Dissertation

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 110

TEMPORALITY AND RHYTHMIC STRUCTURE IN THIRTEEN DRUMS

BY MAKI ISHII AND REBOND A BY IANNIS XENAKIS

Yi-Jan Liu, B.M., M.M.

Dissertation Prepared for the Degree of

DOCTOR OF MUSICAL ARTS

UNIVERSITY OF NORTH TEXAS

August 2014

APPROVED:

Mark Ford, Major Professor


Daniel Arthurs, Committee Member
Christopher Deane, Committee Member
John Holt, Chair of the Division of
Instrumental Studies
Benjamin Brand, Director of Graduate Studies
of the College of Music
James Scott, Dean of the College of Music
Mark Wardell, Dean of the Toulouse Graduate
School
Liu, Yi-Jan. Temporality and Rhythmic Structure in Thirteen Drums by Maki Ishii and

Rebond A by Iannis Xenakis. Doctor of Music Arts (Performance), August 2014, 102 pp., 3

tables, 37 examples, bibliography, 64 titles.

This dissertation will focus on the concepts of musical time of two solo multiple-

percussion compositions, Thirteen Drums (1985) by Maki Ishii and Rebond A (1987-1989) by

Iannis Xenakis. The aesthetic experience of musical works is tied to the perception of musical

time. Performers have to understand the concepts and methods of construction of musical time in

order to interpreting composer’s works.

The model of cognitive process in neuroscience of music and the information processing

theory from cognitive psychology is provided to explain the perception of musical time and its

importance to the aesthetic experience of music. The rhythmic structure, which is essential in

temporal structure to the perception of musical time, is examined in depth to show its significant

influence on the aesthetic experience in both works. Rhythmic tension will also affect the

aesthetic experience.
Copyright 2014

by

Yi-Jan Liu

ii
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

The process of working on this dissertation has been challenging, but at the end, this

experience has been rewarding and priceless. I would not have succeeded without the help of

many people. I would like to thank my committee members: Professor Mark Ford, Dr. Daniel

Arthurs, and Professor Christopher Deane, who have given me valuable advice and support on

this dissertation.

It has been a long and incredible journey studying in the United States. I would like to

thank the professors with whom I have studied who have helped me become a better musician

and prepared me for my future. I would like to thank my major professor Mark Ford and

Professor Christopher Deane who have taught me great deal about percussion and have also

served as my mentors for life. I would also like to thank Dr. Graham Phipps whose enthusiasm

and lectures in music theory have inspired my interests in that area.

I thank my family in Taiwan and all my friends; your support has been my biggest

motivation and comfort. In the journey of pursuing my dream, I could not have made it this far if

not for you.

Finally, I thank my mother and brother for their selfless love and endless support.

Because of you, I feel safe and loved. I am proud to be your daughter and sister. Thank you for

always being there for me and helping me become who I am today.

iii
TABLE OF CONTENTS

Pages

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS ........................................................................................................... iii

LIST OF TABLES .......................................................................................................................... v

LIST OF MUSICAL EXAMPLES ................................................................................................ vi

CHAPTER I
INTRODUCTION ............................................................................................................ 1

CHAPTER II
MAKI ISHII’S AND IANNIS XENAKIS’S
MUSIC AND CONCEPTS OF MUSICAL TIME ........................................................... 3

CHAPTER III
THE PERCEPTION OF MUSICAL TIME AND IMPORTANCE OF RHYTHM
STRUCTURE TO AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE ............................................................ 14

CHAPTER IV
THE CHARACTERISTICS OF RHYTHMIC STRUCTURE TO THE AESTHETIC
EXPERIENCE IN THIRTEEN DRUMS AND REBOND A............................................. 28

CHAPTER V
ANALYSIS OF TEMPORALITY IN THE RHYTHMIC STRUCTURE OF
THIRTEEN DRUMS ......................................................................................................... 44

CHAPTER VI
ANALYSIS OF TEMPORALITY IN THE RHYTHMIC STRUCTURE OF
REBOND A ...................................................................................................................... 70

CHAPTER VII
CONCLUSION ................................................................................................................. 92

APPENDIX
ERRATA IN THE SCORE OF THIRTEEN DRUMS PUBLISHED IN 1986 ................. 95

BIBLIOGRAPHY ......................................................................................................................... 97

iv
LIST OF TABLES

Page

5-1 Thirteen Drums, formal structure ................................................................................... 45

5-2 Thirteen Drums, sectional structure from [page 6, line 2] to [page 9, line 2] ................ 58

6-1 Rebond A, formal structure............................................................................................. 71

v
LIST OF MUSICAL EXAMPLES

All musical examples are reproduced with permission from the Hal Leonard MGB Srl.

Page

4-1a Thirteen Drums, instrumentation .................................................................................... 29

4-1b Rebond A, instrumentation ............................................................................................. 29

4-2 Psappha, 1430-1470 ........................................................................................................ 30

4-3a Rebond A, mm. 15-18 ..................................................................................................... 31

4-3b Rebond A, mm. 21-22 ..................................................................................................... 31

4-4 Thirteen Drums, the rhythmic scheme............................................................................ 32

4-5 Thirteen Drums, page 3, line 2 ...................................................................................... 33

4-6 Thirteen Drums, page 4, from line 2 to line 3 ............................................................... 36

4-7a Rebonds A, mm. 1-6 ....................................................................................................... 37

4-7b Rebonds A, mm. 51-60 ................................................................................................... 38

4-8 Thirteen Drums, page 7 ............................................................................................................ 39

4-9a Rebond A, mm.1-3 .......................................................................................................... 41

4-9b Rebond A, mm. 34-38 ..................................................................................................... 41

4-9c Rebond A, mm. 48-50 ..................................................................................................... 41

4-10a Rebond A, ratios of subdivisions .................................................................................... 42

4-10b Rebond A, ratios of polyrhythms .................................................................................... 42

5-1 Thirteen Drums, the rhythmic scheme............................................................................ 45

5-2 Thirteen Drums, [L1 P1] ~ [4/4 L11 P4] .................................................................................. 48

5-3 Thirteen Drums, [4/4 L11 P4] ~ [4/4 L13 P5] ................................................................ 51

5-4 Thirteen Drums, [3/4 L12 P4] ~ [1/4 L13 P5] ................................................................ 52

vi
5-5 Thirteen Drums, [1/4 L14 P5] ~ [1/4 La5 P5] ................................................................ 54

5-6 Thirteen Drums, [2/4 L16 P6] ~ [3/4 L26 P9] ................................................................ 59

5-7 Thirteen Drums, [4/4 L26 P9] ~ [2/4 L30 P10] .............................................................. 62

5-8 Thirteen Drums, [2/4 L30 P10] ~ [L37 P13] .................................................................. 65

5-9 Thirteen Drums, formation of the variation technique ................................................... 66

6-1 Rebond A, beats 1-3 in m.1 ........................................................................................... 72

6-2 Rebond A, mm. 1-6 ......................................................................................................... 73

6-3 Rebond A, mm. 4-16 ....................................................................................................... 75

6-4 Rebond A, mm. 15-19 ..................................................................................................... 76

6-5 Rebond A, mm. 19-25 ..................................................................................................... 77

6-6 Rebond A, mm. 25-28 ..................................................................................................... 79

6-7 Rebond A, mm. 29-36 ..................................................................................................... 80

6-8 Rebond A, m. 41 ............................................................................................................. 82

6-9 Rebond A, mm. 36-43 ..................................................................................................... 83

6-10 Rebond A, mm. 43-49 ..................................................................................................... 84

6-11 Rebond A, mm. 49-52 ..................................................................................................... 85

6-12 Rebond A, mm. 51-60 ..................................................................................................... 87

vii
CHAPTER I

INTRODUCTION

Maki Ishii (1936-2003) and Iannis Xenakis (1922-2001) were important composers of

contemporary music in the twentieth century and had significant influence over percussion

literature. Both composers had distinct musical styles that were primarily identified by their use

of innovative compositional techniques. In order to appreciate and perform their compositions, it

is necessary to examine their concepts of musical time as it relates to structure and aesthetic

perception. This dissertation focuses on two solo multiple-percussion compositions, Thirteen

Drums (1985) by Maki Ishii and Rebond A (1987-1989) by Iannis Xenakis.

When learning the repertoire of contemporary music, especially the ones considered as

avant-garde or modern works, often performers encounter problems of interpretation due to the

musical complexity in these works. If performers do not understand a composer’s intention (or

meaning) and the compositional concept of the work, it is likely that the performance will be

doubtful, inauthentic, or misleading. For listeners, contemporary music is already difficult to

comprehend. If the performance is misinterpreted, listeners will be confused and have the

inaccurate impression of the work as well as the composer.

Percussionists have more opportunities to be in this situation since much percussion

music is contemporary and modern. Performers have to understand the concepts and methods of

construction of musical time in order to understand contemporary percussion music. This is

important and helpful to professional artists and students interpreting composer’s works. The

performances will be more convincing and closer to the composer’s concept. At the same time,

listeners will know the composer better and will have an authentic concept of the work; therefore,

1
they will have a better appreciation of contemporary percussion music.

This dissertation starts with biographical information of Ishii and Xenakis. A brief

introduction to their music styles and concept of musical time is also provided. Following that is

the discussion on the perception of musical time and its importance to the aesthetic experience of

music by using the model of cognitive process in neuroscience of music and the information

processing theory from cognitive psychology. The rhythmic structure, which is essential in

temporal structure to the perception of musical time, is examined in depth to show its significant

influence on the aesthetic experience. Detailed temporal discussion on Thirteen Drums and

Rebond A is represented respectively in separate chapters.

Thirteen Drums and Rebonds are performed regularly in the modern classical percussion

repertoire. These two works provide a great opportunity to present concepts relating to the

perception of musical time from each composer. By presenting this dissertation, I hope to give

insightful information to performers and listeners alike in order to help them appreciate the value

of these works and their contributions to the world. I also hope to encourage additional research

on musical time in contemporary percussion music.

2
CHAPTER II

MAKI ISHII’S AND IANNIS XENAKIS’S MUSIC AND CONCEPTS OF MUSICAL TIME

Maki Ishii and Iannis Xenakis are important composers of contemporary music and have

significant influence on percussion music. Both composers’ compositions for percussion are also

representative and influential especially to the genre of multiple-percussion. Although Ishii and

Xenakis received western musical education, including the twelve-tone technique, they were not

satisfied with the music of their age, so both turned to seek their ideal music within their

respective cultural backgrounds. Their ethnic heritages (Ishii was a native of Japan and Xenaxis

was Greek), played an essential part in their music.

Biographies and Music Styles

Maki Ishii1

Maki Ishii (1936-2003) is an important Japanese composer of avant-garde music. Born in

Tokyo in 1936, Ishii started his music training with conducting and composition, and also on the

violin and piano in 1948.2 In 1958, Ishii went to Berlin, Germany and studied music at the

Hochschule für Musik under Josef Rufer (twelve-tone technique), Boris Blacher (free

composition), Ernst Pepping (counterpoint), and Heinz Friedrich Harting (harmony).3 In 1962,

he returned to Japan and had his debut concert in Tokyo.4 Invited by the Deutscher

Akademischer Austauschdienst (German Academic Exchange Service, DAAD), Ishii went back

1
The section of Maki is Ishii’s biography is from three sources: the trilingual book, Sounds of West –
Sounds of East: Maki Ishii's Music; entry of “Maki Ishii” on Grove Music Online; Maki Ishii’s official website:
http://ishii.de/maki/en/.
2
From Annex I, “Artistic Background,” in Sounds of West-Sounds of East: Maki Ishii’s Music, ed. Christa
Ishii-Meinecke (Japan: Ongaku No Tomo Sha and Germany: Moeck Musikinstrumente und Verlag, 1997), 243.
3
Ibid..
4
Ibid..

3
to Germany in 1969 and participated in the Artists in Residence Program (Berliner

Künstlerprogramm).5 Since then, Ishii became an active conductor and composer in Japan and

Germany as well as an organizer for various festivals. His works have been performed in many

countries.

Upon his return to Japan when he completed his studies, Ishii devoted his time to finding

a way to combine the sounds of the East and the West together and finally generated his own

voice, which he called a “third image.”6 In the process, Ishii preserved the individual essence of

Western and Japanese music instead of fusing both styles together that could have marred each

style’s identity.

Ishii’s father, Bac Ishii, was a famous Japanese dance teacher and introduced modern

dance to Japan. Through his father, Maki was enculturated in traditional Japanese music since his

childhood. Japanese traditional music such as gagaku, heike-biwa, shakubachi and the chant

(shōmyō) from Japanese Buddhist ceremonies also had a significant influence in Ishii’s works.

He used the compositional techniques from Western music and incorporated the elements of

Japanese music to create works such as Kyō-Sō (1968-1969). Ishii was also fond of traditional

Japanese instruments. He composed works that utilized both Japanese instruments, such as

shakuhachi and koto, and Western instruments. Representative works are Sō-Gu I for shakuhachi

and piano (1970), Sō-Gu II for gagaku and orchestra (1971), and Mono-Prism for Japanese

drums and orchestra (1976).

5
Ibid., 244.
6
Maki Ishii, “Sounds of West-Sounds of East: Striding Two Musical Worlds,” trans. Robin Thompson, in
Sounds of West-Sounds of East: Maki Ishii’s Music, ed. Christa Ishii-Meinecke (Tokyo, Japan: Ongaku No Tomo
Sha and Germany: Moeck Musikinstrumente und Verlag, 1997), 63-65.

4
Iannis Xenakis7

Iannis Xenakis was a composer of avant-garde music and also an architect. Born in 1922

in Romania to Greek parents, Xenakis was sent to a boarding school on the Greek island of

Spetzai at the age of ten and became interested in Greek tragedy and poetry. He returned to

Athens when he was seventeen years old and fought as a soldier against Nazi Germany and the

Axis forces in World War II from 1939-1947. Immediately after the war in 1947, he attended

Athens Polytechnic and earned an engineering degree. That same year, he went to Paris and

continued his music education that he missed during the war. He studied with Arthur Honnegger,

Darius Milhaud and Oliver Messian who had a significant impact on him as a composer. While

at Paris, he began his research in electro-acoustic music.

In order to earn a living, Xenakis worked as an architect with Le Corbusier from 1947 to

1960. He participated in several projects with Le Corbusier including the design of the Philips

Pavilion for the 1958 Brussels Exposition Universelle. Many of the construction’s designs for

architecture can be seen in some of Xenakis’s works.

During his lifetime, Xenakis focused on the synthesis of sound. He was interested in the

timbre and structure of ancient music (Greek) and Byzantine music (in the domain of ancient

Greece) as well as ancient Greek philosophy, all of which had a great influence in his

construction of music.8 Moreover, with his engineering background, Xenakis utilized

mathematical theories such as the Golden Mean (Golden Ratio, Golden Section), Fibonacci

series, and sieve theory to construct the formal structure in compositions. He also developed a

new approach called “stochastic,” a theory of probability to control the sound masses and to

7
The information of Iannis Xenakis’s biography is from several sources: entry of “Iannis Xenakis” on
Grove Music Online; James Harley, Xenakis: His Life in Music; Mario Bois, Iannis Xenakis: the Man and His Music;
Nouritza Matossian, Iannis Xenakis.
8
Interestingly, many of his compositions are in Greek titles.

5
expand his compositional language.9 Representative works of Xenakis are Metastaseis (1953-

1954), Pithoprakta (1955-1956), Duel (1959), Stratégie (1962), Herma (1961),

Terretektorh (1966), and Nomos gamma (1967–8).

Concepts of Musical Time

Music is defined as a chronological art form. Jonathan Kramer stated in his book The

Time of Music, “Music becomes meaningful in and through time.”10 Time in music (musical time)

is perceived through the construction of musical elements such as pitch, rhythm, melody,

harmony, etc..., in a temporal arrangement that is the structure of music. The manipulation of

structure creates the perception of musical time in a piece. Composers in the twentieth century

tried to move away from established forms in Western music and looked for new ways to

construct the formal structure. As unique composers, Ishii and Xenakis both developed distinct

languages to express their ideal of musical time.

Scholars often refer to Ishii’s presentation of musical time in his works as multi-layers of

temporal structure in which different layers of temporal structures, dynamic and static, are

simultaneously presented.11 This time-space concept of Ishii was inspired by the chant of shōmyō

and gagaku performed at the same time and place in a Buddhist temple. The characteristic of

static temporal layer is the static sound that is continuous, repetitive, accumulated, less ordered

and without directionality. In contrast, dynamic temporal layer is the layer of dynamic sounds

9
Iannis Xenakis, Formalized Music: Thought and Mathematics in Composition (Bloomington, IN: Indiana
University Press, 1971). In the book, Xenakis discussed his compositional approaches, including stochastic, and his
philosophy toward music.
10
Jonathan Kramer, The Time of Music: New Meaning, New Temporalities, New Listening Strategies (New
York: Schirmer Books, 1988), 1.
11
The trilingual book, Sounds of West – Sounds of East: Maki Ishii's Music, is a collection of essays from
seven experts on Ishii’s music from Japan and Germany. In this collection, an article written by Ishii himself talked
about his thoughts and techniques, his awakening as a Japanese composer and his philosophy toward music. In all
the articles, an emphasis is especially made on the sounds of Ishii’s music and the aspects of musical time and space
influenced by Japanese traditional music.

6
that offer diversity and directionality. The static sounds/layers are the foundation of Ishii’s

musical structure.

The main concept of Ishii’s music is a “temporal space” existing between multi-temporal

layers.12 Therefore, the concept of “time-space” becomes the core of his compositions. The

sectional development of Ishii’s music is the process of integrating different temporal layers into

a unified space. This structural idea is also inspired by the principle of “jo-ha-kyū (preparation-

breaking-rushing)” from the dance of Nō (as performed in Japanese traditional theater).13

The idea of dynamic temporal layer or static temporal layer can be used extensively in a

section as a horizontal development of musical form. If the characteristics of the dynamic

temporal layers are perceived strongly, the form of the section is “dramatic form.”14 In dramatic

form, the music is “less stratified or becomes uniformed, the whole moving uni-directionally.”15

In the static section, it is Japanese static time that no sense of meter or pulse is perceived and it

may be thought as “a timeless, spatial concept.”16 Ishii’s formal structure is based on the

alternation of sections in different temporalities. Moreover, at the end of the piece, a section of

united temporalities with high intensity concludes Ishii’s music. This stylistic structural closure

is also inspired by his experience at a Buddhist temple where he heard different types of music

taking place at the same time.17 The stylistic structural closure was also inspired by the

12
Koji Sano, “Maki Ishii: The Synthesis of Multi-layered Time,” trans. Robin Thompson, in Sounds of
West-Sounds of East: Maki Ishii’s Music, ed. Christa Ishii-Meinecke, (Tokyo, Japan: Ongaku No Tomo Sha and
Germany: Moeck Musikinstrumente und Verlag, 1997), 83.
13
Maki Ishii, “Sounds of West-Sounds of East: Striding Two Musical Worlds,” trans. Robin Thompson, in
Sounds of West-Sounds of East: Maki Ishii’s Music, ed. Christa Ishii-Meinecke (Tokyo, Japan: Ongaku No Tomo
Sha and Germany: Moeck Musikinstrumente und Verlag), 61.
14
Sano, 89.
15
Ibid., 91.
16
Ibid..
17
Ibid..

7
increasing acoustic density caused by both shōmyō and gagaku.18

In Xenakis’s opinion, aesthetics of music is closely linked to philosophy and humanity.

He was more concerned about the “abstract nature” of music itself.19 Xenakis had claimed that

“To make music means to express human intelligence by sonic means.”20 The techniques used in

the construction of music had to be able to express humanity (logical or illogical). Therefore,

Xenakis was against traditionally established systems such as functional harmony, twelve-tone

technique and all forms of structure. He used his knowledge of mathematics and science,

subjects he thought were closely related to the philosophy of ancient Greece and to the universe,

to construct his music.

Xenakis approached musical time in a different way than that in psychological

comprehension and the aural perception of in-time and out-of-time. 21 He proclaimed the

construction of music is based on three aspects. The first part is the decision of the sonic events

based on the pitch, intensity and duration. The sonic events in this stage are independent of the

procession of time. They are of “structure outside-time.” The second aspect is the “temporal

structure.” Any temporal scheme that has a sense of ordering, including structural designs and

the mathematical formulas, is still considered as outside-time. The third aspect is that the sonic

events of the first aspect are arranged in a “one-on-one” correspondent to the temporal structure

(the second aspect). This is called the “structure in-time.”22

He also related the concept of time to that of space and thought of time as a spatial

18
Lothar Mattner, “Verharrende Zeit: Die Komponist Maki Ishii,” trans. Sidney Corbett, in Sounds of
West-Sounds of East: Maki Ishii’s Music, ed. Christa Ishii-Meinecke (Tokyo, Japan: Ongaku No Tomo Sha and
Germany: Moeck Musikinstrumente und Verlag, 1997), 157.
19
Mario Bois, Iannis Xenakis: The Man and His Music (London: Boosey & Hawkes Music Publishers,
1967),10.
20
Xenakis, Formalized Music, 178.
21
The information on Xenakis’s concept of musical time briefly discussed in this dissertation referred to
Bois’ published interview, “Iannis Xenakis, the Man and His Music,” Xenakis’s book, “Formalized Music,” and an
article, “Concerning Time,” translated by Roberta Brown.
22
Xenakis, Formalized Music, 160-161.

8
concept that musical space was equal to “the area of a sheet of paper of a blackboard.”23 He did

not think time as being only one-dimensional (i.e., linear), but neither did he think of it as three-

dimensional. For him, musical space was “multi-dimensional.”24 Thus Xenakis spatialized

musical time. Once the decision of the outside-time entities were made and arranged according

to a temporal structure, time was then “countable” (i.e., with integers). Therefore, time could be

visualized as “points of a line and could be notated.25

Works for Percussion

Ishii thought that the potential of percussion instruments in Western music during his

time was not fully developed. To that effect, Ishii explored the novelty of sound quality in

percussion like his contemporaries and also focused on "control over rhythm." The construction

of rhythmic structure became essential for percussion in such a way that made it possible for a

performer to play and for the listener to perceive.26

Ishii used percussion instruments, including non-western instruments, in his works of

chamber, orchestra, percussion ensemble and solo. 27 He was especially fond of Japanese

traditional drumming and had invented new techniques for it. Ishii wrote several works for

famous Japanese Taiko-drum groups: Monochrome (1976), Mono-Prism (1976), Dyu-Ha (1981),

Mono-Prism II (1985).

Ishii also contributed to repertoire for multiple-percussion. Representative works are

Search in Gray (1978), Afro-Concerto (1982), Thirteen Drums (1985), For Lily (1988)28,

Percussion Concerto -- South - Fire – Summer (1992) and Fourteen Percussions (2000).

23
Ibid., 173.
24
Bois, 13.
25
Iannis Xenakis and Roberta Brown, “Concerning Time,” Perspectives of New Music 27, no. 1 (Winter,
1989), 89.
26
Ishii, 55.
27
Here percussion instruments refer to non-melodic (non-define pitched) instruments.
28
For Lily is an arrangement from Thirteen Drums for percussion solo and percussion ensemble with
marimba.

9
Xenakis, like Ishii, was fond of percussion and wrote many works for percussion

instruments, including non-western instruments, alone or with other instruments in different

genres such as chamber, orchestra and percussion ensemble. He composed five works using

percussion instruments only. Persephassa (1969) and Pléïades (1979) are for percussion sextet

and were commissioned and premiered by the famous French percussion group, Les Percussions

de Strasbourg. Okho (1989) is a djembe29 trio for Trio Le Cercle (a French percussion group).

Psappha (1975) and Rebonds (1987-1989) are solos for multiple-percussion; these were

premiered by Sylvio Gualda, a renowned French percussionist.30

Thirteen Drums and Rebonds

Thirteen Drums by Ishii and Rebonds by Xenakis, are two of the most performed classic

multiple-percussion solos. Both works were composed within years of each other and only drums

with skin heads were used. Most composers in the twentieth century were attracted to the

colorful sounds generated from various percussion instruments, and the music was more

complex in structure and form. However, in Thirteen Drums and Rebonds, Ishii and Xenakis

demonstrated different techniques to enrich texture through rhythmic structure to make up for the

deficiency of tone color.

Both Thirteen Drums and Rebonds are considered challenging difficult works for

percussionists in the collegiate and professional level. There is no doubt that both pieces are

worth performing and studying. An introduction to the historical background of each piece below

better explains the detailed discussion of each work’s temporality presented in the coming

chapters.

29
A djembe is a kind of African hand drum.
30
James Harley, Xenakis: His Life in Music (New York: Routledge, 2004), 192.

10
Thirteen Drums by Maki Ishii

Thirteen Drums for percussion solo, op. 66 was written in 1985. This piece was

commissioned and premiered by Atsushi Sugahara.31 When he commissioned this work,

Sugahara told Ishii that he wanted only a simple piece which would be like music for the

Japanese taiko drum. Thus Thirteen Drums was born. However, this piece was beyond the

“simple” that Sugahara had pictured with some impossible passages for performance. The work

was revised several times resulting with the current version being very different from the one

original.

The number “thirteen” in the title came from the chromatic scale in one octave. It was an

avant-garde attempt to express musical elements such as melody, harmony and beat and all are

expressed through the tuned-indefinite pitches of drums. Ishii explained his intention of using

drums only in the program note of the premier32 of Thirteen Drums:

…In Thirteen Drums I have flown in the face of this awareness by using only thirteen
membranophones. I make no use here of gongs, cymbals or any other instruments with
long sound envelopes, nor of percussion instruments conventionally used for providing
tonal coloration. My aim in composing this work was to draw attention again to the
dynamic fascination of drums of the membranophone category … This confronts two
main challenges, namely the return to the essence of what it means to strike drums and
the search for new possibilities for the interaction of determinate and indeterminate
rhythms.33

In the detailed analysis of Thirteen Drums which will be discussed in Chapter V, the

determinate and indeterminate designs in the rhythmic structure create the formal structure. The

determinate and indeterminate rhythmic structures create different perception of musical time.

31
In the printed score, Sugahara’s name is misspelled as Sugawara.
32
This is also adopted in the discography with the published recording by Atsushi Sugahara.
33
Ishii, Maki. Program note for Thirteen Drums, in the discography of CD, “Thirteen Drums: Music for
Percussion Solo,” Atsushi Sugahara, percussion, CD-ROM (Camerata Tokyo INC., 30CM-414, 1996).

11
Rebonds by Iannis Xenakis

Rebonds (1987-1989), like Psappha, was written for Sylvio Gualda, a renowned French

percussionist. It was premiered in 1988 by Gualda at the Villa Medici in Rome.34 There are two

movements in Rebonds, A and B, and if both are performed in a concert, movement A can be

played before or after movement B. Compared to Xenakis’s other percussion works such as

Psappha and Pléïdes, Rebonds is a smaller scale of work. Fewer numbers of instruments are

used, and it is in homogeneous instrumentation (seven membranophones). It is also less

complicated in texture and structure.

Xenakis has commented on the homogeneous instrumentation during his interview by

Varga:

Writing percussion is somewhat like writing for piano rather than for orchestra. The piano
has a more homogeneous sound and is consequently much more difficult to write for
interestingly: it has just one color. It was similary a challenge to produce a worth-while
percussion work just for skins, for instance.35

Because of the homogeneous instrumentation, both movements of Rebonds are pure

rhythmic. In Varga’s interview with Xenakis in 1989, Xenakis admitted that

In the past I experimented with music without a rhythmic pulse, that is, having no precise
rhythmic sense. In my latest pieces I seem to have come back to very sharp and simple
structures that are immediately perceptible.36

Compared to his previous works for percussion, the rhythmic structure in Rebonds is

simpler but still complex to a certain degree. The complexity of rhythms in Rebonds is mainly

caused by the irregularity in rhythmic figures, accents patterns, registration of drums, and

polyrhythms. The evolution of each factor together creates a “multi-layered system”37 of

rhythmic structure. The manipulation of these factors affects the perception of the rhythmic

34
Harley, 192.
35
Bálint András Varga, Conversations with Iannis Xenakis (London: Faber and Faber Limited, 1996), 179.
36
Ibid., 147.
37
Ibid..

12
structure and makes a work using homogeneous instrumentation interesting. The detailed

discussion is provided in Chapter IV and Chapter VI.

From the information regarding both composers, it is clear that rhythm is the most

important element in Thirteen Drums and Rebond A. To understand the musical time in both

works, it is necessary to comprehend the rhythmic structure. In Rebond A, the rhythms are

precisely notated; in Thirteen Drums, despite the grace notes and fermatas, all the rhythms are

clearly written. However, even though the rhythms are notated explicitly and can be understood

intelligently by studying the score, the rhythmic structure cannot be comprehended through aural

experience. The following chapter discusses possible reasons why discrepancies exist between

the notated scores and the impact of those discrepancies on the actual experience of musical time.

13
CHAPTER III

THE PERCEPTION OF MUSICAL TIME AND IMPORTANCE OF RHYTHM STRUCTURE

TO AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE

The Perception of Musical Time and the Aesthetics of Music

In daily life, people “experience” time through events happening in the flux of time.

Similarly, the experience of time in music (musical time) is through different musical elements

such as pitch, rhythm, melody, harmony, timbre, and texture. These musical elements together

provide information for listeners about musical time. Some information of musical time is

audible and some is hard to perceive. The ability of recognizing the audible information is

subject to the limitation of perception and cognition of human hearing.38 According to scholars,

the appreciation of music is tied to the aural experience of musical time. Justin London, in his

article “Temporal Complexity in Modern and Post-Modern Music…,”39 claimed that:

… I think it is reasonable to presume that musical works involve the organization and
presentation of sounds in time—sound being the ‘primary medium’ of music. If the
primary medium of music is that of sounds, then a proper experience of a musical work
involves hearing those sounds… While we may have experiences of music that involves
other senses (we may follow Crumb’s score while we listen, we may watch an opera
acted out on stage), it is always our aural experience of it that is key.40

The research of musical time is also found in other fields such as philosophy and

psychology. The perception of musical time has been studied in the cognitive psychology and

neuroscience of music. The studies of perceptual neuroscience of music explain the basic process

from music perception to cognition involving memory systems in the human brain, similar to the

38
Many documents from experts of different fields: music, psychology, philosophy, and physics, discuss
these issues. Please see the bibliography entrees: Kramer, Barry, London, Thaut, Parncutt.
39
Justin London, “Temporal Complexity in Modern and Post-Modern Music…,” in Unfolding Time, ed.
Darla Crispin and Kathleen Snyers (Ghent, Belgium: Leuven University Press, 2009), 45-68.
40
Ibid., 46-47.

14
information processing theory of cognitive psychology. These studies claim that the operation of

memory systems influences our perception of music as well as musical time.

The Cognitive Processes of Musical Time

The following content explains the process of perception and cognition of musical

elements in the human brain and its relations to aesthetic experience. The perception of musical

time happens in the same process. This information is based on memory systems from the

neuroscience and the processing theory of cognitive psychology. The processing of memory

generates different musical experience. In this dissertation, this process is briefly discussed to

explain the perception of musical time. The reference books of this section are Music and

Memory by Bob Snyder,41 Brain & Music by Stefan Koelsch,42 and Rhythm, Music, and the

Brain by Michael H. Thaut.43

The musical experience can be divided into two different kinds of processing: perceptual

processing and cognitive processing. The musical experience starts with the perceptual

processing. To decode the information of musical time, the first step is to extract auditory

features. The sounds the ears perceive as auditory data enter and are processed in the echoic

memory. Auditory features are extracted in this early part of processing; the acoustic variants

such as pitches, loudness, interval, and timbre, are detected. Then according to the relations of

extracted features (simultaneity and correlation), they are bound into separate, individual events.

The sense of tactus or pulse is established here.

The individual events in the echoic memory are then processed in the short-term memory

and into different Gestalt groups according to the similarity, proximity, and continuity of the

41
Bob Snyder, Music and Memory: An Introduction (Cambridge: MIT Press), 2000.
42
Stefan Koelsch, Brain and Music (West Sussex, UK: Wiley-Blackwell, 2012).
43
Michael H. Thaut, Rhythm, Music, and the Brain (New York: Routledge), 2005.

15
stimulus (in correspondence to the auditory “cues” of information processing theory).44 The

melodic and rhythmic groupings all happen in this process and most of the acts are primitive.

Some Gestalt groups are examined further to gain more detailed information such as the qualities

of chords and interpretation of time relations. In this process, the action may not be primitive

because past experience or knowledge stored in the long-term memory may be involved (Snyder

calls this “learned grouping”).45 A sense of meter emerges at this stage. The discrimination of

phrases and the next higher level of groupings may take place in this process. The phrase

grouping also follows Gestalt principles.46

The performance of grouping is very important for information processing because the

storage size of short-term memory is limited. If the perceived information is grouped into

valuable units according to the Gestalt principles, the brain can increase the speed of processing

information thereby stretching the limits of the short-term memory.47

The grouping of information is akin to “chunking” in information processing theory.48

The psychological chunking behavior is to put the interrelated clusters of auditory information

stored in the sensory systems (auditory or visual for example) into groups, allowing the groups

(chunks) to be processed easier in the short-term/working memory. Therefore, more information

can be encoded and then be understood. When the information is understood, it can be stored in

the long-term memory and then be recalled for future use.49

Grouping or chunking can take place in multiple structural levels where the

discrimination of periodicities such as rhythmic figures, motives, fragments, phrases, and

44
Koelsch, 91.
45
Snyder, 33.
46
Ibid., 37-39.
47
Ibid., 36.
48
Ibid., 53-56.
49
Ibid., 54.

16
sections can be intelligible. A chunk of shorter periodicity becomes one of the units of longer

periodicity, which is a “larger chunk.” 50 Thus, structural hierarchy is established. Higher levels

of chunking and the establishment of structural hierarchy involve short-term memory, long-term

memory and working memory. Usable chunks from short-term memory and long-term memory

“rehearse” in the working memory. The performance of rehearsal is important for permanent

memory and emotional stimulation.51 As music goes on in the process of structural building,

listeners keep receiving new elements which may cause the established structural model to be

reanalyzed and then modified. The whole cognitive process may start over.52

The key to chunking performance is the closure and cues for boundaries. A closure is the

act of grouping so the perceived information can be divided into chunks. The cues are for

recalling what the next chunk is in an ordered sequence of a long duration. The chunking cues

are essential to the establishment of structural hierarchy. When the structural hierarchy is formed,

the musical expectancy is born and the sense of entrainment starts to roll. If the information in a

piece of music has better chunking ability (when chunking is easier to perform), the structure

hierarchy will be established easily, and the formal structure as well as rhythmic structure can be

better understood. Only then can musical time be comprehended.

The Perception of Rhythmic Structure

Rhythm is the most important element to the perception of musical time. Most studies of

rhythmic structure in music are on components of rhythm: pulse, beat, tempo, accent, meter, and

rhythmic figures. Understanding how each component is perceived and functions in relation to

the aesthetic experience is important for comprehending the rhythmic structure and musical time.

50
Ibid..
51
Ibid., 53.
52
Koelsch, 93.

17
In this dissertation, the following discussion of the rhythmic structure’s essence is referred to in

several resources: Justin London’s Hearing in Time53 and his several entries about musical time

and rhythm on New Grove Online,54 Christopher Hasty’s Meter as Rhythm,55 Bob Snyder’s

Music and Memory,56 and Michael H. Thaut’s Rhythm, Music, and the Brain.57

Pulse and Beat

London explains both terms as “regularly recurring articulations in the flow of musical

time.”58 Pulse and beat are used interchangeably most of the time. A pulse is sometimes referred

to as a tactus and from it the sense of “motion” is created. However, the meanings of both words

sometimes are different. Pulses are durational. Beats are time points which mark off the musical

events in equal temporal intervals and are durationless.59 Pulses are flexible and are generated

from the rhythmic events. Beats are functional locations in a temporal continuum often

associated with the sense of meter.

Pulse perception is primitive and is perceived through the operation of echoic memory or

short-term memory.60 Many studies show that the shortest/fastest perceivable IOI (interonset

interval) is 100 ms (milliseconds) and the longest/slowest IOI is 5 to 6 seconds.61 However, the

sense of pulse only arises in a range from 250-300 ms62 or 240-200 bpm (beat per measure) to 2

53
Justin London, Hearing in Time: Psychological Aspects of Musical Meter, 2nd ed. (New York: Oxford
University Press, 2012).
54
London, several entrees: “Rhythm,” “Pulse,” “Time” from Grove Music Online.
55
Christopher Hasty, Meter as Rhythm (New York: Oxford University Press, 1997).
56
Bob Snyder, Music and Memory: An Introduction (Cambridge: MIT Press), 2000.
57
Michael H. Thaut, Rhythm, Music, and the Brain (New York: Routledge), 2005.
58
London, “Pulse,” Grove Music Online. Oxford Music Online. Oxford University Press,
http://www.oxfordmusiconline.com/subscriber/article/grove/music/45937 (accessed January 13th, 2013).
59
Christopher Hasty claimed that “if there is sound, it has always had duration” in his book Meter as
Rhythm (pp. 69-76). However, this is a conceptual idea and will help the theoretical explanation for rhythmic
structure.
60
Thaut, 8. This is also discussed in “Brain and Music” by Stefan Koelsch in pp. 103-104.
61
London, Hearing in Time, 27.
62
ms= millisecond.

18
seconds (30 bpm) apart.63 London specifies the characteristics of beats/pulse in different ranges

with the temporal envelope:

…ranges of very short elements (100-400 ms), intervals that are too fast to be heard as
beats but are still countable, giving distinctive shapes to triplets versus quadruplets;
medium elements (400-1,200 ms) that strongly give rise to a sense of beat and that best
afford sensorimotor synchronization; and long elements (1,200-2,000 ms) with which we
can still entrain, though with less accuracy and stability, and tend to induce a subdivision
or counting strategy.64

The durations in the range of 80-120 bpm (500-700 ms) are judged “neither too long nor

too short” and are easily established as pulses.65 In Richard Parncutt’s research, he points out that

600 ms (100 bpm) is the “maximal pulse salience.”66

Pulse is essential to the perception of rhythm and other components. Thaut explains this

importance:

Pulse perception, creating interval templates, must then be considered as a fundamental


component in rhythm formation. It is a universal characteristic of music, forming the
basis for the temporal organization of music in virtually all culture. Pulses serve as
isochronous, equidistant time points generated by interval or period durations. Pulses
divide the flow of time into regular reference points. They serve as critical frameworks
for the essential function of synchronization, which is the fundamental building process
of rhythm.67

Pulse is the primary rhythmic level. Other components of rhythm (which will be

discussed later) could not exist without the perception of pulse. Another important function of

pulse is that the sense of entrainment comes from it. The sense of entrainment involves

anticipation and predictability and these could not happen without the establishment of pulse.

The foregoing research shows that the sense of entrainment is tied to the aesthetic experience.

63
London, Hearing in Time, 30.
64
Ibid., 63.
65
Ibid., 31.
66
Richard Parncutt, “A Perceptual Model of Pulse Salience and Metrical Accents in Musical Rhythms,”
Music Perception 11, no. 4 (1994): 409-464.
67
Thaut, 7.

19
Tempo

The basic concept of tempo is that “the repetition rate of regularly occurring beats or

pulses in a given amount of time defines the pulse speed or frequency.”68 In the entry “Time”

from Grove Music Online, London says that tempo is literally “used to describe musical speed or

pacing.” He also claims that “a true sense of tempo, then, is a product of more than the

successive note-to-note articulations; it involves the perception of motion within rhythmic

groups and across entire phrases.”

In Western music, tempo is marked in beats per minute (bpm). The range of pulse is also

the range for the sense of tempo. However, the rate of tempo is not necessary equal to the pulse

rate. The sense of tempo for listeners is a perceived experience influenced by all the factors in

music such as melodies, rhythms, tonal structure, harmonic progressions and so on.69

Accent

Another important component which will affect the perception of rhythm is accent.

Accents increase the complexity of rhythm and affect rhythmic structure. Theorists distinguish

three types of accents: phenomenal accents, structural accents, and metrical accents.70

Phenomenal accents emphasize a musical event by changes of dynamics, changes of timbre, long

notes (agogic accents) regardless of the phrase structure, rhythmic grouping, and metrical

structure. Structural accents are at melodic or harmonic points which render the impression of

the beginning or ending of a phrase or section. Metrical accents happen when accentuated events

68
Thaut, 9.
69
London, “Hearing in Time,” 33.
70
Fred Lerdahl and Ray Jackendoff , A Generative Theory of Tonal Music (Cambridge : MIT Press, 1983).
In both London’s and Thaut’s books, they adopted the classification from this book.

20
are “relatively strong in its metrical context.”71 The metrical accents are events “marked by

consciousness.”72 When a sense of meter is established, listeners project metrical accents on

events in the felt metrical frame even though there are no indications of dynamic changes or

agogic accents.

Meter

London explains meter in a general sense in Grove Music Online —it is “the temporal

hierarchy of subdivisions, beats and bars that is maintained by performers and inferred by

listeners which functions as a dynamic temporal framework for the production and

comprehension of musical durations.”73 In this sense, meter is a kind of “attentional behavior”74

and a sense of pulse is required to be perceived in order to feel a sense of meter. A sense of meter

is felt when at least two or more levels of periodicity are isochronized: the pulse level (primary

level), the subdivisions of beat/pulse (surface level), and higher levels of combining successive

beats into measures.

The sense of meter also involves psychological behaviors: attention, anticipation,

synchronization and the process of synchronization also known as entrainment behavior.75 In

music meter is a regular and cyclical temporal phenomenon and the sense of entrainment arises

from it. Once the sense of meter is established, listeners will project this metrical temporality

back to the future events. As the music continues, the metrical sense will be maintained because

of the temporal entrainment. This is significant for the aesthetic experience of music.

71
London, Hearing in Time, 18; Thaut, 10.
72
London, Hearing in Time, 19.
73
London, “Metre,” Grove Music Online, Oxford Music Online (Oxford University Press)
http://www.oxfordmusiconline.com/subscriber/article/grove/music/43936 (accessed January 13th, 2013).
74
This is the fundamental concept for London’s book.
75
Ibid., 9-13.

21
The sense of meter is important in Western music while some music from other cultures

do not use the metrical idea. However, the temporal organizations of non-Western music may be

established on rhythmic modes in which rhythmic patterns are built on pulse structures and

combined into extended phrases.76

The temporal envelope of meter is from 200-250 ms to 2 seconds and 600 ms is a

preferred periodicity which is the same range for the sense of pulse. This is because of the

subdivisible characteristic of a beat in a metrical framework. The shortest perceivable IOI for a

human is 100 ms, which serves the subdivision of a perfect duple (1:1) of a beat (200 ms). The

changes of tempo in some musical passages will affect all the levels of periodicities (beat,

subdivision, measure) and also the metrical sense. The choice of tempo also restricts possibilities

of metrical sense.

Rhythmic Figures

At the beginning of the cognitive process, perceived temporal onsets are identified as

separate durations in orders. When durations are in the perceivable range from 100 ms to 5 or 6

seconds, they are grouped into patterns according to their similarity, proximity, and continuity.

The process of configuration for durations is like the chunking of information processing. When

the perceived durations are arranged in a hierarchical structure based on a metric framework, the

rhythmic complexity is intelligible.

The extraction of rhythmic figures is affected by the established pulse and chosen tempo.

The metric context also influences the grouping of rhythmic figures.77 Some research shows that

listeners tend to group auditory onsets into patterns that match certain metrical frameworks

76
Thaut, 9.
77
London, Hearing in Time, 65.

22
(i.e. template matching)78 from their former musical experience and memory (the recall of

information/chunking from long-term memory).

Rhythmic figures occur at the surface level of rhythmic structure. A metric framework is

established well when the durational onsets are grouped into isochronized figures with a cyclic,

hierarchical, temporal organization.79 If entrainment to a meter is well-established prior to the

ongoing sound, listeners’ sense of meter is not likely affected by contrary patterns. This is

because the pulse salience and metrical sense are maintained by human nature for a short amount

of time even when they are not presented in a musical context.80 That is, for a short time (maybe

one or two measures) when the contrary patterns (which carry conflict cues such as phenomenal

accents, structural accents, and periodicity) discord with the entrained patterns created by

established metric sense, the “contextual mismatches” happen until the conflicting rhythmic

patterns disappear, the pulse salience and metrical sense will not be destroyed.81 Rhythmic

figures of syncopations, hemiolas, and missing “articulation on the down beat of the measure”

(London calls it “loud rests”) are likely to cause short-term contextual mismatches.

Nonetheless, a new metric framework may be expected and the refiguring of rhythmic

patterns may occur according to the new extracted salience from the contrary rhythmic figures.

Some musical passages formed by rhythmic figures of high complexity, including the ones that

involve rests, make it difficult to establish a stable pulse resulting in an undetermined or

unavailable metric sense. When rhythmic figures are “metrically malleable” and can generate

more than one metric context,82 the metric framework becomes ambiguous.

Another situation of metric ambiguity happens when two or more rhythmic activities in

78
Ibid., 67.
79
Ibid., 77.
80
Ibid., 70.
81
Ibid., 107-108.
82
Ibid., 99.

23
different layers in music play simultaneously and each rhythmic stream reveals conflicting

dynamic cues or temporal cues. The conflicting temporal cues happen in the musical context of

polyrhythms. London defines polyrhythms as “any two or more separate rhythmic streams in the

musical texture whose periodicities are noninteger multiples.”83 Hemiola is an example of

polyrhythm and its ratio of periodicities is 2:3.

Studies show that listeners can only extract one metric framework from one passage.

When they encounter passages that have ambiguous meters or polyrhythms, listeners will have

two perceptive activities. One is to follow the composite pattern formed by all the rhythmic

streams and match it to the metric framework; the other is to focus on one metric framework

presented by one rhythmic stream.

Rhythmic Structure to the Aesthetic Experience

Scholars from different fields agree that encoding rhythmic structure is essential to the

aesthetic experience of music. Thaut explains why rhythm is the key to grasping meanings in

music. Thaut claims:

Rhythm determines, assigns, and builds time relationships between events in the
perceptual process. Because all efforts in perception must fundamentally include a
multidimensional temporal process, regardless of sense of modality, rhythm assumes a
critical role in the shaping and modulating of meaning in perception.84

Thaut continues:

Rhythm organizes time. In music, as a time-based acoustical language, rhythm assumes a


central syntactical role in organizing musical events into coherent and comprehensible
patterns and forms. Thus, the structure of rhythm communicates a great deal of the actual,
comprehensive “musical meaning” of a musical composition. At the other end of the
musical communication process, rhythm also modulates the attention of the listener in
relationship to the perception of musical events. Rhythm guides the ear and brain to make
sense of acoustical patterns and shapes by directing focus to important moments in the

83
Ibid., 66.
84
Thaut, 4-6.

24
unfolding of the music.85

From the point of view of entrainment or attention-attending theory, when the rhythmic

structure establishes a sense of periodicity (cyclical or metric), psychological behaviors such as

anticipation and entrainment are created for future structural events. When a musical element

violates the structural expectation, tension takes place and a search for tension release or a

resolution follows. The affective experience of music therefore happens during the process of

alternations between “expectation and temporary suspense, tension and release, arousal and de-

arousal, arsis and thesis.”86

Bob Snyder talks about the “rhythmic tension.” Rhythmic tension still happens within a

rhythmic grouping by “the number of events occurring in a given amount of time” even when a

metric frame or pulse is not established. When the amount of events happening in shorter time

spans is larger, the generated rhythmic tension is perceived as higher. 87

The rhythmic structure shows temporal organizations of musical events and these are

revealed in chronological order. The more discernible the events of rhythmic structure, the easier

the rhythmic structure can be understood. The intelligibility of rhythmic structure becomes

essential for the aesthetics of music. The perception of rhythmic structure is subject to aural

experience. The cognitive model from neuroscience and the information processing theory from

psychology introduced earlier in this dissertation also provide the cognitive process for the

comprehension of rhythmic structure.

Issues of Temporal Structures in Contemporary Music

A lot of contemporary music is considered incomprehensible. Composers in the twentieth

85
Ibid., 6.
86
Ibid., 6.
87
Snyder, 161.

25
century tried to move away from the classical musical language in tonality, harmony, forms, and

structures. They explored new possibilities of organizing music which created issues related to

the aesthetic of music. While contemporary composers explored and expanded the horizon of

music, a problem arose: because contemporary music is complex and difficult, many structural

and formal elements cannot be heard and understood. The comprehension of temporal structure

is therefore challenging for listeners and creates challenges to the aesthetic experience of

contemporary music.

According to aforementioned studies, the sense of entrainment established by rhythmic

structure is key to the aesthetic experience. Rhythmic structure in a musical work that renders a

strong sense of entrainment is usually pulse-based and metrical. It also creates the sense of

motion and continuity. From the point of view of the information processing theory, when the

perceived musical elements are organized into a hierarchical structure, the chunking process

makes the performance more approachable; other musical elements and structure is then better

comprehended. Music with an established pulse and a metrical temporal structure is therefore

easier to understand and remember. This is important to the aesthetic experience.

The temporal characteristic in music described above is often referred to as “in time” and

a sense of motion is experienced. In order to explore new experiences of musical time in

contemporary music, composers construct such time with innovative methods. They use pre-

compositional schemes to build the piece’s form such as the block of sounds of different texture,

mathematical formulas and ratios like the golden mean, the twelve-tone technique in pitch or

rhythmic element and so on. The music constructed under those methods is complex in structures

and creates a temporal experience of “out-of-time.” The sense of motion is missing or weak due

to the lack of entrainment. The performance of grouping and structural building is hard for

26
human brains’ capacity due to a lack of clear cues from the inaudible or unclear auditory

information.

Much contemporary percussion music is identified as modern in style and conceptual in

nature. Because modern compositions often are not easily comprehended by aural experience,

applying theoretical methods such as score studying, analysis or commentaries of program notes

will help listeners and even performers understand and appreciate modern works.

In the following chapters, temporal analysis on Thirteen Drums and Rebond A are

examined through their rhythmic structure. Aesthetic experience is discussed through the aural

experience of rhythmic structure. The theory introduced in this chapter provides a basis for

comprehending the rhythmic structure and perception of musical time.

27
CHAPTER IV

THE CHARACTERISTICS OF RHYTHMIC STRUCTURE TO THE AESTHETIC

EXPERIENCE IN THIRTEEN DRUMS AND REBOND A

There are four important aspects of rhythmic structure in both pieces: instrumentation,

compositional scheme/formal structure, absence of notated meter, and rhythmic complexity.

These aspects affect the aesthetic experience of musical time. In Chapter III, scholarship in the

neuroscience of music and cognitive psychology studies on information processing theory of

memory were provided to explain the discrepancies between the written music in the score and

the actual perceived temporal information.

The purpose of the next two chapters is to discuss how certain features of rhythmic

structure affect the perception and cognition of musical time, which is the first and most

important factor in the aesthetic experience. However, these chapters will not identify the

emotions evoked by features of the rhythmic structure because emotional responses are more

complicated; they can be very different according to the individual’s personal response and

because more complex physical reactions are involved.

As discussed in Chapter III rhythmic tension is perceived through rhythmic grouping by

the amount of events taking place within a given time period. The aesthetic experience from

rhythmic tension occurs whether a pulse or a metric sense is established or not. Therefore, even

if the rhythmic structure is not always intelligible in Thirteen Drums or Rebond A, rhythmic

tension is still experienced. The experience of rhythmic tension is discussed when more details

on rhythmic structure are analyzed in the following chapters.

28
Instrumentation

In both Thirteen Drums and Rebond A, only drums are used. In Thirteen Drums, Ishii

called for 13 drums that had calf skin, such as bongos and Japanese drums.88 In Rebond A,

Xenakis asked for 2 bongos, 3 tom-toms, and 2 bass drums (example 4-1).89 These drums are

classified as “indefinite pitch membranophones.”90

Example 4-1. Instrumentation in Thirteen Drums and Rebond A91

a. Instrumentation of Thirteen Drums by Maki Ishii

b. Instrumentation of Rebond A by Iannis Xenakis

The notation system of Western music, including note values, bar lines, and the staff, has

been commonly adopted in works for percussion. It becomes a convenient tool for learning and

reading music. Thirteen Drums uses a staff of thirteen lines (example 3-1a) and Rebond A adopts

a five-line staff of Western music (example 3-1b). Like the notation system of Western music,

the lines of the staff indicate “high to low” indefinite-pitch as they descend.
88
But in performances where 13 drums with calf skin are not available, 13 tom-toms can be used.
89
In Rebond B, it needed 2 bongos, 1 tumba, 1 tom-tom, 1 bass drums and a set of 5 wood blocks.
90
Gary Cook, Teaching Percussion, 2nd ed. (New York: Schirmer Books, 1997), 3.
91
This and all musical examples in this dissertation are for analytical purposes only and are not intended
for reproduction or performance.

29
The homogeneous instrumentation in both works conveys a clear rhythmic structure and

reduces possible issues caused by the contrasting qualities of sounds such as the balance and

blend between the instruments of metal, wood, and skin-heads. The two composers most likely

used drums to allow greater focus on the fundamental character of percussion: attacks and

rhythms. Using multiple drums also creates a melodic quality in a non-pitched drumming work.

Percussive Characteristics

Unlike woodwinds, brass, or strings, the sound generated by striking percussion

instruments primarily creates sharp attacks of sound quality92; if a sustained or “long-note” effect

is desired, special techniques such as a “roll”93 technique must be applied. To relate this

characteristic of percussion instruments to time, the attacks are the events happened in time

points.94 The notated durational values of rhythm therefore can be ignored and the score becomes

a reference timing plate. Xenakis’ Psappha for multiple-percussion solo is a good example:

Example 4-2. An excerpt from Psappha by Iannis Xenakis

92
Harvey White and Donald White, Physics and Music: The Science of Musical Sound (Philadelphia:
Saunders College, 1980), 270. “A percussive note, on the other hand, has a sharp attack, followed immediately by a
decay of variable duration.”
93
A roll is a continuous succession of single strokes performed very close together.
94
Hasty, 69. Hasty claimed that “if there is sound, it has always had duration.” However, for many
percussion instruments, the notated note values, if no special technique is used, are often indeterminate and not stand
for “real” durational indication. The notation system is for “timing” reference.

30
Melodic Quality

Even though the drums are of indefinite pitch, the tuning is done according to a “relative

scale.”95 The relative-pitch contents in both pieces imply melodic qualities. The implied melodic

qualities sometimes form “motives” which are more easily recognized than rhythmic (non-

pitched) motives. The recurring motivic materials can serve as a “grouping principle” for

chunking information and are important for periodic divisions such as motive, phrase and section.

The excerpts from Rebond A is a good example (example 4-3). In Rebond A, membranophones

with different heads create distinct timbres. Therefore, bongos, tom-toms, and bass drums render

different sound qualities that will enhance some motivic materials.

Example 4-3. Examples of registers in Rebond A by Iannis Xenakis


a. High register
3a: high register, mm. 15-18.

b. Low register

95
Sugawara (personal communication) points out that the 13 drums used in Thirteen Drums resemble a
complete octave. In Rebond A, seven drums are in relative non-definite pitches.

31
Compositional Scheme/Formal Plan

Since both Thirteen Drums and Rebond A lack tonal elements, the study of a pre-

composed structural plan helps identify phrases and sections especially in the case of Thirteen

Drums.

Thirteen Drums

In Thirteen Drums, the development of the music is based on a rhythmic series: twelve

rhythmic figures of four-consecutive-sixteenth notes with different accent patterns (referred to as

“accent patterns of the rhythmic scheme”) followed by a grace-note group (“the thirteenth

figure”, example 4-4):

Example 4-4. Rhythmic scheme in Thirteen Drums

When represented in real content, the accented notes and non-accented notes are distinct

because of the dynamic contrast. This rhythmic scheme is modified into various textures by

different techniques to create a diverse experience of musical time. It can be broken into

segments consisting of two or three accent patterns without changing the order. Segments are

flexibly used to expand the periodicity of phrases that becomes the structural feature of form.

The new formations of the rhythmic scheme are further discussed later in this current chapter and

the following one.

Ishii composed the rhythmic scheme for two different effects of musical time: the

determinate rhythms and orders made of the rhythmic scheme’s accent patterns, and the

32
indeterminate “interruption” of the thirteenth figure. He later gradually added grace notes to the

existing accented sixteenth notes (example 4-5). At the same time, he increased the tempo

making it impossible for the performer to keep the timing of sixteenth notes due to the large

setup of thirteen drums. Therefore, the time of an actual performance is distorted which is the

effect that Ishii desired.96

Example 4-5. The temporal distortion, starting in page 3, line 2 in Thirteen Drums

Rebond A

Xenakis did not clarify the pre-composed structure of Rebond A. Greg Beyer claims that

96
This is described on the last page of the score of Thirteen Drums.

33
Xenakis used the Golden Section (Golden Mean) as a formal scheme.97 Xenakis used the Golden

Section (which he called “the Modular”) as a structural frame in other compositions, for example,

Metastasis (1953-1954).98 Xenakis claimed that you can “identify” the proportion of a piece in

the Golden Section through rhythmic exercises.99 However, for most people, it is hard to “hear”

the arrival of the division in the golden ratio. In fact, there is no clear discrimination of phrases

or sections in Rebond A besides the silence caused by longer notated values of notes and rests.

The composition is based on changes of density between the two sixteenth notes in the very

beginning (example 4-1b). Schick roughly divides this piece into six sections according to the

changes of density (“points of articulation” in Schick’s words).100 These changes of density are

achieved through different elements, register of drums, polyrhythms, and various subdivisions of

duration. These elements also affect the perception of rhythmic groupings and the perception of

duration. The continuous mutation of rhythmic figures oscillating between the drums in

polyrhythmic texture obscures the groupings and boundaries for chunking. The formal structure

is therefore more difficult to perceive and understand.

Absence of Notated Meter

These works lack notated time signature (see example 4-1) and both have a weak sense of

meter. The sense of meter (with or without the indication of time signature) is important in

understanding the musical time of a piece because it helps the establishment of the sense of

entrainment (discussed in Chapter III). Because of the instrumentation and the lack of a clear

97
Greg Beyer, “All is Number: Golden Section in Xenakis’ ‘Rebonds’,” Percussive Notes (February 2005):
40-50.
98
Harley,
99
Bálint András Varga, Conversations with Iannis Xenakis (London: Faber and Faber Limited, 1996), 30.
100
Steve Schick, The Percussionist’s Art: Same Bed, Different Dreams (Rochester, NY: University of
Rochester Press, 2006), 211-212.

34
metric sense, the notated rhythmic figures and measures are open for interpretation including

durational determinations such as motives, phrases, and sections.

In Thirteen Drums, there are no divisions of measure: only dashed lines (single or double)

used for phrases and pauses or solid doubled lines for the indication of large sections (only two

places). The repeated sixteenth notes may give a sense of pulse that normally is felt as the length

of two or four sixteenth notes (a value of eighth note or quarter note). Groups of grace notes

interrupt this sense of pulse. Although these groups cause the distortion of time, some of them

also serve as “closures” or cues of chunking boundaries at the ending of a phrase.

In the score of Rebond A, the music is organized/notated in 4/4. Nevertheless, the music

is not felt in 4/4 time. Although the quarter note equals 40 throughout the work, the rhythmic

grouping is not easily perceived and does not help listeners develop a steady pulse and a sense of

meter. The constant changing rhythms do not build the sense of entrainment.

Rhythmic Complexity

This section focuses on the surface level of rhythmic structure: rhythmic grouping.

Studies show that the higher the complexity of rhythms becomes, the more ambiguous rhythmic

groupings can be perceived.101 In addition to the previously discussed factors of instrumentation

and compositional plan, other elements also increase the rhythmic complexity in Thirteen Drums

and Rebond A: use of rests, pervasive accents, and subdivisions of beat/pulse.

Use of Rests

Ishii wrote many rests in Thirteen Drums, and the impact of the silence created by the

rests is significant. Thirteen drums is constructed of a rhythmic scheme and the development of
101
Parncutt, 454.

35
the piece is primarily variations of the rhythmic scheme. Rests become the tool for Ishii in some

of the variations (example 4-6):

Example 4-6. Variation of the original rhythmic scheme, starting in page 4, the end of
second line

In the excerpt above, if each grace-note group at the beginning represents a sixteenth note

and is combined with sixteenth rests, each grace-note group is in the accented sixteenth note’s

timing of each rhythmic figure of four-consecutive-sixteenth notes. The same approach can be

applied to the figure of a single sixteenth-note with an ornament and the variations with rolls in

which the rests are substituted by rolls. It is clear that the musical excerpt in example 4-6 is a

variation from the original rhythmic scheme’s patterns (see example 4-4), in the score, but the

aural experience of accent patterns is not as salient.

Moreover, the duration of silence is indicated by successive sixteenth rests instead of

rests with other values. The intention is clear: Ishii wanted the performer to keep precise timing

of the sixteenth rest. But for the aural experience, listeners can hardly keep track of the timing of

these rests. The grace-note groups are heard as random events. For these reasons discussed, this

excerpt is difficult to recognize as a variation from its original form and the temporal experience

of duration is distorted.

36
In the score of Rebond A, rests appear in the very beginning (example 4-7a) and at the

very end (example 4-7b). Since the development of this piece is a continuous transformation of

rhythms, these rests create silence that serves as chunking cues for rhythmic grouping or a break

between phrases. In fact, rests are not the only ones that have this function. In the section of

instrumentation, the notated note values can be ignored. Note values longer than an eighth note

also give the impression of rests. Besides the excerpts in musical examples 4-7a and 4-7b, eighth

notes in m.20, m.21, m.22, and m.36 also have the same effect.

Example 4-7a. Rebonds A, mm. 1-6

In example 4-7a, even though the tempo is marked at a quarter note as 40 bpm (beats per

minute), the pulse is likely felt as durations of eighth notes or sixteenth notes. In human

perception, the periodicities in the 80-120 bpm (500-700 ms102) are judged “neither too long nor

too short.”103 So the pulse in this range is easily felt and established as the pulse for entrainment.

102
ms= millisecond.
103
London, Hearing in Time, 31.

37
Therefore in the beginning three rhythmic figures, the pulse is the duration of the eighth note and

the rhythmic grouping is identified as the repeated rhythmic figure of two sixteenth-notes

followed by an eighth rest.

However, the pulse of eighth notes is not consistently perceived. Starting in the last

beat104 in m.1, the pulse as eighth notes is interrupted. The rhythmic grouping is a repeated figure

of three sixteenth notes followed by an eighth note rest until the third beat in m. 5. The pulse is

now changed to be felt as a sixteenth note or as odd meter as 5/16 (example 4-7a).

Example 4-7b. Rebonds A, mm. 51-60

In example 4-7b, silence between each quarter note resembles the silence of rests. The

polyrhythms and irregular rhythms reduce the sense of pulse. Like the situation in Thirteen

Drums, listeners can hardly keep track of the timing of these rests. The musical time here seems

out-of-time for the aural experience.

104
I use “beat” here to indicate where the rhythmic figure is in score. The “beat” is not involved in any
metric frame.

38
Pervasive Accents

Pervasive use of accents is prominent in both Thirteen Drums and Rebond A and easily

perceived during performance (please refer to the musical examples in the following chapter).

According to the information of accents discussed in Chapter III, metric accents are hard to

perceive in both pieces because neither have a strong sense of entrainment and lack pitch

elements and metric framework.

In Rebond A, structural accents are not obvious and are unrecognizable most of the time.

This is due to the consistent evolution of rhythms and lack of clear chunking boundaries.

Structural accents are represented in Thirteen Drums at the use of grace-note groups (see

example 4-5) and sustained notes (accomplished by the performance technique of the roll) for

phrase discrimination (example 4-8). The dynamic changes (crescendo and decrescendo) also

contribute to the perception of structural accents. Detailed analysis is provided in the next

chapter.
Example 4-8. Thirteen Drums, page 7

39
Phenomenal accents are pervasive in both pieces. Some phenomenal accents coincide

with the structural accents. In Thirteen Drums, the phenomenal accents are marked by sharp

changes of dynamic level as in the symbol >, which is sometimes paired with other dynamic

markings such as sf, sff, sfff, etc… The accents of the first twelve rhythmic figures of the

rhythmic scheme’s four-consecutive-sixteenth notes are phenomenal accents.

There are two accent symbols used in Rebond A: ˅ (single accent) and (double accents).

Xenakis did not include an explanation of how to interpret both accents in the score of Rebonds.

However, in the score of Psappha, Xenakis indicated the interpretation of both accent types as

follows:105

1. Louder, greater intensity

2. Abrupt change in timbre

3. Abrupt change of weight

4. Addition of another sound played simultaneously

5. Simultaneous combination of all preceding suggestions

These accents increase the complexity of the non-repetitive rhythmic figures and obscure

the identification of phrases. According to Xenakis’s interpretations of the accents, these accents

have different functions depending on their placements in the musical context. When the

functions of accents are evaluated, the execution of the accents is open to the performer. This

breaks the sense of continuity in the mutating rhythmic figures and brings out the periodicities of

motives or phrases which serve as the chunks for listeners to process the music. Some examples

are listed here (examples 4-9a, 9b, 9c) and a detailed analysis is provided in the next chapter.

105
Iannis Xenakis, Psappha (Paris: Editions Salabert, 1975). The original content is in French. This
translation is made by Brian Christopher Tinkel in his dissertation, " 'Rebonds' by Iannis Xenakis: Pedagogical
Study and Performance Analysis," D.M.A. diss. (University of Oklahoma, 2009), 28.

40
Example 4-9a. Rebond A, mm.1-3

In example 4-9a, the first three single accents on the beginning sixteenth-note figures

represent the core of the piece. The accents’ function can be interpreted as having greater

intensity or greater weight to grasp the listeners’ attention and to balance the sound qualities

between the small bongo and large bass drum. The fourth accent on the large bass drum is to

introduce the “new timbre” of different drums following the large bass drum.

Example 4-9b. Rebond A, mm. 34-38

Example 4-9c. Rebond A, mm. 48-50

41
In example 4-9b, accents are added on the sixteenth notes that conclude the rhythmic

sequences of various subdivisions of pulse. These spots break the continuous mutating rhythmic

patterns and periodicities of phrase then emerge. In example 4-9c, the frequent accents enhance

the intensity of the musical context that is created by the high density of rhythms.

Subdivisions of Beat/Pulse

This section deals with another important factor that increases the rhythmic complexity in

Rebond A: subdivisions of beat/pulse. Note that Rebond A’s main effect and compositional

design is in changes of density. The various durations caused by subdivisions of beat/pulse

mostly contribute to this effect. If the beat/pulse is based on the rate of a sixteenth note, the

subdivisions of beat/pulse are in the ratios of 1:2, 1:3, 1:4, 1:5, 2:3, and 2:5 in the monophonic

musical context (example 4-10a). In the context of polyrhythms, the density increases especially

when the sixteenth note is divided into a thirteen-second note or a sixty-fourth note in the ratios

of 2:3, 4:3, and 6:5 (example 4-10b).

Example 4-10a. Ratios of subdivisions in Rebond A

Example 4-10b. Ratios of polyrhythms in Rebond A

The continuous mutation of rhythmic figures obscures the groupings and boundaries for

chunking. Because of irregular rhythms and the lack of reference to a metric framework, the

42
information in the formal structure is therefore more difficult to perceive and understand.

However, the subdivisions of duration dominate the density and intensity that they create

rhythmic tension in the aural experience of the piece (Chapter III). The tension building and

release in Rebond A depends on various subdivisions of a pulse.

43
CHAPTER V

ANALYSIS OF TEMPORALITY IN THE RHYTHMIC STRUCTURE OF

THIRTEEN DRUMS

In this chapter and the next, a temporal analysis of Thirteen Drums and Rebond A will be

presented respectively. The primary focus of each respective chapter examines the effects of

perception and cognition of rhythmic structure and the resulting aesthetic experience. According

to the characteristics of rhythmic structure discussed in the previous chapter, detailed discussion

on musical time is offered. The analysis reveals a framework for a formal structure. Each work is

divided into parts according to different levels of tension created by texture and intensity, and in

the case of Rebond A, density. Each part conveys musical time and rhythmic tension in a

different way.

Rhythmic Scheme and Formal Structure

There are a total of thirteen pages (37 lines) in the score of Thirteen Drums. It was said

earlier that no discrimination of measures are used for organization, so every line is numbered

for reference (like measure numbers). When a specific passage is referred to, the fraction,

number of line and page number will be used to indicate the location in the score106: [2/4 L11 P4],

for example, means the spot approximately on two fourths of line 11 in page four. Thirteen

Drums is divided into 8 parts (Table 5-1).

The rhythmic scheme (example 5-1) introduced in the previous chapter is presented in

different ways, just like theme and variations. Since the metric framework is not established in

106
The referenced score in this dissertation is the version published by Moeck Verlag und
Musikinstrumentenwerk in 1986. According to the rhythmic scheme and musical content, there are several errors in
the score published by Moeck Verlag und Musikinstrumentenwerk in 1986. A list of errata is provided in Appendix.

44
this piece, it is the rhythmic scheme that sets up the formal structure and ties all the parts

together into a large-scale composition. The rhythmic structure of each variation therefore builds

the tension and decides the perception and cognition of musical time in each section. The

aesthetic experience is subject to the different characteristic temporalities created by the

rhythmic structure in each section.

Table 5-1. Formal structure of Thirteen Drums

Part# Location in the score Correspondent sectional marking


1 Beginning ~ [3/4 L11 P4] A
2 [4/4 L11 P4] ~ [4/4 L13 P5] B
3 [1/4 L14 P5] ~ [1/4 L15 P5] Transition I
4 [1/4 L15 P5] ~ [1/4 L17 P6] C
5 [2/4 L17 P6] ~ [3/4 L26 P9] A'
6 [4/4 L26 P9] ~ [2/4 L29 P10] B'
7 [3/4 L29 P10] ~ [1/4 L30 P10] Transition II
8 [2/4 L30 P10] ~ Ending C'

Example 5-1. The rhythmic scheme of Thirteen Drums

According to the musical example, the rhythmic scheme is composed of twelve rhythmic

figures of four-consecutive-sixteenth notes with different accent patterns (only four possibilities);

the seventh to twelfth figures are the retrograde of the first to sixth figures. The twelve sets of

four consecutive sixteenth notes end with the grace note group as the “thirteenth figure.” This

“thirteenth figure” in Ishii’s design concept is an “indeterminate” event. By indeterminate, this

means the figure reveals its openness in the musical content and its contrast to the figures of

determined temporal values.

45
The rhythmic scheme is enriched by phenomenal accents and the “melodic quality” of the

instrumentation: The non-accented sixteenth notes are kept on one drum in a lower level of

dynamics when the accented notes are played on different drums with abrupt emphasis in

dynamics.

Before providing a detailed analysis of each section, it is helpful to understand how

Thirteen Drums is divided. The letters used for sectional marking distinguish between different

“core elements” in the sections. The core elements are basically features that change the

formation of the rhythmic scheme. The core elements are discussed later in this chapter.

The main feature in Sections A and A' is characterized by clearly notated sixteenth notes

denoting the rhythmic scheme. In Sections B and B', only accented sixteenth notes are preserved;

other sixteenth notes are substituted by rests and some are combined into notes of longer values

with rolls. Transitions are located between Sections B and C (or B' and C'). The elements of

rhythmic scheme are either absent or ambiguous.

In Sections C and C', the design has moved away from the rhythmic scheme of earlier

sections. Like the alternation done in Section B, only the notes with rolls substituting the rests

are extended into longer values in Section C. The total duration of the rhythmic scheme is then

prolonged. In Section C', fragments from accent patterns of the rhythmic scheme are formulated

in special arrangements to create new musical context and conclude the piece.

Analysis of Rhythmic Structure of Sections

Section A

Sections A and A' feature clearly-notated sixteenth notes. Section A into three

46
subsections based on changes in melodic quality and rhythmic structure: A1, A2, and A3107

(example 5-2).The phrase discrimination is based on the basic formation of the rhythmic scheme:

rhythmic figures of four-consecutive-sixteenth notes followed by a group of grace notes. The

group of grace notes serves as a cue for boundaries of chunking and break up the long passage

into several phrases. There are six phrases in A1 (A1-1 to A1-3), four phrases in A2 (A2-1 to

A2-4), and seven phrases in A3 (A3-1to A3-7).

The music of Thirteen Drums starts with twelve rhythmic figures of four-consecutive-

sixteenth notes without accents (referred to as ‘sixteenth-note group’) followed by a group of

five grace-notes (A1). The continuous sixteenth notes repeat on the same drum starting in

fortissimo followed by a decrescendo, and are interrupted by the loud grace-note group (marked

as sff). The same design happens five times on different drums. Between some of the repeated

passages, each accent pattern of the rhythmic scheme from material A2 is gradually revealed

when the music unfolds in time. The group of grace-notes in passages of A1 and A2 is the same

figure.

The fragments of accent patterns from the rhythmic scheme characterize the material in

A2. The accented notes are assigned on drums in the order of 5-3-11-1-9-7-5-7-9-1-11-3 in

which, after the seventh drum (drum 5), the order of drums is in retrograde. Ishii inserted a

variety of sixteenth-note groups between accent patterns. All twelve accent patterns are

introduced completely at [L7 P3] (A2-4). During the time when Phrase A2-4 is introduced, all

the drums except the bass drum have been assigned for the non-accented notes.

107
There is a mistake in the score: at the second four-sixteenth note figure, the accent should be on the third
sixteenth note instead of the fourth one.

47
Example 5-2. Section A from [L1 P1] to [4/4 L11 P4], Thirteen Drums by Maki Ishii

48
An explicit appearance of the rhythmic scheme is first introduced at [1/4 L8 P3] in

Section A (example 5-2, A3-1) and the passage of A3 starts here. There is no insertion of any

sixteenth-note group between each accent pattern. The non-accented notes are kept on drum 5;

the accented notes are played on drums 7-4-2-10-9-6-7-4-2-10-9-6, but are gradually changed

every time when the rhythmic scheme is reintroduced. The groups of grace notes are different

from those in passages of A1 and A2.

The rhythmic scheme appears to evolve with the shifting of musical materials from A1 to

A3. The process starts in its simplest form: sixteenth notes, dying away, played on a single drum

followed by an abrupt group of five grace-notes. Accent patterns of the rhythmic scheme are

gradually revealed. As the music unfolds, material from A1 disappears and the formation of the

rhythmic scheme reaches completion. When accent patterns are all represented, the dynamic

intensity also increases (via a crescendo) until the material of A3 is reached at [L8 P3].

In the representations of material of A3 from [L8 P3] to [3/4 L11 P4], Ishii gradually

added one to two grace-notes on the accents; at the same time, the tempo and intensity increases

(please refer to the musical example 4-5).The addition of grace-notes and the acceleration of

speed interrupt the timing of sixteenth note. The rhythmic figures of accent patterns are therefore

enriched and distorted.

Based on the features of the rhythmic structure discussed above, different materials of A1,

A2, and A3 affect the perception of musical time and create different levels of tension as to

aesthetic tension.

At the beginning, the material of A1 sets up the pulse perception as the duration of an

eighth note or quarter note (two-or four- sixteenth notes) according to the possible adopted

tempo. The most commonly chosen tempo is from 80 to 100 bpm of a quarter note, which is in

49
the temporal range for better pulse perception. The sense of pulse is interrupted by the

occurrence of the group of grace notes.

Possible metric frameworks are identified in section A2. The insertions of partial

segments at [3/4 L2 P1], [4/4 L3 P1] and [1/4 L5 P2] can be perceived by the meter as 4/4 if the

quarter note gets the beat. Metric cognitions of 3/4 and 2/4 happen in later passage of A2

(example 5-2). The motion of the music is discernible.

Characteristics of the passage in A3 have been discussed in Chapter III. The pulse is

carried on in this passage, but the sense of meter is ambiguous. As the music goes on, the

addition of grace-notes and acceleration of speed cause temporal distortion, because the pulse

cannot be preserved under that condition (please refer to example 3-5). At the end of Section A,

the sense of pulse and motion is destroyed.

The musical intensity and tension increases along with the process discussed. The

evolution of the rhythmic scheme starts in a plain form, with gradually-revealed accent patterns

added to its explicit presentation. Then the rhythmic scheme evolves until its formation is not

recognizable. At the end of Section A, the level of musical tension reaches its first climax of

Thirteen Drums.

Section B

Section B contains within it two subsections (B1 and B2). Similar to the periodic

formation of phrases in Section A, three phrases are recognized: B1, B2-1 and B2-2 (example 5-

3). The formation of phrase B1 has been analyzed in Chapter III. The group of grace-notes, on

drums 1-3-6-9-12-13, is inherited from A3’s material. However, the function of this group is

different. In the A3 passage, this group serves as the closure of phrases for boundaries of

50
chunking. Here in first half of Phrase B1, it substitutes the spot of accented sixteenth note in each

accent pattern. This group later concludes Phrases B1 and B2-1.

Example 5-3. Section B from [4/4 L11 P4] to [4/4 L13 P5], Thirteen Drums

In the second half of Phrase B1, the group of grace-notes is replaced by flams.108 There

are six flams played on different drums: 51-113-76-49-1012-2-813 (smaller numbers on the top left

are grace notes). All thirteen drums are used. This formation of flams is repeated in the first half

of Phrase B2-1, so the drums used on the figures of flams in Phrase B2-1 are 51-113-76-49-1012-
2-8
13-1112-108-79-104-612-211. This formation is again used in Phrase B2-2, but the drums on the

grace-note and primary note in each figure of the flam are switched.

It was mentioned earlier that non-accented sixteenth notes are substituted with rests in

Phrase B1, and in Phrases B2-1 and B2-2, they are turned into rolls of different durations. In
108
A flam is a percussion technique by which a single grace note precedes the primary note (stroke).

51
these passages, a new formation of the rhythmic scheme is introduced: if the number of sixteenth

rest between two consecutive accented notes is four or above, the durations of sixteenth rests are

combined into longer temporal intervals which are executed by rolls on drums (example 5-4).

Example 5-4. Comparison of Phrase B1 and the original rhythmic scheme, from [3/4 L12 P4]
to [1/4 L13 P5], Thirteen Drums

In Chapter IV, the issues of the rhythmic structure in Section B were discussed. It is clear

that phrases in Section B are variations of the rhythmic scheme at the opening of the work. For

performers, it is possible to keep precise timing of the sixteenth notes while playing rolls.

Listeners, on the other hand, are likely unable to keep track of such timing. The grace-note

groups are heard as random events, and so are the accented figures of flams.

There are several reasons for causing this discrepancy. The musical passages of A3

before Section B end with temporal distortion in which the sense of pulse is destroyed. Therefore,

the music in Section B starts without a sense of pulse. In Section B, successive rests and rolls on

the longer note values do not provide a solid temporal salience for pulse perception. At Phrase

B2-1, the speed starts to slow down (rit.). Because of these reasons, this excerpt is difficult to

recognize as a variation from its original form and the temporal experience of duration of this

excerpt is distorted.

52
Ishii, based on the same rhythmic scheme, creates different musical textures by altering

the articulations. In comparing the aural perception of sixteenth notes, sixteenth rests, and rolls,

consecutive sixteenth notes are the “articulated time points,” and in the preferred range of tempo,

they serve as the “motor” driving music to different directions. Rests and rolls here cause

temporal indeterminacy due to the absence of a sense of pulse. Therefore, in the passages of

Section B, the driving force is taken away, and the music loses its momentum.

The musical tension achieves a climax in the block of massive sound that occurs at the

end of Section A. In Section B, the intense energy is not lost completely. However, the level of

tension drops a little if compared to the end of Section A because of the rhythmic structure and

tempo slowing down. The texture starting in the second half of B1 creates the sound color

reminiscent of Klangfarebenmelodie of the Second Viennese School. Ishii was trained in serial

methods so it is not surprising that he adopted the technique in Thirteen Drums.

Another important element Ishii introduced in Section B is rolls. It was said that the

primary characteristic of percussion instruments is attack. The technique of rolls on most

percussion instruments is to sustain a sound. The contrast of sound is then created (imagine a

string player performing a pizzicato and arco on his/her instrument). The sound quality produced

by rolls becomes the main feature in the transitions and other later sections.

Transition I

Transition I (example 5-5) starts at [L14 P5]. There are four phrases in Transition I (TI-1

to TI-4). Section B concludes with the crescendo of rolls on a dotted-half note followed by a

group of grace notes in dynamic of sfff . Phrase TI-1 is the two figures reversed and the rolls are

in decrescendo. A fermata is inserted between the end of Section B and Transition I.

53
Example 5-5. Transition I from [1/4 L14 P5] to [1/1 L15 P5], Thirteen Drums

There are several features in Transition I: use of rolls, use of fingers, use of small and

medium sizes of drums, and fermata. Ishii uses three kinds of fermata in Thirteen Drums:

(short fermata), (medium fermata), and (long fermata).109 A fermata not only provides the

pause of silence (notated above with dashed barlines) but also serves as a cue for the boundary of

the phrase. Fermatas of are used to separate Phrases TI-1, TI-2 and TI-3.

109
Maki Ishii, Zone for Piano-Percussion (Japan: Ongaku No Tomo Sha Corp., 1968). Ishii did not include
an explanation of them in Thirteen Drums but in the score of Zone for Piano-Percussion.

54
The use of rolls is more legato compared to the articulated attacks of the sixteenth note in

previous sections. In Phrase TI-2, rolls are applied on mid-register drums: 7-6-8-5-7-6. High-

register drums are used in Phrase TI-3: 3-2-4-1-3-2. Phrase TI-4 are repeated half-note rolls on

drum 5. The starting points of the rolls (in general, it is called the attack of the roll) are

accentuated. This is the first appearance of this sonority.

At the end of Section B, the tempo has a marked quarter note of 60bpm. At this tempo, a

performer will have a smooth movement between drums to avoid the accented attack of the roll,

especially in soft dynamics. The dashed slurs confirm the “legato” effect wanted by the

composer.

The dynamics range from pp to sf and ff. However, in the passages of ff, smaller drums

are chosen and are played with fingers. Using fingers on these drums produces softer attacks and

a mellower sonority of rolls compared to using sticks or mallets. Moreover, the chosen drums are

smaller so the volumes in passages of ff actually sound like they are in the dynamics of mf or f.

Consequently, the whole Transition I has a much less intense dynamic.

Although the pulse of the quarter note may be perceived and kept in Phrases TI-2,

TI-3, and TI-4, it feels as if it is lacking forward motion in Transition I. The main reason is

because the shortest notated duration of the rhythmic figure is a quarter note with a rate of about

60 bmp. This is considered slower in the tempo envelope as the sense of entrainment is very

weak. The musical time seems suspended.

From the features discussed above, the music in Transition I is perceived as less intense,

less active, and legato. Therefore, when compared to previous sections, the experience of this

whole section is smooth and tranquil.

55
Section C

The music of Section C starts at [L15 P5]. This section includes two variations of the

rhythmic scheme: C1 and C2 (example 5-5). The structural scheme is similar to the alterations

that occurred in the passages of B2. The use of rolls happens at the temporal intervals of six

consecutive sixteenth notes between accented figures of flams, in which intervals occur during

the accent patterns of ③&④, ⑥&⑦, and ⑨&⑩. Originally, the duration of six sixteenth notes

combined is a dotted quarter-note. Ishii doubles the durations so a dotted half-note is represented.

The thirteenth figure of Phrase C1 is a larger group of nine grace-notes.

The feature of the rolls’ extension is carried through Phrase C2 but in a more complicated

manner. In the musical example 5-5, it shows the comparison between Phrase C2 to the original

rhythmic scheme. Combined durations of sixteenth notes happen during the accent patterns of ①

&②, ③&④, ⑥&⑦, and ⑨&⑩ and like in Phrase C1, the durations are extended. However,

the presentation of each extended-duration is different: rolls on one or two or multiple drums.

Notations of large groups of grace-notes can be interpreted as rolls on multiple drums.

Another important feature of Phrase C2 is that accented sixteenth notes of the rhythmic

scheme are played on the bass drum (the thirteenth drum in the score). The remaining non-

accented notes of the rhythmic scheme that are not combined together are replaced by different

figures such as a sixteenth note, a sixteenth rest, a flame, single sixteenth-note ornamented with

two to four grace-notes, and rolls on sixteenth notes (please refer to example 5-5).

Variations of Phrases C1 and C2 create contrasting textures. Phrase C1 contains rests

and most of this passage is played with fingers until the last figure of flams and the group of

grace-notes. Most of the drums range from small to medium in size. In Phrase C1, every spot of

a sixteenth note is filled with a figure except in one place (marked with * in the score).The

56
accented notes in the original rhythmic scheme are placed on the only bass drum, which

produced a heavy and weighted quality of sound that makes it stand out in the passage. The

whole passage is played with mallets or sticks. The various segments of rolls in Phrase C2

produce much more volume than that produced by the rolls on the single drum. The dynamic in

Phrase C2 is raised by a level (sff to sfff).

Phrase C1 is the return of the rhythmic scheme. It breaks up the smooth atmosphere in

Transition I by reintroducing the articulation of staccato and the isolation between events. In

Phrase C2, activities filled up the silence in Phrase C. Along with the other features discussed

above, the impact is very powerful and intense. The musical tension is again pushed to a high

level.

Section A'

A pervasive feature of Section A, characterized by consecutive sixteenth notes, returns

after the forceful ten-second rolls at the end of Section C. The segments marked earlier as A1,

A2, and A3 appear again but in different order (please refer to examples 5-2 and 5-6). Section A'

is divided into 5 parts. Each section is marked by the shifts of texture and tension which will

soon be discussed. The shift of texture and tension in each part is achieved mainly through the

shapes of dynamics, the manipulation of tempo (accelerando and ritardando), and the thirteenth

figure of the original rhythmic scheme. The chart (Table 5-2) shows the formation of each part. It

indicates the segments corresponding to materials taken from Section A.

According to Table 5-2, the basic formation of each part is {(A1) + A2 + A3

+thirteenth figure} and each part is separated by the new “thirteenth figure.” The thirteenth

figure of the original rhythmic scheme is a group of grace-notes. In Section A', it is replaced by

57
rolls on drums lasting in various length of seconds. The new design of the thirteenth figure is

longer in length and serves as a cue of boundary for chunking like the old one.

Table 5-2. Structure of parts in Section A'

Part Formation New 13th Figure


A' 1 A1 + A2 + A2 + A3-2 ˅ (8")
A' 2 A2 + A3-4 ˅ (7")
A' 3 A2 + A3-5 ˅ (5")
A' 4 A2 + A3-6 ˅ (2")
A' 5 A3-7 + A3-7 (broken) --------

The clear representation of material of A1 is only in the first part. However, during the

end of the last roll of the thirteenth figure, Ishii gives an indication to slow down the speed of roll,

slowing down the rate of the hand-to-hand motion for performers. When the rate of hand-to-hand

motion slows down, the strokes happening in a temporal interval reduce. According to the range

of temporal perception which is discussed in Chapter III, the event that is above 100 ms is heard

as an individual sound. At this point, the rolls actually lose the quality of sustaining sound, and

the timing of sixteenth notes (by what’s notated in the score) is set. Toward the end of the

thirteenth figure, it resembles the material of A1 and the actual material of A1 is then replaced.

The variation of the rhythmic scheme of A2 introduces some new design. A new

performance technique of dead strokes, notated with a cross on a note head, is employed in the

passages of A2 on the timing of the accented sixteenth-note in the original rhythmic scheme. To

perform a dead stroke, a performer lets the stick stay on the drum head after striking to stop the

resonance from the drum head. This produces a sound that is shorter and dryer than that played

by a normal stroke. This particular technique creates a different timbre from the drum. Ishii only

uses this technique in softer dynamics in Sections A' and B'.

58
Example 5-6. Section A' from [2/4 L16 P6] to [3/4 L26 P9], Thirteen Drums

59
Another feature is in the melodic design. In the material of A2 of the first and second

parts, drums used on the timing of the accented sixteenth-note in the original rhythmic scheme

are 11-2-9-7-4-1-12-6-3-8-10-13. But in the third and fourth parts, those notes are played on

drums 2-11-4-6-9-12-1-7-10-5-3-13. The later melody is the “inversion” of the previous melody:

11 2 9 7 4 1 12 6 3 8 10 13

+ 2 11 4 6 9 12 1 7 10 5 3 13

13 13 13 13 13 13 13 13 13 13 13 ---

The design of dynamics and tempo in Section A' is also in a specific plan. Each part starts

slower and in a lower dynamic level. As the passage develops, the dynamics and the speed

gradually increase (indicated as poco a poco accel. e cresc in the score) until the material of A3

is presented. During the thirteenth figure after A3, the volume and speed both drop to a slower

and lower level.

The building up of dynamics and tempo does not only happen during each part but also

during the whole section of A'. Each part starts and ends in a faster tempo compared to the

previous part and dynamically, each part starts and ends stronger. When the tempo and dynamics

increase, the materials of A3 are more complicated (comparing A3-2 to A3-7) and the

periodicities of the thirteenth figure are reduced.

Toward the end of part 5, the material of A3-7 is repeated but is broken into segments

and separated by quarter rests. The segments consist of different amounts of accent patterns of

A3. It starts with three patterns and then reduces to one. The number of rests between the

segments goes from one, then increases to four, and finally reduces to two. The reducing

numbers of activities and separation of rests create a “liquidation” effect that makes a smooth

transition into the texture of Section B'.

60
The perception of the materials’ musical time has been discussed in Section A. Materials

of A1 and A2 establish a sense of pulse but the material of A3 destroys it because of the added

ornamentations (grace notes) and increasing tempo. According to the discussion above, in

Section A', the stretch of tempo also elevates the rhythmic complexity, which affects the

aesthetic experience of time.

The buildup of tension and intensity in Section A is a long process, with the climax

reached at the end. The overall effect in Section A' follows the diagram. Nonetheless, each part

in Section A' proceeds as an organism in which the tension and intensity on each part builds up

and then resolves itself.

Section B'

Following the liquidation of ornamented accent patterns, the music in Section B' unfolds

in the technique of variation from materials of B1. The tempo is marked as a quarter note that

equals about 66 bpm, a much slower tempo than that of the music in Section B. By doing so,

Ishii makes the transition between Sections A' and B' much smoother than the transition between

Sections A and B. Because the temporal rate of sixteenth notes for the material of A3-7, for

Ishii’s preference, is very fast during the liquidation of A3-7, accent patterns and quarter rests are

set up for the texture of Section B'.

The musical example 5-7 shows the formation of variation of the rhythmic scheme in

Section B'. Non-accented sixteenth notes are replaced by rests and a quarter rest is inserted

between each accent pattern (in the middle of [L28 P10], there are two quarter rests). There is no

thirteenth figure, but two drums played in the last accent pattern are 6 and 7 (13!). This

formation of the varied rhythmic scheme is repeated again from [4/4 L27 P9] to [3/4 L28 P10]. A

61
new cycle of the rhythmic scheme begins at [4/4 L28 P10], and is interrupted by the rolls

entering at [1/4 L29 L10]. This shift of texture is the beginning of Transition II.

Example 5-7. Section B' and Transition II, [4/4 L26 P9] to [2/4 L30 P10], Thirteen Drums

62
Dynamically, Section B' starts in dynamics sfff and then gradually gets soft as the music

unfolds. Dead strokes are used on some attacks in the repeat of the variation. The speed in this

section starts at quarter note as 66 bpm and slows down with the dynamic level dropping.

The effect in Section B' is similar to that in Section B. All the articulated sixteenth notes

in the original rhythmic scheme are replaced by rests with only the accented notes left. The

tracking on the timing of sixteenth rests is impossible for listeners, especially when the tempo is

already slow and then slows down more. Unlike Section B, there is no use of the roll to fill up

the silenced time span. Thus, in Section B', the silence between articulated notes is much longer

than in Section B. The energy level decreases significantly and no sense of motion is perceived.

The perception of time seems lost.

Transition II

The materials in Transition II are very different from what is in Transition I. Based on the

texture, Transition II consists of two parts. The first part starts with a roll that leads to accent

patterns. The accent patterns are the continued figures of the cycle of the rhythmic scheme that

started at the end of Section B': patterns, ⑤, ⑥, ⑦, ⑧, ⑨ (please refer to example 5-7). The

accent patterns are broken up by dotted half-notes with crescendo. The accented notes are played

on drums 1 and 2 together.

The second part begins in the middle of [L29 P10]. The materials are the remaining

accent patterns of the rhythmic scheme’s cycle but in mixed order: ⑦, ⑧, ⑨, ⑩, ⑪, ⑫. These

accent patterns are in new forms: the non-accented sixteenth notes, which were replaced by rests,

are now all changed to rolls (marked with in example 5-7). These new forms of accented

patterns are kept and become the feature in the following section.

63
The new forms of accent patterns are played on drums 1 and 2 (one drum is for the rolls

and the other is for the accented note). Ishii introduces them in loud dynamics

(ff, sff). After the accented patterns, the thirteenth figure comes in two figures of eight-note

triplets followed by a dotted half-note roll. Similar material is repeated but in different levels of

dynamics and speed. A prolonged roll marked by long fermata concludes this section.

From the discussed information, Transition II is another variation of the original rhythmic

scheme. In this variation, two techniques are used: the old technique from B and the new

technique that is dominant in Section C. Unlike the characteristics in Transition I, smooth, less

intense, and lacking movements, music in Transition II is more energetic, intense, and contains

various textures.

Section C'

According to the formation of the rhythmic scheme’s variations, Section C' can be

divided in three parts (C'1, C'2, and C'3, example 5-8). Each part can be separated into segments.

The cues of boundaries for chunking are groups of grace-notes. In parts of C'1 and C'2, each

represents a complete variation of the rhythmic scheme.

The new variation technique of accent patterns is discussed in the previous section. The

formation of each segment in C'1 is repeated in accent patterns (three times or two times) and

followed by a group of grace-notes. There are total 11 segments. Every three consecutive

segments can be grouped together as a phrase because of the special arrangement of dynamics

and group of grace-notes.

64
Example 5-8. Section C' from [2/4 L30 P10] to [L37 P13], Thirteen Drums

65
In the excerpt of example 5-8, the accent patterns are repeated: segments 1, 2 and 3 are

the same; 4, 5 and 6 are the same, so are 10 and 11. 110 Each segment contains three continuous

accent-patterns from the original rhythmic scheme. In comparing the accent patterns grouped in

each segment, a new formation then arises.

Accent patterns ②, ③, ④ have the same rhythmic patterns as patterns ⑤, ⑥, ⑦; accent

patterns, ⑥, ⑦, ⑧ are the same as patterns ⑨,⑩, ⑪. The relation of this formation to the

original rhythmic scheme is in the following example (example 5-9):

Example 5-9. Formation of the variation technique in Section C'

Segments 1, 2, 4, 5, 8 are the rolls and accented notes staying on one drum and followed

by a group of grace-notes on drums: 2-3-4-1-5-6. This combination of drums was already used

on the figures of eighth-note triplets in the second part of Transition II (example 5-7, at [4/4 L29

P10]). Accent patterns of segments of 3, 6, 7, and 9 are played on two drums (rolls on one drum

and accents on the other). The group of grace notes followed these segments are 1-2-1-3-2-3-4-3,

which was seen on the figures of eighth-note triplets at the end of Transition II (example 5-8, at

[1/4 L30 P10]).

In segments 10 and 11, more drums are involved in the accent patterns that are only

110
Circled numbers are correspondent to the accent patterns in the original rhythmic scheme.

66
played two times and the dynamics increase gradually. The groups of grace notes are in a new

combination. The second part of C'2 enters after the eleventh segment of C'1. The variation

technique of materials used in the part of C'2 is the same as C'1 with a few adjustments. There

are five segments in C'2. The musical example 5-8 shows the structure and formation

corresponding to the original rhythmic scheme.

The melodic shape created in C'2 is moving up-and-down in a step-wise motion. This

shape is represented in the small portions (C'2-1 for example) at the beginning of each segment

and then is used between portions (C'2-4 for example) and the whole segment (C'2-5). In the

segment of C'2-5, it starts in the second-lowest drum, then gradually moves up The pedaled bass

drum concludes the step-wise motion. A large group of grace-notes played on top-nine drums

follows in a random order and this group lasts for seven seconds. After that, another group of

grace-notes comes in sfff and with tremendous crescendo. This group is the last appearance of

grace notes and leads to the powerful final statement (C'3) in Thirteen Drums. In this final

statement, rolls and accented notes are played on drum 5 and quarter-notes are consistently

repeated on drum 13. The repeated figures on drum 5 are the first and second accent patterns of

the original rhythmic scheme.

In the new variation technique, rolls replacing the articulated sixteenth-notes or rests

become the new “background” of the accented notes in the original rhythmic scheme. Although

rolls are heard and are the opposite of rests (silence), they do not project a clear sense of pulse as

articulated sixteenth notes do. However, the attack of rolls does give a cue of down beats,

especially during drum changing. Thus a sense of pulse may be perceived in some passages such

as segments of C'2. Nevertheless, most of the time, the accents, and, sometimes, fast-moving

‘melody’ obscures the sense of pulse even during drum changing.

67
Based on the information in parts 1 and 2 of Section C', the rhythmic structure in the

score is not easily recognizable by aural experience. The perception of musical time is obscured

until the final statement. In the final statement, a strong pulse of a quarter note is repeated in the

bass drum. Another consistent rhythmic pattern is played against it. Together, both active

rhythmic streams establish a solid metric frame of 2/4. The times of repetitions of this material

are open to the performer’s decision.

From Ishii’s compositional concept, the core of Thirteen Drums evolves two opposite

ideas: determinacy and indeterminacy. Ishii again confirms this concept with the final statement:

determinacy in rhythmic design but indeterminacy in duration.

The Multi-Characteristic Rhythmic Scheme and Aesthetic Experience

The analysis of the rhythmic structure above provides detailed information about how the

aesthetic experience is influenced by the texture, intensity, and rhythmic tension in Thirteen

Drums. The various effects are based on the different variation techniques of the rhythmic

scheme. Although not all the variations of the rhythmic scheme are recognizable and the

rhythmic structure is obscured most of the time, it is the design of the rhythmic scheme that is

fundamental to the formal structure and connects each section as a whole.

As discussed in the first chapter, the feature of Ishii’s music is the co-existence of multi-

temporal layers—the dynamic and static layers. The structure of Thirteen Drums’s rhythmic

scheme contains both characteristics of temporal layers. The repeated sixteenth notes are the

static layer, and the accents represent the dynamic layer, as does the thirteenth figure. Solid

evidence of the temporal space between both temporal layers is shown in Section A3. Ishii

demonstrates this temporal space by adding grace-notes to the accented note. The number of

68
grace-notes existing in the temporal space increases gradually, as does the speed and dynamics.

The dynamic temporal layer becomes obscured and loses its characteristic to static effect. At the

end of Section A, both the dynamic layer and static layer are unified as oneness (this is also an

important feature of Ishii’s musical structure). The same structural design occurs in other

sections. The variations of the rhythmic scheme preserve the characteristics of dynamic and

static temporal layers. The static sound is substituted by silence and sustained sound (rolls).

The formal structure in Thirteen Drums also represents the feature of Ishii’s formal

design. Music in Sections A and A' is of the dramatic form and is strongly directional. In

Sections B, B', and C, the characteristics of static form dominate. In Section C', the

characteristics from dramatic form and static form begin to unify. The repetition of segments

represents the dynamic accented patterns taken from the original scheme mixed with the

characteristic of static form, the repetition. The quick change on the segments’ different drums

adds melodic diversity to the static nature of rolling on a single drum. Characteristics of the

dramatic form and static form are blended. The increasing dynamics and speed intensify the

aggregation of the different forms. The total synchronization of both dramatic and static forms

concludes Thirteen Drums with indeterminate repetitions.

69
CHAPTER VI

ANALYSIS OF TEMPORALITY IN THE RHYTHMIC STRUCTURE OF REBOND A

Unlike Thirteen Drums, a pre-composed structure is not verified in Rebond A.

Nevertheless, many scholars have agreed that the development of this composition lies in the

changes of density between the two sixteenth notes introduced at the very beginning (example

4-1b). The opening statement, a simple rhythmic figure of two consecutive sixteenth notes

assigned individually to the highest bongo and the large bass drum, is the core of the

composition. The development of Rebond A evolves through the density of the time span

(temporal interval) between those notes. The amount and complexity of rhythms occurring in this

time span determines the temporal density.

James Harley described Rebonds: “This is less utopian than Psappha in its demands, but

is, like the earlier work, a study in regularity and irregularity—of pulse, pattern, and form.”111

Just as Harley stated, the regularity and irregularity of the drums’ register, polyrhythm, and

various subdivisions of duration affect the pulse, rhythmic figures, and formal structure. The

continuously-mutating and irregular rhythms oscillating between drums obscure the boundaries

for chunking and also affect the perception of rhythmic groupings and the perception of

periodicities. Therefore, the formal structure is more difficult to perceive and understand.

Compared to Thirteen Drums, the timing of rhythms in Rebond A is precisely written but

the irregularity influences the perception of time. The rhythmic irregularities are caused by

phenomenal accents, complex subdivisions, and polyrhythms. The aesthetic experience of

Rebond A is especially tied to the “rhythmic tension” created by the change of density and

irregularity of rhythms.
111
Harley,192.

70
Formal Structures

The changes of density, already discussed in Chapter IV, are achieved through the

features of rhythmic structure: the register of drums, polyrhythms, accents, and various

subdivisions of duration. According to the change of density and the features of rhythmic

structure, Schick roughly divides Rebond A into six sections.112 In this dissertation, the formal

analysis is based on Schick’s identification of structure. Details in each section are examined

through the features of rhythmic structure discussed earlier in Chapter IV and rhythmic

groupings according to the proximity, similarity, and continuity, which are discussed in Chapter

III. Table 6-1 demonstrates the adjusted formal structure for Rebond A:113

Table 6-1. Formal structure of Rebond A

Section# Location in the score Length ( Approximate)


1 Measure 1, Beats 1-3 12 sixteenth-notes
2 Measure 1, Beat 4 ~ Measure 6, Beat2 76
3 Measure 6, Beat 3 ~ Measure 28 360
4 Measure 29 ~ Measure 36, Beat1 115
5 Measure 36, Beat1~ Measure 51 255.5
6 Measure 52 ~ The End 141.5

This formal structure only provides an approximate boundary between each section. Note

that bar-lines and notated rhythmic groupings are for learning purposes discussed in Chapter IV.

The numbering system for measures and beats used in this analysis are for convenience of

communication.

112
Schick, 211-212.
113
Schick, 211.

71
Density Analysis of Rhythmic Structure

Section 1: Measure 1, Beats 1-3

Example 6-1. Section 1, beats 1-3 in m.1, Rebond A

Schick calls this section the “declamation.” The beginning two sixteenth notes (two-note

grouping), one on high bongo and the other on the large bass drum, are the core of the entire

piece (example 6-1). The repeated rhythmic figures are separated by eighth rests and set up the

pulse of the eighth note or the sixteenth note and a sense of entrainment is established.

The tempo marked in the score shows that the quarter note equals 40 bpm and is kept all

the way to the end. If a performance starts in this tempo in the beginning, the pulse of the eighth

note will be 80 bpm and will fall into the range of 80~120 bpm (in which the pulse is easier to

establish). However, in cases of practice, due to the increasing rhythmic complex, some

performers play this piece at a slower tempo between 30 bpm and 40 bpm. In this temporal

envelope, an eighth note is between 60 bpm and 80 bpm and a sixteenth note is between 120

bpm and 160 bpm. The sense of pulse may then move toward the duration of the sixteenth note

and will affect the discrimination of phrases and sections.

In this dissertation, the analysis is based on the tempo of the 40 bpm quarter note as

marked. Therefore, a pulse of an eighth note is set at the beginning but not for long. In the

following section, the pulse is adjusted to the duration of the sixteenth note and is likely to be

maintained throughout Rebond A. Reasons will be revealed in the discussion of each section.

72
Section 2: Measure 1, Beat 4 ~ Measure 6, Beat2

Example 6-2: Beat 1 in m. 1 ~ Beat 2 in m. 6, Section 2, Rebond A

New drums are gradually introduced from low to high register after each core figure

(example 6-2). The rhythmic figure that was in two-note grouping becomes three-note grouping.

On beat 2 in m. 3 when the high bongo is reached, the drums’ register decreases until the last

three-note figure on the upbeat of beat 4 in the next measure. In three-note groups of decreasing

drum size, the core is moved to the second and third spots of the figure and the large bass drum

is played before the high bongo.

The figure of a four-note grouping starts in the last sixteenth note on beat 1 in m. 5.

Unlike chunking cues for groupings of two notes or three notes, each figure is separated by the

silence of an eighth note’s time interval. The cue of chunking boundaries is the rest; its cue for

the four-note grouping is the ‘melodic’ element. The figure of a four-note grouping has drums of

ascending and descending indefinite-pitches (example 6-2) inserted between the lowest and

highest drums. The large bass drum gives the impression of the grouping’s beginning.

The pulse of an eighth note or a sixteenth note was established in the first appearance of

the core (example 6-1). However, starting in three-note groups, the pulse as eighth notes is

interrupted. In the discussion earlier in Chapter IV, the pulse is now changed to be felt as an

73
sixteenth note or as odd meter as 5/16 (example 4-7a).

During the first appearance of a four-note grouping, the metric sense of 5/16 is kept. But

starting at the second four-note figure, the 5/16 metric frame is hindered, and the metric frame of

4/16 is perceived because of the melodic quality and the pulse of sixteenth note. The phenomenal

accents in this section do not disturb the perception of the proposed metric frame but increase the

complexity of rhythmic structure.

Section 3: Measure 6, Beat 3 ~ Measure 28

The complexity of rhythmic figures and the rhythmic structure’s density start to increase

in this section. According to the texture and the motivic materials in the rhythmic structure, there

are four parts in Section 3. Part 1 starts from the beginning of this section. Part 2 enters on beat 3

in m. 15 when a motivic material is played on bongos. Another piece of motivic material played

on beat 4 in m. 19 begins Part 3. Part 4 starts at the first large bass drum with a single accent in

m. 25.

In Part 1 (example 6-3), new rhythmic figures of different durations, sixteenth-note

triplets, and thirty-second notes are introduced, and the first appearance of polyrhythm occurs on

beat 3 in m. 6. Each new rhythmic figure represents the evidence of increasing density: the

former is achieved horizontally through subdivisions of beat and the latter is done vertically

through polyrhythm. The ratio of the only polyrhythm in Part 1 is 2:3 (two sixteenth-notes

against a sixteenth-note triplet).

The pulse is possibly perceived as the eighth note and sixteenth note in Part 1.

Nonetheless, the rhythms seem random and incoherent. Mutations of rhythms are irregular and

there are no perceived patterns. Most of the time, the single accents appear every four or five

74
sixteenth-notes, so a metric sense of 4/16 (2/8) or 5/16 may be perceived.114 This metric sense is

not as evident as that in previous sections because the melodic quality does not support it and

due to the interruption created by polyrhythm. Therefore, the cues for rhythmic grouping are

ambiguous and the chunking ability of rhythmic figures and phrases are low. The rhythmic

structure is not as intelligible as previous sections.

Example 6-3. Section 3, Part 1 in Rebond A

114
In the marked tempo of quarter note as 40 bpm (1500 ms), a thirty-second note is 188 ms, which is too
fast to be perceived as pulse.

75
In Part 2, the rhythmic structure is organized around a motivic material. Although this

motivic material is not represented in the same rhythmic pattern every time, it still stands out

because of its melodic quality and rhythmic formation. The example 6-4 shows the construction

of this part.

Example 6-4. Section 3, Part 2 in Rebond A

The motivic material occurs nine times and oscillates between bongos, producing a high

and penetrating sound quality. Following this motivic material is the large bass drum (the lowest

drum) that creates a distinctive contrasting sound from the bongos. Thus, this motivic material

stands out in the aural experience even when it is in the texture of polyrhythms. The new ratios

of polyrhythms are represented: 2:3 (two thirty-second notes against a thirty-second-note triplet)

and 4:3 (four thirty-second notes against a sixteenth-note triplet). Sixty-fourth notes, a new

subdivision of the beat, are introduced on beat 1 m. 17.

Rhythmic figures used in the motivic material are mostly in total duration of three

76
sixteenth-notes but arranged in different formations containing various subdivisions of the beat.

The beat here is referred to as the duration of the sixteenth note. Between each appearance of the

motivic material are various figures of sixteenth notes. The density achieved in the motivic

material is higher than single sixteenth notes because of the more complex subdivisions that are

mixed with duple or triple ratios and the polyphonic texture.

Example 6-5. Section 3, Part 3 in Rebond A

In Part 3 of Section 3, the motivic material of total duration of three sixteenth-notes

continues. However, it is not placed on the bongos (which represent the high register of drums in

Rebond A); instead, it occurs in the middle register (tom-toms) and mostly in the low register

77
(lowest tom and two bass drums). Each piece of motivic material is separated by the three-note

grouping with a few exceptions (please refer to example 6-5). The drums used for the three-note

grouping create a melodic shape, high (descending to middle range) to low-high, which is

distinct for an aural experience.

The three-note grouping breaks the continuity of the motivic material. According to its

diverse formation, it may affect the discrimination of periodicities. The formation of the three-

note grouping is one eighth note and two sixteenth notes at the end of m.19 and then the eighth

note is replaced by a sixteenth-note triplet. Starting in the second half of m. 21, the eighth note is

replaced by a sixteenth-note and this formation is kept until m. 24. The figure of an eighth note

(or other rhythms) creates a pause or extension of duration compared to a single sixteenth note.

Therefore, part of the three-note figure may be interpreted as the continuation of the motivic

material or a beginning of a phrase with the motivic figure concluding it. Similar functions are

the addition of single accents.

Starting in the middle of m. 24, the formation of the three-note grouping is unstable. On

beat 1 in m. 25, an accented sixteenth-note is introduced on the large bass drum and begins Part 4

of Section 3. This single sixteenth-note on the large bass drum breaks the continuously mutating

rhythms just like the three-note grouping in Part 3 and later is extended into a three-note group

played between two bass drums. In Part 4, the total duration of the motivic material is expanded

into multiple beats: it is seven and five sixteenth notes; later it is consistently six sixteenth notes

which is the original duration’s double of the motivic figure (example 6-6).

In Part 4, the extended motivic material is mostly played on tom-toms (mid-register). In

passages of polyrhythm, opposing rhythms played on bongos and two bass drums where the

melodic quality is in contrast to mutating rhythms of the motivic material. The motivic material

78
of the six-sixteenth-note period can also be divided into 3+3 according to the melodic quality.

Example 6-6. Section 3, Part 4 in Rebond A

In Section 3, according to the texture and motivic materials in the rhythmic structure

discussed above, the complexity of rhythmic figures and the density of rhythmic structure

increase gradually. The discrimination of periodicities is possibly perceived but is also obscured

by the irregularly mutating rhythms, the melodic quality, accents and the polyrhythm. A sense of

pulse is perceived but metric sense is unstable or absent. Therefore, the sense of entrainment is

weak. The rhythmic structure is not intelligible for aural experience.

Section 4: Measure 29 ~ Measure 36, Beat1

The characteristic of continuously-mutating rhythms from the previous section is

preserved and becomes the main feature of Section 4 (example 6-7). Schick called this section

79
“linear” since the density of texture is achieved through the linear transformation of rhythms by

subdivisions of the beat. From beat 1 in m. 29, the noted values are thirty-second notes or shorter

until beat 1 in m. 34 where a double-accented sixteenth note on the high bongo appears. This

passage is mostly played on drums in the high and mainly middle registers, avoiding the lower

bass drum which only appears once in m. 32 during the polyrhythm. The polyrhythm only

appears three times in this passage.

Example 6-7. Section 4 in Rebond A

80
The single sixteenth note with a double accent concludes the linear passage. Starting in

beat 1 in m. 34, the linear mutating rhythms are interrupted by one or more sixteenth notes, a

situation that is similar to one in Section 3. Accents (single or double) are added to the first

single sixteenth note(s) when they occur. In the passage between the sixteenth notes, the total

duration of the mutating rhythms is in the order of short (3 sixteenth notes)-long (5 sixteenth

notes)-short (3)-long (6)-short (4). During the short passages, drums in the high register are used,

and in the long passages, drums of middle and low registers are chosen. The contrast of melodic

quality between linear passages becomes the main characteristic of the next section.

Two subdivisions of quintuplets are introduced in Section 4: thirty-second-note

quintuplet and sixty-fourth-note quintuplet. The sixty-fourth-note quintuplet provides the shortest

time interval (75 ms) in Rebond A. The sixty-fourth-note quintuplet happens at the end of m. 35

and it enhances the ‘long’ duration of the eighth note occurring in the beginning of the next

measure. This eighth note is important because it is in contrast to the continuous mutating

rhythms of sixteenth-note subdivisions. It is also perceived as a ‘long’ break since the last

appearance at the end of m. 22.

In Section 4, the pulse is based on the sixteenth note. However, it is sometimes obscured

by the constantly changing rhythms from subdivisions of different ratios (especially during the

passage without a single sixteenth-note). The phenomenal accents and melodic quality do not

synchronize to give cues of boundaries for chunking. The first recognizable cue is not

represented until the upbeat on beat1 in m. 34. Due to the high complexity of rhythmic grouping

and low chunking ability, the discrimination of periodicities is harder to perform and thus the

rhythmic structure is difficult to understand.

81
Section 5: Measure 36, Beat1~ Measure 51

The density and intensity gradually increases and reaches the climax toward the end of

this section through several aspects: subdivisions, melodic quality, polyrhythm and accents. The

music in Section 5 can be divided into three parts according to the rhythms and melodic quality.

There are two main contours of the melodic quality: rhythmic figures repeating on single

drums and rhythmic figures oscillating between drums. The rhythmic figures repeating on single

drums are single sixteenth notes, or duple subdivisions (type 1); the subdivisions used in the

rhythmic figures oscillating between drums will include triple divisions (type 2). The second

type of rhythmic figures is inherited from the linear mutating rhythms. The musical example 6-8

shows the materials of type1 and type 2. The development in all three parts is based on the

alternation of these distinct types of rhythmic figures.

Example 6-8. Two featured rhythmic figures in Section 5, Type 1 and Type 2

In Part 1 (example 6-9), the rhythmic figures of type 2 are played in the higher register of

drums and then moved down to the lower register of drums. The periodicity of the linear

mutating rhythms is in different lengths but is regulated in the total duration of three sixteenth-

notes on the last beat in m. 39. The polyrhythm of ratio 4:3 sometimes occurs in the linear

mutating rhythms.

The rhythmic figures of type 1 consist of various numbers of sixteenth-note duration. The

grouping of type 1 starts with a single or double accent on drum 4 or 5 (the high bongo is

counted as drum 1 and counted down to the large bass drum as drum 7). Other accents are

82
considered as phenomenal accents and they add to the complexity of rhythmic structure.

Example 6-9. Section 5, Part 1 in Rebond A

The choice of drums for the rhythmic figures of type 1 is in a special order that gives a

structural cue for chunking. Starting with the first accented note, the order of drums is [513],

[413], [4132], [41424], [51324], [5142], [51424], [51325], [41325], [4124], and the last one

[41243] is before the entrance of the lower bass drum with a double accent. The similar melodic

quality of each combination and the duple rhythmic quality stand out for the aural perception in

comparison with the rhythmic figures of type 2.

83
Example 6-10. Section 5, Part 2 in Rebond A

The transformation in both types of rhythmic figures in Part 2 is similar to that in Part 1.

However, there are two important features in Part 2 that set it apart from Part 1: the large bass

drum with a double accent and increasing use of accents (example 6-10). The large bass drum

with a double accent is repeated twelve times and serves as a chunking cue for the discrimination

of periodicity. Before the appearance of the seventh double accented bass drum, the passages

between each double accent contain the duration of a total of eight sixteenth-notes. In these

passages, the rhythmic figures of type 1 remain the length of three sixteenth-notes. The drums

84
used in the type-1-rhythmic figures, including the double accented bass drum, are in similar

orders of [741253], [751362], [751312], [751413], and [751624]. The fourth drum in each order

descends to 7 after the seventh double accent.

After the seventh double accent, the passages are in various lengths and then are set to the

periodicity of a total of seven sixteenth-notes. The accents reach their most dense form from the

seventh double accent to the eighth one (example 6-10). The drums used in this segment are

[7517], which become the melodic cue for chunking boundaries. In the passage between the

tenth and eleventh double accents, the rhythmic figures of type 2 disappear. In each rhythmic

figure, the subdivisions of each single sixteenth-note are played on a single drum. In the same

passage, the order of the drum, [75174152], is later changed to [75174162]. The order of

[75174162] and the discussed rhythmic feature in this passage become the “skeleton” for Part 3.

Example 6-11. Section 5, P3 in Rebond A

In mm. 49-50 in Part 3, the music reaches its climax through the aggregation of high

densities of different elements: subdivisions, melodic quality, polyrhythm, and accents. There are

four passages of the same design in mm. 49-50: the skeleton from Part 3 and is ornamented with

85
polyrhythm in ratio of 4:3 oscillating between drums (example 6-11). In m. 51, the two-note

figure from the core introduced in the very beginning is reconfirmed. The interval between the

high bongo and the lower bass drum is gradually stretched in this measure. This creates an

impression of rallentando that commonly appears as a structural closure at the end of a phrase or

a piece.

Section 6: Measure 52 ~ the End

As discussed in the previous section, the end of m. 51 feels like the ending of Rebond A.

The music afterwards is the recollection, as Schick calls it, of the core in this piece. So Section 6

is the coda in the formal structure (example 6-12).

Continuing the low density and the entrainment to the core from the end of the previous

section, a note on the large bass drum followed by long silence is expected after the dotted

sixteenth note on the high bongo (example 6-8). The sixteenth note on the large drum occurs as

expected but a passage of high density intrudes the silence. The expected silence is then filled up

with subdivisions of beat and polyrhythms. The temporal density and the time interval between

the two notes of the core are increased by the mutating rhythms as well as the register of drums.

The figure played on the large bass drum is followed by a long silence exactly as

expected; it actually occurs in m. 54 where a single quarter note is. On beat 4 in m. 54, the figure

of the flam between the large bass drum and the high bongo represents the shortest time interval

of the core (the timing on the high bongo and the large bass drum is never together). The figure

of the flam is repeated five times straight and each is in every three sixteenth-note apart.

Dynamically, it starts in fff on the second flam and gradually gets softer (dim.) till it reaches the

dynamics of p.

86
Example 6-12. Section 6 in Rebond A

Following is a long silence until repeated figures occur on the large bass drum. The figure

of a flam reappears twice in dynamics of f with a single accent, and between each appearance, a

long silence with a few notes is played on the large bass drum in dynamics of pp. After the last

occurrence of a flam is another long silence. A note placed on the large bass drum concludes

Rebond A.

The passage from m. 55 to the end is reminiscent of the core material. The decreasing

dynamics and increasing silence signify the resolving of high intensity and tension created by the

high density of the rhythmic structure. Furthermore, as discussed in Chapter IV, the sense of

pulse is lost during the long silence; so too is the perception of musical time. The impression of

rallentando is better established than that in m. 51. This is the actual ending of Rebond A.

87
Rhythmic Tension and Xenakis’s Rhythmic Scheme

As discussed above, features in the rhythmic structure, pervasive use of irregular accents,

melodic quality from oscillating on different drums, irregular mutation of rhythmic figures, and

polyrhythm all contribute to the building up and releasing of rhythmic tension. The aesthetic

experience of Rebond A is especially tied to the rhythmic tension created by the change of

density and irregularity of rhythms. For Xenakis, the manipulation between simple and complex

passages creates the aesthetic experience of tension buildup and release.115

“Simple and complex” can be interpreted as “regular and irregular” in Rebond A.

Although the components of rhythmic structures, rhythmic figures, accents, and choices of

drums are in random designs, some forms of regularity are also found. For example, the core

figures in m.1, recurring total periodicities of accent patterns and motivic passages, types of

rhythmic figures, contrasting timbre in registers of drums, and melodic choices of drums that was

discussed in the analysis of each section carry some ‘identities’ that are recognizable.

Nonetheless, the original form of each identity is not preserved but changed by irregular patterns

of accents, melodic quality, and subdivisions of beat. The regularity is therefore obscured by the

irregularities.

“Change” is an important concept of Xenakis’s rhythmic scheme.116 People entrain to

regularities and when changes happen, the aural experience is shaken. When irregularities of

several rhythmic components aggregate or accumulate, the regularity of the pattern from any

component is then hard to follow, and at times, is lost. Xenakis understood that in order to spot

changes formed by the irregularities a sense of pulse (metronomic beat or underbeat as he

115
Xenakis, “Concerning Time,” 90.
116
Ibid., 92.

88
described it) is required.117 Once a sense of pulse is established by a rhythmic pattern, he altered

one or more of the rhythmic components to ‘shake’ the entrainment as well as the sense of

musical time. So in Rebond A, a sense of pulse is perceived most of the time, and some metric

senses could be experienced, but the rhythms seem distributed randomly because of the constant

changes in the rhythmic structure. Therefore, the rhythmic structure is mostly unintelligible in

Rebond A.

Performance Interpretation and Musical Time

Xenakis did not organize Rebond A in a metric framework. It is rhythmic but the

structure is often unintelligible because of the irregularities that affect the perception of musical

time. From the analysis provided, there are materials that serve as chunking cues for the

formation of motives, phrases, and sections. Based on the information process theory provided in

Chapter III, these materials are essential to the comprehension of the rhythmic structure as well

as musical time.

The experience of listening to Rebond A is challenging as to the aspect of primitive

rhythmic grouping. People may regard Rebond A as a piece of “free rhythms” because of its lack

of tonal elements, a metrical frame and the irregularity of complex rhythms. For performers, it is

important to help listeners grasp a composer’s intention and ideas in the work. Therefore, the

performers’ interpretations are essential to the aesthetic experience and will influence the

perception of musical time.

Here, the performers’ interpretations can add some expressive nuance that helps listeners

capture the motivic materials and understand the formal structure especially the discrimination of

periodicity. The expressive nuance happens through adding dynamic contours, adjusting the
117
Varga, 147.

89
intensity of accents, and creating layers of foreground/background. The nuance of interpretation

needs to be made based on the rhythmic design discussed earlier in this chapter.

Materials in Part 3 of Section 3 are good examples (musical example 6-5). Two

contrasting materials, figures of three-note grouping and linear mutating rhythms, are

represented one after another. The three-note figure has various functions. If the figure itself is

interpreted as an intrusive rhythm breaking the continuation of the mutating rhythms, a little

dynamic adjustment of the different materials can be made. One of them could be emphasized

more than the other without a significant dynamic change. Thus the discrimination of periodicity

is perceived as three-beat units most of the time (sometimes four-beats). Another situation is that

if the first note in the group is interpreted as the last note concluding the continuation from the

mutating rhythmic figure, the perception of periodicity will be changed. The accents in this

section are interpreted differently according to the situation a performer chooses.

The interpretation of polyrhythm also influences the aesthetic experience. Xenakis used

polyrhythm to enrich sound and especially triplets against other note values (2:3, 4:3 for

example).118 His intention was also to enhance the perception of a particular pattern by

introducing an opposing rhythm so the listener would “hold on” to the established pattern.

The discussion of polyrhythm in Chapter III stated that listeners could perceive

polyrhythms as a composite rhythm or would grasp toward one of the coexisting rhythms. So

when playing polyrhythms, a performer can choose to keep one voice stronger than the other or

keep both voices at the same volume. Each interpretation has a different impact on aural

experience. The polyrhythm first appears in Section 3 of Rebond A and it is a sixteenth-note

triplet imposed against two sixteenth-notes. This polyrhythm sometimes obscures boundaries the

rhythmic groupings of four or five sixteenth notes which are based on accent patterns. The accent
118
Varga, 150.

90
patterns may form metric senses. If the triple is played under the duple sixteenth-note, the metric

senses will be better preserved, allowing listeners to better follow the music.

It is not the purpose of this dissertation to judge what kind of interpretation is right or

wrong but to point out the fact that performance interpretation will affect the perception of

musical time and the aesthetic experience. Unlike the design of rhythmic structure in Thirteen

Drums, the discrimination of periodicity of formal structure is not as transparent in Rebond A.

Therefore, the performance interpretation of materials becomes crucial to the aesthetic

experience of musical time.

91
CHAPTER VII

CONCLUSION

Thirteen Drums and Rebonds are classic representative works in solo repertoire for

multiple-percussion. Each work demonstrates the composer’s concept of musical time. Ishii’s

“multi-layered” temporal structure is presented in the rhythmic scheme in Thirteen Drums that

contains two different temporal layers: the static and dynamic layers. The variations of rhythmic

scheme show the “time-space” concept of Ishii’s musical time. In Rebond A, the irregularly

mutating rhythms dominate the density in the “space” between the two notes of the core at the

beginning statement. The various densities are achieved vertically and horizontally through

several elements of rhythmic structure: the register of drums, the accents, subdivisions of beat,

and the polyrhythms. These elements represent musical time as the “multi-dimensional” musical

space.

In the detailed temporal analysis discussed in this dissertation, the characteristics of

rhythmic structure (instrumentation, compositional scheme/formal structure, free of meter, and

rhythmic complexity) determine the perception of musical time of both works. The

homogeneous instrumentation of using drums with skin heads becomes the significant feature of

both pieces. Multiple drums resemble a relative scale of indefinite pitches that produce a melodic

quality. In Thirteen Drums distinctive rhythmic structures of different parts are based on the

compositional rhythmic scheme that connect these parts cohesively; the core material and the

change of density in Rebond A’s rhythmic structure roughly defines the sections. The absence of

meter and the high rhythmic complexity challenge listeners’ aural experience. The high rhythmic

complexity is primarily caused by use of rests, pervasive accents, and subdivisions of beat/pulse.

92
Combined with other characteristics in rhythmic structure, the resulting rhythmic complexity

dominates rhythmic grouping.

Rhythmic grouping is a subjective behavior and is the rhythmic structure’s surface level.

The cognition of temporal structure and the discrimination of periodicities start with this

subjective behavior. This behavior is limited to perception and cognition of human hearing.

Studies of neuroscience and cognitive psychology have provided information processing theories

that are associated with memory systems of the human brain. Listeners encode the perceivable

auditory information through different levels of memory systems: echoic memory, short-term

memory, working memory, and long-term memory. Melodic and rhythmic elements in the

temporal structure are examined through the encoding process. Therefore, musical time is

experienced and understood.

The aesthetic experience of music is also strongly tied to this information processing

model of memory systems. If a piece renders a stable sense of pulse and a strong metric sense, a

sense of entrainment and anticipation to structures are then established. The entrainment and

anticipation are essential to the aesthetic experience: when structural expectation is violated by

upcoming events, the tension takes place and the search for tension release or a resolution

follows. This tension-and-release phenomenon stimulates the affective aesthetic experience.

Another type of aesthetic experience is influenced by rhythmic tension. The formation of

rhythmic tension does not require the establishment of pulse or metric sense and it is determined

by the density of events happening in a given amount of time. The more events happen in a

shorter time span, the higher rhythmic tension will be perceived. Thirteen Drums and Rebond A

are perceived as modern and conceptual to aesthetic experience. In both works the rhythmic

structures are not intelligible. A sense of pulse is sometimes perceived but not stable; the metric

93
sense and entrainment are weak or absent. Nevertheless, the rhythmic tension is still experienced

in aural experience.

In contemporary music, the complexity of temporal structure is challenging for listeners

to grasp and musical time is not easily understood. One of the most significant challenges for

performers is to transmit the conceptual ideas created by composers in modern works into

comprehensive interpretations. It is a performer’s responsibility to transmit the composer's

conceptual work to the audience. To achieve this goal effectively, a performer should study the

temporal structure in order to convey the aesthetic intentions of the composer.

The purpose of this dissertation is not to argue the values of contemporary music. The

purpose is to offer alternative listening strategies that may bring out different aesthetic

experiences toward contemporary music. Since the aesthetic experience is closely related to the

information processing of memory systems in which the function of long-term memory involves

learnt experience and past memories, a comparison of aesthetic experience can be made from the

first-time and after-score-studying listening experiences of Thirteen Drums and Rebond A. A

valuable project for future research would be to conduct a study that measures how informed

listening and score studying improve an individual’s comprehension of musical time in Thirteen

Drums and Rebond A.

94
APPENDIX

ERRATA IN THE SCORE OF THIRTEEN DRUMS PUBLISHED IN 1986

95
Location Description of Errata
Dedication above the
1 The last name should be spelt as Sugahara, not Sugawara.
title
Drum #1 should be located on the 4th sixteenth note of the sixteenth-note
2 [1/4, L6, P2]
pattern.
After the first group of grace-notes, drum #5 should be located on the 4th
3 [2/4, L6, P2]
sixteenth note of the sixteenth-note pattern.
Drum #4 should be located on the 3rd sixteenth note of the sixteenth-note
4 [1/4, L8, P3]
pattern.
The grace-note before drum #2 should be placed on drum #11 instead of
5 [4/4, L9, P3]
#12.
[1/4, L10, P4] and
The grace-note before drum #6 should be placed on drum #12 instead of
6 when it is represented
#13.
later
[4/4, L10, P4] and
The primary stroke after two-grace notes on drums #6 and #9 should be
7 when it is represented
placed on drum #12, instead of #11.
later
[3/4, L11, P4] and
The primary stroke after two-grace notes on drums #7 and #6 should be
8 when it is represented
placed on drum #3, instead of #4.
later
The second-grace note between the grace-note on drum #2 and the
9 [4/4, L12, P4]
primary stroke on drum #13 should be placed on drum #8, instead of #7.
The grace-note before the primary stroke on drum #6 should be placed on
10 [3/4, L13, P5]
drum #12, instead of #13.
11 [2/4, L19, P7] The note on drum #2 should be played on drum #3.
Drum #4 should be located on the 1st sixteenth note of the sixteenth-note
12 [4/4, L23, P8]
pattern.
Drum #6 should be located on the 4th sixteenth note of the sixteenth-note
13 [4/4, L23, P8]
pattern.
14 [3/4, L26, P9] Three quarter-rests should come before two quarter-rests.
In the grace-note group at the very end, drum #4 should be used instead of
15 [4/4, L30, P10]
drum #5.
Drum #6 should be located on the 3rd sixteenth note of the sixteen-note
16 [4/4, L32, P11]
pattern.
The accented note on drum #10 should be located on the 1st sixteenth note
17 [1/4, L34, P12]
of the sixteenth-note pattern.

96
BIBLIOGRAPHY

Information of Musical Time and Percussion

Barry, Barbara. Musical Time: the Sense of Order. Stuyvesant, NY: Pendragon Press, 1990.

Cook, Gary. Teaching Percussion. 2nd ed.. New York: Schirmer Books, 1997.

Cooper, Grosvenor and Leonard B. Meyer. The Rhythmic Structure of Music. Chicago: The
University of Chicago, 1960.

Hasty, Christopher F.. Meter as Rhythm. New York: Oxford University Press, 1997.

Kramer, Jonathan. The Time of Music. New York: Schirmer Books, 1988.

Koelsch, Stefan. Brain and Music. West Sussex, UK: Wiley-Blackwell, 2012.

Lerdahl, Fred and Ray Jackendoff. A Generative Theory of Tonal Music. Cambridge: MIT Press,
1983.

London, Justin. Hearing in Time: Psychological Aspects of Musical Meter. 2nd ed.. New York:
Oxford University Press, 2012

________. “Temporal Complexity in Modern and Post-Modern Music….” In Unfolding Time,


ed. Darla Crispin and Kathleen Snyers, 45-68. Ghent, Belgium: Leuven University Press,
2009.

________. “Metre.” Grove Music Online. Oxford Music Online. Oxford University Press.
http://www.oxfordmusiconline.com/subscriber/article/grove/music/43936
(accessed January 13th, 2013).

________. “Pulse.” Grove Music Online. Oxford Music Online. Oxford University Press,
http://www.oxfordmusiconline.com/subscriber/article/grove/music/45937
(accessed January 13th, 2013).

________. “Rhythm.” Grove Music Online. Oxford Music Online. Oxford University Press,
http://www.oxfordmusiconline.com/subscriber/article/grove/music/45934
(accessed January 13th, 2013)

________. “Time.” Grove Music Online. Oxford Music Online. Oxford University Press.
http://www.oxfordmusiconline.com/subscriber/article/grove/music/43935
(accessed January 13th, 2013).

Parncutt, Richard. “A Perceptual Model of Pulse Salience and Metrical Accents in Musical
Rhythms.” Music Perception 11, no. 4 (1994): 409-464.

97
Schick, Steven. The Percussionist’s Art: Same Bed, Different Dreams. Rochester, NY:
University of Rochester Press, 2006.

Snyder, Bob. Music and Memory: An Introduction. Cambridge: MIT Press, 2000.

Thaut, Michael H. Rhythm, Music, and the Brain. New York: Routledge, 2005.

White, Harvey E. and Donald H. Harvey. Physics and Music: the Science of Musical Sound.
Philadelphia: Saunders College, 1980.

Information of Maki Ishii

Articles

Akiyama, Kuniharu. “Monochrome: the Emotional Aesthetic of Japanese Drums and Maki
Ishii’s ‘Non-Musical Time’.” Translated by Robin Thompson. In Sounds of West-Sounds
of East: Maki Ishii’s Music, edited by Christa Ishii-Meinecke, 165-177. Tokyo, Japan:
Ongaku No Tomo Sha and Germany: Moeck Musikinstrumente und Verlag, 1997.

Berry, Mark. “Thirteen Drums for Percussion Solo, op.66: Interpreting in Concurrence with
Maki Ishii’s ‘Space-Time’ Concept.” Percussive Notes (November 2009): 50-59.

Burde, Wolfgang: “Musikalischer Kosmos der Integration: Notizen sum kompositorischen Werk
Maki Ishiis.” Translated by Sidney Corbett. In Sounds of West-Sounds of East: Maki
Ishii’s Music, edited by Christa Ishii-Meinecke, 99-111. Tokyo, Japan: Ongaku No Tomo
Sha and Germany: Moeck Musikinstrumente und Verlag, 1997.

Ishii, Maki. “Sounds of West-Sounds of East: Striding Two Musical Worlds.” Translated by
Robin Thompson. In Sounds of West-Sounds of East: Maki Ishii’s Music, edited by
Christa Ishii-Meinecke, 13-69. Tokyo, Japan: Ongaku No Tomo Sha and Germany:
Moeck Musikinstrumente und Verlag, 1997.

Kido, Toshirō. “The Iconology of Sounds:The Music of Maki Ishii and Spatial Concept in
Japanese Traditional Music.” Translated by Christa Ishii-Meinecke. In Sounds of West-
Sounds of East: Maki Ishii’s Music, edited by Christa Ishii-Meinecke, 181-225. Tokyo,
Japan: Ongaku No Tomo Sha and Germany: Moeck Musikinstrumente und Verlag, 1997.

Maruyama, Keisaburō. “Anagrams Across Sound Layers: Kyō-Sō.” Translated by Chris Drake.
In Sounds of West-Sounds of East: Maki Ishii’s Music, edited by Christa Ishii-Meinecke,
119-145. Tokyo, Japan: Ongaku No Tomo Sha and Germany: Moeck Musikinstrumente
und Verlag, 1997.

Mattner, Lothar. “Verharrende Zeit: Die Komponist Maki Ishii.” Translated by Sidney Corbett.
In Sounds of West-Sounds of East: Maki Ishii’s Music, edited by Christa Ishii-Meinecke,
151-163. Tokyo, Japan: Ongaku No Tomo Sha and Germany: Moeck Musikinstrumente

98
und Verlag, 1997.

Rheingruber, Patrick James. “An Exploration of Japanese Percussion Composers through


Performance and Analysis.” M.A. thesis, Eastern Illinois University, 2011.

Sano Koji. Takemitsu, Tōru. “My Perception of time in Traditional Japanese Music.”
Contemporary Music Review1, part2 (1987): 9-13.

Web site

Ishii, Maki. http://ishii.de/maki/ (accessed on January 13th, 2013).

Scores

Ishii, Maki. Zone for Piano-Percussion. Japan: Ongaku No Tomo Sha Corp., 1968.

________. Thirteen Drums for Percussion Solo, op. 66. Celle: Moeck Verlag und
Musikinstrumentenwerk, 1986.

Recording

Ishii, Maki. Thirteen Drums. From “Thirteen Drums: Music for Percussion Solo.” Atsushi
Sugahara, percussion. CD-ROM. Camerata Tokyo INC., 30CM-414, 1996.

Information of Iannis Xenakis

Books

Bois., Mario. Iannis Xenakis: the Man and His Music. London: Boosey & Hawkes Music
Publishers, 1967.

Harley, James. Xenakis: His Life in Music. New York: Routledge, 2004.

Kanach, Sharon. Performing Xenakis. Hillsdale, NY: Pendragon Press, 2010.

Matossian, Nouritza. Iannis Xenakis. New York: Kahn & Averill, 1986.

Schick, Steven. The Percussionist’s Art: Same Bed, Different Dreams. Rochester, NY:
University of Rochester Press, 2006.

Varga, Bálint András. Conversations with Iannis Xenakis. London: Faber and Faber Limited,
1996.

Xenakis, Iannis. Formalized Music: Thought and Mathematics in Composition. Bloomington,


IN: Indiana University Press, 1971.

99
Articles and Dissertations

Beyer, Greg. “All is Number: Golden Section in Xenakis’ ‘Rebonds’.” Percussive Notes
(February 2005): 40-50.

Borboudakis, Minas. “Percussion Instruments through Three Generations: Influences and


Evolutions of Composition for Percussion through Varèse’s Ionization, Xenakis’
Rebonds and Borboudakis’ Evlogitária.” Proceedings of the “International Symposium
Iannis Xenakis,” ed. Makis Solomos, Anastasia Georgaki, Giorgos Zervos (Athens, May
2005): 350-354.

DeLio, Thomas. “Xenakis.” Perspectives of New Music 39, no. 1 (2001): 231-243.

Ereuna, Eskhaté. “Extending the Limits of Musical Thought: Comments on and by Iannis
Xenakis.” Computer Music Journal 20, no. 4 (Winter 1996): 11-16.

Flint, Ellen Rennie. “An Investigation of Real Time as Evidenced by the Structural and Formal
Multiplicities in Iannis Xenakis’s Psappha.” PhD diss., University of Maryland, 1989. In
PROQUESTMS ProQuest Dissertations & Theses Full Text,
http://search.proquest.com/docview/303770456?accountid=7113 (accessed January 14th,
2013).

________. “Metabolae Arborecences and the Reconstruction of Time in Iannis Xenakis’


Psappha.” Contemporary Music Review 7, part 2 (1993): 221-248.

Harley, Maria Anna. “Spatial Sound Movement in the Instrumental Music of Iannis Xenakis.”
Journal of New Music Research 23, no. 3 (September 1994): 291-314.

Hill, Peter. “Xenakis and the Performer.” Tempo no. 112 (March 1975)” 17-22.

Lambert, James William. “Multiple Percussion Performance Problem as Illustrated in Five


different Works Composed by Stockhausen, Smith Bindle, Colgrass, Dahl, and Kraft
Between 1959 and 1967.” D.M.A. diss., University of Oklahoma, 1983. In
PROQUESTMS ProQuest Dissertations & Theses Full Text,
http://search.proquest.com/docview/303274999?accountid=7113 (accessed January 14th,
2013).

Longshore, William Terell. “Reflections on the Preparation and Performance of Three Works for
Contemporary Percussion.” D.M.A. diss., University of California, San Diego, 1999. In
PROQUESTMS ProQuest Dissertations & Theses Full Text,
http://search.proquest.com/docview/304509408?accountid=7113 (accessed January 14th,
2013).

Palter, Morris S. “The Solidification of Performance Practice Issues in Solo Percussion


Performance.” D.M.A. diss., University of California, San Diego, 2005. In
PROQUESTMS ProQuest Dissertations & Theses Full Text,

100
http://search.proquest.com/docview/305002487?accountid=7113 (accessed January 14th,
2013).

Rosen, Michael. “Focus on Performance: An Interview with Sylvio Gualda Concerning Psappha.”
Percussive Notes 27, no. 4 (Summer 1989):32-36.

Sallak, bill. “Informed Indeterminacy: Guidelines for Instrument Choice in Iannis Xenakis’s
‘Psappha’.” Percussive Notes (April 2002): 55-59.

Schick, Steven. “X is for Xenakis: Notes and Meditations on the Percussion Music of Iannis
Xenakis.” In Performing Xenakis, edited and translated by Sharon Kanach, 171-202.
Hillsdale, NY: Pendragon Press, 2010.

Shively, David P. “Indeterminacy and Interpretation: Three Realizations.” D.M.A. diss.,


University of California, San Diego, 2001. In PROQUESTMS ProQuest Dissertations &
Theses Full Text, http://search.proquest.com/docview/304688031?accountid=7113
(accessed January 14th, 2013).

Smith, Alyssa Gretchen. "An Examination of Notation in Selected Repertoire for Multiple
Percussion." D.M.A. diss., the Ohio State University, 2005. In PROQUESTMS ProQuest
Dissertations & Theses Full Text,
http://search.proquest.com/docview/305425967?accountid=7113 (accessed January 14th,
2013).

Stuart, Gregory William. "A Percussionist's Practice." D.M.A. diss., University of California,
San Diego, 2009. In PROQUESTMS ProQuest Dissertations & Theses Full Text,
http://search.proquest.com/docview/304859290?accountid=7113 (accessed January 14th,
2013).

Sterken, Sven. “Towards a Space-Time Art: Iannis Xenakis’s Polytypes.” Perspectives of Mew
Music 39, no. 2 (Summer 2001): 262-273.

Tinkel, Brian Christopher. ""Rebonds" by Iannis Xenakis: Pedagogical Study and Performance
Analysis." D.M.A. diss., University of Oklahoma, 2009. In PROQUESTMS ProQuest
Dissertations & Theses Full Text,
http://search.proquest.com/docview/304972691?accountid=7113 (accessed January 14th,
2013).

Xenakis, Iannis. “Towards a Metamusic.” Tempo 93 (1970): 2-19.

________ and Roberta Brown, “Concerning Time.” Perspectives of New Music 27, no. 1 (Winter,
1989), 84-92.

________, Roberta Brown and John Rahn. “Xenakis on Xenakis.” Perspectives of New Music 25,
no.1/2 (1987): 16-63.

101
Yoken, David. “Focus on Performance: Interview with Iannis Xenakis.” Percussive Notes 28, no.
3 (Spring 1990): 53-57.

Web site

Ferguson, Lee. “Rebonds A and B.”, accessed on January 13th, 2013,


http://www.duocontour.org/main_menu.htm

Scores

Xenakis, Iannis. Rebonds pour Percussion Solo. Paris: Editions Salabert, 1991.

________. Psappha. Paris: Editions Salabert, 1975.

102

You might also like