ch3 - 3 Individual Differences, Learning Styles and CELL
ch3 - 3 Individual Differences, Learning Styles and CELL
3.1 Introduction
The argument being proposed in this work is based on the premise that learners differ in
experience and education, their aptitude and motivation for language learning or
learning in general, and their age and level of maturity. These differences in turn affect
choices made by both learners and their teachers as to which materials, resources,
teaching and learning approaches, and forms of interaction are most effective for
being either internal or external to the learner, though there are areas of obvious cross-
over such as learning styles where, as will be discussed below, external features of
tasks, for example, can influence features of perception which are essentially internal to
learners. The extent to which learner choices and differences can be accommodated
difference, in order better to tailor self-access resources and materials to cater for this
range. In such an environment, where there is much less opportunity for teachers to
mediate the learning processes directly, it becomes critical that individual differences
which have been identified in the research literature are incorporated into the
teachers can participate in the mediation of learning, to create an environment that does
enhance language learning. The incorporation of multimedia (sound, video, graphics
of media in the self-access situation. In order for learners to take advantage of multiple
channels of perception, they need to be aware of their preferred styles of perception, the
extent to which the available media correspond to these, and how best to exploit this.
used by learners to work in their own time and at their own pace is one of the principal
advantages in their use. However, in order for learners to use computer resources in this
way, they need to be aware of the variations that have been identified as regards
successful language learning, and to possess and be able to use skills in self-directed or
learners through awareness-raising and practice, while others cannot. The more
information learners have of these differences and variations, the more chance they will
From a teacher’s point of view, decisions on the investment of money for purchase of
equipment, time for the training of staff and learners, or the development of materials,
other media, or to complement them, and the implications of these factors for the
teachers, and learners, and the appropriateness of the use of technology in language
learning, have already been discussed. In the following sections, the human side of the
examined, particularly as regards individual differences and learning styles. This will
then be incorporated into the discussion in the following chapters of certain aspects of
which can be identified as accounting for such differences. As will be discussed in more
detail in later sections, some of these features can be changed, modified, or developed
through instruction and awareness-raising, whereas others are less amenable to change.
learning environment, as it raises the dual question of which changes are desirable, and
The terms ‘learning style’ and ‘cognitive style’, though often used interchangeably, can
comprehensive ‘learning style’. Thus cognitive style has been defined as:
cognitive, affective and physiological behaviors that indicate learners’ characteristic and
consistent way of perceiving, interacting with and responding to the learning environment; more
concrete than cognitive style (Willing, 1988).
second language (L2) learners, Larsen-Freeman and Long (1991: 153) start with the age
factor as being ‘one of the most obvious potential explanations for the comparative lack
of success of second language learners’. L2 learners simply start later than first
language learners. Larsen-Freeman and Long go on to say, however, that there is ‘a
SL [second language] learners’. Among these other factors, they list: ‘language
In addition to these, Altman (1980) offers previous experience with language learning,
sex of the learner, proficiency in the native language, and sense modality preference, as
interaction (whether in groups, alone, or in pairs). These latter two factors correspond
(1989) also deals with most of these factors, though from the perspective of their
implication in theories of second language acquisition (SLA). All of this has led Ellis
(1994: 471) to comment that, while ‘the study of IDs [individual differences] has
attracted a lot of attention in SLA research, and has made considerable advances’, the
constructs used to interpret research findings ‘are often vague and overlap in
While there is obviously some confusion and overlap among the findings of these
approaches to research and theory in this area, as can be seen above, there is sufficient
consistency on the main features for these to be considered and catered for in language
necessary to examine the factors within the individual which relate to success in
language learning in general. Only then can we identify those factors of general
differences in SLA. In this field, the focus has been on individual variation in terms of
acquisition require some examination here, for their relation to the issue of teachability
and learnability mentioned above in section 3.3. In brief, developmental factors in SLA
include such features as stages, rate or order of acquisition, natural sequences (usually
of syntactic morphemes – Dulay and Burt, 1974; Meisel, Clahsen & Pienemann, 1981),
It was Selinker who first coined the term ‘interlanguage’ to describe the form of the
(1) knowledge underlying production in his native language (NL); (2) knowledge –
presumably at more than one level of consciousness – about the TL; and (3)
knowledge underlying attempted production of a TL norm.
(Nemser, 1971). It can be seen, therefore, that researchers from a range of paradigms
In 1974, two researchers into the emergence of natural sequences, Dulay and Burt, were
able to confirm their hypotheses from earlier studies, that the ‘same sequence of
acquisition of 11 functors, obtained by three different methods, provides strong
universal order’ (1974: 50). Dulay and Burt call the process by which children are able
they ‘formulate certain types of hypotheses about the language system being acquired,
until the mismatch between what they are exposed to and what they produce is
resolved’.
Meisel, Clahsen and Pienemann (1981) similarly, for adults learning German as a
second language in natural settings, were able to identify developmental stages in their
learners’ acquisition which were subject to ‘readiness’ constraints. To account for the
variations found among learners at the same developmental level, Meisel and his
acculturation theory discussed below. In other words, the acquisition of some features is
dependent on, for example, the learner being positively disposed towards, or valuing,
linguistic accuracy.
On the basis of this and previous studies, Meisel and colleagues also suggested the
correspond to the natural order of acquisition of those features. One of the major issues
investigated in relation to classroom instruction and the teaching of syntax has been this
complex question of the best way to teach learners syntactic rules, and which rules are
learnt best in what way: in other words, whether it is better in language instruction to
take an inductive or a deductive approach, or whether certain rules are better learnt by
explicit knowledge, Green and Hecht (1992) examined a group of German learners of
English as the L2. They found that, in terms of explicit knowledge instruction, rules
and are independent of the larger context, are easy to learn. Alternatively, when rules
the broader context, such as aspect rules, they were found to be much more difficult to
learn.
Code-oriented
Instruction
Explicit
Knowledge
Studies such as this lead Ellis (1992: 97-8) to hypothesise a weak interface model to
explain the role of explicit knowledge in SLA. According to this model, illustrated in
Figure 3.1 above, explicit knowledge helps learners ‘to notice features in the input that
(Seliger, 1979) also assists learners to notice the gap between the input they are exposed
to and their own interlanguage output.
the necessary and sufficient condition for converting input to intake’ and that ‘paying
attention is probably facilitative, and may be necessary if adult learners are to acquire
redundant grammatical features’ (1990: 129). This issue of conscious noticing will be
discussed in greater detail later in section 3.4.4.4 on the development of the concept of
‘uptaken’ items recorded on her student classroom charts were focussed upon at some
point during instruction. However, viewed differently, 11 percent of uptaken items only
occurred as part of classroom discourse, among peers, and even within the 89 percent,
there was considerable individual variation regarding which aspects of items were
1, in that it is within the interaction among users of the language, even novice users,
that the construction of meaning emerges. The combination of teacher and peer
discourse in the classroom provides both the context, and language and discourse
Also in the area of classroom acquisition of syntactic structures, Felix (1981) found that
the acquisition of his German high school learners of English as a second language
classroom instruction, his learners ‘were continuously forced to produce structures for
which, developmentally, they were not yet ready’ (1981: 87). To compensate for this,
they adopted ‘two basic strategies: (a) they followed principles of naturalistic
acquisition; or (b) they randomly selected any one structure from a finite repertoire’.
From this range of studies on developmental stages or natural sequences, it can be seen
in this work, except to the extent that learners and teachers alike need to be informed of
the existence and nature of this phenomenon. In addition, as the focus here is not
morpho-syntax, but rather the development of strategic listening, through the use of
learners are at the appropriate stage to make meaning from a piece of authentic
material, and use the structures that appear in it, then they will be able to learn or
acquire those structural elements. While some of the listening tasks focus on specific
express cause and effect, for example, there is no explicit focus on syntactic elements.
From the evidence found in the research studies discussed above, however, this focus
on functional features and the negotiation of meaning required of learners in this CELL
environment should make it possible for them to form tenable hypotheses, whether
conscious or not, about the rules relating to the syntactic elements of such features.
As Schmidt (1990: 149) found, ‘incidental learning is certainly possible when task
demands focus attention on relevant features of the input’. In addition, Ellis (1994: 89)
stresses that ‘many formal language lessons are not directed at teaching new properties
but at enabling the learners to use features they have already partly acquired with
greater accuracy’. The listening and viewing material in the program described in this
work is thus not graded to cater for the progressive emergence of morpho-syntactic
the various individual differences introduced earlier in section 3.1.1, the main interest
we have in these is the extent to which they influence learners’ capacity to learn from
identify, among these areas of difference, those features which are more amenable to
change. In order for optimal learning to be promoted for different learners using the
CELL materials, these features will need to be taken into account in the design of the
CELL program, and also it is these features that will need to become the focus of
This process will also allow us to move away from those features which are less
influence they have in order to choose more appropriate materials, or direct learners to
make use of the materials in certain ways. In addition, as will be discussed in the next
possible to help learners compensate for less desirable or less effective characteristics
which they may have. However, within the materials themselves, and within a program
of instruction or learning little change can be effected in learners with regard to these
features. A learner may, for example, be beyond an age when language learning can
easily be achieved through the use of certain techniques, but he/she can be made aware
of, and encouraged to employ, other techniques or strategies which have been shown to
The factors or characteristics being discussed here are specific to individual learners,
and include age and maturity, personality, aptitude for language, hemispheric
strategies. As mentioned above, some of these individual and developmental factors are
obviously more amenable to being changed, developed, or influenced through
identified in the literature as ‘learning strategies’ are the most changeable or ‘trainable’
features. Broadly speaking, these include metacognitive strategies used to organise and
Language learning strategies will become the focus of the next chapter.
aptitude, and hemispheric orientation are less changeable, while attitude, motivation,
possibly learning and cognitive style, and learning strategies are more open to
however, it is important to outline briefly why this is so, and what these effects are,
before discussing in greater depth those aspects of individual learners which can
become the focus of education and awareness-raising. The area of learning strategy
identification and training will then form the major part of the discussion in the
following chapter, as such training forms an integral part of the development of learner
when it comes to language learning (McLaughlin, 1987: 29; Genesee, 1988; Skehan,
1989: 137; Larsen-Freeman & Long, 1991: 154). For younger learners, age may be a
positive factor, not from the simplistic point of view that younger learners learn
languages better (Dulay & Burt, 1974), but rather from the view that they have a longer
period of time to learn when starting younger (Genesee, 1988: 105). In addition, when
absence of Piagetian ‘formal operations’ allows children to learn their second language
using processes much like those involved in learning their first language (Krashen,
1982a). Others, however, find that the cognitive maturity of older learners favours the
way languages are generally taught in schools, using more abstract processes, and
age of learners, while matching instructional style with the cognitive development and
Snow, 1983; Ellis, 1985), and if so, what this age might be (Snow and Hoefnagel-
Hohle, 1978; Dulay and Burt, 1974), whether there might be a point beyond which
language learning abilities stultify (Snow, 1987; Penfield and Roberts, 1959;
Lenneberg, 1967; Krashen, 1974; Neufeld, 1979), the differing approaches to language
and Chesterfield, 1985; Krashen, 1982; O’Malley et al., 1985), and the relative
(Skehan, 1989: 137; Hatch, 1983; McLaughlin, 1984; Krashen, Long and Scarcella,
1979).
To date, the conclusions seem to consistent with the oft-cited adage ‘older is faster, but
younger is better’ (Krashen et al., 1979; Larsen-Freeman & Long, 1991: 155), where
‘faster’ refers to short-term gains found with older learners, while ‘better’ refers to the
eventually approximate. However, there have been several studies, particularly in early
and later bilingual programs, that indicate that early adolescence may be the optimum
time to begin (McLaughlin, 1984; Swain and Lapkin, 1982). In relation to types of
instruction, as mentioned above (Genesee, 1988), on the basis of data from immersion
Studies of the differences between child L2 learning and that of older learners have
Many of these studies have used data collected from immigrants learning their L2 while
living in the target culture, where the age of arrival provides a clear reference point.
Several of these have indicated that age of arrival in the target language culture is
In terms of phonology, other studies indicate that it may even be impossible to achieve
native-like speech if language exposure begins after the age of about six years (Payne,
1980; Tahta et al., 1981; Major, 1987), though some exceptions have been found
(Neufeld, 1979).
Schmidt (1990: 145) was intrigued to note that the age range during which the shift
occurs from child to adult consciousness ‘approximates to the sensitive period for
that includes an open awareness of the environment’, adults exhibit a ‘more controlled
mode that includes the strategic allocation of attention’. Schmidt used studies by Ceci
and Howe (1982) and Miller (1985) on strategic attention and incidental learning in
children, together with his own 1983 study of adults’ failure to learn grammar through
al. (1983) that ‘children learn the rules of grammar as a by-product of trying to
communicate’. From these, he proposes the hypothesis that it is children’s lack of
strategic attention which causes them to notice grammar features that are
communicatively less important. Adults, therefore, having more control over their focus
grammatical features’.
Several other explanations have been offered for the age-related changes in capacity for
language learning referred to above. Larsen-Freeman and Long (1991: 163-7) classify
3.4.3 on personality)
below)
education, namely pre-school and primary, contrasted with secondary and adult
contexts (Curtain and Pesola, 1988; Wankowski, 1973). In pre-school and primary
learning environments, language play is encouraged, and learners are exposed to more
input that is concrete and meaningful, providing simple syntactic models for them to
follow.
Another argument that is advanced to explain the perceived advantages of the kinds of
environments, is that the second language input experienced by younger learners more
closely approximates that of their first language (Krashen, 1981; Larsen-Freeman &
Long, 1991). The latter argument is premised on the principles that, as children acquire
their L2 using processes similar to those activated in the acquisition of the L1 (Krashen
stages. Children and adults would therefore learn second languages differently: the
former, still having access to the Language Acquisition Device (LAD - Chomsky,
1965) used for L1 acquisition, could utilise this in much the same way for their second
language; the latter, having more mature cognitive skills, could approach the second
analytic, and reasoning strategies adolescents and adults use in higher frequency and
number than children (O’Malley & Chamot, 1990). Particularly in instructed learning
environments, problem-solving has been identified as one of the two basic processes,
1994b: 90). This strategic approach has been considered in the choice and design of
tasks in developing the taxonomy of listening comprehension skills discussed in the last
chapter and Chapter 4, as some researchers claim that such ability can develop quite
Currently, however, insufficient investigation has been carried out into the differences
between children and adults as regards the language learning processes operating. As
comparisons will clearly be the key, yet are virtually non-existent to date’ (1991: 166-
7). Larsen-Freeman and Long acknowledge that biological constraints on SLA, such as
that espousd in the critical (Penfield and Roberts, 1959; Lenneberg, 1967) or sensitive
maintain that, in the absence of interlanguage comparisons, and the ‘lack of any known
neurological changes clearly coinciding with changes in SLA ability’, they cannot
different learners helps us to ensure that the language learning materials we design cater
for these preferences, thereby providing as many learners as possible with optimal
learning opportunities.
difficulty of obtaining reliable data on the mechanisms and processes involved (Jacobs,
1988). An unfortunate factor in many of these studies is that the investigation of the
investigation of their processing. While this has to be borne in mind as regards the
conclusions and speculations emerging from these studies illuminate an area of interest
Several researchers (Joiner, 1984; Fountain & Fillmer, 1987; Danesi, 1988; Curro,
activities and a learning program that promote the integration of processing from both
hemispheres of the brain, in order for learners to realise their maximum potential. At
the same time, these researchers and others (Walsh & Diller, 1981) maintain that older
learners can take greater advantage of the ‘higher-order’ language functions that
develop with cognitive maturity. As Walsh & Diller comment (1981: 18): ‘As people
functions [such as semantic relations] allow them to do more than they could before
with their lower-order aptitudes and functions’. This provides further evidence for the
speculative. This is because, despite advances in the area, equipment for measuring
neural activity is still not accurate enough, and because tests are so indeterminate. Thus,
for example, even though the most modern techniques for mapping the activity of
individuals (Begley, et al. 1992; Damasio, 1992), this information cannot yet tell us
how or why the events and activity identified relate to the actual learning process. In
investigating neural activity in living, breathing people, and are therefore constrained in
the range and nature of tests they can conduct. There remains, therefore, no reliable
means of determining what exactly they are seeing, and how this relates specifically to
language learning.
lateralisation, both for the purpose of demonstrating the range of such factors that
from our scope in the design of a learner-centred software package. In addition to those
of lateralisation on modes of teaching and learning, script nature and direction, and type
of first or second language. It is the first of these areas of focus that interests us here for
the design of a software package that caters for individual differences.
occur, is their use of strategies that deploy the specialised processes of the two
The research suggests that children exhibit more right hemispheric involvement in
language processing than adults, and that this is also true for beginning second language
learners. In addition, it is hypothesised from the studies that while left hemisphere
processing seemed greater in formal learning contexts than in informal ones. Therefore
Genesee (1988) both cautions that such studies examine language processing, not
learning, and cites evidence for greater left hemisphere involvement in more formal
learning contexts, with greater right hemispheric processing in more informal contexts.
Possibly the most important aspect of the research into hemispheric lateralisation and
differential processing is the implications of these findings for teaching and learning.
Curro (1995), frames her discussion of this as the necessary consideration of bimodality
in syllabus planning and task selection and design. As she explains, on the basis of
research findings (Goldberg & Costa, 1981; Danesi, 1988; Palotta, 1993):
the right hemisphere has a more compatible anatomical structure for new material.
Given its intricate network of neuronal connectivity, it can accommodate information
for which no preexistent codes or schemes are available. [...] According to Danesi, the
construct of bimodality does not constitute a syllabus format or method. ‘It simply alerts
the teacher to certain fundamental requirements of human learning, such as the need to
start a novel learning task in a sensorial and concrete way and to end it in a more
formalized fashion (the learning flow principle)’ (Danesi, 1991: 28).
processing in the right and left hemispheres, and particularly in teaching and learning
involving both listening and viewing. As Danesi (1987, citing Sperry) reminds us,
while neuroscience has fairly clearly established that verbal thinking is represented in
the left hemisphere and non-verbal thinking in the right, our current teaching and
learning practices tend to neglect the non-verbal. This again reinforces the findings of
Hurley (1992) and Kellerman (1992) that language learning materials incorporating the
incorporating multimedia.
and researchers have hypothesised about the desirability of matching teaching style and
learners. While it is proposed that such matches will promote learning, little evidence
has been provided for any consistency in the findings (Hamayan et al.; 1977; Wong-
Fillmore, 1982).
In addition, personality and aptitude are both features which are relatively difficult to
further complication resides in the experience of researchers that tests of the existence
and nature of personality traits are unreliable (Larsen-Freeman & Long, 1991;
learning than others, particularly as they relate to individual learning. This information
then enables us to make provision in language learning programs and materials for the
3.4.3.1 Personality
The personality traits which occur most frequently in the literature surveyed are
success in general academic performance, it was predicted that introversion would show
higher correlations with academic achievement (Entwistle and Wilson, 1977). This
prediction was based on the assumption that introverted learners would be more self-
reliant and more prepared to study alone. This had, in fact, been shown to be the case
(Entwistle and Entwistle, 1970), though the cross-cultural applicability of the inventory
1981) has been questioned (Watkins, 1982). In Hattie and Watkins’ (1981) study of
Filipino tertiary students, for example, the researchers failed to find evidence of the
secondary school, Wankowski (1973) found that before puberty, extroversion was
associated with higher academic achievement, with introversion being more dominant
an indicator after puberty. The explanation he offered for this was that after puberty, or
at high school, the nature of the teaching and testing approaches changed to favour
introverted learners. In the primary school, tasks were more often oriented towards
group work, while at high school, with greater subject specialisation, the emphasis was
secondary and post-secondary ages, these findings indicate the necessity to provide
activities that range across contexts of working alone, in pairs, and in groups, and to
provide activities which help extroverted learners develop strategies for working alone.
learning contexts, extroversion seems to be the desirable trait, because of the emphasis,
learning is gauged by the proficiency with which the learner can negotiate meaning and
interact in the target language, characteristics such as sociability and seeking out
occasions for interaction with speakers of the target language assist in the acquisition of
A study by Swain (1985) also indicates that not only is it necessary for a language
learner to have useful or comprehensible input (Krashen, 1982) from speakers of the
target language, but that it is equally important for the learner to experiment with
1985). Such behaviours are typical of extroverted personalities. As the ‘Good Language
Learner’ studies of Naiman and colleagues (1978), and others (Wong-Fillmore, 1979;
Rubin, 1975; 1987) have indicated, being active and pursuing occasions for interaction
such as discrete point grammar tests, pronunciation and standardised reading tests
(Suter, 1976; Busch, 1982), the correlations favour characteristics of introversion over
production, and engagement with the second language has been the major teaching
approach since the early 80s (Swan, 1985a, b), we would expect higher correlations
with language proficiency for learners with stronger extroversion than introversion
characteristics. It may be the case that the relationship between extroversion and
introversion and language learning has not yet become clear, because studies are
hampered by the predominantly decontextualised nature of the tests of language
communicative tests have been developed, such as those of Ingram (1984) and
McNamara (1996), these have not been the tests used in studies of
questionable. Indeed, both Skehan (1989) and Ellis (1994) argue for studies using a
the interaction between the more malleable aspects of individual difference’, these can
‘provide measures of learning based on natural language use rather than on tests or
Aspects of gender have also been studied in relation to personality factors in language
al. (1991), Dye (1995), and Meunier (1996) have all found that females tend to be
dominated by males in mixed-gender language learning activities. However, as Meunier
comments (1996: 50): ‘the poor performance of female students seems to be related
more to interaction styles and personality differences than to their level of computer
and keyboard control, and, together with ‘position at the computer’, personality was
As a result of these findings, she recommends that (1996: 67): ‘foreign language
software designers should attempt to integrate more than one path of data exploration
learners in hypertext environments ‘for the purpose of being exposed to other cognitive
styles’ (Meunier, 1996: 67); and incorporating a ‘road map’ to provide linear learners
with some structured support in their exploratory learning. While issues of cognitive
the software package described in Chapters 5 and 6 of this work has been designed to
make provision for learners’ different cognitive styles and to take into account the
systems, and partially through the division of the package into three progressively less
structured layers between which learners can alternate as their needs determine.
characteristics, many of which are inter-related, have been investigated for their
influence on second language learning. The ones which will be dealt with here include:
mentioned by Ellis (1994: 524), along with motivation, as being one of the ‘more
Self-esteem and anxiety have been shown to have only low to moderate correlations in
necessary, and too much being deleterious. Self-esteem can be defined as ‘self-
judgement of worth or value, based on feelings of efficacy’ (Scarcella & Oxford, 1992:
57), and, in common with anxiety described below, can be either a trait or global
Scovel, 1978), with different ratings emerging for the same learners with different
teachers and subject areas. For learners with low global self-esteem, it can be built up
through the use of such affective learning strategies as ‘taking your emotional
20).
In a self-access CELL environment, such as the one described in Chapters 5 and 6, the
succeed at their own level. Referred to in the literature as ‘individualised learning’, this
is one aspect of the oft-cited advantage of computers: that they allow learners to work
at their own pace and level (Underwood, 1984: 38; Batley & Freudenstein, 1991: 10,
12-13; Weible, 1987: 81; Wyatt, 1987: 91). However, this can only apply if the
appropriate materials and activities are provided and if the learner has an understanding
optimum balance in relation to one another (Biggs and Telfer, 1981: 113 - 5). After this
point, performance drops off rapidly as anxiety increases. Researchers in the area of
language learning have defined anxiety as being either facilitating (Scovel, 1978), or
debilitating. The former characteristic, which can also be described as ‘positive tension’
(Scarcella & Oxford, 1992: 54), promotes learning, while the latter can produce worry
and self-doubt, causing the learner to participate less and perform less well. Horwitz
(1990) sees the role of facilitating anxiety as being limited to simple tasks which may,
therefore, exclude language learning (Williams, 1991). Other researchers (Scarcella &
Oxford, 1992: 54) argue that ‘a certain amount of positive tension is helpful for
language learners’. In this light, Scarcella and Oxford make a claim for the use of the
construed.
only ‘witness’ to one’s language interaction and performance is a computer, it has been
suggested that learners build a special relationship with the computer, experiencing a
sense of privacy, and increasing their motivation to learn and their sense of
achievement (Batley & Freudenstein, 1991: 11). It is in contexts such as this that
working with computers is regarded as being less threatening than public (i.e.
classroom) performance.
learning (Rubin, 1975; Ely, 1986), although it may result in reduced performance when
over-used (Beebe, 1980, 1983). Risks here are exemplified variously by guessing
mistakes are also produced, and initiating conversations with native speakers of the
target language. Such risks are a normal part of learning a language in naturalistic
settings, and have been shown to characterise successful language learners (Oxford,
1990; O’Malley & Chamot, 1991). They are also an essential component of the
(Selinker, 1969; 1971; 1972). Naiman (1978), on the other hand, has found little
to risk-taking, and language learning success. One possible explanation for this could
be that the test of language proficiency is not an appropriate instrument for measuring
As well as being difficult to test accurately, empathy has produced mixed correlations
when matched with language learning. In 1972 (b), Guiora and others hypothesised the
found in children for accurate pronunciation, mentioned earlier. Children, who have
low ego boundaries, have less difficulty with, and attain greater accuracy in,
increase in strength with age, this facility for accurate pronunciation diminishes. Guiora
and colleagues also investigated the influence of inhibition (Guiora et al. 1972a) but
found little firm evidence for this in either direction. Variations of both these
Lastly, tolerance for ambiguity, together with risk-taking, seem to exhibit the highest
correlation with success in second language learning (Naiman, 1978; Chapelle and
meaning of a whole text or discourse of a text (Canale & Swain, 1980; Canale, 1983),
rather than that of individual words. In order to be able to function on such a holistic
basis, a high tolerance for ambiguity is necessary to avoid the anxiety which might
ensue when the meaning of individual words within the text may be unknown.
Tolerance for ambiguity has also been linked to self-esteem (Scarcella & Oxford,
1992), in that learners with high self-esteem are more likely to feel confident enough to
accept initial lack of total comprehension or possible mismatches between their existing
as paraphrasing and guessing (Oxford, 1990: 19) may help minimise the perception of
risk on the part of students with lower self-esteem. On the other hand, too high a
tolerance for ambiguity might actually inhibit language learning by not allowing
learners to perceive and analyse some rules of language. As will be discussed in more
detail in the next chapter, such learners need exposure to cognitive strategies such as
practising, analysing and reasoning, and creating structure for input and output (Oxford,
1990).
this section are typical of successful language learners, moderation in each seems to
produce the best effects. Thus, while some anxiety, in the form of ‘positive tension’,
good language learners have sufficient self-esteem to take risks in interpreting and
producing their own utterances, they also need to cultivate a critical internal monitor of
the language to which they are exposed, in order to formulate the appropriate
3.4.3.3 Aptitude
When we come to the question of the role of aptitude in language learning, Skehan
(1989: 136) comments: ‘(s)ome people are endowed with better cognitive/linguistic
ability than others’. But what constitutes aptitude for language learning, and can it be
that was typical of traditional grammar-translation language classes and tests. Based on
Carroll’s (1965) comprehensive testing in the area, Skehan goes on to claim that:
Though Skehan lists the above three components as being the most critical, Carroll also
sensitivity and those of a language analytic capacity, and all four components seem to
section.
The fact that these components of aptitude are identifiable provides strong grounds for
the design of instructional materials that would cater for the range of individual learner
needs represented. In commenting about the strong relationship found by Cummins and
Nakajima (1987) between the L1 and L2 reading skills of Japanese immigrant students,
McLaughlin (1990) makes the claim that some of the strength of this relationship is
[...] aptitude should not be viewed as a static personality trait; novices can become
experts with experience. I believe there is an interdependence between first and second
language in the cognitive/academic domain because experience with one language gives
the learner strategies and metacognitive skills that generalize to subsequent languages.
[...] Teachers, in my view need to do more than provide “comprehensible input”. They
need to make these strategies and metacognitive skills available to learners.
McLaughlin’s comments here are echoed in the research reported in Chapter 1 of this
through adopting the social norms, practices, and patterns of the target culture, and
thereby also the target language (Lantolf, 1994; Donato & McCormick, 1994). For
McLaughlin, then, aptitude can improve with experience that derives from transfer of
skills from the first language, and also through the development of strategies and
metacognitive skills. As we will see in the next chapter, most researchers in the field of
training (O’Malley & Chamot, 1991). This being so, there is a place for education or
strategies.
Politzer and Weiss (1969) and Carroll (1979) as showing evidence of aptitude being
experience. However, while accepting the difficulty of training learners in certain skills
in order to improve their aptitude, Carroll (1965) recognises that some improvement
can be achieved with high quality instruction. As Larsen-Freeman and Long (1991)
argue, when instruction is of lower quality, learners are forced to fall back on their own
In a study of adults studying French in Canada, Wesche (1981) found positive effects
for matching learners with appropriate teaching methods and materials based on their
learning style preferences exhibited in personality profiles. On the basis of this and
Carroll’s findings above, there seem to be clear advantages for a range of learners in
using multimedia CELL materials which are learner-driven. These materials should
vary in the channels of perception they activate (visual, textual, auditory), and learners
should be able to choose the types of tasks and materials they feel most comfortable
using. In this model, more challenging tasks and materials would be those which
Chapters 5 and 6, the software package developed as a prototype of this model, for
example, makes available to learners three levels of working through the material: a
highly structured level, a level which focusses on cognitive demand, and an exploratory
Given that aptitude is an important factor in language learning, and that learners’
accommodation for this variability. For a learner-centred CELL package to make such
clearly the choices and options available. In addition, the feedback mechanisms within
the package need to be task- and choice-specific and to give learners access to
information about the right and wrong answers and the context of and background to
these.
both general learning and second language learning. Work by Hartnett (1975 – cited in
Larsen-Freeman & Long, 1991: 210) has also shown that cognitive style may, in fact,
been driven by the same motivating force that lies behind research in the other areas of
found between modes of instruction and learning or cognitive style. Similarly, the
impetus behind the work on learning strategies has been the extent to which educating
learners in the use of more effective strategies enables them to improve their learning.
As a prelude to this latter area of research, it has first been necessary to identify and,
usually, classify, such strategies. Subsequently, having identified and classified the
desirable strategies, it is proposed that learners of lower aptitude, or those who do not
normally employ such strategies, can then be educated in their efficacy and use.
The distinction between styles and strategies has only been articulated since studies
studying’ (Ramsden and Entwistle, 1981) in study, school or learning contexts (Marton
and Säljö, 1976; Svensson, 1977; Laurillard, 1979), rather than in the previous ‘natural’
contexts (Biggs, 1976; 1978; Entwistle et al., 1979). Questions on the existence of
cognitive or learning styles, their nature, and their capacity for being influenced
through teaching or training, have been the subject of inquiry since the early work of
Witkin (1954; 1967), Witkin et al. (1967; 1977) and Goodenough (1976; 1981) on
It is important that learning or cognitive styles, which refer to ‘the student’s preferred
which, as will be discussed in detail later, ‘concern the way a student elects to tackle a
particular learning task’ (Watkins, 1982: 78). It is only since the mid eighties that this
distinction has begun to clarify, with the work of Oxford on language learning strategy
identification, Dickinson (1987), Holec (1987), and Wenden (1991) on strategies useful
for self-instruction and autonomy, and O’Malley and Chamot (1990) on the relationship
Early studies of learning and cognitive style began to show that there were similar,
identifiable, dichotomous dimensions to cognitive style, in both the study and natural
study and high achievement motivation’ (Watkins, 1982: 77) also seemed to be
operating. Thus, for example, in a qualitative study of student descriptions of how they
approach learning tasks, Marton and Säljö (1976) were able to classify the responses
generally according to whether the students were employing surface or deep levels of
parts rather than the whole in surface processing; and a holistic, structure-oriented
orientation and attitude also emerged as an influencing factor in this and other studies
As illustrated by the variety and nature of terms used by researchers into learning
styles, a lot of confusion resulted from early analyses of learner approaches to study.
nature of these styles: whether they were innate and fixed, or whether they were task-
or context-dependent. Many of these problems arose because researchers had not yet
‘strategic’ processing, discussed in the following sections, which had not yet been
adequately described.
The dichotomous ‘surface <–> deep’ dimensions mentioned above have now been
dimension fits the description of what we now call learning strategies, which will be
discussed in detail in the next chapter. As will become clear by the next chapter, even
though the recognition and accommodation of learning styles and strategies is now
fairly widely accepted as playing a crucial role in the teaching and learning process, we
are still not certain of their exact nature and the interaction between them. First,
however, let us examine the critical features of cognitive and learning styles as
styles was focussed on modes of perception and differences in style among individuals
for making meaning from input gathered by the senses, particularly sight. Later studies
1982; Lam, 1982). In these studies, the basic premises were that the differences in
working through learning tasks. It was at this stage that clear discrepancies began to
emerge as to whether learning styles are stable and static, or variable and dynamic
changeable. The emergence of these discrepancies has led to the distinction being made
between learning styles, which have come to be regarded as mostly unconscious and
innate in individuals for particular contexts (see also Larsen-Freeman & Long, 1991),
and learning strategies, which are, or can become, conscious, and which therefore
criterion in the identification of strategies of learning. [....] [but] those strategies which
are under conscious control are of course easier to identify, to discuss, to manipulate
and to teach.’
Schmidt (1990: 133), on the other hand, argues that consciousness and intentionality
need to be separated, as ‘we often become aware of things we do not intend to notice’.
While denying the possibility of subliminal perception from his data, Schmidt makes a
strong claim for the possibility of incidental learning taking place while the focus of
learning is on other, relevant and salient points. His necessary criterion in this process is
to the phenomenon of consciousness allow the possibility, for example, of new learning
strategies being acquired by learners when the focus of material presented is listening
comprehension tasks incorporating such strategies. This is, in fact, the paradigm
independence (FI), were identified as tendencies only, though quite stable over time.
Females were generally represented slightly more highly at the dependent end, possibly
field’, a low capacity for segregating the self from the non-self, greater use of social
people’, and closer personal space. The articulated, FI cognitive style, on the other
More recently, in the SLA field, FI/D has been defined as ‘a cognitive style, a bipolar,
stable trait affecting how one thinks, feels, and behaves’. Field Independent people are
people are holistic, uncertain, and dependent upon others’ (Chapelle & Green, 1992).
Chapelle and Green go on to note that it is important to distinguish cognitive style from
ability because, among other reasons, ‘a cognitive style is a bipolar trait, indicating
As can be seen in Figure 3.2 below, other researchers in the area of cognitive style have
used various terms to describe FD, including global (Witkin, 1977), holistic (Pask and
Scott, 1972), intuitive (Bruner, 1960), intuitive/random (Scarcella & Oxford, 1992),
and divergent production (Guilford, 1967). These are contrasted with articulated style
(Witkin, 1977), serialist (Pask & Scott, 1972), analytic (Bruner, 1960),
1967) respectively. One aspect on which all researchers seem to agree with regard to
cognitive style is that extremes of the dichotomy are equivalent in learning potential. In
other words, individuals can achieve equally on intellectual tasks, regardless of which
cognitive style they exhibit, though their means of achieving the same end product may
differ.
However, some researchers in the SLA field (Felder & Henriques, 1995) believe that
this equality can be compromised when mismatches occur between the learning styles
of members of a class and the teaching style of the teachers, where the teacher’s
cognitive style predisposes him or her towards presenting, teaching, illustrating, and
testing in certain ways. Other researchers, while recognising the need for awareness on
the part of teachers of their learners’ preferred styles, emphasise the importance also of
challenging their learners to experiment with and acquire new styles or extensions of
their preferred styles (Smith & Renzulli, 1984; Reid, 1987; Larsen-Freeman & Long,
1991).
Although there has been some agreement on the existence of bipolar cognitive styles,
versions of the commonly used [FD/I] tests measure the same thing, and whether they
do not also involve general intelligence’, with only the Rod-and-Frame test1 possibly
Control (Rotter, 1966), male-female role identification (Sherman, 1967; Vaught, 1965),
Along similar lines, Griffiths and Sheen (1992: 145) make the claim, on the basis of ‘so
few substantive findings’ and the lack of consistency in measurement instruments, that
Reid (1987) has questioned the cross-cultural validity of the construct on the grounds
different range of learning style preferences. However, she uses these findings to
response to Griffiths and Sheen, and after a comprehensive overview of the nature of
the construct and the instruments for measuring it, Chapelle concludes that:
life is both too short and too complex to ignore concepts such as cognitive style which
express some of our intuitions about students and which facilitate appreciation for the
divergent approaches to thinking and learning. [...] But, personally, I believe that the
most important and relevant human constructs are those which are neither interesting to
‘authorities’ nor measurable at present.
1The apparatus used here was Oltman's (1968) portable daylight test as described in Vernon
(1972: 369).
therefore seems to be that the original construct of Witkin et al. (1977) should be
which conform to the FD/I distinction, or some similar form of these characteristics.
researchers have applied the construct in various studies. Some of the most prominent
determined education systems (Hansen, 1984; Berry and Dasen, 1974), advantages for
one cognitive style rather than the other on certain kinds of second language tests
(Readance et al., 1980; Stansfield and Hansen, 1983), and possible impacts of
mismatch between cognitive styles of teachers and learners (Frank and Davis, 1982;
Hansen and Stansfield, 1982). This last issue of cognitive style mismatch is of most
self-access context, in order for learners to feel comfortable using the materials and to
be able to progress at their own pace, it is important that the organisation of the
Hansen and Stansfield (1982: 263), for example, discuss the finding that ‘field
independence, as opposed to field dependence, has been shown to play a helpful though
effectiveness, ‘little evidence exists that either style, in and of itself, produces better all-
round teaching or learning’ (Hansen and Stansfield, 1982: 264). While urging further
investigation of the causal factors in their study, Hansen and Stansfield found that the
their study, Frank and Davis (1982) found that teachers with FI preferences produced
much better performances among both FI and FD learners than did teachers with FD
preferences. These researchers hypothesised that FD learners were more aware of the
stylistic differences between them and their (FI) teachers, and therefore attempted to
themselves.
Three major conclusions to emerge from the studies mentioned above are that analytic,
inferencing tasks of the kind represented by cloze tests favour FI-style learners, that
these kinds of tests are therefore culturally biased against members of some societies
which are more FD-oriented, and that teachers should be more aware of the variety of
their own learning style preferences and of those of their learners. As Witkin and
Goodenough (1981) found, people in agrarian and authoritarian societies are generally
more inclined towards FD than FI. Hansen (1984) also found that, of the five Pacific
Island cultures he studied, learners from Hawaii, the least agrarian society, showed the
highest incidence of FI. One additional point made by Readance et al. (1980) was that
beneficial for both types of learners, by providing them with opportunities both to work
within their own stylistic comfort zones, and to extend themselves in new ways.
from diverse L1 backgrounds learning ESL. Jamieson and Chapelle, in common with
Poulisse (1989-90, found that strategy use was task-specific in that more strategies were
evident for the more difficult task of dictation. Only one strategy, monitoring input,
was used more by lower proficiency learners, when they listened repeatedly and
carefully to the dictation passage. As regards cognitive style, the findings showed that
FI learners used less advance preparation and more output monitoring (self-reflection)
Jamieson and Chapelle collected their computer data records on learners’ working
was inferred from the length of time a learner took to respond to a question, output
monitoring was inferred from the number of times answers were edited, while the
number of times learners chose to have the audio segment repeated was used to infer
input monitoring. However, these findings should not be extended to apply to all tasks
involving computers, and should be considered with caution from two viewpoints.
Firstly, the tasks used for this study were very restrictive and computer-managed. A
different model of instructional design such as the one advocated in Chapters five and
six would produce different data. Secondly, as discussed by Pusack and Otto (1995),
computer records such as key strokes and time between strokes provide much
extraneous data that is very uninformative. In addition, such records do not have the
capacity to record more useful information such as the reasons for students’ repeated
these reasons could only be collected using the kinds of prompt questions and learner
these other factors were identified as strategies which could possibly be trained. Though
he distinguished between styles and strategies, and identified that underlying his
‘holistic’ and ‘serialist’ strategies lay distinct learning styles, comprehension and
operation learning respectively2, Pask (1976) failed to specify elements of his strategies
in such a way that they could be classified. For Pask, for example, the difference
between learning styles and strategies was that styles were more generally exhibited,
while strategies are exhibited in strictly constrained circumstances in the form of his
Schmidt (1990) examines the relationship between language processing and learning
from the perspective of consciousness. While assuming that both conscious and
2 Marton and Säljö (1976), following Craik and Lockhart (1972), make the distinction between
'surface' and 'deep' processing in memory and learning. In this approach to depth of processing,
perception is built up through a progression or series of processing stages, where increased
semantic or cognitive analysis is referred to as having greater depth. Greater depth of
processing, in turn, produces a stronger memory trace. For Marton and Säljö, the level of
processing brought into operation depends on students’ perception of the demands of
assessment: ‘the type of test expected affects the kind of processing that subjects engage in’
(1976: 115). Pask, on the other hand, claims that the requirement for understanding is ‘as
strong as, or stronger than, the requirement for deep-level processing’ (1976: 133). He goes on
to hypothesise that in classroom or self-study contexts, the requirement for understanding is
relaxed, allowing learners to be more flexible in their approach, choosing to act like ‘holists’
(comprehension learners) or ‘serialists’ (operation learners), or varying their approach,
depending on the subject matter.
separation helps to clarify some of the dilemmas faced by second language researchers.
(Schmidt & Frota, 1986) Schmidt concludes that, while there is a close association
problem solving, and all types of metacognition, belong. This becomes a crucial
distinction for the discussion in the next chapter on whether, and to what extent,
learning strategies can be taught. As much of the research in this area shows, it is the
instruction, and these strategies are also implicated in successful language learning.
by clarifying the ambiguity between passive awareness and active intent. For Schmidt,
as for us, ‘intentions may be either conscious or unconscious [...] and we may become
aware of things we do not intend to notice’ (1990: 133). Thus, learners may act
strategically, with unconscious intent, or only become aware of their strategy use when
it has noticeable positive or negative effects. In other words, ‘unconscious learning [...]
1990: 135).
Later, Brumby (1982), as a result of her research, saw the dichotomies of cognitive
style as falling into two groups: those relating to the perception of a problem; and those
distinctively more effective than the other. In this latter group, Brumby (1982: 245)
column are hierarchically ‘higher’ in that they are regarded as producing ‘better’
Brumby, however, could not overcome her dilemma of whether cognitive styles were
innate or learnt. In the light of the findings by Readance et al. (1980) that certain
‘styles’ can be trained, and considering that by ‘styles’ they were referring to what we
would now call strategies, it can be determined that while styles may be innate (though
subject to variations as described earlier), at least some strategies can be trained. Based
on the research of Willing (1989: 139 - 146), it may in fact be the case that there are
learning strategies that can be categorised as, and that are used for, becoming
Willing (1985, 1988) has identified four major learning styles as being useful for ESL
teachers of migrants to help them develop sensitivity towards their students’ individual
needs and learning styles. He classified these styles as: communicative, authority-
oriented, concrete, and analytical, where a person may change styles, but generally
tends more toward one of these styles than another (see Figure 3.4 below). Inherent in
that they will then be able to modify consciously their behaviour towards students and
choose tasks for their classes that cater for a range of learning styles in addition to their
Communicative Authority-oriented
Concrete Analytical
Other researchers have focussed on sensory preferences such as visual, auditory, kinesic
and tactile, particularly as regards cultural norms, values, and expectations (Wesche,
1981; Soudek & Soudek, 1985; Reid, 1987). Four major points which emerge from
Reid’s study of cross-cultural learning style preferences among ESL learners are that:
1) learners learn best when their styles match those of their teachers, 2) learning style
in their preferences of sensory mode, so that these need to be catered for, and 4) the
optimal approach is to provide learners with a range of activities incorporating different
learning styles. Meanwhile, Soudek and Soudek (1985: 113) conclude that ‘much of
to help our students not only switch from language to language, but also to switch
Felder and Henriques (1995) also support the need for multiplicity of exposure to
different learning styles, having first identified what those styles might be in any given
For Gremmo and Riley (1995), it is important that we avoid categorising learners, but
instead concentrate on using the research findings on learning styles to help us set up a
way, we can ‘help learners come to terms with their strengths and weaknesses, to learn a
language efficiently in ways which are compatible with their personalities’ (1995: 158).
Similarly, Ehrman and Oxford (1995: 70) advocate learners ‘using strategies that are
not necessarily directly related to their preferred styles’ and that ‘indeed, to learn
effectively, every student must be able to do this some of the time’. This also reflects
This, then, is the approach taken in the software package discussed in Chapters 5 and 6:
a range of tasks and media are provided, and these tasks are designed to incorporate a
strategies, some of which may not come naturally to them. Returning to the question of
the distinction between ‘styles’ and ‘strategies’, it may be the case that styles, which
emerge at the perceptual level, are innate and unchangeable preferences, but that
learners can be trained to become aware of, though not prefer, different styles. Other
‘styles’ emerge at the application level and can be varied by any individual learner,
according to the tasks and context. As identified above, these styles are better identified
materials need to be designed to cater for differences in perceptual style. For application
styles or ‘strategies’, however, learners can be trained, or made aware of, the range of
more effective strategies that can be used when dealing with tasks of varying cognitive
or contextual demands (Oxford, 1993). Lawrence (1984) and O’Malley and Chamot
(1993: 109) also make the claim for ‘a tendency to use certain learning tools (learning
strategies) and to avoid others’, as one interpretation of the term learning style.
when left to their own devices, and if not overly pressured by their environment to use a
certain set of strategies, students typically use learning strategies that reflect their basic
learning style (Ehrman & Oxford 1989). They can, however, learn to develop additional
learning strategies and test the value of ones they already use.
on learning strategies. Scarcella and Oxford’s comment above raises critical questions
of how additional learning strategies can be developed, how learners can be brought to
want to develop or change strategies they already use, which strategies can and should
orientation, and attitudes towards and beliefs about language learning, or the learning of
the language of a specific culture, are also critical in the use of learning styles and
strategies for successful language learning. In fact, according to Gremmo and Riley ‘it
is important to keep in mind the fact that any form of self-directed learning scheme is
an intervention in the social knowledge system, that is, in the set of structures and
legitimization and storage of knowledge’ (1995: 161). The next section will deal with
Prominent among these are learners’ attitudes towards each other, and towards the
target language and target language culture, as well as their motivations to learn. For
changeable characteristics are equally important, if not more so, than those that are less
amenable to change. Where there is the possibility for learners to make changes within
themselves, and for the software program to enhance this process, the functions of the
instructional design model change from that of providing flexibility and variety, to
point out: ‘regardless of whether we believe motivation to be the single primary factor,
dismiss the importance of motivation in language learning’ (1992: 52). Crookes and
Schmidt (1989) define motivation as comprising two equally important factors: external
after interruptions), and activity level. Four factors characterise the internal perspective
Both motivation and attitude are important to the learning process, whether before,
during, or after formal instruction (Scarcella & Oxford, 1992; Skehan, 1989). In turn,
there are a variety of factors which have been shown to influence motivation, though
questions as to the direction of this influence are frequently raised. Thus, while studies
have included parental and teacher attitudes (Feenstra, 1969; Schumann and Schumann,
1977; Buttjes, 1991; Keller, 1991), politics (Byram, 1991), and materials orientation
(Risager, 1991), it is still unclear whether these factors increase or decrease motivation,
or, alternatively, whether it is the level of motivation which affects the factors studied.
Skehan (1989: 49) hypothesises a distinction between Intrinsic motivation, which stems
from the latter area of research have been relatively inconclusive, with surprisingly low
correlations found between motivational levels and success. One explanation for these
motivating factors investigated. Many of these studies have also been criticised for their
lack of rigour in method and design (Skehan, 1989: 64–7). Such studies have, for
attitudes of first and fifth year students towards the target culture (German).
Gardner (1980) has postulated various models to account for influencing factors in
a sum of three factors: effort, desire to achieve a goal, and attitudes (Gardner, 1985: 9–
10). For Gardner, integrative motivation, the desire to achieve integration into the target
culture through learning the language, was preferred over instrumental motivation, or
learning the language to achieve a goal more effectively. However, he has since
Gillette (1987) has shown that Rubin and Thompson’s (1982) argument for pragmatism
learning Indonesian in Australia, where the second language is being learnt in a foreign
language context, that is, the language being learnt is not the language of the majority
population.
Dickinson (1995 166) finds evidence that ‘learning autonomy increases motivation and
motivation (Deci & Ryan, 1985) and attribution theory. In addition, he cites the claims
of the Carnegie Project (deCharms, 1984) that ‘motivation can be enhanced through
encouraging learners to exert personal control over their learning and to take
responsibility for it’ (Dickinson, 1995: 166). As has been shown earlier, learners can
achieve this through an understanding of the values of such an approach, an awareness
supportive environment.
However, the issue of orientation (positive or negative) and attitude towards the target
language and the target language culture remains a strong influencing factor in extrinsic
The latter, however, is more difficult to define, with researchers differing in their
attributions of the same factors. Attitude and orientation are the most extensively
researched aspects of this problematic area, though Peirce (1995) has strong arguments
for a redefinition of the terms under which these aspects are investigated, to include the
multiple social identities of the individual. Peirce (1995: 11–12) argues that ‘although
muted, there is an uneasy recognition by some [SLA] theorists that current theory about
the relationship between the language learner and the social world is problematic’.
She then extends this statement to cite the ambiguities found in various studies of
anxiety (Scovel, 1978), and personality variables and language achievement (Gardner
and McIntyre, 1993), as deriving from the fundamental misapprehension that ‘language
learners can choose under what conditions they will interact with members of the target
language community’ (Peirce, 1995: 12), and that this choice is learner-motivated.
However, for Peirce, many of these ambiguities can be addressed when the learner’s
multiple social identities, across time and different sites, are taken into account. Before
taking full consideration of Peirce’s theory, and its conjunction with the sociocultural
model proposed earlier in this work, we need to look at the other major theories
‘interlingual’ identification as a possible explanation for the fact that some learners just
do not want to sound like native speakers of the target language. She also proposed
learners’ desire to maintain their L1 as another possible cause. Two major hypotheses
Speech Accommodation Theory (Giles & Byrne, 1982; Beebe, 1988) and Schumann’s
Speech Accommodation Theory has developed to help account for the degree to which
different individuals are able to integrate with the target culture, by ‘explaining the
interaction between speakers in terms of their feelings, values, and motives’ (Beebe,
1988: 62). According to this theory, broadly speaking, the degree of learners’
identification with the people of the target culture is in direct proportion to the degree
more linguistically proficient, that is, closer to the speech of the target culture, the more
they are accepted by or integrated with that culture. This in turn produces the desired
social and communicative advantages, as, being accepted, they come to be regarded as
part of the in-group (Tajfel, 1974), thereby benefiting from more positive attitudes on
the relative desirability of, the target culture and language (Schumann uses the terms
one social group, and the degree of contact between this group and another, the
members of which speak another language. Psychological distance, on the other hand,
involves affective factors more central to the individual such as culture shock, different
As McLaughlin puts it: ‘[...] social and psychological distance influence second-
language acquisition by determining the amount of contact learners have with the target
language and the degree to which they are open to the input that is available’
(1987:111). Thus, when the distance is perceived to be great, the learner may miss out
and therefore the learner’s interlanguage (Selinker, 1971) develops no further in the
direction of the target language. However, the validity of this argument is questioned as
to the direction of causality between attitude and motivation. It remains unclear, and,
indeed, may vary with individuals, as to whether a positive attitude motivates a learner
indeed, whether the two factors are mutually supportive. This remains a contentious
For the purposes of the present work, and the determination of the influences of
the direction of causality diminishes the value and applicability of Schumann’s model
causal factor, no effective action can reasonably be taken to improve one or the other.
advocated by theorists in the field such as Oxford (1994), Gardner and Tremblay
(1994), MacIntyre (1995; 1995a), and Sparks and Ganschow (1995), we must await
further empirical research in the area to test the various aspects of theories currently
being propagated.
relationship between various social and psychological factors and language learning.
Macnamara (1973) and Genesee and Hamayan (1980), for example, found evidence
that younger learners are less prone to fixed attitudes which affect their language
learning than older learners. From his studies of Anglophone students learning French
in bilingual contexts in Canada, Gardner (1980) suggests that attitude seems more
critical in second, as against foreign, language learning contexts, where much more
contact is possible between learners and speakers of the target language. In a contrastive
study of learners in a foreign language context, Dörnyei (1990) finds that for such
learners, beliefs about, and attitudes towards, the target culture are less specific, and
integrative motivation (see section 3.5.1) is much lower than for learners in a L2
context. He also finds that integrative motivation may be necessary for learners to
Both these trends are supported by Oxford (1994a), Dörnyei (1994), and Oxford and
Shearin (1994). Oxford and Shearin also explore the implications of motivational
factors from other discipline areas, including Need Theories (Maslow, 1970),
Reinforcement Theories Landy (1985), and Social Cognition Theories (Locke &
Latham, 1990). As a result of their analysis, they suggest a number of ways in which
teachers can increase their learners’ motivation, including identifying their motivational
range of (instrumental) motivational factors that may correspond to the needs and
field, these are fertile areas for future empirical and ethnographic research. For the
purposes of our CELL instructional design model, these issues of motivation are
addressed through the provision for learner control over the materials. As will be
discussed in detail in the following chapter, learner motivation to learn has been shown
to increase when learners have control over the paths their learning takes (Nakhoul,
Robinson, 1991).
To sum up, in the words of Scarcella and Oxford ‘[...] the arguments about instrumental
vs. integrative motivation that have dominated the research literature might be less
important than the absolute degree of motivation possessed by the individual learner’
(1992: 53). As illustrated by Oxford and Nyikos (1989) mentioned above, and Genesee,
Rogers and Holobow (1983), motivation can often be the catalyst needed to progress in
one’s language learning, by stimulating the search for, and acquisition of, more
(Ellis, 1994).
3.6 Conclusion
To reframe this discussion of individual differences within a sociocultural framework,
it is useful to return to the contentions of Peirce (1995), that individuals differ, not only
from one another, but also within themselves from one point in time to another, and
from one context to another. Having reviewed the literature on individual differences
learning styles, and motivation, we see that social factors, as much as psychological or
neurological factors influence a learner’s capacity for, and success in language learning.
Three critical elements emerge from this wide-ranging discussion. Firstly, that learners
Secondly, that learners need to move out of their individual ‘comfort zones’ in order to
participate productively and effectively in the learning process, and to carry the
learning beyond the immediate task to novel situations. Thirdly, that learners need
information, support, and the infrastructure to negotiate this development – that they
Because of the indeterminacy pervading most of the findings in these areas, we cannot,
influences in order that language learning can be enhanced for as many learners as
provision of materials based on a variety of media and content, and utilising a range of
task responses which favour different learning styles and sensory preferences.
teachability of language learning strategies will be examined in the light of research and
listening and viewing comprehension theories, and the concept of learner- and learning-
centred instruction, we will then be able to construct the framework for the multimedia