relationships) when met with a receptive environment. An
objective circumstance like more high-quality social relationships would itself promote
greater happiness.
Another possibility is that the continued practice itself reinforces happiness by help-
ing people actively appreciate their lives as they are. Indeed, happiness researchers have
posited that continued practice is necessary to sustainably boost happiness and therefore
if the gratitude practice stopped, so would the associated gains in well-being (Lyubomir-
sky, Sheldon, et al., 2005, but see Cohn & Fredrickson, 2010). Researchers also theorize
that if continued practice is sustained long enough, the practice may become habitual,
thus sustaining boosts in well-being without effortful practice (Lyubomirsky, Shedon, et
al., 2005). Thus, for example, someone who practices gratitude over time may eventually
have his or her immediate response to a situation be one of gratitude rather than dismay.
This person may then resemble people who are naturally grateful due to personality or
early learned tendencies. No research has been conducted yet to determine whether hap-
piness interventions can become habitual after a certain amount of time and therefore
sustain boosts in well-being.
More research also needs to explore evidence for whether recursive processes, con-
tinued practice, or both drive long-term changes in well-being. If continued practice is
necessary, you may expect longer intervention periods (e.g., 10 weeks vs. 4 weeks) to have
similar (or even perhaps greater) gains in well-being than shorter interventions. Indeed, a
meta-analysis found that interventions that were longer had larger effect sizes than those
that were shorter, but this finding was confounded by the fact that many of the longer
interventions also took place in group or individual therapy, another factor that boosted
efficacy (Sin & Lyubomirsky, 2009). In my own work, I have seen evidence of a quadratic
effect whereby the intervention boosts well-being out to a certain point (e.g., 3 weeks in),
but does not sustain the boosts for everyone out to the end of the intervention (e.g., 6
weeks in; Layous, Kurtz, Wildschut, & Sedikides, 2020). Thus, continued practice alone
does not seem to maintain boosts in well-being.
Some studies have measured the degree to which people continue to practice the
intervention of their own free will after the prescribed intervention period and found that
continued practice was related to sustained increases in well-being (Dickerhoof, 2007;
Proyer, Wellenzohn, Gander, & Ruch, 2015; Seligman et al., 2005; Sheldon & Lyubomir-
sky, 2006). Unfortunately, most of these studies do not provide enough information to
know whether the continued practice was necessary to maintain sustained well-being
increases, or were merely additionally beneficial.
Complicating matters, most of the evidence on continued practice is correlational,
so the relationship between continued practice and sustained well-being could be due to
other factors. For example, Sheldon and Lyubomirsky (2006) found that continued prac-
tice was related to participant self-reports of self-concordance with their activity (e.g.,
the degree to which they enjoyed the activity and identified with its importance vs. felt
forced to engage in the activity by external forces or guilt). Similarly, Cohn and Fredrick-
son (2010) noted that those who continued their happiness-promoting practice were also
the ones who reacted more strongly in the initial weeks of the intervention. This early
Happiness Interventions 201
reactivity could be an indication of fit between the person and the happiness interven-
tion. Thus, quite rationally, those who felt the intervention working for them were more
likely to continue practicing it beyond the prescribed intervention period and those who
were slower to react (not reaping benefits until near the end of the intervention and reap-
ing fewer benefits overall) stopped the practice when the study stopped. Nevertheless,
Cohn and Fredrickson noted that what little gains the noncontinuers reaped in the initial
intervention were sustained at the follow-up, indicating continued practice may not be
necessary because it is possible the intervention helps the person build other sources of
positivity in their lives that sustain well-being regardless of continued practice.
All of this is to say that more research needs to be conducted to explore the degree
to which continued practice is necessary and under which conditions (e.g., under optimal
person-activity fit) continued practice is most beneficial. Following recommendations by
Miller and colleagues (2017), more research also needs to measure behavioral changes
following happiness interventions and how those behavioral changes may sustain boosts
in well-being in a recursive process.
Adding to the difficulty of exploring the “how” of well-being change over time,
many happiness interventions include either no follow-up after the intervention or a rela-
tively short follow-up (e.g., 1 month). Given how difficult behavior change is, longer fol-
low-up periods are needed to know whether and how the changes instigated by happiness
interventions stick beyond the prescribed intervention period. Nevertheless, some articles
point to the potential longevity of these effects. Bolier and colleagues (2013) found nine
happiness interventions that examined follow-up effects from 3 to 6 months after the
intervention and found a small but significant effect of the interventions (vs. control) on
well-being at the follow-up.
Heterogeneity
Theory and research proposes various aspects of the happiness intervention (e.g., its dos-
age) and the people practicing it (e.g., their personality) that moderate the degree to which
the activity works to increase well-being (Layous & Lyubomirsky, 2014; Lyubomirsky &
Layous, 2013). For example, one study found that a gratitude exercise was more effective
if performed once per week rather than three times a week and another study found that
a kindness intervention was more effective if all kind acts were done in 1 day during the
week rather than spread out across the week (Lyubomirsky, Sheldon, et al., 2005). The
authors later reasoned that both studies suggest that happiness interventions performed
once per week are most effective, perhaps because many cultural routines are performed
weekly (e.g., church, television programs; Lyubomirsky & Layous, 2013). Although the
authors cannot be sure why these patterns of implementation were the most effective, at
the very least, these studies point to the dosage of the intervention affecting its efficacy.
Lyubomirsky and Layous discuss various other aspects of happiness intervention imple-
mentation that may affect its efficacy (e.g., variety, built-in social support).
In addition, research points to aspects of the person that might make him or her
more likely to improve in well-being following a happiness intervention. For example,
in one study, people who were relatively more extraverted gained more in well-being
from happiness interventions indicating the role of personality (Senf & Liau, 2013). Past
research has demonstrated that extraverted people consistently experience more positive
emotions than their more introverted counterparts (Costa & McCrae, 1980), perhaps
202 II. Health and Well-Being
because they react more positively to similar daily events (Larsen & Ketelaar, 1991).
Thus, it seems that extraverted people are predisposed to construe events as positive and,
when given overtly positive material like a happiness intervention, they benefit more than
their peers. Lyubomirsky and Layous (2013) discuss other characteristics of people that
may affect their likelihood of gaining in well-being following a happiness intervention
(e.g., how motivated they are to boost their happiness, how much they believe that hap-
piness interventions can increase well-being).
Importantly, the optimal dosage of a happiness intervention (i.e., how often it is
practiced) may vary by person, and whether particular personality profiles (i.e., extraver-
sion) gain more in well-being from a happiness intervention may depend on the happiness
intervention itself. Thus, perhaps more important than the main effects of activity or
person characteristics is the fit between a particular person and a particular activity (i.e.,
person-activity fit; Lyubomirsky & Layous, 2013). For example, giving random acts of
kindness to complete strangers may not be the best intervention for introverted people
(Pressman, Kraft, & Cross, 2015). Indeed, participants who indicate that an activity
feels relatively more natural and enjoyable to them are more likely to increase well-being
over time (Dickerhoof, 2007) and to continue practicing the intervention beyond the pre-
scribed period (Sheldon & Lyubomirsky, 2006). Similarly, people who indicate a good fit
with the happiness intervention are more likely to sustain benefits to well-being (Proyer et
al., 2015; see also Cohn & Fredrickson, 2010). More research needs to address the issue
of person-activity fit to best maximize well-being benefits for happiness seekers.
COUSINS
Happiness interventions incorporate a broad range of activities, both inside (e.g., Fava &
Ruini, 2003; Seligman, Rashid, & Parks, 2006) and outside (e.g., Seligman et al., 2005)
of therapy, so I am not able to discuss all of them here. The activities mentioned below
(and the gratitude and kindness activities already mentioned) are similar in that they all
help people view themselves, their relationships, or their environments in ways that pro-
mote happiness, but they are different in the exact way they focus attention.
Various interventions—broadly classified as savoring interventions—direct people’s
attention to the positive in their past, present, or future experiences. Some researchers
characterize savoring as an umbrella term that encompasses gratitude, with gratitude
being a specific type of savoring in which one recognizes and appreciates a direct benefit
received from the goodwill of another person (Adler & Fagley, 2005). Many savoring
interventions have successfully boosted positive emotions by prompting people to remem-
ber positive experiences from their past (e.g., Burton & King, 2004; Sedikides et al.,
2015). Perhaps unsurprisingly given the fundamental need for humans to feel connected
to others (e.g., Baumeister & Leary, 1995), the two most frequent subjects of these posi-
tive memories is close others and momentous events (e.g., weddings, graduations) during
which close others are an important part (Wildschut, Sedikdies, Arndt, & Routledge,
2006). Following this theme, the boost in positivity following the recollection of positive
memories was predicted by an increased sense of connectedness to others, as well as an
increased sense of meaning in life, self-esteem, and self-continuity (one’s sense that they
are connected with their past and that their personality has remained the same across
time; Sedikides et al., 2015).
Happiness Interventions 203
practices not only reduce negativity, but also boost positivity (Davidson et al., 2003).
Other meditation practices directly focus on boosting positivity. For example, loving-
kindness meditation prompts participants to direct love and compassion toward them-
selves, loved ones, acquaintances, strangers, and all living beings, and increases positive
emotions, mindfulness, meaning in life, perceptions of social support, and perceptions of
physical health (Fredrickson et al., 2008).
A theme in all of the above interventions is the importance of social relationships
for happiness. Many other wise interventions also focus on helping people view their
relationships in a positive light, although not typically with the primary goal of increas-
ing personal happiness. For example, across multiple studies, the social-belonging inter-
vention has helped students construe their challenges as normal and temporary, not as
a sign they do not belong in their educational setting (e.g., Walton & Cohen, 2011). The
primary goal of the social-belonging intervention is to improve academic achievement
among marginalized groups, but it also boosts happiness and physical health (Walton
& Cohen, 2011). Thus, a common theme across wise interventions, including happiness
interventions, is the focus on positive social relationships.
Happiness interventions within therapy can be quite intensive (Fava & Ruini, 2003;
Seligman et al., 2006), but the self-guided variety are often brief and typically performed
once per week for about 6 weeks (although 6 weeks is an arbitrary time frame to permit
completion of a study during a school term). Writing activities (e.g., gratitude) are often
scheduled for as few as 2 minutes (Burton & King, 2008) to as many as 15 minutes
(Lyubomirsky et al., 2011), whereas kindness activities can take much longer with par-
ticipants often reporting spending quality time with loved ones.
Gratitude interventions typically focus on either keeping a gratitude journal (e.g.,
“counting blessings” or “three good things”; Emmons & McCullough, 2003; Seligman
et al., 2005) or writing gratitude letters (e.g., Layous, Lee, et al., 2013; Lyubomirsky et
al., 2011).
Typical instructions for keeping a gratitude journal are as follows: “There are many
things in our lives, both large and small, that we might be grateful about. Think back
over the past week and write down on the lines below up to five things in your life that
you are grateful or thankful for” (Emmons & McCullough, 2003). This activity prompts
participants to reflect upon and appreciate aspects of their lives that they may take for
granted, giving them an opportunity to extract positivity from mundane experiences that
they otherwise would have taken for granted.
Typical instructions for a gratitude letter writing activity are as follows:
“Please take a moment to think back over the past several years of your life and
remember an instance when someone did something for you for which you are
extremely grateful. For example, think of the people—parents, relatives, friends,
teachers, coaches, teammates, employers, and so on—who have been especially kind
to you but have never heard you express your gratitude. Although you should try to
write your letter of gratitude to a new person each week; if you prefer, you can write
another letter to the same person you wrote to previously.”
Happiness Interventions 205
“In our daily lives, we all perform acts of kindness for others. These acts may be large
or small and the person for whom the act is performed may or may not be aware of
the act. Examples include helping your parents cook dinner, doing a chore for your
sister or brother, helping a friend with homework, visiting an elderly relative, or
writing a thank you letter. During one day this week (any day you choose), please
perform five acts of kindness—all five in one day. The acts do not need to be for the
same person, the person may or may not be aware of the act, and the act may or may
not be similar to the acts listed above. Next week you will report what acts of kind-
ness you chose to perform. Please do not perform any acts that may place yourself
or others in danger.”
Asking people to report the kind acts they performed (i.e., the kindness check-in)
has two purposes. The first purpose is to hold people accountable. Researchers do not
want participants to blow off the activity, so they remind them that they will report their
kind acts the following week. If participants do not perform kind acts, they can leave the
kindness check-in blank and the researcher can decide whether to include them in the
analyses. That said, even if participants did not purposely perform kind acts during the
week, they likely still did nice things for others that they might report. Research suggests
that recounting the kindnesses one has performed throughout the day boosts well-being
(Kerr et al., 2015; Otake, Shimai, Tanaka-Matsumi, Otsui, & Fredrickson, 2006). Thus,
the second purpose of the kindness check-in is to give participants one more chance to
benefit. Kindness interventions that ask people to go out and intentionally do kind acts
cannot be sure that participants actually did anything additional to what they usually do,
but, by asking participants to report their kind acts, they can be sure that participants
who report their kind acts are becoming more aware of the kindnesses they perform.
For a comprehensive list of happiness-promoting activities, visit the Greater Good
in Action website (https://ggia.berkeley.edu), part of the Greater Good Science Center
at the University of California, Berkeley, which houses 58 research-tested practices for
boosting well-being.
206 II. Health and Well-Being
NUANCES AND MISCONCEPTIONS
Research has found that valuing happiness to an extreme degree actually undermines
it, so there seems to be a risk in people endorsing statements like “If I don’t feel happy,
maybe there is something wrong with me,” and “I am concerned about my happiness
even when I feel happy” (Mauss, Tamir, Anderson, & Savino, 2011). Alternatively,
research also suggests that simply prioritizing positivity as an important part of daily life
(e.g., “A priority for me is experiencing happiness in everyday life”) is predictive of well-
being (Catalino, Algoe, & Fredrickson, 2014). Thus, Catalino et al. demonstrate that the
explicit intention to pursue happiness is likely positive, but Mauss et al. demonstrates that
overvaluing happiness is distinct from a simple intention and can be detrimental. These
two different approaches to thinking about happiness and their distinct relationships
with well-being inform the administration of happiness interventions.
Indeed, most happiness interventions do not focus participants directly on their own
happiness (e.g., “Go make yourself happy”) but instead prompt participants to engage
in activities that take the focus off of their personal happiness (e.g., write a gratitude let-
ter, perform kind acts). A person engaging in these research-tested practices is obviously
showing signs of prioritizing positivity by seeking out happiness-promoting strategies,
but, when engaged in the activity itself, will not be thinking about their own happiness in
an “Are we there yet?” unhealthy way. Indeed, in general, self-focus is related to negative
affect, particularly when that self-focus involves ruminative thoughts like “Why do I feel
the way I do?” (Mor & Winquist, 2002). Thus, happiness interventions like gratitude and
kindness often take the focus off the self and direct it to others. This is a nuance of hap-
piness interventions that has not been discussed much, but I think may be an important
part of their success—happiness interventions do not typically focus directly on happi-
ness.
Simply telling people to make themselves happy without giving them efficacious
happiness-promoting activities may not work because people often do not know what
will make them happy. For example, many people engage in retail therapy to boost their
mood, but research has shown that experiential purchases promote greater happiness
than materialistic ones, likely because experiential purchases are more open to positive
reinterpretation and contribute to social relationships (Van Boven & Gilovich, 2003;
see also Kasser & Ryan, 1993). In another experiment, participants randomly assigned
to perform kind acts for others reported greater flourishing than those assigned to per-
form kind acts for themselves (Nelson et al., 2016). Presumably, the self-kindness group
engaged in activities meant to boost their well-being, but they were unsuccessful in doing
so. Thus, people may want to pursue happiness, but they may not know what will actu-
ally make them happy.
Although people may not know exactly what to do to pursue their happiness, theory
suggests that their intention to pursue happiness is important to their success, as happi-
ness pursuit takes at least some effort (Lyubomirsky, Sheldon, et al., 2005). In one test
of this theory, researchers advertised their study as either a “happiness intervention” or a
“cognitive exercises” study (Lyubomirsky et al., 2011). Researchers thought that partici-
pants who signed up for the happiness intervention study were likely motivated to pursue
their own happiness and had the intention of doing so, whereas participants who signed
up for the cognitive exercises were less motivated and had no explicit intention to pursue
their happiness. Regardless of how participants initially entered the study (via happiness
Happiness Interventions 207
intervention or cognitive exercises title), all were randomly assigned to complete one of
two happiness interventions or a control activity weekly for 6 weeks. Participants who
were motivated to improve their happiness and were assigned to one of the happiness
interventions reported greater increases in well-being than those who practiced happi-
ness interventions but were not motivated, or the control groups. Researchers concluded
that participants needed both a will (i.e., motivation/intention to pursue happiness) and
a proper way (i.e., an efficacious happiness-promoting activity) to increase well-being
(Lyubomirsky et al., 2011; see also Ferguson & Sheldon, 2013, for a replication with a
completely experimental design).
The positive effects of intention during happiness pursuit is interesting compared to
other wise interventions in which the purpose is rarely made explicit to participants. For
example, some research suggests that making the purpose of self-affirmation interven-
tions known undermines their effect (Sherman et al., 2009), but upon further examina-
tion, the awareness may only be detrimental if the activity is viewed to be externally
imposed rather than internally chosen (Silverman, Logel, & Cohen, 2013). I further dis-
cuss the importance of intrinsic motivation later in this section.
One reason intention to pursue happiness may predict higher happiness following a
happiness intervention is because intentional happiness seekers may put more effort into
happiness-increasing activities. Lyubomirsky and colleagues (2011) had independent cod-
ers rate the degree to which participants put effort into completing their assigned activi-
ties (either the happiness interventions or the control activity). Regardless of whether
participants self-selected into the happiness activity or not, those who put more effort
into the happiness interventions (but not the control activity) saw greater increases in
well-being. This again supports the “will and proper way” logic—participants needed
to put effort into an efficacious activity. Unfortunately, Lyubomirsky and colleagues did
not report whether those who self-selected into the happiness intervention study also put
forth more effort. Possibly, anyone who puts effort into happiness-promoting activities
will reap the benefits, with or without the explicit intention to become happier.
The Ferguson and Sheldon (2013) and Lyubomirsky and colleagues’ (2011) studies
suggest that intentionality is an important component of benefiting from a happiness
intervention, but many questions remain. For example, can the effect of intention and
effort be disentangled and, if so, which is more important? What exactly needs to be
intentional—the pursuit of happiness more generally or just intentionality toward the
practice of a specific happiness-promoting activity (e.g., intentionally practicing grati-
tude or kindness)? I suspect that the latter would be just as effective as the former, but no
research to my knowledge has disentangled these effects. At what stage in happiness pur-
suit is intentionality necessary? In light of the potential for recursive processes to unfold,
maybe intentionality is only necessary at the beginning of a happiness pursuit, but ceases
to become necessary when activities either become habitual or trigger other positive pro-
cesses that contribute to happiness. Finally, even at the beginning of a happiness interven-
tion, is intention truly necessary, or is it merely helpful? I suspect if a group of nonhappi-
ness seekers (i.e., people who do not have the goal of improving their happiness) started
writing in a gratitude journal weekly, at least some of them would experience improve-
ments in well-being, indicating that the initial intention to pursue happiness was not
necessary for some. Similarly, in an experiment described earlier, participants randomly
assigned to read information about happiness being malleable (vs. fixed) reported higher
happiness despite never expressing any intention to pursue happiness (Van Tongeren &
208 II. Health and Well-Being
Burnette, 2018). Thus, although initial theory suggested that intentionality is necessary
for happiness pursuit (Lyubomirsky, Sheldon, et al., 2005), many questions surround the
exact nature of this intentionality.
Another nuance is that, as much as possible, happiness interventions need to be
autonomy supportive (see also Silverman et al., 2013; Vansteenkiste, Simons, Lens, Shel-
don, & Deci, 2004). People need to engage in happiness interventions because they want
to and because the activities feel natural and enjoyable to them (intrinsically motivating).
Even if people hold personal happiness as an intrinsically motivated goal because it feels
good in and of itself, the way one pursues happiness (e.g., via gratitude expression or
prosocial behavior) could start to feel unnatural or forced. Similarly, if people hold hap-
piness as a goal only because others seem happier than they do or because society expects
them to be happy, happiness pursuit may also feel mandated by extrinsic forces or by
one’s own guilt or shame (cf. Crocker & Park, 2004). Thus, as much as possible, people
need to come to happiness pursuit on their own terms and the happiness interventions
themselves need to underscore this autonomy. One experiment randomly assigned both
U.S. and South Korean students to perform kind acts or focus on their academics (the
control group) and then either provided autonomy-supportive messages from a peer who
purportedly completed this study previously (e.g., “Just wanted to let ya know that where
u do these acts and who u do them for is totally up to u. Feel free to do this however u
want! :) ”), or just provided the other instructions with no peer support messages (i.e., no
autonomy support; Nelson et al., 2015). Regardless of culture, the autonomy-supportive/
kindness group showed greater linear improvements in well-being over the 6-week inter-
vention than the other three groups, indicating that autonomy support boosted the effi-
cacy of the kindness activity, but not the control group.
IMPLICATIONS FOR PRACTICE
The majority of kindness interventions give participants a choice in how they perform
their kind acts, thus the likelihood of person-activity fit and autonomous motivation is
higher. Researchers and practitioners need to consider the participants in their happi-
ness interventions and what type of complications they may experience, as well as leave
enough flexibility in their administration as to give participants control and choice in
their implementation.
In another example of a backfiring effect, one study found that writing gratitude let-
ters actually decreased well-being among mildly depressed participants (Sin, Della Porta,
& Lyubomirsky, 2011; see also Layous, Lee, et al., 2013, for the null effect of gratitude
letters among South Koreans). The authors reasoned that writing a letter of gratitude
might have been too taxing for depressed participants. Indeed, in casual conversations
with clinical psychologists, they caution that some of their patients with depression find it
difficult to think of people in their lives who care about them, or, alternatively, if they can
think of someone, patients quickly feel guilty about their family and friends wasting time
on them (see also Layous et al., 2017). In a clinical setting, therapists can guide patients
into a different way of thinking, but if people with depression are completing the grati-
tude letter activity by themselves, they may not have the proper support or wherewithal
to benefit from the activity. Over half of happiness seekers on the Internet qualify as clini-
cally depressed (Parks et al., 2012), so researchers need to be careful about which activi-
ties they recommend and which caveats they may place. In one successful customization,
researchers adapted the “visualize your best possible self” instructions for chronic pain
patients by acknowledging their pain in the instructions (Peters et al., 2017). Thus, par-
ticipants were asked to imagine their good life in the future “despite their pain.” This
small customization may have made participants feel understood and may have made the
activity feel less unreasonable given their current difficult circumstances.
IMPLICATIONS FOR PSYCHOLOGICAL THEORY
First, happiness interventions inform the degree to which a relatively stable and geneti-
cally influenced aspect of one’s personality—happiness—can be meaningfully and sus-
tainably changed through intentional practice. Although the heritability of happiness is
well established at about 40–50%, empirical evidence indicates that even identical twins
can have substantially different well-being trajectories due to nonshared environmental
influences (which could include intentional practice; see Layous, 2018, for a review).
Many view heritability estimates to be concrete and deterministic, but they are highly
influenced by nongenetic factors, like variability in the environment and variability in
the trait across people in the sample. Even if happiness was 100% heritable, improv-
ing environmental factors could still raise happiness in an entire population, boosting
people’s well-being relative to their own levels. For example, in one multiweek happiness
intervention among identical and fraternal twins, the average well-being across the whole
sample improved even though the estimates of the genetic effects on happiness remained
consistent across the study (Haworth et al., 2016). Genetically sensitive designs with an
experimental intervention component can continue to help researchers understand the
interplay between genes and environment on happiness and other personality processes.
Second, intervening to improve happiness among people in different cultures helps
psychologists learn about processes of well-being that are culturally specific and those
210 II. Health and Well-Being
that might be more universally important. For example, although gratitude interventions
consistently boost well-being among Western samples, one study found that gratitude
letters were not effective in boosting well-being in a South Korean sample (e.g., Lay-
ous, Lee, et al., 2013). On the other hand, kindness interventions have been equally
effective among Eastern (South Korean) and Western (U.S.) samples (Layous, Lee, et al.,
2013; Nelson et al., 2015), suggesting a universality not characteristic of gratitude letters.
Exploring what activities do or do not boost happiness among different cultures, and the
psychological processes by which they do so, can promote theory development surround-
ing what leads to thriving in different cultures.
Lastly, intervening to improve happiness could develop theory in clinical psychology
by pointing to the role of positive affect in the manifestation and treatment of mental
illness. Currently, treatment for depression largely relies on drug therapy or cognitive-
behavioral therapy, approaches that have been successful, but perhaps incomplete and
not a fit for all patients. Researchers have speculated that drug therapy, cognitive-
behavioral therapy, and happiness interventions act on different neural circuitry (Layous,
Chancellor, Lyubomirsky, Wang, & Doraiswamy, 2011). A three-arm randomized trial
comparing changes in brain functioning among depressed patients undergoing happiness
interventions, cognitive-behavioral therapy, or drug therapy could reveal whether the
theorized neural circuitry is supported. Future studies could also test the combination of
one or more of these therapies to explore whether addressing multiple neural circuits at
once has an additive effect on depression outcomes.
In addition, adding happiness interventions to existing therapeutic approaches could
address limitations of current treatment. For example, it takes several weeks for patients
to respond to drug therapy, but, on average, people respond to happiness interventions
quickly, showing immediate boosts to mood (e.g., Seligman et al., 2005). Thus, happi-
ness interventions may complement slower-acting drug therapies. Similarly, happiness
interventions help focus people’s attention on the positive in their lives, so may be a good
complement to cognitive-behavioral therapy’s focus on reducing maladaptive thoughts
(e.g., Beck, 2011). More research will further our understanding of the neural circuitry
surrounding depression and the way that happiness interventions could be incorporated
into existing treatment plans or even act as an effective stand-alone treatment (e.g., Selig-
man et al., 2006).
FUTURE DIRECTIONS
The late Chris Peterson, a prominent thought leader in positive psychology, summarized
positive psychology with three words: “other people matter” (Peterson, 2006). Although
the happiness interventions reported in this chapter have leveraged the perception of
positive relationships with others to increase personal happiness, very few have explored
the effect of happiness interventions on relationships and functioning in closed networks
like workplaces and schools (but see Chancellor, Margolis, Jacobs Bao, & Lyubomirsky,
2018; Layous, Nelson, Oberle, Schonert-Reichl, & Lyubomirsky, 2012). Theoretically,
improving perceptions of relationships within a closed network could trigger positive
recursive processes that build upon each other quickly, reaping immediate relational
and well-being benefits. Thus, a ripe area for future research is intervening to improve
happiness among closed networks and exploring the cumulative effects on individual or
Happiness Interventions 211
REFERENCES
Adler, M. G., & Fagley, N. S. (2005). Appreciation: Individual differences in finding value and
meaning as a unique predictor of subjective well-being. Journal of Personality, 73(1), 79–114.
Aknin, L. B., Sandstrom, G. M., Dunn, E. W., & Norton, M. I. (2011). It’s the recipient that
counts: Spending money on strong social ties leads to greater happiness than spending on
weak social ties. PLOS ONE, 6(2), e17018.
Algoe, S. B. (2012). Find, remind, and bind: The functions of gratitude in everyday relationships.
Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 6(6), 455–469.
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Freeman.
Bartlett, M. Y., & DeSteno, D. (2006). Gratitude and prosocial behavior: Helping when it costs
you. Psychological Science, 17(4), 319–325.
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attach-
ments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529.
Beck, J. S. (2011). Cognitive behavior therapy: Basics and beyond (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford
Press.
Boehm, J. K., & Kubzansky, L. D. (2012). The heart’s content: The association between positive
psychological well-being and cardiovascular health. Psychological Bulletin, 138(4), 655–691.
Boehm, J. K., Lyubomirsky, S., & Sheldon, K. M. (2011). A longitudinal experimental study com-
paring the effectiveness of happiness-enhancing strategies in Anglo Americans and Asian
Americans. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 1263–1272.
Bolier, L., Haverman, M., Westerhof, G. J., Riper, H., Smit, F., & Bohlmeijer, E. (2013). Positive
psychology interventions: A meta-analysis of randomized controlled studies. BMC Public
Health, 13(1), 119–139.
212 II. Health and Well-Being
Burton, C. M., & King, L. A. (2004). The health benefits of writing about intensely positive expe-
riences. Journal of Research in Personality, 38(2), 150–163.
Burton, C. M., & King, L. A. (2008). Effects of (very) brief writing on health: The two-minute
miracle. British Journal of Health Psychology, 13(1), 9–14.
Busseri, M. A., & Sadava, S. W. (2011). A review of the tripartite structure of subjective well-
being: Implications for conceptualization, operationalization, analysis, and synthesis. Per-
sonality and Social Psychology Review, 15(3), 290–314.
Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D. M., & Charles, S. T. (1999). Taking time seriously: A theory of
socioemotional selectivity. American Psychologist, 54(3), 165–181.
Catalino, L. I., Algoe, S. B., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2014). Prioritizing positivity: An effective
approach to pursuing happiness? Emotion, 14(6), 1155–1161.
Chancellor, J., Margolis, S., Jacobs Bao, K., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2018). Everyday prosociality in
the workplace: The reinforcing benefits of giving, getting, and glimpsing. Emotion, 18(4),
507–517.
Cohen, G. L., & Sherman, D. K. (2014). The psychology of change: Self-affirmation and social
psychological intervention. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 333–371.
Cohn, M. A., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2010). In search of durable positive psychology interventions:
Predictors and consequences of long-term positive behavior change. Journal of Positive Psy-
chology, 5(5), 355–366.
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1980). Influence of extraversion and neuroticism on subjective
well-being: Happy and unhappy people. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38(4),
668–678.
Crocker, J., & Park, L. E. (2004). The costly pursuit of self-esteem. Psychological Bulletin, 130(3),
392–414.
Curry, O. S., Rowland, L. A., Van Lissa, C. J., Zlotowitz, S., McAlaney, J., & Whitehouse, H.
(2018). Happy to help?: A systematic review and meta-analysis of the effects of performing
acts of kindness on the well-being of the actor. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
76, 320–329.
Davidson, R. J., Kabat-Zinn, J., Schumacher, J., Rosenkranz, M., Muller, D., Santorelli, S. F., . . .
Sheridan, J. F. (2003). Alterations in brain and immune function produced by mindfulness
meditation. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65(4), 564–570.
Davis, D. E., Choe, E., Meyers, J., Wade, N., Varjas, K., Gifford, A., . . . Worthington, E. L., Jr.
(2016). Thankful for the little things: A meta-analysis of gratitude interventions. Journal of
Counseling Psychology, 63(1), 20–31.
Dickens, L. R. (2017). Using gratitude to promote positive change: A series of meta-analyses inves-
tigating the effectiveness of gratitude interventions. Basic and Applied Social Psychology,
39(4), 193–208.
Dickerhoof, R. M. (2007). Expressing optimism and gratitude: A longitudinal investigation of
cognitive strategies to increase well-being. Dissertation Abstracts International, 68, 4174
(UMI No. 3270426).
Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 95(3), 542–575.
Diener, E. (2000). Subjective well-being: The science of happiness and a proposal for a national
index. American Psychologist, 55, 34–43.
Diener, M. L., & Lucas, R. E. (2004). Adults’ desires for children’s emotions across 48 countries.
Journal of Cross-C ultural Psychology, 35, 525–547.
Disabato, D. J., Goodman, F. R., Kashdan, T. B., Short, J. L., & Jarden, A. (2016). Different types
of well-being?: A cross-cultural examination of hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Psycho-
logical Assessment, 28(5), 471–482.
Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (1998). Parental socialization of emotion. Psy-
chological Inquiry, 9(4), 241–273.
Emmons, R. A., & McCullough, M. E. (2003). Counting blessings versus burdens: An experimental
Happiness Interventions 213
investigation of gratitude and subjective well-being in daily life. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 84(2), 377–389.
Fava, G. A., & Ruini, C. (2003). Development and characteristics of a well-being enhancing psy-
chotherapeutic strategy: Well-being therapy. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental
Psychiatry, 34(1), 45–63.
Ferguson, Y. L., & Sheldon, K. M. (2013). Trying to be happier really can work: Two experimental
studies. Journal of Positive Psychology, 8(1), 23–33.
Fordyce, M. W. (1977). Development of a program to increase personal happiness. Journal of
Counseling Psychology, 24(6), 511–521.
Fordyce, M. W. (1983). A program to increase happiness: Further studies. Journal of Counseling
Psychology, 30(4), 483–498.
Fredrickson, B. L. (2013). Positive emotions broaden and build. Advances in Experimental Social
Psychology, 47(1), 1–53.
Fredrickson, B. L., Cohn, M. A., Coffey, K. A., Pek, J., & Finkel, S. M. (2008). Open hearts build
lives: Positive emotions, induced through loving-kindness meditation, build consequential
personal resources. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(5), 1045–1062.
Fritz, M. M., Armenta, C. N., Walsh, L. C., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2019). Gratitude facilitates
healthy eating behavior in adolescents and young adults. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 81, 4–14.
Fritz, M. M., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2018). Whither happiness?: When, how, and why might positive
activities undermine well-being. In J. P. Forgas & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), The social psychol-
ogy of living well (pp. 101–115). New York: Psychology Press.
Gable, S. L., Reis, H. T., Impett, E. A., & Asher, E. R. (2004). What do you do when things go
right?: The intrapersonal and interpersonal benefits of sharing positive events. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 87(2), 228–245.
Haworth, C. M., Nelson, S. K., Layous, K., Carter, K., Bao, K. J., Lyubomirsky, S., & Plomin,
R. (2016). Stability and change in genetic and environmental influences on well-being in
response to an intervention. PLOS ONE, 11(5), e0155538.
Heintzelman, S. J. (2018). ENHANCE: Evidence for the efficacy of a comprehensive intervention
program to promote durable changes in subjective well-being. Presented at the Happiness
and Well-Being Preconference at the annual meeting of the Society of Personality and Social
Psychologists, Atlanta, GA.
Jackowska, M., Brown, J., Ronaldson, A., & Steptoe, A. (2016). The impact of a brief grati-
tude intervention on subjective well-being, biology and sleep. Journal of Health Psychology,
21(10), 2207–2217.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in context: Past, present, and future. Clin-
ical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10(2), 144–156.
Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1993). A dark side of the American dream: Correlates of financial suc-
cess as a central life aspiration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(2), 410–422.
Kerr, S. L., O’Donovan, A., & Pepping, C. A. (2015). Can gratitude and kindness interventions
enhance well-being in a clinical sample? Journal of Happiness Studies, 16(1), 17–36.
King, L. A. (2001). The health benefits of writing about life goals. Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy Bulletin, 27(7), 798–807.
King, R. B., & Datu, J. A. D. (2018). Grateful students are motivated, engaged, and successful in
school: Cross-sectional, longitudinal, and experimental evidence. Journal of School Psychol-
ogy, 70, 105–122.
Koo, M., Algoe, S. B., Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2008). It’s a wonderful life: Mentally sub-
tracting positive events improves people’s affective states, contrary to their affective forecasts.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(5), 1217–1224.
Kurtz, J. L. (2008). Looking to the future to appreciate the present: The benefits of perceived tem-
poral scarcity. Psychological Science, 19(12), 1238–1241.
214 II. Health and Well-Being
Kurtz, J. L. (2015). Seeing through new eyes: An experimental investigation of the benefits of
photography. Journal of Basic and Applied Sciences, 11, 354–358.
Lambert, N. M., & Fincham, F. D. (2011). Expressing gratitude to a partner leads to more rela-
tionship maintenance behavior. Emotion, 11(1), 52–60.
Lambert, N. M., Fincham, F. D., & Stillman, T. F. (2012). Gratitude and depressive symptoms:
The role of positive reframing and positive emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 26(4), 615–633.
Larsen, R. J., & Ketelaar, T. (1991). Personality and susceptibility to positive and negative emo-
tional states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(1), 132–140.
Layous, K. (2018). Malleability and intentional activities. In E. Diener, S. Oishi, & L. Tay (Eds.),
E-handbook of well-being. Salt Lake City, UT: DEF.
Layous, K., Chancellor, J., Lyubomirsky, S., Wang, L., & Doraiswamy, P. M. (2011). Deliver-
ing happiness: Translating positive psychology intervention research for treating major and
minor depressive disorders. Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 17(8),
675–683.
Layous, K., Kurtz, J., Chancellor, J., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2018). Reframing the ordinary: Imagin-
ing time as scarce increases well-being. Journal of Positive Psychology, 13(3), 301–308.
Layous, K., Kurtz, J., Margolis, S., Chancellor, J., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2018). Make someone
happy and you will be happy too: An other-oriented path to well-being. Unpublished manu-
script.
Layous, K., Kurtz, J., Wildschut, T., & Sedikides, C. (2020). The effect of a multi-week nostalgia
intervention on well-being: Mechanisms and moderation. Manuscript under revision.
Layous, K., Lee, H., Choi, I., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2013). Culture matters when designing a suc-
cessful happiness-increasing activity: A comparison of the United States and South Korea.
Journal of Cross-C ultural Psychology, 44, 1294–1303.
Layous, K., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2014). The how, what, when, and why of happiness: Mechanisms
underlying the success of positive interventions. In J. Gruber & J. Moskowitz (Eds.), Posi-
tive emotion: Integrating the light sides and dark sides (pp. 473–495). New York: Oxford
University Press.
Layous, K., Nelson, S. K., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2013). What is the optimal way to deliver a positive
activity intervention?: The case of writing about one’s best possible selves. Journal of Happi-
ness Studies, 16, 635–654.
Layous, K., Nelson, S. K., Oberle, E., Schonert-Reichl, K. A., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2012). Kindness
counts: Prompting prosocial behavior in preadolescents boosts peer acceptance and well-
being. PLOS ONE, 7(12), e51380.
Layous, K., Sweeny, K., Armenta, C., Na, S., Choi, I., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2017). The proximal
experience of gratitude. PLOS ONE, 12(7), e0179123.
Lykken, D., & Tellegen, A. (1996). Happiness is a stochastic phenomenon. Psychological Science,
7(3), 186–189.
Lyubomirsky, S. (2001). Why are some people happier than others?: The role of cognitive and
motivational processes in well-being. American Psychologist, 56(3), 239–249.
Lyubomirsky, S., Dickerhoof, R., Boehm, J. K., & Sheldon, K. M. (2011). Becoming happier takes
both a will and a proper way: An experimental longitudinal intervention to boost well-being.
Emotion, 11, 391–402.
Lyubomirsky, S., King, L., & Diener, E. (2005). The benefits of frequent positive affect: Does hap-
piness lead to success? Psychological Bulletin, 131(6), 803–855.
Lyubomirsky, S., & Layous, K. (2013). How do simple positive activities increase well-being? Cur-
rent Directions in Psychological Science, 22(1), 57–62.
Lyubomirsky, S., & Lepper, H. S. (1999). A measure of subjective happiness: Preliminary reliabil-
ity and construct validation. Social Indicators Research, 46(2), 137–155.
Lyubomirsky, S., Sheldon, K. M., & Schkade, D. (2005). Pursuing happiness: The architecture of
sustainable change. Review of General Psychology, 9(2), 111–131.
Happiness Interventions 215
Mauss, I. B., Tamir, M., Anderson, C. L., & Savino, N. S. (2011). Can seeking happiness make
people unhappy?: Paradoxical effects of valuing happiness. Emotion, 11(4), 807–815.
Miller, D. T., Dannals, J. E., & Zlatev, J. J. (2017). Behavioral processes in long-lag intervention
studies. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(3), 454–467.
Mor, N., & Winquist, J. (2002). Self-focused attention and negative affect: A meta-analysis. Psy-
chological Bulletin, 128(4), 638–662.
Nelson, S. K., Della Porta, M. D., Jacobs Bao, K., Lee, H. C., Choi, I., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2015).
“It’s up to you”: Experimentally manipulated autonomy support for prosocial behavior
improves well-being in two cultures over six weeks. Journal of Positive Psychology, 10, 463–
476.
Nelson, S. K., Layous, K., Cole, S. W., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2016). Do unto others or treat your-
self?: The effects of prosocial and self-focused behavior on psychological flourishing. Emo-
tion, 16(6), 850–861.
Otake, K., Shimai, S., Tanaka-Matsumi, J., Otsui, K., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2006). Happy people
become happier through kindness: A counting kindnesses intervention. Journal of Happiness
Studies, 7(3), 361–375.
Parks, A. C., Della Porta, M. D., Pierce, R. S., Zilca, R., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2012). Pursuing
happiness in everyday life: The characteristics and behaviors of online happiness seekers.
Emotion, 12(6), 1222–1234.
Peters, M. L., Smeets, E., Feijge, M., van Breukelen, G., Andersson, G., Buhrman, M., & Lin-
ton, S. J. (2017). Happy despite pain: A randomized controlled trial of an 8-week Internet-
delivered positive psychology intervention for enhancing well-being in patients with chronic
pain. Clinical Journal of Pain, 33(11), 962–975.
Peterson, C. (2006). A primer in positive psychology. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. (2004). Character strengths and virtues: A handbook and classifica-
tion. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association Press.
Pressman, S. D., Jenkins, B. N., & Moskowitz, J. T. (2019). Positive affect and health: What do we
know and where next should we go? Annual Review of Psychology, 70, 627–650.
Pressman, S. D., Kraft, T. L., & Cross, M. P. (2015). It’s good to do good and receive good: The
impact of a “pay it forward” style kindness intervention on giver and receiver well-being.
Journal of Positive Psychology, 10(4), 293–302.
Proyer, R. T., Wellenzohn, S., Gander, F., & Ruch, W. (2015). Toward a better understanding of
what makes positive psychology interventions work: Predicting happiness and depression
from the person × intervention fit in a follow-up after 3.5 years. Applied Psychology: Health
and Well-Being, 7(1), 108–128.
Quoidbach, J., Hansenne, M., & Mottet, C. (2008). Personality and mental time travel: A differen-
tial approach to autonoetic consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(4), 1082–1092.
Redwine, L., Henry, B. L., Pung, M. A., Wilson, K., Chinh, K., Knight, B., . . . Mills, P. J. (2016).
A pilot randomized study of a gratitude journaling intervention on HRV and inflammatory
biomarkers in stage B heart failure patients. Psychosomatic Medicine, 78(6), 667–676.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic moti-
vation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55(1), 68–78.
Sandstrom, G. M., & Dunn, E. W. (2014). Social interactions and well-being: The surprising
power of weak ties. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40(7), 910–922.
Sedikides, C., Wildschut, T., Routledge, C., Arndt, J., Hepper, E. G., & Zhou, X. (2015). To nos-
talgize: Mixing memory with affect and desire. Advances in Experimental Social Psychol-
ogy, 51(10), 189–273.
Seligman, M. E. P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: An introduction. Ameri-
can Psychologist, 55, 5–14.
Seligman, M. E. P., Rashid, T., & Parks, A. C. (2006). Positive psychotherapy. American Psy-
chologist, 61(8), 774–788.
216 II. Health and Well-Being
Seligman, M. E. P., Steen, T. A., Park, N., & Peterson, C. (2005). Positive psychology progress:
Empirical validation of interventions. American Psychologist, 60, 410–421.
Senf, K., & Liau, A. K. (2013). The effects of positive interventions on happiness and depressive
symptoms, with an examination of personality as a moderator. Journal of Happiness Studies,
14(2), 591–612.
Sheldon, K. M., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2006). How to increase and sustain positive emotion: The
effects of expressing gratitude and visualizing best possible selves. Journal of Positive Psy-
chology, 1(2), 73–82.
Sherman, D. K., Cohen, G. L., Nelson, L. D., Nussbaum, A. D., Bunyan, D. P., & Garcia, J. (2009).
Affirmed yet unaware: Exploring the role of awareness in the process of self-affirmation.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(5), 745–764.
Silverman, A., Logel, C., & Cohen, G. L. (2013). Self-affirmation as a deliberate coping strategy:
The moderating role of choice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(1), 93–98.
Sin, N. L., Della Porta, M. D., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2011). Tailoring positive psychology interven-
tions to treat depressed individuals. In S. I. Donaldson, M. Csikszentmihalyi, & J. Nakamura
(Eds.), Applied positive psychology: Improving everyday life, health, schools, work, and
society (pp. 79–96). New York: Routledge.
Sin, N. L., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2009). Enhancing well-being and alleviating depressive symptoms
with positive psychology interventions: A practice-friendly meta-analysis. Journal of Clinical
Psychology, 65(5), 467–487.
Smith, M. L., & Glass, G. V. (1977). Meta-analysis of psychotherapy outcome studies. American
Psychologist, 32(9), 752–760.
Van Boven, L., & Gilovich, T. (2003). To do or to have?: That is the question. Journal of Personal-
ity and Social Psychology, 85(6), 1193–1202.
Van Tongeren, D. R., & Burnette, J. L. (2018). Do you believe happiness can change?: An investi-
gation of the relationship between happiness mindsets, well-being, and satisfaction. Journal
of Positive Psychology, 13(2), 101–109.
Vansteenkiste, M., Simons, J., Lens, W., Sheldon, K. M., & Deci, E. L. (2004). Motivating learn-
ing, performance, and persistence: The synergistic effects of intrinsic goal contents and
autonomy-supportive contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(2), 246–
260.
Walsh, L. C., Boehm, J. K., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2018). Does happiness promote career success?:
Revisiting the evidence. Journal of Career Assessment, 26(2), 199–219.
Walsh, L. C., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2018). To share or not to share?: The effects of sharing gratitude
on actors and targets. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the Society for Personality
and Social Psychologists, Atlanta, GA.
Walton, G. M., & Cohen, G. L. (2011). A brief social-belonging intervention improves academic
and health outcomes of minority students. Science, 331(6023), 1447–1451.
Walton, G. M., & Wilson, T. D. (2018). Wise interventions: Psychological remedies for social and
personal problems. Psychological Review, 125(5), 617–655.
Watkins, P. C., Woodward, K., Stone, T., & Kolts, R. L. (2003). Gratitude and happiness: Devel-
opment of a measure of gratitude, and relationships with subjective well-being. Social Behav-
ior and Personality: An International Journal, 31(5), 431–451.
Wildschut, T., Sedikides, C., Arndt, J., & Routledge, C. (2006). Nostalgia: Content, triggers,
functions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(5), 975–993.
Williamson, G. M., & Clark, M. S. (1989). Providing help and desired relationship type as deter-
minants of changes in moods and self-evaluations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 56(5), 722–734.
Yeager, D. S., & Walton, G. M. (2011). Social-psychological interventions in education: They’re
not magic. Review of Educational Research, 81(2), 267–301.
C H A P TE R 9
The substantial body of research depicting the damaging effects of stress has spurred a
decidedly negative cultural narrative around stress. There is also considerable evidence,
however, that stress can improve people’s health, well-being, and performance. In this
chapter, we detail an intervention designed to shift people’s mindset about stress from the
prevailing mindset that stress is debilitating to the mindset that stress can be enhancing. We
describe several lab- and field-based studies that demonstrate improvements in health, well-
being, and performance from this mindset intervention via attentional, affective, motivational,
and physiological mechanisms. Situating this intervention in the wise intervention framework,
we detail the structure and content of the intervention, outline important considerations for
implementing the intervention, and discuss the intervention’s contribution to existing psycho-
logical theory.
BACKGROUND
Hans Selye, often referred to as the “father of stress,” began his career as a medical
student in Prague in the 1920s. During his medical training, he noted a specific sickly
appearance of his patients; regardless of their diagnosis or circumstance, they all dis-
played the same haggard quality. These observations inspired his experimental work with
rats at McGill University in the 1930s and 1940s, leading to his exploration of the stress
response, or “Selye’s syndrome,” as he sometimes referred to it (Jackson, 2014; Selye,
1974). Selye notes in his seminal 1936 letter to the editor in Nature—considered by many
historians as the inception of modern research on the biological and psychological effects
of stress—that the experience of stress triggered by “non-specific nocuous agents” (e.g.,
physical injury, extreme amounts of exercise, exposure to cold) causes rats to undergo a
consistent and predictable physiological response (p. 32). He describes this “general adap-
tation syndrome” as consisting of three stages: an alarm reaction as the body prepares to
217
218 II. Health and Well-Being
defend itself, a period of resistance as the body begins adapting to the heightened alarm
state, and, ultimately, exhaustion as the body can no longer sustain the taxing effects
of the alarm state (Selye, 1936, p. 32). Selye’s work, documented in the roughly 1,500
articles and 32 books he authored over his lifetime, came to dominate the field of stress
research (Jackson, 2014; Szabo, Tache, & Somogyi, 2012).
Although stress can be defined neutrally as the experience or anticipation of chal-
lenge or threat in one’s goal-related efforts, much of the research following in the wake of
Selye’s depiction of stress documented damaging physiological and psychological effects
of stress. For example, research has demonstrated that chronic stress can lead to memory
loss (Sapolsky, 1996), posttraumatic stress disorder (Lupien, McEwen, Gunnar, & Heim,
2009), and depression (Hammen, 2005). Stress has also been linked to greater suscep-
tibility to upper respiratory viruses, faster progression of HIV to acquired immunodefi-
ciency syndrome (AIDS), and higher incidence of cardiovascular disease (Schneiderman,
Ironson, & Siegel, 2005). Spurred by this research, popular media outlets have enthusi-
astically saddled up for the war on stress with headlines declaring, “Even the Youngest
of Us Need to Learn to Battle Stress” (Newsweek, 2018) and “Work-Related Stress Can
Kill, Study Finds” (Kahn, 2008).
The notoriety of these research findings and the media attention they inspired have
spawned the rigid cultural perspective that stress is bad. Accompanying this attitude,
scores of stress management programs have cropped up, promising to help people miti-
gate stress and its negative effects. The idea of stress management can be attributed in
part to the critical insight that the outcomes of stress are not driven only by the sheer
amount of stress one experiences but also by how one copes with that stress—in other
words, the manner in which people appraise stress and mobilize cognitive and behav-
ioral resources to combat stress (Billings & Moos, 1981; Carver, Scheier, & Weintraub,
1989; Folkman & Lazarus, 1980; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Penley, Tomaka, & Wiebe,
2002). Yet, stress management programs all too often focus solely on reducing stress,
and while a few stress management programs have shown promising results (e.g., the
mindfulness-based stress-reduction program; Creswell, 2017), many of them have failed
to produce reliable benefits (Somerfield & McCrae, 2000).
A stress-reduction approach has several practical shortcomings. First, some stress
may be too significant or variable to be effectively reduced though coping mechanisms.
Poverty, for example, is notoriously difficult to escape and can sprout a host of additional
stressors and cognitive demands (Mullainathan & Shafir, 2013), rendering the stress
associated with it a poor candidate for “reduction” techniques. Second, a life of avoid-
ing stress would likely leave one without much of what people treasure most—getting
an education, pursuing a career, maintaining strong friendships, and raising children,
among other meaningful goals; all generally involve experiencing some degree of stress.
Last, the irony of the stress-reduction movement is that it may be inadvertently adding
to the stress many people experience. Experiencing stress, while being bombarded with
the message that stress is bad, can cause people to worry about the negative effects of
that stress (Brady, Hard, & Gross, 2018), and similar types of recurring and intrusive
thoughts, and the cognitive resources they take up, have been shown to have negative
outcomes (e.g., Logel, Iserman, Davies, Quinn, & Spencer, 2009; Schmader, Johns, &
Forbes, 2008). McGonigal (2015) captures this stressed-about-stress phenomenon in the
book The Upside of Stress: “When a friend of mine was pregnant with her first child, she
saw a study online that put her in a panic. The headline warned that a mother’s stress
The Stress‑Mindset Intervention 219
during pregnancy is passed on to the baby. My friend was under a lot of pressure at work,
and she began to worry. Was she permanently harming her baby by not going on early
maternity leave?” (p. 43). This “meta-stress” is an ironic and unfortunate by-product of
well-intentioned attempts to help people reduce their stress.
What has come of this negative portrayal of stress in our culture? We spend vast
amounts of money on stress management programs, but people’s stress levels do not
seem to be improving (Gallup Daily: U.S. Mood, 2018; Saad, 2017). Of course, there are
a multitude of ecological, societal, and cultural factors influencing stress levels, but we
have little encouraging evidence that our current stress-reduction culture is living up to
its promise. Furthermore, by focusing on the negative outcomes of stress, we have inad-
vertently created a culture that is now stressed about stress.
It stands to reason that Selye’s early work would have focused on the negative conse-
quences of stress. As a medical student, Selye observed sick people day after day, and the
people he saw regularly were those who remained patients. What perspective might he
have adopted had he continued to observe the people who made swift recoveries from the
same conditions that plagued those still bedridden in his hospital? Or what about those
who never entered his hospital?
Later in his career, Selye (1976) recognized another form stress could take: eustress,
or “good” stress. Selye noted the importance of distinguishing between eustress and
distress—“the former being agreeable or healthy, and the latter, disagreeable or patho-
genic” (p. 54)—and even attributed his own health and well-being to eustress: “What is
important is to live so that one’s distress is converted into eustress. In any event, that is
what has kept me healthy and happy” (p. 55). Despite Selye’s endorsement, public atten-
tion around eustress was far outpaced by that around distress. As the concept of eustress
faded to academic obscurity, stress and distress soon became synonymous for most prac-
titioners and consumers of stress research.
There is a strong body of research demonstrating the beneficial effects of stress as
Selye’s distinction suggests. Stress has been linked with enhanced cognitive performance,
personal initiative, and emotional stability (Dienstbier, 1989; Epel, McEwen, & Ickov-
ics, 1998; Fay & Sonnentag, 2002). Stress can promote physiological thriving, facilitat-
ing the healing response and enhancing immunity (Dienstbier, 1989; Epel et al., 1998).
Research on “posttraumatic growth” has even shown that severe bouts of stress triggered
by traumas, such as life-threatening illness or military combat, can, in some instances,
have positive outcomes, such as improved relationships, greater appreciation for life, and
enhanced perceptions of personal strength (Tedeschi & Calhoun, 2004).
This research on the benefits of stress does not render the research illustrating the
harmful effects of stress any less important or real. Rather, it suggests that the effects
of stress are paradoxical: stress has the capability to either help or harm our health,
well-being, and performance. The question then is: What is the distinguishing factor
that determines whether the stress we experience will manifest in positive or negative
outcomes?
This chapter reviews the manner in which mindset serves as a moderating factor in
determining the stress response. In the tradition of wise interventions (Walton & Wilson,
220 II. Health and Well-Being
2018), the stress-mindset intervention was designed to change the subjective meaning
people ascribe to stress in order to influence how stress affects their health, well-being,
and performance. Specifically, this intervention educates people about the ways in which
stress can have enhancing effects in their daily lives in order to shift people’s mindsets
from the prevailing view that stress is harmful (a “stress-is-debilitating mindset”) to the
view that stress can be beneficial to health, well-being, and performance (a “stress-can-
be-enhancing mindset”).
PSYCHOLOGICAL PROCESSES
The core psychological construct of stress mindset can be measured by the Stress
Mindset Measure (SMM; Crum et al., 2013), an eight-item measure that asks partici-
pants the extent to which they agree or disagree with a series of statements about the
consequences of stress for health and vitality, learning and growth, and performance
and productivity. This measure has been validated and explored in a number of sam-
ples, including college students (Crum, Akinola, Martin, & Fath, 2017; Goyer, Akinola,
Grunberg, & Crum, 2019), employees at a large financial firm (Crum et al., 2013) and a
large technology firm (Crum et al., 2019), Navy SEALs (Smith, Young, & Crum, 2019),
and a sample of middle school students (tested using a youth version of the SMM; Park et
al., 2017). Most research suggests that, prior to intervention, people typically fall on the
“stress-is-debilitating” side of the scale, reflecting our cultural milieu. The SMM can be
accessed at https://mbl.stanford.edu/materials-measures/stress-mindset-measure.
EMPIRICAL EVIDENCE
Outcomes
Evidence from existing stress-mindset research indicates that preexisting stress mind-
sets are associated with health, well-being, and performance outcomes. Having a stress-
can-be-enhancing mindset has been tied to improved self-reported health and energy
levels, reduced symptoms of anxiety and depression, and greater life satisfaction and
performance at work (Crum et al., 2013). While these initial results were correlational,
subsequent randomized controlled trials established causal links between stress-can-be-
enhancing mindsets and these domains (Crum et al., 2019; Crum et al., 2013, 2017;
Goyer et al., 2019).
In the initial experimental study manipulating stress mindsets, a randomly assigned
group of employees at a large Fortune 500 company watched a series of short videos
about the positive effects of stress. Meanwhile, another group watched videos on the
negative effects of stress that presented similar information to what they might encounter
reading a news article or participating in a traditional stress management workshop and
a third control group watched no videos. Participants in the treatment group adopted
more of a stress-can-be-enhancing mindset (as assessed by the SMM) and, 1 week later,
reported fewer negative health symptoms associated with depression and anxiety than
both of the other groups (Crum et al., 2013). As this intervention took place in a work
setting, we also investigated the effects of adopting a stress-can-be-enhancing mindset on
people’s self-reported work performance. One week after the video intervention, those
in the treatment group reported stronger performance on composite measures of both
“hard” skills—those related to productivity, including the quality, quantity, accuracy,
and efficiency of their work; and “soft” skills—those related to the abilities to sustain
focus, communicate effectively, generate new ideas, and contribute to the overall work
environment (Crum et al., 2013).
In a pair of follow-up studies to the seminal intervention, we tested many of the
same primary outcomes, but implemented more involved versions of the intervention. In
the first study, employees were recruited from a large financial institution shortly after
the 2008 financial collapse to participate in a 2-hour live training on stress mindset. In
222 II. Health and Well-Being
the second study, employees from a large technology company took a 90-minute online
training about stress mindset. (See the “Intervention Content and Implementation” sec-
tion for more information on the features of this intervention approach compared to the
approach used in earlier studies.) Both randomized interventions found positive effects
on symptoms of anxiety and depression, and participants from the financial institution
also reported higher satisfaction with their physical health 4 weeks after the intervention
(Crum et al., 2019). Furthermore, results from these interventions again suggested that
shifting stress mindsets has the potential to influence hard and soft skills in the work-
place (Crum et al., 2019).
We also adapted our intervention messages for an education setting by conduct-
ing a study with low-income, ethnic-minority students entering an elite university. This
population of students is especially likely to experience stress and pressure in a collegiate
setting. One objective of this study was to compare the stress-mindset intervention to a
traditional stress management program. As such, there were four conditions: (1) the treat-
ment condition in which students received an in-person stress-mindset intervention simi-
lar to that used with the employees from the large financial institution described above;
(2) a stress management intervention condition in which students received an in-person
stress management training of comparable length and format to the treatment condition;
(3) a low-socioeconomic status (SES), no-treatment control group; and (4) a high-SES,
no-treatment control group. Results showed that participants in the mindset condition
experienced higher levels of positive affect during periods of high stress—in this case,
finals weeks—than college students who received either no intervention or a traditional
stress management intervention (Goyer et al., 2019). Furthermore, in the low-SES, no-
treatment control condition, there was a significant negative relationship between levels
of perceived stress immediately before the freshman final exam week and self-reported
exam scores, such that higher perceived stress prior to the final exam week was associ-
ated with lower exam performance; the same relationship was nonsignificant for those in
the stress-mindset, stress management, and high-SES conditions. This suggests that the
stress-mindset intervention may have helped mitigate the negative association between
perceived stress and exam performance for low-SES students.
This series of experimental studies offer substantial evidence that we can make sig-
nificant differences in people’s lived experiences by encouraging them to revise the sub-
jective meaning they assign to stress. The next section explores how a shift in mindset can
produce these changes in health, well-being, and performance.
Mechanisms
We theorize that individuals’ mindsets about the nature of stress can impact health, well-
being, and performance outcomes through four mechanisms: attention, affect, motivation,
and physiology. We review each of these mechanisms below. Although these mechanisms
represent simplified theoretical pathways, we suspect that a set of recursive processes
occur in which each of these mechanisms mutually reinforce one another. Furthermore,
we recognize that many of the proposed mechanisms are important outcomes in and of
themselves depending on the research question, but provide them here with a focus on
how these mechanisms might affect global levels of psychological and physical flourish-
ing. An illustration of the mechanisms of stress mindset can be found in Figure 9.1.
The Stress‑Mindset Intervention 223
STRESS-CAN-BE-ENHANCING MINDSET
STRESS-IS-DEBILITATING MINDSET
Attention
When experiencing stress, the mindset people have about whether it is debilitating or
enhancing is likely to influence attention in logical ways. In theory, having a stress-is-
debilitating mindset will lead people to focus on all the ways stress might be a negative
force in their life, whereas having a stress-can-be-enhancing mindset will lead them to
look for and be more mindful of the ways stress has positive effects. Indeed, research sug-
gests that people who are led to adopt a stress-can-be-enhancing mindset are more likely
to display an attentional bias toward positive stimuli (Crum et al., 2017).
More generally, we posit attention as an important mechanism of stress-mindset
interventions because what people attend to (and how they think about what they attend
to) has been shown to impact performance (e.g., attending to information critical to suc-
ceeding at a particular task; Chajut & Algom, 2003), mental health and well-being (e.g.,
not ruminating on negative events or self-perceptions; Nilly & Winquist, 2002), and even
physical health (e.g., attending to stimuli that reinforce or bring to mind positive expec-
tations about healing; Zion & Crum, 2018). Given these straightforward pathways and
the encouraging experimental results reported above, more research should be conducted
on the specific attentional outcomes of stress-mindset interventions. We would expect
changes in both what one focuses on as well as how—and how long—he or she thinks
about it. Such attributional and rumination patterns are likely to differ depending on
one’s mindset about stress (see Amirkhan, 1990), but these mechanisms have not yet been
tested in experimental settings.
Affect
The second mechanism through which stress mindsets are expected to impact health, well-
being, and performance outcomes is changing how people feel when stressed. Again, the
logic is fairly straightforward. If people hold the mindset that stress is debilitating, then
they are likely to experience stress even more negatively because not only are they stressed
but they are stressed about the effects of stress. If that same stress is experienced through
the mindset that stress can be enhancing, the affective consequences of that stress are likely
to be better. While the stress-can-be-enhancing mindset does not necessarily remove the
stress, and therefore will not completely mitigate negative feelings, such as nervousness or
irritability, the belief that the stress can have positive effects is likely to provide people with
more positive affect, such as feelings of excitement, determination, and inspiration.
Indeed, research supports these mindset-based alterations in affect. Participants
completing a mock job interview who were taught about the enhancing aspects of stress
experienced increases in positive affect when confronted with the stressful experience,
whereas those taught about the debilitating aspects of stress experienced decreases in
positive affect throughout the course of the study (Crum et al., 2017). Additionally,
incoming first-year students at an elite university exposed to the stress-mindset interven-
tion experienced higher levels of positive affect during stressful exam weeks than those in
the stress management and no-treatment control groups (Goyer et al., 2019). These two
studies found no evidence of changes in negative affect. Employees at the large technol-
ogy company who participated in the stress-mindset intervention experienced a small
trending decrease in negative effect, which could reflect a reduction in stress about stress
since they also reported lower levels of perceived distress, but future research should
The Stress‑Mindset Intervention 225
investigate this link further (Crum et al., 2019). Across studies, observed changes in
positive affect have been much stronger and more robust than changes in negative affect,
indicating that viewing stress as enhancing boosts positive emotions, but does not neces-
sarily eliminate the stress and the negative affect stress produces.
Such mindset-generated improvements in positive affect during stress can be a criti-
cal determinant in shaping whether or not stress will have lasting effects on physiological
health and well-being. A wealth of research supports the benefits of positive affect for
mental and physical health, including psychological resilience, physiological immunity,
and healthier behavior (see Fredrickson & Joiner, 2018; Salovey, Rothman, Detweiler,
& Steward, 2000, for an overview). Positive affect has also been shown to promote a
host of skills and behaviors associated with performance in a broad range of contexts,
including creativity and problem solving (see Lyubomirsky, King, & Diener, 2005, for an
overview).
Motivation
Another mechanism that we propose underlies the outcomes of stress-mindset interven-
tions is what people are motivated to do when in a state of stress. People demonstrate a
range of behavioral responses to stress, but a simple, yet highly meaningful dimension of
these responses is the degree to which individuals approach or avoid stressful informa-
tion and events (Roth & Cohen, 1986). If people view stress as enhancing, it stands to
reason that they would be motivated to approach and actively address a stressful situa-
tion, whereas those who view stress as debilitating would be motivated to reduce, avoid,
or distance themselves from the stressful situation.
To test these proposed mechanisms, Crum and colleagues (2013) measured the stress
mindsets of a group of college students taking an introductory psychology class. Several
weeks later, these students were told that a number of them would be selected to give
presentations during that class session. Students were then asked whether they would
like to receive feedback on their presentation from their peers and/or experts at the busi-
ness school. Individuals who had a stress-can-be-enhancing mindset were significantly
more likely than those with a stress-is-debilitating mindset to request feedback, which
represented a more approach-motivated response to the stressful act of public speaking
(Crum et al., 2013). While this finding is only correlational, it provides initial evidence
that approach motivation may contribute to the positive outcomes we see from stress-
mindset interventions. Indeed, research suggests that for stressors that are recurring and
can be addressed, actively approaching the stressor (in this case, seeking feedback so as
to improve one’s public speaking skills) generally results in less long-term distress than
avoiding the stressor (in this case, opting out of feedback so as to avoid thinking further
about the stressful event), primarily because the latter approach does nothing to resolve
the cause of the stress (Carver & Connor-Smith, 2010).
Physiology
The final mechanism we propose as core to the success of stress-mindset interventions
is direct physiological effects. Research on placebo and nocebo effects has demonstrated
the power of our associations and expectations to elicit physiological change, even with-
out behavioral change (see Zion & Crum, 2018, for a review). Because our mindset
226 II. Health and Well-Being
about stress will, almost by definition, influence the associations and expectations we
hold for stress, we expected shifts in stress mindsets to have measurable physiological
consequences.
When in a state of stress, our bodies release catabolic and anabolic hormones.
Catabolic hormones, such as cortisol, stimulate the breakdown of proteins to generate
energy for the body, but prolonged exposure can be damaging, leading to conditions
such as osteoporosis, hypertension, and cardiovascular disease. Anabolic hormones such
as dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate (DHEA-S) act in opposition to catabolic hormones,
stimulating cell repair to facilitate healing processes (Charney, 2004; Epel et al., 1998).
In one study, we measured the salivary cortisol levels of students who were told they
might be selected to give an impromptu presentation to their class. Results revealed that
among students with a stress-can-be-enhancing mindset during this task, those with a
high baseline cortisol response experienced reduced cortisol levels and those with a low
baseline cortisol response experienced increased cortisol levels (Crum et al., 2013). As
suggested by Crum et al., this moderating effect may represent a more adaptive response
than a simple across-the-board decrease in cortisol levels because deficient cortisol levels
have been associated with negative outcomes, such as increased susceptibility to inflam-
matory and autoimmune diseases (Sternberg, 2001), and moderate levels of arousal tend
to promote peak performance (Yerkes & Dodson, 1908). While correlational, these find-
ings hint at a connection between stress mindsets and physiological responses to stress.
Moreover, in the study in which participants engaged in a mock job interview, sali-
vary DHEA-S and cortisol levels were also measured. While those in the stress-can-be-
enhancing condition did not experience a significant change in cortisol levels in response
to the challenge,1 they did experience a boost in the anabolic hormone DHEA-S (Crum et
al., 2017), again suggesting a more adaptive response to stress. In addition to facilitating
healing responses, DHEA-S is thought to protect neurons in the hippocampus from some
of the harmful effects of cortisol and evidence suggests that it may promote psychologi-
cal resilience (Charney, 2004). More research is needed to understand the physiological
mechanisms of stress mindsets.
Effects over Time
Although little data exist on the long-term effects of stress-mindset interventions, the
limited data we do have hints that effects of the intervention hold up over time. Crum et
al. (2019) report positive outcomes 4 weeks postintervention—however, these findings do
not indicate how the effects might diminish or extend past this point. The strongest test
of longitudinal efficacy over multiple time points comes from the intervention conducted
with low-income students beginning college at an elite university. These students received
the stress-mindset intervention during the summer before their freshman year and then
received a “booster” version of the intervention during the fall semester of their freshman
year. Compared to both a group that received a traditional stress management training
and a no-treatment control group, those who received the stress-mindset intervention
1 The effects of cortisol in this study were in response to an acute stressor on a single day. Because we did
not have cortisol response on a baseline day, we were unable to test the more complex reactivity pattern
found in Crum et al. (2013) in which a stress-can-be-enhancing mindset was associated with elevated
cortisol for hyporesponders and decreased cortisol for hyperresponders.
The Stress‑Mindset Intervention 227
experienced higher levels of positive affect during the finals weeks of both their freshman
spring and sophomore spring. In fact, the difference between the stress-mindset group
and the other two groups was directionally larger the second year—a difference of 0.43
standard deviation between the no-treatment control and the stress-mindset intervention
the first year and a difference of 0.48 standard deviation the second year (Goyer et al.,
2019).
These results align with the theory behind mindset interventions, which predicts
that mindset interventions can have lasting—and perhaps even compounding—effects.
If after partaking in a stress-mindset intervention an individual construes a new bout
of stress as enhancing, he or she should be more likely to think and behave in ways that
make him or her more likely to experience positive outcomes from that stress. Experienc-
ing positive outcomes, in turn, should bolster his or her stress mindset. This recursive
process, characteristic of wise interventions (Walton & Wilson, 2018), may explain why
the gap in positive affect between students who took part in the stress-mindset interven-
tion and students in the other two conditions increased between freshman and sopho-
more year, even though these students did not receive a stress-mindset booster in that
time frame. Nevertheless, more research is needed before we can say with confidence how
this intervention impacts long-term outcomes.
Heterogeneity
While there has been very little exploration to date of potential moderators of the stress-
mindset intervention (Jamieson, Crum, Goyer, Marisa, & Akinola, 2018), our theory
suggests that stress itself could be a moderator. If people are not experiencing stress,
their newfound stress-can-be-enhancing mindset would have no fuel to produce mean-
ingful outcomes. Similarly, we would expect that the more stress a person is experienc-
ing, the bigger the effects of this intervention would be, which exploratory analyses have
supported (Smith et al., 2019). However, if people are particularly obstinate in their
view that stress is debilitating—perhaps due to a lifetime spent attributing stress as the
cause of negative events—they may not benefit from this intervention. Additionally, a
rather obvious point that nonetheless bears stating is that this intervention would also be
unlikely to have an effect if participants already hold a strong stress-can-be-enhancing
mindset. As such, this intervention may be less effective in the aggregate among cultures
or subpopulations that already have a more positive view of stress. On the other hand, in
line with wise intervention theory (Walton & Wilson, 2018), people in these populations
who do not yet have a stress-can-be-enhancing mindset might see longer-lasting benefits
from stress-mindset interventions because the cultural messaging they are exposed to on
a daily basis will help maintain and bolster a stress-can-be-enhancing perspective. Future
research should directly test these hypotheses.
COUSINS
This work draws on decades of research on the impact of mindsets (sometimes referred to
as “implicit theories”) that people hold about the nature and workings of things and pro-
cesses in the world, such as the nature of intelligence (Dweck & Leggett, 1988), personal-
ity (Chiu, Hong, & Dweck, 1997), and emotions (Romero, Master, Paunesku, Dweck, &
228 II. Health and Well-Being
Gross, 2014). Stress-mindset work is similar in that it further demonstrates that mindsets
can affect behavior (Dweck & Leggett, 1988), well-being (Smith et al., 2018), and perfor-
mance (Blackwell, Trzesniewski, & Dweck, 2007), and that these mindsets are malleable
through intervention (e.g., Paunesku et al., 2015; Yeager et al., 2016). It also extends this
research by showing that mindsets need not reflect only beliefs about the extent to which
something is fixed versus malleable, but can also reflect beliefs about other characteris-
tics, such as whether something will have enhancing or debilitating effects.
Research on stress mindsets, akin to research on mindset more generally, is related to
but distinct from research and theory on attributions and appraisals (see Wilson & Lin-
ville, 1982). Mindsets are similar to appraisals in that they are subjective judgments regard-
ing a complex and ambiguous reality. Mindsets are distinct, however, in that they can exist
in the mind (consciously or subconsciously) in an ever-ready fashion, whereas appraisals
and attributions occur in response to a specific situation. More precisely, stress mindset
encompasses the beliefs one ascribes to stress, whether they are currently in a stressful situ-
ation or not. Appraisals refer to situation-specific evaluations, such as how much stress one
perceives and the degree to which one evaluates it as a threat (appraised demands exceed
appraised resources) versus a challenge (appraised resources meet appraised demands; see
Blascovich et al., 2003; Blascovich, Mendes, Hunter, & Lickel, 2000).
The stress reappraisal intervention is especially similar to the stress-mindset inter-
vention. Described in more detail by Jamieson and Hangen (Chapter 10, this volume),
this intervention teaches people that the physiological arousal they may experience while
stressed can be useful (as opposed to detrimental). For example, students who were
informed about the potential benefits that physiological arousal can have on performance
prior to taking a practice Graduate Record Examination (GRE) not only performed bet-
ter on the math portion in that lab setting but went on to report higher scores on the
actual GRE (Jamieson, Mendes, Blackstock, & Schmader, 2010; see also Brady et al.,
2018; Jamieson, Peters, Greenwood, & Altose, 2016; Jamieson & Hangen, Chapter 10,
this volume). This can be seen as a targeted substantiation of stress-can-be-enhancing
mindset, in which one particular indicator of stress (physiological arousal) in one par-
ticular instance (during tests) improves one particular outcome (performance). This
approach could also be applied more broadly, and indeed, our recent theory and research
is focusing on synergizing these two interventions into what we call “stress optimization”
(Crum, Jamieson, & Akinola, 2020; Jamieson et al., 2018).
The stress-mindset intervention aims to expose participants to the many ways that stress
can, in fact, be enhancing—a perspective that counters the prevailing cultural narrative
on stress. Using examples from a variety of domains, the intervention seeks to broaden
people’s understanding of how stress can enhance everyday experiences, such as focus
and productivity, recovery from illness, and appreciation for life. To date, the interven-
tion has been delivered using two different structures: (1) a series of short videos that
review selective facts about the effects of stress (Crum et al., 2013, 2017), and (2) a 1- to
2-hour training employing a metacognitive approach that provides a balanced view of
the positive and negative effects of stress and incorporates personal reflection exercises
(Crum et al., 2019).
The Stress‑Mindset Intervention 229
Stress‑Mindset Intervention 1.0
The shorter, selective-information approach first employed by Crum et al. (2013) included
three 3-minute videos on the topics of health, performance, and learning and growth.
The videos contained a combination of music, images, and text, and delivered messages
such as “The secretion of stress hormones does more than just protect the body from
harm. These hormones help rebuild cells, synthesize proteins, and enhance immunity.”
In Crum et al. (2017), only one 3-minute video on the topic of cognitive performance was
used to shift mindsets, and included messages such as “This stress response is designed to
enhance your focus, decision making, memory, [and] performance.” Table 9.1 provides
Learning and Message: Stress threatens your well- Message: Stress can help you grow.
growth being.
Example evidence: The experience of
(Crum et al.,
Example evidence: Stress can lead to stress and adversity can promote deeper
2013)
irritability, emotional exhaustion, low relationships, new perspectives, and
morale/self-esteem, loss of enjoyment, greater appreciation for life.
and memory loss.
Park, Cohen, & Murch (1996); Park &
Schneiderman et al. (2005); Shapiro, Helgeson (2006); Tedeschi & Calhoun
Brown, & Biegel (2007); Shapiro, (2004)
Shapiro, & Schwartz (2000)
Performance Message: Stress makes you perform Message: Stress can help you perform
and productivity worse. better.
(Crum et al.,
Example evidence: Stress deteriorates Example evidence: Stress enhances
2013)
your focus and decision making. your focus and drive.
Keinan (1987); McEwen & Sapolsky Cahill, Gorski, & Le (2003); Fay &
(1995); Schwabe & Wolf (2010) Sonnentag (2002); Hancock & Weaver
(2005)
Cognitive Message: Stress deteriorates your Message: Stress can improve your
performance cognitive performance. cognitive performance.
(Crum, Akinola,
Example evidence: Feeling anxious Example evidence: Feeling anxious
Martin, & Fath,
while completing cognitive tasks can while engaging in cognitive
2017)
hurt performance. performance tasks can help
performance.
Keinan (1987); McEwen & Sapolsky
(1995); Schwabe & Wolf (2010) Cahill et al. (2003); Hancock & Weaver
(2005); Jamieson, Mendes, Blackstock,
& Schmader (2010)
230 II. Health and Well-Being
Stress‑Mindset Intervention 2.0
Although the interventions described above were short, easy to administer, and effectively
shifted participants’ mindsets, there were a handful of potential shortcomings. First, the
original stress-mindset interventions delivered selective information—in this case, tout-
ing the positive effects of stress but not volunteering the negative effects. In reality, stress
can be both enhancing and debilitating, and we may lose credence as scientists if we do
not acknowledge these conflicting messages. Authenticity and noncoercive messages have
been found to be important in past intervention work (e.g., Bryan et al., 2016; Yeager,
Dahl, & Dweck, 2018), and thus we may discover interventions are more effective when
they acknowledge the paradoxical nature of stress (see Crum et al., 2019).
Moreover, as psychological interventions are only a small portion of people’s full
psychological experience, participants will likely come across inconsistent evidence
against a single-sided mindset intervention. For example, a participant of a stress-can-be-
enhancing intervention who reads a news article on the negative health effects of stress or
experiences a dip in his or her work performance due to stress may start to question the
mindset the intervention intended to communicate. Thus, another limitation is that once
people leave the context of the intervention and are inevitably assailed with information
about the ill effects of stress, their mindset might drift back to its preintervention state.
And finally, employing informational videos that confer a single viewpoint is an
inherently passive approach that does not offer people much agency over their mindsets.
On the other hand, conveying the message that mindsets are changeable and offering
people the opportunity to choose their own mindset might help interventions produce
long-term shifts in participants’ mindsets.
To patch these shortcomings, Study 1 of Crum et al. (2019) tested a new version
of the stress-mindset intervention: a 2-hour live training consisting of three modules.
Module 1 targeted the limitations of providing a one-sided view of stress by introduc-
ing the paradox of stress—the fact that stress can simultaneously have enhancing and
debilitating effects on health, well-being, and performance. In addition to providing a
more complete account of stress, this module was designed to help inoculate participants
against future stress-is-debilitating messages. Addressing both sides of stress allows par-
ticipants to reconcile this contrasting information in their mindsets. Additionally, in line
with research on self-persuasion (Aronson, 1999), the module prompted participants to
actively reflect on personal experiences in which stress was beneficial to make these mes-
sages more personally meaningful and more likely to be internalized.
Modules 2 and 3 addressed the lack-of-agency shortcoming by demonstrating the
self-fulfilling nature of mindsets and empowering participants with a concrete technique
for incorporating the information and shifting their stress mindset. Module 2 taught
participants that mindsets can become self-fulfilling such that believing that stress can
be enhancing can increase its beneficial effects. Likening the consequences of mindsets
to the placebo effect, the training explored the power of mindsets with several examples
of self-fulfilling physiological effects in contexts such as healing, aging, and exercising.
The Stress‑Mindset Intervention 231
NUANCES AND MISCONCEPTIONS
Stress versus Stressors
When delivering a stress-mindset intervention, it is important to distinguish between stress
and stressors. Stress is the experience or anticipation of challenge or threat in one’s goal-
related efforts and stressors are any events or conditions that lead to this anticipation. The
stress-mindset intervention makes no claim that individual stressors are good things—
rather, the intervention emphasizes that the stress that emanates from a stressor can, in
some cases, have enhancing outcomes. At first blush, this may seem an inconsequential dis-
tinction. One might argue that if a stressor causes stress, and if that stress can be enhanc-
ing, then surely the stressor should be considered a good thing. But this view neglects the
other outcomes that stressors can spawn. Take the loss of a loved one, for example. The
occurrence itself is, of course, not a good thing. However, the resultant stress could be
harnessed to motivate someone to seek out a support group where they then might develop
strong new friendships. Or perhaps the stress could give them the impetus to make a change
in their life that they had been meaning to make for a while (e.g., change jobs, rekindle a
lost friendship, move to a new area). But accompanying that stress are likely many other
outcomes—perhaps loneliness, fear, or loss of meaning. Although the stress from losing
a loved one might have led to positive outcomes, such as new friends and a better job, it
does not mean that this stressor was a positive thing. Many stressors—poverty, divorce,
illness—are not in and of themselves good things, yet the process of undergoing these expe-
riences can have enhancing outcomes (Tedeschi & Calhoun, 2004).
Managing versus Utilizing Stress
Given the popularity of stress management and reduction programs, participants may
assume that an unspoken objective of the stress-mindset intervention is to diminish the
amount of stress people are experiencing or to help people cope with the negative effects
of stress. On the contrary, the goal is to help people adopt a mindset that will allow them
to harness the stress that they will inevitably encounter in their daily lives. The focus,
then, is on utilizing—not managing or reducing—the stress response to achieve improve-
ments in health, well-being, and performance. One could channel the stress associated
with a moral dilemma into connecting more deeply with their values. One could embrace
the stress of rehabilitating an injury as an opportunity to grow more resilient. One could
convert the stress of a difficult project at work into motivation and focus to complete
the project faster. None of these examples involve reducing stress—rather, the goal is to
refocus the energy inherent to stress so that your stress response can better serve you.
232 II. Health and Well-Being
Seeking Out Stress
Although the intervention centers on utilizing stress, it is not meant to encourage tak-
ing on additional stress. While it is likely the case that additional stress could be useful
in some circumstances, like attempting a challenging task or taking on a new role, the
intervention clarifies that stress can have simultaneous positive and negative effects so it
is not necessarily a good idea to seek out more stress for its enhancing properties. From
an organizational perspective, just as this intervention is not an endorsement of taking
on extra stress, it also should not come across as a rationale for the perpetuation of a
stressful work, school, or caregiving environment. For example, in workplace settings,
these interventions would backfire if employees felt that the implicit goal of the interven-
tion was to get employees to work harder. It is important that this intervention comes
from a place of care and a genuine desire to help people utilize inevitable and/or necessary
stress in their lives, not a desire to help them pile on more. This may require a targeted
and purposeful introduction to the intervention to make it clear to participants why this
intervention is valuable and how the organization hopes its members will benefit from it.
IMPLICATIONS FOR PRACTICE
Together, this research suggests that stress management approaches may be substantially
improved to the degree that they foster more useful mindsets about the nature of stress.
To better disseminate a stress-can-be-enhancing mindset, we need to (1) collaborate with
global organizations and technological platforms to deliver scaled-up interventions, (2)
create trainings for practitioners (train the trainer) on methods of delivering these mes-
sages to reach broader audiences, and (3) influence health organizations (e.g., National
Institutes of Health, World Health Organization) to be more mindful of what mindsets
their messages convey.
In doing so, it is important to acknowledge that many people have spent their entire
lives considering stress as harmful and something to be avoided whenever possible. Some
participants may therefore experience knee-jerk skepticism to the notion that stress can
be enhancing. To combat this response, the intervention incorporates a discussion of the
paradox of stress, emphasizing that stress is by no means always good, and in fact can
have simultaneous negative and positive consequences. However, organizations and prac-
titioners hoping to capitalize on these ideas should ensure they do not suggest that people
should hold a stress-can-be-enhancing mindset, but rather present scientific evidence for
its benefits and allow participants to come to their own conclusion without coercion or
condescension. As Walton and Wilson (2018) note, interventions are likely to be less effec-
tive when they are prescriptive—rather, participants should be given the space to generate
their own narrative and incorporate these ideas in ways they find personally meaningful.
Of course, it is also important to note that this intervention is not a cure-all across any
context in which we might deploy it. Research has suggested that stress-can-be-enhancing
mindsets are impactful during times of stress (e.g., layoffs, military training; Crum et al.,
2013; Smith et al., 2019), but little work has examined the impact of these mindsets in
settings where stress may be less salient. As mentioned earlier, exploratory analyses have
suggested that stress-can-be-enhancing mindsets may be more beneficial to those report-
ing greater amounts of stress (Smith et al., 2019), indicating that these messages may be
The Stress‑Mindset Intervention 233
less impactful for those not experiencing stress when participating in the intervention.
Some participants may even feel concerned that their health, well-being, or performance
could be hampered by their lack of stress, which could be similarly counterproductive. For
example, a student given a stress-can-be-enhancing intervention may be worried that not
being stressed about an exam could result in worse performance. Thus, paying attention
to context is necessary, as with the deployment of any intervention.
IMPLICATIONS FOR PSYCHOLOGICAL THEORY
FUTURE DIRECTIONS
Several questions should be explored in future research, including how stress mindsets
are developed and how stress mindsets function in different types of stress, including
acute versus chronic, as well as more severe conditions, such as anxiety disorders and
depression. Of particular interest from an intervention standpoint is to reconsider the
use of biased information to convey mindset messages. Although an unbiased narra-
tive of conflicting mindsets may be less powerful in some contexts and populations
given the difficulty of incorporating paradoxical beliefs (e.g., Yeager et al., 2016), other
234 II. Health and Well-Being
interventions may be limited in both their application and long-term impact when they
do not address the paradoxical and ambiguous nature of life. In these cases, “meta-
mindsets” or mindsets about the nature of mindsets may themselves be impactful. Tar-
geting these beliefs may be an effective path to improve future interventions, as it suggests
a more agentic pathway to mindset activation. For example, people who believe that
mindsets are self-fulfilling (i.e., those who hold a “self-fulfilling meta-mindset”) might be
more likely to actively recognize and deliberately switch on adaptive mindsets. Further
research is needed to determine what happens when someone thinks stress is generally
debilitating but believes that activating a stress-can-be-enhancing mindset can benefit
their performance. Will this person be motivated to activate this mindset and will it
prove self-fulfilling? Or are there psychological barriers to utilizing mindsets when not
fully believed? In addition, rather than explicitly stating how stress-can-be-enhancing
mindsets can help participants (as this message has the potential to undermine effects;
see Sherman et al., 2009; Yeager et al., 2016), future interventions could convey informa-
tion about meta-mindsets and the nature of stress, then allow participants to act on that
information without explicit influence.
CONCLUSION
Throughout this chapter, we suggested that the predominant view in our society is that
stress is something that should be avoided or mitigated, and we have proposed that a
shift in mindsets, in which stress can be a motivating and energizing force, will promote
optimal functioning. By shifting the mindset we hold about stress, we can begin noticing
and attending to the ways stress benefits our daily lives, experiencing better emotional
responses to stress, motivating ourselves to actively make use of the stressors in our
lives, and benefiting from the physiological reactions stress induces—ultimately achiev-
ing improved health, well-being, and performance.
While seeking to reduce our stress levels is not inherently bad—unless it leads to
further stress about stress or reinforces a stronger stress-is-debilitating mindset—it is
counterproductive to believe that we could simply eradicate stress from our lives. Hans
Selye (1974), whose early work arguably precipitated our cultural adoption of a stress-is-
debilitating mindset, noted toward the end of his career: “Above all, stress is not some-
thing to be necessarily avoided. It is associated with the expression of all our innate
drives. . . . Indeed, complete freedom from stress is death!” (p. 137). We humbly propose
one tweak to this sentiment: above all, stress is not something to be necessarily avoided
but rather something to be harnessed.
REFERENCES
Amirkhan, J. H. (1990). Applying attribution theory to the study of stress and coping. In S. Gra-
ham & V. S. Folkes (Eds.), Attribution theory: Applications to achievement, mental health,
and interpersonal conflict (pp. 79–102). New York: Psychology Press.
Aronson, E. (1999). The power of self-persuasion. American Psychologist, 54(11), 875–884.
Billings, A. G., & Moos, R. H. (1981). The role of coping responses and social resources in attenu-
ating the stress of life events. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 4(2), 139–157.
The Stress‑Mindset Intervention 235
Blackwell, K. L., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Dweck, C. S. (2007). Implicit theories of intelligence pre-
dict achievement across an adolescent transition: A longitudinal study and an intervention.
Child Development, 78(1), 246–263.
Blascovich, J., Mendes, W. B., Hunter, S. B., & Lickel, B. (2000). Stigma, threat, and social inter-
actions. In T. F. Heatherton, R. E. Kleck, M. R. Hebl, & J. G. Hull (Eds.), The social psychol-
ogy of stigma (pp. 307–333). New York: Guilford Press.
Blascovich, J., Mendes, W. B., Tomaka, J., Salomon, K., Seery, M., Kemeny, M., . . . Haley, D.
(2003). The robust nature of the biopsychosocial model challenge and threat: A reply to
Wright and Kirby. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 3(3), 234–243.
Brady, S. T., Hard, B. M., & Gross, J. J. (2018). Reappraising test anxiety increases academic per-
formance of first-year college students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 110(3), 395–406.
Bryan, C. J., Yeager, D. S., Hinojosa, C. P., Chabot, A., Bergen, H., Kawamura, M., & Steub-
ing, F. (2016). Harnessing adolescent values to motivate healthier eating. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 113(39), 10830–10835.
Cahill, L., Gorski, L., & Le, K. (2003). Enhanced human memory consolidation with post-
learning stress: Interaction with the degree of arousal at encoding. Learning and Memory,
10(4), 270–274.
Carver, C. S., & Connor-Smith, J. (2010). Personality and coping. Annual Review of Psychology,
61, 679–704.
Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Weintraub, J. K. (1989). Assessing coping strategies: A theoreti-
cally based approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(2), 267–283.
Chajut, E., & Algom, D. (2003). Selective attention improves under stress: Implications for theo-
ries of social cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 231–248.
Charney, D. S. (2004). Psychobiological mechanisms of resilience and vulnerability: Implications
for successful adaptations to extreme stress. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161(2), 195–
216.
Chiu, C., Hong, Y., & Dweck, C. S. (1997). Lay dispositionism and implicit theories of personal-
ity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(1), 19–30.
Conner, A. L., Boles, D. Z., Markus, H. R., Eberhardt, J. L., & Crum, A. J. (2018). Americans’
health mindsets: Content, cultural patterning, and associations with physical and mental
health. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 53(4), 321–332.
Creswell, J. D. (2017). Mindfulness interventions. Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 491–516.
Crum, A. J., Akinola, M., Martin, A., & Fath, S. (2017). The role of stress mindset in shaping cog-
nitive, emotional, and physiological responses to challenging and threatening stress. Anxiety,
Stress, and Coping, 30(4), 379–395.
Crum, A. J., Corbin, W. R., Brownell, K. D., & Salovey, P. (2011). Mind over milkshakes: Mind-
sets, not just nutrients, determine ghrelin response. Health Psychology, 30(4), 424–429.
Crum, A. J., Jamieson, J. P., & Akinola, M. (2020). Optimizing stress: An integrated intervention
for regulating stress responses. Emotion, 20(1), 120–125.
Crum, A. J., & Langer, E. J. (2007). Mind-set matters: Exercise and the placebo effect. Psycho-
logical Science, 18(2), 165–171.
Crum, A. J., Salovey, P., & Achor, S. (2013). Rethinking stress: The role of mindsets in determin-
ing the stress response. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(4), 716–733.
Crum, A. J., Santoro, E. W., Smith, E. N., Achor, S., Moraveji, N., & Salovey, P. (2019). Evalua-
tion of a metacognitive stress mindset intervention. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Dienstbier, R. A. (1989). Arousal and physiological toughness: Implications for mental and physi-
cal health. Psychological Review, 96(1), 84–100.
Dweck, C. S. (2006). Mindset: The new psychology of success. New York: Random House.
Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality.
Psychological Review, 95(2), 256–273.
236 II. Health and Well-Being
Epel, E. S., McEwen, B. S., & Ickovics, J. R. (1998). Embodying psychological thriving: Physical
thriving in response to stress. Journal of Social Issues, 54(2), 301–322.
Even the youngest of us need to learn to battle stress. (2018). Newsweek. Retrieved from www.
newsweek.com/even-youngest-us-need-learn-battle-stress-932282.
Fay, D., & Sonnentag, S. (2002). Rethinking the effects of stressors: A longitudinal study on per-
sonal initiative. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 7(3), 221–234.
Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1980). An analysis of coping in a middle-aged community sample.
Journal of Health and Social Behaviour, 21(3), 219–239.
Fredrickson, B. L., & Joiner, T. (2018). Reflections on positive emotions and upward spirals. Per-
spectives on Psychological Science, 13(2), 194–199.
Gallup Daily: U.S. Mood. (2018). Retrieved October 31, 2018, from https://news.gallup.com/
poll/106915/gallup-d aily-us-mood.aspx.
Goyer, J. P., Akinola, M., Grunberg, R., & Crum, A. J. (2019). A stress mindset intervention
improves affect for minority students during exam periods in freshman and sophomore
years. Manuscript in preparation.
Hammen, C. (2005). Stress and depression. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 293–319.
Hancock, P. A., & Weaver, J. L. (2005). On time distortion under stress. Theoretical Issues in
Ergonomics Science, 6(2), 193–211.
Herbert, T. B., & Cohen, S. (1993). Stress and immunity in humans: A meta-analytic review. Psy-
chosomatic Medicine, 55, 364–379.
Jackson, M. (2014). Evaluating the role of Hans Selye in the modern history of stress. In D. Cantor
& E. Ramsden (Eds.), Stress, shock, and adaptation in the twentieth century (pp. 21–48).
Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.
Jamieson, J. P., Crum, A. J., Goyer, J. P., Marisa, E. M., & Akinola, M. (2018). Optimizing stress
responses with reappraisal and mindset interventions: An integrated model. Anxiety, Stress,
and Coping, 31(3), 245–261.
Jamieson, J. P., Mendes, W. B., Blackstock, E., & Schmader, T. (2010). Turning the knots in your
stomach into bows: Reappraising arousal improves performance on the GRE. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 208–212.
Jamieson, J. P., Peters, B. J., Greenwood, E. J., & Altose, A. J. (2016). Reappraising stress arousal
improves performance and reduces evaluation anxiety in classroom exam situations. Social
Psychological and Personality Science, 7(6), 579–587.
Kahn, M. (2008). Work-related stress can kill, study finds. Reuters. Retrieved from www.reuters.com/
article/us-heart-stress/work-related-stress-c an-kill-study-finds-idUSL2284632220080123.
Keinan, G. (1987). Decision making under stress: Scanning of alternatives under physical threat.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(3), 639–644.
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer.
Logel, C., Iserman, E. C., Davies, P. G., Quinn, D. M., & Spencer, S. J. (2009). The perils of
double consciousness: The role of thought suppression in stereotype threat. Journal of Exper-
imental Social Psychology, 45(2), 299–312.
Lupien, S. J., McEwen, B. S., Gunnar, M. R., & Heim, C. (2009). Effects of stress throughout
the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(6),
434–445.
Lyubomirsky, S., King, L., & Diener, E. (2005). The benefits of frequent positive affect: Does hap-
piness lead to success? Psychological Bulletin, 131(6), 803–855.
Mangels, J. A., Butterfield, B., Lamb, J., Good, C., & Dweck, C. S. (2006). Why do beliefs about
intelligence influence learning success?: A social cognitive neuroscience model. Social Cogni-
tive and Affective Neuroscience, 1, 75–86.
McEwen, B. S., & Sapolsky, R. M. (1995). Stress and cognitive function. Current Opinion in
Neurobiology, 5(2), 205–216.
The Stress‑Mindset Intervention 237
McGonigal, K. (2015). The upside of stress: Why stress is good for you, and how to get good at it.
New York: Penguin Random House.
Mullainathan, S., & Shafir, E. (2013). Scarcity: Why having too little means so much. New York:
Holt.
Nilly, M., & Winquist, J. (2002). Self-focused attention and negative affect: A meta-analysis. Psy-
chological Bulletin, 128(4), 638–662.
Park, C. L., Cohen, L. H., & Murch, R. L. (1996). Assessment and prediction of stress-related
growth. Journal of Personality, 64(1), 71–105.
Park, C. L., & Helgeson, V. S. (2006). Introduction to the special section: Growth following
highly stressful life events—current status and future directions. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 74(5), 791–796.
Park, D., Yu, A., Metz, S. E., Tsukayama, E., Crum, A. J., & Duckworth, A. L. (2017). Beliefs
about stress attenuate the relation among adverse life events, perceived distress, and self-
control. Child Development, 89(6), 2059–2069.
Paunesku, D., Walton, G. M., Romero, C., Smith, E. N., Yeager, D. S., & Dweck, C. S. (2015).
Mind-set interventions are a scalable treatment for academic underachievement. Psychologi-
cal Science, 26(6), 784–793.
Penley, J. A., Tomaka, J., & Wiebe, J. S. (2002). The association of coping to physical and psy-
chological health outcomes: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 25(6),
551–603.
Romero, C., Master, A., Paunesku, D., Dweck, C. S., & Gross, J. J. (2014). Academic and emo-
tional functioning in middle school: The role of implicit theories. Emotion, 14(2), 227–234.
Roth, S., & Cohen, L. J. (1986). Approach, avoidance, and coping with stress. American Psycholo-
gist, 41(7), 813–819.
Saad, L. (2017, December 20). Eight in 10 Americans afflicted by stress. Retrieved from https://
news.gallup.com/poll/224336/eight-americans-afflicted-stress.aspx.
Salovey, P., Rothman, A., Detweiler, J., & Steward, W. (2000). Emotional states and physical
health. American Psychologist, 55(1), 110–121.
Sapolsky, R. M. (1996). Stress, glucocorticoids, and damage to the nervous system: The current
state of confusion. Stress, 1, 1–19.
Schmader, T., Johns, M., & Forbes, C. (2008). An integrated process model of stereotype threat
effects on performance. Psychological Review, 115(2), 336–356.
Schneiderman, N., Ironson, G., & Siegel, S. D. (2005). Stress and health: Psychological, behav-
ioral, and biological determinants. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 607–628.
Schwabe, L., & Wolf, O. T. (2010). Learning under stress impairs memory formation. Neurobiol-
ogy of Learning and Memory, 93(2), 183–188.
Selye, H. (1936). A syndrome produced by diverse nocuous agents. Nature, 138, 32.
Selye, H. (1974). Stress without distress. In G. Serban (Ed.), Psychopathology of human adapta-
tion (pp. 137–146). Boston: Springer.
Selye, H. (1976). Forty years of stress research: Principal remaining problems and misconceptions.
Canadian Medical Association Journal, 115, 53–56.
Shapiro, S. L., Brown, K. W., & Biegel, G. M. (2007). Teaching self-care to caregivers: Effects of
mindfulness-based stress reduction on the mental health of therapists in training. Training
and Education in Professional Psychology, 1(2), 105–115.
Shapiro, S. L., Shapiro, D. E., & Schwartz, G. E. R. (2000). Stress management in medical educa-
tion: A review of the literature. Academic Medicine, 75(7), 748–759.
Sherman, D. K., Cohen, G. L., Nelson, L. D., Nussbaum, A. D., Bunyan, D. P., & Garcia, J. (2009).
Affirmed yet unaware: Exploring the role of awareness in the process of self-affirmation.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(5), 745–764.
Smith, E. N., Romero, C., Donovan, B., Herter, R., Paunesku, D., Cohen, G. L., . . . Gross, J. J.
238 II. Health and Well-Being
(2018). Emotion theories and adolescent well-being: Results of an online intervention. Emo-
tion, 18(6), 781–788.
Smith, E. N., Young, M. D., & Crum, A. J. (2019). Stress, mindsets, and success in Navy SEALs
special warfare training. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2962.
Somerfield, M. R., & McCrae, R. R. (2000). Stress and coping research: Methodological chal-
lenges, theoretical advances, and clinical applications. American Psychologist, 55(6), 620–
625.
Sternberg, E. M. (2001). Neuroendocrine regulation of autoimmune/inflammatory disease. Jour-
nal of Endocrinology, 169, 429–435.
Szabo, S., Tache, Y., & Somogyi, A. (2012). The legacy of Hans Selye and the origins of stress
research: A retrospective 75 years after his landmark brief “letter” to the editor of Nature
Stress, 15(5), 472–478.
Tedeschi, R. G., & Calhoun, L. C. (2004). Posttraumatic growth: Conceptual foundations and
empirical evidence. Psychological Inquiry, 15(1), 1–18.
Turnwald, B. P., Jurafsky, D., Conner, A., & Crum, A. J. (2017). Reading between the menu lines:
Are restaurants’ descriptions of “healthy” foods unappealing? Health Psychology, 36(11),
1034–1037.
Walton, G. M., & Wilson, T. D. (2018). Wise interventions: Psychological remedies for social and
personal problems. Psychological Review, 125(5), 617–655.
Wilson, T. D., & Linville, P. W. (1982). Improving the academic performance of college freshmen:
Attribution therapy revisited. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42(2), 367–376.
Yeager, D. S., Dahl, R. E., & Dweck, C. S. (2018). Why interventions to influence adolescent
behavior often fail but could succeed. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(1), 101–122.
Yeager, D. S., Romero, C., Paunesku, D., Hulleman, C. S., Schneider, B., Hinojosa, C., . . . Dweck,
C. S. (2016). Using design thinking to improve psychological interventions: The case of the
growth mindset during the transition to high school. Journal of Educational Psychology,
108(3), 374–391.
Yerkes, R. M., & Dodson, J. D. (1908). The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-
formation. Journal of Comparative Neurology and Psychology, 18(5), 459–482.
Zion, S. R., & Crum, A. J. (2018). Mindsets matter: A new framework for harnessing the placebo
effect in modern medicine. In International review of neurobiology (Vol. 138, pp. 137–160).
London: Elsevier.
C H A P TE R 10
Jeremy P. Jamieson and Emily J. Hangen
Stress is typically perceived as having negative effects on health and performance, and
people exert substantial efforts to reduce or avoid experiencing stress. Contrary to lay
beliefs, however, stress is not only a negative state to be unilaterally avoided. Adaptive stress
responses can also facilitate performance, help people achieve goals, and even confer
protective health benefits. This chapter presents stress reappraisal, a psychological interven-
tion developed to help optimize stress responses, and reap performance, cognitive, and
health benefits. Rather than reducing or down-regulating stress arousal, stress reappraisal
educates individuals about the adaptive benefits of stress such that bodily signs of stress
(e.g., racing heart) are conceptualized as coping resources. Reappraising stress arousal as
functional and adaptive increases appraisals of coping resources, which in turn, improve
downstream outcomes. This chapter reviews the extant literature on the background,
mechanisms, and outcomes of the intervention, and discusses heterogeneity, implementa-
tion, and implications of the intervention.
Acute social stressors rooted in achievement pressures or social interactions are ubiqui-
tous in modern society. For instance, in educational settings, nearly all students experi-
ence testing situations as negative stressors (Bradley et al., 2010). Beyond the economic
and performance costs of stress, the accumulation of acute, maladaptive stress has sub-
stantial health consequences. Notably, repeated maladaptive acute stress responses are
associated with increased rates of cardiovascular disease, reduced immune function, and
cognitive impairment, to name a few outcomes (e.g., Jefferson et al., 2010; Lundberg,
2005; Matthews, Gump, Block, & Allen, 1997).
Although stress is typically perceived and experienced as negative and maladaptive,
stress can also facilitate cognitive performance, promote active coping, and protect the
239
240 II. Health and Well-Being
body against the damaging effects of catabolic hormones (e.g., Dienstbier, 1989; Jamie-
son, Crum, Goyer, Marotta, & Akinola, 2018; Mendes, Gray, Mendoza-Denton, Major,
& Epel, 2007). Thus, without experiencing the benefits of (the right kind of) stress,
people can underachieve, miss opportunities for growth, and even suffer negative health
outcomes. The notion that stress can have facilitative and debilitative effects (Crum,
Akinola, Martin, & Fath, 2017), however, is not represented in lay theories. People typ-
ically only consider the negative facets of stress and equate stress with distress. This
perspective is even echoed by social scientists. Scales developed to measure stress are
overwhelmingly constructed of negative-valence items that present stress as a negative
state (e.g., Cohen, Kamarck, & Mermelstein, 1983). The result is that people believe that
efforts to cope with stressors should focus on eliminating or avoiding stress altogether
rather than seeking to promote adaptive stress responses (Brooks, 2014).
Given the societal costs of negative stress responses, what can be done to discourage
maladaptive responses and promote thriving during acute stress contexts? Toward this
end, the stress reappraisal intervention reviewed here posits that the way people think
about stress responses can unlock adaptive benefits and attenuate costs. Stress reappraisal
focuses on promoting more adaptive stress response profiles by altering cognitive appraisal
processes (Jamieson, Hangen, Lee, & Yeager, 2018). Specifically, this intervention presents
the arousal experienced during stressful situations as a functional coping resource that aids
performance—that is, signs of stress arousal (e.g., racing heart) are interpreted as coping
tools that facilitate adaptive patterns of cognitive appraisals to affect subsequent physi-
ological, affective, and motivational processes. For example, consider a job applicant sit-
ting down for an important interview with a prospective employer. The applicant’s heart is
racing and his or her palms are sweaty. These bodily responses could be interpreted as signs
the applicant is nervous and that stress levels need to be reduced through, for instance,
deep breathing. Alternatively, the bodily stress responses could be interpreted as signs the
applicant is “amped up” and ready to perform. Whereas the former interprets stress as
maladaptive and to be avoided, the later interprets stress as functional and to be optimized.
Contrary to traditional relaxation techniques that seek to reduce stress, stress reappraisal
seeks to help individuals make meaning of their stress responses as helpful. The extant
literature suggests that this intervention can improve physiological responses, facilitate
performance, and reduce attention for negative cues in acute stress contexts.
BACKGROUND
Stress reappraisal grew directly out of the biopsychosocial (BPS) model of challenge and
threat (Blascovich, 1992). Challenge and threat theory, in turn, built on the framework
provided by the appraisal theory of emotion (Lazarus, 1991) and mapped the biological
underpinnings of acute stress responses (upstream appraisal processes) onto downstream
health, behavior, and decision outcomes in “motivated performance situations,” which
are goal-relevant performance situations requiring instrumental (i.e., active) responses
(Blascovich, Mendes, Hunter, & Salomon, 1999; Blascovich & Mendes, 2010; Mendes,
2009). For instance, job interviews, academic exams, performance reviews, public
speeches, and negotiations are all examples of motivated performance situations.
A core contribution of the BPS model of challenge and threat is specifying the psy-
chological processes underpinning stress responses. Specifically, in challenge and threat
Stress Reappraisal Interventions 241
Task Engagement
by Stress Reappraisal
Processes Targeted
Psychological
Challenge Threat
Physiological
aversive, avoidance motivation (e.g., Jamieson, Valdesolo, & Peters, 2014). Downstream,
challenge predicts positive behavioral and performance outcomes (e.g., Blascovich et al.,
1999), but threat impairs decision making, and is associated with cognitive decline and
cardiovascular disease (Jefferson et al., 2010; Matthews et al., 1997). Moreover, in a
study that directly informed the development of stress reappraisal, individuals who were
assigned to appraise a speech preparation task as a challenge experienced more positive
emotions (Tugade & Fredrickson, 2004).
Together this research suggests that manipulating or modifying resource and
demand appraisal processes has the potential to improve physiological stress responses
and cognitive and behavioral outcomes. Indeed, a growing literature on stress reappraisal
provides support for the idea that acute stress responses can, indeed, be improved via
modifying appraisal processes (e.g., Beltzer, Nock, Peters, & Jamieson, 2014; Brady,
Hard, & Gross, 2018; Hangen, Elliot, & Jamieson, 2019; Jacquart, Papini, Freeman,
Bartholomew, & Smits, 2020; Jamieson et al., 2020; Jamieson, Mendes, Blackstock, &
Schmader, 2010; Jamieson, Mendes, & Nock, 2013; Jamieson, Nock, & Mendes, 2012,
2013; Jamieson, Peters, Greenwood, & Altose, 2016; John-Henderson, Rheinschmidt, &
Mendoza-Denton, 2015; Moore, Vine, Wilson, & Freeman, 2015; Rozek, Ramirez, Fine,
& Beilock, 2019; Sammy et al., 2017; Oveis, Gu, Ocampo, Hangen, & Jamieson, 2020).
PSYCHOLOGICAL PROCESSES
The focal goal of the stress reappraisal intervention is to increase resource appraisals by
defining the stress response itself as a coping resource (e.g., Jamieson, 2017). In the stress
reappraisal intervention literature, individuals are provided with instructions that pres-
ent the arousal experienced during stressful situations as a functional coping resource
that can facilitate performance. The theory is that this information will alter the ratio
of resources and demands such that the individual is more likely to view the stress as a
challenge as opposed to a threat.
Contrary to other stress management approaches, the stress reappraisal intervention
is not aimed at convincing individuals that stressful situations are not demanding—that
is, reappraising stress does not decrease perceptions of effort required to address stress-
ors. The focus of stress reappraisal manipulations on resource appraisals is an important
mechanistic distinction when individuals encounter acutely stressful situations that can-
not be avoided or mitigated. For example, in the U.S. (and many others) educational
system, students frequently must take evaluative exams (i.e., a stressful situation); these
exams have important consequences for grades, placements, and applications, and are
generally unavoidable. However, students who reframe stress responses as functional can
help optimize performance in stressful academic testing contexts (e.g., Brady et al., 2018;
Jamieson et al., 2010, 2016, 2020).
PHYSIOLOGICAL PROCESSES
Importantly for the effectiveness of the stress reappraisal intervention, challenge and
threat psychological states are associated with patterns of physiological responding (see
Mendes & Park, 2014, for a biologically oriented review). Dienstbier’s (1989) work on
Stress Reappraisal Interventions 243
physiological toughness helped to provide the organization for the physiological response
patterns delineated by challenge and threat. Specifically, challenge and threat physiologi-
cal responses derive from activation of the sympathetic–adrenal–medullary (SAM) and
hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axes. Both challenge- and threat-t ype responses
are accompanied by SAM activation, which leads to the synthesis and secretion of cat-
echolamines, particularly epinephrine (or adrenaline) and norepinephrine. More down-
stream, catecholamines increase ventricular contractility (decreasing preejection period
and increasing heart rate), constrict veins (facilitating venous return of blood to the heart),
and dilate atrial blood vessels (via binding of epinephrine to beta-2 receptors; Brownley,
Hurwitz, & Schneiderman, 2000). Challenge-type responses, which are dominated by
SAM activation, are thus characterized by increased cardiac output (CO)—the volume
of blood pumped by the heart across a period of time (usually 1 minute)—and decreased
resistance in the total peripheral vasculature resistance (TPR). Challenge-t ype responses
also allow for a rapid onset and offset of responses: Resources are mobilized rapidly and
individuals return to homeostasis quickly after stress offset.
In addition to activating the SAM axis, threat also strongly activates the HPA axis,
which is frequently assessed by measuring cortisol (its end product). Given the shorter
half-lives of catecholamines relative to cortisol (e.g., a few minutes vs. 1+ hours, respec-
tively), HPA activation elicits more prolonged stress responses after stress offset relative
to SAM-dominated challenge responses. HPA activation also tempers effects of the SAM
axis and results in reduced (or little change in) CO and increased TPR downstream in
the cardiovascular system (see Figure 10.1). Thus, the end consequences of the cardiovas-
cular changes accompanying challenge and threat states is that challenge results in more
oxygenated blood being delivered to peripheral sites, such as the brain, whereas blood is
concentrated in the core of the body when individuals experience threat.
As patterns of appraisals shift from more threat oriented to more challenge ori-
ented (i.e., as resource appraisals increase relative to demand appraisals, respectively),
stress axes are differentially recruited to direct downstream stress responses—thus, by
increasing appraisals of resources, the stress reappraisal intervention seeks to elicit SAM-
dominated patterns of stress that facilitate thriving under stress.
EMPIRICAL EVIDENCE
Outcomes
Initial findings from the stress reappraisal literature suggest that the intervention improves
outcomes in acute stress contexts. The first empirical test of the intervention was a com-
bined laboratory/field experiment (Jamieson et al., 2010). In this study, students prepar-
ing to take the Graduate Record Examination (GRE) were first recruited for a laboratory
practice GRE session. They were randomly assigned to receive stress reappraisal instruc-
tions or no instructions prior to taking practice exams. Reappraisal participants out-
performed controls on the quantitative section of the practice GRE. Then, 1–3 months
after the practice exam, participants provided score reports from their actual, real GRE
tests. Again, reappraisal participants outperformed controls on the quantitative section.
Reappraisal participants also reported that stress arousal was more beneficial for perfor-
mance.
244 II. Health and Well-Being
Mechanisms
Although initial stress reappraisal studies focused on assessing the effects of the interven-
tion on downstream outcomes, subsequent work has more clearly elucidated the mecha-
nisms underlying these effects. As touched on previously, appraisals of resources (rather
than demands) are the focal psychological mechanisms of stress reappraisal. To demon-
strate, in a double-blind field experiment, community college students completed either
stress reappraisal or placebo control materials prior to taking an in-class mathematics
exam (Jamieson et al., 2016). Reappraisal instructions educated students about the adap-
tive benefits of stress arousal, whereas placebo materials instructed students to ignore
stress. After the instruction materials, math anxiety and stress appraisals were assessed.
Consistent with performance effects observed previously, reappraisal participants scored
higher on their exams, and also reported less math evaluation anxiety and higher resource
appraisals. Bootstrap mediation analyses suggested the effect of the intervention on exam
performance manifested through appraisals of resources.
The emphasis on changing cognitive appraisals of coping resources can also be seen
in a laboratory study (Beltzer et al., 2014). Prior to performing a stressful public speaking
task, participants were randomly assigned stress reappraisal or were given no instruc-
tions, and then reported on their stress appraisals in anticipation of speaking. Relative
to controls, reappraisal participants displayed less anxiety. The effect of reappraisal on
behavioral displays of anxiety was mediated by the ratio of resources to demands, such
that the higher resources were relative to demands, the less anxiety participants displayed
while speaking.
Taken together, the extant literature suggests that stress appraisals, and resource
appraisals in particular, are core mechanisms of the stress reappraisal intervention (see
Jamieson, 2017, for a review of stress appraisals). To date, however, all direct tests of
mechanisms have relied on self-reports of appraisals, which have inherent shortcomings.
Notably, stress appraisals are processed both consciously and unconsciously/automati-
cally (Blascovich, 2008). Thus, individuals may or may not have access to all compo-
nents of multifaceted psychological appraisal processes when completing explicit self-
reports. Moreover, self-reports cannot be obtained in vivo during stressful situations.
For these reasons (and others; see Blascovich, 2008, for a review), research grounded in
challenge and threat theory—and by extension, the stress reappraisal intervention—has
Stress Reappraisal Interventions 245
emphasized mapping psychological processes (e.g., challenge and threat, appraisal pat-
terns) by measuring physiological responses (e.g., Blascovich et al., 1999)—that is, if
appraisals of resources exceed demands, this pattern of psychological responding will
necessitate challenge-t ype physiological responses (see Figure 10.1), even if the individual
cannot (retro- or proactively) report on the full array of his or her appraisals with a high
degree of accuracy. Future lines of research on stress reappraisal, and other BPS-oriented
research, could seek to develop implicit measures of appraisals to more tightly yoke psy-
chological mechanisms to downstream outcomes.
Effects over Time
Beyond the basic questions of an intervention’s effectiveness it is important to consider
how long effects persist, and how effects unfold over time. An effective intervention that
produces brief effects requiring repeated delivery to maintain the effects has limited effi-
cacy for producing lasting change. Addressing questions of the duration and dynamics of
intervention effects necessitates elucidating mechanisms. Although the majority of stress
reappraisal research focuses on in vivo stress processes, research has observed subsequent
effects of the intervention following stressful contexts. As noted previously, reappraising
stress arousal reduced attention for emotionally negative cues in a laboratory setting
(Jamieson et al., 2012)—that is, reappraisal participants exhibited less vigilance for nega-
tive cues than control participants even though there was no evaluative pressure (i.e., no
social stress) during the attention task. Thus, the effects of the intervention on attentional
processes persisted after stress offset.
Research has also examined longer-term performance effects of stress reappraisal in
educational contexts. In initial examinations of duration of effects in an educational con-
text, students were randomly presented with reappraisal or control instructions embed-
ded in an e-mail message delivered prior to exams, and performance was measured across
an academic semester. Reappraisal instructions led to higher end-of-semester course
grades relative to controls via reducing exam worries (Brady et al., 2018). Moreover, as
touched on previously, a study with students taking the GRE observed performance ben-
efits as a function of the intervention up to 3 months after initial delivery without boost-
ers (Jamieson et al., 2010). Taken together, these lagged effects of the stress reappraisal
intervention suggest students are capable of applying instructions beyond the specific
context in which the intervention is delivered. However, these studies do not elucidate
the mechanisms underlying precisely how stress reappraisal might feed forward to impact
future outcomes.
Exploring processes in longitudinal designs to answer mechanistic questions of
temporal dynamics is potentially important because challenge/threat appraisals are
not encapsulated to specific situations or within individuals—rather, prior appraisals
influence future appraisals in subsequent similar (or even unfamiliar) situations (e.g.,
Gross, 2015). Toward this end, a recently completed study examined temporal dynam-
ics of stress reappraisal in an educational setting. More specifically, a double-blind field
experiment examined how stress reappraisal impacts students across several exam situa-
tions (Jamieson et al., 2020). To do so, students were randomly assigned to receive stress
reappraisal or expectancy control instructions immediately prior to their second in-class
exams (to allow for comparisons to Exam 1). Then, psychological processes—stress
appraisals, achievement goals (Elliot, 1999), and procrastination (Steel, 2010)—were
246 II. Health and Well-Being
assessed between Exams 2 and 3 with the aim of examining how stress reappraisal might
impact psychological processes outside of classroom exam situations, and how these pro-
cesses might feed forward to affect future academic performance and neuroendocrine
responses. Preliminary results suggest that teaching students that stress is functional
and adaptive improved academic performance and neuroendocrine functioning during
Exam 2. Importantly for understanding temporal dynamics, students using reappraisal
reported they procrastinated less and had more performance-approach goals in at-home
diary measures, which then directly predicted subsequent Exam 3 scores (e.g., more pro-
crastination led to lower scores). Thus, reappraisal may not only improve proximal stress
responses and performance but also improve psychological processes that are predictive
of longer-term academic success as has been seen in other interventions reported through-
out this volume.
Heterogeneity of Effects
Like all psychological interventions, stress reappraisal is not a “silver bullet” for improv-
ing stress outcomes. The effectiveness of reappraising stress arousal on outcomes depends
on myriad factors, including demographics, individual differences, and contextual fac-
tors, to name a few. This section focuses on reviewing and proposing moderating fac-
tors of the intervention, including both situational and individual difference factors. The
proposed moderating factors were developed from an understanding of how stress reap-
praisal counters traditional lay beliefs—namely, presenting stress arousal (and even sub-
sequent negative affective experiences of anxiety) as a coping resource stands in stark
contrast to how people typically attempt to manage stress. In other words, stress reap-
praisal is not aimed at eliminating or dampening stress arousal. It does not encourage
relaxation or reduction of sympathetic arousal as most stress management approaches
do, but instead focuses on changing the type of stress response experienced (threat →
challenge). In fact, negative lay conceptualizations of stress play an important role in why
stress reappraisal is effective. If people already believed stress was beneficial for coping,
teaching them to reappraise stress as functional and adaptive would do little to impact
challenge and threat responses.
Context and Situational Factors
Given typical “stress-is-bad” perspectives, the classic stress regulation literature has often
(but not always) instructed individuals to reappraise external cues to decrease stress
arousal in passive situations (e.g., Gross, 2002). However, situational factors are impor-
tant to consider when implementing classic reappraisal methods or arousal-maintaining
stress reappraisal methods. For instance, in clinically relevant samples/situations, the
arousal experienced is often not appropriate/functional. To demonstrate, an individual
with posttraumatic stress disorder may experience a stress response in situations that are
not acutely demanding but include trauma-inducing cues, such as a veteran responding
to hearing a car backfire because it resembles the noise of a gunshot. Thus, reappraising
stress arousal as beneficial in these situations is not appropriate because arousal is not
needed to actively address acute task demands. Acutely stressful motivated performance
situations, however, necessitate instrumental responding where increased sympathetic
arousal can be functional. Notably, stress reappraisal provides a method for regulating
Stress Reappraisal Interventions 247
COUSINS
The stress reappraisal intervention seeks to harness the power of cognitive appraisal pro-
cesses to promote active coping with acute stressors. Resource and demand appraisal
processes, however, target specific stressful situations, and can (and often do) fluctuate
substantially from situation to situation. For instance, one may consider oneself an adept
skier. Presented with a demanding trail (e.g., steep, icy, and narrow), the expert skier per-
ceives his or her coping resources (ability, skills, familiarity, etc.) to exceed task demands.
However, when the same expert skier is placed in a mathematics achievement context,
such as taking an important standardized test, he or she may perceive the demands (high
difficulty of problems, uncertainty processes, etc.) as exceeding the ability to cope (poor
math knowledge, little experience, etc.). Thus, the effectiveness of any intervention for
which appraisal processes are focal mechanisms will likely vary from situation to situa-
tion because of differences in basal appraisal patterns and affective response tendencies.
To improve the likelihood of observing effects across many different contexts, other cog-
nitive processes (in addition to appraisals) can be harnessed.
Stress Reappraisal Interventions 249
Notably, research from the mindsets literature suggests another avenue for inter-
vention. In addition to targeting domain-specific appraisal processes, improving stress
responses can also be achieved by targeting domain-general, more meta-level beliefs.
For example, research on stress mindsets, which are beliefs about the nature of stress
in general not tied to specific situations—demonstrates how manipulating mindsets can
produce downstream benefits (e.g., Crum et al., 2013). Individuals holding a “stress-is-
enhancing mindset” believe that stress can have enhancing effects, whereas those holding
a “stress-is-debilitating mindset” believe that stress has debilitating effects. Although the
stress can have either or both enhancing and/or debilitating effects, these stress mindsets
orient individuals to a set of expectations and motivations.
Similar to effects observed in the stress reappraisal literature, experimental mindset
research demonstrates that intervention materials that teach a stress-is-enhancing mind-
set can improve stress responses, performance, and health outcomes. To demonstrate,
employees taught about the enhancing nature of stress self-reported greater improve-
ments in work performance and general health compared to employees taught about the
debilitating effects of stress and no instruction controls (Crum et al., 2013). Subsequent
research has observed longer-term effects of stress-mindset training across weeks or pos-
sibly even years. For instance, employees taught a stress-is-enhancing mindset reported
better health and well-being up to 4 weeks after intervention delivery (Crum et al., 2019),
and in a college student sample, a stress-is-enhancing mindset intervention delivered
prior to the start of college predicted positive outcomes across students’ freshman and
sophomore years (Goyer, Akinola, Grunberg, & Crum, 2018).
Importantly for research on stress reappraisal, altering general beliefs about stress
may benefit outcomes by shaping situation-specific appraisal processes (cf. Yeager, Lee, &
Jamieson, 2017). Individuals with a more stress-is-enhancing mindset could be expected
to benefit more from stress reappraisal manipulations because they are already “precon-
ditioned” to appraise stress responses as functional and adaptive. Conversely, individuals
who hold strong stress-is-debilitating mindsets might be resistant to stress reappraisal
interventions because the materials align poorly with their worldview that stress is harm-
ful for them.
To date, only one study has explored stress mindsets and appraisal processes in the
same design (Crum et al., 2019). Compared to a stress-is-debilitating mindset, a stress-is-
enhancing mindset improved affective responses in a negative social-evaluative situation
that elicited threat appraisals. As might be expected, however, optimal outcomes were
observed when mindsets and appraisals aligned: participants who both held a stress-is-
enhancing mindset and received positive feedback that elicited challenge appraisals expe-
rienced the most benefits. However, appraisals and mindsets were not integrated. Taken
together, the growing literature on stress mindsets suggests that stress responses can be
improved by altering meta-level belief systems. This approach provides a nice comple-
ment to the situationally grounded stress reappraisal approach, and suggests avenues for
potential integrations of the two interventions to optimize effects (see Jamieson et al.,
2018, for a review).
In addition to focal grounding in challenge and threat theory, the development of
stress reappraisal methods have relied on the theory of constructed emotion (Barrett,
2017) and the extended process model of emotion regulation (Gross, 2015). Construc-
tionist models of emotion posit that emotions (e.g., anxiety or excitement) are experi-
enced when categorized using knowledge from cognitive concepts. Thus, emotions are
250 II. Health and Well-Being
NUANCES AND MISCONCEPTIONS
One misconception about stress reappraisal is that it is a stress reduction or stress man-
agement technique—that is, traditional efforts to regulate stress responses have focused
on reducing or “managing” stress responses because lay conceptualizations of stress are
overwhelmingly negative (e.g., Brooks, 2014). However, as noted previously, stress reap-
praisal interventions do not seek to attenuate arousal or reduce perceptions of situational
demands. Informing individuals about the functional benefits of stress responses (e.g.,
increased heart rate helps to deliver oxygenated blood to the brain and periphery) does
not imply that stressful situations are devoid of demands, nor does this approach sug-
gest the best method for coping with stressful experiences is to down-regulate arousal
and “keep calm.” Consider an athlete about to compete in an important match. Stress
experienced in anticipation of performing can be appraised as challenging, such as “I
possess the necessary training and ability to beat my competitor” (i.e., resources out-
weigh demands), or threatening, such as “I worry that I cannot beat this difficult oppo-
nent” (i.e., demands exceed resources). Stress reappraisal seeks to encourage the athlete
to appraise as the stress experienced in anticipation of and during performance as a
resource that will help him or her achieve victory (i.e., boosting resource appraisals). The
aim of this intervention is not to encourage the athlete to view his or her competitors as
worse, or the match as less important or meaningful (i.e., reducing demand appraisals).
Another misconception of stress reappraisal is that it is easily applied across all indi-
viduals and all stressful domains. As touched on previously in the “Heterogeneity” sec-
tion, though, a prerequisite to exhibiting acute stress responses in challenge and threat
theory is engagement. People must care about performing, coping, and so on in a particu-
lar stressful context for challenge and threat responses to manifest. If an individual does
not care, invest effort, or disengages, he or she is not actively responding to the stressor.
Only when individuals actively engage is stress reappraisal hypothesized to help improve
responses—that is, stress reappraisal is unlikely to benefit individuals when there is no
stress to appraise/reappraise. For example, consider a stressful standardized exam situ-
ation. A student could not care about the standardized test, disengage with the situa-
tion, and walk out of the room without answering any questions. Such a response pat-
tern would not be associated with any stress response because there are no situational
demands, and thus no resources are needed to cope with demands. The stressful task (the
standardized test) is simply avoided before appraisal processes are engaged. Reappraising
stress arousal is unlikely to promote initial engagement with a stressful situation unless
levels of anxiety (or a similar affective state) are so high in anticipation of the stressor that
the negative affect leads to complete avoidance of the stress context (cf. Peters, Overall,
& Jamieson, 2014). The prior example, however, is not a particularly realistic depic-
tion of how the vast majority of students engage with and respond to testing situations.
To advance through educational systems, students must take exams and cannot sim-
ply remove themselves from situations. Thus, students must engage, which explains why
nearly all students experience exams as stressful (Bradley et al., 2010). Once engaged,
appraisal processes come online to evaluate personal coping resources relative to situ-
ational demands (Blascovich & Mendes, 2010). Stress reappraisal can then help improve
responses by increasing the ratio of resources to demands.
Another nuance of the stress reappraisal intervention is the kind of stress tar-
geted. The research reviewed here provides empirical evidence for the benefits of stress
252 II. Health and Well-Being
IMPLICATIONS FOR PRACTICE
To date, laboratory and field research has utilized two general forms of stress reap-
praisal: (1) a ~10-minute reading exercise comprising summaries of scientific articles on
the adaptive benefits of stress responses and responding to questions aimed at encourag-
ing endorsement of the material via “saying-is-believing” processes (e.g., Jamieson et
al., 2012; see Walton & Wilson, 2018, for a discussion; materials available for down-
load at http://socialstresslab.wixsite.com/urochester/research); and (2) a “short-form,”
single-paragraph instruction presented below (see also John-Henderson et al., 2015). For
instance, Jamieson and colleagues (2010) used the following short-form instructions to
manipulate stress appraisals prior to GRE tests:
“People think that feeling anxious while taking a standardized test will make them
do poorly on the test. However, recent research suggests that arousal doesn’t hurt
performance on these tests and can even help performance . . . people who feel
‘anxious’ during a test might actually do better. This means that you shouldn’t feel
concerned if you do feel anxious while taking today’s GRE test. If you find yourself
feeling anxious, simply remind yourself that your arousal could be helping you do
well.”
the instructions that are provided to them. To demonstrate, a field experiment found
that teaching community college students to appraise their stress arousal as a coping
tool immediately before an exam reduced their test anxiety and improved performance
(Jamieson et al., 2016). In this experiment, the research team visited participating class-
rooms on the days of exams and administered the intervention materials immediately
prior to students taking their exams, which required substantial coordination between
the research team and instructors, as well as steps to ensure that instructors administer-
ing and scoring exams remained blind to condition assignment and hypotheses.
This targeted approach requires that practitioners seeking to implement this inter-
vention should be cognizant of when the materials are delivered. If a disconnection exists
between when/where materials are completed and when/where stress is experienced, there
is a possibility that some effectiveness of the intervention will be lost. However, it should
be noted that one prior study used a more distal and passive delivery method—students
were e-mailed the “short-form” intervention materials the night before an exam—and
still observed psychological and performance effects of the intervention (Brady et al.,
2018). Thus, it may be possible to use a less immersive delivery method, though care
should be taken to ensure that intervention materials are recalled or applied in the same
context where effects are measured.
IMPLICATIONS FOR PSYCHOLOGICAL THEORY
In addition to emerging from research on the BPS model of challenge and threat, stress
reappraisal also has roots in concepts derived from emotion regulation and achievement
goal theories. Because of these shared underlying processes, research on stress reappraisal
has the potential to inform the development of these broader models. For example, the
classic process model of emotion regulation highlights “cognitive change” processes
such as reappraisal as effective (and heavily studied) methods for regulating emotional
experiences (Gross, 2002). Along these lines, stress reappraisal changes cognitive factors
(appraisals) to regulate emotional experiences (stress responses). Recent advances in emo-
tion regulation theory have emphasized the importance of valuations: when an event/
experience/situation is “good for me” versus “bad for me” (Gross, 2015). Importantly
for regulating stress responses, reappraising stress as functional and adaptive has the
potential to promote the valuation that stress can be “good for me.” In this way, stress
reappraisal can be conceptualized as an emotion regulation tool operating within the
framework provided by the extended process model.
The emphasis of stress reappraisal on improving physiological, cognitive, and per-
formance outcomes in performance contexts makes it ripe for integration with research
on achievement goals: cognitive representations of future competence-based outcomes
that one approaches or avoids (Elliot & Fryer, 2008). Crossing dimensions of evaluative
standard and valence yields four basic goals: mastery-approach goals (focused on doing
well relative to task requirements or one’s own performance trajectory), performance-
approach goals (focused on doing well relative to others), mastery- avoidance goals
(focused on not doing poorly relative to task requirements or one’s own performance
trajectory), and performance-avoidance goals (focused on not doing poorly relative to
others; Elliot, 1999). Importantly for integration with stress reappraisal, achievement
goals are situation-specific, self-regulatory processes.
254 II. Health and Well-Being
Once instantiated, achievement goals can then create a framework through which
people interpret and experience future achievement situations (e.g., Dweck, 1986). For
instance, reappraising stress arousal can increase subsequent performance- approach
goals (Jamieson et al., 2019). The commitment to pursuing these approach goals may
then influence a multitude of affective, cognitive, and behavioral processes and outcomes.
Along these lines, in one set of studies, the positive focus stemming from performance-
approach goals of attaining academic competence was linked to appraising a specific
course or exam as a challenge (McGregor & Elliot, 2002). Moreover, as with cyclical
emotion regulation processes (Gross, 2015), achievement scenarios are usually not one-
off experiences but instead operate in sequences. Thus, with regard to integrating goals
with stress reappraisal, achievement goals may operate as both antecedents of appraisal
processes and consequences of challenge–threat responses resulting from stress reap-
praisal manipulations. Moreover, it is possible that goals could serve both these roles
sequentially and reciprocally.
CONCLUSIONS AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS
Lay conceptualizations of stress are generally negative. In fact, when asked to choose
the best advice for optimally performing in an upcoming stressful task, 91% of people
indicated that remaining calm and relaxed (i.e., trying not to be “stressed”) was the best
option (Brooks, 2014). This negative conceptualization of stress can play an important
role in maintaining and promoting negative health and well-being outcomes associated
with stressful experiences because appraisals and perceptions of stress directly impact
biological responses (e.g., Crum et al., 2013; Blascovich & Mendes, 2010; Jamieson et
al., 2018). Thus, intervening to change how people appraise and engage with stressful sit-
uations can help improve acute stress responses. This review highlights the physiological,
cognitive, and performance benefits of reappraising stress arousal. In this line of research,
stress responses are presented as a functional coping resource that aids performance—
that is, signs of stress arousal are interpreted as coping tools that facilitate challenge
appraisals to affect outcomes.
Although this chapter focuses on stress reappraisal interventions, we advocate for
an integrative approach—advancing research by synthesizing and consolidating exist-
ing models—to develop interventions to facilitate active coping. Rather than “discover-
ing islands” by creating new models, a potentially more efficacious approach suggested
herein may be building bridges by integrating processes, constructs, and themes across
multiple models (i.e., “connecting islands”). As highlighted in the “Cousins” section,
stress reappraisal and stress mindsets are ideal candidates for integration into a unified
stress optimization intervention. Such an intervention would have the potential to com-
bine domain-general benefits of mindset interventions with tools for optimizing coping
in specific stressful situations emphasized by stress reappraisal. These potentially additive
effects could be disseminated with little to no cost to recipients as well. Future research,
however, is needed to explicate basic effects of an integrated stress optimization interven-
tion, and subsequently, to maximize efficiency to scale the intervention for widespread
dissemination.
In sum, the research on stress reappraisal reviewed here contributes to a growing
corpus of evidence demonstrating the efficacy of brief, social-psychological interventions
Stress Reappraisal Interventions 255
for improving health and well-being outcomes (see Walton & Wilson, 2018, for a review).
As emphasized by Yeager and Walton (2011), however, these interventions are not
“magic” panaceas—rather, social-psychological interventions, such as stress reappraisal,
target specific processes to enact positive change. Ultimately, it is our hope that this and
other intervention approaches can be integrated, scaled, and disseminated to potentially
improve people’s lives at near zero cost.
REFERENCES
Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E., & Teasdale, J. D. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans:
Critique and reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 49–74.
Barrett, L. F. (2006). Are emotions natural kinds? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1(1),
28–58.
Barrett, L. F. (2017). The theory of constructed emotion: An active inference account of interocep-
tion and categorization. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(1), 1–23.
Barrett, L. F., Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., & Barsalou, L. W. (2014). A psychological construction
account of emotion regulation and dysregulation: The role of situated conceptualizations.
In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (2nd ed., pp. 447–465). New York:
Guilford Press.
Beltzer, M. L., Nock, M. K., Peters, B. J., & Jamieson, J. P. (2014). Rethinking butterflies: The
affective, physiological, and performance effects of reappraising arousal during social evalu-
ation. Emotion, 14, 761–768.
Blascovich, J. (1992). A biopsychosocial approach to arousal regulation. Journal of Social and
Clinical Psychology, 11(3), 213–237.
Blascovich, J. (2008). Challenge, threat, and health. In J. Y. Shah & W. L. Gardner (Eds.), Hand-
book of motivation science (pp. 481–493). New York: Guilford Press.
Blascovich, J., & Mendes, W. B. (2010). Social psychophysiology and embodiment. In S. T. Fiske,
D. T. Gilbert, & L. Gardner (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (pp. 194–227). Hoboken,
NJ: Wiley.
Blascovich, J., Mendes, W. B., Hunter, S. B., & Salomon, K. (1999). Social “facilitation” as chal-
lenge and threat. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 68–77.
Bradley, R. T., McCraty, R., Atkinson, M., Tomasino, D., Daugherty, A., & Arguelles, L. (2010).
Emotion self-regulation, psychophysiological coherence, and test anxiety: Results from an
experiment using electrophysiological measures. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeed-
back, 35(4), 261–283.
Brady, S. T., Hard, B. M., & Gross, J. J. (2018). Reappraising test anxiety increases academic per-
formance of first-year college students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 110(3), 395–406.
Brooks, A. W. (2014). Get excited: Reappraising pre-performance anxiety as excitement. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(3), 1144–1158.
Brownley, K. A., Hurwitz, B. E., & Schneiderman, N. (2000). Cardiovascular psychophysiology.
In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G., Tassinary, & G. G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology
(2nd ed., pp. 224–264). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Butler, E. A., & Randall, A. K. (2013). Emotional coregulation in close relationships. Emotion
Review, 5(2), 202–210.
Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. Journal
of Health and Social Behavior, 24, 385–396.
Croson, R., & Gneezy, U. (2009). Gender differences in preferences. Journal of Economic Litera-
ture, 47(2), 448–474.
Crum, A. J., Akinola, M., Martin, A., & Fath, S. (2017). The role of stress mindset in shaping
256 II. Health and Well-Being
cognitive, emotional, and physiological responses to challenging and threatening stress. Anx-
iety, Stress, and Coping, 30(4), 379–395.
Crum, A. J., Salovey, P., & Achor, S. (2013). Rethinking stress: The role of mindsets in determin-
ing the stress response. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(4), 716–733.
Crum, A. J., Santoro, E., Smith, E. N., Salovey, P., Achor, S., & Mooraveji, N. (2020). Rethink-
ing stress: Changing mindsets to harness the enhancing effects of stress. Manuscript under
review.
Dienstbier, R. A. (1989). Arousal and physiological toughness: Implications for mental and physi-
cal health. Psychological Review, 96(1), 84–100.
Dweck, C. S. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning. American Psychologist, 41(10),
1040–1048.
Elliot, A. J. (1999). Approach and avoidance motivation and achievement goals. Educational Psy-
chologist, 34(3), 169–189.
Elliot, A. J., & Fryer, J. W. (2008). The goal concept in psychology. In J. Shah & W. Gardner
(Eds.), Handbook of motivational science (pp. 235–250). New York: Guilford Press.
Goyer, J. P., Akinola, M., Grunberg, R., & Crum, A. J. (2018). Evaluation of a stress mindset
intervention to improve performance and wellbeing in underrepresented minority college
students at a selective institution. Manuscript in preparation.
Gross, J. J. (2002). Emotion regulation: Affective, cognitive, and social consequences. Psycho-
physiology, 39(3), 281–291.
Gross, J. J. (2015). The extended process model of emotion regulation: Elaborations, applications,
and future directions. Psychological Inquiry, 26(1), 130–137.
Gross, J. J., & Barrett, L. F. (2011). Emotion generation and emotion regulation: One or two
depends on your point of view. Emotion Review, 3(1), 8–16.
Hangen, E. J., Elliot, A. J., & Jamieson, J. P. (2019). Stress reappraisal during a mathematics
competition: Testing effects on cardiovascular approach-oriented states and exploring the
moderating role of gender. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 32(1), 95–108.
Jacquart, J., Papini, S., Freeman, Z., Bartholomew, J. B., & Smits, J. A. (2020). Using exercise
to facilitate arousal reappraisal and reduce stress reactivity: A randomized controlled trial.
Mental Health and Physical Activity, 18, 100324.
Jamieson, J. P. (2017). Challenge and threat appraisals. In A. J. Elliot, C. Dweck, & D. Yeager
(Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition (2nd ed., pp. 175–191). New York: Guilford
Press.
Jamieson, J. P., Black, A. E., Pelaia, L. E., Gravelding, H., Gordils, J., Altose, A. J., & Reis, H. R.
(2020). The effects of reappraising stress arousal in a naturalistic academic context. Manu-
script under review.
Jamieson, J. P., Crum, A. J., Goyer, J. P., Marotta, M. E., & Akinola, M. (2018). Optimizing stress
responses with reappraisal and mindset interventions: An integrated model. Anxiety, Stress,
and Coping, 31(3), 245–261.
Jamieson, J. P., Hangen, E. J., Lee, H. Y., & Yeager, D. S. (2018). Capitalizing on appraisal pro-
cesses to improve social stress responses. Emotion Review, 10, 30–39.
Jamieson, J. P., Koslov, K., Nock, M. K., & Mendes, W. B. (2013). Experiencing discrimination
increases risk taking. Psychological Science, 24(2), 131–139.
Jamieson, J. P., Mendes, W. B., Blackstock, E., & Schmader, T. (2010). Turning the knots in your
stomach into bows: Reappraising arousal improves performance on the GRE. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 46(1), 208–212.
Jamieson, J. P., Mendes, W. B., & Nock, M. K. (2013). Improving acute stress responses the power
of reappraisal. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 51–56.
Jamieson, J. P., Nock, M. K., & Mendes, W. B. (2012). Mind over matter: Reappraising arousal
improves cardiovascular and cognitive responses to stress. Journal of Experimental Psychol-
ogy: General, 141(3), 417.
Stress Reappraisal Interventions 257
Jamieson, J. P., Nock, M. K., & Mendes, W. B. (2013). Changing the conceptualization of stress
in social anxiety disorder affective and physiological consequences. Clinical Psychological
Science, 1(4), 363–374.
Jamieson, J. P., Peters, B. J., Greenwood, E. J., & Altose, A. J. (2016). Reappraising stress arousal
improves performance and reduces evaluation anxiety in classroom exam situations. Social
Psychological and Personality Science, 7(6), 579–587.
Jamieson, J. P., Valdesolo, P., & Peters, B. J. (2014). Sympathy for the devil?: The physiological
and psychological effects of being an agent (and target) of dissent during intragroup conflict.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 55, 221–227.
Jefferson, A. L., Himali, J. J., Beiser, A. S., Au, R., Massaro, J. M., Seshadri, S., . . . Manning,
W. J. (2010). Cardiac index is associated with brain aging: The Framingham Heart Study.
Circulation, 122, 690–697.
Jesus, S. N., & Lens, W. (2005). An integrated model for the study of teacher motivation. Applied
Psychology: An International Review, 54(1), 119–134.
John-Henderson, N. A., Rheinschmidt, M. L., & Mendoza-Denton, R. (2015). Cytokine responses
and math performance: The role of stereotype threat and anxiety reappraisals. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 56, 203–206.
Juster, R. P., McEwen, B. S., & Lupien, S. J. (2010). Allostatic load biomarkers of chronic stress
and impact on health and cognition. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(1), 2–16.
Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K. M., & Hellhammer, D. H. (1993). The “Trier Social Stress Test”—a
tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsycho-
biology, 28(1–2), 76–81.
Kray, L. J., Thompson, L., & Galinsky, A. (2001). Battle of the sexes: Gender stereotype confir-
mation and reactance in negotiations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(6),
942–958.
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Progress on a cognitive–motivational–relational theory of emotion. Ameri-
can Psychologist, 46(8), 819–834.
Lundberg, U. (2005). Stress hormones in health and illness: The roles of work and gender. Psycho-
neuroendocrinology, 30(10), 1017–1021.
Matthews, K. A., Gump, B. B., Block, D. R., & Allen, M. T. (1997). Does background stress
heighten or dampen children’s cardiovascular responses to acute stress? Psychosomatic Medi-
cine, 59(5), 488–496.
McGregor, H. A., & Elliot, A. J. (2002). Achievement goals as predictors of achievement-relevant
processes prior to task engagement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(2), 381–395.
Mendes, W. B. (2009). Assessing autonomic nervous system activity. In E. Harmon-Jones & J. S.
Beer (Eds.), Methods in social neuroscience (pp. 118–147). New York: Guilford Press.
Mendes, W. B., Gray, H. M., Mendoza-Denton, R., Major, B., & Epel, E. S. (2007). Why egali-
tarianism might be good for your health: Physiological thriving during stressful intergroup
encounters. Psychological Science, 18(11), 991–998.
Mendes, W. B., Major, B., McCoy, S., & Blascovich, J. (2008). How attributional ambiguity
shapes physiological and emotional responses to social rejection and acceptance. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 94(2), 278–291.
Mendes, W. B., & Park, J. (2014). Neurobiological concomitants of motivational states. In A. J.
Elliot (Ed.), Advances in motivation science (pp. 233–270). New York: Academic Press.
Moore, L. J., Vine, S. J., Wilson, M. R., & Freeman, P. (2015). Reappraising threat: How to opti-
mize performance under pressure. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 37(3), 339–343.
Niederle, M., & Vesterlund, L. (2007). Do women shy away from competition?: Do men compete
too much? Quarterly Journal of Economics, 122(3), 1067–1101.
Oveis, C., Gu, Y., Ocampo, J. M., Hangen, E. J., & Jamieson, J. P. (2020). Emotion regulation
contagion: Stress reappraisal promotes challenge responses in teammates. Journal of Experi-
mental Psychology: General. [Epub ahead of print]
258 II. Health and Well-Being
Peters, B. J., Overall, N. C., & Jamieson, J. P. (2014). Physiological and cognitive consequences of
suppressing and expressing emotion in dyadic interactions. International Journal of Psycho-
physiology, 94(1), 100–107.
Peters, B. J., Reis, H. T., & Jamieson, J. P. (2018). Cardiovascular indexes of threat impair respon-
siveness in situations of conflicting interests. International Journal of Psychophysiology,
123, 1–7.
Reis, H. T., Clark, M. S., & Holmes, J. G. (2004). Perceived partner responsiveness as an orga-
nizing construct in the study of closeness and intimacy. In D. J. Mashek & A. Aron (Eds.),
Handbook of closeness and intimacy (pp. 201–225). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Rozek, C. S., Ramirez, G., Fine, R. D., & Beilock, S. (2019). Reducing sociodemographic dispari-
ties in the STEM pipeline through student emotion regulation. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences of the USA, 116(5), 1553–1558.
Sammy, N., Anstiss, P. A., Moore, L. J., Freeman, P., Wilson, M. R., & Vine, S. J. (2017). The
effects of arousal reappraisal on stress responses, performance and attention. Anxiety, Stress,
and Coping, 30(6), 619–629.
Sbarra, D. A., & Hazan, C. (2008). Coregulation, dysregulation, self-regulation: An integrative
analysis and empirical agenda for understanding adult attachment, separation, loss, and
recovery. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 12, 141–167.
Schmader, T., Forbes, C. E., Zhang, S., & Mendes, W. B. (2009). A metacognitive perspective on
the cognitive deficits experienced in intellectually threatening environments. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 35(5), 584–596.
Seery, M. D., Weisbuch, M., & Blascovich, J. (2009). Something to gain, something to lose: The
cardiovascular consequences of outcome framing. International Journal of Psychophysiol-
ogy, 73(3), 308–312.
Steel, P. (2010). The procrastination equation: How to stop putting things off and start getting
stuff done. Toronto: Random House Canada.
Tugade, M. M., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). Resilient individuals use positive emotions to bounce
back from negative emotional experiences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
86(2), 320–333.
Walton, G. M., & Wilson, T. D. (2018). Wise interventions: Psychological remedies for social and
personal problems. Psychological Review, 125(5), 617–655.
Waters, S. F., West, T. V., Karnilowicz, H. R., & Mendes, W. B. (2017). Affect contagion between
mothers and infants: Examining valence and touch. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
General, 146(7), 1043–1051.
Williams, J. M. G., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional Stroop task and psycho-
pathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120(1), 3–24.
Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., Barrett, L. F., Simmons, W. K., & Barsalou, L. W. (2011). Grounding
emotion in situated conceptualization. Neuropsychologia, 49, 1105–1127.
Yeager, D. S., Lee, H. Y., & Jamieson, J. P. (2016). How to improve adolescent stress responses:
Insights from integrating implicit theories of personality and biopsychosocial models. Psy-
chological Science, 27(8), 1078–1091.
Yeager, D. S., & Walton, G. M. (2011). Social-psychological interventions in education: They’re
not magic. Review of Educational Research, 81(2), 267–301.
C H A P TE R 11
Values-Alignment Interventions
An Alternative to Pragmatic Appeals
for Behavior Change
Christopher J. Bryan
The science of behavior change has become an increasingly important frontier in the quest
to improve human health and well-being. Recognition of this is now widespread in the scien-
tific community, but the science of behavior change—the development of effective, empiri-
cally validated techniques for producing lasting, internalized motivation for the behavior
choices people know they should be making but usually do not—is still badly underdevel-
oped. Most public appeals to engage in such “should” behaviors (e.g., exercise, eat health-
ily, save for the future, conserve energy) focus on the pragmatic reasons why those behav-
iors are important. The problem with this approach is that such pragmatic appeals lack the
motivational immediacy to drive the needed changes in behavior for reasons psychologists
have understood for decades. Here, I suggest an alternative approach: reframing should
behaviors in terms that emphasize how those behaviors serve the values that are already
immediate and important to the people whose behavior one seeks to change. I demonstrate
the potential of this approach using the example of an intervention to get adolescents to
adopt healthier dietary habits by framing manipulative food marketing as a subversion of
important adolescent values, including autonomy from adult control and social justice.
BACKGROUND
Perhaps now more than ever before, human thriving depends on individual behavior
choices. To effectively address problems ranging from climate change to obesity, we must
find ways to foster sustained changes in behavior on a population scale. In particular,
we need good ways to create sustained motivation for “should” behaviors—the things
people know they should be doing but struggle to muster the motivation for. We see pub-
lic appeals for should behaviors every day; we are encouraged to get a flu shot, eat more
vegetables, exercise, conserve energy, and put our phones away while driving. And those
259
260 II. Health and Well-Being
appeals almost always focus on the pragmatic reasons for should behaviors; they empha-
size how those behaviors will make us healthier, safer, and more financially secure in the
future. This is not an effective approach. The big problem with it is that psychologists
(and many economists) have known for decades that people tend to overvalue immedi-
ate outcomes relative to those in the future (Ainslie & Haslam, 1992; Berns, Laibson, &
Loewenstein, 2007). So, if people see should behavior as the sacrifice of a reward in the
immediate term (e.g., enjoying dessert, having money to spend right now) for a reward
in the future (e.g., health, financial security), they are unlikely to be motivated to choose
them—even if they know they should. This is a problem because, in fact, should behav-
iors usually do require forgoing an immediate reward in favor of one in the future. So
what’s the alternative?
In this chapter, I describe a new approach to creating sustained, internalized motiva-
tion for should behaviors, called values alignment. Values alignment involves identifying
values that have motivational immediacy for people and that could plausibly be served
by the behavior one seeks to motivate, then framing appeals for the behavior in terms
of those values. In particular, values that are central to people’s (culturally defined) con-
ception of what makes someone deserving of acceptance, approval, or admiration from
the members of their important social reference groups tend to be powerful motivators
because seeing oneself as living up to those values is critical to self-integrity—the sense
that one is a worthwhile person (Leary & Baumeister, 2000; Sherman & Cohen, 2006;
Steele, 1988).
Although values alignment is a general approach to motivating should behaviors
in a wide range of domains, the theory largely grew out of work on a specific interven-
tion aimed at motivating adolescents to adopt healthier eating habits by framing them
as consistent with the important adolescent values of autonomy from adult control and
the pursuit of social justice (Bryan, Yeager, & Hinojosa, 2019; Bryan et al., 2016). That
intervention is the central focus of this chapter. My collaborators and I framed healthy
eating as rebellious and autonomy assertive by teaching adolescents about deceptive food
marketing designed to manipulate them and others into eating junk food. We framed
healthy eating as social justice-oriented behavior by emphasizing the ways in which food
marketers deliberately target vulnerable groups, like very young children and the poor.
Our goal was to portray healthy eating not as a way to be healthier in the future—a
pragmatic motive that lacks motivational immediacy—but rather as a way to be a “good
teenager” who stands up against unjust adult authority. We hypothesized that the reward
of knowing they were living up to the important values they share with their peers would
have the motivational immediacy to compete with the reward of eating junk food.
PSYCHOLOGICAL PROCESS
The psychological process behind values alignment relies on two basic psychological phe-
nomena: myopia and construal.
Berns et al., 2007; Frederick, Loewenstein, & O’Donoghue, 2002; Loewenstein, 1996;
Mischel & Staub, 1965; Thaler, 1981). Often referred to as myopia, this “pervasive deval-
uation of the future” (Ainslie & Haslam, 1992, p. 59), helps to explain why the long-term
benefits of should behaviors seem to provide such a weak incentive for people to make the
immediate sacrifices those behaviors typically require.
Compounding this challenge, motivation to delay gratification depends on people’s
confidence that the distant reward will eventually be achieved (Kidd, Palmeri, & Aslin,
2013; Mischel, Cantor, & Feldman, 1996; Mischel & Staub, 1965). And people generally
have enough experience with temptation to realize that even if they manage to delay grat-
ification in a given instance, the likelihood that they will be able to do so in a sustained
and consistent enough way to reap long-term benefits is quite low. These psychological
realities impose a persistent barrier to the effective motivation of should behaviors. The
values-alignment approach draws on one of social psychology’s earliest and most power-
ful insights to circumvent this barrier: the central role of construal in shaping people’s
experiences and decisions.
purpose of should behaviors as the pursuit of a pragmatic benefit in the distant future,
myopia necessarily undermines motivation for those behaviors: Most decisions about
should behavior pit an immediate hedonic reward (e.g., enjoying junk food) that people
are predisposed to overvalue against a distant pragmatic benefit (e.g., being healthy) that
people are predisposed to undervalue.
The aim of values alignment is to circumvent the myopia problem by shifting people
away from the pragmatic construal. When you see a should behavior as an expression of
your existing deeply held values, this introduces a new reason to perform it: the power-
ful eudaimonic reward that comes from feeling that you are living up to your important
values. And, critically, because this reward is mostly abstract and symbolic, it is experi-
enced immediately so its motivational power is not blunted by myopia (see Figure 11.1).
In addition to the immediate reward for choosing should behavior, the values-aligned
construal introduces a eudaimonic cost for succumbing to temptation and not choosing
should behavior. Because the should behavior is now construed as an expression of one’s
important values, not engaging in that behavior is now construed as inconsistent with
those values. So when a person fails to choose should behavior, she not only forgoes the
reward of living up to her values but must incur the cost of knowing that she is making a
choice that is inconsistent with those values.
are currently the single leading cause of premature death and disease in the developed
world (Forouzanfar et al., 2015; Gakidou et al., 2017).
The two values the intervention was designed to harness—autonomy from adult con-
trol and the pursuit of social justice—have been shown in past research to be especially
important to adolescents. The first will come as no surprise; adolescents are famously (or
infamously, depending on your perspective) rebellious against adult authority (Lee, Siegle,
Dahl, Hooley, & Silk, 2014; Vansteenkiste, Simons, Lens, Sheldon, & Deci, 2004). This
psychology likely hampers the success of many traditional health interventions, which
typically involve adults advising adolescents about what they should be eating for their
own good—just the sort of message that tends to provoke teens’ rebellious impulses. The
second is less widely appreciated but adolescence is a time when people tend to become
especially concerned about fairness, social justice, and beyond-the-self aims (Damon,
Menon, & Bronk, 2003; Robinson, 2010; Yeager et al., 2014). For the first time in their
lives, adolescents are beginning to feel like independent agents in the world and they want
to make a difference (Robinson, 2010; Yeager et al., 2014).
To connect to the autonomy value, we framed the intervention as an exposé of
manipulative food-industry marketing practices designed to inveigle people into consum-
ing more junk food than they would otherwise be inclined to consume. This includes
practices like using deceptive labeling to conceal the sugar content in foods or devoting
huge research budgets to discovering new ways to make junk foods maximally addictive
(Kessler, 2009; Moss, 2013). To connect to the social justice value, we emphasized the
ways in which food marketing contributes to injustice. This refers to practices like dispro-
portionately targeting vulnerable populations (e.g., very young children, the poor, and
minority groups) with marketing of the unhealthiest products (Kessler, 2009). Through
this framing, we sought to replace what we imagined was adolescents’ default construal
of healthy eating—something like “Healthy eaters are lame nerds who do what adults
tell them to do”—with an alternative construal: “Healthy eaters are independent-minded
people who are fighting to make the world a fairer place.” This construal, we theo-
rized, would provide adolescents with the immediate symbolic reward of feeling like they
were living up to the important values they share with their peers every time they made
a healthy dietary choice. Perhaps equally important, every time adolescents made an
unhealthy dietary choice, it would impose the immediate symbolic cost of knowing they
were not living up to their important values.
EMPIRICAL EVIDENCE
Outcomes
Using randomized, controlled field experimental methods, the values-alignment healthy-
eating intervention has been shown to improve adolescents’ dietary choices in naturalistic
school settings that they believed were unconnected to the research. The first test of its
effectiveness (Bryan et al., 2016) was conducted in a rural/suburban middle school in
Texas with the entire eighth-grade class in 2 consecutive years (N = 536). Participants
were randomly assigned, at the individual level, to read one of three short informational
articles: an exposé article that linked healthy dietary choices to adolescent values, an
active control article based on current best practices that used material from middle school
264 II. Health and Well-Being
health curricula and the U.S. government’s health education website and emphasized the
long-term health consequences of healthy and unhealthy diets, or a “no-treatment” con-
trol article that was similar to the other two in length but did not deal with food or diet.
After reading their respective articles, all students completed a series of survey questions
measuring the extent to which they construed healthy eating as relevant to the two target
adolescent values and some brief writing exercises to help them internalize the key mes-
sages of the article they read (Aronson, 1999).
The day after the intervention session we measured participants’ dietary choices in a
situation they believed to be unconnected with the research. This was possible thanks to
the school principal, who adapted his yearly practice of giving students a “snack pack”
each spring as a reward for their hard work in preparing for the statewide standardized
tests. To ensure students would not see the snack pack as part of the research, we asked
the principal to announce it to students in his normal fashion, long before the research
took place (and long before any student at the school knew about the study). Then, the
morning after the study, during homeroom period (which was a different class from
the one the intervention was administered in), the principal announced over the school
public address (PA) system that students would be receiving their snack packs that day.
Homeroom teachers distributed order forms and the principal explained that snack packs
containing the items they selected would be waiting for them at the main office at the
end of the school day. Students were invited to select two snacks and one drink. The
snack options included three healthy choices (baby carrots, fresh-cut fruit, and nuts with
dried fruit) and three unhealthy choices (cookies, potato chips, and cheese puffs). Drink
options included two healthy choices (bottled water and sparkling mineral water) and
three unhealthy choices (two sodas and a sweetened fruit drink).
Our primary measure of dietary choices was simply the number of unhealthy options
participants chose to include in their snack packs. We found that participants who
received the values-aligned exposé intervention ordered slightly but significantly fewer
unhealthy options (M = 1.38 unhealthy selections) than those who received either of the
control articles (M = 1.49 unhealthy selections).
There was no significant difference in the number of unhealthy choices participants
made after reading the traditional health education intervention (active control) versus
the article that was unrelated to diet or nutrition (no-treatment control). The lack of dif-
ference between these two control conditions is itself notable: even just 1 day later, we
found no evidence that traditional health and nutrition education had any effect on par-
ticipants’ dietary choices. Although this is not surprising in light of the psychological bar-
riers preventing people from choosing should behaviors under the traditional pragmatic
construal of that behavior, it is still a striking indictment of the approach that schools and
governments currently invest enormous resources in.
There are various ways to quantify the exposé intervention’s effect on participants’
snack pack choices and each provides a different and complementary perspective on the
intervention’s effects. For example, examining the percentage of participants in each
group who ordered snack packs that were entirely composed of unhealthy options sheds
light on how effectively the exposé intervention motivated participants to make at least
some sacrifice of their hedonic experience and take a step in the direction of healthier
diets. In the two control conditions, 57% of participants chose the maximum possible
number of unhealthy options (three). Adolescents who read the values-aligned exposé
Values-Alignment Interventions 265
article were substantially more willing to forgo a tempting, unhealthy treat in favor of
something healthier but much less hedonically rewarding: only 46% of them chose snack
packs that contained the maximum of three unhealthy options.
The reduction in the number of unhealthy choices among adolescents who received
the exposé intervention also translated into differences in the overall nutritional content
of the snack packs they ordered. While participants in the two control conditions ordered
snack packs with an average of 40.4 total grams of sugar and 60.2 total grams of car-
bohydrates, those who received the exposé treatment ordered items containing 3.6 fewer
grams of sugar and 4.3 fewer grams of carbohydrates, on average.
Although I have argued strongly that emphasizing the practical benefits of should
behaviors is an ineffective way to motivate those behaviors, practical outcomes are of
course still the most relevant criterion when evaluating an intervention from a policy
perspective. Although improving the nutritional content of a single meal or snack cannot
possibly have any meaningful impact on long-term health, it is possible to estimate what
the health significance would be of differences of the size observed in this experiment if
they were sustained over months and years. For example, a relative reduction in carbo-
hydrate consumption of the size observed in this study, if sustained, would translate into
roughly 1 pound of body fat lost (or not gained) every 6–8 weeks in adolescents of this
age.1 So can a values-alignment intervention produce lasting changes in adolescents’ daily
dietary choices? We examined this in a second experiment.
Effects over Time
Our first iteration of the values-aligned healthy-eating intervention provided “proof of
concept” for the values-alignment approach: It got adolescents to care enough about
healthy eating that they were willing to forgo rewarding junk food in favor of healthier but
less rewarding options 1 day later in a situation they believed was unrelated. To go beyond
that and produce lasting changes in adolescents’ daily dietary choices, we must overcome
some additional obstacles. Perhaps most daunting among those is that adolescents’ lives
are pervaded by junk food marketing. This marketing fosters strong positive emotional
associations with unhealthy food and drives overconsumption (Harris, Pomeranz, Lob-
stein, & Brownell, 2009; Hastings & Cairns, 2010; Kessler, 2009; Moss, 2013; Nestle,
2006; Swinburn et al., 2011). So, are the changes prompted by our relatively brief inter-
vention doomed to be wiped away by the barrage of pro-junk food messaging participants
are exposed to in their daily lives? Not necessarily. After all, the exposé intervention is
designed to change participants’ construal of junk food and of junk food marketing. If it is
effective at changing the meaning adolescents attach to the junk food marketing messages
they are exposed to, maybe it can neutralize their effect—or possibly even harness their
ubiquity to support the intervention’s message. Imagine if every time adolescents saw an
ad for junk food, it served as a reminder of unjust, controlling manipulation by the food
industry instead of acting as a tempting inducement to consume the product.
1 Because participants’ snack pack choices were limited to a small set of options, the size of the reduc-
tion in carbohydrate content must be considered an imprecise estimate of the reduction we might see if
participants were choosing from the universe of food and drink options available to them in their daily
lives.
266 II. Health and Well-Being
Mechanism
Shifting Construal of Healthy Eating
In the “Psychological Process” section of this chapter, above, I argued that values align-
ment motivates sustained increases in should behavior by changing people’s construal of
that behavior in ways that circumvent the barriers imposed by myopia. Was the exposé
intervention successful in doing this? In both field experiments I described, we measured
the extent to which participants in the treatment and control conditions construed healthy
eating as consistent with the values of autonomy and social justice. Participants were
asked, for example, how much they agreed with statements like “Eating healthy is a way
to stand up to people who are trying to control us” (autonomy) and “When I eat healthy,
I’m doing my part to protect kids who are being controlled by food companies” (social
justice). As expected, we found that participants in the exposé (values-aligned) condition
agreed more strongly with both the autonomy and social justice construals of healthy eat-
ing than those in either the no-treatment or the traditional health education control group.
Introduction of Symbolic Reward
I also argued that the construal of should behavior that is produced by values alignment
creates a reward for choosing should behavior that comes from knowing one is living up
to his or her core values. Because this reward is largely symbolic, it can be experienced
immediately—likely before the relevant behavior (e.g., healthy or unhealthy eating) even
happens in some cases, since the symbolic implications of a behavior can presumably be
experienced in an anticipatory form as soon as one decides to perform it. And because
this reward for choosing should behavior is experienced immediately, its power is not
undermined by myopia and it can compete with the hedonic rewards that would come
from eating rich, unhealthy foods. Essentially, this theoretical argument boils down to a
claim that values alignment makes should behavior feel more (immediately) rewarding.
It is difficult to measure a symbolic reward directly but we were able to obtain
data that provide indirect support for this hypothesis. We obtained saliva samples from
approximately half of the participants in the second intervention experiment a few weeks
before the intervention was administered and assayed them for testosterone (T) levels
(Medrano et al., 2019). Levels of T increase dramatically in adolescence (for boys and
girls) as a result of pubertal development and T is known to increase the brain’s sensitiv-
ity to reward (Braams et al., 2015). T has also been shown to motivate reward-seeking
behavior. Both correlational and experimental studies in animals and in humans have
shown that increased levels of T drive increases in behaviors that are expected to be
rewarding (Casey, 2015; van Duijvenvoorde, Peters, Braams, & Crone, 2016). So if val-
ues alignment motivates should behavior by making that behavior feel more immediately
rewarding, it should be most effective in people who experience reward most intensely.
It is worth noting that this is quite a bold prediction. Unhealthy food is also power-
fully rewarding, so people with higher levels of T should also be the people who are most
powerfully attracted to junk food under the traditional pragmatic construal of healthy
eating. Nevertheless, we found, as predicted, that the values-aligned exposé intervention
was most effective at motivating healthy eating among those with higher levels of T.
Because the effect of the intervention on daily cafeteria choices was limited to boys,
we focus on boys in this analysis as well. First, consistent with our assumption that,
Values-Alignment Interventions 269
under the traditional pragmatic construal, T should attract boys more strongly to the
hedonic reward of eating junk food, we found that T levels were positively associated
with junk food purchases among boys in the control condition. But the key test of the
theoretical process model is whether the exposé intervention was more effective at reduc-
ing junk food purchases among boys who were higher in T than among boys who were
lower in T. This prediction was also supported by the data; the exposé intervention was
significantly more effective among boys with higher levels of T (Medrano et al., 2019).
The difference was striking: while higher levels of T were associated with higher rates of
unhealthy, hedonically rewarding purchases among boys in the control condition, boys in
the exposé condition made relatively few daily junk food purchases irrespective of their
T levels (see Figure 11.3). Among boys in the exposé condition, although the hedonic
reward of junk food undoubtedly remained a powerful motivator, it was countered by a
powerful eudaimonic reward for making healthier choices.
0.3
0.2
0.1
0.0
FIGURE 11.3. Interaction of T × condition on boys’ junk food purchases. Values along the
y-axis represent fitted values with our covariates (age, date, and body mass index [BMI]). The
dashed vertical line indicates the region of significance (ROS) cutoff point: To the right of the line,
the control and expose conditions are significantly different.
270 II. Health and Well-Being
Heterogeneity
We have already seen two cases of heterogeneity in the effect of the exposé intervention:
its effectiveness at improving daily dietary choices (relative to traditional health educa-
tion) in boys but not in girls and among boys with high, but not low, levels of T. Each of
these instances of heterogeneity helped us learn something important, providing empiri-
cal support for the underlying theory in one case (T) and suggesting updates to our theory
in the other case (gender). This is an important lesson: We frequently think of treat-
ment effect heterogeneity in primarily practical terms—as “boundary conditions” that
set limits on how broadly the usefulness of an intervention generalizes. Although true,
this misses the point that identifying sources of heterogeneity in an intervention’s effect
is often a good way to develop and test the underlying theory about how the interven-
tion works and what adjustments might allow it to work more effectively, or in different
segments of the target population. The two sources of heterogeneity I have discussed so
far are individual differences, but another sort of heterogeneity—in context—can also
have important effects that are often equally useful in testing theory (Spencer, Zanna, &
2 Thispresentation duration is just barely above the threshold for conscious processing. Participants are
instructed not to let the initial image influence their evaluation of the neutral image but they can only
control influence of the initial image that they are consciously aware of.
Values-Alignment Interventions 271
FIGURE 11.4. Representation of a single AMP trial. The target object here is the Coke ad. Par-
ticipants are presented with it for 75 milliseconds, followed by a dark screen for 125 milliseconds,
then the neutral character they are asked to judge for 300 milliseconds before it is replaced with a
visual mask, which remains on screen until the participant responds by tapping either the U or the
P (to indicate whether they judge the neutral character to be unpleasant or pleasant, respectively,
relative to other such characters).
Fong, 2005). So far, we have not had the opportunity to study systematically how this
intervention might be affected by context but I describe some anecdotal evidence below
in the “Nuances and Misconceptions” section that suggests contextual variation is also
likely important for this intervention.
COUSINS
3 This belief may be caused at least in part by the misguided emphasis on pragmatic appeals for should
behaviors that I described at the start of this chapter. Emphasizing health as the primary reason to eat
foods like fruits and vegetables might inadvertently suggest to people that this is the only reason one
would want to eat those foods.
272 II. Health and Well-Being
concrete, hedonic reward of a pleasurable taste experience. One interesting question for
future research is whether these two approaches could be combined in a way that would
allow them to reinforce each other. For example, by emphasizing the appealing hedonic
properties of healthy foods, taste-focused labeling might make it easier for adolescents
to make dietary choices that are consistent with their values. Moreover, once the exposé
intervention successfully prompts adolescents to decide that they will make healthier
choices by harnessing their important values, adolescents might become more amenable
to beliefs (e.g., “Healthy foods are delicious too”) that support the wisdom of that deci-
sion (Kunda, 1990).
The exposé intervention to influence dietary choices is one instantiation of the
broader values-alignment approach to motivating should behaviors. Although the theo-
retical framework for values alignment that I have articulated here was developed in the
context of work on the exposé intervention for healthy eating, other interventions that
came before it have employed values alignment even if their designers did not conceive
of them explicitly in those terms. One example is an intervention to increase high school
students’ motivation for tedious schoolwork by linking education to a self-transcendent
prosocial purpose (Yeager et al., 2014). In this intervention, adolescents are encouraged
to think of a way in which they believe the world could be a better place (e.g., less hunger,
disease, or prejudice). Once they have identified a self-transcendent purpose they person-
ally care about, the intervention reframes the sometimes-boring work they are required
to do in high school as teaching them skills that they can use to make a positive contri-
bution to the world. Compared with control treatments that either provide useful advice
about navigating high school life or that emphasized self-interested motives for learning,
the self-transcendent-purpose intervention increased participants’ willingness to invest
effort in learning difficult material—even in the face of tempting alternatives like watch-
ing viral videos—and, in one study, produced a substantial improvement in students’
science and math grades. Like the values-aligned healthy-eating intervention, the self-
transcendent-purpose intervention works by shifting participants’ construal of a should
behavior—devoting energy to schoolwork students find boring—such that it is seen as
serving a deeply held value. The most obvious difference between the two interventions is
that the healthy-eating intervention identified specific values that we knew, from previous
research, tend to be important in adolescence. In contrast, the self-transcendent-purpose
intervention invites participants to identify their own specific value and then encourages
them to think about how their schoolwork might serve that value. According to our
theory, one advantage of the former approach is that harnessing a shared group value
enhances an intervention’s power by drawing on the desire people generally feel to fit
within their social groups (e.g., Asch, 1956). On the other hand, focusing on a value that
is tailored to each individual’s personal priorities might allow the intervention to tap into
values that participants are especially passionate about. More research is needed to test
these hypotheses and assess which contexts, behaviors, or types of people each approach
is best suited to.
Another example is a classic intervention by psychologist Milton Rokeach (1971) that
used values alignment to increase White undergraduates’ support for African Americans’
civil rights in the late 1960s by confronting them with inconsistencies in their own constel-
lation of values and attitudes. Rokeach had participants rank a list of 18 human values
(e.g., freedom, happiness, a world at peace, equality). He then showed them fabricated
data indicating (1) that students at their school, on average, ranked freedom much more
Values-Alignment Interventions 273
highly than equality; and (2) that students who were supportive of civil rights, on average,
ranked those two values almost identically, while students who were less supportive of
civil rights ranked freedom much more highly than equality. Rokeach then highlighted the
hypocrisy of the latter ranking, saying, “This raises the question as to whether those who
are against civil rights are really saying that they care a great deal about their own freedom
but are indifferent to other people’s freedom” (p. 454). Highlighting this hypocrisy had a
profound and lasting effect on students. Three to 5 months later, when they received a let-
ter inviting them to join the National Association for the Advancement of Colored People
(NAACP), they were substantially more likely to send in the $1 membership dues. In the
17 months following this intervention, participants also updated their ranking of the two
focal values, prioritizing freedom less and equality more. What deeply held and widely
shared value did this intervention harness to motivate these increases in support for civil
rights? Although the values of freedom and equality were central features of the interven-
tion, I would argue that neither was the critical driver of motivation—rather, I believe it
was the strong desire not to feel like or be seen as a hypocrite (see also Stone, Aronson,
Crain, Winslow, & Fried, 1994). Rokeach’s intervention caused a shift in how participants
construed the significance of prioritizing freedom so far above equality. Highlighting
the hypocrisy of this disparity caused participants to feel substantial discomfort, which
Rokeach then channeled to motivate support for civil rights.
Another class of related interventions uses a subtle linguistic cue to frame behavior
as having implications for identity. In a series of experiments, my collaborators and I have
shown how referring to should behaviors with a predicate noun instead of a verb (e.g.,
“be a voter” vs. “vote”; “don’t be a cheater” vs. “don’t cheat”; “be a helper” vs. “help”)
can cause people to behave more in accordance with their values—to exert the effort to
vote and to help others, and to resist the temptation to cheat (Bryan, Adams, & Monin,
2013; Bryan, Master, & Walton, 2014; Bryan, Walton, Rogers, & Dweck, 2011; Bryan,
Yeager, & O’Brien, 2019). Using nouns in this way frames should behavior as a reflec-
tion of the kind of person one would reveal oneself to be by performing it—it signals that
the behavior is reflective of a person’s essential, underlying identity (Gelman & Heyman,
1999; Gelman, Hollander, Star, & Heyman, 2000). By signaling that elements of one’s
identity hinge on a behavior choice, noun wording causes people to behave more in accor-
dance with the core values that constitute their (culturally informed) standards for what
constitutes a “good” or worthy person.
Context and Logistics
The values-aligned food marketing exposé intervention is delivered in a classroom setting
and introduced as material being considered for inclusion in future middle school health
curricula. Because it is critical not to threaten participants’ sense of autonomy, the pur-
pose of the classroom session is described in autonomy-supportive terms, as a request for
feedback from participants about whether the material will be seen as engaging to other
students of their age. The intervention consists of an informational article that describes
the core content (read on a tablet computer with narration through earphones), a pair of
brief writing exercises to reinforce the lessons of the article, and the Make It True activity,
274 II. Health and Well-Being
described above, in which participants modify images of real junk food ads to make them
more accurate.
Core Content
The content comes mostly from journalistic accounts of the ways in which food mar-
keters intentionally mislead and manipulate consumers into buying and eating more
unhealthy, processed foods than they otherwise would; the harm this does to people’s
health and well-being; and the ways in which that harmful influence is disproportionately
targeted at vulnerable populations, including very young children and the poor (e.g., Kes-
sler, 2009; Moss, 2013). The content is described in terms that make clear and obvious
the link to adolescents’ drive for autonomy from adult control. For example, see the fol-
lowing excerpt from the informational article:
The companies that make the unhealthy food spend billions of dollars trying to get
people to overeat. The reason is simple: the more people eat, the more money they make.
If people only ate when they were actually hungry—if corporations just let you choose
what you and your body wanted to eat—they would make a lot less money. So, instead
they spend lots of money to come up with new ways to trick our brains into eating more
than our bodies want, and more than our bodies can handle.
The direct reference to the counterfactual in this passage (“if corporations just let
you choose what you and your body wanted to eat . . . ”) helps cast food marketing in
clear opposition to adolescent autonomy. Tone and language are important here, too. The
use of simple, direct language (“just let you choose”; “trick our brains”) prevents read-
ers from feeling they are being talked down to, which could provoke autonomy threat
or defensiveness. The importance of making the reader feel respected is evident in the
following passage as well:
Why do the companies go after kids? Previous company executives have admitted that
it’s because they are easy targets. Children that young usually believe what adults tell
them. Corporations put ads on Saturday morning cartoons for the most sugar-packed,
addictive junk foods. They teach kids as young as 3 and 4—who don’t know any better—
that eating lots of junk food is cool, fun and normal.
The above excerpt is obviously intended to provoke a prosocial concern for social
justice but it accomplishes another goal, too. Focusing on the intended effects of food
marketing on very young children makes it possible to portray a belief likely held by
many of the readers “that eating lots of junk food is cool, fun and normal” as childish
and naïve without insulting the reader or provoking defensiveness.
We also sought to link the desire for the hedonic reward of junk food directly with
food marketers’ subversion of the reader’s autonomy. That was the purpose of the follow-
ing passage, for example:
Our brains are naturally designed to tell us to stop eating when we’ve had enough. But
they have laboratories and hire scientists to figure out the brain’s blind spots. Then they
create foods that cause the brain to crave more and more sugar and fat, even when we’re
not hungry.
Values-Alignment Interventions 275
This passage helps to externalize even the internal experience of craving junk food.
We worried that if participants construed their desire for junk food as coming from
within, they would feel compelled to justify the validity of those cravings (Aronson,
1969; Festinger, 1957; Festinger & Carlsmith, 1959). Externalizing those cravings makes
clear that they are threats to autonomy rather than genuine internal desires.
The article also contains examples of real food industry executives, all middle-age
White men, to serve as objects of the reader’s rebellion. These executives were featured
because, in addition to representing a controlling adult authority, they have all been iden-
tified as avoiding their own companies’ products for health reasons. One such example is
John Ruff who, the article explains, “is an executive at Kraft (maker of Kool-Aid, Tang,
Oreos, and lots of other high-sugar, high-fat snacks). He dropped high fat and high calo-
rie snacks to stay healthy. His diet calls for avoiding his company’s snacks. He continues
to sell them to others.” Mentioning this hypocrisy helped underscore the ways in which
junk food marketing directly undermines the values of autonomy and social justice. This
was expected to provoke righteous outrage and fuel participants’ motivation for behavior
change to defend those core adolescent values.
Supporting Psychology
As is common in many wise interventions, the core content of the exposé intervention
is complemented with components designed to evoke supportive psychological processes
(Walton, 2014; Walton & Wilson, 2018). To communicate a descriptive norm (Cialdini,
2003; Goldstein, Cialdini, & Griskevicius, 2008) supportive of the intervention’s core
message, participants read about a survey of the previous year’s ninth graders in which
“almost all” respondents were “shocked and angry” about food marketers’ manipulation
and planned to “fight back against the companies by eating less of their processed foods.”
Those survey results were followed by quotes from previous participants reiterating key
points from the article and explaining how they planned to fight back by avoiding junk
food.
Next, participants completed a pair of short writing activities in which they were
asked why they thought so many people were “outraged and upset” about food market-
ers’ manipulation, invited to share their own feelings on the topic, and finally to describe
what kinds of food choices “students like [them]” could make, starting right away, to
fight back against this injustice. These activities were designed to serve a self-persuasion
function—to help students internalize and feel ownership over the persuasive arguments
in the article (Aronson, 1999).
Make It True Exercise
The primary goal of the Make It True exercise was to highlight the role of food ads in
supporting the deception and manipulation of the food industry. The activity was framed
in terms that recalled the key messages we had communicated the day before:
“What is advertising? It’s a way for companies to sell their products (and make
money). A big problem is that junk food ads often target young kids and don’t tell
the whole truth. How? Companies use bright colors and cartoons to paint a fake pic-
ture about their food. And they put these ads everywhere—in children’s TV shows,
276 II. Health and Well-Being
schools, websites, and even phone apps. Surrounding little kids with these ads is a
big way that companies try to take over their power to think for themselves. But we
don’t have to let a big company do the thinking for them.”
Note again the emphasis on protecting young children from marketing, which simul-
taneously reinforces the alignment of our message with prosocial justice and allows us
to call out the food industry’s deception with less risk that we might provoke defen-
siveness by making participants themselves feel like dupes. Next, we provided examples
of marked-up ads that the previous year’s eighth graders had produced during piloting
to help inspire participants. To scaffold participants’ creative process, we also provided
thought questions (e.g., “When you pick an ad, ask yourself . . . does it seem false or like
it’s hiding part of the truth?”; “Does it seem like it’s trying to make junk food seem way
more fun than it really is?”) and suggested ways they might modify the ads (e.g., “Add
speech or thought bubbles”; “Cross out words to make a point”; “Do a play on words”;
and “Add a whole new slogan that shows the reality of the product”).
NUANCES AND MISCONCEPTIONS
clear that the exposé intervention as we had drafted it (and as we have tested it so far)
was well aligned with White, middle class adolescent values in particular. In relying on
the published research literature to characterize adolescent values, we likely fell prey to
a common and persistent misconception implicit in most psychological literature: the
characterization of “human” psychology from research that is based overwhelmingly
on samples of White, Western, relatively educated, middle class people (Henrich, Heine,
& Norenzayan, 2010; Sears, 1986). The social-psychological literature on social class
makes clear why an intervention designed to motivate action by triggering the desire
for personal autonomy might be less well aligned with the values of adolescents from
working-class backgrounds—namely, working-class culture tends to value individual
choice less than middle-class culture does, privileging interdependence among ingroup
members and attentiveness to the needs of others in one’s group (Stephens, Fryberg, &
Markus, 2011; Stephens, Hamedani, Markus, Bergsieker, & Eloul, 2009; Stephens,
Markus, & Townsend, 2007).
In addition to this major misalignment of the exposé intervention with the values
of lower-socioeconomic status (SES) students, the social justice component of the inter-
vention, which discusses the ways in which the food industry disproportionately targets
low-income communities, has a very different meaning to low-SES adolescents than it
does to middle-class adolescents. This material is designed to create a sense of prosocial
purpose for rejecting junk food in participants by emphasizing the ways in which groups
more vulnerable than participants themselves are being targeted. It is well suited to that
purpose when the audience is middle class; not so when the audience is the low-income
group being targeted by companies.
This analysis makes clear why the current version of the exposé intervention seemed
to resonate less strongly with the pilot participants from the low-SES school in the South
Bronx. It also suggests ways in which relatively minor modifications might help align
that intervention better with the values of lower-SES adolescents. If the emphasis were
on the ways in which the food industry targets very young members of low-income com-
munities and communities of color, the call to action in the intervention could be framed
less in terms of “sticking it to the man” or “taking back control” and more in terms of
the importance of being role models and “protecting the younger members of our com-
munity” from harmful outside influence. Pilot work and rigorous testing are needed to
be confident that such a modified framing would be effective in lower-SES communities
of color but, as a starting place, this framing certainly seems better aligned with values
in those communities.
Another nuance that is relevant to values-alignment interventions generally is worth
noting. I have emphasized that values alignment involves framing should behavior in
terms of values other than long-term pragmatic benefit. But this does not mean that the
pragmatic benefits of should behavior are made to seem irrelevant; the difference between
pragmatic and values-aligned appeals is about emphasis. The health implications of junk
food consumption, for example, are still important to the logic of the exposé interven-
tion but they are not emphasized as the sole or central reason to make healthier choices.
Instead, they help motivate why deceptive marketing undermines the values of autonomy
and social justice, which are the central focus of the intervention—that is, the food indus-
try’s manipulation of people’s choices and its targeting of vulnerable populations feel like
such violations of these values in part because greedy executives are profiting by under-
mining people’s well-being. Pragmatic concerns play a similar role in other values-aligned
278 II. Health and Well-Being
IMPLICATIONS FOR PRACTICE
Research on values alignment more generally and on the food marketing exposé in par-
ticular, is still at a relatively early stage and more research is needed before it is possible to
offer a comprehensive set of guidelines to practitioners. In the case of the food marketing
exposé intervention, one key principle is the importance of supporting the autonomy of
participants (Yeager, Dahl, & Dweck, 2018). The conceit we used to achieve this in the
studies—framing the content as draft material for future middle school health courses—
would not likely wear well as subsequent classes of students received the same material
year after year. But an alternative framing of the material, simply as facts that food
companies try to conceal but that participants have a right to know about, would likely
accomplish the same purpose and be more sustainable.
More broadly, as illustrated by the example of how the exposé intervention seemed
much less resonant for students of color in a low-SES school, the question of how to
identify the right values to focus on in a values-alignment intervention is an important
and difficult one. Especially in Western, educated populations, psychological literature
can sometimes inform answers to this question. But for many populations and in many
contexts, existing literature does not provide a sufficient basis for designing an interven-
tion. I believe a big part of the answer is that we must begin to incorporate qualitative
approaches, borrowed from ethnography, into the intervention-design process. As more
research is conducted on values alignment, a systematic, scalable process for this can be
developed and serve as a guide to researchers and practitioners. For now, I suggest the
guiding principle (which I have alluded to throughout this chapter) that values that are
central to a culture’s conception of what makes a “good” member of the social group,
worthy of respect and admiration from other group members (Leary & Baumeister,
2000), tend to be powerful motivators.
IMPLICATIONS FOR PSYCHOLOGICAL THEORY
In using construal processes to tackle challenges related to myopia, this work brings
together two important lines of theory that, historically, have mostly existed separately
from each other. The pervasiveness and stubborn consistency with which people discount
the value of the future and the negative consequences this tendency has for people’s health
and well-being has made it a major topic of research in psychology, economics, and related
fields. Research on ways to encourage more future-oriented behavior has been intense
and has resulted in numerous discoveries of methods for reducing the influence of myopic
Values-Alignment Interventions 279
FUTURE DIRECTIONS
The work I have described in this chapter constitutes an initial demonstration that values
alignment can generate enduring motivation for should behavior. Much more research is
needed to take this from a promising idea to a viable, scalable program. I have already
mentioned two high-priority directions for future research in earlier sections. First, to
better understand the reason for the gender difference in the effect of the exposé inter-
vention on cafeteria purchases in the second intervention study, a follow-up experiment
that includes both a traditional nutrition education control and a no-treatment control
is a high priority. Second, my theorizing about why the exposé intervention material
seemed to resonate less strongly with low-SES adolescents of color and how the interven-
tion could be amended to be effective in that population cries out for testing. Improving
dietary habits and health outcomes in the communities most affected by food marketing
should be a high priority.
280 II. Health and Well-Being
Another high-priority question is whether the effects of the food marketing exposé
intervention could be made even stronger by incorporating social components. For rea-
sons of methodological rigor, the intervention studies we have conducted to date have
used individual-level random assignment to experimental conditions, within classrooms.
Because roughly half of the participants in each classroom were in the exposé condi-
tion and the other half in the control condition, we had to take steps to prevent par-
ticipants from discussing the intervention material with one another (to prevent cross-
contamination). But an intervention like this one, which is predicated on linking dietary
choices to the important values adolescents share with one another, might have even
stronger and longer-lasting effects if participants felt like they were part of a social move-
ment to take a stand against the manipulation and injustice perpetrated by a heinous
group of adult authority figures (see Paluck, Shepherd, & Aronow, 2016). Testing this
would require a much larger scale than has been possible in experiments to date, with
enough schools to allow classroom, or preferably, school-level random assignment.
In addition, one constraint on improvement in the health profile of students’ cafete-
ria purchases in the last study was a lack of appealing healthy options. Combining the
food marketing exposé intervention with a program that made more appealing healthy
options available in the school cafeteria might result in an even greater improvement in
nutritional outcomes. Indeed, this suggestion could even be combined with the previous
one. The intervention could be amended to encourage students to lobby their school to
make healthier options available. This combination might have the dual benefits of intro-
ducing a sense of group cohesion around a constructive purpose (thus fostering stronger
commitment to the cause) and increasing students’ commitment to healthier diets because
they had to work hard to gain access to them (Aronson, 1969; Aronson & Mills, 1959).
And, of course, the values-alignment approach is not specific to autonomy and social
justice or to influencing dietary preferences and choices. It is a general theoretical frame-
work for fostering lasting, internalized motivation for a wide range of should behaviors.
For example, in a newer line of research with Ashley Whillans, a psychologist at Har-
vard and Liz Dunn, a psychologist at the University of British Columbia, I am explor-
ing whether values related to parenting can be harnessed to motivate charitable giving.
This work is predicated on the observation that most people can only afford to donate
amounts of money that feel trivial relative to the massive scope of the need and that this
may prevent many people from giving at all. So we are examining whether emphasizing
to parents of young children that giving can serve a purpose other than charitable impact
might motivate them to give more often. In particular, we frame giving as a way to create
a teachable moment for their children. We encourage parents to include their young chil-
dren in the giving process and to use this as an opportunity to teach their children about
the importance of caring about the needs of others. In a series of laboratory studies, we
have found that inviting parents to consider giving to a charitable cause along with their
children (vs. on their own while a researcher entertained their children) made them more
willing to give to the cause and to share their e-mail address with the charitable organiza-
tion for solicitation of future donations. We are currently conducting a field experiment,
in partnership with a large online charitable giving organization, to test whether a ver-
sion of this teachable-moment framing could motivate parents to make ongoing, monthly
donations. Parents who visit the site are encouraged to begin giving their young children
a “charitable allowance” and sit down with their children each month to discuss the dif-
ferent causes they could donate the money to.
Values-Alignment Interventions 281
CONCLUSION
In the coming decades, human thriving is likely to depend on finding ways to produce
consistent changes in the everyday behavior choices that are behind many of our most
daunting problems. Values alignment offers a way to produce lasting, internalized moti-
vation for such changes. It does this by circumventing the persistent psychological bar-
riers that often prevent people from making the daily choices they know they should be
making. Much more research will be needed before this approach can begin to offer scal-
able solutions to the many challenges humanity is faced with, but results so far are cause
for optimism that this goal can be achieved.
REFERENCES
Spencer, S. J., Zanna, M. P., & Fong, G. T. (2005). Establishing a causal chain: Why experiments
are often more effective than mediational analyses in examining psychological processes.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(6), 845–851.
Steele, C. M. (1988). The psychology of self-affirmation: Sustaining the integrity of the self. In L.
Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 21, pp. 261–302). New
York: Elsevier.
Steinberg, L. (2007). Risk taking in adolescence: New perspectives from brain and behavioral sci-
ence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(2), 55–59.
Stephens, N. M., Fryberg, S. A., & Markus, H. R. (2011). When choice does not equal freedom:
A sociocultural analysis of agency in working-class American contexts. Social Psychological
and Personality Science, 2(1), 33–41.
Stephens, N. M., Hamedani, M. G., Markus, H. R., Bergsieker, H. B., & Eloul, L. (2009). Why
did they “choose” to stay?: Perspectives of Hurricane Katrina observers and survivors. Psy-
chological Science, 20(7), 878–886.
Stephens, N. M., Markus, H. R., & Townsend, S. S. M. (2007). Choice as an act of meaning: The
case of social class. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(5), 814–830.
Stone, J., Aronson, E., Crain, A. L., Winslow, M. P., & Fried, C. B. (1994). Inducing hypocrisy
as a means of encouraging young adults to use condoms. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 20(1), 116–128.
Strahan, E. J., Lafrance, A., Wilson, A. E., Ethier, N., Spencer, S. J., & Zanna, M. P. (2008).
Victoria’s dirty secret: How sociocultural norms influence adolescent girls and women. Per-
sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(2), 288–301.
Swinburn, B. A., Sacks, G., Hall, K. D., McPherson, K., Finegood, D. T., Moodie, M. L., &
Gortmaker, S. L. (2011). The global obesity pandemic: Shaped by global drivers and local
environments. Lancet, 378(9793), 804–814.
Thaler, R. H. (1981). Some empirical evidence on dynamic inconsistency. Economics Letters, 8(3),
201–207.
Thaler, R. H., & Benartzi, S. (2004). Save more tomorrow: Using behavioral economics to increase
employee saving. Journal of Political Economy, 112(S1), S164–S187.
Thompson, J. K., Heinberg, L. J., Altabe, M., & Tantleff-Dunn, S. (1999). Exacting beauty:
Theory, assessment, and treatment of body image disturbance. Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Turnwald, B. P., Bertoldo, J. D., Perry, M. A., Policastro, P., Timmons, M., Bosso, C., . . . Crum,
A. J. (2019). Increasing vegetable intake by emphasizing tasty and enjoyable attributes: A
randomized controlled multi-site intervention for taste-focused labeling. Psychological Sci-
ence, 30(11), 1603–1615.
van Duijvenvoorde, A. C. K., Peters, S., Braams, B. R., & Crone, E. A. (2016). What motivates
adolescents?: Neural responses to rewards and their influence on adolescents’ risk taking,
learning, and cognitive control. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 70, 135–147.
Vansteenkiste, M., Simons, J., Lens, W., Sheldon, K. M., & Deci, E. L. (2004). Motivating learn-
ing, performance, and persistence: The synergistic effects of intrinsic goal contents and
autonomy-supportive contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(2), 246–
260.
Walton, G. M. (2014). The new science of wise psychological interventions. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 23(1), 73–82.
Walton, G. M., & Wilson, T. D. (2018). Wise interventions: Psychological remedies for social and
personal problems. Psychological Review, 125(5), 617–655.
Weber, E. U., Johnson, E. J., Milch, K. F., Chang, H., Brodscholl, J. C., & Goldstein, D. G. (2007).
Asymmetric discounting in intertemporal choice: A query–theory account. Psychological Sci-
ence, 18(6), 516–523.
Values-Alignment Interventions 285
Yeager, D. S., Dahl, R. E., & Dweck, C. S. (2018). Why interventions to influence adolescent
behavior often fail but could succeed. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(1), 101–122.
Yeager, D. S., Henderson, M. D., Paunesku, D., Walton, G. M., D’Mello, S., Spitzer, B. J., & Duck-
worth, A. L. (2014). Boring but important: A self-transcendent purpose for learning fosters
academic self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(4), 559–580.
Yeager, D. S., & Walton, G. M. (2011). Social-psychological interventions in education: They’re
not magic. Review of Educational Research, 81(2), 267–301.
C H A P TE R 12
The Taste‑Focused‑Labeling
Intervention
Emphasizing the Tasty
and Enjoyable Attributes of Healthy Foods
Bradley P. Turnwald and Alia J. Crum
Rising rates of obesity, diabetes, and cardiovascular disease call for interventions that
promote healthier dietary intake. While most interventions emphasize the long-term impor-
tance of eating healthily and highlight the nutritional benefits of healthy options, these
health-focused approaches have had limited success. Here we discuss a novel interven-
tion that focuses instead on promoting healthy foods based on their tasty and enjoyable
characteristics—qualities that most people prioritize over healthiness in the moment of food
choice. Highlighting tasty and enjoyable properties changes people’s reason for choos-
ing healthy foods from a choice that is perceived as effortful, instrumental, and misaligned
with taste priorities to a choice that is aligned with the short-term goal of choosing tasty
and enjoyable food. Compared to the classic approach of emphasizing health qualities and
benefits, taste-focused labeling increases the proportion of people who choose healthy
foods across a range of field settings and for a variety of vegetables and plant-based foods.
In this chapter, we describe (1) theoretical foundations that led to the formulation of the
taste-focused-labeling intervention, (2) experimental evidence that it increases healthy food
choices, (3) mechanisms and moderators of the effects, (4) how to implement the taste-
focused-labeling intervention, and (5) directions for future research.
BACKGROUND
Poor dietary intake (diet low in fruits and vegetables and high in sugar, sodium, and
saturated fat) is a major driver of chronic disease (Lim et al., 2013). An estimated 45%
of deaths associated with heart disease, stroke, and type 2 diabetes can be attributed
286
The Taste‑Focused‑Labeling Intervention 287
to unhealthy eating (Micha et al., 2017), and nearly half of Americans’ dietary intake
qualifies as “poor” (Rehm, Penalvo, Afshin, & Mozaffarian, 2016). Improving the health
qualities of the foods that Americans choose to eat is a public health priority.
To promote healthier eating, three major approaches have been emphasized: (1) edu-
cating individuals so that they understand what constitutes a healthy diet and the impor-
tance of consuming a healthy diet (e.g., nutrition education programs), (2) using nutri-
tional labeling (e.g., calorie labeling, traffic light labeling, warning labels) so that people
can more easily identify healthy choices, and (3) changing aspects of the food choice
environment to make healthy choices more accessible or likely (e.g., access to healthy
foods in disadvantaged communities, monetary incentives, nudging, and choice architec-
ture). Despite each of these approaches improving attitudes, intentions, or behaviors in
some studies or for some populations, substantial progress on improving dietary intake
or slowing obesity rates has not been made (Gortmaker et al., 2011; Hawkes et al., 2015;
Mann, Tomiyama, & Ward, 2015; Roberto et al., 2015).
One potential explanation for the limited success of many health-focused approaches
is a shared underlying feature: reliance on a fundamental assumption that most people
will prioritize healthiness above other motives in the moment of food choice if they can
simply identify what the healthy choice is. This seemingly logical assumption is, more
often than not, flawed. People certainly must have access to healthy foods, but large
national surveys across decades show that most Americans prioritize tastiness more than
healthiness when making food choices (Aggarwal, Rehm, Monsivais, & Drewnowski,
2016; Glanz, Basil, Maibach, Goldberg, & Snyder, 1998). Making matters worse, many
Americans perceive healthy foods as less tasty (Raghunathan, Naylor, & Hoyer, 2006)
and less filling (Suher, Raghunathan, & Hoyer, 2016) compared with unhealthy foods.
Given such preferences and expectations, it is not surprising that in spite of improved
education, access, and nutritional labeling, people forgo healthy options and opt instead
for unhealthier foods that they expect will be tastier and more satisfying.
Are healthy foods objectively less tasty, filling, and appealing than unhealthier
foods? The notion that healthy foods are not tasty is not an objective fact but rather a
mindset—a core assumption about the nature and processes about something (in this
case, healthy foods) that is culturally developed and psychologically informed, and
orients people toward a particular set of expectations, associations, and attributions
(Crum, Salovey, & Achor, 2013; Dweck, 2008). Research shows that experiences with
food are shaped by prior experiences, information, and culture. First, food preferences
are learned, change over time, and improve with repeated exposure (Aldridge, Dovey,
& Halford, 2009; Birch, 1998; Wardle & Cooke, 2008). Second, foods can be experi-
enced as more or less tasty, filling, physiologically satiating, and neurologically reward-
ing depending upon how they are labeled or framed. Specifically, when the same food is
labeled as healthy (vs. not), people experience it as less tasty (Fenko, Kersten, & Bialkova,
2016; Lähteenmäki et al., 2010; Raghunathan et al., 2006), less filling (Finkelstein &
Fishbach, 2010; Suher et al., 2016), less appealing (Fenko et al., 2016; Lähteenmäki et
al., 2010), less physiologically satiating, (Crum, Corbin, Brownell, & Salovey, 2011),
and less neurologically rewarding (Veldhuizen, Nachtigal, Flammer, de Araujo, & Small
2013). Third, culture, rather than objective nutrients alone, influences the foods that
people experience as most pleasing. While Americans believe that the healthier the food,
the less tasty (Raghunathan et al., 2006), Indian populations endorse this mindset to a
288 II. Health and Well-Being
lesser degree (Dube, Fatemi, Lu, & Hertzer, 2016), and the French endorse the opposite
mindset—that the healthier the food, the tastier it is (Werle, Trendel, & Ardito, 2013).
Together, this body of research shows that, like other mindsets, while the mindset that
healthy foods are not tasty or enjoyable is not necessarily true or false, it has an impact.
By orienting people to a particular set of associations, expectations, and motivational
responses, the mindset that healthy food is not tasty or satisfying can create the reality
that is implied.
If not from objective qualities of the food itself, how are these mindsets formed?
Negative mindsets about healthy foods are, in part, rooted in and reinforced by American
cultural messages. For example, national fruit and vegetable promotion campaigns (e.g.,
“5 A Day,” “Fruits and Veggies: More Matters”) place more emphasis on the importance
of eating healthy foods for fighting disease than on tasty and enjoyable attributes (Marty,
Chambaron, Nicklaus, & Monnery-Patris, 2018; Pettigrew, 2016). In contrast, unhealthy
foods are promoted in more appealing ways than healthier foods on restaurant menus
(Turnwald, Jurafsky, Conner, & Crum, 2017; Turnwald, Anderson, Jurafsky, & Crum,
in press), through celebrity endorsements (Bragg, Miller, Elizee, Dighe, & Elbel, 2016),
popular character endorsements (Kelly et al., 2010), television advertisements (Harris,
Bargh, & Brownell, 2009; Kelly et al., 2010), and major sports sponsorships (Bragg et al.,
2018). Emerging work demonstrates a systematic bias in how healthy versus unhealthy
foods are portrayed even in nonadvertisement contexts. Unhealthy foods are systemati-
cally portrayed with less exciting, indulgent, social, and overall appealing language than
healthy foods across America’s top-selling movies, television shows, social media outlets,
and food reviews (Turnwald et al., 2020). These social, marketing, and informational
influences in American culture shape and reinforce the mindset that healthy foods are
not tasty or enjoyable.
In this chapter, we present the taste-focused-labeling intervention. In contrast to tra-
ditional health-focused approaches, taste-focused-labeling challenges negative mindsets
about healthy foods by highlighting the tasty and enjoyable characteristics of healthy
dishes. This intervention is designed to not only align healthier foods with diners’ taste
motives in the moment of food choice but to also improve people’s mindsets about healthy
foods as a general category by changing the traditional health-focused lens through which
they think about them. In the following sections, we describe the psychological processes
involved, experimental evidence, mechanisms and moderators, implementation instruc-
tions, and questions for future research.
PSYCHOLOGICAL PROCESSES
Taste-focused labeling is a wise intervention (Walton & Wilson, 2018) that works to
improve both people’s momentary expectation of how tasty and enjoyable a specific
healthy dish will be and to broadly improve people’s mindsets about all healthy foods.
Figure 12.1 summarizes the hypothesized psychological processes through which the
taste-focused-labeling intervention operates.
Describing the solid line paths from left to right, promoting the tasty and enjoy-
able attributes of healthy foods enhances people’s expectations of how delicious the food
will taste (Liem, Aydin, & Zandstra, 2012; Turnwald et al., 2019). Since most people
The Taste‑Focused‑Labeling Intervention 289
FIGURE 12.1. Conceptual model of how taste-focused labeling works. Solid arrows represent
paths that are empirically tested. Dashed arrows represent hypothesized paths.
prioritize tastiness when making food decisions, increased expected tastiness of the dish
leads more people to choose it.1 It also shifts people’s reason for choosing a healthy food
toward choosing for a tasty experience rather than for long-term health benefits, thereby
aligning a healthy choice more closely with taste priorities. Since the subjective experi-
ence of taste is a combination of a food’s qualities and one’s expectations, expecting that
a food will taste better can enhance the actual experienced taste of foods (Maimaran &
Fishbach, 2014; Raghunathan et al., 2006; Turnwald & Crum, 2019). For this specific
healthy food with a taste-focused label, a positive taste experience makes consumption
of that same healthy food with a taste-focused label on repeated occasions more likely
because people have learned that it tastes good. Moreover, a positive taste experience
with this healthy food (labeled as taste focused) has the potential to influence mindsets
that healthy foods, as a general category, can be tasty and enjoyable. This is because a
positive taste experience with a healthy food can challenge the mindset that healthy foods
are not tasty or satisfying by providing a personally experienced counterexample.
Within this process, there are two important questions for future research, as we
later describe and as depicted in the model with dashed lines. First, does a positive mind-
set shift transfer to a person’s subsequent experiences with the same healthy foods and
other healthy foods (with and without taste-focused labels)? An improvement in the
mindset that healthy foods can be tasty could increase a person’s expectations about
how tasty healthy foods in new environments are and increase the likelihood of choos-
ing healthy foods for taste reasons in new environments, particularly if those foods also
have taste-focused labels. Second, does a positive mindset shift occur only after a positive
taste experience or can exposure to the label alone shift mindsets about how tasty healthy
foods can be?
1 In the “Heterogeneity” section, we address moderators in this model, including one’s prior experiences
with a particular dish or setting, the extent to which one prioritizes tastiness more than healthiness, and
how flavorfully dishes are prepared.
290 II. Health and Well-Being
EMPIRICAL EVIDENCE
Three experiments and one multisite replication experiment tested whether labeling
could change food choices in real-world contexts. Table 12.1 presents a summary of these
experiments and key results.
The first experiment tested whether more students in a university dining hall sam-
pled a small bowl of raw vegetables with dipping sauce when labeled and promoted
as taste focused versus health focused. On each of two test days, two cafeteria staff
members prompted each diner (total N = 1,116) upon entry to try a serving of mixed
vegetables (carrots, jicama, and green beans) with miso dipping sauce. On one day, the
vegetable dish was labeled as “Fiber-Packed Vegetables with Nutritional Miso Sauce”
and verbally advertised as “healthy,” “nutritious,” and “good for you” by staff. On the
other day, the same dish was labeled with a taste-focused description (“Crispy Veggie
Straws with Decadent Miso Dip”) and verbally promoted as “delicious” and “tasty” by
staff. We found that 49% more diners chose the vegetables when labeled as taste focused
compared to health focused (Turnwald & Crum, 2019).
The second experiment expanded to a lunch-buffet setting in which healthy foods
(salad, vegetable wrap) competed among other food choices to test the hypothesis. Par-
ticipants (N = 202) walked through one of two lunch-buffet lines and had their choice
of salad, quinoa, vegetable wrap, turkey or steak sandwich, and dessert. On one serv-
ing line, the salad and vegetable wrap were labeled as health focused (“Light n’ Healthy
Salad,” “Healthy Choice Vegetable Wrap”), and on the other as taste focused (“Indulgent
Creations Deluxe Salad,” “Mouthwatering Grilled Vegetable Wrap”). Other items were
given the same, nondescriptive labels on both lines, and labels were not visible to diners
Note. All studies were conducted at lunchtime. N represents the total number of diner choices observed in each study.
Duration represents the number of days for which data were collected in the study, and % increase represents the
percent increase in the outcome (proportion of diners choosing healthy foods, mass of food chosen, or mass of food
consumed) when labeled taste focused versus health focused.
The Taste‑Focused‑Labeling Intervention 291
prior to selecting a buffet line. We observed that 18% more participants chose salad and
84% more participants chose the vegetable wrap when labeled as taste focused compared
to health focused (Turnwald & Crum, 2019).
These two experiments showed that, on a single occasion in a naturalistic field set-
ting, a greater proportion of diners chose various healthy foods when labeled as taste
focused than as health focused. However, these results do not inform whether this inter-
vention has lasting effects, whether food choices revert to baseline on repeated exposure
due to novelty effects, or whether they might reverse due to potential backfire effects. To
drive improvements in dietary intake and health, this intervention must sustain healthy
food choices over the long term. Therefore, we scaled the intervention to test the effect of
repeated exposure to taste-focused and health-focused labeling over the course of several
months.
The third experiment assessed whether taste- focused labeling impacted healthy
food choices in a naturalistic field setting on repeated exposures over a 10-week period
with a large sample (Turnwald, Boles, & Crum, 2017). We also tested whether the type
of healthy language impacted choice (restriction-focused healthy language vs. positive-
focused healthy language). Across 46 days of data collection in a university dining hall in
which 27,933 diner food choices were observed, taste-focused labeling (e.g., “Dynamite
Chili and Lime-Seasoned Beets”) increased the proportion of diners choosing vegetables
by 25% compared to basic labeling (e.g., “Beets”), 35% compared to healthy-positive
labeling (e.g., “Vitamin-Packed Beets”), and 41% compared to healthy-restrictive label-
ing (e.g., “Low-Carb Beets”). The healthy-positive and healthy-restrictive labeling did not
differ from each other, demonstrating that a focus on healthiness decreased choice even
when the health-focused language was positive. Additionally, we measured the weight of
vegetables that diners chose each day, and found that, similarly, a 16–33% greater mass
of vegetables was chosen on days that vegetables were labeled as taste focused compared
to basic, healthy-positive, and healthy-restrictive labels. Importantly, the benefits of taste-
focused labeling were stable over time. There was no label condition × time interaction on
the proportion of diners selecting vegetables or the mass of vegetables selected for taste-
focused labeling compared to any of the other three labeling types.
In a fourth experiment, we replicated the taste-focused-labeling intervention in five
universities across the United States to test whether taste-focused labeling was scalable,
whether the effects would replicate, and to test for heterogeneity (Turnwald et al., 2019).
Each day, the five university dining halls labeled their main-course vegetable as taste
focused, basic, or health focused, and measured the number of diners who chose the vege-
table. Results showed that across 137,842 diners, 185 days, and 71 different preparations
involving 24 different types of vegetables, taste-focused labeling increased the proportion
of diners choosing vegetables by 29%. Once again, the beneficial effect of taste-focused
labeling compared to health-focused labeling on vegetable selection did not change over
time at any of the five research sites.
At one of the sites in this experiment, we also measured how much of the vegetables
people were actually eating, as it is possible that people were taking more vegetables
when labeled as taste focused but then throwing them away. To measure the total mass
of vegetables that diners served themselves, we weighed the serving dishes each time
they were filled throughout the lunch period. We then collected each diner’s plate waste
at the entrance to the dining hall waste rooms and scraped off any of the remaining
daily vegetable on their plate into a separate trash pail. Since we removed all other trash
292 II. Health and Well-Being
pails from the dining hall, the total mass of vegetables selected minus the total mass
of vegetables wasted represented the mass of vegetables that diners actually consumed.
Using these methods on each of 16 days revealed that taste-focused labeling significantly
increased actual consumption of vegetables by 39% per day, on average. This effect on
actual vegetable consumption was also stable over time. Together, these studies provide
strong evidence that taste-focused labeling increases people’s choice of a large variety of
healthy foods (e.g., raw vegetables, hot vegetable dishes, vegetarian wraps, salads) across
a variety of institutional field settings (tasting tables, buffet lines, and dining halls).
How does taste-focused labeling work? The first part of our theory (solid black paths
in Figure 12.1) is that taste-focused labeling increases diners’ expectations of having a
positive taste experience with a particular healthy food, which increases the likelihood
that they will choose that food for taste reasons and have a positive taste experience.
Although many of the field studies measured behavior and not psychological processes,
several follow-up studies provide evidence in support of this hypothesis. In two indepen-
dent preregistered experiments with online participant samples, taste-focused labels sig-
nificantly increased participants’ expectations of a positive taste experience compared to
basic and health-focused labels (Turnwald et al., 2019). Furthermore, in all samples, the
effect of taste-focused labeling on increased likelihood of choosing was fully mediated
by increased expectations of a positive taste experience. Evidence from our longitudinal
behavior experiments (see the “Empirical Evidence” section Study 3 and multisite replica-
tion) also supports this first part of the mechanism. The effects of taste-focused labeling
compared to health-focused labeling on vegetable selection did not diminish over time
when the same group of diners were exposed to different healthy dishes with different
taste-focused labels over the course of 10–12 weeks, suggesting that this process may
occur with each new exposure to a healthy food with a taste-focused label.
We also found that taste-focused labels change the reason why people say they would
choose a vegetable dish. When participants in an online study were asked why they would
choose a variety of vegetable dishes (on a scale of 1 = For health benefits to 5 = For a
tasty experience), they indicated that it would be primarily for health benefits when these
vegetable dishes had basic labels (e.g., “Broccoli,” “Carrots”). When these same dishes
were presented with health-focused labels, participants were even more likely to indicate
that they would choose these dishes primarily for health benefits. However, when we pre-
sented these same dishes with taste-focused labels, participants shifted their reason to be
primarily for a tasty experience (above the midpoint of the scale; Turnwald et al., 2019).
This provides evidence that participants view healthy foods with taste-focused labels as
being less in conflict with their taste goals.
To confirm that taste-focused labeling can enhance the actual taste experience of
consuming healthy foods, we asked participants (N = 203) in a large university dining
hall to taste green beans labeled as health focused (“Light n’ Low-Carb Green Beans and
Shallots”) or taste focused (“Sweet Sizzlin’ Green Beans and Crispy Shallots”). Com-
pared to individuals who tasted green beans with the health-focused label, those who
tasted green beans with the taste-focused label rated them as more delicious. They also
endorsed the mindset that “in general, healthy foods taste delicious” postconsumption to
The Taste‑Focused‑Labeling Intervention 293
a greater extent than individuals who ate the same green beans labeled as health focused
(Turnwald & Crum, 2019).
These data preliminarily speak to the effect that choosing a healthy food with a taste-
focused label can lead to a more positive taste experience and improve mindsets about
the tastiness of healthy foods as a category. Yet more work is needed to understand this
process. Does the improved mindset result from merely seeing healthy foods with taste-
focused labels, or can this change be cemented only after having a positive taste experience
with a tastily labeled healthy food? Does this mindset improvement increase how tasty a
person expects other healthy foods to be (with and without taste-focused labels) and, if
so, for how long do the effects last? Future work is needed to understand these questions.
HETEROGENEITY
Our research to date suggests at least three important moderating factors of the taste-
focused-labeling intervention: (1) how deliciously the foods are prepared, (2) a person’s
priorities when making food decisions, and (3) a person’s cultural background.
First, taste-focused labeling is more effective in settings that serve tastier vegetable
dishes. Taste is an interaction between its qualities and expectations and thus both matter.
Prior lab research (Woolley & Fishbach, 2016) showed that priming taste goals increased
participants’ consumption of a raw vegetable compared to priming health goals, but this
effect was moderated by how tasty the food was (e.g., apple vs. raw spinach). Our mul-
tisite intervention extended these findings to field settings by showing that taste-focused
labeling was more effective at schools that served tastier vegetable recipes on average than
schools that served more bland preparations (Turnwald et al., 2019). Moderation at the
level of setting occurs because one’s belief regarding whether this particular distributor
or setting generally serves tasty food influences how credible the taste-focused label is in
this setting. Further downstream in the model depicted in Figure 12.1, once a diner has
chosen the dish, how flavorfully this dish is prepared may moderate the extent to which a
positive taste experience occurs because the actual experienced taste must meet expecta-
tions set by the label. Backfire effects could occur in cases where blandly prepared veg-
etables do not meet expectations, disappointing diners and confirming existing negative
mindsets that healthy foods are not tasty.
A second factor that influences the efficacy of taste-focused labeling is the extent to
which a person prioritizes tastiness and healthiness when making food decisions. Many
Americans prioritize tastiness over healthiness (Glanz et al., 1998; Aggarwal et al., 2016).
Yet a relatively small proportion of people, such as dieters and restrictive eaters, do pri-
oritize healthiness in the moment of food choice and may be more attracted to health-
focused labels because they affirm intentions to choose something that is, above all else,
healthy (Irmak, Vallen, & Robinson, 2011; Papies & Veling, 2013). In an online sample
of participants (N = 277), we found that those who prioritized healthiness to a greater
extent were more likely to say that they would order foods with health-focused language
than those who prioritized healthiness to a lesser extent, consistent with this moderation
hypothesis and with prior studies on health-focused-labeling effects on dieters (Irmak et
al., 2011; Papies & Veling, 2013).
Third, other studies suggest that culture and geography likely influence the effects
of health-focused and taste-focused labeling. The model depicted in Figure 12.1 assumes
294 II. Health and Well-Being
an American context. While the mindset that healthy is not tasty or filling was observed
in American contexts (Raghunathan et al., 2006; Suher et al., 2016), other cultures do
not endorse this mindset to the same degree (Dube et al., 2016; Werle et al., 2013). For
any group that already holds positive mindsets about the tastiness of healthy foods, the
theory would predict that taste-focused labeling would have a smaller effect because
there is little room for improvement in the mindset. However, even for people who have
the general mindset that healthy foods are tasty, taste-focused labeling may still help in
cases of individual foods that they do not like (e.g., for someone who has the mindset that
healthy foods, in general, are tasty except for asparagus). While this list is not exhaustive,
these moderators are important to consider when implementing taste-focused labeling for
various populations and settings.
COUSINS
(Hulleman & Harackiewicz, 2009). Yeager and colleagues showed that identifying an
important social problem, reading motivational stories from other students who were
driven by a desire to contribute positively to the world, and writing their own motiva-
tional story for future students also improved high school students’ grades. In a health
care context, Grant and Hofmann demonstrated that health care providers increased
their soap use when primed with messages about protecting patients’ health compared to
messages about protecting their own health. Because goal pursuit in education and health
contexts typically involves sustained effort over the course of years, imbuing the daily
processes with pleasure or meaning beyond the long-term instrumental benefits is a use-
ful psychological approach that can inform future work in these domains.
caramelized, juicy) if a sauce is used or the dish is particularly satisfying; (3) traditional
words (e.g., old-fashioned, classic, countryside, Abuelita’s, homestyle, Mama’s) if the
dish is hearty, comforting, nostalgic, or rooted in tradition; or (4) location-based words
(e.g., New Orleans, Shanghai, tavern style, Thai, Provence) if the dish draws from ingre-
dients or preparation methods that are positively associated with a particular culture,
location, or setting.
NUANCES AND MISCONCEPTIONS
There are three common misconceptions about taste-focused labeling. A first common
misconception is that any fancy or positive word would work just as well as taste-focused
labels. However, taste-focused labeling increased diners’ expectations of how delicious
healthy foods will taste and their hypothetical choices of vegetable dishes compared
to fancy labels (e.g., “Viridescent,” “Ebullient”) and vague-positive labels (e.g., “Awe-
some,” “Incredible”; Turnwald et al., 2019). While vague-positive labels and fancy labels
may sound novel because people are not used to seeing vegetables described with vague-
positive or fancy words, these words do not provide specific flavor expectations and so
are less effective than taste-focused labels.
A second common misconception is that this approach is merely a behavioral nudge.
Nudges are aspects of the choice architecture that alter people’s behavior in a predictable
way without forbidding any options or significantly changing their economic incentives
(Thaler & Sunstein, 2008). However, nudges are limited in that they try to influence
behavior without considering how people make sense of the choices before them. In con-
trast, taste-focused labeling was informed both by people’s taste goals in the moment of
food choice and the psychological obstacles (e.g., culturally entrenched mindsets that
healthy foods are not tasty) that may prevent them from construing healthy foods as
consistent with those goals.
A third common misconception regards the moderator of preparing vegetable dishes
more flavorfully. Taste-focused labels work better when applied to vegetable dishes that
are prepared flavorfully, but some may feel that preparing vegetable dishes with flavor-
ful ingredients may reduce their nutritional benefits. Ingredients that are perceived as
unhealthy (e.g., cheese, butter, salt) can be mindfully incorporated into vegetable dishes
in small amounts that enhance flavor but do not override health benefits. Vegetables in
our studies were often prepared with ingredients that enhance both flavor and health
benefits, such as plant oils, herbs, and spices. Since dieting and restriction are ineffective
long-term strategies (Mann et al., 2007), preparing healthier foods more flavorfully using
chefs’ culinary strategies (Cohen et al., 2015; Spencer, Kurzer, Cienfuegos, & Guinard,
2018) and labeling them in enticing ways to highlight those flavors are both important
steps for enhancing vegetable consumption in ways that last.
IMPLICATIONS FOR PRACTICE
“This research reinforces what I have experienced in the field—the more that I empha-
size the health aspects of a food, the less enthused people are to eat it. Taste-focused
labeling is an exciting strategy because it is relatively easy and inexpensive to imple-
ment, and it has the potential to boost customer satisfaction and positively enhance
the dining experience. Plant-based options are also less expensive than meat-based
options. Using this strategy to increase the extent to which customers choose plant-
based foods and actually enjoy eating them is a win–win–win for food service opera-
tors and food companies concerned about their bottom line.”
“One of the most important lessons we’ve learned in our operations is the importance
of engaging chefs in the process. Not only do they have a lot of creative insight for
generating delicious names, but they also play a critical role in developing healthy
dishes that taste and look as delicious as they sound.”
IMPLICATIONS FOR PSYCHOLOGICAL THEORY
This intervention has implications for psychological theory regarding health promotional
approaches. Most interventions to improve healthy behaviors have focused on educating
people to identify healthy choices or about the importance of making healthy choices.
They have also assumed that a long-term desire to be healthy will be enough to prioritize
healthiness among many competing desires in the moment of food choice (e.g., tastiness).
However, taste-focused labeling capitalizes on emphasizing the attributes of healthy
298 II. Health and Well-Being
foods that align with most people’s top priority in the moment of food choice: tastiness.
Instead of leading with healthiness, which people associate with poorer taste, this inter-
vention changes how foods that are healthy are construed to the individual, shifting his
or her mindset by providing a new lens through which to experience and perceive these
foods—as delicious and enjoyable. As mentioned in the “Cousins” section of this chapter,
taste-focused labels are also similar to a class of interventions that help endow instrumen-
tal, long-term pursuits (e.g., school lessons, homework, work, safety) with meaning or
relevance to motivate greater engagement in the process.
Broadly, this intervention suggests that health interventions may be more effec-
tive when they match how target health behaviors are portrayed with people’s intrinsic,
short-term motives and priorities (Woolley & Fishbach, 2016, 2017). Research and policy
approaches have called for limiting appealing advertising of unhealthy foods (Kelly et
al., 2010), using labeling strategies like calorie labels or warning labels that make it easy
to recognize unhealthy foods (e.g., Block & Roberto, 2014; Donnelly, Zatz, Svirsky, &
John, 2018; Thorndike, Gelsomin, McCurley, & Levy, 2019), or teaching cognitive train-
ing strategies that emphasize health consequences of choosing unhealthy foods (Boswell,
Sun, Suzuki, & Kober, 2018; Van Dessel, Hughes, & De Houwer, 2018). However,
few approaches leverage tasty and enticing components of healthier foods. The present
research shows that it is possible to increase the lure of healthy foods by changing the
experience that people expect to have when eating them and making available the inter-
pretation that healthy foods can satisfy taste motives. Because we need people to not only
avoid healthy foods but to choose healthy foods, these efforts are just as critical as the
large body of research on strategies for decreasing the appeal of unhealthy foods.
FUTURE DIRECTIONS
Expanding to Other Settings
Are there certain locations, dining settings, or food types that taste-focused labeling is
especially effective or not effective for? We hypothesize that labels may be most effective
when they are the only piece of information upon which to make decisions (e.g., menus)
and when a diner is in a new situation compared to when the label is only one of multiple
sources of information (e.g., dining halls in which one can see the dish, smell the dish, see
others choosing or avoiding the dish), or when the diner has prior experience with a dish.
Much of the work to date is limited in that it was conducted in dining settings within
a college campus and all within a North American cultural context with participants
who are likely younger and more educated than the general population. More research is
needed across different populations and commercial settings.
2008; Nosek, Banaji, & Greenwald, 2002, for work on the importance of identity rele-
vance in other domains). This potentially could mean that taste-focused labeling may bet-
ter align with groups that have the least healthy diets and for whom emphasis on health
is counter to their cultural values, but research is needed to test these ideas thoroughly. It
remains to be tested whether these differences also depend upon which themes are used in
a taste-focused label to elevate expectations of a positive experience (e.g., exciting, indul-
gent, traditional) because some themes may be more identity relevant for some groups.
CONCLUSION
REFERENCES
Aggarwal, A., Rehm, C. D., Monsivais, P., & Drewnowski, A. (2016). Importance of taste, nutri-
tion, cost and conveniece in relation to diet quality: Evidence of nutrition resilience among
300 II. Health and Well-Being
US adults using National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) 2007–2010.
Preventive Medicine, 90, 184–192.
Aldridge, V., Dovey, T. M., & Halford, J. C. G. (2009). The role of familiarity in dietary develop-
ment. Developmental Review, 29(1), 32–44.
Berger, J., & Rand, L. (2008). Shifting signals to help health: Using identity signaling to reduce
risky health behaviors. Journal of Consumer Research, 35(3), 509–518.
Birch, L. L. (1998). Development of food acceptance patterns in the first years of life. Proceedings
of the Nutritional Society, 57, 617–624.
Block, J. P., & Roberto, C. A. (2014). Potential benefits of calorie labeling in restaurants. Journal
of the American Medical Association, 312, 887–888.
Boswell, R. G., Sun, W., Suzuki, S., & Kober, H. (2018). Training in cognitive strategies reduces
eating and improves food choice. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
USA, 115(48), E11238–E11247.
Bragg, M. A., Miller, A. N., Elizee, J., Dighe, S., & Elbel, B. D. (2016). Popular music celebrity
endorsements in food and nonalcoholic beverage marketing. Pediatrics, 138(1), e20153977.
Bragg, M. A., Miller, A. N., Roberto, C. A., Sam, R., Sarda, V., Harris, J. L., & Brownell, K. D.
(2018). Sports sponsorships of food and nonalcoholic beverages. Pediatrics, 141(4), e20172822.
Bryan, C. J., Yeager, D. S., Hinojosa, C. P., Chabot, A., Bergen, H., Kawamura, M., & Steub-
ing, F. (2016). Harnessing adolescent values to motivate healthier eating. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 113(39), 10830–10835.
Cohen, J. F. W., Richardon, S. A., Cluggish, S. A., Parker, E., Catalano, P. J., & Rimm, E. B. (2015).
Effects of choice architecture and chef-enhanced meals on the selection and consumption of
healthier schools foods: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA Pediatrics, 169(5), 431–437.
Crum, A. J., Corbin, W. R., Brownell, K. D., & Salovey, P. (2011). Mind over milkshakes: Mind-
sets, not just nutrients, determine ghrelin response. Health Psychology, 30(4), 424–429.
Crum, A. J., Salovey, P., & Achor, S. (2013). Rethinking stress: The role of mindsets in determin-
ing the stress response. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(4), 716–733.
Donnelly, G. E., Zatz, L. Y., Svirsky, D., & John, L. K. (2018). The effect of graphic warnings on
sugary-drink purchasing. Psychological Science, 29(8), 1321–1333.
Dube, L., Fatemi, H., Lu, J., & Hertzer, C. (2016). The healthier the tastier?: USA–Inda com-
parison studies on consumer perception of a nutritious agricultural product at different food
processing levels. Frontiers in Public Health, 4, 6.
Dweck, C. S. (2008). Mindset: The new psychology of success. New York: Random House Digital.
Fenko, A., Kersten, L., & Bialkova, S. (2016). Overcoming consumer scepticism toward food
labels: The role of multisensory experience. Food Quality and Preference, 48, 81–92.
Finkelstein, S. R., & Fishbach, A. (2010). When healthy food makes you hungry. Journal of Con-
sumer Research, 37, 357–367.
Glanz, K., Basil, M., Maibach, E., Goldberg, J., & Snyder, D. (1998). Why Americans eat what
they do: Taste, nutrition, cost, convenience, and weight control concerns as influences on
food consumption. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 98(10), 1118–1126.
Gortmaker, S. L., Swinburn, B. A., Levy, D., Carter, R., Mabry, P. L., Finegood, D., . . . Moodie,
M. (2011). Changing the future of obesity: Science, policy, and action. Lancet, 378(9793),
838–847.
Grant, A. M., & Hofmann, D. A. (2011). It’s not all about me: Motivating hand hygiene among
health care professionals by focusing on patients. Psychological Science, 22(12), 1494–1499.
Hanks, A. S., Just, D. R., & Brumberg, A. (2016). Marketing vegetables in elementary school
cafeterias to increase uptake. Pediatrics, 138(2), e20151720.
Harris, J. L., Bargh, J. A., & Brownell, K. D. (2009). Priming effects of television food advertising
on eating behavior. Health Psychology, 28(4), 404–413.
Hawkes, C., Smith, T. G., Jewell, J., Wardle, J., Hammond, R. A., Friel, S., . . . Kain, J. K. (2015).
Smart food policies for obesity prevention. Lancet, 385(9985), 2410–2421.
The Taste‑Focused‑Labeling Intervention 301
Hulleman, C. S., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2009). Promoting interest and performance in high
school science classes. Science, 326(5958), 1410–1412.
Irmak, C., Vallen, B., & Robinson, S. R. (2011). The impact of product name on dieters’ and non-
dieters’ food evaluations and consumption. Journal of Consumer Research, 38(2), 390–405.
Kelly, B., Halford, J. C., Boyland, E. J., Chapman, K., Bautista-Castano, I., Berg, C., . . . Sum-
merbell, C. (2010). Television food advertising to children: A global perspective. American
Journal of Public Health, 100(9), 1730–1736.
Lähteenmäki, L., Lampila, P., Grunert, K., Boztug, Y., Ueland, Ø., Åström, A., & Martinsdóttir,
E. (2010). Impact of health-related claims on the perception of other product attributes. Food
Policy, 35(3), 230–239.
Letona, P., Chacon, V., Roberto, C., & Barnoya, J. (2014). Effects of licensed characters on chil-
dren’s taste and snack preferences in Guatemala, a low/middle income country. International
Journal of Obesity, 38(11), 1466–1469.
Liem, D., Aydin, N. T., & Zandstra, E. (2012). Effects of health labels on expected and actual taste
perception of soup. Food Quality and Preference, 25(2), 192–197.
Lim, S. S., Vos, T., Flaxman, A. D., Danaei, G., Shibuya, K., Adair-Rohani, H., . . . Memish, Z. A.
(2013). A comparative risk assessment of burden of disease and injury attributable to 67 risk
factors and risk factor clusters in 21 regions, 1990–2010: A systematic analysis for the Global
Burden of Disease Study 2010. Lancet, 380(9859), 2224–2260.
Maimaran, M., & Fishbach, A. (2014). If it’s useful and you know it, do you eat?: Preschoolers
refrain from instrumental food. Journal of Consumer Research, 41(3), 642–655.
Mann, T., Tomiyama, A. J., & Ward, A. (2015). Promoting public health in the context of the
“obesity epidemic”: False starts and promising new directions. Perspectives on Psychological
Science, 10, 706–710.
Mann, T., Tomiyama, A. J., Westling, E., Lew, A. M., Samuels, B., & Chatman, J. (2007). Medi-
care’s search for effective obesity treatments: Diets are not the answer. American Psycholo-
gist, 62(3), 220–233.
Marty, L., Chambaron, S., Nicklaus, S., & Monnery-Patris, S. (2018). Learned pleasure from eat-
ing: An opportunity to promote healthy eating in children? Appetite, 120, 265–274.
Micha, R., Penalvo, J. L., Cudhea, F., Imamura, F., Rehm, C. D., & Mozaffarian, D. (2017). Asso-
ciation between dietary factors and mortality from heart disease, stroke, and type 2 diabetes
in the United States. Journal of the American Medical Association, 317(9), 912–924.
Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Math = male, me = female, therefore
math ≠ me. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(1), 44–59.
Oyserman, D., Fryberg, S. A., & Yoder, N. (2007). Identity-based motivation and health. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(6), 1011–1027.
Papies, E. K., & Veling, H. (2013). Healthy dining: Subtle diet reminders at the point of purchase
increase low-calorie food choices among both chronic and current dieters. Appetite, 61, 1–7.
Pettigrew, S. (2016). Pleasure: An under-utilised “P” in social marketing for healthy eating. Appe-
tite, 104, 60–69.
Raghunathan, R., Naylor, R. W., & Hoyer, W. D. (2006). The unhealthy = tasty intuition and its
effects on taste inferences, enjoyment, and choice of food products. Journal of Marketing,
70(4), 170–184.
Rehm, C. D., Penalvo, J. L., Afshin, A., & Mozaffarian, D. (2016). Dietary intake among US
adults, 1999–2012. Journal of the American Medical Association, 315(23), 2542–2553.
Roberto, C. A., Baik, J., Harris, J. L., & Brownell, K. D. (2010). Influence of licensed characters
on children’s taste and snack preferences. Pediatrics, 126(1), 88–93.
Roberto, C. A., Swinburn, B., Hawkes, C., Huang, T. T., Costa, S. A., Ashe, M., . . . Brownell, K.
D. (2015). Patchy progress on obesity prevention: Emerging examples, entrenched barriers,
and new thinking. Lancet, 385(9985), 2400–2409.
Spencer, M., Kurzer, A., Cienfuegos, C., & Guinard, J.-X. (2018). Student–consumer acceptance of
302 II. Health and Well-Being
plant-forward burrito bowls in which two-thirds of the meat has been replaced with legumes
and vegetables: The Flexitarian Flip in university dining venues. Appetite, 131, 14–27.
Suher, J., Raghunathan, R., & Hoyer, W. D. (2016). Eating healthy or feeling empty?: How the
“healthy = less filling” intuition influences satiety. Journal of the Association for Consumer
Research, 1(1), 26–40.
Thaler, R. H., & Sunstein, C. R. (2008). Nudge: Improving decisions about health, wealth, and
happiness. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Thorndike, A. N., Gelsomin, E. D., McCurley, J. L., & Levy, D. E. (2019). Calories purchased by
hospital employees after implementation of a cafeteria traffic light-labeling and choice archi-
tecture program. JAMA Network Open, 2(7), e196789.
Turnwald, B. P., Anderson, K. G., Jurafsky, D., & Crum, A. J. (in press). Five-star prices, appeal-
ing healthy item descriptions?: Expensive restaurants’ descriptive menu language. Health
Psychology.
Turnwald, B. P., Bertoldo, J. D., Perry, M. A., Policastro, P., Timmons, M., Bosso, C., . . . Crum,
A. J. (2019). Increasing vegetable intake by emphasizing tasty and enjoyable attributes: A
randomized controlled multisite intervention for taste-focused labeling. Psychological Sci-
ence, 30(11), 1603–1615.
Turnwald, B. P., Boles, D. Z., & Crum, A. J. (2017). Association between indulgent descriptions
and vegetable consumption: Twisted carrots and dynamite beets. JAMA Internal Medicine,
177(8), 1216–1218.
Turnwald, B. P., & Crum, A. J. (2019). Smart food policy for healthy food labeling: Leading
with taste, not healthiness, to shift consumption and enjoyment of healthy foods. Preventive
Medicine, 119, 7–13.
Turnwald, B. P., Jurafsky, D., Conner, A., & Crum, A. J. (2017). Reading between the menu lines:
Are restaurants’ descriptions of “healthy” foods unappealing? Health Psychology, 36(11),
1034–1037.
Turnwald, B. P., Perry, M. A., Jurgens, D., Prabhakaran, V., Jurafsky, D., Markus, H. R., & Crum,
A. J. (2020). Healthy food cast as unappealing in language of American movies, television,
restaurants, government websites, and users on social media. Manuscript in preparation.
Van Dessel, P., Hughes, S., & De Houwer, J. (2018). Consequence-based approach–avoidance
training: A new and improved method for changing behavior. Psychological Science, 29(12),
1899–1910.
Veldhuizen, M. G., Nachtigal, D. J., Flammer, L. J., de Araujo, I. E., & Small, D. M. (2013). Ver-
bal descriptors influence hypothalamic response to low-calorie drinks. Molecular Metabo-
lism, 2(3), 270–280.
Walton, G. M., & Wilson, T. D. (2018). Wise interventions: Psychological remedies for social and
personal problems. Psychological Review, 125(5), 617–655.
Wardle, J., & Cooke, I. (2008). Genetic and environmental determinants of children’s food prefer-
ences. British Journal of Nutrition, 99, S15–S21.
Werle, C. O. C., Trendel, O., & Ardito, G. (2013). Unhealthy food is not tastier for everybody: The
“healthy = tasty” French intuition. Food Quality and Preference, 28(1), 116–121.
Woolley, K., & Fishbach, A. (2016). For the fun of it: Harnessing immediate rewards to increase
persistence in long-term goals. Journal of Consumer Research, 42, 952–966.
Woolley, K., & Fishbach, A. (2017). Immediate rewards predict adherence to long-term goals.
Personality and Social Pscyhology Bulletin, 43(2), 151–162.
Yeager, D. S., Henderson, M. D., Paunesku, D., Walton, G. M., D’Mello, S., Spitzer, B. J., & Duck-
worth, A. L. (2014). Boring but important: A self-transcendent purpose for learning fosters
academic self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(4), 559–580.
PA R T III
CONFLICT AND
RELATIONSHIPS
C H A P TE R 13
David S. Yeager and Hae Yeon Lee
This chapter explains how an incremental theory of personality—the belief that people can
change their socially relevant characteristics—can cause people to cope with their social
difficulties more effectively. We review evidence from randomized trials showing that even
short exposures to an intervention teaching an incremental theory of personality can shift
people’s construals, so that adolescents see social difficulties as experiences that could
be changed with time and therefore as less threatening. Our interventions have improved
adolescents’ maladaptive stress responses, mental health, and academic achievement. We
explain how the incremental theory of personality intervention works through attributional
and biopsychosocial mechanisms. We conclude with a discussion of future directions for
research and practice, such as the implications of implicit theories of personality for school
or home environments and for theories of stress and coping.
Making the transition to high school typically comes with social challenges. In some
of our studies, we asked ninth-grade students every day for several days in the first month
of high school to tell us about negative events that happened to them that day (e.g., Yea-
ger, Lee, & Jamieson, 2016). It was common for students to write about events such as
these:
“Well sometimes people call me fat or stupid . . . I didn’t want to tell them, but it
killed me inside.”
“This morning I was walking by and all the person could do was act as if I weren’t
there. Seeing them and they just look you in the face without a ‘hi’ or a smile
made me feel invisible.”
“All my friends were acting weird and wouldn’t tell me why. I feel out of place since
they’re all keeping something from me.”
305
306 III. Conflict and Relationships
“I had an old friend look at me with disgust, even though I’m not the one who did
anything wrong.”
“In lunch I only see people in groups of 3–10, never alone. I feel extremely lonely
again.”
What can be done to help adolescents deal with these challenges and others like them?
We developed the incremental theory of personality intervention to answer this question.
The core assumption underlying our intervention is that the meaning of stressful
experiences can be ambiguous. Are they happening to me because of something wrong
with me or my context, or are they normal events that I can learn from? The incremental
theory of personality intervention offers people a new lens for seeing the world—one that
focuses on humanity’s potential for dynamism and change. In doing so, the intervention
gives people reason to expect that even socially painful experiences, such as peer victim-
ization or exclusion, are not causes for despair, because they might get better with time.
The effect of inviting people into this more dynamic worldview has been to improve
adolescents’ emotional and academic lives, as they confront the inevitable social adver-
sities that accompany a difficult life transition. As we will see, randomized trials have
evaluated the intervention and have documented improvements in adolescents’ behavior,
well-being, and grades. Now that the intervention has shown effects in the context of
high school bullying and social exclusion, the stage is set for research to generalize the
intervention to other socially challenging periods of life and to test it in more—and more
diverse— settings.
BACKGROUND
Quite often, adolescents jockey for social status in a way that harms their peers (Cole-
man, 1961; Crosnoe, 2011; Hawley, 1999). Aggressive behaviors can range from bully-
ing, defined as a repeated use of aggression against someone with lower social power
(Hong & Espelage, 2012), to more subtle forms of relational aggression, such as targeting
others’ reputations or weakening their social networks through indirect insults, rumors,
or exclusion (Prinstein, Boergers, & Vernberg, 2001). Adolescents are not only victims of
these behaviors but are also initiators of them. Some young people perceive a need to pro-
tect their status and signal their social power before others do the same to them. Many
seek revenge after victimization.
Managing social challenges such as these can feel like more than one can handle.
As such, the normative difficulties of adolescence can evoke maladaptive stress responses
that feed into symptoms of anxiety and depression (Hammen, 2005). Epidemiological
data suggest that recent birth cohorts of adolescents may be even more affected by this
than previous cohorts (Twenge, Joiner, Rogers, & Martin, 2017) perhaps because con-
nected technologies (e.g., social media) have made it easier than ever for students to
engage in relational aggression, such as targeting others’ reputations (George & Odgers,
2015; Underwood & Ehrenreich, 2017).
To make matters worse, social difficulties co-occur with academic difficulties. Ado-
lescents have been said to have two jobs in secondary school: the job of doing well socially
and the job of doing well academically (Coleman, 1961; Crosnoe, 2011). When the first
job is going poorly, it can compromise adolescents’ cognitive ability to do the second.
The Incremental Theory of Personality Intervention 307
Worries about social status can interfere with the basic cognitive processing required
to perform well in classes (Yang, Lee, Crosnoe, & Yeager, 2020), and threats to social
belonging in a setting can undermine motivation to learn (Deci & Ryan, 1985; Walton,
Cohen, Cwir, & Spencer, 2012). In sum, adolescents’ reactions to social status threats
can act as a “psychological hub” that connects diverse outcomes, from aggressive behav-
ior to mental health to academic achievement.
The incremental theory of personality intervention targets a belief system, the entity
theory of personality, that lies underneath this “psychological hub,” and in doing so it
simultaneously affects a number of related outcomes (see Yeager et al., 2014, for a dis-
cussion). The entity theory of personality is the belief that people cannot change. High
school students with an entity theory have been prone to despair, as we explain below.
The incremental theory of personality intervention seeks to neutralize the entity theory
and improve adolescents’ coping with social stressors by teaching a simple belief: People
can change and improve their personal characteristics, under the right conditions, with
the right experiences, and with social support.
The effectiveness of this message grows out of the insight that adolescents who think
that current social challenges are permanent will cope more poorly with them. That
poorer coping in turn can spill over into their social behavior, well-being, and academic
achievement. But if adolescents can put their ongoing social difficulties into perspec-
tive—if they can see that social difficulties are temporary and not indicative of people’s
core character traits—then adolescents may cope more effectively. By coping better, ado-
lescents may show more prosocial behavior and better mental health and academic per-
formance.
PSYCHOLOGICAL PROCESSES
social goals). When judging others, an entity theory furthermore creates a heightened
focus on underlying traits as causes of behavior, as well as the expectation that traits will
remain consistent far into the future. This leads to surprise when traits are not shown to
be the most important determinants of individuals’ behavior or when they change (see
Plaks, 2017, for a review). Then, when confronted with others’ social failures, those with
more of an entity theory respond in ways that condemn or exclude the targets, as opposed
to working productively to educate or change the targets (Rattan & Dweck, 2010).
An incremental theory of personality intervention seeks to target and change the
entity theory meaning system. To understand how and why this is effective, below we
preview two mediators of an entity theory’s effects on behavior and stress responses:
attributions and stress appraisals. (These will show up again when we review the media-
tors of the intervention’s effects.)
Attributions
One psychological process affected by implicit theories of personality is a person’s attri-
butions concerning the causes of ongoing difficulties (Dweck, 1975; Weiner, 1985). This
is an important psychological process because the start of high school is a moment with
excessive attributional ambiguity.
The attributional ambiguity of high school is illustrated well by a 16-year-old author,
Florida Frenz (2013), in her inspiring book How to Be Human. Frenz is on the autism
spectrum and is a sophisticated observer of the high school transition:
Some [friends] have abruptly decided they don’t want to hang out with me. . . . But more
often “friends” indicate their disinterest more subtly. They may not nod or wave at me
anymore. Their eyes may wander or glaze over when I try to talk to them. Their replies
may go from being stories or jokes to terse answers such as “uh-huh” or “oh.” The gray
areas are most frustrating. Is this about me . . . or her? Will my friendships with the
other members of the group start fraying as well? Is this a phase or a permanent thing?
(emphasis added)
The sentences in italics highlight the attributional questions. Prior research has
shown that how adolescents answer those questions can have important effects on how
they cope. When individuals attribute an event such as being socially excluded or being
targeted by bullying to factors that are stable and outside of one’s control (e.g., the flawed
moral character of one’s peers or one’s own inherent social inadequacy), then they may be
expected to cope more poorly, for instance, by showing signs of helplessness or despair
(Chiu, Hong, & Dweck, 1997; Gervey, Chiu, Hong, & Dweck, 1999; see also Janoff-
Bulman, 1979). But when a person attributes an event such as social rejection or bullying
to factors that could be changed (e.g., others’ more temporary motivations or erroneous
beliefs, or one’s misunderstanding of the current social milieu), then the person may be
expected to cope better, and may be willing to proactively solve the problem (Yeager &
Miu, 2011). Thus, whether one construes social difficulties as stemming from fixed ver-
sus malleable causes can be an important determinant of how one responds to the event
(Beck, 2008; Janoff-Bulman, 1979; Weiner, 1985), and in particular whether one copes
well or poorly.
The Incremental Theory of Personality Intervention 309
Physiological Stress Responses
The fixed-trait attributional styles that follow from an entity theory of personality can
give rise to maladaptive physiological stress responses that may explain how individuals
who differ in their implicit theories of personality go on to show differences in symptoms
over time (Schleider & Weisz, 2016; Yeager et al., 2016). The key to understanding why is
to recognize that adolescents are not simply disinterested observers of the causes of their
social world. The attributions for social difficulties that they make flow directly into their
assessments of whether they are likely to be able to deal with or change stressors such as
peer victimization or exclusion. Illustrating this fact is a quotation from a high school
student participating in a focus group we conducted for one of the original incremental
theory studies:
“I’m in my first month of high school and I have already had one or two melt
downs. . . . It is already getting to be a bit much to handle all at once. I can already
see I am in a heck of a ride for high school. . . . ”
What stands out to us in this quotation is how students’ predictions about the future—
whether their stressors are likely to continue throughout high school—seem closely linked
to their appraisals of whether they have what it takes to handle the stressors. Quotations
like this one led us to test the hypothesis that individuals with more of an entity theory of
310 III. Conflict and Relationships
personality may tend to construe their social stressors as demands that they do not have
sufficient resources to handle what is called a threat state in the biopsychosocial (BPS)
model of challenge and threat (Blascovich & Tomaka, 1996; Jamieson, Hangen, Lee, &
Yeager, 2018).
Threat-t ype responses to stressors, which we now know are more likely to grow out
of the entity theory meaning system in which negative social stressors are construed as
demands that cannot be changed, have been known for some time to contribute to long-
term physical and mental health problems (Jamieson et al., 2018; Juster, McEwen, &
Lupien, 2010). Threat states elicit greater cortisol responses, greater constriction of the
blood vessels, and less efficient cardiac output, all of which could help the body to survive
acute physical trauma but are poorly suited for physical and mental health in the absence
of acute physical threats (Jamieson et al., 2018; Mendes & Park, 2014).
Daily diary data have shown that an entity theory of personality elicits threat-t ype
stress responses and this can explain why an entity theory might be related to down-
stream health outcomes (Seo et al., 2019). Threat-t ype stress responses, such as elevated
cortisol levels, tend to linger and become self-reinforcing (Lee, Jamieson, Miu, Josephs,
& Yeager, 2019), and predict the onset of more global outcomes like depression (Dick-
erson & Kemeny, 2004) or poorer cognitive performance (Lupien, McEwen, Gunnar, &
Heim, 2009). Hence, if an incremental theory of personality intervention could reduce
threat-type responses—by leading adolescents to perceive their stressors as things that
they can handle—then the intervention could result in better long-term outcomes via its
effects on physiological stress responses.
approach avoids the disrespectful implication that the intervening parties view the ado-
lescents’ beliefs or adjustment to be deficient (Yeager et al., 2018). It can also make the
intervention more credible; adolescents who are told to believe that bullies can change
may begin to think of specific bullies who may never change. Adolescents may have a
more optimistic response when they are asked to generate an argument about why peers
who are sometimes mean could potentially change.
EMPIRICAL EVIDENCE
Outcomes
The incremental theory of personality intervention has reduced both externalizing behav-
ior (e.g., aggression) and internalizing symptoms (e.g., maladaptive stress responses and
depression). In a few studies, it improved adolescents’ academic performance.
Externalizing Behavior
The first incremental theory of personality intervention sought to reduce adolescents’
aggressive retaliation in response to a peer provocation (Yeager, Trzesniewski, & Dweck,
2013). The intervention took the form of a multisession classroom workshop delivered
to ninth- and tenth-grade students (total N = 230) at a relatively lower-achieving school
with rather high levels of peer conflict. It occurred over 3 weeks, in six classroom ses-
sions each lasting under 50 minutes. Adolescents were presented with scientific informa-
tion about changes in the brain and in personality—that changes were always possible,
even if they were not automatic or easy. They read stories from older students who had
learned about the incremental theory of personality (during a pilot study) and completed
activities (skits, games, small-group discussions) that allowed them to use the incremental
theory for specific, hypothetical instances of victimization or exclusion. Like growth-
mindset- of-
intelligence interventions (Aronson, Fried, & Good, 2002), participants
were guided to internalize the message by completing a writing exercise—a “saying-is-
believing” exercise—in which they wrote a letter to a future student encouraging him or
her to remember that people can change (Aronson, 1999).
In the first trial, the primary behavioral measure of aggression was assessed 1
month after the end of the intervention. Behavior was measured via a standardized
task adapted from social psychology: the number of grams of hot sauce participants
allocated to a peer to consume when they believed that the peer had excluded them in
an online game, and that the peer strongly disliked spicy food (Lieberman, Solomon,
Greenberg, & McGregor, 1999; i.e., Cyberball; Williams & Jarvis, 2006). In truth,
there was no peer (the provocation was controlled by a computer) and no one actu-
ally ate the hot sauce. Yeager, Trzesniewski, et al. (2013) showed that adolescents who
received the incremental theory of personality intervention, compared to two separate
control groups allocated about 40% less hot sauce. Improvement was also visible in
students’ daily behaviors in schools. Three months after the intervention, those who
received the incremental theory of personality intervention were more likely to be nomi-
nated by their teachers (who were unaware of students’ conditions) for having improved
their social behavior in school.
312 III. Conflict and Relationships
Subsequent studies have shortened the intervention and made it easier to replicate in
larger samples, taking it from six sessions led by trained facilitators to one session self-
administered either on paper or via an online module (Yeager et al., 2014; Yeager, Miu,
Powers, & Dweck, 2013). The shortened intervention kept the same basic elements—
scientific evidence about the brain’s and personality’s potential for change, stories from
upper-year students who received and endorsed the incremental theory of personality,
and the “saying-is-believing” letter to a peer—but the content was communicated more
efficiently. Two experiments found that this shortened, self-administered incremental
theory of personality intervention effectively reduced adolescents’ willingness to respond
aggressively to a hypothetical scenario relative to a control group, via changes in attribu-
tional tendencies (Yeager, Miu, et al., 2013).
It may seem surprising that a briefer version of the intervention could show measur-
able effects. But an inspection of the letters students wrote to a peer shows that they often
processed the message and found it to be powerful. Here are two examples from one of
our recent studies:
“All the people that are mean and hurtful to you are just missing out on a good friend
they could’ve had. But with all that said people can change and learn what they did
and said are wrong. Just because someone is one way right now doesn’t mean they
can’t change at all in time. Because in reality they can change.”
“When we were starting the school year I went [to] where my friends hang out. Then
they just looked at me and kept on talking. . . . I felt really left out especially because
we were all really good friends in middle school . . . [Although] it may seem like your
life is crashing down, and that nothing will ever get better, it is not the end of the
world. Your life will not be like this forever. . . . People can change. It’s only your
first day of school.”
Internalizing Symptoms
The first evaluation of the incremental theory of personality intervention found an effect
on symptoms of depression (Yeager, Trzesniewski, et al., 2013), even though, at the time,
it was designed primarily to reduce aggression. That study showed that although in the
control condition adolescents who were more victimized were also more depressed, this
was not the case in the intervention condition. With an incremental theory of personality,
depression was lower for victimized adolescents compared to control participants who
were victimized.
Why did the effect on internalizing symptoms appear? We began to dig into this
result more deeply, which led us to the theoretical synthesis with the BPS model of chal-
lenge and threat described above. We conducted new studies focusing on internalizing
symptoms in general and maladaptive stress responses in particular (e.g., Yeager et al.,
2014), and we found that the one-session incremental theory of personality interven-
tion could reduce negative stress responses to an experimenter-controlled social challenge
(again, exclusion via Cyberball; Williams & Jarvis, 2006). Then in a field experiment that
tracked students’ levels of cortisol over 1 week after the intervention—which, as noted,
represents one hormonal indicator of threat-type responses to social stressors—Yeager
The Incremental Theory of Personality Intervention 313
and colleagues (2016, Study 2) found that the incremental theory intervention reduced
cortisol among adolescents who reported more intensely negative social stressors.
Can the short-term reductions in threat-t ype stress responses from the incremental
theory of personality last? Evidence is beginning to show that they can. Yeager et al.
(2014, Studies 2 and 3) reported two experiments showing that at a 9-month follow-up
there were lower levels of global negative stress responses (measured via the Perceived
Stress Scale; Cohen, Kamarck, & Mermelstein, 1983) among those who had received the
incremental theory of personality at the beginning of the school year.
Could the incremental theory go on to reduce clinically significant depressive symp-
toms? Individuals who appraise a social stressor as something that they can handle should, in
principle, be less likely to show increases in their depressive symptoms (Hammen, 2005)—
therefore, an intervention to improve social stress coping might prevent those increases in
depression. Consistent with this logic, Miu and Yeager (2015) reported 9-month effects of
the incremental theory intervention on self-reported depressive symptoms (measured via a
short form of the Children’s Depression Inventory [CDI]; Kovacs, 2003).
Two teams have recently found significant effects of the incremental theory inter-
vention on clinical symptoms of depression. Schleider and colleagues (2015) found that
an incremental theory of personality intervention led to lower levels of depressive symp-
toms, compared to controls, in a longitudinal study conducted with clinical populations
of adolescents who reported elevated symptoms prior to random assignment to condi-
tion. Calvete and colleagues (2019), conducting a replication study with adolescents in
Spain (between ages 13 and 18), reported lower levels of depressive symptoms at 6- and
12-month follow-up among those receiving the incremental theory intervention, com-
pared to controls—but this result was moderated by age in unpredicted ways that would
ideally lead to additional replication studies.
Academic Performance
The incremental theory of personality intervention was designed to improve responses to
social stressors, and did not have an academic component—that is, the short interven-
tion made no mention of trying harder in school. Therefore, initially we did not expect
to find that the incremental theory of personality intervention would improve grades, but
because social success is so closely tied to academic success in high school, we thought
it would be worth testing. In the three studies we conducted so far that tested for it, we
found evidence for effects on grades (Yeager et al., 2014, Studies 2 and 3; Yeager et al.,
2016, Study 2). This result was conceptually replicated in a laboratory study focusing on
performance on a speech and math task (Yeager et al., 2016, Study 1). Thus, an intriguing
result is that an intervention that focuses primarily on helping adolescents to deal with
their social lives has also produced cross-domain benefits for academic outcomes as well.
This supports the “psychological hub” idea that we described above.
Mechanisms
Several experiments have generated preliminary evidence in support of psychological
mechanisms growing out of the two theoretical models reviewed earlier: attribution the-
ory and the BPS model of challenge and threat.
314 III. Conflict and Relationships
Stress Physiology
The effects of the incremental theory intervention have also appeared on physiological
responses to social stress—responses that are thought to lead to long-run differences in
well-being (Goodyer, Park, Netherton, & Herbert, 2001; Gunnar & Quevedo, 2007;
Sapolsky, 2000; Staufenbiel, Penninx, Spijker, Elzinga, & van Rossum, 2013). In the
first experiment described in the previous section, adolescents receiving the incremental
theory of personality intervention and undergoing the TSST showed more adaptive car-
diovascular responses relative to controls, in the form of greater vascular dilation and
cardiac efficiency, and also more adaptive adrenal responses (i.e., lower cortisol response;
Yeager et al., 2016). A similar pattern of results emerged in an experiment conducted
by Schleider and Weisz (2016), which also showed improvements in acute physiologi-
cal responses to an experimentally induced stressor within a population of adolescents
already showing higher depression at baseline.
Could stress physiology improvements explain the effects of the incremental theory
of personality on academic performance? A correlational mediation analysis reported
by Yeager and colleagues (2016, supplemental online material), reproduced in Figure
13.1, found that the intervention’s effect on lower threat-type appraisals of the stressor
predicted changes in vascular dilation from baseline to during the stressor (i.e., reactivity
in “total peripheral resistance” [TPR]), and this difference in stress reactivity in turn pre-
dicted performance on the public speech and mental math tasks. This is consistent with
The Incremental Theory of Personality Intervention 315
FIGURE 13.1. Mediation model showing the path from the incremental theory of personality
intervention to improved cognitive performance during a social stressor (the Trier Social Stress
Test [TSST]). Coefficients represent standardized estimates. TPR, total peripheral resistance,
an indication of vascular constriction, which is a physiological indication of threat-type stress
responses. N = 52. Data from Yeager, Lee, and Jamieson (2016, Study 1).
the conclusion that adolescents who received the incremental theory intervention circu-
lated more oxygenated blood to their brains, resulting in better cognitive performance.
Effects over Time
So far, the incremental theory of personality intervention has been evaluated for the
length of one school year—usually 9 months (e.g., Miu & Yeager, 2015). This was inten-
tional; students are far less likely to transfer or drop out of high school within ninth
grade relative to between ninth and tenth grade. However, a critical question is how and
whether the effects persist beyond the 1-year period.
COUSINS
The incremental theory of personality intervention owes much to the incremental theory
of intelligence intervention (also known as the growth-mindset-of-intelligence interven-
tion; see Aronson et al., 2002; Dweck & Yeager, 2019; Yeager & Dweck, 2012). And the
“grammar” of wise interventions that it implements comes directly from interventions to
address belonging uncertainty (Walton & Cohen, 2011).
Like the social-belonging intervention (see Walton & Brady, Chapter 2, this volume),
the incremental theory of personality intervention targets the “psychological hub” of
social relationships during a challenging life transition. Both interventions rely on the
possibility of attributional ambiguity in the meaning of everyday adversities, and seek
to tilt patterns of attributions in favor of changeable causes rather than fixed causes of
difficulty. And both interventions have shown evidence of affecting both academic and
health outcomes (see Walton & Cohen, 2011). One way to distinguish the two interven-
tions is in the arguments they make about how adversities can change. The incremental
theory of personality intervention conveys evidence that people’s internal traits change
(e.g., via changes in the brain that come with development and experience), whereas the
social-belonging intervention argues that people’s social circumstances change (e.g., by
joining study groups or meeting with professors during office hours). A second way that
the interventions differ is in who is targeted by the message and who tends to benefit.
The incremental theory of personality intervention targets students who are undergo-
ing social difficulties in general (e.g., bullying or exclusion), but the social-belonging
intervention focuses on alleviating identity threat (e.g., students facing stereotype threat).
Therefore, we do not expect the incremental theory of personality intervention to be
moderated by race, ethnicity, or socioeconomic status unless in a given sample these are
correlated with the vulnerability targeted by the intervention (i.e., prior susceptibility to
social adversity). But the social-belonging intervention should be moderated by groups
that tend to experience identity threat, because it is designed to clear up attributional
ambiguity for identity-threatened individuals.
The incremental theory of personality was also informed by interventions seeking
to reduce the “hostile attributional bias,” which is the tendency for aggressive individu-
als to attribute causally ambiguous social difficulties to hostile intent (Dodge & Frame,
1982; Dodge, Godwin, & Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group, 2013). The
incremental theory of personality intervention targeted a belief system that was thought
to give rise to a pattern of attributions, but did not attempt to directly change hostile
intent attributions.
The incremental theory has some alignment with the stress- is-
enhancing mind-
set (Crum, Salovey, & Achor, 2013). The latter involves the mindset that one’s stress
responses can be positive and energizing and can help one address and overcome the
stressor. This should also reduce “threat-t ype” responses to stressors, and yield patterns
of cortisol reactivity that are analogous to the incremental theory of personality. The dif-
ference between the two is that the incremental theory of personality focuses on people’s
appraisals of the external stimulus (the stressful event) and the stress-is-enhancing mind-
set focuses on people’s appraisals of the internal stimulus (your ongoing stress response).
An interesting future direction could be to combine these two approaches into a sin-
gle intervention; such an intervention would presumably convey that negative, stressful
events and negative stress responses can be changed and improved over time.
The Incremental Theory of Personality Intervention 317
NUANCES AND MISCONCEPTIONS
Although the incremental theory of personality intervention can have replicable effects,
it relies on a number of nuances that we think are important for its effectiveness. The
first is that the intervention does not seek to teach an extreme incremental theory of per-
sonality so much as it seeks to reduce the extreme entity theory. Said differently, for the
intervention to work adolescents do not need to believe that all people can greatly change
at any time or in any social circumstances—instead, they probably only need to have a
reasonable doubt about the assumption that people’s characteristics cannot ever change.
The former might be tough for many students to believe; the latter might provide a basis
for optimistic outlooks. The intervention simply conveys that it is possible for people to
change, under the right conditions, with the right support, and with sufficient motivation
on behalf of the person seeking to change.
Another nuance is that the intervention does not argue that you deserve to be bullied
or excluded (which could imply you have weak social skills that need to be improved)
or that you are responsible for changing every peer who is unkind or aggressive. Such a
message could have two negative side effects: It could cause adolescents to blame them-
selves if their peers do not change, or it could cause adolescents to put themselves into
dangerous situations where change is unlikely but harm at the hands of the peer is likely.
The incremental theory of personality intervention makes the argument that change hap-
pens in many ways—sometimes, just through chronological maturation, or natural life
transitions—and it is not any one person’s responsibility to change another person. But,
regardless of how change happens, it is always possible.
Finally, as the intervention begins to be tested in clinical settings (Schleider & Weisz,
2016), one open question concerns how the intervention is presented to participants. In
the universal, preventative settings where the intervention was initially tested, it has been
framed as a survey about the transition to high school, but participants have not been told
that they are getting an “intervention” designed to help them. The goal, as noted, is to be
autonomy supportive and prevent reactance (see also Yeager et al., 2018). However, ado-
lescents who are seeking treatment for clinically elevated mental health problems, such
as depression and anxiety, may be aware that they are being treated and actively seek out
treatment. An intriguing question is whether the intervention needs to be “stealthy” or
not (see Walton & Wilson, 2018; Yeager & Walton, 2011, for discussions). (We are grate-
ful to Jessica Schleider, a psychological scientist at Stony Brook University, for suggesting
this important future direction for research.)
IMPLICATIONS FOR PRACTICE
This research has implications for how schools and parents talk about and address
bullying. Research on essentialism (Gelman, 2004), which refers to social judgments
that focus on a person’s or group’s fixed “essence,” raises the concern that describ-
ing socially aggressive students with generic noun phrases (e.g., “bullies” or “victims”
or “troublemakers”) could contribute to an entity theory culture and climate. Using
generic phrases, defined as statements that take the form “bullies do X” (or, in past
studies, “bears eat ants”) to describe the characteristics or behaviors of groups of indi-
viduals, has led listeners to infer that the group’s characteristics or behaviors are rooted
318 III. Conflict and Relationships
IMPLICATIONS FOR PSYCHOLOGICAL THEORY
experience of lingering threat-t ype stress responses to social stressors that could interfere
with cognition and performance.
FUTURE DIRECTIONS
Apart from research on the implications for practice and theory noted above, we see at
least three exciting avenues for future research. The first is to continue to replicate the
effects of the intervention and identify the subgroups and school contexts where effects
are weaker and stronger. This will be critical for understanding the public health impact
of the intervention, but it is also for advancing mindset × context theory, which may apply
to many kinds of wise interventions.
The second future direction is to continue to understand the recursive (or self-
reinforcing) processes (Cohen, Garcia, Purdie- Vaughns, Apfel, & Brzustoski, 2009;
Walton & Wilson, 2018) that sustain the treatment effects long after the treatment
administration has ended. One way to do so is by carrying out finer-grained research on
moment-to-moment stress processes. This could co-occur with ambulatory measurement
of cardiovascular responses to provide a more precise picture about when implicit theo-
ries improve coping outcomes over time.
Another way to examine recursive processes is to measure the emergent social inter-
actions and access to resources that might result from an incremental theory. Presum-
ably, an intervention that promotes more adaptive stress responses might also embolden
adolescents to seek out new and more variable social relationships. Those relationships
might then provide a buffer from real-world adversities, or even reduce the prevalence of
bullying or exclusion. In general, an important possible mechanism for enduring effects
of the intervention is changes in the social world brought about by personal agency (see
Walton & Wilson, 2018, Figure 1C).
A primary future direction for our research groups is to think more about develop-
mental timing (Dahl, Allen, Wilbrecht, & Suleiman, 2018; Yeager et al., 2018). What is
the optimal time—in terms of chronological age, in terms of pubertal maturation, and in
terms of educational or professional transitions—to change mindsets? We have presumed
that the change in mindset is best done at the beginning of a school transition (Miu &
Yeager, 2015; Yeager et al., 2014), but this has not been directly tested in a sample that
controls for differences across school systems. Should implicit theories be changed pre-
ventatively, well before the socially challenging start of high school? Could this depend
on pubertal timing—and therefore, vary across gender, racial, and ethnic groups since
groups differ in the onset of pubertal maturation? Advances in hormone assays that indi-
cate pubertal maturation (i.e., testosterone and estradiol) and validation of self-reported
pubertal indices will make tests of these questions more feasible than before. Now that
the intervention can be made precise, lasting one session, then research can be carried
out on when and how it can best be administered and how it interacts with development.
Finally, now that the intervention and its mechanisms have been studied in the con-
text of one socially trying circumstance—peer victimization and exclusion during the
transition to high school—we think an important future direction is to examine its appli-
cation to other social difficulties. How could the intervention help individuals transition-
ing to college or the workplace and struggling to feel included? We are excited to see how
the intervention could be adapted and evaluated in these or other contexts.
320 III. Conflict and Relationships
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Support for writing this chapter came in part from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute
of Child Health and Human Development (R01 HD084772-01), the Raikes Foundation, the Wil-
liam T. Grant Foundation, and Hope Lab. This research was supported by grant, P2CHD042849,
Population Research Center, awarded to the Population Research Center at The University of
Texas at Austin by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human
Development. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily rep-
resent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.
REFERENCES
Lee, H. Y., Jamieson, J. P., Josephs, R. A., Reis, H. T., Beevers, C. G., & Yeager, D. S. (2020).
Adolescents’ implicit theories of personality shape their responses to social media stressors.
Unpublished manuscript, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX.
Lee, H. Y., Jamieson, J. P., Miu, A. S., Josephs, R. A., & Yeager, D. S. (2019). An entity theory
of intelligence predicts higher cortisol levels when high school grades are declining. Child
Development, 90, e849–e867.
Levy, S. R., & Dweck, C. S. (1999). The impact of children’s static versus dynamic conceptions of
people on stereotype formation. Child Development, 70(5), 1163–1180.
Levy, S. R., Stroessner, S. J., & Dweck, C. S. (1998). Stereotype formation and endorsement: The
role of implicit theories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1421–1436.
Lieberman, J. D., Solomon, S., Greenberg, J., & McGregor, H. A. (1999). A hot new way to mea-
sure aggression: Hot sauce allocation. Aggressive Behavior, 25(5), 331–348.
Lupien, S. J., McEwen, B. S., Gunnar, M. R., & Heim, C. (2009). Effects of stress throughout the
lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(6), 434–445.
Mendes, W. B., & Park, J. (2014). Neurobiological concomitants of motivational states. In A. J.
Elliot (Ed.), Advances in motivation science (pp. 233–270). Waltham, MA: Elsevier.
Miu, A. S., & Yeager, D. S. (2015). Preventing symptoms of depression by teaching adolescents
that people can change: Effects of a brief incremental theory of personality intervention at
9-month follow-up. Clinical Psychological Science, 3(5), 726–743.
Molden, D. C., & Dweck, C. S. (2006). Finding “meaning” in psychology: A lay theories approach
to self-regulation, social perception, and social development. American Psychologist, 61(3),
192–203.
Molden, D. C., Plaks, J. E., & Dweck, C. S. (2006). “Meaningful” social inferences: Effects of
implicit theories on inferential processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42(6),
738–752.
Plaks, J. E. (2017). Implicit theories: Assumptions that shape social and moral cognition. In J. M.
Olson (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 56, pp. 259–310). Cambridge,
MA: Academic Press.
Prinstein, M. J., Boergers, J., & Vernberg, E. M. (2001). Overt and relational aggression in ado-
lescents: Social-psychological adjustment of aggressors and victims. Journal of Clinical Child
Psychology, 30(4), 479–491.
Rattan, A., & Dweck, C. S. (2010). Who confronts prejudice?: The role of implicit theories in the
motivation to confront prejudice. Psychological Science, 21(7), 952–959.
Ryan, A. M., & Shim, S. S. (2008). An exploration of young adolescents’ social achievement
goals and social adjustment in middle school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(3),
672–687.
Sapolsky, R. M. (2000). Stress hormones: Good and bad. Neurobiology of Disease, 7(5), 540–542.
Schleider, J. L., Abel, M. R., & Weisz, J. R. (2015). Implicit theories and youth mental health
problems: A random-effects meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 35, 1–9.
Schleider, J. L., & Weisz, J. R. (2016). Reducing risk for anxiety and depression in adolescents:
Effects of a single-session intervention teaching that personality can change. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 87, 170–181.
Seo, E. J., Lee, H. Y., Jamieson, J., Beevers, C., Reis, H., Josephs, R., & Yeager, D. S. (2019).
Trait attributions and threat appraisals explain why an entity theory of personality predicts
greater internalizing symptoms during adolescence. Unpublished manuscript, University of
Texas at Austin.
Slavich, G. M., O’Donovan, A., Epel, E. S., & Kemeny, M. E. (2010). Black sheep get the blues: A
psychobiological model of social rejection and depression. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral
Reviews, 35(1), 39–45.
Staufenbiel, S. M., Penninx, B. W., Spijker, A. T., Elzinga, B. M., & van Rossum, E. F. (2013). Hair
cortisol, stress exposure, and mental health in humans: A systematic review. Psychoneuroen-
docrinology, 38(8), 1220–1235.
The Incremental Theory of Personality Intervention 323
Twenge, J. M., Joiner, T. E., Rogers, M. L., & Martin, G. N. (2017). Increases in depressive symp-
toms, suicide-related outcomes, and suicide rates among US adolescents after 2010 and links
to increased new media screen time. Clinical Psychological Science, 6(1), 3–17.
Underwood, M. K., & Ehrenreich, S. E. (2017). The power and the pain of adolescents’ digital
communication: Cyber victimization and the perils of lurking. American Psychologist, 72(2),
144–158.
Vansteenkiste, M., Simons, J., Lens, W., Sheldon, K. M., & Deci, E. L. (2004). Motivating learn-
ing, performance, and persistence: The synergistic effects of intrinsic goal contents and
autonomy-supportive contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(2), 246–
260.
Walton, G. M., & Cohen, G. L. (2011). A brief social-belonging intervention improves academic
and health outcomes of minority students. Science, 331(6023), 1447–1451.
Walton, G. M., Cohen, G. L., Cwir, D., & Spencer, S. J. (2012). Mere belonging: The power of
social connections. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(3), 513–532.
Walton, G. M., & Wilson, T. D. (2018). Wise interventions: Psychological remedies for social and
personal problems. Psychological Review, 125(5), 617–655.
Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of emotion and motivation. Psychological Review,
92(4), 548–573.
Williams, K. D., & Jarvis, B. (2006). Cyberball: A program for use in research on interpersonal
ostracism and acceptance. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 38(1),
174–180.
Wolf, W., Levordashka, A., Ruff, J. R., Kraaijeveld, S., Lueckmann, J.-M., & Williams, K. D.
(2015). Ostracism online: A social media ostracism paradigm. Behavior Research Methods,
47(2), 361–373.
Yang, M., Lee, H. Y., Crosnoe, R., & Yeager, D. S. (2020). The cognitive cost of thinking you’re
popular in high school. Unpublished manuscript, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX.
Yeager, D. S., Dahl, R. E., & Dweck, C. S. (2018). Why interventions to influence adolescent
behavior often fail but could succeed. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(1), 101–122.
Yeager, D. S., & Dweck, C. S. (2012). Mindsets that promote resilience: When students believe
that personal characteristics can be developed. Educational Psychologist, 47(4), 302–314.
Yeager, D. S., Johnson, R., Spitzer, B. J., Trzesniewski, K. H., Powers, J., & Dweck, C. S. (2014).
The far-reaching effects of believing people can change: Implicit theories of personality shape
stress, health, and achievement during adolescence. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 106(6), 867–884.
Yeager, D. S., Lee, H. Y., & Jamieson, J. P. (2016). How to improve adolescent stress responses:
Insights from integrating implicit theories of personality and biopsychosocial models. Psy-
chological Science, 27(8), 1078–1091.
Yeager, D. S., & Miu, A. S. (2011). Implicit theories of personality predict motivation to use pro-
social coping strategies after bullying in high school. In E. Frydenberg & G. Reevy (Eds.),
Personality, stress and coping: Implications for education (pp. 47–62). Charlotte, NC: Infor-
mation Age.
Yeager, D. S., Miu, A. S., Powers, J., & Dweck, C. S. (2013). Implicit theories of personality and
attributions of hostile intent: A meta-analysis, an experiment, and a longitudinal interven-
tion. Child Development, 84(5), 1651–1667.
Yeager, D. S., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Dweck, C. S. (2013). An implicit theories of personality
intervention reduces adolescent aggression in response to victimization and exclusion. Child
Development, 84(3), 970–988.
Yeager, D. S., Trzesniewski, K. H., Tirri, K., Nokelainen, P., & Dweck, C. S. (2011). Adolescents’
implicit theories predict desire for vengeance after peer conflicts: Correlational and experi-
mental evidence. Developmental Psychology, 47(4), 1090–1107.
Yeager, D. S., & Walton, G. M. (2011). Social-psychological interventions in education: They’re
not magic. Review of Educational Research, 81(2), 267–301.
C H A P TE R 14
Jason Anthony Okonofua and Michael Ruiz
Suspensions are the most common form of exclusionary discipline, discipline that removes
students from the learning environment. They are associated with a negative consequence
for education and life outcomes. Research suggests that suspensions result from a default
punitive approach to curb student misbehavior. In this chapter, we detail an intervention
designed to shift teachers’ mindsets about misbehavior to show more empathy. An empathic
mindset is one in which teachers value students’ perspectives, nurture students’ growth,
and prioritize the maintenance of positive relationships with students. We describe lab
studies and a large-scale field experiment that demonstrate how the shift from a punitive to
empathic mindset can produce more productive outcomes for both teachers and students.
The intervention ultimately halved suspension rates in middle school across three school
districts (N = 1,682 students) and helped students with a history of suspension to maintain a
perception of respect from adults at their school. This chapter situates the intervention in the
wise intervention framework with information about the content and delivery of the interven-
tion and the mechanisms by which it operates. We end with details about implications of the
intervention for theory and integration with policy and practice.
BACKGROUND
Students across the United States are removed from learning environments by way of
suspensions at an alarming rate. In 2011, more than 5 million students were suspended
from schools throughout the United States, which marked a substantial increase since
a few decades ago when less than 2 million students were suspended in 1974 (Losen
& Wald, 2003; U.S. Department of Education, Office for Civil Rights, 2016). These
rates are of particular concern, because suspensions are an exclusionary discipline that
removes students from environments where learning is the priority. And some students,
especially those with multiple suspensions, are placed in an environment where they are
324
The Empathic‑Discipline Intervention 325
more likely to enter a life trajectory of school dropout, unemployment, mental and physi-
cal illness, and incarceration—a process called the “school-to-prison pipeline” (Jordan,
Lara, & McPartland, 1996; Gottfried, 2010; Couch & Fairlie, 2010; Pager, Western, &
Sugie, 2009; Boynton, O’Hara, Covault, Scott, & Tennen, 2014; Rocque & Paternoster,
2011). Further, one student’s discipline problems can affect other students’ outcomes in
the classroom (Bill and Melinda Gates Foundation, 2013; Ferguson, 2012). The effects
of discipline problems can also extend beyond the school. Recent research showed that
taxpayers must contribute millions of dollars to offset the lifetime consequences of school
suspensions for society at large—by way of incarceration costs and lower future earnings
and tax revenue (Rumberger & Losen, 2017).
These effects have exploded concurrently with zero-tolerance policies put in place
to deter threats to school safety with punitive repercussions. An unexpected drawback
lies in how suspensions have become a more common response to relatively minor and
ambiguous misbehaviors compared to weapon or drug possession (Skiba, 2014). In recent
years, the most common reasons for office referrals that result in suspensions are for
misbehaviors classified as insubordination or classroom disruption. However, according
to Skiba, “no data exist to show that out-of-school suspensions and expulsions reduce
disruption or improve school climate” (p. 27).
Stigmatized groups are impacted by these school policies at a disproportionate rate
(Pager et al., 2009). Research has shown that lesbian, bisexual, gay, transgender, Black,
Latinx, Native American, and students in special education are at a heightened risk for
suspension from school (U.S. Department of Education, 2014; Himmelstein & Bruckner,
2011; Poteat, Scheer, & Chong, 2015). For example, Black students are two to three
times more likely to be suspended than their White peers (Fabelo et al., 2011). These
disparities exist from preschool through high school (Skiba et al., 2011) with Black pre-
schoolers being 3.6 times more likely to receive a suspension than White preschoolers
(U.S. Department of Education, Office for Civil Rights, 2016).
Teacher–student relationships suffer under these punitive— nonempathic—
conditions. For teachers, they become disheartened when they feel that dealing with mis-
behavior gets in the way of their teaching goals (Johnson, Yarrow, Rochkind, & Ott,
2009). For students, they can feel threatened and question their teachers’ intentions when
teachers are more likely to critically respond to them without communicating that they
care (e.g., Yeager et al., 2014). This process can be of particular risk for students from
stigmatized groups who may already be vigilant to cues that they do not belong at school
(Goyer et al., 2019) or will not receive fair treatment (Mendoza-Denton, Downey, Purdie,
Davis, & Pietrzak, 2002; Steele, 1997). Students tend to behave with more defiance and
less cooperation when they perceive a teacher to be an untrustworthy authority figure
(Fenning & Rose, 2007; Gregory & Weinstein, 2008)—a process that is exacerbated for
stigmatized students who may already expect unfair treatment. In this way, the default
punitive context can deteriorate the quality of teacher–student relationships and cause
both teachers and students to feel their goals cannot be reached in school (Okonofua,
Walton, & Eberhardt, 2016). Discipline problems can then arise, a process also especially
likely for students from stigmatized groups.
Public and private institutions have attempted a variety of strategies to curb the exor-
bitant rates of exclusionary discipline. From a policy standpoint, many states have enacted
laws that prohibit schools from suspending students for reasons such as insubordination
326 III. Conflict and Relationships
FIGURE 14.1. Empathic discipline (top path) activates empathic mindset, as opposed to default
punitive mindset (bottom path). In turn, a more productive recursive cycle ensues.
The Empathic‑Discipline Intervention 327
What is the default punitive mindset? Teacher responses to misbehavior can be shaped
by the extent to which a teacher thinks the misbehavior will be an ongoing or consistent
hindrance. Teachers can come to think misbehavior will be ongoing when they attribute
the misbehavior to an inherent characteristic of the student. We refer to this belief as a
“punitive mindset.” In turn, teachers may seek more punitive discipline, often discipline
that removes the student from the learning environment (e.g., referral to the principal’s
office). Researchers evidenced such a belief and process in a series of experiments about
race disparities in disciplinary action (Okonofua & Eberhardt, 2015). Teachers were pre-
sented with a series of misbehaviors by a student and were asked questions about how
they would respond. Teachers were more likely to respond to a student they labeled as
a troublemaker with a desire for more severe discipline and were more likely to see the
student being suspended in the future. The troublemaker labeling was thus pivotal in the
discipline process and it was a process that played out over the course of multiple mis-
behaviors. Further, teachers became more distressed and wanted more severe discipline
from one misbehavior to the next, and this escalation was steeper for students viewed
as troublemakers. All of these effects were most pronounced if the student was assumed
to be Black, because the student was more likely to be a troublemaker if assumed to be
Black as compared to White. However, for either a Black or White student, being labeled
a troublemaker predicted the process of harsh responses to misbehavior. The empathic-
discipline intervention seeks to replace this punitive mindset with an empathic mindset,
one that appreciates the potential for students to behave better and for relationships with
students to improve over time.
PSYCHOLOGICAL PROCESSES
The adoption of the empathic mindset creates opportunity to build respect, rather than
mistrust, in a teacher’s relationship with a student who has misbehaved. Over time, each
mindset (punitive or empathic) can contribute to recursive cycles such that either the
relationship deteriorates and thus discipline problems grow, or a positive relationship
remains intact and thus future conflict is prevented (see Figure 14.1). In this section, we
describe the process by which a psychological component (a teacher’s mindset and a stu-
dent’s feeling of respect) and a behavioral component (how a teacher and a student then
respond to each other) can contribute to recursive cycles that ultimately lead to severe
exclusionary discipline, or not.
A teacher can have distinct mindsets or models for dealing with student misbehavior.
As mentioned, a teacher’s punitive mindset can lead him or her to view misbehavior as
a stable pattern and thus respond with severe or exclusionary punishment. Also, due to
many teachers entering the profession with a desire to support and help children grow
(Johnson et al., 2009), that same teacher might also harbor an empathic mindset, one
that prioritizes the maintenance of high-quality and productive relationships with stu-
dents who struggle, including students who misbehave. Each mindset can be activated
and affects the way a teacher will respond to misbehavior. When the empathic mindset is
activated teachers are less likely to label a misbehaving student as a troublemaker and are
more likely to want to find out more about why the student misbehaved.
The researchers conducted an initial experiment to determine whether a targeted
exercise could activate distinct mindsets in teachers and in turn shift their responses
328 III. Conflict and Relationships
1 Ideally,middle school students would have been the participants in this study. However, we decided
that college students would be better able to express how they would have felt in the situation.
The Empathic‑Discipline Intervention 329
while the findings are short term—based in scenarios—they suggest that a productive
recursive process would ensue between the teacher and student.
The productive recursive process is coined “empathic discipline” and as a whole
makes way for a mindset shift and a behavioral shift in the effects of discipline. The
mindset shift pertains to the change in teachers’ beliefs about students and their behavior.
When teachers engage with materials that remind them of the powerful positive impact
of quality teacher–student relationships—as opposed to a default punitive mindset—they
become less likely to attribute a student’s behavior to a rigid component of the student’s
character (i.e., labeling the student as a troublemaker). In turn, the student respects the
teacher more and is more motivated to behave well in class in the future.
The behavioral component is evident in how the teacher and student interact in their
behaviors toward each other. The teacher seeks to find out more about the student’s per-
spective and how to use discipline as a vehicle to gain or maintain the student’s respect
and trust in the relationship. The student wants to follow the teacher’s instructions and
to behave well in the future. Over time, the quality of the teacher–student relationship
is protected and there is a reduced likelihood of conflict in the future due to these inter-
twined mindset and behavioral shifts.
The empathic-discipline intervention is geared to offset the punitive path to disci-
pline problems. It aims to shift teachers’ mindsets away from default troublemaker label-
ing and punitive responses to misbehavior. Instead, it strategically highlights (1) listening
to and seeking to understand students’ perspective in periods of misbehavior, even when
this perspective is not productive; (2) prioritizing and sustaining positive relationships
with students, especially in times of misbehavior; and (3) helping students grow and
improve within the context of a trusting relationship. The intervention seeks to remind
teachers that they are in a unique position to do each of these three things, which will
allow them to make meaningful contributions to their students’ lives. In turn, teachers
will create a context for students to feel more respect in the teacher–student relationship
and be more motivated to behave well.
EMPIRICAL EVIDENCE
The researchers conducted a randomized controlled field experiment to test whether the
effects described in the previous section can extend to actual teacher–student relation-
ships and discipline rates. Can an opportunity for teachers to reflect on, articulate, and
commit to an empathic mindset cause reductions in discipline problems?
Outcomes
The experiment was evaluated at five middle schools in three California districts with
math teachers (N = 31) and students (N = 1,682; 52% female; 17% Asian, 2% Black,
54% Latino, 7% White, 20% other/unknown). The schools varied in the percentage of
their student population that received free or reduced-price lunch, an indicator of socio-
economic status (37%, 68%, 70%, 61%, and 62%, in order of largest to smallest total
student enrollment at each of the five schools that participated).
In the experiment, math teachers were randomly assigned to engage either with
modules about how technology use is important in involving students (control) or with
330 III. Conflict and Relationships
modules about the importance of sustaining positive relationships with students by way
of valuing and seeking to understand their perspectives, especially when students mis-
behave. In each module, teachers read brief articles and narratives about the topic, and
answered questions about their understanding and experiences related to the topic.
Math teachers completed two online modules, a 45-minute session in the fall
(October–November) and a 25-minute session in the winter (January–February).2 All
materials were delivered online such that teachers completed the sessions from their own
computers and did so at their convenience during a 2-week window. The fall and spring
time line was chosen to ensure that teachers had experiences with their current students
before engaging in the first module—that is, teachers would be able to engage with the
materials in a meaningful way that would directly apply to their students. The second
module was designed to serve as a booster, or reminder, when the school year reconvened
after holiday breaks.
As described earlier, suspensions are especially impactful because they remove stu-
dents from the learning environment and they are significantly more common than other
exclusionary disciplines, like expulsions or referrals to law enforcement. Further, this was
the only discipline outcome schools tracked across each school district. This intervention
halved year-long student suspension rates from 9.6 to 4.8% (see Figure 14.2). Similar to
national suspension rates, control-condition suspension rates were highest among boys,
Black and Latinx students, and students with a history of suspensions. The reduction in
suspension rates was comparably large for the following groups: boys, from 14.6 to 8.4%;
African Americans and Latinx, from 12.3 to 6.3%; and previously suspended students,
from 51.2 to 29.4%.
There was also a notable shift in students’ experiences of respect. Students were
asked the extent to which they agreed with the statement “Teachers and other adults at
my school treat me with respect.” The intervention bolstered the respect of the most at-
risk students and previously suspended students, perceived from all teachers and adults
at their school. It is important to note that the felt respect was not solely from their math
teacher but rather all teachers at the school. This point is further explained in the next
section.
Mechanism
Did suspensions drop solely due to a change in math teachers’ interactions or discipline
standards with students, or did they drop, as well, because students experienced fewer
suspensions from interactions with adults across school contexts (e.g., nonmath teach-
ers)? Evidence suggests the latter.
Not only did students report experiencing greater respect from all their teachers at
school (not just their math teacher) but records also indicated that the fewer suspensions
were not likely due solely to fewer referrals for discipline from math teachers. One school
district in the sample kept records of the faculty member who referred a student for ulti-
mate suspension. Students from this school accounted for 33% of the full student sample.
2 Math teachers were recruited because all students at the schools had one math teacher only. This
allowed for a design that determines efficacy of the treatment with only one of the students’ teachers.
Otherwise, it would be difficult to determine effects on students if they had some teachers randomly
assigned to the treatment condition and others to the control condition.
331
FIGURE 14.2. Middle school students (N = 1,682) whose math teacher (N = 31) completed the empathic-mindset intervention as compared to random-
ized control materials were half as likely to be suspended over the school year. From Okonofua, Paunesu, and Walton (2016). Error bars represent 95%
confidence intervals after 10,000 bootstraps. Reprinted with permission from the authors.
332 III. Conflict and Relationships
In this school, students of math teachers who received the intervention were 55% less
likely to be suspended from school (treatment = 5.4%, control = 12.1%). Yet, the effect
was not due to a change only in math class. Only 7.4% of suspensions were referred by
math teachers. Furthermore, all students referred for suspension by a math teacher were
also referred for suspension by other faculty (e.g., a science teacher). Thus, even exclud-
ing suspensions referred by math teachers from the analysis yields an identical reduc-
tion in suspension rates. This supplemental finding suggests that, at least in this school,
improving the experience with at least one teacher led to a broad improvement in student
behavior across diverse classroom contexts. It notably also suggests that the effect was
not merely due to math teachers being more lenient in their discipline practices nor to an
improvement in students’ behavior solely in math classes—rather, students’ perceptions
of and experiences in the entire school context became more productive and less condu-
cive to discipline problems.
These findings suggest that part of the effect results from a shift in students’ percep-
tions and experiences throughout the school and the school day. Students’ entire school
experiences can be improved when a single teacher presumably treats them as more
deserving of respect and as having a valuable perspective. This is consistent with the
aforementioned recursive process of respect in teacher–student relationships (see Figure
14.1). This is also consistent with the finding that the most at-risk students, those with
a history of suspensions, were less likely to lose respect for adults at their school when
they had a teacher who received the empathic-discipline intervention. It also suggests the
importance of students’ perspectives in this process (see Goyer et al., 2019).
Effects over Time
The intervention’s effects lasted for several months. Teachers began participation in
October and suspension records were evaluated for that entire school year. The sustained
effects seem to also be associated with protecting teacher–student relationships from
deterioration. This is evident in how, several months after the intervention, students with
a history of suspension perceived more respect from adults at their school when they have
a teacher with the empathic mindset, as opposed to not having it. Future research might
explore outcomes beyond the year of the intervention, such as how teachers interact with
new students in following years or how students interact with new teachers in the follow-
ing years.
Heterogeneity
In the initial test of the intervention, the proportional reduction in suspension rates was
comparable for all students. Also, due to certain groups being more at risk of receiving
suspensions, the absolute impact was relatively larger for them (males: from 14.6 to 8.4%;
African American and Latinos: from 12.3 to 6.3%; and previously suspended students:
from 51.2 to 29.4%). The intervention was tested at only five schools and with only the
math faculty at those schools (N = 31; 77% female; 39% sixth grade, 29% seventh grade,
32% eighth grade). While this can attest to the intervention’s strength to bring about
large and lasting effects with a small sample of high-impact players, it does not allow for
definitive heterogeneity information.
The Empathic‑Discipline Intervention 333
Future research is needed to confirm more specific conditions for the intervention’s
efficacy. For example, randomized controlled trials are currently in place to evaluate the
effects of teacher characteristics (e.g., race, gender, and stress) and school characteristics
(e.g., school size, student demographics, and grade levels) on the extent to which the
intervention reduces suspension rates (see “Future Directions” section).
COUSINS
Framing
Following past social-psychological interventions (e.g., Walton & Cohen, 2011), teachers
were not told that they were receiving an intervention, that the exercise was intended to
reduce discipline problems, or that teachers are biased in their discipline practices. Teach-
ers could have interpreted that framing as controlling or as stigmatizing. Instead, teachers
were treated as experts asked to offer their feedback on best practices and how to not show
bias in discipline. They were told that the researchers were interested in learning more from
them about effective discipline practice so they could pass on their insights to new teachers.
The intervention was delivered through a 45-minute online session in the fall and a
25-minute online session in the winter. Each session was introduced as an opportunity
The Empathic‑Discipline Intervention 335
for future teachers to learn from participants’ past experiences as teachers and that the
researchers would present them with brief articles and stories to guide their feedback.
These different forms of reflection were geared to cohesively remind teachers of the three
primary themes for empathic discipline: (1) seek to understand students’ perspective when
misbehavior occurs, (2) prioritize the maintenance of positive relationships with students,
and (3) help students develop and control their behaviors.
Articles
The articles detailed how it is important for teachers to bring out the best in their students
through communication of care and respect relationships. They highlighted how situa-
tional factors such as stigma and puberty can cause students to worry about unfair treat-
ment and can affect their behavior. A student can also come to feel less threatened in school
and more motivated to behave well when teachers consider situational factors and value the
student’s perspective. The following is an example of language used in the articles:
Of course, it takes more time to reach some students than others because their previ-
ous experiences and expectations differ. Some students have had good experiences with
teachers. Others have had negative experiences. . . . But teachers who consistently reach
out and engage students do make a difference. They help students see that they do have
a fair shot and that people in authority are there to help them grow and develop, not
stand in their way.
Last, the articles explain how this consideration is especially important in the heat of
the moment, when conflicts or misbehavior arise. One article states:
Teachers told us that some of the greatest challenges they faced—and some of their best
opportunities for helping students—occurred after students misbehaved or struggled
academically. These situations offer teachers an opportunity to talk with students and
help them understand their experiences in class and in middle school more positively.
Turmoil is an opportunity to show students care and respect in a way that can be
especially meaningful for students and impactful for long-term gains in quality teacher–
student relationships.
Stories
The content also included brief stories to drive home major points in the articles. The
stories were told from the perspectives of various students and a teacher, and they each
included multiple psychological methods to persuasively remind teachers of how key
themes emphasized in the articles play out in real-life situations (see Table 14.1). For
example, norms were established in most of the stories. These norms ranged from how
many students misbehave when they feel anxious to how students become less anxious
when they feel they receive care and respect, especially from teachers. The objective was
to remind teachers that misbehavior is to be expected from growing children and a nor-
mal response is to show care to students. Research shows that establishing norms in this
way can lead a person to change his or her behavior to avoid deviating from it (Goldstein,
Cialdini, & Griskevicius, 2008).
336 III. Conflict and Relationships
Messages: Students worry about respect; stigma can affect students’ perspectives.
Racially stigmatized student quotation 1. Show that it is normal for students to worry
about unfair treatment and how that feeling
“Whenever I get a new teacher, I think ‘Is she gonna
can be heightened by risk of discrimination
treat me fairly? Does she call on the White students
due to a student’s background.
more? Does she expect them to know the right
2. Provide an example of how a teacher’s
answers and us to get them wrong?”
intentions may not always be clear to a
student.
Racially stigmatized student quotation 1. Show that students from stigmatized groups
may expect unfair treatment.
“I always thought school wasn’t for me, or for
2. Place teachers in the perspective of a student
people like me. It seemed that people like me just
to help him or her remember the situational
get in trouble in school. But my 6th-grade math
reasons why a student might misbehave.
teacher really changed my mind. She told us that she
3. Provide an example of how a teacher can
knew that every one of us could learn and that she
help a stigmatized student to feel more
would work hard to help us get there. . . . ”
certain that he or she can belong at school.
The Empathic‑Discipline Intervention 337
Saying Is Believing
After reading and reflecting on the materials, teachers wrote essays describing how they
use the kinds of practices described to build positive relationships with students during
difficult disciplinary contexts. For instance, one participating teacher wrote, “I NEVER
hold grudges. I try to remember that they are all the son or daughter of someone who
loves them more than anything in the world. They are the light of someone’s life” (see
Table 14.2 for more teacher quotations). Teachers were told that these essays would be
shared with new teachers to help them in their practice. This procedure, in which people
freely advocate for an idea to a receptive audience (“saying is believing”), is a powerful
persuasive technique. It makes the experience active, not passive, promoting deep pro-
cessing. It also encourages people to commit themselves to an idea and to connect this
idea to their own lives and practice (Walton & Wilson, 2018; Yeager & Walton, 2011).
1. “At the start of the year I introduce myself to each student individually. We do several journal and
other assignments that allow me to learn more about my students. I write comments on the pages
so the students know that I actually do read their work. I take note of anything my students share
that I may want to reference in conversation.”
2. “I make myself available to students after school to provide them with more personalized support.
When students are struggling I try to get on their level and find out what is going on not focusing
on what they are doing wrong but trying to focus more on what I can do to ameliorate the situation
and look for solutions moving forward. I also try to attend events like sports or theatre that they
are interested in and talk to them about the things they like.”
3. “I feel that one way to build positive relationships is to talk to the students. Often times students
feel that they are judged even before they walk into the classroom. So if you listen to them and talk
with them they are willing to work for you.”
4. “We share good news each week—building a strong classroom community. Each student who
wants to share can share. No one is left out. I am fair. I say hello to each student as they walk
through the classroom door. I also try and say good-bye to students as they leave. I smile at each
student as they enter the classroom. I also try to listen to what a student is telling me and try to be
fair and consistent in my discipline in the classroom.”
5. “Pull students aside to talk with them about behavior or grades; help students set goals and create
steps for meeting them; incorporating student interests into activities and lessons; giving students
choice in projects (i.e., students can select, research, focus, determine format for presenting
information, etc.); allowing students to create own groups for work; chatting with students about
their interests and their daily lives.”
6. “I try to find out interests and hobbies outside of school. I attend these activities and talk with
parents to build relationships. I talk to the students back at school about the activity that I
attended.”
7. “I do ice-breaker activities & ask students their hobbies and interests. I also make a point of letting
the class know that I am human & make mistakes as well. Our motto is It’s OK to make a mistake
as [long] as you have grown from it. That could mean apologizing, fixing or having the Ah-Ha
moment. We also have ‘Bad Day’ plans and students can let me know if it is a Bad Day and I will
work with their comfort level for participation.”
338 III. Conflict and Relationships
Control Condition
In the randomized controlled trial, half of the math teachers were assigned to a
“technology-engagement” control condition. The content of this condition was about
how to leverage technology to engage students in lessons and assignments. Thus, like the
treatment condition, it was about means to improve student outcomes. The key difference
is that this condition did not talk about seeking students’ perspectives or ways to think
about student misbehavior.
The control condition was similar in structure and in time to complete. Like the
treatment condition, the control condition consisted of two online sessions: a 45-minute
session in the fall and a 25-minute session in the winter. Also, the content was delivered
in the form of articles, stories, and exercises similar in length.
NUANCES AND MISCONCEPTIONS
The empathic-discipline intervention is still not fully understood. Current and future
research is needed to determine the specifics for implementation and expectations for
effects in various contexts. So far, there is one major nuance or misconception about
the intervention. The psychological message is about seeking to understand a student’s
perspective when the student misbehaves, not merely imagining or assuming his or her
perspective.
teachers to guide students to better manage their thoughts and feelings, a key lesson in
child development.
IMPLICATIONS FOR PRACTICE
Scalability
The empathic- discipline intervention was administered with teachers in five schools
across three school districts. This required a focus on scalability that maintained fidel-
ity to the treatment—in other words, it was important to make sure the cohesion of the
psychological experience was sufficient while also done in a way that can be administered
across many contexts. This form of consideration is key for future steps to increase the
scalability of the intervention.
Critical components for scalability was that the intervention could be administered
online and at teachers’ convenience. By constructing the intervention materials in an
online forum, it could be implemented remotely from the research base—the location of
the research team. The intervention was thus able to be administered at schools in differ-
ent cities while still being able to be monitored in a single location.
The online platform also made it possible for teachers to participate in the inter-
vention at their convenience within a 3-week window. As mentioned, context matters.
Schools have varying schedules and planning in place that determine teachers’ day-to-day
schedules. For example, in some schools, teachers have planning periods when participa-
tion would work best. In other schools, teachers have dedicated times for professional
development meetings when participation would work best. While a strict participation
schedule (e.g., all participation at a single time and in a single place) would allow for more
control over the delivery of the materials and fidelity to the participation procedure, it
could disrupt schools’, teachers’, and students’ regular working and learning schedules.
In turn, it could lead teachers to not appropriately engage with the materials or opt to not
participate at all. The implementation schedule of providing a set number of weeks for
teachers to participate at their leisure allowed for relative control over timing of imple-
mentation while also being flexible to schools’ various schedules.
Context Matters
As with all psychological interventions, the context matters. Schools have different
policies in place and different theories for how to approach improvements to student
340 III. Conflict and Relationships
outcomes. The empathic- discipline intervention will likely work best when tactfully
integrated with policy and skill-building approaches. While future empirical research is
needed to confirm the efficacy of integration, theory suggests that it will be beneficial in
contexts with certain policy and skill-building interventions in place.
In modern times, research suggests that schools can have a default punitive climate
in which teachers become more likely to respond to misbehavior with punishment instead
of care. In this social climate, teachers can come to view a misbehaving student in terms
of a label (e.g., a “troublemaker” or a “bad kid”; Okonofua & Eberhardt, 2015; Okono-
fua, Walton, et al., 2016). Their discipline might then focus on getting rid of the student
as opposed to adjust the context to make it more conducive to better behavior. The inter-
vention takes a psychological approach (i.e., how teachers interpret student misbehavior)
to shift classrooms from the default punitive social climate to one that is more conducive
to teachers and students feeling less disgruntled or threatened. Therefore, the interven-
tion may be particularly beneficial in school contexts that can provide time and space
for teachers and students to nurture their relationships. Put differently, teacher–student
relationships can be strengthened and discipline problems reduced when policies are in
place to enable local or district leaders to effectively create nonpunitive social climates in
schools.
IMPLICATIONS FOR PSYCHOLOGICAL THEORY
Many psychological interventions have shown large positive effects from a direct focus on
students’ own mindsets about their experiences. Empathic discipline advances that work
with evidence for how a focus on students’ environments (i.e., teachers’ responses to their
students’ behavior) can also improve student outcomes. This intervention attests to the
power of the situation to affect outcomes, for better or for worse (Ross & Nisbett, 1991).
This is most apparent in how the intervention highlights the default punitive context in
schools. Lab studies show that when teachers are reminded that punishment is critical for
students to learn self-control, the teachers are more likely to view a student as a trouble-
maker when he or she misbehaves. They are also more likely to start students on a path to
suspension and less likely to try to find out more about the cause of the misbehavior. The
default context is so normalized that a brief intervention that reminds teachers to value
students’ perspectives and to help students perceive respect from them can significantly
change the likelihood of a student getting in trouble throughout the school day.
Second, psychologically wise interventions can contribute to lasting change in real-
world outcomes. These interventions are low cost and brief, which can cause them to be
interpreted as magic (Yeager & Walton, 2011). However, they are carefully crafted to
shift how people interpret their experiences in a way that can build on itself with new
experiences over time (see Walton & Wilson, 2018). This is the case with empathic dis-
cipline. It shifts the way teachers interpret misbehavior and the students who misbehave.
Over time, this can change the way teachers interact with students, and students can
come to feel more respected at school. In this way, the intervention provides an example
of how a strategic nudge can be embedded in patterns of interaction such that it can build
on itself and ultimately influence an entire context (see Harackiewicz, Rozek, Hulleman,
& Hyde, 2012; Outes, Sanchez, & Vakis, 2017; Paluck, Shepherd, & Aronow, 2016;
Powers et al., 2016, for other examples).
FUTURE DIRECTIONS
The high-priority next steps for the empathic-discipline intervention include investiga-
tions of mechanisms by which the intervention’s effects benefit teachers, benefit students,
and persist over time, and also include explorations of conditions in which the interven-
tion is most effective, or not.
How might the intervention affect teachers? The preliminary findings indicate that
teachers who engage with the empathic mindset or participate in the empathic-discipline
intervention are less likely to view students as troublemakers and more likely to respond
to misbehavior in ways that communicate respect to students. Previous research suggests
342 III. Conflict and Relationships
higher levels of empathy are associated with decreased teacher stress (Platsidou & Agali-
otis, 2017). Other research indicates that decreases in teachers’ stress (e.g., by way of less
perceived threat in teacher–student relationships) are associated with fewer discipline
problems for their students (O’Brennan, Pas, & Bradshaw, 2017; Pas, Bradshaw, Hersh-
feldt, & Leaf, 2010). Might the empathic-discipline intervention’s effect on relationships
be in part associated with reductions in less anxiety or stress? Further, research shows
that people’s perception, judgment, and decision making are more likely to be shaped
by stereotypes when they lack cognitive resources (e.g., when stressed or exhausted; see
Spencer, Charbonneau, & Glaser, 2016). If the intervention’s effects are associated with
reductions in stress, might it also reduce the likelihood of stereotyping, as is suggested by
the reduction of troublemaker labeling?
How might the intervention affect students? When college students imagined them-
selves as receiving treatment from a teacher with the empathic mindset, they felt more
respect in the relationship and more motivation to behave well. Also, previously sus-
pended students of teachers who received the empathic-discipline intervention were more
likely to feel respect in their relationships with all adults at their school. These findings
suggest that students might experience a shift in their construal of respect throughout
the school day. Might the intervention lead students to feel less stress or anxiety in their
relationships with teachers or in school at large? Recent research suggests that such a
shift in construal can lead to long-term reductions in discipline problems (Goyer et al.,
2019).
How might the intervention’s effects extend beyond single teacher–student relation-
ships? Preliminary results mark a 50% reduction in year-long suspension rates. Also,
previously suspended students reported a heightened perception of respect several months
after teachers participated in the intervention. Might future students (e.g., the next year)
of a teacher who receives the intervention also be less likely to be suspended and more
likely to feel respect with adults at their school? Also, might students of teachers who
receive the intervention continue to be less likely to be suspended and more likely to feel
respect in future years with new teachers?
Under what conditions might the intervention not work? So far, the empathic-
discipline intervention has been tested in middle schools in adjacent districts that serve
racially diverse student populations (17% Asian, 2% Black, 54% Latino, 7% White,
20% other/unknown). Future research is needed to determine the intervention’s efficacy
in other middle school contexts and at other grade levels. For example, it will be useful to
discover how well the intervention works in schools with (1) more or less racial and socio-
economic status diversity in the student population (e.g., more Black students); (2) vari-
ous default cultural contexts, such as policies for responses to student misbehavior (see
Pre-K Student Discipline Amendment Act of 2015) and school structure and support (see
Gregory, Cornell, & Fan, 2011); and (3) various teacher characteristics, such as their race
(see Egalite, Kisida, & Winters, 2015), stress levels, job satisfaction, and burnout—each
of which has been associated with the quality of teacher–student relationships (Johnson
et al., 2009). Research suggests that factors like punitive policies and stressed teachers are
associated with more suspensions for students. The empathic-discipline intervention may
then be especially effective for teachers affected by these factors.
Answers to these questions will provide a better understanding of the mechanisms
that lead to the overall shift in how teachers and students view each other following
the intervention. It is important to better understand these mechanisms to ensure it can
The Empathic‑Discipline Intervention 343
predictably improve teacher and student outcomes in various contexts throughout the
country that suffer from high and disproportionate rates of discipline problems.
REFERENCES
Bill and Melinda Gates Foundation. (2013). Learning about teaching: Initial findings from the
Measures of Effective Teaching project. Retrieved from www.metproject.org/downloads/
Preliminary_Findings-Research_Paper.pdf.
Boynton, M. H., O’Hara, R. E., Covault, J., Scott, D., & Tennen, H. (2014). A mediation model
of racial discrimination and alcohol-related problems among African American students.
Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 76(2), 229–236.
Cohen, G., Garcia, J., Purdie-Vaughns, V., Apfel, N., & Brzustoski, P. (2009). Recursive processes
in self-affirmation: Intervening to close the minority achievement gap. Science, 324, 400–403.
Couch, K. A., & Fairlie, R. (2010). Last hired, first fired? Black–white unemployment and the
business cycle. Demography, 47(1), 227–247.
Dee, T., & Penner, E. (2016). The causal effects of cultural relevance: Evidence from an ethnic
studies curriculum (CEPA Working Paper No. 16-01). Stanford, CA: Stanford Center for
Education Policy Analysis.
Egalite, A. J., Kisida, B., & Winters, M. A. (2015). Representation in the classroom: The effect
of own-race teachers on student achievement. Economics of Education Review, 45, 44–52.
Eyal, T., Steffel, M., & Epley, N. (2018). Perspective mistaking: Accurately understanding the
mind of another requires getting perspective, not taking perspective. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 114(4), 547–571.
Fabelo, T., Thompson, M. D., Plotkin, M., Carmichael, D., Marchbanks, M. P., III, & Booth, E.
A. (2011). Breaking schools’ rules: A statewide study of how school discipline relates to stu-
dents’ success and juvenile justice involvement. Retrieved from http://justicecenter.csg.org/
resources/juveniles.
Fenning, P., & Rose, J. (2007). Overrepresentation of African American students in exclusionary
discipline: The role of school policy. Urban Education, 42(6), 536–559.
Ferguson, R. F. (2012). Can student surveys measure teaching quality? Phi Delta Kappan, 94(3),
24–28.
Finkel, E. J., Slotter, E. B., Luchies, L. B., Walton, G. M., & Gross, J. J. (2013). A brief interven-
tion to promote conflict reappraisal preserves marital quality over time. Psychological Sci-
ence, 24(8), 1595–1601.
Goldstein, N. J., Cialdini, R. B., & Griskevicius, V. (2008). A room with a viewpoint: Using social
norms to motivate environmental conservation in hotels. Journal of Consumer Research,
35(3), 472–482.
Gottfried, M. A. (2010). Evaluating the relationship between student attendance and achievement
in urban elementary and middle schools: An instrumental variables approach. American
Educational Research Journal, 47(2), 434–465.
Gregory, A., Cornell, D., & Fan, X. (2011). The relationship of school structure and support to
suspension rates for Black and White high school students. American Educational Research
Journal, 48, 904.
Gregory, A., & Weinstein, R. S. (2008). The discipline gap and African Americans: Defiance or
cooperation in the high school classroom. Journal of School Psychology, 46, 455–475.
Goyer, J. P., Cohen, G. L., Cook, J. E., Master, A., Apfel, N., Lee, W., . . . Walton, G. M. (2019).
Targeted identity-safety interventions cause lasting reductions in discipline citations among
negatively stereotyped boys. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 117(2), 229–259.
Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, R. C. (2001). Early teacher–child relationships and the trajectory of chil-
dren’s school outcomes through eighth grade. Child Development, 72(2), 625–638.
344 III. Conflict and Relationships
Harackiewicz, J. M., Rozek, C. S., Hulleman, C. S., & Hyde, J. S. (2012). Helping parents to moti-
vate adolescents in mathematics and science: An experimental test of a utility-value interven-
tion. Psychological Science, 23(8), 899–906.
Himmelstein, K. E. W., & Bruckner, H. (2011). Criminal-justice and school sanctions against non-
heterosexual youth: A national longitudinal study. Pediatrics, 127, 49–57.
Johnson, J., Yarrow, A., Rochkind, J., & Ott, A. N. (2009). Teaching for a living: How teach-
ers see the profession today. Public Agenda. Retrieved from www.publicagenda.org/pages/
teaching-for-a-living.
Jordan, W. J., Lara, J., & McPartland, J. M. (1996). Exploring the causes of early dropout among
race-ethnic and gender groups. Youth and Society, 28(1), 62–94.
Libby, L. K., & Eibach, R. P. (2011). Visual perspective in mental imagery: A representational
tool that functions in judgment, emotion, and self-insight. In M. Zanna & J. Olson (Series
Eds.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 44, pp. 185–245). San Diego, CA:
Academic Press.
Losen, D. J., & Wald, J. M. (2003). Deconstructing the school-to-prison pipeline. San Francisco:
Jossey-Bass.
Mendoza-Denton, R., Downey, G., Purdie, V. J., Davis, A., & Pietrzak, J. (2002). Sensitivity to
status-based rejection: Implications for African American students’ college experience. Jour-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 896–918.
O’Brennan, L., Pas, E., & Bradshaw, C. (2017). Multilevel examination of burnout among high
school staff: Importance of staff and school factors. School Psychology Review, 46(2), 165–
176.
Okonofua, J. A., & Eberhardt, J. L. (2015). Two strikes: Race and the disciplining of young stu-
dents. Psychological Science, 26(5), 617–624.
Okonofua, J. A., Paunesku, D., & Walton, G. M. (2016). Brief intervention to encourage empathic
discipline cuts suspension rates in half among adolescents. Proceedings of the National Acad-
emy of Sciences of the USA, 113(19), 5221–5226.
Okonofua, J. A., Walton, G. M., & Eberhardt, J. L. (2016). A vicious cycle: A social-psychological
account of extreme racial disparities in school discipline. Perspectives on Psychological Sci-
ence, 11(3), 381–398.
Outes, I., Sanchez, A., & Vakis, R. (2017). Project: Growth mindset at scale- increasing
school attainment by affecting the mindset of pupils and teachers. Retrieved from www.
riseprogramme.org/sites/www.riseprogramme.org/files/65%20Outes-Leon,%20Ingo,%20
Sanchez,%20 Alan,%20Vakis,%20Renos.%20%20Project-%20Growth%20Mindset%20
at%20Scale.pdf.
Pager, D., Western, B., & Sugie, N. (2009). Sequencing disadvantage: Barriers to employment fac-
ing young Black and White men with criminal records. Annals of the American Academy of
Social and Political Science, 623, 195–213.
Paluck, E. L., Shepherd, H., & Aronow, P. M. (2016). Changing climates of conflict: A social
network experiment in 56 schools. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
USA, 113, 566–571.
Pas, E. T., Bradshaw, C. P., Hershfeldt, P. A., & Leaf, P. J. (2010). A multilevel exploration of
the influence of teacher efficacy and burnout on response to student problem behavior and
school-based service use. School Psychology Quarterly, 25(1), 13.
Platsidou, M., & Agaliotis, I. (2017). Does empathy predict instructional assignment-related
stress?: A study in special and general education teachers. International Journal of Disability,
Development and Education, 64(1), 57–75.
Poteat, V. P., Scheer, J. R., & Chong, E. S. K. (2015). Sexual orientation-based disparities in school
and juvenile justice discipline: A multiple group comparison of contributing factors. Journal
of Educational Psychology, 108, 229–241.
The Empathic‑Discipline Intervention 345
Powers, J. T., Cook, J. E., Purdie-Vaughns, V., Garcia, J., Apfel, N., & Cohen, G. L. (2016).
Changing environments by changing individuals: The emergent effects of psychological inter-
vention. Psychological Science, 27, 150–160.
Prater, M. A., Wilder, L. K., & Dyches, T. T. (2008). Shaping one traditional special educator
preparation program toward more cultural competence. Teaching Education, 19(2), 137–151.
Pre-K Student Discipline Amendment Act of 2015. D.C. Assemb B, 21-0001 (2014-2015).
Pupil Discipline: Suspensions and Expulsions: Willful Defiance. Cal. Education Code §8900(k)
(2015).
Reinke, W. M., Herman, K. C., & Stormont, M. (2013). Classroom-level positive behavior sup-
ports in schools implementing SW-PBIS: Identifying areas for enhancement. Journal of Posi-
tive Behavior Interventions, 15(1), 39–50.
Rocque, M., & Paternoster, R. (2011). Understanding the antecedents of the “school-to-jail” link:
The relationship between race and school discipline. Journal of Criminal Law Criminology,
101(2), 633–666.
Rosenthal, R., & Jacobson, L. (1968). Pygmalion in the classroom. New York: Holt, Rinehart &
Winston.
Ross, L., & Nisbett, R. E. (1991). Person and the situation: Perspectives of social psychology.
New York: McGraw-H ill.
Rumberger, R. W., & Losen, D. J. (2017). The hidden costs of California’s harsh school dis-
cipline: And the localized economic benefits from suspending fewer high school students
(Civil Rights Project/Proyecto Derechos Civiles). Los Angeles: Civil Rights Project/Proyecto
Derechos Civiles.
Skiba, R. J. (2014). The failure of zero tolerance. Reclaiming Children and Youth, 22(4), 27.
Skiba, R. J., Horner, R. H., Chung, C., Rausch, M. K., May, S. L., & Tobin, T. (2011). Race is
not neutral: A national investigation of African American and Latino disproportionality in
school discipline. School Psychological Review, 40(1), 85–107.
Spencer, K. B., Charbonneau, A. K., & Glaser, J. (2016). Implicit bias and policing. Social and
Personality Psychology Compass, 10(1), 50–63.
Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and perfor-
mance. American Psychologist, 52(6), 613–629.
U.S. Department of Education, Office of Civil Rights. (2016, June). 2013–2014 Civil rights data
collection: Key data highlights on equity and opportunity gaps in our nation’s public schools.
Retrieved from www2.ed.gov/about/offices/list/ocr/docs/crdc-2013-14.html.
U.S. Government Department of Education. (2014). Guided principles: A resource guide for
improving school climate and discipline. Washington, DC: Author.
Walton, G. M., & Cohen, G. L. (2011). A brief social-belonging intervention improves academic
and health outcomes among minority students. Science, 331, 1447–1451.
Walton, G. M., & Wilson, T. D. (2018). Wise interventions: Psychological remedies for social and
personal problems. Psychological Review, 125(5), 617.
Yeager, D. S., Purdie-Vaughns, V., Garcia, J., Apfel, N., Brzustoski, P., Master, A., . . . Williams,
M. E. (2014). Breaking the cycle of mistrust: Wise interventions to provide critical feedback
across the racial divide. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(2), 804–824.
Yeager, D. S., & Walton, G. M. (2011). Social-psychological interventions in education: They’re
not magic. Review of Educational Research, 81, 267–301.
C H A P TE R 15
In the past few decades, social scientists have sought new tools to address and change
psychological barriers to conflict resolution. One such barrier is group members’ resistance
to changes that promote peace, due to the fear that such changes may lead to disappoint-
ment or be perceived as weakness by the outgroup. A suggested solution to this barrier
involves manipulating individuals’ beliefs regarding the possibility for group change. In the
present study, a field intervention involving Israelis (N = 508) from three locations in Israel
extended previous lab findings by testing the durability of a group-malleability interven-
tion over a 6-month period of frequent violence. Three different 5-hour interventions were
administered as leadership workshops: The group-malleability intervention was compared to
a neutral coping-with-stress intervention and, importantly, to a state-of-the-art perspective-
taking intervention. The group-malleability intervention proved superior to the coping inter-
vention in improving attitudes, hope, and willingness to make concessions, and maintained
this advantage over a 6-month period of intense intergroup conflict. Moreover, it was as
good as, and in some respects superior to, the perspective-taking intervention. These find-
ings provide the first naturalistic examination of the potential of a group-malleability interven-
tion to increase openness to conflict resolution.
BACKGROUND
346
The Group‑Malleability Intervention 347
Recent analyses suggest that the number of state-based armed conflicts has reached a
new peak since the collapse of the Soviet Union (Melander, Pettersson, & Themner,
2016), supporting the notion that we are in the midst of a global escalation of intergroup
conflicts. These recent developments emphasize the importance of finding ways to atten-
uate the destructive effects of intergroup conflicts.
In the past few decades, social scientists have sought new tools to address and change
psychological barriers to conflict resolution (Hameiri, Porat, Bar-Tal, Bieler, & Halperin,
2014; Paluck, 2009). The assumption that undergirds this approach is that conflicts are
fueled and perpetuated by certain psychological barriers that may arise as an attempt to
cope with the challenging reality of living in a conflict (Bar-Tal, 2007; Ross & Stittinger,
1991). One such barrier is the resistance to change toward peace, due to the fear that any
change would lead to disappointment or be perceived as weakness and be taken advan-
tage of by the outgroup (Bar-Tal, 2013; Bar-Tal, Oren, & Nets-Zehngut, 2014). The goal
of the current intervention is to attempt to influence a belief that seems to be central to
groups’ resistance to change, which is the belief that the other side will never change.
PSYCHOLOGICAL PROCESSES
One central mindset commonly held by members of groups involved in intractable con-
flicts is the notion that the other side is inherently malevolent and will never change
(Halperin, 2008; Peterson, 2002). Believing that the other side will never change serves
a variety of adaptive functions. First, it shields the individual from disappointment. If
one expects nothing from the other side, one cannot be disappointed by any violent or
harmful actions taken by the other side (Norem & Cantor, 1986). Second, it helps the
individual to justify aggression and violence that are perpetrated by one’s own group
toward the other side (Bar-Tal, 2013; Bar-Tal et al., 2014). If those on the other side are
inherently evil and will never change, they must be resisted by force if necessary, and no
other means (e.g., negotiations, gestures) can be beneficial.
The mindset that the other side cannot change is central to the narratives of groups
involved in intractable conflicts. Indeed, this mindset is often echoed by the leaders of
these groups—for example, take the famous quote by former Israeli Prime Minister
Yitzhak Shamir, who, on the brink of the Madrid Peace Conference in 1991, said, “The
sea is the same sea, and the Arabs are the same Arabs,” implying that just as the sea never
changes, so too will the Palestinians never change (Goldenberg, 2012). In 2012, Israeli
Prime Minister Binyamin Netanyahu reiterated Shamir’s quote, suggesting he was right
in his evaluations of the Palestinians (Podolsky, 2012). These quotes by Israeli leaders
represent a narrative held by many people in Israel and in other countries involved in
conflicts, which is that the other side will never change.
Believing that the other side will always stay the same is a psychological moat that
helps to keep victims of conflicts away from the stress and distress of the conflict, but also
one that hinders progress toward peace. If there is no chance that the Palestinians will
change, why even try (Bar-Tal & Halperin, 2011; Bar-Tal, Halperin, & Pliskin, 2015)?
If one believes that the other side can never change, investing efforts in actions that may
lead to a reduction in violence or even to reconciliation is perceived as useless. Instead, it
makes more sense to invest resources in order to eliminate potential threats using force
and aggression. The belief that the other side will not change may serve short-term goals
348 III. Conflict and Relationships
apology by the outgroup would lead to intergroup forgiveness. This research laid the
psychological foundation from which the group-malleability intervention was designed.
EMPIRICAL EVIDENCE
Although there has been ample lab-based evidence for the effectiveness of a group-
malleability intervention, to date there has not been any intervention in a field setting
that examined the long-term effects of a group-malleability manipulation. This interven-
tion was therefore initiated in order to learn more about the potential of using group
malleability as a tool to change conflict-related attitudes, emotions, and perhaps even
willingness to make concessions in the field. The group-malleability intervention was
compared to two other interventions. The first one was a perspective-taking intervention,
which focused on the importance of taking the other side’s perspective when leading a
group, even in challenging situations. It was chosen because of its suggested potential for
conflict resolution and prejudice reduction (Bilali & Vollhardt, 2013; Galinsky & Mos-
kowitz, 2000). However, after a series of three pilot studies, it was altered to enhance its
effectiveness by de-emphasizing perspective taking in the local Israeli–Palestinian context
(which evoked resistance from some participants; Sassenrath, Hodges, & Pfattheicher,
2016; Vorauer & Sasaki, 2009; Zaki & Cikara, 2015) and instead discussed the impor-
tance of perspective taking in general. The second intervention was a coping-with-stress
intervention that was chosen as a control comparison to both the group-malleability
and perspective-taking interventions. The coping-with-stress intervention was designed
to teach participants useful coping skills to overcome stressors that leaders often encoun-
ter. Coping was chosen and prepiloted as a control condition because it provided useful
leadership-related skills, but also because it was unrelated to group-malleability or con-
flict resolution. Furthermore, a similar workshop has been used as a neutral control con-
dition for a malleability intervention (Yeager, Trzesniewski, & Dweck, 2013). In order to
examine the longer-term effects of these interventions, participants’ attitudes and behav-
ior were measured in five time points, up to 6 months after the intervention.
Outcomes
We recruited a sample of Jewish–Israeli participants (N = 508) from around the country
with diverse backgrounds, ages, and political affiliations (191 males and 317 females,
M = 28.81 years, SD = 8.69). Participants’ attitudes, hope, and support for conciliatory
behavior were measured one time before the intervention and four times following the
intervention: postworkshop, 2 weeks, 2 months, and 6 months after the intervention.
Some participants dropped out of the study during each follow-up period, leaving us with
59% of the sample in the 6-month follow-up (N = 300). However, no differences were
found in attrition rate between the groups. The period in which the data were collected
was relatively a tumultuous period in Israel, often referred to as the “silent intifada”
(Caspit, 2014), in which there were multiple terrorist attacks in Israel, conducted primar-
ily by desperate individual Palestinians who were not directly sent by a known terrorist
organization.
We first examined differences in participants’ negative attitudes toward the Palestin-
ians. The scale that was used to estimate these negative attitudes was similar to the one
used in Halperin and colleagues’ (2011) lab study. The scale focused primarily on the
350 III. Conflict and Relationships
perception that Palestinians are inherently malevolent and included items such as “To
what extent would you say that the Palestinians are evil?” Looking first at the differences
between our three conditions after the intervention, results suggested that participants
in the malleability intervention held significantly fewer negative attitudes compared to
the control coping intervention (see Figure 15.1, Panel A). Analysis suggested no differ-
ence between the group-malleability intervention and the perspective-taking intervention
(which was also significantly better compared to the control condition).
Following this comparison of participants’ negative attitudes, we examined differ-
ences in participants’ hope regarding a shared future with the Palestinians. We focused
our attention on hope, as previous work suggested that it was an important outcome
of group-malleability manipulations (Cohen-Chen, Halperin, Saguy, & van Zomeren,
2014; Wohl et al., 2015). Comparing the group-malleability and coping conditions fol-
lowing the intervention suggested that participants in the group-malleability interven-
tion reported significantly higher hope compared to the control coping intervention (see
Figure 15.1, Panel B). Unlike in the case of negative attitudes, we did find a significant
difference between the two interventions. These findings point to at least one outcome
that seems to differentiate between our group-malleability and perspective-taking inter-
ventions.
Influencing psychological constructs, such as negative attitudes and hope for the
future, do not necessarily translate into change in support for conciliatory behavior. We
therefore examined three such measures. The first was a scale that examined support for
a two-state solution. A two-state solution represented the most popular solution for the
Israeli–Palestinian conflict when the original study was conducted in 2011. The general
principles of the two-state solution include an establishment of a Palestinian state, a divi-
sion of Jerusalem, territory exchanges between Israel and Palestine, and a symbolic pay-
ment of reparations by Israel to Palestinian refugees. Between 2010 and the end of 2014
(which is when our intervention was conducted), dramatic changes in public opinion led
FIGURE 15.1. Average ratings of participants’ negative attitudes (A) and hope (B) at each point
for all three conditions, obtained from the longitudinal model. Error bars are standard errors. For
the sake of simplicity, the distance between points has been constrained to be equal despite the
differences in time.
The Group‑Malleability Intervention 351
to shifts in terms of public support for that idea, which rendered the items on our old
scale obsolete (Hilal, 2007). This is because some major parties in the Israeli government
had completely abandoned the two-state solution as a viable resolution to the conflict.
Looking at differences among all of our intervention participants’ support for a two-
state solution revealed no difference between any of our conditions. To overcome this
limitation, we created a more timely measure of concessions that included symbolic ges-
tures, such as acknowledgment of Palestine as an independent and sovereign country, and
allowing the Palestinian soccer team to play in international games. Indeed, our modified
concession scales revealed a significant difference between the group-malleability and
the coping interventions, but not between the perspective-taking and group-malleability
interventions, providing initial evidence that our intervention was able to shift at least
more symbolic concessions compared to the traditional two-state solution measure.
In addition to our concession measures, we wanted to examine whether our interven-
tion actually affected participants’ decisions involving dividing resources between Israelis
and Palestinians. We therefore added two conciliatory behavior measures that were influ-
enced by economics games. The first such measure was a dictator game (Bolton, Katok,
& Zwick, 1998), in which participants were asked to divide resources worth hundreds of
millions of Israeli shekels (NIS) between Israelis and Palestinians. Similar to our revised
concessions measure, we found significant differences between the group-malleability
and the coping interventions, but no differences between the group-malleability and the
perspective-taking interventions (see Figure 15.2). Our second measure was modeled on a
trust game (Berg, Dickhaut, & McCabe, 1995), in which participants were actually given
10 NIS (approximately $3.00), and were asked to transfer some or all of the amount to
a Palestinian player, in the hope that the Palestinian player would reciprocate. Partici-
pants were told that any amount that would be given back to them by their Palestinian
confederate would be tripled before being given to them. Therefore, if they trusted that
their partner would reciprocate, it made sense to share all of their money with their con-
federate with the hope that they would make more than the initial 10 NIS that were given
to them. Results suggested that participants in the group-malleability intervention gave
significantly more money to their Palestinian confederate than participants in either the
coping intervention or the perspective-taking intervention (see Figure 15.2).
Overall, these results point to the difference between the malleability and coping
conditions on all measures except for support for a two-state solution. We found sig-
nificant differences between the group-malleability and perspective-taking interventions
only in participants’ hope and in the trust game. Further research should be done to
examine whether these effects are replicated in other field interventions—however, both
of these results point to a difference in participants’ ability to be optimistic in estimating
outcomes from interacting with the other side. These findings are congruent with recent
work that shows that a group-malleability intervention mainly influenced Israeli and
Palestinian participants’ ability to cooperate with each other (Goldenberg et al., 2017).
Mechanisms
The group-malleability intervention influenced participants’ perceptions regarding the
possibility of group change (assessed in a manipulation check). We believe that this gen-
eral perception is relatively easy to influence, as it is an implicit belief that is not con-
stantly reevaluated and reaffirmed as are other beliefs, such as ideology or religiosity
352 III. Conflict and Relationships
FIGURE 15.2. Performance in two decision-making tasks (standardized) at the 6-month follow-
p. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals.
u
*
–.42*
)
(.12
* ** Hope
–.4
. 37 (6 months)
4* .33**
6)
* (–.1
*
–.11*
–
Malleability mindset –.35** (–.17) New concessions
*
vs. coping condition (6 months)
–. 3 0*
(–.16)
–. 5
9 **
* (– Dictator game
.48
–. 2 6
* **) (6 months)
* **
Trust game
(6 months)
FIGURE 15.3. Mediation model for all outcomes measured at the 6-month follow-up. We used
the postworkshop measure of malleability beliefs as a mediator of outcomes at the 6-month fol-
low-up. Results suggested a significant indirect effect for all measured outcomes. p ≤ .10, p ≤ .05*,
p ≤ .01**, p ≤ .001***.
were irrelevant and therefore are not presented. Overall, results suggest significant indi-
rect effects for all of our outcomes, indicating that differences in malleability beliefs
between the malleability and coping conditions in the postworkshop measure mediated
all of the outcomes in the 6-month follow-up.
Effects over Time
So far, we have compared the three interventions by aggregating all postworkshop mea-
sures that were taken following the intervention. However, since we have four measures
that were taken after the workshops (postworkshop, 2 weeks, 2 months, and 6 months),
we can also examine changes in our outcomes over time. We focused our analyses on
participants’ hope and negative attitudes toward Palestinians, which were measured dur-
ing these four follow-ups. Importantly, in our analysis, we examined models that allowed
varied changes across time points (growth curve analysis). However, in most of these
cases, model comparisons suggested that a simple linear model was strongest compared
to all other models. Finally, similar to all other analyses, we controlled for premeasures
of each outcome in the analysis.
Results revealed no change over time in negative attitudes in the group-malleability
condition, suggesting that the changes that occurred following the intervention were
sustained over time. Indeed, comparing negative attitudes in the 6-month follow-ups
between the group- malleability and coping interventions suggested that differences
were still significant. There were still no differences between the group-malleability and
perspective-taking conditions (which were also significantly lower than the control con-
dition; see Figure 15.1, Panel A). Unlike negative attitudes, participants’ hope did seem
354 III. Conflict and Relationships
to significantly decline in the postworkshop measures. One potential reason for this
decrease is the increase in terrorist attacks that occurred during these 6 months that may
have specifically affected participants’ hope regarding the future. However, even after
this decline, participants’ ratings were still significantly higher compared to the prework-
shop measures and compared to both the perspective-taking and coping interventions.
Heterogeneity
One important question regarding the heterogeneity of the group-malleability interven-
tion is whether its impact differs among participants with different political affiliations.
Two opposite hypotheses are offered. First, it is possible that liberals are more influenced
by a group-malleability intervention, as a plethora of work suggests that liberals are more
susceptible to the notion of change (Graham, Haidt, & Nosek, 2009; Jost, Federico, &
Napier, 2009; Schein & Gray, 2015). However, liberals’ susceptibility to the notion of
change may mean that the impact of the intervention is weaker, since perceptions of mal-
leability are already an integral part of their thinking (Kahn et al., 2018). The current
sample is big enough to examine this important question, which bears on the target audi-
ence of such an intervention.
To address this question, we divided our participants into three political groups:
liberals, centrists, and conservatives. We decided to use a centrist group because unlike
the United States, the Israeli political system includes many different parties and a large
portion of the population considers itself as centrist. For each of these three groups, we
compared the levels of hope and negative attitudes across the group-malleability and
control coping interventions. Results suggest that the difference between the incremental
and coping intervention groups was mainly driven by differences within rightists and
centrists. In both cases, we found no significant difference between the incremental and
coping conditions for liberals. These results suggest that the malleability intervention
may be especially useful for conservatives, which suggests the possibility that targeting
conservatives might lead to maximal changes.
COUSINS
The idea that changing perceptions of group malleability can influence people’s emotions,
attitudes, and behaviors toward other group members can be associated with two types
of cousins. The first and most obvious cousin is an intervention that is focused on chang-
ing perceptions of malleability of the self and others as a tool to motivate individuals to
confront challenging situations. The second cousin is an intervention that is focused on
changing one’s perception of the outgroup, either through interaction or through per-
spective taking. Here we review both of these approaches.
The idea that perceptions of malleability can serve as a tool for intervention was
developed by Carol Dweck, who suggested that beliefs regarding one’s own malleability
were a factor influencing people’s motivation, especially in light of challenging tasks (see
Dweck, 2006, 2012, for reviews). Focusing initially on the domain of education, Dweck
argued and showed that children who perceived their intelligence as malleable (compared
to those who perceived their intelligence as fixed) were more motivated to overcome edu-
cational challenges that threatened their sense of self (Hong, Chiu, Dweck, Lin, & Wan,
The Group‑Malleability Intervention 355
1999; Nussbaum & Dweck, 2008). These children therefore showed improved perfor-
mance in school compared to children who perceived their intelligence as fixed. Dweck
not only showed that believing in malleability is associated with group performance but
also that this belief can be manipulated, and that changing students’ malleability beliefs
can thus improve their performance in school (Blackwell, Trzesniewski, & Dweck, 2007;
Kamins & Dweck, 1999).
Dweck’s insights in the domain of education were then examined in the context
of interpersonal relations. For example, in a series of studies, Chiu, Dweck, Tong, and
Fu (1997) showed that people who perceived others’ personalities as fixed tended to
make more strict trait judgments regarding other people, whereas those with incremental
beliefs were more lenient in their judgment of others (Chiu, Hong, & Dweck, 1997). Fur-
ther interventions were conducted by Yeager and colleagues, exploring the contribution
of perceived malleability to interpersonal relations. In a series of projects, Yeager and
colleagues showed that convincing participants that other people can change was associ-
ated with reduced aggression (Yeager, Trzesniewski, et al., 2013), hostility (Yeager, Miu,
Powers, & Dweck, 2013), and vengeance after peer conflict (Yeager, Trzesniewski, Tirri,
Nokelainen, & Dweck, 2011). Overall, this large body of work provides ample evidence
of the potential of perceived malleability in improving interpersonal relations, especially
in cases of challenging interactions. We assume some overlap between the individual and
the group-malleability interventions. Naturally, to believe that groups can change one
has to believe in change among individuals as well. At the same time, we believe that
changes in intergroup emotions and beliefs are driven mainly by people’s views regarding
groups as separate entities and that explains part of the potential embedded in the group-
malleability interventions, as supported by the data presented above.
Beyond interventions that focus on the notion of malleability of individuals and
groups, there are many intergroup interventions that attempt to change people’s inherent
perception of their outgroup, often with an emphasis on the similarities between one’s
ingroup and the outgroup. These interventions can be divided into a few subtypes. The
first type involves attempting to convince group members that they are actually similar
to the outgroup. For example, priming common ingroup identity among racial groups
leads to a reduction in intergroup bias (see Gaertner, Dovidio, Anastasio, Bachman, &
Rust, 1993, for a review). Even convincing rival groups that they have genetic similarities
can also reduce explicit bias and in turn lead to an increase in support for peacemaking
(Kimel, Huesmann, Kunst, & Halperin, 2016). Eliciting a sense of similarity seems to be
efficient in blurring intergroup boundaries and thus changing group members’ percep-
tions of their ingroup.
A second and related type of intervention does not explicitly try to convince group
members of similarity to the outgroup but rather asks group members to take the per-
spective of the outgroup, either with or without the presence of the outgroup. This is done
with the hope that taking the other side’s perspective would either lead to emphasizing the
similarities between the experiences of both groups or at least to a greater understanding
of their position. For example, in a study by Broockman and Kalla (2016), canvassers
asked voters to think about a time when they themselves were judged negatively for being
different, in order to emphasize their similarity with transgender people, showing that
this had an effect on attitudes toward transgendered individuals. In general, perspective-
taking interventions seem to lead to a variety of positive outcomes, including decreases
in prejudice (Galinsky & Moskowitz, 2000), increases in ingroup help (Batson, Chang,
356 III. Conflict and Relationships
Orr, & Rowland, 2002; Bilewicz, 2009), and increased willingness to engage in contact
(Wang, Kenneth, Ku, & Galinsky, 2014). Importantly, however, some research suggests
that perspective-taking interventions may backfire. One reason for this is that when par-
ticipants are asked to take the perspective of the outgroup, they either realize how big the
gap in views between them and their outgroup actually is, or focus on how much effort
would be required to bridge the gap between the two groups (see Maoz, 2011; Sassenrath
et al., 2016; Vorauer & Sasaki, 2009; Zaki & Cikara, 2015, for similar arguments).
The perspective-taking workshop used in the current project took advantage of
insights from these studies. Participants were told that taking others’ perspective is an
important way to understand their motivations and needs and therefore an extremely
important tool for decision making. Indeed, we found that our perspective-taking inter-
vention led to reduced negative attitudes compared to our control coping intervention
and to increased willingness to make concessions. We did find in our pilot studies, how-
ever, that mentioning the local context of the conflict led to a reversed order and to an
increase in negative attitudes. These findings reveal a potential moderator to perspective-
taking outcomes.
Each group-malleability workshop was 5 hours long and included a preworkshop ques-
tionnaire (30 minutes), general leadership content (similar to all conditions, 1.5 hours),
and the relevant content of each condition (3 hours; see Table 15.1). The general leader-
ship content section was identical in all three groups and was inspired by a leadership
development model created by the Harvard Kennedy School’s Center for Public Leader-
ship. Participants learned about transformational leaders (Bass, 1985) and how different
leadership styles influence group performance. After the general leadership content, the
9:30–10:30 General leadership content Identical in all three groups and inspired by a leadership
development model created by the Harvard Kennedy
School’s Center for Public Leadership.
10:30–11:00 Break
11:00–12:15 Intervention content, part 1 Introduction of the central idea of the intervention
using a story line, a video, and an active discussion.
12:15–12:30 Break
workshop shifted to fit with each of the conditions. The structure of the interventions
was similar for all conditions in terms of schedule and type of activities—however, the
main message of each intervention was tailored to each condition. In addition to making
sure that the structure of the interventions would be similar, a second goal was that these
three interventions would be similar in how much they were engaging and entertaining
for participants. Indeed, our tests of participants’ engagements suggested that partici-
pants enjoyed our interventions in a similar way.
The malleability workshop focused on the benefits of remaining a relevant leader in
an ever-changing context, highlighting the importance of identifying and encouraging
change in groups. In the first part of this section of the workshop, participants were told
that they would focus on a specific aspect of leadership—namely, the ability to identify
and facilitate group change. To open this segment, the instructors presented the story of
Amir, head of a research and development group in a large company who failed to recog-
nize positive changes in his employees. Ignoring his employees’ changes decreased Amir’s
relevance and eventually led to a deterioration in his relationship with them. After going
over the story, instructors led a discussion on the importance of believing in change, iden-
tifying it, and facilitating it. Next, participants were introduced to the concept of change
in human development and then learned about brain plasticity (by watching a short video).
Moving from the individual to the group level, participants learned about the possibility
of change in groups, focusing on different aspects that groups could change, such as their
stereotypes of others or of themselves, their ideologies, and their lifestyle. Participants
were asked to provide examples in each domain following a discussion with the group.
Instructors then provided participants with their own examples for each of these domains.
The second part of the workshop (after the second break) focused on group change
and leadership. Workshop instructors emphasized the fact that change is never easy, and
that leaders must be willing to embrace and facilitate transformations in order to remain
relevant. Participants were given three examples of leaders who were able to identify and
amplify group change: Steve Jobs, Martin Luther King Jr., and Ellen Johnson Sirleaf.
Steve Jobs, Apple’s legendary chief executive officer (CEO), recognized changes in con-
sumer preference in the domain of personal computing and developed products that fit
these emerging needs. Martin Luther King Jr., a leader and activist for race equality in
the United States, was able to recognize that affordances created societal changes within
the United States that allowed him to lead a successful collective action movement call-
ing for equality. Ellen Johnson Sirleaf, the first female head of state in Africa, was able
to recognize and capitalize on important changes in her home country of Liberia and
lead the country to a better future. We chose these three figures as they could serve
as excellent examples not only for the group-malleability interventions but also to the
perspective-taking and coping interventions. Indeed, in each workshop we used the same
three people, focusing the participants’ attention on different aspects in these three fig-
ures’ biography, behaviors, and statements.
Following this section, participants took part in a simulated negotiation between
the management group of a paper facility and the facility’s union representatives. The
context of the negotiation was the need to cut costs in order to improve the facility’s
profits. Participants were divided into three groups: management, employees, and observ-
ers. Each group was given different materials prior to the negotiation. After conduct-
ing the negotiation, participants analyzed the position of each group, emphasizing the
notion of group change. For example, one of the key points during the negotiation was
358 III. Conflict and Relationships
management’s belief in the employees’ ability to change a few crucial work-related norms
in order to improve production.
Finally, to conclude the workshop, we focused on the change in groups that can lead
to improved intergroup relations. A few historical examples were used, including changes
in European countries such as England and France that led to tremendous improvements
in their relationship, changes in Arab society that led to the Arab Spring, and the con-
flict in Ireland. For example, when examining the story of the Irish conflict, partici-
pants learned about the bloody history between the unionists (mostly Protestants) and
the Irish nationalists (mostly Catholics). They then learned about changes occurring in
Ireland, and especially change in the approach of the Sinn Féin political party that led to
a greater willingness for peace. Participants observed the processes that occurred within
each group, leading to these intergroup changes.
NUANCES AND MISCONCEPTIONS
Two nuances played an important role in the success of our group-malleability inter-
vention. The first nuance is that the workshops were framed as a leadership workshop
rather than a conflict-resolution intervention. Both during the recruiting and during the
workshops, participants were explicitly told that they were taking part in a pilot study
designed to examine the utility of different leadership workshops. Leadership was chosen
as a focus to provide an appealing framework that would be broad enough to encom-
pass all three interventions. We believe that using the leadership framework was a good
strategy, as it reduced potential reactance to the notion of conflict resolution (Bar-Tal &
Halperin, 2011; Brehm, 1966).
The second and perhaps more important nuance of our workshop is that we did not
mention the specific context of the Israeli–Palestinian conflict throughout the interven-
tion but rather talked about group malleability in general and used examples that were
far from the local context. This decision was made after we conducted two pilot studies
to test whether mentioning the Israeli–Palestinian conflict would be helpful or harmful.
Results of our pilot studies indicated that not mentioning the Israeli context yielded the
best outcomes in changing participants’ negative attitudes toward Palestinians, increas-
ing participants’ hope regarding a mutual future, and increasing willingness to make
concessions. In fact, in pilot studies in which the Israeli–Palestinian conflict was highly
emphasized during the intervention, we even saw an increase in negative attitudes for
some of the participants. We therefore believe that not mentioning the specific context of
the conflict was crucial to the success of our intervention.
IMPLICATIONS FOR PRACTICE
Our findings provide encouraging evidence for the possibility of using the group-
malleability intervention to reduce negative attitudes, increase hope, and increase the
willingness to make concessions necessary to promote the resolution of protracted inter-
group conflict. Our longitudinal analysis suggests that changing people’s perceptions
of group malleability may have long-lasting effects. With this knowledge at hand, it is
important to consider how our intervention can be adapted by practitioners and used in
tandem with other known interventions. Next we examine some of these opportunities.
The Group‑Malleability Intervention 359
Adoption at Scale
As part of the process of adopting our interventions with a larger audience, we are enthu-
siastic about the possibility of examining the effects of our group-malleability interven-
tion in other challenging intergroup contexts. For example, our workshop could poten-
tially be used to train professionals who are vulnerable to developing intergroup bias in
their daily lives. Police officers, teachers, nurses and doctors, judges, and many other
professionals whose work is highly affected by their relations to negatively stereotyped
groups could potentially benefit from our workshops. The general message of the work-
shop has the advantage, as discussed above, that it does not raise the resistance that these
professionals may have to (or the offense they might take from) a direct message about
changing their attitudes or reducing their prejudice (Paluck, 2009). We have already
started using such workshops in Israel, as part of the work of aChord: Social Psychology
for Social Change, a research-based organization aimed at promoting tolerant equal and
respectful relations among different groups within the Israeli society. Under the umbrella
of that organization, training and workshops are provided to school principals, teachers,
and business managers in an attempt to reduce stereotypes and promote less biased and
more harmonious relations among different identity groups within the Israeli society.
In addition to using our workshops in a classroom setting, another area of growth
for our intervention is in finding ways to convey the message of our intervention using
other modes of communication. The most readily available option is to conduct the
group-malleability intervention online. Indeed, there has been recent growth in the use of
psychological interventions—and specifically mindset interventions—online (Paunesku
et al., 2015; Yeager et al., 2016). We believe that as in those studies, the general message
of group malleability can be effective even if disseminated online. However, future work
should examine the aspects of the intervention that are lost in the transition to an online
intervention. For example, based on our initial analysis, it seems that participants who
came to our workshop with someone they knew were more affected by its content. Such
effects are probably less strong in online interventions, which might diminish the overall
impact of a group-malleability intervention delivered online.
In addition to conducting our intervention online, the general message of group mal-
leability might be disseminated in other ways that could reach a larger audience. For
example, one of the big supporters of the idea of group malleability as a motivating tool
for conflict resolution is the Israeli president Reuven Rivlin, who initiated a large social
initiative focusing on change beliefs under the name “Israeli Hope.” In a recent speech,
Rivlin (2015) said, “I stand as one who really believes. Not because there is no choice, but
because right before my eyes I see a process of change taking place.” In addition to being
used by public figures, our interventions might also be used as an advertising campaign
for organizations interested in using efficient messaging in order to change population-
level attitudes in the contexts of conflicts (see, e.g., Hameiri, Porat, Bar-Tal, & Halperin,
2016). It is still unclear how changing the method of dissemination would affect the
impact of the intervention, and future work will be needed to examine these effects.
may enhance the impact of other well-known interventions as it would increase group
members’ hopefulness regarding a potential mutually beneficial outcome. One type of
intervention that might especially benefit from group-malleability interventions is the
contact-encounter intervention. Contact is perhaps the most used and well-researched
approach to alleviate tension and reduce prejudice between groups (Paluck, Green, &
Green, 2019; Tropp, 2015; Zhou, Page-Gould, Aron, Moyer, & Hewstone, 2018). The
idea behind contact encounters is that by meeting members of the opposite side in a con-
structive environment, group members may be able to develop a better understanding of
the other side, as well as acknowledge similarities between the two groups.
Since the contact approach was first suggested (Allport, 1954; Pettigrew, 1998; Petti-
grew & Tropp, 2006; Williams, 1947), its popularity has increased substantially (Hews-
tone & Brown, 1986; Maoz & McCauley, 2008; Pettigrew & Tropp, 2006). In Israel, for
example, a series of representative public opinion surveys conducted during 2002–2005
indicated that about 16% of the entire population of Israel had attended organized con-
tact workshops (Maoz, 2011). However, alongside optimism regarding the potentially
positive outcomes of contact, there have been some findings that suggest that contact may
not be as useful in leading to real and long-term change as hoped, especially in intergroup
conflicts (MacInnis & Page-Gould, 2015; Maoz, 2011; Paluck et al., 2019). One of the
challenges that has been suggested in the literature is that during contact encounters,
group members learn about the position of the other side, which makes them realize how
challenging reconciliation will be for the two groups. It seems possible, therefore, that
changing perceptions of group malleability may be useful to alleviate some of these con-
cerns by making the possibility of group change more readily available.
We recently conducted an intervention that provides an initial investigation of the pos-
sibility of extending our intervention to the Palestinian population (Goldenberg et al., 2017).
This intervention was designed with the intention of using a group-malleability manipu-
lation as a way to improve a contact encounter between Jewish Israeli and Palestinian–
Israeli adolescents. Both Jewish and Palestinian students were assigned to either a group-
malleability or a coping intervention. Unlike the adult intervention that lasted 5 hours, the
content of these workshops was divided into three separate meetings that were designed
to prepare participants for a fourth contact encounter between those schools. During the
contact encounter, participants played a few introduction games, as well as completed a
set of tasks that measured their ability to cooperate with one another. Results suggested
that participants’ performance in the group-malleability condition was significantly bet-
ter compared to the coping condition (Goldenberg et al., 2017). Although these findings
cannot directly speak to the impact of the intervention on the Palestinian participants com-
pared to the Israeli participants, the results are encouraging. Further work should be done
to examine the specific effects of our intervention on Palestinian participants.
IMPLICATIONS FOR PSYCHOLOGICAL THEORY
One of the more influential insights from psychology is that we operate in the world on
the basis of how we perceive the world to be (rather than how the world actually is). From
a motivational perspective, our motivation to influence the world around us is deter-
mined in part by our estimation of whether we are actually able to impact the world or
not (Bandura, 1997, 2006). Evidence for this insight can also be seen at the group level:
groups are more likely to support a collective-action movement if they believe that it may
The Group‑Malleability Intervention 361
change the reality and lead to the desired outcome (van Zomeren, Postmes, & Spears,
2008). The belief that reality can change increases the motivation to change it and forms
a self-perpetuating cycle of change.
But the belief that one can influence the world is conditioned on a more basic belief
that change and development are possible (Dweck, 2000, 2006). If a person does not
believe that people can change in general, then it’s likely that they won’t believe that
investing energy in a specific situation may lead to change. A similar rationale can be
applied to the group context. If group members hold the belief that other groups cannot
change their inherent nature, this is most likely to affect their belief regarding a specific
group change. Our current intervention provides further evidence relevant to this theo-
retical background and to the fact that changing this belief can have important down-
stream implications.
FUTURE DIRECTIONS
The current intervention opens many interesting questions that call for further research.
Here we present two main next steps that are currently planned. The first step involves
further understanding of the active ingredient in our group-malleability manipulation.
One important question that stems from our work is when participants are led to believe
that groups can change, do they focus their attention on their own group or on the other
group? Groups in conflicts often develop strong systems of moral justification for their
behavior (Bar-Tal, 2007). It is therefore likely that group members think to themselves,
“My group is completely OK, but now there is hope that the other side could change.”
We therefore hope to investigate this question and further examine whether convinc-
ing group members that their own group can also change may increase or decrease the
impact of the intervention.
The second future direction involves the broader impacts of our intervention for
those not directly targeted. The current work was done with a focus on just the individu-
als who took part in the interventions, ignoring their social networks. However, just like
other behaviors, interventions may also spread (see, e.g., Centola, 2010). Participants in
our intervention may share the content they learn with friends or family, and these people
may either be influenced by the message, resist its effect, or even polarize their own opin-
ion as a reaction to the message. Learning more about the potential impacts on others of
the content of our intervention is key to leading a real change in the Israeli society. In a
recent project, we began examining this question by creating predesigned social networks
to manipulate and monitor real-time interactions among group members (Centola, 2010;
Coman, Momennejad, Drach, & Geana, 2016). A certain portion of our network will go
through our intervention, which allows us to examine its effects on their social network.
Overall, the impact that our intervention has on influencing challenging intergroup rela-
tions motivates us to continue our research in this domain with the hope that it could be
utilized for conflict resolution and also for real group change.
REFERENCES
of immoral groups facilitate collective action. Social Psychological and Personality Science,
5(2), 203–210.
Coman, A., Momennejad, I., Drach, R. D., & Geana, A. (2016). Mnemonic convergence in social
networks: The emergent properties of cognition at a collective level. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 113(29), 8171–8176.
Dweck, C. S. (2000). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. Phila-
delphia: Psychology Press.
Dweck, C. S. (2006). Mindset: The new psychology of success. New York: Random House.
Dweck, C. S. (2012). Mindsets and human nature: Promoting change in the Middle East: The
schoolyard, the racial divide, and willpower. American Psychologist, 67(8), 614–622.
Gaertner, S. L., Dovidio, J. F., Anastasio, P. A., Bachman, B. A., & Rust, M. C. (1993). The com-
mon ingroup identity model: Recategorization and the reduction of intergroup bias. Euro-
pean Review of Social Psychology, 4(1), 1–26.
Galinsky, A. D., & Moskowitz, G. B. (2000). Perspective-taking: Decreasing stereotype expres-
sion, stereotype accessibility, and in-group favoritism. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 78(4), 708–724.
Goldenberg, A. (2012, April 7). How convenient. Haaretz. Retrieved from https://www.haaretz.
co.il/opinions/1.1747338.
Goldenberg, A., Endevelt, K., Ran, S., Dweck, C. S., Gross, J. J., & Halperin, E. (2017). Making
intergroup contact more fruitful: Enhancing cooperation between Palestinian and Jewish–
Israeli adolescents by fostering beliefs about group malleability. Social Psychological and
Personality Science, 8(1), 1–8.
Graham, J., Haidt, J., & Nosek, B. A. (2009). Liberals and conservatives rely on different sets of
moral foundations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(5), 1029–1046.
Halperin, E. (2008). Group-based hatred in intractable conflict in Israel. Journal of Conflict Reso-
lution, 52(5), 713–736.
Halperin, E., Cohen-Chen, S., & Goldenberg, A. (2014). Indirect emotion regulation in intractable
conflicts: A new approach to conflict resolution. European Review of Social Psychology,
25(1), 1–31.
Halperin, E., Crisp, R. J., Husnu, S., Trzesniewski, K. H., Dweck, C. S., & Gross, J. J. (2012).
Promoting intergroup contact by changing beliefs: Group malleability, intergroup anxiety,
and contact motivation. Emotion, 12(6), 1192–1195.
Halperin, E., Russell, A. G., Trzesniewski, K. H., Gross, J. J., & Dweck, C. S. (2011). Promot-
ing the Middle East peace process by changing beliefs about group malleability. Science,
333(6050), 1767–1769.
Hameiri, B., Porat, R., Bar-Tal, D., Bieler, A., & Halperin, E. (2014). Paradoxical thinking as a
new avenue of intervention to promote peace. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sci-
ences of the USA, 111(30), 10996–11001.
Hameiri, B., Porat, R., Bar-Tal, D., & Halperin, E. (2016). Moderating attitudes in times of vio-
lence through paradoxical thinking intervention. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences of the USA, 113(43), 12105–12110.
Hewstone, M., & Brown, R. (1986). Contact is not enough: An intergroup perspective on the
‘contact hypothesis.’ In M. Hewstone & R. Brown (Eds.), Social psychology and society:
Contact and conflict in intergroup encounters (pp. 1–44). Cambridge, MA: Basil Blackwell.
Hilal, J. (Ed.). (2007). Where now for Palestine?: The demise of the two state solution. London:
Zed Books.
Hong, Y., Chiu, C., Dweck, C. S., Lin, D. M.-S., & Wan, W. (1999). Implicit theories, attribu-
tions, and coping: A meaning system approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 77(3), 588–599.
Jost, J. T., Federico, C. M., & Napier, J. L. (2009). Political ideology: Its structure, functions, and
elective affinities. Annual Review of Psychology, 60(1), 307–337.
364 III. Conflict and Relationships
Kahn, D. T., Reifen Tagar, M., Halperin, E., Bäckström, M., Vitriol, J. A., & Liberman, V. (2018).
If they can’t change, why support change?: Implicit theories about groups, social dominance
orientation and political identity. Journal of Social and Political Psychology, 6(1), 151–173.
Kamins, M. L., & Dweck, C. S. (1999). Person versus process praise and criticism: Implications for
contingent self-worth and coping. Developmental Psychology, 35(3), 835–847.
Kimel, S. Y., Huesmann, R., Kunst, J. R., & Halperin, E. (2016). Living in a genetic world: How
learning about interethnic genetic similarities and differences affect peace and conflict. Per-
sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 42(5), 688–700.
MacInnis, C. C., & Page-Gould, E. (2015). How can intergroup interaction be bad if intergroup
contact is good?: Exploring and reconciling an apparent paradox in the science of intergroup
relations. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(3), 307–327.
Maoz, I. (2011). Does contact work in protracted asymmetrical conflict?: Appraising 20 years
of reconciliation-aimed encounters between Israeli Jews and Palestinians. Journal of Peace
Research, 48(1), 115–125.
Maoz, I., & McCauley C. (2008). Threat, dehumanization, and support for retaliatory aggressive
policies in asymmetric conflict. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 52(1), 93–116.
Melander, E., Pettersson, T., & Themner, L. (2016). Organized violence, 1989–2015. Journal of
Peace Research, 53(5), 727–742.
Norem, J. K., & Cantor, N. (1986). Defensive pessimism: Harnessing anxiety as motivation. Jour-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1208–1217.
Nussbaum, A. D., & Dweck, C. S. (2008). Defensiveness versus remediation: Self-theories and
modes of self-esteem maintenance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(5), 599–
612.
Paluck, E. L. (2009). Reducing intergroup prejudice and conflict using the media: A field experi-
ment in Rwanda. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(3), 574–587.
Paluck, E. L., Green, S. A., & Green, D. P. (2019). The contact hypothesis re-evaluated. Behavioral
Public Policy, 3(2), 129–158.
Paunesku, D., Walton, G. M., Romero, C., Smith, E. N., Yeager, D. S., & Dweck, C. S. (2015).
Mind-set interventions are a scalable treatment for academic underachievement. Psychologi-
cal Science, 26(6), 784–793.
Peterson, R. D. (2002). Understanding ethnic violence: Fear, hatred, and resentment in twentieth-
century Eastern Europe. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Pettigrew, T. F. (1998). Intergroup contact theory. Annual Review of Psychology, 49(1), 65–85.
Pettigrew, T. F., & Tropp, L. R. (2006). A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 90(5), 751–783.
Podolsky, P. (2012, July 1). Netanyahu intimates Shamir was right in wariness of Arabs. Times of
Israel. Retrieved from www.timesofisrael.com/netanyahu-shamir-was-right-in-criticism-of-
arabs.
Rivlin, R. (2015). Address to the 15th annual Herzliya Conference. Retrieved from http://archive.
president.gov.il/English/ThePresident/Speeches/Pages/news_070615_01.aspx.
Ross, L., & Stittinger, C. (1991). Barriers to conflict resolution. Negotiation Journal, 7(4), 389–
404.
Rydell, R. J., Hugenberg, K., Ray, D., & Mackie, D. M. (2007). Implicit theories about groups and
stereotyping. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(4), 549–558.
Sassenrath, C., Hodges, S. D., & Pfattheicher, S. (2016). Its all about the self: When perspective
taking backfires. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25(6), 405–410.
Schein, C., & Gray, K. (2015). The unifying moral dyad: Liberals and conservatives share the same
harm-based moral template. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41(8), 1147–1163.
Tropp, L. R. (2015). Dismantling an ethos of conflict: Strategies for improving intergroup rela-
tions. In E. Halperin & K. Sharvit (Eds.), The social psychology of intractable conflicts
(pp. 159–171). New York: Springer.
The Group‑Malleability Intervention 365
van Zomeren, M., Postmes, T., & Spears, R. (2008). Toward an integrative social identity model
of collective action: A quantitative research synthesis of three socio-psychological perspec-
tives. Psychological Bulletin, 134(4), 504–535.
Vorauer, J. D., & Sasaki, S. J. (2009). Helpful only in the abstract? Psychological Science, 20(2),
191–197.
Wang, C. S., Kenneth, T., Ku, G., & Galinsky, A. D. (2014). Perspective-taking increases willing-
ness to engage in intergroup contact. PLOS ONE, 9(1), e85681.
Williams, R. M. (1947). The reduction of intergroup tensions, Vol 57. New York: Social Science
Research Council.
Wohl, M. J. A., Cohen-Chen, S., Halperin, E., Caouette, J., Hayes, N., & Hornsey, M. J. (2015).
Belief in the malleability of groups strengthens the tenuous link between a collective apology
and intergroup forgiveness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41(5), 714–725.
Yeager, D. S., Miu, A. S., Powers, J., & Dweck, C. S. (2013). Implicit theories of personality and
attributions of hostile intent: A meta-analysis, an experiment, and a longitudinal interven-
tion. Child Development, 84(5), 1651–1667.
Yeager, D. S., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Dweck, C. S. (2013). An implicit theories of personality
intervention reduces adolescent aggression in response to victimization and exclusion. Child
Development, 84(3), 970–988.
Yeager, D. S., Trzesniewski, K. H., Tirri, K., Nokelainen, P., & Dweck, C. S. (2011). Adolescents’
implicit theories predict desire for vengeance after peer conflicts: Correlational and experi-
mental evidence. Developmental Psychology, 47(4), 1090–1107.
Yeager, D. S., Walton, G. M., Brady, S. T., Akcinar, E. N., Paunesku, D., Keane, L., . . . Dweck,
C. S. (2016). Teaching a lay theory before college narrows achievement gaps at scale. Pro-
ceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113(24),
3341–3348.
Zaki, J., & Cikara, M. (2015). Addressing empathic failures. Current Directions in Psychological
Science, 24(6), 471–476.
Zhou, S., Page-Gould, E., Aron, A., Moyer, A., & Hewstone, M. (2018). The extended contact
hypothesis: A meta-analysis on 20 years of research. Personality and Social Psychology
Review, 23(2), 132–160.
C H A P TE R 16
Erica B. Slotter and Laura B. Luchies
Marital quality normatively declines over time, which is especially unfortunate given that
it is a key contributor to overall life satisfaction. In an effort to preserve marital quality, we
designed an intervention that uses emotional reappraisal to help spouses manage the
negative emotions they experience during marital conflict. We tested the Couples Activity for
Reappraising Emotions (CARE) intervention in a 2-year study of married couples (N = 120).
Couples that spent 21 minutes over the course of a year engaging in exercises in which they
viewed their marital conflicts from the perspective of a benevolent third party experienced
stable marital quality, whereas those in a control group experienced a typical decline in mari-
tal quality. Moreover, couples in the CARE intervention group also reported better psycho-
logical health than those in the control group. In this chapter, we discuss procedures of the
CARE intervention, processes that may underlie its effectiveness, predictions about couples
for whom it may be less effective, and possibilities for scaling the intervention to make it
available and easy for anyone to use. The success of the CARE intervention demonstrates
that brief, inexpensive, theory-based social-psychological interventions can address ongo-
ing, costly social problems, such as marital dissatisfaction.
BACKGROUND
Marital quality is one of the most crucial social factors in predicting people’s health and
well-being (e.g., Myers, 2000; Parker-Pope, 2010). Copious research has documented
that lower-quality marriages come with health costs, whereas higher-quality marriages
366
The CARE Intervention 367
yield health benefits (e.g., Burman & Margolin, 1992; Carr & Springer, 2010; Kiecolt-
Glaser & Newton, 2001; Proulx, Helms, & Buehler, 2007; Slatcher, 2010; Whisman,
2001). In terms of psychological health, greater marital quality is associated with greater
feelings of general life satisfaction and well-being, with over half of people who report
being “very happy” in their marriages also report high levels of general happiness. In
contrast, only 10% of people who report being “pretty happy” in their marriages report
similarly high levels of overall happiness. Indeed, the association of life satisfaction with
marital quality is stronger than life satisfaction’s ties to job satisfaction or overall health
(Heller, Watson, & Hies, 2004). Low levels of marital happiness have been associated
with an increased risk of negative mental health outcomes, including both subclinical and
clinical levels of depression (e.g., Beach, 2014; Whisman, 2001).
Greater marital quality also confers benefits for physical health. For example, among
patients who have had a coronary artery bypass graft, those who reported high rather
than low marital satisfaction 1 year following the surgery were 3.2 times more likely to
be alive 15 years after the surgery. This effect could not be explained by demographic,
behavioral, or baseline health measures (King & Reis, 2012; see also Coyne et al., 2001).
A recent meta-analysis demonstrated that the associations of marital quality with vari-
ous health outcomes, ranging from cardiovascular events to chronic illness, had effect
sizes similar in magnitude to the effects of diet and exercise on health outcomes (Robles,
Slatcher, Trombello, & McGinn, 2014). The benefits of high-quality marriages and the
tolls of strained marriages have been documented across adulthood, with analogous
effects emerging among the newly married, those in midlife, and older adults (e.g., Beach,
Katz, Kim, & Brody, 2003; Carr, Freedman, Cornman, & Schwarz, 2014).
Despite the fact that a strong marital bond is an important factor in determining
health outcomes, marital quality normatively declines over time. Across couples, marital
quality typically declines throughout the course of a marriage, with couples especially at
risk for a drop in marital quality around the time they first become parents (e.g., Glenn,
1998; VanLaningham, Johnson, & Amato, 2001). Although the fact that marital quality
tends to decline over time is well-known, exactly why this happens is not. Some contrib-
uting factors likely include a ceiling effect (i.e., couples typically start their marriages
so happy that their satisfaction has nowhere to go but down), habituation (i.e., spouses
simply get used to each other’s positive traits and behaviors and take them for granted),
and an accumulation of frustrations over time.
Given the unfortunate, almost paradoxical, effects wherein happy marriages are
essential for well-being and yet marital happiness tends to decline, relationship schol-
ars have sought to identify factors that predict marital quality. These factors may, in
turn, be risk factors for negative psychological and physical health outcomes. Dozens
of factors have been identified, including conflict dynamics within the marriage, social
support provided by the marriage, and external factors placing stress on the marriage
(e.g., F
incham & Beach, 1999; Neff & Karney, 2007; Reis, 2012). One recent theoreti-
cal model (Slatcher & Schoebi, 2017) categorized risk factors into three groups: external
stressors, a lack of marital strengths, and marital strains. External stressors include fac-
tors outside of the marital dyad, such as financial pressures, parenting demands, or work
concerns. Inadequate marital strengths include a lack of positive behaviors and interac-
tion patterns, such as poor partner responsiveness, low levels of intimacy, or insuffi-
cient self-disclosure. Marital strains include negative behaviors and interaction patterns,
such as hostility, demand/withdraw conflict patterns, or negative affect reciprocity. The
368 III. Conflict and Relationships
effects of the factors within each category can be moderated by individual differences,
such as attachment style or self-esteem.
Interventions designed to promote marital quality could target any of these catego-
ries. However, one marital strain has been shown to be an especially robust predictor
of marital quality: negative affect reciprocity. Negative affect reciprocity is a chain of
retaliatory negativity between spouses during marital conflict (Gottman, 1998). Nega-
tive affect reciprocity occurs during conflict when spouses counter their partner’s com-
plaint or criticism with an escalated complaint or criticism of their own. This pattern
of interaction creates a destructive cycle in which behaviors become increasingly hostile
as the conflict progresses. Negative affect reciprocity reliably distinguishes couples that
are experiencing low marital quality and marital distress from higher-quality, nondis-
tressed couples (e.g., Gottman, 1998; Fincham & Beach, 1999). One of the greatest chal-
lenges for couples locked into negative exchanges is to find an adaptive way to exit from
such cycles (Weiss & Heyman, 1997). Moreover, the effects of negative affect reciprocity
within a marriage are likely cumulative—the longer the pattern of behavior persists in
the relationship, the less happy the relationship becomes. Thus, breaking the pattern of
negative affect reciprocity should benefit couples at any stage in their relationship.
Scholars and practitioners have attempted to develop therapeutic interventions to
interrupt cycles of negativity before they become all consuming (e.g., Baucom, Shoham,
Mueser, Daiuto, & Stickle, 1998). For example, behavioral marital therapy uses social
learning approaches to help spouses gain insight into problematic interaction patterns
and learn to modify their behavior (e.g., Baucom & Epstein, 1990). Although traditional
psychotherapy approaches can help spouses learn to manage their emotions and behavior
more constructively, they come with drawbacks. They tend to require considerable invest-
ment of time and money on the part of the couple. Couples must have both the insight to
know their relationship could be improved and the resources to seek and follow through
with such treatment. Additionally, couples that are seeing a marital therapist may already
be sufficiently distressed in their marriages that therapeutic interventions are rendered
less effective than if they had been implemented sooner.
Inspired by research demonstrating that brief, theory-based, social-psychological
interventions can yield remarkably enduring improvements in people’s lives by fostering
thoughts and behaviors that self-reinforce over time (see Walton & Wilson, 2018, for
a review), we developed a brief intervention aimed at reducing conflict-based negative
affect reciprocity. Given that marital happiness is strongly influenced by recursive, self-
reinforcing dynamics, such as negative affect reciprocity, it represents a promising target
for a social-psychological intervention. The relative ease and low cost of the interven-
tion’s implementation, together with its directed, theoretically driven approach, make
it a promising way for couples to interrupt the cycle of negative affect reciprocity in the
relationship. Alleviating this marital strain can help preserve marital quality over time,
thereby allowing people to reap the psychological and physical health benefits of being
in a satisfying marriage.
PSYCHOLOGICAL PROCESSES
Our intervention, called the Couples Activity for Reappraising Emotions (CARE) inter-
vention, capitalized on the power of emotional reappraisal—reinterpreting the meaning
The CARE Intervention 369
spouses view their disagreements from a more distanced, less emotionally “hot” perspec-
tive. In the CARE intervention, we did not ask participants to view conflicts with their
spouse from the perspective of any third party. Instead, we asked them to adopt the per-
spective of a benevolent third party who “wants the best for all involved” and to consider
how this person might “find the good that could come from” disagreements. With this
distanced and benevolent perspective, spouses are prompted to see the bigger picture of
their relationship and respond constructively instead of reacting destructively. Compared
to destructive responses, which escalate conflict-related distress, constructive responses
would reduce conflict-related distress; this dissipation represents a disruption in the cycle
of negative affect reciprocity. Disrupting negative affect reciprocity helps preserve marital
quality by reducing one type of marital strain that predicts declines in relational happi-
ness. Furthermore, emotional reappraisal of marital conflict should be associated with
better psychological well-being for the people in these happier marriages.
EMPIRICAL EVIDENCE
Outcomes
To test the effectiveness of the CARE intervention, we conducted a 2-year study of het-
erosexual married couples (N = 120; see Finkel, Slotter, Luchies, Walton, & Gross, 2013,
for details). Both members of the couple participated in the study, although each individ-
ual completed study measures independently. At the beginning of the study and every 4
months thereafter, participants completed an online survey and reported on their marital
quality, including feelings of trust, love, passion, intimacy, satisfaction, and commitment.
At each follow-up survey, which occurred every 4 months for the remainder of the study,
participants recalled and provided a brief, fact-based summary of the most significant
conflict they had experienced in their marriage since the previous follow-up. They also
reported on their feelings of postconflict distress as a proxy for negative affect reciproc-
ity.
The first year of the 2-year study did not include the CARE intervention, and all par-
ticipants underwent identical procedures. During the first year of the study, participants’
reports showed decreases in marital quality, replicating the well-documented normative
decline in marital quality (e.g., Glenn, 1998; VanLaningham et al., 2001). This decline
was evident in all six aspects of marital quality, as well as in an overall composite mea-
sure of marital quality.
One year into the study, we randomly assigned half of the couples to the CARE
intervention and the other half to a control group. During Year 2, all couples recalled
and described a conflict they had experienced, just as they had in Year 1. Couples in the
control group did not receive any additional instructions, whereas couples in the CARE
intervention condition were instructed to engage in emotional reappraisal techniques
after describing the conflict. They completed an additional writing task that involved
three prompts and lasted approximately 7 minutes. In it, they were asked to take the per-
spective of a neutral third party who wants the best for everyone and consider how they
could overcome barriers to adopting this perspective during future conflicts. Full details
on the intervention and its implementation are available in the “Intervention Content and
Implementation” section.
The CARE Intervention 371
Notes T0 is a SEPARATE variable, intake is not part of this central analysis….T1 is the first followup wave
Overall Composite made by z-scoring composites and averaging them into 1
0.1
0.0
-0.1
CARE Intervention Begins
-0.2
-0.3
0 4 8 12 16 20 24
FIGURE 16.1. Trajectories of marital quality for participants in the CARE intervention condi-
tion and participants in the control condition. Reprinted with permission from Finkel, Slotter,
Luchies, Walton, and Gross (2013).
372 III. Conflict and Relationships
End-of-study stress
Intake stress .80 11.51**
CARE intervention status –.11 –2.44*
Note. Degrees of freedom ranged from 115 to 119; slight variations were due to missing data.
*p < .05; **p < .001.
Mechanism
We next sought to examine the mechanism by which the CARE intervention might be
buffering participants against drops in marital quality over time. We tested two possible
mechanisms. The first tested mechanism was the possibility that those in the CARE
intervention condition experienced less severe marital conflicts than control participants.
Results showed that this was not the case; there was no notable difference in how severe
participants in the two conditions rated the conflicts they had experienced.
The second tested mechanism was the possibility that those in the CARE interven-
tion experienced less distress related to their marital conflicts than control participants—
even though the conflicts they had experienced were just as severe. Results provided
evidence for this mechanism. CARE intervention participants experienced less distress
about their marital conflicts over the course of the second year of the study, which was
the same time they were engaging in the intervention. Additional mediation analyses
showed that this reduction in conflict-related distress went on to predict more stable
levels of marital quality. In sum, couples that participated in the CARE intervention
experienced less conflict-related distress over time, and this led them to experience more
stable marital quality over time.
Effects over Time
Because we have tested the CARE intervention in only one study to date, we have limited
evidence about its long-term effects. However, from this study we do know that marital
quality stabilized among couples that engaged in the CARE intervention, and that this
stabilization lasted throughout the year that the intervention took place. On the other
hand, marital quality continued a downward trajectory among control participants. We
expect that both trends would continue, causing the gap in marital quality between those
who engaged in the CARE intervention and those who did not to widen over time.
Learning, practicing, and mastering new skills takes much practice, repetition, and
time. Yet, the CARE intervention took only 7 minutes at each of three occurrences, total-
ing just 21 minutes in a year’s time. This small amount of time would not be enough for
participants to learn, master, and implement a skill that would directly lead to better
The CARE Intervention 373
marital quality. Instead, we think that the CARE intervention instilled in participants
a new mindset to use when facing marital conflicts. Unlike skills, new mindsets can be
adopted quickly (Walton & Wilson, 2018). Once people adopt a new mindset about mar-
ital conflict, that mindset may remain, perhaps for the duration of their marriage. To the
extent that this is true, continuing to complete reappraisal exercises may not yield addi-
tional benefits. However, to lessen the possibility of shifting back into the preintervention
default tendency to see conflicts from an emotionally “hot” egocentric perspective, it may
be helpful for people to continue to invest 21 minutes a year into their marriages by sus-
taining the CARE intervention over time. We do not foresee any downsides of continuing
the intervention long term—doing so might help and would not hurt.
Heterogeneity
We tested whether the CARE intervention would be more or less effective among dif-
ferent groups of participants, including men versus women, younger versus older par-
ticipants, or couples that had been married for shorter versus longer lengths of time. In
our study, none of these factors affected the CARE intervention’s effectiveness. We also
examined six indicators of marital quality, including feelings of trust, love, passion, inti-
macy, satisfaction, and commitment. All six indicators yielded similar results.
However, it is important to note that all of our participants volunteered to take part
in a study of romantic relationships. Couples whose relationships are very distressed are
unlikely to want to participate in a study like this. We suspect that the CARE interven-
tion would be less effective among clinically distressed couples living in a climate of con-
tempt. Instead, this intervention presupposes that people who use it are living in a general
climate of decency, believe their relationship is worth maintaining, and have a partner
who warrants the benefit of the doubt. If a relationship is so troubled that wanting the
best for all involved means that it may be best for the relationship to end, the CARE
intervention is unlikely to work well, if at all. Moreover, it could give people living in
a toxic relationship false hope for improvement, potentially putting them at risk if that
relationship is abusive.
COUSINS
The CARE intervention is both similar to and different from several other social psy-
chological interventions. Because marital conflicts are stressful, it is instructive to com-
pare the CARE intervention to other interventions that have focused on people’s views
of stress and the implications of those views. For example, people can hold different
appraisals about specific stressful situations. They can see stress arousal either as a use-
ful tool that can be harnessed to help one rise to a challenge, or they can see stress
arousal as a detriment to one’s ability to address a threat (see, e.g., Jamieson & Hangen,
Chapter 10, this volume; Jamieson, Nock, & Mendes, 2012). Interventions that induce
people to see stress as indicating a challenge rather than a threat predict more optimal
physiological responses and enhance one’s performance. Other research has examined
people’s general mindsets about stress (see, e.g., Crum, Handley-M iner, & Smith, Chap-
ter 9, this volume; Crum, Salovey, & Achor, 2013). Although people tend to see stress
as negative, stress has enhancing as well as debilitating consequences. Inducing people
to adopt a stress-is-enhancing mindset rather than a stress-is-debilitating mindset also
374 III. Conflict and Relationships
predicts more optimal physiological responses and enhances one’s performance. Both
stress appraisal and stress-mindset interventions have focused on outcomes related to
performance on specific, stressful tasks. In contrast, although marital conflicts are stress-
ful, they are neither a task to be accomplished nor a challenge to be overcome. In fact,
if spouses view marital conflicts as something to perform well on or “win,” they may
double down on their default egocentric view of the conflict—the opposite of the CARE
intervention’s goal. Therefore, we caution against broadly applying these other interven-
tions to the stress of marital conflict.
A type of intervention that is more like the CARE intervention is one that uses self-
distancing strategies (e.g., Kross et al., 2005; Ray et al., 2008). In a seminal study in
this line of research, participants recalled an interpersonal situation in which they felt
overwhelming anger and hostility (Kross et al., 2005). Then they were asked to view this
experience in one of two ways. In the self-immersed condition, they viewed the situation
through their own eyes all over again. In the self-distanced perspective, they viewed the
situation as if they were a fly on the wall. In addition, half of the participants in both
the immersed and distanced conditions were asked to think about what they were feel-
ing, whereas the other half were asked to think about why they were feeling the way
they were feeling. People who had adopted the self-distanced perspective and thought
about the reasons underlying their emotions experienced less negative affect than those
in the other conditions. These results showed that a self-distanced and abstract perspec-
tive allows people to think about negative experiences without eliciting high levels of
negative emotions. The CARE intervention employed a self-distancing strategy for view-
ing marital conflicts. Yet, it went further than asking participants to view their marital
conflict from the perspective of a fly on the wall. Instead, participants were asked to take
the perspective of a third party who wants the best for all involved and to consider how
this person might see the good that could come from the conflict. These instructions may
have encouraged CARE intervention participants to adopt an abstract perspective, much
like those in Kross et al.’s (2005) why condition.
Still other social psychological interventions have prompted mindset changes that
have benefited dyadic relationships. For example, one intervention led teachers to adopt
an empathic mindset about discipline, in which they were encouraged to understand and
value their students’ circumstances and perspectives (Okonofua, Paunesku, & Walton,
2016). Students in these teachers’ classes were only half as likely to be suspended as those
in a control group; they also perceived greater respect from their teachers. Another inter-
vention was administered to mothers of medically at-risk infants during home visits (e.g.,
Bugental, Beaulieu, & Silbert-B eiger, 2010; Bugental et al., 2002). The intervention facil-
itated the mothers’ own problem-solving and information search, whereas the control
home visiting program provided mothers with ideas on how to solve problems and gave
them information. It also discouraged mothers from holding pejorative views about them-
selves and their children, such as the belief that they are a bad mom or that their child
is a bad baby. The intervention yielded several positive outcomes over time, including
decreased use of corporal punishment, increased maternal investment of time in the child,
and better child health. In both lines of research, the intervention targeted one member of
a dyad, yet benefited both members. As currently designed, the CARE intervention targets
both members of a married couple, but it could be adapted to involve only one spouse. We
expect that the CARE intervention would be beneficial even if only one spouse partici-
pated in it, although its benefits are maximized when both spouses participate.
The CARE Intervention 375
The CARE intervention was designed to be a brief, directed intervention to reduce nega-
tive conflict-related emotions, thus reducing negative affect reciprocity, with the aim of
protecting marital quality in ongoing relationships. The study designed to test the CARE
intervention was conducted largely online, with all procedures of the CARE intervention
and its effects completed entirely online. As stated previously, participants in the study
completed an intake session, as well as six follow-up waves, every 4 months over 2 years.
Participants received e-mails when it was time to complete a follow-up wave with a link
directing them to the assessment. They could complete the follow-up at their convenience
within 2 weeks of receiving the e-mail. Each follow-up wave took an average of 20–30
minutes to complete.
Participants wrote about a recent conflict in their relationship at each wave of the
study. They provided a “fact-based summary of the most significant disagreement” they
had experienced with their spouse over the preceding 4 months, “focusing on behavior,
not on thoughts or feelings.” We asked them to “provide enough information that some-
one unfamiliar with the disagreement would be able to understand what happened.”
Participants were asked to respond to this prompt for about 2 minutes but could write as
long as they liked.
During Year 2 (Waves 5–7), half of the couples were randomly assigned to the CARE
intervention. The participants were asked to engage in an additional, approximately
7-minute writing task in which they were asked to emotionally reappraise the conflict
they had written about from the perspective of a neutral third party who wanted the best
376 III. Conflict and Relationships
outcome for all persons involved. Specifically, they were asked to respond to the follow-
ing three prompts:
“Think about the specific disagreement that you just wrote about having with your
partner. Think about this disagreement with your partner from the perspective of
a neutral third party who wants the best for all involved; a person who sees things
from a neutral point of view. How might this person think about the disagreement?
How might he or she find the good that could come from it? (Please write about this
for 3 minutes.)”
“Some people find it helpful to take this third-party perspective during their interac-
tions with their romantic partner. However, almost everybody finds it challenging to
take this third-party perspective at all times. In your relationship with your partner,
what obstacles do you face in trying to take this third-party perspective, especially
when you’re having a disagreement with your partner? (Please write about this for
2 minutes.)”
“Despite the obstacles to taking a third-party perspective, people can be successful
in doing so. Over the next 4 months, please try your best to take this third-party
perspective during interactions with your partner, especially during disagreements.
How might you be most successful in taking this perspective in your interactions
with your partner over the next 4 months? How might taking this perspective help
you make the best of disagreements in your relationship? (Please write about this for
2 minutes.)”
Each prompt and a space for typing responses appeared on a separate page in the
online survey. Participants clicked a button marked “Next” to proceed through the
pages. For each prompt, participants were asked to consider the questions at hand and
write about them for a set amount of time. They could take longer if they wished, but
they could not advance the page until the base amount of time had passed. The CARE
intervention occurred at the very end of the follow-up assessments before participants
were thanked for their participation and dismissed.
Participants in the CARE intervention group also received reminder e-mails about
the reappraisal task they had completed during their follow-up assessments. The e-mails
were automatically sent 2 months after the completion of each wave in the second year of
the study. The e-mail was designed to remind participants to engage in emotional reap-
praisal regarding conflict in their marriage. The e-mail text, slightly edited for space,
was:
Hello [name],
We’re writing to check in. The next follow-up questionnaire isn’t for another two
months, but we wanted to thank you again for participating in the study.
When you completed your most recent questionnaire, you wrote about a specific
disagreement with your partner. You thought about this disagreement from the perspec-
tive of a neutral third party who wants the best for all involved and about how this
person might find the good that could come of the disagreement.
You then thought about obstacles you face in your own life that might interfere with
your ability to take a neutral, third-party perspective regarding disagreements in your
own relationship. Finally, you thought about specific ways that taking this third-party
The CARE Intervention 377
perspective in your own relationship can help you make the most of disagreements with
your partner.
As you go through your daily life, please keep in mind the benefits of adopting a
third-party perspective regarding your relationship. Sometime today (now, if the timing
works), please take a few moments to think about ways you can adopt this perspective
about conflicts in your relationship, about ways to find the good that can come from
them.
Thanks again for staying involved in the study. We’ll check back with you in a
couple of months. In the meantime, if you have any questions about the study, feel free
to e-mail us.
Participants in the control group received e-mails in between their follow-up assessments
in the second year of the study as well, but these e-mails simply thanked them for being
involved in the study and told them that they would receive their next follow-up survey
in 2 months.
The CARE intervention, as it was designed, has the advantage of being brief—it was
completed in 7 minutes per wave, making participants’ total investment in the interven-
tion 21 minutes in a year—and yet, it was powerful enough to eliminate declines in cou-
ples’ marital quality over a year. It is also easy and flexible to administer—participants
in our study completed the intervention procedures online, largely at their convenience,
in locations of their choosing. Overall, the design of the CARE intervention makes it an
efficient, and yet powerfully effective, intervention to protect marital quality and psycho-
logical well-being.
NUANCES AND MISCONCEPTIONS
A dangerous misconception that some people may have about the CARE intervention is
seeing it as a panacea that can replace marital therapy and fix troubled marriages. We
tested the CARE intervention in couples that volunteered to participate in our study; few
of them were likely to be highly distressed. In our view, the CARE intervention should be
used by couples that want to maintain, not end, their relationship and in which partners
have a foundation of care for each other and warrant the benefit of the doubt. If a rela-
tionship is so toxic that a reasonable third party could not see the good that could come
from disagreements in it, the CARE intervention would not help. Instead, these spouses
should consider whether their relationship should continue, and if so, they should seek
professional counsel about how to improve it. Moreover, the CARE intervention stabi-
lized marital quality, but it did not increase it. If the current level of marital quality in a
relationship is not satisfying, the CARE intervention might be able to keep it from getting
worse but probably won’t improve it.
Another potentially important nuance about the CARE intervention is that we asked
participants to take the perspective of a benevolent third party who wants the best for
all involved. Participants were prompted to imagine how this person could see the good
that could come from the disagreement and consider how they could make the best of
future conflicts in the relationship. Although we did not compare this benevolent third-
party perspective to a neutral one, we expect that it contributed to participants’ ability to
respond constructively to their marital conflicts, leading to less conflict-related distress.
Other potential third-party perspectives, such as a friend’s or family member’s view, may
378 III. Conflict and Relationships
not have been as effective, especially if people would choose to take the perspective of a
person who might be on “their side” more than their spouse’s side. It is also important
to note that we did not ask participants to put themselves into their spouse’s shoes and
try to see things from their spouse’s perspective. In the midst of marital conflict, people
are likely to believe that their spouse’s perspective is overblown, irrational, or just plain
wrong. Trying to take a spouse’s view is likely to heighten these beliefs and backfire by
promoting the default egocentric and emotionally hot perspective, which would only
make things worse.
A few other details of how we administered the CARE intervention may be critical
to its effectiveness. First, both spouses participated in the CARE intervention. Although
we do not think it is absolutely necessary for both spouses to complete the CARE inter-
vention for it to yield some benefit, we expect that it is most powerful when both spouses
take part because of the beneficial synergy that would occur. Second, participants com-
pleted the reappraisal exercises about specific conflicts in their own relationship, not
interpersonal conflicts in general. We expect that reappraising concrete, personal con-
flicts was effectual because these are the conflicts about which participants can benefit
from shifting from an emotionally hot, egocentric perspective to that of a distanced,
rational, benevolent third party. Third, participants anticipated obstacles that they might
encounter when adopting the benevolent third-party perspective during future marital
conflicts and how they might overcome these obstacles. We expect that doing this helped
participants push through challenges they might have encountered, rather than seeing
them as evidence that taking the third-party perspective is too difficult. Fourth, par-
ticipants were not forced to complete the intervention exercises or write in response to
each prompt, although the survey did not allow them to move forward until the base
amount of time had passed. Other social-psychological interventions have been shown
to work best or only when people voluntarily take part in them (e.g., Bryan et al., 2016;
Vansteenkiste, Simons, Lens, Sheldon, & Deci, 2004; Lyubomirsky, Dickerhoof, Boehm,
& Sheldon, 2011; Silverman, Logel, & Cohen, 2013), and the CARE intervention might
be less effective if it were a required activity.
Although these nuances may enhance or reduce the effectiveness of the CARE inter-
vention, we do not anticipate ways in which it could backfire. We designed the CARE
intervention to be straightforward, easily administered, conducted online, and scalable.
Variations in the CARE intervention’s procedures may render it less beneficial but are
unlikely to make it harmful.
IMPLICATIONS FOR PRACTICE
Given the beneficial effects of the CARE intervention on marital quality and psychologi-
cal health, it would be advantageous to make it available to couples on a larger scale.
One way to roll out the CARE intervention to more people is through professionals and
practitioners who work directly with couples. Although we have publicized our results
in the popular media and are aware of practitioners who have adopted or adapted this
intervention in their practice, there is much more opportunity to actively promote the
intervention among those who work directly with couples. As previously discussed, the
CARE intervention maintains, but does not improve, marital quality. We also caution its
use among highly distressed couples. Therefore, it would be most helpful for practitioners
The CARE Intervention 379
who work with couples early in their relationships, such as those who provide premarital
counseling or officiate weddings, to promote the CARE intervention.
A second and even more scalable way to roll out the CARE intervention to more
couples is to build an application (app) that delivers the intervention to any person who
would like to use it in his or her relationship. The app could lead users through the 7-min-
ute intervention at regular intervals and send push notifications or e-mail reminders about
adopting a third-party perspective during conflicts. As discussed in the “Nuances and
Misconceptions” section, the CARE intervention was designed to be self-administered
online, and we do not anticipate ways in which it could be harmful when used by peo-
ple who are involved in average to high-quality relationships. The app could include
an optional research component, perhaps varying the frequency of the intervention and
reminder notifications. Users would report on their relationship quality periodically, cre-
ating a cost-effective way to test variations of the intervention while delivering the inter-
vention to an unlimited number of couples at minimal cost.
IMPLICATIONS FOR PSYCHOLOGICAL THEORY
FUTURE DIRECTIONS
Even though the intervention itself is inexpensive and easy to implement, conducting a
standard study to test it is not. Recruiting a sufficient number of couples and tracking
them for 2 years or more is expensive and time-consuming. To date, we have tested the
380 III. Conflict and Relationships
intervention’s outcomes only one time, and we are not aware of other studies that have
replicated or extended our work. With that in mind, we outline six avenues for future
empirical work.
First, as mentioned in the “Implications for Practice” section, future research could
test the CARE intervention using an app that delivers the intervention to many users via
their mobile devices. Doing so would be a cost-effective way to extend research on the
CARE intervention.
Second, future research could test the CARE intervention with a more diverse sam-
ple of participants. Our participants varied in age and in the length of their marriages—
however, they were predominantly White, of midrange socioeconomic status, and all of
them were in heterosexual relationships. Given recent work showing that some of these
factors can impact marital conflict and marital quality dynamics (e.g., Jackson et al.,
2016), examining the effectiveness of the CARE intervention within a broader popula-
tion is essential. One possibility would be to test the CARE intervention among newly-
weds to determine whether it could help maintain the high level of marital quality that
couples tend to experience in the so-called honeymoon phase. Another possibility would
be to test the CARE intervention in couples undergoing the transition to parenthood, a
time during which marital quality is especially likely to decline. It would also be informa-
tive to examine the CARE intervention across cultures. It is possible that the intervention
would be less effective among couples living in interdependent cultures to the extent that
their default perspective is less egocentric than it tends to be among people living in inde-
pendent cultures (see Cohen & Gunz, 2002; Wu & Keysar, 2007).
Third, future research could follow up with the original participants some years
after the original study period to gauge whether those who had been assigned to the
CARE intervention condition have continued to reappraise conflicts in their relation-
ships and/or have maintained higher levels of marital quality than those in the control
condition. We expect that the CARE intervention brings about a mindset shift that yields
recursive, self-perpetuating processes over time; these processes would continue to yield
benefits even after the intervention ended. Nonetheless, it would be fruitful for future
research to investigate the longer-term effects of the intervention, both with and without
additional iterations of the intervention.
Fourth, future research could involve a study in which only one member of each
couple completes the intervention to determine whether one or both members of the
couple should participate to yield maximal benefits. As discussed in the “Nuances and
Misconceptions” section, we think that having both members participate would create a
synergy and make the intervention the most effective. However, it may yield some ben-
efits if only one spouse engages in the intervention, especially because either partner can
stop a cycle of negative affect reciprocity. Couples in which one spouse is truly behaving
more negatively than the other are an interesting case. On the one hand, it would be most
helpful to change the behavior of the spouse who is behaving most negatively. On the
other hand, it may be the most helpful to change the mindset of the spouse who is on the
receiving end of the negativity; doing so may avoid further instigating the spouse who
is behaving most negatively. However, keep in mind our caution about using the CARE
intervention while couples are highly distressed.
Fifth, future research could systematically vary aspects of the CARE intervention
to determine the minimally necessary elements for the intervention to remain effective.
This type of investigation would be similar to Marigold et al.’s (2007) work that isolated
The CARE Intervention 381
the core of an intervention to help people with low self-esteem to respond positively to a
romantic partner’s compliments. Furthermore, it would enable further refinement of the
psychological processes that are at work in the CARE intervention. Relatedly, we used
conflict-related distress as a proxy measure for negative affect reciprocity, the primary
mechanism we targeted. A better measure that could be used in future research would be
behavioral evidence of negative affect reciprocity, which could be captured in videotaped
conflict discussions.
Sixth, future research could extend the scope of the CARE intervention’s outcomes
to include physical health. As reviewed at the beginning of this chapter, marital quality
is an important predictor of physical health, but this finding is based on correlational
and longitudinal studies. A future experiment of the CARE intervention’s effectiveness
on both marital quality and physical health could provide even stronger evidence of the
causal effect of marital quality on physical health.
CONCLUSION
For decades, psychological researchers and practitioners have grappled with the paradox
of marital quality: it is essential for people’s psychological and physical well-being, and
yet it declines over time within relationships. Fortunately, this decline is not inevitable.
The CARE intervention, grounded in appraisal theories of emotion and capitalizing on
people’s ability to reinterpret their emotional experiences, interrupts the spiral of nega-
tive affect reciprocity that can occur during marital conflict. It both stops declines in
marital quality and benefits psychological well-being. Compared to previous attempts
to intervene in declining marital quality, the CARE intervention has the benefit of being
brief (couples completed it in 7-minute sessions, three times during a year period), theory
driven (it was developed out of a robust literature on the benefits of emotional reap-
praisal), and flexible (the current variant of the procedure was applied online but could be
adapted to many other settings). Because of its effectiveness and ease of implementation,
the CARE intervention represents a promising avenue for both researchers and practitio-
ners to aid people in protecting and preserving their marital happiness.
REFERENCES
Baucom, D. H., & Epstein, N. (1990). Cognitive-behavioral marital therapy. New York: Brunner/
Mazel.
Baucom, D. H., Shoham, V., Mueser, K. T., Daiuto, A. D., & Stickle, T. R. (1998). Empirically sup-
ported couple and family interventions for marital distress and adult mental health problems.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66(1), 53–88.
Beach, S. R. H. (2014). The couple and family discord model of depression: Updates and future
directions. In C. R. Agnew & S. C. South (Eds.), Interpersonal relationships and health:
Social and clinical psychological mechanisms (pp. 133–155). New York: Oxford University
Press.
Beach, S. R. H., Katz, J., Kim, S., & Brody, G. H. (2003). Prospective effects of marital satisfac-
tion on depressive symptoms in established marriages: A dyadic model. Journal of Social and
Personal Relationships, 20(3), 355–371.
Bryan, C. J., Yeager, D. S., Hinojosa, C. P., Chabot, A., Bergen, H., Kawamura, M., & Steubing,
382 III. Conflict and Relationships
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes:
Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 85(2), 348–362.
Heller, D., Watson, D., & Hies, R. (2004). The role of person versus situation in life satisfaction:
A critical examination. Psychological Bulletin, 130(4), 574–600.
Jackson, G. L., Trail, T. E., Kennedy, D. P., Williamson, H. C., Bradbury, T. N., & Karney, B. R.
(2016). The salience and severity of relationship problems among low-income couples. Jour-
nal of Family Psychology, 30(1), 2–11.
Jamieson, J. P., Nock, M. K., & Mendes, W. B. (2012). Mind over matter: Reappraising arousal
improves cardiovascular and cognitive responses to stress. Journal of Experimental Psychol-
ogy, 141(3), 417–422.
John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2004). Healthy and unhealthy emotion regulation: Personality pro-
cesses, individual differences, and life span development. Journal of Personality, 72(6),
1301–1334.
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Newton, T. L. (2001). Marriage and health: His and hers. Psychological
Bulletin, 127(4), 472–503.
King, K. B., & Reis, H. T. (2012). Marriage and long-term survival after coronary artery bypass
grafting. Health Psychology, 31(1), 55–62.
Kiosses, D. N., Alexopoulos, G. S., Hajcak, G., Apfeldorf, W., Duberstein, P. R., Putrino, D.,
& Gross, J. J. (2018). Cognitive reappraisal intervention for suicide prevention (CRISP) for
middle-aged and older adults hospitalized for suicidality. American Journal of Geriatric Psy-
chiatry, 26(4), 494–503.
Kross, E., Ayduk, O., & Mischel, W. (2005). When asking “why” does not hurt: Distinguishing
rumination from reflective processing of negative emotions. Psychological Science, 16(9),
709–715.
Lazarus, R. S., & Alfert, E. (1964). Short-circuiting of threat by experimentally altering cognitive
appraisal. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 69(2), 195–205.
Lyubomirsky, S., Dickerhoof, R., Boehm, J. K., & Sheldon, K. M. (2011). Becoming happier takes
both a will and a proper way: An experimental longitudinal intervention to boost well-being.
Emotion, 11, 391–402.
Marigold, D. C., Holmes, J. G., & Ross, M. (2007). More than words: Reframing compliments
from romantic partners fosters security in low self-esteem individuals. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 92, 232–248.
Marigold, D. C., Holmes, J. G., & Ross, M. (2010). Fostering relationship resilience: An interven-
tion for low self-esteem individuals. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 624–
630.
Murray, S. L., Holmes, J. G., Griffin, D. W., Bellavia, G., & Rose, P. (2001). The mismeasure of
love: How self-doubt contaminates relationship beliefs. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 27(4), 423–436.
Myers, D. G. (2000). The funds, friends, and faith of happy people. American Psychologist, 55(1),
56–67.
Neff, L. A., & Karney, B. R. (2007). Stress crossover in newlywed marriage: A longitudinal and
dyadic perspective. Journal of Marriage and Family, 69(3), 594–607.
Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2005). The cognitive control of emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sci-
ences, 9(5), 242–249.
Okonofua, J. A., Paunesku, D., & Walton, G. M. (2016). Brief intervention to encourage empathic
discipline cuts suspension rates in half among adolescents. Proceedings of the National Acad-
emy of Sciences of the USA, 113(19), 5221–5226.
Parker-Pope, T. (2010, April 14). Is marriage good for your health? New York Times. Retrieved
from www.nytimes.com/2010/04/18/magazine/18marriage-t .html.
Proulx, C. M., Helms, H. M., & Buehler, C. (2007). Marital quality and personal well-being: A
meta-analysis. Journal of Marriage and Family, 69(3), 576–593.
384 III. Conflict and Relationships
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general
population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1(3), 385–401.
Ray, R. D., Wilhelm, F. H., & Gross, J. J. (2008). All in the mind’s eye?: Anger rumination and
reappraisal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(1), 133–145.
Reis, H. T. (2012). Perceived partner responsiveness as an organizing theme for the study of rela-
tionships and well-being. In L. Campbell & T. J. Loving (Eds.), Interdisciplinary research on
close relationships (pp. 27–52). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Richards, J. M., Butler, E. A., & Gross, J. J. (2003). Emotion regulation in romantic relationships:
The cognitive consequences of concealing feelings. Journal of Social and Personal Relation-
ships, 20(5), 599–620.
Robinson, J. A., & Swanson, K. L. (1993). Field and observer modes of remembering. Memory,
1(3), 169–184.
Robles, T. F., Slatcher, R. B., Trombello, J. M., & McGinn, M. M. (2014). Marital quality and
health: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 140(1), 140–187.
Sbarra, D. A., Boals, A., Mason, A. E., Larson, G. M., & Mehl, M. R. (2013). Expressive writing
can impede emotional recovery following marital separation. Clinical Psychological Science,
1(2), 120–134.
Silverman, A. M., Logel, C., & Cohen, G. L. (2013). Self-affirmation as a deliberate coping strat-
egy. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 93–98.
Slatcher, R. B. (2010). Marital functioning and physical health: Implications for social and person-
ality psychology. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4(7), 455–469.
Slatcher, R. B., & Schoebi, D. (2017). Protective processes underlying the links between marital
quality and physical health. Current Opinion in Psychology, 13, 148–152.
Slotter, E. B., & Ward, D. E. (2015). Finding the silver lining: The relative roles of redemptive nar-
ratives and cognitive reappraisal in individuals’ emotional distress after the end of a romantic
relationship. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 32(6), 737–756.
VanLaningham, J., Johnson, D. R., & Amato, P. (2001). Marital happiness, marital duration, and
the U-shaped curve: Evidence from a five-wave panel study. Social Forces, 79(4), 1313–1341.
Vansteenkiste, M., Simons, J., Lens, W., Sheldon, K. M., & Deci, E. L. (2004). Motivating learn-
ing, performance, and persistence: The synergistic effects of intrinsic goal contents autonomy-
supportive contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(2), 246–260.
Verduyn, P., Van Mechelen, I., Kross, E., Chezzi, C., & Van Bever, F. (2012). The relationship
between self-distancing and the duration of negative and positive emotional experiences in
daily life. Emotion, 12(6), 1248–1263.
Walton, G. M., & Wilson, T. D. (2018). Wise interventions: Psychological remedies for social and
personal problems. Psychological Review, 125(5), 617–655.
Watt, L. M., & Cappeliez, P. (2000). Integrative and instrumental reminiscence therapies for
depression in older adults: Intervention strategies and treatment effectiveness. Aging and
Mental Health, 4(2), 166–177.
Weiss, R. L., & Heyman, R. E. (1997). A clinical-research overview of couples interactions. In W.
K. Halford & H. J. Markman (Eds.), Clinical handbook of marriage and couples interven-
tions (pp. 13–41). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Whisman, M. A. (2001). The association between depression and marital dissatisfaction. In S. R.
H. Beach (Ed.), Marital and family processes in depression: A scientific foundation for clini-
cal practice (pp. 3–24). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Wu, S., & Keysar, B. (2007). The effect of culture on perspective taking. Psychological Science,
18(7), 600–606.
C H A P TE R 17
Denise C. Marigold
Insecurity in romantic relationships can create a self-fulfilling prophecy, whereby people who
doubt their partner’s love and commitment consequently behave in ways that drive their
partner away. Yet relational insecurity is difficult to overcome. Insecure individuals tend to
ascribe considerable meaning to a partner’s negative behavior but trivial meaning to their
positive behavior. The intervention described in this chapter was designed to address the
latter phenomenon. Participants recall an instance of receiving a compliment from their
partner, then are encouraged to view this compliment more broadly as a meaningful and
significant example of their partner’s admiration for them. I have demonstrated in numerous
studies that this abstract reframing intervention can foster a greater sense of relationship
security and subsequent positive relationship behavior among individuals who are typically
insecure in their relationships.
BACKGROUND
Romantic relationships have much potential to be rewarding and affirming. The confi-
dent belief that one is cared for and supported is hugely beneficial to both psychological
and physical well-being (Myers, 2000), and relationship satisfaction is a strong contribu-
tor to overall life satisfaction (Gustavson, Røysamb, Borren, Torvik, & Karevold, 2016).
Yet many such relationships are marred by conflict and doubt. Several of the factors that
contribute to relationship troubles can be targets for intervention. For example, dyadic
factors like poor communication can be addressed with training in problem-solving skills
385
386 III. Conflict and Relationships
either through psychoeducational programs (Halford, Markman, & Stanley, 2008), con-
flict reappraisal exercises (Finkel, Slotter, Luchies, Walton, & Gross, 2013; see Slotter &
Luchies, Chapter 16, this volume), or couple therapy (e.g., Sevier, Eldridge, Jones, Doss,
& Christensen, 2008). Contextual factors like external stressors can fray relationships
(Neff & Karney, 2004), so stress management strategies may be helpful for alleviating
relationship troubles. The research reported in this chapter focuses on a psychological
intervention designed to improve relationships for individuals with low self-esteem (LSEs)
by addressing a social-cognitive factor: the tendency to interpret the meaning of partners’
behavior in a maladaptive way.
Although there is little evidence that LSEs are any less attractive, intelligent, or capa-
ble than their high-self-esteem counterparts (HSEs), LSEs tend to view various aspects
of their lives more negatively (Baumeister, Campbell, Krueger, & Vohs, 2003; Cameron
& Granger, 2019; Orth, Robins, & Widman, 2012) and they are inclined to interpret
positive information about the self in a relatively negative light (Danielsson & Bengtsson,
2016). In romantic relationships specifically, LSEs are generally less secure and satisfied
(Fincham & Bradbury, 1993; Murray, Holmes, & Griffin, 1996) and they underestimate
how much their partners love and value them (Murray, Holmes, & Griffin, 2000). These
tendencies push LSEs in the direction of defensive self-protection; if they can keep a bit
of distance from their partner, it won’t be as painful when they eventually receive the
rejection they expect is inevitable. Moreover, when feeling acutely threatened, such as
when facing a partner’s transgression, LSEs often become cold and critical toward their
partner, again as a means of protecting themselves against hurt. Derogating their partner
allows them to feel they are not losing anything of substantial value. In an unfortunate
self-fulfilling prophecy, these defensive responses to perceived threats contribute to a
decrease in partners’ satisfaction over time (Murray, Bellavia, Rose, & Griffin, 2003).
There are at least two related psychological processes in which LSEs engage that
serve to maintain their insecurities. One, they abstract from negative relationship events,
reading broader meaning and significance into even minor, everyday kinds of slights (e.g.,
they might interpret a partner’s bad mood as a sign of their partner’s waning affection for
them). Second, they fail to abstract from positive relationship events, often seeing them as
little more than isolated incidents that do not signify anything of importance about their
partner’s feelings toward them. What’s more, LSEs may even make a negative attribution
about their partner’s motivation for something like a compliment—perhaps their partner
felt sorry for them, or was preparing to ask for a favor.
There is an extensive literature on the causes and consequences of partner attribu-
tion processes, but it has focused primarily on the meaning people make of their partner’s
negative or potentially threatening behavior, like during a conflict discussion (Pearce &
Halford, 2008). Indeed, a number of individual difference variables linked to insecurity
(low self-esteem, anxious attachment style, neuroticism, depression) predict the tendency
to make distress-maintaining attributions for partners’ transgressions (Bellavia & Mur-
ray, 2003; Collins, Ford, Guichard, & Allard, 2006; Karney, Bradbury, Fincham, & Sul-
livan, 1994; Pearce & Halford, 2008; Sümer & Cozzarelli, 2004; Uebelacker & Whis-
man, 2005). Critically, these attributions have long-term consequences for relationship
well-being (Karney & Bradbury, 2000).
Far less research has focused on the kinds of attributions for positive partner behav-
ior that may also serve to maintain insecurity. Theoretically, one should be less likely to
attribute a partner’s positive behavior to caring motives if that behavior is perceived to
The Abstract Reframing Intervention 387
be facilitated by external factors, or to benefit the partner (e.g., the partner’s act of kind-
ness is public and makes a good impression on others; Kelley, 1973). Additionally, when
people who are insecure believe their partner is aware of their insecurities, they are more
likely to suspect that their partner’s positive affirmations are not authentic (Lemay &
Clark, 2008).
How can LSEs be made to see their value in their partner’s eyes? Some attempts to
boost LSEs’ sense of security with positive feedback have actually backfired. For example,
giving LSEs feedback that their questionnaire responses showed they were high in “con-
siderateness” made them feel more insecure (compared to a no-feedback control condi-
tion; Murray, Holmes, MacDonald, & Ellsworth, 1998). Even prompting LSEs to affirm
their own worth as a partner, by repeating “I am a lovable person,” made LSEs feel more
anxious (Wood, Perunovic, & Lee, 2009). More broadly, thinking about personal suc-
cesses of various sorts can also increase LSEs’ anxious feelings (Wood, Heimpel, Newby-
Clark, & Ross, 2005). Such efforts are consistent with the “self-esteem movement” that
aimed to increase self-esteem for the purpose of preventing associated social problems
(Singal, 2017). Unfortunately, LSEs have defenses against such efforts. When positive
feedback or self-statements are externally imposed, participants question whether they
truly apply to themselves. HSEs are likely to conclude they do apply and then move on.
LSEs, on the other hand, are also liable to be more equivocal, and search their memories
for evidence that both supports and weakens that conclusion.
In designing the abstract reframing intervention (ARI), I considered how one might
“fly under the radar” of LSEs’ defenses. How could LSEs be affirmed without activating
self-doubts and self-evaluation concerns? One key component was to have LSEs choose
their own personal quality on which they were affirmed. False feedback from an experi-
menter may not be easily believed if there is clear evidence to the contrary. I expected that
when LSEs had a chance to recall their own example of a compliment from their partner,
they would likely recall one that they felt fairly confident about already. The second com-
ponent was to have LSEs take their recalled compliment and subtly encourage them to
ratchet up its meaning.
The intervention thus appears like this: On one page, participants are asked to
“Think of a time when your current romantic partner told you how much he/she liked
something about you. For example, a personal quality or ability you have that he/she
thinks very highly of, or something you did that really impressed him/her.” They are then
asked to write down a few cue words that would identify that memory to them, note how
long ago it occurred, and then turn to the next page to describe the compliment more
fully. On the next page, the intervention instructions are as follows: “Explain why your
partner admired you. Describe what it meant to you and its significance for your rela-
tionship.” These instructions assume that the compliment reflected the partner’s broader
view of the participant (i.e., admired them), and that the compliment must have been
meaningful and significant in some way.
The Linguistic Category Model (Semin & Fiedler, 1988; Semin & De Poot, 1997)
was the primary inspiration for the wording of the ARI. This model makes the distinction
between action verbs like said and state verbs like admired, with the state verb implying
that the behavior has lasted for a longer period and is more likely to recur in the future.
I expected that the use of a state verb would lead LSEs to perceive the compliments to be
more broad and global in their implications about their value to their partner. Research
in a number of other domains has supported the contention that subtle linguistic cues
388 III. Conflict and Relationships
can indeed have significant effects on self-perceptions and behavior (e.g., helping: Bryan,
Master, & Walton, 2014; voting: Bryan, Walton, Rogers, & Dweck, 2011).
One of the other conditions used for comparison instructed participants, after think-
ing of a compliment, to “Describe exactly what your partner said to you. Include any
details you can recall about where you two were at the time, what you were doing, what
you were both wearing, etc.” Labeled as the “concrete” condition, this allowed us to test
whether immersion in and elaboration on the incidence of positive feedback, which LSEs
might typically avoid, was sufficient to yield beneficial outcomes. A control condition
instructed participants to simply “Describe the event below.” This condition allowed us
to get a sense of self-esteem differences in spontaneous responses to compliments (see
Appendix 17.1 for full materials).
PSYCHOLOGICAL PROCESSES
The participants in these intervention studies have been primarily young adults in dating
relationships. The purpose of the ARI is to encourage them to consider the potential for
broader meaning in their partner’s feedback, and to consider the impact such affirma-
tions may have on their relationship more globally. In doing so, there seems to be a cor-
responding move from primarily past-tense language to also include some present-tense
language. The compliment becomes something their partner thinks or feels about them
that is indicative of the kind of relationship they have, rather than something their part-
ner simply said at one time. This is particularly important for LSEs, who are less inclined
to reach this kind of broad conclusion naturally. Our research showed that when asked
to describe compliments from their romantic partners with no further instructions (i.e.,
the control condition), LSEs tend to use more past-tense verbs than do HSEs (Marigold,
Holmes, & Ross, 2007). What follows are two examples of participants in the control
condition whose partner complimented them on their intelligence. The first is from a par-
ticipant who scored low in self-esteem (over 1 standard deviation below the mean). Note
the restriction to a single incident in the past.
“I got the best score in a course in university. He took the course in the term after my
course and the professor mentioned the best score for the previous term was exactly
my score. So he told me afterwards that I was smart.”
The second is from a participant who scored high in self-esteem (over 1 standard devia-
tion above the mean). Note the description of the partner’s ongoing positive view of the
participant.
“I get nervous about university, because I am in first year, and my boyfriend keeps
reassuring me. He says I am very smart and that he is so proud of me. He is so loving
and makes me feel like I can do anything.”
How does the intervention affect how people reflect on a compliment? To see this, we
compared the concrete condition and the ARI condition. In the concrete condition, par-
ticipants are asked for further details about the time the compliment was given—where
The Abstract Reframing Intervention 389
they were, who was there, what they were doing. Again, for the two examples that follow,
the participants indicated that their partner complimented them on their intelligence.
In the concrete condition, people stuck to what happened in the past. For example, one
participant in this condition explained:
“A week ago I won an award for being a top student at U of W. I told my boyfriend
over the phone, on his way home from work. He said, ‘That’s awesome! I’m proud
of you, you’re so intelligent.’ That same day he told everyone he knew that I was a
top student at U of W.”
In the abstract condition, people talked about how the past event indicated ongoing posi-
tive meaning for their relationship. For example, a different participant in the ARI condi-
tion went on to explain:
In another set of examples, two participants indicated that their partner compli-
mented them on being caring. In the concrete condition, the participant went on to say:
“He admired me because he was going through a tough time, and he realized he
wasn’t alone and that I cared about him. This was important to me because it meant
that he knew my feelings for him and that I want to help him the best I can.”
It is important to note that LSEs are not always less positive than HSEs about the
exchange of compliments between romantic partners. I conducted one study (Hoplock,
Stinson, Marigold, & Fisher, 2018) to compare LSEs’ reactions to compliments directed
at the self versus others, using hypothetical compliment scenarios. Would LSEs also mini-
mize the meaning of compliments shared between other couples? Half of the participants
were randomly assigned to read about a compliment one hypothetical person gave to their
romantic partner. The other half of the participants were asked to imagine receiving the
same compliment from their romantic partner. As expected, LSEs in the self-compliment
condition were less enthusiastic about the compliment, and believed it was less sincere
than did LSEs in the other-compliment condition. The simple effect of self-esteem was
significant in the self-compliment condition but not in the other-compliment condition.
LSEs did not differ from HSEs in their judgments about compliments exchanged between
other people. These findings confirm that LSEs do have the capacity to see the potential
for enjoyment and meaning in partners’ praise; it’s only when the compliment applies to
themselves that they respond differently than HSEs, which can be taken as evidence of
motivated thinking.
390 III. Conflict and Relationships
EMPIRICAL EVIDENCE
Outcomes
The first set of published studies established the basic effect of the ARI, both immediately
after completing the intervention and in a follow-up 2 weeks later. In Study 2 of Marigold
et al. (2007), all participants first thought of a time their partner said something nice to
them. The control condition was simply asked to describe that event, with no further
instructions. The concrete condition was asked to describe the concrete details surround-
ing what was said, where, who was there, and so forth. The ARI condition was asked
to explain why their partner admired them, and describe what it meant to them and
why it was significant to their relationship. There were a number of dependent variables
that showed similar patterns. Among LSEs, those in the ARI condition reported greater
positive feelings about the compliment, state self-esteem, felt security, and relationship
valuing than did those in the control or concrete conditions. In fact, LSEs in the ARI
condition reported outcomes that were just as high as HSEs (who did not differ between
conditions). The typical pattern is illustrated in Figure 17.1.
Effects over Time
This effect persisted over time. In two studies, results of a follow-up questionnaire deliv-
ered 2–3 weeks later showed that LSEs who had been in the ARI condition at Time 1
continued to feel more secure than did LSEs in either the control or concrete conditions,
mirroring the pattern of results from Time 1. LSEs who had been in the ARI condition
6.5
6.0
ARI
Concrete
Control
5.5
5.0
LSE HSE
FIGURE 17.1. Self-reported felt security as a function of self-esteem and condition (Study 2 from
Marigold, Holmes, & Ross, 2007).
The Abstract Reframing Intervention 391
also reported a greater frequency of positive partner behavior since Time 1. Although
participants did not receive an explicit reminder of the compliment prior to making their
ratings at Time 2, it is possible that receiving the follow-up questionnaire was sufficient
to remind participants of their positive experience at Time 1. That may account for the
increased felt security ratings and perceptions of partner positivity reported by LSEs in
the ARI condition (i.e., they felt more secure when completing both Time 1 and Time 2
questionnaires but not in between). Another possibility is that receiving the ARI at Time
1 actually changed LSEs’ experience, or the perception of their experience, in the inter-
vening 2- to 3-week period (i.e., their felt security and perceptions of positive partner
behavior remained high between Time 1 and Time 2). The next set of studies suggest
some support for the latter explanation.
A study reported in a later paper employed partner reports to verify that the ARI
had an effect beyond participants’ perceptions, on their observable behavior (Marigold,
Holmes, & Ross, 2010, Study 2). In this study, participants were randomly assigned to
complete the ARI or a control condition with no instructions for describing the compli-
ment (see Marigold et al., 2007). Findings on relationship ratings replicated previous
research (Marigold et al., 2007) in that LSEs rated their relationships more positively
in the ARI condition than in the control condition, as high as HSEs in either condi-
tion. Moreover, 2 weeks later, partners were contacted to report on the participants’
behavior over the preceding 2 weeks. Results showed that LSE participants who had
initially received the ARI had partners who reported lower frequency of critical, nega-
tive behavior from the participants in that time period (compared to LSEs who were in
the control condition), as low as HSE participants in both conditions (see Figure 17.2).
2.9
2.5
ARI
Control
2.1
1.7
LSE HSE
This finding is consistent with the notion that LSEs who have received the ARI are actu-
ally changing their behavior. In the previous set of studies (Marigold et al., 2007), when
LSEs reported noticing more positive partner behavior in the weeks following the ARI,
they may have actually been eliciting that positive behavior as a response to their own
behavior changes.
Mechanism
In Marigold et al. (2007), the Linguistic Inquiry Word Count (LIWC; Pennebaker, Fran-
cis, & Booth, 2001) program was used to analyze the verb tense used in the narratives
participants wrote to describe their compliment. These results showed that when partici-
pants were given no specific instructions on how to describe the compliment they recalled
(simply, “Describe the event”), LSEs used more past-tense verbs than did HSEs. In the
ARI condition, LSEs used fewer past-tense verbs and more present-tense verbs than did
LSEs in the control or concrete conditions. HSEs did not differ in their verb use among
the three conditions.
In addition to feeling more positively about the compliment in the ARI condition,
LSEs also rated it as more sincere and thought they were more deserving of the com-
pliment (Marigold et al., 2007). Thus, the abstraction seemed to lead them to a more
internal attribution for the motivation behind their partner’s praise, which mediated fur-
ther effects on state self-esteem and relationship ratings. In unpublished data (Marigold,
2011), I found that LSEs who think of an “admired” quality compared to a “compli-
mented” quality are more confident in their possession of that quality, which further
supports the use of a state verb (admired) to connote a sense of endurance.
Process
Another study (Marigold et al., 2010, Study 1) provided evidence that the ARI could
reduce LSEs’ defensive reactions to relationship threats. The threat in Study 1 involved a
“Secret Selves” exercise (Murray, Rose, Bellavia, Holmes, & Kusche, 2002) where par-
ticipants were instructed to think about several aspects of themselves they try to keep
hidden from their partner, knowing conflict could arise as a result. LSEs who completed
this exercise rated their relationship significantly more negatively than LSEs who did not
(HSEs were unaffected by the manipulation). It is thought that contemplating a nega-
tive evaluation from their partner prompts LSEs to become more negative and critical
about their relationship as a means of self-protection. It is less painful to be rejected by a
partner who is not all that great anyway. As described earlier, this kind of self-protective
behavior in response to threat has important consequences in that it actually contributes
to the demise of the relationship over time (Murray, Griffin, Rose, & Bellavia, 2003). Yet
in our study, for LSEs who were asked to complete the ARI prior to the threat exercise,
no such relationship derogation effect was found. These LSEs remained just as positive
about their relationship as were those who were not threatened.
Furthering the evidence for behavioral effects of the ARI, another paper reported the
effects of the ARI on a subsequent conflict discussion among romantic partners (Mari-
gold & Anderson, 2016). In this study, partners’ conflict history was the moderating vari-
able. A history of difficult or destructive conflict interactions might function similarly
to LSE in this context in that concerns about partners’ care and responsiveness become
The Abstract Reframing Intervention 393
activated. Indeed, partners with a higher conflict history had lower positive expectations
of how a conflict discussion would go in the lab. However, the ARI increased posi-
tive expectations among couples with a stronger conflict history (those with less history
of conflict were not affected by the manipulation). Subsequently, the increased positive
expectations translated to less distress, more positive behavior (according to self- and
partner reports), and better quality conflict discussions (according to coding by observers
who were blind to both condition and conflict history).
It is worth noting that in all of these studies, coding by objective observers revealed
that there was no association of self- esteem with the positivity of the compliments
selected—that is, LSEs and HSEs recalled equally positive compliments; the difference
lay in the way in which they naturally described them and the meaning they made of
them. As Walton and Wilson (2018) point out, psychologically wise interventions assume
that objective opportunities for enhancement may already be present, but the meaning
people make of social information may limit them from taking advantage.
Heterogeneity
In one unpublished study (Marigold, 2010), I attempted to replicate the basic ARI effect
with married couples, using self-esteem as a moderator, but the effect was not significant.
Although one cannot make any firm conclusions based on this single sample, it seems
reasonable that one incidence of praise has less potential to be consequential to partners
who are seriously committed. It may take much larger or sustained gestures of affection
to influence the felt security of long-term coupled or married LSEs. Further research can
explore this notion more fully.
COUSINS
Self-affirmation is one intervention that bears some similarity to the ARI (see Sherman,
Lokhande, Müller, & Cohen, Chapter 3, this volume). The self-affirmation exercise typi-
cally has participants choose their most cherished value from a list provided and describe
how they have acted consistently with it in the past. This process seems to generate a
more expansive view of the self and its resources, which buffers people from subsequent
threats to the self. The similarity between the two interventions lies in the process of hav-
ing participants generate their own affirmations, which may be particularly important
for individuals who are chronically insecure, as discussed earlier in this chapter. Affirma-
tions that are externally imposed are more likely to activate self-doubts and evaluative
concerns.
Numerous positive outcomes of the self-affirmation intervention have been reported
in relationships as well as in the domains of education and health (see Sherman et al.,
Chapter 3, this volume). In regard to relationships, for example, self-affirmation has been
shown to improve relational security more broadly (i.e., with family, friends, and roman-
tic partners), and facilitate more welcoming social behavior with a stranger (Stinson,
Logel, Shepherd, & Zanna, 2011). In another study, self-affirmation increased adoles-
cents’ general prosocial feelings and behaviors over a 3-month period (Thomaes, Bush-
man, Orobio de Castro, & Reijntjes, 2012). Self-affirmation may not be as well suited
to improving positive feelings and behavior in one’s romantic relationship. The ARI is a
394 III. Conflict and Relationships
As described earlier, the ARI requires participants to “Think of a time when your current
romantic partner told you how much he/she liked something about you. For example, a
personal quality or ability you have that he/she thinks very highly of, or something you
did that really impressed him/her.” They are then asked to write down a few cue words
that would identify that memory to them, note how long ago it occurred, and then turn
to the next page to “Explain why your partner admired you. Describe what it meant to
you and its significance for your relationship.” (See Appendix 17.1 for full materials.)
This intervention has been delivered both in person and online, with no apparent dif-
ference.
The intervention can be adapted to encourage abstraction of other positive partner
behaviors. In one unpublished study (Marigold & Anderson, 2011), the manipulation
was adapted to reflect on a partner’s caring behavior rather than positive feedback. Spe-
cifically, participants were asked to “Think of a time when your romantic partner did
something nice for you. For example, helped you with homework, brought you a present,
put your preferences ahead of his or her own.” In the abstract reframing condition, they
were then asked to “Explain why your partner cared for you so much. Describe what
it meant to you and its significance for your relationship.” Participants in the control
condition were simply asked to “Describe the event in the space below.” Results showed
that in the control condition, LSEs experienced less positive affect and made fewer posi-
tive attributions for the behavior than did HSEs. LSEs in the abstract condition reported
more positive affect and attributions than did LSEs in the control condition, as high as
HSEs in either condition.
A study published by Gaucher et al. (2012, Study 4) used the same ARI but sub-
stituted “close friend” for “partner.” Participants were then asked to make a video that
would ostensibly be shown to that friend in which they described a negative experience
they had at their university. Results showed that LSEs were more expressive (according to
both self-report and observer coding) in the ARI condition than in the control condition.
The authors theorized that LSEs are generally reticent to share their true thoughts and
feelings because that leaves them vulnerable to rejection, which may seem likely given
their lower self-worth. Once LSEs’ sense of perceived regard was increased by the ARI,
they felt more comfortable expressing themselves authentically, which is critical to foster-
ing closeness and intimacy.
In a completely different domain, Zunick, Fazio, and Vasey (2015) adapted the ARI
for use with participants who hold relatively negative self-views to encourage them to gen-
eralize from success experiences in public speaking. They used a “directed abstraction”
technique for which the ARI “was one of the primary inspirations” (Zunick et al., 2015,
p. 4). After recalling a past public speaking success (Study 2) or having a public speaking
success in the lab (Study 3), participants in the directed abstraction condition were asked
to “Explain why you were able to achieve such a successful performance. Begin by com-
pleting the sentence stem below. ‘I was able to achieve a successful performance because I
The Abstract Reframing Intervention 395
am . . . ’ ” Participants in the control condition instead responded to the prompt “Describe
how you performed as you did in this situation. What did you do?” Generalization from
success was measured with a composite variable including participants’ views of their
overall public speaking abilities, expectations for future performance, and self-related
affect. The directed abstraction led people with negative self-views to generalize more
from a successful public speaking experience than they would have otherwise, leading to
a greater likelihood of returning to the activity and more persistence in the face of dif-
ficulty.
NUANCES AND MISCONCEPTIONS
A second way that shifting the original materials can undermine, and in this case,
reverse the effects of the ARI involves applying it to the partner. In one study (Marigold,
2018), participants were asked to “Think of a time when you told your current romantic
partner how much you liked something about him/her. For example, a personal quality
or ability he/she has that you think very highly of, or something he/she did that really
impressed you.” The abstract condition was then asked to “Explain why you admired
your partner. Describe what you think it meant to your partner and its significance for
your relationship.” The control condition was simply asked to “Describe the event in the
space below.” Essentially, the original wording of the ARI was taken and then shifted to
focus on a compliment the participant gave to, rather than received from, their partner.
Although writing abstractly about a compliment received from one’s partner increases
LSEs’ felt security, in the modified version, writing abstractly about a compliment given
to one’s partner decreased LSEs’ felt security. It is thought that this occurs because LSEs
are particularly sensitive to feelings of inferiority in their relationship (Murray et al.,
2005), and these feelings may be activated when considering their admiration for their
partner.
It is also worth noting that giving participants false positive feedback is quite dif-
ferent from the ARI’s approach of encouraging participants to generate their own affir-
mations, and in fact, the former can backfire. One example comes from Murray et al.
(1998), where participants received feedback from the experimenter that a questionnaire
they completed indicated they were high (or low) in considerateness. The positive feed-
back actually made LSEs feel more insecure (Murray et al., 1998). The ARI is clearly
more effective than this type of approach.
IMPLICATIONS FOR PRACTICE
Therapists may use this research in either individual or couple therapy. With LSEs, thera-
pists may challenge the notion that they are not well loved or will assuredly be rejected
by partners by asking them to bring to mind evidence of their partner’s love and care.
However, if they stop there, it will be easy for the LSEs to dismiss past compliments or
generous behaviors as isolated incidents, perhaps brought on by the partner’s sense of
obligation or pity for the LSE. Therapists can prevent this dismissal by prompting LSEs
to describe the meaning and significance of these acts of admiration or care.
In couple therapy, therapists may encourage partners to express appreciation or
admiration for each other (e.g., Gottman & Silver, 1999). Whether done in therapy or
in everyday interactions, I believe there are three key considerations that can make or
break the success of delivering compliments to LSEs. One is the particular quality cho-
sen for the compliment. Recall that in the ARI, participants select their own quality on
which they’ve been complimented, which they then go on to write about in an abstract
way. This allows them to select a quality about which they are relatively confident, or
view as important to their sense of self. Thus, people who are asked to express admira-
tion to their partners should take care to choose a quality that their partner can easily
embrace. It might intuitively seem more impactful to compliment partners on a quality
about which they frequently express doubt, but this strategy could ultimately backfire if
the complimenting further fuels the doubts (e.g., “Why is my partner saying this? Do they
really mean it? Do they just feel bad for me?”).
The Abstract Reframing Intervention 397
Second, given LSEs’ suspicions about the authenticity of positive feedback (Lemay
& Clark, 2008), partners should take care to reserve compliments for qualities that they
truly appreciate in their partner, to be given in times in which they are feeling genuinely
loving and affectionate. Given how readily LSEs attribute ulterior motives to their part-
ner’s expression of affection, it seems prudent to ensure those expressions are made with-
out any ulterior motive that might be detected.
A third consideration is phrasing the compliment with some positive meaning for the
relationship implied. For example, rather than saying, “You are really helpful,” one might
say, “I appreciate how willing you are to help when I need it. It makes me feel like we
are a team.” One might need to be careful to keep it simple and brief; going on too long
about the partner’s helpfulness and its significance for the relationship may eventually be
perceived as manipulative.
Ultimately, these are speculations based on prior related research. More targeted
research should be conducted on couples delivering compliments to each other, both
in and out of a clinical setting. Further, therapists must use judgment to determine the
context in which this activity may ultimately be helpful rather than harmful. If there is
overwhelming evidence that the partner really does not care for the individual and treats
them poorly, they may need to be coached to leave the relationship rather than increase
an unwarranted sense of security and commitment.
Beyond therapeutic settings, people may benefit from the ARI by learning about
it through literature in which it has been described. For example, in Eli Finkel’s (2017)
popular book The All-or-Nothing Marriage, the ARI is cited as one example of a “love
hack,” a strategy that involves “tweaking how we think about our partner and relation-
ship” (p. 183). Love hacks on their own are not sufficient to rescue a failing relationship
or even to make a stable relationship flourish. But they are relatively easy and accessible,
they do not require much investment of time, and they do not rely on cooperation from
the partner. The hope is that couples may benefit in at least some small way from using a
variety of love hacks and even developing their own.
Presenting the ARI as one simple yet meaningful strategy, among others, for facili-
tating relationship well-being may be an effective way to reach a larger audience. How-
ever, no research has yet addressed whether awareness of the ARI and its intended
effects may undermine its usefulness. Research on self-affirmation has shown that it
is not awareness per se that can undermine its effect but rather the feeling of being
manipulated or controlled (Silverman, Logel, & Cohen, 2013). This is an important
distinction to make in future research investigating whether LSEs may still benefit from
compliment abstraction once they know its intended effects; perhaps the answer is yes if
they feel personally motivated to try the exercise themselves, but no if they feel someone
is imposing it upon them.
Most of the previous research has focused on self-esteem as the moderator of the
intervention’s effect, but changing the context may change the moderator. At a concep-
tual level, the most appropriate moderator may be one that distinguishes reactions to
the social information being conveyed in that particular context. Thus, self-esteem is
most appropriate when considering reactions to feedback about the self; a measure of
conflict history seems to be more appropriate when considering reactions to anticipated
and actual conflict. Indeed, as discussed earlier in this chapter, a dyadic study of couples
discussing a conflict in their relationship showed the ARI was particularly beneficial for
couples with a stronger history of destructive conflict (Marigold & Anderson, 2016).
398 III. Conflict and Relationships
IMPLICATIONS FOR PSYCHOLOGICAL THEORY
These findings reinforce a long history of research showing that meaning matters (see
Walton & Wilson, 2018, for a review). To the extent that people believe their partners’
negative statements and behaviors signify a lack of care or concern for the self, they may
become more insecure or dissatisfied. To the extent that people believe their partner’s
positive statements and behaviors fail to signify care and concern for the self, their secu-
rity and satisfaction will not benefit. Insecurity and dissatisfaction further predispose
people to make relationship-threatening attributions, and so the cycle continues (Brad-
bury & Fincham, 1990).
This research also tells us something about how relational insecurities operate. Peo-
ple’s motivations shape their perceptions, such that those who are primarily motivated to
protect themselves from hurt (e.g., LSEs) actually “see” things differently. Just as LSEs
more readily identify acceptance cues directed at others than at themselves (Cameron,
Stinson, Gaetz, & Balchen, 2010), they make more positive meaning of compliments
directed toward others than to themselves (Hoplock et al., 2018). Thus there is little evi-
dence to suggest that LSEs have some kind of perception deficit that prevents them from
being able to accurately recognize cues of acceptance and positive regard—rather, they
falter in these tasks only when their own sense of self and security are on the line. Under-
standing this phenomenon should be helpful to partners of individuals who are insecure,
who often find that their behavior does not get interpreted as intended.
As with other “wise” interventions, the ARI research shows that we can target small
changes that, with some reinforcement, can have larger, long-term effects. It also tells
us that when we cannot change people’s reality (e.g., by mandating the number or kind
of compliments people pay their partners), there is still some utility in changing the way
people think about their reality (e.g., seeing the potential for broader meaning in the
compliments they do receive).
FUTURE DIRECTIONS
The highest priority next step for this research is to determine how partners of LSEs may
deliver compliments more effectively, in a manner that prevents LSEs from dismissing
them as genuine. People may naturally feel compelled to give their LSE partners feed-
back on qualities about which they know their partner is insecure in an effort to combat
that insecurity. However, there is good evidence to suggest that approach may very well
backfire (e.g., Brummelman, Thomaes, Overbeek, et al., 2014; Lemay & Clark, 2008).
Instead, people may be better off giving LSE partners compliments on qualities about
which they appear to be relatively confident—such qualities may lend themselves bet-
ter to spontaneous abstracting by the LSEs. As discussed earlier in the chapter, people
may also be able to give their partners compliments in a way that implies some meaning,
though they must take care that those efforts to affirm are not too heavy-handed.
For the LSEs’ part, ideally they would not continually require a researcher’s gentle
encouragement toward making meaning out of positive partner feedback. Over time,
they can practice moving away from reflexively dismissing compliments outright and
giving them further consideration. Of course not all compliments will be that meaning-
ful, and sometimes their partner may actually be just trying to make them feel better or
The Abstract Reframing Intervention 399
prepare them to be asked a favor. Another option would be to simply say “thank you”
when they receive a compliment, allowing it to stand as is without any further consid-
eration of whether the evidence actually backs it up. Perhaps this practice of acceptance
would eventually extend beyond the moment.
Given the enormous potential of romantic relationships to be rewarding and affirm-
ing, and to benefit both psychological and physical well-being more broadly (Gustavson
et al., 2016; Myers, 2000), the problem of LSEs’ relationship insecurity is an important
one. There is much opportunity to alleviate this problem when one understands the psy-
chological processes involved in the experience of low self-esteem and of meaning making
in relationships, and the “wise interventions” that are designed with this in mind.
REFERENCES
Baumeister, R. F., Campbell, J. D., Krueger, J. I., & Vohs, K. D. (2003). Does high self-esteem
cause better performance, interpersonal success, happiness, or healthier lifestyles? Psycho-
logical Science in the Public Interest, 4, 1–44.
Bellavia, G., & Murray, S. (2003). Did I do that?: Self esteem-related differences in reactions to
romantic partners’ mood. Personal Relationships, 10, 77–95.
Bradbury, T. N., & Fincham, F. D. (1990). Attributions in marriage: Review and critique. Psycho-
logical Bulletin, 107, 3–33.
Brummelman, E., Thomaes, S., Orobio de Castro, B., Overbeek, G., & Bushman, B. J. (2014).
“That’s not just beautiful—that’s incredibly beautiful!”: The adverse impact of inflated praise
on children with low self-esteem. Psychological Science, 25, 728–735.
Brummelman, E., Thomaes, S., Overbeek, G., Orobio de Castro, B., van den Hout, M. A., &
Bushman, B. J. (2014). On feeding those hungry for praise: Person praise backfires in children
with low self-esteem Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 9–14.
Bryan, C. J., Master, A., & Walton, G. M. (2014). “Helping” versus “being a helper”: Invoking the
self to increase helping in young children. Child Development, 85, 1836–1842.
Bryan, C. J., Walton, G. M., Rogers, T., & Dweck, C. S. (2011). Motivating voter turnout by
invoking the self. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 108, 12653–
12656.
Cameron, J. J., & Granger, S. (2019). Does self-esteem have an interpersonal imprint beyond self-
reports?: A meta-analysis of self-esteem and objective interpersonal indicators. Personality
and Social Psychology Review, 23, 73–102.
Cameron, J. J., Stinson, D. A., Gaetz, R., & Balchen, S. (2010). Acceptance is in the eye of the
beholder: Self-esteem and motivated perceptions of acceptance from the opposite sex. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 513–529.
Collins, N. L., Ford, M., Guichard, A. C., & Allard, L. M. (2006). Working models of attachment
and attribution processes in intimate relationships. Personality and Social Psychology Bul-
letin, 32, 201–219.
Danielsson, M., & Bengtsson, H. (2016). Global self-esteem and the processing of positive infor-
mation about the self. Personality and Individual Differences, 99, 325–330.
Fincham, F. D., & Bradbury, T. N. (1993). Marital satisfaction, depression, and attributions: A
longitudinal analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 442–452.
Finkel, E. (2017). The all-or-nothing marriage: How the best marriages work. New York: Dutton.
Finkel, E. J., Slotter, E. B., Luchies, L. B., Walton, G. M., & Gross, J. J. (2013). A brief interven-
tion to promote conflict reappraisal preservers marital quality over time. Psychological Sci-
ence, 24, 1595–1601.
Gaucher, D., Wood, J. V., Stinson, D. A., Forest, A. L., Holmes, J. G., & Logel, C. (2012). Perceived
400 III. Conflict and Relationships
Cue words:
Concrete Condition
Describe exactly what your partner said to you. Include any details you can recall about where you
two were at the time, what you were doing, what you were both wearing, etc.
Control Condition
Describe the event in the space below.
PA R T IV
SUSTAINABILITY
C H A P TE R 18
Social norms interventions work by communicating to a target audience that other people
are engaging in the desired behavior, or will approve of you if you do the behavior (or con-
versely that other people are avoiding an undesired behavior, or will disapprove of you if you
do it). Social norms interventions have been used to address a number of social and envi-
ronmental problems with positive effects that have been documented to persist over time.
Key considerations in social norms interventions are that they are believable, utilize cred-
ible sources, and reference meaningful groups. Importantly, social norms interventions are
often most effective at changing the behavior of those who frequently engage in undesired
behaviors (e.g., consuming electricity) or who infrequently engage in desired behaviors (e.g.,
recycling). This chapter includes sample messages and advice for successfully implement-
ing a social norms intervention. The process by which social norms affect behavior and the
implications for theory and practice are also discussed.
On a hillside in Italy, a large crowd has gathered to sing about how they would “like
to buy the world a Coke” (www.youtube.com/watch?v=C2406n8_rUw). As the camera
pans the crowd we see people with different skin colors and clothing presumably repre-
senting a multitude of countries and ethnicities from around the world. The crowd con-
tinues their heartfelt lip-syncing with “It’s the real thing. Coke is what the world wants
today.” This commercial, released by Coca-Cola in 1971, went on to become one of its
most famous (Andrews & Barbash, 2016). The theme of the ad is clear: People all over
the world are drinking Coke and you should, too.
In this chapter, we describe how social norms can be harnessed as a wise interven-
tion to influence behavior. As in the Coca-Cola commercial, social norms interventions
405
406 IV. Sustainability
provide “social proof” to the audience that other people engage in a desired behavior (or
do not engage in an undesired behavior) in an attempt to leverage an individual’s need
to belong and the tendency to conform to the behavior of others. Because social norms
interventions more frequently utilize descriptive social norms, that is the primary focus
of this chapter. However, research on injunctive norms is included when it helps to clarify
the differences between the two types of norms and their underlying mechanisms. We
also focus primarily on research that utilizes explicit normative appeals and that attempts
to impact behavior, rather than just attitudes or beliefs.
BACKGROUND
There are two origin stories to be told for social norms interventions: one for the aca-
demic discussion of social norms in social psychology, and the other for the develop-
ment of social norms marketing to curb problematic drinking. We begin with the former.
Many social norms interventions carried out today have been influenced by the work
of Cialdini and colleagues in the early 1990s on the focus theory of normative conduct.
This work was built on an understanding of social norms as “rules and standards that
are understood by members of a group, and that guide and/or constrain social behavior
without the force of laws” (Cialdini & Trost, 1998, p. 152). This definition allows social
norms to exist at multiple levels, representing a shared understanding among members
of a group that can be as small as a family unit or group of friends, or as large as a
whole society. Cialdini and colleagues took this definition a step further by distinguish-
ing between two types of social norms that can influence behavior: descriptive norms
that describe what most people do, and injunctive norms that prescribe or proscribe what
people think should or should not be done. Furthermore, more than a half-dozen experi-
ments, many looking across real behavior in the field, provide substantial support for the
focus theory of normative conduct.
The focus theory of normative conduct proposes that norms must be salient or acti-
vated to guide behavior. Both descriptive and injunctive norms can be activated, and a
variety of manipulations can be used to bring a particular norm into focus. For example,
in one of their early experiments, Cialdini, Reno, and Kallgren (1990) exposed partici-
pants to either a clean (antilittering descriptive norm) or littered (prolittering descriptive
norm) parking garage and then had a confederate either walk by (low norm salience),
drop a crumpled bag (high-descriptive norm salience), or pick up a crumpled bag (high-
injunctive norm salience). Researchers recorded how many participants in each condition
littered a large handbill that had been left on the windshield of their car. The results
showed, first, that participants were more likely to litter the handbill when the environ-
ment was already littered versus when it was clean. Second (and more importantly), con-
sistent with the prediction of focus theory (Cialdini et al., 1990) that salient norms will
guide behavior, there was an interaction between the norm salience manipulation and the
manipulation of the environment. In the high-descriptive norm salience condition, par-
ticipants who saw the confederate drop the crumpled bag in an already littered parking
garage were just as likely as those who saw the confederate walk by to litter the handbill.
However, those who saw the confederate litter the crumpled bag into a clean environment
were much less likely to litter compared to those who saw the confederate just walk by.
When the confederate littered, they brought attention to the prevailing descriptive norm
The Social Norms Approach 407
and made it more likely that the participant would use that norm to inform their own
behavior. In the high-injunctive norm salience condition, participants littered less when
the confederate picked up the crumpled bag versus just walked by, regardless of whether
the environment was clean or littered (see Figure 18.1). Subsequent research showed
that a salient piece of litter, like a watermelon rind, in an otherwise clean environment,
could also activate the antilittering descriptive norm and that sweeping litter into a pile
and using an antilittering message on the handbill left on the windshield could activate
injunctive norms against littering with similar effects (Cialdini et al., 1990).
Let’s turn now to the second origin story for social norms interventions. The devel-
opment of social norms marketing began in the late 1980s with research at Hobart and
William Smith Colleges. This early research on college drinking norms showed that col-
lege students frequently overestimated how much and how regularly their peers were con-
suming alcohol (Perkins & Berkowitz, 1986). Subsequent research found that exagger-
ated beliefs about the prevalence of alcohol use and abuse was widespread across college
campuses of various sizes and in different regions of the country (Perkins, 2003). This
discovery of exaggerated or “misperceived” norms, combined with correlational research
showing that normative beliefs about the behavior of others are often a strong predictor
of one’s own behavior (e.g., Clapp & McDonnell, 2000), led to the development of the
social norms marketing approach.
The social norms marketing approach provided an alternative to the traditional
health promotion model. In the traditional model, problematic drinking was seen as a
consequence of favorable attitudes toward drinking. As a result, interventions designed
in the context of the traditional model focused on changing attitudes toward alcohol
use as a way to promote responsible drinking behavior. Typically, these attitude-change
interventions focused on educating the target audience about the dangers associated with
the harmful effects of alcohol, whether they be health related or legal risks. The social
40
Percent who littered handbill
35
30
25
20
15 Environment clean
Environment littered
10
0
None Descriptive Injunctive
(confederate (confederate (confederate
walks by) litters bag) picks up bag)
Activated norm
FIGURE 18.1. Percentage of people who littered a handbill based on which norm was activated
and the state of the environment. Figure redrawn using data from Reno, Cialdini, and Kallgren
(1993).
408 IV. Sustainability
norms marketing approach acknowledged that problematic drinking behavior might also
be caused by peer and social influences, real or imagined (see also Perkins, 2003, for a
detailed history of the emergence of the social norms approach).
PSYCHOLOGICAL PROCESSES
How does the social norms approach achieve lasting effects? As a wise intervention, the
social norms approach alters the way that people make sense of the world around them
by providing answers to psychologically important questions, such as “How can I behave
effectively in a given situation,” or “How should I behave to avoid disapproval from
important others?” The desire to find answers to these questions is driven by two of the
fundamental motives that underlie meaning making: the need to understand and the need
to belong (Walton & Wilson, 2018). Later in the chapter we discuss the details of three
mechanisms that result from these fundamental needs:
We also discuss how the social norms approach engages recursive processes between an
individual and their social environment that help to sustain changes into the future.
EMPIRICAL EVIDENCE
Outcomes
There is abundant evidence for the effectiveness of the social norms approach. Social
norms interventions have been used successfully to promote a wide range of health, pro-
social, and environmental behaviors. In the health domain, social norms interventions
have increased vegetable consumption (Robinson, Thomas, Aveyard, & Higgs, 2014),
hand washing (Lapinski, Maloney, Braz, & Shulman, 2013), and use of designated driv-
ers (Perkins, Linkenbach, Lewis, & Neighbors, 2010), and decreased sedentary behav-
ior (Priebe & Spink, 2015), binge drinking (Haines, 1998), pregame drinking (Burger,
LaSalvia, Hendricks, Mehdipour, & Neudeck, 2011), drinking and driving (Perkins et
al., 2010), and inappropriate antibiotic prescription by physicians (Linder et al., 2017).
In the prosocial domain, social norms interventions have spurred signing petitions (Mar-
getts, John, Escher, & Reissfelder, 2011), donating to charitable causes (Agerström, Carls-
son, Nicklasson, & Guntell, 2016), and voting (Gerber & Rogers, 2009). As evidenced
The Social Norms Approach 409
from this list, social norms interventions have broad generalizability across behaviors.
In the environmental domain, social norms interventions have increased curbside recy-
cling (Schultz, 1999), towel reuse among hotel guests (Goldstein, Cialdini, & Griskevi-
cius, 2008), water conservation (Ferraro & Price, 2013), composting household organics
(Phelps, Large, Schultz, & Ettlinger, 2017), and decreased energy consumption (Nolan,
Schultz, Cialdini, Goldstein, & Griskevicius, 2008), littering (Cialdini et al., 1990), and
theft of petrified wood at a national park (Cialdini et al., 2006).
In our own research on household energy conservation, we found that a message
utilizing social norms outperformed more traditional approaches (Nolan et al., 2008).
The research began with a survey of Californians to assess their energy-conservation
behavior, normative beliefs, and beliefs about what motivated them to conserve. A major-
ity of Californians reported engaging in many of the energy-conserving behaviors that
we asked about, including using fans instead of air-conditioning at night and turning off
unused lights. The results of this correlational study also showed that although respon-
dents were most likely to attribute their motivation for conserving energy to a desire to
protect the environment and future generations, in fact, the best predictor of their self-
reported energy-conservation behavior was their descriptive normative beliefs about the
energy-conserving behavior of others.
We followed up the statewide survey with a large-scale field experiment comparing
the effectiveness of a social norms message inviting households to “join your neighbors”
to more traditional appeals to “protect the environment,” “do your part for future gen-
erations,” and “save money.” Door hangers were distributed to participating households
for 4 weeks. Each week the door hanger promoted a different energy-saving behavior
along with a motivational appeal specific to the type of message. Those in the environ-
mental protection condition were told how much carbon dioxide emission they would
prevent, those in the social responsibility condition were told how they could reduce
demand for electricity, and those in the self-interest condition were told how performing
the recommended behavior would save them money. In the social norms condition, we
used information from the statewide survey to provide households with true information
about the percentage of neighbors like them who were engaging in the recommended
energy-saving behavior (see Figure 18.2 for a sample door hanger). We found that partici-
pants who received the social norms intervention used significantly less energy during the
intervention compared to those in the other conditions. In addition to measuring home
energy consumption, we also interviewed households to inquire about their perception of
the effectiveness of the door hangers. Consistent with the results of the statewide survey,
the results of the door-to-door survey revealed that the social norms message was rated
as the least motivating, despite the fact that it had been most effective at reducing home
energy consumption.
This and other related research (e.g., Schultz, Nolan, Cialdini, & Griskevicius, 2007)
inspired companies such as Opower to adopt the social norms approach for large-scale
application, providing customers with normative feedback on their home energy bills.
Over a 10-year period, Opower reduced energy consumption by an average of 2–5%
in more than 100 utility districts and reached nearly 60 million households (Allcott,
2011). The cumulative impact of this large-scale application is estimated to have reduced
household energy costs by $1 billion and carbon dioxide emissions by 13 billion pounds
(Schultz, Nolan, Cialdini, Goldstein, & Griskevicius, 2018).
410 IV. Sustainability
By using fans
instead of A/C!
Why?
In a recent survey of households in
your community, researchers at Cal
State San Marcos found that _____%
of San Marcos residents often use
fans instead of air conditioning to
keep cool in the summer.
FIGURE 18.2. Sample door hanger used in the study by Nolan, Schultz, Cialdini, Goldstein,
and Griskevicius (2008).
Mechanism
Social Proof
There is evidence to suggest that conformity to social norms, and descriptive norms in par-
ticular, occurs through a heuristic process (Göckeritz et al., 2010; Jacobson, Mortensen,
& Cialdini, 2011). Individuals, motivated by a need to understand how to behave effec-
tively, use the behavior of others to quickly make decisions about their own behavior.
Thus, activated social norms can serve as an efficient guide for the decision maker, pro-
viding “social proof” for the effectiveness of a particular behavior (Cialdini, 2001). This
heuristic process is also consistent with evolutionary explanations of conformity. For
example, in the domain of eating, adhering to descriptive norms may have provided an
easy way to ensure the selection of safe foods and may have promoted food sharing in our
environment of evolutionary adaptation (Higgs, 2015).
The Social Norms Approach 411
Support for the heuristic nature of this approach comes from research showing
that social norms interventions can impact behavior, even when participants do not
self-report greater intentions to engage in the behavior (Stok, Mollen, Verkooijen, &
Renner, 2018). Furthermore, participants with fewer cognitive resources are more likely
to conform to a descriptive norm message (Jacobson et al., 2011), and other research
showing that individuals who are personally involved with the behavior being promoted
(Göckeritz et al., 2010), or who are encouraged to elaborate on a message (Kredentser,
Fabrigar, Smith, & Fulton, 2012), are less likely to conform to a descriptive norm mes-
sage. In contrast, the reverse is true for social norms interventions that employ injunc-
tive norms. Depleting cognitive resources makes injunctive norm messages less effec-
tive, while elaboration makes them more effective. These results support the notion that
descriptive norms operate as a heuristic cue that help to achieve the goal of efficient and
effective action with minimal cognitive resources, whereas injunctive norms are more
focused on the maintenance of interpersonal interactions and thus require greater cogni-
tive resources to process them.
Changing Normative Beliefs
The social norms approach can also be construed as an intervention that harnesses indi-
viduals’ need to belong by linking belonging to beliefs about what constitutes desired
behavior. In many cases, individuals overestimate the prevalence of undesirable behav-
iors and underestimate the prevalence of desirable behaviors. Social norms interventions
correct these misperceptions of the norm by providing people with accurate information
about what constitutes typical behavior in a valued social community. Norms and nor-
mative beliefs, though related, represent distinct constructs. Whereas the former speaks
to the actual state of affairs regarding a target behavior’s prevalence or acceptance, the
latter encompasses individuals’ perceptions of these actualities (Nolan, 2011). It is these
normative beliefs that may become the proximate drivers of behavior (Morris, Hong,
Chiu, & Liu, 2015).
Social norms marketing campaigns have been shown to be quite effective at cor-
recting misperceptions of campus drinking norms (Clapp, Lange, Russel, Shillington, &
Voas, 2003; Neighbors, Larimer, & Lewis, 2004). For example, a social norms market-
ing campaign was designed to reduce problematic drinking among young people from the
United States crossing the border to go to the bars in Tijuana, Mexico (Johnson, 2012).
The results showed that providing these young people with accurate information about
how much alcohol previous border crossers had consumed succeeded in changing their
normative beliefs about drinking and that this change in beliefs was negatively correlated
with their blood alcohol levels as they crossed back into the United States. Other evidence
for the role of normative beliefs comes from research showing that social norms inter-
ventions can be enhanced by first asking people to guess the norm before it is revealed
(Bartke, Friedl, Gelhaar, & Reh, 2017). Importantly, asking people to guess the norm
increased charitable giving only among people who perceived it to be less common than it
actually was (i.e., those who underestimated the prevalence of the desired behavior). For
those who overestimated the true descriptive norm, guessing only served to undermine
their motivation as they learned that charitable giving was in fact less frequent than they
had previously assumed.
412 IV. Sustainability
Internalized Norms
A third way that social norms interventions may operate to change behavior (and main-
tain those changes) is via the internalization of social norms as personal norms—that
is, a personal moral obligation to act in a certain way. It is accepted, and expected, that
over time social norms will become internalized as personal norms (Schwartz, 1977;
Sherif, 1966). According to Sherif, “Norms develop in man and become part of him as he
develops in a social environment” (p. 46). Personal norms are internally sanctioned and
backed by the anticipation of shame, guilt, and/or embarrassment when a personal norm
has been violated (Horne, 2003; Kerr, Garst, Lewandowski, & Harris, 1997; Posner &
Rasmusen, 1999; Rozin, Lowery, Haidt, & Imada, 1999) and as a “warm glow” when
one has behaved consistently with their own personal standards (e.g., Andreoni, 1995).
Thus, in addition to having short-term effects on behavior, social norms interventions
may also be able to change behavior in the long term.
The long-term impact of social norms interventions is likely the result of a recur-
sive process between the individual and their social environment. For example, someone
who reduces their home energy consumption as a result of a social norms intervention
may receive approval for this change from friends and colleagues or may be more likely
to notice or associate with others who also conserve energy. These interactions rein-
force the new pattern of behavior and help to maintain, or even strengthen, the energy-
conservation norm, as well as to help the norm become internalized.
Effects over Time
Not only do social norms interventions have an immediate effect on behavior, they can
continue to affect behavior for days, weeks, or even years later. The strongest effects
seem to come from interventions that provide ongoing exposure to normative informa-
tion or provide “boosters” on a semiregular basis. In the environmental domain, large-
scale longitudinal research by Allcott and Rogers (2014) showed that the most dramatic
decrease in energy consumption came immediately after the introduction of normative
feedback. While these large reductions in energy usage were not sustained, households
that continued to receive monthly or quarterly reports over the next 2 years did con-
tinue to conserve, just at a lower rate. In addition, these households continued to con-
serve energy over the next 2 years even after they stopped receiving regular home energy
reports. It is possible that the continuation in energy conservation over time may, in part,
be the result of households having implemented one-time behaviors, such as installing an
energy-efficient appliance, that have a long-term impact on consumption. However, simi-
lar long-term effects have been found for social norms interventions designed to discour-
age overprescribing of antibiotics by physicians (e.g., Linder et al., 2017). In this problem
space, behaviors must be repeated to be sustained, which suggests that these long-term
effects cannot be attributed entirely to engaging in one-time behaviors with enduring
effects. Instead, individuals must come to believe that these newly adopted behaviors are
the correct and effective way to behave, and so continue to do so, even after the interven-
tion has ended. On a smaller scale, college students continued to conserve water 3 weeks
after a social norms sign encouraging them to do full loads of laundry was posted in their
dorm laundry room (Sparkman & Walton, 2017). In this case, the sign remained pres-
ent for the duration of the intervention. However, even one-shot treatments can persist
The Social Norms Approach 413
over short periods of time. For example, participants exposed to a descriptive norm mes-
sage for water conservation continued to use less water 1 week later, even with no new
exposure to the message (Richetin, Perugini, Mondini, & Hurling, 2016). Nonetheless,
one-shot treatments do tend to wane over longer periods of time (Ferraro & Price, 2013).
Another demonstrated way to prolong the effects of a social norms intervention is to
couple it with commitment (Jaeger & Schultz, 2017). In a study of water conservation,
households that received a social norms message and made a commitment to use water
efficiently consumed less water 4 months later, compared to those that received a strong
warning reminding them of the penalties associated with violating the water restrictions
and that then made a commitment. The authors argue that these results are caused by
cognitive dissonance resulting from an insufficient justification for making the commit-
ment in the social norms condition. Because social norms interventions are minimally
directive (Walton & Wilson, 2018), households that made a commitment after receiving
the norms message perceived their behavior as freely chosen rather than attributing it to
the coercive nature of the message. As a result, the behavior was more likely to become
intrinsically motivated. In contrast, those in the strong warning condition could attribute
their commitment to the coercive nature of the message and associated laws.
The goal of most interventions is to create long-term changes in behavior that are
intrinsically motivated, and thus do not require external rewards, incentives, or remind-
ers. The take-home message from these studies is that one-shot interventions are likely to
have immediate effects on behavior but may not persist beyond a few days. This may be
because normative beliefs are dynamic and constructed from a continuous flow of inputs
that includes observing the behavior of others and communications about the behavior
of others, as well as one’s own behavior (Miller & Prentice, 1996). As time goes by, the
information provided in a one-shot social norms intervention may be outweighed by
other inputs suggesting that the behavior is less common than was suggested. In contrast,
ongoing campaigns that last for several weeks, months, or years are more likely to persist
far into the future. Like most wise interventions (Walton & Wilson, 2018), the key to
success is to create a durable change in the way people make sense of themselves and/or
the situation. For example, a student who previously saw binge drinking on campus as
normal and expected may reconsider that view after being exposed to an ongoing social
norms campaign. As we mentioned earlier, ongoing exposure to social norms, combined
with repeated production of the behavior, may lead to internalization of those norms
and the development of new habits. When new habits are formed and/or personal norms
are developed, the intervention is no longer needed to spur behavior. Instead, behavior
becomes internally motivated and free from external controls.
Heterogeneity
Like most persuasive efforts, social norms interventions can be more or less effective
depending on important attributes of the message, the behavior, and the target audience.
Attributes of the Message
In order to be effective, messages must use credible sources and advertise social norms
that are believable. Social norms marketing may be less effective, or ineffective, in situ-
ations in which the source of the message can be perceived to have a vested interest and
414 IV. Sustainability
stands to gain personally from participants conforming to the implied request of the
message. For example, participants perceived a grocery store ad promoting natural prod-
ucts to be more deceptive when it included a descriptive norm (Raska, Nichols, & Shaw,
2015). While these ads did successfully change participants’ perception of the norm, they
were rated more negatively, and made participants less willing to purchase the advertised
products. Likewise, descriptive norms must be believable to be effective. Smith, Atkin,
Martell, Allen, and Hembroff (2006) found that normative messages vary in the extent to
which the target audience accepts them as believable and propose that messages that uti-
lize information that falls within the latitude of “noncommitment” will be most effective.
Information that is too easily accepted may not provide a motivation to change, while
information in the latitude of rejection may be dismissed as deceptive.
The most effective social norms interventions often use reference groups that are
meaningful to the individual in some way. Messages that reference ingroups (Abrams,
Wetherall, Cochrane, Hogg, & Turner, 1990) and proximal others (Agerström et al.,
2016; Stok, de Vet, de Ridder, & de Wit, 2016) are more effective than those that refer-
ence outgroups and distal others. Similarly, identifying strongly with the reference group
can increase the impact of social norms messages (Stok, de Ridder, de Vet, & de Wit,
2014), and in general, reference groups that are valued by the individual are better able to
influence behavior. Social identity theory (Tajfel, 1974) predicts that people will be most
inclined to follow the norms of groups that are important to them because they derive
part of their sense of self (and self-esteem) from these social identities. Conforming to
social norms provides a way to remain a member in good standing and to satisfy our
fundamental need to belong.
Perhaps surprisingly, messages utilizing seemingly meaningless referents, such as
“people who used this soap” (Richetin et al., 2016) and “guests in this room” (Goldstein
et al., 2008; Reese, Loew, & Steffgen, 2014), can also be effective. Indeed, advertis-
ing that a majority of guests that had stayed in a particular hotel room reused their
towels was more effective at motivating guests to reuse their towels than a traditional
appeal to environmental protection or a patriotic social norms message that referenced
citizens. Provincial norms are defined as “the norms of one’s local setting and circum-
stances” (Goldstein et al., 2008, p. 476). Importantly, the results of this initial study
showed that while the provincial norm had the biggest effect on behavior, it was rated as
being the least important to participants’ personal identity. Instead, Goldstein and col-
leagues speculate that adherence to these provincial norms is guided by a desire to behave
effectively in one’s immediate setting. These results are also consistent with predictions
that would be made according to the logic-of-appropriateness framework (Weber, Kopel-
man, & Messick, 2004): “What does a person like me do in a situation like this?” If the
guest assumes that they are similar to other guests that have stayed in that room, then
the provincial norm provides clear guidance for what a person “like you” has done under
similar circumstances.
Attributes of the Behavior
Two important aspects of the behavior that have been explored are the extent to which
the behavior can be publicly observed and the level of uncertainty associated with what
constitutes correct action in a given situation. Social norms interventions may be more
effective at influencing publicly observable behaviors (Stok et al., 2016), but research has
The Social Norms Approach 415
shown that they can also be used successfully to promote private or semiprivate behav-
iors, such as hand washing (Lapinski et al., 2013) and home energy consumption (Nolan
et al., 2008). With respect to uncertainty or ambiguity about what constitutes correct
action, the research clearly shows that social norms interventions are more likely to influ-
ence behavior when the situation is ambiguous. Because of our need to understand, when
we are unsure about what constitutes safe, effective, or appropriate behavior we are
more likely to rely on social norms (Gelfand & Harrington, 2015; Higgs, 2015; White &
Simpson, 2013).
Attributes of the Target Audience
What you say in a message is important but so is understanding to whom you are saying
it. In terms of demographic characteristics, social norms interventions tend to be more
effective with younger people (Rivas & Sheeran, 2003), women (Higgs, 2015), and indi-
viduals from collectivistic cultures or those who have a more collectivistic orientation
(Cialdini, Wosinska, Barrett, Butner, & Gornika-Durose, 1999). Younger people, and
adolescents in particular, may be more susceptible to social norms associated with their
peer groups because this is a developmental period that is heavily focused on identity
development (Erikson, 1950). Women may be more likely to conform because of evolu-
tionary factors that predispose men to conform less as part of a mating strategy to stand
out, but only when nonconformity does not lead to behavior that is objectively less accu-
rate (Griskevicius, Goldstein, Mortensen, Cialdini, & Kenrick, 2006). In collectivistic
societies, there is greater societal pressure to conform to group norms and to elevate the
needs of the group above self-interest (Triandis, 1995).
Whether it be energy or alcohol, social norms interventions designed to decrease
undesirable levels of consumption are often more effective among high users. For exam-
ple, high users of water accounted for the bulk of the savings achieved by providing
comparative feedback indicating how an individual’s household water consumption com-
pared to their neighbor’s average consumption (Ferraro & Price, 2013). High-use house-
holds may be more susceptible to social norms interventions for a few reasons. First, the
personal behavior of high users is most discrepant from the advertised norm, which may
produce stronger motivations to correct the deviant behavior. Second, it is easier for high-
use households to identify easy ways to reduce their consumption. Third, high consump-
tion of resources is often associated with greater wealth. These households tend to be less
sensitive to prices, making the social norms approach even more important. Although the
reasoning may be slightly different, a similar pattern has been found in the domain of
alcohol use with heavy drinkers showing the largest reductions in consumption following
exposure to personalized normative feedback (Prince, Reid, Carey, & Neighbors, 2014).
Similarly, social norms interventions designed to increase desirable behaviors are often
more effective among low users. For example, registered voters who had voted infre-
quently or only occasionally in the past were more likely than those who voted often or
always to show greater intentions to vote when told that a majority of voters in the state
had voted in the last election (Gerber & Rogers, 2009).
Highly empathic people and people with low self-esteem, or who are temporarily
made to feel a lack of social acceptance, are more likely to imitate a confederate, and by
extension may be more likely to conform to social norms (Robinson, Tobias, Shaw, Free-
man, & Higgs, 2011). Similarly, Gelfand and Harrington (2015) argue that low-power or
416 IV. Sustainability
COUSINS
Of course, the social norms approach is not the only one that has been used to promote
desirable behaviors. Two techniques that are similar to social norms and that have been
used to target a similar range of behaviors are “modeling” and “feedback.” Interventions
that use modeling to change behavior are based on Bandura’s (1977) social learning
theory. According to social learning theory, individuals use the behavior of models to
inform their own behavior and are more likely to imitate valued models and behaviors
for which the models are rewarded. For children, these valued models are often parents
and other caregivers, whereas for adolescents, valued models are more likely to be peers.
Modeling can be an effective way to change behavior. For example, in the now classic
study by Aronson and O’Leary (1982–1983), the number of college males who turned
the water off while soaping up in a locker room shower more than doubled to 49%
when a student confederate modeled this behavior, compared to when only a large sign
was present. Compliance “jumped” further to 67% when two confederates modeled the
water-saving behavior. At first glance, modeling and social norms might seem to be the
same. Both approaches use the behavior of others as a guide to one’s own behavior, so
how are they different? While modeling has its effect by focusing on the behavior of a
particular individual, social norms have their effect by conveying an impression of what
most people do. Cialdini and colleagues (1990) conducted an experiment that nicely
illustrates this distinction. Participants returning to their car in a hospital parking garage
either encountered a confederate who littered a large handbill onto the ground or simply
walked by in the opposite direction. Research on modeling would predict that those who
saw the confederate litter the handbill should be more likely to litter as well. However,
this was the case only when the ground was already littered. When participants saw the
confederate litter the handbill in an otherwise clean garage, they were actually less likely
to litter the handbill they found on their own vehicle, compared to when the confeder-
ate just walked by. Cialdini and colleagues argue that the confederate littering into the
clean environment decreased littering because it made the descriptive antilittering norm
salient. Thus, this example illustrates the importance of norm focus, but also the superior
strength of social norms, compared to modeling by strangers. While both social norms
and modeling can be used to resolve questions related to the need to understand and
the need to belong, modeling may be effective only when the model is a valued other. In
contrast, because social norms convey what most people approve or disapprove of, indi-
viduals can be more confident that conforming to social norms will lead to effective or
socially rewarding behavior.
The Social Norms Approach 417
There are a variety of options available for implementing a social norms intervention that
utilizes direct appeals to change behavior and that explicitly references a social norm. For
example, descriptive social norms can be communicated several ways (see Table 18.1 for
sample messages). Probably the most common approach is to advertise that a majority of
people are engaging in the desired behavior, either by saying so directly or by providing
an exact percentage (see Table 18.1, examples A–D). Variations on this approach might
also use other numeric descriptors, such as “most” or “many” to quantify the extent to
which the behavior is commonly done (see example E). A second option is to report that
a specific (desired) frequency of the behavior is common, such as advertising that most
college students have zero to five drinks when they party (see examples B and F). A third
option is to report average usage, either alone, or more often in conjunction with person-
alized feedback (see example G).
As discussed previously, social norms messages can use proximal referents, such
as neighbors or fellow students at a particular university (see example A), or provincial
norms that reference a group that shares a connection to the location (see example D).
There are also options for how to talk about the frequency of the behavior. Research
suggests that using verbal quantifiers with positive polarity, such as “at least” and “a
few” will be more effective at increasing a desired behavior compared to quantifiers with
negative polarity, such as “at most” and “few” (Demarque, Charalambides, Hilton, &
Waroquier, 2015; see example C). In all of these examples the goal of the message is to
convince the reader that the recommended behavior is commonly done and therefore the
right and correct thing to do.
NUANCES AND MISCONCEPTIONS
One of the most important points to remember when designing a social norms interven-
tion is that people conform to social norms whether the advertised behavior is desirable
or undesirable. This has two important implications for practice. First, the common tactic
418 IV. Sustainability
B Scribner et al. (2011) Majority percentage Binge “67% of university students have 4
drinking or fewer drinks when they party.”
Frequency of
behavior
D Goldstein, Cialdini, Majority percentage Towel reuse “75% of the guests who stayed
& Griskevicius in this room [#xx] participated in
Provincial norm
(2008) our new resource savings program
by using their towels more than
once.”
F Robinson, Thomas, Typical referent Vegetable “A lot of people aren’t aware that
Aveyard, & Higgs and fruit the typical student eats their five
Frequency of
(2014) consumption servings of fruits and vegetables
behavior
each day.”
H Sparkman & Walton Dynamic norm Meat “Some people are starting to limit
(2017) consumption how much meat they eat. . . .
Specifically, recent research has
shown that, over the last 5 years,
30% of Americans have started to
make an effort to limit their meat
consumption. That means that,
in recent years, 3 in 10 people
have changed their behavior and
begun to eat less meat than they
otherwise would.”
simultaneously, inquiring about normative beliefs may call attention to the message in
ways that undermine its effectiveness (Kredentser et al., 2012). Walton and Wilson (2018)
argue that one of the features that makes interventions “wise” is that they are minimally
directive—that is, it is possible that the covertness of the intervention is part of what
makes it effective. People do not see social norms interventions as motivating, but asking
questions about norms may draw attention to the influence attempt and increase resis-
tance to the request. Taken together, the available research would suggest that while it
is important to make the descriptive norm salient, it is also wise to discourage the target
audience from thinking too much about the message.
IMPLICATIONS FOR PRACTICE
To effectively implement a social norms intervention in the field a series of steps should
be followed. First, survey research should be used to determine the prevalence and/or
frequency of the behavior(s) of interest and the prevailing normative beliefs about those
behaviors among the target audience. Second, survey research can also be used to under-
stand the target audience in terms of demographics and other variables deemed to be
important, including those mentioned in this chapter. Third, structural barriers and
opportunities should be identified. For example, it would not make sense to try to reduce
heating among apartment dwellers who do not have control over their thermostats. The
second and third steps are common to most community-based social marketing efforts
(McKenzie-Mohr, Lee, Kotler, & Schultz, 2011). Fourth, a behavior should be selected
that is performed by many people, but misperceived to be infrequent, and that has the
potential to make a substantial impact in terms of units (e.g., pounds of greenhouse
gases, number of students blacking out) deemed to be important by the community or
organization.
Social norms interventions can be delivered effectively through various channels.
In relatively small-scale applications, the intervention is typically delivered via posters,
flyers (Robinson, Fleming, & Higgs, 2014), face-to-face discussion groups, or online
via computer-mediated feedback (Prince et al., 2014). Small-scale applications might
also manipulate the physical environment to convey social norms (Reese, Loeschinger,
Hamann, & Neubert, 2013)—however, this approach typically requires time and
resources that can make the intervention cost prohibitive. That said, there would be no
harm, for example, in asking a computer lab manager with time on their hands to shut
down unused monitors, knowing that it will make users more likely to turn them off
themselves when they leave (Bator, Tabanico, Walton, & Schultz, 2014). In large-scale
applications, the intervention is typically delivered via media campaigns that include
print, television, and radio advertisements or PSAs (Cialdini, 2003; Perkins et al., 2010),
or by collaborating with companies that already have direct access to customers (and
consumption data), such as electricity, natural gas, and water utility companies (Allcott,
2011). Social norms can also be harnessed in online environments by advertising the
number of times a post or video has been liked and/or shared. For example, participants
who saw a description of a climate change video with a high number of views were
more likely to perceive that climate change was an important issue for “most Americans”
(Spartz, Su, Griffin, Brossard, & Dunwoody, 2017).
One of the questions we are frequently asked when presenting our research is “What
happens when the behavior you want to promote is not performed by a majority of
The Social Norms Approach 421
people?” and, sometimes, “Why promote behaviors that are already normative? After
all, isn’t it more important to increase the prevalence of unpopular but highly impactful
behaviors?” Indeed, the usefulness of social norms interventions would be severely lim-
ited (although not altogether useless) if they could not be adapted to promote behaviors
that are not descriptively normative. When we first started working on this research in
the early 2000s there were really only two answers to this question: lie (a less than ideal
option when working in field settings) or focus on injunctive norms. While focusing on
injunctive norms is certainly a viable alternative when a behavior is not descriptively
normative, we are happy to report that a flurry of activity in the last decade has pro-
duced additional options for using social norms marketing to promote behaviors that
are not yet descriptively normative. One new option is to use positive polarity numeric
qualifiers (Demarque et al., 2015). For example, a minority of people might be recycling
paper, glass, and plastic each, but a majority of people may be recycling at least one type
of recyclable item. This adaptation is most applicable in domains that consider a sphere
of behaviors or instances of a behavior, rather than one unique behavior at one point in
time. A second new option is to report dynamic or trending norms. Trending norms are
“norms in which the number of people engaging in a behavior is increasing” (Mortensen
et al., 2017; see also Sparkman, Chapter 19, this volume). Trending norms can be used
successfully on their own (see example H in Table 18.1; Sparkman & Walton, 2017), or
they can be added to a message that communicates the minority norm (see example I;
Mortensen et al., 2017).
Like most interventions there is a tension between scaling up versus customizing
social norms messages. This is especially true with respect to choosing a reference group
for the message. Under ideal conditions, messages would be customized to refer to the
group that is most meaningful for the target audience, for which there are data available.
However, if customization is not possible, we would advise using a referent that has broad
appeal among the target audience. If possible, we recommend pilot-testing messages prior
to large-scale application to ensure that they are well received (though recall that they
need not be perceived as motivational), ideally using a behavioral outcome measure.
The main challenge in conducting a social norms intervention is that it does require
the practitioner to acquire normative information about the target behavior, and not
everyone is in a position to conduct a survey, either due to limited time, money, or
resources. The good news is that many of the behaviors targeted in social norms inter-
ventions are heavily studied by government agencies. For example, in the United States,
information about transportation behavior can be found at www.transportation.gov,
and about energy consumption at www.energy.gov. Public opinion polls can also be a
good source of normative information about health and environmental behaviors.
Below is the story of how the social norms approach was used to reinvent home
energy bills and save more energy than the Hoover Dam produces in a year:
Alex Laskey
Cofounder of Opower
In early 2007, my longtime friend Daniel Yates and I were introduced to the work of Profes-
sors Cialdini and Schultz by Rhea Suh of the Hewlett Foundation. Dan and I had recently
decided to team up and work together to have a positive impact on the environment. At
the time, we were exploring several novel ideas of projects—both for-profit and not-for-
profit—to work on together. We met with Rhea because we were interested in the work
422 IV. Sustainability
Hewlett was funding to protect temperate rain forests in British Columbia. Toward the end
of our meeting, Rhea asked, “What else are the two of you thinking of working on?” It
was at this point that we shared with her our preliminary idea to reinvent the electric util-
ity bill. We had cooked up this idea while driving in my uncomfortable old Honda Civic,
which we routinely chose to drive instead of Dan’s relatively luxurious Nissan Pathfinder.
We subjected ourselves—and our wives—to cramped rides in the Civic because we couldn’t
stomach the notion of unnecessarily wasting gasoline. At some point, we wondered out loud,
“Wouldn’t it be ironic if we were driving a Civic around town, but were returning home to
Pathfinder apartments? And shouldn’t everyone have a right to know whether their families
were efficient or inefficient in their homes?”
We were aware that utilities were increasingly incentivized to help their customers save
energy. And we were optimistic that if we helped utilities provide better information to
their customers, we could change behaviors. When we shared this idea with Rhea, she said,
“Surely you’ve read the research we’ve funded about the application of behavioral economics
to conservation, right?” We responded, “No. And what is ‘behavioral economics’?” She then
introduced us to the work of Drs. Cialdini and Schultz, showing how normative information
could reduce energy consumption in hotel guest rooms and suburban households. I remem-
ber very clearly the excitement Dan and I both felt after reading this research and learning
more about the field of behavioral economics. After reading that the communication of
social norms could significantly increase hotel visitors’ likelihood to hang up their towels or
motivate homeowners in the heat of summer to turn off their air conditioners and turn on
their fans, we developed growing confidence that our idea to show homeowners how their
energy use compared to their neighbors might actually make a difference.
Rhea soon connected us with Bob Cialdini. Although it’s not easy to persuade a persua-
sion researcher, because of Bob’s commitment to the environment, he agreed to serve as our
chief scientist during the initial years of the company we formed, known as Opower. Bob
worked closely with our communication team to design home energy reports sent to util-
ity customers that communicated the energy use norms of their neighborhoods. Although
Opower has since been purchased by Oracle, those energy reports, employed by over a
hundred utilities worldwide, have now saved more than 35 billion pounds of CO2 emissions
and more than 23 trillion watts per hour of electricity. What’s more, the energy reports are
presently generating 5 trillion watts per hour per year in savings to utility customers. That
works out to roughly $700 million in bill savings each year. The Hoover Dam, by compari-
son, produces only 4 trillion watts per hour of electricity. It’s remarkable to me that our little
behavioral science experiment is now roughly 20% bigger than the Hoover Dam!
IMPLICATIONS FOR PSYCHOLOGICAL THEORY
Research on social norms interventions reveals important qualities of our human nature,
and also the nature of environmental problems. The research presented in this chapter
supports the idea that, as human beings, we use the behavior of others as a guide for our
own behavior. The evidence also suggests that people may not spend a great deal of time
deliberating about daily choices, such as whether or not to recycle or turn the water off
while brushing their teeth. Instead, people rely on heuristic cues, like descriptive norms,
to satisfy their need to understand what is expected and to make these decisions about
everyday behaviors.
There are several ways that the application of the social norms approach has
affected our theoretical understanding of social norms and conformity. For example,
while research does support original theorizing suggesting that individuals prefer to use
The Social Norms Approach 423
the behavior of proximate, ingroup members to inform their own behavior, additional
research shows that other referents can also be effective. Specifically, provincial or “situ-
ational” reference groups can also inform the behavior of targets. These results suggest
that while meaningful ingroups may be more powerful than outgroups, conformity is not
limited to those groups with which we identify. Instead, we use the behavior of others
who are similar in some way—either by nature of their group membership or in having
had contact with a particular product or location at different points in time.
Research on trending norms also forces us to reconsider our understanding of con-
formity to social norms. Original theorizing about descriptive social norms suggested
that individuals will go along with what most people seem to be doing. However, research
on trending norms shows that individuals will also conform to the behavior of a trend-
ing minority. This seems to be due in part to a belief that the behavior is moving toward
majority status in the near future or that the changes described are expected to continue.
Thus, conformity to social norms can occur when a behavior has already become norma-
tive, or when there is evidence that it is moving in that direction.
The social norms approach also reveals important qualities of environmental prob-
lems. Environmental problems possess the qualities of social dilemmas—situations in
which the self-interest of the individual is at odds with the collective good. The costs
of cooperating are immediate and accrue to the individual (e.g., the inconvenience of
taking the bus vs. driving a personal automobile), while the benefits of cooperating are
delayed and spread across society (e.g., benefits to air quality; see Rogers, Goldstein, &
Fox, 2018, for a more general review of factors, including social norms interventions that
mobilize individuals to cooperate in such situations). In addition, an individual cooperat-
ing alone cannot make much of an impact on large-scale problems such as global climate
change. Individuals are understandably concerned that their efforts to conserve will be
wasted if not enough others take action. One of the reasons why social norms interven-
tions may be so effective at promoting environmental behaviors is that they assure the
target that they will not be alone in their efforts.
FUTURE DIRECTIONS
As we have shown in this chapter, the social norms intervention has broad applicability
across different domains of behavior and across populations. Companies like Opower
have also shown that social norms interventions can be adapted for large-scale applica-
tions that benefit society and the environment. In addition, technologies such as smart
thermostats provide yet another opportunity for harnessing the power of social norms.
Indeed, some smart thermostat companies, such as Ecobee, have already incorporated
comparative feedback that provides users with information about how their energy con-
sumption compares to similar households as part of their “home IQ” interface. Future
research should explore how these new technologies can be used to deliver social norms
messages to optimize energy savings.
Going forward there are two key areas that should be explored in greater depth:
boundary conditions of the intervention and the underlying mechanism. With few excep-
tions (e.g., Kormos, Gifford, & Brown, 2015), social norms research to date has focused
primarily on relatively simple, everyday behaviors that must be enacted repeatedly to
have a substantive impact. For example, in the environmental domain, application of the
424 IV. Sustainability
social norms intervention has focused primarily on reducing energy and water use via
curtailment behaviors. These curtailment behaviors are often easy, inexpensive behav-
iors, such as taking shorter showers and doing full loads of laundry. Future research
should explore whether the social norms intervention can be used to influence more
difficult or expensive behaviors, such as purchasing a fuel-efficient vehicle or planting a
rain garden.
There is also more to learn about the psychological mechanism that drives confor-
mity to social norms. In particular, it would be interesting to explore whether measuring
normative beliefs following exposure to a social norms intervention has the paradoxical
effect of making the intervention less effective. This question could be tested by reviewing
past research that has measured both beliefs and behavior, as well as with new research
specifically designed to test the hypothesis. This prediction could be directly tested by
exposing participants to the same message but asking some of them to report on their
beliefs before versus after the behavior change opportunity is presented. In conducting
the review of research, it would be important to distinguish between studies that mea-
sured behavioral intentions, self-reported past behavior, and actual behavior. Given the
heuristic nature of social norms interventions, behavioral intentions are less likely to
provide an accurate measure of the interventions’ impact. Interference created by asking
participants to think about the message may be common to other “minimally directive”
techniques that rely on a heuristic mechanism.
REFERENCES
Abrams, D., Wetherall, M., Cochrane, S., Hogg, M. A., & Turner, J. C. (1990). Knowing what to
think by knowing who you are: Self-categorization and the nature of norm formation, con-
formity, and group polarization. British Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 97–119.
Agerström, J., Carlsson, R., Nicklasson, L., & Guntell, L. (2016). Using descriptive social norms
to increase charitable giving: The power of local norms. Journal of Economic Psychology,
52, 147–153.
Alfonso, J. (2015). The role of social norms in personalized alcohol feedback: A dismantling study
with emerging adults. Journal of Child and Adolescent Substance Abuse, 24(6), 379–386.
Allcott, H. (2011). Social norms and energy conservation. Journal of Public Economics, 95(9–10),
1082–1095.
Allcott, H., & Rogers, T. (2014). The short-run and long-run effects of behavioral interventions:
Experimental evidence from energy conservation. American Economic Review, 104(10),
3003–3037.
Andreoni, J. (1995). Warm-glow versus cold prickle: The effects of positive and negative framing
on cooperation in experiments. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 110, 1–21.
Andrews, T. M., & Barbash, F. (2016). “I’d like to buy the world a Coke”: The story behind
the world’s most famous ad, in memoriam its creator. Retrieved from www.washington-
post.com/news/morning-mix/wp/2016/05/17/id-like-to-buy-the-world-a-coke-the-story-
behind-the-worlds-most-famous-ad-whose-creator-has-died-at-89.
Aronson, E., & O’Leary, M. (1982). The relative effectiveness of models and prompts on energy
conservation: A field experiment in a shower room. Journal of Environmental Systems,
12(3), 219–224.
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Bartke, S., Friedl, A., Gelhaar, F., & Reh, L. (2017). Social comparison nudges—guessing the
norm increases charitable giving. Economics Letters, 152, 73–75.
The Social Norms Approach 425
Bator, R. J., Tabanico, J. J., Walton, M. L., & Schultz, P. W. (2014). Promoting energy conserva-
tion with implied norms and explicit messages. Social Influence, 9(1), 69–82.
Burger, J. M., LaSalvia, C. T., Hendricks, L. A., Mehdipour, T., & Neudeck, E. M. (2011). Party-
ing before the party gets started: The effects of descriptive norms on pre-gaming behavior.
Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 33, 220–227.
Cialdini, R. B. (2001). Influence: Science and practice. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Cialdini, R. B. (2003). Crafting normative messages to protect the environment. Current Direc-
tions in Psychological Science, 12(4), 105–109.
Cialdini, R. B., Demaine, L. J., Sagarin, B. J., Barrett, D. W., Rhoads, K., & Winter, P. L. (2006).
Managing social norms for persuasive impact. Social Influence, 1(1), 3–15.
Cialdini, R. B., Reno, R. R., & Kallgren, C. A. (1990). A focus theory of normative conduct:
Recycling the concept of norms to reduce littering in public places. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 58(6), 1015–1026.
Cialdini, R. B., & Trost, M. R. (1998). Social influence: Social norms, conformity and compliance.
In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th
ed., pp. 151–192). Boston: McGraw-H ill.
Cialdini, R. B., Wosinska, W., Barrett, D. W., Butner, J., & Gornik-Durose, M. (1999). Compli-
ance with a request in two cultures: The differential influence of social proof and commit-
ment/consistency on collectivists and individualists. Personality and Social Psychology Bul-
letin, 25(10), 1242–1253.
Clapp, J. D., Lange, J. E., Russel, C., Shillington, A., & Voas, R. B. (2003). A failed norms social
marketing campaign. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 64, 409–414.
Clapp, J. D., & McDonnell, A. L. (2000). The relationship of perceptions of alcohol promotion
and peer drinking norms to alcohol problems reported by college students. Journal of College
Student Development, 41(1), 19–26.
Demarque, C., Charalambides, L., Hilton, D. J., & Waroquier, L. (2015). Nudging sustainable
consumption: The use of descriptive norms to promote a minority behavior in a realistic
online shopping environment. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 43, 166–174.
Dwyer, P. C., Maki, A., & Rothman, A. J. (2015). Promoting energy conservation behavior in
public settings: The influence of social norms and personal responsibility. Journal of Envi-
ronmental Psychology, 41, 30–34.
Erikson, E. H. (1950). Childhood and society. New York: Norton.
Faruqui, A., Sergici, S., & Sharif, A. (2010). The impact of informational feedback on energy
consumption—a survey of the experimental evidence. Energy, 35(4), 1598–1608.
Ferraro, P. J., & Price, M. K. (2013). Using nonpecuniary strategies to influence behavior: Evi-
dence from a large-scale field experiment. Review of Economics and Statistics, 95(1), 64–73.
Gelfand, M. J., & Harrington, J. R. (2015). The motivational force of descriptive norms: For whom
and when are descriptive norms most predictive of behavior? Journal of Cross-C ultural Psy-
chology, 46(10), 1273–1278.
Gerber, A. S., & Rogers, T. (2009). Descriptive social norms and motivation to vote: Everybody’s
voting and so should you. Journal of Politics, 71(1), 178–191.
Göckeritz, S., Schultz, P. W., Rendón, T., Cialdini, R. B., Goldstein, N. J., & Griskevicius, V.
(2010). Descriptive normative beliefs and conservation behavior: The moderating roles of
personal involvement and injunctive normative beliefs. European Journal of Social Psychol-
ogy, 40(3), 514–523.
Goldstein, N. J., Cialdini, R. B., & Griskevicius, V. (2008). A room with a viewpoint: Using social
norms to motivate environmental conservation in hotels. Journal of Consumer Research,
35(3), 472–482.
Griskevicius, V., Goldstein, N. J., Mortensen, C. R., Cialdini, R. B., & Kenrick, D. T. (2006).
Going along versus going alone: When fundamental motives facilitate strategic (non) confor-
mity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(2), 281–294.
426 IV. Sustainability
Haines, M. P. (1998). Social norms: A wellness model for health promotion in higher education.
Wellness Management, 14(4), 1–10.
Higgs, S. (2015). Social norms and their influence on eating behaviours. Appetite, 86, 38–44.
Horne, C. (2003). The internal enforcement of norms. European Sociological Review, 19(4), 335–
343.
Jacobson, R. P., Mortensen, C. R., & Cialdini, R. B. (2011). Bodies obliged and unbound: Differ-
entiated response tendencies for injunctive and descriptive social norms. Journal of Personal-
ity and Social Psychology, 100(3), 433–448.
Jaeger, C. M., & Schultz, P. W. (2017). Coupling social norms and commitments: Testing the
underdetected nature of social influence. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 51, 199–
208.
Johnson, M. B. (2012). Experimental test of social norms theory in a real-world drinking environ-
ment. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 73(5), 851–859.
Keizer, K., Lindenberg, S., & Steg, L. (2008). The spreading of disorder. Science, 322(5908),
1681–1685.
Keizer, K., Lindenberg, S., & Steg, L. (2011). The reversal effect of prohibition signs. Group Pro-
cesses and Intergroup Relations, 14(5), 681–688.
Keizer, K., Lindenberg, S., & Steg, L. (2013). The importance of demonstratively restoring order.
PLOS ONE, 8(6), e65137.
Kerr, N. L., Garst, J., Lewandowski, D. A., & Harris, S. E. (1997). That still, small voice: Com-
mitment to cooperate as an internalized versus a social norm. Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy Bulletin, 23(12), 1300–1311.
Kluger, A. N., & DeNisi, A. (1996). The effects of feedback interventions on performance: A his-
torical review, a meta-analysis, and a preliminary feedback intervention theory. Psychologi-
cal Bulletin, 119(2), 254–284.
Kormos, C., Gifford, R., & Brown, E. (2015). The influence of descriptive social norm informa-
tion on sustainable transportation behavior: A field experiment. Environment and Behavior,
47(5), 479–501.
Kredentser, M. S., Fabrigar, L. R., Smith, S. M., & Fulton, K. (2012). Following what people think
we should do versus what people actually do: Elaboration as a moderator of the impact of
descriptive and injunctive norms. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3(3), 341–
347.
Lapinski, M. K., Maloney, E. K., Braz, M., & Shulman, H. C. (2013). Testing the effects of social
norms and behavioral privacy on hand washing: A field experiment. Human Communication
Research, 39(1), 21–46.
Linder, J. A., Meeker, D., Fox, C. R., Friedberg, M. W., Persell, S. D., Goldstein, N. J., & Doctor,
J. N. (2017). Effects of behavioral interventions on inappropriate antibiotic prescribing in
primary care 12 months after stopping interventions. JAMA, 318(14), 1391–1392.
Margetts, H., John, P., Escher, T., & Reissfelder, S. (2011). Social information and political par-
ticipation on the Internet: An experiment. European Political Science Review, 3(3), 321–344.
McKenzie-Mohr, D., Lee, N. R., Kotler, P., & Schultz, P. W. (2011). Social marketing to protect
the environment: What works. Newbury Park, CA: SAGE.
Miller, D. T., & Prentice, D. (1996). The construction of social norms and standards. In E. T. Hig-
gins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 799–
829). New York: Guilford Press.
Morris, M. W., Hong, Y. Y., Chiu, C. Y., & Liu, Z. (2015). Normology: Integrating insights about
social norms to understand cultural dynamics. Organizational Behavior and Human Deci-
sion Processes, 129, 1–13.
Mortensen, C. R., Neel, R., Cialdini, R. B., Jaeger, C. M., Jacobson, R. P., & Ringel, M. M.
(2017). Trending norms: A lever for encouraging behaviors performed by the minority. Social
Psychological and Personality Science, 10(2), 201–210.
The Social Norms Approach 427
Neighbors, C., Larimer, M. E., & Lewis, M. A. (2004). Targeting misperceptions of descriptive
drinking norms: Efficacy of a computer-delivered personalized normative feedback interven-
tion. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 434–447.
Nolan, J. M. (2011). The cognitive ripple of social norms communications. Group Processes and
Intergroup Relations, 14(5), 689–702.
Nolan, J. M., Schultz, P. W., Cialdini, R. B., Goldstein, N. J., & Griskevicius, V. (2008). Normative
social influence is underdetected. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 913–923.
Perkins, H. (Ed.). (2003). The emergence and evolution of the social norms approach to substance
abuse prevention. In The social norms approach to preventing school and college age sub-
stance abuse: A handbook for educators, counselors, and clinicians (pp. 3–17). San Fran-
cisco: Jossey-Bass.
Perkins, H., & Berkowitz, A. D. (1986). Perceiving the community norms of alcohol use among
students: Some research implications for campus alcohol education programming. Interna-
tional Journal of the Addictions, 21(9–10), 961–976.
Perkins, H., Linkenbach, J. W., Lewis, M. A., & Neighbors, C. (2010). Effectiveness of social
norms media marketing in reducing drinking and driving: A statewide campaign. Addictive
Behaviors, 35(10), 866–874.
Phelps, K., Large, L., Schultz, P. W., & Ettlinger, J. (2017). Keeping compostables out of the trash.
BioCycle, 58, 16.
Posner, R. A., & Rasmusen, E. B. (1999). Creating and enforcing norms, with special reference to
sanctions. International Review of Law and Economics, 19, 369–382.
Priebe, C. S., & Spink, K. S. (2015). Less sitting and more moving in the office: Using descriptive
norm messages to decrease sedentary behavior and increase light physical activity at work.
Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 19, 76–84.
Prince, M. A., Reid, A., Carey, K. B., & Neighbors, C. (2014). Effects of normative feedback for
drinkers who consume less than the norm: Dodging the boomerang. Psychology of Addictive
Behaviors, 28(2), 538–544.
Raska, D., Nichols, B. S., & Shaw, D. (2015). When descriptive norm cues fail as persuasion agents
in green supermarket advertising. Journal of Promotion Management, 21(6), 721–738.
Reese, G., Loeschinger, D. C., Hamann, K., & Neubert, S. (2013). Sticker in the box!: Object–
person distance and descriptive norms as means to reduce waste. Ecopsychology, 5(2), 146–
148.
Reese, G., Loew, K., & Steffgen, G. (2014). A towel less: Social norms enhance pro-environmental
behavior in hotels. Journal of Social Psychology, 154(2), 97–100.
Reno, R. R., Cialdini, R. B., & Kallgren, C. A. (1993). The transsituational influence of social
norms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 104–112.
Richetin, J., Perugini, M., Mondini, D., & Hurling, R. (2016). Conserving water while washing
hands: The immediate and durable impacts of descriptive norms. Environment and Behavior,
48(2), 343–364.
Rivas, A., & Sheeran, P. (2003). Social influences and the theory of planned behavior: Evidence for
a direct relationship between prototypes and young people’s exercise behavior. Psychology
and Health, 18(5), 567–583.
Robinson, E., Fleming, A., & Higgs, S. (2014). Prompting healthier eating: Testing the use of
health and social norm based messages. Health Psychology, 33(9), 1057–1064.
Robinson, E., Thomas, J., Aveyard, P., & Higgs, S. (2014). What everyone else is eating: A system-
atic review and meta-analysis of the effect of informational eating norms on eating behavior.
Journal of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics, 114(3), 414–429.
Robinson, E., Tobias, T., Shaw, L., Freeman, E., & Higgs, S. (2011). Social matching of food
intake and the need for social acceptance. Appetite, 56(3), 747–752.
Rogers, T., Goldstein, N. J., & Fox, C. R. (2018). Social mobilization. Annual Review of Psychol-
ogy, 69, 357–381.
428 IV. Sustainability
Rozin, P., Lowery, L., Haidt, J., & Imada, S. (1999). The CAD triad hypothesis: A mapping
between three moral emotions (contempt, anger, disgust) and three moral codes (community,
autonomy, divinity). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 574–586.
Schultz, P. W. (1999). Changing behavior with normative feedback interventions: A field experi-
ment on curbside recycling. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 21, 25–36.
Schultz, P. W., Nolan, J. M., Cialdini, R. B., Goldstein, N. J., & Griskevicius, V. (2007). The
constructive, destructive, and reconstructive power of social norms. Psychological Science,
18, 429–433.
Schultz, P. W., Nolan, J. M., Cialdini, R. B., Goldstein, N. J., & Griskevicius, V. (2018). The
constructive, destructive, and reconstructive power of social norms: Reprise. Perspectives on
Psychological Science, 13(2), 249–254.
Schwartz, S. H. (Ed.). (1977). Normative influences on altruism. New York: Academic Press.
Scribner, R. A., Theall, K. P., Mason, K., Simonsen, N., Schneider, S. K., Towvim, L. G., &
Dejong, W. (2011). Alcohol prevention on college campuses: The moderating effect of the
alcohol environment on the effectiveness of social norms marketing campaigns. Journal of
Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 72(2), 232–239.
Sherif, M. (1966). The psychology of social norms. New York: HarperCollins.
Smith, S. W., Atkin, C. K., Martell, D., Allen, R., & Hembroff, L. (2006). A social judgment
theory approach to conducting formative research in a social norms campaign. Communica-
tion Theory, 16(1), 141–152.
Sparkman, G., & Walton, G. M. (2017). Dynamic norms promote sustainable behavior, even if it
is counternormative. Psychological Science, 28(11), 1663–1674.
Spartz, J. T., Su, L. Y. F., Griffin, R., Brossard, D., & Dunwoody, S. (2017). YouTube, social
norms and perceived salience of climate change in the American mind. Environmental Com-
munication, 11(1), 1–16.
Stok, F. M., de Ridder, D. T., de Vet, E., & de Wit, J. B. (2014). Don’t tell me what I should do, but
what others do: The influence of descriptive and injunctive peer norms on fruit consumption
in adolescents. British Journal of Health Psychology, 19(1), 52–64.
Stok, F. M., de Vet, E., de Ridder, D. T., & de Wit, J. B. (2016). The potential of peer social norms
to shape food intake in adolescents and young adults: A systematic review of effects and mod-
erators. Health Psychology Review, 10(3), 326–340.
Stok, F. M., Mollen, S., Verkooijen, K. T., & Renner, B. (2018). Unravelling social norm effects:
How and when social norms affect eating behavior. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 738.
Stok, F. M., Verkooijen, K. T., de Ridder, D. T., de Wit, J. B., & de Vet, E. (2014). How norms
work: Self-identification, attitude, and self-efficacy mediate the relation between descrip-
tive social norms and vegetable intake. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being, 6(2),
230–250.
Tajfel, H. (1974). Social identity and intergroup behaviour. Social Science Information, 13, 65–93.
Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism and collectivism. Boulder, CO: Westview.
Walton, G. M., & Wilson, T. D. (2018). Wise interventions: Psychological remedies for social and
personal problems. Psychological Review, 125(5), 617–655.
Weber, J. M., Kopelman, S., & Messick, D. M. (2004). A conceptual review of decision making
in social dilemmas: Applying a logic of appropriateness. Personality and Social Psychology
Review, 8(3), 281–307.
White, K., & Simpson, B. (2013). When do (and don’t) normative appeals influence sustainable
consumer behaviors? Journal of Marketing, 77(2), 78–95.
C H A P TE R 19
Gregg Sparkman
Social problems where a majority of people contribute to the problem are especially difficult
to solve. For example, how can we motivate people to live sustainably when many unsustain-
able behaviors are currently the norm? More generally, how can we dislodge problematic
and widely endorsed norms? One answer is to focus people’s attention on dynamic norm
information: changes in others’ behavior or attitudes over time. The research on dynamic
norms reviewed here finds that drawing attention to positive changes in others can inspire
observers to change their ways even when that change goes against the current norm.
Further, dynamic norm interventions have a variety of strengths that may make them more
effective than traditional norm interventions that focus solely on static information about the
norms in the present moment. These findings substantially extend the contexts in which
social norm interventions can be employed to those where current norms are problematic
and give a clearer understanding of how social influence may perpetuate social problems,
as well as solve them.
BACKGROUND
Social norms constitute one of the most reliable and powerful sources of influence on
human behavior (Asch, 1952; Cialdini & Goldstein, 2004). Given their influence, social
norms have been used in interventions across a wide range of contexts, including those
related to health (Lewis & Neighbors, 2006), environmental sustainability (Goldstein,
Cialdini, & Griskevicius, 2008; Schultz, Nolan, Cialdini, Goldstein, & Griskevicius,
2007), intergroup conflict (Paluck, 2009), civic action (Gerber & Rogers, 2009), and
more (Tankard & Paluck, 2016; Miller & Prentice, 2016). Generally, these interven-
tions operate by highlighting a positive norm—that others typically act in an appropriate
way—in order to sway people to abandon counternormative behaviors or attitudes that
429
430 IV. Sustainability
are problematic. For instance, one might send a letter to those who are late in paying their
taxes that includes a statement that most people have already paid their taxes, and they
are part of a very small minority of people who have not, in order to increase the payment
rates of recipients (see Hallsworth, List, Metcalfe, & Vlaev, 2017).
But what can you do if the current norm is not “good”? In some cases, behaviors
we hope to encourage may be done by the population very infrequently, done by only
a small minority, or otherwise be counternormative. Similarly, current norms may be
problematic or maladaptive, and do not reflect the desired outcome for those designing
interventions. This is the case for many social problems. Indeed, severe social problems
are, almost by definition, contexts where many or most people contribute to the problem.
In such cases, conformity to existing norms can perpetuate the problem and forestall
change. Fortunately, recent research on social influence has provided a possible solution:
Learning that norms are changing over time and heading in a positive direction can help
motivate people to abandon current norms to create positive social change (Sparkman &
Walton, 2017). In other words, learning that others are changing can motivate people to
follow suit, even if it means going against the current norm.
This form of intervention requires differentiating between static and dynamic norm
information. Static norms refer to information about the behaviors and attitudes of oth-
ers in the present moment (e.g., “Many people text while driving”). Dynamic norms refer
to information about the trends in norms or changes in others’ behavior and attitudes
over time (e.g., “More and more people are starting to avoid texting while driving”). This
conceptual distinction is fairly recent, and, upon reflection, nearly all past work in social
influence and related interventions has focused on static norms. Prior research has shown
that people conformed to whatever norm information their attention was focused on,
whether it be about others’ attitudes or behavior (Cialdini, Kallgren, & Reno, 1991). But
until recently, it was unknown whether people also conformed to information that other
people are changing.
This question was initially pursued in a behavioral context with substantial envi-
ronmental and health impacts: high levels of meat consumption (see Gerber et al., 2013;
Nijdam, Rood, & Westhoek, 2012). In countries like the United States, meat consump-
tion is a salient and well-reinforced norm: People eat meat in social and public settings
and the norm is echoed by default options at almost all restaurants. However, there was
a substantial multiyear decline in meat consumption per capita in the United States start-
ing in the late 2000s (Bittman, 2012). Would drawing people’s attention to this dynamic
norm increase their interest in eating less meat, even though that behavior was coun-
ternormative? A series of studies compared the effects of static and dynamic norms by
randomizing participants to either learn about the static norm that some people make an
effort to limit how much meat they eat, or learn about the dynamic norm that, in recent
years, some people had changed and started to make an effort to eat less meat (Spark-
man & Walton, 2017). We found that learning about the dynamic norm—that others
were changing over time—was more influential and piqued people’s interest in eating less
meat, even though only a minority of people currently did so. In other words, the actions
of a minority were more influential when people knew it was a growing minority (see also
Mortensen et al., 2017).
These findings show that a growing minority can help entice people to abandon prob-
lematic norms. More generally, they showed that people were sensitive to information
Dynamic Norm Interventions 431
about others changing over time. This raised another possibility: In cases where the cur-
rent norms are already desirable, would learning about a growing majority be more influ-
ential than learning about a static majority? Further research, discussed below, suggests
this is the case. Thus, the broad implications of dynamic norms for interventions are
twofold: first, dynamic norm interventions can help create change that goes against the
grain of current norms; second, in contexts where current norms already reflect desirable
outcomes, dynamic norm interventions can strengthen traditional norm interventions by
adding information about positive changes in others’ behavior over time.
PSYCHOLOGICAL PROCESSES
Dynamic norms are influential for many of the same reasons that past research has found
static norm information to be influential. When we witness a majority of people behav-
ing a certain way, we are likely to infer that this behavior is effective, efficient, or wise.
We may also suspect that most people disapprove of those who act in a counternormative
way. Therefore, norms are said to possess both “informational influence” and “norma-
tive influence” (Deutsch & Gerard, 1955). Essentially, we may be persuaded to conform
to a norm we see because we assume there is good reason it is the norm (informational
influence), and because we may fear the reputational costs of going against the grain
(normative influence). Indeed, motives to be accurate and to be liked by others drive con-
formity to norms across a wide range of contexts (Cialdini & Goldstein, 2004; Cialdini
& Trost, 1998). Just as someone may infer that something is the norm for a good reason,
they may also infer that widespread changes are happening for a good reason. And just as
someone may be worried that acting against the group will lead to social rejection, they
may also worry that if they do not keep up with trends, they will be “left behind” and
fall out of favor with others.
Although there are some similarities in how static and dynamic norms function,
there are also unique inferences people may draw from dynamic norms that make them
especially suited to encourage people to change. For instance, when we learn that some-
thing is becoming more popular, we may infer that a larger number of people will act
that way in the future (Mortensen et al., 2017). In the aforementioned work on meat
consumption, participants who read a dynamic norm message were more likely to assume
that a larger number of people would make some effort to eat less meat in the coming
years as well (Sparkman & Walton, 2017). Further, believing that more people would be
cutting back on meat in the future increased people’s interest in reducing one’s own meat
consumption. More generally, seeing people change now invites us to consider a future
world where those trends have continued and where the norms may be different. Then we
may conform to those anticipated changes as if they were a current reality. This process
is referred to as “preconformity,” and enables dynamic norms to encourage conformity
to future norms that have not yet arrived. Consistent with this theory, we found in a
follow-up study that when people were told about the recent decline in meat consump-
tion, but then told that the trend was not expected to continue, this information was no
longer influential.
Further, dynamic norms may be especially well equipped to address a variety of
potential concerns about change. Consider the wide variety of psychological barriers that
432 IV. Sustainability
could stand in the way of change: people may doubt that change is possible, that change
is important enough to pursue, that change is “for people like me,” or have other identity-
related concerns. Imagine you shared these concerns, but then witnessed many people
changing: Would it still seem like change wasn’t possible if many people were doing it?
Would it still feel unimportant if people, en masse, put in the effort to alter their habits
and make changes? If the people changing are people who, like you, did not do it before,
would it still seem incompatible with the kind of person you are? Broadly, dynamic norms
convey that others have changed despite whatever reasons were anticipated to stand in
the way. By providing social proof that contradicts our expectations, dynamic norms
invite us to reconsider these common psychological barriers to change.
Further, the barriers that loom largest in a context are the ones that should be called
into question when we learn that other people are changing. This would suggest that
dynamic norms operate by resolving whichever barriers people anticipate in a given con-
text. A series of experiments explored this idea (Sparkman & Walton, 2019). One study
examined the context of quitting smoking, where doubts of being able to quit often pre-
vent people from trying. Here, we found that learning that others were successful in their
attempts to change led participants to believe they could succeed in changing, too.
Another study examined a very different context, where identity-related barriers
are present: men identifying as feminist and supporting feminist legislation. Here, we
found that learning that some men were changing and starting to identify as feminist
helped men see feminism as more compatible with their own identity, increased self-
identification as feminist, and increased policy support for the Paycheck Fairness Act. In
these and other contexts, dynamic norms signal that others overcame expected barriers
to change, or that the barriers they imagined were perhaps not barriers at all (see Table
19.1). Moreover, across all of these studies, static norms alone did not help resolve these
barriers, or did so significantly less than dynamic norms. Thus, it appears that dynamic
norms have a unique ability to challenge these barriers in contexts where they loom large.
In a follow-up study designed to experimentally assess whether dynamic norms help
resolve barriers that are more salient or loom large, participants read about late-night
screen use, including an op-ed that discussed how it can disrupt one’s quality of sleep
(Sparkman & Walton, 2019, Experiment 5). Participants saw one of four versions of this
op-ed, each written to highlight a different barrier that made it hard to avoid late-night
screen use. One version, for example, was written to raise concerns that participants
might lack the ability to change this habit. In it, participants read about how experts
thought late-night screen use was similar to an addiction. It also included testimonies
“Change is not important enough to pursue” Increased belief that others feel change
(e.g., late-night screen use). is important enough to pursue
“Change is not for people like me” (e.g., men Increased belief that a change would be
identifying as feminist). consistent with one’s sense of identity
Note. In each context, dynamic norms help whichever barrier looms large within these contexts.
Dynamic Norm Interventions 433
from people who had tried to quit late-night screen use but had repeatedly failed to do
so despite its negative impacts on their lives. In a different version of the op-ed designed
to highlight identity-related concerns, participants read about how experts thought that
late-night screen use had become a normal part of people’s lives and identities. It also
gave testimonies from people who remarked that the only people they knew who did not
look at screens at night were people they did not identify with, such as Luddites or older
adults. Next, half of the participants read a dynamic norm statement about how many
people had recently changed and started to avoid late-night screen use. Those who only
saw the op-ed, but did not learn that people had changed, were more likely to be con-
cerned about whichever psychological barrier that had been discussed in the op-ed (e.g.,
if they read that people had struggled to quit, they were less likely to believe they could
quit, too). But those who read about these concerns in the op-ed, and then read about
the dynamic norm, no longer showed concern over the barrier that had been discussed
in the op-ed. Notably, dynamic norms helped improve perceptions only about whichever
barrier had been discussed in the op-ed, and did not help with the barriers that seemed
irrelevant. This study helps confirm that dynamic norms operate by calling into question
whichever barriers loom largest for people. More generally, these findings highlight how
dynamic norms can help resolve a variety of different psychological barriers that stand in
the way of change and may be helpful across a wide range of contexts.
EMPIRICAL EVIDENCE
Dynamic norm interventions have been developed in a variety of domains. In one field
experiment aimed at encouraging people to act against current meat- eating norms,
participants waiting in line to order lunch at a café were given a survey in exchange
for a discount on lunch. Surveys were randomized to include either static or dynamic
norm information about meat consumption, or information about something irrelevant
to meat consumption as the control group. Those in the static norm read that “Recent
research has shown that 30% of Americans make an effort to limit their meat consump-
tion. That means that 3 in 10 people eat less meat than they otherwise would.” Those in
the dynamic norm read “Recent research has shown that, over the last 5 years, 30% of
Americans have started to make an effort to limit their meat consumption. That means
that, in recent years, 3 in 10 people have changed their behavior and begun to eat less
meat than they otherwise would.” In the control survey, participants read about a decline
in Facebook use. When the survey was complete, participants were given a coupon for
a discount on lunch. Unbeknown to participants, each coupon could be used to identify
their experimental condition and track their food-ordering behavior later. Orders among
the static norm and control conditions were roughly similar—about one in six persons
bought a meatless lunch. But these numbers doubled in the dynamic norm condition,
with one in three persons buying a meatless lunch (Sparkman & Walton, 2017).
Dynamic norms research conducted independently by Mortensen and colleagues
(2017) has examined the effects of being told about a growing minority, but in this case
in the domain of conserving water. In one study, they found that people were more likely
to conserve water in the laboratory when they learned that a growing minority had done
so than when simply learning about a static norm that a minority had done so. In a
second study, they found that participants were more likely to donate their time to an
434 IV. Sustainability
environmental charity when they had learned that a growing minority of people had
done so, as compared to both a neutral control, and that a (static) minority had done so.
These studies examined contexts of “trending norms” in which the minority was on the
precipice of becoming a majority (48%, specifically).
This work has found that a dynamic norm message about a growing minority was
more influential than a static norm about a minority.1 But what about in contexts where
the desired behavior is already the norm and done by a majority? Is there some benefit of
adding dynamic norm information here as well? In other words, can a growing majority
be more influential than a static one? One study examined whether dynamic norms would
help encourage water conservation during a drought by encouraging university residents
to use full loads of laundry (Sparkman & Walton, 2017). In this randomized controlled
trial, residential buildings were either assigned to a static norm, dynamic norm, or con-
trol condition. In the static and dynamic norm conditions, signs were hung in the laundry
facilities with conservation messages. The static norm signs highlighted that most resi-
dents used full loads of laundry, while the dynamic norm signs highlighted that residents
were changing and now most used full loads of laundry. The static norm messages led to
a (nonstatistically significant) 10% reduction in water use over the 3-week intervention
period, while the dynamic norm messages led to a (significant) 29% reduction in usage.
This work demonstrates that dynamic norms can strengthen traditional norm interven-
tion approaches.
MECHANISM
How are dynamic norms able to achieve these behavioral outcomes? As discussed,
research has found that dynamic norms can impact several psychological factors that
are important to behavior change. In the context of meat consumption, for instance,
research finds that learning that others are changing and eating less meat leads them to
infer that others must have put in some effort to make this change because they felt it was
of substantial importance (Sparkman & Walton, 2017). Therefore, dynamic norms can
help shift perceptions of prescriptive norms that others care about this behavior change
enough to pursue it. As mentioned, “preconformity” was also a source of influence in
this context: It was found that information about others eating less meat also led partici-
pants to infer that the norm in the future would be different (i.e., that the trend of eating
less meat would continue), which increased their motivation to eat less meat. Further,
expectations of future norm levels were also shown to be a driving factor in motivating
participants to conserve water in the laboratory research conducted by Mortensen and
colleagues (2017). However, given that dynamic norms can impact a variety of important
psychological processes, depending in part on which barriers loom largest, it is important
to note that different mechanisms are likely to drive conformity to dynamic norms in
different contexts.
1 Sincethese original findings others have conducted conceptual replications and found similar results.
For instance, one intervention found that posting signs in a café that contained a dynamic norm message
about an increase in people choosing to use a reusable coffee mug (something done only by a minority)
led to more people choosing to use a reusable mug and reduced the number of disposable cups used
(Loschelder, Siepelmeyer, Fischer, & Rubel, 2019).
Dynamic Norm Interventions 435
EFFECTS OVER TIME
Prior research finds that social norms are a prevalent factor in our decision making
(Ajzen, 1985; Terry, Hogg, & White, 1999). However, this literature also emphasizes
that norms have to be salient in order to have effects (Cialdini et al., 1991). This would
suggest that norm interventions will have direct influence over others over time to the
extent they maintain salience in people’s minds. For instance, research on home energy
interventions where households are sent monthly reports comparing their home energy
use to their neighbors finds positive effects persist over the course of years so long as
the reports continue to be sent (Allcott, 2011). Work on dynamic norms specifically has
mostly examined effects on immediate behaviors. In one notable exception, the research
on water conservation in laundry usage assessed repeated behavior over a course of 2–3
weeks and did not find any attenuation of effects during that time (Sparkman & Wal-
ton, 2017). More generally, the pattern of effects for dynamic norms over time could be
similar to the broader body of norms research, which finds that repeated exposure can
generate longitudinal effects.
Given that dynamic norms deal with contemporary trends and changes in behavior,
those designing interventions need to consider how to give “booster shots” of the inter-
vention that still feel contemporary. For instance, consider the aforementioned interven-
tion where signs were hung in residential laundry facilities that mentioned that many
people had changed and now most used full loads to reduce water consumption. If those
signs were still up 6 months or a year later, should we assume that residents will look at a
dusty sign and still believe this trend is continuing? Would people even notice the signs if
they became a long-term part of the landscape? Perhaps not. In this case, new signs with
updated norm content could need to be put up from time to time both to increase salience
of the signs, and to help the perception that the trends being reported are, in fact, credible
and contemporary ones. The general takeaway here is that the content of dynamic norm
messages, especially if that content discusses “recent” or “current” changes in others, or
responses of others to current events, may need to be edited over time to ensure it is still
perceived to be true and relevant at the time it is being seen.
In some cases, we are able to intervene only once in a given population. To what
extent can we expect a onetime norm intervention to permanently change behavior? Prior
research suggests this is by no means guaranteed, as brief norm messages may be later
forgotten and lose impact. But a few general approaches may be considered to extend the
duration of a onetime dynamic norm intervention where booster shots are not feasible.
First, one may try to incorporate the perception of dynamic norms into people’s general
day-to-day experiences and meaning-making process. One way to do this would be to
create a reminder of the dynamic norm perception that is reinforced every time the behav-
ior in question is observed. For instance, in the context of meat consumption intervention
materials one could say, “Every time you see someone eating a meatless dish or see veg-
etarian dishes listed at a restaurant, remember that’s the direction things are heading,”
or similarly, “Every time you see someone who eats large amounts of meat for every meal
and refuses to try a vegetarian dish from time to time, remember that you are seeing
something that is quickly becoming ‘old-fashioned.’ ” There are certainly cases where
people look at some behavior and think, “Oh, I have noticed more and more people doing
that,” or cases where people see something and think, “Wow, how out of touch!” To the
extent that dynamic norms interventions can manipulate these beliefs, they may have
436 IV. Sustainability
long-term effects. Much like work on “goal priming” and “goal-relevant cues” (Papies,
2016), the idea here is to help create cues or help people recognize existing cues in their
environment that lead them to think about the dynamic norm at or before the point of
decision making, so effects persist over time.
One method to achieve long-term outcomes may be to use dynamic norms to change
beliefs that underlie important goals, such as goals related to one’s well-being or one’s
identity. For example, as previously noted, dynamic norms about many people success-
fully quitting smoking can lead smokers to believe that quitting smoking is easier than
they imagined (Sparkman & Walton, 2019). So even while the trend of people quitting
may lose salience over time or even be forgotten, the belief that they may be able to quit
smoking may persist, which could help smokers attempt to quit. Similarly, men who
learned that other men are changing and starting to identify as feminist can come to
believe that being a feminist is not at odds with who they are. And while the trend may
quickly decline in salience, the notion that being a feminist is “for people like me” could
persist. This may lead them to be more open or feel more comfortable engaging when
issues pertaining to gender inequality come up, like discussions in the classroom or work-
place. More generally, the duration of dynamic norm effects will likely depend on how
long lasting the dynamic norm’s impact is on beliefs that are of major consequence to the
behaviors one is trying to intervene on.
Another approach used by many interventions is to operate as a “foot in the door”
for a lengthier commitment that comes next. For instance, one could first offer partici-
pants a dynamic norm about meat consumption, and then once their interest in eating
less meat is piqued, get them to sign up for a weekly e-mail letter that gives tips on how
to cook more vegetarian dishes and highlights local restaurants that have well-reviewed
plant-based options. In the laundry context, one could imagine first using the dynamic
norm message, and then once residents are motivated to conserve water, offer them a
larger laundry bin that has a “full” line indicating when they have enough laundry to do
a load. Here, dynamic norms may be especially effective as the first step in a two-part
intervention that is followed by an “implementation-intention intervention” (see Leven-
thal, Singer, & Jones, 1965, for an example). This would work by using dynamic norms
to motivate people to create an intention to change their behavior, and then intervening
to channel those intentions into a concrete plan that makes it simple to follow through on
their actions in the long haul.
Finally, it is worth noting that in many cases, onetime behaviors have long-term ben-
efits. Deciding to install solar panels or water-saving shower heads, or persuading voters
to pass a ballot measure, may only require an appeal to succeed once. Dynamic norm
interventions, such as those in the existing literature, can be readily translated to these
kinds of structural or political decision-making contexts without as much concern over
how to maintain the longevity of the desired effects.
HETEROGENEITY
In what contexts should dynamic norms be effective, and when will they be less helpful?
One obvious context where dynamic norms should not be effective are cases where key
barriers to personal change are structural or otherwise not psychological. For instance,
if an intervention sought to increase the use of mass transit in a city, but the city’s mass
Dynamic Norm Interventions 437
transit system was inaccessible to the target audience, dynamic norms about an increase
in people using mass transit would not be likely to help. Generally, dynamic norms are
unlikely to enable change or challenge salient barriers if people have no real affordance
to change.
Even where change is technically possible, the effectiveness of dynamic norms may
still depend on contextual factors. Existing research on dynamic norms shows they are
effective largely because they signal inferences that can help encourage personal change,
such as altering perceptions of future norms, self-efficacy beliefs, identity-related beliefs,
and so on. While this work suggests dynamic norms should be effective against a variety
of psychological barriers in a variety of contexts, it is also possible that certain contex-
tual factors will impede these change-promoting inferences and impact the effectiveness
of dynamic norm interventions. In particular, existing narratives about change in a given
behavioral context (such as beliefs about why or how a change is happening) are likely to
have a dramatic impact on the inferences people draw from dynamic norms. For instance,
a smoker who learns that others are succeeding in quitting smoking may doubt that they
too can quit if a common narrative conveys that only a privileged few can quit smok-
ing, such as those who are wealthy enough to purchase helpful resources, or who live
less stress-filled lives because of relative high status. Generally, we have to interpret the
changes we see, and as we attempt to make sense of change, we are likely to reach for
existing narratives to help us explain that change. In behavioral contexts with narratives
that steer people away from inferences that encourage personal change, we should not
expect dynamic norms to be effective.
Just as behavioral contexts vary in their existing narratives for change, so too do
individuals vary in their personal narratives and existing understandings of what a par-
ticular change may mean. For example, if some men have an existing narrative that the
only reason other men are starting to identify as feminist is because they feel pressured
to do so, they may not find it compatible with their identity to follow suit. Generally, we
should expect dynamic norms to be more effective for subpopulations that do not possess
narratives that hamper change-promoting inferences.
However, dynamic norm interventions dealing with behavioral contexts or individu-
als who have problematic existing narratives of change are not a lost cause. Those design-
ing an intervention could hone the dynamic norm content to actively resist such narra-
tives. Using the prior smoking example, one could highlight that many of the people who
quit were, in fact, low income. In the feminism example, one could highlight that a lot
of men began to identify as feminist not because they felt pressured to do so but because
they were finally willing to stand up for their beliefs despite being pressured not to do so.
In any event, successful dynamic norm interventions will have to be aware of the existing
narratives of change for the behavioral context and across individuals in order to better
understand how the dynamic norm will be understood, and whether that understanding
will help promote change.
Some may wonder whether dynamic norms are more effective for those thought to
be more favorable to any kind of social change, such as those who are politically liberal
or young in age. Thus far, dynamic norms examined in the contexts of meat consump-
tion, quitting smoking, avoiding late-night screen use, avoiding sugary beverages, and
endorsing feminist politics have not found any moderation by political orientation, age,
or gender. In all of these contexts, dynamic norm effects on interest in personal change
were found to have similar effect sizes across these demographic dimensions. However,
438 IV. Sustainability
for a given behavioral context, if a particular demographic is more likely to have an inter-
pretation of change that does not promote personal change, we should expect weaker
effects for that group.
Further, in some contexts, it may be easier or harder to manipulate the salience of
the dynamic norm as desired. Even when a dynamic norm is stated directly, it does not
guarantee a rise in salience (e.g., see Sparkman, Weitz, Robinson, Malhotra, & Walton,
2020, Studies 5–7). In some contexts, dynamic norm information may already be highly
salient, in which case the intervention is effectively redundant with naturally occurring
norm observations. In such cases, the salience of a dynamic norm may effectively be at
ceiling level and unable to be raised. Alternatively, discussing the rise of an unfamiliar
behavior that is highly unusual may, ironically, lead people to fixate on the counternor-
mative status of that behavior (the static norm), instead of the information about change.
For instance, consider the dynamic norm statement “More and more people are install-
ing composting toilets in their home.” This behavior is so deviant from the present norm
that people may not even process this as information about norms changing over time, or
many individuals changing, but simply get stuck thinking about how strange or unusual
composting toilets may be. Therefore, raising the salience of dynamic norms may prove
difficult for behavior changes that are either already salient, or completely unknown.
Conceptually, dynamic norm interventions aim not just to provide information about
change but to make the fact that others are changing stand out more than it otherwise
would.
COUSINS
Dynamic norm interventions are an extension of prior norm interventions research. The
core features of these interventions are similar as they both utilize information about
others to instigate personal change. But what makes dynamic norms unique is that, at
their core, they deal with representations of others’ change. As such, they can serve as
a psychological road map of how to get from point A to point B. This provides some
advantages over traditional static norm interventions. Specifically, three benefits have
thus far been mentioned. First, one can use dynamic norm interventions in cases where
the desired behavior is obviously counternormative, so long as there is improvement
over time. Second, in contexts where the desired behavior is already normative, learning
about a growing majority may be more influential than just a static one. And third, that
information about others changing can convey inferences helpful for others considering
change (e.g., that change is possible; that others feel that something is important enough
to change for; and that it is normal for “people like me,” who have never done something
before to change, to start changing).
A fourth potential benefit is that dynamic norm interventions may be able to avoid
condemning those who have not yet changed and thereby may avoid making people feel
defensive. Consider the basic conceptual appeal (implicit or explicit) of a typical static
norm message hoping to change someone’s behavior: “Most people do this, and if you are
in the minority of people who do not do this, you are wrong and should change.” This
may be problematic as people often struggle to accept negative information about them-
selves or their current behavior (Sherman & Cohen, 2006; Sherman, Nelson, & Steele,
2000) that can prevent people from being inspired by others’ actions (Minson & Monin,
Dynamic Norm Interventions 439
2012; Sparkman & Attari, 2020) and hamper positive behavior change (Armitage, Har-
ris, & Arden, 2011; Epton & Harris, 2008). In fact, norm messaging that leads people
to feel overly pressured can, ironically, be less effective (Howe, Carr, & Walton, 2020).
By contrast, dynamic norm appeals essentially convey that “others are changing, and you
should, too.” So, while static norms convey that one is outside of the majority and ought
to change to conform to it, dynamic norms convey that people like you are currently
changing and thereby avoid the implication that you were foolish for not having changed
yet. Dynamic norms are therefore unique as they can still apply social pressure to change,
but without positioning the target of the intervention as a member of a mistaken minor-
ity. In contexts where people are likely to be defensive, dynamic norms may allow for an
approach to get people to consider change that is more sympathetic to their current state.
However, there may also be some drawbacks of using a dynamic norm intervention
rather than a static norm intervention. For instance, if a problematic behavior is done by
a growing minority of people, highlighting this negative change could yield ironic effects.
Instead, one should use a static norm intervention to motivate conformity to the present
norm and curb the negative changes. Additionally, in some cases, the rapid adaptation of
a behavior can serve as a sign that it is a fad and will soon pass; such is the case with the
rapid rise in popularity of specific baby names (Berger & Le Mens, 2009). In cases where
existing beliefs about change strongly suggest something is a fad, a static norm may be
more persuasive.
How was information about dynamic norms constructed and delivered in past inter-
ventions? As mentioned in the café study, participants encountered the dynamic norm
materials in a survey where they were paid to closely examine it and respond to it shortly
before they chose something for themselves for lunch. Participants therefore had a high
level of engagement with the materials, which they experienced immediately before the
point of decision making in order to maximize impact. The full dynamic norm statement,
as it was presented, read:
Some people are starting to limit how much meat they eat. This is true both nationally
and here at Stanford.
Specifically, recent research has shown that, over the last 5 years, 30% of Americans
have started to make an effort to limit their meat consumption.
That means that, in recent years, 3 in 10 people have changed their behavior and begun
to eat less meat than they otherwise would.
Each sentence directly indicates that people are changing or have changed. The first
statement is meant to help participants recognize that the change is large in magnitude to
emphasize the importance of the matter (“nationally”), while still pertaining to a relevant
ingroup (“and here at Stanford”), which has been shown to strengthen norm statements
(Rimal & Real, 2005). Subsequent statements add detail that clarify that these changes
are due to people who have intentionally altered their behavior, which was chosen to
convey that others believe this is something worth changing for. Participants were then
440 IV. Sustainability
asked, “Why do you think this is?” in order to guide them in elaborating the reasons why
one would change. In doing so, we are inviting participants to articulate reasons they find
to be compelling to change and eat less meat.
In the study targeting full loads of laundry, participants in the dynamic norm condi-
tion read the following message on signs hung in laundry facilities: Stanford R esidents
A re C hanging: Now Most Use F ull Loads! accompanied by an illustration of a
laundry machine with a “full” line indicated, and H elp Stanford Conserve Water!
Similar to the café study, the first line clearly articulates that a relevant ingroup is making
intentional changes in their behavior, and this change is portrayed as substantial (such
that with these changes, those who use full loads now constitute a majority). The signs
were hung on the wall behind the laundry machines so that seeing them would occur
naturally as one was about to do laundry. Thus, the information was available immedi-
ately before deciding whether to combine different types of clothing into a full load, or
divide up one’s clothing into many smaller loads (lights, darks, delicates, etc.). In the sec-
tion below, I further articulate the logic behind these choices and how to create effective
dynamic norm interventions.
NUANCES AND MISCONCEPTIONS
In designing the content to be used in a dynamic norm intervention, one must make
decisions about how to represent social change. For instance, should one simply give a
qualitative description of the trend? “More and more people are doing this.” Or would
a detailed quantitative representation be more effective? “In the past 5 years, 20% of
people have started doing this.” Or would a visual representation of the change, such as
a line graph over time, be better yet? Further, one needs to decide exactly which dynamic
norm information to include: simply the direction of change? The direction, duration,
and pace of change? Who is changing and who is not? How are people making this
change? Are people successful or not in making this change? Are they making this change
intentionally or not? Should it be said or implied that the change is intrinsically motivated
(e.g., people really care about this), or extrinsically motivated (e.g., people are responding
to a financial incentive or legal punishment)? Additionally, one must decide whether and
how to present peripheral information, such as the source of the information, who the
messenger is, and what their motives are. Aside from the content of the dynamic norm,
there is also a choice of medium (Posters hung in public places? Letters sent to homes?
E-mails?) and a variety of aesthetic choices. The answer to all of these questions likely
depends in large part upon the population one is working with, the behavioral context,
and practical limitations, such as the project budget. However, there are some general
guidelines in designing successful dynamic norm interventions that are discussed below,
including having the message be believable, easily noticed, personally elaborated (by the
target audience), signaling intentionality, and being delivered at relevant times for the
decision-making process. While these guidelines are informed by prior research in other
domains, it is important to note that they remain a prospect for future research to better
ascertain how important each is for dynamic norm interventions.
The intervention materials should be believable. When people feel they are asked
to change something substantial, the credibility and quality of the message is a key fac-
tor in their decision making (Langer, Blank, & Chanowitz, 1978; Petty, Cacioppo, &
Dynamic Norm Interventions 441
Goldman, 1981). If the information provided seems unbelievable to the target audience,
it is very possible that it will simply be dismissed. In some cases, providing supporting
evidence may be necessary. For example, someone reading the statement “More men are
starting to consider themselves feminist” may wonder what exactly they were just told:
Is this supposed to be the author voicing their opinion? Is it grounded in some truth?
And if it is true, how did the author come to know this information? It is not simply vis-
ible by looking around a public space. Further, is the change due to cohort differences
(younger men identify as feminist more than older ones)? Or is it that specific men have
actually changed? Readers may better understand that the message contains dynamic
norm information and have fewer lingering questions if they read “More men are start-
ing to identify as feminist. Specifically, a recent poll finds that many men, who did not
consider themselves feminist before, have changed and now do.” While no quantitative
numbers were introduced, and no specific sources were listed (or a specific time frame or
population, for that matter), the information presented does give a simple and complete
narrative regarding what kind of social change has occurred and how we know about it.
Of course, skeptics may still want to know details, such as what the actual percentage of
change was, in what population, who conducted the poll, when it was conducted, how
“feminist” was defined in the poll, and so on. But in cases where the population will take
the information that is presented at face value, a short and coherent message is likely best.
The intervention materials should be noticed and, ideally, contemplated. People con-
form to norm information more when it is salient to them (Cialdini et al., 1991). How-
ever, getting people to open a letter that was mailed to them, or read a sign in public, is
not easy to do. It is harder yet to get people to take their time and actively engage with
intervention materials so that they understand the social change being conveyed and
arrive at specific inferences that will induce personal change. In a survey context, one
can explicitly ask respondents to discuss why they think a change is happening or what
that change means in order to ensure they give it adequate attention and time. But in the
context of a sign hung in a hallway, or a billboard along a roadway, there is no captive
audience—the content has to be visually interesting and sufficiently brief to engage the
observer. In some contexts where people are so busy that contemplation is unlikely, like
a billboard next to a freeway, it may be ideal to explicitly state the important inferences
alongside the dynamic norm: M any P eople A re Quitting Smoking, and You Can
Too. The advantage being that we do not need to gamble on whether busy drivers reach
the important inference on their own. But, this does come at a cost: had they made the
inference on their own that they could quit, they might more strongly believe it to be true,
as discussed below.
The intervention materials should lead observers to generate a persuasive argument
to change on their own, rather than explicate the full argument for them. In many cases,
when people are directly told what to do, they may rebel in order to maintain a sense of
freedom and personal choice (Brehm, 1966). Imagine you read a billboard that said Stop
Looking at Electronic Devices before Going to Bed, It’s Bad for Your Sleep!
This might invite someone to think, “C’mon, how bad could it really be?” Or perhaps
they’ll think, “Thanks, Mom,” and reject the nagging tone of the message. To avoid such
a reaction, interventions should gently guide people to a point where they draw conclu-
sions that are favorable to change on their own so that they engage in “self-persuasion”
(see Aronson, 1999). Dynamic norms can enable this process: others’ change often gar-
ners our attention, and may lead us to wonder how or why are people changing as we try
442 IV. Sustainability
to make sense of the world around us. Those designing interventions should capitalize on
people’s intrigue with change, and use it to get the audience to create persuasive reasons
for why others are changing. Consider the following message: “More and more people
are choosing to avoid looking at their electronic devices at night before going to bed.”
After reading this, people are likely to automatically start trying to explain why many
people would do this, and try to recall whether they have heard anything about the detri-
ments of looking at electronic screens late at night. They might recall an article they once
saw saying it disrupted sleep, or maybe they think about their own late-night device use,
and how it often keeps them up later than they want to be. If so, they are more likely to
internalize the idea that it may not be such a good idea to spend hours at night looking
at a screen before bed.
Importantly, some dynamic norm messages may be better than others at leading
people to identify reasons to change behavior. For instance, imagine you read that “Time
spent using electronic devices late at night has declined in recent years.” Compared to the
prior dynamic norm message on screen use, this one does not remark on decision making
or intentionality of others, and is generally distant from the psychological experience of
people choosing to change their behavior. People reading this may wonder whether oth-
ers are losing interest in smartphones and tablets given their waning novelty, or whether
other late-night activities have become more popular. Critically, it does not lead the
reader to envision strong reasons to avoid or change this behavior. Further, in cases where
one learns about a decline in smoking, it may be ambiguous whether that decline is due
to smokers actually quitting, or simply to younger generations not smoking as much as
prior ones. The latter interpretation would not lead one to believe in others’ or their own
ability to quit. Generally, in order for dynamic norms to lead to helpful inferences about
personal change, they should be written to clearly convey that social changes are due to
many people intentionally making changes in their lives.
The intervention materials should be delivered in a place and time that facilitate
change. In many cases, it is ideal to deliver information about others’ changes just before
the point of decision making. Consider the café study mentioned earlier where we gave
people dynamic norm information as they waited in line before reaching the café’s menu
stand. Had the message come much earlier, such as on a billboard they read that morn-
ing, people may simply have forgotten about the dynamic norm information, and any
positive shifts in opinion they had may have faded away before they had the opportunity
to act. If we had given them the message after they reached the menu stand and already
made a selection for lunch, then the task of encouraging change would have been much
harder: We would no longer be trying to persuade someone that it is a good idea to
order a meatless dish, but instead we would have to persuade participants to abandon
their existing preference, even though they were already anticipating eating that dish.
Generally, dynamic norm interventions should review when and where people are mak-
ing decisions in order to find the proper moment to interject information about others’
change.
Taken together, one should think carefully about how to deliver the dynamic norm
information in a way that attracts attention and succeeds in getting people to engage with
the content. That content should ring true to observers, and lead them to think about why
change would be suitable for themselves. This process should occur in a time and a place
shortly before important junctions in their decision making for that behavior.
Dynamic Norm Interventions 443
IMPLICATIONS FOR PRACTICE
IMPLICATIONS FOR PSYCHOLOGICAL THEORY
What do dynamic norms teach us about how people think and how to solve social prob-
lems? We have long known that people are highly attentive to others and often conform
to how they act, and what they believe. It has been argued that our interest and confor-
mity to others is driven by a fundamental psychological need to share common ground
with others in understanding and purpose (Asch, 1952; Hardin & Higgins, 1996). We
feel compelled to see things the way others do, and for them to see things the way that
we do. People therefore converge on a common shared reality from which any divergence
is acting against the grain. But research on dynamic norms suggests that this is not the
whole story. It demonstrates that social influence is an evolving process—one where peo-
ple are particularly sensitive to information about other people changing in ways that can
drive people to alter how they see the world and how they decide to act. Therefore, social
influence is not only a force of greater convergence on current norms, it is also a force
that can be harnessed for social change. Indeed, looking at wide-scale social changes that
have taken place historically, large numbers of people often change rapidly, abandoning
prior norms and converging on new ones.
It is notable that dynamic norms can help resolve diverse barriers to change in a
variety of contexts, and do so by helping resolve the barriers that loom largest for people.
Thus, dynamic norms interventions may be a particularly flexible and resilient way to
create change, and those designing interventions may not need to identify the specific
barriers to change in a given context, so long as they seem more surmountable when oth-
ers change.
FUTURE DIRECTIONS
Many important areas for future research on dynamic norm interventions have already
been discussed, including the need to design and assess interventions aimed at longitudi-
nal outcomes, and systematically test which presentations of dynamic norm information
are the most effective. In addition, further research can help clarify how to translate
dynamic norm effects into highly scalable formats, how understanding dynamic norm
effects can help curb undesirable behaviors that are becoming more popular over time,
and whether dynamic norm interventions can improve the effectiveness of current inter-
ventions that typically rely on static norms.
Scaling up dynamic norm interventions presents a number of questions to be further
explored. In the context of reducing meat consumption, for instance, it may be possible
to incorporate dynamic norm information about the rise in popularity of meatless dishes
into restaurant menus. While highly scalable, and delivered very near the point of deci-
sion making, such an intervention would no longer be able to ensure that people notice or
actively engage customers. Generally, more research is needed to better understand how
dynamic norm messages can be translated to passive reading contexts and still manage
to actively engage the target audience to elucidate psychological inferences that have been
shown to drive effects.
Dynamic norms may hold the answer to another kind of important intervention con-
text besides those investigated here: What should one do when undesirable behaviors are
becoming more popular over time? Understanding how the direction of change influences
Dynamic Norm Interventions 445
others may hold the key to stopping problematic changes before they spread. The cur-
rent research suggests that making novel problematic behaviors less salient should help.
It also suggests that it may be worth assessing whether portraying changes as likely to be
short-lived (“a fad”), or done for reasons that go against the target audience’s broader val-
ues, would be helpful. However, research has not yet assessed a dynamic norm interven-
tion aiming to intervene on existing norm changes to halt them. Further, more research
is needed to understand whether regressive changes spread via the same psychological
mechanisms as those discussed here or because of novel processes.
Further, there are a number of places where static norm interventions are presently
used that might be strengthened by using dynamic norm information instead. Many util-
ity companies, for instance, provide static social norm feedback to their customers that
compares their home’s energy use to their neighbors’ energy use in order to encourage
users to decrease their monthly usage. If results are similar to those examined in the
studies here, providing dynamic norm information about the positive changes in one’s
neighbors’ energy savings over time may improve the effects of such interventions. Given
how widely social norm interventions are used, there are many areas where compar-
ing the effectiveness of dynamic and static norm interventions may highlight areas for
improvement.
REFERENCES
Ajzen, I. (1985). From intentions to actions: A theory of planned behavior. In J. Kuhl & J. Beck-
mann (Eds.), Action control (pp. 11–39). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Allcott, H. (2011). Social norms and energy conservation. Journal of Public Economics, 95(9–10),
1082–1095.
Armitage, C. J., Harris, P. R., & Arden, M. A. (2011). Evidence that self-affirmation reduces alco-
hol consumption: Randomized exploratory trial with a new, brief means of self-affirming.
Health Psychology, 30(5), 633–641.
Aronson, E. (1999). The power of self-persuasion. American Psychologist, 54(11), 875.
Asch, S. E. (1952). Group forces in the modification and distortion of judgments. In Social psy-
chology (pp. 450–501). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Berger, J., & Le Mens, G. (2009). How adoption speed affects the abandonment of cultural tastes.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 106(20), 8146–8150.
Bittman, M. (2012, January). We’re eating less meat: Why? Retrieved February 1, 2019, from
https://opinionator.blogs.nytimes.com/2012/01/10/were-eating-less-meat-why.
Brehm, J. W. (1966). A theory of psychological reactance. Oxford, UK: Academic Press.
Cialdini, R. B., & Goldstein, N. J. (2004). Social influence: Compliance and conformity. Annual
Review of Psychology, 55(1), 591–621.
Cialdini, R. B., Kallgren, C. A., & Reno, R. R. (1991). A focus theory of normative conduct: A
theoretical refinement and reevaluation of the role of norms in human behavior. Advances in
Experimental Social Psychology, 24(20), 201–243.
Cialdini, R. B., & Trost, M. R. (1998). Social influence: Social norms, conformity and compliance.
In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th
ed., Vol. 1, pp. 151–192). New York: McGraw-H ill.
Deutsch, M., & Gerard, H. B. (1955). A study of normative and informational social influences
upon individual judgment. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51(3), 629–636.
Dorn, M., & Stöckli, S. (2018). Social influence fosters the use of a reusable takeaway box. Waste
Management, 79, 296–301.
446 IV. Sustainability
Epton, T., & Harris, P. R. (2008). Self-affirmation promotes health behavior change. Health Psy-
chology, 27(6), 746–752.
Gerber, A. S., & Rogers, T. (2009). Descriptive social norms and motivation to vote: Everybody’s
voting and so should you. Journal of Politics, 71(1), 178–191.
Gerber, P. J., Steinfeld, H., Henderson, B., Mottet, A., Opio, C., Dijkman, J., & Tempio, G.
(2013). Tackling climate change through livestock: A global assessment of emissions and
mitigation opportunities. Rome: Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations.
Goldstein, N. J., Cialdini, R. B., & Griskevicius, V. (2008). A room with a viewpoint: Using social
norms to motivate environmental conservation in hotels. Journal of Consumer Research,
35(3), 472–482.
Hallsworth, M., List, J. A., Metcalfe, R. D., & Vlaev, I. (2017). The behavioralist as tax collector:
Using natural field experiments to enhance tax compliance. Journal of Public Economics,
148, 14–31.
Hardin, C. D., & Higgins, E. T. (1996). Shared reality: How social verification makes the subjec-
tive objective. In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and
cognition: Vol.3. The interpersonal context (pp. 28–84). New York: Guilford Press.
Howe, L., Carr, P., & Walton G. M. (2020). Normative appeals are more effective when they
invite people to work together toward a common cause. Manuscript under review.
Keizer, K., Lindenberg, S., & Steg, L. (2008). The spreading of disorder. Science, 322(5908),
1681–1685.
Langer, E. J., Blank, A., & Chanowitz, B. (1978). The mindlessness of ostensibly thoughtful
action: The role of “placebic” information in interpersonal interaction. Journal of Personal-
ity and Social Psychology, 36(6), 635–642.
Leventhal, H., Singer, R., & Jones, S. (1965). Effects of fear and specificity of recommendation
upon attitudes and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2(1), 20.
Lewis, M. A., & Neighbors, C. (2006). Social norms approaches using descriptive drinking norms
education: A review of the research on personalized normative feedback. Journal of Ameri-
can College Health, 54(4), 213–218.
Loschelder, D. D., Siepelmeyer, H., Fischer, D., & Rubel, J. A. (2019). Dynamic norms drive sus-
tainable consumption: Norm-based nudging helps café customers to avoid disposable to-go-
cups. Journal of Economic Psychology, 75(A).
Miller, D. T., & Prentice, D. A. (2016). Changing norms to change behavior. Annual Review of
Psychology, 67, 339–361.
Minson, J. A., & Monin, B. (2012). Do- gooder derogation: Disparaging morally motivated
minorities to defuse anticipated reproach. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3(2),
200–207.
Mortensen, C. R., Neel, R., Cialdini, R. B., Jaeger, C. M., Jacobson, R. P., & Ringel, M. M.
(2017). Trending norms: A lever for encouraging behaviors performed by the minority. Social
Psychological and Personality Science, 10(2), 201–210.
Nijdam, D., Rood, T., & Westhoek, H. (2012). The price of protein: Review of land use and
carbon footprints from life cycle assessments of animal food products and their substitutes.
Food Policy, 37(6), 760–770.
Paluck, E. L. (2009). Reducing intergroup prejudice and conflict using the media: A field experi-
ment in Rwanda. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 574–587.
Papies, E. K. (2016). Health goal priming as a situated intervention tool: How to benefit from
nonconscious motivational routes to health behaviour. Health Psychology Review, 10(4),
408–424.
Petty, R. E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Goldman, R. (1981). Personal involvement as a determinant of
argument-based persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41(5), 847–855.
Rimal, R. N., & Real, K. (2005). How behaviors are influenced by perceived norms: A test of the
theory of normative social behavior. Communication Research, 32(3), 389–414.
Dynamic Norm Interventions 447
Schultz, P. W., Nolan, J. M., Cialdini, R. B., Goldstein, N. J., & Griskevicius, V. (2007). The
constructive, destructive, and reconstructive power of social norms. Psychological Science,
18(5), 429–434.
Sherman, D., & Cohen, G. L. (2006). The psychology of self-defense: Self-affirmation theory.
Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 183–242.
Sherman, D., Nelson, L. D., & Steele, C. M. (2000). Do messages about health risks threaten the
self?: Increasing the acceptance of threatening health messages via self-affirmation. Personal-
ity and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(9), 1046–1058.
Sparkman, G., & Attari, S. Z. (2020). Credibility, communication, and climate change: How
lifestyle inconsistency and do-gooder derogation impact decarbonization advocacy. Energy
Research and Social Science, 59, 101290.
Sparkman, G., & Walton, G. M. (2017). Dynamic norms promote sustainable behavior, even if it
is counternormative. Psychological Science, 28(11), 1663–1674.
Sparkman, G., & Walton, G. M. (2019). Witnessing change: Dynamic norms help resolve diverse
barriers to personal change. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 82, 238–252.
Sparkman, G., Weitz, E., Robinson, T. N., Malhotra, N., & Walton, G. M. (2020). Developing a
scalable dynamic norm menu-based intervention to reduce meat consumption. Sustainability,
12(6), 2453.
Tankard, M. E., & Paluck, E. L. (2016). Norm perception as a vehicle for social change. Social
Issues and Policy Review, 10(1), 181–211.
Terry, D. J., Hogg, M. A., & White, K. M. (1999). The theory of planned behaviour: Self-identity,
social identity and group norms. British Journal of Social Psychology, 38(3), 225–244.
Author Index
448
Author Index 449
Balchen, S., 398 Blascovich, J., 228, 240, 242, 244, 245, 247, 248, 251,
Banaji, M. R., 299 254, 310, 318
Bandura, A., 103, 156, 195, 360, 416 Block, D. R., 239
Banko, K. M., 81 Block, J. P., 298
Barbash, F., 405 Bloom, H. S., 20, 51
Bargh, J. A., 288 Blumberg, S. J., 65
Barnett, W. S., 11 Boaler, J., 28
Barnoya, J., 294 Boals, A., 369
Barrett, D. W., 415 Boehm, J. K., 194, 198, 203, 378
Barrett, L. F., 249, 250 Boergers, J., 306
Barron, K. E., 100, 103, 105, 106, 113, 116, 120, 155 Bolen, D., 150
Barsalou, L. W., 250 Boles, D. Z., 233, 291
Bar-Tal, D., 347, 358, 359, 361 Bolier, L., 192, 197, 201, 211
Bartholomew, J. B., 242 Bolton, G. E., 351
Bartke, S., 411 Boniecki, K. A., 61
Bartlett, M. Y., 199 Booth, R. J., 392
Basil, M., 287 Borman, G. D., 42, 44, 46, 70, 74, 75, 77, 78, 80
Bass, B. M., 356 Borren, I., 385
Bastian, B., 130 Boswell, R. G., 298
Bator, R. J., 420 Boulay, B., 20, 21
Batson, C. D., 355 Bowen, N. K., 78
Baucom, D. H., 368 Bowman, N. A., 130
Baumeister, R. F., 39, 53, 196, 202, 260, 278, 386 Boykin, C. M., 43
Beach, S. R. H., 367, 368 Boynton, M. H., 325
Beaulieu, D. A., 374 Braams, B. R., 262, 268
Beck, A. T., 308, 309, 318 Bradbury, T. N., 386, 398
Beck, J. S., 210 Bradley, R. T., 239, 251
Beilock, S., 242 Bradshaw, C. P., 342
Bell, L. A., 139 Brady, S. T., 2, 3, 36, 38, 39, 43, 45, 53, 68, 82, 83,
Bellavia, G. M., 375, 386, 392 109, 155, 218, 228, 233, 242, 245, 247, 253, 316
Beltzer, M. L., 242, 244 Bragg, M. A., 288
Bembenutty, H., 167 Brandstätter, V., 174
Benartzi, S., 279 Brannon, T. N., 127, 131, 142
Bengtsson, H., 386 Bransford, J. D., 119
Ben-Zeev, A., 139 Bratter, J. L., 70
Ben-Zeev, T., 74 Braz, M., 408
Berg, J., 351 Brehm, J. W., 358, 441
Berger, J., 298, 439 Brewer, M. B., 141
Berger, R., 25, 28 Brick, C., 67
Bergin, S., 168 Brickman, P., 150
Bergsieker, H. B., 277 Briñol, P., 83
Berkowitz, A. D., 407 Brock, T., 51
Bernecker, K., 23 Brockner, J., 68
Berns, G. S., 260, 261 Broda, M., 17, 41, 46, 53
Bertoldo, J. D., 296, 297 Brody, G. H., 54, 367
Bettinger, E. P., 18, 22 Bronfenbrenner, U., 159
Bialkova, S., 287 Bronk, K. C., 263
Biegel, G. M., 229 Broockman, D., 355
Bieler, A., 347 Brookfield, J., 66
Bilali, R., 349 Brooks, A. W., 240, 251, 254
Bilewicz, M., 356 Brossard, D., 420
Billings, A. G., 218 Browman, A. S., 82, 151, 161
Binet, A., 12 Brown, A. L., 119
Binning, K. R., 41, 44, 46, 51, 67, 68, 70, 82, 83 Brown, E., 423
Birch, L. L., 287 Brown, E. R., 108, 109, 121
Bittman, M., 430 Brown, J., 198
Bjork, R. A., 168 Brown, J. S., 176
Black, A. R., 20 Brown, K. W., 229
Blackstock, E., 228, 229, 242 Brown, R., 360
Blackwell, K. L., 228 Brownell, K. D., 220, 265, 287, 288, 294
Blackwell, L. S., 9, 11, 12, 14, 15, 16, 19, 25, 128, 155, Brownley, K. A., 243
173, 174, 355 Bruch, S. K., 75
Blair, S. S., 70 Bruckner, H., 325
Blank, A., 440 Brumberg, A., 294
Blanton, H., 79 Brummelman, E., 395, 398
450 Author Index
Bryan, C. J., 2, 3, 156, 230, 259, 260, 262, 263, 266, Chatman, J., 127
273, 294, 378, 388 Chavez, O., 167, 171
Brzustoski, P., 70, 92, 319, 334 Chen, E., 54, 159
Buehler, C., 367 Chen, J., 155
Buffardi, L. E., 140 Chen, P., 2, 3, 166, 167, 168, 169, 170, 171, 172, 174,
Bugental, D. B., 374 175, 177, 178, 179, 183, 184, 185
Bunyan, D. P., 67 Cheng, C. M., 270
Burack, J. A., 24 Cheng, K. M., 179
Burger, J. M., 77, 408 Cheryan, S., 161
Burman, B., 367 Cheung, A. C. K., 20, 21
Burn, F., 82 Chezzi, C., 369
Burnette, J. L., 22, 193, 208 Chiu, C. Y., 11, 29, 130, 143, 227, 307, 308, 354, 355,
Burson, A., 71 411
Burton, C. M., 198, 202, 204, 211 Choi, I., 194
Bushman, B. J., 393, 395 Chong, E. S. K., 325
Busseri, M. A., 197 Christensen, A., 386
Butler, E. A., 250, 369 Chukhray, I., 70
Butner, J., 415 Chung, C. K., 76
Butterfield, B., 220 Cialdini, R. B., 2, 275, 335, 405, 406, 407, 409, 410,
Bybee, D., 151 415, 416, 419, 420, 421, 422, 429, 430, 431, 435,
441
Cable, D. M., 127 Cienfuegos, C., 296
Cabrera, N. L., 130 Cikara, M., 349, 356
Cacioppo, J. T., 440 Cimpian, A., 318
Cahill, L., 229 Clapp, J. D., 407, 411
Cain, K. M., 307 Clark, E. K., 108
Cairns, G., 265 Clark, M. S., 196, 199, 250, 387, 397, 398
Calhoun, L. C., 219, 229, 231 Claro, S., 79
Calvete, E., 313 Clayton, J., 128
Cameron, J. J., 386, 398 Cochrane, S., 414
Campbell, J. D., 386 Cocking, R. R., 119
Canning, E. A., 22, 27, 28, 30, 70, 102, 105, 106, 107, Coffey, K. A., 199
112, 115, 121, 155, 158 Cohen, D., 380
Cantor, N., 261, 347 Cohen, G. L., 2, 38, 39, 40, 42, 43, 44, 45, 46, 47, 49,
Capó Crucet, J., 131 50, 53, 54, 58, 63, 64, 65, 66, 67, 68, 70, 71, 72,
Cappeliez, P., 369 73, 74, 75, 76, 77, 78, 79, 80, 81, 82, 83, 92, 109,
Carey, K. B., 415 120, 127, 136, 139, 154, 167, 169, 172, 184, 195,
Carlsmith, J. M., 78, 275 199, 204, 207, 260, 307, 316, 319, 334, 378, 393,
Carlsson, R., 408, 418 397, 438
Carr, D., 367 Cohen, J. F. W., 296
Carr, P. B., 23, 53, 439 Cohen, L. H., 229
Carstensen, L. L., 203 Cohen, L. J., 225
Carswell, K. L., 151 Cohen, S., 54, 229, 240, 313, 371
Carver, C. S., 218, 225 Cohen-Chen, S., 348, 350, 352
Case, K. A., 130 Cohn, M. A., 199, 200, 201, 202
Casey, B. J., 262, 268 Cole, E. R., 130, 139
Caspit, B., 349 Cole, S. W., 205
Castillo, C., 159 Coleman, J. S., 306
Catalino, L. I., 206 Collins, A., 176
Caudle, K., 262 Collins, N. L., 386
Cehajic-Clancy, S., 67, 68 Colyar, J. E., 149
Centola, D., 361 Coman, A., 361
Chacon, V., 294 Connell, J. P., 171
Chaiken, S., 66 Conner, A. L., 139, 143, 233, 288
Chajut, E., 224 Connor-Smith, J., 225
Chambaron, S., 288 Contreras, F., 79
Chancellor, J., 196, 203, 210 Cook, A., 81
Chandon, P., 83 Cook, J. E., 45, 71, 76, 78, 80
Chang, J., 355 Cooke, I., 287
Chanowitz, B., 440 Cooper, H., 116, 141
Chao, M. M., 130 Coppola, B. C., 167
Charalambides, L., 417, 418 Corbin, W. R., 220, 287
Charbonneau, A. K., 342 Cordova, D. I., 110
Charles, S. T., 203 Cordray, D. S., 115
Charney, D. S., 226 Cornell, D., 342
Author Index 451
Levy, D. E., 298 Major, B., 71, 139, 140, 240, 248
Levy, S. R., 307 Maki, A., 416
Lewandowski, D. A., 412 Malhotra, N., 438
Lewin, K., 64, 79, 261 Mallett, R. K., 39
Lewis, M. A., 408, 411, 418, 429 Maloney, E. K., 408
Lewis, N. A., 149 Mang, J., 80
Liau, A. K., 201, 203 Mangels, J. A., 220
Libby, L. K., 333 Mann, T., 287, 296
Liberman, A., 66 Manzo, V., 132
Liberman, V., 67, 261 Maoz, I., 356, 360
Lickel, B., 228 Margetts, H., 408
Lieberman, J. D., 311 Margolin, G., 367
Liem, D., 288 Margolis, S., 196, 210
Lim, S. S., 286 Marigold, D. C., 2, 53, 375, 380, 385, 388, 389, 390,
Lin, D. M. S., 11, 354 391, 392, 393, 394, 395, 396, 397
Lindenberg, S., 419, 443 Marisa, E. M., 227
Linder, J. A., 408, 412 Markman, E. M., 318
Linkenbach, J. W., 408, 418 Markman, H. J., 386
Lin-Siegler, X., 155 Marksteiner, T., 42
Linville, P. W., 72, 77, 155, 228 Markus, H. R., 28, 67, 127, 129, 135, 139, 141, 142,
Lipsey, M. W., 20 143, 150, 233, 277
List, J. A., 430 Marotta, M. E., 240
Liu, T. J., 65 Martell, D., 414
Liu, Z., 411 Martens, A., 68
Lobstein, T., 265 Martin, A., 221, 229, 240
Locke, E. A., 102 Martin, C., 68
Lockwood, P., 46, 47, 50 Martin, G. N., 306
Loeschinger, D. C., 420 Martinez-Pons, M., 167
Loew, B., 174 Marty, L., 288
Loew, K., 414 Marx, R. W., 130
Loewenstein, G., 260, 261 Mason, A. E., 369
Logel, C., 39, 45, 58, 68, 71, 78, 207, 218, 378, 393, 397 Mason, A. Z., 121
Lokhande, M., 2, 45, 63, 79, 80, 81, 154, 393 Master, A., 68, 92, 172, 227, 273, 388
Long, S. M., 127, 131 Mathews, A., 244
Loomis, C. C., 307 Matthews, K. A., 239, 242
Lopez, D. F., 66 Matzel, L. D., 12
Lopez, M. H., 79 Mauss, I. B., 206
Lord, S., 68 McCabe, K., 351
Loschelder, D. D., 434 McCaul, K. D., 77
Losen, D. J., 324, 325 McCauley C., 360
Lovenheim, M. F., 149 McCoy, S., 248
Lowery, L., 412 McCrae, R. R., 201, 218
Lu, J., 288 McCullough, M. E., 192, 194, 198, 204
Lucas, R. E., 191 McCurley, J. L., 298
Luchies, L. B., 2, 3, 333, 366, 370, 371, 386 McDonnell, A. L., 407
Ludden, J., 38 McEwen, B. S., 218, 219, 229, 252, 310
Ludvigsen, S., 18 McGinn, M. M., 367
Luna-Lucero, M., 155 McGonigal, K., 218
Lundberg, K., 270 McGregor, H. A., 254, 311
Lundberg, U., 239 McKay, T. A., 179
Lupien, S. J., 218, 252, 310 McKeachie, W. J., 167
Luykx, A., 81 McKenzie-Mohr, D., 420
Lykken, D., 192 McPartland, J. M., 325
Lyubomirsky, S., 191, 192, 193, 194, 196, 197, 198, McQueen, A., 77
199, 200, 201, 202, 203, 204, 205, 206, 207, 208, Medrano, F. N., 268, 269
209, 225, 378 Mehdipour, T., 408
Mehl, M. R., 369
Ma, J., 149 Meissner, L., 159
MacDonald, G., 387 Melander, E., 347
MacInnis, C. C., 360 Mendes, W. B., 228, 229, 240, 241, 242, 247, 248, 250,
Mackie, D. M., 348 251, 254, 310, 373
MacLeod, C., 244 Mendoza-Denton, R., 38, 39, 43, 72, 240, 242, 325
Maibach, E., 287 Menon, J., 263
Maier, S. F., 10 Mermelstein, R., 240, 313, 371
Maimaran, M., 289 Messick, D. M., 414
Author Index 457
Reay, D., 128 Rozek, C. S., 42, 70, 102, 109, 242, 341
Redwine, L., 198 Rozin, P., 412
Reese, G., 414, 420 Rubel, J. A., 434
Rege, M., 12, 18, 20 Ruch, W., 200
Reh, L., 411 Rueda, M. R., 12
Rehm, C. D., 287 Ruini, C., 202, 204
Reich, J., 82 Ruiz, M., 3, 324
Reid, A., 415 Rumberger, R. W., 325
Reiff, J. S., 279 Russel, C., 411
Reijntjes, A., 393 Russell, A. G., 22, 307, 348
Reilly, R., 168 Rust, M. C., 355
Reinke, W. M., 326 Ruvolo, A. P., 150
Reis, H. T., 203, 250, 367 Ryan, A. M., 307
Reissfelder, S., 408 Ryan, R. M., 103, 193, 196, 206, 307
Renner, B., 411 Ryan, S., 79
Renninger, K. A., 103, 105 Ryda, C., 82
Reno, R. R., 406, 407, 430 Rydell, R. J., 348, 352
Rheinberg, F., 81
Rheinschmidt-Same, M., 72, 135, 242 Saad, L., 219
Richards, J. M., 369 Sadava, S. W., 197
Richeson, J. R., 135 Saguy, T., 350
Richetin, J., 413, 414 Salomon, K., 240
Riggs, L., 131 Salovey, P., 23, 220, 225, 229, 248, 287, 316, 373
Rimal, R. N., 439 Saltarelli, A. J., 82
Rios, D., 130 Sälzer, C., 80
Rivas, A., 415 Sammy, N., 242
Rivlin, R., 359 Samuels, S. M., 261
Roberto, C. A., 287, 294, 298 Sánchez, A., 18, 341
Robins, R. W., 11, 12, 386 Sanchez-Burks, J., 140
Robinson, E., 408, 415, 418, 420 Sandstrom, G. M., 196
Robinson, J. A., 369, 379 Sansone, C., 103
Robinson, J. C., 141 Santerre, C., 65
Robinson, M. L., 38 Santoro, E. W., 221, 222, 223, 225, 226, 228, 230
Robinson, S. R., 293 Sapolsky, R. M., 67, 218, 229, 314
Robinson, T. N., 263, 438 Sasaki, S. J., 349, 356
Robles, T. F., 367 Sassenrath, C., 349, 356
Rochkind, J., 325 Sauce, B., 12
Rocque, M., 325 Savino, N. S., 206
Rogers, L. O., 154 Sbarra, D. A., 250, 369, 379
Rogers, M. L., 306 Schanzenbach, D. W., 149
Rogers, T., 156, 273, 388, 408, 412, 415, 423, 429 Scheer, J. R., 325
Rokeach, M., 76, 77, 82, 272, 273 Scheier, M. F., 218
Romero, C., 9, 10, 12, 17, 18, 21, 27, 227 Schein, C., 354
Ronaldson, A., 198 Schimel, J., 68, 81
Rood, T., 430 Schkade, D., 192
Rose, J., 325 Schleider, J. L., 22, 309, 313, 314, 315, 317
Rose, P., 375, 386, 392 Schmader, T., 38, 139, 218, 228, 229, 242, 247
Rosenthal, L., 139 Schmeichel, B. J., 71, 82, 94, 155
Rosenthal, R., 72, 334 Schneider, B., 149
Rosenzweig, E. Q., 100, 106, 108, 113, 116 Schneiderman, N., 218, 229, 243
Ross, L. D., 53, 67, 68, 78, 139, 177, 261, 339, 341, 347 Schoebi, D., 367
Ross, M., 375, 387, 388, 390, 391, 392 Schonert-Reichl, K. A., 159, 210
Rossignac-Milon, M., 154 Schreier, H. M. C., 159
Roth, S., 225 Schroder, H. S., 11
Rothbart, M. K., 12 Schultz, P. W., 2, 405, 409, 410, 413, 417, 419, 420,
Rothman, A. J., 225, 416 421, 422, 429
Rothschild, L., 129 Schuman, H., 178
Rothstein, J., 149 Schumann, K., 22
Rothstein, R., 79 Schunk, D. H., 156, 176
Rouse, D. I., 70 Schwabe, L., 229
Routledge, C., 202 Schwalbe, M., 70
Rowe, M. L., 22 Schwartz, G. E. R., 229
Rowland, J., 356 Schwartz, S. H., 412
Rowley, K. J., 70 Schwarz, N., 94, 367
Røysamb, E., 385 Scillitoe, J., 68
460 Author Index
Walton, M. L., 420 Wilson, T. D., 1, 29, 40, 42, 44, 49, 53, 65, 72, 77, 79,
Wan, W., 11, 354 109, 128, 136, 143, 152, 155, 169, 172, 174, 184,
Wang, C. S., 356 192, 193, 196, 199, 203, 219, 220, 227, 228, 232,
Wang, L., 210 252, 255, 275, 276, 279, 288, 310, 317, 319, 333,
Ward, A., 68, 78, 261, 287 337, 341, 368, 371, 379, 393, 398, 408, 413, 420
Ward, D. E., 369, 379 Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., 250
Wardle, J., 287 Winant, H., 129
Waroquier, L., 417, 418 Wingate, U., 173, 174
Waters, S. F., 250 Winquist, J., 206, 224
Watkins, P. C., 198 Winslow, M. P., 273
Watson, D., 367 Winters, M. A., 342
Watt, L. M., 369 Wohl, M. J. A., 348, 350
Weaver, J. L., 229 Wolf, O. T., 229
Webber, K. C., 78 Wolf, W., 314
Weber, E. U., 279 Wolfe, C., 139
Weber, J. M., 414 Wood, J. V., 387, 395
Weeks, L., 38 Woodfin, L., 25
Wegmann, K. M., 78 Woodward, K., 198
Weiner, B., 10, 308, 318 Woolley, K., 293, 295, 298
Weinstein, R. S., 325 Wormington, S. V., 105, 106
Weintraub, J. K., 218 Wosinska, W., 415
Weisbuch, M., 247 Wright, D. L., 79
Weiss, M. J., 51 Wu, S., 380
Weiss, R. L., 368 Wurf, E., 150
Weisz, E., 22 Wynn, S. R., 116
Weisz, J. R., 22, 309, 314, 317
Weitz, E., 438 Yaeger, P., 127
Welch, M., 149 Yang, M., 307
Wellborn, J. G., 171 Yarrow, A., 325
Wellenzohn, S., 200 Yates, D., 421, 422
Werle, C. O. C., 288, 294 Yeager, D. S., 2, 3, 4, 9, 10, 11, 12, 16, 17, 18, 19, 21, 22,
Wesley Perkins, H., 407, 408, 418, 420 23, 27, 41, 42, 43, 44, 45, 46, 49, 50, 51, 52, 54, 58,
West, T. V., 250 72, 78, 81, 109, 110, 121, 132, 134, 142, 143, 155, 156,
Western, B., 325 172, 173, 174, 183, 184, 199, 220, 228, 230, 233, 234,
Westhoek, H., 430 240, 249, 255, 260, 262, 263, 266, 272, 273, 276, 278,
Wetherall, M., 414 294, 295, 305, 307, 308, 309, 310, 311, 312, 313, 314,
Wheatley, T. P., 65 315, 316, 317, 318, 319, 325, 337, 341, 349, 355, 359
Whillans, A., 280 Yerkes, R. M., 226
Whisman, M. A., 367, 386 Yeung, C. W. M., 23
White, K. M., 415, 435 Yoder, N., 160, 298
Widman, K. F., 386 Young, M. D., 221
Wiebe, J. S., 218
Wiesenfeld, B. M., 68 Zajonc, R. B., 83
Wigfield, A., 100, 103, 106, 108, 113, 116 Zaki, J., 22, 349, 356
Wilbrecht, L., 319 Zandstra, E., 288
Wilder, L. K., 340 Zanna, M. P., 39, 68, 270, 393
Wildschut, T., 200, 202 Zatz, L. Y., 298
Wilkins, C. L., 140 Zhang, S., 247
Williams, A. M., 68 Zhou, S., 360
Williams, C., 42 Zilca, R., 208
Williams, J. M. G., 244 Zimmerman, B. J., 167, 169, 184
Williams, K. D., 311, 312 Zion, S. R., 224, 225
Williams, M. J., 130 Zlatev, J. J., 134, 199
Williams, R. M., 360 Zuckerman, B., 22
Williamson, G. M., 196, 199 Zunick, P. V., 394
Williamson, I. O., 127 Zúñiga, X., 136
Wilson, M. R., 242 Zwick, R., 351
Subject Index
Ability, 10, 14–21, 15f, 25–27. See also Intellectual Achievement, 27–29, 245–246, 253–254
abilities Achievement gap, 17–18, 22, 127. See also Social class
Abstract reframing intervention (ARI) achievement gap
background, 385–388 Action readiness, 150
content and implementation of, 394–395 Acts of kindness. See Happiness interventions;
Couples Activity for Reappraising Emotions (CARE) Kindness, acts of
intervention and, 375 Acute stress, 251–252. See also Stress
cousins of, 393–394 Adaptive outcomes, 73f. See also Outcomes
effects over time, 390–392, 391f Affect misattribution procedure (AMP), 270, 271f
evidence for, 390–393, 390f, 391f Affirmation interventions. See also Self-affirmation
future directions in, 398–399 interventions
heterogeneity of intervention outcomes, 393 abstract reframing intervention (ARI) and, 393–394,
implications for practice and, 396–397 397
implications for psychological theory and, 398 future directions in, 82–83
mechanisms of, 392 nuances and misconceptions regarding, 79
nuances and misconceptions regarding, 395–396 overview, 83–84
outcomes associated with, 390, 390f social-belonging intervention and, 45
overview, 385 African American students, 70, 71, 73, 74–75. See also
psychological processes and, 388–389 Black individuals
study materials for, 402 Aggression, 311–312
Academic motivation, 153–154, 154f. See also Anxiety, 247–248
Motivation Appraisals. See also Couples Activity for Reappraising
Academic outcomes. See also Intellectual abilities; Emotions (CARE) intervention; Stress reappraisal
Outcomes interventions
difference-education intervention and, 133–134 of coping resources, 244
growth-mindset interventions and, 14–21, 15f empathic-discipline intervention and, 333–334
incremental theory of personality and, 313, group malleability and, 348
314–315, 318–319 incremental theory of personality and, 314–315,
mindset research and, 12, 12f 315f
pathways intervention and, 153, 154f stress-mindset intervention and, 228
self-affirmation interventions and, 69–70 Asian individuals, 74–75
social class achievement gap and, 127–130 Attribution theory. See also Attributions
social functioning and, 306–307 incremental theory of personality and, 313, 314, 318
social-belonging intervention and, 42f, 43 mindset research and, 10–13, 12f
strategic resource use intervention and, 167, 173 overview, 10
utility-value intervention and, 105–108, 106t Attributional retraining interventions, 40, 44, 155–156
463
464 Subject Index
Attributions. See also Attribution theory taste-focused-labeling intervention and, 287, 296
incremental theory of personality and, 308–310, 314 utility-value intervention and, 116, 120, 121
overview, 10 Chronic stress, 251–252. See also Stress
relationship problems and, 386–387 Classroom contexts, 54, 68–69, 69t, 77–78, 105. See
self-affirmation interventions and, 64–65 also Educational interventions
strategic resource use intervention and, 183 Clinical implications. See Practice implications
Autonomy, 310–311 Cognitive approaches, 173–174, 218–219
Cognitive dissonance theory, 66
Background information for interventions Cognitive learning strategies, 173–174
abstract reframing intervention (ARI), 385–388 Communal-value intervention, 109
Couples Activity for Reappraising Emotions (CARE) Compliments, 396–397
intervention, 366–368 Conflict resolution, 358, 359–360, 392–393. See also
difference-education intervention, 127–130 Couples Activity for Reappraising Emotions (CARE)
dynamic norm interventions, 429–431 intervention; Marital quality and interventions
empathic-discipline intervention, 324–327, 326f Conflicts, intergroup, 346–349. See also Group-
group-malleability intervention, 346–347 malleability intervention
happiness interventions, 191–193 Connection. See also Belongingness; Relationships
incremental theory of personality, 306–307 gratitude interventions and, 196–197
mindset research, 10–13, 12f happiness interventions and, 202, 210–211
overview, 2 kindness interventions and, 196–197
pathways intervention, 148–149 needs for, 196–197
self-affirmation interventions, 63–64 Construal. See also Interpretations; Mindsets and
social norms approach, 406–408, 407f mindset research; Perceptions
social-belonging intervention, 36–37, 37f incremental theory of personality and, 314
strategic resource use intervention, 166–167 values-alignment interventions and, 261–262,
stress reappraisal interventions, 240–242, 241f 268–270, 269f, 271f, 278–279
stress-mindset intervention, 217–219 Constructionist models, 250
taste-focused-labeling intervention, 286–288 Contagion, 250
utility-value intervention, 100–101 Content and implementation of an intervention. See
values-alignment interventions, 259–260 also Practice implications
Beliefs. See also Interpretations; Mindsets and mindset abstract reframing intervention (ARI) and, 394–395
research; Perceptions Couples Activity for Reappraising Emotions (CARE)
dynamic norm interventions and, 431–433, 432t intervention and, 375–377
mindset research and, 10–11 difference-education intervention and, 137–138
nuances and misconceptions regarding, 440–441 dynamic norm interventions and, 439–440
social norms approach and, 411 empathic-discipline intervention and, 334–338,
social-belonging intervention and, 42–43, 42f 336t, 337t
Belonging Guide, 45, 58–60 group-malleability intervention and, 356–358, 356t
Belonging uncertainty. See also Threat growth-mindset interventions and, 23–25
incremental theory of personality and, 316 happiness interventions and, 204–205
self-affirmation interventions and, 71 overview, 3–4
social-belonging intervention and, 38–40, 53, 54 pathways intervention and, 156–157
stories from others regarding, 46–49 self-affirmation interventions and, 77–78
Belongingness, 193, 196–197. See also Connection social norms approach and, 417, 418t
Belongingness interventions, 136. See also Social- social-belonging intervention and, 45–50, 58–60
belonging intervention strategic resource use intervention and, 175–178
“Best possible self” intervention, 203. See also taste-focused-labeling intervention and, 295–296
Happiness interventions utility-value intervention and, 110–117, 111f, 112f,
Bias, 23, 65, 141–142 113f, 114f, 121
Biopsychosocial model (BPS) values-alignment interventions and, 273–276
incremental theory of personality and, 310, Context-personalization interventions, 110
312–313, 318–319 Contextual factors
stress reappraisal interventions and, 240–242, 241f empathic-discipline intervention and, 339–340
Bisexual individuals, 325 growth-mindset interventions and, 19
Black individuals. See also African American students happiness interventions and, 209–210
belonging uncertainty and, 38–40 self-affirmation interventions and, 73f
empathic-discipline intervention and, 325, 342–343 social group differences and, 127
social-belonging intervention and, 36–37, 37f, 40–43 social norms approach and, 419
Build Connections intervention, 114, 114f, 117, 118– social-belonging intervention and, 54
119, 118f, 119f. See also Utility-value intervention strategic resource use intervention and, 175
Contextual influence, 158–161, 159f
Career outcomes, 43, 173. See also Outcomes Contextual theory
Choice. See also Content and implementation of an difference-education intervention and, 131–132,
intervention 131f, 133, 134, 138
difference-education intervention and, 140–141 essentialist theory and, 129–130
nuances and misconceptions regarding, 440–442 incremental theory of personality and, 315
Subject Index 465
Identity salience, 150–151, 153, 158 implications for practice and, 208–209
Implementation of an intervention, 3–4, 334–338, nuances and misconceptions regarding, 206–208
336t, 337t. See also Content and implementation outcomes associated with, 197–201
of an intervention overview, 195–196
Implementation-intention interventions, 156
Implications. See Practice implications; Theory Labeling, nutritional, 287. See also Taste-focused-
implications labeling intervention
Incremental theory of intelligence, 316 Latino American students, 72, 74–75, 325
Incremental theory of personality Leadership content, 356–357
background, 306–307 Learned helplessness, 10
cousins of, 316 Learning goals, 11, 105
effects over time, 315 Learning processes. See also Strategic resource use
evidence for, 311–315, 315f intervention
future directions in, 319 learning strategies, 167
heterogeneity of intervention outcomes, 315 metacognition and, 167–168
implications for practice and, 317–318 strategic resource use intervention and, 172–174,
implications for theory and, 318–319 180t, 181–182
mechanisms of, 313–315, 315f Legal contexts, 73f
nuances and misconceptions regarding, 317 Length of interventions, 115–116, 121. See also
outcomes associated with, 311–313 Content and implementation of an intervention
overview, 305–306 Lesbian individuals, 325
psychological processes and, 307–311 Letter writing activities. See Writing activities
Individual differences. See also Cultural contexts; Life satisfaction, 197–198, 367. See also Well-being
Group differences; Student factors and individual Low-self-esteem individuals (LSEs). See also Self-
differences esteem
Couples Activity for Reappraising Emotions (CARE) conflict resolution and, 392–393
intervention and, 380 implications for practice and, 396–397
empathic-discipline intervention and, 325 nuances and misconceptions regarding, 395–396
happiness interventions and, 193–194 outcomes associated with, 390–393, 390f, 391f
stress reappraisal interventions and, 247–248 relationship problems and, 386–389
taste-focused-labeling intervention and, 298–299 social norms approach and, 415–416
values-alignment interventions and, 277, 278
In-person experiences, 51–52 Make It True exercise, 275–276
Institutional climate, 159f, 160–161 Malleability, group. See Group malleability; Group-
Integrative approaches, 254–255 malleability intervention
Intellectual abilities. See also Ability Marital quality and interventions, 366–368, 377,
growth-mindset interventions and, 14–21, 15f, 381. See also Abstract reframing intervention
23–27 (ARI); Conflict resolution; Couples Activity for
mindset research and, 10–12, 12f Reappraising Emotions (CARE) intervention
psychological processes and, 13–14 Mastery-oriented strategies, 11
stress-mindset intervention and, 227–228 Meaning systems, 307–308, 408. See also Mindsets
Intentionality, 207–208 and mindset research
Interactive change processes, 72–73, 73f Mechanisms involved in an intervention. See also
Interest, 103 Effects over time; Heterogeneity of intervention
Intergroup conflicts. See also Group-malleability outcomes; Psychological processes addressed by
intervention an intervention
conflict resolution and, 359–360 abstract reframing intervention (ARI) and, 392
overview, 346–347 Couples Activity for Reappraising Emotions (CARE)
perspective taking and, 348–349, 353–354, intervention and, 372
355–356, 359–360 difference-education intervention and, 133–134
psychological processes and, 347–349 dynamic norm interventions and, 434
Internalization mechanisms, 408, 412 empathic-discipline intervention and, 330, 332
Internalizing symptoms, 312–313 group-malleability intervention and, 351–353, 352f,
International context, 79–81 353f
Interpersonal factors, 250, 368, 369–370. See also happiness interventions and, 198–199
Relationships; Social functioning incremental theory of personality and, 313–315,
Interpretations, 1–2, 10, 150–151. Construal; Mindsets 315f
and mindset research; Perceptions overview, 3
Intervention processes, 103, 104f pathways intervention and, 153, 154f
Introversion, 201–202. See also Personality self-affirmation interventions and, 70–76, 73f
Involvement, 103 social norms approach and, 410–412
IQ testing, 11–12 social-belonging intervention and, 42–43, 42f
strategic resource use intervention and, 170–171,
Kindness, acts of. See also Happiness interventions 171f
cultural contexts and, 210 stress reappraisal interventions and, 244–245
implementation of, 204–205 stress-mindset intervention and, 222–226, 223f
Subject Index 469
Social norms approach. See also Dynamic norm Strategic resource use intervention
interventions background, 166–167
background, 406–408, 407f content and implementation of, 175–178
content and implementation of, 417, 418t cousins of, 173–174
cousins of, 416–417 effects over time, 172
effects over time, 412–413 evidence for, 169–173
evidence for, 408–416, 410t future directions in, 184–185
future directions in, 423–424 implications for practice and, 179–183, 180t
heterogeneity of intervention outcomes, 413–416 implications for theory and, 183–184
implications for practice and, 420–422 mechanisms of, 170–171, 171f
implications for psychological theory and, 422–423 mistakes in implementation of, 181–182
mechanisms of, 410–412 nuances and misconceptions regarding, 178
nuances and misconceptions regarding, 417, outcomes associated with, 169–170
419–420 overview, 166, 185
outcomes associated with, 408–409, 410f psychological processes and, 167–169, 180t, 181–182
overview, 405–406 Strategizing, 171–172
psychological processes and, 408 Stress. See also Stress management and reduction
Social policy, 162. See also Policy factors approaches; Threat
Social system, 73f beneficial effects of, 219–220
Social-belonging intervention Couples Activity for Reappraising Emotions (CARE)
background, 36–37, 37f intervention and, 373–374
content and implementation of, 45–50, 58–60 empathic-discipline intervention and, 342
cousins of, 44–45 implications for practice and, 232–233
effects over time, 42f, 43–44 incremental theory of personality and, 309–310,
evidence for, 40–44, 42f 312–313, 314–315, 315f
future directions in, 53–54 new perspective on, 219–220
heterogeneity of intervention outcomes, 44 overview, 217–219, 231–232, 239–240, 251–252
implications for practice and, 50–52 relationship problems and, 386
implications for theory and, 53 Stress management and reduction approaches. See also
incremental theory of personality and, 316 Stress; Stress reappraisal interventions; Stress-
mechanisms of, 42–43, 42f mindset intervention
nuances and misconceptions regarding, 50 implications for practice and, 232–233
outcomes associated with, 40–42 overview, 218–219, 231
overview, 2, 36 relationship problems and, 386
pathways intervention and, 155 self-affirmation interventions and, 66–67
practitioner experiences, 52, 61–62 stress reappraisal interventions and, 251
psychological processes and, 38–40 Stress Mindset Measure (SSM), 221
Sociocultural context, 36–37, 37f, 129 Stress mindsets, 249, 373–374. See also Mindsets and
Socioeconomic backgrounds mindset research
Couples Activity for Reappraising Emotions (CARE) Stress reappraisal interventions. See also Appraisals;
intervention and, 380 Reappraisals
pathways intervention and, 148–149, 151, 153, 161 background, 240–242, 241f
self-affirmation interventions and, 73f cousins of, 248–250
taste-focused-labeling intervention and, 298–299 effects over time, 245–246
values-alignment interventions and, 277, 278 evidence for, 243–248
Special education, 325 future directions in, 254–255
Stereotypes heterogeneity of intervention outcomes, 246–248,
difference-education intervention and, 138–139, 251
141–142 implications for practice and, 252–253
empathic-discipline intervention and, 342 implications for psychological theory and, 253–254
gender differences and, 247 mechanisms of, 244–245
self-affirmation interventions and, 65–66, 68, nuances and misconceptions regarding, 251–252
74–75 outcomes associated with, 243–244
stereotype threat, 38 overview, 239–240
Stigmatized groups, 325. See also Group differences physiological processes and, 242–243, 244–245
Stories from others. See also Peer stories and psychological processes and, 242, 245–246
testimonials Stress-is-debilitating mindset, 248–249, 373–374
difference-education intervention and, 133 Stress-is-enhancing mindset, 248–249, 316, 373–374
empathic-discipline intervention and, 335, 336t Stress-mindset intervention. See also Mindsets and
giving voice to underrepresented narratives and, 140 mindset research
in growth-mindset interventions and, 24 background, 217–219
incremental theory of personality and, 311 content and implementation of, 228–231, 229t
social norms approach and, 421–422 cousins of, 227–228
social-belonging intervention and, 40, 46–49, effects over time, 226–227
58–60 evidence for, 221–227, 223f
Subject Index 473