Behavior
Behavior
ESTER F. C. SLEDDENS
Department of Health Promotion
NUTRIM School for Nutrition, Toxicology and Metabolism
Maastricht University Medical Center+
Method
Respondents and Procedure
United States English-speaking caregivers of children ages 3 to 5 years
were recruited for this cross-sectional online survey about child tempera-
ment, in which both the 36-item and the one-item temperament scale were
administered. Potential participants were approached from July 2010 un-
til February 2011 by (a) posting flyers in the vicinity of the Texas Medical
Center, community centers, public libraries, universities, sports centers,
and museums throughout Houston, Texas; (b) posting the study on the
website of Baylor College of Medicine and the Children’s Nutrition Re-
search Center (CNRC); and (c) listing the study in the CNRC’s nationally
distributed newsletter and recruiting from the participant database. Com-
pletion time for the survey was approximately 10 minutes. From all com-
pleted entries, two drawings were conducted for two $100 gift cards. Only
participants who agreed to take part in the raffles (n = 224) had a chance to
win one of the gift cards.
A total of 301 participants entered the website. Of those, 13 were dis-
qualified because they did not agree to participate (n = 2), did not have a
3- to 5-year-old child (n = 10), or did not live with the child during most
of the week (n = 1). Another 51 entries were deleted from the database: 11
were duplicates (assessed by checking email and IP addresses) and 40 par-
ticipants did not complete the 36-item Children’s Behavior Questionnaire
scale. The final data set contained 237 participants, mostly female caregiv-
ers (93.2%). Children’s mean age was 3.9 yr. (SD = 0.8), and sex of the child
was almost equally divided (boys: 53.6%, girls: 46.4%). The study was re-
viewed and approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Baylor Col-
lege of Medicine; all caregivers completed online informed consent prior
to data collection.
Measures
Background characteristics.—Parents or guardians were asked to report
some family demographics in the beginning of the online survey includ-
ing child’s age, sex, and their relationship to the child. The participant’s
sex, race, living situation, highest educational level attained, employment,
and annual household income were assessed at the end of the survey.
Temperament scales.—The 36-item scale of the Children’s Behavior
Questionnaire (Putnam & Rothbart, 2006) was used to assess the three
broad factors of temperament: Surgency, Negative Affectivity, and Effort-
ful Control, consisting of 12 statements for each trait. Parents were asked
to report using a 7-point scale, with anchors 1: Extremely untrue of your
child and 7: Extremely true of your child, on how well each statement de-
scribed their child’s reaction to a given situation within the past 6 months;
Validation of CBQ and One-Item Temperament Scale 201
a “not applicable” option was provided if parents could not answer be-
cause they had not seen their child in the situation described. The Chil-
dren’s Behavior Questionnaire higher order factors have been replicated
across multiple age samples among different countries and showed ad-
equate internal consistency reliability ranging from .61 to .94 (e.g., Aha-
di, et al., 1993; Kochanska, De Vet, Goldman, Murray, & Putnam, 1994;
Rothbart, et al., 2001; Majdandžić & Van den Boom, 2007; Sleddens, et al.,
2011). The very short scale (Putnam & Rothbart, 2006; Hughes, et al., 2008;
Ward, et al., 2008; O’Connor, et al., 2010; Sleddens, et al., 2011) also showed
adequate internal consistency (Cronbach’s alphas ranging from .63 to .76
for Surgency, from .65 to .79 for Negative Affectivity, and from .67 to .74
for Effortful Control). Convergent validity included parental agreement
(Rothbart, et al., 2001; Majdandžić & Van den Boom, 2007; Majdandžić, Van
den Boom, & Heesbeen, 2008) and prediction of social (Rothbart, Ahadi,
& Hershey, 1994; Rotbhart, et al., 2001) and laboratory behavior patterns
(Kochanska, Murray, Jacques, Koenig, & Vandegeest, 1996; Majdandžić &
Van den Boom, 2007; Majdandžić, et al., 2008).
One-item scale.—The one-item temperament scale included three re-
sponse options reflecting the three higher order temperament factors (Sur-
gency, Negative Affectivity, and Effortful Control) from the Children’s
Behavior Questionnaire. For a description of the one-item scale and op-
erational definitions, see Table 1. This one-item scale was developed by
two of the authors (SH and TO) and agreed upon by the whole group. The
two co-authors (SH and TO), one an expert in child development (SH) and
the other a pediatrician (TO), wrote single item statements that reflected
Table 1
Single-item Temperament Scale
Please read the statements below and select which one best
describes your 3- to 5-year-old child (select just the best one)
The “Surgency/Extraversion” child
This child has lots of energy, is easily excited, and often goes fast on the playground. This
child enjoys meeting new people and going to new places.
The “Negative Affectivity” child
This child often shows his frustration or discomfort, and easily becomes sad when not
able to finish a project. This child is often afraid of the dark, and when upset may be dif-
ficult to calm down.
The “Effortful Control” child
This child likes to listen to rhymes and songs. When working on a project, this child can
concentrate deeply, and carefully follows rules and instructions. When something chang-
es, this child quickly notices.
Note.—Operational definition of the three higher order temperament factors: Surgency (ten-
dency to perform impulsive and active behavior), Negative Affectivity (predisposition to ex-
perience negative affective states), and Effortful Control (the ability of a child to control at-
tentional processes and behavior).
202 E. F. C. SLEdDens, et al.
the general theory and the items (Rothbart, et al., 2001; Putnam & Roth-
bart, 2006) for each of the three major child temperament factors. Each vi-
gnette attempted to capture the original subscales defined by the higher
order factor (Rothbart, et al., 2001): Surgency by impulsivity, lack of shy-
ness, activity level, and high intensity pleasure; Negative Affectivity by
anger, discomfort, sadness, difficulty to sooth, and fear; and Effortful Con-
trol by inhibitory control, attentional focusing, low intensity pleasure, and
perceptual sensitivity. Each statement was reviewed and revised by the
co-authors until consensus was reached about optimal wording. A single
item was selected to identify the dominant temperament characteristic of
the child in order to simplify the task of tailoring an intervention to the
child’s temperament.
Statistical Analyses
Classical test theory item analysis, 36-item temperament scale.—Rele-
vant scale items of the 36-item scale were combined to create mean fac-
tor scores for Surgency, Negative Affectivity, and Effortful Control. Items
marked “does not apply” were not included in the scores. Missing data
were not present because the participants were forced to answer all of the
questions or the computer would not progress. Internal consistency reli-
ability was evaluated by calculating both Cronbach’s alpha and average
corrected item-total correlations. The minimum acceptable cutoff point for
Cronbach’s alphas was .70, and for item-scale correlations cutoff points
were between .15 and .30 (Nunnally & Bernstein, 1994). Correlations of .30
or above were considered good, and below .15 were considered unreliable
since they would indicate lack of homogeneity of the items within a scale.
Principal factor analysis, 36-item temperament scale.—Principal factor
analysis (exploratory in nature) with oblique rotation yielded factor load-
ings for each item as well as the percent variance explained by each factor.
In accordance with the original validation studies (Rothbart, et al., 2001;
Putnam & Rothbart, 2006), a factor loading of .25 or higher was an indi-
cation of a reasonably high factor loading. Items with no numerical re-
sponse were imputed with the mean score of the factor to which the item
belonged. On average, respondents chose the “not applicable” option less
than 1% of the time. The frequency of “not applicable” responses was
identified for each item. Item 6 (“Prepares for trips and outings by plan-
ning things he/she will need”) had the largest number of “not applicable”
answers (n = 12, 5.1%). To verify the robustness of the results, a principal
factor analysis was performed on only complete cases (n = 183), excluding
54 respondents with missing values. Parallel analysis was performed us-
ing the SPSS syntax (O’Connor, 2000) to determine accurately the number
of factors to retain in the factor analysis. The program was forced to gen-
erate 1,000 samples of random number matrices and corresponding ei-
Validation of CBQ and One-Item Temperament Scale 203
genvalues, each of which was based on 183 cases and 36 variables. The
eigenvalues resulting from this procedure were compared with the eigen-
values found following principal factor analysis on the data. Factors were
retained when eigenvalues from the actual data (following principal fac-
tor analysis) were greater than the eigenvalues from the random data.
Item response modeling analyses, 36-item temperament scale.—Rasch mod-
eling (partial credit model) was performed on all cases using the “Con-
Quest” software program (Wu, Adams, & Haldane, 2007) which provided
item parameter difficulty estimates, item fit statistics, Wright maps, and
reliability indices. Item difficulty refers to the item’s location on the un-
derlying temperament trait, and reflects how difficult it was for a per-
son to respond affirmatively to an item (higher values = more difficult).
Item fit was determined by computing weighted mean square fit statis-
tics for each item, which indicated whether residuals varied as much as
expected given the observed distribution. Items with a weighted fit sta-
tistic < .75 or > 1.33, and for which the weighted t statistic was < −2.00 or
> 2.00, were considered to fit poorly (Adams & Khoo, 1996). The Wright
map visually linked the distribution of respondents (indicated by Xs on
the left side of the Wright map) on the latent temperament factors to the
distribution of individual item difficulties (represented on the right side
by item number), using the same metric, i.e., a logit scale centered at a
mean of zero. The items and respondents should be located between −3
and +3 logits. Also included in the Wright map is the location of the items
by threshold (the 7-point Likert-type scale is separated by six threshold
points, where Threshold 1 refers to the threshold between response op-
tion 1: Extremely untrue of your child and response option 2: Quite un-
true of your child. Item separation reliability, which indicated “how well
the sample of subjects had spread the items along the measure of the test”
(Fisher, 1992, p. 238), were calculated for the three factors. Finally, visual
examination of the item response functions evaluated the functioning of
the 7-point response format.
Analysis of variance and Pearson chi-square analyses of the one-item tem-
perament scale.—The average scores of the three temperament factors (Sur-
gency, Negative Affectivity, Effortful Control) were compared for each of
the three temperament groups as defined by the one-item temperament
scale. This was conducted by means of a three-level group ANOVA (Sur-
gency, Negative Affectivity, Effortful Control), followed by Bonferroni and
Tukey’s HSD post hoc analyses, adjusting p values for multiple testing to
detect differences in mean scores on each of the three temperament fac-
tors between the three groups. Pearson chi-square analyses with categori-
cal variables were used with a 3 × 3 contingency table comparing tempera-
ment characteristics of the children, defined by parents using the one-item
204 E. F. C. SLEdDens, et al.
scale, and the 36-item scale. Mean scores were calculated for the three tem-
perament factors of Surgency, Negative Affectivity, and Effortful Control,
and a child was characterized by a temperament characteristic based on
the highest mean score across the three factors.
Results
The study sample was ethnically diverse (Table 2). The majority were
White (39.2%), but Hispanics (25.3%) and African-Americans (23.6%) were
also represented. A minority of the participants were combined into “oth-
er,” consisting of American Indians, Native Hawaiians, Pacific Islanders,
and Asians (10.5%). In total, 209 participants (88.2%) were from Texas
(United States). The other 28 participants reported living in other states
within the United States (n = 24), Canada (n = 1), or these data were missing
(n = 3). Most participants indicated they were married or lived with a signif-
icant other (75.5%). A majority reported higher levels of education (59.1%
indicated having a college degree or higher) and high annual household
income (46.0% indicated a household income above $60,000 a year).
Psychometric Properties of the 36-item Temperament Scale
Classical test theory item analysis.—Internal consistency (Cronbach’s al-
pha) ranged from .69 to .78 (Table 3). The average item-total correlations
among the three factors suggested good consistency of the items (Surgen-
cy = .41, Negative Affectivity = .37, Effortful Control = .33). For the Surgen-
cy factor, the corrected item-total correlations of all items were above the
value of .30. For the two other higher order factors, some items fell below
the value of .30, but still above the critical cut-off point of .15 (Negative Af-
fectivity = two items; Effortful Control = five items). One Effortful Control
item had an item-total correlation (after correlation for overlap) of .05, far
below the critical value of .15. Cronbach’s alpha increased from .69 to .71
when this item was removed.
Principal factor analysis.—Principal factor analysis revealed 12 factors
having eigenvalues greater than 1.00. Forcing a three-factor structure, as
proposed by Rothbart, et al. (2001) and Putnam and Rothbart (2006), re-
vealed the three higher order factors Surgency, Negative Affectivity, and
Effortful Control (Table 3). Results of the parallel procedure (eigenvalues
from random data were lower than the eigenvalues of the three factors
found following PFA) supported the retention of the three-factor struc-
ture. The three-factor solution accounted for 25.1% of the total variance.
Most of the variance was explained by the Surgency factor (9.4%) with Ef-
fortful Control explaining 8.4% and Negative Affectivity explaining 7.3%,
respectively. Item 30 on Effortful Control (“My child approaches places
he/she has been told are dangerous slowly and cautiously”) had a factor
loading below .25 (factor loading of .09). Factor analyses on the complete
Validation of CBQ and One-Item Temperament Scale 205
Table 2
Participant Characteristics (N = 237)
Description n %
Sex
Male 16 6.8
Female 221 93.2
Relationship to child
Parent 227 95.8
Grandmother 5 2.1
Aunt 4 1.7
Female guardian 1 0.4
Race
Black or African-American 56 23.6
White or Euro-American 93 39.2
Hispanic 60 25.3
Other 25 10.5
No response 3 1.3
Living situation
Married or living with a significant other 179 75.5
Single, never married 21 8.9
Divorced, separated or widowed 35 14.8
No response 2 0.8
Education
Attended some high school 12 0.8
High school graduate or GED 17 7.2
Technical school 12 5.1
Some college 64 27.0
College graduate 72 30.4
Post-graduate study 68 28.7
No response 2 0.8
Employed
Yes 160 67.5
No 75 31.6
No response 2 0.8
Annual household income
Less than $10,000 10 4.2
$10,000–$19,999 15 6.3
$20,000–$39,999 45 19.0
$40,000–$59,999 56 23.6
$60,000 or more 109 46.0
No response 2 0.8
cases (n = 183) obtained similar results (data not reported in table), and the
factor loading of Item 30 only slightly improved from .09 to .11.
Item response modeling analyses.—All items of the three higher order
factors (Surgency, Negative Affectivity, and Effortful Control) had both
weighted mean square statistic and t statistic within the acceptable rang-
es (Table 3). Fig. 1 presents the item-respondent Wright map for the three
Table 3
206
Psychometric Properties for the Items of the 36-item Scale of the Children’s Behavior
Questionnaire Clustered by the Three Higher Order Temperament Factors
No. 36-item Scale Children’s Behavior Questionnaire M SD CITC Factor Est. SE MNSQ t
Surgency/Extraversion, Factor 1: Cronbach’s alpha = .78 4.59 0.86
25 Is full of energy, even in the evening 5.80 1.23 0.41 0.47 −0.69 0.04 0.97 −0.2
4 Likes going down high slides or other adventurous activities 5.56 1.56 0.34 0.473 −0.37 0.03 1.08 0.8
16 Likes to go high and fast when pushed on a swing 5.27 1.60 0.38 0.46 −0.23 0.03 1.05 0.6
28 Likes rough and rowdy games 5.03 1.68 0.50 0.58 −0.13 0.03 0.94 −0.6
13 Prefers quiet activities to active games (R) 4.99 1.29 0.38 0.46 −0.11 0.04 0.99 −0.1
31 Is slow and unhurried in deciding what to do next (R) 4.38 1.51 0.37 0.45 0.02 0.03 1.03 0.4
E. F. C. SLEdDens, et al.
10 Seems to be at ease with almost any person 4.42 1.84 0.44 0.46 0.09 0.03 1.05 0.7
19 Takes a long time in approaching new situations (R) 4.33 1.49 0.49 0.542 0.10 0.03 0.94 −0.7
22 Is sometimes shy even around people (s)he has known a long
time (R) 4.14 1.94 0.41 0.42 0.16 0.03 1.09 1.1
1 Seems always in a big hurry to get from one place to another 3.94 1.68 0.41 0.49 0.28 0.03 1.03 0.4
7 Often rushes into new situations 3.86 1.54 0.50 0.603 0.39 0.03 0.93 −0.9
34 Sometimes turns away shyly from new acquaintances (R) 3.37 1.67 0.36 0.363 0.49 0.11 1.08 0.9
Negative Affectivity, Factor 2: Cronbach’s alpha = .74 4.13 0.85
2 Gets quite frustrated when prevented from doing something
(s)he wants to do 5.41 1.38 0.31 0.431 −0.51 0.03 1.04 0.4
32 Gets angry when (s)he can’t find something (s)he wants to play
with 4.41 1.56 0.50 0.59 −0.13 0.03 0.89 − 1.4
5 Is quite upset by a little cut or bruise 4.14 1.73 0.47 0.52 −0.06 0.03 0.92 −1.1
35 Becomes upset when loved relatives or friends are getting ready
to leave following a visit 4.48 1.81 0.28 0.28 −0.08 0.10 1.15 1.8
8 Tends to become sad if the family’s plans don’t work out 4.40 1.48 0.45 0.53 −0.06 0.03 0.91 −1.0
11 Is afraid of burglars or the ‘boogie man’ 4.20 1.84 0.30 0.34 −0.03 0.03 1.08 1.0
26 Is not afraid of the dark (R) 3.69 1.93 0.26 0.35 −0.01 0.03 1.11 1.4
20 Hardly ever complains when ill with a cold (R) 3.70 1.64 0.35 0.42 0.09 0.03 0.99 −0.1
29 Is not very upset at minor cuts or bruises (R) 3.89 1.63 0.37 0.40 0.12 0.03 1.02 0.3
23 Is very difficult to soothe when (s)he has become upset 3.58 1.77 0.34 0.45 0.16 0.03 1.03 0.4
(continued on next page)
Table 3 (cont’d)
Psychometric Properties for the Items of the 36-item Scale of the Children’s Behavior
Questionnaire Clustered by the Three Higher Order Temperament Factors
207
208 E. F. C. SLEdDens, et al.
Fig. 1a. Item-respondent map for Surgen- Fig. 1b. Item-respondent map for Negative
cy. N = 237; each “X” represents 1.4 cases; Affectivity. N = 237; each “X” represents 1.6
*items by thresholds show the item first cases; *items by thresholds show the item
followed by the threshold (e.g., 7.4 refers to first followed by the threshold (e.g., 8.4 re-
Item 7 and Threshold 4). fers to Item 8 and Threshold 4).
210 E. F. C. SLEdDens, et al.
Fig. 1c. Item-respondent map for Effortful Control. N = 237; each “X” represents 1.8 cases;
*items by thresholds show the item first followed by the threshold (e.g., 9.4 refers to Item 9
and Threshold 4).
Validation of CBQ and One-Item Temperament Scale 211
Table 4
Average Scores on the Three Factors of the 36-item Temperament
Scale for Parents Classifying their Children on the One-item
Scale and p Values For Pairwise Between-Groups Comparisons
36-item Scale One-item Temperament Scale (1–2) (1–3) (2–3)
(3 Factors)
Surgency Negativ Effortful
(n = 108) (1) Affectivity Control (n = 99)
(n = 30) (2) (3)
M SD M SD M SD
Surgency 5.02 0.72 4.30 0.96 4.22 0.75 < .001* < .001* 1.0
Negative Affectivity 4.08 0.83 4.68 0.78 4.00 0.84 .002* 1.0 < .001*
Effortful Control 5.18 0.68 4.92 0.72 5.45 0.70 .209 .014* .001*
Note.—N = 237; using Bonferroni correction for adjusting post hoc multiple comparisons;
statistical significance was assigned at the p < .05 level; *p value remained significant using
Tukey α, 36-item Temperament Scale with answer options ranging from 1 to 7.
ured by the two methods. A sensitivity score was calculated to identify the
percentage of caregivers identifying their child on a particular tempera-
ment trait with both temperament scales (36-item scale is the “gold stand-
ard”). Thus, it is the probability a parent correctly identifies their child on
a particular temperament trait. This score ranged from 54.17% for Nega-
tive Affectivity to 76.79% for Surgency. However, a substantial number of
participants (n = 56) characterized their child as “Surgency” on the one-
item temperament scale, but the highest mean score was for the Effortful
Control factor (Table 5).
Secondary analyses showed that for the group of children whose
parents chose “Surgency” on the one-item scale, the means of the three
temperament factors were: Surgency 4.71 (SD = 0.56), Negative Affectiv-
ity 4.04 (SD = 0.77), and Effortful Control 5.61 (SD = 0.44). Pearson’s corre-
lation analysis showed that Surgency and Effortful Control were related
(r = .35, p < .01), whereas the correlations between Effortful Control-Nega-
tive Affectivity and Surgency–Negative Affectivity were not statistically
significant. Thirteen participants identified their child’s temperament as
“Negative Affectivity” on both scales. Thirteen other participants also de-
scribed their child’s temperament as being “Negative Affectivity” using
the one-item scale, but had a higher mean score on Effortful Control using
the 36-item Children’s Behavior Questionnaire scale (Table 5). Secondary
analyses showed that the means for this group of children for the three
temperament factors were as follows: Surgency 3.76 (SD = 0.68), Negative
Affectivity 4.29 (SD = 0.81), and Effortful Control 5.45 (SD = 0.62) on the
one-item scale. Findings of the correlation analysis showed that none of
the temperament factors were significantly correlated.
Discussion
The psychometric properties of the very short scale of the Children’s
Behavior Questionnaire, using classical test theory, principal factor analy-
sis, and item response modeling, were found to be acceptable to outstand-
ing. These methods complement each other and are needed for a thorough
psychometric evaluation of the temperament scale. The factor analytic
results closely resembled those of the original United States (Putnam &
Rothbart, 2006) and Dutch studies (Sleddens, et al., 2011), after forcing the
principal factor analysis to retrieve three factors. Most of the variance in
the present study was explained by Surgency, in contrast to the study of
Putnam and Rothbart (2006) and the Dutch study of Sleddens, et al. (2011),
where Negative Affectivity was the factor accounting for the highest vari-
ance. Factor loadings in the present study were slightly higher than in the
Dutch study, which may have been due to the Children’s Behavior Ques-
tionnaire in this study being administered in isolation from the longer
scales of the Children’s Behavior Questionnaire, or to differences in cul-
Validation of CBQ and One-Item Temperament Scale 213
Heesch, K. C., Mâsse, L. C., & Dunn, A. L. (2006) Using Rasch modeling to re-evaluate
three scales related to physical activity: enjoyment, perceived benefits and per-
ceived barriers. Health Education Research, 21, i58-i72.
Hughes, S. O., Shewchuk, R. M., Baskin, M. L., Nicklas, T. A., & Qu, H. (2008) Indul-
gent feeding style and children’s weight status in preschool. Journal of Develop-
mental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 29, 403-410. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097%2FDBP.0b
013e318182a976.
Kochanska, G., Aksan, N., & Joy, M. E. (2007) Children’s fearfulness as a moderator
of parenting in early socialization: two longitudinal studies. Developmental Psy-
chology, 43, 222-237.
Kochanska, G., De Vet, K., Goldman, M., Murray, K., & Putnam, S. P. (1994) Mater-
nal reports of conscience development and temperament in young children. Child
Development, 65, 852-868.
Kochanska, G., Murray, K., Jacques, T. Y., Koenig, A. L., & Vandegeest, K. A. (1996) In-
hibitory control in young children and its role in emerging internalization. Child
Development, 67, 490-507. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111%2Fj.1467-8624.1996.tb01747.x.
Majdandžić, M., & Van den Boom, D. C. (2007) Multimethod longitudinal assessment
of temperament in early childhood. Journal of Personality, 75, 121-168.
Majdandžić, M., Van den Boom, D. C., & Heesbeen, D. G. M. (2008) Peas in a pod:
biases in the measurement of sibling temperament? Developmental Psychology, 44,
1354-1368.
Miller, K. S., Vannatta, K., Compas, B. E., Vasey, M., McGoron, K. D., Salley, C. G., &
Gerhardt, C. A. (2009) The role of coping and temperament in the adjustment
of children with cancer. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 34, 1135-1143. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1093%2Fjpepsy%2Fjsp037.
Nunnally, J. C., & Bernstein, I. H. (1994) Psychometric theory. (2nd ed.) New York:
McGraw-Hill.
Nygaard, E., Smith, L., & Torgersen, A. M. (2002) Temperament in children with
Down syndrome and in prematurely born children. Scandinavian Journal of Psy-
chology, 43, 61-71.
O’Connor, B. P. (2000) SPSS and SAS programs for determining the number of com-
ponents using parallel analysis and Velicer’s MAP test. Behavior Research Meth-
ods, Instrumentation, and Computers, 32, 396-402. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017%2
FS1368980009005916.
O’Connor, T. M., Hughes, S. O., Watson, K. B., Baranowski, T., Nicklas, T. A., Fisher,
J. O., Beltran, A., Baranowski, J. C., Qu, H., & Shewchuk, R. M. (2010) Parent-
ing practices are associated with fruit and vegetable consumption in pre-school
children. Public Health Nutrition, 13, 91-101.
Putnam, S. P., & Rothbart, M. K. (2006) Development of short and very short forms of
the Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment, 87, 102-112.
Putnam, S. P., Sanson, A. V., & Rothbart, M. K. (2002) Child temperament and par-
enting. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting. Vol. 1. Mahwah, NJ: Erl-
baum. Pp. 255-277.
Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., & Evans, D. E. (2000) Temperament and personality:
origins and outcomes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 122-135.
Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., & Hershey, K. L. (1994) Temperament and social be-
havior in childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 40, 21-39.
Validation of CBQ and One-Item Temperament Scale 217