BOWES - An Exploratory Study of The Use of Imagery by Vocal Professionals
BOWES - An Exploratory Study of The Use of Imagery by Vocal Professionals
BOWES - An Exploratory Study of The Use of Imagery by Vocal Professionals
Scholar Commons
Graduate School Theses and Dissertations Graduate School
6-1-2009
This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at Scholar Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in
Graduate School Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Scholar Commons. For more information, please contact
[email protected].
An Exploratory Study of the Use of Imagery by Vocal Professionals:
by
Date of Approval
June 4, 2009
This dissertation is dedicated to my heart and joy, my son, David Alistair Bowes.
I further dedicate this study to all the current and future generations of imaginative and
creative performers around the world. I am hopeful that this investigation in imagery
helps to light your way and gives you greater ease in achieving optimal performance.
Acknowledgements
deeply indebted to my Committee Chair, C. Victor Fung for his tireless diligence and
integrity, whose direction and support, made dissertation possible and successful. To the
members of this research committee, Sheila Woodward, Janet Moore, and Constance
Hines, I offer my heartfelt thanks for all your support, guidance, and outstanding effort.
To the participants of this study, whose identities remain confidential, I thank you for all
the time, effort, and thoughtfulness you shared with me through the entire process and
finally supporting the completion of this research. Your contributions were most valuable
and I especially thank you for listening to your inner voice and developing your imagery
debriefings and coding the transcriptions. To the many thousands of students I have
taught over the course of my life, thank you for helping me begin to learn and understand
imagery, creativity, excellence, and how all these work in life and in education. To all my
many teachers through the years who have taught me to be a better musician, artist, actor,
and teacher. To my mother, Paula Bowes, thank you for graciously editing one of the
early drafts. To my son, David, who offered kind support, excellent meals, and
and indebted to Ronald T. Jeffers, for his kind and generous support, dedication, and
helpful assistance in seeing me through all the levels of this eventful process.
Table of Contents
Abstract..........................................................................................................................ix
i
When or Time of Imagery Use .............................................................. 62
What: Content of Images ....................................................................... 64
What: Imagery Types ............................................................................ 65
Execution Images ...................................................................... 65
Metaphoric/Artistic Images ........................................................ 68
Character/Role Images ............................................................... 72
Context images .......................................................................... 74
Body-Related Images ................................................................. 75
Irrelevant images ....................................................................... 78
What: Imagery Characteristics ............................................................... 78
Senses......................................................................................... 79
Perspective ................................................................................ 85
Ability ........................................................................................ 87
Direction .................................................................................... 89
Deliberation................................................................................ 91
Amount and Duration ................................................................ 93
Why or Reasons for Imagery Use .......................................................... 95
Analytic Framework of Imagery Use ......................................... 96
Cognitive Reasons ..................................................................... 96
Motivational Reasons .............................................................. 103
Artistic Reasons ....................................................................... 111
Health Reasons ........................................................................ 112
Research in Imagery Use of Musical Performers.............................................. 113
Trusheim Study.................................................................................... 113
Carter Study ......................................................................................... 116
Bellon Study ........................................................................................ 117
Conclusion....................................................................................................... 119
v
List of Tables
Table 7 Research Questions Directly Addressed in Singer’s Interview Guide ........ 134
Table 9 Participant Pseudonym, Gender, Voice Range, Age Range, and Highest
Degree of Education ............................................................................... 141
Table 12 Vocal Participants’ Responses to Where They Used Imagery .................... 151
Table 13 Vocal Participants’ Responses to When They Used Imagery ..................... 154
Table 14 Vocal Participants’ Imagery Types, Categories, and Subcategories ........... 161
Table 16 Vocal Participants’ Rank Order of Sense Imagery Use .............................. 182
Table 19 Vocal Participants’ Cognitive Reasons for Imagery Use Categories .......... 189
vi
Table 20 Vocal Participants’ Motivational Reasons for Imagery Use: Categories,
and Subcategories .................................................................................... 204
Table 21 Vocal Participants’ Artistic Reasons for Imagery Use Categories .............. 220
Table 22 Vocal Participants’ Healing Reasons for Imagery Use Categories ............. 243
vii
List of Figures
viii
An Exploratory Study of the Use of Imagery by Vocal Professionals:
ABSTRACT
The purpose of this study was to identify and describe the use of imagery in vocal
psychology. The four Ws of imagery use: where, when, what, and why have been applied
to this study from the fields of athletics (Munroe, Giacobbi, Hall, & Weinberg, 2000) and
dance (Nordin & Cumming, 2005). These were adopted after many years of research in
the Cognitive and Motivational Functions of Imagery (Paivio, 1985; Hall, Mack, Paivio,
& Hausenblas, 1998). This theoretical framework was combined with those of previous
(Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987) and provided a foundation for this study.
Theories from psychology explaining the effects of imagery were also integrated,
investigate the comprehensive nature of this inquiry using a purposeful sample (N = 15).
A semi-structured interview based on the four Ws was conducted with 15 solo classical
vocal professionals, 10 females (sopranos and mezzos) and 5 males (tenors and a
ix
baritone) ranging in age from late twenties to late sixties, residing in several major cities
of the United States. Vocalists used imagery during practice, before performance, at
home, traveling, and a range of other times. Imagery content was divided into types and
context, body-related, musical sound, and character/role images. Imagery was employed
to perfect vocal production and quality, embody the character’s qualities and emotional
aspects for performance, visualize metaphorical and anatomical images to achieve proper
vocal execution, achieve goals, and communicate with the audience. Imagery
primarily internal and some external perspectives. These professionals’ imagery abilities
were deliberate, controllable, and positive. Similar to competitive athletes, solo vocal
performers engaged in imagery for many cognitive and motivational functions. Vocalists
replicated dancers in artistic, and healing functions of imagery use in preparing for and
x
Chapter 1
Introduction
technical, musical, and artistic skills in their performances. Technical skills involve many
aspects of correct vocal production, breath management and control, diction and text
articulation, often in many different languages. Skills in music require training in theory,
history and styles, and interpretation. Artistic skills involve those in acting, creative, and
affective methods of interpretation (Caldwell & Wall, 2001; Leyerle, 1986). Other areas
front of live audiences, and being physically and mentally competent to cope with the
stresses of performance life. While these skills require varying degrees of cognitive
processing, developing the full faculties of the mind is often neglected in arts
performance training programs (Connolly, 2002; Connolly & Williamon, 2004; Dayme,
2006; Kohut, 1992). However, in sports, imagery and mental training have been given a
Imagery and its relationship to execution and outcome assume different meanings
for athletic and artistic performers. In vocal performance, imagery is often used as a
(Averino, 1989; Freed, 2000; Miller, 1996; Moorcroft, 2002; Vennard, 1968). Vennard
(1967) defined imagery as “Figures of speech to express concepts which are difficult to
1
understand literally” (p. 261). The two major reasons that the vocal profession generally
practices imagery in this form could be related to vocal training being primarily an oral
tradition, and the practice of singing professionals who become teachers, passing on their
own experience and understanding of the voice (Freed, 2000). The fact that singing is
strongly based on experience and behavior, emphasizing knowing of the body rather than
knowing intellectually also reinforces the need for this imagery. However, based on
several qualitative research studies, it may be deduced that singers seem to use imagery
in a wide variety of ways (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993), including those detailed in sport
psychology literature (e.g., Morris, Spittle, & Watt, 2005; Murphy & Martin, 2002).
Artistic and musical performers may regard imagery differently from how it is
environmental elements experienced through one’s senses and possibly filtered through a
one’s lifetime, imagery can be conscious, subconscious, or unconscious and can take the
form of daydreaming, mentally running through past experience, future planning and goal
formation, and even spontaneous intuitive flashes. The following definition of imagery is
experiences of which (2) we are self-consciously aware, and which (3) exist for us
in the absence of those stimulus conditions that are known to produce their
2
have different consequences from their sensory or perceptual counterparts
This definition has been borrowed and adapted extensively in sport imagery literature.
Perry and Morris (1995) described mental imagery as the “central pillar of applied
sport psychology” (p. 339). In this field imagery is considered a key component of mental
training in which “athletes can learn to use it systematically to aid performance, reduce
anxiety, increase confidence, enhance endurance, speed recovery from injury or heavy
exercise, and much more” (Morris et al., 2005, p. 5). Connecting the applications and
benefits of imagery in sport psychology with its use by professional singers may reveal
Increasingly over the past few decades, the athletic profession has recognized that
developing the psychological faculties, particularly imagery, allows the athlete to achieve
outstanding results well beyond which have been derived from concentration on physical
practice and skill development (Gould, Eklund, & Jackson, 1992). These were often
(Csikszentmihalyi, 1990), two terms that have often been used interchangeably. Their
meanings have been blurred in some of the literature; however, they are quite different in
terms is highly relevant to the present investigation and its relationship to consciously
Maslow (1954, 1968, 1970, 1971) initiated the psychological study of outstanding
human experience in his theory of hierarchy of needs, the penultimate of which was self-
3
actualization and the corresponding peak experiences. According to Maslow, peak
experiences are those involving cognitive and emotional changes including egoless
perception, complete and total attention, wonder and awe, disorienting time and space,
illumination. Maslow’s peak experience has the connotation that there is one outstanding
given point and all other experiences either lead to or occur after this.
Csikszentmihalyi explained:
[W]e feel a sense of exhilaration, a deep sense of enjoyment that is long cherished
and that becomes a landmark in memory for what life should be like. . . . The best
moments usually occur when a person’s body or mind is stretched to its limits in a
control what happens in consciousness moment by moment, each person has to achieve it
on the basis of his own individual efforts and creativity” (p. 5). Therefore, the term
preferred in this study, optimal performance, described the results of mental and physical
4
Imagery plays an important part in achieving optimal performance and has been a
When singing is ‘right,’ it’s the most exhilarating physical and mental experience
all. It’s all happening out there and it’s all happening in my projected feeling and
This is just one example of the intermingling of imagination and flow in performance
when all the physical, mental, and musical elements were working together and seemed
to lift the individual out of normal consciousness beyond time into a heightened
experience of the present moment. In the effort to understand the many components of
optimal performance, sport psychologists have pursued this research area extensively
(Gould, Dieffenbach, & Moffett, 2002; Orlick, 1990; Orlick & Parrington, 1988).
Although imagery has been empirically investigated for over one hundred years
(e.g., Betts, 1909; Galton, 1880, 1883, 1907; James, 1890; Jastrow, 1892), extensive
progress has been made over the past forty years through hundreds of studies in sport
psychology (e.g., Morris et al., 2005; Murphy & Martin, 2002; Murphy, Nordin, &
Additionally, the human potential movement of the last decades of the twentieth century
(Gallway, 1974; Maltz, 1960; Raiport, 1988) further stimulated growing interest in
5
Athletes’ use of imagery and mental practice has been applied in all facets of
training, practice, competition, and performance and has been found to facilitate athletic
analyses (Driskell, Cooper, & Moran, 1994; Feltz & Landers, 1983; Hall, 2001;
Hinshaw, 1991). While the main focus of this research has been in studying the effects of
mental practice, imagery has also been associated with confidence enhancement (Callow,
Hardy, & Hall, 1998; Moritz, Hall, Martin, & Vadocz, 1996), reduction of anxiety and
stress (Vadocz, Hall, & Moritz, 1997), imagery ability (Rodgers, Hall, & Buckholz,
1991), imagery perspective, (Hale, 1982; Harris & Robinson, 1986; Hardy & Callow,
Soulard, 2003). The body of this research is so extensive that only those studies directly
There is substantial anecdotal evidence that mental rehearsal and imagery practice
endeavors. For example, the world-class professional golfer, Jack Nicklaus, described
details of his use of imagery in achieving world status in golf (Nicklaus & Bowden,
1974). One of the more compelling anecdotal records of mental practice in musical
performance was that of a Chinese professional pianist. After six brutal years of
imprisonment, the second place winner of the 1958 International Tchaikovsky Piano
Competition, Liu Chi Kung, expertly played a concert with the visiting Philadelphia
Orchestra in Beijing, solely from mentally practicing the piano all those years (Korn,
1984). In the field of singing, the late Italian tenor, Luciano Pavarotti, identified mental
6
imagery as an extremely important element of his success (Günter, 1992a). Athletes and
musicians seem to share similarities in their applications of imagery that extend beyond
The similarities and contrasts of athletes and musical performers are worthy of
consideration with regard to imagery use. For a comprehensive comparison, see Table 1
below. The common practices of vocalists and athletes include: (a) use and complete
reliance of the body in performing; (b) highly specialized training and skill in
coordinating specific cognitive and motor behaviors in order to achieve even a modicum
of success (Hays, 2002); (c) the requirement of focus and concentration to perform both
audience; (d) mental disposition which can positively or negatively effect the outcome of
a performance (Chapman, 2006; Emmons & Thomas, 1998); and (e) meeting the
expectations (Bellon, 2006; Gregg, Hall, & Hanton, 2007; Kohut, 1992). These
requirements of both athlete and artistic performer suggest that mental preparation is no
There are five identified differences that set singers apart from athletes,
concerning the areas in which imagery could be applied. First, singers must control fine
motor mechanisms of the vocal apparatus, most of which are often not even visible to the
naked eye. Second, singers are required to develop much more discreet cognitive and
affective skills for executing expressive and artistic performances than athletes, such as
gaining facility with meaning and words in many languages (Caldwell & Wall, 2001;
Emmons & Thomas, 1998; Moyer, 1992). Third, singers are required to vocally,
7
physically, and artistically communicate directly to an audience. Fourth, vocalists must
dramatic, historical, and cultural styles (Lehmann, 1945/1985; Hays, 2002). Finally,
well as the rudiments of music, such as rhythm, pitch, harmony, and composition.
Illustrating the differences between classical solo vocalists and other performing
ensembles and instrumental groups, such as symphonies and orchestras, are required to
read music notation when the music is not memorized and to watch the conductor, while
avoiding overt gazing at the audience. Unlike most ensemble performers, traditionally
singers must memorize their repertoire and communicate musically using text, often in
many different languages. Instrumentalists may perform as soloists but they generally do
not directly interact visually and are usually separated from the audience by their
instruments and sometimes by their music stands. Solo singers, particularly in recital and
oratorio performances, are generally the only musicians who perform directly facing the
audience with no instrument, conductor, or other performers to separate them from the
audience (Carter, 1993). This intimate and vulnerable position requires vocalists to
images, thoughts, self-criticisms, and other distractions, while maintaining positive self-
confidence, body image, and self-consciousness. Many of these skills, which successful
vocalists must master, represent requirements for cognitive and affective development
8
and training along with technical and artistic facility in being able to perform to
Table 1
Use of Sensory Kinesthetic & Kinesthetic, visual, Visual, aural, Visual, aural,
Management arousal are trained anxiety often a anxiety often a often a major issue.
for individual and major issue. major issue. Breathing & full
preparation preparation
Health & Injury High stress High stress Repetitive strain Physical fatigue
9
Rationale
The rationale for this study includes several important elements: (a) traditional
terminology issues, (c) mental and physical practice research, (d) mental practice studies
using university students, (e) correlating sport psychology research with musical
performance, (f) imagery use in achieving artistic and musical performance excellence,
and (g) researchers’ call for empirical studies of imagery use by vocal performers.
from that of psychology. Traditionally, imagery has been employed in learning and
executing musical skills in musical study and performance. For years, vocal pedagogues
have recognized the value of specific imagery applications in securing singing technique
and achieving artistic and emotionally sensitive performances (Brower & Cooke,
1920/1996; Fields, 1947; Lehmann, 1960; Leyerle, 1986; Vennard, 1968; Ware, 1998).
For the past century, general and sport psychologists have examined imagery extensively
in ways that extend beyond the conventional uses assumed in vocal pedagogy. According
to psychologists (e.g., Sheehan, 1983; Singer, 1966), imagery is a natural cognitive skill
that has been well exploited in various performance areas (Sheikh, 1983; Sheikh & Korn,
1994). Several vocal pedagogues (e.g., Cleveland, 1989a; Fields, 1972; Ware, 1998) have
embraced the psychological concepts of mental imagery and its function in training
singers. According to Günter (1992a), “It is my understanding that in artistic singing and,
concepts, without training mental awareness, without training the ear, and without
training the ability of the imagination” (p. 46). It could be that the time has come to
10
update and extend vocalists’ comprehension and implementation of imagery in ways that
reflect more current and research-supported applications for the purposes of improving
explicated. The language of imagery in the sports context focuses on mental practices and
execution, goal orientation, arousal, and mastery (Hall, Mack, Paivio, & Hausenblas,
1998; Paivio, 1985), while music imagery assumes metaphorical and artistic contexts in
skill learning and acquisition and in vocal literature interpretation (Carter, 1993: Fields,
1947; Lehmann, 1960). As dance researchers have embraced these sport-based imagery
terms, more aspects of imagery have been identified, including imagery types,
characteristics, and artistic qualities (Nordin & Cumming, 2005). Linking these terms in
how they are used in vocal performance could provide a foundation for applying the
1997). However, exclusive or extensive physical practice of a skill does not ensure
proficiency (LaFosse, 1973). The cognitive aspects of performance are often neglected in
many training programs (Kohut, 1992). Researchers agree that most motor skills begin in
the individual as a thought (Feltz & Landers, 1983; Oxendine, 1969; Richardson, 1969)
and operate on a cognitive to motor continuum (Feltz & Landers, 1983; Richardson,
1967a). While there are many athletic skills that are considered more motor than
cognitive, researchers have reported that appropriate mental practice and imagery use
11
have facilitated motor performance (Driskell et al., 1994; Feltz & Landers, 1983).
Singing could be considered more cognitive than motor, especially since the internal
vocal apparatus cannot directly be seen and most of the skills involved in coordinating
internally. Therefore, perhaps even more than athletics, singers could benefit from
Sport imagery researchers, Murphy and Jowdy (1992), argued that imagery research
needs to extend beyond athletic performance, and said, “Future research needs to be
directed towards a better understanding of the roles that imagery plays in human
performance so that we can help all persons to optimally utilize their innate capacities”
(p. 245).
been performed primarily in a controlled and sterile laboratory setting, using the ever
prevalent and convenient subject pool of university students (cited in Gregg et al., 2008).
In these studies, participants, content, and all relevant factors have been removed from
the natural environment, modifying the behavior and the results. Consequently, Yuille
(1985) and Lindauer (1983) have recommended investigating highly experienced subjects
in their natural practice and performance environment in order to gain a more complete
understanding of imagery use. These researchers suggest that university students too
often lack the maturity and understanding that seasoned professionals gain after years of
meeting the demands of performance requirements. It was also assumed that universities
and conservatories of music did not traditionally provide students with psychological
skills instruction, including imagery, although this trend seems to be changing (Caldwell
12
& Wall, 2001; Connolly, 2002; Connolly & Williamon, 2004; Emmons & Thomas,
1998). Musicians, teachers, and coaches may be unaware of or unfamiliar with the
including imagery, after years of meeting the various challenges involved in the highly
The results of a study such as this could provide singing pedagogues and
professionals with an understanding of how singers’ imagery use directly relates to that
of professional athletes and dancers in achieving optimal performance. Too often highly
gifted, talented, and even trained singers abandon a professional career because of
memorize, and insurmountable physical, psychological, and affective blocks that could
(Caldwell & Wall, 2001; Chapman, 2006; Emmons & Thomas, 1998; Freymuth, 1999).
Imagery use, as found in sport psychology research, offers techniques and skills which
could help singers overcome some of the well-known barriers to performance excellence,
such as performance anxiety, memory loss, or vocal problems. A detailed inquiry into the
various aspects of imagery use of professional singers may reveal if or how those
performance in ways that are similar to those employed by their elite athletic
13
counterparts. Texts in musical performance have promoted the development of imagery
and mental aspects of achieving excellence as it is used in sports (Green & Gallwey,
1986; Freymuth, 1999; Kohut, 1992). Other recent publications have been helpful in
excellence (Caldwell & Wall, 2001; Chapman, 2006; Emmons & Thomas, 1989; Ristad,
1982). Researchers have examined the use of imagery and mental practice in artistic
performers (Castellano, 1983; Hays, 2002; Rosenberg & Trusheim, 1989). Specifically,
imagery has been studied in dancers (Fish, Hall, Cumming, 2004; Hanrahan, & Salmela,
1990; Nordin & Cumming, 2005), musicians (Connolly, 2002; Freed, 2000; Gregg,
Clark, & Hall, 2008), brass players (Trusheim, 1987; Ross, 1985a, 1985b), pianists
(Coffman, 1987, 1988, 1990; Lim & Lippman, 1991; Rubin-Rabson, 1941; Sisterhen,
2005) and singers (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993). This literature strongly supports the link
between sport psychology imagery research and imagery use by vocal professionals.
However, relatively little research has been conducted directly linking these two areas.
A number of researchers have called for further study investigating the various
2006; Carter, 1993; Connolly, 2002; Murphy & Jowdy, 1992; Nordin & Cumming, 2005;
Perhaps singers have the most to gain from their imagery experiences. Vocal
approaches. Vocalists must rely on their own physical structure and its
14
There is therefore need for a study to determine how professional vocal performers use
imagery, the extent to which it is used successfully by them, and its relative value in
training professionals. If the use of imagery could provide the same excellent success in
music educators, and particularly vocal instructors, to espouse and teach these imagery
The use of imagery to achieve optimal performance in sports has been extensively
researched for the past fifty years (for a review see Morris et al., 2005) resulting in
widespread training and dissemination of the various components and aspects of its
performing at higher competitive levels were found to use imagery more than novices
and those with less skill and experience (Barr & Hall, 1992; Calmels et al., 2003; Driskell
et al., 1994; Feltz & Landers, 1983; Hall, Rodgers, & Barr, 1990; Hall, et al., 1998).
Although several exploratory studies have reported the various aspects of imagery use in
musical performers (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987), few investigations
have focused on the recent findings in imagery research in achieving optimal results in
sport psychology in relation to classical solo singing professionals (Gregg et al., 2008;
Research and text in vocal pedagogy, which include mental training and imagery
limited and is rarely empirically supported. Carter (1993) summarized how voice teachers
15
Imagery used by teachers was employed in five specific teaching situations: first,
matching pitch, breath control, and tone quality. Second, imagery was used to
vocal mechanism. And last, imagery helped the singer to identify more closely
with textual and musical material, thereby bringing new freshness and spirit to
These functions of imagery in this context have been employed for many years and
remain valid and important. The focus has been primarily on vocal technique acquisition
and strengthening and musical interpretation for performance. However, uses and
functions of imagery in the sport and dance areas may be more pervasive and offer more
While vocal performers have used imagery, particularly metaphors and artistic
applications for many years (Averino, 1989; Carter, 1993; Fields, 1945; Lehmann,
1945/1985), only recently has imagery, as it is defined and used in sport psychology,
been included in vocal performance literature (Caldwell & Walls, 2001; Dayme, 2005,
2006; Emmons & Thomas, 1998; Günter, 1992a, 1992b; Williamon, 2004). It is possible
that professional vocalists have learned to use imagery for performance excellence as an
element intrinsic to the nature of singing, by their own efforts, or by some supplementary
method or training. If singers already use imagery as defined in sport in their professional
efforts, this could have serious implications on vocal training and performance
16
possibilities. Understanding specific details of the four Ws of imagery use: where, when,
what, and why from research in sport (Munroe, Giacobbi, Hall, Weinberg, 2000) and
dance (Nordin & Cumming, 2005) as it relates to vocal performance, may be especially
relevant to singers striving to be able to access their optimal performing abilities and
vocalists’ use of imagery has not been identified in terms heretofore accepted by the
vocal profession or linked to what has been considered imagery and its employment as
singers in a variety of ways, including these seven: (a) begin to determine the relevance
of sport and dance imagery research, and applications to that of singing excellence, (b)
help to create a foundation of understanding of terms and usage, (c) identify specific
content and purposes of imagery in achieving optimal vocal performance, (d) facilitate
specific guidelines for training, (e) direct effective applications and interventions, (f)
guide further research, and (g) develop and provide an imagery framework specific to
these vocal performers. The researcher has found only a paucity of the literature available
in English pertaining to the systematic study of the use of imagery in vocal performance,
especially those correlated to imagery research in sport and dance. It was therefore
believed that investigation in this field would provide an important contribution to the
The purpose of this study was to investigate imagery use in achieving optimal
performance in vocal professionals. Research from sport psychology and particularly the
17
framework of the four Ws of imagery use: where, when, why, and what, (Munroe et al.,
2000; Nordin & Cumming, 2005) was used to compare and contrast singers’ imagery
performance practice and development. This study sought to bridge the gap between
music performance research, dance, and sport psychology in the uses of mental imagery.
Questions applied in the study were based on general and sport psychology, dance, and
music imagery research as it related to vocal performance. The study focused on solo
singers for their extensive professional experience. It was assumed that these vocalists
were more adept at applying imagery in their artistic endeavors and professional efforts,
and were more extensively developed and grounded in the practical requirements of a
vocal career than novice singers in a university training program (Bellon, 2006; Carter,
The four Ws framework previously applied to athletes (Munroe et al., 2000) and
dancers (Nordin & Cumming, 2005) addressing where (location), when (time periods),
what (content and qualities), and why (functions) of imagery were adopted in this
exploratory study for vocal professionals. It was hypothesized that similar themes as
those found in athletes and dancers would emerge, including uses of imagery as it related
to the five areas: skill acquisitions and refinement, strategy of performance, goal-oriented
activities, motivational arousal, and mastery. Additionally, it was thought that themes of
artistic and healing imagery as reported in dance research would also appear. It was
further thought likely that new significant themes of imagery applications would emerge
that had not been identified in adult athletes or dancers, which were exclusive to the
18
information would lead to the creation of a framework of the use of imagery specific to
vocal professionals. Furthermore, the results from this study could have implications in
developing models for teaching imagery to vocal and instrumental musical performers.
Research Questions
The research questions addressed in this study employed the framework of the
four Ws of imagery use: where, when, what, and why, and are outlined below.
performance?
5. Why and for what purpose do vocal professionals use imagery to achieve optimal
performance?
Employing an inductive and deductive approach seemed appropriate for the study for
several reasons (Patton, 2002). Deductive aspects were represented in the findings of
previous studies investigating imagery use incorporating the above framework for
athletes (Munroe et al., 2000) and dancers (Nordin & Cumming, 2005) and the analytical
framework of the cognitive and motivational functions of imagery (Paivio, 1985; Hall et
al., 1998). The author hypothesized that some aspects of imagery use by vocal performers
would be similar to that of elite athletes, dancers, and musical performers (Bellon, 2006;
Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987), which represented the deductive portion of the analyses.
Additionally, inductive analysis allowed for the possibility of new categories to emerge
from the data (Patton, 2002), since very little was known about how professional singers’
19
use of imagery was related to the above questions and frameworks. The following
theoretical framework represented some of the major developments and research in the
use of imagery in sport psychology and relevant findings in musical performance studies.
Theoretical Framework
The theoretical framework of this study of imagery in vocal performance has its
roots in historical psychology theory, extensive studies in sport psychology, and the
limited research in imagery use by music professionals. Early imagery theories emerged
with the development of the study of psychology (e.g., Carpenter, 1874; Galton, 1880)
including various theories of psychology (Bandura, 1977; Lang, 1979a; Neisser, 1976)
and the most recent developments of neuro-imaging in brain research (Decety, 1996a,
1996b; Jeannerod, 1994). Imagery in sport psychology began being investigated more
rigorously in the past three decades and included various elements such as comparing the
efficacy of physical with mental practice (e.g., Driskell, Cooper, & Moran, 1994), use of
imagery abilities (Hall, 1998), imagery perspectives (Hale, 1982; Callow & Hardy,
2004), and use of imagery by elite athletes (Orlick, 1990). These and the more recent
many quantitative answers to the questions: where, when, what, and why, through a
developed and implemented to gather a more complete spectrum of the use of imagery by
elite athletes (Munroe et al., 2000), which also served to clarify and corroborate the
20
used to investigate imagery by dancers (Nordin & Cumming, 2005), which revealed more
relevant information of artistic uses of imagery than those found in sports. Several studies
providing in-depth interviews of vocal and instrumental imagery use were also relevant to
this study.
performance. One of the earliest to develop was the psychoneuromuscular theory, which
resulted from the work of James (1890) and Jacobson (1927, 1929, 1930a, 1930b, 1930c,
1930d, 1931), posited that mental imagery produced muscular innervation in the specific
muscles involved in the imagery. In symbolic learning theory, Sackett (1934, 1935)
argued that mental imagery facilitated memory in creating symbols for learning, which
facilitated subsequent performances. Paivio’s (1971) dual coding theory proposed that
two different codes in imagery, the verbal and the image, aided memory and facilitated
learning. In the bio-informational theory, Lang (1977, 1979a, 1979b) argued that imagery
was composed of stimulus and response propositions, which enhanced learning and
performance. In the self-efficacy theory, Bandura (1977, 1997) proposed that imagery
improving performance. Schmidt’s (1982) attention-arousal set theory put forward that
performance preparation.
equivalence theory held that mental practice and the experiences of images were
functionally equivalent to specific aspects of actual physical practice of the same activity
(Decety, 1996a, 1996b; Farah, 1984; Jeannerod, 1994, 1995; Kosslyn, Ganis, &
21
Thompson, 2001). Many of these studies found that areas and pathways of the brain that
were activated during imagery were the same as those that occurred in the mental
preparation of an individual just prior to engaging in that act. This field of research has
potentially many implications for using imagery for practice and performance purposes.
performance, however they may not have addressed all the issues involved. More
importantly, these theories may vary in their application to imagery use by professional
singers. Munroe and others (2000) first examined the four Ws of imagery use in
answering the questions: where, when, what, and why did athletes use imagery. Where
and when dealt with the time and place in which the individuals actually engaged in
imagery. What was being imaged represented the content and qualities of imagery and the
reasons and functions of imagery use were addressed in the category of why. Studies
relevant to the various uses of imagery in performing artists also addressed some of these
issues and included dancers (Fish et al., 2004; Nordin & Cumming, 2005), musicians
(Bellon, 2006; Ross, 1985a, 1985b; Trusheim, 1987), pianists (Coffman, 1990; Sisterhen,
athletes’ practice and competition settings (Weinberg & Gould, 2003). Athletes seemed
to use imagery most in direct relation to competition and less in practice (Hall et al.,
1990; Salmon, Hall, & Haslam, 1994). Athletes used imagery in almost any setting,
including at home, work and school, (Salmon et al., 1994), and particularly in bed at
22
night (Hall et al., 1990; Hall & Buckholz, 1991). Some athletes reported using more
With regard to when imagery was being used, sport competitors were found to
engage in imagery just prior to competition, less during and following competition or in
practice settings (Hall et al., 1990; Munroe, Hall, Simms, & Weinberg, 1998). Athletes
were also found to use imagery more during practice than beforehand or afterwards
performance enhancement, while using imagery in the context of practice was considered
skill learning (Munroe et al., 2000). Outside of these times, athletes used imagery on
breaks in their normal day (Salmon et al., 1994), and particularly at night just before
sleeping (Hall et al., 1990; Hall & Buckholz, 1991). These findings were also reported in
The category, what, entailed the content and quality of imaging. Traditionally
practice to physical practice (Driskell et al., 1994; Hinshaw, 1991, Feltz & Landers,
1993). Over the years, the content of athletes’ imagery research was expanded and
included use of the senses, sessions, effectiveness, controllability, and nature of imagery
as defined in by Munroe and others (2000). In investigating dancers’ imagery use, Nordin
and Cumming (2005) identified two major traits: imagery types and imagery
and role, those that related to the body, and irrelevant images. Imagery characteristics
23
dealt with qualities of the image itself and included the various senses, perspective,
Paivio (1985) generated the analytic framework of functions of imagery use and
identified two main reasons that athletes’ use of imagery, cognitive and motivational.
This model was later extended by Hall and others (1998), resulting in five distinct
categories: (a) cognitive specific, or skill and technique building; (b) cognitive general, or
general – arousal, in “psyching up” or calming down of arousal or affect; and (e)
control in performance. This analytic framework was the result of years of development
various athletic fields (Barr & Hall, 1992; Hall et al., 1990; Rodgers et al., 1991). This
led to the development of the Sport Imagery Questionnaire (SIQ) (Hall et al., 1998; Hall,
Stevens, & Paivio, 2005), while valid and reliable in assessing athletes’ use of imagery,
which seemed to be inadequate in reflecting the full use of imagery, including metaphor,
artistic, health, and spontaneous images of dancers (Fish et al., 2004; Monsma & Overby,
2004; Nordin & Cumming, 2005) or classical musicians (Gregg et al., 2008). Using the
SIQ or other imagery instruments did not seem to be able to adequately represent the
entire picture of how singers use imagery. Gregg and colleagues (2008) adapted the Sport
Imagery Questionnaire (SIQ) (Hall et al., 1998; Hall et al., 2005) for use with musicians,
24
this instrument uncovered many similarities between imagery use by musicians and
sense imagery and musical and artistic characteristics and functions. Therefore the more
broadly based exploratory inquiry of the four Ws of imagery use of Munroe and others
(2000) and Nordin and Cumming (2005) was chosen for the purpose of understanding the
elite brass orchestral instrumentalists (Trusheim, 1987) and top vocal professionals
(Carter, 1993). Although these studies were extensive and valuable, these researchers
used artistic and creative frameworks of imagery and included only minimal research of
imagery in sport psychology. Bellon’s (2006) study examined the correlations of specific
musical performers and their incorporation of the main elements of sport psychology, of
which imagery was a part. These studies were examined individually with regard to their
relevance to imagery use in vocal professionals and in relation to the four Ws framework
It was anticipated that the present study might provide important contributions to
the literature on vocal performance practice, with implications about its application in
1960s and 1970s, Maslow’s (1968, 1971) humanistic psychology promoted the trend of
human potential movement in sport and later artistic performers. After decades of
research and training in the applications of imagery for achieving performance excellence
in sports, music researchers as well as pedagogues had recently begun to recognize the
25
potential for similar success in musical performance using these similar cognitive
methods (Caldwell & Wall, 2001Connolly, 2002; Connolly & Williamon, 2004; Dayme,
2005). Investigators and psychologists who had experience and understanding in the
positive effects of imagery were found to be “demonstrating anew that people tutored in
the use of imagery skills can perform at remarkable levels in comparison to those
common to rote learning situations” (Sheehan, 1972, p. xiv). It was suggested that, by
applying the advances achieved in the research of imagery in sport psychology over the
past forty years (Morris et al., 2005; Murphy et al., 2008), singers’ uses of imagery would
compare favorably and therefore appropriate findings in sports and dance research could
science with vocal training in the hopes of enhancing this ancient art. He stated:
The science of voice draws upon many fundamental sciences; notably physics,
into an applied science which we may call the ‘psychology of vocal arts.’ (p. 340)
Over the years, extensive research has been conducted to this end (Deutsch, 1999). With
the exception of several studies (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Gregg et al., 2008), the
imagery use in musical and singing performance. It was hoped that this study would help
bridge the gap of imagery research in sport and dance with music and vocal performance
26
research. In examining the uses of imagery in solo vocal professionals, findings were
compared with those in sport and dance, the results of which could be applied in the
Generally, music students have been left to their own devices as to how to
mentally cope with the many challenges they had to face in their training (Chapman,
2006; Dayme, 2006; Emmons & Thomas, 1998; Freymuth, 1999). A number of cognitive
aspects of musical performance may have a direct relationship to sport concerns, such as
learning and perfecting skills and strategies, setting and achieving goals, preventing or
grappling with arousal problems, and performance anxiety. It was suggested that, in
identifying and analyzing the special mental skills and practices of professional
musicians and singers, teachers, professors, and coaches may be better prepared to train
and equip their students with the skills necessary for optimal performance as has been
practiced in sports training for decades. If imagery could be used successfully to deal
with the normal psychological, technical, or affective problems that arise in the practice
and performance of singing, many more talented musicians and singers could enjoy and
participate in the profession in which they may have exceptional talent. The gap in vocal
excellence. It was also possible that implementing early training in imagery could assist
singers in coping with previously insurmountable blocks that could be alleviated and
While vocal pedagogues have begun including some mental aspects of singing in
their texts (Caldwell & Wall, 2001; Chapman, 2006; Emmons & Thomas, 1998;
27
Williamon, 2004), exploratory studies such as this could add to the growing body of
research on the uses of imagery in musical performers. Few investigations have been
psychology (Bellon 2006; Gregg et al., 2008; Stedman, 1985). Gaining an understanding
of professional singers’ experience and use of imagery could assist vocal teachers and
students in applying effective practices, skills, and concepts in creatively using imagery
to reach beyond skill proficiency and achieve optimal performance in the professional
arena. A study such as this could contribute to facilitating and understanding how
imagery use can enhance vocal performance. The findings could possibly extend into
other forms of musical performance and training, contributing to the body of literature in
imagery use for optimal performance. A full inquiry into imagery uses in professional
vocalists could benefit the body of knowledge in all three areas: sport, dance, and musical
Definitions of Terms
The following is a list of terms pertinent to this study and is not meant to
represent general use of imagery, sport psychology, musical, or vocal performance. The
• Audiation was a term coined by Gordon (1976, 2003a) who rejected the label of
auditory image because of its connotation with the visual sense and lack of its ability to
describe the internal process of music cognition. He defined audiation in the following
way:
hear and understand in our minds music that we have just heard performed or
28
have heard performed sometime in the past. . . . We also audiate when we hear
and understand in our minds music that we may or may not have heard but are
Audiation was also defined as the process of internally hearing and understanding
musical elements such as rhythm, pitch, and tonality in sequences and patterns (Gordon,
1993). Audiation differed from inner hearing or auralizing (Karpinski, 2000), pitch
internalization (Klonoski, 2003), and auditory imagery in that it dealt with elements and
patterns of musical sounds that had meaning and required understanding in context
physical appearance of their body (Nordin & Cumming, 2005). Singers’ body image
could differ in how they regarded themselves on or off stage (Carter, 1993).
• Cognitive psychology was defined as the study of the process whereby people
transformed, reduced, elaborated, stored, recovered, and used sensory input (Neisser,
1976).
• Goals were images of concepts, skills, or behaviors that were created and held for
future achievement, exemplified in the charge “[b]egin with the end in mind” (Covey,
29
(Weinberg & Gould, 1999, p. 308). Singers set goals and worked for many
were more able to control the achievement of these types of goals than outcome
goals. For vocalists, these goals may have included presenting a character in
• Imagery derived from several Latin terms, imago, “a likeness,” im-itari, “to
imitate,” imaginer “to imagine, think,” and imaginatus “to picture one’s self” (Skeat,
1943, p. 282). For many years Richardson’s (1969) definition set the standard in the
sport psychology:
Imagery is intriguing for its close relationship to perception and action. It is such
environment and contained in the execution of motor skills. Images bind personal
30
experience attitudes and actions mentally in ways that have not yet been
Imagery has had many other terms such as picture-like visual imaginations,
controllable images and retaining them for sufficient time to effect the desired imagery
rehearsal” (Morris, 1997, p. 37). Ability to imagine and manipulate images could be
developed over time, with an individual’s conscious effort and support from a
knowledgeable guide. For the purposes of this study, imagery ability also included
only vivid but also persistent images, yet persistent only for the time they
should be used, that is, both persistent and interruptable [sic] on request”
(Denis, 1985, p. 8S). Controllability dealt with how easily an image was
oDirection of imagery was whether the image was helpful and facilitative or
2005).
31
oVividness connoted clarity of detail, which has had implications on internal
processing. Vivid imagers learned more than poor imagers (Marks, 1977).
• Mental practice (MP) was a specific type of imagery wherein the person imagined
motor movements and cognitive processes, thought about the activity, practiced various
aspects of a skill, both at the initial stages of learning through the end stages of
issues, concepts, or even skills that could not readily, fully, or accurately be described in
factual terms. It was derived from the Greek “metapherein, to carry over” meaning “a
and primarily used of one thing is applied to another” (Guralnik, 1982, p. 893). It also
came from the Latin word, metaphora meaning “a transferring of a word from its proper
signification to another” (Skeat, 1943, p. 365). Metaphoric image was used extensively in
effective expression in vocal and instrumental music (Barten, 1992, 1998). An example
The projection of imagery falls under two general headings: mimicry and sensual
‘produce a hooty tone quality’ are often useful in the conceptualization of a pure
32
• Modeling was also known as vicarious experience, identified in Bandura’s (1977)
and/or external forces that produce the initiation, direction, intensity and persistence of
behavior” (Vallerand & Thill, cited in Short et al., 2006, p. 54). Motivation in this study
included the energy gathered to inspire drive and enthusiasm to accomplish some action,
(1990) “the state of mind in which people are so involved in an activity that nothing else
seems to matter” (p. 4) and “being in the zone,” or the feeling of performance excellence
• Perception stemmed from the Latin, percipiere, “to take hold of . . . to become
aware of through sight, hearing, touch, taste, or smell” (Guralnik, 1984, p. 1054).
“Perception is the larger process of making meaningful sense out of sensation” (Klinger,
1981, p. 2). This differed from imagination in which the individual was not responding
directly with any one of the senses to stimuli in the environment. Neisser (1972) argued
that imagery lay at “the intersection of memory and perception” (p. 233).
33
• Perspective in imagery referred to how the item or occurrence was being
phenomenology such that the person actually imagines being inside his/her body
situation” (Mahoney & Avener, 1977, p. 137). Imagining the experience “from
outside observer, as if watching a video (Epstein, 1980; Hale, 1982; Mahoney &
Avener, 1977).
• Professional singer/vocalist, for the purposes of this study, was a male or female
singer, trained at a university or conservatory, who had earned at least half of his
professionally in varying venues including opera, recital, classical concert, and chamber
the self. . . . serving at least two functions; instructional and motivational” (Hardy, 2005,
• Sense imagery was categorized into six different divisions: (a) visual, or sight; (b)
auditory, or hearing; (c) kinesthetic, or bodily feeling; (d) tactile, or feeling of touch; (e)
34
olfactory, or smell, and (f) gustatory, or taste. The three senses: visual, auditory,
kinesthetic, and combining several of these were most reported in musical performance
studies (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987) and are further defined here:
o Visual imagery connoted the use of the sense of sight to see a scene from
actions.
hearing sounds internally when they were not present including audience
above).
Munzert, 2005, pp. 4-5), was derived from “Kinesthesia: Greek kinein, ‘to
35
equipment)” (pp. 444-445). Günther (1992b) expanded this definition as,
movement in muscles and joints. These sensations are real and with an
adequate sensitivity can create a mental image which one can use in
• Skills were understood on the cognimotor continuum. Cognitive and motor skills
physical, or overt (Feltz & Landers, 1983; Hinshaw, 1991; Richardson, 1967a).
movement. Cognition was the act of conceiving and processing these overt
actions.
36
Delimitations
delimitations (see Chapter 3). The criteria for participation in this study were limited to
solo vocal classical performers who had at least four years of professional experience
singing classical repertoire in opera, oratorio, concert, recital, and art song. These were
male and female vocalists who sang in major metropolitan areas of the United States and
represented many different experience levels of singing from early in their career to
retirement. Their voices ranged from soprano, mezzo, tenor, and baritone. Participants
from this population were chosen because of their extensive experience in vocal
performance and their willingness to share their experiences of using imagery in their
profession. The findings of this study were not intended to be generalized to any group
study. Specifically, these consisted of (a) sampling considerations, (b) the lack of formal
assessment of the participants in imagery ability and experience levels, (c) response
biases, (d) issues of temporal validity, (e) the elusive and subjective nature of imagery,
and (f) researcher bias. These were individually addressed and steps were taken to
Selection of participants for this study was purposeful and involved snowball
sampling of professional singers from various areas of the United States. This method
was deemed more effective than random sampling (Patton, 2002) since the population of
all professional singers was not available for selection in this study (Onwuegbuzie, 2003).
37
The sample size was relatively small and may not have reflected how the majority of
professional singers practiced and used imagery in their profession. In order to address
this limitation, singers working in major metropolitan areas of the United States were
represented. Furthermore, both male and female vocalists with different levels of
experience and voice types were included and their identities were kept confidential.
Even so, the results of this study were limited to this study participant and were not
generalizable to the entire population of professional classical solo singers in the United
States.
limitation, singers in this study were asked to give details of their early experiences and
their imagery ability including accuracy, vividness, their ability to manipulate their
images, and whether their imagery was helpful or hurtful. This self-reported information
allowed the researcher to more thoroughly analyze and interpret the data set. Early
training and experiences, as well as ability levels of these participants represented factors
that could have influenced individual use and facility of imagery particularly in the effort
A possible internal credibility threat may have been observational bias, which
occurs when the researcher has “obtained an insufficient sampling of behaviors or words
from the study participants” (Onwuegbuzie, 2003). This could also be due to the
limitations of the number of participants chosen for the study or insufficient time of
gathering data during each interview. Revising and adapting a respected interview guide
38
used in previous studies of various populations of athletics and dance for appropriate use
with singers attempted to address this latter threat. In addition, the subjects were invited
to contribute answers at any time during the interview or add to the given question in
their own way. Furthermore, interview questions were repeated and reworded to assure
complete responses from the interviewees. Member checking was also used in addressing
this threat (Johnson & Christensen, 2004), in which each transcribed interview was sent
Response biases occurred when subjects distort their answers to seem more
favorable to other important people such as colleagues, teachers, and researchers (Vella-
Brodrick & MacRae, 2004). The two most common response biases were acquiescence or
the tendency to respond in a certain direction, either negative or positive and social
desirability where subjects answered in what they thought they should say in order to
appear more favorably in the eyes of peers or other people (Patton, 2002; Vella-Brodrick
& MacRae, 2004). It was possible that the participants, for any number of reasons, did
not want to reveal all pertinent information regarding their use of imagery for optimal
performance. However, an attempt was made to minimize this effect through the
Mental imagery was a highly subjective and difficult behavior to directly observe,
describe, and analyze due to its internal and private cognitive nature (Mackay, 1981). The
recall, lack of understanding of imagery terms used in this study, and possible discomfort
however, this remained the most reliable method in gaining a descriptive and in-depth
39
understanding of imagery use in optimal performance. Providing initial imagery
definitions, asking open-ended questions, probing for further responses, and using
questions that cross-referenced each subject for two different directions were employed
to minimize this threat. Every effort was made to avoid leading the participant and the
researcher endeavored to maintain a neutral stance and attitude during the interview
process. At the conclusion of the interviews, these singers were asked if they had been
led in any way either by the questions or in any manner of the interviewer. Maintaining
confidentiality also allowed the subjects to respond more honestly and accurately.
Furthermore, participants’ responses were compared with other studies of imagery use in
athletes (Munroe et al., 2000), dancers (Nordin & Cumming, 2005), vocalists (Carter,
A further threat to external validity in this study was temporal validity since the
results may not have been fixed across time and represented a cross-section of the
specific population (Onwuegbuzie, 2003). The time of year in which the interviews were
vocalists working during a heightened performance season. Singers may have used
imagery differently in relation to the time of year and season. In an attempt to minimize
this possible threat, the interview questions were sent in an email attachment to all
participants prior to the interview appointment in order to assist them gaining familiarity
by previewing the definitions, questions, and probes. All of the interviews were
way conflicted with vocalists’ holiday concert schedules. Furthermore, through member
checking, participants were given the opportunity to amend their transcribed interviews in
40
any way they felt appropriate in describing their use of imagery. This provided time for
the participants to contemplate their responses and give as complete and accurate account
as possible. As a result, the exploratory design of the study added to the existing body of
inherent problems with validity. Terminology of imagery and its definitions as used in
this study were not universal since singers regarded imagery differently from sport
psychologists. In an effort to avoid confusion, explanation of some of the basic terms was
given in the introductory letter and embedded in the interview questions. Furthermore, it
was possible that the interview format and the individual interview with each participant
did not adequately cover the various topics of interest in the use of imagery in singing.
The extensive review of the relevant literature and a pilot study were made to diminish
this threat. The interview guide was based on several studies in the line of research
gaining information as to where, when, what, and why (Munroe et al., 2000; Nordin &
Cumming, 2005). The pilot interview was administered to a regional singing professional
any awkward or confusing questions. This was an important step in assuring the highest
The threat of researcher bias is inherent in qualitative study and was avoided
wherever possible in order to increase the trustworthiness of this study (Anfara, Brown,
& Mangione, 2002; Patton, 2002). This type of bias happens when the researcher has
personal thoughts about the questions, answers, or the overall subject of the inquiry that
may have an either conscious or subconscious effect on the responses of the participants
41
(Onwuegbuzie, 2003). Peer debriefing, rigorous methodology, and attention to detail
were conducted and maintained as well as avoiding bias in data collection, analysis, and
interpretation in order to remain true to reporting the responses of the participants. Rigor
in this research made the results and conclusions as valid as possible in this exploratory
format. This investigator has had considerable training and experience as a professional
vocalist as well as prior knowledge and experience in imagery, which may have served to
bias responses in the interview. However, it was reasonable to expect the researcher to
have some knowledge of the subject she was investigating (Patton, 2002).
interpreting, and reporting this information, often leading to investigator bias. For this
reason, debriefing with peers was conducted throughout the research process in an effort
researchers with advanced degrees in music education and training in research methods
assisted in the data analysis and in categorizing the results of the interviews (Johnson &
Christensen, 2004).
uses of imagery in the athletic, artistic, and musical performers, including pertinent
theories, studies, and applications. The framework of the four Ws of imagery use: where,
when, what, and why as proposed in athletes (Munroe et al., 2000) and dancers (Nordin &
imagery. Chapter 3 provides details of the research design, the participants, methodology,
data collection procedures, and methods of analysis for this study, including
42
legitimization. The results of the data analyses are provided in Chapter 4 along with
tables and figures. Emergent themes and areas of similarity to previous research are also
interpretations and significant findings. The implications for music and vocal education
are included, followed by limitations of the study and recommendations for further
research.
43
Chapter 2
Literature Review
This chapter provides support for understanding of the four Ws framework for
vocalists’ use of imagery by drawing upon the literature primarily from sport psychology
(e.g., Hall et al., 1998; Martin et al., 1999; Munroe et al., 2000) and dance research (e.g.,
Nordin & Cumming, 2005). The paucity of imagery research in music performance
limited the author’s references in this area to only a small number of studies including
those in instrumental brass (Trusheim, 1987), musical performance (Bellon, 2006), and
singing (Carter, 1993). The chapter begins with a review of the theories of imagery
effectiveness on performance from general and sport psychology, which provided the
groundwork in understanding the various aspects of imagery research. Then, each query
in the main framework of the four Ws: where, when, what, and why is examined
individually. Results from the major studies from sports, dance, and musical performance
A number of theories have been developed over the years to explain and
understand the underlying mechanisms of imagery and its effects on performance. Four
categories of theories have been identified primarily in sport psychology as those relevant
to understanding the workings of imagery and mental practice: (a) early developments
44
and theories, (b) cognitive-based theories (c) psychological states theories, and (d)
functional equivalence theory. The early developments and theories included several
symbolic learning theory (Sackett, 1934; 1935). The focus of these studies was in testing
mental processes involved in imagery. These included Paivio’s (1971) dual code theory,
bio-informational theory of Lang (1977, 1979a, 1979b), and Ahsen’s (1972, 1984) triple
code theory. Theories emphasizing psychological states, such as arousal and motivation,
included the Bandura’s (1977, 1982, 1986, 1997) self-efficacy theory and the attention-
arousal set theory of Schmidt (1982). The theory of functional equivalence (Decety,
1996a, 1996b; Farah, 1984; Finke, 1980, 1985, 1989; Finke & Shephard, 1986;
Jeannerod, 1994, 1995, 1999, 2006) represented the newest developments in imagery
theories and involved neuroimaging. Functional equivalence studies found that imagining
motor tasks seemed to replicate the same mental patterns involved in preparing to execute
the task. Although this theory was relevant to imagery in performers, it could not be
supported or disputed in this study. However, it did offer evidence and understanding of
imagery use beyond what has been presented in prior studies. Further details of these
elements and supporting research as identified in sport psychology (see Table 2).
45
Table 2
Symbolic Learning Sackett (1934, Imagery was effective in learning and rehearsing particularly
Theory 1935) cognitive tasks through symbols representing tasks in the brain
Dual Coding Paivio (1971, Imagery represented dual coding modes: imaginal and verbal,
Theory 1979a, 1979b) (emotional and physical reactions), and meaning propositions
Triple Code Ahsen (1984) Imagery was composed of three elements: image (I), somatic
Functional Decety (1996) Imagery and perception of a specific action were functionally
Equivalence Jeannerod (1994) equivalent in the same neural substrates action was inhibited in
46
Early Developments and Theories of Imagery
Various aspects of mental imagery have been investigated since the early study of
psychology. Examination of the effects of mentally practicing a motor task began with
of muscular actions produced slight nerve impulses. James (1890) argued that mental
images of actions was produced in slight muscular discharges generated in the related
muscles and further postulated that by imagining, one could learn to perform skills
without overt practice. Washburn (1916) supported the assumption that mental
1931) studies were among the earliest to formally investigate neuromuscular effects from
arousal of the normal motor output, which were strong enough to generate slight
variety of muscle groups, not just the ones involved in the imagery.
earliest theories addressing mental imagery, which stated that mental practice effects
muscular activity as that produced during actual physical motor activity. These muscle
(Corbin, 1972; Richardson, 1967b; Schmidt, 1987; Start & Richardson, 1964). This
theory had been developed through many studies, using the most innovative
instrumentation available at the time. Even though it enjoyed popularity for decades, the
47
positive relationship between actual muscular activity and its imagined counterpart has
yet to be established (Feltz & Landers, 1983; Murphy & Martin, 2002).
Symbolic learning theory. Sackett’s (1934, 1935) symbolic learning theory held
that specific representations of skills or “mental blueprints” (Vealey & Walter, 1993)
were stored in the brain as they were acquired. Learning a task resulted in forming a
“representation,” code, or symbol of that task, which could aid learning, recall, and
that more mental rehearsals increased learning retention, but after a certain amount of
practice there were no further gains, creating a negative acceleration relationship between
rehearsals and retention. The theory did not address how elite athletes or performers
effectively use mental practice when these “mental blueprints” were already well
established. Furthermore, the design of the study may not have represented the actual use
of imagery in the practices of performing artists or athletes. Yuille (1985) argued that
researchers would be more apt to generalize and benefit from these experiments if actual
Imagery studies fell out of favor for many years in the early decades of the
twentieth century. Both psychoneuromuscular and symbolic learning theories lacked the
ability to fully explain the wide variety of applications and aspects of imagery use, such
Watson’s (1931) behaviorism supported his demand for empirical and scientific
inquiry into imagery was heralded by Holt’s article, “Return of the Ostracized” (1964)
whereby he provided compelling evidence and called for renewed research in imagery.
48
Various groups in psychology began to embrace and include imagery in their clinical
transformed, reduced, elaborated, stored, recovered, and used sensory input (Neisser,
1976). Most of the work in imagery concerned acquisition, storage, retrieval and use of
imagery: (a) Paivio’s (1971, 1986) dual code theory which included the pictoral and
verbal representation of the image; (b) bio-informational theory (Lang, 1977, 1979a,
1979b), wherein imagery contained both a stimulus, a response proposition, and its
intrinsic meaning (c) Ahsen’s (1984) triple code theory, in which the (I) image, (S)
somatic response, and (M) meaning of the image were necessary to affect performance.
These cognitive-based theories were investigated for their relevance to imagery use in
performance execution.
Dual code theory. In dual code theory, Paivio’s (1971, 1986) theoretical
information in two basic coding systems operating in cognitive functions, verbal and
pictoral, which have been reported to aid in memory and retrieval. This theory was
functional localization” (Jeannerod, 1994). Hall, Moore, Annett, and Rodgers (1997)
both, or no practice control. Results indicated that the combination of imagery and verbal
guide led to the most successful pattern recall. Annett (1986, 1994) extended the dual
49
coding approach rendering it specific to motor applications. Annett developed the action-
language-imagination (ALI) model whereby the two methods assisting skill acquisition,
verbal or demonstration, and were linked in the action-language bridge making it possible
to act on verbal instructions or encode actions in language. Ahsen (1984) criticized the
dual coding theory for being “an abstract, linear model” (p. 18), omitting the function and
importance of meaning or somatic effect. It could be argued that athletes and performers
used verbal coding in a variety of ways to learn and master skills, however dual coding
would only account for a limited portion of artistic performance experience, particularly
According to Lang, images were organized into a two basic propositions: stimulus, and
response with its relevant meaning characteristics. “Stimulus propositions are those
descriptive details which establish the context and designate specific stimuli. The
response propositions are the things the subject does in the scene” (Lang, 1979b, p. 19).
Lang recommended that response proposition scripts include as many specific details of
the intended emotional and physical states to achieve the most significant and desired
as demonstrated in studies by Lang and his colleagues. Lang, Melamed, and Hart (1970)
compared the emotional imagery, particularly those verbally invoked by use of specific
scripts, with biological responses to fear images using two groups: those with a phobia of
live snakes and the other the social phobia of public speaking. Lang found the social
50
phobia groups’ arousal leveled off while the images in the snake phobia group were much
more vivid, showed highest increases of heart rate, and registered at the upper end of the
hierarchy. Study participants were able to gain more control of their performance by
modifying their responses, producing greater change in behavior. Similar results occurred
in a cross-validation study with participants having public speaking anxiety and spider
phobias (Weerts & Lang, 1978). In desensitization studies treating these phobic patients
with stimulus and response propositions, Lang (1979a) subsequently found successfully
treated phobic patients reported less fear reactions to imagery and calmer heart-rates.
Budney, Murphy, and Woolfolk (1994) supported Lang’s theory that imagery
instructions, in which more response items were included and produced more
applicable for addressing arousal concerns of singers since they, like public speakers,
perform before audiences. Many vocalists have struggled with crippling performance
anxiety and inability to achieve proper arousal levels appropriate for performance on
stage. In controlling inner thoughts, emotions, and images by including the desired
response propositions, vocal performers could possibly move beyond their fears or
phobias into more free and expressive delivery of their art to the audience. However,
Lang’s theory may have fallen short of explaining some of the cognitive and motivational
functions of imagery and the use of language in processing information into action,
Triple code theory. Ahsen’s (1984) triple code theory, similar to Lang’s theory,
also addressed imagery more in terms of the emotional effect imagery had on the
51
individual. Ahsen’s (1984) triple code theory was developed in his response criticizing
Paivio’s (1971) dual code theory. Ahsen diminished the importance of the verbal content
and argued that the somatic element and meaning required representation for imagery to
be effective. Also emerging from his studies with mental patients and phobias, Ahsen
(1972) included three main components of his ISM imagery theory, which were: (a) the
image itself (I) internally containing all the sensations of actual experience; (b) the
emotions elicited by the specific image; and (c) meaning (M), which was specific to the
image and the individual, including their history, experience, and previous conceptions.
Three components of triple code theory, the image, the somatic response, and the
meaning (ISM), reflected issues and concerns for vocal professionals, making it
The cognitive theories addressing imagery issues such as verbal coding, image
itself, the specific somatic responses to images, and the meanings applied to these
appropriate since imagery in this context was highly individualized. The interaction
between these imagery elements was important, however they were limited in other
details of how imagery affects performance. More recent research has emerged
addressing the psychological states and how these affect imagery performance.
52
included two main theories: (a) Bandura’s (1977) self-efficacy theory where imagery was
partly seen as the product of social interaction, and (b) attention-arousal set theory
(Schmidt, 1982), which addressed the optimal levels of arousal necessary for specific
activities.
learning theory and cognitive-behavioral therapy, which dealt with psychology patients’
theory held that consciously directed, task specific imagery increased expectations of
success, which facilitated confidence and positive outcome of the actual performance. “A
substantial body of literature shows that efficacy beliefs regulate human functioning
through four major processes [including] cognitive, motivational, affective and selective
processes” (Bandura, 1997, p. 116). Bandura (1986) posited that self-efficacy and self-
evaluation cognition such as attaining goals and achieving success enhanced intrinsic
experience (modeling), verbal reinforcement, and emotional arousal” (Morris et al., 2005,
p. 48).
movement and skills with an observed standard or representative. Feedback was required
to check for accuracy, corrections, and refinement of these skills. This learning had a
cognitive effect on the individual as to the degree of proficiency to which they were
53
When a person observes a model’s behavior, but otherwise performs no overt
responses, he can acquire the modeled responses while they are occurring only in
modeling stimuli have been coded into images or words for memory
Athletes and performers have benefited from the practice of modeling for a long time.
Athletes have watched other athletes to learn how to execute their skills, just as singers
have been encouraged to listen to excellent vocalists to replicate and be inspired by the
quality and practices of other excellent singers (Tosi & Galliard, 1723/1968). Both
athletes and performers also learned, from others and by experience, how to regulate their
referred to as arousal or activation theory, proposed that imagery allows for an optimal
state of arousal for each performer and each activity, which facilitated performance
excellence and success. Attention-arousal set theory also proposed that imagery prepared
the athlete (and performer) for action to embody a perfect arousal state while focusing
attention on task-relevant cues rather than distractions (Murphy, Woolfolk, & Budney,
1988). Landers (1980) reported that high arousal or emotional activity had the potential
of narrowing or decreasing attentional range while low levels broaden attention. This
inverted-U theory stated that optimal performances were associated with moderate levels
of arousal, while high and low arousal could have a deteriorating effect on performance.
54
Each one of these theories: psychoneuromuscular theory, symbolic learning
the current issues present during the time in which they were addressed and often
developed in response to earlier theories. Furthermore, they were tested and analyzed in
the best empirical manner possible at the time. Functional equivalence theory used the
More recent research and developments using highly technical electronic imaging
capacities especially those from medical science have taken mental imagery out of its
subjective and therefore questionable category into one of more extensive, rigorous, and
scientific investigation. This represented a departure from the psychological models and
equivalence theory (Decety, 1996a, 1996b, 2002; Farah, 1984; Finke, 1980, 1985, 1989;
Finke & Shephard, 1986; Jeannerod, 1994, 1995, 2006) postulated that the same brain
functions involved in imagery were functionally equivalent in the brain activities of the
preparation of that same skill or activity even though physical motor execution was
usually blocked during imagery (Holmes & Collins, 2002). Both activated the same
neural substrates, pathways, and processes in specific areas of the brain. While motor
preparation was activated below the level of consciousness, imagery of that act was
consciously directed in the same action (Jeannerod, 1994). This effectively placed
intended actions and skills on a covert to overt continuum (Jeannerod, 1994, 2006;
Kosslyn, 1994; Kosslyn & Koenig, 1992). Decety (1996b) reviewed studies involving
55
mental chromatic and autonomic responses while imaging. He concluded that the studies
“converge to support the notion that motor imagery shares the same neural mechanisms
objectify imagery activity, possibly resolving decades of controversy over its elusive and
subjective nature particularly in the area of motor performance. Holmes and Collins
imagery training for use in sports performance. They have found that imagery as a
“motor representation can be used to explain the multifaceted and variable motor
behaviour [sic] of elite sport athletes and to develop a mechanistic explanation for the
effectiveness of imagery generally” (p. 123). They further argued that: “if physical and
mental practice can be shown to possess high functional equivalence, then many
procedures that are efficacious in physical practice should be relevant to mental practice
as well” (p. 124). Holmes and Collins concluded that the most effective imagery
reflected, as closely as possible, all the cognitive, motor, sensory, and affective elements
musical expression, and observational learning were particularly relevant to this study.
found that auditory imagery was associated positively with auditory and musical training.
Meister, Krings, Foltys, Boroojerdi, Müller, Töpper, & Thron (2004) found similar brain
activity during imaginary as in actual musical performance. Halpern and Zatorre (1999)
observed that vocal song performance activated specific areas of the brain. More recently,
56
Decety (2006) found that emotional imagery and empathy reflected actual expression of
emotion in brain activity. In studying the neural activity of humans viewing another
individual performing actions, Fadiga, Fogassi, Pavesi, and Rizzolatti (1995) observed
increased stimulation of the same parts of the motor system in the brain as when subjects
both imagined and performed that motor action. Similarly, Corchin, Barthelemy,
executing gymnastic movements. Kosslyn and others (2001) concluded that “most of the
neural processes that underlie cognitive like-modality perception are also used in
imagery; and imagery, in many ways, can stand in for (re-present, if you will) a
perceptual stimulus or situation” (p. 641). These studies were particularly relevant to the
issues of training vocal performers in how they learn to employ modeling, observation,
skill acquisition and technique, and embody a variety of affect in portraying different
All these imagery theories had been developed over many decades and seemed to
dealt with specific coding in the brain. The cognitive-based theories, including Paivio’s
(1971) dual-coding theory, Lang’s (1977, 1979a, 1979b) bio-informational theory, and
Ahsen’s triple code theory, addressed information processing. The psychological state
theory of Bandura’s (1977) self-efficacy theory dealt with environmental and social
57
individual. Schmidt’s (1982) attention-arousal set theory posited that there were certain
affective and arousal states, which made optimal performance achievable. Functional
equivalence was based on neural imaging evidence that imagery and motor activity share
the same cognitive processes. With the recent developments in brain imaging, functional
equivalence seemed to provide the most comprehensive explanation, the results of which
may impact the relevancy of the earlier theories. However, these theories may have been
The framework of the four Ws of imagery use: where, when, what, and why, from
athletes (Munroe et al., 2000) and dancers (Nordin & Cumming, 2005) provided a basis
for this study. Where and when imagery was examined first, followed by what performers
why different imagery content was employed followed. The major studies investigating
the use of imagery in musical performers were examined and compared with relevant
The framework of the four Ws was initially proposed in imagery of athletes and
included six levels (Munroe et al., 2000), as seen in Figure 1. Levels one and two
depicted where and when, respectively, and the third level revealed a connection of the
function and content of imagery in answering why and what athletes used imagery. In
adapting the four Ws framework for dancers (see Figure 2), Nordin and Cumming (2005)
eliminated the use of levels and created a hierarchical tree and separated why and what.
58
Figure 1. Four Ws of imagery use framework for athletes.
From “The Four Ws of Imagery Use: Where, When, Why, and What,” by K. J. Munroe, P. R. Giacobbi, Jr.,
C. R. Hall, and P. Weinberg, 2000, The Sport Psychologists, 14, p. 126. Copyright 2000 by Human
59
Figure 2. Four Ws of imagery use framework for dancers.
From “Professional Dancers Describe Their Imagery: Where, When, What, Why, and How,” by S. M.
Nordin and J. Cumming, 2005, The Sport Psychologists, 19, p. 400. Copyright 2005 by Human Kinetics.
related, and character/role. In the why category, cognitive and motivational reasons were
carried over from the sports framework (Munroe et al., 2000). Three additional imagery
reasons categories emerged from the dance study: (a) artistic reasons of choreography,
movement enhancement, and audience communication; (b) healing reasons, and (c) no
reason. These adaptations were considered to more appropriately apply to use of imagery
in artistic performers and specifically to the vocal professionals in this study. Therefore,
these two frameworks were used in this study to analyze and compare to the imagery uses
of these professional singers and to allow a new singers’ imagery framework to emerge
from the data analyses. The main elements of the two imagery frameworks are analyzed
which answered the questions: where, when, what, and why. This is separated into the
following sections: (a) where; (b) when; (c) what, imagery types and imagery
characteristics; and (d) why, and the cognitive, motivational, artistic, and healing reasons.
The chapter ends with a review of the relevant imagery research in music performance.
venue; (c) at home; (d) in transit, commuting, or traveling; and (e) other places.
Researchers initially reported that athletes used imagery primarily in training and practice
areas. However, more recent studies found that athletes used more imagery on the
competition field than training areas (Barr & Hall, 1992; Hall et al., 1990). Salmon and
61
the home, school, and at work. Dancers and musicians imagined in all areas of the
rehearsal or practice studio and the performing venue, such as the dressing room,
backstage, and on stage (Bellon, 2006; Nordin & Cumming, 2005; Trusheim, 1987).
They also reported using imagery while in transit or commuting, on the train, driving, in
quiet places, and at home, especially in bed. Both athletes and performers used imagery
in many places and settings, particularly those in which they could concentrate and
mentally practice and prepare for the performance. These varied by time, place, personal
According to the studies cited below, the specific times, or when, athletes and
performers participated in imagery constituted ten categories: (a) practice or rehearsal, (b)
performance or competition, (c) during quiet times, (d) in transit or traveling, (e) certain
types or styles of performances, (f) times of the day or night, (g) periods of the season or
year, (h) anytime or all the time, (i) in times of difficulty, and (j) during injury or health
problems. Many of these categories were similar to the location where imagery was
employed, since times chosen by these performers were connected with their scheduling
during or after competition (Barr & Hall, 1992; Rodgers et al., 1991; Weinberg, Butt,
Knight, Burke, & Jackson, 2003; Ungerleider & Golding, 1992). Other researchers
(Munroe et al., 2000; Salmon et al., 1992) found athletes using imagery more during
practice than before or after training sessions, however it was not known how directly
this practice was connected with performance. Oxendine (1969) proposed that “effective
62
mental rehearsal techniques may enable learners to practice at times when they are not
able to actively perform the tasks” (p. 755). Later, researchers (Weinberg & Gould, 2003;
White & Hardy, 1998) reported that athletes also used imagery when they were
experiencing tough or difficult times, under a lot of pressure, during sickness or injury, or
fatigue while convalescing, or while physical practice was not possible. Outside of
school (Weinberg & Gould, 2003). Athletes and artistic performers were found to use
imagery just prior to going to sleep (Bellon, 2006; Hall et al., 1990; Munroe et al., 2000;
Musicians and dancers imagined more during practice and rehearsals than before
or after, and more before performances than during or after (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993;
Nordin & Cumming, 2005; Trusheim, 1987). Performers also used more imagery when
For example, dancers imagined more if metaphors were needed in an expressive piece
(Nordin & Cumming, 2005), and singers used more imagery during song recitals than
operatic performances (Carter, 1993). Both dancers and musicians engaged in mental
rehearsal when they were tired, injured, or in a place where they could not overtly
rehearse. They also used imagery extensively to alleviate performance anxiety, for
instance when they felt nervous, anxious, or distracted. Furthermore, musicians and
dancers imagined more during the time of year when their schedules were heavy with
rehearsals and performances (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Nordin & Cumming, 2005;
Trusheim, 1987). Generally, athletes and artistic performers used imagery throughout the
63
normal day, and specifically when different types of imagery were used to facilitate
practice or performance.
and her colleagues (2000) identified six specific categories: (a) sessions, (b)
effectiveness, (c) nature of imagery, (d) surroundings, (e) types of imagery, and (f)
controllability. Sessions referred to the duration of the imagery engagement which were
directly related to findings in the where and when category. Effectiveness involved the
participants’ evaluation of the usefulness of their imagery practices. Whether the images
were positive, negative, or the degree of their accuracy comprised the nature of imagery.
Types of imagery encompassed the specific uses of four senses: (a) vision, including
internal and external perspective and vividness; (b) auditory, or sounds; (c) kinesthetic or
the feel of the movements; and (d) olfactory, or smells. Athletes used the kinesthetic and
vision senses the most and involved experiences of the specific sport or venue of
the image and the speed with which the image was executed. While all these categories
are critical in describing athletes’ imagery, the organization of this was preferred in
Nordin and Cumming (2005) dance imagery investigation due to its closer approximation
to the artistic experiences of the professional vocalists in this study. Nordin and
Cumming (2005) adapted the results from the framework derived from athletes’ imagery
findings of Munroe and her colleagues (see Figure 1), and determined that dancers’
imagery content was divided between types and characteristics. Henceforth in this
64
chapter, each of the important findings from athletes and related literature was reported
dancers’ imagery, Nordin & Cumming, 2005) identified six different types of imagery
investigated were: (a) execution, (b) metaphoric and artistic, (c) context, (d) body-related,
(e) character and role, and (e) irrelevant images. Each content area was examined in
detail for its use in achieving optimal athletic, artistic, and musical performance.
Execution images. The image types described as execution included (a) skill
learning and technique; (b) sequencing, planning, and strategies; and (c) goal imagery.
Skill learning images included specific images in mentally practicing actions and
methods for performance. Mental practice studies in sport and musical performance are
addressed in the section on why later in this chapter. Munroe and others (2000)
distinguished execution images in two distinct areas: (a) skill development in technique
learning and skill building, and (b) performance enhancement, including correctional
efforts.
Skill images were generally regarded as imagery functions in the why category
and not a part of content. However, athletes have illustrated the value of being able to
visualize specific skills and its relationship to physical execution of that skill. Mac
Basically it boils down to the fact that if you’re trying to accomplish something, a
particular athletic movement, if you can’t visualize it then it’s pure chance you
will be able to perform the movement. If you visualize it and can really see it . . .
65
you have a clear target to aim for and a much better chance of realizing that
This quote underscores the connection between being able to mentally execute skills and
physically performing them. For athletes, skill images depended upon the type of sport in
which the athlete was engaged. Similarly, specific images of instrumental musicians
for brass players (Trusheim, 1987) or holding the wrist in a way that facilitated playing
arpeggios on the piano. For singers, images of opening the sinuses and the throat to
achieve better resonance or performing octave leaps correctly would be considered skill
execution images.
Planning and sequencing images in performance also dealt with development and
execution of strategies and routines (Munroe et al., 2000; Nordin & Cumming, 2005).
(Fenker & Lambiotte, 1987), strategies in wrestling (Rushall, 1988), and routines such as
those in figure skating and gymnastics (White & Hardy, 1998). Munroe and her
colleagues (2000) reported strategy and sequence imagery in the cognitive aspects of the
why category and not as a separate content. Musicians imagined strategies and plans in
strategies of handling difficult passages, and ways of coping with distractions (Bellon,
Research in imagery with regard to goal attainment has only recently emerged.
Sports research in goal setting and achievement involved three different types of goals:
(a) outcome, (b) performance, and (c) process goals (Weinberg & Gould, 1995). Outcome
66
goals in this study focused on the final result of the effort or endeavor. Performance goals
involved the projection of exceeding one’s own efforts in a particular setting without
regard to other competitors. Process goals contained the desired effect of improving more
specific performance functions. Weinberg and Gould (1995) suggested that athletes
Woolfolk, Murphy, Gottesfeld, and Aitken (1985) found that athletes who imagined the
outcome of their specific sport activity just prior to performance subsequently influenced
that same activity more than mental rehearsal. Reports of goal imagery were included as
motivational aspects of imagery reasons in the study by Munroe and colleagues (2000).
conceptions of performance goals. The more complete and descriptively detailed the
performance goals were in the mind of the performer, the more successful the actual
performers keep focused on their goals by visualizing ahead of time the specific
performance both in parts and as a whole. This gave the individual a clear blueprint of all
the components involved in its successful execution. The application of different goals
Artistic performers used goal images to improve skill and performances, identify
and execute desired roles to perform, and mentally project future stagings (Carter, 1993).
Performance goals were found to often intermingle with outcome goals for artistic
professionals. Dancers used goal images to imagine being the best, perform difficult
skills, and set long and short-term goals (Nordin & Cumming, 2005). Professional singers
expected and projected positive outcomes, regularly setting achievable and effective
67
goals (Carter, 1993). The following quote exemplified outcome and performance goals
made by the world-renowned tenor, Luciano Pavarotti, early in his illustrious career. He
stated:
When I began to study, I made lists of all the roles I wanted to sing, and the age at
which I planned to sing them. At the top I put ‘Rudolfo – 22.’ I did it at 26.
Everything has come later than I planned, and I am not sorry. I thought to retire
myself at 55, but for now the voice feels [fine]. (Luciano Pavarotti, cited in
Crutchfield, 1985)
Pavarotti was 50 when he gave this interview and was still singing until very close to his
death at 71. Christine Harel, a freelance singer, (Bellon, 2006) described process goals in
I also have objectives such as: I would really like, this time, to be able to stand in
a particular way while singing; or also: I would like for my voice to stay in a
particular place in my body while singing. I can have various objectives based on
where I am at with my instrument [voice] at the time. This comes from the
feedback that I got from previous performances and what I would like to improve.
(p. 17)
used in artistic performance and have been included in more current investigations in
several sports, such as figure skating and artistic gymnastics (Ruiz & Hanin, 2004).
Recent studies have revealed the successful use of metaphorical imagery in athletes
68
regulating affect in achieving flow states (Hanin & Stambolva, 2002; Ruiz & Hanin,
2004). Nordin and Cumming (2005) reported dancers using various kinds of metaphorical
imagery with performance, “including images of color, objects that are not present,
actions that cannot actually be performed, the environment, and themes” (p. 403).
Musicians have used metaphorical and poetic imagery for technical execution and
and historic genre of the piece have dictated specific imagery and interpretation. Carter
(1993) reported singers using more imagery in performing recitals than oratorio or opera.
Conductors were known to share certain metaphors or poetic images for interpreting
phrases, movements, and entire symphonies (Barten, 1992, 1998; Bellon, 2006; Carter,
1993; Trusheim, 1987). Conductors, directors, and musical instructors also used
allegorical and descriptive language and imagery, story telling, and kinesthetic and visual
metaphoric or poetic images related to affect and artistic expression (Bellon, 2006;
Carter, 1993; Coffin, 1987; Leyerle, 1986; Miller, 1996) as opposed to the definition of
imagery used in sport psychology. Professional singers in Carter’s (1993) and Bellon’s
69
Pavarotti imagining an airplane ascending as he sang a rising series of pitches (Hines,
1982).
Within the vocal pedagogical community there had been much controversy
concerning the appropriate use of imagery in teaching voice (Titze, 1986). According to
Vennard (1971), imagery was often a description a teacher would employ to help students
philosophy is that while the physical details are either unknown or not directly
controllable, the experience of good tone production can be described in figures of speech
which will enable the student to grasp it” (p. 15). Vennard cautioned the singing teacher
to be careful only to use imagery with ideas that the student singer could grasp easily
through past experience. He referred to “float the tone on the breath” and “don’t reach for
high notes; approach them from above,” as statements that had particular meaning only to
and scientific approach to singing teaching. Reid (1983) strongly cautioned against the
the use of imagery cannot be considered pertinent to the central issue. Imagery,
70
therefore, is not an important factor in the voice building process. (Reid, 1983, p.
155)
realized that metaphorical imagery was sometimes necessary in teaching but held the
conviction that subjective uses of imagery could go counter to the more scientific, and
therefore more technical approach of vocal training. Dayme (1982) also argued for a
more integrated approach, and said, “When all the available knowledge of the mechanics
and artistry of singing is used, then the result can only be a better understanding and
vocal texts from the early part of the twentieth century with its more recent usage. Many
of these terms were still employed in contemporary pedagogy and were directly related to
skill acquisition in voice training (e.g., learning breath support; achieving and executing
proper sensations and placement of the voice). Singers in Carter’s (1993) study reported
using such imagery (e.g. “bellows,” “bouncing ball on a water fountain,” “stretching a
rubber band”), which were used to facilitate vocal production. Freed recommended that
experiences. Given the subjective and artistic definition of imagery, advising caution in
its use was understandable. Nonetheless, metaphorical imagery in vocal pedagogy was
often required.
While the dangers in using metamorphic imagery were clear, its use in music was
well established and was not likely to change, due to the abstract nature of music (Barten,
1998). However, imagery, as it has been used and investigated in psychology and sport
71
psychology, offers a much broader definition and encompasses mental aspects of musical
and singing development, as exemplified in the work of several pedagogues (Caldwell &
Wall, 2001; Fields, 1972; Ware, 1998). However, music performance has remained a
creative art in which metaphor was valid, especially in singers who created and
emotional, and artistic requirements in dance, music, and singing. Dancers, as well as
singers, imagined the behaviors and emotions of their characters as well as their physical
movements and appearance (Carter, 1993; Craig, 1992; Hanrahan & Vergeer, 2000;
Nordin & Cumming, 2005). These were related to the experience of certain qualities,
human or non-human, for instance, a dancer recreating the swan in Swan Lake. Even
instrumentalists in musical compositions have had certain roles in which they created
characters, such as those in Prokofiev’s Peter and the Wolf, wherein specific instruments
represented the different characters in the story. Many musical compositions suggested
some kind of affective ideal or image from which the performer could embody and
present the work. However, in singing, often the interaction of music with the text more
Operatic singers recreated their characters using words presented vocally, dramatically,
musically, as well as physically, mentally, and affectively (Barten, 1992; Carter, 1993;
Creating characters and roles in art song recitals was considered more rigorous
and difficult than those in opera or oratorio. Songs for recitals require an entire subjective
world to be recreated mentally, dramatically, and emotionally for each song in order to
72
achieve an effective performance (Barten, 1992; Carter, 1993; Emmons & Sonntag,
2002). Lotte Lehman (1945/1985) in More Than Singing, offered suggestions for
interpreting lieder of many composers (e.g., Brahms and Schumann). She often included
entire scenarios performers could imagine in recreating the song. For example, in
performing Gute Nacht from Die Winterreise by Schubert, she described the specific
Imagine that you are this man who is on the verge of complete disintegration. It is
a cold winter night. You have decided to leave during the night for if you left in
the daylight, you might see her, from whom you are fleeing, and perhaps if you
should see her again you would weaken and would not find the strength to go. But
you can no longer bear the torture of being near her. You will lose your mind if
The poet and composer created an entire dramatic world in these lieder songs, the dictates
Even though Lehmann maintained that specific images were dictated in the text of
these songs, elite singers in Carter’s (1993) study often felt images were private and were
flexible according to specific need. Some singers created entire stories and scenarios to
act out the images of their songs. Most singers felt that embodying subtext, or underlying
meaning, came from the text or story of the opera or song. Feeling appropriate emotions
and imagining past personal experiences in order to more completely embody the
character of the song best accomplished this. The dramatic skill of imagining the subtext
allowed the performer to unlock emotional significance and communicate the intentions
73
Many singers’ accounts of their lives in biographies dealt with their experience of
vocal production and interpretation (Ferrier, 1955; Horne & Scorvell, 1984).
Autobiographies such as Lehmann, (1945/1985) and Ferrier (1955) revealed how these
singers brought intuition and imagination into their characters and roles in performances.
Hines’ (1982) interviews of 40 internationally recognized male and female opera singers
illustrated some experiences in creating roles for many operas. Developing singers could
use these accounts as models for their own work with bringing characters to life on the
Context images. Both athletes and performers have reported the use of context
images involving the environment, other people, and specific situations and venues.
Imagery context has been recognized as an important element since the beginning of
Before the 1976 Summer Olympics, representatives from the Soviet Union shot
pictures of the Olympic facilities in Montreal. These pictures were returned to the
Soviet Union and studied by the athletes. Although the Soviet athletes had not
the athletes with the Olympic environment before they arrived. (Vealey, 1986, p.
211)
This type of image was reported in athletes’ imagery as various elements in the
surroundings (e.g., sounds from spectators, smell of the grass) (Munroe et al., 2000).
Nordin and Cumming (2005) identified this type as another category that emerged from
dancers’ imagery research. Prior to this sport imagery research related to context had
74
rarely been a separate subject and was incorporated in various studies such as those on
qualities of specific performances spaces. These and the musicians in Bellon’s (2006) and
Carter’s (1993) studies described images with other musicians in the performing space or
venue in front of an audience. Furthermore, these singers imagined the entire scenarios of
songs they had created in their minds while mentally and physically performing on stage.
appearance, musical expression, and health concerns (Carter, 1993; Hanrahan & Vergeer,
2000; Munroe et al., 2000; Nordin & Cumming, 2005). Much of the sport research of
body-related imagery has been included in studies investigating kinesthetic imagery, and
The body related category emerged from the Nordin and Cumming (2005) dance
imagery study. Dancers often employed a number of body-related images. Physical and
emotional feelings were connected with arousal, muscular tension, and affective
sensations. Several dancers imagined various and specific parts of their anatomy for
individual performance execution. Dancers also used images of their appearance and that
of their characters in more effectively relating to the audience (Nordin & Cumming,
2005).
Singers who used body-related imagery shared some of the same concerns as
dancers, and had some that differed. Carter (1993) discussed vocalists’ use of imagery
75
based on Leyerle’s (1986) five theoretical categories of organic imagery in singing.
psychological problem areas. Since Garcia II developed the laryngoscope in 1854 (Garcia
& Paschke, 1975), singers have been able to see and understand the actual anatomy of the
larynx. Subsequently vocal pedagogues have advocated for the increased incorporation of
vocal anatomical images, which were considered more scientific than metaphorical
imagery (Dayme, 1982; Leyerle, 1986; Miller, 1996; Vennard, 1968). More recent texts
have included extensively detailed illustrations of the various aspects of vocal and human
anatomy as it relates to singing (Chapman, 2006; Miller, 2004). Singers imagined their
entire physical body and vocal mechanism, or “interior posture” in controlling the vocal
sound and physically creating their character (Bellon, 2006). Vocalists were found to
have had images arise concerning their ability to sing or not, especially having fears of
losing their voice (Stedman, 1985). Due to the fact that singers depended on the health of
their body as their musical instrument, they used many different body-related images
The use of body-related imagery in achieving musical sound and expression is not
(Carter, 1993) used kinesthetic imagery to mental hearing of pitches and musical
passages. Furthermore, one of the singers in Carter’s (1993) study practiced physical
movements related to the rhythm of the music as the result of training in Dalcroze
and subsequently apply the use of kinesthetic imagery to strengthen rhythm and connect
with the natural tension and expression in the particular piece of music. Learning the
76
words in physical rhythmic motion also facilitated memory for several of Carter’s
singers.
investigation of the cognitive and motivational functions of imagery, Sordoni, Hall and
Forwell (2000) developed the Athletic Injury Imagery Questionnaire (AIIQ) and found
runners, older athletes, and those who had been to a sports physician were more likely to
monitor their body for pain and injury than younger and less educated athletes
(Ungerleider, 1992). Athletes reported that recovery from injury was enhanced using
positive self-talk and images of healing (Ievleva & Orlick, 1991). Green (1992) suggested
possible uses of imagery to facilitate the many stages of healing athletic injury. Some
musical performers also used imagery in healing their bodies after injury or coping with
instrument or genre to continue and augment physical practice during fatigue and injury
The mind-body paradigm literature had supported the connection of imagery with
review). Early studies with cancer patients using imagery to combat cancer reported
& Sparks, 1971; Simonton, Simonton, & Creighton, 1978). Studies (Schwartz, 1984;
Schwartz & Kussek, 2003) have suggested that the human being is a complete system
made up of a number of subsystems including mind and body. These parts interact
77
resulting in new emergent properties which are facilitated and achieved in the use of
Irrelevant images. Irrelevant images emerged in the dance research of Nordin and
Cumming (2005) and were not directly addressed in the sport imagery research in Hall
and others (1998) and Munroe and colleagues (2000). Irrelevant images were identified
as those types that spontaneously arose that may have had nothing to do with practice or
performance. Musicians and singer were reported to have images that were not wanted
and that emerged spontaneously (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987). These
were found intertwined in the images they reported. One singer in Carter’s (1993) study
however these images directly related to her performance and could not be considered
irrelevant.
the content of imagery with which performers, more than athletes, were involved. Most
of these represented the vehicle in which artistic expression was achieved in creative
performance. The general qualities in how athletes and performers engage in imagery
product rather than imagery type or content previously discussed. According to Nordin
and Cumming (2005) the characteristics of imagery included six specific qualities: (a)
senses, (b) perspective, (c) ability, (d) direction, (e) deliberation, and (f) amount and
duration.
78
Senses. According to Richardson (1969), sensory imagery differed from
experience, but replicated the experience of that sensory information in the cognitive
processes (Richardson, 1969). Senses used in imagery were the same as those involved in
perception and included six categories: (a) visual, (b) auditory, (c) kinesthetic, (d) tactile,
In some of the earliest studies of the use of senses, Galton (1880, 1883) surveyed
visual imagery ability and found it more profoundly developed in engineers, women, and
schoolboys than scientists. In a diary study, Kosslyn, Seger, Pani, and Hillger (1990)
found that two-thirds of imagery in people’s normal life was visual. However, the use of
specific senses often depended on the individual and their chosen activities, or
experiences, as well as the type of sport, art form or music, the various skills and
requirements of the athlete or performer. For example, field athletes used more visual
imagery than marathon runners (Ungerleider & Golding, 1992), which may be related to
the visual components in field athletics. Furthermore, the extensive physical requirements
over time for long-distance athletes may require more kinesthetic imagery.
various sports have been examined in a number of studies. Results of sports research
have differed as to which imagery sense athletes use more, visual or kinesthetic. Murphy
and Martin (2002) found athletes preferred kinesthetic to visual imagery, however other
studies reported athletes were better visual than kinesthetic imagers (Barr & Hall, 1992;
Gregg et al., 2005; Hall & Martin, 1997; Moran, 2004; Murphy et al., 2008). Ryan and
Simons (1982) found that better visual or kinesthetic imagers showed more improvement
79
than those with weak visual or kinesthetic skills. Furthermore, soccer players not only
used visual and kinesthetic imagery extensively, but some also reported using auditory
imagery (Salmon et al., 1994). Munroe and others (2000) reported athletes in their study
used visual, kinesthetic, auditory, and olfactory sense imagery. Sport psychologists
generally recommended that athletes use as many sense modalities as possible in their
imagery to increase effectiveness (Moran, 2004; Morris et al., 2005; Murphy et al. 2008;
of their art. Dancers in Nordin and Cumming’s (2005) study reported experiencing multi-
sensory imagery: kinesthetic, visual, and aural. Elite musicians used the same three sense
modalities in their profession (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Holmes, 2005; Moyer, 1992;
Ross, 1985; Trusheim, 1987). MacPherson (1997) found more experienced musicians
were able to connect their kinesthetic and auditory image with playing wherein the “link
between fingers and singers was automatic and seemed to occur without any conscious
effort” (p. 70). In her interview of a solo cellist and guitarist, Holmes (2005) reported that
instrumentalists imagined the sound, the feel of muscles, the visual image of movement,
as well as the appropriate affect and expression in preparing the actual execution of
music. Recreating the kinesthetic feeling of the proper breath control, together with
imagining the perfect embouchure of the mouth for brass players (Trusheim, 1987) or
position of the fingers for string players (Holmes, 2001), also played a major part in
Although reports of athletes experiencing sounds in imagery are less frequent than
other senses, the difference between their use of auditory imagery must be distinguished
80
from how auditory imagery is generally employed by professional musicians. Athletes
heard the sounds of their sport in their inner hearing, such as splashing of water in the
pool, footsteps on the track, or crowds cheering (Munroe et al., 2000). Most of the
music in context (Gordon, 1999), which is exemplified in the studies that follow.
Research in assessing sensory ability in imagery with regard to music began being
reported over a century ago. Betts (1909) investigated pitch accuracy and found that
student musicians who reported using auditory imagery scored higher than those who did
not. In a subsequent experiment of the same study, Betts (1909) reported that in sight-
reading or hearing a familiar tune, participants used kinesthetic and auditory images for
adults and children, Agnew (1922a) found that children and musicians scored higher than
non-musical adults in auditory vividness tests. Seashore (1919) worked extensively in the
area of cognitive music assessment and auditory imagery in his 10-point inventory of
musical talent. Seashore (1938/1967) and later Farnsworth (1958) recognized that
auditory imagery interacting with kinesthetic, or motor imagery, was often accompanied
by visual images. Seashore wrote, “The motor imaginal [sic] type is ordinarily also well
developed . . . the motor tendency to image the tone or execute it in inceptive movements
is highly developed in the musical mind” (1938/1967, p. 6). These early studies served to
strengthen the investigation of vocalists’ imagery use and the ability in mentally
81
Instrumentalists and singers reported first learning their music internally prior to
playing or singing, and later continuing mental rehearsal in relearning and perfecting
pieces (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Holmes, 2001; Moyer, 1992; Trusheim, 1987). James
Olin, a brass professional in Trusheim’s study was quoted as saying, “What I try to do is
to visualize or auralize [sic] or whatever what I want to hear coming out of my horn.”
(cited in Trusheim, 1987, p. 179). Many musicians reported internally hearing their line
of music as well as those of others in the musical ensemble, and often every note in the
entire orchestra, opera, or composition (Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987). Singers who were
trained to play the keyboard reported internally hearing more of the accompaniment than
those who had no keyboard skills (Carter, 1993). This represented elements of Gordon’s
(1999) idea of audiation, which “takes place when we hear and understand in our minds
music that we have just heard performed or have heard performed sometime in the past”
(p. 41). Audiation was not just a high level of auditory imagery but one developed
which elite singers, instrumentalists, and conductors seem to share. This understanding of
imagery in musicians has been examined extensively over the years (e.g., Azzara, 1999;
As singers and musicians developed their art through playing, observational, and
which brain activity of hearing and imagining music was found to be similar (Kosslyn et
al., 2001; Zatorre & Halpern, 2005). Ideal vocal or instrumental sound was used to guide
singing and playing and became more sophisticated over time as influenced by listening
82
and remembering past models of others and one’s own performance experiences (Carter,
1993; Trusheim, 1987). The great violin master teacher, Dorothy DeLay, emphasized
p. 40). Musicians combined aural images of the ideal musical sound with the required
kinesthetic feeling necessary to produce specific qualities of timbre and expression of the
The practice of internally hearing the sound and replicating that ideal auditory
image on the instrument or in the voice replicate in performance has been examined in
the literature (Averino, 1989; Fields, 1972; Ware, 1998). In his treatise on Ideo-Kinetics,
Bonpensiere (1953) promoted concentration on the auditory image to achieve the desired
sound wherein one need only “ideate our end results in a process of flow . . . we ideate
our absolute unconcern with the ways and means to obtain them” (p. 14). Unlike
traditional piano pedagogy, Bonpensiere promoted the primary use of auditory, visual,
and kinesthetic intention while allowing the body to respond to the vividness of the
Never think of your music in terms of execution (of what your hands and fingers
should or are going to do) but in terms of interpretive rendering (what you would
expect it to sound like if a performer from heaven were executing it for you). (p.
69)
individual idea musical sound prior to playing (Trusheim, 1987). Using auditory and
83
In vocal pedagogy, Averino (1989) stated that “[i]n order to produce a song, the
singer must have it clearly in mind: pitch, rhythm, musical form and the emotional
content of the words. The performer is the imagination” (emphasis hers, p. 40). Fields
(1972) advocated that vocalists create a clear mental ideal auditory image of the sound
they desired to produce prior to its execution. In producing that aural image, Geraldine
Farrar’s statement reflected this in the following, “[W]e learn to know the sensations
produced in muscles of throat, head, face, lips and other parts of the anatomy, which
vibrate in a certain manner to correct tone production. We learn the feeling of the tone”
(cited in Brower & Cooke, 1996, p. 53). Lamperti concurred when he said: “Do not listen
to yourself sing! Feel yourself sing! When internal conditions are right and ready, the
singing voice appears not before” (cited in Brown, 1931/1973, p. 16). Since the body was
considered the singers’ instrument, they were not able to hear their own voices as others
hear them. Therefore singers learned early to equate the desired tone in auditory imagery
with the kinesthetic feeling of what was required to produce that exact sound (Bellon,
2006; Carter, 1993; Stedman, 1985). Furthermore, pitch and vowel production often
For vocalists, kinesthetic, auditory, and visual imagery were considered the most
important senses used for expressive and artistic performance (Bellon, 2006; Carter,
1993; Emmons & Thomas, 1998, 2008). Singers were found to use kinesthetic imagery to
control breath, feel rhythm, and physiologically respond with emotion and expression in
portraying a character in an aria or song (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993). Those who had
experienced viewing the internal mechanisms of their voice through laryngeal image
biofeedback (LIB) were able to connect these images both visually and kinesthetically,
84
imagining the vocal anatomy in preparing and singing a variety of desired sounds
(Cleveland, 1989a, 1989b, 1989c). With training and experience imagery using all the
senses can be trained and strengthened, which has been corroborated since imagery
seems to become more complex over time (Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987). The use of
sensory imagery seemed to be essential to the singers’ and musicians’ expressive and
artistic performance. How athletes and performers executed their images through the
external imagery was tested in many studies yielding a variety of results. Mahoney and
Avener (1977) were some of the first researchers to identify imagery perspective as an
important element in imagery use for the performer. Lane (1980) described internal
perspective (first person) as one of “involvement,” while the external viewpoint (third
“spectator.” Internal imagery was more highly correlated with motor movement than
external imagery (Hale, 1992; Harris & Robinson, 1986). More skilled athletes reported
more frequent use of internal than external imagery (Mahoney & Avener, 1977; Rotella,
Gansneder, Ojala, & Billing, 1980). Conversely, several studies found no significant
differences between the effectiveness of internal and external imagery (Barnes, 1982;
Highlen & Bennett, 1979; Mumford & Hall, 1985). However, in Hall and others (1990),
elite and novice athletes reported equal use of internal and external imagery, often
switching between them. Imagery perspective effectiveness was also correlated with
types of sport and performance task requirements (Hardy, 1997) with external imagery
more associated with sports in which form was essential. Sequence of actions were
85
improved with external imagery while internal perspectives were preferred if there was
only a single task to perform. Results of imagery benefit depended on the athlete’s (a)
imagery ability, (b) the ability to imagine the suggested image, and (c) the type of task
(Taylor, 1993). Performance skills with a concentration on form were more associated
and often perfected with external imagery, while using internal imagery perspective
The intermingling of kinesthetic senses with visual perspective imagery has been
the subject of some controversy (Callow & Hardy, 2004). In a study investigating the US
track and field participants of the Seoul Olympics, Ungerleider and Golding (1991, 1992)
found that more successful athletes used external perspective and stronger kinesthetic
imagery. Hale (1982) argued that the kinesthetic sense was more associated with internal
imagery, while others (White & Hardy, 1995; Hardy & Callow, 1999) held that
kinesthetic imagery involved both internal and external perspectives. Hardy (1997) and
White and Hardy (1995) proposed that different task requirements seemed to suggest
both internal and external imagery to increase effectiveness. Future studies investigating
the specific brain functions of vision and motor centers may shed more light on this
subject.
particularly in regard to their concern with bodily appearance on stage. Bellon (2006)
reported singers using internal perspective, especially regarding the mechanics of vocal
production, and external perspective for perceiving their appearance in relation to the
86
hall, stage, and audience. Singers also reported combining internal and external
perspectives to imagine the entire venue in which they were performing, including the
hall or arena, stage, audience, and dressing rooms. Musicians as well as dancers imagined
how they would feel and look to be in a new competition or performance space in order
to familiarize themselves with its many features (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Nordin &
Cumming, 2005; Trusheim, 1987). Although Carter (1993) did not directly identify the
descriptions suggested employment of both internal and external imagery. Caldwell and
Wall (2001) and Emmons and Thomas (1998) recommended that singers learn and
become proficient in using both internal and external imagery. Imagery perception also
Ability. One of the variables in imagery use was the ability of the individual and
the degree to which he or she could vividly produce mental images. It was also important
to discern what types of imagery the person preferred to employ, or those with which one
was familiar. The effectiveness of imagery was dependent on a number of factors: (a)
skill level, (b) past training and experience in imagery, and (c) degree to which imagery
was consciously directed. The type of athletic or artistic skill in which one was engaged
to perform may have also influenced this imagery ability. For example, because of
extensive training, musicians typically may have had the opportunity to develop more
auditory imagery ability than athletes who may have excelled in kinesthetic images.
Types of imagery ability included all the various sense modalities just as they were
experienced in real life. The differences in the development of mental use could have
been a result of the reinforcement and feedback, or lack of it, for using the various types
87
of imagery. The desired outcome for which the imagery was being used may have been
dependent on the individual’s ability to use imagery (Arieti, 1976; Martin et al., 1999;
Several instruments have been devised to assess imagery ability in athletes. One
of the earliest was Betts’ (1909) study, Distribution and Functions of Mental Imagery,
the instrument of which was later shortened by Sheehan (1972) and widely used in
imagery research. Marks (1973) created the Vividness of Visual Imagery Questionnaire,
or VVIQ. The two most popular and useful in sport were Vividness of Movement
Imagery Questionnaire (VMIQ; Isaac, Marks, & Russell, 1986) and the revised
Movement Imagery Questionnaire (MIQ-R; Hall & Martin, 1997). Two criticisms of
these instruments in assessing imagery ability were that much of the support comes from
the research team in which these tests were developed, and that these self-reported tests
were hard to replicate with subjects often reported positively at the upper end of the
The effects of imagery ability differences have been most examined in sport
performing motor skills, good imagers often had the advantage over poor imagers (Goss,
Hall, Buckolz, & Fishburne, 1986; Isaac, 1992). Rodgers and others (1991) reported
athletes’ imagery ability as being fairly well developed. Elite athletes used clear and
accurate imagery and were found to be better imagers than non-elite athletes (Barr &
Hall, 1992; Orlick & Parrington, 1988; Salmon et al., 1994; Ungerleider & Golding,
1992; Vadocz et al., 1997). Athletes better imagined skills in which they had already had
gained mastery and their imagery abilities increased the more they were used (Gregg et
88
al., 2005; Rodgers et al., 1991; Vadocz et al., 1997; Ungerlieder & Golding, 1991) and
the higher the effectiveness of that imagery became (Hall et al., 1990; Vadocz et al.,
1997; Vealey, 1986; Weinberg & Gould, 2003). Athletes with better imagery ability in
vividly producing and controlling images were thought to enjoy superior performances
(Isaac, 1992; Ryan & Simons, 1982). Researchers agreed that imagery was a skill that
could be developed (Hall, 1998; Hall & Martin, 1997; Morris et al., 2005) and recently
has been investigated more thoroughly using advanced medical imaging techniques.
(1922a) study reported that music teachers and children possessed good auditory
imagery. Most of the dancers in Nordin and Cumming’s (2005) investigation offered self-
reports of possessing the ability to produce vivid, life-like images, depending on the type
of imagery and the sense modality used. Some had difficulty with imagery in developing
or portraying characters or controlling imagery involving injury or pain. All but one of
the brass musicians in Trusheim’s (1987) study had no difficulty in creating and
controlling images. Many of these professionals had performed for many years and
reported that particular uses of imagery were generally quite vivid. Both singers (Carter,
1993) and musicians (Bellon, 2006) reported having facility with imagery. Generally,
artistic performers possessed fairly adequate ability in imagery. The next imagery
characteristic was the direction in which imagery was used by athletes and performers.
(2004), dealt with whether the image was facilitative or debilitative. In one of the first
experiments, Powell (1973) examined the effects of using positive and negative imagery
89
positively imagined the darts hitting in the center of the target, which led to higher scores,
another group imagined a negative performance of missing the bull’s eye, which resulted
in significantly poorer scores. Despite the lack of a control group, Powell concluded that
positive imagery content could be facilitative, and negative imagery could debilitate
Weinberg, & Jackson, 1980; Short et al., 2004; Woolfolk, Parrish, & Murphy, 1985). In
examining performance effects, Epstein (1980) and Woolfolk, Murphy and others (1985)
debilitated performance. Therefore, it could not be assumed that positive images were
facilitative or negative images were debilitative (Short, et al., 2004). Further research is
negative images with poorer performances in athletes (Hall et al., 1990), instrumental
musicians (Bellon, 2006; Trusheim, 1987), and vocalists (Carter, 1993), with some
exceptions (Bellon, 2006). Positively imaging the desired outcome improved athletic and
artistic performance, particularly during competition, while negatively imagining the task
seemed to be detrimental (Munroe et al., 2000; Woolfolk, Murphy et al., 1985). Positive
imagery was correlated with increased confidence (Moritz et al., 1996; Trusheim, 1987)
and decreased anxiety (Vadocz et al., 1997). Not all imagery was positive. Sometimes the
negative image, as in rerunning mistakes over and over, could degrade performance in
athletics (Murphy et al., 2008), dance (Nordin & Cumming, 2005), and music (Bellon,
2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987). Some of these issues were connected with imagery
deliberation.
90
Deliberation. Deliberation in imagery involved the individual’s ability to
1994), imagery facilitated learning especially for acquiring perceptual motor skills if it
from those spontaneously appearing to the ones that were fully manipulated (Murphy et
al., 2008). The area of deliberation was also connected with whether imagery was used
that could impact performance. Imagining inappropriate action, such as the negative
Using the wrong imagery type for a certain outcome could have perhaps negatively
and lack of control of spontaneous or certain kinds of imagery, and images producing
1999; Ristad, 1982). Researchers have investigated various ways in which athletes and
1994) and had been assessed by several instruments. Early psychological investigations
into controllability and manipulation of images focused on the problem of mental rotation
of an object (Shepard, 1978; Shepard & Metzler, 1971). Many other scales of imagery
ability have been developed and implemented in various settings. Gordon’s (1949) Test
of Visual Imagery Control (TVIC) was devised to assess control, the ability to
manipulate, or hold images in the mind. More recently, Vella-Brodrick and MacRae
(2004) reviewed the imagery assessment instruments used in the athletic context, the
91
discussion of which was beyond the scope of this study. However, the results were
promising for validating imagery use in performers and supported the need for more
It could be assumed that imagery practice improved performance but research has
shown that this was not always the case. In an early study investigating imagery control,
shooting only to imagine that it would not bounce and stuck to the floor. This
disturbed him to a point where he could not successfully visualize the shooting
Clark (1960) reported gains in self-confidence and error identification with participants
who learned to increase their imagery ability and control. Athletes with better imagery
ability in consciously producing and controlling images were found to enjoy superior
interpretation in imagery use, Murphy and Martin (2002) offered an account of several
skaters imitating the imagery of another skater who used an image of a ball of light to
facilitate his performance. Using this model image produced a variety of negative effects
in the other young skaters, highlighting the individualistic nature of imagery and the need
imagery; whether it is used to help the performer often depended on experience and
intrusive images such as rerunning a poor performance (Bellon, 2006; Nordin &
92
Cumming, 2005; Trusheim, 1987). Dancers reported actions or sensations “triggering”
Amount and duration. Amount and duration were two ways in which time spent
engaging in imagery has been examine. Amount of imagery referred to how often
individuals engage in the actual imagery session. Imagery duration was the length of time
it took for the image to appear, remain or proceed, and fade. It seemed to vary by
individuals, different sports and performance types, skill level of performer, imagery
The type of execution in which the individual was engaged may have determined
the length of the imagery session. For instance, imaging application of a single skill
would have only taken a short amount of time, while imaging an entire performance of a
role in an opera would have taken a much longer time, perhaps as long as it would have
taken to actually perform it. Hall (2001) recommended more imagery practice for best
results. Ungerleider and Golding (1992) reported that the amount of mental practice time
was directly related to the amount in which track and field athletes engaged in training.
Only a few studies have examined the amount necessary for imagery
effectiveness. Short imagery sessions of one to three minutes were found to be more
effective than longer ones of five to seven minutes for basketball players (Etnier &
Landers, 1996). In their meta-analysis, Driskell and others (1994) reported that the longer
one engages in mental practice, the less effective are the results. They recommended that
93
imagery sessions and training last about 20 minutes for optimal results. More research is
Imagery duration included the amount of time it took for the emergence of an image, the
time in which one was actually engaged in imaging, the speed of the imagery, or how
long its usefulness lasted (Nordin & Cumming, 2005). Mackay (1981) demonstrated that
imagery reflected internal cognitive processing and therefore it could be assumed that
imagery directly correlated to one’s relationship with how performance was executed.
Imagery times were highly correlated with actual time it took to execute that skill in
slalom races (MacIntyre & Moran, 1996) and canoe-slalom (Hall & Martin, 1997).
Experts’ imagery of their springboard dives times were closest to actual times (Reed,
2002), whereas divers with less experience employed longer imagery durations than those
Studies focusing on the amount and duration of imagery use in artistic performers
remained sparse. However, Nordin and Cumming (2005), Carter, (1993), and Trusheim
(1987) reported that performers often engaged in mental imagery, depending on the time
of season, type of performance requirements, and the individual’s experience. One of the
instrumentalists in Bellon’s (2006) study reported up to one third of his practice time was
spent in mental practice. It was possible that the more years involved with the
performance, the more the individual engaged in imagery and the more effective mental
practice became. Durations of imagery varied for different performers. Dancers reported
using slow, actual, and fast speeds in using imagery especially in running through
94
reported brass instrumentalists using a compressed mental practice session. A singer
reported imagining in real time, the same as actual performance (Bellon, 2006). Imagery
speed seemed to vary with the individual needs, requirements of the task, and personal
preferences. The various aspects of why imagery was used by athletes’ and artistic
In examining the reasons why athletes and performers used the various types of
imagery, a number of empirical developments changed how imagery use and function
were regarded. It was originally thought that athletes used imagery for skill acquisition
and training (Feltz & Landers, 1983). However, more evidence surfaced that imagery was
also being used to enhance performance and motivation (Hall et al., 1990; Munroe et al.,
1998; Paivio, 1985; Weinberg & Gould, 2003). Athletes were found to use both cognitive
and motivational imagery in varying degrees (Hall et al., 1998; Paivio, 1985) depending
on the sport, the individual athlete, level of competition in which they were involved, and
the time of season (Cumming & Hall, 2002a; Hall et al., 1998; Munroe et al., 1998).
Furthermore, one image could be used for several reasons as readily as several types of
imagery could be used for a single purpose (Callow & Waters, 2005; Fish et al., 2004;
Murphy et al., 2008; Nordin & Cumming, 2005; Short et al., 2004; Short et al., 2006). In
examining the purposes dancers used imagery, Nordin and Cumming (2005) added
artistic and healing reasons, which were deemed appropriate to how singers used imagery
sport psychology and its adaptations and correlations to artistic and musical performance
95
Analytic framework of imagery use. In analyzing the research on why athletes and
(1985) analytic framework of imagery provided a foundation for understanding use and
functions, both of which operated at a specific and general level. This 2 X 2 orthogonal
The resulting four imagery areas were: (a) cognitive specific (CS), or skill acquisition,
development, and performance; (b) cognitive general (CG), strategies and routines; (c)
motivational specific (MS), goal oriented behavior; and (d) motivational general (MG),
physiological and affective arousal. Later, Hall and others (1998) divided motivational
mental toughness, self-efficacy and concentration (see Table 3). The major contributions
and findings of studies addressing the analytic framework of imagery use in sport, dance,
and music, including cognitive, motivational, artistic, and healing reasons were pertinent
to why singing professionals use imagery in achieving optimal performance and will now
be examined.
understand certain challenges, or simply change concepts and beliefs (Nordin &
Cumming, 2005; Murphy et al., 2008). These cognitive reasons for using imagery
encompassed functions of both the CS, or skills and mental practice, and SG, or game
96
Table 3
Cognitive Motivational
Mastery (MG-M)
Note: Adapted from Paivio (1985) and Hall, Mack, Paivio, and Hausenblas (1998, p.74).
Cognitive specific (CS), or mental practice, had been the most widely researched
area of imagery, and was generally understood as the internal learning, practice, and
performance of both cognitive and motor, or physical, skills (Driskell et al., 1994; Feltz
& Landers, 1983; Hall et al., 1990; Hall, Schmidt, Durand, & Buckolz, 1994; Mahoney &
Avener, 1977). Results of the extensive volume of mental practice studies were
summarized in several meta-analyses (e.g., Driskell et al., 1994; Feltz & Landers, 1983;
Hinshaw, 1991; Richardson, 1967a, 1967b). Athletes used mental practice imager to
learn skills, techniques, and actions in order for them to become automatic in
97
performance. Mental practice was found to be more effective for novices in more
cognitive than physical tasks, while “experienced subjects benefited equally well from
mental practice regardless of task type” (Driskell et al., 1994, p. 488). Generally it was
accepted that the effects of mental practice were moderated by a several factors: (a) type
of task, (b) retention interval or duration of time between treatment and assessment, and
(c) experience levels of the subjects. While a full analysis of the many studies in mental
practice was beyond the scope of this study, the findings as they directly related to
The use of mental practice in musical performance had been explored in a number
practice (PP) with mental practice (MP) in memorization of specific musical excerpts.
These included studies in piano (Coffman, 1987, 1990; Highben & Palmer, 2004; Lim &
Lippman, 1991; Lo, 1976; Rubin-Rabson, 1941), trombone (Ross, 1985a, 1985b),
clarinet (Coffman, 1987), guitar, and vocal performance (Theiler & Lippman, 1995). The
of these mental practice studies listed alphabetically by author. These studies were
conducted in testing different applications of physical practice (PP) and mental practice
98
Table 4
Coffman 80:40 College a) PP, b) Yes, sight- 4 part church Performance Second half MP
(1987) male, music majors MP reading hymn for time duration, of alternating
study on 40 novice piano alternating motivational children pitch, rhythm composition w/PP
no feedback
movement removed
or sound
Lim & 7 College piano a) MP with No 6-16 m. (25- Note accuracy, 2 posttests Physical
99
Table 4 (continued)
Lo Piano College class a) PP, b) Yes (PP Rhythm, Yes, Pre and MP group
a four-part PP group
hymn
movements,
d) control
Rabson 3 groups pianists (ages prestudy, etudes from later & 7 facilitated
Theiler 7 College (a) all PP, Control 4 17th C. Pitch & rhythm 2 MP &
& guitarists, guitar and (b) MP w/ mixed Guitar accuracy, posttests continuous
Lippman, 7 voice majors PP, (c) MP, with PP Dance articulation & w/score, PP most
(1995) vocalists (ages 19-29 aural model excerpts, phrasing, 2 effective for
100
One of the first researchers to conduct mental practice studies in musical
students using combined mental practice (MP) and physical practice (PP) with all
which included: (a) PP, MP, PP; (b) PP then MP; and (c) PP plus extra PP. Results
indicated that subjects who used mental practice in the middle of learning had the higher
scores for memory than subjects who practiced mentally after learning. Scores for this
group were retained weeks later, while the other groups’ scores declined.
significantly better than the PP only group in class piano subjects. Coffman (1988) found
that alternating MP and PP was superior to the PP only or MP only groups in trials of
novice pianists learning a composition. Lim and Lippman (1991) examined seven piano
majors’ mental practice effects. After 10 minutes of rehearsal, scores were higher in the
group using physical practice followed by listening to an aural model and mental
practice. In examining mental practice in guitarists and vocalists, Theiler and Lippman’s
(1995) design included a control and three treatments: (a) PP only; (b) PP and MP; and
(c) a combination of PP, MP, and an aural model. For guitarists, MP treatment was
superior to all others for pitch accuracy. The results for vocalists in this study revealed
Mental practice studies have also examined the use of movement, or simulated
performance, and aural ability in conjunction with imagery. In dance, Hanrahan, Tetreau,
and Sarrazin (1995) also found that MP enhanced performance when movement was
101
incorporated. Ross (1985a) reported that combining physical and mental practice was the
most effective method of mastering a musical task, however the inclusion of movement
simulating practice with either mental or physical practice was not significant. Highben
and Palmer (2004) found that aural ability, more than motor ability significantly affected
the memory of a newly learned piece for 16 pianists. Highben and Palmer suggested
focus on the aural image rather than the kinesthetic activity of hand movements
those of physical practice, although physical practice still seemed optimal, especially in
benefit slightly less from MP, than those with intermediate experience. Novices lacked
technical skill and experts achieved a ceiling effect since they had less to learn.
memory, change their habits, and solidify corrections, which related to CS functions.
Imagery aided their physical practice by making it more conceptually concrete and
streamlining their rehearsal (Nordin & Cumming, 2005). Artistic performers were found
to often use imagery for learning and refining skills and techniques, and memorization in
order to automate skills and interpretations (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Nordin &
execution of sequences and strategies included rehearsing game plans and routines
102
(Munroe et al., 2000; Murphy et al., 2008). Researchers investigated various CG imagery
in two similar ways: (a) routines such as those in artistic gymnastics (White & Hardy,
1998) and on the pommel horse (Mace, Eastman, & Carroll, 1987) and (b) strategies as
found in slalom canoe races (MacIntyre & Moran, 1996), football (Fenker & Lambiotte,
1987), and wrestling (Rushall, 1988). Results in using CG imagery were reported to be
familiarizing themselves with performance spaces and venues (Nordin & Cumming,
organizing their practice for better performance execution, and to execute conductors’
directions and plan routines (Bellon, 2006; Trusheim, 1987). They also used imagery to
coordinate difficult aspects of performance, deal with technical and musical challenges,
and cope with performance distractions and varying acoustical effects of different
performance halls (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987). Furthermore, singers
costumes, props, and sets, and dealing with blocking and venue considerations (Carter,
1993).
reasons: (a) goal setting and achievement, or motivational specific (MS); (b) modifying
103
Athletes reported using various types of imagery in the MS function to set and
1990; Murphy et al., 2008). Orlick and Parrington (1988) interviewed 75 Canadian 1984
Olympian athletes in their practices and reported, “The best athletes had clear daily goals.
They knew what they wanted to accomplish each day, each workout, each sequence or
interval. They were determined to accomplish these goals and focused fully on doing so”
(p. 111).
Dancers reported using imagery to aid their use of goals (MS) and to increase
their incentive to continue, particularly in the face of difficulty (Nordin & Cumming,
2005). This was also found in singers who also used imagery to progress in their goals of
gaining roles, winning competitions, or just improving certain skills (Bellon, 2006,
Carter, 1993). The goal imagery most associated with motivation seemed to involve
mastery (Lacaille, Whipple, & Koestner, 2005), which also related to preparation and
stress, and competitive anxiety levels, that is “psyching-up” and relaxation or calming
down (Barr & Hall, 1992; Monsma & Overby, 2004; Orlick, 1990; Salmon et al., 1994;
Vadocz et al., 1997; White & Hardy, 1998). Munroe and others (2000) identified three
elements of arousal and stress in motivational imagery: (a) excitement, (b) control, and
(c) relaxation. Athletes and artistic performers differed in arousal requirements due to
104
Extensive research in general psychology has been conducted in the use of
Relaxation (PMR) was a scheme in which Jacobson (1957) employed imagery techniques
to systematically tense and then relax each muscle. Meichenbaum (1977, 1985)
subjects were taught to use imagery and self-instruction and self-talk directives, such as
“stay calm” and “easy does it.” In extending the work of Jacobson, Suinn’s (1986) Visuo-
Motor Behavior Rehearsal (VMBR) was originally developed for sports and focused
primarily on creating a relaxed state in which imagery could better affect specific areas of
performance.
facilitate successful performance (e.g., Benson, 1987; Gawain, 1978, 2002; Harris, 1986).
Performers have often had to contend with seemingly uncontrollable levels of nervous
tension, the effort of which has tended to increase negative effects. Green and Galway
(1986) and Ristad (1982) have recommended that performers accept the symptoms of
anxiety by reframing the context of their individual meaning. Singers in Carter’s (1993)
study differentiated between positive or anticipation and working tension and negative
relaxation imagery to facilitate performance in sport and music (Bellon, 2006; Carter,
Optimal arousal levels in performance varied with athletes, type of sport, as well
as specific task requirements (Schmidt, 1982). Athletes used MG-A images to control
105
emotions and stress levels, particularly prior to performance (Hecker & Kaczor, 1988);
Munroe et al., 1998). Athletes used many types of imagery to change their emotional
condition, the MG-A function, particularly to relax, “psych up,” and energize themselves,
Murphy et al., 2008; Orlick, 1990; Oxendine, 1980; Vadocz et al., 1997). Oxendine
(1980) reported that highly anxious participants in his study were successful in low
difficulty tasks and low anxious subjects were more successful in high difficulty tasks.
This was illustrated in the Yerkes-Dodson Law, which stated, “complex tasks are
performed better when one’s drive is low, while simple tasks are performed better when
drive is high” (Oxendine, 1980, p.104). In contrast to the high levels of MG-A imagery
used by athletes to facilitate performance, dancers and musicians required much more
subdued levels of arousal (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Cumming & Nordin, 2005;
Stedman, 1985; Trusheim, 1987). Musicians generally performed better when they were
relaxed and calmed, however, many believed that some anxiety, or adrenaline, was
The practice of relaxation in imagery has been the subject of some controversy. It
had long been an accepted practice to require relaxation prior to imagery engagement as
proposed in Jacobson’s (1957) Progressive Muscular Relaxation (PMR) and Suinn (1976,
1986) Visuo-Motor Behavior Rehearsal (VMBR). Little empirical support for this did not
affect the insistence of sport psychologists to require relaxation before motor imagery
(Weinberg & Gould, 1999). However, Holmes and Collins (2002), in supporting imagery
applications that complied with the findings of functional equivalence theory, argued that
arousal levels in imagery should reflect the optimal levels of the performance of that
106
specific task. Since pre- imagery relaxation inhibited sport performance, which was
supported in Perry and Morris (1995), actual arousal and emotional levels should be
embedded in the imagery in order to precisely replicate the task in the brain.
could modulate arousal and performance success. Extreme feelings of vulnerability and
performance (Bellon, 2006; Ristad, 1982; Stedman, 1985). The nature of singers
embodying the instrument often precluded personal identification with singing ability and
the approval of the audience. As a result, singers may have perceived or imagined the
audience regarding them in the three possible ways: (a) negatively or with hostility, (b)
positively and with support for the singer, or (c) accurately responding to each moment in
the performance (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993). All these factors and variables impacted
sports literature (e.g., Abma et al., 2002; Hall et al., 1998; Munroe et al., 2000; Weinberg
& Gould, 2003) and was related to five themes: (a) being mentally tough, (b) focus and
concentration, (c) self-efficacy and confidence, (d) being positive, and (e) modeling.
usually just prior to or during performance. Focus and concentration dealt with the mental
ability to directly control images, thoughts, and distractions, which could affect optimal
107
In almost all cases the best focus was one that kept the athlete connected to what
he or she was doing (his or her job). In contrast, the worst focus was one in which
the athlete was dwelling on factors over which he or she had no direct control,
on the specific tasks of the performance, characterization, and communication with the
sustained involvement in the task was directly correlated in those individuals who
problem for such a person. Instead of worrying about how he is doing, how he
awareness, while sometimes it is the other way around: it is the very lack of self-
This deep personal connection with the performance seemed to be regulated by the level
108
Imagery relating to confidence and self-efficacy were involved with feeling
successful, positive, and up to the task. Self-efficacy theory (Bandura, 1977, 1982, 1997)
Bandura (1997) stated that “[e]fficacy beliefs affect thought pattern that could enhance or
undermine performance” (p. 116). He further proposed that self-efficacy was belief in
one’s own capabilities in acting in specific ways. In sport research, MG-M imagery was
1996; Vadocz et al., 1997; Vealey, 1986) and was a factor distinguishing elite and less
successful performers (Gould, Weiss, & Weinberg, 1981; Woolfolk, Murphy, Gottesfeld,
& Aitken, 1985). Employing and maintaining positive imagery and outlook was found to
facilitate athletic performance (Munroe et al., 2000). Positive self-talk and stopping
negative thoughts were also used in achieving confidence (Gammage, Hardy, & Hall,
2001; Hardy, Gammage, & Hall, 2001). Sport researchers (Orlick, 1990; Rushall, 1988;
Suinn, 1972, 1986) supported the use of confidence-building techniques in imagery in the
applied athletic fields. Athletes who imagined performing in a confident manner were
better able to develop, maintain, and regain confidence (Moritz et al., 1996).
confident, and mentally fit for the task of performing was not new. In reflecting some of
the qualities of MG-M imagery, the renowned vocal teacher, Vennard (1971), rigorously
supported the idea of inner composure and self-efficacy in the following description:
in himself. This is why natural singers are often insufferable egotists. They are not
egotistical because they sing well; on the contrary, they control their voices well
109
because they are so self-confident. . . . This is their priceless asset. Dampen the
exhibitionist pleasure they take in their voices, and you will have damaged the
strength of character for the long pull. (Vennard, 1971, pp. 24-25)
regard to vocal control and mental toughness. Carter (1993) and Bellon (2006) reported
elite singers used strong, positive self-images and self-talk continually in rehearsing,
mental, physical, and environmental distractions through focus and concentration. Fields
(1972) advocated that the joy of singing consisted of “a sense of well-being and
exhilaration of spirit that comes from the experience of being free from worry or restraint
of any kind” (pp. 9-10). Stedman (1985) reported that singers used imagery for
The use of modeling ideal performances, whereby performers learned a new skill
(Bandura & Jefferies, 1973). The image of the ideal performance served to motivate the
individual to perform beyond what had been previously considered possible. Feltz (1984)
argued, “ just mentally seeing oneself successfully performing the desired task is enough
to convince an athlete that he or she has the ability to successfully execute the task” (p.
193). Rushall (1988) employed covert modeling, to assist performance anxiety conditions
110
in imagining how an outstanding performer would execute a specific performance to
increase their motivation and self-confidence (Bellon, 2006; Caldwell & Wall, 2001;
Emmons & Thomas, 1999). Singers modeled performance excellence in front of a mirror
for immediate feedback to see how they were physically projecting the character.
functions were rarely addressed in sport research this was an important aesthetic aspect in
figure skating, synchronized swimming, and artistic routines in gymnastic floor exercises
(Hays, 2002; Murphy et al., 2008). Likewise, dancers used imagery to evaluate their
performance and to check external body awareness such as stage blocking, musical
timing, and coordination with other dancers to achieve appropriate artistic imagery in
performance (Nordin & Cumming, 2005). Instrumental and vocal musicians used
and effectively communicating with the audience (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Stedman,
1985). Elite vocalists in Carter’s (1993) investigation recognized that technique and vocal
material had to be completely solid and automatic in order for positive interaction with
the audience to occur. Carter (1993) reported that singers in her study exchanged
imaginal interactions of “electricity,” “vibrations,” and “love” (p. 245) with their
audiences. They also responded to positive nonverbal, kinesthetic feedback from their
111
specific sense imagery in achieving appropriate artistic images relevant to the desired
performance.
Health reasons. Due to the extensive physical requirements in both athletics and
artistic performance, the body was highly regarded and maintaining excellent health was
a paramount concern for performers. Imagery in the process of maintaining health had
been the focus of many studies (Sheikh, 2003; Sheikh & Jordan, 1983; Sheikh & Korn,
1994). Imagery use in illness and healing was popularized with the work of Simonton and
his colleagues (Simonton et al., 1971; Simonton et al., 1978) in their treatise in the mental
applications for treating disease, especially cancer, as previously mentioned. This method
of imagery use in rehabilitating injury and pain management has been applied in sports
(Orlick, 1990). Green (1992) suggested imagery applications for facilitating sport injury.
Athletes and performers used imagery for healing purposes in several ways. Elite
gymnasts imagined their routines to forget their pain and become more involved in the
performance (Calmels et al., 2003). According to Nordin and Cumming (2005), dancers
used imagery for four different healing reasons: injury prevention and healing, pain
management, spiritual healing, and rejuvenation. Imagery was used as a substitute for
actual practice to save energy and rest the body when they were too tired to perform.
Dancers also used imagery to improve the internal awareness of their body such as
posture, coordination, balance, and weight distribution (Nordin & Cumming, 2005).
Singers and instrumentalists often used mental practice when they were sick or needed to
preserve their voices and fingers (Bellon, 2006). Brass musicians used imagery during
112
1987). Injury prevention, healing, and recovery were an important aspect represented in
musical performers in their use of imagery, which extended beyond those elements
instrumentalists used imagery. Using a similar framework, Carter (1993) examined the
use of imagery in elite singers. Most recently, Bellon (2006) interviewed several different
mental imagery. Many of the specific findings from these studies have previously been
reported in the various categories, however these studies represent extant research more
presented in the sport psychology research literature. These three studies will be analyzed
individually in regard to their relevance to the present study. Table 5 shows the three
major imagery studies in musical performance, the participant performers, general uses of
imagery as they relate to dance and sport research, as well as the findings of each study.
Trusheim Study
imagery, Trusheim (1987) investigated 26 top brass instrumentalists who were members
of five of the most prestigious symphonies in major cities of the United States, using the
iii Framework representing the image, imagining, and imagination (Rosenberg, 1987).
Trusheim (1987) interviewed these musicians concerning their use of imagery in the
various aspects of training, rehearsal, and performance. These elite brass players reported
113
Table 5
tactile imagery
Carter (1993) Elite Singers/ 11 Auditory, Artistic Internal and Singers use
w/audience expressively
relaxation and
energizing
extensive use of metaphoric and artistic imagery. They also used imagery in modeling
other revered players and developed a high degree of proficiency in aural and kinesthetic
imagery in executing their musical skills and talents. Trusheim did not compare his
findings with those in sport research, but in his literature review, he did devote a page to
imagery studies primarily focused on Suinn’s (1983, 1986) VMBR techniques for
the cognitive types and functions of skill learning and acquisition, as well as strategies in
practice to secure passages and augment physical rehearsal, often in various locations and
when playing their instruments was not appropriate. These professionals also employed
motivational imagery types and functions to regulate their arousal to appropriate levels
for optimal performance. Furthermore, the brass players in the study engaged in imagery
to achieve various goals, boost confidence, and master the mental and emotional
including artistic purposes in musically expressing the compositions and healing purposes
when they were recovering from illness, reflecting dancers’ imagery purposes (Nordin &
Cumming, 2005). Their use of imagery compared quite favorably to the research in
imagery types, times, locations, and functions addressed in sport and dance research.
Trusheim’s study raised several concerns that were relevant to the present study.
Although the participants represented some of the most prestigious positions in the
orchestras of the United States, these subjects were all male ensemble musicians and their
identities were all revealed in the study. Their high standing in the musical community
could have influenced their responses. These musicians did not perform regularly as
the norm in classical solo vocal performance. Furthermore, they were not responsible for
memorizing words or text, often in another language, to songs, arias, and entire roles in
operas, representing vocal skills that require years of development and refinement. Many
115
of these requirements and issues were addressed in the Carter (1993) study of vocal
professionals.
Carter Study
moderately scheduled interview format, topics included: career preparation, teachers’ use
of imagery, performance anxiety issues, mental rehearsal, and mental imagery in vocal
pedagogy. These issues related to the present study perhaps more than any other music
investigation.
The results of this comprehensive study directly addressed many of the aspects of
using imagery outside of the context of sport psychology. Vocalists in the Carter study
reported being involved with imagery from an early age and continued to use various
Many of them learned to use metaphorical imagery from their teachers, especially in
developing proper vocal technique building and breath management, diction, and
imagery most frequently, followed by auditory imagery, then visual imagery. Imagery
was used for technique building as an aid in interpreting vocal literature. Imagery assisted
these vocalists in developing their characters and roles in affective expression and
effectively communicating with the audience. Singers used mental practice to augment
physical practice during injury or times when they could not physically sing, for example
when traveling, at night, or just prior to performance. Imagery use assisted these vocalists
116
in reinforcing positive beliefs, calming performance anxiety, and facilitating performance
Various aspects of the Carter study were cause for concern in relation to the
present study. The participants in the study were all located in the New York and Boston
area of the United States. It is not certain that their use of imagery was a result of their
locality and proximity to other performers who practice imagery in their profession.
Furthermore, many of these singers were directly involved in teaching other vocalists,
which may have facilitated their own use and propensity to use imagery in singing. One
of the interviewees, an outstanding singer also known for her excellent use of imagery in
her teaching, had previously taught had two of the other participants. Even though Carter
(1993) did not connect the imagery uses of vocalists to those in sport psychology, there
were many references that correlated with the uses of imagery found in dancers (Nordin
& Cumming, 2005). Carter’s imagery definitions were more in keeping with vocal
pedagogy than the more athletic uses such as the imagery types, characteristics, and
Bellon Study
Bellon (2006) interviewed six musical performers from different fields in the
effort to compare their mental practices with the main principles of sport psychology,
(energy levels), and optimal performance” (p. 1). Although Bellon (2006) used a general
model from sport psychology texts, her interviews did provide a wealth of information
for how professional musical performers use specific types of imagery as defined in sport
117
texts. Specific studies in sport psychology research were not included. Only one section
investigated imagery and visualization; however, many of the issues in the study dealt
with the mental aspect of practice, preparation, and performance. Bellon’s results
concluded that all of the musicians in her study used imagery in ways that mirrored much
Some of the criticism of the Bellon (2006) study concerned sampling issues,
interview questions, and terminology, as they were related to the present study. The small
sample of six participants included two singers from Quebec (a retired mezzo soprano, a
part-time singer and choral conductor), two oboe instrumentalists (one from France and
the other from Puerto Rico), an instrumental conductor, and a collaborative pianist, both
of whom were professors at Arizona State University. Of the two singers, one had
enjoyed a somewhat shortened international career and had just retired from singing,
while the other was a part time professional vocalist. This was a very broad sample,
raising questions of the degree to which the results were generalizable to other
performers, particularly professional singers. Some of the interview questions may have
elicited specific results that could be judged as leading the participant rather than probing
specific subjective responses. For example, the question was asked, “What distracts you
most during or before a performance?” (p. 3). This question presupposed that the
individual could initially be distracted during or before a performance, leaving little room
imagery as visualization rather than differentiating between the various sensory aspects
118
One of the limitations shared among the Trusheim (1987), Carter (1993), and
Bellon (2006) studies was the use of professionals whose names were revealed and often
highly recognized in the field. Sharing the names of those individuals included in an
interview study allowed for the risk that the participants would shape their answers in
order to appear better than what they may share if their identities remained confidential.
The Trusheim (1987), Carter (1993), and Bellon (2006) studies represented the
primary interview investigations of the use of imagery in musical performers, which were
relevant to the present study. They could be considered part of a growing body of
research in using imagery in musical performance. Bellon’s (2006) study was the only
researcher to base her study on the principles of sport psychology applications. These
investigators did not attempt to correlate the findings in musical performance with the
Therefore, it was with caution that the four Ws of the use of imagery: where, when, what,
and why (Munroe et al., 2000; Nordin & Cumming, 2005) was applied to these studies.
Information gathered on musical performers’ imagery use in the Trusheim (1987), Carter
(1993), and Bellon (2006) studies provided part of a strong foundation for this study.
Conclusion
This chapter has provided an examination of the theories relevant to imagery use
in sport, dance, musical, and vocal performance. These included the early contributions
and development of theories, the cognitive-based theory, the psychological states theory,
and the more recent developments in the theory of functional equivalence. The body of
literature focused on important aspects of imagery using the four Ws framework: where,
when, what, and why from research in sports (Munroe et al., 2000) and dance (Nordin &
119
Cumming, 2005). Also included were the results from imagery research with performing
musicians (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1998). Other relevant theories were
(Munroe et al., 2000) and dance (Nordin & Cumming, 2005), and the related musical
framework (see Table 6). This framework was created to guide the development of this
study in gathering and reporting the data from the solo professionals’ interviews.
Specifically, it was intended to assist in constructing codes and themes in the analyses of
the data and as an initial step in creating the vocalists’ imagery framework for this study.
120
Table 6
Stage
Home Bed
Transit Commuting
Anywhere
During
Post
Performance Pre
During
Post
At Home
Traveling
Character/Role
Body-Related
Ability
Direction
121
Table 6 (continued)
Improving
Memorizing
Planning
Drive
Thought/feeling change
Accompaniment
Rhythm
Style
Interpretation
Communicating w/audience
Health Maintenance
Recuperation
Irrelevant Spontaneous
In the remaining chapters, the research design of this study is presented in Chapter
Three. Results of the interviews and the subsequent categories and subcategories are
explained in Chapter Four. Chapter Five includes a discussion on the findings in relation
122
Chapter 3
Method
The purpose of this study was to investigate imagery use in achieving optimal
performance in vocal professionals. Research from sport psychology and particularly the
framework of the four Ws of imagery use: where, when, why, and what, (Munroe et al.,
2000; Nordin & Cumming, 2005) were used to compare and contrast singers’ imagery
performance practice and development. The inquiry was also based on imagery research
in the professional practice of other musical performers such as brass players (Trusheim,
1987), various musicians (Bellon, 2006), and vocalists (Carter, 1993). This chapter
describes the research design, participants, instrumentation, and procedures used in this.
Research Design
A basic interpretive qualitative research design was chosen for this study since the
new and understudied area of research. Patton (2002) recommended: “In new fields of
study where little work has been done, few definitive hypotheses exist and little was
known about the nature of the phenomenon, qualitative inquiry was a reasonable
beginning point for research” (p. 193). This research approach allowed for one to gain an
in-depth understanding of how vocal professionals used imagery in their profession. Data
123
were collected through interviews designed to gather a broad and rich body of
have some background and experience in the area being investigated, particularly since
background to allow the reader to understand how the researcher interacted with the
various phases and aspects of the study (Patton, 2002). The researcher held undergraduate
and graduate degrees in applied art, theatre, and music, particularly vocal performance.
She also participated in a young artists’ training program in a regional opera company
and has performed in operatic roles, concerts, and recitals professionally. The researcher
has taught general and choral music in public schools as well as in a private studio
teaching voice and other musical instruments. Additionally, she has been an avid sport
exerciser for her entire life. Through this training and experience, the researcher was
familiar with the language and practices of professional vocalists as well as imagery.
During the course of conducting this study, she was in the process of discovering the
relevance of imagery in sport psychology. Despite the possible threat of bias, this
background made the researcher uniquely qualified to conduct, analyze, report, and
Sample Selection
Purposeful, homogeneous sampling was the method used to select the participants
for examining imagery use by professional vocalists. The sample consisted of 15 out of a
124
total number of 18 vocal professionals who were invited to participate and met one of the
following criteria:
accredited university or conservatory and had worked and made the majority
vocalist.
2. Singers without a four year college degree, who had worked and made the
vocalist.
In either case, participants were solo singers who had primarily sung in the Western
classical singing tradition, including work in opera, concert, recital, and oratorio. This
population of vocalists was chosen as the best source for providing information on the
use of imagery by singing professionals. “Information-rich cases are those from which
one can learn a great deal about issues of central importance to the purpose of the
was initiated by asking singers and professors of vocal performance known to the
researcher to identify and suggest others who fit the criteria of this study and were willing
different parts of the United States and had experience in the many areas of solo classical
repertoire performance. They represented various stages of their career, beginning with at
least four years of experience as a professional to retirement age. Both males and females
were targeted with voices in the major vocal registers: soprano, mezzo, tenor, baritone,
125
and bass. In contacting these individuals by email, the researcher invited singers she
knew to participate and asked them to suggest other singers who might have been
interested in being interviewed for the study (see Appendix B). Vocal professors and
New Jersey, New York, Massachusetts, and Pennsylvania were also emailed and asked to
identify and contact other potential participants who had been or were currently
performing professionally (see Appendix C). The minimum sample size was determined
to be at least five male and ten female singers from varying vocal registers and from
different regions of the United States in an attempt to include singers not restricted to one
or two cities.
Solo classical professional vocalists were chosen as participants of this study for
five reasons. First, the specific requirements of the professional singer were extensive and
singers could benefit extensively from knowledge about the specifics of the use of
various aspects of imagery in vocal texts (e.g., Caldwell & Wall, 2001; Chapman, 2006;
Dayme, 2005; Emmons & Thomas, 1998; Williamon, 2004) suggested the need for
further scientific inquiry in how professional singers use imagery. Third, this researcher
was able to use her training and expertise in singing and imagery research in interviewing
the professional vocal participants. Fourth, an initial sample of professional singers was
available to this researcher through past experience and network in the field. Finally,
since many of the athletic and music studies in imagery have used the ever prevalent and
convenient subject pool of college students, professional singers with a minimum of four
years of professional experience were chosen for their expertise in their field. Yuille
126
(1985) as well as Lindauer (1983) called for more appropriate methods of investigating
imagery studies than the highly controlled experimental designs. He stated, “We need to
and replace it with field-based research” (p. 146). He also criticized the use of the
inquiry focused. For these reasons, singers whose primary source of income was solo
Measures
Interview Protocol
The singers’ interview guide (see Appendix D) was developed for use in this
study. Questions included in the protocol were designed to gather information about
where and when singers used imagery, what kinds of imagery they engaged in, and why,
or the reason they used imagery. Using an interview guide approach as suggested by
Patton (2002), this semi-structured format allowed for inclusion of ideas relevant to the
inquiry, introduction of new concepts in the interview, and exclusion of items not
The Nordin and Cumming (2005) dancers’ interview guide (see Appendix E),
from the four Ws of imagery use study served as a basis for gaining information and
allowing the participants to share their thoughts and expertise about their use of imagery
professional vocalists. The dancers’ interview guide was adapted with permission (see
Appendix F) to reflect the findings in the music literature and what the researcher, as a
127
professional singer, considered pertinent to eliciting imagery responses from the
professional vocalists. Adaptations also included personalizing the interview guide for
this research and adding a working definition of optimal performance. In order to avoid
the assumption that the participant used imagery in the first section of the protocol,
imagery was defined and described. The first question asked of the participant was if he
or she ever engaged in this type of imagery. If the participant affirmed that imagery was
used, the interview continued. Additionally, a new category was added to the Why section
called Your Sound since it was assumed that musicians might imagine musical sounds or
use imagery to recreate vocal sounds (Averino, 1989; Carter, 1993; Fields, 1972; Hines,
The singers’ interview protocol was pilot tested with a professional singer who
met the requirements for participation. Only minimal suggestions to include several
appropriate probes were made. The interview guide was revised based on these
suggestions. Therefore the instrument protocol was then administered in the actual
interviews with the chosen participants. The use of the singers’ interview guide assisted
Ethical Considerations
Upon receiving approval from the Institutional Review Board (IRB), the
researcher contacted identified vocalists by email and invited them to participate in the
study. Email correspondence was chosen for convenience in the recruitment process, due
to logistics, time, and financial considerations. Vocal performance professors were sent
an email and letter by the researcher requesting them to suggest eligible participants for
the study (see Appendix C). Eighteen eligible vocalists were sent a letter of invitation to
128
participate (see Appendix B). Included in these documents was an explanation of the
purpose and rationale of the study, the general definition of imagery, the intended use of
the data, the intention and purpose of recording the interview, and the details of
confidentiality of their identity (see Appendix G). The response rate resulting from the
and requirements of the study by being read the Consent Script (see Appendix H) in a
purpose of the study, description of procedures, intent of the researcher to record and
transcribe verbatim, the invitation to review and edit their transcriptions including a
timeline for return, and the risks, inconveniences, and benefits of being involved in the
study. In order to gain more candid and accurate responses of imagery use by these vocal
professionals, their identities were not revealed. This was articulated in the statement of
confidentiality and singers were also informed that their participation was completely
voluntary. The participants agreed to the terms in the Consent Script, the conditions of
the requirements of the study, to be recorded, and to allow their recorded responses to be
Procedures
In order to familiarize participants with the study, the imagery terms as used in
sport psychology and the requirements for participation were described in an attached
letter to both potential participants and vocal performance professors (see Appendix G).
129
Imagery is an experience that mimics real experience. We can be aware of
smell, taste or sounds without experiencing the real thing. Sometimes people find
that it helps to close their eyes. It differs from dreams in that we are awake and
The participants understood the requirements that they had to be earning at least half of
their income from vocal performance engagements. Participants were sent a copy of the
Consent Script (Appendix H) and the Singer’s Interview Guide (Appendix D). Next, a
convenient time and date for the subsequent telephone pre-interview was scheduled.
study participants, during which time the Consent Script (see Appendix H) was read to
them. Participants were able to clarify any concerns with the procedure of the inquiry,
logistics of the telephone interview, the intention to record the discussion, and the
was scheduled at an appointed time convenient for the participant. At the designated time
the interview was conducted and digitally taped using the Zoom H2 Handy Recorder, and
The telephone interview began with a statement of the purpose of the study,
verbal verification of the participant’s name and agreement to participate in the study and
interview, and the White and Hardy (1998) imagery definition (see above). The interview
commenced and the length for each session lasted between 40 and 150 minutes.
Participants were asked to answer questions in the interview protocol (see Appendix D),
which addressed each of the four Ws, that is, where, when, what, and why they used
130
imagery were then specifically discussed. Probes were used to gain understanding of
imagery as defined in sport psychology and reported in the literature (Bellon, 2006;
verbatim and the document was sent via an email attachment to each respective
checking (Johnson & Christensen, 2004). Each vocalist was given the opportunity to
scrutinize his or her own interview for accuracy and clarity. Participants were asked to
return their comments and any changes within seven days of receipt of the document. If
there was no response by that date, the researcher sent a second email and when
necessary, telephoned the participant to verify receipt of the interview transcription and
to request a response. A second date was offered and if there was no response, the
interview was assumed to be accurate. All of the participants made minor revisions, and
most requested that superfluous words, “like,” “um,” and “you know” be removed. These
Along with the final copy of the transcription, each participant was sent via email
demographics, education, and experience in the field. All singers completed and returned
this survey and the results were also submitted for analysis.
Prospective participants and vocal teachers and professors were sent initial email
invitations to participate in the study in early December 2008. A week later, a second
invitation was sent to any person who did not respond to the first invitation. Pre-interview
131
telephone calls were made to the vocalists who had met the participation criteria by mid-
December, 2008. All interviews took place between the middle of December 2008 to
early January 2009. Interviews were then transcribed and emailed to each respective
participant for member checking between early January and the middle of February 2009.
transcriptions, data analysis began. The pilot participant’s interview was also included in
the body of interviews. During the course of listening carefully to the participants’
responses in the earlier interviews, more probes were included in the remaining
interviews. Transcription responses were coded manually. Each unit of meaning, whether
it was a word, a phrase, or an entire chunk of text, was coded using a priori categories
derived from the interview protocol that was based on the previous research in the four
Ws of imagery use and the literature. Each code was color coded and entered in the right
margin of each interview quote. Units of meaning fit into major themes and new
categories and subcategories emerged where they were not able to be included in the a
priori set. Similar codes were then categorized together to identify new themes. Each of
these entries was then categorized, subcategorized, and counted for utilization frequency
(Kvale, 1996; Kvale & Brinkmann, 2009; Leech & Onwuegbuzie, 2007).
Both deductive and inductive approaches (Patton, 2002) were used. Deductive
analyses connoted that which was previously known in an area of inquiry, represented
here by the prescribed conceptual framework of the four Ws of imagery use: where,
when, what, and why in athletes (Munroe et al., 2000) and dancers (Nordin & Cumming,
2005), cognitive and motivational functions of imagery (Paivio, 1985; Hall, 1998) and
132
the artistic and healing reasons (Nordin & Cumming, 2005). Inductive analysis and
interviews and allowed for new categories to emerge from the data. This exploratory
method was used for this study rather than testing a proposed hypothesis.
Primary data sources for the singers’ interview guide for each of the research
challenge in separating the specific units of meaning provided in the interview quotes.
Participants’ responses to the research questions were not only found in their direct
answers to the questions posed during the interview but were intricately interwoven
throughout the interview, as Patton (2002) stated, “the relevant data won’t be found in the
same place in each interview” (p. 440). Often the participants’ responses included
information relating to many themes, not only those directly addressing the proposed
Legitimization
The research process itself served to secure efforts to legitimize the findings of
this study. Validity was strengthened by using these expert participants in this specific
field, the professional vocalists, who were chosen due to their first hand knowledge,
experience, and understanding of how singers can effectively use imagery for achieving
optimal performance. “Consulting with experts who possess conceptual and practical
experience in the field of investigation is an ideal method for ensuring satisfactory face
and content validity” (Vella-Brodrick & MacRae, 2004, p. 124). Internal validity was
133
Table 7
professionals use
imagery to achieve
optimal performance?
professionals use
imagery to achieve
optimal performance?
What do vocal 27) Describe any other reasons that you have for using imagery to achieve
optimal performance, that are different from the ones we have mentioned?
professionals use in
29) To what extent do you use imagery relating to vision?
their imagery to
30) To what extent do you use imagery relating to sound and hearing?
achieve optimal
31) To what extent do you use imagery relating to smell?
35) Describe any other kinds of sensations that you feel in your imagery?
36) Which of the above would be the primary senses you use in your
imagery?
Note: Interview questions taken from the Singers’ Interview Guide (see Appendix D).
134
Table 7 (continued)
Why and for what 15) Describe any imagery you use that is based on skill learning and
execution?
purpose do vocal
16) Describe any imagery you use relating to sequences?
professionals use
17) Describe any imagery you use relating to strategies?
imagery to achieve
18) Describe any imagery you use relating to arousal and anxiety?
optimal performance? 19) Describe any imagery you use relating to self-confidence and mastery?
28) Describe any other reasons that you have for using imagery to achieve
optimal performance, that are different from the ones we have mentioned?
Note: Interview questions taken from the Singers’ Interview Guide (see Appendix D).
used in this study, as well as allowing for accurate comparisons to other studies and
participants. Member checking (Johnson & Christensen, 2004) was conducted by sending
a transcribed copy of the interview to each participant for review. These singers were told
that they could change, add, or clarify anything in the interview they felt was unclear,
understanding of the processes of the results and offer an opportunity to make individual
Trustworthiness
Mental imagery was difficult to objectively measure because it was not an overtly
observable behavior. Singers might not have revealed all of the pertinent information
reasons including these: (a) there may have been a lack of agreement in the definitions
and uses of imagery terminology between athletes and vocalists, (b) it was possible that
these subjective experiences were present in vocalists but they had not previously been
required to verbalize or analyze them, or (c) this internal process of imagery use may
have been too personal and the vocalist may not have been completely comfortable with
conversing about this topic. These issues were addressed beginning with the initial stages
of contact with the potential participant. The White and Hardy (1998) imagery definition
was given to bridge any latent gaps in the use of this term between sports and music. The
concerns, gain rapport with the researcher, and facilitate understanding of the imagery
subject in general.
Researcher Bias
An important concern in this study throughout the interviews and the analysis was
studies to have some degree of expertise in the area of inquiry (Patton, 2002). Although
this researcher, trained in both singing and imagery, was a necessary aspect of this study,
her participation in the interviews also posed the threat of researcher bias. A number of
136
elements were put in place to control for this potential problem. As recommended by
Miles and Huberman (1994), detailed records of the process to collect and interpret data
was conducted throughout all stages of the research process. Peer debriefing was defined
and for the purpose of exploring aspects of the inquiry that might otherwise remain only
implicit within an inquirer’s mind” (Lincoln & Guba, 1985, p. 308). These stages of the
research process included the design of the study, choosing and adapting the interview
the results. This was done to help uncover the researcher’s personal assumptions, biases,
and perspectives that may have threatened the credibility of the study.
During the course of conducting the interviews, the investigator assumed a neutral
stance and allowed the interviewee to answer freely from his or her experience. Leading
questions were avoided wherever possible and each of the imagery issues were presented
vocalists to elaborate on their answers and bring in responses not previously expected on
techniques of this nature could not be completely free of bias, the researcher endeavored
Frequency Counts
Frequency counts could be misleading but were included in the results for the
137
the small sampling, the personal nature of imagery experience, or quality and specifics of
the interview protocol, which served to elicit certain responses. Sometimes rare
experiences were revealing and created more understanding of the research questions,
therefore a category was created even if one singer only mentioned it once. Initial codes
were checked and recoded repeatedly in order that the responses were categorized as
effectively as possible in the effort to verify coding reliability. It was important to note,
however, that frequency was not necessarily an indicator of importance (Gammage et al.,
Summary
This chapter presented the design of this exploratory study of mental imagery use
use taken from sport psychology research was employed to gain information in athletics
(Munroe et al., 2000), dance (Nordin & Cumming, 2005), and imagery findings from the
literature review, especially music performance studies in imagery (Bellon, 2006; Carter,
1993; Trusheim, 1987). The interview guide was borrowed and adapted to be appropriate
for vocal professionals. Professional vocalists volunteered for the study and were
(Munroe et al., 2000; Nordin & Cumming, 2005). Participants were described, the
interview process was given, and data analyses were explained. The details of the results
of the investigation are discussed in Chapter Four. These results are discussed in relation
to the results and findings of previous research in Chapter Five, including implications
138
Chapter 4
Results
singers as described in the previous chapter. In this chapter, the backgrounds of the
participants are presented first, the interview protocol and process is discussed, followed
by results for each of the research questions based on the framework of the four Ws of
imagery use. Analysis of the data is presented for each of the major questions -- where,
Participants
were interviewed for this study. Participants’ ages ranged from the late twenties to the
late sixties and were reported in five-year groupings. Summary data on study
participants’ age and gender are reported in Table 8. The majority of participants (n = 11)
were between 30-45 years old, 67% (n = 10) were female and 33% (n = 5) were male.
each participant’s interview order, pseudonym, age, voice type, and highest educational
degree earned. Pseudonyms were chosen with first letters in alphabetical order according
to their gender and the order of their interview. For example, the male participant, who
was the seventh interviewee, was given the random name Gerald, which began with the
letter G, the seventh letter of the English alphabet. The thirteenth interviewee was female
and arbitrarily given the name Monique, since the letter M was thirteenth in the alphabet.
139
Table 8
n % n % n %
30-35 4 26.7 4
40-45 3 2 3 20
N = 15
Participants included seven sopranos (average age = 37.4), three mezzo sopranos
(average age = 55.8), four tenors (average age = 40), and one baritone (age = 42). Of the
15 participants, 13 held a master’s degree or higher, including a tenor and baritone with
doctorates.
Florida, Maryland, New Jersey, New York, and Pennsylvania. Particular information on
each participant’s current residence remained general and confidential in order to obscure
the singer’s identity. Furthermore, it was not considered pertinent to this study since
Participants’ Pseudonym, Gender, Voice Type, Age Range, and Highest Degree
N = 15
Singers reported the vocal performance genre in order of their most frequent,
second most frequent, and third most frequently performed singing genres (see Table 10).
Thirteen participants reported that their most frequent genre was opera, one reported
141
oratorio, and one reported an equal distribution among oratorio, recital, art song, early
music, liturgical music, and concert singing. The second most frequent genre identified
was recital and cited by seven singers, followed by oratorio, and concert types.
Participants’ reports of their third genre varied widely among other forms of concerts and
recitals.
participants, all of whom had performed regionally. There were four singers who had
performed in only one region of the United States and, for the purposes of this study,
were considered regional singers. Eight of these professionals with experience in two or
more United States regions were given the classification of national experience. There
were ten vocalists who had been engaged to sing outside the United States and were
single region of the United States as well as another country, and were also considered
international singers. Years of experience were given along with the locations in which
these participants had sung, ranging from six to over 40 years of total professional
experience (see Table 11). The average total years of professional solo singing
142
Table 10
Liturgical
Concert Equally
N = 15
143
Table 11
3 Catherine 6 0 6 NE UK
4 Dorine 7 0 0 SE 0
5 Eloise 13 0 0 SE 0
6 Francine 18 6 0 All 0
MA AU
10 Josephine 25 3 0 NE, MW 0
UK, EU, AF
12 Louis 20 15 10 MW CN
EU, AU, AF
14 Nicholas 10 0 2 MW AS
15 Ophelia 8 0 3 MA AS
N = 15
Note: NE: Northeast; SE: Southeast; S: South; MW: Midwest; SW: Southwest; NW: Northwest; MA: Mid-Atlantic;
CN: Canada; CA: Central America; SA: South America; AS: Asia; UK: United Kingdom; EU: Europe; AU: Australia;
144
Interview Protocol and Process
gathering both breath and depth of responses from these singers. The open-ended format
of the questions allowed for probing as was deemed appropriate for each participant. The
singers’ interview guide was sent to study participants prior to the interviews, which
helped the singers to understand imagery terms and be mentally prepared to respond to
In addition to the changes that were made to the original Nordin and Cumming
(2005) dancers’ interview guide (see Appendix E), a few terms required clarification
distinguishing meanings of terms. These were not uncovered during the pilot test. For
example, a misunderstanding relative to the question of times when singers used imagery
occurred in differentiating between breaks and holidays. Singers were perplexed by the
word “holiday” in the fourth probe of question 39 regarding when singers used imagery,
“To what extent does your imagery use differ between rehearsal, performance, and
holiday periods?” Since the height of most vocalists’ performance season was during the
Christmas and New Years holidays, the word “holiday” was changed to “break”, meaning
the period during the year when professionals had time off and were not engaged in
during breaks only minimally, if any at all. However, in the sport (Munroe et al., 2000)
and dance (Nordin & Cumming, 2005) studies, breaks meant quiet periods throughout the
day, which contrasted with how this issue was addressed in this study. The meanings of
145
the terms “tactile” or “touch” and “kinesthetic feeling” had to be explained to many of
these participants since they were not accustomed to using the word “kinesthetic” to
describe feelings and movements inside their body. Singers were also somewhat unsure
as to how to answer the sequences and strategies questions (Items 16 and 17). Adding a
single probing question on “planning” seemed to have provided more clarity for these
singers. These changes should be addressed and resolved in future uses of the singers’
interview guide.
Although the first three sections of the singers’ interview guide generated
responses about singers’ background, these responses did not prove to be critical to
answering the research questions of where, when, what, and why singers used imagery.
Upon reviewing the transcripts, it was found that singers’ personal information was not
concise enough to be included in this study. Therefore, in the effort to accurately and
efficiently report the demographics of the participants, the vocal participants’ survey was
designed (see Appendix I) and administered to the participants along with their
transcriptions. This greatly aided in maintaining confidentially and more generally and
Several different aspects of the interview process itself helped shape this study.
The researcher was an experienced vocal professional, was a student of imagery, and had
more familiar with the process and further refined the interviewing technique with each
subsequent participant. Slight changes were made in terminology of the probes and
singers’ answers more reflected the intentions for which the guide was used. Generally,
the participants were articulate and willing to share their knowledge and experience. A
146
number of them had prior experience being interviewed for radio, television, and
newspaper, and were comfortable with the process. The interviewer generally took a
neutral stance and refrained from offering too much direction so that singers’ responses
The nature of the responses of participants to the interview and the specific
questions varied widely. The duration of the interviews lasted between 40 and 150
minutes. Several singers were more verbal and shared a wealth of information, regardless
of my efforts to invite them to be more concise, while other vocalists answered more to
the point and needed continuous prompting. Similarly, participants’ responses ranged
from extensively detailed, insightful, and rich to some that were more perfunctory or less
focused. The quality of responses depended on the degree to which the interviewee
stayed on topic and answered the specific given question. Several of these participants
were teachers and answered in the form of what they would share their responses with
their students. Most of these responses were not included since it was not certain if the
quotes that were difficult to understand and unclear were also omitted in the analyses, if,
after making a concerted effort through emailing and telephoning the participant, the
researcher failed to gain clarity. Overall, the extensive breadth and depth of these
Coding
predetermined categories and subcategories derived from the four Ws framework, the
147
singers’ interview guide, and the related literature. Data were segmented into units of
meaning, which ranged from a single word to chunks of text containing one item of
information. These were initially coded using the a priori categories and subcategories. In
the case where a meaning unit did not fit into the predetermined codes, a new category or
subcategory was created as appropriate. The final data analyses included six main
themes: (a) where, (b) when, (c) imagery types, (d) sense imagery, (e) imagery ability,
Each meaning unit was labeled by theme, category and subcategory. For example,
Production.” This represented the imagery type of kinesthetic sensory imagery response
taken from interview question 34, “To what extent do you use imagery relating to
kinesthesia?” and involving images of vocal production. Another example of the coding
was “IU:VS:MO,” indicating “Imagery Use: Vocal Sound: Modeling,” representing the
imagery type of vocal sound from question 26, “Describe any imagery you use based on
your sound?” and the use of modeling. The complete list of themes and codes are
provided in Appendix J.
When it appeared that the codes were beginning to be organized cohesively, four
other researchers trained in coding qualitative data “check-coded” (Miles & Huberman,
apparent that the first three sections of the singers’ interview guide did provide sufficient
content for understanding the participants’ backgrounds but did not adequately address
the research questions in this study. Initially, the inter-coder agreement was .78 and was
deemed satisfactory but could be improved. The themes and codes were again analyzed
148
and reviewed, revealing several redundancies, which resulted in further consolidation of
the codes. The same transcript, without the responses to the first three sections, was
analyzed by the four researchers, resulting in an inter-coder agreement of .83, which was
Themes and codes that had emerged from the first coding were used to analyze
the transcripts a second time and each meaning unit was placed by the participant’s name
and code (by color) into each cell of an Excel document. At the end of the second
analysis, all data were analyzed and redundant categories and codes were collapsed into
codes that represented the findings as efficiently as possible. Another analysis was
employed to scour each transcript for any remaining meaning unit that could correspond
to the individual codes. Transcriptions were saved in Word documents and key words
were entered to find quotes using various descriptive words connected to that code. For
example, in looking for the all the quotes for the theme of “arousal,” specific words such
as “nerves,” “performance anxiety,” “stage fright,” and “afraid” were used to search the
entire document and locate every quote, which had anything to do with “arousal.” This
action was performed in every transcript and all codes (see in Appendix J).
All the coded quotes were transferred into their own Word documents and
analyzed and compared to other coded data in that category for emerging themes and
patterns. These themes and codes were compared with those of the four Ws frameworks
for a final check before confirmation. These data were interpreted and a framework
emerged, which constituted further deductive analyses processes (Strauss & Corbin,
1998). Quotes in all coded categories were analyzed by frequency and general emerging
patterns.
149
Frequency counts were reported according to the number of participants who
responded in these categories rather than the number of times the singer mentioned the
item. This was primarily due to the semi-structured nature of the interview guide and the
repetitive nature of some of the questions. This was exemplified by the researcher’s
statement given at the beginning of the interview, “Also, do not worry if some questions
seem repetitive. This is partly to make sure that I am covering all aspects of your singing
imagery and not leaving anything out, and partly useful to obtain quotes for later
analyses” (see Appendix D). Since so many singers offered answers to questions in
several ways, at times revealing replications, it was deemed that reporting frequency
In reporting the results of the interviews, it was determined that first knowing
where and when singers engaged in imagery provided a better understanding of what was
imagined and why images were used. An attempt was made to separate what, or the
content of imagery, from why, or the purpose. However, participants’ responses were so
integrated in the content and purposes of their imagery that it presented certain challenges
in reporting these findings. It was determined that the cohesive nature of this information
be maintained as much as possible. Therefore, where, when, and what imagery was
reported first, and the section on why offered a more complete and integrated description
of how these singers incorporated imagery in their profession. What follows is the report
of the analyses of the vocal participants’ responses as organized under the four major
themes of the four Ws of imagery use: where, when, what, and why.
150
Where Do Vocal Professionals Use Imagery to Achieve Optimal Performance?
Results of where singers use imagery are followed by when imagery was used.
Vocalists’ reports of where, or the location of imagery engagement, was separated into
four categories: (a) general locations, (b) home, (c) other places, and (d) in practice or
performance settings. Responses are given here according to the divisions that emerged
which resulted in a number of subcategories within each category as shown in Table 12.
Table 12
Rehearsal Space 4
At the piano 2
Dressing Room 2
Home Bed 13
General Alone 7
Anywhere/Everywhere 6
Quiet/Relaxing Places 5
Exercising/Outside 6
151
Results of the singers’ responses in the where category, from the most frequent to
the fewest, were: (a) practice/rehearsal/performance settings, (b) home, (c) general, and
(d) other places. All of the participants reported using imagery on stage for auditions and
performances, followed by six who imagined in the practice or studio space. Four
vocalists used imagery in the rehearsal space, two at the piano, and two in the dressing
room. Referring to the places she used imagery, Francine said, “I use it in the practice
room. I use it in the rehearsal hall, and I use it most specifically in the wings before a
performance, or the hallways before an audition.” The home was the next most frequently
named space with 13 specifically imagining in bed and nine naming various areas in the
home. For example, Dorine said, “I’m usually in the comfort of my own bed, or on my
couch where I can stretch out and relax. I like to be lying down and I try to get myself as
comfortable as possible.” In the general category, less than half of the participants chose
being alone, anywhere or everywhere, and in quiet or relaxing places. Other places
included eight preferring commuting and driving and six either outside or exercising.
It was significant that all of the vocalists considered using imagery at auditions to
more frequently than the older, more experienced professionals. Being successful in
auditions allowed the singer to actually perform, since roles were so few and many
performers vie for these in this format. Therefore, with regard to professional vocalists’
These solo singers used imagery in the performance setting where they were
already set to practice or perform. Outside of these settings, vocalists engaged in imagery
wherever it was convenient for them to process their thoughts and feelings in imagery, as
152
evidenced by the high number of responses for being alone, at home, in bed, and
commuting and driving (see Table 12). Their particular performance requirements and
living situations and environments may have influenced these results. Furthermore, some
singers reported imagining in many different places, particularly those who seemed to
Responses giving the time periods when imagery was used resulted in six
subcategories: (a) practice, (b) rehearsal, (c) performance, (d) performance season, (e)
breaks, and (f) other times. These are presented in this particular order by when these
singers reported their use of imagery beginning with the primary and specific time
periods of practicing, through performance, and ending with break periods and times not
With regard to using imagery around practice times, all participants reported
engaging in imagery during practice, two singers used it before, and three singers
reported using imagery to assist in retrospection of what they did and to decide what to
do in the future. Ophelia identified the cyclical nature of what was considered after
practice and the next before practice times. She explained that, “afterwards you are
studying and imaging what happened and you remember what happened and you’re
learning from those things and improving. But if you think about it, the afterward is
really just the beginning of the practice.” Upon further investigation, this distinction held
some weight. Ophelia and Catherine expressed the desire to use more of this type of
153
Table 13
Practice Before 4
After 4
Performance Before 15
During 9 (5 some)
After 6
During 15
7 no imagery used
154
imagery during the initial stages of practice. Ophelia mused, “it behooves me to have the
Six singers used imagery during the process of learning the material and four used
it after most of the basic notes and music were secured, as illustrated by Francine’s
The only time I wouldn’t [use imagery] is when I’m being very, very, very
technical and I’m learning a piece or I’m just learning basic plunking of notes . . .
But even in my own private studio work, once a piece has gotten to a certain level
where I need to start making more out of it than just learning the notes and
Louis also engaged in imagery after the rudiments of the piece were learned. He stated:
During the preparation process for musical literature that I’m preparing, during
there. But I need to hone that knowledge into a more detailed mastery of the
music.
Generally, all vocalists engaged in imagery during practice in relation to the learning
process which considerably outweighed the responses given for before or after practice.
Regarding times these singers used imagery, a distinction between practice and
rehearsal arose as an issue particular to the classical vocal profession. For solo singers,
practice was usually conducted alone, at various times most convenient for the individual.
Rehearsals were more formal events scheduled and organized for the purpose of staging
an opera or recital, usually involving other singers, musicians, and possibly directors and
coaches, depending on the singing genre and performance venue. Singers were required
155
to show up and participate having already memorized their roles and arias. Josephine
explained:
I prepare myself outside of rehearsal and then I come there and just rehearse with
the other musicians. So it’s really a lot of instant music. So it’s not a lot of work.
Louis’ response also illustrated this when he said, “I’m much more likely to use imagery
Nicholas differentiated his rehearsal and practice efforts when he said, “if I’m in
rehearsal in a show and trying to study a second or third show at the same time then it’s
tricky balance to stay rested enough to accomplish everything.” He was in rehearsals for
one opera while learning and practicing other roles. Differentiating practice, rehearsal,
and performance periods was important when examining professional singers’ imagery
use.
For the purposes of this study, “before performance” referred to periods following
the completion of all practices and rehearsals. All participants used imagery just prior to
quiet time on the day of the performance to mentally prepare for optimal performance
The day it all comes together into one thing, and, when I put my makeup and hair
on, I’ll take three hours if I can, if I have it, if I have the time I’ll try to do an hour
to three hours. . . . I’m really just focused on the entirety of the performance.
Nicholas’ value of his extensive pre-performance (or pre-audition) imagery time was
reflected in the following quote, “I feel that giving myself enough time to achieve focus
156
before an audition or performance is almost more important than anything else on a
represented a great portion of when singers used imagery and both of these types of
Vocalists’ responses for using imagery during performances varied. Nine singers
used imagery during performance; five reported a significantly diminished amount while
performance, I only employ imagery if I fall out of character and need to get back in.
When I'm actually performing, it is distracting to be thinking about performing, it's better
to just do it!” Most of the imagery that singers used in performance was set beforehand
and was intended to render automaticity to their execution and portrayals on stage.
Francine further illustrated, “I’m aware that I’m using it when I’m performing, but I’m so
involved in the performance, that I don’t necessarily remember using it. I just remember
making the conscious decision beforehand to do it.” Conversely, two vocalists reported
using no imagery during performance, including Barry, who said, “I don’t use imagery in
out, comes out.” Harriet explained some of the reasons why she used less imagery in
performance. She said for example, “I use it all beforehand, so that by the time I’m on
157
[U]sually I give myself at least four or five days before I start to take apart the
performance, and rework in, ‘next time I’m going to do this, it’s gonna feel like
Few participants articulated this type of retrospection and intention to change behavior
for the future performances. Imagery engagement after performance was more common
in the responses of singers, such as Monique, who repeatedly performed the same roles
Vocalists used imagery differently throughout the year. All participants engaged
in imagery during the performance season whether it was associated with practice,
rehearsal, or performance. The six participants who reported using imagery to prepare for
the next season also reported using some imagery during breaks. Seven singers said they
took a rest from imagery while on extended vacations, five used less imagery, and three
reported incorporating it more. For example, Louis said, “I might utilize it in anticipation
of the next section of the rehearsal.” Still some confusion remained as to whether these
breaks were rest periods throughout the day, between practice and performing, or during
and performance. Eight participants reported using imagery at anytime or most of the
time, five preferred the day and of those, three imagined upon waking up in the morning.
Night proved to be a more favorable time for 11 participants, including seven who
specified using imagery before going to sleep. One singer had no preference between day
and night. Engaging in imagery required quiet times for seven participants, especially
Eloise who said, “I’m by myself and there’s like nothing going on and I can just do it in
158
my mind, with nothing to interfere.” Karen and Ophelia described using imagery while
To summarize the findings of where and when, all singers used imagery in stage
and practice areas during practice, just prior performance, and during the performance
season. Many participants used imagery more during rehearsal and performance than
before and after. At other times, these participants used imagery at home in bed at night,
and during quiet periods outside, exercising, and during travel. These results have helped
to lay the foundation of what imagery singers use and later why they use it.
four Ws of imagery use (Nordin & Cumming, 2005) was used to differentiate imagery
types from imagery characteristics. Imagery types were all those responses that the singer
images were experienced, such as through the senses, perspective, and various aspects of
challenging to extract the singers’ imagery content from its intended purpose.
Furthermore, the same image could be used for a variety of reasons, and many different
images were used for a single purpose. Even during the interviews, participants pointed
this out. Ivan clearly illustrated his difficulty in analyzing his own imagery practices
when he said, “It’s hard to separate it out when I think about it minimally because when I
am visualizing, it’s all these things combined. In other words, they’re all together for
159
me.” Excessively dissecting this information may have reflected too much of an atomistic
approach to analyzing the very natural and holistic human function that is imagery as
are reported as concisely and efficiently as possible in order to provide a clear foundation
of the various qualities of images these singers used. The section in this chapter on why
Imagery Types
Imagery types are reported in categories that emerged from the singers’ responses
and included the content of the image itself. Their responses were organized into six pre-
determined subcategories that were adapted from dancers’ imagery research (Nordin &
Cumming, 2005). In the original four Ws of imagery use framework for dancers (see
Figure 2) the last category was irrelevant images, which represented an imagery type that
was not found in the singers’ responses. However the category of musical sound was
added to the types of imagery singers described in their interviews. The resulting six
categories of imagery types included: (a) execution, (b) metaphorical, (c) context, (d)
body-related, (e) musical sound, and (f) character/role images. Table 14 provides a
Execution images. Execution images for singers were divided into three sections:
(a) skill learning and technique, (b) sequences and planning, and (c) goal images.
All singers reported an abundance of images of skill learning and techniques and their
responses resulted in five subcategories: (a) vocal production, (b) text and phrases, (c)
page notation, and (e) breath control. Images of vocal production and technique were
described in all singers’ interviews. For example, in maintaining consistency through her
160
Table 14
Page Notation 10
Breath Control 6
Creating Stories 3
Performance 11
Process 15
Song Scenarios 6
Distant Places 1
Home 1
Other People 3
Non-human 4
161
Table 14 (continued)
Types Participants
Calm Images 4
Health 6
Accompaniment 14
Pitch 10
Ideal Sounds 7
Past Experiences 6
Gender Change 1
162
vocal registers, Monique said, “I see my sound as a steady flowing thing, even from top
to bottom.” Eloise described her images of her efforts to establish continuity through her
registers. She offered, “in order to sing scales that go up and down, I imagine the
opposite direction and the opposite flow of energy.” In securing another type of vocal
technique, Ophelia imagined a “soft palate lift, and maybe flattening of the tongue” to
Singing text and words of one language or another have been considered a basic
imagery included words, text, or phrases, which emerged in the interviews as an imagery
element specific to singers. Francine aptly illustrated this in the following quote:
I see the words. When I study just the language, I see the IPA of the pronunciation
of the words. And most importantly with the languages that I don’t speak fluently,
I see, also in my head, the English translation. So as I’m singing in German, I will
Experience of words, text, and phrases in visual imagery emerged naturally from the data
and seemed appropriate since these elements were an integral part of singing training for
performance.
Ten singers reported other uses of visual images with regard to performance,
particularly page notation in relation to memorizing and executing songs. Karen had an
acute ability in visualizing the score. She stated, “I see that all the time. I could tell you
what side of the page something’s on. I don’t have a photographic memory, but it’s
almost like that.” Barry and Francine reported having a photographic memory and page
163
notation images were easy for them to recreate. Regarding his experience of mentally
seeing the score, Nicholas said, “When I’m memorizing, a little bit. I try to get beyond
that soon, as quick as I can, so that I don’t have to think about that and can concentrate on
other things.” Ivan also had remembered his images of notation and described, “there’s a
sense of the music, the sheet, hovering in front in my consciousness. Then that
disappears.” Page notation images often subsided when the music became more
internalized. Mentally reading from a score was sometimes necessary in certain instances.
Ophelia said she used notation imagery “if I do have a difficult passage where this
cadenza for instance has a lot of technically challenging aspects, I would see, yes, the
music notation in front of me.” Monique and Francine mentally imagined the score
[W]ith contemporary music, very specifically when it’s something that may be
atonal, it doesn’t even make sense harmonically, again, I see the score. I see the
compared to the A.
Page notation images were connected to performing the song and helped singers to
Barry: “imagine the path of your breath”). While performing a long phrase, Anna would
imagine inhaling at the same time she was singing. These techniques and breathing
Every singer reported mentally rehearsing and silently singing the songs and
roles. Many of their images directly reflected or were related to all the skills they
164
practiced physically. Differences appeared in the degree to which they focused on vocal
production, words, breath, or various combinations of these musical elements and may
learning or performing music ranging from a simple phrase to an entire recital or role.
These types of images comprised four sections: (a) phrasing and patterns, (b) planning
songs and arias, (c) sequences of song cycles and recitals, and (d) creating stories. Five
vocalists imagined phrases and distinct sections especially while learning repertoire, such
as Josephine who said, “I see the patterns in the music.” Eight participants described
images of planning through the execution of their songs and arias. Catherine illustrated
this by saying, “I imagine myself singing through an entire piece vocally and singing
everything correctly.” For six singers, sequencing imagery was integrated into their
recital arrangement and progression through song cycles. Nicholas created “specific ideas
about how each piece is going to go.” Likewise, Josephine imagined “the shape of the
music and what each song was in the program.” Three singers reported creating stories
that linked the different songs in their recitals together in a cohesive manner. Eloise said
she had “to come up with some kind of a story that makes sense in order to sequence
through them.” Singers used elements naturally intrinsic in music as well as those related
kinds: (a) outcome goals, (b) performance goals, and (c) process goals. These images
ranged from vocalists who imagined singing in a role in a famous opera house to
165
Outcome goal images were reported in 14 participants and ranged from specific,
clearly imagined goals to those more general. Specific outcome goals included images
that were sometimes competitive, the results of being chosen from an audition,
instance, images involved visualizing the final results of a competition, as when Ivan
imagined himself “with the award in hand.” Catherine and others imagined details of a
specific role and actually being engaged to perform it as a result of the audition.
Catherine stated, “I imagine myself in the costume and in the show I'm auditioning for,
imagining already having gotten the gig and performing it on stage.” Francine organized
a routine for imagining the outcome for performing in different venues. She said:
In order to accomplish successfully singing for them and getting the job, what
I’ve done is go into the house [theatre], see a show there, and imagine myself on
that stage, so putting myself on the other side already before they even give me
the opportunity. Saying ‘I already got the job, I already have gone through
Other specific outcome goals included images of performing in prestigious venues such
as the Metropolitan Opera. For example, Nicholas stated, “the MET is one of the places
I’m hoping to sing before I’m done, and so I’ve used that a little bit to imagine myself
singing there.” General outcome goals were less focused and were evident in six quotes.
To illustrate, Gerald had “goals of singing at international performing venues, and being
like other highly sought-after performers,” and Josephine wanted to “sing on the next
level of the business, on a higher level of the business.” These vocalists used outcome
166
Performance goals primarily dealt with singers achieving standards of excellence
These goal images included a desire to experience general positive affect as expressed by
Eloise who wanted to “really enjoying what I’m doing,” and the somewhat more specific
concentrated on the immediate moment with the assumption that her efforts could lead to
better opportunities. She stated, “I try to focus on the task at hand, and the tasks at hand
always lead to the bigger goal.” She also expressed continued interested in “getting
better, and better, and better. It’s something I’ll never be able to attain.” Ophelia’s
performance goals were more general as compared to Louis’ goals which were more
I want to give the absolute best artistic performance that I can. And when I do
that, regardless of what stage I’m on or what role it is in, that’s when I feel like
Nicholas’ imagined executing his ideal audition and said, “I will imagine it all. I’ll
picture the scene usually ahead of time.” This type of goal work included all three types
skills were so frequent in this study and seemed to be an intrinsic aspect of singers’
imagery content. These ranged from securing techniques in vocal production, achieving a
certain sound quality, embodying the character and emotion of a role, and imagining the
body to have a certain appearance in the effort to achieve automaticity, or habit, in the
desired skill or technique. The specific findings are not reported here since they were
167
found to pervade nearly every aspect of the imagery these professional vocalists used and
relating to vocal production. While most of the participants used some kind of metaphor
in their imagery, all were familiar with a number of metaphors used in traditional vocal
categories: (a) imaginary actions, (b) objects not present, and (c) colors. These
Cumming, 2005).
production and technique, such as regulating the breath and vocal placement. Gerald
controlled his breath by using the image of “very gently, gently, gently, blowing a spec of
dust.” For vocal support, Josephine imagined that her “voice is a shelf. I think of putting
those notes on a shelf.” Francine’s imagery of her vocal production was like “catching
the baseball behind my head.” Related to enlarging the throat for singing, Eloise said,
“Imagine that you’re a seal and you’re going to swallow the fish whole.” Some of these
Twelve singers described objects that were not present, constituting the widest
variety of metaphorical images, all of which related to vocal technique and production.
Examples of some object images were, “potato sacks in the back” (Barry), “a conveyor
belt” (Monique), and “a string of spaghetti” (Harriet). Many of these singers reported
using the same images, which may have represented those generally accepted in vocal
pedagogy (e.g., “smelling a rose”; “fireplace bellows”). Many singers created their own
168
metaphors, such as Eloise’s image of a “crescent moon, which is behind you, embracing
you.” Some of these images were used in place of actual mechanical processes of the
voice. More examples of these images and the specific applications were included in the
For nine of these vocalists, imagery of color represented visual hues and shades
primarily relating to the timbre or the quality of vocal sound. Anna experienced the visual
aspect of color in relation to parts of music. She said, “I’ll see colors, different notes have
different colors and different phrases have different colors, reds or greens or blues.” Ivan
A passionate line will invariably, for me, have reddish tint. A passionate line with
an ascending line, towards the top will be that passionate red that goes to a
brighter color, almost always towards . . . gold, a very bright gold, for the upper
part of the voice. No matter what it is, no matter if it’s blue that I’m envisioning,
it always goes to gold on top, red to gold on top. The top is always the same color.
Unless I using something like a fille voce, sotto voce, pianissini, up top, then it’s a
silver.
This example could arguably be described as chromesthesia in which the visual colors
mix with sound in imagery. Conversely, other singers regarded color images as those
relating to the emotional expression in the voice. Nicholas illustrated this by saying, “I’ll
either case, color images for these singers described a spectrum of vocal qualities they
169
Context images. Vocalists’ imagery encompassed specific contexts and included
three different categorical aspects: (a) environments, (b) imaginary people, and (c)
audiences. All 15 singers imagined various environments, which were classified into four
subcategories: (a) being on stage or in the theatre, (b) the scene of a song, (c) distant
places, and (d) home. Vocalists described various scenarios of themselves performing on
a stage. For example, Catherine said, “I would just imagine being on stage, taking a
curtain call.” Harriet imagined singing to an audience member “in the last row of the
theatre.” Monique was one of six singers who created her own artistic environment in her
mind when she performed. She said, “I try to put myself in the scene of the picture that
that song is painting. So in my mind, I see the sun; I see the water; I see the mountains.
They’re all around me.” Karen imagined being in a far away land. Ivan described images
of his old age with his grandchildren surrounding him at home “in front of a fireplace.”
images. These included audition panelists and people who were directly related to the
Other singers imagined panelists present in their auditions. Four participants reported
non-humans such as angels and devils. Francine imagined “an angel on one shoulder and
a devil on the other.” At times just prior to performing, Monique reported contending
Nine vocalists imagined interactions with the audience. Most of these singers
regarded the audience members favorably, as Karen came to realize. She said, “I actually
imagined an audience really not that frightening.” Other images involved sharing
something with the audience. Eloise would “picture the audience and try to imagine what
170
they’re hoping for,” and Josephine imagined her voice was “reaching that last seat, not
just the people in the very front.” Other audience images are reported in the Artistic
feeling or seeing their physical body and was classified into four categories: (a) arousal,
(b) appearance, and (c) kinesthetic images. With regard to the body, feelings and images
of arousal were the most frequent, with 13 vocalists sharing their images of breath
control, feelings of calm, and specific areas of physical tension. Breath images in relation
to calming thoughts and feelings were most common in regulating arousal. Gerald
imagined the “feeling of the breath calmly moving through my body,” while Josephine
used “the imagery of seeing the breath coming in and out of my body.” Four singers
experienced calmness throughout their body, including Dorine who imagined “this
feeling of peace, peacefulness and calmness that starts at the top of my head and goes
down my body.” Four singers mentioned images of feeling nervousness and tension that
Eleven participants used appearance images, which was not surprising since much
category primarily included images for stage performance outside of appearance imagery
and appealing to the audience.” Anna said, “sometimes I’ll look in the mirror to see what
I’m doing right then, and then to create what I want it to look like in my head instead.”
Louis stated, “[I] visualize myself looking the way that I want to.” Generally, appearance
171
imagery was used to create at least the illusion of appropriate visual presentation for
performance.
Singers’ responses varied in the level of detail they imagined their appearance to
be on stage. Several held a more general image in their minds as to what they wanted to
look like on stage (e.g., Josephine, “looking beautiful and appealing to the audience. . . .
looking like a princess,” Ophelia: “poised, trying to be as in control, even though you
completely feel out of control”). Harriet proposed, “I guess there’s a standard that I
expect of myself when on stage. Yes. I expect to have a finished product.” Other singers’
I know how I will walk on stage, how I will look, how I will hold my hands, who
am I, how do I want to present myself, how I go on, how I look when I go on,
Each detail concerning her appearance was fully planned and vividly imagined.
Reports of kinesthetic images included those of (a) vocal production, (b) body
feeling and movement, (c) posture and alignment, and (d) health. Ten responses in vocal
production images involved specific mechanisms in the body. Catherine and Ivan
provided the best examples of these images. Catherine imagined the feeling of “where in
your body the resonance [of the sound] will strike or where it will vibrate.” Ivan used
imagery to experience “the ease, the vibrato, the blood flow, the vibrations of the tone
when my body is supporting the tone, what that feels like in my body.” Along with the
six vocalists who mentioned body movement, Josephine felt it was important to “imagine
yourself as a young child, or as a baby lamb or something to get your body to move a
certain way.” Six singers described images of posture and alignment which either
172
represented optimal qualities or those representing the physical character depicted in a
song or aria. Six participants reported health images. During periods of sickness, Barry
imagined “the times when I was perfectly healthy.” Before even vague symptoms of
illness set in, Gerald would tell himself “that I’m healthy, so that my body functions at its
optimal level.” Images relating to the body seemed to be an intrinsic part of performing
as a singer.
Musical sound images. Singers reported hearing both musical and vocal sound
images in relation to practice and performance. All singers engaged in silent musical
practice and mentally heard various sounds in their head related to vocal and musical
production. Musical auditory images were classified into three subcategories: (a) pitch,
(b) melody, and (c) accompaniment. Ten singers mentally heard the specific pitch just
prior to initiating that sound in their voice. Monique illustrated this perfectly when she
said:
I always breathe in the note I’m about to sing. . . . so that the note is existing
before I actually sing it, so that I’m right in the middle of the note. . . .I can’t tell
you how that helps you mentally. There’s no searching for it, you see, you’ve
Exact pitches were imagined in the beginning of phrases, in approaching a high note, as
well as notes throughout the aria. Fourteen participants mentioned hearing the melody in
their heads just before singing the line or during mental practice. Josephine imagined
“hearing a passage in my head before I would sing it.” All but one participant reported
hearing the accompaniment, especially the musical intervals that occurred between their
vocal phrases. During silent practice, Dorine said she was “hearing the piano, imagining
173
the accompaniment from start to finish.” Gerald stated, “I do imagine the accompaniment
[and] hear the interludes.” Harriet reported mentally hearing “another instrument’s part
that plays in sort of duet with me while I’m singing my line.” Some singers mentioned
being able to mentally hear several musical elements simultaneously. Nicholas said, “I
will hear basically everything going on in my head. So I’ll hear my line and hear the text,
and hear the orchestra.” It seemed that these participants used sound images to assist
Participants reported using images of vocal sounds which were divided into the
subcategories of (a) modeling sounds of expert singers, (b) ideal sounds, and (c) hearing
one’s own singing voice. Anna was one of 10 vocalists who used sound modeling in her
imagery, which she developed by “[w]atching other singers, and listening to their music,
especially ones that are similar to you.” Likewise, Eloise said, “I have sort of imitated or
imagined performances that I’ve seen other people do.” Several of these participants
included the names of famous vocal professionals they used as inspiration for their
images. Ideal sound images were possibly a culmination of what that particular singer
had seen, heard, and experienced throughout the years as a professional. Seven singers
reported using these images, including Karen who stated, “I know the sound that I want
to make.” Four singers heard their own voice in silent practice and anticipated an ideal
production of that specific sound. These sound images provided an ideal and appropriate
professional was the element of acting. From the participants’ responses, character and
role images included (a) emotions, (b) behaviors, and (c) appearance.
174
Emotional images emerged as being vital to bringing a character, song, or role to
life for all these professionals. Several of the participants expressed their conviction that
emotional imagery gave them motivation for the behavior of the character. Eliciting the
correct emotional image was critical in providing an appropriate framework for how the
character or story of the song progressed in performance. Ivan said, “the mood changes,
yeah, the moment before the change happens.” Six of the singers reported using their own
recreating a sorrowful feeling for a particular role, Barry said he accomplished this “by
going to that place, and bringing back images of a sadder time.” In order to maintain the
ability to sing in times of great emotion during an aria, Josephine would simply “touch on
memories to be able to bring you to that place that you need to be able to portray the
song.” Specific and appropriate emotional imagery was deemed critical to performance
excellence and served as internal guide and motivation for movement of the character.
included 12 images of motivation for action and four images of physically staging the
specific characters and roles. Nicholas allowed his images to naturally develop by
“setting myself within the text of the plot of the opera and letting the motivation of the
character, and getting ideas that the character would be having, shape the phrases.” Anna
said, “I do some of this visualization where I’ll go through and go, ‘I want this to look
like this,’ especially if you’re dying or you’re angry” as part of the requirements of
staging. Some of these singers employed movement and behavior images in specific
constantly having images going through my brain at the same time I was singing, of
175
someone who was wounded, and physically crippled and disabled to portray that type of
character.” For staging Carmen, Francine conjured images of being in Spain many years
ago and described, “Imagine that you are digging through the earth, you’re in Seville . . . .
What does the floor feel like underneath your [sic] feet?” This imagery included sensory
and context details helped recreate the character’s experience in the scene.
costumes, makeup, and historical and cultural settings as dictated by the composer or the
production. Four singers described their imagery of appearance including Eloise, who
I imagine myself in the costume and on the set, and in the castle or whatever it
was, the mansion, or in the country home or wherever, and what was I wearing,
In acting out roles, singers often needed to imagine a change in their physique for
presentation on stage. Being a mezzo, Francine had to appear to change genders, which
Imagery types described here represented the content the participants reported
using in their professional efforts. The following sections describe some of the imagery
Imagery Characteristics
subcategories from Nordin and Cumming (2005: (a) sense imagery, (b) perspective, (c)
ability, (d) direction and deliberation, and (e) amount and duration.
176
Senses. Six categories of imagery involving the senses emerged in the analysis:
(a) sight or visual, (b) sound or auditory, (c) kinesthetic or feeling, (d) touch or tactile, (e)
smell or olfactory, and (f) taste or gustatory. During the interviews, singers were given
the opportunity to describe the specific extent of their uses of each of the senses. All
singers reported using vision, auditory, and kinesthetic imagery in their artistic
endeavors. The senses of touch, smell and taste were used to a much less extent in
practice and performance. Participants’ specific responses to each of the senses involving
some Imagery Types (addressed in the previous section) are given in Table 15.
All 15 singers reported using visual imagery with six who said they used it a great
deal. Barry and Francine each stated they had a photographic memory and Karen
declared that she was “a visual learner.” The sense of sight was used primarily in
visualizing scenarios, vocal production and technique, and page notation. Auditory
imagery was employed by all the vocalists to imagine hearing various aspects of their
vocal production, the music, and the text involved in their silent practice and
performance. Kinesthetic imagery included feeling the internal technical, physical, and
emotional aspects of singing and acting. Specific images using these three senses were
Tactile and kinesthetic senses had been confused in seven of the interviewees’
responses, which may have originated from simple terminology differences. For example,
Louis declared, “I do experience the music in a very tactile, visceral kind of way.” Even
though tactile refers to touch and visceral connotes deeper feelings, the assumption could
reasonably be made that he was referring to the internal vocal mechanism and perhaps
not the epidermis of the neck and jaw. This was later confirmed in a subsequent
177
Table 15
Page Notation 13
Sound/Auditory Sound 15
Words/Text/Phrase 14
Pitch 10
Melody 14
Accompaniment 14
Silent Singing 15
Vocal Production 15
Kinesthetic Kinesthetic 15
Character/Emotion 14
Vocal Production 15
Body Check 10
Pitch Preparation 3
Automaticity 2
178
Table 15 (continued)
Character Based 8
Future Performance 2
Vocal Production 3
Venue 3
Text-based 9
Vocal Production 3
kinesthetically in singing and tactile sense imagery was used more in the physical act of
vocalizing and securing technique. Furthermore, when the terms kinesthetic and tactile
were explained as defined in this study, all seven singers agreed they meant the
179
The sense of touch was usually employed in imagery when its use was dictated in
the text of a song or the action of a character. For example, Louis was one of two
participants who used the tactile sense in imagining future auditions and performances:
If I know that I’m going to be wearing a certain suit for an audition, I try to feel
hands on the piano during a recital and if so, what will that piano feel like? If I’m
going to have a wig on during an operatic performance, how will that constrict my
scalp?
Smell and taste imagery was employed the least with roughly half of the singers
reporting that they did not use it very often. Only two participants seemed to enjoy taste
and smell images and referenced their extensive cooking experience. Otherwise these
vocalists reported using smells and tastes in context of the song or role. Using olfactory
imagery in performing in the opera, La Boheme, Ivan would “imagine what kind of
perfume Mimi would wear. What kind of cologne does Rudolpho put on when he’s
getting ready to go out?” Gerald used the sense of smell to assist him in portraying his
In some operas, you find yourself in particular scenes where you’re either in a
dungeon or you’re on the gallows, or some place, and where people are dying
around you, so that imagining the smell of death, or whatever that gives a fear
Singers also used smell imagery in association with the odor of specific venues.
180
like felice mi fa, [from Musetta’s Waltz in La Boheme] it’s like, it’s got to be yummier
each time you say it. So I think of darker and darker chocolate as I go up.” These
participants used touch, and particularly smell and taste the least of all the senses, but
these senses remained available for use in imagery to more specifically embody the
specific sense imagery, two participants used no tactile, one singer never used smell, and
From the above accounts, vocalists used their senses extensively for imagery in
of use, singers were asked the interview question 36, “Which of the above would be the
primary senses you use in your imagery?” Subsequent probes during the interview invited
vocalists to rate the second and third sense they used in imagery. Ivan and Monique felt
the senses of vision, hearing, and kinesthesia held equal and topmost importance. The
final results of the sense imagery rating by the remaining 13 participants revealed vision
to be first with seven votes, auditory second with eight, and kinesthesia third with six (see
Table 16). Several mentioned touch usually as the third choice, but later changed it to
kinesthesia. Taste was used slightly more than smell, and mostly employed by two
participants who enjoyed cooking. Therefore, by a very small margin in the first three,
the rank order of senses used in imagery by these professional soloists from the most to
the least was: (a) vision, (b) auditory, (c) kinesthesia, (d) tactile, (e), taste, and (f) smell.
181
Table 16
Vision/Sight 7 3 3
Sound/Auditory 1 8 4
Kinesthesia/Feeling 5 2 6
TOTAL 13 13 13
Note: Two participants choose sound, kinesthetic, and vision together as equally important.
Perspective. Capabilities of visual imagery also involved the use of internal and
external perspectives. Singers in this study favored internal perspective, however several
used external with some employing both as seen in Table 17. Internal perspective,
reported by 14 singers, was seeing things from the first person point of view. Nicholas
stated, “I’m always picturing it from my perspective, because I never see myself in the
scene.” Conversely, five participants used the external, or the third person viewpoint, as
was the preference of Gerald who said, “I’d see me, watching from the audience.” Ivan
was one of three singers, who were able to switch between inner and outer perspective,
It flips from the inner eye to the outer eye, back and forth. . . .[I]f I’m stuck with
just the outer eye, then I’m not as complete as I could be. . . .What’s most
powerful is the inner eye. If I can get into the inner eye of what, what I am initial
seeing, then I know I’m on track. . . . That’s when I know it’s real. If I can do that
transition in my visualization.
182
Anna sometimes used "external to get the perspective the audience.” Again these visual
perspective images were used for various reasons and depended on individual abilities
and skills.
Table 17
Internal 14
External 5
Switching 3
imagery ability, or how well the singers were able to create their images. These data were
classified into three subcategories: (a) ability, accuracy, and level of detail; (b)
deliberation and direction; (c) amount and duration. The frequency of results of the
When singers were asked about their imagery ability and degree of accuracy
separately, the responds were identical. Twelve singers said it was good while three
others rated theirs as fair. Regarding vividness, 10 vocalists reported having very vivid
images and five felt that theirs were fairly vivid. Four singers described their images as
being very accurate, with Louis adding, “right down to the last level of minutia.” Some
183
Table 18
Helpful 10 (5 no answer)
and the degree to which it was controlled or manipulated. Singers’ rated their imagery
deliberation as being primarily controlled, with 11 participants reporting that they were
able to easily manipulate their images, and four rating their ability to do this as fair. Some
Nicholas who said, “I found that probably just because I have a lot of practice on the
stage things now, that I can imagine. Imagery was good.” Some exceptions and problems
spontaneously and could have debilitated her performance if she had not exercised
response to imagery direction, 10 singers reported that their images were generally more
helpful and facilitative than hurtful. However, eight felt that sometimes their imagery
It’s hurtful if you don’t control it. . . . If you are discovering technique and you
find the wrong image and it doesn’t help you and you continue to use it over and
At times Anna’s images were not helpful, as she stated, “if I don’t do a good job
[imaging], it can be destructive.” Singers reported this occurring especially when their
images were not appropriate to their specific needs. Catherine found that sometimes
“they’re not helpful, because they’re really not the right image.” Francine discovered, “If
it’s hurtful, it’s because I’ve chosen to do something that’s unsuccessful, and then, again,
I have to make an adjustment.” Several participants discovered problems when they did
not take the time to imagine clearly, such as Ophelia discovering her “blind spots” on
stage, where she had inadvertently omitted specific imagery in her performance
preparation.
Regarding amount and duration, the former dealt with the time given to engage in
imagery, while the latter involved how long the image appeared, remained and
duration of the image itself, a few participants offered glimpses of their experiences in
this regard. Amount of time in imagery sessions seemed to vary with the individual from
very quick flashes (Gerald) to consciously practicing imagery all night long in preparing
185
for an upcoming role (Eloise). Regarding duration, Louis described using high-speed
I go through the recital very quickly and very frantically in my mind in an attempt
to make sure that I can recall the text right to the forefront of my mind very
quickly. It’s a memory drill to make sure that I have the text memorized really,
but I am thinking the notes and the pitches all the way through. Oftentimes what
I’ll do in this type of practice is eliminate any rests or sections where I’m not
as quickly as I can and I’ll move from one section to the next immediately
without break.
This detailed example of high speed, or compressed mental practice, seemed to help
solidify the song in memory for Louis. Just prior to auditions, Nicholas tried, in his
words, “to visualize pretty much everything that happens for about 10 minutes.” Dorine
imagery amount and duration in vocal performance seemed to depend on the need of the
imagery changed particularly with regard to the uses of their senses and abilities. Gerald
reflected this, when he said, “my first criticism I would make is that I need to make my
visualization more specific, more detailed.” In evaluating her imagery strengths and
weaknesses as a result of answering the questions, Eloise realized that she could shift her
use of sense imagery and include those that were less developed. She said, “I think I’ve
stacked a little bit too much in the kinesthetic and I should be more aware of the
186
emotional. I think I should think about taste, smell, and touch, which I’ve said I don’t
like, and experiment with those.” Catherine’s retrospection also revealed her desire for
I am not a master. I feel a little bit lazy at it all. Like I said, you’re bringing up all
these things that I could be doing. This is so cool. I think I go to a comfort level,
and then don’t push it, because I don’t necessarily take the time. . . . I’m
constantly aware that the imagery is a good way to go, but in practice, it does not
always play out as well as I would hope. But that could be because I’m not very
Catherine and some of the other singers reflected a belief that there may be a link
between the specificity and clarity of the image and performance outcome. Furthermore,
it became apparent to many of these participants that imagery ability could be continually
developed and strengthened and that it was a powerful tool in their quest for performance
excellence. Results of singers’ reasons for using imagery content and ability will now be
discussed.
Responses regarding why, or the reasons for which imagery was used, were
similar and at times quite different compared to athletic and dance imagery research.
Singers’ imagery functions seemed to generally reflect aspects of the athletic frameworks
of Paivio (1985), Hall and associates (1998), Munroe and others (2000), and perhaps
more specifically those of dance imagery research (Nordin & Cumming, 2005). In
analyzing the data, emerging categories were constantly compared with those found in
187
the sport and dance frameworks. Vocal professionals’ reasons for using imagery were
classified into four categories: (a) cognitive, (b) motivational, (c) artistic, and (d) healing.
Many of these reasons were presented in previous sections to allow the reader to
understand the details of how the content of images interacted intrinsically with the
purposes for which they were being used in these professional vocalists’ experiences.
Cognitive Reasons
The cognitive reasons for using imagery addressed the functions of thinking
through or planning out behaviors. These were divided between cognitive specific (CS),
which included skill learning and execution, and cognitive general (CG), relating to
specific categories and subcategories of singers’ cognitive reasons for using imagery.
Learning vocal techniques and skills (cognitive specific). Cognitive reasons for
singers’ use of imagery were those in which singers reported using images for the
purposes of learning, developing, strengthening, and correcting skills. Results from the
production and quality and resulted in two distinct subcategories of vocal production and
188
Table 19
Vocal Participants’ Cognitive Reasons for Imagery Use Categories and Subcategories
Breath Support 7
Kinesthetic 6
Audiation 2
Note Duration 2
Projection 2
Metaphors in Breath 11
High Notes 3
Vocal Registers 2
Resonance 2
Changing Quality 2
Vocal Line 4
Stories 2
Staging Movements 7
Motivation 2
189
Vocal production involved specific aspects of using imagery directly involved
with producing, extending, or projecting the sound. Each of the 15 singers offered several
technique, (b) ease and comfort of producing sound, (c) breath support, (d) kinesthetic
connection, (e) audiation, (f) extending note duration, and (g) projection.
singers, such as Catherine used technique images in warm-ups “to help me produce
proper vocal technique.” Eloise illustrated her experience of this, and said that:
in order to sing scales that go up and down, I imagine the opposite direction and
the opposite flow of energy. . . . in order to sing from low to high, I’m gonna sink
Imagery to keep the voice grounded in the high notes and lifted in the low ones assisted
this singer in maintaining continuity of sound execution throughout the registers. Other
singers were also concerned with gaining skills in singing in specific registers. For
example, Nicholas was looking to produce “a little more focus and ring in the upper part
of my voice.” In producing a vocal sound particular to mezzos, Karen said, “In order to
do that whistle range, I just see these imaginary folds going almost together. It just pops
right into my mind.” Imagery for vocal placement concerned the muscles of the vocal
apparatus producing tones in a specific way, as Ivan illustrated. He said, “I can anticipate
what the voice is going to feel like in its optimal conditions related to the sequence of
music that I want to sing.” Conversely, several vocalists found that over the years they
tended, as Louis stated, “to rely more on mechanics and more on the science of vocal
technique rather than on sensation.” Instead of kinesthetic feeling, Louis preferred more
190
informed technical imagery for singing. It seemed that as vocal skill developed, imagery
preferences changed. These two elements of vocal skill and imagery interacted and grew
In producing vocal sound, nine participants mentioned using imagery to feel ease
or comfort in various areas of the body, and particular parts of the vocal apparatus. In
specifically striving for relaxation in the neck and jaw, Ophelia maintained, “you should
never think about having a voice box, having a neck at all.” To this end, Gerald wanted to
experience “the sound of the passage being easy, the image of freedom and free flow.”
Imagery was particularly useful in creating the feeling of effortlessness of singing. Louis
explained, “I do try for a gentle, easy sound, but oftentimes it’s difficult to interpret
mechanically. Instead you just do it sensorily [sic] and you try to experience a little
gentler, easier style of singing.” These singers believed that creating the image of singing
ease produced the proper vocal sounds and a comfortable feeling in their bodies.
Imagery for skill learning also involved the development and maintenance of
breath support for seven participants. Four singers talked about breath support by
imagining their breath deepening while they sang an ascending vocal line. Gerald
illustrated his use of imagery to gain more breath support. He stated, “I would go back to
breathing and visualization. When I am taking a breath or using a breath, imagining the
air filling places where oxygen really doesn’t go but you want to find expansion.” This
example also incorporated Gerald’s kinesthetic use of imagery in gaining more control in
vocal production.
described a physiological feeling of producing the ideal sound image. Anna exemplified
191
this aptly, and said, “It’s a combination of the feeling and what sounds you’re going for. .
. . You get that exact thing in your head and then you breathe into what you’re already
imagining.” More specifically, Eloise imagined parts of her body in creating a strong
sound, saying:
I think of that through my back, and a weight trainer body, I imagine a very strong
body. . . . I create this huge instrument in my mind and then in my body, just
actually feel the muscles and gauge them with the depths of the voice, which is
what carries.
Nicholas tried to imagine the feeling of sound being “well grounded from deep in my
body.” Francine experienced this deep placement in executing lower notes when she said,
“what I feel is a relaxed letting myself go and letting the sound just drop in the middle of
my body. . . . So I have that image every single time I go into my chest voice.” Vocal
production for these singers was rooted more deeply in their entire body than just the
Ophelia was one of two who mentioned kinesthetic imagery in relation to gaining
[A] lot of it comes with doing it again and again and just getting it into the body
and getting that kinesthetic memory, so that when it comes down to performance,
honest to God, you’re not thinking about it, because you have so many other
things to think about, that you want it to just be rote memory. But there are
occasionally those spots that are very difficult and even in performance you need
to think about it, when you have to focus on the kinesthetic memory.
192
Kinesthetic imagery in learning through the body seemed to facilitate the movements and
The last three elements in this section on vocal production were audiation,
extending note durations, and projecting the sound. Eloise heard the sound she wanted to
make before she sang it. She said, “It was to audiate, so to hear it, listen to it, mentally so
to speak, before you try to actually make the sound.” Imagery in audiation often
improved pitch accuracy and proper mental and physical preparation to produce the
sound in the specific context of the music. Extending note durations began with breath
control and mental thought processes in preparing that sound. Ophelia described using
imagery to help learn this technique. She said, “if I have to sing a high C of 16 beats, then
I would just keep thinking of an image that would lengthen the note.” Anna used imagery
[E]ven if you have a note that you need to hold. . . if you imagine while you’re
singing that you’re breathing in at the same time, so you actually are breathing in
at the same time. . . . it restructures your body without out using valuable energy
and voice.
Not only do singers have to find ways to hold extensively long notes, but they have to
make their sound carry through a large theatre or venue without the use of microphone.
Josephine mentioned singing to the “old lady in the back row” as a mental image to help
her voice carry all the way through the theatre. Dorine would “envision a small point on
the wall and I sing to that point.” She imagined the sound touching or going beyond that
chosen point in the back of the theatre. Singers used different kinds of imagery to find
better ways of producing their sound. These accounts included many instances of
193
imagery use in improving various aspects of vocal skill and technique. This next section
production. Although several singers preferred not to use metaphors, they were all
familiar with them and how they were used in achieving proper vocal technique.
Metaphors were connected with vocal production in seven distinct areas: (a) support
including breath, (b) throat opening, (c) execution of high notes, d) singing through the
vocal registers, (e) creating resonance, (f) changing sound quality, and (g) connecting the
vocal line.
breathing and was vital for classical singing. Metaphors were plentiful in this area and
were reported by 11 singers (e. g., Gerald: “smelling a rose for inhalation,” Francine, “a
steel rod coming out of my feet and going all the way through the core of the earth”). In
order to relieve muscular tension, Anna imagined “making everything really relaxed
underneath like the anchor.” Eloise created the image of “a three-dimensional pyramid
that goes out from my body, out behind me, very wide. . . . Your voice goes effortlessly
through the peaks of the pyramid because it has such wide, wonderful support at the
base.” Many singers mentioned that using metaphors greatly aided in establishing support
and controlling the breath, however as they developed as singers these skills became
more automatic and many began to prefer anatomical images of the actual vocal
mechanism.
194
In classical singing training, one of the first techniques most students were taught
was to lift the soft palate in the back of the throat to create more space in singing. It was
most easily accomplished by maintaining the feeling of a yawn during singing. Six
singers in this study mentioned various metaphors to help them achieve that continuous
feeling (e.g., Nicholas: “egg in the back of my throat,” Dorine: “plum in the mouth,”
In order to secure high notes and to keep the sound light and pleasant, three
sopranos used metaphors. Dorine provided a vivid example of this. She stated:
[W]hen I’ve taken a deep enough breath, or if I’m going for higher notes, then I
want my breath to stay strong beneath me. . . . I pretend that I’m sliding down a
banister, moving down physically while the voice is moving up, and it helps to
This idea of singing high while thinking low was repeated several times. To achieve a
more delicate sound, Josephine felt that the metaphor of birdsong was the “perfect
imagery for a soprano, high sounds, like something that’s pretty and easy and floating.”
Josephine imagined supported high notes when she spoke of placing the notes on a shelf
as previously quoted. The image of a shelf helped her think of her high notes as having a
place to rest, rather than mentally pushing her sound higher. Singers consistently found
Regardless of the vocal category, singers’ voices were considered to have vocal
registers that were delineated by “breaks” in separating the high, middle, and low parts of
their voice. Singers needed to develop and maintain consistency through these areas or
risk “cracking,” or a vocal shift, in passing from one register to another. Two singers
195
mentioned metaphors for contending with this. Gerald felt “the voice narrowing, that sort
of feeling that my voice registers blend there.” In order to successfully deal with this,
accomplish as singers is making our voice consistent from low to high and all the
way through the middle. . .but it’s never 100% just straight chest, there’s always a
thread of head in that. And then as you go up through the middle, how does that
shift on every note as you go up into the high register. So if you visualize a dial,
you have a high level of chest and a low level of head, and then it slowly and
gradually shifts up until the high registers where you have a high level of head
Consistency through the vocal registers was facilitated with metaphorical imagery as well
production, these singers also concentrated on shaping the quality of their sound as was
expressed in nine of the transcriptions. Four singers mentioned the connection of human
sound capabilities such as a sigh, a grunt, a groan, or a yawn with providing a variety of
vocal possibilities and extending the quality of their sound. This was best exemplified in
The most nutty metaphor has to do with different noises that we naturally make.
But they’re so wacky, like your conscious intellect doesn’t give you permission to
make these noises. . . . See, we have the entire orchestral family of instruments
right in our bodies, in our voices. Wheew, or quack, waaa, [she demonstrated the
196
sounds] because you’re really hot. I mean it’s like there’s a whole bunch of really
crazy people inside of you that need to make all of these different noises and they
In her interview, Eloise provided a detailed explanation of each of these specific imagery
sounds representing all the families of the orchestra. However, due to space
considerations, these are not given here. Although some of these singers had extensive
familiarity with vocal sound varieties, it did not appear as general knowledge in many of
the singers in this study. This aspect of singing could be further investigated.
create more resonance in her voice. She said, “I imagined that the space that one creates
in the cavity, the head, is sort of shaped like a cone, and that the voice functions within
the resonance of the voice is within that cone.” Harriet also used the image of her ribs
acting like fireplace bellows to increase deeper resonance in her torso. Francine achieved
more vocal resonance in her head when she imagined what “it feels like biting into a big
juicy apple.” These metaphors seemed to connect with kinesthetic feeling to facilitate
In performing an aria or song, soloists imagined connecting the entire line of the
music and text in performance. Four singers gave many examples of using metaphors to
help them stay connected throughout the song (e. g., Anna: “like the train is already
running and you just need to get on the train;” Dorine: “hanging each note on the
clothesline;” Monique: “my conveyor belt, a constant flowing line”). More specifically,
Barry used a metaphor taken from nature to help control the voice when the melody line
descended:
197
A lot of times singers crash, and they get lazy when they descend into a line and
the sound will go flat. And what I always say is the metaphor of a bird landing on
a small branch. You can’t just crash on the branch, you have to gently land on it
Using various metaphors these singers were able to create a smooth and continuous line
throughout the song. These participants were exceptionally adept at using metaphors in
imagery for developing and perfecting extensive vocal technique. Imagery used for the
function of imagery use was found in responses and resulted in five sections: (a) learning
and memorizing repertoire, (b) sequence in song or aria, (c) program planning, (d)
Learning and memorizing songs, arias, and roles was a skill vocalists developed
and perfected throughout their profession. Six singers described various uses of imagery
in the process of learning the rudiments of a song. Gerald explained that he would
“mentally sing through with all the correct words, correct rhythm, high notes in place,
everything is there, voice tapped in the way I want it to, gestures where they need to be.”
Catherine imagined herself “singing through an entire piece vocally and singing
everything correctly.” Dorine emphasized kinesthetic feeling when learning new music
and said, “if there are passages that are difficult or I’m not doing it correctly, I just keep
doing it until the feeling is there.” Ivan also used repetition by mentally singing the song
“over and over again until it becomes really clear and then as I start to sing. I trace, in
effect, that imagery. I follow it. And I do that for most all my music.” Louis preferred
198
both physical and mental practice to fully memorize and embody the aria. He said, “sub-
vocal or trying to learn literature without really singing it, I find takes a lot more time
and practice to adequately prepare.” Mentally rehearsing the song served to reinforce all
the elements of the song rendering them automatic in the minds and voices of these
singers.
For seven participants, memorization was easier when a song they had to learn
was grouped in sequences and patterns. Two singers visualized these musical patterns on
the page to help them remember the song. Others used movements to assist them in
memory, as Harriet was quoted as saying, “I can imagine myself walking through
staging. I can see myself on stage going through staging outside of me actually doing it. I
use that to memorize things.” Louis ran through the music very quickly to test whether he
could “recall the text right to the forefront of my mind very quickly.” When singers used
Elements in songs and arias were imagined in ordered sequences in order to give
anticipate “what’s coming up next, and how much energy, and how much vocal power
and breath, and all that it requires” in order to properly perform the song. Singers
specifically planned out how the song was to be executed. Harriet demonstrated this by
saying, “I’ll mark spaces where I’ll give a little less intensity. I’ll give myself a little bit
of a break here.” Ivan found it was important to “anticipate what the voice is going to feel
like in its optimal conditions related to the sequence of music that I want to sing.” Two
singers mentioned sequencing in terms of pacing, especially if the song was long. For
these arias, Ophelia found that “there’s a sequence that’s followed [that allowed her] to
199
go back to previous images or moods or colors [within the song] that . . . draws the
audience in.” In order to keep the song interesting and make it appealing to the listener,
these singers imagined sequential rudiments to deliver a cohesive and interesting aria.
For singers who were involved in giving concerts, one of the most obvious
reported mentally preparing these performances and elaborated on the details. In the case
of song cycles, where the composer has provided the connection between each song,
Each piece had its own mood, and so you would do one, and the next one. You
would have to prepare in between. You got in the moment, it was almost like
opera because you went into this next character, and the whole mood and
character, and everything. . . . But that sort of imagery had to happen very quickly
because, of course, you stopped the one piece and then you were in another
In practicing song cycles, Dorine said she “pictured the whole thing from start to finish. .
. . singing each piece through, and then singing in my head and making sure each piece
was correct.” In this case, sequence imagery facilitated memorizing the entire song cycle.
Some recitals required the vocalist to choose the sequence and create some
element to link these songs together in a logical progression. Francine said she had to
“come up with some kind of a story that makes sense in order to sequence through them.”
For Monique, planning her programs was very specifically arranged, as she illustrated:
[Y]ou build your program so that you pace yourself by what you’re singing. . . . I
plan my program so that, especially my solo recitals, that whatever I’m singing
200
first is really pretty easy to sing so that I can get over my nerves. Then the
program builds from there, and the biggest piece being just before the interval
[intermission]. And then you come back after the interval. And I always end my
Connecting one song to another and creating the entire recital to be, as Catherine
described, “an organic whole,” took planning and the ability to imagine the effect of
had to be synchronized precisely with the music. Seven participants reported using
imagery for sequencing performance movement. Ophelia explained that “you have to
prepare ahead of time, where you’re singing on stage, and prepare your singing ahead of
time to get all of those aligned correctly so you get the right effect.” Louis illustrated his
imagery of staging when he said, “it will include everything from blocking, to text, to
pitch, to my particular vocal approach through to the music.” He was able to include
many aspects of his performing experience in imagery. Some singers had to physically
practice the staging prior to imagining it. Louis discovered that “because I have a lot of
practice on the stage things now, that I can imagine, imagery was good.” Harriet also
I have to see things I physically did for things to physically work in my body
before I can imagine myself walking through staging. [Then] I can see myself on
stage going through staging outside of me actually doing it. I use that to memorize
201
things. That and the sequence of the scene. . . the order of staging for a scene or
These singers connected these sequences with the physical aspect of performing.
Staging sequencing also was connected with the subtext and motivation of the
character of song. For example, Francine found that “the moment to moment reality is
very much again about ‘Why am I doing this?’ and the subtext, what’s not being said . . .
the motivation . . . every single moment has to be justified.” For Ivan, staging and
I rarely have to write my staging in the book because what I do is that when I go
again. The directions, where I was going, why I was going, everything, the music
The sequences in movement and music in staging were deeply connected for these
singers and further assisted them in memory of both staging and singing the song or aria.
In performing a role in opera, strategies were used to help the singer embody the
character. Six singers mentioned motivation and clarifying the underlying reasons for
acting out the role. Monique said, “I try to find out who my character is. And therefore
several ways that singers suggested in order to accomplish this. One was through
understanding the strategies of how to feel emotions. Nicholas explained, “once I have
music learned, or a role learned, then I will plot out an emotional path through the aria or
the role, and think of different emotional directions I would like to take it.” Barry
elaborated, “the basic premises for acting is ‘What do I want to achieve?’ or ‘How do I
202
want to make you feel?’ as the other person. . . . Those are strategies that you experiment
with during the rehearsal process.” Eloise eloquently presented reasons subtext was vital
It really does have to be internalized. Like walking for a role. I have to be able to
feel the motivation. Why am I doing this walking? Why is this physical thing
necessary? What is it expressing? And I have to feel it, and then my body is going
to respond. . . . So that it doesn’t interrupt the flow of the story and the emotion. It
For Eloise and many others, using imagery in understanding and working out the
strategies for the character was essential in feeling or embodying the essence of the
character.
the singers’ responses. They included execution of skill, sequencing and strategy and
provided.
Motivational Reasons
Motivation reasons for using imagery were three-fold: (a) motivational specific
regulating arousal, anxiety, and energy; and (c) motivational general-mastery (MG-M)
for self-confidence and mastery (Paivio, 1985; Hall et al., 1998). Categories and
203
Table 20
Subcategories
Participants
Short-term 11
Other Issues 5
Body Concerns 6
Preparation 10
Self-talk 10
Psyching-Up 13
Self-talk 9
Mastery Mastery 5
Fixing Problems 7
204
Goal attainment (motivational specific). The motivational specific (MS) imagery
reasons included identifying goals in order to inspire the singer to continue to strive to
achieve ideals or specific roles. This purpose for using imagery in participants’ responses
resulted in three categories: (a) long-term, (b) short-term, and (c) other issues regarding
performance, and process has previously been provided. Further concerns involved
distinguishing goal imagery from daydreaming and relative value and success of goal
attainment.
Differences arose between long-term and short-term goals for many participants.
Thirteen singers had long-term goals, mostly replicating those already described in
outcome goals, which inspired them to improve. Louis entertained a general long-term
I do tend to try to goal-set for myself, and tell myself where I would like to be. . . .
If I have a five or a ten year accomplishment that I would like, I do make a note of
that goal and I do recognize that that’s something that I want to achieve. . . .
Eleven singers imagined attaining their desired roles in succession and relied more on a
break down the long-term goals and asked himself, “How did I arrive at this goal?
That’s what I did, okay. How did I arrive at that place? . . . just kind of come like that in
In contrast, several participants had concerns about the usefulness and benefit in
imagining short-term goals to help inspire them. Harriet stated, “the minutia is not
205
role. Actually, I sort of skip over all of that in my imagery.” Similar to this sentiment,
Catherine said, “all the steps between my one-year goal and that are ridiculously
unimaginable for me.” Eloise also became frustrated with short-term goals, and said, “I
imagery.
Other issues and concerns with imagery emerged in goals changing over the
years, and how goals were affected by parenting children. Eloise and other participants
experienced marked changes in their work in goal attainment imagery as they progressed
in their careers and became more accomplished. In describing her experience of finally
I came back and sang for them, didn’t care anymore, got straight in . . . because I
didn’t care so much anymore. . . . But it was a wonderful feeling, getting into the
[company] that I’d so desperately wanted to be in. But once I went to the first
Francine agreed, “goals change as you start to become aware.” The decision to raise
children was a major consideration in how two singers allowed themselves to imagine
achieving their goals. Ivan felt, “It’s difficult as a father and a husband, because you have
to consider your kids, you affect your kids as well.” Josephine, who also had children,
wanted to progress in her career, but had “made the choice not to be away from them for
six weeks or eight.” As a result she was satisfied with regional engagements. Both singers
had to make adjustments in their careers goals and consequently in their imagery.
206
Concerning goal imagery, some of the participants also commented on the
distinction between goal imagery and daydreaming, the effectiveness of imagery, and the
imagining a specific goal, Ivan said, “It’s not so much a wishful sense of ‘Oh, I wish this
might happen.’ But, I allow myself to completely feel it as if it’s real.” In analyzing her
use of imagery to secure a particular role which she finally won after years of effort,
Monique came to realize that “imagining it is like this expression: ‘See it, be it.’ You see
yourself in it. And I think I was longing to be in it, but not seeing myself in it.” Monica
distinguished that goal attainment involved specificity in imagery; longing for something
begets continuing the feeling of longing, while seeing and embodying the goal help
manifest that image. These singers varied as to their efforts and applications in goal
imagery, but generally felt that the more clearly the goals were imagined, the better their
Several singers elaborated on the successes they had experienced with clearly and
several of her roles using this method, wherein she said, “they offered me a contract in
the audition, because I had already gone through the whole process in my brain of
having already performed it.” Francine’s imagery was as though she was actually acting
in that role, convincing herself it was a present reality. Likewise Harriet gave an account
of achieving two different roles. She stated, “maybe a year ago I said . . . I wanted to be
singing Traviata and Lucia. And I imagined myself in that role, and I can imagine
myself on stage, and I had it.” As a result of his years of success with goal imagery,
Barry suggested:
207
[T]he more specific you can be, the more, I use the word, visualization, the more
you can visualize what the actual end product is, and articulate it, write it down,
say it, get really it out of your body on to something concrete, a piece of paper,
For these vocalists, goal imagery was a vital part of their motivation and their
imagery, however it was not clear how they initially began and developed this.
This desire to engage in more goal imagery emerged in three of the singers. In
considering using imagery to achieve his goal to sing at the MET, Nicholas said, “I’ve
used that a little bit to imagine myself singing there. . . . I haven’t really gotten into any
detail into it yet, although it could possibly be helpful. It’s more of generalized idea at
this point.” When asked if she imagined achieving her goals, Catherine replied, “I don’t
think I’ve ever actually done it, but it sounds like a good idea, to imagine myself in the
position I want to be in.” Some singers who shared less experience with imagery in
achieving their goals did not feel as strongly about using goal imagery as a tool of
motivation.
All these singers used goal imagery to motivate themselves in their profession but
valued goal work differently. Those who spent a great deal of time engaged in imagining
their desired results did so because they were convinced of its effectiveness. Others
imagined with much less intent and hope for results. In any case, goal imagery was used
(MG-A) imagery comprised the second reason or purpose for which vocal professionals
208
used imagery. Singers’ responses describing this imagery function resulted in the sole
category of efforts to calm anxiety or tension and energy for psyching up, for which the
Imagery methods to regulate arousal and energy to maintain appropriate levels for
singing revealed seven subcategories, which were (a) calming efforts, (b) managing the
breath, (c) coping with body concerns, (d) focus and concentration, (e) preparation, (f)
self-talk, and (g) psyching-up. The ability for a singer to calm anxiety and arousal levels
and control nerves was regarded as essential in performing excellence. All participants
described using imagery for calming, which they practiced most particularly before and
anxiety. High arousal levels often severely diminished a singers’ ability to breath. In
describing how her breath was affected during times of intense performance anxiety,
Monique said, “it’s very hurtful, and it affects my breathing badly.” One of the primary
methods fourteen of these singers suggested to achieve calmness was gaining proper
breath control. Nine singers used their breath to specifically calm down (e.g., Harriet: “a
lot of stress and anxiety and things like that can be harnessed when you just get a hold of
your breath”). Several mentioned the physiological benefits of slow, relaxed breathing.
Nicholas said, “I’m breathing slowly and I’ve found that that helps control or helps me
control my adrenalin and my heart rate a little bit.” Three singers were reluctant to
attribute the calming effects of breathing to imagery. For example, Gerald stated that
“using and moving the air through my body to calm down. I’m not sure if that’s as much
209
the physical body to breath in a way that calms nerves, especially when that was the
practice to make this skill automatic and fully accessible to the professional vocalist.
Adding to the benefits of good breathing, Ophelia said, “If I can’t breathe fully then I
don’t feel like the performance would be as satisfactory for me, or the audience.”
Singers also shared their use of imagery in dealing with physiological concerns
and the effects of nervousness. Over the years, six participants discovered ways to use
their nerves for improving their performances. Eloise reframed her experience of anxiety
and said, “It’s not so much nerves as adrenalin.” Changing the way a singer regarded
nerves seemed to facilitate calmer feelings during performances. Ivan said, “[I] stopped
resisting against the nerves and started embracing them. . . . and just understand that this
is something my body did for whatever reason and I didn’t view it as a negative.” Two
singers identified specific bodily reactions to stress they eventually accepted and found
ways to cope with it. Eloise would locate the area of tension in her body and feel it relax.
She said, “imagine a fist, and that’s the tension, and – tension is not bad. It’s just in the
fist, so that the hand is open and the energy can flow.” Physical manifestations of
nervousness and stage fright were often alleviated or resolved by mentally imagining
their solutions.
overwhelming especially considering the enormous stress singers had to endure. Karen
[I]magery saved my life in stage fright. . . . this particular season, and I think it
was coupled with getting my graduate degree, and I actually had a baby that I was
210
nursing at the same time, and I think I did have a little bit of pressure on me. I
think the combination of all that probably entered into it. . . . I decided that I
would use the imagery to back down the anxiety. . . . it was just a matter of
visualizing the worst and how I could pull it off. And it was just that exit door that
Often performers were able to regulate arousal when they could identify specific mental
thoughts and feelings that cause fear and those that calmed them.
singers to feel the ideal levels of arousal during performance. Specifically, eight singers
mentioned focusing on the task at hand. Some developed skills to deal with their thoughts
wondering during singing by practicing concentration exercises. Staying positive and not
entertaining any negative thoughts was a solution for four singers. Josephine suggested:
[I]f you feel nervous about a performance. . . . focus only on the image of the
music, to focus only on the picture of the music of what you’re singing, not to
allow other thoughts to come into your head. . . . and shut down all the thoughts
Anna and Eloise felt that thoughts of connecting with the audience helped her to more
completely focus on her performance. Eloise described her experience in the following
excerpt:
I picture the audience and try to imagine what they’re hoping for. And when I
remember that they came for something good and to feel good, and that they’re
not there to pick me apart, then I calm down, because then I feel like they’re on
my team.
211
Concentrating and focusing were best practiced in rehearsal so controlling mental
however, they also used imagery to visualize the perfect performance, mentally plan its
execution, and imagine technical excellence. Ten singers cited the importance of
anxiety. Louis’ response clearly exemplified this when he said, “I think I am more calm
visualizing the perfect performance to calm their nerves. Ivan illustrated the difference in
If I am not able to visualize clearly what I want, then what happens is that
inevitably my nerves will increase a little bit. I’ll feel a little unsettled. New roles
are this way for me. . . . If, for pieces that I repeat and I know, I just know to the
point that I can just hear it, I can feel it, feel myself singing it, see every
production detail and staging, then the level of stress decreases greatly.
Six singers felt more secure when they mentally planned the details of a performance. For
example, Karen declared, “I don’t like surprises, so I pretty much have in my mind
everything that I’m going to do.” Imagining as many details as possible in the entire
performance calmed performers minds and allowed them to concentrate on executing the
song.
adjusting their anxiety, or motivational drive. Four singers used self-talk in alleviating
212
a little Satan inside me. As I’m breathing for the next line, saying, ‘You’re going
to forget this line.’ . . . And as the curtain opened, this devil in me said, ‘Oh,
what’s your first line?’ And at least I had time to say, ‘Go away, just relax. This
Louis recognized his anxiety and would mentally respond with self-talk when he said, “I
picture myself on stage, I picture myself nervous, I picture myself ready to give the
performance. But then I follow through and say that even though I am nervous or
apprehensive, I will perform well anyway.” Other singers repeated phrases to strengthen
their motivation, such as Eloise who continually said to herself, “adrenalin is fantastic
energy, it’s flowing through you, you’re a lion, and you’re about to go on stage and
possess the stage and own it.” Similarly, Barry said, “if I’m nervous because I’m not at
100% and I’ll close my eyes and say a mantra, if you will, that allows me to relieve some
of the stress.” Self-talk was another form of imagery these singers used to successfully
With regard to imagery use in calming arousal, the other elements emerging from
the interviews involved grounding, meditation, thoughts of family, flow, and balancing
energy. Anna said, “I’ll try to imagine my feet growing into the ground” to relax and
stabilize her nerves. Two performers practiced meditation and one entertained warm
thoughts of family for feeling peaceful and ready to go on stage. Another two singers
imagined being in a flow with the audience (e.g., Anna, “that flow that comes through me
and back out to them, and that infinity thing”). These performers strove to achieve the
213
[I]f I’m too hyper, again, if I just think about that flow, for some reason that’s a
stopped up, if I just kind of let it, keep it flowing steadily, then it either calms
Singers seemed to find their individual balance for optimal performance, which usually
required calming.
participants reported. Eloise illustrated the need for feeling more motivation and her
[W]hen I have to perform and I feel kind of low energy or maybe even down
emotionally . . . you get it flowing and to free up any negative feelings so that
they’ll go away, because you just can’t perform with negativity. . . . [It] is like
waterfalls or rivers flowing, and it’s just this cleaning, this clean flow.
To psyche up, Anna thought about the efforts the audience expended to attend the theatre.
Two singers were motivated by the privilege they felt by being able to sing. Francine
would tell herself, “I get to do this, this is a treat, this is a privilege. Many people work
their entire lives to be able to do this.” Eloise would say, “This is what I love to do. I get
to do what I love to do, which makes me happy.” Gerald created the metaphor of “the
energy of a volcano eruption” to feel more energy for performing. Conversely, three
participants did not think they used imagery to psyche up, using primarily physical
214
I don’t think that that’s an imagery or visualization issue. I think that’s just a
physical issue. You just have to get to the practice room when you need to,
For these singers, psyching up was gained by dedication or rigorous physical activity.
Using imagery to control nerves, relax, and calm down were more critical than
employing those to increase energy to perform on stage. This may have been a distinct
difference from how arousal and energy were regarded in the more physical activities
involved in sport and dance. It is also possible that too much energy especially aggressive
energy could have adversely affected the intricacies of vocal performance for these
singers.
and mastery imagery category included managing difficulties, staying positive, and
mastering situations. All participants reported using images of confidence and mastery,
resulting in five subcategories, (a) self-efficacy feelings, (b) successful image, (c) self-
Imagery to gain and maintain confidence was important to all participants and
was separated between images of self-efficacy (eight singers) and imagining successful
being strong and self-assured. Several mentioned such images as getting “a diva attitude,
like I just know I look good on stage” (Eloise). Josephine preferred to use the modeling
method, and suggested, “imagine yourself as [being someone] . . . you really look up to.
Who do you see as very successful and a very good performer?” This helped her embody
the appearance and feelings of the self-efficacy and confidence she wanted to portray on
215
stage. Other singers combined confident images with successfully imagining a
imagine what I could do to make the performance better, I guess, or to make it really,
really good.” Other singers such as Anna believed “If I could imagine through the whole
thing without singing, I felt more secure.” Realizing the importance of directing her own
an unsuccessful outcome. And that’s a hard thing to do because I’ve become more
and more aware how we do subconsciously project to people our own insecurities.
And if we’re not keeping that stuff in check, you really can convey all of your
Many singers recognized the value in making sure their imagery and thoughts of future
self-talk. Nine singers’ responses revealed the use of self-talk in performing songs and
general feelings of confidence. Three singers reported using self-talk to execute songs
successfully. Gerald would mentally say, “This is going to be fine, this is going. I’m
going to be successful. I’m going to sing everything correctly. I’m going to remember my
words, and it’s going to be great.” In singing challenging passages, Monique said:
[W]hen there’s something very difficult coming up, a very high note or difficult
passagio note, or something like that, I would say, ‘I can do all things through
Christ.’ Or I would cut out the Christ part and say ‘I can do all things,’ so that I
216
saw that immediately just before I was going to sing it and it just sort of girded
my loins.
Self-talk, religiously oriented or otherwise, was used to secure notes that were potentially
troublesome for this singer, which also strengthen feelings of self-efficacy in mastering
its execution. Self-talk was also used by six singers to instill general feelings of
confidence. Examples of these included such repetitive affirmations such as, “you are a
wonderful singer. You are a beautiful” (Anna). Just before stepping on stage to perform,
Monique declared to herself, “I AM the Queen!” This helped strengthen her self-
Five singers described using motivational imagery for mastery in more general terms.
Nicholas stated, “I use imagery to . . . master the elements that are within my control, the
ones that I have responsibility for.” Karen expanded this idea by saying:
Being focused, thinking through what I’m going to do and how I’m going to do it,
and all from knowing that I can do it. ‘All right, I have done it. I’m capable of
doing it,’ and the only reason I won’t do it is that I do myself in. So, in order not
Feeling completely competent and clearly practicing the performance helped her to
Seven singers used mastery images specifically to prepare for difficulties or to fix
problems that arose while performing. Mastery imagery for dealing with difficulties
included vocal execution, managing change, and prior preparation. In classical singing, it
217
was common for singers to switch from one voice type to another during the course of
their career due to physiological changes. Executing higher or lower notes than what the
I had switched from baritone to tenor. . . anything that any note above the staff
and approaching it with confidence and not being so either apologetic, or fearful,
specifically just the confidence of the sound. . . [in] my approach to those pitches.
Ivan used imagery to remedy this particular problem and was able to gain mastery.
Singers often had to revise methods of executing the song and craft more
appropriate ways, as Anna related, “it’s easier to go, ‘Okay. I know that I have that
issue,’ and to work through it. It’s almost like a re-knitting of the fabric in your head.”
Part of mastering the voice was recognizing when a technique was no longer appropriate.
Anna realized this and used imagery to re-pattern her skills for the next performance.
Similarly, Louis felt that preparation was vital in mastery and said:
Just the pure advanced repetition of going through the event mentally. I find that
in the moment of performance, if one has not anticipated that moment properly,
it’s very challenging to calm the situation, or to get a mental grasp over what
you’re going through. That work has to be done before you arrive there. At the
Imagery used for mastery assisted these singers in building strong beliefs and self-
efficacy that they were capable and prepared for the task of singing and overcoming any
218
Artistic Reasons
The category of artistic reasons was borrowed from the four Ws framework of
Nordin and Cumming (2005) in dance imagery research and was found to be appropriate
in reporting vocalists’ interview responses. Artistic reasons for using imagery in optimal
performance in this study emerged slightly differently than those of dance. For these
singers, preparing to perform a song or role was considered primarily artistic when their
imagery extended beyond the technical or cognitive and motivational functions. These
resulted in four imagery categories: (a) vocal quality, (b) appearance, (c) character
development, (d) flow, and (e) communication with the audience. Artistic reasons for
singers to use imagery, the categories, details, and frequency of responses are presented
in Table 21.
Vocal quality. Achieving optimum vocal quality was one of the primary artistic
reasons vocalists used imagery. All singers mentioned imagery of some ideal quality of
how they wanted their sound to be heard. Imagery in singers’ vocal quality resulted in
four sections: (a) color, (b) modeling, (c) ideal sound, and (d) sound characteristics.
Eleven singers in this study reported experiencing vocal quality combining the two senses
of seeing visual colors and hearing tones and phrases. These had been divided into two
contexts, visual color and color as timbre or emotion of different vocal sounds. Two
responders spoke primarily of the visual aspect of color as it relates to tones. In striving
to produce the most accurate pitch, Anna reported that for her “different notes have
different colors and different phrases have different colors, reds or greens or blues.”
219
Table 21
Modeling 11
Ideal Sound 9
Sound Characteristics 6
Posture 10
Fitness 10
Appearance 12
Past Experiences 8
Metaphors 2
Staging Emotion 15
Kinesthetic 8
Sound 6
Zone 4
Flowing Energy 3
Spiritual Interaction 4
220
Eloise described her experience of vocal quality in color imagery when she said,
“you’ve got your chest voice or your low voice is red, and your high voice is yellow, and
they need to mix, so you have all these different beautiful oranges constantly going on.”
The image of yellow or gold in the higher tones was also found in responses from Ivan,
whose quote was included in the previous section on sense imagery. These singers used
visual color imagery to execute the precise vocal quality and tone in the musical context.
Six participants related color in terms of timbre and the emotional aspects of expressing a
I do use imagery in terms of color, relating color or images to tone. Some songs
require different timbre than other songs, or a different quality to the voice than
other songs, for example with songs that are more melancholy or more sultry or
more passionate or more sad, I’ll envision things like velvet or chocolate or
something rich.
Descriptive terms combining sound and color have often been used in vocal pedagogy to
Modeling the sound of other more experienced singers was a tool 11 singers said
they used to improve their own sound quality. Several vocalists recalled the sounds of
actually try and imagine what their tongue is doing or what their body is doing, or
what the sound that they created and try and recreate that in my own practices.
Anna would imagine replicating the kinesthetic production of an ideal singer’s sound, as
221
I can listen to Joan Sutherland and I feel where the vibration is. I’m not even
honestly sure if the vibration really is occurring or we’re just doing imagery for it,
but you have the points on your head where you feel that buzz, and so when I
Catherine struggled with modeling because she used the mental image of a singer’s voice
that was inappropriate in that it did not match her own instrument. She said, “I have other
people’s voices in my head. I have Jessye Norman’s sound in my head [but] nope, you
have a Barbara Bonney sound, and make sure that you don’t try to sing like Tosca.”
Ophelia recalled, “I’ve run into a lot of problems by trying to copy others.” These singers
may have tried to replicate the vocal quality of expert singers with extensive experience
and characteristics differing from the natural abilities of their own voice. Four singers
mentioned that modeling was something they did more when they were younger, but as
they became more secure and found their own voice, the need for modeling diminished.
Nine of these singers retained an ideal image in their mind of a sound quality they
aimed to recreate in their own voices. Three participants spoke of invoking a wide variety
of human vocal capabilities in their singing, as reflected by Josephine who said, “the
human voice mimics so many different sounds and has such capacity to do so.” Eloise
described an array of sounds that mimicked all the families of the orchestra that she
imagined emanating from various parts of her body. Several singers felt their sound
quality was directly linked to its mental origins. Ophelia endeavored to create “an image
of what you want to come out, how you want it to sound.” Barry’s vocal efforts were
he aptly illustrated:
222
[You] don’t do a sound, you do the image. . . . You let your mind go to
somewhere, you don’t actually affect the sound directly. There’s something else
It is possible that Barry had many of the techniques solidly incorporated in his singing
and now concentrated on executing specific vocal qualities. Singers used their imagery to
affect and change the quality of their vocal sounds in a variety of ways.
reported in nine responses. Six singers offered illustrations of imagery to achieve greater
depth and richness in their sound. However, Monique felt that a singer’s individual vocal
quality was meant to be consistent. Gerald concurred, “I usually don’t think of the sound
changing from aria to aria. The sound is always the same.” For many singers, vocal
development involved finding your individual sound, which some considered as unique
as your thumbprint.
Notwithstanding, three singers had to cope with changes in their vocal quality as
a result of getting older. Josephine said, “I started out with a very like light, sweet sound.
And it’s grown as I’ve grown older. And it’s become warmer and richer. And I’ve had to
try to figure out how to manage change in my sound.” Not only did the sound quality
change for Nicholas as he matured, but he had to change his vocal quality for certain
songs. He recalled, “I’ve got to rework it just a little bit, so I’m looking for a little more
focus and ring in the upper part of my voice.” Monique shifted her voice quality as
needed for the production. She felt, “it depends on what I’m singing. . . .I have a flexible
voice and I can sing straight or I can sing curly, as I call it, depending on what the
223
conductor wants, I will do it.” There seems to be certain qualities of the voice that are
maintained, others that can be consciously altered, and some that change with age.
Appearance. All the participants used imagery to change and improve their
appearance, which is hardly surprising given that vocal professionals usually give live
performances on a stage where they are fully visible to the audience. Results from this
category revealed three subcategories of (a) look on stage, (b) posture, and (c) fitness
concerns.
Look on stage comprised how the vocalist imagined the audience viewed him or
her in performance and was cited in all of the responders. This involved the need for a
feedback mechanism, level of imagery detail, and preferred perspective. In order for
singers to know how they appeared on stage, some sort of feedback system was required,
such as a teacher, coach, director, mirror, or video. Anna illustrated this by saying:
[S]ometimes I’ll look in the mirror to see what I’m doing right then, and then to
create what I want it to look like in my head instead. . . . But, if there’s a focus too
much on the external, you will look contrived. . . . or you need to get the feedback
When other people are not available to give feedback, singers had to rely on experience, a
mirror, or video. Ophelia, one of the younger participants, said, “the only way you get
good at this is practice because how you mean to portray yourself may not be how you’re
actually perceived.” In order for some performers to better imagine how the audience
224
viewed them, several participants specifically mentioned using external visual imagery
perspective to gain information on their appearance. Harriet stated, “I can see myself, I
guess from the audience perspective. I’ll imagine certain gestures and things like that.
And I’ll decide what reads better to the audience versus others.” Aligning the ideal
appearance in imagery with the actual look on stage was the goal for singers who wanted
Singers imagined their posture in how they appeared to hold their body, the
position of their head, shoulders, chest, and arms, as 10 of these singers mentioned. These
elements from special training to achieve their optimal posture. In learning to hold her
body erect but relaxed in singing, Dorine explained, “my teacher used to say, ‘Pretend
you’re on a pole, a posto, the Italians say, that your body is on a pole, it should act
straight.’” Using a different metaphor, Dorine imagined “a string coming out of the top of
Karen imagined “always looking refined. . . . and it would include head up and confident,
and more in the kind of regal category,” when she performed. Likewise, Monique
envisioned, “’I AM the Queen!’ Now you cannot say that sentence without holding your
head up high, throwing your chest out.” This worked particularly well when she sang
stand proudly. She told him, “You have to stand like a bullfighter in the ring,” which was
an enduring image for him even to the present. Even using the image of a stereotypical
person helped these singers stand in the way they felt best for proper vocal production
225
and appearance on stage. Posture was learned in the early stages of life and if developed
incorrectly, singers benefited from retraining to stand properly. One singer found great
assistance in this type of training. Ophelia shared, “I think the most helpful imagery that a
coach gave me recently had to do with alignment of the body. . . in that you need
optimum posture.” Part of achieving excellent posture for these singers was in staying
physically fit.
presentation. Monique recognized this and said, “Singing is an extremely athletic pursuit,
because you’re using your stomach, and your back, and your breathing mechanism, and
you can’t have tension in your neck or your jaw.” Several of these responses reflected
their opinion that this was more a physical matter of just exercising than an imagery
issue. Catherine illustrated, “I’d like to lose some girth, but I go exercise. . . . I don’t sit
around and say, ‘I wish I looked better.’ I either make myself look better, or don’t worry
about it.” Others recognized that imagery could be used to shape how they appeared on
If I’m not feeling 100% in shape, or feel like I need to loose five or ten vanity
pounds here or there, I will imagine myself in the physical condition that I want to
be in order to get back in shape next role. . . . that’s my of sort of staying in shape
or preparing that way physically for a role. I’ll use that . . . to manifest how I work
out and what I need to do in order to get to that. I use it as my building block to
226
Harriet used imagery to help her identify what kind of body she wanted personally and
for performances. This imagery also inspired her to achieve it through exercise and
healthy habits.
Singers used appearance imagery to project and maintain an ideal look for stage
presentations. Vocalist’s appearance, attitudes and gestures, posture, fitness, and body
image all comprise the elements of how an audience perceives him or her while
performing. This next section encompasses singers’ use of imagery in developing the
Character development was often a process that extended from the early stages of
learning the role past the last performance, especially when singers repeated their roles
throughout their career. For this reason, character development was divided between
the components for embodying the role or qualities of a song to be performed. Character
Singers had to prepare the character or artistic quality of their repertoire whether it was
an entire role in an opera or a single song presentation. Responses for preparing the
character revealed five sub-categories: (a) modeling, (b), appearance, (c) history and
For 14 singers, the use of modeling, or observing exemplary performers who had
227
Modeling use for these participants ranged from catching glimpses “as inspiration”
(Harriet) to pretending to be that model performer entirely (e.g., Anna: “when you’re up
there, you actually picture that you’re that person. . . . you actually embody that person”).
Ophelia also reported, “I’m guilty of watching a movie and liking a character and
identifying with that character and then having it rubbing off on me. Then I’m acting like
that actor or something in certain situations.” She and others cautioned that modeling use
should be limited. Eloise was inspired by another professional’s presentation and “kind of
tried to feel it and internalize it in myself, and then make it my own.” Francine also
illustrated this by saying, “I think of the things that they did successfully and I try and use
them. . . [This] helped me learn and understand stylistically, how to approach things, and
understand what the result is that people are looking for.” Modeling also served singers in
eliciting images and intentions often used by performers of greater experience, as Louis
explained, “I don’t try to mimic that but I certainly respect the artistry and the
performance involvement that they create. I then try to create something that is just as
that significant in my own singing.” Since singing was considered primarily a profession
singers’ responses.
historical and cultural background of the opera or song. This represented a major part of
creating a foundation for understanding the character for 14 singers. Anna’s account
defined some of the details and reasons she used in imagery of the character, “You do a
lot of history work on where . . . the clothing they wore in the 1800s or the 1600s, and
228
how that affects you as a person. And it all helps create this whole living character.”
Being thorough in her research was key to successful rendering the character according to
Dorine, who added, “I have to do the research before I can create the images to go with
it.” This practice also helped Eloise “to imagine myself there, and in all kinds of different
roles. . . . what motivated them, and to feel the culture.” Details of the history and culture
Creating the mental image of the character’s appearance was a natural element
and benefit of researching the history and culture. For 12 singers, differences regarding
stage presentation also arose in relation to the venue in which the singer was performing
(e. g., operatic, concert, or audition). These vocalists imagined their characters’ physical
appearances with varying degrees of detail and in various settings. After doing
[I]magine what each character looks like as me, what color hair I have, how long
my hair is, what type of makeup I wear, how much makeup I wear, what kind of
clothes I have, how I walk, how I stand. . . .I imagine myself in the costume.
When Karen performed on the concert stage she had already secured how she wanted to
I have to imagine first what that character would look like. . . . I sort of decide
who I want to be at that time, how I want to look to the audience. Do I want to be
somebody very regal, or what, and a lot of it is the music that I’m doing.
Karen took time to carefully craft the image and appearance appropriate to each
character.
229
Since mezzos often perform as both male and female, Francine had to use
imagery to quickly change between the genders during auditions, as she described:
If I’m wearing a dress and I’m singing a boy, I have to make a mental image of
that fact that, okay, now I’m a boy. And how does that affect the way that I stand,
in the way that my arms express themselves. . . . imagining that I’m wearing a
In embodying the character of the song, singers used imagery to recreate the look and
external feeling to enhance the presentation. Appearance imagery also depended on the
Eight singers reported using their own past experiences to elicit emotions and
feelings in preparing their roles and songs. Gerald used his past experiences to connect
with what “the character has experienced or something that would elicit the response that
I am trying to get, and reliving that, in other words, I’m trying to capture that feeling.”
Dorine concurred, adding that using her own experiences often provided “a much more
believable performance for the audience.” When past experiences were not sufficient,
singers used those of other people they knew or had seen somewhere. The most important
element of employing images of these experiences was the connection with its
These images were specific to some of the roles these two singers had performed in the
wounded animal, a wounded bird, or . . . someone who was wounded, and physically
230
crippled and disabled” to help her more completely portray this character. Harriet
described, “Gilda in Rigoletto . . . as a trapped bird that wants to fly. . . . But then she’s in
a cage and her house is the cage that her father keeps her in.” Both of these examples
represent simple acting metaphors to help the singer feel the conflicts in the characters.
incorporated, the performer was ready for staging rehearsals. Character development in
staging involves aspects of using imagery to bring the character to life while performing
before an audience. The five sub-categories emerging from the interviews were: (a) being
in character, (b) emotion, (c) kinesthetic, (d) sound, and (e) being in the zone.
All singers mentioned that they imagined being in character and performing in
ways that encompassed all dramatic aspects of human behavior. In trying to make the
[S]etting myself within the text of the plot of the opera and letting the motivation
of the character, and getting ideas that the character would be having. . . .
picturing myself in the scene, not necessarily in the operatic stage, but trying to
imagine if it were real life, how the character would be seeing, and feeling.
Some singers went to great extents to create all the details of the character in their
You have to go through and do your whole history of that character. So you’ve
imagined . . . everything that led up to who they are today. You already have that
implanted in your head, and you walk out with all the baggage loaded up on your
shoulders, so that you have, it’s almost like arsenal, ready to use at any minute,
231
based on the kinds of experiences that you’re gonna walk through with this
I think very deeply about each piece I sing. . . . it’s my acting, I try to put myself
in the scene of the picture that that song is painting. . . . so [I become] that person,
involved in those emotions, the lost love, the whatever. I am that person.
For these singers, imagining being in character included choosing methods that best
All these vocalists mentioned the importance of recreating the emotion to bring
the character of a song or role to life, particularly in performance. “You always have to
go back to the foundation, which is the emotional, human experience” (Anna). This was
also reflected in a quote by Barry, who said, “your goal as an artist is to live the moment
performance because “You can’t convey that to an audience unless I think you’ve felt
that emotion.” The inner motivation of a character was depicted in how the performer
recreated the emotions. According to Nicholas, emotions changed throughout the aria or
the entire opera and this helped him more effectively deliver the proper intent to the
audience. He stated, “I think of the emotional changes as I’m maneuvering around the
emotional pathway of the character. The bigger and stronger I can hit each emotional
change the more effective I think it’s going to be.” Emotional imagery in recreating a role
Furthermore, all the participants recognized the natural connection between emotion and
singing, not just in character development in roles for opera. As Josephine put it, “It’s
232
hard to separate emotion and singing because . . . singing is emotion . . . . set to words
and music. . . . it’s just organized emotion.” These singers felt emotional imagery was
Although emotion certainly was considered vital in creating the proper mood,
nine singers had specific words of caution in applying excessive feeling in performance.
Many singers recalled specific experiences that taught them to be careful not to pour too
much passion into their performances. In a recent audition that he described, Barry said
he “allowed it to get too emotional, i.e., [sic] cross the line. You can cross the line where,
especially vocal technique-wise, you get verklempt. If you get verklempt as a singer,
you’re dead in the water.” Francine had to adjust her emotions so she could technically
execute certain songs. She stated, “if your emotions get in the way, you won’t be
successful at it. So I had to learn how to adapt it and make it still functional but not allow
I’ll often compare myself with a figure skater. A figure skater has to constantly be
order to accomplish that triple axel, because if they don’t do that, they will fall
Monique recognized the dangers of being overcome with emotion in singing and said:
[Y]ou mustn’t be the emotion, because it will tear your voice apart, but you must
agony, if you did that you would loose your voice. . . . Where you cry is where
233
The singers with more experience were able to find the proper balance between
Along with emotion, eight singers connected this to a bodily feeling in order to
participants felt the need to feel qualities of the character in their body. In discovering the
various aspects of her character, Karen incorporated the kinesthetic feeling when she
said:
I guess it’s visual first, then how that visual fits into the auditory part of it, and
then just movements, of course, any kind of movements. Would they be slow,
would they be fast. If you’re heavy you’re going to be probably slower. I just
Others mentioned the importance of physically staging the character first in order to more
[In] A Barber of Seville . . . there’s a complicated shaving scene that happens very
quick and . . . I practiced imagery with it, but if I hadn’t done [that] probably the
physical timing of it then actually the physical movements were more important
Barry also used movements and behaviors of a specific man he had seen on the street. He
said, “I can use that to create my character, and use the body language, the facial
expressions, or the way somebody says something” in creating the details of acting out
the role. Using imagery to understanding the character kinesthetically assisted these
234
Singers had the opportunity to choose from a variety of sound qualities to best
represent the individual aria and character. In creating the appropriate sound for a rough
character she portrayed, Karen said, “Her voice would have to project her body. The
body was easy enough to pad, but her voice would have to project that body and the
crudeness, and so forth. I had to just really feel the heaviness.” This practice did not
always display the best qualities of her singing voice but was necessary to better depict
[In] these wonderful roles in opera, you’re being judged on just how wonderful
you are in singing. But really, in many cases, you should be in kind of a character
on that. It wouldn’t necessarily be the most refined singing that you would do if
Anna concurred, by saying, “you want a character to come through.” Sound imagery was
performance only appeared in a four singers’ responses even though it was not included
as a separate question in the interview guide. Flow is worthy of including here since it is
part of what would be considered an optimal performing experience. For these singers,
being in the zone did not emerge as a reason or separate purpose as it had for athletes
(Munroe et al., 2000) but as an experience that culminated after all separate components
of singing the character were incorporated. These were specific times when a singer
performed a song, all the effort faded away and the present experience became all
encompassing, involving different kinds of imagery. Louis experienced this early in his
career. He recounted:
235
I was singing a certain passage that I’d been struggling with maybe in lessons or
weeks prior to and it was really working. And I sang several minutes in sort of a
groove where I was just singing pretty effortlessly and it was working. She [his
teacher] said that she could equate that maybe to a basketball player in the middle
of the game that just got into the zone. And the basket looks wide. She said
sometimes in your singing, the basket will look wide and you know that it’s
working. You can just sort of accomplish things that you might not at other times
be able to do.
This example showed that experiencing flow was possible in a small span of time after
this and said, “when I’ve really nailed something, in my mind it will make me laugh, it
might make me cry a little bit, but I may feel in my whole body.” While acting out her
character, Ophelia’s experience of being in the zone was also quite poignant, when she
performer, there’s just the music. And in order to get there I really think you have to
move beyond this whole thing [religious conviction].” Being in the zone to Ophelia was
an experience that transcended any religious beliefs and took place when she was clearly
singing as that character. Harriet also felt this flow feeling when she fully immersed
herself in the character. She recalled, “when I start a performance, my self checks out,
just, I get into the zone, and I’m just that character. . . . cause it’s no longer about me, and
I’m in my character.” It could have been that all the imagery used to secure techniques,
skill, emotion, and the kinesthetic aspects of portraying the character help made these
236
elements so automatic that the singer could just enjoy being that character. This may be
part of what constituted the experience of singers being in the zone or in flow.
Communication with the audience. All but one of the 15 participants shared
thoughts of interacting with the audience in their imagery. This emerged as a separate
category under artistic reasons. Vocalists used imagery to communicate specific ideas to
the audience in four specific areas, which consisted of: (a) communicating ideas and
emotions, (b) sharing intimate feelings, (c) flowing energy, and (d) spiritual interaction.
Some images were used in helping the audience to understand the meaning of
what was being performed. The effort to share specific elements of the composition with
the audience was important to Harriet, who said, “I will take specific moments and
especially with text or something that needs to be communicated. . . . I intend for certain
things to be conveyed to the audience.” Other singers felt it was important for the
audience to understand and appreciate their genuine portrayal of the character. Louis
explained, “I’m looking to communicate authenticity. I’m looking for them to experience
my sincere thought and connection to the music in a way that expresses reality rather than
emotions and feelings to the audience. Dorine felt that, as a singer, one of her best assets
was her passion. Barry also felt that a major element in singing was conveying emotion
through his singing. He offered, “your goal as an artist is to live the moment and . . . the
emotion of your character.” Sharing emotion with an audience was accomplished using
[W]e’re instilling emotion and feeling into other people, and in doing that, they
are getting visual stimulation too, they’re getting imagery. We’re implanting
237
imagery into them by the way that we’re singing. So we need to start with our
own.
Theses singers communicated their ideas and emotional feelings with the audience
For some singers, the idea of touching the audience went beyond the physical
tactile sense and represented the image of connecting with the audience. It is included
here because Josephine and Ophelia directly answered the 33rd question in the interview
guide, “To what extent do you use imagery relating to touch?” using this meaning. While
performing, Josephine intended to mentally contact all the members of the audience. She
said she would “use the image of while you’re standing on stage, to touch or reach the
person in the last row.” She felt she could touch the audience with her vocal sound or an
inner feeling or thought. For Ophelia, touching the audience concerned shaping the voice
in a way that deeply impacted the members of the audience. She illustrated:
[I]t’s about the intimacy and using your voice to touch people both in a feeling,
but also physically getting the voice to reach them in a way. . . . You can’t touch
someone just by singing loud all the time. You have to shape things and in order
to do that, how you touch people through the shaping of the phrase.
Ophelia used a combination of sensory imageries to achieve this intimate and multi-
Three participants in this study were sensitive to their personal interaction with
the audience. These singers used imagery to share deep feelings of intimacy with the
audience. Dorine felt keenly aware of her vulnerability in performing as a singer when
she stated:
238
[T]here’s nothing between the audience and the singer. With instrumentalists, it’s
like they’re able to be hiding. They can hide behind an instrument. But when I’m
exposed to that audience. And I’m baring my soul to them. My chest is exposed,
heart’s exposed. They’re seeing my heart. So I need to give to them and to let
She used her sense of feeling exposed as an opportunity to share her innermost feelings.
Ivan and Josephine had images of good feelings going out into the audience that
helped them achieve greater connection with their listeners. Ivan’s image was intimacy
[It’s] a sense of, this gets into a little bit of metaphysical thing, but if sound, in
some ways [had] an ability to caress the listener in a way, energetically. And so I
would think it is much like healing, that way. . . just peace, calm, invitation to
participate. Not going at them, but inviting participation. . . . It’s an invitation for
them to come sharing in the embrace. . . . It’s not going out to embrace them but
rather just opening arms to let them meet somewhere in the middle.
This endearing image helped Ivan give him the feeling of drawing his audience more
deeply into his performance. Josephine’s imagery also helped her share good feelings and
a sense of being conveyed to another place together with the audience. She related:
I try to imagine that what I’m doing is giving them so much joy and happiness . . .
I do imagine just like a blanket, I guess, a blanket of joy or bliss reaching out over
the audience while I’m singing. It’s sort of something, I guess, I feel transported
239
to a place, to a different place in between. I’m saying I feel transported that’s
between me and the audience. And I just don’t feel on the stage, I feel somewhere
Dorine, Ivan, and Josephine imagined offering a deep connection and sense of intimacy
to the audience.
and their audience. During her performances, Dorine imagined the following:
[T]aking all of the good energy that these people are sending me out of the ground
. . . through my feet, and then I’m going to pull it up through my body and give it
Imagining that the audience gave back energy was important to these singers. Anna also
[The] infinity symbol is such a precise visualization for me for what happens
between you and the audience . . . because you have to have two whole solid
halves interacting with each other. You can’t have a wimpy side. It just won’t
flow back. . . . I believe that if I’m more open to the audience and to being with
them and the love that goes back and forth. . . . it gets me out of it being about me.
Eloise also was inspired by her metaphorical imagery of continuous cyclical activity with
the audience:
I imagine how ocean waves actually roll under against the ocean floor, and that’s
why then the water peaks on top. . . . it’s the circular thing that keeps, it’s just
rolling. That’s how I see what’s going on between me and the audience. It’s this
rolling wave of energy and mutual love, and we all want each other to have a
240
good time. They’re supporting me, and I’m giving them something in return, or
visa-versa. It’s like you can’t even tell where it starts and ends. That’s what I love.
I just like that, unspoken conversation. And that’s what makes me want to
During performance, these singers used imagery with this intention of being a part of
flowing goodness with the audience, which inspired them to perform better and more
meaningfully.
sharing. In discussing her motivation for singing, Monique said she imagined herself as:
[A] channel and the energy comes through me and out to the audience. . . . I want
to be a blessing. . . . And that’s your purpose as a singer, too, that that audience
Ivan also felt that the spiritual aspect was a vital ingredient in singing. He said, “what we
ultimately get to share is love, and we must be willing to open ourselves to sharing that
love, and taking part in love. And the art, as a whole, is an ultimate dimension of
picturing God filling up the space. . . between the audience and the stage and all
the space left in the air to the ceiling . . . with love, with a capital L, like an active,
I really do feel like I’m pastoring [sic] to people when I’m singing. I really do feel
a spiritual connection. I feel like a lot of times I feel like a real spiritual leader
while I’m singing to people, that I really am feeding their souls with my singing.
241
And that’s actually one of the biggest parts of what’s gratifying what singing I do
right now. . . .And I get that back from them and I feel like I’m giving it to them
as well.
something very precious was intrinsic to their singing and communication with their
audience.
For these singers, interacting with the audience involved the intrinsic reasons why
they became performers, the elements they actually imagined communicating to and with
the audience, feeling a sense of intimacy with each audience member, imagining a flow
of energy with them, and spiritual components. This emerged as a significant and
Healing Reasons
Healing reasons for using imagery were cited in 13 singers’ interviews. These
results included the two main issues of overall health and healing oneself, and more
specifically, vocal health. Table 22 shows singer’s healing reasons for imagery use
since the body was the exclusive instrument used in vocal singing. Thirteen participants
used imagery in relation to their health and self-healing. This category included: (a)
simple mental processes in staying positive, (b) body checking systems, and (c) using
alternative healing methods to achieve holistic balance. Francine said, “sometimes when
you’re fatigued or when it’s just not happening, you need to then rely on imagery.”
242
Table 22
Body Checking 10
Alternative Methods 2
Silent Practice 4
Gerald said:
I don’t wait to get sick to heal myself, feeling what my voice feels like when I am
healthy, before I even get to that place, telling myself that I’m healthy, so that my
This also exemplified Gerald’s use of imagery in self-talk and the memory of previous
There were ten participants who mentioned the use of kinesthetic imagery to
check physical conditions and alignment in the body for ideal singing and performing.
Barry employed a method of checking his body for functioning excellence in singing, as
[B]y the time you get to the performance level, you go through and identify your
sticky parts, the parts of you that are locking up, or physically not operating at a
243
peak level. So in performance you do have to go back to some of the relaxation
techniques.
Singers spent time and effort to make sure their bodies and its various parts were healthy
and active in facilitating performance excellence. Ophelia spoke about training in this
specific area, and said, “I actually read a book recently and that imagery is focused on, in
that you need your posture optimum. . . . It’s called the Egoscue Method.” This was part
of a training program her vocal coach had provided in using imagery for good posture.
modalities that appeared in the interviews. Catherine and Ivan mentioned using Reiki, or
energy work, and three other singers used chakrah balancing techniques, or East Indian
I have used a lot of that when I’ve been sick. I connect to Mother Gaia, clear out
the chakrahs, directly to the core of the earth. . . . First it’s always the earth for
me, and then out, reaching to the center of the universe. Always two points
grounding cord, and then to the center of what I would image the center of the
These meditation techniques were completely immersed in imagery use and these
Vocal health. Seven singers included responses specific to vocal health in their
imagery. Three singers used imagery to contend with singing during bouts with illnesses.
Four vocalists reported using imagery to practice while their voices rested due to fatigue
244
and sickness. Karen and Ivan had to contend with periodic symptoms of colds or allergies
and found imagery useful in identifying and implementing ease in breathing and singing.
Several singers also used imagery as an alternative to singing when their voices
could not physically sing. Being able to use imagery to practice beyond what the voice
could physically sing was a considered an added asset. For example, Louis used imagery
in this way and said, “if I am not well or the voice is not healthy, then I’ll use imagery as
an alternative to real singing, because the instrument might not be functioning properly so
I can’t physically sing.” Anna also used supplement mental practice and expressed:
And the great thing about it is you could do it without ruining your voice, because
you can’t sing all day. You just can’t. So you have to be able to keep your mouth
shut, and do this work. It’s imperative that you do it, or you’ll wreck your voice.
These vocalists considered silent singing a great asset to continuing practice when it was
damaging to vocal quality. After traveling extensively to perform, singers used imagery
[G]etting enough rest. That’s usually the biggest determination of whether I feel
retired baritone [would do] . . . I’m sure he was in situations where he was still
tired and had to perform. . . . to his optimal best, in trying to keep my energy
about me and do the best I could do. . . . Sometimes you’ve just got to make the
most out of what you have and make it through, not push vocally, but endurance-
245
Nicholas’ use of modeling imagery inspired him to continue with his performing oversees
engagement. Several other singers cautioned against pushing the voice during critical
times of stress and illness. Preventing and treating sickness and fatigue could be
Summary
description, categorization, and analyses of the data taken from the 15 vocalists’
interviews. In answering the four Ws questions of where, when, what, and why, a full
description of singers’ use of imagery emerged. These vocal professionals were found to
use imagery around the performance venues, at home, and other places. Times included
primarily during practice, prior to performance, in bed at night, and commuting and
imagery singers used were senses and abilities. Imagery characteristics included vocal
why, or purposes, singers used imagery included cognitive, motivational, artistic, and
healing reasons. Audience interactions emerged as an additional artistic reason for using
imagery in these performers. Other findings revealed specific uses exclusive to solo
singing professionals.
Emergent themes of imagery uses were often consistent with those found in sport
and dance research. Many of the themes reflected more dance imagery uses than those of
these commonalities and differences warrant further discussion in relation to the findings
from sport and dance research and related literature. A singers’ framework for the four
246
Ws of imagery is presented, as well as limitations, implications, and suggestions for
further research.
247
Chapter 5
This chapter provides a summary of the results of the study, interpretation of the
findings as related to the research questions, and compares the results to the four Ws
framework of imagery use in sport (Munroe et al., 2000), dance (Nordin & Cumming,
2005), and related research. Based on the analyses of the data, a new four Ws framework
of imagery use for singers is proposed. Limitations are reviewed and implications of the
In this study, the data of where and when professional solo singers used imagery,
what they imagined, and why they used imagery were gathered, analyzed, and reported.
In analyzing the results of the study, many similarities and contrasts with sport, dance and
related music imagery research were identified, and new categories and findings
these questions is presented individually and the resulting singers’ imagery framework is
offered.
Findings of the results regarding where vocalists’ imagery was carried out,
included practice and stage areas, at home, outside, and anywhere, which replicated
findings in dance (Nordin & Cumming, 2005) and musicians (Carter 1993; Trusheim).
All 15 vocal professionals in this study reported using it in practice and even more in
248
performance venues. This supported reports of athletes using imagery more in
competition for performance enhancement than learning situations in practice (Barr &
Hall, 1992; Hall et al., 1990; Munroe et al., 2000). Outside of their formal training and
stage settings, reportedly engaged in imagery at home and primarily in bed, commuting,
in places where they were alone, exercising, or anywhere. These findings were also
reported studies including dance (Nordin & Cumming, 2005), music (Bellon, 2006;
Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987), and sports (Salmon et al., 1994; Munroe et al., 2000).
and even more prior to performance, which was similar to the findings in athletics (Barr
& Hall, 1992; Rodgers et al. 1991; Munroe et al., 2000) and dance (Nordin & Cumming,
practice and rehearsal for musical performers with regard to times of imagery use. Other
performance studies mentioned this distinction (Carter, 1993; Nordin & Cumming, 2000;
careers. In this investigation, as well as Carters’ (1993), vocal practice was identified as a
private time of study and singing in a studio or private space, whereas a rehearsal was a
formal session to prepare an entire ensemble for an upcoming performance for which
singers were expected to be prepared. While all vocalists regularly engaged in imagery
during practice, fewer participants reported imagining during rehearsals. This distinction
emerged as a new consideration with regard to both where and when singers used
preparation.
249
Some confusion regarding breaks and holidays arose during the interviews as a
result of the specific word “holiday” in the singers’ interview guide, which required
immediate clarification. Singers in this study performed more during the holidays (i.e.,
Christmas and New Years) than at any other time of the year. The word “break” was
substituted, meaning an extended period of time (such as days or weeks) away from
work. Participants reported using little if any imagery during this time. Conversely,
Munroe and others (2000) reported break results only with regard to rest periods
Other times when vocalists used imagery were during quiet times when they
could think or when there were few distractions, breaks throughout the day, commuting
and driving, any or all the time, and especially in bed at night just prior to sleeping. This
was similar to what was found in many studies in sport (Munroe et al., 2000; Salmon et
al., 1994) and artistic performance (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Nordin & Cumming,
The results of what singers imagined were divided between imagery types, or
content of the image, and imagery characteristics in how they produced the images.
Generally, singers’ use of imagery seemed to reflect more of the elements reported in
dance (Nordin & Cumming, 2005) and somewhat less of those in sport (Munroe et al.,
2000). The six imagery types were categorized as: (a) execution, (b) metaphorical, (c)
context, (d) body-related, (e) musical sound, and (f) character/role, as adapted from the
dance imagery framework of Nordin and Cumming (2005). These represented the
250
significant areas of singer’s imagery content, while the category of musical sounds
including strategies and planning for practicing and performing, as well as goal
support, text, and diction. Singers extensively engaged in all kinds of images, such as
raising the uvula with a yawn, lifting the palate, and maintaining proper positioning of the
mouth and throat for proper singing. They described many images of expanding the ribs,
back, and lowering the diaphragm for breath control and support. These participants also
imagined text and diction execution as well as phrasing words of the song. These images
were consistent with those of other singers (Bellon, 2006; Gregg, 1998), particularly in
building vocal technique in elite professionals (Carter, 1993). These findings also support
the idea of Vennard (1968) and Arieti (1976) that musical sound and words fused in song
through imagery. The intermingling with skill learning of language and motor behavior
provided some support for Annett’s (1986, 1994) dual coding in motor applications,
motor behavior in imagery reflected the preparation and execution of that task could
support some of the findings of functional equivalence (Decety, 1996b; Halpern &
Zatorre, 1999; Holmes & Collins, 2002; Jeannerod, 1994; Meister et al. 2004).
Participants used sequence and strategy images in songs, entire concerts, recitals,
and operatic roles. Images of strategies and sequences were connected with processes of
learning and executing musical requirements of performance, which were also images of
other vocalists (Carter, 1993). The six participants who had to plan their own recitals
251
used sequence and strategy imagery to develop a cohesive program, which was similar to
findings in sports (Fenker & Lambiotte, 1987; Rushall, 1988; White & Hardy, 1998).
Furthermore, three singers created stories to connect songs they sang in their own
concerts. This use of stories and plans in imagery also appeared in several of the
musicians in the Bellon (2006), Carter (1993) and Trusheim (1987) studies. These
vocalists imagined strategies and plans in organizing their practice and pre-performance
ways of coping with distractions which reflected the results of musicians’ use of imagery
in other studies (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993, Trusheim, 1987). Singers also used sequence
imagery to facilitate memory of songs and phrases, which seems to support Sackett’s
(1934, 1935) “mental blueprints” which were complete concepts stored in the brain.
All singers used goal images, which primarily comprised outcome and process
with less performance types as described in Weinberg and Gould (1995). These results
were similar to other music performers’ imagery (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim,
performances, and single songs, arias, and entire roles in opera. This supported the study
by Woolfolk, Murphy, Gottesfeld, and Aitken (1985) who found that athletes who
imagined their desired outcome subsequently influenced that same activity. Several
results while consciously directed and detailed goal imagery more consistently affected
specifically desired outcomes. This issue was addressed in Ungerleider and Golding
252
The vocalists also imagined various process goals such as accurately executing
sounds, phrases, proper breath support, and other skills, which were also found in
Carter’s (1993) study. It seemed that process goals were used when singers needed to
(1972). The fact that singers discussed how goals evolved over the years further supports
employing goal images of whole and parts for achieving performance excellence. While
athletes (Munroe et al., 2000) and dancers (Cumming & Nordin, 2005) were found to use
all these goal images, they used more performance goals than singers in this study.
images in relation to vocal production, the findings of which were similar to those used
by singers in studies by Bellon (2006) and Carter (1993) and to a lesser extent in brass
instrumentalists (Trusheim, 1987). Just as some athletes used metaphors to help enhance
performance in different sports (Ruiz & Hanin, 2004), singers and dancers used
metaphors to learn and secure vocal technique and enhance performance, particularly in
creating characters and roles (Carter, 1993; Hanrahan & Vergeer, 2000; Nordin &
Cumming, 2005). Specifically, singers reported imagery of objects not present and
imaginary actions, which was similar to dancers’ metaphorical imagery (Hanrahan &
Vergeer, 2000; Nordin & Cumming, 2005; Vergeer & Hanrahan, 1998). Metaphoric
images of actions (e.g., bellows opening and closing; ball balancing on a fountain;
stretching a rubber-band) used to facilitate vocal production in this study were similar to
those in the literature (Fields, 1947; Freed, 2000; Lehman, 1945/1985; Ware, 1992). Just
253
as in Carter’s (1993) study, participants felt that using metaphorical images diminished
technical and anatomic images regarding vocal production. Some singers felt that
metaphorical images helped in achieving comfort and ease in sound production without
imagery when they were younger and when they gained a better understanding of the
continued studying the voice over the years, they became more familiar with the
anatomical workings and were able to accurately visualize them, as advocated by various
voice pedagogues (Dayme, 1982, 2005; Reid, 1983; Vennard, 1968). Similarly, singers in
viewing their own voices through laryngeal image biofeedback (LIB) medical imaging
system. Two sopranos and all the men in the study were familiar but indifferent to
production. Singers in the study varied in their choices of whether to use metaphorical or
anatomical imagery and their preferences often changed over time, usually in favor of
had to serve the intended function as has been suggested in previous studies (Carter,
1945/1985). While some singers used various colors to represent different timbres of
254
vocal qualities, several vocalists described very specific applications of visual color with
specific sounds. A few singers were very explicit in illustrating the connection of visual
color with tones and phrases, which was a type of multi-sensory imagery known as
chromesthesia (Polzella & Kuna, 1981). Trusheim (1987) also reported brass
professionals who saw different colors associated with the ideal sounds they aimed to
produce on their instruments. Conversely, dancers’ color images were more often
connected with the movement in their performances rather than musical sound (Nordin &
panelists and friends for whom they were performing. All participants imagined
performing on stages, for audiences, or specific people, and included details of the venue,
which were reflected other musician’s imagery (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim,
1987). This finding was similar to sport and dance in that the singers were imagining the
venues in which they generally or specifically performed (Munroe et al., 2000; Nordin &
Cumming, 2005). These context images facilitated performance best when they were as
support for the functional equivalence theory (Holmes & Collins, 2002). One singer aptly
described this in her account of imagining a previously unfamiliar theatre in which she
had hoped to sing. This imagery method of the Soviet Union’s elite athletic trainers using
photographs of the Montreal Olympic facility to familiarize athletes with this new venue
255
Furthermore, singers in this and Carter’s (1993) study imagined the details of
mentally created scenarios they devised for the settings of recital songs and arias.
Sometimes singers argued that the images needed to support certain songs were
imbedded in the text or the music, as in Lehmann’s (1945/1985) argument for the
tradition of creating the “right” image for performing a specific song. Conversely, singers
said that the content of the image was immaterial as long as it produced the right effect in
delivering the song, which reflected the views of the singers in Carter’s (1993) study who
arousal images directly dealing with the specific parts of their body most affected by
nerves. This was often related to relaxing the body, focusing attention in areas of tension,
and deepening the breathing, as mentioned by singers in other studies (Bellon, 2005;
Carter, 1993).
health, which seemed to reflect vocalists’ recognition that their bodies were a significant
concern in singing optimally. This was reported in the imagery of elite singers (Carter,
1993) and dancers (Nordin & Cumming, 2005), and vocal texts (Averino, 1989; Ware
1998). Participants used the mirror and other methods for direct feedback on the look of
their performances so that their ideal appearances, body positions, and movement could
singers performed with their full body visible to the audience, it was understandable that
256
Singers’ body-related images also encompassed the physically imagined feelings
of how they produced, executed, and memorized vocal sound and in characterizing the
song or role, which was consistent in singers’ interviews in other studies (Carter, 1993;
Hines, 1982; Moyer, 1992). This lends support for Ahsen’s (1984) triple code theory,
image, somatic response, and meaning (ISM) all must be rooted in the body. Participants
in this study were keenly aware of their physical bodies in relation to their vocal
mechanism and its relationship to physical posture and alignment for singing optimally.
Singers in Carter’s (1993) study also reported employing these body-related images.
These findings also support Leyerle’s (1986) five theoretical categories of organic
use in vocal or psychological problem areas. The ideal sounds singers intended to make
were directly connected with the physical positions and kinesthetic feelings used to
secure those tones, especially since singers argued that they could not trust their ears in
achieving proper sound. This provides some support for the idea of securing sound in
anatomical feeling rather than hearing it through the ears (Carter, 1993; Geraldine Farrar
cited in Brower & Cooke, 1996; Lamperti cited in Brown, 1931/1973). Several singers
support of using anatomical images over metaphorical imagery for vocal production
Bodily movement and kinesthetic imagery was also used to facilitate memory,
rhythmic expression, and dynamics of music, which was also found in the literature
(Carter, 1993; Jaques-Dalcroze, 1920/1972; Laban, 1975, 1984). This supported the
257
argument for including simulated movement with motor imagery for increasing
performance execution in athletics (Holmes & Collins, 2002), dance (Hanrahan et al.,
1995) and piano (Highben & Palmer, 2004). Sound production was intrinsically
connected to the kinesthetic feeling of where vocalization originated in the body, as was
also found in Carter (1993). Trusheim (1987) reported that brass instrumentalists
well.
Musical sound images. A major category that emerged from the interviews was
images of musical sound. This extended beyond hearing sounds in relation to imagined
venues and those specific to their performance activities reported by athletes (Munroe et
al., 2000) as well as some singers in this study. More specifically, musical sounds were
reported in dancers imagery (Nordin & Cumming, 2005) and were a major imagery
element of all vocalists in this study. All the singers reported hearing internally their
beginning pitches and 14 of the 15 participants said they heard the accompaniment while
practicing either silently or out loud. Auditory images of musical accompaniment were
also found in Carter’s (1993) study, particularly for singers that had keyboard training.
Participants also mentioned mentally seeing the notes on the page in front of them, as did
singers in Carter’s (1993) study. Gordon’s (1976, 1999, 2003a) music learning theory
extensively described the phenomena of hearing musical sounds and elements internally,
developing an internal ideal sound image. Ten singers used modeling of other
258
observing experts has been promoted in vocal pedagogy in a number of sources (Fields,
1992; Günter, 1992a, 1992b; Tosi & Galliard, 1723/1968; Ware, 1998). This lent support
to the observational studies of Fadiga and others (1995) in neuroimaging and functional
equivalence.
Seven of the vocalists heard ideal sounds in their head to help them produce
excellence in vocal quality, an element that also appeared in other music studies (Bellon,
2006; Bonpensiere, 1953; Carter, 1993; Hines, 1982; Trusheim, 1987). Miller (1996) and
others (Averino, 1989; DeLay cited in Stockholm, 1975; Fields, 1972; Ware, 1998)
supported the practice of developing a storehouse of strong ideal sounds in which singers
could compare and align with their own personal vocal sounds. During silent practice,
four singers said they heard the ideal sound of their own voices rather than those of other
experience, it was not surprising that all these singers had developed this faculty. This
connection between ideal sound images and vocal execution may provide support for
imagining music was found to be similar (Kosslyn et al., 2001; Zatorre & Halpern, 2005).
Musical sounds emerged as a distinct category in this study since singers’ imagery
content in this area was so extensive and diverse, and differed markedly from the
environmental and sport specific sound images most athletes reported (Munroe et al.,
behavior, emotions, and appearance, which also followed the imagery content category
set by Nordin and Cumming (2005). These images reflected vocal professionals’
259
requirements, movements, and efforts in dramatics from performing a simple aria to an
entire role in opera. All the interviewees expressed the idea that characterization,
emotion, and memories of past experiences were essential elements to expressive singing
(Craig, 1992; Emmons & Sonntag, 2002; Lehman, 1945/1985). Vocalists’ character and
role images were much more detailed and specific to acting than what dancers reported in
this category (Hanrahan & Vergeer, 2000; Nordin & Cumming, 2005). Singers in this
study and those in Carter’s (1993) began methodologically recreating their individual
characters with the words of the song, the melody and rhythm, the context and
embody the character or song. This supported the methods suggested in a number of
vocal texts (Barten, 1992; Emmons & Thomas, 1998, 2008 Emmons & Sonntag, 2002;
Lotte Lehman, 1945/1985; Ware, 1998). This also concurred with Lang’s (1977, 1979a,
stimulus, response, and meaning propositions as related to both the individual singer and
deliberation and direction, and amount and duration. These vocalists’ responses reflected
many similarities of athletes and dancers use of sense imagery and general abilities
(Munroe et al., 2000; Nordin & Cumming, 2005; White & Hardy, 1995). Munroe and
colleagues (2000) categorized sense imagery as imagery types. However, the distinction
was made between specific content categories of imagery (as detailed above) and
function in the subsequent dance imagery investigation of Nordin and Cumming (2005).
260
These investigators relegated sense imagery as a characteristic of imagery, and more a
vehicle by which performers experienced certain images, which more closely represented
Senses. The results of these singers’ ranking of their sense imagery practices
(from highest to lowest) were visual imagery, auditory imagery, and kinesthetic imagery,
which accurately reflected Betts’ (1909) results gathered exactly one hundred years ago
in his vividness studies of musicians (e.g., 93% vision, 88% sound, and 83% kinesthetic
imagery). Other musical performers were reported to use the same three sense modalities
in their profession (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Holmes, 2005; Moyer, 1992; Ross, 1985;
Trusheim, 1987). However, Carter’s (1993) ranking differed in that elite singers use
kinesthetic, followed by aural, then visual sense imagery. The use of these three main
senses were also discussed in Seashore (1938/1967) and Farnsworth (1958) who
recognized that musicians experienced kinesthetic and visual images interacting with
auditory imagery.
imagery interspersed with auditory environmental and sport specific images, including
olfactory, gustatory (Munroe et al., 2000; Salmon et al., 1994), and tactile senses (Vealey
& Greenleaf, 2006). Similar to dancers (Nordin & Cumming, 2005), singers’ auditory
abilities were centered on musical elements rather than environmental sound images as
was found in athletes (Munroe et al., 2000). Vocalists in this study included touch and to
a lesser extent smell and taste, the reports of which appeared primarily with participants
who enjoyed cooking. The use of these three senses was also limited to the dictates of the
text and character of the song. The inclusion of all these varied senses, or what Vealey
261
and Greenleaf (2006) termed, the polysensory experience, served to strengthen the
recommendation that athletes (and performers) use as many sense modalities as possible
in their imagery to increase effectiveness (Moran, 2004; Morris et al., 2005; Murphy et
kinesthetic feeling was the source of some confusion for seven of these singers. When
asked to rate their three primary senses in imagery, many included touch. However, when
the definitions of both tactile and kinesthetic senses were given, they all changed their
answers to kinesthesia. From subsequent conversations with vocal singers and professors,
the term, kinesthesia, was not as common in voice training as touch or tactile in referring
to the feeling in the body (Ware, 1998). Tactile imagery was not included in the four Ws
framework for athletes (Munroe et al., 2000) but touch did emerge in dancers’ imagery
(Nordin & Cumming, 2005). Brass professionals in Trusheim’s (1987) study did employ
instruments. This difference in use of tactile and kinesthetic terms should be considered
for any reference to bodily feeling concerning sense imagery use by singers and possibly
the visual sense with their preference and ability. Vocalists primarily used internal
perspective, believing it to be the most valuable, as had been reported in sport studies
(e.g., Lane, 1980; Mahoney & Avener, 1977; Rotella et al., 1980). Imagery in internal
perspective was helpful for understanding the feeling of actually performing, which
supported findings that internal perspective was more highly correlated with motor
262
movement than external perspective (Hale, 1992; Harris & Robinson, 1986). Singers’ use
as also reported by dancers (Nordin & Cumming, 2005). However, Ungerleider and
Golding (1991) and Holmes and Collins (2002) found that more successful athletes used
external perspective. Earlier studies had revealed no significant differences between the
effectiveness of internal and external imagery (Barnes, 1982; Highlen & Bennett, 1979;
Mumford & Hall, 1985). More recently sport researchers (Hardy, 1997; Moran, 2004:
White & Hardy, 1995) have argued that imagery perspective preferences relied on the
needs of the individuals and type of sports in which they performed. An internal
viewpoint was found to be more useful for perception in execution and external
perspective was used more for skills involving form (Hardy, 1997). Five singers
visualized externally, and some switched between internal and external, as was preferred
by some athletes (Gould, Weinberg, & Jackson, 1980; Jowdy, Murphy, & Durtschi,
1989). One of the tenors in this study, as well as a singer in Bellon’s (2006) study,
thought that using both perspectives were required in determining the success of the
outcome. Both execution and form were incorporated in vocal performance imagery,
strengthening the argument for using each perspective appropriately for the specific
needs of the performers as previously advocated (e.g. Caldwell & Wall, 2001; Emmons
An unexpected finding that emerged from these singers’ interviews was the issue
Hardy & Callow, 1999; Munroe et al., 2000; White & Hardy, 1995) and dancers (Nordin
263
& Cumming, 2005). Singers reported hearing their own vocal sounds internally,
especially when it was associated with kinesthetic feelings. They also described mentally
stage from the audience’s point of view. A singer imagining another vocalist executing a
the singer imagined being in the act of producing vocal sound. Bandura (1977) argued
that when individuals internalized the modeled behavior as their own, performance of the
perspective since other people played these instruments and these were not vocal sounds
internal and external auditory perspective. More investigation is needed to determine the
effects of each perspective on performance outcome and its correlation with ability and
deliberation, ability to manipulate and control images, and amount and duration of
was generally favorable in accuracy, vividness, and being able to manipulate the images.
Elite athletes were also found to have clear and accurate images (Barr & Hall, 1992;
Isaac, 1992; Orlick & Parrington, 1988; Salmon et al., 1994; Ungerleider & Golding,
1991; Vadocz et al., 1997). Some singers said their images were extremely vivid and
264
accurate while a few others felt they needed improvement (e.g., Nicholas reported that
some of his images were “veiled”). Athletes also reported that their images were
controllable (Weinberg & Gould, 2003; Vealey & Walter, 1993), however Munroe and
others (2000) argued that this was not important in the athletes they interviewed. More
experienced singers in this study said that the images they used were accurate since they
had become accustomed to them. The findings suggested that singers were aware that
they could improve their imagery accuracy, vividness, and learn to control their images
more directly. Athletes had improved their imagery abilities after a 16-week training
program (Rodgers et al., 1991), suggesting that these abilities were modifiable and
It was interesting to note that those singers in the study that reported high self-
assessment in imagery ability, accuracy, vividness, and detail also shared a number of
instances in which their images were associated with successful outcomes such as getting
an award or a role in an opera. Francine and Ivan imagined all the aspects of singing in an
audition and the specific part they intended to perform and were subsequently given the
appointment. Ivan even drew himself “with the award in hand.” Other singers whose
imagery was less accurate or detailed often reflected that they wanted to increase their
practice and get better at the details. Those participants who devoted a significant amount
of time in developing their imagery used it more and reported more successful results, as
was reflected in imagery of athletes (Vadocz et al., 1997). It was previously reported in
sport imagery research that more proficient imagers enjoyed more benefits in
performance and skill learning (Goss et al., 1986; Isaac, 1992). Similarly, athletes
reported that the more experience they gained in imagery, the higher the effectiveness of
265
that imagery became (Hall et al., 1990; Vadocz et al., 1997; Vealey, 1986; Weinberg &
Gould, 2003). Martin and others (1999) determined that these abilities could moderate the
performance, as defined by Short and others (2004), was also addressed in this study.
Generally, singers reported that their images were positive and helpful, as found in sports
investigations (Gould et al., 1980; Hall et al., 1990; Powell, 1973; Short et al., 2004;
Woolfolk, Parrish et al., 1985). Most participants positively imagined the outcomes of
their performances, which they reported yielded beneficial effects. Several singers
grappled with negative imagery prior to performance and felt their confidence and
execution was in danger of deteriorating. This supported the previous findings that
positively imagining the desired outcome improved athletic and artistic performance,
detrimental (Munroe et al., 2000; Woolfolk, Murphy et al., 1985). Some participants
included the caveat that the degree to which imagery was helpful or hurtful depended on
the appropriateness of the image to the task, which was described in performance studies
of musicians (Emmons & Thomas, 1999; Ristad, 1982) and athletes (Murphy & Martin,
2002). If the image was debilitating in some way, they simply adjusted the image.
images to those fully controlled, as described in Murphy and others (2008). Participants
who enjoyed greater ability to control their images reported greater positive effects on
266
their performances, which supported the findings of athletes’ imagery use (Clark, 1960;
Amount, duration, and speed of image engagement varied widely among the
vocalists. Imagery sessions lasted between fleeting seconds and all night mental
experience. Those singers who had learned to use imagery early in life more regularly
reported using a significant portion of their practice time in mental imagery (Bellon,
2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987). Participants reported their images as both
replicating and exceeding the actual execution times especially with regard to securing
compressed mental rehearsal (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987), while
dancers engaged in slow, actual, and accelerated imagery speeds to aid memory for
routines and sequences (Nordin & Cumming, 2005). Singers briefly mentioned mentally
slowing down images when they were engaged in the early stages of learning a skill,
piece, or section of music. However, most imagery took place in real time, which could
be attributed to the temporal nature of the musical activity and the importance of
matching the speed of imagery to the actual timing of the motor skill as neural
equivalents (Holmes & Collins, 2002). Sport researchers generally have agreed that real-
time was superior to slow-motion imagery (Holmes & Collins, 2002; Gould et al., 2002;
267
Why Vocalists Used imagery
The purposes for which solo professional vocalists used imagery were similar to
those reasons in sport and dance while other themes emerged from the data. The
cognitive and motivational reasons as found in athletics (Paivio, 1985; Hall et al., 1998;
Munroe et al., 2000) were supported, as was the artistic and healing purposes as revealed
in imagery of dancers (Nordin & Cumming, 2005). Within these categories additional
Cognitive reasons. These vocalists used imagery for cognitive specific (CS)
purposes in many aspects of vocal production including technique, efforts to sing easily
and comfortably, supporting the breath, and using the body and vocal mechanism to
produce the desired sounds, which was also found in other musicians’ experiences
(Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987). Imagery functions of building and strengthening skill
have been recognized in many studies (e.g., Feltz & Landers, 1983; Driskell et al., 1994;
Hall et al., 1990; Hall et al., 1994; Mahoney & Avener, 1977). Purposes of using imagery
for skill learning and technique development differed in vocalists based somewhat on
level of imagery ability (Driskell et al., 1994). For example, younger singers reported
using more imagery to gain breath support and lifting the palate than more mature
singers, who, it could be assumed, had already made these techniques a intrinsic part of
their performance behavior. This lent support to the argument of Fields (1972) and Marks
(1977) that conscious use of imagery was used to render motor action automatic. It is
possible that the more experienced singers no longer needed to use imagery for acquiring
268
skills that they had already incorporated into their performance behavior, which supports
the Holmes and Collins (2002) assertion that skill imagery uses changed over time.
after learning the rudiments of the musical composition. Singers combined mental with
physical practice in achieving optimal performance. This supported the finding that
combining mental and physical practice facilitated performance success in studies such as
piano (Coffman, 1987, 1990; Highben & Palmer, 2004; Lim & Lippman, 1991; Lo, 1976;
Rubin-Rabson, 1941), voice Theiler & Lippman, 1995), various instruments (Coffman,
1987; Ross, 1985a, 1985b; Theiler & Lippman, 1995), and dance (Hanrahan, Tetreau, &
Sarrazin, 1995). Similar findings were reported in the sports literature (Driskell et al.,
1994; Feltz & Landers, 1983; Hinshaw, 1991; Richardson, 1967a, 1967b).
Singers in this study used the cognitive general (CG) functions of imagery,
strategy and sequence, in ways that were somewhat different than what had been
described in sports. Those singers who had planned and performed many recitals reported
using imagery to practice strategy and sequence and to carefully project the proper
artistic experience they intended to provide for their audiences. Vocalists in this study
and in Carter (1993) and instrumentalists in Trusheim (1993) reported similar uses of
imagery, particularly to execute conductors’ directions and plan routines. Using imagery
in planning and strategizing was also found in athletes (Fenker & Lambiotte, 1987;
MacIntyre & Moran, 1996; Munroe et al., 2000; Murphy et al., 2008; Rotella et al., 1980;
Rushall, 1988) and dancers (Fish et al., 2004). Vocalists were concerned more with
finding strategies to contend with difficulties in performance, as had been found in other
musicians (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987), gymnasts, and figure skaters
269
(White & Hardy, 1998). Operatic singers also mentioned using more imagery for
sequencing and staging than concert vocalists who sometimes created individualized
stories to connect their recital songs. Singers in Carter (1993) and orchestral brass
sports, also used imagery in sequencing to assist in memorization (Nordin & Cumming,
2005). These findings supported Sackett’s (1934, 1935) symbolic learning theory of
memorization of text also provided some support for Paivio’s (1986) dual coding since
specific (MS) function of goal identification, setting, and attainment, which reflected
findings in studies in imagery in sport (Hall et al., 1998; Munroe et al., 2000) and dance
(Fish et al., 2004; Monsma & Overby, 2004; Nordin & Cumming, 2005). These singers’
responses reflected goal motivation, which varied by the type of repertoire they were
2005), and self-efficacy beliefs (Bandura, 1982). Vocalists combined outcome and
process goals in imagining a variety of desired results from executing the ideal sounds
they intended to produce to aspiring to secure a difficult operatic role. Participants who
had a high degree of success with goal imagery used it more often and in more detail,
mentioned their goals had changed over the years particularly with regard to the
270
development of their vocal technique and as a result of achieving past goals. This seemed
to imply that singers had a fairly high regard for the effects their imagery had on their
professional efforts. Regardless of the singers’ varying levels of use and understanding of
its effects, goal imagery for motivational reasons seemed to be an intrinsic part of
and energy in these singers was closely related to reports of dancers (Hanrahan &
Vergeer, 2000; Nordin & Cumming, 2005; Vergeer & Hanrahan, 1998), athletes (Hall et
al., 1998; Munroe et al., 2000; Paivio, 1985; White & Hardy, 1998), and other musicians
(Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987). Singers’ efforts of using imagery to
particularly in the younger, less experienced singers. Nine singers adjusted their nervous
feelings by using imagery to focus on breath management, which was similar to other
musicians’ imagery (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987), especially in lowering
their heart rates, as found in athletes (Hecker & Kaczor, 1988). Singers reported using
1987; Gawain, 1978, 2002; Harris, 1986). Other methods of using imagery to calm
nerves included prior preparation, focus, and concentration. Ten participants mentioned
the importance of thorough prior preparation and the knowledge that they were fully
ready to perform, which helped to alleviate their anxiety. Musicians in the studies of
Trusheim (1987), Carter (1993) and Bellon (2006) also mentioned the direct correlation
271
between extensive preparation and level of anxiety in their performances. This
corresponded with the findings in Fish and colleagues (2004) that dancers used cognitive
specific imagery of strengthening skill level to alleviate anxiety. Thirteen singers gave
accounts of focusing and concentrating on the task at hand to calm their nerves, which
also supported Landers (1980) and Murphy, Woolfolk, and Budney (1988), who found
that focusing on task-relevant cues helped athletes achieve the proper arousal levels for
performer and using this energy to benefit them in performance. Singers in Carter’s
working tension and the negative unwanted tension with which they had to learn to
sometimes caused distraction, anxiety, and nervous tension. Learning to reframe these
feelings had been previously promoted in various performance texts (e.g., Dayme, 2005;
Emmons & Thomas, 1998; Green, 2002; Green & Gallway, 1986; Leyerle, 1986; Ristad,
1982) as well as studies in dance (Fish et al., 2004) and sport (Munroe et al., 2000;
Vadocz et al., 1997). Several singers reported used a similar ways of calming their fears
by various methods found in general psychology, including dealing with stress with self-
theory of Lang and his colleagues (Lang, 1977, 1979a, 1979b; Lang, Melamed, & Hart,
1970).
272
similar responses in his older brass professionals. Higher levels of energy were required
when more physical movements were involved as in sport and dance (Harris, 1986;
Landers, 1980; Murphy et al., 2008; Orlick, 1990; Oxendine, 1980; Vadocz et al., 1997).
Schmidt’s (1982) theory of attention-arousal set was supported in that each participant
identified and embodied the optimal arousal state for each specific performance.
Achieving optimal levels in imagery for the given task supported Holmes and
Collins’ (2002) position that imagery should include the specific affective responses and
arousal levels used in the desired performance. Most singers agreed that they required a
certain degree of arousal or calm readiness, which precluded energy levels above
relaxation, in order to successfully perform. The Yerkes-Dodson Law stating that tasks
with more complexity or difficulty required lower levels of drive or arousal was
supported (Oxendine, 1980). This went counter to the assertions of Jacobson (1957) and
Suinn’s VMBR research (1976, 1980a, 1980b, 1980c, 1983, 1992) both of whom
Matching arousal levels and emotional affect in imagery with preparation and execution
of performance lent support to the functional equivalence theory (Holmes & Collins,
included thoughts and feelings of self-confidence and mastery. Specifically the findings
included (a) being mentally tough and positive, (b) focus and concentration, (c) self-
Vocalists’ mental toughness and efforts to be positive was exemplified in the way
many of them handled distractions and resolve to pull through difficult situations. These
273
issues were often connected with maintaining proper arousal levels particularly just prior
confidence and being competent to perform (Gammage, Hardy, & Hall, 2001; Hardy,
Gammage, & Hall, 2001). Negative thought stopping methods, being positive, and
maintaining positive self-images and expectations helped to stifle inner judges and
excellence (Emmons & Thomas, 1998; Kohut, 1992; Ristad, 1982). This grappling with
finding that supported those of other studies (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim
1987).
focus and concentration on the task at hand. Several participants even mentioned that
they concentrated solely those elements over which they had control and which could
specific tasks of the performance, characterization, and communication with the audience
(Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987). Orlick and Parrington (1988) found that Olympians’ best
focus was achieved by concentrating on factors within their control. Fields (1972) found
were well aware that their beliefs and patterns of self-assessment greatly affected the
274
success of their performances, as stated in Bandura (1997). Furthermore, singers who had
enjoyed success over a longer period of time also had increased self-efficacy concerning
their own expectations to continue performing than those with less experience, which was
also reported by other musicians (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; and Trusheim, 1987).
seemed to undergird how these vocalists regarded their professional abilities and efforts,
which reflected the ideas offered by various pedagogues (Fields, 1972; Stedman, 1985;
Vennard, 1971) and sport researchers (Callow & Hardy, 2001; Martin et al., 1999; Mills
et al., 2000-2001; Moritz et al., 1996; Vadocz et al., 1997; Vealey, 1986). High self-
efficacy and strong confidence beliefs and images were factors that distinguished elite
from less successful athletes (Gould et a., 1981; Woolfolk, Murphy et al., 1985), and
allowed for more facility in developing, maintaining, and regaining confidence (Moritz et
al., 1996).
1973). Singers described the development of modeling from observing elite vocalists, to
desired skills as an automatic habits as illustrated in Bandura (1977, 1986, 1997) and
Fields (1972). Feltz (1984) had maintained that mental rehearsal of successfully
performing a task served to motivate and strengthen athletes’ beliefs in their abilities.
Artistic reasons. In creating the four Ws imagery framework for dancers, Nordin
and Cumming (2005) included the cognitive and motivational reasons (Munroe et al.,
2000) and identified two new categories of imagery reasons: artistic and healing.
275
Vocalists’ imagery for artistic purposes differed from dancers’ due to the nature of the art
and included vocal quality, appearance, character development, and communication with
the audience. While these categories had been discussed previously in imagery types,
they are addressed here because singers used imagery for other purposes as well as the
specific image itself. As researchers had established, one image could be used for several
reasons as readily as several types of imagery could be used for a single purpose (Callow
& Waters, 2005; Fish et al., 2004; Murphy et al., 2008; Nordin & Cumming, 2005; Short
encompassed changes in their color or timbre, modeling, hearing an ideal sound, and
increasing depth and richness. Vocalists often imagined certain colors, which were
associated with specific sounds and phrases. This use of imagery was very similar to
those singers who held an ideal sound in their minds to match in vocalizing. Many
mentioned this in trying to achieve a certain depth and richness to their voice. Two
singers felt that each individual had their own sound, however they also admitted it
sometimes took years of training to achieve this. These concerns were also discussed in
the other music imagery studies (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987). Singers
performance practices they had observed in expert singers to affect more successful
behaviors and feelings of mastery. These were elements included in Bandura’s self-
276
efficacy theory (1977, 1986, 1997), as well as vocal pedagogues (Tosi & Galliard,
1723/1968; Ware, 1998). Musical performers in other studies also mentioned many
different aspects of modeling use in solidifying, developing, and perfecting their musical
efforts and production (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; and Trusheim, 1987).
Singers also used imagery to affect their appearance on stage. Ten singers
described images to achieve and maintain appropriate posture and execute staging in
connection with the character or a song. Several singers who performed specific songs in
recitals mentioned using imagery to achieve the right stage movement and gestures,
which more reflected how appearance imagery in dancers was employed (Nordin &
skating also reported using imagery to secure their artistic appearance and form in their
particular sports (Hays, 2002; Murphy et al., 2008). Professional vocalists recognized the
importance of their onstage image as perceived by the audience as a vital element in their
preparation and staging. For character preparation, vocalists imagined the backgrounds,
history, culture, and appearance of their characters (Emmons & Sonntag, 2002; Lehmann,
1945/1985). All but one participant recalled being inspired by either live or recorded
images of other professional singers performing in the roles and characters these
participants were engaged to play. Eight singers used images of their own past
emotion, the body, vocal sound, and being fully in the character. All singers used various
277
images for feeling the emotions and being in character, which reflected other singers’
accounts (Averino, 1989; Carter, 1993; Hines, 1982). Ahsen (1984) underscored the
importance of including emotion in his triple code theory, ISM, image, somatic response,
and meaning. Similarly, Lang’s (1977. 1979a, 1979b) emphasis on emotion was
through vocal production and interpretation as had been discussed in the literature
(Ferrier, 1955; Hines, 1982; Horne & Scorvell, 1984). Addressing and incorporating
these character elements were also consistent with how Craig (1993), Lehmann
(1945/1985), and Emmons and Sonntag (2002) suggested vocalists prepare and imagine
Several issues arose for some singers concerning being in the zone, or in flow
(Csikszentmihalyi, 1990). Munroe and others (2000) categorized flow as one of the
motivational functions of imagery in athletes. Flow was not included in imagery for
dancers (Nordin & Cumming, 2005). Similar to Bellon’s (2006) musicians, singers in this
study reported either being in the zone as a result and requirement of their profession or
only rarely achieving the experience of flow. Others described being in a magical moment
where nothing else existed but the music or feeling a responsibility as a performer to be
in the zone as much as possible in order to benefit the audience. Furthermore, the flow
reflecting Bellon’s (2006) findings that being in the zone was synonymous with
professionalism. Participants’ responses often reflected that being in the zone was
278
tantamount to embodying the character in the performance. This idea supported Carter’s
(1993) finding that elite vocalists achieved flow as a result of surpassing the experience of
synthesized text, music, and character. More research is needed to find how imagery and
Another artistic reason for using imagery was the variety of regard these singers
held for the audience, as initially categorized in dancers’ imagery (Nordin & Cumming,
2005). Nordin and Cumming (2005) identified many of the previously mentioned artistic
singers in this study distinguished most of those artistic reasons separately from their
specific intentions to communicate more directly with the audience than had been
music, and embodying the character was a prerequisite for mentally engaging with the
audience. Therefore the audience communication category was redefined for this study
and included: (a) sharing musical intent, (b) flow of energy, (c) sharing love and joy, (d)
spiritual engagement, and (e) spiritual responsibility. These findings supported and
extended Carter’s (1993) reports of elite vocalists interacting with their audience with
supported in their performances, which was analogous to the other vocalists’ mental
regard for the audience (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993). Most singers imagined ways of
communicating with the audience, ranging from making sure the audience understood the
intentions of the composer and the text to engaging members of the audience in a flow of
energy, the findings of which were consistent with other professional vocalists (Carter,
279
1993). Several participants imagined the audience feeling their love and joy, while others
shared a more spiritual engagement with those in the theatre. The reasons for specific
audience interaction were often tied to singers’ purpose for which they initially became
being a beneficial or healing force for their audience. It was possible that presenting
songs directly facing the audience, as has been the tradition in classical singing, could
have inspired certain vocalists to feel this for the people that came to see them perform.
Furthermore, many of these singers were well experienced in liturgical and oratorio
performances, which precluded a spiritual or religious context. These findings were not
addressed as such in instrumentalists or athletes’ imagery and were far more personally
directed than audience communication imagery of dancers (Nordin & Cumming, 2005).
These specific results also elaborated and extended the findings of Carter (1993), and
Healing reasons. Healing reasons for using imagery by these vocal professionals
varied in the interviews and reflected some of the findings in dancers (Nordin &
Cumming, 2005). Most singers’ healing images focused on efforts to maintain good
physical and mental health to be able to optimally perform. Ten singers described some
kind of method to check their physical body, the alignment, and status of the vocal
apparatus prior to and during performance, as found in other musicians (Carter, 1993;
initial symptoms of sickness or during illness and injury by engaging in healthy images
and feelings, which replicated imagery of other musicians’ (Trusheim, 1987) and athletes
(Calmels et al., 2003; Orlick, 1990). Singers also used imagery in healing methods such
280
as Reiki and other visualization techniques to maintain health (Gawain, 1978, 2002;
Sheikh, 2003; Sheikh & Jordan, 1983; Sheikh & Korn, 1994; Simonton et al, 1971;
Simonton et al., 1978). Singers often employed mental practice when they were sick or
needed to preserve their voices, as was also found in imagery uses by musicians (Bellon,
2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987) and dancers (Nordin & Cumming, 2005).
Vocalists in this study reported using imagery to contend with having to sing
while they were excessively tired, during times of illness, and outbreaks of chronic
physical ailments. Nicholas used modeling in recalling how another famous singer would
have dealt with the same stress he experienced. Sickness and fatigue were also times
when these professionals practiced mentally in silence to preserve their voices, which
was also reported in Bellon (2006), Carter, (1993), and Trusheim (1987). These
participants seemed to be consciously connected to how their bodies and voices were
feeling and responded readily with imagery to maintain optimal health for performance
excellence.
analyzing the responses of the interviews deductively from imagery research in sport,
dance, music, and inductively from themes and categories that emerged directly from the
participants’ interviews. Figure 3 gives the details of this framework including where,
when, what, and why singers imagine, which was adapted primarily from the frameworks
shown in Figure 1 of athletes (Munroe et al., 2000) and Figure 2 of dancers (Nordin &
Cumming, 2005).
281
Practice/Performance
Settings
General Locations
Where At Home
Other Places
Practice
Rehearsal
When
Performance Execution Images
Performance Season Metaphorical Images
Breaks Context Images
Other Times Body-related Images
Musical Sound Images
Imagery Types Character/Role Images
What Senses
Perspective
Singers’ Ability/Accuracy/Detail
Imagery Imagery Characteristics
Deliberation/Direction
Amount/Duration
Vocal Production
Metaphors in Vocal
Cognitive Reasons Production
Memorizing/Planning
Goal Attainment
Arousal/Energy
Motivational Reasons Mastery/Confidence
282
This proposed singers’ framework differs from the four Ws of imagery
framework for athletes (Figure 1) and dancers (Figure 2) in a number of important ways.
The landscape of the dance framework has been maintained in favor of the levels
presented in the athletic framework since a fuller description of singers’ imagery use
could be represented. Singers as well as dancers frameworks extended the areas of where
and when imagery was used. Furthermore, in the dancer’s framework content areas were
more fully represented to include specific image types and characteristics that were also
present in singers’ imagery. Additions to the dance framework included vocal production,
The four Ws of imagery framework for singers is offered here for several reasons.
It is hoped that the singers’ framework would be used as a guide in future studies of
imagery use in vocalists, instrumentalists, and other musicians. It could also be used to
inform students and professionals in possible ways they could develop and employ their
own imagery in their artistic endeavors. Furthermore, teachers and vocal professors could
use this framework to train singers and possibly musicians to incorporate and develop
imagery applications in their practice and performance. It is further hoped that future
research and applications would serve to improve this framework in any way that would
Several limitations emerged as a result of the exploratory nature and design of this
investigation and were worthy of consideration. Seven limitations were cause for concern
and included: (a) sample size, (b) sample configuration, (c) instrumentation, (d) pilot
study, (e) interview responses, (f) data analysis, and (g) researcher bias.
283
Sample Size
The first limitation concerned threats due to sample size. The small number of
participants in the sample restricted the ability to generalize to the entire classical solo
vocal professional population in the United States. However, extensive effort was made
to secure the widest range of professional vocalists that qualified for participation and
included soloists residing throughout the United States. Professional vocalists and
participate in the study. Generalizabilty was sacrificed in order to gain an in-depth and a
rich data set from highly qualified and experienced professionals. It was determined that
this population provided the most complete responses to the research questions, which
allowed the results of study to contribute to the growing body of research in the use of
Sample Configuration
Another limitation was the sample configuration. The use of snowball sampling
limited the generalizability to those singers who participated in this study. The
exploratory and in-depth design of the study limited the number of participants that could
be included in the interviews. Snowball sampling also was limited to those singers who
were known to the researcher or recommended by other vocalists and singing teachers
and professors who the researcher knew. Due to the researchers’ prior experience in
opera and liturgical singing, many of the vocalists were involved in these performance
areas which possibly limited representation for other types of solo classical vocal
284
While generalizability is not possible with this small sample, attempts a broader
representation were made through a sample selection that included (a) male and female
singers, (b) a wide range of ages, (c) a wide range of professional experience, (d) a
variety of vocal types, (e) varied geographical locations across the United States, and (e)
female, with sopranos’ average age of 37.4, mezzos’ average age of 58.5. Of the five
men, four were tenors whose average age was 40 and one was a baritone. Sopranos
represented the least experience with mezzos the most, including two who had over 40
years of professional experience each. In spite of many efforts to personally invite bass
singers to participate, none responded. Also missing were singers representing the
Midwest and Northwest regions of the United States. The limiting factors of the sample
The wide variety of classical singers’ genre served to represent a broad range of
the general employment options for professional vocalists, however it revealed an even
broader use of imagery and differences between the singers. There was also a high
number of singers who performed primarily in operatic roles, which may not represent
the entire profession of classical solo singers who sang other styles such as concerts,
recitals, and oratorios. Usually the acting requirements of staging and physical movement
in opera were not the same as concert singing. These issues may have influenced the
Instrumentation
The third limitation was the threat of instrumentation (Onwuegbuzie, 2003). The
singers’ interview guide was the initial instrument used to collect data for this study. This
285
interview protocol was borrowed from the dancers’ interview guide of Nordin and
Cumming (2005). Each item in this guide was scrutinized and adapted for the purposes of
this study using related literature and music research pertinent to the use of imagery in
vocal performers. Other efforts to control for this threat included the use of member
checking (Johnson & Christensen, 2004), whereby participants were each sent their
several music researchers analyzed the interview protocol for appropriateness in use by
vocal professionals. Although efforts were made to create a guide most appropriate to
gathering data from solo singing professionals, several issues arose during the interview
and breaks, tactile and kinesthesia, and sequencing and planning, caused some confusion
for a few of the participants. In addition, the first three sections of the singers’ interview
guide included inquiries that gathered less relevant data relative to the research questions.
descriptive data on the participants. Therefore the participant survey instrument had to be
designed and implemented, which proved adequate for gathering this information.
Pilot Study
The fourth limitation addressed threats in the administration of the pilot study. It
became apparent during the course of the study that the use of only one participant for the
pilot study was not sufficient to identify and clarify several terminology concerns in the
singers’ interview guide. Interviewing more pilot study participants would also have
clarified the inclusion of appropriate probes in the protocol questions. It is possible that
286
including more participants in the pilot study would have prevented these problems and
Interview Responses
The fifth limitation involved the participants’ responses during the interview.
These self-report and personal introspection responses were subjective at best but
represented a viable method in an exploratory study such as this. In order to control for
this threat, the identities of the individual participants were kept confidential.
Interviewees were given pseudonyms for reporting purposes and any personal
information, which could have identified them, was omitted. This helped ensure that the
participants had nothing to lose or gain by falsifying or inflating their answers. It was also
assumed that the responses of the participants were as honest, candid, and forthright as
could be expected in describing their experiences with imagery, since they had no reason
only enough general information was provided to allow the reader to understand basic
background and experience of each individual but not enough to specifically identify him
or her.
Data Analysis
In the sixth limitation concerning the data analyses, methods were used to reduce
threats of bias. In coding, four researchers with advanced degrees in music research and
training in data analysis were employed in coding one of the transcriptions, which
Guba, 1985) was conducted in order to expose the researchers’ implicit mental
assumptions so that data coding more adequately reflected the theoretical framework, the
287
related literature, and the responses of the participants. In the effort to allow the reader to
understand the researcher’s coding process, specific descriptive quotes from the
Research Bias
The final threat of researcher bias was a major consideration even though the
familiarity with imagery and the vocal profession offered a unique understanding to these
two fields. This background assisted the investigator in understanding and interpreting
the data in explaining the meaning of their quotes involving the experiences specific to
the vocal profession and imagery. However, the researcher’s prior experience may have
created certain expectations and influenced the direction and interpretation of the
interviews. In the effort to maintain objectivity and reduce bias, the researcher divulged
her training and related experience to another researcher throughout the course of this
study. Furthermore, peer debriefings were conducted throughout all stages of the research
process (Lincoln & Guba, 1985). Additionally, at the end of each interview, the
participants were asked if they were influenced in any way. Only one responded
positively and offered suggestions to alleviate this problem in future interviews. This
participant felt that she was being probed to answer in a way that sometimes confused her
and suggested that I could patiently wait slightly longer for the answers. This change was
immediately implemented with the remaining ten interviews. Subsequently, all other
participants reported that they had not been influenced in their interviews. Many vocalists
recalled that the interviewer helped to clarify concepts and provided ideas to help them
288
Implications of the Study
Since there were no known models of imagery use that have connected sport
unique and novel approach. Since these performers found imagery to be useful and
effective, it could be assumed that other singers and musical performers may already be
employing many of the same techniques in their artistic endeavors, even if their imagery
use was not specifically identified as those practices identified in sport psychology
research. The results of this study have served to distinguish these vocal professionals’
use of imagery in achieving optimal performance as they relate to the findings of sport
psychology imagery. Implications for vocal and music education on all levels, private and
class music instruction, and general education are discussed. These areas included: (a)
location and times, (b) developing mental capacities, (c) matching imagery content with
function, (d) mental practice, (e) metaphorical and anatomical images, (f) context
imagery, (g) goal attainment, (h) modeling, (i) musical sound imagery, (j) use of the
senses, (k) body-related imagery, (l) healing imagery, (m) arousal modification, (n) self-
efficacy and mastery, (o) character development, (p) communication with the audience,
practice, the finding of which was revealed in athletes (Munroe et al., 2000) and dancers
(Nordin & Cumming, 2005). Other places and times included being at home, traveling
and commuting, exercising, and any time or place. Becoming comfortable with using
289
professional singers as a way to use imagery in augmenting practice and developing
concentration and focus. The more vocal participants could concentrate, the longer they
were motivated to engage in imagery. However, singers varied in their imagery times
imagery times should begin with short durations and in quiet places then proceeding to
increased time lengths and levels of distractions as indicated by these singers’ responses.
Ultimately, with practice, directed, successful, and effective imagery would take place in
is a skill that can be taught and developed (Arieti, 1976). Participants in this study have
described the development of their imagery from memories of playing creatively as very
young children. Valuing and incorporating this important skill could begin in early
childhood musical and preschool programs and possibly provide innumerable benefits in
the academic and creative life of the individual regardless of their profession. Singers and
performing musicians in training programs of all levels including the private studio, the
general music classroom, and undergraduate and graduate private and group classes in
imagery skills. Professional musical performing skills would be incomplete with out
mental training and development. This is a strong implication for a holistic approach to
particularly in their use of imagery, could serve to guide vocal pedagogy, just as sport
290
psychology research has positively influenced athletic training programs (Martin et al.,
1999, Morris et al., 2005). Even though singing training is considered to be an oral
tradition, teachers with minimum vocal experience continue to teach vocalists, which
may impact how imagery is incorporated into their educational practices. It is possible
that teachers with or without professional singing experience may have preconceived
opinions concerning imagery applications and could inadvertently omit some aspects of
mental training vital to the performance success of their students. While it may be ideal
for all voice teachers to be successful vocalists, teachers should at least be knowledgeable
may not have been top athletes, but they seek to provide the tools to equip elite sport
competitors with excellent mental skills and techniques. Singing teachers may greatly
benefit not only themselves as educators but the future success of their students if they
learned the various applications of imagery to musical and particularly vocal performance
development and excellence. Even though only a few students may reach professional
status, all students deserve the best training available, including those in mental skills.
For these participants, imagery was used in cognitive functions for learning and
securing vocal technique and execution, motivational reasons of maintaining arousal and
energy balance, and artistic purposes of developing the character and emotional elements
of a song or role. It was also used to augment practice through mental rehearsal, check
and resolve execution problems, generate artistic interpretation, sustain optimal health for
performing, and much more. Given these findings, it is possible that other musical
performers might use similar imagery content and purposes in their artistic processes.
While many music pedagogues have recognized that developing the mental side of
291
performance is intrinsic to training (Caldwell & Wall, 2001; Chapman, 2006; Emmons &
Thomas, 1998; Ware, 1991), current methods of guidance, training, and education in this
area may be insufficient and may not be addressed in ways to achieve optimal
performance.
Sport psychologists have recognized for years that mental training was vital to
athletic success (Hall, 2001; Morris, 2005). Vocal teachers could be guided by research
such as this to provide all their students with excellent and thorough preparation. The
imagery components were brought out during training by the assistance of astute vocal
teachers, singers could learn to consciously direct their use of imagery, which may allow
for more autonomy and natural development in vocal development. Given the extensive
skills and techniques the singer must learn, training in imagery could facilitate building,
securing, and refining these efforts. Furthermore, the teacher could use a variety of
methods in helping the student learn to integrate, mentally and physically, many of the
other skills involved in being a professional singer. The use of software, graphic, video,
and audio media, and the Internet and can all assist in building the repertoire of sensory
and technical imagery upon which the singer could develop a career.
Mental Practice
Many of these singers said that they initially used and developed their imagery in
mental rehearsal primarily during quiet times when there were minimal distractions,
292
which served to augment their physical practice efforts. Implementing a program to
develop mental practice in general and private music programs would require specific
training as well as periods of silence in and outside the classroom or studio to allow
students to imagine recreating and practicing the piece mentally in between their physical
practice sessions. Mixing mental and physical practice was found to be ideal for
1990; Highben & Palmer, 2004; Lim & Lippman, 1991; Ross, 1985a, 1985b; Rubin-
Rabson, 1941). More specifically, mental rehearsal was most effective after the rudiments
of the music had been learned, then physical and mental practice was alternated for best
effect. Too often music teachers feel compelled to give their students as much tangible
and active material as possible. Teachers could assist students and children in their own
seemed to depend on personal preference and experience, familiarity with the anatomical
features of the vocal mechanism, and the requirements of the specific task. Regarding
vocal production, metaphorical imagery was used to secure technique and execute artistic
learning for some singers with less knowledge or familiarity of the vocal anatomy. In her
experience as a vocal performer and a teacher, one participant in this study found that
many young singers regarded their voices as somewhat magical and responded to
metaphorical images in achieving better vocal quality and advancing their technique.
293
However, as vocalists matured and gained more experience and understanding, they often
acquired knowledge of the intricacies of the vocal mechanism in executing their sound.
the voice with images of the corresponding muscles. This finding was consistent with
singers’ responses in Carter (1993). If a student was given the mechanical explanations
too soon, it sometimes caused confusion and unnecessary tension possibly resulting in
vocal injury. Several participants in the present study reported that similar experiences
caused them to seek another teacher. Other singers responded to the technical aspects of
the vocal production, preferring to know exactly how their vocal mechanism operated so
they could learn to understand its functions and manipulate it more precisely. For these
participants, especially those with the most experience, this knowledge base grew over
time and became quite extensive, which was reflected in several studies of vocal
pedagogues (Cleveland, 1989a, 1989b, 1989c; Vennard, 1958, 1961, 1968, 1971).
Providing graphic and video images of vocal anatomy could facilitate greater
understanding in singers who preferred these images. In taking it a step further, allowing
singers to see their own vocal mechanism by using a flexible fibroscope or a laryngeal
image biofeedback (LIB) medical imaging system (Cleveland, 1998a, 1998b) to see the
vocal folds could greatly enhance their anatomical understanding. Vocalists’ needs and
Therefore with sensitivity and flexibility, teachers should continue developing their own
knowledge and understanding of the voice and help students learn how imagery can best
294
be employed. These results point to the benefits teachers could gain from a full
with their students as needs arise. Teachers’ knowledge of when and how to apply
students. This highlights the necessity of the teacher to put aside personal preference
regarding the uses of imagery in favor of meeting the individual imagery needs of each
singer.
Context Imagery
auditions to their most coveted dream of singing at famous venues, such as the
Metropolitan Opera House. Many participants shared their extensively detailed images of
their future performances, including their affective reactions to the experience and the
uses of many of their senses, which helped them prepare for the actual experiences. This
linked imagined tasks with the same executed motor activity (Decety 1996a, 1996b,
2002; Holmes & Collins, 2002). Researching the venue by attending, acquiring pictures,
or viewing video of the theatre in which the singer planned to perform allowed personal
teachers in a private or group singing classes, could easily implement the inclusion of
visual media in imagery of various theatres around the world, especially since
295
Goal Attainment
An important area of imagery use that these participants stressed was their efforts
to identify, clarify, and attain goals. These outcome goals were also mixed with
performance and process goals (Weinberg & Gould, 1999) and were imagined with as
much detail and accuracy as the individual singer was able. Some participants mentioned
making lists, writing down details, drawing pictures, and clearly identifying the
particulars of what they wanted to accomplish. Several singers continually held in mind
the long-term overall vision of their goals while others were more interested in enjoying
and artistically crafting each performance. Teachers could help students by encouraging
them specifically identify, describe, illustrate, and vividly imagine their immediate and
performance, and process goals in their musical efforts as well as ways to use imagery to
accomplish them. Allowing students to internalize the music by providing time and
training in imagery, and giving students opportunities to direct their own educational and
personal goals could significantly increase learning and understanding as well as develop
a life-long skill and personal autonomy. This could provide the opportunity for the singer
to share the responsibility for their development and eventually assume the full conscious
Modeling
Vocal professionals in this study recalled watching, listening to, and being
appearance, movement, and characterization for the singers to emulate in their own
296
1997; Bandura & Jefferies, 1973). Providing excellent models in various capacities for
singers, instrumentalists, chorus, general music, and young children could positively
influence their vocal and musical development. Furthermore, the practice of listening to
Often music programs have emphasized playing and physical involvement with
performance, however these results indicated that watching and hearing exemplary
models of performances could serve to guide a growing musician in ways that would far
exceed constant physical practice. Employing expert models as teachers and educators
Another important finding that emerged in these interviews was that singers
mentally heard a wide variety of sounds in their imagery. All singers mentally practiced
songs and roles, hearing various elements such as the first attack, pitch, tone quality, text,
phrases, entire song, and various parts of the accompaniment. Over the years, many had
developed an every evolving ideal sound image, which served to guide vocal production.
This individualized sound image developed as the vocalist grew as a professional and
incorporated the storehouse of musical sound qualities they had heard and experienced
over the years. Building this personalized vocal or musical sound can be provided
beginning in infancy and even prior to being born (e.g., Woodward, 1992) and continue
297
“recalling and performing familiar music from memory” (p. 15). While most singers
reported mentally hearing the melody and accompaniment, especially with regard to
memorization, very few mentioned attending to the musical elements of rhythm, tonality,
or context. Even though it could not be assumed that these were not a part of their
auditory imagery, the fact that these musical images were omitted may be cause for
concern. No singer mentioned hearing any alteration of these phrases or melodies, such
provided evidence for more developed audiation abilities as defined by Gordon (1999,
2007). However, learning and applying further training in audiation could provide facility
develop auditory abilities in music. This has been exemplified in a number of texts on
music learning (Azarra, 1992,1999; Grunow, 2001; Liperote, 2006; Reynolds, Valerio,
Another vocal production issue, which emerged from the interviews, was singers’
inability to trust their vocal sounds and therefore their auditory images, preferring to
kinesthetically feel the placement for each tone. Several of them purposefully declared
that they did not hear musical sounds internally as a result of their teachers preventing
them from developing any reliance on their internal auditory images. This dependence on
the kinesthetic feeling of the components to produce specific sounds seemed to supplant
their use and development of internalized musical sound images, a practice which had
been supported in the pedagogic texts (e.g., Farrar cited in Brower & Cooke, 1996;
Lamperti cited in Brown, 1931/1973). With the extensive work done in audiation and
298
music learning theory of Gordon (1976, 1999, 2003z, 2003b, 2007), heavily depending
intonation, developing musical auditory imagery ability would seem to take precedence.
If teachers of voice would allow their students to connect both aurally and kinesthetically
to the sounds they need to produce, perhaps this would further buttress singers’ skill,
Singers’ imagery primarily involved sight, auditory, and kinesthetic senses which
were considered to be intrinsic to vocal performance. This has significant implications for
music education programs of all kinds to strengthen use of these senses in imagery.
Traditional music programs have often focused on music literacy, which developed
certain aspects of sight as it related to vocal and instrumental sound. Holmes and Collins
(2002) have acknowledged that learning usually begins with visual or auditory imagery
(in the case of musical training) and then the kinesthetic sense was subsequently
incorporated. Programs that incorporated singing, playing, and movement have helped
Laban (1975, 1984) and Dalcroze (Jaques-Dalcroze, 1920/1972; Joseph, 1982) had long
Elementary music methods such as Orff Schülwerk, Kodály, Dalcroze (see Choksy,
Abramson, Gillespie, Woods, & York, 2001; Landis & Carter, 1972) and music learning
299
theory (Gordon, 2007; Reynolds et al., 1998) have incorporated different aspects of
visual, auditory, and kinesthetic senses in their programs. Developing these senses in
imagery could be incorporated into all general music programs as well as private
instruction. Music education text series would benefit children’s music development if
they incorporated imagery training in developing these three senses in music play and
performance. This would also require the general music teacher to understand how to
implement specific strategies in valuing imagery, directing specific mental tasks, and
Body-Related Imagery
body-related imagery. Their images of the effects of their feelings and experiences had on
their physical body were remarkable. These singers were sensitive to the many intricacies
of the vocal anatomy as well as how they appeared on stage, whether it was how they
wore their costumes, the position and alignment of their body, or how their movements
affected the believability of their character. Several of the singers mentioned requiring
feedback in these areas so they could incorporate the corresponding kinesthetic feelings
in order to reproduce these effects at will. This was accomplished by soliciting the
and movement with kinesthetic images grew over time and with continued experience.
vocal training program could assist in building a storehouse of kinesthetic memories and
images to be used appropriately for specific performance purposes. This would build
300
autonomy and effective self-governing behavioral and performance techniques so
Healing Imagery
Healing images included both the overall health of the body and, at times more
importantly, the vocal mechanism. Participants revealed using imagery to assist them
during times of illness, fatigue, and exhaustion. Images of optimal health helped these
singers avert illness and take the necessary precautions so that imminent sickness would
not take root in their body. Many used mental rehearsal to augment physical practice
during times when singing was not possible or advisable. Other singers imagined the
release of all unnecessary tension, particularly in the face, neck, and throat, so that injury
to the vocal mechanism could be avoided. These imagery practices could be taught to
applications could be made so that singers could maintain excellent health and recover
quickly from illness or injury. As recommended in the sport literature (Morris et al.,
2005), the singer or musician would need to have clear and accurate mental pictures of
the healthy state of the specific or affected area of the body. Anatomical pictures or
videos of the actual movement of that part of the body could assist in imagery practices.
Arousal Modification
These vocalists used imagery extensively to help calm, energize, and ultimately
balance their arousal levels. Most of the participants felt alert and ready to perform on
stage and had alleviated most of their problems with stage fright and maintained a
comfortable and enjoyable level of arousal in performing. The various methods these
singers used to achieve the proper energy balance for optimal performance could be
301
incorporated into vocal training. Imagining the ease and control of breath, calming
anxiety, feeling release of unwanted tension in the body, or brushing the devil off a
shoulder were all employed to align the mental and emotional thoughts and feelings for
optimal performance. Music students of all ages have had to cope with adjusting arousal
levels when they perform. These issues could be addressed and resolved with proper and
theory. The help of a sensitive teacher knowledgeable in arousal imagery could easily
alleviate these fears. Teachers could offer their students a variety of imagery applications
from which to choose to help every individual they taught learn to embody the optimal
These participants all described feelings of confidence, mastery, and high levels
of self-efficacy. The process usually began with singers focusing, concentrating, and
controlling attention to the specific performance task and diminishing distractions. The
use of thought stopping and positive self-talk also greatly contributed to strengthening
which also positively affected future performances. Providing developing singers with
audition and performance simulations could help them develop the skills in
feelings related to the actual experiences of performance. Teachers could also assist
students in ways to deal with negative self-criticism and use imagery to strengthen their
302
self-image, thoughts, and feelings that facilitate optimal performances early in their
training.
Character Development
Further implications for developing the many aspects of imagery are directly
and artistically embody the character of the role or song. Precisely imagining the desired
sound quality, appearance, historical and cultural background, and physical gestures and
movement of the character was critical to achieving optimal performance for these
professionals. All singers highly valued recreating the emotion of the song or character
effort in incorporating these imagery elements in vocal training could be made. Learning
proper vocal technique represents only a small part of the required skills to become a
professional. Often these acting essentials are left for singers to learn much later in their
(Stanislavsky; 1961; Strasberg & Morphos, 1987), and found that their abilities to
embody their characters increased and deepened immensely, resulting in more expressive
vocal performances. Singers as well as voice teachers could greatly benefit from training
and experience in acting and dramatics. The sooner young singers incorporate these
mental, sensory, and emotional components in their performance, the more adept at
embodying them they could become, which could positively influence their work on
303
Communication with the Audience
Singers in this study were very articulate and somewhat passionate about their
professionals with more experience on stage than those with less. It appeared that
vocalists were able to engage in this type of imagery when they had achieved a certain
degree of automaticity with vocal technique, the music to be performed, and a level of
personal comfort in being in front of a live audience. These images varied by the degree
of interaction with which the singers were comfortable, their creative images, and the
very personal, intimate, and deeply spiritual images which directly correlated with their
own understanding of why they had become professional singers. This was an interaction
that seemed to develop over time and was extremely individualized. However, it could be
an area of imagery vocalists could develop either on their own or with the assistance of a
caring and sensitive teacher since these images are relatively personal. These images can
evolve and mature to a significant level, which could positively engage the singer in a
Singers regularly reported the benefits they had long enjoyed in using imagery in
their profession, employing different kinds of imagery for many purposes. By using
advanced technology, such as the flexible fibroscope or the laryngeal image biofeedback
(LIB) medical imaging system, singers could view their own vocal mechanism, which
could assist them in clearly imagining the healthy state of their voice. It could also
304
(1989a) found in his singers. Furthermore, teachers could incorporate Internet, graphic,
audio, and video images in providing excellent models and examples of elite singers for
musical performance that has heretofore not been available. In building ideal images of
sound, execution, and variety of images for performers, this could be a potential gold
mine and the use of good models from it could be strongly encouraged. With the
guidance of knowledgeable vocal instructors, the student singers would use modeling in
the effort to find the great qualities of their own voices and avoid the adverse effects of
purpose, singers, teachers, vocal professors, and music educators of all kinds could assist
provided evidence that imagery use by athletes and dancers compares quite favorably to
how vocal professionals have used imagery, even if the terminology and semantics may
differ. Teachers, students, and professionals could understand and appropriately use the
types, content, and functions of imagery in addressing vocal, musical, and performance
instrumental and ensemble groups, general music education, and even early childhood
music. Therefore, the link between imagery use in athletics and vocal performers, as
exemplified in this study and the proposed singers’ imagery framework, provides a basis
305
Recommendations for Further Research
This study provided evidence that extensive future research in the area of imagery
in performance is warranted. Specific ideas for further research included nine areas: (a)
replication of this study, (b) imagery in the performance process, (c) imagery for vocal
production, (d) applications in imagery training, (e) musical performers’ use of imagery,
(f) sensory imagery, (g) children’s use of imagery, (h) quantitative studies, (i) music
teachers’ imagery, (j) relationship of flow and imagery, (k) functional equivalence of
Recommendations for replicating this study would involve shortening the first
three sections of the singers’ interview guide, as well as adding the changes mentioned
previously in this and the previous chapter. It may be advisable to narrow the field of
classification. Future studies could also be designed to compare imagery of one vocal or
instrumental group with another, perhaps older professionals with younger, basses with
tenors, mezzos with sopranos, concert performers with opera singers, or string with
may prove advantageous to limit the interview to a specific area of imagery use by
specific populations. This may allow for a larger sample and more in depth data may
to uncovering deep feelings and images that are sometimes only barely conscious are
306
Imagery in the Performance Process
Several singers described imagery use in their learning sequences and processes.
imagery they employ in the performance process, beginning with first learning a piece to
formal presentation on stage. Furthermore, vocalists in this study reported using imagery
primarily during practices and just prior to performances. Investigating the differences in
how musical performers use imagery before, during, and after practice sessions, formal
rehearsals, and performances could reveal important findings that assist musicians in
them to journal either in writing or by audio or video recording their thoughts and images
as they progress from initial practice through the last performance, which was similar to
The issue of using metaphors and anatomical imagery in vocal production could
be investigated. More details and uses of metaphors could be compiled along with its
intended purpose, extending the work of Barten (1992, 1998), Fields, (1945), and Freed
(2000). Studies investigating reasons and variables for using metaphor and anatomy in
vocal production could provide understanding in matching the type of image with the
needs of the singer. Further investigation is also needed to as how to most appropriately
train singers in vocal mechanics and anatomy (Chapman, 2006; Vennard, 1968) and
apply this knowledge in their imagery through the senses of vision, hearing, and
kinesthetic feeling. Knowledge and imagery resulting from singers’ training in anatomy
and their personal experiences of viewing their own vocal mechanism through a flexible
307
fibroscope and a laryngeal image biofeedback (LIB) medical imaging system could also
imagery framework, for different musical performers and tested for effectiveness in
performance experiences. Changes in imagery use over time as a result of training have
been investigated in athletics (Martin et al., 1999), and could be applied and researched in
applications could be devised and studied in various musical teachers and performers
such as solo instrumentalists, small and large ensemble groups, chorus and band, and
many more, extending the work of Connolly and Williamon (2004). Studies could also be
planned in order that imagery use match the musical needs of the performers and be
variety of musical performing genres (e.g., blues, popular, hip-hop, and rap artists).
Imagery in musicians in different cultural settings within the United States could be
musicians, especially jazz artists, could be examined and also compared with imagery
in other cultures could be investigated on their own terms and compared with those that
308
Sensory Imagery
of the various senses. Singers in this study reported primarily using vision, hearing, and
Gordon, and Orff) and compared with other applications or a control group. The sense of
sight through different skills such as visualizing the musical score or internal images of a
and audiation research could extend the work of Gordon (1999, 2003b, 2007) and his
colleagues (Azarra, 1992,1999; Grunow, 2001; Liperote, 2006; Reynolds et al., 1998).
The interaction of different sense imagery such as chromesthesia (Polzella & Kuna,
How imagery is developed, nurtured, and used in children in a variety of age groups
could provide a wealth of information. This could extend beyond the music setting, into
imaginary play, creative endeavors such as fine art, dramatics, and movement, extending
Piaget’s (1962) work. Their use of the different senses in imagery could also be studied
longitudinally in these areas. The use of imagery in the general educational setting could
309
inform teachers as to how to value and develop imagery in a variety of academic
applications.
Quantitative Studies
Functions of Imagery in Music Questionnaire (FIMQ) (Gregg et al., 2008) adapted from
the Sport Imagery Questionnaire (SIQ) (Hall et al., 1998; Hall et al., 2005) could
could be adapted and administered to singers, teachers, and other musicians. Other
visualize, imagine, audiate, feel, and think about), providing definitions and examples so
that music participants would easily grasp each meaning. Manipulation checks to verify
that participants were imagining what they professed to imagine or what they were
related studies (Morris et al., 2005). Findings could be compared among musicians, such
as soloists with ensemble members, conductors with orchestra members, and students
with professionals.
educational settings could be investigated. Private and general music educators could be
310
surveyed as to their value for imagery and how they specifically use mental practice,
programs for teachers and professors in using imagery and its connections to sport and
Extended areas of investigation could include several issues that emerged from
performers in their actual daily imagery experiences, the practice of which was employed
imagery during their normal days as a professional or just their experiences relating to
professional in relation to their performance imagery could also be a topic for a research
study. Performers’ spiritual imagery and experiences in relation to flow is also worthy of
future investigation.
The data suggest that singers were generally convinced of the usefulness of
imagery in benefiting many aspects of their work. These vocalists also found imagery to
be intrinsically connected to their physical, technical, and artistic execution. The recent
311
imagery and performance. Functional equivalence studies (Decety, 1996a, 1996b; Finke,
furthering previous studies in this area (e.g., Alemen et al., 2000 Meister et al., 2004;
Halpern & Zatorre, 1999). For instance, singers’ images and thoughts could be mapped
using fMRI techniques and compared with those produced prior and during the actual
execution, in various aspects of performance and practice. The results of these studies
could contribute to this growing body of research in ways that would lend even more
Cross-Disciplinary Studies
support crossing the lines of musical and artistic performance and sport research. Singers
used imagery in ways similar to those of athletes (Munroe et al., 2000) and dancers
(Nordin & Cumming, 2005). In this study, music research was guided successfully by
findings in dance and sport psychology. Connections to many other areas emerged in the
data as pertinent to singers’ efforts to achieve optimal performance. Several vocalists had
specific training in acting in which they used imagery to create intentions and emotions
of their character. Other participants were trained in visual art and mentioned drawing
and creating specific graphic images that were created internally. Singing, as in other
professions, was not an entity unto itself, especially with regard to imagery use, but
and compared as to their uses of imagery and effects on performance. For example, sports
312
applications for healing purposes in the health professions could inform performers
perhaps as effectively as athletic and musical performers’ healing and arousal imagery
could provide information to the medical and psychology community. The possibilities in
correlating these seemingly diverse areas could be quite promising in revealing important
Summary
This chapter has presented the findings of where, when, what, and why 15 solo
vocal professionals used imagery based on the four Ws framework from sport and dance.
The results in each of these areas were summarized and discussed. Limitations included
and other issues of imagery in music training and performance. Recommendations for
Exploring the imagery used by solo vocalists served to broaden the findings and
the data pool providing further understanding of performance imagery. These singers
were articulate and willing participants, and contributed to the growing link in connecting
sport psychology and musical performance. The similarities are remarkable and serve to
313
The researcher hopes that this study will be a stimulus for further cross-
disciplinary research among music, sport, dance, psychology and more. Sharing the
extensive benefits in this area has potential advantage for all these fields. Further
qualitative and quantitative research in imagery involving musical and other types of
performance would involve applied and theoretical fields of study and continue to add to
314
References
Abma, C., Fry, M., Li, Y., & Relyea, G. (2002). Differences in imagery content and
imagery ability between high and low confident track and field athletes. Journal
Ahsen, A. (1972). Eidetic parents test and analysis. New York: Brandon House.
Ahsen, A. (1984). The triple code model for imagery and psychophysiology. Journal of
Aleman, A., Nieuwenstein, M. R., Böcker, K. B. E., & de Haan, E. H. F. (2000). Music
Anfara, V. A., Brown, K. M., & Mangione, T. L. 2002). Qualitative analysis on stage:
Making the research process more public. Educational Researcher, 31(7), 28-38.
Annett, J. (1994). The learning of motor skills: Sports science and ergonomics
Arieti, S. (1976). Creativity: The magic synthesis. New York: Basic Books.
Averino, S. (1989). The principals and art of singing. Aarhus, Denmark: Forlaget
Intention.
315
Azzara, C. D. (1992). Audiation-based improvisation techniques and elementary
86(3), 21-25.
37, 122-147.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundation of thought and action: A social cognitive theory.
Bandura, A., & Jefferies, R. W. (1973). Role of symbolic coding and rehearsal processes
130.
Barnes, P. R. (1982). Internal vs. external imagery as a mental preparation when applied
United States -- Texas. Retrieved February 11, 2008, from ProQuest Digital
Barr, K., & Hall, C. (1992). The use of imagery by rowers. International Journal Sport
26(2), 53-61.
316
instruction. Journal of Aesthetic Education 32(2), 89-97.
Betts, G. H. (1909). The distribution and functions of mental imagery. New York:
between mind and body with special reference to piano playing. New York:
Philosophical Library.
Brower, H., & Cooke, J. F. (1996). Great singers on the art of singing. Mineola, NY:
Budney, A., Murphy, S. M., & Woolfolk, R. L. (1994). Imagery and motor performance:
Caldwell, R., & Wall, J. (2001). Excellence in singing: Multilevel learning and multilevel
317
Callow, N., & Hardy, L. (2001). Types of imagery associated with sport confidence in
netball players of varying skill levels. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 13, 1-
17.
Callow, N., Hardy, L., & Hall, C. (2001). The effects of a motivational general-mastery
Callow, N., & Waters, A. (2005). The effect of kinesthetic imagery on the sport
Calmels, C., D’Arripe-Longuefille, F., Fournier, J. F., & Soulard, A. (2003). Competitive
Carter, M. R. (1993). Mental imagery in the science and art of singing: An inquiry into
The State University of New Jersey - New Brunswick, United States. (Publication
Castellano, R. L. (1983). An anecdotal inquiry into the nature and function of mental
dissertation, Rutgers – the State University of New Jersey. (Publication No. AAT
8325866).
318
Choksy, L., Abramson, R. M., Gillespie, A. E., Woods, D. & York, F. (2001). Teaching
Music in the Twenty-First Century (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice
Hall.
Clark, L V. (1960). Effect of mental practice on the development of a certain motor skill.
Cleveland, T. (1989a). Vocal pedagogy in the twenty-first century: Mental imaging and
45(3), 41-42.
Corchin, S., Barthelemy, C., Lejeune, B., Roux, S., & Martineau, J. (1998). Perception of
Corbin, C. (1972). Mental practice. In W. Morgan (Ed.), Ergogenic aids and muscular
Coffman, D. D. (1987). The effects of mental practice, physical practice, and aural
319
University of Kansas, United States -- Kansas. Retrieved February 11, 2008, from
Coffman, D. D. (1988). Rehearsing in your mind: Review of the mental practice literature
196.
Connolly, C., & Williamon, A. (2004). Mental skill training. In A. Williamon (Ed.),
Corchin, S., Barthelemy, C., Lejeune, B., Roux, S., & Martineau, J. (1998). Perception of
Covey, S. (1998). Seven habits of highly effective people. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Craig, D. (1993). The performer prepares: A guide to song preparation for actors,
320
Crutchfield, W. (1985, September 24). Pavarotti reflects upon the celebrated voice:
[interview]. New York Times (Late Edition (East Coast)). P. C. 13. Retrieved
August 23, 2008 from New York Times database, (Document ID: 954235151).
Cumming, J., & Hall, C. (2002a). Deliberate practice: The development of imagery skills
Cumming, J., & Hall, C. R. (2002b). Athletes’ use of imagery in the off-season. The
Cumming, J., Hall, C. R., & Shambrook, C. (2004). The influence of an imagery
Dayme, M. D. (2005). The performer’s voice: Realizing your vocal potential. New York:
Norton.
Dayme, M. B. (2006). An argument for whole body and holistic approaches to research
Decety. J. (1996b). Do imagined and executed actions share the same neural substrate?
321
Imitative mind: Development, evolution, and brain bases (pp. 291-310). New
(Eds.), Imitation of the social mind: Autism and typical development (pp. 251-
Denis, M. (1985). Visual imagery and education and the use of mental practice in the
development of motor skills. Canadian Journal of Applied Sport Science, 10, 4S-
16S.
Deutsch, D. (Ed.). (1999). The psychology of music (2nd ed.) New York: Academic Press.
Driskell, J. E., Cooper, C., & Moran, A. (1994). “Does mental practice enhance
Emmons, S., & Sonntag, S. (2002). The art of the song recital. Prospect Heights, IL:
Waveland Press.
Emmons, S., & Thomas, A. (1998). Power performance for singers: Transcending the
Fadiga, L., Fogassi. L., Pavesi, G., & Rizzolatti, G. (1995). Motor facilitation during
73, 2608-2611.
322
Farah, J. (1984). The neurological basis of mental imagery: A componential analysis.
Feltz, D. L. & Landers, D. M. (1983). The effects of mental practice on motor skill
57.
college football team: One incredible season. The Sport Psychologist, 1, 224-236.
Fields, V. A. (1947). Training the singing voice. Morningside Heights, NY: King’s
Crown Press.
Fields, V. A. (1972). How the mind governs voice. The NATS Bulletin, 22(2), 2-10.
Fish, L., Hall, C., & Cumming, J. (2004). Investigating the use of imagery by elite ballet
323
Finke, R. A., & Shephard, R. N. (1986) Visual functions of mental imagery. In K. R.
Freymuth, M. S. (1999). Mental practice and imagery for musicians: A practical guide
Gallwey, W. T. (1974). The inner game of tennis. New York: Random House.
Galton, F. (1883). Inquiries into human faculty and its development. New York:
Macmillan
Galton, F. (1907). Inquiries into human faculty and its development. London: Dent.
Galvan, M. G. (1992) Kinesthetic imagery and mental practice: Teaching strategies for
Gammage, K. L., Hardy, J., & Hall, C. R. (2001). A description of self-talk in exercise.
Garcia, M., & Paschke, D. V. (1975). A complete treatise on the art of singing: Complete
Gawain, S. (1987). Creative visualization. San Rafael, CA: New World Library.
324
Gawain, S. (2002). Creative visualization. Navato, CA: New World Library.
Gordon, E. E. (1999). All about audiation and music aptitudes. Music Educators Journal,
86(2), 41-44.
Gordon, E. E. (2003b). A music learning theory for newborn and young children.
Chicago: GIA.
Gordon, R. (1949). An investigation into some of the factors that favour the formation of
Goss, S., Hall, C. R., Buckoltz, E., & Fishburne, G. (1986). Imagery ability and the
Gould, D., Dieffenbach, K., & Moffett, A. (2002). Psychological characteristics and their
172-204.
Gould, D., Eklund, R. C., & Jackson, S. A. (1992). 1988 Olympic wrestling excellence: I.
6, 358-362.
Gould, D., Weinberg, R., & Jackson, A. (1980). Mental preparation strategies, cognitions
3, 69-81.
Green B., & Gallwey, W. T. (1986). The inner game of music. Garden City: Doubleday.
325
Green, D. (2002). Performing success: Performing your best under pressure. New York:
Routledge.
Green, L. (1992). The use of imagery in the rehabilitation of injured athletes. The Sport
Psychologist, 6, 416-428.
Gregg, J. W. (1998). On requirements for a singing career. The NATS Journal, 54(5), 51-
52.
Gregg, M., Clark, T. W., & Hall, C. R. (2008). Seeing the sound: An exploration of the
Gregg, M., Hall, C., & Hanton, S. (2007). Perceived effectiveness of heptathletes’ mental
Gregg, M., Hall, C., & Nederhoff, E. (2005). The imagery ability, imagery use, and
Grunow, R. F. (2001). Jump right in: The instrumental series – Teacher’s guide for winds
Günter, H. (1992b). Mental concepts in singing: A psychological approach, Part II. The
Guralnik, D. B. (Ed.). (1984). Webster’s new world dictionary of the American language
Hackfort, D., & Munzert, J. (2005). Mental Simulation. In D. Hackfort, J. L. Duda, & R.
326
International perspectives (pp. 3-18). Morgantown, WV: University of West
Virginia.
Hale, B. D. (1982). The effects of internal and external imagery on muscular and ocular
Hall, C. R. (1998). Measuring imagery abilities and imagery use. In J. Duda (Ed.),
C. M. Janell (Eds.), Handbook of sport psychology (2nd ed., pp. 529-549). New
York: Wiley.
Hall, C. R. & Buckolz, E. (1991). The effect of an imagery training program on imagery
ability, imagery use, and figure skating performance. Journal of Applied Sport
Hall, C. R., Mack, D., Paivio, A., & Hausenblas, H. (1998). Imagery use by athletes:
Hall, C. R., & Martin, K. A. (1997). Measuring movement imagery abilities: A revision
154.
Hall, C. R., Moore, J., Annett, J., & Rodgers, W. (1997). Recalling demonstrated and
327
Hall, C. R., Rodgers, W., & Barr, K. (1990). The use of imagery by athletes in selected
Hall, C. R., Schmidt, D., Durand, M., & Buckoltz, E. (1994). Imagery and motor skills
Hall, C. R., Stevens, D. E., & Paivio, A. (2005) Sports Imagery Questionnaire: Test
Hallam, S. (1997). What do we know about practicing? Toward a model synthesizing the
perfect? Current theory and research on instrumental music practice (pp. 179-
Halpern, A. R., & Zatorre, R. J. (1999). When that tune runs through your head: A PET
704.
Hanrahan, C., & Salmela, J. H. (1990). Dance images: Do they really work or are we just
imagining things? Journal of Physical Education, Recreation, and Dance, 61, 18-
21.
Hanrahan, C., Tertreau, B., & Sarrazin, C. (1995). Use of imagery while performing
Hanrahan, C., & Vergeer, I. (2000). Multiple uses of mental imagery by professional
328
Hardy, J. (2005). Speaking clearly: A critical review of the self-talk literature.
Hardy, L. (1997). Three myths about applied consultancy work. Journal of Applied Sport
Psychology, 9, 277-294.
Hardy, L., & Callow, N. (1999). Efficacy of external and internal visual imagery
Hardy, L., Gammage, K., & Hall, C. R. (2001). A descriptive study of athlete self-talk.
Harris, D. V., & Robinson, W. J. (1986). The effects of skill level on EMG activity
Highben Z., & Palmer, C. (2004). Effects of auditory and motor mental practice in
329
Highlen, P. & Bennett, B. (1979). Psychological characteristics of successful and non-
Hines, J. (1982). Great singers on great singing. Garden City, NJ: Doubleday.
11(1), 3-35.
of two experienced solo performers. British Journal of Music Education, 22, 217-
235.
Holmes, P. S., & Collins, D. (2002). Functional equivalence solutions for problems with
Holt, R. R. (1964). Imagery: The return of the ostracized. American Psychologist, 19,
254-264.
Horne, M., & Scovell, J. (1983). Marilyn Horne, my life. New York: Atheneum.
Crofts.
Ievleva, L., & Orlick, T. (1991). Mental links to enhanced healing. Sport Psychologist,
5(1), 25-40.
Isaac, A. R. (1992). Mental practice: Does it work in the field. The Sport Psychologist, 6,
192-198.
330
Isaac, A. R., Marks, D. F., & Russell, E. (1986). An instrument for assessing imagery of
Jackson, S. A., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1999). Flow in sports. Champaign, IL: Human
Kinetics.
331
Jacobson, E. (1931). Electrical measures of neuromuscular states during mental activities:
Jacobson, E. (1957). You must relax: A practical method of reducing the strains of
James, W. (1890). Principles of psychology. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Jaques-Dalcroze, E. (1972). Eurhythmics, art and education. New York: Benjamin Blom
4(3), 398-407.
Jeannerod, M. (1994). The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and
Jeannerod, M. (1995). Mental imagery in the motor context. Neuropsychologia, 33, 1419-
1432.
Jeannerod, M. (2006). Motor cognition: What actions tell the self. New York: Oxford.
Jørrgensen, H., & Lehmann, A. C. (Eds.). (1997). Does practice make perfect? Current
Musickkhørgskole.
332
Joseph, A. S. (1982). A Dalcroze Eurhythmics approach to music learning in
Jowdy, D. P., Murphy, S. M., & Durtschi, S. (1989). An assessment of the use of imagery
Karpinski, G. (2000). Aural skills acquisition: The development of listening, reading, and
Plenum Press.
Klonoski, E. (2003). Connecting the inner ear and the voice. Choral Journal, 44(3), 35-
40.
Stipes.
Kosslyn, S. M. (1994). Image and brain: The resolution of the imagery debate.
333
Kosslyn, S. M., Ganis, G., & Thompson, W. L. (2001). Neural foundations of imagery,
Kosslyn, S. M., & Koenig, O. (1992) Wet mind: The new cognitive neuroscience. New
Kosslyn, S. M., Seger, C., Pani, J. R., & Hillger, L. A. (1990). When is imagery used in
Kvale, S., & Brinkmann, S. (2009). InterViews: Learning the craft of qualitative research
Laban, R. (1984). A vision of dynamic space. Philadelphia, PA: The Falmer Press.
Lacaille, N., Whipple, N., & Koestner, R. (2005). Reevaluating the benefits of
LaFosse, L. (1973). Teaching the art of practicing, The Instrumentalist, 23(5), 42-43.
Landis, B., & Carder, P. (1972). The eclectic curriculum in American music education:
National Conference.
334
Lane, J. F. (1980). Improving athletic performance through visuo-motor behavior
16, 495-512.
Price (Eds.), Clinical applications of biofeedback: Appraisal and status (pp. 12-
Leech, A. L., & Onwuegbuzie, A. J. (2007). An array of qualitative data analysis tools: A
call for data analysis triangulation. School Psychology Quarterly, 22(4), 557-584.
Lehmann, L. (1985). More than singing: The interpretation of songs. New York: Dover
Leyerle, W. (1986). Vocal development through organic imagery. (2nd ed.). Geneseo,
Lim, S., & Lippman, L. (1991). Mental practice and memorization of piano music.
Lincoln, Y. S., & Guba, E. G. (1985). Naturalistic inquiry. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
335
Lindauer, M. S. (1983). Imagery and the arts. In A. A. Sheikh (Ed.), Imagery: Current
Lo, L. N. (1976). The effect of visual memory training on the ability to memorize music
Mace, R. D., Eastman, C., & Carroll, C. (1987) The effects of stress inoculation training
MacIntyre, T., & Moran, A. (1996). Imagery use among canoeists: A worldwide survey
8, S132.
performance. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 133, 64-
71.
Mahoney, M. J., & Avener, M. (1977). Psychology of the elite athlete: An exploratory
Maltz, M. (1960). Psycho-cybernetics: A new way to get more living out of life.
336
Psychology, 64, 14-24.
Martin, K. A., Moritz, S. E., & Hall, C. R. (1999). Imagery use in sport: A literature
Maslow, A. (1968). Toward a psychology of being (2nd ed.). New York: Van Norstrand.
Maslow, A. (1970). Religion, values and peak experiences. New York: Viking Press.
Maslow, A. (1971). The farther reaches of human nature. New York: Viking Press.
McKinney, J. (1994). Diagnosis and correction of vocal faults: A manual for teachers of
singing and for choir directors (Revised ed.). Nashville: Genevox Music Group.
Meister, I. G., Krings, T., Foltys, H., Boroojerdi, B., Müller, M., Tüpper, R., & Thron, A.
(2004). Playing piano in the mind: An fMRI study on music imagery and
337
Miller, R. (1996). On the art of singing. New York: Oxford University Press.
Mills, K. D., Munroe, K. J., & Hall, C. R. (2000-2001). The relationship between
Monsma, E. V., & Overby, L. Y. (2004). The relationship between imagery and
8(1), 11-18.
Schubert, E., & Renwick (Eds.). Proceedings of the 7th International Conference
Moran, A. (2004). Sport and exercise psychology: A critical introduction. Hove, East
Moritz, S. E., Hall, C. R., Martin, K. A., & Vadocz, E. (1996). What are confident
171-179.
Morris, T. (1997). Psychological skills training in sport: An overview. (2nd ed.). Leeds,
Morris, T., Spittle, M., & Watt, A. (2005). Imagery in sport. Champaign, IL: Human
Kinetics.
338
Texas, United States -- Texas. Retrieved February 11, 2008, from ProQuest
Mumford, B. & Hall, C. R. (1985). The effects of internal and external imagery on
10(4), 171-177.
Munroe, K. J., Giacobbi, P. R., Jr., Hall, C., & Weinberg, P. (2000). The four Ws of
imagery use: Where, when, why, and what. The Sport Psychologists, 14, 119-137.
Munroe, K. J., Hall, C. R., Simms, S., & Weinberg, R. (1998). The influence of type of
sport and time of season on athletes’ use of imagery. The Sport Psychologist, 12,
440-449.
Murphy, S. M. (1985). Emotional imagery and its effects on strength and fine motor skill
New Brunswick, United States. Retrieved February 11, 2008, from ProQuest
Murphy, S. M., & Jowdy, D. P. (1992). Imagery and mental practice. In T. S. Horn (Ed.),
Murphy, S. M., & Martin, K. A. (2002). The use of imagery in sport. In T. Horn (Ed.),
Advances in sport psychology (2nd ed., pp. 405-439). Champaign, IL: Human
Kinetics.
339
Murphy, S. M., Nordin, S. M., & Cumming, J. (2008). Imagery in sport, exercise, and
dance. In T. Horn (Ed.), Advances in Sport Psychology (3rd ed., pp. 297-324).
Murphy, S. M., Woolfolk, R. L., & Budney, A. J. (1988). The effects of emotive imagery
345.
Nicklaus, J., & Bowden, K. (1974). Golf my way. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Nordin, S. M., & Cumming, J. (2005). Professional dancers describe their imagery:
Where, when, what, why, and how. The Sport Psychologist, 19, 395-416.
Orlick, T. (1990). In pursuit of excellence (2nd ed.). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Orlick, T., & Parrington, J. (1988). Mental links to excellence. Sport Psychology, 2, 105-
130.
340
Overby, L. Y., Hall, C., & Haslam, I. (1998). A comparison of imagery used by dance
teachers, figure skating coaches, and soccer coaches. Imagination, Cognition and
Oxendine, J. B. (1969). Effect of mental and physical practice on the learning of three
Burgess.
University Press.
Patton, M. Q. (2002). Qualitative evaluation and research methods. (3rd ed.). Thousand
Perry, C., & Morris, T. M. (1995). Mental imagery in sport. In T. M., Morris & J.
Summers (Eds.), Sport psychology: Theory, applications and issues (pp. 339-
Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton.
341
Powell, G. (1973). Negative and positive mental practice in motor skill acquisition.
Raiport, G. (1988). Red gold: Peak performance techniques of the Russian and East
1178.
children 18 months to three years of age and their caregivers to rhythm chants in
duple and triple meters. Dissertation Abstracts International, 56(04), 186. (AAT
No. 9527531)
Reynolds, A. M., Valerio, W. H., Bolton, B. M., Taggart, C. C., & Gordon, E. E. (1998).
Current theory, research, and application (pp. 3-42). New York: John Wiley &
Sons.
342
Richardson, A. (1994). Individual differences in imaging: Their measurement, origins
Ristad, E. (1982). Soprano on her head. Moab, UT: Real People Press.
Rodgers, W. M., Hall, C. R., & Buckolz. E. (1991). The effect of an imagery training
program on imagery ability, imagery use, and figure skating performance. Journal
Rosenberg, H. S. (1987). Creative drama and imagination. New York: Holt, Reinhart and
Winston.
Rosenberg, H. S., & Trusheim, W. (1989). Creative Transformations: How visual artists,
musicians, and dancers use mental imagery in their work. In J. E. Shore, P. Robin,
Rotella, R. J., Gansneder, B., Ojala, D., & Billing, J. (1980). Cognitions and coping
343
Rubin-Rabson, R. K. (1941). Studies in the psychology of memorizing piano music, VI:
Ruiz, M. C., & Hanin, Y. L. (2004). Metaphoric description and individualized emotion
Ryan, E. D., & Simons, J. (1982). Efficacy of mental imagery in enhancing mental
Sackett R. S. (1934). The influence of symbolic rehearsal upon the retention of a maze
Sackett, R. S. (1935). The relationship between the amount of symbolic rehearsal and
Salmon, J., Hall, C., & Haslam, I. (1994). The use of imagery by soccer players, Journal
Schmidt, R. A. (1982). Motor control and learning. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Schmidt, R. A. (1987). Motor control and learning (2nd ed.). Champaign, IL: Human
Kinetics.
Baywood.
Schwartz, G. & Russek, L. G. (2003). Imagery, love, and health: How dynamic energy
344
systems theory can integrate conventional and complementary medicine. In A.
Sheikh (Ed.), Healing images: The role of imagination in health (pp. 91-14).
Seashore, C. E. (1919). The psychology of musical talent. Boston: Silver, Burdett and
Company.
Seashore, C. E. (1939). New approaches to the science of voice. Scientific Monthy, 49,
340.
McGraw-Hill, 1938)
Sheehan, P. W. (1972). The function and nature of imagery. New York: Academic Press.
Sheikh, A. A. (Ed.) (1983). Imagery: Current theory, research, and application. New
York: Wiley.
Sheikh, A. (Ed.). (2003). Healing images: The role of imagination in health. Amityville,
NY: Baywood.
Sheikh, A. A., & Jordan, S. C. (1983). Clinical uses of mental imagery. In A. A. Sheikh
(Ed.), Imagery: Current theory, research, and application. New York: Wiley.
Sheikh, A. A., & Korn, E. R. (1994). Imagery in sports and physical performance.
345
Short, S., Monsma, E., & Short, M. (2004). Is what you see really what you get?
Short, S., Ross-Stewart, L., Monsma, E. (2006). Onwards with the evolution of imagery
Simons, J. (2000). Doing imagery in the field. In M. B. Anderson (Ed.), Doing sport
Simonton, O. C., Simonton, S., & Creighton, J. L. (1978). Getting well again: A step-by-
step, self-help guide to overcoming cancer for patients and their families. New
Sisterhen, L. A. (2005). The use of imagery, mental practice, and relaxation techniques
Oklahoma, United States. Retrieved January 16, 2008, from ProQuest Digital
Skeat, W. W. (1943). An etymological Dictionary of the English language (2nd ed.). New
York: Macmillan.
Sommer, R. (1980). A practical guide to behavioral research: Tools and techniques. New
Sordoni, C., Hall, C. R., & Forwell, L. (2000). The use of imagery by athletes during
346
Stanislavsky, K. (1961). Creating a role. New York: Theatre Arts Books.
Start, K. B., & Richardson, A. (1964). Imagery and mental practice. British Journal of
Stockholm, G. (1975). Dorothy DeLay discusses teaching. The Instrumentalist, 30(5), 40-
43.
Strasberg, L., & Morphos, E. (1987). A dream of passion: The development of the
Strauss, A., & Corbin, J. (1998). Basics of qualitative research: Techniques and
procedures for developing Grounded Theory (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Suinn, R. M. (1972). Behavior rehearsal training for ski racers: Brief report. Behavior
Therapy, 2, 210-212.
Krumboltz & C. Thoresen (Eds.), Counseling Methods (pp. 360-366). New York:
Holt.
Minneapolis: Burgess.
347
Suinn, R. M. (1980b). Psychology and sports performance: Principles and applications.
Macmillan.
Suinn, R. M. (1986). Seven steps to peak performance: The mental training manual for
Baywood.
Suinn, R. M. (1997). Mental practice in sport psychology: Where have we been, where
Handbook of research on sport psychology (2nd ed., pp. 496-510). New York:
Macmillan.
Taggart, C. C., Bolton, B. M., Reynolds, A. M., Valerio, W. H., & Gordon, E. E. (2000).
Taylor, R. (1993). Imagery: Its role in mental rehearsal to improve sports performance:
Research Report No. 27. Edinburgh, EH: The Scottish Sports Council, Caledonia
348
House.
Theiler, A. M., & Lippman, L. G. (1995). Effects of mental practice and modeling on
Titze, I. (1986). Standardization in voice research. The NATS Journal, 42(2), 30-31.
Tosi, P. F., & Galliard, J. E. (1968). Observations on the florid song. New York: Johnson
imagery use by eminent orchestral brass players in the United States. Ed.D.
United States -- New Jersey. Retrieved February 11, 2008, from ProQuest Digital
Ungerleider, S., & Golding, J. M. (1991). Mental practice among Olympic athletes.
Vadocz, E., Hall, C., & Moritz, S. (1997). The relationship between competitive anxiety
(Ed.), Applied sport psychology: Personal growth for peak performance (pp. 209-
Vealey, R. S., & Greenleaf, C. A. (2001). Seeing is believing: Understanding and using
349
growth to peak performance (4th ed., pp. 247-272). Mountain View, CA:
Mayfield.
Vealey, R. S., & Greenleaf, C. A. (2006). Seeing is believing: Understanding and using
growth to peak performance (5th ed., pp. 306-343). Mountain View, CA:
Mayfield.
Vealey, R. S., Walter, S. M. (1993). Imagery training for performance enhancement and
Personal growth to peak performance (2nd ed., pp. 200-224). Mountain View,
CA: Mayfield.
Vennard, W. (1961). Singers and science. Selected Articles from the writings of William
Vennard, W. (1968). Singing: The mechanism and the technic. New York: Carl Fisher.
Vennard, W. (1971). Selected Articles from the writings of William Vennard (Gurnee, R.
Vergeer, I., & Hanrahan, C. (1998). What modern dancers do to prepare: Content and
Ware, C. (1998). Basics of vocal pedagogy: The foundations and process of singing.
350
Warner, B. (1991). Orff-Schulwerk: Applications for the classroom. Englewood Cliffs,
Watson, J. B. (1913). Psychology as the behaviorist sees it. Lancaster, PA: Psychological
Review Company.
between focal phobia and social performance anxiety. Journal of Consulting and
Weinberg, R., Butt, J., Knight, B., Burke, K. L., & Jackson, A. (2003). The relationship
Weinberg, R. S., & Gould, D. G. (1995). Foundations of sport and exercise psychology.
Weinberg, R. S., & Gould, D. G. (1999). Foundations of sport and exercise psychology
Weinberg, R. S., & Gould, D. G. (2003). Foundations of sport and exercise psychology
White, A. & Hardy, L. (1995). Use of different imagery perspectives on the learning and
White, A. & Hardy, L. (1998). An in-depth analysis of the uses of imagery by high-level
slalom canoeists and artistic gymnasts. The Sport Psychologist, 12, 387-403.
351
Woodward, S. (1992). Intrauterine Rhythm and Blues? British Journal of Obstetrics and
Woolfolk, R. L., Murphy, S. M., Gottesfeld, D., & Aitken, D. (1985). Mental rehearsal of
motor task activity and mental depiction of task outcome on motor skill
Woolfolk, R. L., Parrish, M. W., & Murphy, S. M. (1985). The effects of positive and
(3), 335-341.
Zatorre, R. J., & Halpern, A. R. (2005). Mental concerts: Musical imagery and auditory
352
Appendices
353
Appendix A
354
Appendix B
Patricia L. Bowes
6712 Sandscape Lane
Temple Terrace, FL 33617
813 985-1199, cell 561 523-2223
[email protected]
December 1, 2008
I am a candidate for the Ph.D. degree in music education at the University of South
Florida, Tampa. I am currently beginning field work on my dissertation, titled: An
Exploratory Study of the Use of Imagery by Vocal Professionals: Applications of a Sport
Psychology Framework.
I am looking for male and female participants in all vocal registers from major cities
throughout the United States who
(a) hold a four year degree in vocal performance, and
(b) have been working for at least four years making at least half their income as a solo
vocal professional
or
(c) if they have no performance degree, they have eight years making at least half their
income as a solo vocal professional.
In order to identify vocal professionals who might wish to participate in my study, I am
respectfully requesting your assistance. I have contacted you because you are a
recognized vocal professional currently working in the field.
355
Appendix B (continued)
There are a few potential benefits to you and members of the vocal profession. You may
learn more about your use of imagery in achieving optimal performance. Your responses
may also be of possible benefit to other singers, voice teachers, and music education in
general. Unfortunately, there is no pay for your time.
The telephone call for the interview should be free for you since I will call you. All
names, locations and identifying references will remain anonymous in order to ensure
your most candid and honest responses.
I sincerely hope you decide to participate in this research study, and that it will be a
rewarding experience for you. As soon as you have a chance, please send me an email
([email protected]) and let me know whether you decide to participate.
Furthermore, if you are able to suggest possible participants for my dissertation study
who meet the above criteria, please send an email to me that includes the names of
singers you recommend, and either their email and/or telephone numbers where they can
be reached.
If you would like more information about my study, please take a look at the attached
information sheet. If you have additionally questions or concerns, please feel free to
email me at the above address. Thank you for your kind consideration. I look forward to
hearing from you soon.
Sincerely,
Patricia L. Bowes
356
Appendix C
Patricia L. Bowes
6712 Sandscape Lane
Temple Terrace, FL 33617
813 985-1199, cell 561 523-2223
[email protected]
December 1, 2008
I am a candidate for the Ph.D. degree in music education at the University of South
Florida, Tampa. I am currently beginning field work on my dissertation, titled An
Exploratory Study of the Use of Imagery by Vocal Professionals: Applications of a Sport
Psychology Framework.
I am looking for male and female participants in all vocal registers from major cities
throughout the United States who
(a) hold a four year degree in vocal performance, and
(b) have been working for at least four years making at least half their income as a solo
vocal professional
or
(c) if they have no performance degree, they have eight years making at least half their
income as a solo vocal professional.
I sincerely hope that you are willing and able to assist me in identifying possible
participants for my dissertation study. I am asking you to take a few moments to think of
singers who meet the above criteria. Then, please send an email to me
([email protected]) that includes the names of singers you recommend, and
either their email and/or telephone numbers where they can be reached.
357
Appendix C (continued)
If you would like more information about my study, please take a look at the attached
information sheet. If you have additionally questions or concerns, please feel free to
email me at the above address. Thank you for your kind consideration. I look forward to
hearing from you soon.
Sincerely,
Patricia L. Bowes
Ph.D. Candidate
School of Music
College of Visual and Performing Arts
University of South Florida
Tampa, FL 33620
358
Appendix D
359
Appendix D (continued)
As we go through the interview, I have definitions and examples of the various kinds of
imagery available so please ask if you are not quite clear about what I am asking for.
Also, do not worry if some questions seem repetitive. This is partly to make sure that I
am covering all aspects of your singing imagery and not leaving anything out, and partly
useful to obtain quotes for later analyses.
5) Describe how your use of imagery has developed over your singing career?
i. i.e., is it different now in any way from what it was to start with?
10) Did your teacher ever use imagery with you in connection with your training in
achieving optimal performance? (If yes, go on to #11. If no go on to Section 4, #
15.)
11) Describe what kinds of imagery your singing teacher involved in the lessons when
you were younger?
i. Elaboration probe:
1. e.g. suggesting you could imagine an aria in your head?
2. When was this? / How old were you then?
3. What did this imagery involve?
13) What kinds of imagery do your teachers/coaches encourage you to use yourself,
outside of lessons/rehearsals?
i. Describe some of that imagery that they encourage?
ii. To what extent do you use what they recommend?
14) Where else did you learn to use imagery in vocal performance?
i. Did you read books, collaborate with peers, or suggested by a
teacher?
Section 4: Why
Imagery use for optimal performance can serve different functions for singers, and in the
following sections, I will be asking you to elaborate on the reasons why you use imagery.
15) Describe any imagery you use that is based on skill learning and execution?
i. i.e. imagery based on technical instruction and doing things
correctly?
ii. e.g. imagining a certain aria performed perfectly.
iii. elaboration probes:
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)
361
Appendix D (continued)
ii. e.g. imagining a stage entry going exactly according to plan and
practice.
iii. elaboration probes:
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)
18) Describe any imagery you use relating to arousal and anxiety?
i. i.e. imagery based on stress, nervousness, stage fright etc.?
ii. e.g. imagining the excitement associated with performing,
imagining handling your performance anxiety or stage fright?
iii. elaboration probes:
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)
19) Describe any imagery you use relating to self-confidence and mastery?
i. i.e. imagery based on being in control, being focused, and/or
working successfully through difficult situations?
ii. e.g. imagining feeling really confident when walking onto the
stage?
iii. elaboration probes:
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)
362
Appendix D (continued)
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)
363
Appendix D (continued)
27) Describe any other kinds of imagery you use to achieve optimal performance,
which are different from the ones we have mentioned?
i. e.g. healing, spiritual imagery?
ii. elaboration probes:
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content
28) Describe any other reasons that you have for using imagery to achieve optimal
performance, that are different from the ones we have mentioned?
i. e.g. motivating yourself to sing?
ii. elaboration probes:
1. What does it consist of? (content)
364
Appendix D (continued)
Section 5: What
This section of the interview focuses on what senses you involve in your imagery for
optimal performance. As you know, you have the five senses of vision, hearing, smell,
taste, and touch. Additionally, you can experience kinaesthetic imagery, and the emotion-
based imagery we talked about earlier.
29) To what extent do you use imagery relating to vision?
i. What perspective does that imagery take?
a. i.e. is it Internal/External/Switching?
30) To what extent do you use imagery relating to sound and hearing?
35) Describe any other kinds of sensations that you feel in your imagery?
i. e.g. heat or other temperature changes? Pain? Body Image?
36) Which of the above would be the primary senses you use in your imagery?
i. Which would be not so important/by-products?
365
Appendix D (continued)
Section 8: Conclusions
Ok, this just about wraps up the interview. However, before we finish, let me ask you a
few final questions:
41) How did you think the interview went?
44) Do you have any comments or suggestions about the interview itself?
366
Appendix E
Hello, I am Sanna Nordin from the School of Sport and Exercise Sciences at The
University of Birmingham. Thank you for agreeing to participate in this interview! In this
project I am talking to dancers about why, when, and where dancers use imagery, and
what their imagery consists of. First of all, I am going to define imagery to you:
“Imagery is an experience that mimics real experience. We can be aware of ‘seeing’ an
image, feeling movements as an image, or experiencing an image of smell, taste or
sounds without experiencing the real thing. It differs from dreams in that we are awake
and conscious when we form an image.”
(White and Hardy, 1998).
Imagery is not simply talking to yourself, and it is not just watching demonstrations by
other people. A lot of the time it is seeing and feeling as if you were doing something
else. Imagery can be done whilst performing a movement and in an appropriate setting,
but just as well whilst being in a completely different environment, and being completely
still.
Do you have any questions about what I mean with imagery? Is this similar to how your
would define imagery?
As we go through the interview, I have definitions and examples of the various kinds of
imagery available so please ask if you are not quite clear about what I am asking for.
Also, do not worry if some questions seem repetitive. This is partly to make sure that I
am covering all aspects of your dance imagery and not leaving anything out, and partly
useful to obtain quotes for later analyses.
367
Appendix E (continued)
3) Describe how your use of imagery has developed over your dancing career?
i. i.e., is it different in any way from what it was to start with?
8) Describe what kinds of imagery your dance teacher involved in the lessons when
you were younger?
i. Elaboration probe: e.g. suggesting you could imagine a dance
routine in your head?
i. When was this? / How old were you then?
ii. What did this imagery involve?
iii. Was this a regular occurrence?
368
Appendix E (continued)
11) Describe any imagery you use that is based on skill learning and execution?
i. i.e. imagery based on technical instruction and doing things
correctly?
ii. e.g. imagining a double pirouette performed perfectly.
iii. elaboration probes:
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)
14) Describe any imagery you use relating to arousal and anxiety?
i. i.e. imagery based on stress, nervousness, stagefright etc.?
ii. e.g. imagining the excitement associated with performing,
imagining handling your stagefright?
iii. elaboration probes:
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)
369
Appendix E (continued)
15) Describe any imagery you use relating to self-confidence and mastery?
i. i.e. imagery based on being in control, being focused, and/or
working successfully through difficult situations?
ii. e.g. imagining feeling really confident when walking onto the
stage?
iii. elaboration probes:
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)
vii. i.e. imagery based on what it will be like to reach your goals?
viii. e.g. imagining great applause from the audience for your solo
performance, and/or how it would feel to get accepted to a certain
academy/school, or likewise?
ix. elaboration probes:
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)
370
Appendix E (continued)
3. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
4. What does it consist of? (content)
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)
22) Describe any other kinds of imagery you use, that is different from the ones we
have mentioned?
i. e.g. healing imagery?
ii. elaboration probes:
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)
23) Describe any other reasons that you have for using imagery, that are different
from the ones we have mentioned?
i. e.g. motivating yourself to go dancing?
ii. elaboration probes:
1. What does it consist of? (content)
371
Appendix E (continued)
Section 5: What
This section of the interview focuses on what senses you involve in your imagery. As you
know, you have the five senses of vision, hearing, smell, taste and touch. Additionally,
you can experience kinaesthetic imagery, and the emotion-based imagery we talked about
earlier.
24) To what extent do you use imagery relating to vision?
ii. What perspective does that imagery take?
1. i.e. is it Internal/External/Switching?
31) To what extent do you use imagery relating to sound and hearing?
36) Describe any other kinds of sensations that you feel in your imagery?
i. e.g. heat or other temperature changes? Pain?
37) Which of the above would be your primary senses in your imagery?
i. Which would be not so important/by-products?
ii.
Section 6: What continued: Imagery details
This section of the interview relates to some of the details of your imagery use.
38) Describe your ability to use imagery?
i. i.e. how good do you think you are at imaging?
ii. Elaboration probes:
Section 8: Conclusions
Ok, this just about wraps up the interview. However, before we finish, let me ask you a
couple of final questions:
42) How did you think the interview went?
45) Do you have any comments or suggestions about the interview itself?
373
Appendix F
374
Appendix G
Study Title:
An Exploratory Study of Vocal Professionals Use of Imagery: Applications of a Sport
Psychology Framework
Researcher:
Patricia L. Bowes
Ph.D. Candidate in Music Education, University of South Florida
813 985-1199, Cell: 561 523-2223
[email protected]
Researcher Qualifications:
Ph.D. Candidate in Music Education, University of South Florida
Bachelors and Masters Degrees in Music in Voice
Bachelors and Master Degrees in Art and Drama
Professional experience as solo vocalists
10 years teaching university classes in arts and music.
11 years experience as arts educator in public and private K-12 schools
Florida Teacher Certification: Music, Art, and Drama, to 2013
Participants:
The participants must:
(a) hold a four year degree in vocal performance, and
(b) have been working for at least four years making at least half their income as a solo
vocal professional
or
(c) if they have no performance degree, they have eight years making at least half their
income as a solo vocal professional.
375
Appendix G (continued)
Responsibilities of Participants:
Study participants will complete and sign the agreement to participate in this study. An
initial telephone discussion will clarify the purpose of the study, intention of participant’s
anonymity, intention to digitally record and transcribe verbatim the interviews, and the
use of their responses. Participants will participate in a telephone interview lasting from
30 to 75 minutes. After the interviews are transcribed using a pseudonym, they will be
sent to the participant for clarification or any changes they would like to make within five
business days. Upon receiving the responses, the results will be categorized according to
the framework of the four Ws of imagery use. Participants can request a copy of the final
copy of the results upon completion of the study.
376
Appendix H
Consent Script
Patricia Bowes
Invitation to Participate
Hello, my name is Patricia Bowes and I am a Ph.D. candidate in Music Education at the
University of South Florida, conducting a dissertation study on the use of imagery by
vocal professionals. You are invited to participate because you have been working as a
professional vocalist for at least 4 years with a musical performance degree or 8 years
without the degree and you make at least half your income in this manner.
Purpose
The purpose of this study is
1. To better understand the questions where, when, what, and why of imagery use by
professional vocalists
2. To link the use of imagery in sport psychology research to the use of imagery by
vocal professionals.
Description of Procedures
You will be interviewed by telephone in an open-ended interview that will take place
between 30 and 75 minutes, and with your permission, it will be audio-taped. The
interview will take place at an agreed upon time and date and the interviewer will call
you so as to incur no telephone charges to you. It is up to the participant to cover any
charges for their own incoming calls, if any. Please make sure that you set up a time
when you can concentrate and will not be interrupted for the duration of this interview.
Upon completion of transcribing the interview verbatim, you will be emailed a copy for
your revision and approval. You will be given 5 business days to return this document.
Upon your agreement, the document will be included in the data for the study.
Benefits
Although there is no direct benefit to you for participating in this study, I feel your
participation will likely benefit vocal teachers, students, and professionals. You may even
learn something about your own use of imagery that you didn’t know before. Upon
completion of the study, each participant will be emailed a link to a website where they
can read and/or download the study for your use.
Confidentiality
Interview tapes will be locked in a safe place. Only the research team will listen and
transcribe the information you give to us.
Interview responses will not be linked to your name, address, email, or telephone
number, and therefore there will be no follow-up sessions. You should know that the
University of South Florida Institutional Review Board (IRB) may inspect study records
as part of its auditing program, but these reviews only focus on the researchers and the
study, not on your responses or involvement. The IRB is a committee that reviews
research studies to make sure that they are safe and that the rights of the participants are
protected.
Voluntary Participation
Participation is voluntary. You do not have to participate in this study if you do not want
to. If you agree to be in this study, but later change your mind, you may withdraw at any
time. There are no consequences of any kind if you decide you do not want to
participate.
Questions
If you have any questions about this study I will be happy to answer them now. If you
have any questions in the future, please contact me Patricia Bowes, at 561 523-2223. If
you have any questions about your rights as a research participant, you may contact our
Institutional Review Board at 813 974-5638.
Consent
Now that you have been given the details of this study, I need to ask you for your verbal
agreement to participate in this study.
When would be a good time for the telephone interview, which should last between 30
and 75 minutes? _____ time ______ date
What number would you like me to call? _____________________________________
And may I have your email address, please? __________________________________
That’s great, so I will call you at ______ (time) on _______ (date) at
__________________________ (phone number).
I will email you this information so you can verify it. If there are any questions or
conflicts you will be able to tell me and we will adjust interview time accordingly.
I just want to thank you for taking the time to participate in this study.
379
Appendix I
Please answer the following questions by checking, or completing the appropriate spaces.
Personal Information
3. Age (check one): ___25-30, ___ 30-35, ___35-40, ___40-45, ___45-50, ___50-55,
Educational Information
6. Highest degree earned? __4 year degree __graduate degree _post graduate _doctorate.
380
Appendix I (continued)
11. Types of music primarily (most often) performed: __Opera, __Oratorio, __Recital,
381
Appendix J
Key of Emergent Codes and Categories/Themes
Code Categories/Themes
WR Where
WN When
382
Appendix J (continued)
Code Categories/Themes
WT What
IT Imagery Type
383
Appendix J (continued)
Code Categories/Themes
384
Appendix J (continued)
Code Categories/Themes
Imagery Characteristics
SI Sense Imagery
385
Appendix J (continued)
Code Categories/Themes
386
Appendix J (continued)
Code Categories/Themes
IA Imagery Ability
WY Why
IU Imagery Use
387
Appendix J (continued)
Code Categories/Themes
388
Appendix J (continued)
Code Categories/Themes
389
Appendix J (continued)
Code Categories/Themes
390
Appendix J (continued)
Code Categories/Themes
RI Review of Interview
391
About the Author
Patricia Bowes has taught music and art on the college level for over 10 years in
universities around South Florida. Dr. Bowes has trained pre-service music teachers,
instructed Elementary Music Methods and World Music Ensemble classes at the
University of South Florida and the University of Tampa. In addition, Dr. Bowes has
taught general and choral music, fine arts, and drama from K-12 for over ten years in
Florida public and private schools. Dr. Bowes holds a BA in Theatre from Indiana
University, a BFA in Fine Art, a BM in Music, an MAT in Music from Florida Atlantic
University, and an MFA in Theatre from the University of Arizona. Dr. Bowes has
presented lectures and workshops regionally as well as internationally. She has sung
professionally in New York City and South Florida in Western Classical and Jazz genres