BOWES - An Exploratory Study of The Use of Imagery by Vocal Professionals

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 406

University of South Florida

Scholar Commons
Graduate School Theses and Dissertations Graduate School

6-1-2009

An exploratory study of the use of imagery by vocal


professionals: Applications of a sport psychology
framework
Patricia Louise Bowes
University of South Florida

Follow this and additional works at: http://scholarcommons.usf.edu/etd


Part of the American Studies Commons

Scholar Commons Citation


Bowes, Patricia Louise, "An exploratory study of the use of imagery by vocal professionals: Applications of a sport psychology
framework" (2009). Graduate School Theses and Dissertations.
http://scholarcommons.usf.edu/etd/1867

This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at Scholar Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in
Graduate School Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Scholar Commons. For more information, please contact
[email protected].
An Exploratory Study of the Use of Imagery by Vocal Professionals:

Applications of a Sport Psychology Framework

by

Patricia Louise Bowes

A dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment


of the requirements for the degree of
Doctor of Philosophy
School of Music
College of The Arts
University of South Florida

Major Professor: C. Victor Fung, Ph.D.


Sheila Woodward, Ph.D.
Janet Moore, Ed.D.
Constance Hines, Ph.D.

Date of Approval
June 4, 2009

Keywords: music performance, music education, mental rehearsal,


singing performance, functional equivalence

 Copyright 2009, Patricia L. Bowes


Dedication

This dissertation is dedicated to my heart and joy, my son, David Alistair Bowes.

I further dedicate this study to all the current and future generations of imaginative and

creative performers around the world. I am hopeful that this investigation in imagery

helps to light your way and gives you greater ease in achieving optimal performance.
Acknowledgements

In the effort to prepare this document, it is my privilege to acknowledge the

assistance of a number of people to whom I extend my gratitude. First and foremost, I am

deeply indebted to my Committee Chair, C. Victor Fung for his tireless diligence and

integrity, whose direction and support, made dissertation possible and successful. To the

members of this research committee, Sheila Woodward, Janet Moore, and Constance

Hines, I offer my heartfelt thanks for all your support, guidance, and outstanding effort.

To the participants of this study, whose identities remain confidential, I thank you for all

the time, effort, and thoughtfulness you shared with me through the entire process and

finally supporting the completion of this research. Your contributions were most valuable

and I especially thank you for listening to your inner voice and developing your imagery

so diligently and extensively. I am thankful to the researchers who assisted in peer

debriefings and coding the transcriptions. To the many thousands of students I have

taught over the course of my life, thank you for helping me begin to learn and understand

imagery, creativity, excellence, and how all these work in life and in education. To all my

many teachers through the years who have taught me to be a better musician, artist, actor,

and teacher. To my mother, Paula Bowes, thank you for graciously editing one of the

early drafts. To my son, David, who offered kind support, excellent meals, and

continuous encouragement, I extend sincere appreciation. Finally, I am deeply grateful

and indebted to Ronald T. Jeffers, for his kind and generous support, dedication, and

helpful assistance in seeing me through all the levels of this eventful process.
Table of Contents

List of Tables ................................................................................................................vi

List of Figures ..............................................................................................................viii

Abstract..........................................................................................................................ix

Chapter 1: Introduction ................................................................................................... 1


Rationale ........................................................................................................... 10
Statement of the Problem ................................................................................... 15
Purpose of the Study .......................................................................................... 17
Research Questions............................................................................................ 19
Theoretical Framework ..................................................................................... 20
Where Imagery Was Used ..................................................................... 22
When Imagery Was Used ...................................................................... 23
What Was Being Imagined .................................................................... 23
Why Imagery Was Used ........................................................................ 24
Significance of the Study ................................................................................... 25
Definitions of Terms ......................................................................................... 28
Delimitations ..................................................................................................... 37
Limitations of the Study .................................................................................... 37
Organization of Remaining Chapters ................................................................ 42

Chapter 2: Literature Review ........................................................................................ 44


Theories of Imagery Use ................................................................................... 44
Early Developments and Theories of Imagery ........................................ 47
Psychoneuromuscular Theory ..................................................... 47
Symbolic Learning Theory ......................................................... 48
Cognitive-Based Theories ...................................................................... 49
Dual Code Theory ...................................................................... 49
Bio-Informational Theory .......................................................... 50
Triple Code Theory .................................................................... 51
Psychological States Theory................................................................... 52
Self-Efficacy Theory .................................................................. 53
Attention-Arousal Set Theory .................................................... 54
Functional Equivalence Theory ............................................................. 55
The Four Ws of Imagery Use ............................................................................ 58
Where or Location of Imagery Use ........................................................ 61

i
When or Time of Imagery Use .............................................................. 62
What: Content of Images ....................................................................... 64
What: Imagery Types ............................................................................ 65
Execution Images ...................................................................... 65
Metaphoric/Artistic Images ........................................................ 68
Character/Role Images ............................................................... 72
Context images .......................................................................... 74
Body-Related Images ................................................................. 75
Irrelevant images ....................................................................... 78
What: Imagery Characteristics ............................................................... 78
Senses......................................................................................... 79
Perspective ................................................................................ 85
Ability ........................................................................................ 87
Direction .................................................................................... 89
Deliberation................................................................................ 91
Amount and Duration ................................................................ 93
Why or Reasons for Imagery Use .......................................................... 95
Analytic Framework of Imagery Use ......................................... 96
Cognitive Reasons ..................................................................... 96
Motivational Reasons .............................................................. 103
Artistic Reasons ....................................................................... 111
Health Reasons ........................................................................ 112
Research in Imagery Use of Musical Performers.............................................. 113
Trusheim Study.................................................................................... 113
Carter Study ......................................................................................... 116
Bellon Study ........................................................................................ 117
Conclusion....................................................................................................... 119

Chapter 3: Method ...................................................................................................... 123


Research Design ............................................................................................. 123
Sample Selection ............................................................................................ 124
Measures ......................................................................................................... 127
Interview Protocol................................................................................ 127
Ethical Consideration ...................................................................................... 128
Procedures ....................................................................................................... 129
Timeline for Data Collection ................................................................ 131
Data Analysis Procedures .................................................................... 132
Legitimization ..................................................................................... 133
Trustworthiness ................................................................................... 136
Researcher Bias ................................................................................... 136
Frequency Counts ................................................................................ 137
Summary ........................................................................................................ 138

Chapter 4: Results ...................................................................................................... 139


Participants ..................................................................................................... 139
Interview Protocol and Process ....................................................................... 145
ii
Coding ............................................................................................................ 147
Where Do Vocal Professionals Use Imagery to Achieve Optimal
Performance?.................................................................................... 151
When Do Vocal Professionals Use Imagery to Achieve Optimal
Performance?..................................................................................... 153
What Do Vocal Professionals Use in their Imagery to Achieve Optimal
Performance?.................................................................................... 159
Imagery Types ..................................................................................... 160
Execution Images ..................................................................... 160
Metaphorical Images ................................................................ 168
Context Images ........................................................................ 170
Body-Related Images ............................................................... 171
Musical Sound Images ............................................................. 173
Character/Role Images ............................................................. 174
Imagery Characteristics ....................................................................... 176
Senses....................................................................................... 177
Perspective .............................................................................. 182
Imagery Ability ........................................................................ 183
Why Do Vocal Professionals Use in their Imagery to Achieve Optimal
Performance?.................................................................................... 187
Cognitive Reasons ............................................................................... 188
Learning Vocal Skills and Techniques (Cognitive Specific) ..... 188
Memorizing and Planning (Cognitive General) ........................ 198
Motivational Reasons .......................................................................... 203
Goal Attainment (Motivational Specific) ................................. 205
Arousal and Energy (Motivational General-Arousal) ............... 208
Self-Confidence and Mastery (Motivational General-Mastery) . 215
Artistic Reasons ................................................................................... 219
Vocal Quality ........................................................................... 219
Appearance............................................................................... 224
Character Development ........................................................... 227
Communication with Audience ................................................ 237
Healing Reasons .................................................................................. 242
Overall Health ......................................................................... 242
Vocal Health ............................................................................ 244
Summary ........................................................................................................ 246

Chapter 5: Summary of Findings, Implications, and Conclusions ................................ 248


Summary and Interpretation of Significant Findings ........................................ 248
Where Vocalists Used Imagery ............................................................ 248
When Vocalists Used Imagery.............................................................. 249
What Imagery Types Vocalists Used .................................................... 250
Execution Images ..................................................................... 251
Metaphorical Images ................................................................ 253
Context Images ........................................................................ 255
Body-Related Images ............................................................... 256
iii
Musical Sound Images ............................................................. 258
Character/Role Images ............................................................. 259
What Imagery Characteristics Vocalists Used....................................... 260
Senses....................................................................................... 261
Perspective .............................................................................. 262
Imagery Ability ........................................................................ 264
Why Vocalists Used imagery ............................................................... 268
Cognitive Reasons ................................................................... 268
Motivational Reasons .............................................................. 270
Artistic Reasons........................................................................ 275
Healing Reasons ....................................................................... 280
Four Ws of Imagery Use Framework for Singers.................................. 281
Limitations of the Study .................................................................................. 283
Sample Size ......................................................................................... 284
Sample Configuration .......................................................................... 284
Instrumentation .................................................................................... 285
Pilot Study ........................................................................................... 286
Interview Responses............................................................................. 287
Data Analysis ....................................................................................... 287
Researcher Bias.................................................................................... 288
Implications of the Study ................................................................................. 289
Location and Times ............................................................................. 289
Developing Mental Capacities.............................................................. 290
Matching Imagery Content With Function ........................................... 292
Mental Practice .................................................................................... 292
Metaphorical And Anatomical Images ................................................. 293
Context Imagery................................................................................... 295
Goal Attainment .................................................................................. 296
Modeling ............................................................................................. 296
Musical Sound Imagery ....................................................................... 297
Use of the Senses ................................................................................. 299
Body-Related Imagery ......................................................................... 300
Healing Imagery................................................................................... 301
Arousal Modification .......................................................................... 301
Self-Efficacy and Mastery .................................................................... 302
Character Development ....................................................................... 303
Communication with the Audience ...................................................... 304
Using Advanced Technology To Facilitate Imagery Development........ 304
Recommendations for Future Research ........................................................... 306
Replication of This Study..................................................................... 306
Imagery in the Performance Process..................................................... 307
Imagery for Vocal Production .............................................................. 307
Applications in Imagery Training......................................................... 308
Musical Performers’ Use of Imagery.................................................... 308
Sensory Imagery .................................................................................. 309
Children’s Use Of Imagery................................................................... 309
iv
Quantitative Studies ............................................................................. 310
Music Teachers’ Imagery ..................................................................... 310
Relationship of Flow and Imagery........................................................ 311
Functional Equivalence of Performers’ Imagery................................... 311
Cross-Disciplinary Studies ................................................................... 312
Summary ........................................................................................................ 313

References .................................................................................................................. 315

Appendices ................................................................................................................. 353


Appendix A Permission to Publish Imagery Framework ................................ 354
Appendix B Introductory Letter and Scripts: Vocal Professionals .................. 355
Appendix C Introductory Letter and Scripts: Vocal Professors....................... 357
Appendix D Singers’ Interview Guide .......................................................... 359
Appendix E Dancers’ Interview Guide ......................................................... 367
Appendix F Permission to Use the Dancers’ Interview Guide........................ 374
Appendix G Research Participant Request .................................................... 375
Appendix H Consent Script ........................................................................... 377
Appendix I Vocal Participant Survey............................................................ 380
Appendix J Key of Emergent Codes and Categories/Themes ....................... 382

About the Author................................................................................................ End Page

v
List of Tables

Table 1 Comparison of Potential Areas of Imagery Use among Athletes, Dancers,


Instrumentalists, and Solo Vocalists............................................................. 9

Table 2 Major theories explaining the effects of imagery on performance................. 46

Table 3 Analytic Framework of Imagery Effects ...................................................... 97

Table 4 Musical Mental Practice Studies Comparison............................................... 99

Table 5 Major Imagery Studies in Musical Performance ........................................ 114

Table 6 Proposed Framework for Vocalists’ Use of Imagery .................................. 121

Table 7 Research Questions Directly Addressed in Singer’s Interview Guide ........ 134

Table 8 Participants’ Age and Gender Distribution ................................................ 140

Table 9 Participant Pseudonym, Gender, Voice Range, Age Range, and Highest
Degree of Education ............................................................................... 141

Table 10 Participants’ Singing Genre Frequencies ................................................... 143

Table 11 Participants’ Professional Solo Singing Experience ................................... 144

Table 12 Vocal Participants’ Responses to Where They Used Imagery .................... 151

Table 13 Vocal Participants’ Responses to When They Used Imagery ..................... 154

Table 14 Vocal Participants’ Imagery Types, Categories, and Subcategories ........... 161

Table 15 Vocal Participants’ Specific Sense Imagery Use ........................................ 178

Table 16 Vocal Participants’ Rank Order of Sense Imagery Use .............................. 182

Table 17 Vocal Participants’ Imagery Perspective ................................................... 183

Table 18 Vocal Participants’ Imagery Ability .......................................................... 184

Table 19 Vocal Participants’ Cognitive Reasons for Imagery Use Categories .......... 189
vi
Table 20 Vocal Participants’ Motivational Reasons for Imagery Use: Categories,
and Subcategories .................................................................................... 204

Table 21 Vocal Participants’ Artistic Reasons for Imagery Use Categories .............. 220

Table 22 Vocal Participants’ Healing Reasons for Imagery Use Categories ............. 243

vii
List of Figures

Figure 1. Four Ws of Imagery Use Framework for Athletes ........................................ 59

Figure 2. Four Ws of Imagery Use Framework for Dancers ....................................... 60

Figure 3. Four Ws of Imagery Use Framework for Singers ....................................... 282

viii
An Exploratory Study of the Use of Imagery by Vocal Professionals:

Applications of a Sport Psychology Framework

Patricia Louise Bowes

ABSTRACT

The purpose of this study was to identify and describe the use of imagery in vocal

professionals’ efforts to achieve optimal performance based on research from sport

psychology. The four Ws of imagery use: where, when, what, and why have been applied

to this study from the fields of athletics (Munroe, Giacobbi, Hall, & Weinberg, 2000) and

dance (Nordin & Cumming, 2005). These were adopted after many years of research in

athletic performance excellence, with particular reference to the Analytic Framework of

the Cognitive and Motivational Functions of Imagery (Paivio, 1985; Hall, Mack, Paivio,

& Hausenblas, 1998). This theoretical framework was combined with those of previous

interview studies investigating professional musical performers in their use of imagery

(Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987) and provided a foundation for this study.

Theories from psychology explaining the effects of imagery were also integrated,

including functional equivalence and neuroimaging. An exploratory design was chosen to

investigate the comprehensive nature of this inquiry using a purposeful sample (N = 15).

A semi-structured interview based on the four Ws was conducted with 15 solo classical

vocal professionals, 10 females (sopranos and mezzos) and 5 males (tenors and a

ix
baritone) ranging in age from late twenties to late sixties, residing in several major cities

of the United States. Vocalists used imagery during practice, before performance, at

home, traveling, and a range of other times. Imagery content was divided into types and

characteristics. Vocalists’ use of imagery types encompassed execution, metaphorical,

context, body-related, musical sound, and character/role images. Imagery was employed

to perfect vocal production and quality, embody the character’s qualities and emotional

aspects for performance, visualize metaphorical and anatomical images to achieve proper

vocal execution, achieve goals, and communicate with the audience. Imagery

characteristics of vocalists involved visual, auditory, and kinesthetic senses, using

primarily internal and some external perspectives. These professionals’ imagery abilities

were deliberate, controllable, and positive. Similar to competitive athletes, solo vocal

performers engaged in imagery for many cognitive and motivational functions. Vocalists

replicated dancers in artistic, and healing functions of imagery use in preparing for and

achieving optimal performance.

x
Chapter 1

Introduction

Professional vocalists require exceptional capabilities to coordinate highly

technical, musical, and artistic skills in their performances. Technical skills involve many

aspects of correct vocal production, breath management and control, diction and text

articulation, often in many different languages. Skills in music require training in theory,

history and styles, and interpretation. Artistic skills involve those in acting, creative, and

affective methods of interpretation (Caldwell & Wall, 2001; Leyerle, 1986). Other areas

of expertise requirements include extensive memory capabilities, ability to perform in

front of live audiences, and being physically and mentally competent to cope with the

stresses of performance life. While these skills require varying degrees of cognitive

processing, developing the full faculties of the mind is often neglected in arts

performance training programs (Connolly, 2002; Connolly & Williamon, 2004; Dayme,

2006; Kohut, 1992). However, in sports, imagery and mental training have been given a

prominent position when preparing athletes to achieve performance excellence.

Imagery and its relationship to execution and outcome assume different meanings

for athletic and artistic performers. In vocal performance, imagery is often used as a

representation of metaphorical, poetic, artful pictures, feelings, or concepts that facilitate

specific performance requirements, be they dramatic, artistic, mechanical, or technical

(Averino, 1989; Freed, 2000; Miller, 1996; Moorcroft, 2002; Vennard, 1968). Vennard

(1967) defined imagery as “Figures of speech to express concepts which are difficult to

1
understand literally” (p. 261). The two major reasons that the vocal profession generally

practices imagery in this form could be related to vocal training being primarily an oral

tradition, and the practice of singing professionals who become teachers, passing on their

own experience and understanding of the voice (Freed, 2000). The fact that singing is

strongly based on experience and behavior, emphasizing knowing of the body rather than

knowing intellectually also reinforces the need for this imagery. However, based on

several qualitative research studies, it may be deduced that singers seem to use imagery

in a wide variety of ways (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993), including those detailed in sport

psychology literature (e.g., Morris, Spittle, & Watt, 2005; Murphy & Martin, 2002).

Artistic and musical performers may regard imagery differently from how it is

defined in the field of psychology (Richardson, 1983; Sheikh, 1983). In psychology,

imagery is distinguished from perception by how the individual responds to

environmental elements experienced through one’s senses and possibly filtered through a

number of personal cognitive and affective variables. Imagery includes internal

experiences from memory or fantasy where no outside stimulus is evident. Throughout

one’s lifetime, imagery can be conscious, subconscious, or unconscious and can take the

form of daydreaming, mentally running through past experience, future planning and goal

formation, and even spontaneous intuitive flashes. The following definition of imagery is

one of the most widely accepted and often quoted:

Mental imagery refers to (1) all those quasi-sensory or quasi-perceptual

experiences of which (2) we are self-consciously aware, and which (3) exist for us

in the absence of those stimulus conditions that are known to produce their

genuine sensory or perceptual counterparts, and which (4) may be expected to

2
have different consequences from their sensory or perceptual counterparts

[emphasis his]. (Richardson, 1969, pp. 2-3)

This definition has been borrowed and adapted extensively in sport imagery literature.

Perry and Morris (1995) described mental imagery as the “central pillar of applied

sport psychology” (p. 339). In this field imagery is considered a key component of mental

training in which “athletes can learn to use it systematically to aid performance, reduce

anxiety, increase confidence, enhance endurance, speed recovery from injury or heavy

exercise, and much more” (Morris et al., 2005, p. 5). Connecting the applications and

benefits of imagery in sport psychology with its use by professional singers may reveal

important similarities and differences, facilitating higher levels of performance success

beyond those achieved by traditional training methods.

Increasingly over the past few decades, the athletic profession has recognized that

developing the psychological faculties, particularly imagery, allows the athlete to achieve

outstanding results well beyond which have been derived from concentration on physical

practice and skill development (Gould, Eklund, & Jackson, 1992). These were often

referred to as peak experiences (Maslow, 1968) or optimal experiences

(Csikszentmihalyi, 1990), two terms that have often been used interchangeably. Their

meanings have been blurred in some of the literature; however, they are quite different in

their relationship to performance preparation and outcome. Distinguishing these two

terms is highly relevant to the present investigation and its relationship to consciously

controlled imagery in performance excellence.

Maslow (1954, 1968, 1970, 1971) initiated the psychological study of outstanding

human experience in his theory of hierarchy of needs, the penultimate of which was self-

3
actualization and the corresponding peak experiences. According to Maslow, peak

experiences are those involving cognitive and emotional changes including egoless

perception, complete and total attention, wonder and awe, disorienting time and space,

with a possible transcendence of unity, characterized by feelings of blissful joy, and

illumination. Maslow’s peak experience has the connotation that there is one outstanding

occurrence in an individual’s life representing the climax of an entire career at some

given point and all other experiences either lead to or occur after this.

By contrast, Csikszentmihalyi (1990) defined optimal experience, or flow, being

in the zone, or in the groove as controllable and attainable outstanding experiences.

Csikszentmihalyi explained:

[W]e feel a sense of exhilaration, a deep sense of enjoyment that is long cherished

and that becomes a landmark in memory for what life should be like. . . . The best

moments usually occur when a person’s body or mind is stretched to its limits in a

voluntary effort to accomplish something difficult and worthwhile. Optimal

experience is thus something that we make happen [emphasis his]. (p. 3)

Csikszentmihalyi continued “Because optimal experience depends on the ability to

control what happens in consciousness moment by moment, each person has to achieve it

on the basis of his own individual efforts and creativity” (p. 5). Therefore, the term

preferred in this study, optimal performance, described the results of mental and physical

efforts of performers, particularly athletic, artistic, or musical, in challenging themselves

to continually strive for excellence throughout their entire career.

4
Imagery plays an important part in achieving optimal performance and has been a

recognized element in vocalists’ experience. The outstanding American soprano, Phyllis

Curtin, gave this account of her experience of an optimal performance:

When singing is ‘right,’ it’s the most exhilarating physical and mental experience

imaginable or beyond imagination. It’s transcendental. It’s as if I’m not doing it at

all. It’s all happening out there and it’s all happening in my projected feeling and

imagination on that particular piece. (cited in Carter, 1933, p. 207)

This is just one example of the intermingling of imagination and flow in performance

when all the physical, mental, and musical elements were working together and seemed

to lift the individual out of normal consciousness beyond time into a heightened

experience of the present moment. In the effort to understand the many components of

optimal performance, sport psychologists have pursued this research area extensively

(Gould, Dieffenbach, & Moffett, 2002; Orlick, 1990; Orlick & Parrington, 1988).

Although imagery has been empirically investigated for over one hundred years

(e.g., Betts, 1909; Galton, 1880, 1883, 1907; James, 1890; Jastrow, 1892), extensive

progress has been made over the past forty years through hundreds of studies in sport

psychology (e.g., Morris et al., 2005; Murphy & Martin, 2002; Murphy, Nordin, &

Cumming, 2008; Weinberg & Gould, 2003). As behaviorism’s favor diminished,

imagery’s significance and applications began to be investigated (Holt, 1964).

Additionally, the human potential movement of the last decades of the twentieth century

and the popularity of a variety of publications focusing on performance enhancement

(Gallway, 1974; Maltz, 1960; Raiport, 1988) further stimulated growing interest in

imagery and mental practice in a variety of performance areas.

5
Athletes’ use of imagery and mental practice has been applied in all facets of

training, practice, competition, and performance and has been found to facilitate athletic

performance and increase success in a variety of sports as reported in several meta-

analyses (Driskell, Cooper, & Moran, 1994; Feltz & Landers, 1983; Hall, 2001;

Hinshaw, 1991). While the main focus of this research has been in studying the effects of

mental practice, imagery has also been associated with confidence enhancement (Callow,

Hardy, & Hall, 1998; Moritz, Hall, Martin, & Vadocz, 1996), reduction of anxiety and

stress (Vadocz, Hall, & Moritz, 1997), imagery ability (Rodgers, Hall, & Buckholz,

1991), imagery perspective, (Hale, 1982; Harris & Robinson, 1986; Hardy & Callow,

1999), and other psychological skills (Calmels, D’Arripe-Longuefille, Fournier, &

Soulard, 2003). The body of this research is so extensive that only those studies directly

contributing to understanding the use of imagery in sports as it specifically relates to

vocalists’ experience was considered in this study.

There is substantial anecdotal evidence that mental rehearsal and imagery practice

is a major part of elite athletes’, dancers’, musicians’, and singers’ professional

endeavors. For example, the world-class professional golfer, Jack Nicklaus, described

details of his use of imagery in achieving world status in golf (Nicklaus & Bowden,

1974). One of the more compelling anecdotal records of mental practice in musical

performance was that of a Chinese professional pianist. After six brutal years of

imprisonment, the second place winner of the 1958 International Tchaikovsky Piano

Competition, Liu Chi Kung, expertly played a concert with the visiting Philadelphia

Orchestra in Beijing, solely from mentally practicing the piano all those years (Korn,

1984). In the field of singing, the late Italian tenor, Luciano Pavarotti, identified mental

6
imagery as an extremely important element of his success (Günter, 1992a). Athletes and

musicians seem to share similarities in their applications of imagery that extend beyond

the effects of physical practice.

The similarities and contrasts of athletes and musical performers are worthy of

consideration with regard to imagery use. For a comprehensive comparison, see Table 1

below. The common practices of vocalists and athletes include: (a) use and complete

reliance of the body in performing; (b) highly specialized training and skill in

coordinating specific cognitive and motor behaviors in order to achieve even a modicum

of success (Hays, 2002); (c) the requirement of focus and concentration to perform both

physically and mentally at high standards of excellence often before a demanding

audience; (d) mental disposition which can positively or negatively effect the outcome of

a performance (Chapman, 2006; Emmons & Thomas, 1998); and (e) meeting the

challenge of competitive pressure to perform under various conditions, environments, and

expectations (Bellon, 2006; Gregg, Hall, & Hanton, 2007; Kohut, 1992). These

requirements of both athlete and artistic performer suggest that mental preparation is no

less vital to success than physical preparation.

There are five identified differences that set singers apart from athletes,

concerning the areas in which imagery could be applied. First, singers must control fine

motor mechanisms of the vocal apparatus, most of which are often not even visible to the

naked eye. Second, singers are required to develop much more discreet cognitive and

affective skills for executing expressive and artistic performances than athletes, such as

gaining facility with meaning and words in many languages (Caldwell & Wall, 2001;

Emmons & Thomas, 1998; Moyer, 1992). Third, singers are required to vocally,

7
physically, and artistically communicate directly to an audience. Fourth, vocalists must

employ creative and artistic expression through embodying various qualities of

characterization, such as subtext engagement, emotional dexterity, and expertise in

dramatic, historical, and cultural styles (Lehmann, 1945/1985; Hays, 2002). Finally,

singers must develop musical technique by becoming proficient in vocal production as

well as the rudiments of music, such as rhythm, pitch, harmony, and composition.

Illustrating the differences between classical solo vocalists and other performing

musicians is relevant to this study in regard to imagery uses. Members of chorus

ensembles and instrumental groups, such as symphonies and orchestras, are required to

read music notation when the music is not memorized and to watch the conductor, while

avoiding overt gazing at the audience. Unlike most ensemble performers, traditionally

singers must memorize their repertoire and communicate musically using text, often in

many different languages. Instrumentalists may perform as soloists but they generally do

not directly interact visually and are usually separated from the audience by their

instruments and sometimes by their music stands. Solo singers, particularly in recital and

oratorio performances, are generally the only musicians who perform directly facing the

audience with no instrument, conductor, or other performers to separate them from the

audience (Carter, 1993). This intimate and vulnerable position requires vocalists to

employ a high degree of mental skills including concentration in controlling unwanted

images, thoughts, self-criticisms, and other distractions, while maintaining positive self-

confidence, body image, and self-consciousness. Many of these skills, which successful

vocalists must master, represent requirements for cognitive and affective development

8
and training along with technical and artistic facility in being able to perform to

demanding and often critical audiences.

Table 1

Comparison of Potential Areas of Imagery Use among Athletes, Dancers,

Instrumentalists, and Solo Vocalists

Performance Athletes Dancers Instrumental Solo Vocal


Requirements Musicians Musicians
Artistic Affective/ Specific sports: Bodily, facial, Musical, artistic, Musical, artistic,

Expression Figure skating, artistic, and some affective expression, affective

Training artistic gymnastics affective expression versatility, verbal,

expression bodily, facial

Use of Sensory Kinesthetic & Kinesthetic, visual, Visual, aural, Visual, aural,

Modality in visual & aural kinesthetic, & kinesthetic, & some

Imagery some touch touch

Audience Not essential, Indirect but Indirect but Most direct

Interaction financially essential, essential, connection,

supportive financially financially financially

supportive supportive supportive

Arousal Optimal levels of Performance Performance Performance anxiety

Management arousal are trained anxiety often a anxiety often a often a major issue.

for individual and major issue. major issue. Breathing & full

sport Breathing & full Breathing & full preparation

preparation preparation

Health & Injury High stress High stress Repetitive strain Physical fatigue

Concerns training, overuse training, injuries injury from vocal health

syndrome, overuse syndrome problems

9
Rationale

The rationale for this study includes several important elements: (a) traditional

uses of imagery in vocal pedagogy as compared with those of psychology, (b)

terminology issues, (c) mental and physical practice research, (d) mental practice studies

using university students, (e) correlating sport psychology research with musical

performance, (f) imagery use in achieving artistic and musical performance excellence,

and (g) researchers’ call for empirical studies of imagery use by vocal performers.

Vocalists’ understanding of the function of imagery differs rather extensively

from that of psychology. Traditionally, imagery has been employed in learning and

executing musical skills in musical study and performance. For years, vocal pedagogues

have recognized the value of specific imagery applications in securing singing technique

and achieving artistic and emotionally sensitive performances (Brower & Cooke,

1920/1996; Fields, 1947; Lehmann, 1960; Leyerle, 1986; Vennard, 1968; Ware, 1998).

For the past century, general and sport psychologists have examined imagery extensively

in ways that extend beyond the conventional uses assumed in vocal pedagogy. According

to psychologists (e.g., Sheehan, 1983; Singer, 1966), imagery is a natural cognitive skill

that has been well exploited in various performance areas (Sheikh, 1983; Sheikh & Korn,

1994). Several vocal pedagogues (e.g., Cleveland, 1989a; Fields, 1972; Ware, 1998) have

embraced the psychological concepts of mental imagery and its function in training

singers. According to Günter (1992a), “It is my understanding that in artistic singing and,

consequently in vocal pedagogy, no satisfactory results can be obtained without mental

concepts, without training mental awareness, without training the ear, and without

training the ability of the imagination” (p. 46). It could be that the time has come to

10
update and extend vocalists’ comprehension and implementation of imagery in ways that

reflect more current and research-supported applications for the purposes of improving

performance outcome and success.

Use of imagery terms between sports and musical performance needs to be

explicated. The language of imagery in the sports context focuses on mental practices and

functions in relation to performance, such as skill learning and building, strategy of

execution, goal orientation, arousal, and mastery (Hall, Mack, Paivio, & Hausenblas,

1998; Paivio, 1985), while music imagery assumes metaphorical and artistic contexts in

skill learning and acquisition and in vocal literature interpretation (Carter, 1993: Fields,

1947; Lehmann, 1960). As dance researchers have embraced these sport-based imagery

terms, more aspects of imagery have been identified, including imagery types,

characteristics, and artistic qualities (Nordin & Cumming, 2005). Linking these terms in

how they are used in vocal performance could provide a foundation for applying the

findings of imagery research in sport psychology to musical performance.

In training individuals for athletic or artistic performance, practice has been

regarded as paramount in achieving success (Hallam, 1997; Jørrgensen & Lehmann,

1997). However, exclusive or extensive physical practice of a skill does not ensure

proficiency (LaFosse, 1973). The cognitive aspects of performance are often neglected in

many training programs (Kohut, 1992). Researchers agree that most motor skills begin in

the individual as a thought (Feltz & Landers, 1983; Oxendine, 1969; Richardson, 1969)

and operate on a cognitive to motor continuum (Feltz & Landers, 1983; Richardson,

1967a). While there are many athletic skills that are considered more motor than

cognitive, researchers have reported that appropriate mental practice and imagery use

11
have facilitated motor performance (Driskell et al., 1994; Feltz & Landers, 1983).

Singing could be considered more cognitive than motor, especially since the internal

vocal apparatus cannot directly be seen and most of the skills involved in coordinating

vocal performance excellence are largely controlled cognitively, affectively, and

internally. Therefore, perhaps even more than athletics, singers could benefit from

cognitive training, such as imagery, as a component of their practice and performance.

Sport imagery researchers, Murphy and Jowdy (1992), argued that imagery research

needs to extend beyond athletic performance, and said, “Future research needs to be

directed towards a better understanding of the roles that imagery plays in human

performance so that we can help all persons to optimally utilize their innate capacities”

(p. 245).

Mental practice studies, representing the majority of imagery investigations, have

been performed primarily in a controlled and sterile laboratory setting, using the ever

prevalent and convenient subject pool of university students (cited in Gregg et al., 2008).

In these studies, participants, content, and all relevant factors have been removed from

the natural environment, modifying the behavior and the results. Consequently, Yuille

(1985) and Lindauer (1983) have recommended investigating highly experienced subjects

in their natural practice and performance environment in order to gain a more complete

understanding of imagery use. These researchers suggest that university students too

often lack the maturity and understanding that seasoned professionals gain after years of

meeting the demands of performance requirements. It was also assumed that universities

and conservatories of music did not traditionally provide students with psychological

skills instruction, including imagery, although this trend seems to be changing (Caldwell

12
& Wall, 2001; Connolly, 2002; Connolly & Williamon, 2004; Emmons & Thomas,

1998). Musicians, teachers, and coaches may be unaware of or unfamiliar with the

benefits, techniques, and specific applications of imagery as used in sport psychology in

athletic or artistic performance. It is possible that professional singers would have a

greater understanding and knowledge of incorporating appropriate mental skills,

including imagery, after years of meeting the various challenges involved in the highly

technical and artistic endeavor of vocal performance.

The results of a study such as this could provide singing pedagogues and

professionals with an understanding of how singers’ imagery use directly relates to that

of professional athletes and dancers in achieving optimal performance. Too often highly

gifted, talented, and even trained singers abandon a professional career because of

seemingly unmanageable cognitive issues such as performance anxiety, lack of ability to

memorize, and insurmountable physical, psychological, and affective blocks that could

possibly be easily rectified using appropriate imagery techniques and applications

(Caldwell & Wall, 2001; Chapman, 2006; Emmons & Thomas, 1998; Freymuth, 1999).

Imagery use, as found in sport psychology research, offers techniques and skills which

could help singers overcome some of the well-known barriers to performance excellence,

such as performance anxiety, memory loss, or vocal problems. A detailed inquiry into the

various aspects of imagery use of professional singers may reveal if or how those

practices can be incorporated in training professional vocalists. This knowledge could

inform vocal practice and contribute to music performance research.

Professional vocalists may already use imagery techniques in achieving optimal

performance in ways that are similar to those employed by their elite athletic

13
counterparts. Texts in musical performance have promoted the development of imagery

and mental aspects of achieving excellence as it is used in sports (Green & Gallwey,

1986; Freymuth, 1999; Kohut, 1992). Other recent publications have been helpful in

providing a variety of useful mental practice applications in achieving vocal performance

excellence (Caldwell & Wall, 2001; Chapman, 2006; Emmons & Thomas, 1989; Ristad,

1982). Researchers have examined the use of imagery and mental practice in artistic

performers (Castellano, 1983; Hays, 2002; Rosenberg & Trusheim, 1989). Specifically,

imagery has been studied in dancers (Fish, Hall, Cumming, 2004; Hanrahan, & Salmela,

1990; Nordin & Cumming, 2005), musicians (Connolly, 2002; Freed, 2000; Gregg,

Clark, & Hall, 2008), brass players (Trusheim, 1987; Ross, 1985a, 1985b), pianists

(Coffman, 1987, 1988, 1990; Lim & Lippman, 1991; Rubin-Rabson, 1941; Sisterhen,

2005) and singers (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993). This literature strongly supports the link

between sport psychology imagery research and imagery use by vocal professionals.

However, relatively little research has been conducted directly linking these two areas.

A number of researchers have called for further study investigating the various

aspects of imagery in musical performance, particularly in the vocal profession (Bellon,

2006; Carter, 1993; Connolly, 2002; Murphy & Jowdy, 1992; Nordin & Cumming, 2005;

Trusheim, 1987). After investigating imagery in professional brass instrumentalists,

Trusheim (1987) recommended investigation in vocalists and wrote:

Perhaps singers have the most to gain from their imagery experiences. Vocal

production is perhaps the most personalized and individualized of all musical

approaches. Vocalists must rely on their own physical structure and its

manipulation for every aspect of their tone production. (p. 344)

14
There is therefore need for a study to determine how professional vocal performers use

imagery, the extent to which it is used successfully by them, and its relative value in

training professionals. If the use of imagery could provide the same excellent success in

vocal performance as has been documented in athletic performance, then it behooves

music educators, and particularly vocal instructors, to espouse and teach these imagery

practices in specific ways that have been employed by successful professionals.

Statement of the Problem

The use of imagery to achieve optimal performance in sports has been extensively

researched for the past fifty years (for a review see Morris et al., 2005) resulting in

widespread training and dissemination of the various components and aspects of its

applications in achieving athletic performance excellence. Elite athletes and those

performing at higher competitive levels were found to use imagery more than novices

and those with less skill and experience (Barr & Hall, 1992; Calmels et al., 2003; Driskell

et al., 1994; Feltz & Landers, 1983; Hall, Rodgers, & Barr, 1990; Hall, et al., 1998).

Although several exploratory studies have reported the various aspects of imagery use in

musical performers (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987), few investigations

have focused on the recent findings in imagery research in achieving optimal results in

sport psychology in relation to classical solo singing professionals (Gregg et al., 2008;

Gregg, Hall, & Nederhoff, 2005; Sisterhen, 2005).

Research and text in vocal pedagogy, which include mental training and imagery

applications in achieving optimal performance as described in sport psychology, is

limited and is rarely empirically supported. Carter (1993) summarized how voice teachers

have used imagery to assist singers in the following:

15
Imagery used by teachers was employed in five specific teaching situations: first,

to transcend or circumvent verbal explanations with regard to rhythm, line,

matching pitch, breath control, and tone quality. Second, imagery was used to

address defective or inadequate technique. Third, imagery was employed to

enhance good technique. Fourth, imagery was capable of connecting exterior

kinesthetic sensations and movements to less understood interior sensations of the

vocal mechanism. And last, imagery helped the singer to identify more closely

with textual and musical material, thereby bringing new freshness and spirit to

expressive interpretation of vocal literature. (Carter, 1993, p. 182)

These functions of imagery in this context have been employed for many years and

remain valid and important. The focus has been primarily on vocal technique acquisition

and strengthening and musical interpretation for performance. However, uses and

functions of imagery in the sport and dance areas may be more pervasive and offer more

advantages to performers than the above description.

While vocal performers have used imagery, particularly metaphors and artistic

applications for many years (Averino, 1989; Carter, 1993; Fields, 1945; Lehmann,

1945/1985), only recently has imagery, as it is defined and used in sport psychology,

been included in vocal performance literature (Caldwell & Walls, 2001; Dayme, 2005,

2006; Emmons & Thomas, 1998; Günter, 1992a, 1992b; Williamon, 2004). It is possible

that professional vocalists have learned to use imagery for performance excellence as an

element intrinsic to the nature of singing, by their own efforts, or by some supplementary

method or training. If singers already use imagery as defined in sport in their professional

efforts, this could have serious implications on vocal training and performance

16
possibilities. Understanding specific details of the four Ws of imagery use: where, when,

what, and why from research in sport (Munroe, Giacobbi, Hall, Weinberg, 2000) and

dance (Nordin & Cumming, 2005) as it relates to vocal performance, may be especially

relevant to singers striving to be able to access their optimal performing abilities and

achieving success in an enduring professional career. It is possible that professional

vocalists’ use of imagery has not been identified in terms heretofore accepted by the

vocal profession or linked to what has been considered imagery and its employment as

such in athletics for many years.

Increasing understanding of professional vocalists’ uses of imagery could serve

singers in a variety of ways, including these seven: (a) begin to determine the relevance

of sport and dance imagery research, and applications to that of singing excellence, (b)

help to create a foundation of understanding of terms and usage, (c) identify specific

content and purposes of imagery in achieving optimal vocal performance, (d) facilitate

specific guidelines for training, (e) direct effective applications and interventions, (f)

guide further research, and (g) develop and provide an imagery framework specific to

these vocal performers. The researcher has found only a paucity of the literature available

in English pertaining to the systematic study of the use of imagery in vocal performance,

especially those correlated to imagery research in sport and dance. It was therefore

believed that investigation in this field would provide an important contribution to the

vocal performance profession.

Purpose of the Study

The purpose of this study was to investigate imagery use in achieving optimal

performance in vocal professionals. Research from sport psychology and particularly the

17
framework of the four Ws of imagery use: where, when, why, and what, (Munroe et al.,

2000; Nordin & Cumming, 2005) was used to compare and contrast singers’ imagery

experiences in order to contribute to the body of knowledge of imagery in musical

performance practice and development. This study sought to bridge the gap between

music performance research, dance, and sport psychology in the uses of mental imagery.

Questions applied in the study were based on general and sport psychology, dance, and

music imagery research as it related to vocal performance. The study focused on solo

singers for their extensive professional experience. It was assumed that these vocalists

were more adept at applying imagery in their artistic endeavors and professional efforts,

and were more extensively developed and grounded in the practical requirements of a

vocal career than novice singers in a university training program (Bellon, 2006; Carter,

1993; Yuille, 1985).

The four Ws framework previously applied to athletes (Munroe et al., 2000) and

dancers (Nordin & Cumming, 2005) addressing where (location), when (time periods),

what (content and qualities), and why (functions) of imagery were adopted in this

exploratory study for vocal professionals. It was hypothesized that similar themes as

those found in athletes and dancers would emerge, including uses of imagery as it related

to the five areas: skill acquisitions and refinement, strategy of performance, goal-oriented

activities, motivational arousal, and mastery. Additionally, it was thought that themes of

artistic and healing imagery as reported in dance research would also appear. It was

further thought likely that new significant themes of imagery applications would emerge

that had not been identified in adult athletes or dancers, which were exclusive to the

experience of solo vocal performers. It was anticipated that compilation of this

18
information would lead to the creation of a framework of the use of imagery specific to

vocal professionals. Furthermore, the results from this study could have implications in

developing models for teaching imagery to vocal and instrumental musical performers.

Research Questions

The research questions addressed in this study employed the framework of the

four Ws of imagery use: where, when, what, and why, and are outlined below.

1. To what extent do vocal professionals use imagery in achieving optimal

performance?

2. Where do vocal professionals use imagery to achieve optimal performance?

3. When do vocal professionals use imagery to achieve optimal performance?

4. What do vocal professionals use in their imagery to achieve optimal performance?

5. Why and for what purpose do vocal professionals use imagery to achieve optimal

performance?

Employing an inductive and deductive approach seemed appropriate for the study for

several reasons (Patton, 2002). Deductive aspects were represented in the findings of

previous studies investigating imagery use incorporating the above framework for

athletes (Munroe et al., 2000) and dancers (Nordin & Cumming, 2005) and the analytical

framework of the cognitive and motivational functions of imagery (Paivio, 1985; Hall et

al., 1998). The author hypothesized that some aspects of imagery use by vocal performers

would be similar to that of elite athletes, dancers, and musical performers (Bellon, 2006;

Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987), which represented the deductive portion of the analyses.

Additionally, inductive analysis allowed for the possibility of new categories to emerge

from the data (Patton, 2002), since very little was known about how professional singers’

19
use of imagery was related to the above questions and frameworks. The following

theoretical framework represented some of the major developments and research in the

use of imagery in sport psychology and relevant findings in musical performance studies.

Theoretical Framework

The theoretical framework of this study of imagery in vocal performance has its

roots in historical psychology theory, extensive studies in sport psychology, and the

limited research in imagery use by music professionals. Early imagery theories emerged

with the development of the study of psychology (e.g., Carpenter, 1874; Galton, 1880)

including various theories of psychology (Bandura, 1977; Lang, 1979a; Neisser, 1976)

and the most recent developments of neuro-imaging in brain research (Decety, 1996a,

1996b; Jeannerod, 1994). Imagery in sport psychology began being investigated more

rigorously in the past three decades and included various elements such as comparing the

efficacy of physical with mental practice (e.g., Driskell, Cooper, & Moran, 1994), use of

imagery abilities (Hall, 1998), imagery perspectives (Hale, 1982; Callow & Hardy,

2004), and use of imagery by elite athletes (Orlick, 1990). These and the more recent

investigations of imagery in sports generally focused on various areas which provided

many quantitative answers to the questions: where, when, what, and why, through a

variety of methods. However, it became apparent that exploring elite professionals’

personal experiences of imagery proved more valuable in gaining in-depth understanding

of varieties of its practical application. The four Ws of imagery framework was

developed and implemented to gather a more complete spectrum of the use of imagery by

elite athletes (Munroe et al., 2000), which also served to clarify and corroborate the

previous findings gathered in quantitative research. Subsequently, this framework was

20
used to investigate imagery by dancers (Nordin & Cumming, 2005), which revealed more

relevant information of artistic uses of imagery than those found in sports. Several studies

providing in-depth interviews of vocal and instrumental imagery use were also relevant to

this study.

Numerous theories have attempted to explain imagery’s effects in human

performance. One of the earliest to develop was the psychoneuromuscular theory, which

resulted from the work of James (1890) and Jacobson (1927, 1929, 1930a, 1930b, 1930c,

1930d, 1931), posited that mental imagery produced muscular innervation in the specific

muscles involved in the imagery. In symbolic learning theory, Sackett (1934, 1935)

argued that mental imagery facilitated memory in creating symbols for learning, which

facilitated subsequent performances. Paivio’s (1971) dual coding theory proposed that

two different codes in imagery, the verbal and the image, aided memory and facilitated

learning. In the bio-informational theory, Lang (1977, 1979a, 1979b) argued that imagery

was composed of stimulus and response propositions, which enhanced learning and

performance. In the self-efficacy theory, Bandura (1977, 1997) proposed that imagery

assisted the development of confidence and positive behavior in the individual,

improving performance. Schmidt’s (1982) attention-arousal set theory put forward that

imagery assisted in accessing specific optimal levels of attention and arousal in

performance preparation.

As a result of the recent technological advances in medical imaging, functional

equivalence theory held that mental practice and the experiences of images were

functionally equivalent to specific aspects of actual physical practice of the same activity

(Decety, 1996a, 1996b; Farah, 1984; Jeannerod, 1994, 1995; Kosslyn, Ganis, &

21
Thompson, 2001). Many of these studies found that areas and pathways of the brain that

were activated during imagery were the same as those that occurred in the mental

preparation of an individual just prior to engaging in that act. This field of research has

potentially many implications for using imagery for practice and performance purposes.

Many of these theories addressed certain aspects of imagery effects in

performance, however they may not have addressed all the issues involved. More

importantly, these theories may vary in their application to imagery use by professional

singers. Munroe and others (2000) first examined the four Ws of imagery use in

answering the questions: where, when, what, and why did athletes use imagery. Where

and when dealt with the time and place in which the individuals actually engaged in

imagery. What was being imaged represented the content and qualities of imagery and the

reasons and functions of imagery use were addressed in the category of why. Studies

relevant to the various uses of imagery in performing artists also addressed some of these

issues and included dancers (Fish et al., 2004; Nordin & Cumming, 2005), musicians

(Bellon, 2006; Ross, 1985a, 1985b; Trusheim, 1987), pianists (Coffman, 1990; Sisterhen,

2005), and singers (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993).

Where Imagery Was Used

The location, or where, imagery was previously reported in direct relation to

athletes’ practice and competition settings (Weinberg & Gould, 2003). Athletes seemed

to use imagery most in direct relation to competition and less in practice (Hall et al.,

1990; Salmon, Hall, & Haslam, 1994). Athletes used imagery in almost any setting,

including at home, work and school, (Salmon et al., 1994), and particularly in bed at

22
night (Hall et al., 1990; Hall & Buckholz, 1991). Some athletes reported using more

imagery outside of practice or competition settings (Salmon et al., 1994).

When Imagery Was Used

With regard to when imagery was being used, sport competitors were found to

engage in imagery just prior to competition, less during and following competition or in

practice settings (Hall et al., 1990; Munroe, Hall, Simms, & Weinberg, 1998). Athletes

were also found to use imagery more during practice than beforehand or afterwards

(Salmon et al., 1994). Imagery use in relation to competition was regarded as

performance enhancement, while using imagery in the context of practice was considered

skill learning (Munroe et al., 2000). Outside of these times, athletes used imagery on

breaks in their normal day (Salmon et al., 1994), and particularly at night just before

sleeping (Hall et al., 1990; Hall & Buckholz, 1991). These findings were also reported in

dancers’ imagery (Nordin & Cumming, 2005).

What Was Being Imagined

The category, what, entailed the content and quality of imaging. Traditionally

imagery research concentrated on laboratory studies comparing the efficacy of mental

practice to physical practice (Driskell et al., 1994; Hinshaw, 1991, Feltz & Landers,

1993). Over the years, the content of athletes’ imagery research was expanded and

included use of the senses, sessions, effectiveness, controllability, and nature of imagery

as defined in by Munroe and others (2000). In investigating dancers’ imagery use, Nordin

and Cumming (2005) identified two major traits: imagery types and imagery

characteristics. Imagery types encompassed execution, metaphorical, context, character

and role, those that related to the body, and irrelevant images. Imagery characteristics

23
dealt with qualities of the image itself and included the various senses, perspective,

ability, direction, deliberation, amount, and duration.

Why Imagery Was Used

Paivio (1985) generated the analytic framework of functions of imagery use and

identified two main reasons that athletes’ use of imagery, cognitive and motivational.

This model was later extended by Hall and others (1998), resulting in five distinct

categories: (a) cognitive specific, or skill and technique building; (b) cognitive general, or

strategy applications; (c) motivational specific, as in goal attainment; (d) motivational

general – arousal, in “psyching up” or calming down of arousal or affect; and (e)

motivational general – mastery, such as being mentally tough, having self-control, or in

control in performance. This analytic framework was the result of years of development

through numerous studies in sports on imagery.

Based on this framework, sport psychologists investigated the use of imagery in

various athletic fields (Barr & Hall, 1992; Hall et al., 1990; Rodgers et al., 1991). This

led to the development of the Sport Imagery Questionnaire (SIQ) (Hall et al., 1998; Hall,

Stevens, & Paivio, 2005), while valid and reliable in assessing athletes’ use of imagery,

which seemed to be inadequate in reflecting the full use of imagery, including metaphor,

artistic, health, and spontaneous images of dancers (Fish et al., 2004; Monsma & Overby,

2004; Nordin & Cumming, 2005) or classical musicians (Gregg et al., 2008). Using the

SIQ or other imagery instruments did not seem to be able to adequately represent the

entire picture of how singers use imagery. Gregg and colleagues (2008) adapted the Sport

Imagery Questionnaire (SIQ) (Hall et al., 1998; Hall et al., 2005) for use with musicians,

resulting in the Functions of Imagery in Music Questionnaire (FIMQ). Although using

24
this instrument uncovered many similarities between imagery use by musicians and

athletes, it failed to reveal many aspects of musicians’ imagery, particularly auditory

sense imagery and musical and artistic characteristics and functions. Therefore the more

broadly based exploratory inquiry of the four Ws of imagery use of Munroe and others

(2000) and Nordin and Cumming (2005) was chosen for the purpose of understanding the

use of imagery by professional vocalists.

Several investigations have focused on musical performers’ use of imagery in

elite brass orchestral instrumentalists (Trusheim, 1987) and top vocal professionals

(Carter, 1993). Although these studies were extensive and valuable, these researchers

used artistic and creative frameworks of imagery and included only minimal research of

imagery in sport psychology. Bellon’s (2006) study examined the correlations of specific

musical performers and their incorporation of the main elements of sport psychology, of

which imagery was a part. These studies were examined individually with regard to their

relevance to imagery use in vocal professionals and in relation to the four Ws framework

in the following chapter.

Significance of the Study

It was anticipated that the present study might provide important contributions to

the literature on vocal performance practice, with implications about its application in

vocal pedagogy. As an outgrowth of Freud’s psychoanalysis and behaviorism in the

1960s and 1970s, Maslow’s (1968, 1971) humanistic psychology promoted the trend of

human potential movement in sport and later artistic performers. After decades of

research and training in the applications of imagery for achieving performance excellence

in sports, music researchers as well as pedagogues had recently begun to recognize the

25
potential for similar success in musical performance using these similar cognitive

methods (Caldwell & Wall, 2001Connolly, 2002; Connolly & Williamon, 2004; Dayme,

2005). Investigators and psychologists who had experience and understanding in the

positive effects of imagery were found to be “demonstrating anew that people tutored in

the use of imagery skills can perform at remarkable levels in comparison to those

common to rote learning situations” (Sheehan, 1972, p. xiv). It was suggested that, by

applying the advances achieved in the research of imagery in sport psychology over the

past forty years (Morris et al., 2005; Murphy et al., 2008), singers’ uses of imagery would

compare favorably and therefore appropriate findings in sports and dance research could

be applied in vocal programs and singing practices.

Seashore (1939) recognized the importance of including applied psychology and

other sciences in vocal performance. In an effort to merge scientific innovation with

voice development, he invited vocal pedagogues to incorporate the new developments in

science with vocal training in the hopes of enhancing this ancient art. He stated:

The science of voice draws upon many fundamental sciences; notably physics,

physiology, anatomy, anthropology, neurology and psychology. . . . [I]t has

become the function of psychology to integrate these basic scientific approaches

into an applied science which we may call the ‘psychology of vocal arts.’ (p. 340)

Over the years, extensive research has been conducted to this end (Deutsch, 1999). With

the exception of several studies (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Gregg et al., 2008), the

researcher was only aware of a paucity of investigations focusing on the psychology of

imagery use in musical and singing performance. It was hoped that this study would help

bridge the gap of imagery research in sport and dance with music and vocal performance

26
research. In examining the uses of imagery in solo vocal professionals, findings were

compared with those in sport and dance, the results of which could be applied in the

vocal training programs.

Generally, music students have been left to their own devices as to how to

mentally cope with the many challenges they had to face in their training (Chapman,

2006; Dayme, 2006; Emmons & Thomas, 1998; Freymuth, 1999). A number of cognitive

aspects of musical performance may have a direct relationship to sport concerns, such as

learning and perfecting skills and strategies, setting and achieving goals, preventing or

grappling with arousal problems, and performance anxiety. It was suggested that, in

identifying and analyzing the special mental skills and practices of professional

musicians and singers, teachers, professors, and coaches may be better prepared to train

and equip their students with the skills necessary for optimal performance as has been

practiced in sports training for decades. If imagery could be used successfully to deal

with the normal psychological, technical, or affective problems that arise in the practice

and performance of singing, many more talented musicians and singers could enjoy and

participate in the profession in which they may have exceptional talent. The gap in vocal

research of addressing the psychological considerations related to imagery use of singers

could begin to be filled, assisting future generations of performers in achieving

excellence. It was also possible that implementing early training in imagery could assist

singers in coping with previously insurmountable blocks that could be alleviated and

possibly eliminated all together.

While vocal pedagogues have begun including some mental aspects of singing in

their texts (Caldwell & Wall, 2001; Chapman, 2006; Emmons & Thomas, 1998;

27
Williamon, 2004), exploratory studies such as this could add to the growing body of

research on the uses of imagery in musical performers. Few investigations have been

conducted on the use of imagery by vocalists based on imagery research in sport

psychology (Bellon 2006; Gregg et al., 2008; Stedman, 1985). Gaining an understanding

of professional singers’ experience and use of imagery could assist vocal teachers and

students in applying effective practices, skills, and concepts in creatively using imagery

to reach beyond skill proficiency and achieve optimal performance in the professional

arena. A study such as this could contribute to facilitating and understanding how

imagery use can enhance vocal performance. The findings could possibly extend into

other forms of musical performance and training, contributing to the body of literature in

imagery use for optimal performance. A full inquiry into imagery uses in professional

vocalists could benefit the body of knowledge in all three areas: sport, dance, and musical

performance, especially in applied fields and training programs.

Definitions of Terms

The following is a list of terms pertinent to this study and is not meant to

represent general use of imagery, sport psychology, musical, or vocal performance. The

terms and their definitions are listed in alphabetical order.

• Audiation was a term coined by Gordon (1976, 2003a) who rejected the label of

auditory image because of its connotation with the visual sense and lack of its ability to

describe the internal process of music cognition. He defined audiation in the following

way:

Audiation is to music what thought is to language. Audiation takes place when we

hear and understand in our minds music that we have just heard performed or

28
have heard performed sometime in the past. . . . We also audiate when we hear

and understand in our minds music that we may or may not have heard but are

reading in notation or are composing or improvising. We may audiate while we

are reading music. (Gordon, 1999, p. 41)

Audiation was also defined as the process of internally hearing and understanding

musical elements such as rhythm, pitch, and tonality in sequences and patterns (Gordon,

1993). Audiation differed from inner hearing or auralizing (Karpinski, 2000), pitch

internalization (Klonoski, 2003), and auditory imagery in that it dealt with elements and

patterns of musical sounds that had meaning and required understanding in context

(Klonoski, 2003). (Auditory imagery is further defined later in these definitions.)

• Body image was identified as an individual’s mental concept of the external

physical appearance of their body (Nordin & Cumming, 2005). Singers’ body image

could differ in how they regarded themselves on or off stage (Carter, 1993).

• Cognitive psychology was defined as the study of the process whereby people

transformed, reduced, elaborated, stored, recovered, and used sensory input (Neisser,

1976).

• Goals were images of concepts, skills, or behaviors that were created and held for

future achievement, exemplified in the charge “[b]egin with the end in mind” (Covey,

1989, p. 95). Goals in performance or competition were separated into outcome,

performance, and process.

o Outcome goals “typically focus on a competitive result of an event, such as

winning a race, earning a medal, or scoring more points than an opponent”

29
(Weinberg & Gould, 1999, p. 308). Singers set goals and worked for many

years striving to win coveted competitions and roles.

o Performance goals “focus on achieving standards or performance objectives

independently of other competitors, usually making comparisons with one’s

own previous performances” (Weinberg & Gould, 1999, p. 308). Individuals

were more able to control the achievement of these types of goals than outcome

goals. For vocalists, these goals may have included presenting a character in

opera in a way more believable to an audience, or polishing a particular vocal

skill throughout a concert.

o Process goals “focus on actions an individual must engage in during

performance to execute or perform well” (Weinberg & Gould, 1999, p. 308).

Gaining better breath control on a specific section of an aria would be

considered a singer’s process goal.

• Imagery derived from several Latin terms, imago, “a likeness,” im-itari, “to

imitate,” imaginer “to imagine, think,” and imaginatus “to picture one’s self” (Skeat,

1943, p. 282). For many years Richardson’s (1969) definition set the standard in the

majority of research studies, particularly in sport psychology (see definition in

Introduction). The following represented a comprehensive definition of imagery use in

sport psychology:

Imagery is intriguing for its close relationship to perception and action. It is such

a rich memory system, matching the complexity of information presented by the

environment and contained in the execution of motor skills. Images bind personal

thoughts and emotions to experience. . . . Imagery can be creative, allowing one to

30
experience attitudes and actions mentally in ways that have not yet been

encountered in real performance. (Simons, 2000, p. 92)

Imagery has had many other terms such as picture-like visual imaginations,

conceptualization, ideational functioning, introspection, imaginary practice, implicit

practice, conceptualizing practice, sofa training, symbolic rehearsal, visualization, and

many others (Murphy & Martin, 2002).

• Imagery ability was defined as “an individual’s capacity of forming vivid,

controllable images and retaining them for sufficient time to effect the desired imagery

rehearsal” (Morris, 1997, p. 37). Ability to imagine and manipulate images could be

developed over time, with an individual’s conscious effort and support from a

knowledgeable guide. For the purposes of this study, imagery ability also included

imagery controllability, direction, deliberation, and the degree of vividness.

oControllability referred to “the capacity of the individual to generate not

only vivid but also persistent images, yet persistent only for the time they

should be used, that is, both persistent and interruptable [sic] on request”

(Denis, 1985, p. 8S). Controllability dealt with how easily an image was

manipulated by the intentions of the individual.

oDirection of imagery was whether the image was helpful and facilitative or

hurtful and debilitative to the person imagining (Nordin & Cumming,

2005).

oDeliberation of imagery was degree to which the individual spontaneously

or purposefully created the image (Nordin & Cumming, 2005).

31
oVividness connoted clarity of detail, which has had implications on internal

processing. Vivid imagers learned more than poor imagers (Marks, 1977).

• Mental practice (MP) was a specific type of imagery wherein the person imagined

motor movements and cognitive processes, thought about the activity, practiced various

aspects of a skill, both at the initial stages of learning through the end stages of

performance, without external or physical movement (Denis, 1985).

• Metaphoric image referred to the transference of word meanings in relation to

issues, concepts, or even skills that could not readily, fully, or accurately be described in

factual terms. It was derived from the Greek “metapherein, to carry over” meaning “a

figure of speech containing an implied comparison, in which a word or phrase ordinarily

and primarily used of one thing is applied to another” (Guralnik, 1982, p. 893). It also

came from the Latin word, metaphora meaning “a transferring of a word from its proper

signification to another” (Skeat, 1943, p. 365). Metaphoric image was used extensively in

effective expression in vocal and instrumental music (Barten, 1992, 1998). An example

of the use of imagery in vocal production was:

The projection of imagery falls under two general headings: mimicry and sensual

awareness. In the first category, directives such as ‘imitate a steamboat whistle’ or

‘produce a hooty tone quality’ are often useful in the conceptualization of a pure

falsetto (Reid, 1983, p. 156).

More traditional metaphors used by singers include, “Breathe in as if smelling a rose. . . .

Breathe in as if beginning a yawn. . . . Breathe in as if drinking a glass of water”

(McKinney, 1994, p. 55-56).

32
• Modeling was also known as vicarious experience, identified in Bandura’s (1977)

social learning and self-efficacy theories as one of the components of successful

performance expectations. It was referred to as:

a process in which observers copy or reproduce behaviors or actions

demonstrated by others. The idea is that imagining oneself performing a task

successfully is similar to observing someone else perform the skill (modeling, or

overtly performing the skill (past experience) and therefore provides

reinforcement and increased expectations of success (Morris et al., p. 48).

• Motivation referred to “the hypothetical construct used to describe the internal

and/or external forces that produce the initiation, direction, intensity and persistence of

behavior” (Vallerand & Thill, cited in Short et al., 2006, p. 54). Motivation in this study

included the energy gathered to inspire drive and enthusiasm to accomplish some action,

task, feat, or performance.

• Optimal experience, also known as flow, was a term coined by Csikszentmihalyi

(1990) “the state of mind in which people are so involved in an activity that nothing else

seems to matter” (p. 4) and “being in the zone,” or the feeling of performance excellence

(see discussion in Introduction earlier in this chapter).

• Perception stemmed from the Latin, percipiere, “to take hold of . . . to become

aware of through sight, hearing, touch, taste, or smell” (Guralnik, 1984, p. 1054).

“Perception is the larger process of making meaningful sense out of sensation” (Klinger,

1981, p. 2). This differed from imagination in which the individual was not responding

directly with any one of the senses to stimuli in the environment. Neisser (1972) argued

that imagery lay at “the intersection of memory and perception” (p. 233).

33
• Perspective in imagery referred to how the item or occurrence was being

experienced, that is, either internally or externally.

o Internal imagery was the “first person perspective” (Denis, 1985) or

“involvement” imagery (Lane, 1980) requiring “an approximation of the real-life

phenomenology such that the person actually imagines being inside his/her body

and experiencing those sensations which might be expected in the actual

situation” (Mahoney & Avener, 1977, p. 137). Imagining the experience “from

the first-person perspective which enables the singer to replicate a performance

of practice situation utilizing kinesthetic imagery” (Moyer, 1992).

o External imagery involved the “third person perspective” (Denis, 1985) or

“spectator” imagery (Lane, 1980) in which a person viewed himself/herself as an

outside observer, as if watching a video (Epstein, 1980; Hale, 1982; Mahoney &

Avener, 1977).

• Professional singer/vocalist, for the purposes of this study, was a male or female

singer, trained at a university or conservatory, who had earned at least half of his

livelihood by singing solo in public. These particular singers had performed

professionally in varying venues including opera, recital, classical concert, and chamber

works for at least four consecutive years.

• Self-talk was an individual’s internal “verbalizations or statements addressed to

the self. . . . serving at least two functions; instructional and motivational” (Hardy, 2005,

p. 84). These were mental directions or comments a singer processed internally.

• Sense imagery was categorized into six different divisions: (a) visual, or sight; (b)

auditory, or hearing; (c) kinesthetic, or bodily feeling; (d) tactile, or feeling of touch; (e)

34
olfactory, or smell, and (f) gustatory, or taste. The three senses: visual, auditory,

kinesthetic, and combining several of these were most reported in musical performance

studies (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987) and are further defined here:

o Visual imagery connoted the use of the sense of sight to see a scene from

either the internal or external perspective (see perspective definition

above). It included the mode of visual representation of objects and

actions.

o Auditory imagery, also known as auralization (Martin, 1952), represented

hearing sounds internally when they were not present including audience

applause, environmental sounds, and other musical and nonmusical

sounds. In music, auditory imagery assumed these internal sounds to be

various aspects of musical production such as tone, rhythm, and timbre

(Seashore, 1938/1967). In the musician, auditory imagery was most

developed when it became audiation (Gordon, 1999) (see this definition

above).

o Kinesthetic imagery, also known as “kinesthetic simulation” (Hackford &

Munzert, 2005, pp. 4-5), was derived from “Kinesthesia: Greek kinein, ‘to

move,’ and aesthesis, ‘perception;’ an awareness of movement through

sense impressions caused by changes in the internal state of the body”

(Reid, 1983, p. 169). According to Callow and Waters (2005), kinesthetic

mode was: “[i]magery involving the sensations of how it feels to perform

an action, including the force and effort involved in movement and

balance, and spatial location (either of a body part or piece of sports

35
equipment)” (pp. 444-445). Günther (1992b) expanded this definition as,

“kinesthesis is a sensuous perception of tension in the muscles and of

movement in muscles and joints. These sensations are real and with an

adequate sensitivity can create a mental image which one can use in

singing” (p. 4).

o Chromesthesia was a multi-sensory image reportedly experienced by

musicians. It was defined as “colored hearing, a particular form of

synesthesia in which color images (photisms) are evoked by auditory

stimuli” (Polzella & Kuna, 1981, p. 165).

• Skills were understood on the cognimotor continuum. Cognitive and motor skills

were thought to exist on a continuum from cognitive, mental, or covert, to motor,

physical, or overt (Feltz & Landers, 1983; Hinshaw, 1991; Richardson, 1967a).

o Cognitive, or ideational (Sackett, 1934) skills were the function of mental

information processing and organization. It stemmed from the Latin term,

“cognitio, finding out, acquisitions of knowledge” (Skeat, 1943, p. 120).

Cognitive skills involved the covert process of thinking and comparing

and were required in processing perceptual and symbolic tasks or those

without any physical movement.

o Motor skills involved overt use of fine or gross muscular or physical

movement. Cognition was the act of conceiving and processing these overt

actions.

36
Delimitations

Criteria for selection of the participants in this study represented certain

delimitations (see Chapter 3). The criteria for participation in this study were limited to

solo vocal classical performers who had at least four years of professional experience

singing classical repertoire in opera, oratorio, concert, recital, and art song. These were

male and female vocalists who sang in major metropolitan areas of the United States and

represented many different experience levels of singing from early in their career to

retirement. Their voices ranged from soprano, mezzo, tenor, and baritone. Participants

from this population were chosen because of their extensive experience in vocal

performance and their willingness to share their experiences of using imagery in their

profession. The findings of this study were not intended to be generalized to any group

beyond the participants of the study.

Limitations of the Study

There were a number of threats to validity needing clarification in this exploratory

study. Specifically, these consisted of (a) sampling considerations, (b) the lack of formal

assessment of the participants in imagery ability and experience levels, (c) response

biases, (d) issues of temporal validity, (e) the elusive and subjective nature of imagery,

and (f) researcher bias. These were individually addressed and steps were taken to

minimize each threat.

Selection of participants for this study was purposeful and involved snowball

sampling of professional singers from various areas of the United States. This method

was deemed more effective than random sampling (Patton, 2002) since the population of

all professional singers was not available for selection in this study (Onwuegbuzie, 2003).

37
The sample size was relatively small and may not have reflected how the majority of

professional singers practiced and used imagery in their profession. In order to address

this limitation, singers working in major metropolitan areas of the United States were

represented. Furthermore, both male and female vocalists with different levels of

experience and voice types were included and their identities were kept confidential.

Even so, the results of this study were limited to this study participant and were not

generalizable to the entire population of professional classical solo singers in the United

States.

There were no formal instruments implemented to assess participants’ ability or

previous training in imagery (Munroe-Chandler & Hall, 2004-2005). In addressing this

limitation, singers in this study were asked to give details of their early experiences and

training in imagery. Additionally, participants were invited to assess various aspects of

their imagery ability including accuracy, vividness, their ability to manipulate their

images, and whether their imagery was helpful or hurtful. This self-reported information

allowed the researcher to more thoroughly analyze and interpret the data set. Early

training and experiences, as well as ability levels of these participants represented factors

that could have influenced individual use and facility of imagery particularly in the effort

to achieve optimal performance (Martin et al., 1999).

A possible internal credibility threat may have been observational bias, which

occurs when the researcher has “obtained an insufficient sampling of behaviors or words

from the study participants” (Onwuegbuzie, 2003). This could also be due to the

limitations of the number of participants chosen for the study or insufficient time of

gathering data during each interview. Revising and adapting a respected interview guide

38
used in previous studies of various populations of athletics and dance for appropriate use

with singers attempted to address this latter threat. In addition, the subjects were invited

to contribute answers at any time during the interview or add to the given question in

their own way. Furthermore, interview questions were repeated and reworded to assure

complete responses from the interviewees. Member checking was also used in addressing

this threat (Johnson & Christensen, 2004), in which each transcribed interview was sent

to the participant for revision and editing.

Response biases occurred when subjects distort their answers to seem more

favorable to other important people such as colleagues, teachers, and researchers (Vella-

Brodrick & MacRae, 2004). The two most common response biases were acquiescence or

the tendency to respond in a certain direction, either negative or positive and social

desirability where subjects answered in what they thought they should say in order to

appear more favorably in the eyes of peers or other people (Patton, 2002; Vella-Brodrick

& MacRae, 2004). It was possible that the participants, for any number of reasons, did

not want to reveal all pertinent information regarding their use of imagery for optimal

performance. However, an attempt was made to minimize this effect through the

confidentiality of data collected.

Mental imagery was a highly subjective and difficult behavior to directly observe,

describe, and analyze due to its internal and private cognitive nature (Mackay, 1981). The

dependency on self-report may have resulted in inaccuracy due to lapses in memory

recall, lack of understanding of imagery terms used in this study, and possible discomfort

in self-disclosure. The heavy reliance on accuracy of self-report made results suspect,

however, this remained the most reliable method in gaining a descriptive and in-depth

39
understanding of imagery use in optimal performance. Providing initial imagery

definitions, asking open-ended questions, probing for further responses, and using

questions that cross-referenced each subject for two different directions were employed

to minimize this threat. Every effort was made to avoid leading the participant and the

researcher endeavored to maintain a neutral stance and attitude during the interview

process. At the conclusion of the interviews, these singers were asked if they had been

led in any way either by the questions or in any manner of the interviewer. Maintaining

confidentiality also allowed the subjects to respond more honestly and accurately.

Furthermore, participants’ responses were compared with other studies of imagery use in

athletes (Munroe et al., 2000), dancers (Nordin & Cumming, 2005), vocalists (Carter,

1993), and musicians (Bellon, 2006; Trusheim, 1987).

A further threat to external validity in this study was temporal validity since the

results may not have been fixed across time and represented a cross-section of the

specific population (Onwuegbuzie, 2003). The time of year in which the interviews were

conducted (December 2008 to February 2009) produced a snapshot of professional

vocalists working during a heightened performance season. Singers may have used

imagery differently in relation to the time of year and season. In an attempt to minimize

this possible threat, the interview questions were sent in an email attachment to all

participants prior to the interview appointment in order to assist them gaining familiarity

by previewing the definitions, questions, and probes. All of the interviews were

scheduled at convenient times designated by the participants, during periods, which in no

way conflicted with vocalists’ holiday concert schedules. Furthermore, through member

checking, participants were given the opportunity to amend their transcribed interviews in

40
any way they felt appropriate in describing their use of imagery. This provided time for

the participants to contemplate their responses and give as complete and accurate account

as possible. As a result, the exploratory design of the study added to the existing body of

knowledge in the use of imagery in professional vocal performance.

The nature of investigating imagery in professional vocalists may have presented

inherent problems with validity. Terminology of imagery and its definitions as used in

this study were not universal since singers regarded imagery differently from sport

psychologists. In an effort to avoid confusion, explanation of some of the basic terms was

given in the introductory letter and embedded in the interview questions. Furthermore, it

was possible that the interview format and the individual interview with each participant

did not adequately cover the various topics of interest in the use of imagery in singing.

The extensive review of the relevant literature and a pilot study were made to diminish

this threat. The interview guide was based on several studies in the line of research

gaining information as to where, when, what, and why (Munroe et al., 2000; Nordin &

Cumming, 2005). The pilot interview was administered to a regional singing professional

to determine strengths and weaknesses leading to minimal revisions and rewordings of

any awkward or confusing questions. This was an important step in assuring the highest

content-related validity possible for the data collection.

The threat of researcher bias is inherent in qualitative study and was avoided

wherever possible in order to increase the trustworthiness of this study (Anfara, Brown,

& Mangione, 2002; Patton, 2002). This type of bias happens when the researcher has

personal thoughts about the questions, answers, or the overall subject of the inquiry that

may have an either conscious or subconscious effect on the responses of the participants

41
(Onwuegbuzie, 2003). Peer debriefing, rigorous methodology, and attention to detail

were conducted and maintained as well as avoiding bias in data collection, analysis, and

interpretation in order to remain true to reporting the responses of the participants. Rigor

in this research made the results and conclusions as valid as possible in this exploratory

format. This investigator has had considerable training and experience as a professional

vocalist as well as prior knowledge and experience in imagery, which may have served to

bias responses in the interview. However, it was reasonable to expect the researcher to

have some knowledge of the subject she was investigating (Patton, 2002).

Subjectively analyzing participants’ reports can also invite problems in gathering,

interpreting, and reporting this information, often leading to investigator bias. For this

reason, debriefing with peers was conducted throughout the research process in an effort

to strengthen trustworthiness and validity of this study. Additionally, four other

researchers with advanced degrees in music education and training in research methods

assisted in the data analysis and in categorizing the results of the interviews (Johnson &

Christensen, 2004).

Organization of Remaining Chapters

Chapter 2 includes an extensive literature review in the research involving the

uses of imagery in the athletic, artistic, and musical performers, including pertinent

theories, studies, and applications. The framework of the four Ws of imagery use: where,

when, what, and why as proposed in athletes (Munroe et al., 2000) and dancers (Nordin &

Cumming, 2005) is examined in relationship to musical and vocal performers’ use of

imagery. Chapter 3 provides details of the research design, the participants, methodology,

data collection procedures, and methods of analysis for this study, including

42
legitimization. The results of the data analyses are provided in Chapter 4 along with

tables and figures. Emergent themes and areas of similarity to previous research are also

reported. The summary of the study is discussed in Chapter 5 as well as themes,

interpretations and significant findings. The implications for music and vocal education

are included, followed by limitations of the study and recommendations for further

research.

43
Chapter 2

Literature Review

This chapter provides support for understanding of the four Ws framework for

vocalists’ use of imagery by drawing upon the literature primarily from sport psychology

(e.g., Hall et al., 1998; Martin et al., 1999; Munroe et al., 2000) and dance research (e.g.,

Nordin & Cumming, 2005). The paucity of imagery research in music performance

limited the author’s references in this area to only a small number of studies including

those in instrumental brass (Trusheim, 1987), musical performance (Bellon, 2006), and

singing (Carter, 1993). The chapter begins with a review of the theories of imagery

effectiveness on performance from general and sport psychology, which provided the

groundwork in understanding the various aspects of imagery research. Then, each query

in the main framework of the four Ws: where, when, what, and why is examined

individually. Results from the major studies from sports, dance, and musical performance

are analyzed, compared, and synthesized in relation to vocal professionals’ use of

imagery in achieving optimal performance. The chapter concludes with a proposed

framework of imagery supporting the current study.

Theories of Imagery Effectiveness on Performance

A number of theories have been developed over the years to explain and

understand the underlying mechanisms of imagery and its effects on performance. Four

categories of theories have been identified primarily in sport psychology as those relevant

to understanding the workings of imagery and mental practice: (a) early developments
44
and theories, (b) cognitive-based theories (c) psychological states theories, and (d)

functional equivalence theory. The early developments and theories included several

historical inquiries into imagery (Carpenter, 1874, James, 1890), the

psychoneuromuscular theory (Jacobson, 1930a, 1930b, 1930c, 1930d, 1931) and

symbolic learning theory (Sackett, 1934; 1935). The focus of these studies was in testing

mental practice in motor skills. Cognitive-based theories included a foundation in the

mental processes involved in imagery. These included Paivio’s (1971) dual code theory,

bio-informational theory of Lang (1977, 1979a, 1979b), and Ahsen’s (1972, 1984) triple

code theory. Theories emphasizing psychological states, such as arousal and motivation,

included the Bandura’s (1977, 1982, 1986, 1997) self-efficacy theory and the attention-

arousal set theory of Schmidt (1982). The theory of functional equivalence (Decety,

1996a, 1996b; Farah, 1984; Finke, 1980, 1985, 1989; Finke & Shephard, 1986;

Jeannerod, 1994, 1995, 1999, 2006) represented the newest developments in imagery

theories and involved neuroimaging. Functional equivalence studies found that imagining

motor tasks seemed to replicate the same mental patterns involved in preparing to execute

the task. Although this theory was relevant to imagery in performers, it could not be

supported or disputed in this study. However, it did offer evidence and understanding of

imagery use beyond what has been presented in prior studies. Further details of these

theories were briefly outlined to provide a background in understanding the major

elements and supporting research as identified in sport psychology (see Table 2).

45
Table 2

Major Theories of Effects of Imagery on Performance

Theory Theorists Concepts

Psychoneuro- Carpenter (1874) Imagery of a motor skill produced minute muscular

muscular James (1890) innervations, similar to actual execution. Muscular memory

Theory Jacobson (1930s) provided feedback of that action, improving performance

Symbolic Learning Sackett (1934, Imagery was effective in learning and rehearsing particularly

Theory 1935) cognitive tasks through symbols representing tasks in the brain

Dual Coding Paivio (1971, Imagery represented dual coding modes: imaginal and verbal,

Theory 1975, 1986) which facilitated the action and memory

Bio-Informational Lang (1977, Imagery consisted of stimulus (the scenario), response

Theory 1979a, 1979b) (emotional and physical reactions), and meaning propositions

Triple Code Ahsen (1984) Imagery was composed of three elements: image (I), somatic

Theory response (S), and meaning (M)

Self-Efficacy Bandura (1969, Imagery of performance excellence increased self-efficacy and

Theory 1977, 1982, 1986) self-efficacy beliefs regulated performance outcomes

Attention-Arousal Schmidt (1982) Appropriate imagery assisted performers to achieve optimal

Set Theory levels of arousal for performance success

Functional Decety (1996) Imagery and perception of a specific action were functionally

Equivalence Jeannerod (1994) equivalent in the same neural substrates action was inhibited in

Theory Finke (1980) imagery

46
Early Developments and Theories of Imagery

Various aspects of mental imagery have been investigated since the early study of

psychology. Examination of the effects of mentally practicing a motor task began with

development of the ideo-motor principle (Carpenter, 1874), in which images or intentions

of muscular actions produced slight nerve impulses. James (1890) argued that mental

images of actions was produced in slight muscular discharges generated in the related

muscles and further postulated that by imagining, one could learn to perform skills

without overt practice. Washburn (1916) supported the assumption that mental

representations undergirded muscular activity. Jacobson’s (1930a, 1930b, 1930c, 1930d,

1931) studies were among the earliest to formally investigate neuromuscular effects from

mental activities. According to Jacobson mental practices produced a sub-threshold

arousal of the normal motor output, which were strong enough to generate slight

neuromuscular or kinesthetic sensations. Contrary to Jacobson’s findings, Shaw (1938)

reported that electromyographic (EMG) activity during imagery was distributed in a

variety of muscle groups, not just the ones involved in the imagery.

Psychoneuromuscular theory. Psychoneuromuscular theory was one of the

earliest theories addressing mental imagery, which stated that mental practice effects

were intervened by minute muscular movements, subsequently reflective of the same

muscular activity as that produced during actual physical motor activity. These muscle

movements provided kinesthetic feedback so that future adjustments could be made

(Corbin, 1972; Richardson, 1967b; Schmidt, 1987; Start & Richardson, 1964). This

theory had been developed through many studies, using the most innovative

instrumentation available at the time. Even though it enjoyed popularity for decades, the

47
positive relationship between actual muscular activity and its imagined counterpart has

yet to be established (Feltz & Landers, 1983; Murphy & Martin, 2002).

Symbolic learning theory. Sackett’s (1934, 1935) symbolic learning theory held

that specific representations of skills or “mental blueprints” (Vealey & Walter, 1993)

were stored in the brain as they were acquired. Learning a task resulted in forming a

“representation,” code, or symbol of that task, which could aid learning, recall, and

performance, facilitating automaticity of the activity (Paivio, 1985). Sackett concluded

that more mental rehearsals increased learning retention, but after a certain amount of

practice there were no further gains, creating a negative acceleration relationship between

rehearsals and retention. The theory did not address how elite athletes or performers

effectively use mental practice when these “mental blueprints” were already well

established. Furthermore, the design of the study may not have represented the actual use

of imagery in the practices of performing artists or athletes. Yuille (1985) argued that

researchers would be more apt to generalize and benefit from these experiments if actual

experts were examined in the various fields.

Imagery studies fell out of favor for many years in the early decades of the

twentieth century. Both psychoneuromuscular and symbolic learning theories lacked the

ability to fully explain the wide variety of applications and aspects of imagery use, such

as affect, motivation, and cognition (Murphy, 1985). Furthermore, the popularity of

Watson’s (1931) behaviorism supported his demand for empirical and scientific

validation of such elusive forms of human functions. The reemergence of scientific

inquiry into imagery was heralded by Holt’s article, “Return of the Ostracized” (1964)

whereby he provided compelling evidence and called for renewed research in imagery.

48
Various groups in psychology began to embrace and include imagery in their clinical

practices and research, including cognitive and psychological states theorists.

Cognitive-Based Theories of Imagery

Cognitive psychology (Neisser, 1976) encompassed the processes whereby people

transformed, reduced, elaborated, stored, recovered, and used sensory input (Neisser,

1976). Most of the work in imagery concerned acquisition, storage, retrieval and use of

thought, known as information-processing. There were three cognitive-based theories of

imagery: (a) Paivio’s (1971, 1986) dual code theory which included the pictoral and

verbal representation of the image; (b) bio-informational theory (Lang, 1977, 1979a,

1979b), wherein imagery contained both a stimulus, a response proposition, and its

intrinsic meaning (c) Ahsen’s (1984) triple code theory, in which the (I) image, (S)

somatic response, and (M) meaning of the image were necessary to affect performance.

These cognitive-based theories were investigated for their relevance to imagery use in

performance execution.

Dual code theory. In dual code theory, Paivio’s (1971, 1986) theoretical

assumptions for imagery effectiveness in memory involved parallel processing of

information in two basic coding systems operating in cognitive functions, verbal and

pictoral, which have been reported to aid in memory and retrieval. This theory was

supported by several studies including those of “the current neuropsychological model of

functional localization” (Jeannerod, 1994). Hall, Moore, Annett, and Rodgers (1997)

investigated recalled movement patterns using imagery, a verbal guide, a combination of

both, or no practice control. Results indicated that the combination of imagery and verbal

guide led to the most successful pattern recall. Annett (1986, 1994) extended the dual

49
coding approach rendering it specific to motor applications. Annett developed the action-

language-imagination (ALI) model whereby the two methods assisting skill acquisition,

verbal or demonstration, and were linked in the action-language bridge making it possible

to act on verbal instructions or encode actions in language. Ahsen (1984) criticized the

dual coding theory for being “an abstract, linear model” (p. 18), omitting the function and

importance of meaning or somatic effect. It could be argued that athletes and performers

used verbal coding in a variety of ways to learn and master skills, however dual coding

would only account for a limited portion of artistic performance experience, particularly

in the area of affect and emotion.

Bio-informational theory. Lang (1977, 1979a, 1979b) developed his bio-

informational theory in the psychophysiological studies of phobic and anxiety patients.

According to Lang, images were organized into a two basic propositions: stimulus, and

response with its relevant meaning characteristics. “Stimulus propositions are those

descriptive details which establish the context and designate specific stimuli. The

response propositions are the things the subject does in the scene” (Lang, 1979b, p. 19).

Lang recommended that response proposition scripts include as many specific details of

the intended emotional and physical states to achieve the most significant and desired

behavioral and physiological changes.

Imagery effectiveness was most supported by how a person responded to stimuli

as demonstrated in studies by Lang and his colleagues. Lang, Melamed, and Hart (1970)

compared the emotional imagery, particularly those verbally invoked by use of specific

scripts, with biological responses to fear images using two groups: those with a phobia of

live snakes and the other the social phobia of public speaking. Lang found the social

50
phobia groups’ arousal leveled off while the images in the snake phobia group were much

more vivid, showed highest increases of heart rate, and registered at the upper end of the

hierarchy. Study participants were able to gain more control of their performance by

modifying their responses, producing greater change in behavior. Similar results occurred

in a cross-validation study with participants having public speaking anxiety and spider

phobias (Weerts & Lang, 1978). In desensitization studies treating these phobic patients

with stimulus and response propositions, Lang (1979a) subsequently found successfully

treated phobic patients reported less fear reactions to imagery and calmer heart-rates.

Budney, Murphy, and Woolfolk (1994) supported Lang’s theory that imagery

instructions, in which more response items were included and produced more

physiological responses than stimulus-only scripts.

The use of bio-informational theory of imagery could prove particularly

applicable for addressing arousal concerns of singers since they, like public speakers,

perform before audiences. Many vocalists have struggled with crippling performance

anxiety and inability to achieve proper arousal levels appropriate for performance on

stage. In controlling inner thoughts, emotions, and images by including the desired

response propositions, vocal performers could possibly move beyond their fears or

phobias into more free and expressive delivery of their art to the audience. However,

Lang’s theory may have fallen short of explaining some of the cognitive and motivational

functions of imagery and the use of language in processing information into action,

particularly in highly skilled athletes and performers.

Triple code theory. Ahsen’s (1984) triple code theory, similar to Lang’s theory,

also addressed imagery more in terms of the emotional effect imagery had on the

51
individual. Ahsen’s (1984) triple code theory was developed in his response criticizing

Paivio’s (1971) dual code theory. Ahsen diminished the importance of the verbal content

and argued that the somatic element and meaning required representation for imagery to

be effective. Also emerging from his studies with mental patients and phobias, Ahsen

(1972) included three main components of his ISM imagery theory, which were: (a) the

image itself (I) internally containing all the sensations of actual experience; (b) the

somatic response (S), which represented the psychophysiological changes, including

emotions elicited by the specific image; and (c) meaning (M), which was specific to the

image and the individual, including their history, experience, and previous conceptions.

Three components of triple code theory, the image, the somatic response, and the

meaning (ISM), reflected issues and concerns for vocal professionals, making it

applicable in musical performance.

The cognitive theories addressing imagery issues such as verbal coding, image

itself, the specific somatic responses to images, and the meanings applied to these

experiences as well as their verbal connections needed to be considered in imagery use in

singers. Application of cognitive theories to vocal performance may have proven

appropriate since imagery in this context was highly individualized. The interaction

between these imagery elements was important, however they were limited in other

details of how imagery affects performance. More recent research has emerged

addressing the psychological states and how these affect imagery performance.

Psychological States Theories

Psychological states theories addressed the internal processes involved in arousal

and confidence when using imagery in performance. Information-processing approach

52
included two main theories: (a) Bandura’s (1977) self-efficacy theory where imagery was

partly seen as the product of social interaction, and (b) attention-arousal set theory

(Schmidt, 1982), which addressed the optimal levels of arousal necessary for specific

activities.

Self-efficacy theory. Bandura’s (1977) self-efficacy theory grew out of social

learning theory and cognitive-behavioral therapy, which dealt with psychology patients’

beliefs and understandings in influencing problematic affect and behavior. Self-efficacy

theory held that consciously directed, task specific imagery increased expectations of

success, which facilitated confidence and positive outcome of the actual performance. “A

substantial body of literature shows that efficacy beliefs regulate human functioning

through four major processes [including] cognitive, motivational, affective and selective

processes” (Bandura, 1997, p. 116). Bandura (1986) posited that self-efficacy and self-

evaluation cognition such as attaining goals and achieving success enhanced intrinsic

motivation. Furthermore, an individual’s performance and resulting confident beliefs

were based on personal expectations from “past performance success, vicarious

experience (modeling), verbal reinforcement, and emotional arousal” (Morris et al., 2005,

p. 48).

In modeling, another aspect of self-efficacy theory, the learner matched motor

movement and skills with an observed standard or representative. Feedback was required

to check for accuracy, corrections, and refinement of these skills. This learning had a

cognitive effect on the individual as to the degree of proficiency to which they were

capable of performing these movements. Bandura explained:

53
When a person observes a model’s behavior, but otherwise performs no overt

responses, he can acquire the modeled responses while they are occurring only in

cognitive, representational forms . . . an imaginal and a verbal one. After

modeling stimuli have been coded into images or words for memory

representation they function as mediators for subsequent response retrieval and

reproduction. (1997, p. 133)

Athletes and performers have benefited from the practice of modeling for a long time.

Athletes have watched other athletes to learn how to execute their skills, just as singers

have been encouraged to listen to excellent vocalists to replicate and be inspired by the

quality and practices of other excellent singers (Tosi & Galliard, 1723/1968). Both

athletes and performers also learned, from others and by experience, how to regulate their

arousal levels for optimal performance.

Attention-arousal set theory. Attention-arousal set theory (Schmidt, 1982), also

referred to as arousal or activation theory, proposed that imagery allows for an optimal

state of arousal for each performer and each activity, which facilitated performance

excellence and success. Attention-arousal set theory also proposed that imagery prepared

the athlete (and performer) for action to embody a perfect arousal state while focusing

attention on task-relevant cues rather than distractions (Murphy, Woolfolk, & Budney,

1988). Landers (1980) reported that high arousal or emotional activity had the potential

of narrowing or decreasing attentional range while low levels broaden attention. This

inverted-U theory stated that optimal performances were associated with moderate levels

of arousal, while high and low arousal could have a deteriorating effect on performance.

54
Each one of these theories: psychoneuromuscular theory, symbolic learning

theory, cognitive-based theories, and psychological states theories, addressed specific

aspects of imagery effects on performance. Most of these theories emerged in response to

the current issues present during the time in which they were addressed and often

developed in response to earlier theories. Furthermore, they were tested and analyzed in

the best empirical manner possible at the time. Functional equivalence theory used the

most advanced technology in examining the workings of imagery available to date.

Functional Equivalence Theory

More recent research and developments using highly technical electronic imaging

capacities especially those from medical science have taken mental imagery out of its

subjective and therefore questionable category into one of more extensive, rigorous, and

scientific investigation. This represented a departure from the psychological models and

theories previously designed and used to explain imagery function. Functional

equivalence theory (Decety, 1996a, 1996b, 2002; Farah, 1984; Finke, 1980, 1985, 1989;

Finke & Shephard, 1986; Jeannerod, 1994, 1995, 2006) postulated that the same brain

functions involved in imagery were functionally equivalent in the brain activities of the

preparation of that same skill or activity even though physical motor execution was

usually blocked during imagery (Holmes & Collins, 2002). Both activated the same

neural substrates, pathways, and processes in specific areas of the brain. While motor

preparation was activated below the level of consciousness, imagery of that act was

consciously directed in the same action (Jeannerod, 1994). This effectively placed

intended actions and skills on a covert to overt continuum (Jeannerod, 1994, 2006;

Kosslyn, 1994; Kosslyn & Koenig, 1992). Decety (1996b) reviewed studies involving

55
mental chromatic and autonomic responses while imaging. He concluded that the studies

“converge to support the notion that motor imagery shares the same neural mechanisms

that are involved in motor control of actual actions” (p. 91).

Due to the recent developments in neuroimaging, researchers were able to

objectify imagery activity, possibly resolving decades of controversy over its elusive and

subjective nature particularly in the area of motor performance. Holmes and Collins

(2002) have implemented the findings of functional equivalence into applications of

imagery training for use in sports performance. They have found that imagery as a

“motor representation can be used to explain the multifaceted and variable motor

behaviour [sic] of elite sport athletes and to develop a mechanistic explanation for the

effectiveness of imagery generally” (p. 123). They further argued that: “if physical and

mental practice can be shown to possess high functional equivalence, then many

procedures that are efficacious in physical practice should be relevant to mental practice

as well” (p. 124). Holmes and Collins concluded that the most effective imagery

reflected, as closely as possible, all the cognitive, motor, sensory, and affective elements

present in the preparation and execution of the given task.

Studies specific to brain functions in images involved in auditory processing,

musical expression, and observational learning were particularly relevant to this study.

Using neuroimaging capabilities, Aleman, Nieuwenstein, Böcker, and de Haan (2000)

found that auditory imagery was associated positively with auditory and musical training.

Meister, Krings, Foltys, Boroojerdi, Müller, Töpper, & Thron (2004) found similar brain

activity during imaginary as in actual musical performance. Halpern and Zatorre (1999)

observed that vocal song performance activated specific areas of the brain. More recently,

56
Decety (2006) found that emotional imagery and empathy reflected actual expression of

emotion in brain activity. In studying the neural activity of humans viewing another

individual performing actions, Fadiga, Fogassi, Pavesi, and Rizzolatti (1995) observed

increased stimulation of the same parts of the motor system in the brain as when subjects

both imagined and performed that motor action. Similarly, Corchin, Barthelemy,

Lejeune, Roux, and Martineau (1998) found changes in electroencephalography (EEG)

cartography of both hemispheres of the brain in subjects watching videos of a person

executing gymnastic movements. Kosslyn and others (2001) concluded that “most of the

neural processes that underlie cognitive like-modality perception are also used in

imagery; and imagery, in many ways, can stand in for (re-present, if you will) a

perceptual stimulus or situation” (p. 641). These studies were particularly relevant to the

issues of training vocal performers in how they learn to employ modeling, observation,

skill acquisition and technique, and embody a variety of affect in portraying different

characters and roles.

All these imagery theories had been developed over many decades and seemed to

follow psychological innovations and medical imaging inventions in addressing

imagery’s effects on cognitive and motor performance (see Table 2).

Psychoneuromuscular theory addressed muscular aspects while symbolic learning theory

dealt with specific coding in the brain. The cognitive-based theories, including Paivio’s

(1971) dual-coding theory, Lang’s (1977, 1979a, 1979b) bio-informational theory, and

Ahsen’s triple code theory, addressed information processing. The psychological state

theory of Bandura’s (1977) self-efficacy theory dealt with environmental and social

stimuli as interacting with imagery development of self-efficacy and confidence in the

57
individual. Schmidt’s (1982) attention-arousal set theory posited that there were certain

affective and arousal states, which made optimal performance achievable. Functional

equivalence was based on neural imaging evidence that imagery and motor activity share

the same cognitive processes. With the recent developments in brain imaging, functional

equivalence seemed to provide the most comprehensive explanation, the results of which

may impact the relevancy of the earlier theories. However, these theories may have been

relevant to the framework of the four Ws in providing a foundation for understanding

imagery’s effects on performance.

The Four Ws of Imagery Use

The framework of the four Ws of imagery use: where, when, what, and why, from

athletes (Munroe et al., 2000) and dancers (Nordin & Cumming, 2005) provided a basis

for this study. Where and when imagery was examined first, followed by what performers

imagine, including imagery characteristics and types. A discussion of the purposes, or

why different imagery content was employed followed. The major studies investigating

the use of imagery in musical performers were examined and compared with relevant

sport and performance imagery research.

The framework of the four Ws was initially proposed in imagery of athletes and

included six levels (Munroe et al., 2000), as seen in Figure 1. Levels one and two

depicted where and when, respectively, and the third level revealed a connection of the

function and content of imagery in answering why and what athletes used imagery. In

adapting the four Ws framework for dancers (see Figure 2), Nordin and Cumming (2005)

eliminated the use of levels and created a hierarchical tree and separated why and what.

58
Figure 1. Four Ws of imagery use framework for athletes.

From “The Four Ws of Imagery Use: Where, When, Why, and What,” by K. J. Munroe, P. R. Giacobbi, Jr.,

C. R. Hall, and P. Weinberg, 2000, The Sport Psychologists, 14, p. 126. Copyright 2000 by Human

Kinetics. Reprinted with permission (see Appendix A).

59
Figure 2. Four Ws of imagery use framework for dancers.

From “Professional Dancers Describe Their Imagery: Where, When, What, Why, and How,” by S. M.

Nordin and J. Cumming, 2005, The Sport Psychologists, 19, p. 400. Copyright 2005 by Human Kinetics.

Reprinted with permission (see Appendix A).


60
The category of what, or content was divided between imagery types and imagery

characteristics. Imagery types included images of execution, metaphors, context, body-

related, and character/role. In the why category, cognitive and motivational reasons were

carried over from the sports framework (Munroe et al., 2000). Three additional imagery

reasons categories emerged from the dance study: (a) artistic reasons of choreography,

movement enhancement, and audience communication; (b) healing reasons, and (c) no

reason. These adaptations were considered to more appropriately apply to use of imagery

in artistic performers and specifically to the vocal professionals in this study. Therefore,

these two frameworks were used in this study to analyze and compare to the imagery uses

of these professional singers and to allow a new singers’ imagery framework to emerge

from the data analyses. The main elements of the two imagery frameworks are analyzed

which answered the questions: where, when, what, and why. This is separated into the

following sections: (a) where; (b) when; (c) what, imagery types and imagery

characteristics; and (d) why, and the cognitive, motivational, artistic, and healing reasons.

The chapter ends with a review of the relevant imagery research in music performance.

Where or Locations of Imagery Use

Location, or where, athletes and artistic performers engaged in imagery was

represented in five categories: (a) practice or rehearsal; (b) performance or competition

venue; (c) at home; (d) in transit, commuting, or traveling; and (e) other places.

Researchers initially reported that athletes used imagery primarily in training and practice

areas. However, more recent studies found that athletes used more imagery on the

competition field than training areas (Barr & Hall, 1992; Hall et al., 1990). Salmon and

others (1994) reported athletes engaging in imagery outside of practice or competition, in

61
the home, school, and at work. Dancers and musicians imagined in all areas of the

rehearsal or practice studio and the performing venue, such as the dressing room,

backstage, and on stage (Bellon, 2006; Nordin & Cumming, 2005; Trusheim, 1987).

They also reported using imagery while in transit or commuting, on the train, driving, in

quiet places, and at home, especially in bed. Both athletes and performers used imagery

in many places and settings, particularly those in which they could concentrate and

mentally practice and prepare for the performance. These varied by time, place, personal

preferences, and imagery abilities.

When or Times of Imagery Use

According to the studies cited below, the specific times, or when, athletes and

performers participated in imagery constituted ten categories: (a) practice or rehearsal, (b)

performance or competition, (c) during quiet times, (d) in transit or traveling, (e) certain

types or styles of performances, (f) times of the day or night, (g) periods of the season or

year, (h) anytime or all the time, (i) in times of difficulty, and (j) during injury or health

problems. Many of these categories were similar to the location where imagery was

employed, since times chosen by these performers were connected with their scheduling

requirements of rehearsals and performances.

Athletes reportedly engaged in imagery primarily just prior to competing than

during or after competition (Barr & Hall, 1992; Rodgers et al., 1991; Weinberg, Butt,

Knight, Burke, & Jackson, 2003; Ungerleider & Golding, 1992). Other researchers

(Munroe et al., 2000; Salmon et al., 1992) found athletes using imagery more during

practice than before or after training sessions, however it was not known how directly

this practice was connected with performance. Oxendine (1969) proposed that “effective

62
mental rehearsal techniques may enable learners to practice at times when they are not

able to actively perform the tasks” (p. 755). Later, researchers (Weinberg & Gould, 2003;

White & Hardy, 1998) reported that athletes also used imagery when they were

experiencing tough or difficult times, under a lot of pressure, during sickness or injury, or

fatigue while convalescing, or while physical practice was not possible. Outside of

practice or competition, athletes engaged in imagery during breaks while at work or

school (Weinberg & Gould, 2003). Athletes and artistic performers were found to use

imagery just prior to going to sleep (Bellon, 2006; Hall et al., 1990; Munroe et al., 2000;

Rodgers et al., 1991; Trusheim, 1987; Weinberg & Gould, 2003).

Musicians and dancers imagined more during practice and rehearsals than before

or after, and more before performances than during or after (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993;

Nordin & Cumming, 2005; Trusheim, 1987). Performers also used more imagery when

they needed to engage in or communicate with artistic elements or metaphorical imagery.

For example, dancers imagined more if metaphors were needed in an expressive piece

(Nordin & Cumming, 2005), and singers used more imagery during song recitals than

operatic performances (Carter, 1993). Both dancers and musicians engaged in mental

rehearsal when they were tired, injured, or in a place where they could not overtly

rehearse. They also used imagery extensively to alleviate performance anxiety, for

instance when they felt nervous, anxious, or distracted. Furthermore, musicians and

dancers imagined more during the time of year when their schedules were heavy with

rehearsals and performances (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Nordin & Cumming, 2005;

Trusheim, 1987). Generally, athletes and artistic performers used imagery throughout the

63
normal day, and specifically when different types of imagery were used to facilitate

practice or performance.

What: Content of Images

In the initial investigation of the four Ws of athletes’ imagery content, Munroe

and her colleagues (2000) identified six specific categories: (a) sessions, (b)

effectiveness, (c) nature of imagery, (d) surroundings, (e) types of imagery, and (f)

controllability. Sessions referred to the duration of the imagery engagement which were

directly related to findings in the where and when category. Effectiveness involved the

participants’ evaluation of the usefulness of their imagery practices. Whether the images

were positive, negative, or the degree of their accuracy comprised the nature of imagery.

Surroundings included the venue of practice or competition and those in attendance.

Types of imagery encompassed the specific uses of four senses: (a) vision, including

internal and external perspective and vividness; (b) auditory, or sounds; (c) kinesthetic or

the feel of the movements; and (d) olfactory, or smells. Athletes used the kinesthetic and

vision senses the most and involved experiences of the specific sport or venue of

competition. Image controllability had to do with the individual’s ability to manipulate

the image and the speed with which the image was executed. While all these categories

are critical in describing athletes’ imagery, the organization of this was preferred in

Nordin and Cumming (2005) dance imagery investigation due to its closer approximation

to the artistic experiences of the professional vocalists in this study. Nordin and

Cumming (2005) adapted the results from the framework derived from athletes’ imagery

findings of Munroe and her colleagues (see Figure 1), and determined that dancers’

imagery content was divided between types and characteristics. Henceforth in this

64
chapter, each of the important findings from athletes and related literature was reported

according to the dancers’ framework (see Figure 2).

What: Imagery Types

Imagery types were those of content, or the “image” itself. In investigating

dancers’ imagery, Nordin & Cumming, 2005) identified six different types of imagery

investigated were: (a) execution, (b) metaphoric and artistic, (c) context, (d) body-related,

(e) character and role, and (e) irrelevant images. Each content area was examined in

detail for its use in achieving optimal athletic, artistic, and musical performance.

Execution images. The image types described as execution included (a) skill

learning and technique; (b) sequencing, planning, and strategies; and (c) goal imagery.

Skill learning images included specific images in mentally practicing actions and

methods for performance. Mental practice studies in sport and musical performance are

addressed in the section on why later in this chapter. Munroe and others (2000)

distinguished execution images in two distinct areas: (a) skill development in technique

learning and skill building, and (b) performance enhancement, including correctional

efforts.

Skill images were generally regarded as imagery functions in the why category

and not a part of content. However, athletes have illustrated the value of being able to

visualize specific skills and its relationship to physical execution of that skill. Mac

Wilkins, winner of four Olympic gold medals described this:

Basically it boils down to the fact that if you’re trying to accomplish something, a

particular athletic movement, if you can’t visualize it then it’s pure chance you

will be able to perform the movement. If you visualize it and can really see it . . .

65
you have a clear target to aim for and a much better chance of realizing that

target. (cited in Ungerleider & Golding, 1992, p. 20)

This quote underscores the connection between being able to mentally execute skills and

physically performing them. For athletes, skill images depended upon the type of sport in

which the athlete was engaged. Similarly, specific images of instrumental musicians

could include playing an instrument in a certain fashion, executing a certain embouchure

for brass players (Trusheim, 1987) or holding the wrist in a way that facilitated playing

arpeggios on the piano. For singers, images of opening the sinuses and the throat to

achieve better resonance or performing octave leaps correctly would be considered skill

execution images.

Planning and sequencing images in performance also dealt with development and

execution of strategies and routines (Munroe et al., 2000; Nordin & Cumming, 2005).

Strategies in athletic performance included images of rehearsing game plans in football

(Fenker & Lambiotte, 1987), strategies in wrestling (Rushall, 1988), and routines such as

those in figure skating and gymnastics (White & Hardy, 1998). Munroe and her

colleagues (2000) reported strategy and sequence imagery in the cognitive aspects of the

why category and not as a separate content. Musicians imagined strategies and plans in

organizing their practice and pre-performance routines, specific performance sequences,

strategies of handling difficult passages, and ways of coping with distractions (Bellon,

2006; Carter, 1993, Trusheim, 1987).

Research in imagery with regard to goal attainment has only recently emerged.

Sports research in goal setting and achievement involved three different types of goals:

(a) outcome, (b) performance, and (c) process goals (Weinberg & Gould, 1995). Outcome

66
goals in this study focused on the final result of the effort or endeavor. Performance goals

involved the projection of exceeding one’s own efforts in a particular setting without

regard to other competitors. Process goals contained the desired effect of improving more

specific performance functions. Weinberg and Gould (1995) suggested that athletes

focusing on performance or process goals more readily achieved performance success.

Woolfolk, Murphy, Gottesfeld, and Aitken (1985) found that athletes who imagined the

outcome of their specific sport activity just prior to performance subsequently influenced

that same activity more than mental rehearsal. Reports of goal imagery were included as

motivational aspects of imagery reasons in the study by Munroe and colleagues (2000).

Woody (1999) proposed that expressive performances required mental

conceptions of performance goals. The more complete and descriptively detailed the

performance goals were in the mind of the performer, the more successful the actual

performance seemed to be. Jackson and Csikszentmihalyi (1999) suggested that

performers keep focused on their goals by visualizing ahead of time the specific

performance both in parts and as a whole. This gave the individual a clear blueprint of all

the components involved in its successful execution. The application of different goals

corresponded to the varying requirements of the specific task.

Artistic performers used goal images to improve skill and performances, identify

and execute desired roles to perform, and mentally project future stagings (Carter, 1993).

Performance goals were found to often intermingle with outcome goals for artistic

professionals. Dancers used goal images to imagine being the best, perform difficult

skills, and set long and short-term goals (Nordin & Cumming, 2005). Professional singers

expected and projected positive outcomes, regularly setting achievable and effective

67
goals (Carter, 1993). The following quote exemplified outcome and performance goals

made by the world-renowned tenor, Luciano Pavarotti, early in his illustrious career. He

stated:

When I began to study, I made lists of all the roles I wanted to sing, and the age at

which I planned to sing them. At the top I put ‘Rudolfo – 22.’ I did it at 26.

Everything has come later than I planned, and I am not sorry. I thought to retire

myself at 55, but for now the voice feels [fine]. (Luciano Pavarotti, cited in

Crutchfield, 1985)

Pavarotti was 50 when he gave this interview and was still singing until very close to his

death at 71. Christine Harel, a freelance singer, (Bellon, 2006) described process goals in

endeavoring to improve her performances. She said:

I also have objectives such as: I would really like, this time, to be able to stand in

a particular way while singing; or also: I would like for my voice to stay in a

particular place in my body while singing. I can have various objectives based on

where I am at with my instrument [voice] at the time. This comes from the

feedback that I got from previous performances and what I would like to improve.

(p. 17)

Professional singers seemed to be familiar with various aspects and methods of

employing outcome, performance, and process goals throughout their careers.

Metaphoric/artistic images. Metaphoric and artistic imagery was traditionally

used in artistic performance and have been included in more current investigations in

several sports, such as figure skating and artistic gymnastics (Ruiz & Hanin, 2004).

Recent studies have revealed the successful use of metaphorical imagery in athletes

68
regulating affect in achieving flow states (Hanin & Stambolva, 2002; Ruiz & Hanin,

2004). Nordin and Cumming (2005) reported dancers using various kinds of metaphorical

imagery with performance, “including images of color, objects that are not present,

actions that cannot actually be performed, the environment, and themes” (p. 403).

Musicians have used metaphorical and poetic imagery for technical execution and

expressive interpretation in expressive performances of a composition. Often the style

and historic genre of the piece have dictated specific imagery and interpretation. Carter

(1993) reported singers using more imagery in performing recitals than oratorio or opera.

Conductors were known to share certain metaphors or poetic images for interpreting

phrases, movements, and entire symphonies (Barten, 1992, 1998; Bellon, 2006; Carter,

1993; Trusheim, 1987). Conductors, directors, and musical instructors also used

allegorical and descriptive language and imagery, story telling, and kinesthetic and visual

imagery to help create a certain feeling or intention in a piece. In her observations of

instrumental classes, Barten (1992, 1998) reported instructors described countless

varieties of metaphors and similes to elicit appropriate musical expression in their

students’ performances. Instrumentalists and singers have used a wide range of

metaphoric imagery in achieving a cohesive artistic interpretation of piece of music.

In the context of vocal performance imagery was often assumed to mean

metaphoric or poetic images related to affect and artistic expression (Bellon, 2006;

Carter, 1993; Coffin, 1987; Leyerle, 1986; Miller, 1996) as opposed to the definition of

imagery used in sport psychology. Professional singers in Carter’s (1993) and Bellon’s

(2006) studies emphasized metaphorical and artistic use of imagery in affecting

expressive performances and to describe methods to produce different vocal sounds, as in

69
Pavarotti imagining an airplane ascending as he sang a rising series of pitches (Hines,

1982).

Within the vocal pedagogical community there had been much controversy

concerning the appropriate use of imagery in teaching voice (Titze, 1986). According to

Vennard (1971), imagery was often a description a teacher would employ to help students

gain understanding of vocal production if direct explanation was ineffective. “Its

philosophy is that while the physical details are either unknown or not directly

controllable, the experience of good tone production can be described in figures of speech

which will enable the student to grasp it” (p. 15). Vennard cautioned the singing teacher

to be careful only to use imagery with ideas that the student singer could grasp easily

through past experience. He referred to “float the tone on the breath” and “don’t reach for

high notes; approach them from above,” as statements that had particular meaning only to

those who have experienced these vocal techniques.

Other pedagogues have argued in favor of employing a fully integrated artistic

and scientific approach to singing teaching. Reid (1983) strongly cautioned against the

use of imagery in favor of a more scientific approach to singing. He argued:

To a certain extent, vocal instruction is impossible without imagery, since even

the most elemental scale pattern must be conceptualized before it can be

executed. However, the successful realization of a preconcept [sic] depends upon

an ability to respond, which is a matter of physical coordination. Since the

student has presented himself in order to overcome obvious physical disabilities,

the use of imagery cannot be considered pertinent to the central issue. Imagery,

70
therefore, is not an important factor in the voice building process. (Reid, 1983, p.

155)

Although Reid was “philosophically opposed to imagery” (Carter, 1993, p. 183), he

realized that metaphorical imagery was sometimes necessary in teaching but held the

conviction that subjective uses of imagery could go counter to the more scientific, and

therefore more technical approach of vocal training. Dayme (1982) also argued for a

more integrated approach, and said, “When all the available knowledge of the mechanics

and artistry of singing is used, then the result can only be a better understanding and

enjoyment of vocal artistry” (p. 3).

Freed (2000) compared metaphorical use of imagery terms included in American

vocal texts from the early part of the twentieth century with its more recent usage. Many

of these terms were still employed in contemporary pedagogy and were directly related to

skill acquisition in voice training (e.g., learning breath support; achieving and executing

proper sensations and placement of the voice). Singers in Carter’s (1993) study reported

using such imagery (e.g. “bellows,” “bouncing ball on a water fountain,” “stretching a

rubber band”), which were used to facilitate vocal production. Freed recommended that

imagery in vocal pedagogy be connected with the more grounded physiological

experiences. Given the subjective and artistic definition of imagery, advising caution in

its use was understandable. Nonetheless, metaphorical imagery in vocal pedagogy was

often required.

While the dangers in using metamorphic imagery were clear, its use in music was

well established and was not likely to change, due to the abstract nature of music (Barten,

1998). However, imagery, as it has been used and investigated in psychology and sport

71
psychology, offers a much broader definition and encompasses mental aspects of musical

and singing development, as exemplified in the work of several pedagogues (Caldwell &

Wall, 2001; Fields, 1972; Ware, 1998). However, music performance has remained a

creative art in which metaphor was valid, especially in singers who created and

interpreted roles and characters.

Character/role images. Character and role images were limited to dramatic,

emotional, and artistic requirements in dance, music, and singing. Dancers, as well as

singers, imagined the behaviors and emotions of their characters as well as their physical

movements and appearance (Carter, 1993; Craig, 1992; Hanrahan & Vergeer, 2000;

Nordin & Cumming, 2005). These were related to the experience of certain qualities,

human or non-human, for instance, a dancer recreating the swan in Swan Lake. Even

instrumentalists in musical compositions have had certain roles in which they created

characters, such as those in Prokofiev’s Peter and the Wolf, wherein specific instruments

represented the different characters in the story. Many musical compositions suggested

some kind of affective ideal or image from which the performer could embody and

present the work. However, in singing, often the interaction of music with the text more

specifically suggested its placement as to how it was to be interpreted and performed.

Operatic singers recreated their characters using words presented vocally, dramatically,

musically, as well as physically, mentally, and affectively (Barten, 1992; Carter, 1993;

Emmons & Thomas, 1998, 2008).

Creating characters and roles in art song recitals was considered more rigorous

and difficult than those in opera or oratorio. Songs for recitals require an entire subjective

world to be recreated mentally, dramatically, and emotionally for each song in order to

72
achieve an effective performance (Barten, 1992; Carter, 1993; Emmons & Sonntag,

2002). Lotte Lehman (1945/1985) in More Than Singing, offered suggestions for

interpreting lieder of many composers (e.g., Brahms and Schumann). She often included

entire scenarios performers could imagine in recreating the song. For example, in

performing Gute Nacht from Die Winterreise by Schubert, she described the specific

scenario when she suggested:

Imagine that you are this man who is on the verge of complete disintegration. It is

a cold winter night. You have decided to leave during the night for if you left in

the daylight, you might see her, from whom you are fleeing, and perhaps if you

should see her again you would weaken and would not find the strength to go. But

you can no longer bear the torture of being near her. You will lose your mind if

you cannot escape from her. (Lehmann, 1945/1985, p. 110)

The poet and composer created an entire dramatic world in these lieder songs, the dictates

of which helped the performer to bring the character to life.

Even though Lehmann maintained that specific images were dictated in the text of

these songs, elite singers in Carter’s (1993) study often felt images were private and were

flexible according to specific need. Some singers created entire stories and scenarios to

act out the images of their songs. Most singers felt that embodying subtext, or underlying

meaning, came from the text or story of the opera or song. Feeling appropriate emotions

and imagining past personal experiences in order to more completely embody the

character of the song best accomplished this. The dramatic skill of imagining the subtext

allowed the performer to unlock emotional significance and communicate the intentions

of the composer and poet to the audience.

73
Many singers’ accounts of their lives in biographies dealt with their experience of

vocal production and interpretation (Ferrier, 1955; Horne & Scorvell, 1984).

Autobiographies such as Lehmann, (1945/1985) and Ferrier (1955) revealed how these

singers brought intuition and imagination into their characters and roles in performances.

Hines’ (1982) interviews of 40 internationally recognized male and female opera singers

illustrated some experiences in creating roles for many operas. Developing singers could

use these accounts as models for their own work with bringing characters to life on the

stage both in imagination and in physical execution.

Context images. Both athletes and performers have reported the use of context

images involving the environment, other people, and specific situations and venues.

Imagery context has been recognized as an important element since the beginning of

mental training in sports, as in this example:

Before the 1976 Summer Olympics, representatives from the Soviet Union shot

pictures of the Olympic facilities in Montreal. These pictures were returned to the

Soviet Union and studied by the athletes. Although the Soviet athletes had not

been to Montreal, they used the pictures to create images of themselves

performing in those facilities. Creating these types of images served to familiarize

the athletes with the Olympic environment before they arrived. (Vealey, 1986, p.

211)

This type of image was reported in athletes’ imagery as various elements in the

surroundings (e.g., sounds from spectators, smell of the grass) (Munroe et al., 2000).

Nordin and Cumming (2005) identified this type as another category that emerged from

dancers’ imagery research. Prior to this sport imagery research related to context had

74
rarely been a separate subject and was incorporated in various studies such as those on

vividness and manipulation of images.

In reporting context images of musicians, Trusheim (1987) found brass

instrumentalists including details of auditions rooms with reviewers and acoustical

qualities of specific performances spaces. These and the musicians in Bellon’s (2006) and

Carter’s (1993) studies described images with other musicians in the performing space or

venue in front of an audience. Furthermore, these singers imagined the entire scenarios of

songs they had created in their minds while mentally and physically performing on stage.

Body-related images. Another imagery type concerned the different body-related

images in which athletes and performers engaged, including anatomy, feeling,

appearance, musical expression, and health concerns (Carter, 1993; Hanrahan & Vergeer,

2000; Munroe et al., 2000; Nordin & Cumming, 2005). Much of the sport research of

body-related imagery has been included in studies investigating kinesthetic imagery, and

is examined in the category of sense imagery later in this section.

The body related category emerged from the Nordin and Cumming (2005) dance

imagery study. Dancers often employed a number of body-related images. Physical and

emotional feelings were connected with arousal, muscular tension, and affective

sensations. Several dancers imagined various and specific parts of their anatomy for

individual performance execution. Dancers also used images of their appearance and that

of their characters in more effectively relating to the audience (Nordin & Cumming,

2005).

Singers who used body-related imagery shared some of the same concerns as

dancers, and had some that differed. Carter (1993) discussed vocalists’ use of imagery

75
based on Leyerle’s (1986) five theoretical categories of organic imagery in singing.

These consisted of posture, respiration, phonation, resonation, and use in vocal or

psychological problem areas. Since Garcia II developed the laryngoscope in 1854 (Garcia

& Paschke, 1975), singers have been able to see and understand the actual anatomy of the

larynx. Subsequently vocal pedagogues have advocated for the increased incorporation of

vocal anatomical images, which were considered more scientific than metaphorical

imagery (Dayme, 1982; Leyerle, 1986; Miller, 1996; Vennard, 1968). More recent texts

have included extensively detailed illustrations of the various aspects of vocal and human

anatomy as it relates to singing (Chapman, 2006; Miller, 2004). Singers imagined their

entire physical body and vocal mechanism, or “interior posture” in controlling the vocal

sound and physically creating their character (Bellon, 2006). Vocalists were found to

have had images arise concerning their ability to sing or not, especially having fears of

losing their voice (Stedman, 1985). Due to the fact that singers depended on the health of

their body as their musical instrument, they used many different body-related images

(Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993).

The use of body-related imagery in achieving musical sound and expression is not

new to musicians. Brass instrumentalists (Trusheim, 1987) and professional singers

(Carter, 1993) used kinesthetic imagery to mental hearing of pitches and musical

passages. Furthermore, one of the singers in Carter’s (1993) study practiced physical

movements related to the rhythm of the music as the result of training in Dalcroze

Eurythmics (Jaques-Dalcroze, 1920/1972). This helped the vocalist physically execute

and subsequently apply the use of kinesthetic imagery to strengthen rhythm and connect

with the natural tension and expression in the particular piece of music. Learning the

76
words in physical rhythmic motion also facilitated memory for several of Carter’s

singers.

Several studies focused on the use of imagery in athletic injury. In an

investigation of the cognitive and motivational functions of imagery, Sordoni, Hall and

Forwell (2000) developed the Athletic Injury Imagery Questionnaire (AIIQ) and found

that injured athletes employed motivational imagery to facilitate rehabilitation. Marathon

runners, older athletes, and those who had been to a sports physician were more likely to

monitor their body for pain and injury than younger and less educated athletes

(Ungerleider, 1992). Athletes reported that recovery from injury was enhanced using

positive self-talk and images of healing (Ievleva & Orlick, 1991). Green (1992) suggested

possible uses of imagery to facilitate the many stages of healing athletic injury. Some

musical performers also used imagery in healing their bodies after injury or coping with

physiological, psychological, and emotional aspects of injury and its consequences

(Trusheim, 1987). Performers consistently used mental practice of their particular

instrument or genre to continue and augment physical practice during fatigue and injury

(Carter, 1993; Nordin & Cumming, 2005; Trusheim, 1987).

The mind-body paradigm literature had supported the connection of imagery with

psychophysical conditions especially in healing (see Sheikh, 2003, for a comprehensive

review). Early studies with cancer patients using imagery to combat cancer reported

positive results of recovery and remission of the disease (Simonton, Matthews-Simonton,

& Sparks, 1971; Simonton, Simonton, & Creighton, 1978). Studies (Schwartz, 1984;

Schwartz & Kussek, 2003) have suggested that the human being is a complete system

made up of a number of subsystems including mind and body. These parts interact

77
resulting in new emergent properties which are facilitated and achieved in the use of

imagery to recover and maintain optimal health.

Irrelevant images. Irrelevant images emerged in the dance research of Nordin and

Cumming (2005) and were not directly addressed in the sport imagery research in Hall

and others (1998) and Munroe and colleagues (2000). Irrelevant images were identified

as those types that spontaneously arose that may have had nothing to do with practice or

performance. Musicians and singer were reported to have images that were not wanted

and that emerged spontaneously (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987). These

were found intertwined in the images they reported. One singer in Carter’s (1993) study

reported the regular occurrence of spontaneous images while using self-hypnosis,

however these images directly related to her performance and could not be considered

irrelevant.

These imagery types as established by Nordin and Cumming (2005) comprised

the content of imagery with which performers, more than athletes, were involved. Most

of these represented the vehicle in which artistic expression was achieved in creative

performance. The general qualities in how athletes and performers engage in imagery

will now be examined.

What: Imagery Characteristics

Imagery characteristics included qualities of imagery as a modality or a by-

product rather than imagery type or content previously discussed. According to Nordin

and Cumming (2005) the characteristics of imagery included six specific qualities: (a)

senses, (b) perspective, (c) ability, (d) direction, (e) deliberation, and (f) amount and

duration.

78
Senses. According to Richardson (1969), sensory imagery differed from

perception in that imagery was not dependent on experiencing outside sensory

experience, but replicated the experience of that sensory information in the cognitive

processes (Richardson, 1969). Senses used in imagery were the same as those involved in

perception and included six categories: (a) visual, (b) auditory, (c) kinesthetic, (d) tactile,

(e) olfactory, and (f) gustatory.

In some of the earliest studies of the use of senses, Galton (1880, 1883) surveyed

visual imagery ability and found it more profoundly developed in engineers, women, and

schoolboys than scientists. In a diary study, Kosslyn, Seger, Pani, and Hillger (1990)

found that two-thirds of imagery in people’s normal life was visual. However, the use of

specific senses often depended on the individual and their chosen activities, or

experiences, as well as the type of sport, art form or music, the various skills and

requirements of the athlete or performer. For example, field athletes used more visual

imagery than marathon runners (Ungerleider & Golding, 1992), which may be related to

the visual components in field athletics. Furthermore, the extensive physical requirements

over time for long-distance athletes may require more kinesthetic imagery.

The specific sensory modality, or its combinations, that athletes employed in

various sports have been examined in a number of studies. Results of sports research

have differed as to which imagery sense athletes use more, visual or kinesthetic. Murphy

and Martin (2002) found athletes preferred kinesthetic to visual imagery, however other

studies reported athletes were better visual than kinesthetic imagers (Barr & Hall, 1992;

Gregg et al., 2005; Hall & Martin, 1997; Moran, 2004; Murphy et al., 2008). Ryan and

Simons (1982) found that better visual or kinesthetic imagers showed more improvement

79
than those with weak visual or kinesthetic skills. Furthermore, soccer players not only

used visual and kinesthetic imagery extensively, but some also reported using auditory

imagery (Salmon et al., 1994). Munroe and others (2000) reported athletes in their study

used visual, kinesthetic, auditory, and olfactory sense imagery. Sport psychologists

generally recommended that athletes use as many sense modalities as possible in their

imagery to increase effectiveness (Moran, 2004; Morris et al., 2005; Murphy et al. 2008;

Vealey & Greenleaf, 2001).

Artistic performers used multiple sense images in ways specific to requirements

of their art. Dancers in Nordin and Cumming’s (2005) study reported experiencing multi-

sensory imagery: kinesthetic, visual, and aural. Elite musicians used the same three sense

modalities in their profession (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Holmes, 2005; Moyer, 1992;

Ross, 1985; Trusheim, 1987). MacPherson (1997) found more experienced musicians

were able to connect their kinesthetic and auditory image with playing wherein the “link

between fingers and singers was automatic and seemed to occur without any conscious

effort” (p. 70). In her interview of a solo cellist and guitarist, Holmes (2005) reported that

instrumentalists imagined the sound, the feel of muscles, the visual image of movement,

as well as the appropriate affect and expression in preparing the actual execution of

music. Recreating the kinesthetic feeling of the proper breath control, together with

imagining the perfect embouchure of the mouth for brass players (Trusheim, 1987) or

position of the fingers for string players (Holmes, 2001), also played a major part in

producing the desired quality of sound.

Although reports of athletes experiencing sounds in imagery are less frequent than

other senses, the difference between their use of auditory imagery must be distinguished

80
from how auditory imagery is generally employed by professional musicians. Athletes

heard the sounds of their sport in their inner hearing, such as splashing of water in the

pool, footsteps on the track, or crowds cheering (Munroe et al., 2000). Most of the

auditory imagery of musicians involved musical elements including pitch, rhythm,

harmony which required a high degree of training, understanding, and awareness of

music in context (Gordon, 1999), which is exemplified in the studies that follow.

Research in assessing sensory ability in imagery with regard to music began being

reported over a century ago. Betts (1909) investigated pitch accuracy and found that

student musicians who reported using auditory imagery scored higher than those who did

not. In a subsequent experiment of the same study, Betts (1909) reported that in sight-

reading or hearing a familiar tune, participants used kinesthetic and auditory images for

internally playing or singing a given composition. In comparing the auditory imagery in

adults and children, Agnew (1922a) found that children and musicians scored higher than

non-musical adults in auditory vividness tests. Seashore (1919) worked extensively in the

area of cognitive music assessment and auditory imagery in his 10-point inventory of

musical talent. Seashore (1938/1967) and later Farnsworth (1958) recognized that

auditory imagery interacting with kinesthetic, or motor imagery, was often accompanied

by visual images. Seashore wrote, “The motor imaginal [sic] type is ordinarily also well

developed . . . the motor tendency to image the tone or execute it in inceptive movements

is highly developed in the musical mind” (1938/1967, p. 6). These early studies served to

strengthen the investigation of vocalists’ imagery use and the ability in mentally

experiencing music through the various sense modalities.

81
Instrumentalists and singers reported first learning their music internally prior to

playing or singing, and later continuing mental rehearsal in relearning and perfecting

pieces (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Holmes, 2001; Moyer, 1992; Trusheim, 1987). James

Olin, a brass professional in Trusheim’s study was quoted as saying, “What I try to do is

to visualize or auralize [sic] or whatever what I want to hear coming out of my horn.”

(cited in Trusheim, 1987, p. 179). Many musicians reported internally hearing their line

of music as well as those of others in the musical ensemble, and often every note in the

entire orchestra, opera, or composition (Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987). Singers who were

trained to play the keyboard reported internally hearing more of the accompaniment than

those who had no keyboard skills (Carter, 1993). This represented elements of Gordon’s

(1999) idea of audiation, which “takes place when we hear and understand in our minds

music that we have just heard performed or have heard performed sometime in the past”

(p. 41). Audiation was not just a high level of auditory imagery but one developed

through a foundation of understanding musical structures and context, the experience of

which elite singers, instrumentalists, and conductors seem to share. This understanding of

imagery in musicians has been examined extensively over the years (e.g., Azzara, 1999;

Liperote, 2006; Reynolds, 1995).

As singers and musicians developed their art through playing, observational, and

listening experiences, they eventually gathered a storehouse of sound images in their

minds (Fields, 1972). This seemed to be supported in recent research in neuroimaging, in

which brain activity of hearing and imagining music was found to be similar (Kosslyn et

al., 2001; Zatorre & Halpern, 2005). Ideal vocal or instrumental sound was used to guide

singing and playing and became more sophisticated over time as influenced by listening

82
and remembering past models of others and one’s own performance experiences (Carter,

1993; Trusheim, 1987). The great violin master teacher, Dorothy DeLay, emphasized

building “a well-differentiated mental vocabulary of sounds” (cited in Stockholm, 1975,

p. 40). Musicians combined aural images of the ideal musical sound with the required

kinesthetic feeling necessary to produce specific qualities of timbre and expression of the

notes or phrases (Bellon, 2006; Trusheim, 1987).

The practice of internally hearing the sound and replicating that ideal auditory

image on the instrument or in the voice replicate in performance has been examined in

the literature (Averino, 1989; Fields, 1972; Ware, 1998). In his treatise on Ideo-Kinetics,

Bonpensiere (1953) promoted concentration on the auditory image to achieve the desired

sound wherein one need only “ideate our end results in a process of flow . . . we ideate

our absolute unconcern with the ways and means to obtain them” (p. 14). Unlike

traditional piano pedagogy, Bonpensiere promoted the primary use of auditory, visual,

and kinesthetic intention while allowing the body to respond to the vividness of the

mental image. He illustrated this in the following quote:

Never think of your music in terms of execution (of what your hands and fingers

should or are going to do) but in terms of interpretive rendering (what you would

expect it to sound like if a performer from heaven were executing it for you). (p.

69)

Brass instrumental professionals reported performing as a result of clearly imagining the

individual idea musical sound prior to playing (Trusheim, 1987). Using auditory and

kinesthetic imagery served to facilitate proper execution of musical sound in both

instrumental and vocal production.

83
In vocal pedagogy, Averino (1989) stated that “[i]n order to produce a song, the

singer must have it clearly in mind: pitch, rhythm, musical form and the emotional

content of the words. The performer is the imagination” (emphasis hers, p. 40). Fields

(1972) advocated that vocalists create a clear mental ideal auditory image of the sound

they desired to produce prior to its execution. In producing that aural image, Geraldine

Farrar’s statement reflected this in the following, “[W]e learn to know the sensations

produced in muscles of throat, head, face, lips and other parts of the anatomy, which

vibrate in a certain manner to correct tone production. We learn the feeling of the tone”

(cited in Brower & Cooke, 1996, p. 53). Lamperti concurred when he said: “Do not listen

to yourself sing! Feel yourself sing! When internal conditions are right and ready, the

singing voice appears not before” (cited in Brown, 1931/1973, p. 16). Since the body was

considered the singers’ instrument, they were not able to hear their own voices as others

hear them. Therefore singers learned early to equate the desired tone in auditory imagery

with the kinesthetic feeling of what was required to produce that exact sound (Bellon,

2006; Carter, 1993; Stedman, 1985). Furthermore, pitch and vowel production often

elicited kinesthetic imagery by singers in Moyer’s (1992) study.

For vocalists, kinesthetic, auditory, and visual imagery were considered the most

important senses used for expressive and artistic performance (Bellon, 2006; Carter,

1993; Emmons & Thomas, 1998, 2008). Singers were found to use kinesthetic imagery to

control breath, feel rhythm, and physiologically respond with emotion and expression in

portraying a character in an aria or song (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993). Those who had

experienced viewing the internal mechanisms of their voice through laryngeal image

biofeedback (LIB) were able to connect these images both visually and kinesthetically,

84
imagining the vocal anatomy in preparing and singing a variety of desired sounds

(Cleveland, 1989a, 1989b, 1989c). With training and experience imagery using all the

senses can be trained and strengthened, which has been corroborated since imagery

seems to become more complex over time (Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987). The use of

sensory imagery seemed to be essential to the singers’ and musicians’ expressive and

artistic performance. How athletes and performers executed their images through the

senses, including perspective and various abilities is being addressed as follows.

Perspective. The question of efficacy of the use of internal compared with

external imagery was tested in many studies yielding a variety of results. Mahoney and

Avener (1977) were some of the first researchers to identify imagery perspective as an

important element in imagery use for the performer. Lane (1980) described internal

perspective (first person) as one of “involvement,” while the external viewpoint (third

person) was as if the person was looking at himself in a video of a performance as a

“spectator.” Internal imagery was more highly correlated with motor movement than

external imagery (Hale, 1992; Harris & Robinson, 1986). More skilled athletes reported

more frequent use of internal than external imagery (Mahoney & Avener, 1977; Rotella,

Gansneder, Ojala, & Billing, 1980). Conversely, several studies found no significant

differences between the effectiveness of internal and external imagery (Barnes, 1982;

Highlen & Bennett, 1979; Mumford & Hall, 1985). However, in Hall and others (1990),

elite and novice athletes reported equal use of internal and external imagery, often

switching between them. Imagery perspective effectiveness was also correlated with

types of sport and performance task requirements (Hardy, 1997) with external imagery

more associated with sports in which form was essential. Sequence of actions were

85
improved with external imagery while internal perspectives were preferred if there was

only a single task to perform. Results of imagery benefit depended on the athlete’s (a)

imagery ability, (b) the ability to imagine the suggested image, and (c) the type of task

(Taylor, 1993). Performance skills with a concentration on form were more associated

and often perfected with external imagery, while using internal imagery perspective

seemed more successful with wrestlers or tennis players.

The intermingling of kinesthetic senses with visual perspective imagery has been

the subject of some controversy (Callow & Hardy, 2004). In a study investigating the US

track and field participants of the Seoul Olympics, Ungerleider and Golding (1991, 1992)

found that more successful athletes used external perspective and stronger kinesthetic

imagery. Hale (1982) argued that the kinesthetic sense was more associated with internal

imagery, while others (White & Hardy, 1995; Hardy & Callow, 1999) held that

kinesthetic imagery involved both internal and external perspectives. Hardy (1997) and

White and Hardy (1995) proposed that different task requirements seemed to suggest

specific perspectives, recommending the use of kinesthetic imagery in combination with

both internal and external imagery to increase effectiveness. Future studies investigating

the specific brain functions of vision and motor centers may shed more light on this

subject.

Nordin and Cumming (2005) found dancers using a combination of internal

imagery, such as imagining the emotions of a character, and external imagery,

particularly in regard to their concern with bodily appearance on stage. Bellon (2006)

reported singers using internal perspective, especially regarding the mechanics of vocal

production, and external perspective for perceiving their appearance in relation to the

86
hall, stage, and audience. Singers also reported combining internal and external

perspectives to imagine the entire venue in which they were performing, including the

hall or arena, stage, audience, and dressing rooms. Musicians as well as dancers imagined

how they would feel and look to be in a new competition or performance space in order

to familiarize themselves with its many features (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Nordin &

Cumming, 2005; Trusheim, 1987). Although Carter (1993) did not directly identify the

different visual imagery perspectives used by singers in her study, participants’

descriptions suggested employment of both internal and external imagery. Caldwell and

Wall (2001) and Emmons and Thomas (1998) recommended that singers learn and

become proficient in using both internal and external imagery. Imagery perception also

was influenced by various capabilities to engage in the image itself.

Ability. One of the variables in imagery use was the ability of the individual and

the degree to which he or she could vividly produce mental images. It was also important

to discern what types of imagery the person preferred to employ, or those with which one

was familiar. The effectiveness of imagery was dependent on a number of factors: (a)

skill level, (b) past training and experience in imagery, and (c) degree to which imagery

was consciously directed. The type of athletic or artistic skill in which one was engaged

to perform may have also influenced this imagery ability. For example, because of

extensive training, musicians typically may have had the opportunity to develop more

auditory imagery ability than athletes who may have excelled in kinesthetic images.

Types of imagery ability included all the various sense modalities just as they were

experienced in real life. The differences in the development of mental use could have

been a result of the reinforcement and feedback, or lack of it, for using the various types

87
of imagery. The desired outcome for which the imagery was being used may have been

dependent on the individual’s ability to use imagery (Arieti, 1976; Martin et al., 1999;

Murphy & Martin, 2002; Richardson, 1983).

Several instruments have been devised to assess imagery ability in athletes. One

of the earliest was Betts’ (1909) study, Distribution and Functions of Mental Imagery,

the instrument of which was later shortened by Sheehan (1972) and widely used in

imagery research. Marks (1973) created the Vividness of Visual Imagery Questionnaire,

or VVIQ. The two most popular and useful in sport were Vividness of Movement

Imagery Questionnaire (VMIQ; Isaac, Marks, & Russell, 1986) and the revised

Movement Imagery Questionnaire (MIQ-R; Hall & Martin, 1997). Two criticisms of

these instruments in assessing imagery ability were that much of the support comes from

the research team in which these tests were developed, and that these self-reported tests

were hard to replicate with subjects often reported positively at the upper end of the

distribution (Sommer, 1980).

The effects of imagery ability differences have been most examined in sport

psychology research, especially with regard to performance excellence. In learning and

performing motor skills, good imagers often had the advantage over poor imagers (Goss,

Hall, Buckolz, & Fishburne, 1986; Isaac, 1992). Rodgers and others (1991) reported

athletes’ imagery ability as being fairly well developed. Elite athletes used clear and

accurate imagery and were found to be better imagers than non-elite athletes (Barr &

Hall, 1992; Orlick & Parrington, 1988; Salmon et al., 1994; Ungerleider & Golding,

1992; Vadocz et al., 1997). Athletes better imagined skills in which they had already had

gained mastery and their imagery abilities increased the more they were used (Gregg et

88
al., 2005; Rodgers et al., 1991; Vadocz et al., 1997; Ungerlieder & Golding, 1991) and

the higher the effectiveness of that imagery became (Hall et al., 1990; Vadocz et al.,

1997; Vealey, 1986; Weinberg & Gould, 2003). Athletes with better imagery ability in

vividly producing and controlling images were thought to enjoy superior performances

(Isaac, 1992; Ryan & Simons, 1982). Researchers agreed that imagery was a skill that

could be developed (Hall, 1998; Hall & Martin, 1997; Morris et al., 2005) and recently

has been investigated more thoroughly using advanced medical imaging techniques.

Investigations of artistic performers’ imagery ability were few indeed. Agnew’s

(1922a) study reported that music teachers and children possessed good auditory

imagery. Most of the dancers in Nordin and Cumming’s (2005) investigation offered self-

reports of possessing the ability to produce vivid, life-like images, depending on the type

of imagery and the sense modality used. Some had difficulty with imagery in developing

or portraying characters or controlling imagery involving injury or pain. All but one of

the brass musicians in Trusheim’s (1987) study had no difficulty in creating and

controlling images. Many of these professionals had performed for many years and

reported that particular uses of imagery were generally quite vivid. Both singers (Carter,

1993) and musicians (Bellon, 2006) reported having facility with imagery. Generally,

artistic performers possessed fairly adequate ability in imagery. The next imagery

characteristic was the direction in which imagery was used by athletes and performers.

Direction. Research in imagery direction, a term coined by Short and others

(2004), dealt with whether the image was facilitative or debilitative. In one of the first

experiments, Powell (1973) examined the effects of using positive and negative imagery

in 18 female clinical psychology students in a dart-throwing task. While one group

89
positively imagined the darts hitting in the center of the target, which led to higher scores,

another group imagined a negative performance of missing the bull’s eye, which resulted

in significantly poorer scores. Despite the lack of a control group, Powell concluded that

positive imagery content could be facilitative, and negative imagery could debilitate

performance. A number of subsequent studies supported Powell’s findings (e.g. Gould,

Weinberg, & Jackson, 1980; Short et al., 2004; Woolfolk, Parrish, & Murphy, 1985). In

examining performance effects, Epstein (1980) and Woolfolk, Murphy and others (1985)

found no effect on performance using positive imagery, while negative imagery

debilitated performance. Therefore, it could not be assumed that positive images were

facilitative or negative images were debilitative (Short, et al., 2004). Further research is

needed to clarify this distinction.

Nonetheless, positive imagery was correlated with successful outcomes and

negative images with poorer performances in athletes (Hall et al., 1990), instrumental

musicians (Bellon, 2006; Trusheim, 1987), and vocalists (Carter, 1993), with some

exceptions (Bellon, 2006). Positively imaging the desired outcome improved athletic and

artistic performance, particularly during competition, while negatively imagining the task

seemed to be detrimental (Munroe et al., 2000; Woolfolk, Murphy et al., 1985). Positive

imagery was correlated with increased confidence (Moritz et al., 1996; Trusheim, 1987)

and decreased anxiety (Vadocz et al., 1997). Not all imagery was positive. Sometimes the

negative image, as in rerunning mistakes over and over, could degrade performance in

athletics (Murphy et al., 2008), dance (Nordin & Cumming, 2005), and music (Bellon,

2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987). Some of these issues were connected with imagery

deliberation.

90
Deliberation. Deliberation in imagery involved the individual’s ability to

volitionally control and consciously direct images. According to Richardson (1983,

1994), imagery facilitated learning especially for acquiring perceptual motor skills if it

was voluntary and controllable. Imagery deliberation seemed to range on a continuum

from those spontaneously appearing to the ones that were fully manipulated (Murphy et

al., 2008). The area of deliberation was also connected with whether imagery was used

effectively or produced adverse effects. There were a number of deliberation variables

that could impact performance. Imagining inappropriate action, such as the negative

examples previously mentioned, could result in experiencing performance deterioration.

Using the wrong imagery type for a certain outcome could have perhaps negatively

affected outcome. Other elements included presence of debilitating anxiety, distraction

and lack of control of spontaneous or certain kinds of imagery, and images producing

overconfidence, adversely affecting performance (Carter, 1993; Emmons & Thomas,

1999; Ristad, 1982). Researchers have investigated various ways in which athletes and

performers could gain better control of their imagery.

Controllability was the ability to consciously manipulate images (Richardson

1994) and had been assessed by several instruments. Early psychological investigations

into controllability and manipulation of images focused on the problem of mental rotation

of an object (Shepard, 1978; Shepard & Metzler, 1971). Many other scales of imagery

ability have been developed and implemented in various settings. Gordon’s (1949) Test

of Visual Imagery Control (TVIC) was devised to assess control, the ability to

manipulate, or hold images in the mind. More recently, Vella-Brodrick and MacRae

(2004) reviewed the imagery assessment instruments used in the athletic context, the

91
discussion of which was beyond the scope of this study. However, the results were

promising for validating imagery use in performers and supported the need for more

research in this area.

It could be assumed that imagery practice improved performance but research has

shown that this was not always the case. In an early study investigating imagery control,

Clark (1960) recounted a participant’s lack of control:

[O]ne subject reported mentally attempting to bounce the ball preparatory to

shooting only to imagine that it would not bounce and stuck to the floor. This

disturbed him to a point where he could not successfully visualize the shooting

technique. (p. 567)

Clark (1960) reported gains in self-confidence and error identification with participants

who learned to increase their imagery ability and control. Athletes with better imagery

ability in consciously producing and controlling images were found to enjoy superior

performances (Isaac, 1992). To illustrate the importance of individual meaning and

interpretation in imagery use, Murphy and Martin (2002) offered an account of several

skaters imitating the imagery of another skater who used an image of a ball of light to

facilitate his performance. Using this model image produced a variety of negative effects

in the other young skaters, highlighting the individualistic nature of imagery and the need

to match imagery type for intended outcome.

Research has indicated that most performers experienced some spontaneous

imagery; whether it is used to help the performer often depended on experience and

ability. Dancers and musicians experienced spontaneous imagery including negative,

intrusive images such as rerunning a poor performance (Bellon, 2006; Nordin &

92
Cumming, 2005; Trusheim, 1987). Dancers reported actions or sensations “triggering”

spontaneous imagery. It appeared that images could be controlled or uncontrolled as well

as debilitative or facilitative. Further variables such as amount and duration helped to

increase our understanding of imagery use in performance.

Amount and duration. Amount and duration were two ways in which time spent

engaging in imagery has been examine. Amount of imagery referred to how often

individuals engage in the actual imagery session. Imagery duration was the length of time

it took for the image to appear, remain or proceed, and fade. It seemed to vary by

individuals, different sports and performance types, skill level of performer, imagery

types, and time of season (Munroe et al., 1998).

The type of execution in which the individual was engaged may have determined

the length of the imagery session. For instance, imaging application of a single skill

would have only taken a short amount of time, while imaging an entire performance of a

role in an opera would have taken a much longer time, perhaps as long as it would have

taken to actually perform it. Hall (2001) recommended more imagery practice for best

results. Ungerleider and Golding (1992) reported that the amount of mental practice time

was directly related to the amount in which track and field athletes engaged in training.

Only a few studies have examined the amount necessary for imagery

effectiveness. Short imagery sessions of one to three minutes were found to be more

effective than longer ones of five to seven minutes for basketball players (Etnier &

Landers, 1996). In their meta-analysis, Driskell and others (1994) reported that the longer

one engages in mental practice, the less effective are the results. They recommended that

93
imagery sessions and training last about 20 minutes for optimal results. More research is

needed in this area.

The duration of specific imagery sessions was examined in several studies.

Imagery duration included the amount of time it took for the emergence of an image, the

time in which one was actually engaged in imaging, the speed of the imagery, or how

long its usefulness lasted (Nordin & Cumming, 2005). Mackay (1981) demonstrated that

imagery reflected internal cognitive processing and therefore it could be assumed that

imagery directly correlated to one’s relationship with how performance was executed.

Imagery times were highly correlated with actual time it took to execute that skill in

slalom races (MacIntyre & Moran, 1996) and canoe-slalom (Hall & Martin, 1997).

Experts’ imagery of their springboard dives times were closest to actual times (Reed,

2002), whereas divers with less experience employed longer imagery durations than those

of their actual dives.

Studies focusing on the amount and duration of imagery use in artistic performers

remained sparse. However, Nordin and Cumming (2005), Carter, (1993), and Trusheim

(1987) reported that performers often engaged in mental imagery, depending on the time

of season, type of performance requirements, and the individual’s experience. One of the

instrumentalists in Bellon’s (2006) study reported up to one third of his practice time was

spent in mental practice. It was possible that the more years involved with the

performance, the more the individual engaged in imagery and the more effective mental

practice became. Durations of imagery varied for different performers. Dancers reported

using slow, actual, and fast speeds in using imagery especially in running through

routines or memorizing sequences (Nordin & Cumming, 2005). Trusheim (1987)

94
reported brass instrumentalists using a compressed mental practice session. A singer

reported imagining in real time, the same as actual performance (Bellon, 2006). Imagery

speed seemed to vary with the individual needs, requirements of the task, and personal

preferences. The various aspects of why imagery was used by athletes’ and artistic

performers will now be examined.

Why or Reasons for Imagery Use

In examining the reasons why athletes and performers used the various types of

imagery, a number of empirical developments changed how imagery use and function

were regarded. It was originally thought that athletes used imagery for skill acquisition

and training (Feltz & Landers, 1983). However, more evidence surfaced that imagery was

also being used to enhance performance and motivation (Hall et al., 1990; Munroe et al.,

1998; Paivio, 1985; Weinberg & Gould, 2003). Athletes were found to use both cognitive

and motivational imagery in varying degrees (Hall et al., 1998; Paivio, 1985) depending

on the sport, the individual athlete, level of competition in which they were involved, and

the time of season (Cumming & Hall, 2002a; Hall et al., 1998; Munroe et al., 1998).

Furthermore, one image could be used for several reasons as readily as several types of

imagery could be used for a single purpose (Callow & Waters, 2005; Fish et al., 2004;

Murphy et al., 2008; Nordin & Cumming, 2005; Short et al., 2004; Short et al., 2006). In

examining the purposes dancers used imagery, Nordin and Cumming (2005) added

artistic and healing reasons, which were deemed appropriate to how singers used imagery

(Carter, 1993). Investigating the developments of imagery study in how it evolved in

sport psychology and its adaptations and correlations to artistic and musical performance

help to clarify the main questions in this study.

95
Analytic framework of imagery use. In analyzing the research on why athletes and

artistic performers imagined, a number of important developments transpired. Paivio’s

(1985) analytic framework of imagery provided a foundation for understanding use and

posited that imagery influenced performance in both cognitive and motivational

functions, both of which operated at a specific and general level. This 2 X 2 orthogonal

model created one dimension as cognitive-motivation, and the other as specific-general.

The resulting four imagery areas were: (a) cognitive specific (CS), or skill acquisition,

development, and performance; (b) cognitive general (CG), strategies and routines; (c)

motivational specific (MS), goal oriented behavior; and (d) motivational general (MG),

physiological and affective arousal. Later, Hall and others (1998) divided motivational

general into two distinct areas, resulting in motivational general-arousal (MG-A), or

arousal modification or psyching up, and motivational general-mastery (MG-M), or

mental toughness, self-efficacy and concentration (see Table 3). The major contributions

and findings of studies addressing the analytic framework of imagery use in sport, dance,

and music, including cognitive, motivational, artistic, and healing reasons were pertinent

to why singing professionals use imagery in achieving optimal performance and will now

be examined.

Cognitive reasons. Generally, imagery was often used to solve problems,

understand certain challenges, or simply change concepts and beliefs (Nordin &

Cumming, 2005; Murphy et al., 2008). These cognitive reasons for using imagery

encompassed functions of both the CS, or skills and mental practice, and SG, or game

plans, strategies, and routines.

96
Table 3

Analytic Framework of Imagery Effects

Cognitive Motivational

Specific Skills (CS) Goal-oriented (MS)

(e.g., motor skills, technique) (e.g., imagining scenarios and

performance in future projection)

General Strategy (CG) Arousal (MG-A)

(e.g., game plans, strategies, (e.g., relaxation, psyching up and

routines, sequences, and planning) increasing arousal)

Mastery (MG-M)

(e.g., confidence, mental toughness,

positivism, focus, and concentration)

Note: Adapted from Paivio (1985) and Hall, Mack, Paivio, and Hausenblas (1998, p.74).

Cognitive specific (CS), or mental practice, had been the most widely researched

area of imagery, and was generally understood as the internal learning, practice, and

performance of both cognitive and motor, or physical, skills (Driskell et al., 1994; Feltz

& Landers, 1983; Hall et al., 1990; Hall, Schmidt, Durand, & Buckolz, 1994; Mahoney &

Avener, 1977). Results of the extensive volume of mental practice studies were

summarized in several meta-analyses (e.g., Driskell et al., 1994; Feltz & Landers, 1983;

Hinshaw, 1991; Richardson, 1967a, 1967b). Athletes used mental practice imager to

learn skills, techniques, and actions in order for them to become automatic in

97
performance. Mental practice was found to be more effective for novices in more

cognitive than physical tasks, while “experienced subjects benefited equally well from

mental practice regardless of task type” (Driskell et al., 1994, p. 488). Generally it was

accepted that the effects of mental practice were moderated by a several factors: (a) type

of task, (b) retention interval or duration of time between treatment and assessment, and

(c) experience levels of the subjects. While a full analysis of the many studies in mental

practice was beyond the scope of this study, the findings as they directly related to

imagery use in professional performers were included here.

The use of mental practice in musical performance had been explored in a number

of experimental investigations. Studies in musical performance compared physical

practice (PP) with mental practice (MP) in memorization of specific musical excerpts.

These included studies in piano (Coffman, 1987, 1990; Highben & Palmer, 2004; Lim &

Lippman, 1991; Lo, 1976; Rubin-Rabson, 1941), trombone (Ross, 1985a, 1985b),

clarinet (Coffman, 1987), guitar, and vocal performance (Theiler & Lippman, 1995). The

majority of these investigations involved primarily university students rather than

professional classical instrumentalists or singers. Table 4 provides a comparison of seven

of these mental practice studies listed alphabetically by author. These studies were

conducted in testing different applications of physical practice (PP) and mental practice

(MP) in music performance, including general information on participants, variables,

tasks, assessments, and results.

98
Table 4

Musical Mental Practice Studies Comparison*

Study N Experience Variables Control Musical Performance Posttest Results

Level Task Assessed

Coffman 80:40 College a) PP, b) Yes, sight- 4 part church Performance Second half MP

(1987) male, music majors MP reading hymn for time duration, of alternating

study on 40 novice piano alternating motivational children pitch, rhythm composition w/PP

the female players w/PP, group (pretest- superior

piano first half)

Highben 16 Half college Feedback Yes 4 pieces: 2 posttests: Subjects

& music majors, conditions 2 measures Auditory w/ strong

Palmer half piano a) normal, of early ability, aural

(2004) performers b) auditory Baroque Motor skills were

study on from the hearing style, ability least

the community only compositions affected

piano (min. 6 years c) motor- 2 major, by

training) hands only 2 minor. memory

d) covert- test with

no feedback

movement removed

or sound

Lim & 7 College piano a) MP with No 6-16 m. (25- Note accuracy, 2 posttests Physical

Lippman majors score, b) 37s) of rhythmic practice

(1991) (min, 11 MP with Weiner, accuracy, was best

study on years formal score and Faure, phrasing & followed

the training) listening, c) Haydn, articulation, by

piano PP with Schumann, dynamics & listening

score Valenti, & musical to an aural

Mendelssohn expression model

Note: MP: Mental practice; PP: physical practice.

99
Table 4 (continued)

Study N Experience Variables Control Musical Performance Posttest Results

Level Task Assessed

Lo Piano College class a) PP, b) Yes (PP Rhythm, Yes, Pre and MP group

(1976) piano MP only) melody, memorizing 4 posttest improved

students harmony of part hymns more than

a four-part PP group

hymn

Ross 30 College a) PP Yes Etudes Correct notes Posttest Combination

(1985) music majors w/MP, b) and rhythm of PP & MP

study on PP, c) MP superior

the guitar w/slide

movements,

d) control

Rubin- 9 Skilled 5 min, MP None Challenging Memory 2 weeks MP-PP-MP

Rabson 3 groups pianists (ages prestudy, etudes from later & 7 facilitated

(1941) of 3 21-25 years) Gr. 1 MP- Zipoli, months learning and

study on PP-MP-PP; Pasquini, later memory,

the piano Gr. 2 MP- Galuppi, PP and MP

PP-MP; Frey, resulted in

Group 3 Cohnany, less

PP-PP Gal, and retention and

Toch. more trials

Theiler 7 College (a) all PP, Control 4 17th C. Pitch & rhythm 2 MP &

& guitarists, guitar and (b) MP w/ mixed Guitar accuracy, posttests continuous

Lippman, 7 voice majors PP, (c) MP, with PP Dance articulation & w/score, PP most

(1995) vocalists (ages 19-29 aural model excerpts, phrasing, 2 effective for

study on years) listening, sight- dynamics & posttests guitarists

the guitar w/PP, singing expression, from

(d) control exercises tempo, tonal memory

w/PP for singers. quality

Note: MP: Mental practice; PP: physical practice.

100
One of the first researchers to conduct mental practice studies in musical

performance was Rubin-Rabson (1941) who examined memory of compositions in piano

students using combined mental practice (MP) and physical practice (PP) with all

physical practice. Rubin-Rabson employed analytical pre-study prior to three treatments,

which included: (a) PP, MP, PP; (b) PP then MP; and (c) PP plus extra PP. Results

indicated that subjects who used mental practice in the middle of learning had the higher

scores for memory than subjects who practiced mentally after learning. Scores for this

group were retained weeks later, while the other groups’ scores declined.

Other researchers modified this analytical pre-study model in examining mental

practice in pianists. Lo (1976) reported MP groups memorized four-part hymn

significantly better than the PP only group in class piano subjects. Coffman (1988) found

that alternating MP and PP was superior to the PP only or MP only groups in trials of

novice pianists learning a composition. Lim and Lippman (1991) examined seven piano

majors’ mental practice effects. After 10 minutes of rehearsal, scores were higher in the

group using physical practice followed by listening to an aural model and mental

practice. In examining mental practice in guitarists and vocalists, Theiler and Lippman’s

(1995) design included a control and three treatments: (a) PP only; (b) PP and MP; and

(c) a combination of PP, MP, and an aural model. For guitarists, MP treatment was

superior to all others for pitch accuracy. The results for vocalists in this study revealed

that combining MP and PP with an auditory model was superior.

Mental practice studies have also examined the use of movement, or simulated

performance, and aural ability in conjunction with imagery. In dance, Hanrahan, Tetreau,

and Sarrazin (1995) also found that MP enhanced performance when movement was

101
incorporated. Ross (1985a) reported that combining physical and mental practice was the

most effective method of mastering a musical task, however the inclusion of movement

simulating practice with either mental or physical practice was not significant. Highben

and Palmer (2004) found that aural ability, more than motor ability significantly affected

the memory of a newly learned piece for 16 pianists. Highben and Palmer suggested

focus on the aural image rather than the kinesthetic activity of hand movements

facilitated better performance memory.

Generally, the results of mental practice in musical performance extended the

findings in sport research. The effects of mental practice seemed to be comparable to

those of physical practice, although physical practice still seemed optimal, especially in

less experienced musicians. Combining MP with PP was found to be as effective, if not

more effective than PP alone. Novices as well as experienced musicians seemed to

benefit slightly less from MP, than those with intermediate experience. Novices lacked

technical skill and experts achieved a ceiling effect since they had less to learn.

Dancers reported using imagery in learning to clarify their work, strengthen

memory, change their habits, and solidify corrections, which related to CS functions.

Imagery aided their physical practice by making it more conceptually concrete and

streamlining their rehearsal (Nordin & Cumming, 2005). Artistic performers were found

to often use imagery for learning and refining skills and techniques, and memorization in

order to automate skills and interpretations (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Nordin &

Cumming, 2005; Trusheim, 1987), particularly for solo performances.

In sports, cognitive general (CG) functions of imagery of development and

execution of sequences and strategies included rehearsing game plans and routines

102
(Munroe et al., 2000; Murphy et al., 2008). Researchers investigated various CG imagery

in two similar ways: (a) routines such as those in artistic gymnastics (White & Hardy,

1998) and on the pommel horse (Mace, Eastman, & Carroll, 1987) and (b) strategies as

found in slalom canoe races (MacIntyre & Moran, 1996), football (Fenker & Lambiotte,

1987), and wrestling (Rushall, 1988). Results in using CG imagery were reported to be

beneficial to organizational performance.

Related to cognitive specific (CG) imagery functions, dancers imagined entire

performances, planning, or adapting to differing variables such as stage blocking or

familiarizing themselves with performance spaces and venues (Nordin & Cumming,

2005). Musicians reported using imagery in strategizing pre-performance routines,

organizing their practice for better performance execution, and to execute conductors’

directions and plan routines (Bellon, 2006; Trusheim, 1987). They also used imagery to

coordinate difficult aspects of performance, deal with technical and musical challenges,

and cope with performance distractions and varying acoustical effects of different

performance halls (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987). Furthermore, singers

imagined coordinating details of entire performances, for example, entrances, managing

costumes, props, and sets, and dealing with blocking and venue considerations (Carter,

1993).

Motivational reasons. Motivational functions of imagery use included three

reasons: (a) goal setting and achievement, or motivational specific (MS); (b) modifying

and controlling arousal, included in motivational general-arousal (MG-A); and (c)

developing and maintaining confidence, self-efficacy, mental strength as in motivational

general-mastery (MG-M) (Hall et al., 1998).

103
Athletes reported using various types of imagery in the MS function to set and

achieve goals, such as improving performance, mastering specific skills, overcoming

challenges, winning competitions, and motivating themselves to succeed (Hall et al.,

1990; Murphy et al., 2008). Orlick and Parrington (1988) interviewed 75 Canadian 1984

Olympian athletes in their practices and reported, “The best athletes had clear daily goals.

They knew what they wanted to accomplish each day, each workout, each sequence or

interval. They were determined to accomplish these goals and focused fully on doing so”

(p. 111).

Dancers reported using imagery to aid their use of goals (MS) and to increase

their incentive to continue, particularly in the face of difficulty (Nordin & Cumming,

2005). This was also found in singers who also used imagery to progress in their goals of

gaining roles, winning competitions, or just improving certain skills (Bellon, 2006,

Carter, 1993). The goal imagery most associated with motivation seemed to involve

mastery (Lacaille, Whipple, & Koestner, 2005), which also related to preparation and

strengthening confidence in optimal performance.

Motivational general-arousal MG-A was related to controlling affective states,

stress, and competitive anxiety levels, that is “psyching-up” and relaxation or calming

down (Barr & Hall, 1992; Monsma & Overby, 2004; Orlick, 1990; Salmon et al., 1994;

Vadocz et al., 1997; White & Hardy, 1998). Munroe and others (2000) identified three

elements of arousal and stress in motivational imagery: (a) excitement, (b) control, and

(c) relaxation. Athletes and artistic performers differed in arousal requirements due to

physical and mental performance requirements.

104
Extensive research in general psychology has been conducted in the use of

relaxation and effective stress techniques in arousal modification. Progressive Muscular

Relaxation (PMR) was a scheme in which Jacobson (1957) employed imagery techniques

to systematically tense and then relax each muscle. Meichenbaum (1977, 1985)

developed stress inoculation training (SIT) in cognitive behavior modification, in which

subjects were taught to use imagery and self-instruction and self-talk directives, such as

“stay calm” and “easy does it.” In extending the work of Jacobson, Suinn’s (1986) Visuo-

Motor Behavior Rehearsal (VMBR) was originally developed for sports and focused

primarily on creating a relaxed state in which imagery could better affect specific areas of

performance.

Various texts focused on the applications and techniques of relaxation imagery to

facilitate successful performance (e.g., Benson, 1987; Gawain, 1978, 2002; Harris, 1986).

Performers have often had to contend with seemingly uncontrollable levels of nervous

tension, the effort of which has tended to increase negative effects. Green and Galway

(1986) and Ristad (1982) have recommended that performers accept the symptoms of

anxiety by reframing the context of their individual meaning. Singers in Carter’s (1993)

study differentiated between positive or anticipation and working tension and negative

anxiety and unwanted tension or the spontaneous emergence of little “devils”.

Additionally, performers used different methods of combining breathing techniques with

relaxation imagery to facilitate performance in sport and music (Bellon, 2006; Carter,

1993; Harris, 1986; Sisterhen, 2007).

Optimal arousal levels in performance varied with athletes, type of sport, as well

as specific task requirements (Schmidt, 1982). Athletes used MG-A images to control

105
emotions and stress levels, particularly prior to performance (Hecker & Kaczor, 1988);

Munroe et al., 1998). Athletes used many types of imagery to change their emotional

condition, the MG-A function, particularly to relax, “psych up,” and energize themselves,

thus modifying arousal to appropriately execute performances (e.g., Harris, 1986;

Murphy et al., 2008; Orlick, 1990; Oxendine, 1980; Vadocz et al., 1997). Oxendine

(1980) reported that highly anxious participants in his study were successful in low

difficulty tasks and low anxious subjects were more successful in high difficulty tasks.

This was illustrated in the Yerkes-Dodson Law, which stated, “complex tasks are

performed better when one’s drive is low, while simple tasks are performed better when

drive is high” (Oxendine, 1980, p.104). In contrast to the high levels of MG-A imagery

used by athletes to facilitate performance, dancers and musicians required much more

subdued levels of arousal (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Cumming & Nordin, 2005;

Stedman, 1985; Trusheim, 1987). Musicians generally performed better when they were

relaxed and calmed, however, many believed that some anxiety, or adrenaline, was

beneficial to performing optimally.

The practice of relaxation in imagery has been the subject of some controversy. It

had long been an accepted practice to require relaxation prior to imagery engagement as

proposed in Jacobson’s (1957) Progressive Muscular Relaxation (PMR) and Suinn (1976,

1986) Visuo-Motor Behavior Rehearsal (VMBR). Little empirical support for this did not

affect the insistence of sport psychologists to require relaxation before motor imagery

(Weinberg & Gould, 1999). However, Holmes and Collins (2002), in supporting imagery

applications that complied with the findings of functional equivalence theory, argued that

arousal levels in imagery should reflect the optimal levels of the performance of that

106
specific task. Since pre- imagery relaxation inhibited sport performance, which was

supported in Perry and Morris (1995), actual arousal and emotional levels should be

embedded in the imagery in order to precisely replicate the task in the brain.

The extensive personal exposure in solo singing performance before an audience

could modulate arousal and performance success. Extreme feelings of vulnerability and

self-criticism could deteriorate confidence, increase arousal levels, and debilitate

performance (Bellon, 2006; Ristad, 1982; Stedman, 1985). The nature of singers

embodying the instrument often precluded personal identification with singing ability and

the approval of the audience. As a result, singers may have perceived or imagined the

audience regarding them in the three possible ways: (a) negatively or with hostility, (b)

positively and with support for the singer, or (c) accurately responding to each moment in

the performance (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993). All these factors and variables impacted

MG-A imagery use by singers.

Motivational general-mastery (MG-M) imagery was examined frequently in

sports literature (e.g., Abma et al., 2002; Hall et al., 1998; Munroe et al., 2000; Weinberg

& Gould, 2003) and was related to five themes: (a) being mentally tough, (b) focus and

concentration, (c) self-efficacy and confidence, (d) being positive, and (e) modeling.

Mental toughness involved handling difficult situations and elements of adversity

usually just prior to or during performance. Focus and concentration dealt with the mental

ability to directly control images, thoughts, and distractions, which could affect optimal

performance. In a study investigating qualities and behaviors of 235 Canadian Olympic

athletes, Orlick and Parrington (1988) concluded that:

107
In almost all cases the best focus was one that kept the athlete connected to what

he or she was doing (his or her job). In contrast, the worst focus was one in which

the athlete was dwelling on factors over which he or she had no direct control,

such as other competitors, final outcome, or other distractions. (p. 116)

When distractions were allowed to debilitate performance, confidence levels decreased.

Therefore, ability to concentrate and focus could influence self-confidence and

perceptions of mental toughness of the performer.

Levels of concentration seemed to affect performers’ involvement in their

presentations (Fields, 1972). Debilitating distractions decreased when performers focused

on the specific tasks of the performance, characterization, and communication with the

audience (Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987). According to Csikszentmihalyi (1990),

sustained involvement in the task was directly correlated in those individuals who

experienced optimal performance. In describing a performer completely involved this

way in a role, Csikszentmihalyi (1990) stated:

Self-consciousness, which is the most common source of distraction, is not a

problem for such a person. Instead of worrying about how he is doing, how he

looks from the outside, he is wholeheartedly committed to his goals. In some

cases it is the depth of involvement that pushes self-consciousness out of

awareness, while sometimes it is the other way around: it is the very lack of self-

consciousness that makes deep involvement possible. (pp. 211-212)

This deep personal connection with the performance seemed to be regulated by the level

of confidence, focus, and concentration of the performer.

108
Imagery relating to confidence and self-efficacy were involved with feeling

successful, positive, and up to the task. Self-efficacy theory (Bandura, 1977, 1982, 1997)

posited that motor performance was enhanced by images of successful expectations.

Bandura (1997) stated that “[e]fficacy beliefs affect thought pattern that could enhance or

undermine performance” (p. 116). He further proposed that self-efficacy was belief in

one’s own capabilities in acting in specific ways. In sport research, MG-M imagery was

correlated to self-efficacy in competitive athletes (Mills et al., 2000-2001; Moritz et al.,

1996; Vadocz et al., 1997; Vealey, 1986) and was a factor distinguishing elite and less

successful performers (Gould, Weiss, & Weinberg, 1981; Woolfolk, Murphy, Gottesfeld,

& Aitken, 1985). Employing and maintaining positive imagery and outlook was found to

facilitate athletic performance (Munroe et al., 2000). Positive self-talk and stopping

negative thoughts were also used in achieving confidence (Gammage, Hardy, & Hall,

2001; Hardy, Gammage, & Hall, 2001). Sport researchers (Orlick, 1990; Rushall, 1988;

Suinn, 1972, 1986) supported the use of confidence-building techniques in imagery in the

applied athletic fields. Athletes who imagined performing in a confident manner were

better able to develop, maintain, and regain confidence (Moritz et al., 1996).

In vocal pedagogy text, the idea of mentally perceiving oneself as strong,

confident, and mentally fit for the task of performing was not new. In reflecting some of

the qualities of MG-M imagery, the renowned vocal teacher, Vennard (1971), rigorously

supported the idea of inner composure and self-efficacy in the following description:

The first requirement of the singer-personality is poise. He must have confidence

in himself. This is why natural singers are often insufferable egotists. They are not

egotistical because they sing well; on the contrary, they control their voices well

109
because they are so self-confident. . . . This is their priceless asset. Dampen the

exhibitionist pleasure they take in their voices, and you will have damaged the

voices themselves. . . . Another requirement of the singer-personality, related to

poise, is perseverance. Poise is courage for the challenge at hand; perseverance is

strength of character for the long pull. (Vennard, 1971, pp. 24-25)

For Vennard, successful singing required a strong sense of self-confidence, especially in

regard to vocal control and mental toughness. Carter (1993) and Bellon (2006) reported

elite singers used strong, positive self-images and self-talk continually in rehearsing,

auditioning, and performance. Furthermore, they were found to be adept at controlling

mental, physical, and environmental distractions through focus and concentration. Fields

(1972) advocated that the joy of singing consisted of “a sense of well-being and

exhilaration of spirit that comes from the experience of being free from worry or restraint

of any kind” (pp. 9-10). Stedman (1985) reported that singers used imagery for

confidence building, personal enjoyment, and becoming more proficient in executing

techniques and embodying qualities necessary for mastery.

The use of modeling ideal performances, whereby performers learned a new skill

or technique by watching an expert performer, also related to MG-M imagery use

(Bandura & Jefferies, 1973). The image of the ideal performance served to motivate the

individual to perform beyond what had been previously considered possible. Feltz (1984)

argued, “ just mentally seeing oneself successfully performing the desired task is enough

to convince an athlete that he or she has the ability to successfully execute the task” (p.

193). Rushall (1988) employed covert modeling, to assist performance anxiety conditions

in an elite wrestler, strengthening confidence. Musicians used modeling and role-playing

110
in imagining how an outstanding performer would execute a specific performance to

increase their motivation and self-confidence (Bellon, 2006; Caldwell & Wall, 2001;

Emmons & Thomas, 1999). Singers modeled performance excellence in front of a mirror

for immediate feedback to see how they were physically projecting the character.

Artistic reasons. Artistic reasons addressed the creative, expressive, and

interpretive aspects of meaning in performance interpretation. Although artistic image

functions were rarely addressed in sport research this was an important aesthetic aspect in

figure skating, synchronized swimming, and artistic routines in gymnastic floor exercises

(Hays, 2002; Murphy et al., 2008). Likewise, dancers used imagery to evaluate their

performance and to check external body awareness such as stage blocking, musical

timing, and coordination with other dancers to achieve appropriate artistic imagery in

performance (Nordin & Cumming, 2005). Instrumental and vocal musicians used

imagery to create artistically expressive performances and following conductors’ artistic

directions (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987).

Singers imagined engaging in artistic expressivity, assuming the character or role,

and effectively communicating with the audience (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Stedman,

1985). Elite vocalists in Carter’s (1993) investigation recognized that technique and vocal

material had to be completely solid and automatic in order for positive interaction with

the audience to occur. Carter (1993) reported that singers in her study exchanged

imaginal interactions of “electricity,” “vibrations,” and “love” (p. 245) with their

audiences. They also responded to positive nonverbal, kinesthetic feedback from their

audiences, by feeling strengthened confidence and self-efficacy. Artistic performers used

111
specific sense imagery in achieving appropriate artistic images relevant to the desired

performance.

Health reasons. Due to the extensive physical requirements in both athletics and

artistic performance, the body was highly regarded and maintaining excellent health was

a paramount concern for performers. Imagery in the process of maintaining health had

been the focus of many studies (Sheikh, 2003; Sheikh & Jordan, 1983; Sheikh & Korn,

1994). Imagery use in illness and healing was popularized with the work of Simonton and

his colleagues (Simonton et al., 1971; Simonton et al., 1978) in their treatise in the mental

applications for treating disease, especially cancer, as previously mentioned. This method

of imagery use in rehabilitating injury and pain management has been applied in sports

(Orlick, 1990). Green (1992) suggested imagery applications for facilitating sport injury.

Athletes and performers used imagery for healing purposes in several ways. Elite

gymnasts imagined their routines to forget their pain and become more involved in the

performance (Calmels et al., 2003). According to Nordin and Cumming (2005), dancers

used imagery for four different healing reasons: injury prevention and healing, pain

management, spiritual healing, and rejuvenation. Imagery was used as a substitute for

actual practice to save energy and rest the body when they were too tired to perform.

Dancers also used imagery to improve the internal awareness of their body such as

posture, coordination, balance, and weight distribution (Nordin & Cumming, 2005).

Singers and instrumentalists often used mental practice when they were sick or needed to

preserve their voices and fingers (Bellon, 2006). Brass musicians used imagery during

health recovery and recreated memories of former performance excellence (Trusheim,

112
1987). Injury prevention, healing, and recovery were an important aspect represented in

the major musical performance studies in imagery.

Research in Imagery Use of Musical Performers

Several extensive studies have been conducted in interviewing outstanding

musical performers in their use of imagery, which extended beyond those elements

previously mentioned. Trusheim (1987) investigated how professional orchestral brass

instrumentalists used imagery. Using a similar framework, Carter (1993) examined the

use of imagery in elite singers. Most recently, Bellon (2006) interviewed several different

kinds of performing musicians in their use of sport psychology interventions, including

mental imagery. Many of the specific findings from these studies have previously been

reported in the various categories, however these studies represent extant research more

closely addressing the use of imagery in musical performers, especially singers, as

presented in the sport psychology research literature. These three studies will be analyzed

individually in regard to their relevance to the present study. Table 5 shows the three

major imagery studies in musical performance, the participant performers, general uses of

imagery as they relate to dance and sport research, as well as the findings of each study.

Trusheim Study

In one of the first extensive interview studies on musical performers’ use of

imagery, Trusheim (1987) investigated 26 top brass instrumentalists who were members

of five of the most prestigious symphonies in major cities of the United States, using the

iii Framework representing the image, imagining, and imagination (Rosenberg, 1987).

Trusheim (1987) interviewed these musicians concerning their use of imagery in the

various aspects of training, rehearsal, and performance. These elite brass players reported

113
Table 5

Major Imagery Studies in Musical Performance

Researchers Performers N Sense Metaphoric/ Perspective Findings


Imagery Artistic

Trusheim Elite Orchestral 26 Auditory, Artistic Internal and Instrumentalists

(1987) Brass visual, expression, external use imagery

kinesthetic, conductors’ extensively

tactile imagery

Carter (1993) Elite Singers/ 11 Auditory, Artistic Internal and Singers use

Teachers kinesthetic, expression, external imagery

visual communicating artistically and

w/audience expressively

Bellon (2006) Singers, 6 Auditory, Artistic Internal and Performers use

Instrumentalists, kinesthetic, expression external imagery,

Conductors visual visualization,

relaxation and

energizing

extensive use of metaphoric and artistic imagery. They also used imagery in modeling

other revered players and developed a high degree of proficiency in aural and kinesthetic

imagery in executing their musical skills and talents. Trusheim did not compare his

findings with those in sport research, but in his literature review, he did devote a page to

imagery studies primarily focused on Suinn’s (1983, 1986) VMBR techniques for

visualization and relaxation.


114
Trusheim reported these musicians also used imagery in what could be considered

the cognitive types and functions of skill learning and acquisition, as well as strategies in

executing performance requirements. These instrumentalists regularly used mental

practice to secure passages and augment physical rehearsal, often in various locations and

when playing their instruments was not appropriate. These professionals also employed

motivational imagery types and functions to regulate their arousal to appropriate levels

for optimal performance. Furthermore, the brass players in the study engaged in imagery

to achieve various goals, boost confidence, and master the mental and emotional

requirements of performance. These musicians used imagery in a variety of applications,

including artistic purposes in musically expressing the compositions and healing purposes

when they were recovering from illness, reflecting dancers’ imagery purposes (Nordin &

Cumming, 2005). Their use of imagery compared quite favorably to the research in

imagery types, times, locations, and functions addressed in sport and dance research.

Trusheim’s study raised several concerns that were relevant to the present study.

Although the participants represented some of the most prestigious positions in the

orchestras of the United States, these subjects were all male ensemble musicians and their

identities were all revealed in the study. Their high standing in the musical community

could have influenced their responses. These musicians did not perform regularly as

soloists without instruments, standing face-to-face singing to a demanding audience, as is

the norm in classical solo vocal performance. Furthermore, they were not responsible for

memorizing words or text, often in another language, to songs, arias, and entire roles in

operas, representing vocal skills that require years of development and refinement. Many

115
of these requirements and issues were addressed in the Carter (1993) study of vocal

professionals.

Carter Study

Following Trusheim’s (1987) study, Carter (1993) used Horowitz’s (1978)

psychological model to investigate the use of imagery in 11 elite vocalists. Using a

moderately scheduled interview format, topics included: career preparation, teachers’ use

of imagery, performance anxiety issues, mental rehearsal, and mental imagery in vocal

pedagogy. These issues related to the present study perhaps more than any other music

investigation.

The results of this comprehensive study directly addressed many of the aspects of

using imagery outside of the context of sport psychology. Vocalists in the Carter study

reported being involved with imagery from an early age and continued to use various

aspects of imagery in their profession. A number of vocalists stressed the importance of

acquiring a “storehouse of aural images” throughout life, beginning in early childhood.

Many of them learned to use metaphorical imagery from their teachers, especially in

developing proper vocal technique building and breath management, diction, and

expressive performance. Concerning sense imagery, vocalists reported using kinesthetic

imagery most frequently, followed by auditory imagery, then visual imagery. Imagery

was used for technique building as an aid in interpreting vocal literature. Imagery assisted

these vocalists in developing their characters and roles in affective expression and

effectively communicating with the audience. Singers used mental practice to augment

physical practice during injury or times when they could not physically sing, for example

when traveling, at night, or just prior to performance. Imagery use assisted these vocalists

116
in reinforcing positive beliefs, calming performance anxiety, and facilitating performance

preparation, including imagining performing a new role or in a new venue or opera in

which they had yet to perform.

Various aspects of the Carter study were cause for concern in relation to the

present study. The participants in the study were all located in the New York and Boston

area of the United States. It is not certain that their use of imagery was a result of their

locality and proximity to other performers who practice imagery in their profession.

Furthermore, many of these singers were directly involved in teaching other vocalists,

which may have facilitated their own use and propensity to use imagery in singing. One

of the interviewees, an outstanding singer also known for her excellent use of imagery in

her teaching, had previously taught had two of the other participants. Even though Carter

(1993) did not connect the imagery uses of vocalists to those in sport psychology, there

were many references that correlated with the uses of imagery found in dancers (Nordin

& Cumming, 2005). Carter’s imagery definitions were more in keeping with vocal

pedagogy than the more athletic uses such as the imagery types, characteristics, and

functions described in this study.

Bellon Study

Bellon (2006) interviewed six musical performers from different fields in the

effort to compare their mental practices with the main principles of sport psychology,

including “goal setting, pre-event routines, visualization, focus, arousal regulation

(energy levels), and optimal performance” (p. 1). Although Bellon (2006) used a general

model from sport psychology texts, her interviews did provide a wealth of information

for how professional musical performers use specific types of imagery as defined in sport

117
texts. Specific studies in sport psychology research were not included. Only one section

investigated imagery and visualization; however, many of the issues in the study dealt

with the mental aspect of practice, preparation, and performance. Bellon’s results

concluded that all of the musicians in her study used imagery in ways that mirrored much

of content and functions of imagery used in dance and athletics.

Some of the criticism of the Bellon (2006) study concerned sampling issues,

interview questions, and terminology, as they were related to the present study. The small

sample of six participants included two singers from Quebec (a retired mezzo soprano, a

part-time singer and choral conductor), two oboe instrumentalists (one from France and

the other from Puerto Rico), an instrumental conductor, and a collaborative pianist, both

of whom were professors at Arizona State University. Of the two singers, one had

enjoyed a somewhat shortened international career and had just retired from singing,

while the other was a part time professional vocalist. This was a very broad sample,

raising questions of the degree to which the results were generalizable to other

performers, particularly professional singers. Some of the interview questions may have

elicited specific results that could be judged as leading the participant rather than probing

specific subjective responses. For example, the question was asked, “What distracts you

most during or before a performance?” (p. 3). This question presupposed that the

individual could initially be distracted during or before a performance, leaving little room

for an alternative affective experience. Furthermore, Bellon continually referred to

imagery as visualization rather than differentiating between the various sensory aspects

of imagery: visual, kinesthetic, auditory, and metaphorical types or functions of images.

118
One of the limitations shared among the Trusheim (1987), Carter (1993), and

Bellon (2006) studies was the use of professionals whose names were revealed and often

highly recognized in the field. Sharing the names of those individuals included in an

interview study allowed for the risk that the participants would shape their answers in

order to appear better than what they may share if their identities remained confidential.

The Trusheim (1987), Carter (1993), and Bellon (2006) studies represented the

primary interview investigations of the use of imagery in musical performers, which were

relevant to the present study. They could be considered part of a growing body of

research in using imagery in musical performance. Bellon’s (2006) study was the only

researcher to base her study on the principles of sport psychology applications. These

investigators did not attempt to correlate the findings in musical performance with the

theoretical framework provided in recent developments of imagery in sport psychology.

Therefore, it was with caution that the four Ws of the use of imagery: where, when, what,

and why (Munroe et al., 2000; Nordin & Cumming, 2005) was applied to these studies.

Information gathered on musical performers’ imagery use in the Trusheim (1987), Carter

(1993), and Bellon (2006) studies provided part of a strong foundation for this study.

Conclusion

This chapter has provided an examination of the theories relevant to imagery use

in sport, dance, musical, and vocal performance. These included the early contributions

and development of theories, the cognitive-based theory, the psychological states theory,

and the more recent developments in the theory of functional equivalence. The body of

literature focused on important aspects of imagery using the four Ws framework: where,

when, what, and why from research in sports (Munroe et al., 2000) and dance (Nordin &

119
Cumming, 2005). Also included were the results from imagery research with performing

musicians (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1998). Other relevant theories were

examined on the effects of imagery.

In synthesizing the theories of imagery, the four Ws of imagery use in sport

(Munroe et al., 2000) and dance (Nordin & Cumming, 2005), and the related musical

performance research, the researcher proposed the vocalists’ four Ws of imagery

framework (see Table 6). This framework was created to guide the development of this

study in gathering and reporting the data from the solo professionals’ interviews.

Specifically, it was intended to assist in constructing codes and themes in the analyses of

the data and as an initial step in creating the vocalists’ imagery framework for this study.

120
Table 6

Proposed Vocalists’ Four Ws of Imagery Framework

Question Category Subcategory

Where (place) Singing Places Practice Areas

Stage

Home Bed

Other places Hotels

Transit Commuting

Anywhere

When (time) Practice Pre

During

Post

Performance Pre

During

Post

At Home

Traveling

Anytime/all the time

What (content) Imagery Types Metaphorical/Artistic

Character/Role

Body-Related

Imagery Characteristics Senses

Ability

Direction

Amount & Duration

121
Table 6 (continued)

Question Category Subcategory

Why (reasons) Cognitive Learning

Improving

Memorizing

Planning

Motivational Specific (goals)

Drive

Thought/feeling change

Musical Sound Pitch/Tonal

Accompaniment

Rhythm

Style

Artistic Aesthetic Style

Interpretation

Communicating w/audience

Health Maintenance

Healing Injury Prevention

Recuperation

Irrelevant Spontaneous

In the remaining chapters, the research design of this study is presented in Chapter

Three. Results of the interviews and the subsequent categories and subcategories are

explained in Chapter Four. Chapter Five includes a discussion on the findings in relation

to the relevant research.

122
Chapter 3

Method

The purpose of this study was to investigate imagery use in achieving optimal

performance in vocal professionals. Research from sport psychology and particularly the

framework of the four Ws of imagery use: where, when, why, and what, (Munroe et al.,

2000; Nordin & Cumming, 2005) were used to compare and contrast singers’ imagery

experiences in order to contribute to the body of knowledge of imagery in musical

performance practice and development. The inquiry was also based on imagery research

in the professional practice of other musical performers such as brass players (Trusheim,

1987), various musicians (Bellon, 2006), and vocalists (Carter, 1993). This chapter

describes the research design, participants, instrumentation, and procedures used in this.

The chapter concludes with a summary.

Research Design

A basic interpretive qualitative research design was chosen for this study since the

use of imagery, as defined by sport psychology, in professional vocalists was a relatively

new and understudied area of research. Patton (2002) recommended: “In new fields of

study where little work has been done, few definitive hypotheses exist and little was

known about the nature of the phenomenon, qualitative inquiry was a reasonable

beginning point for research” (p. 193). This research approach allowed for one to gain an

in-depth understanding of how vocal professionals used imagery in their profession. Data

123
were collected through interviews designed to gather a broad and rich body of

information on imagery use in these vocalists.

In a qualitative study such as this, it was reasonable to expect the researcher to

have some background and experience in the area being investigated, particularly since

the researcher is considered an instrument in qualitative study. In an effort to gain

reflexivity in the study, it was important to disclose specifics of the researcher’s

background to allow the reader to understand how the researcher interacted with the

various phases and aspects of the study (Patton, 2002). The researcher held undergraduate

and graduate degrees in applied art, theatre, and music, particularly vocal performance.

She also participated in a young artists’ training program in a regional opera company

and has performed in operatic roles, concerts, and recitals professionally. The researcher

has taught general and choral music in public schools as well as in a private studio

teaching voice and other musical instruments. Additionally, she has been an avid sport

exerciser for her entire life. Through this training and experience, the researcher was

familiar with the language and practices of professional vocalists as well as imagery.

During the course of conducting this study, she was in the process of discovering the

relevance of imagery in sport psychology. Despite the possible threat of bias, this

background made the researcher uniquely qualified to conduct, analyze, report, and

discuss a study such as this.

Sample Selection

Purposeful, homogeneous sampling was the method used to select the participants

for examining imagery use by professional vocalists. The sample consisted of 15 out of a

124
total number of 18 vocal professionals who were invited to participate and met one of the

following criteria:

1. Singers who held a four-year degree in musical performance from an

accredited university or conservatory and had worked and made the majority

of their living for at least four consecutive years as a professional solo

vocalist.

2. Singers without a four year college degree, who had worked and made the

majority of their living for at least eight consecutive years as a professional

vocalist.

In either case, participants were solo singers who had primarily sung in the Western

classical singing tradition, including work in opera, concert, recital, and oratorio. This

population of vocalists was chosen as the best source for providing information on the

use of imagery by singing professionals. “Information-rich cases are those from which

one can learn a great deal about issues of central importance to the purpose of the

research” (Patton, 2002, p. 46).

In order to locate these participants, snowball or chain sampling (Patton, 2002)

was initiated by asking singers and professors of vocal performance known to the

researcher to identify and suggest others who fit the criteria of this study and were willing

to participate. These information-rich individuals, or key persons, were sampled from

different parts of the United States and had experience in the many areas of solo classical

repertoire performance. They represented various stages of their career, beginning with at

least four years of experience as a professional to retirement age. Both males and females

were targeted with voices in the major vocal registers: soprano, mezzo, tenor, baritone,

125
and bass. In contacting these individuals by email, the researcher invited singers she

knew to participate and asked them to suggest other singers who might have been

interested in being interviewed for the study (see Appendix B). Vocal professors and

teachers previously known to the researcher in Alabama, California, Michigan, Florida,

New Jersey, New York, Massachusetts, and Pennsylvania were also emailed and asked to

identify and contact other potential participants who had been or were currently

performing professionally (see Appendix C). The minimum sample size was determined

to be at least five male and ten female singers from varying vocal registers and from

different regions of the United States in an attempt to include singers not restricted to one

or two cities.

Solo classical professional vocalists were chosen as participants of this study for

five reasons. First, the specific requirements of the professional singer were extensive and

singers could benefit extensively from knowledge about the specifics of the use of

imagery in performance excellence. Second, the increasing incidences of including

various aspects of imagery in vocal texts (e.g., Caldwell & Wall, 2001; Chapman, 2006;

Dayme, 2005; Emmons & Thomas, 1998; Williamon, 2004) suggested the need for

further scientific inquiry in how professional singers use imagery. Third, this researcher

was able to use her training and expertise in singing and imagery research in interviewing

the professional vocal participants. Fourth, an initial sample of professional singers was

available to this researcher through past experience and network in the field. Finally,

since many of the athletic and music studies in imagery have used the ever prevalent and

convenient subject pool of college students, professional singers with a minimum of four

years of professional experience were chosen for their expertise in their field. Yuille

126
(1985) as well as Lindauer (1983) called for more appropriate methods of investigating

imagery studies than the highly controlled experimental designs. He stated, “We need to

discard experimental methodology for the investigation of context-dependent processes

and replace it with field-based research” (p. 146). He also criticized the use of the

convenient sampling of undergraduates and recommended using a sample on which the

inquiry focused. For these reasons, singers whose primary source of income was solo

classical professional vocal performance were chosen for this study.

Measures

Interview Protocol

The singers’ interview guide (see Appendix D) was developed for use in this

study. Questions included in the protocol were designed to gather information about

where and when singers used imagery, what kinds of imagery they engaged in, and why,

or the reason they used imagery. Using an interview guide approach as suggested by

Patton (2002), this semi-structured format allowed for inclusion of ideas relevant to the

inquiry, introduction of new concepts in the interview, and exclusion of items not

germane to the participant’s experience of imagery. Furthermore, specific probes were

applied, where appropriate, as a follow-up to responses by the individual interviewee.

The Nordin and Cumming (2005) dancers’ interview guide (see Appendix E),

from the four Ws of imagery use study served as a basis for gaining information and

allowing the participants to share their thoughts and expertise about their use of imagery

in vocal performance, in order to address the specific qualities and concerns of

professional vocalists. The dancers’ interview guide was adapted with permission (see

Appendix F) to reflect the findings in the music literature and what the researcher, as a

127
professional singer, considered pertinent to eliciting imagery responses from the

professional vocalists. Adaptations also included personalizing the interview guide for

this research and adding a working definition of optimal performance. In order to avoid

the assumption that the participant used imagery in the first section of the protocol,

imagery was defined and described. The first question asked of the participant was if he

or she ever engaged in this type of imagery. If the participant affirmed that imagery was

used, the interview continued. Additionally, a new category was added to the Why section

called Your Sound since it was assumed that musicians might imagine musical sounds or

use imagery to recreate vocal sounds (Averino, 1989; Carter, 1993; Fields, 1972; Hines,

1982; Miller, 1996; Trusheim, 1987).

The singers’ interview protocol was pilot tested with a professional singer who

met the requirements for participation. Only minimal suggestions to include several

appropriate probes were made. The interview guide was revised based on these

suggestions. Therefore the instrument protocol was then administered in the actual

interviews with the chosen participants. The use of the singers’ interview guide assisted

in addressing more holistically the subjective nature of imagery (Lindauer, 1983).

Ethical Considerations

Upon receiving approval from the Institutional Review Board (IRB), the

researcher contacted identified vocalists by email and invited them to participate in the

study. Email correspondence was chosen for convenience in the recruitment process, due

to logistics, time, and financial considerations. Vocal performance professors were sent

an email and letter by the researcher requesting them to suggest eligible participants for

the study (see Appendix C). Eighteen eligible vocalists were sent a letter of invitation to

128
participate (see Appendix B). Included in these documents was an explanation of the

purpose and rationale of the study, the general definition of imagery, the intended use of

the data, the intention and purpose of recording the interview, and the details of

confidentiality of their identity (see Appendix G). The response rate resulting from the

initial 18 invitations to participate in the study included 15 singers.

Vocalists who were interested in participating were informed of the procedures

and requirements of the study by being read the Consent Script (see Appendix H) in a

preliminary telephone conversation. This included the invitation to participate, the

purpose of the study, description of procedures, intent of the researcher to record and

transcribe verbatim, the invitation to review and edit their transcriptions including a

timeline for return, and the risks, inconveniences, and benefits of being involved in the

study. In order to gain more candid and accurate responses of imagery use by these vocal

professionals, their identities were not revealed. This was articulated in the statement of

confidentiality and singers were also informed that their participation was completely

voluntary. The participants agreed to the terms in the Consent Script, the conditions of

the requirements of the study, to be recorded, and to allow their recorded responses to be

used for this, and possibly future research.

Procedures

In order to familiarize participants with the study, the imagery terms as used in

sport psychology and the requirements for participation were described in an attached

letter to both potential participants and vocal performance professors (see Appendix G).

The following definition of imagery was provided by email to the participants:

129
Imagery is an experience that mimics real experience. We can be aware of

‘seeing’ an image, feeling movements as an image, or experiencing an image of

smell, taste or sounds without experiencing the real thing. Sometimes people find

that it helps to close their eyes. It differs from dreams in that we are awake and

conscious when we form an image. (White & Hardy, 1998, p. 389)

The participants understood the requirements that they had to be earning at least half of

their income from vocal performance engagements. Participants were sent a copy of the

Consent Script (Appendix H) and the Singer’s Interview Guide (Appendix D). Next, a

convenient time and date for the subsequent telephone pre-interview was scheduled.

The researcher conducted a pre-interview telephone conversation with each of the

study participants, during which time the Consent Script (see Appendix H) was read to

them. Participants were able to clarify any concerns with the procedure of the inquiry,

logistics of the telephone interview, the intention to record the discussion, and the

definition of imagery as it relates to sport psychology. The actual telephone interview

was scheduled at an appointed time convenient for the participant. At the designated time

the interview was conducted and digitally taped using the Zoom H2 Handy Recorder, and

a telephone equipped with a speakerphone.

The telephone interview began with a statement of the purpose of the study,

verbal verification of the participant’s name and agreement to participate in the study and

interview, and the White and Hardy (1998) imagery definition (see above). The interview

commenced and the length for each session lasted between 40 and 150 minutes.

Participants were asked to answer questions in the interview protocol (see Appendix D),

which addressed each of the four Ws, that is, where, when, what, and why they used

130
imagery were then specifically discussed. Probes were used to gain understanding of

imagery as defined in sport psychology and reported in the literature (Bellon, 2006;

Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987).

Upon completion of the telephone interview, each conversation was transcribed

verbatim and the document was sent via an email attachment to each respective

participant for review, additions, verification, and validation as a procedure of member

checking (Johnson & Christensen, 2004). Each vocalist was given the opportunity to

scrutinize his or her own interview for accuracy and clarity. Participants were asked to

return their comments and any changes within seven days of receipt of the document. If

there was no response by that date, the researcher sent a second email and when

necessary, telephoned the participant to verify receipt of the interview transcription and

to request a response. A second date was offered and if there was no response, the

interview was assumed to be accurate. All of the participants made minor revisions, and

most requested that superfluous words, “like,” “um,” and “you know” be removed. These

suggestions were honored.

Along with the final copy of the transcription, each participant was sent via email

the vocal participant survey (see Appendix I) requesting information on participants’

demographics, education, and experience in the field. All singers completed and returned

this survey and the results were also submitted for analysis.

Timeline for Data Collection

Prospective participants and vocal teachers and professors were sent initial email

invitations to participate in the study in early December 2008. A week later, a second

invitation was sent to any person who did not respond to the first invitation. Pre-interview

131
telephone calls were made to the vocalists who had met the participation criteria by mid-

December, 2008. All interviews took place between the middle of December 2008 to

early January 2009. Interviews were then transcribed and emailed to each respective

participant for member checking between early January and the middle of February 2009.

Data Analysis Procedures

Upon completion of the telephone interviews and participants’ verification of the

transcriptions, data analysis began. The pilot participant’s interview was also included in

the body of interviews. During the course of listening carefully to the participants’

responses in the earlier interviews, more probes were included in the remaining

interviews. Transcription responses were coded manually. Each unit of meaning, whether

it was a word, a phrase, or an entire chunk of text, was coded using a priori categories

derived from the interview protocol that was based on the previous research in the four

Ws of imagery use and the literature. Each code was color coded and entered in the right

margin of each interview quote. Units of meaning fit into major themes and new

categories and subcategories emerged where they were not able to be included in the a

priori set. Similar codes were then categorized together to identify new themes. Each of

these entries was then categorized, subcategorized, and counted for utilization frequency

(Kvale, 1996; Kvale & Brinkmann, 2009; Leech & Onwuegbuzie, 2007).

Both deductive and inductive approaches (Patton, 2002) were used. Deductive

analyses connoted that which was previously known in an area of inquiry, represented

here by the prescribed conceptual framework of the four Ws of imagery use: where,

when, what, and why in athletes (Munroe et al., 2000) and dancers (Nordin & Cumming,

2005), cognitive and motivational functions of imagery (Paivio, 1985; Hall, 1998) and

132
the artistic and healing reasons (Nordin & Cumming, 2005). Inductive analysis and

reasoning was employed to accommodate new information gathered from participant

interviews and allowed for new categories to emerge from the data. This exploratory

method was used for this study rather than testing a proposed hypothesis.

Primary data sources for the singers’ interview guide for each of the research

questions is shown in Table 7. However, the intrinsic nature of imagery created a

challenge in separating the specific units of meaning provided in the interview quotes.

Participants’ responses to the research questions were not only found in their direct

answers to the questions posed during the interview but were intricately interwoven

throughout the interview, as Patton (2002) stated, “the relevant data won’t be found in the

same place in each interview” (p. 440). Often the participants’ responses included

information relating to many themes, not only those directly addressing the proposed

questions. Therefore, each transcription was analyzed line-by-line, revealing units of

meaning ranging from single words to entire paragraphs.

Legitimization

The research process itself served to secure efforts to legitimize the findings of

this study. Validity was strengthened by using these expert participants in this specific

field, the professional vocalists, who were chosen due to their first hand knowledge,

experience, and understanding of how singers can effectively use imagery for achieving

optimal performance. “Consulting with experts who possess conceptual and practical

experience in the field of investigation is an ideal method for ensuring satisfactory face

and content validity” (Vella-Brodrick & MacRae, 2004, p. 124). Internal validity was

enhanced by providing a complete description of participants, procedures, and analyses

133
Table 7

Research Questions as Directly Addressed in Singers’ Interview Guide

Research Question Singers’ Interview Guide Questions

Where do vocal 40) Where do you use imagery?

professionals use

imagery to achieve

optimal performance?

When do vocal 39) When do you use imagery?

professionals use

imagery to achieve

optimal performance?

What do vocal 27) Describe any other reasons that you have for using imagery to achieve

optimal performance, that are different from the ones we have mentioned?
professionals use in
29) To what extent do you use imagery relating to vision?
their imagery to
30) To what extent do you use imagery relating to sound and hearing?
achieve optimal
31) To what extent do you use imagery relating to smell?

performance? 32) To what extent do you use imagery relating to taste?

33) To what extent do you use imagery relating to touch?

34) To what extent do you use imagery relating to kinesthesia?

35) Describe any other kinds of sensations that you feel in your imagery?

36) Which of the above would be the primary senses you use in your

imagery?

37) Describe your ability to use imagery to achieve optimal performance?

38) To what extent is your imagery facilitative, debilitative, or both?

Note: Interview questions taken from the Singers’ Interview Guide (see Appendix D).

134
Table 7 (continued)

Research Question Singers’ Interview Guide Questions

Why and for what 15) Describe any imagery you use that is based on skill learning and

execution?
purpose do vocal
16) Describe any imagery you use relating to sequences?
professionals use
17) Describe any imagery you use relating to strategies?
imagery to achieve
18) Describe any imagery you use relating to arousal and anxiety?

optimal performance? 19) Describe any imagery you use relating to self-confidence and mastery?

20) Describe any imagery you use relating to goals?

21) Describe any imagery you use relating to metaphors?

22) Describe any imagery you use relating to character development?

23) Describe any imagery you use relating to emotions?

24) Describe any imagery you use relating to energy?

25) Describe any imagery you use based on appearance?

26) Describe any imagery you use based on your sound?

28) Describe any other reasons that you have for using imagery to achieve

optimal performance, that are different from the ones we have mentioned?

Note: Interview questions taken from the Singers’ Interview Guide (see Appendix D).

used in this study, as well as allowing for accurate comparisons to other studies and

participants. Member checking (Johnson & Christensen, 2004) was conducted by sending

a transcribed copy of the interview to each participant for review. These singers were told

that they could change, add, or clarify anything in the interview they felt was unclear,

omitted, or unintended. In order to provide the reader with a more complete

understanding of the processes of the results and offer an opportunity to make individual

conclusions, examples of quotes were provided to illustrate categories and groupings.


135
Furthermore, participants were asked at the end of each interview if they had been led in

any way during the course of the interview.

Trustworthiness

Mental imagery was difficult to objectively measure because it was not an overtly

observable behavior. Singers might not have revealed all of the pertinent information

they experienced internally concerning imagery. This could be due to a number of

reasons including these: (a) there may have been a lack of agreement in the definitions

and uses of imagery terminology between athletes and vocalists, (b) it was possible that

these subjective experiences were present in vocalists but they had not previously been

required to verbalize or analyze them, or (c) this internal process of imagery use may

have been too personal and the vocalist may not have been completely comfortable with

conversing about this topic. These issues were addressed beginning with the initial stages

of contact with the potential participant. The White and Hardy (1998) imagery definition

was given to bridge any latent gaps in the use of this term between sports and music. The

preliminary telephone conversation was intended to alleviate any of the participants’

concerns, gain rapport with the researcher, and facilitate understanding of the imagery

subject in general.

Researcher Bias

An important concern in this study throughout the interviews and the analysis was

the threat of researcher bias. It was reasonable to expect investigators of qualitative

studies to have some degree of expertise in the area of inquiry (Patton, 2002). Although

this researcher, trained in both singing and imagery, was a necessary aspect of this study,

her participation in the interviews also posed the threat of researcher bias. A number of

136
elements were put in place to control for this potential problem. As recommended by

Miles and Huberman (1994), detailed records of the process to collect and interpret data

were maintained. In order to strengthen authenticity and trustworthiness, peer debriefing

was conducted throughout all stages of the research process. Peer debriefing was defined

as “exposing oneself to a disinterested peer in a manner paralleling an analytic session

and for the purpose of exploring aspects of the inquiry that might otherwise remain only

implicit within an inquirer’s mind” (Lincoln & Guba, 1985, p. 308). These stages of the

research process included the design of the study, choosing and adapting the interview

protocol, interviewing participants, coding issues, methods of reporting, and discussion of

the results. This was done to help uncover the researcher’s personal assumptions, biases,

and perspectives that may have threatened the credibility of the study.

During the course of conducting the interviews, the investigator assumed a neutral

stance and allowed the interviewee to answer freely from his or her experience. Leading

questions were avoided wherever possible and each of the imagery issues were presented

in as balanced a method as possible. Questions in the form of probes encouraged

vocalists to elaborate on their answers and bring in responses not previously expected on

the subject of imagery use in achieving optimal performance. Although qualitative

techniques of this nature could not be completely free of bias, the researcher endeavored

to employ methods as described above to control for such bias.

Frequency Counts

Frequency counts could be misleading but were included in the results for the

purpose of clarity in understanding the results of the interviews. In using qualitative

techniques, frequency counts could be misunderstood for a number of reasons including

137
the small sampling, the personal nature of imagery experience, or quality and specifics of

the interview protocol, which served to elicit certain responses. Sometimes rare

experiences were revealing and created more understanding of the research questions,

therefore a category was created even if one singer only mentioned it once. Initial codes

were checked and recoded repeatedly in order that the responses were categorized as

effectively as possible in the effort to verify coding reliability. It was important to note,

however, that frequency was not necessarily an indicator of importance (Gammage et al.,

2001; Munroe et al., 2000).

Summary

This chapter presented the design of this exploratory study of mental imagery use

in achieving optimal performance in solo vocal professionals. The four Ws of imagery

use taken from sport psychology research was employed to gain information in athletics

(Munroe et al., 2000), dance (Nordin & Cumming, 2005), and imagery findings from the

literature review, especially music performance studies in imagery (Bellon, 2006; Carter,

1993; Trusheim, 1987). The interview guide was borrowed and adapted to be appropriate

for vocal professionals. Professional vocalists volunteered for the study and were

interviewed through recorded telephone conversation. These interviews were transcribed

verbatim and analyzed through the frameworks of previous four Ws investigations

(Munroe et al., 2000; Nordin & Cumming, 2005). Participants were described, the

interview process was given, and data analyses were explained. The details of the results

of the investigation are discussed in Chapter Four. These results are discussed in relation

to the results and findings of previous research in Chapter Five, including implications

for music education and recommendations for further research.

138
Chapter 4

Results

Results of this exploratory study were based on the interviews of 15 professional

singers as described in the previous chapter. In this chapter, the backgrounds of the

participants are presented first, the interview protocol and process is discussed, followed

by results for each of the research questions based on the framework of the four Ws of

imagery use. Analysis of the data is presented for each of the major questions -- where,

when, what and why. The chapter is concluded with a summary.

Participants

A total of 15 professional vocalists, 10 females (66.7%) and five males (33.3%)

were interviewed for this study. Participants’ ages ranged from the late twenties to the

late sixties and were reported in five-year groupings. Summary data on study

participants’ age and gender are reported in Table 8. The majority of participants (n = 11)

were between 30-45 years old, 67% (n = 10) were female and 33% (n = 5) were male.

In order to provide a more detailed account of each participant, Table 9 details

each participant’s interview order, pseudonym, age, voice type, and highest educational

degree earned. Pseudonyms were chosen with first letters in alphabetical order according

to their gender and the order of their interview. For example, the male participant, who

was the seventh interviewee, was given the random name Gerald, which began with the

letter G, the seventh letter of the English alphabet. The thirteenth interviewee was female

and arbitrarily given the name Monique, since the letter M was thirteenth in the alphabet.
139
Table 8

Participants’ Age and Gender Distribution

Age Range Female Male Total

(in years) (n = 10) (n = 5) (N = 15)

n % n % n %

25-30 1 6.7 1 6.7

30-35 4 26.7 4

35-40 2 13.3 2 13.3 4 26.6

40-45 3 2 3 20

45-50 1 6.7 1 6.7

65-70 2 13.3 2 13.3

TOTAL 10 66.7 5 33.3 15 100

N = 15

Participants included seven sopranos (average age = 37.4), three mezzo sopranos

(average age = 55.8), four tenors (average age = 40), and one baritone (age = 42). Of the

15 participants, 13 held a master’s degree or higher, including a tenor and baritone with

doctorates.

Participants resided in various areas of the United States including California,

Florida, Maryland, New Jersey, New York, and Pennsylvania. Particular information on

each participant’s current residence remained general and confidential in order to obscure

the singer’s identity. Furthermore, it was not considered pertinent to this study since

many traveled far distances to their professional engagements.


140
Table 9

Participants’ Pseudonym, Gender, Voice Type, Age Range, and Highest Degree

Order of Pseudonym Gender Voice Type Age Highest Degree


Interview (in years) Earned
1 Anna Female Soprano 35-40 Bachelor’s

2 Barry Male Tenor 40-45 Bachelor’s

3 Catherine Female Soprano 30-35 Master’s

4 Dorine Female Soprano 30-35 Master’s

5 Eloise Female Soprano 35-40 Master’s

6 Francine Female Mezzo 30-35 Master’s

7 Gerald Male Tenor 35-40 Post Graduate

8 Harriet Female Soprano 30-35 Master’s

9 Ivan Male Tenor 35-40 Master’s

10 Josephine Female Soprano 45-50 Post Graduate

11 Karen Female Mezzo 65-70 Post Graduate

12 Louis Male Tenor 40-45 Doctorate

13 Monique Female Mezzo 65-70 Master’s

14 Nicholas Male Baritone 40-45 Doctorate

15 Ophelia Female Soprano 25-30 Master’s

N = 15

Singers reported the vocal performance genre in order of their most frequent,

second most frequent, and third most frequently performed singing genres (see Table 10).

Thirteen participants reported that their most frequent genre was opera, one reported
141
oratorio, and one reported an equal distribution among oratorio, recital, art song, early

music, liturgical music, and concert singing. The second most frequent genre identified

was recital and cited by seven singers, followed by oratorio, and concert types.

Participants’ reports of their third genre varied widely among other forms of concerts and

recitals.

There were a variety of levels of professional vocal experience among study

participants, all of whom had performed regionally. There were four singers who had

performed in only one region of the United States and, for the purposes of this study,

were considered regional singers. Eight of these professionals with experience in two or

more United States regions were given the classification of national experience. There

were ten vocalists who had been engaged to sing outside the United States and were

considered internationally experienced. However, three singers reported singing in a

single region of the United States as well as another country, and were also considered

international singers. Years of experience were given along with the locations in which

these participants had sung, ranging from six to over 40 years of total professional

experience (see Table 11). The average total years of professional solo singing

experience was 18.6 years.

142
Table 10

Participants’ Singing Genre Frequencies

Order of Pseudonym Singing Genre Frequencies


Interview
Most Frequent Second Most Third Most
Frequent Frequent
1 Anna Opera Oratorio Recital

2 Barry Opera Recital Liturgical

3 Catherine Opera Recital Choral

4 Dorine Opera Art Song Liturgical

5 Eloise Opera Recital Art Song

6 Francine Opera Concert Liturgical

7 Gerald Opera Liturgical Oratorio

8 Harriet Opera Recital Concert

9 Ivan Opera Recital Oratorio

10 Josephine Opera Recital Early Music,

Liturgical

11 Karen Opera Oratorio EM

12 Louis Oratorio Opera Early Music

13 Monique Oratorio, Recital, Art Song, Early Music, Liturgical,

Concert Equally

14 Nicholas Opera Recital Oratorio

15 Ophelia Opera Art Song Recital

N = 15

143
Table 11

Participants’ Professional Solo Singing Experience

Order of Pseudonym Years of Experience Location Experience

Interview Regional National International Regions Nations and


World Areas
1 Anna 10 4 0 NE, MW, SW 0

2 Barry 15 15 15 All but SW CN, EU

3 Catherine 6 0 6 NE UK

4 Dorine 7 0 0 SE 0

5 Eloise 13 0 0 SE 0

6 Francine 18 6 0 All 0

7 Gerald 14 8 9 All but MA UK, EU, AF

8 Harriet 6 10 5 All but NW, CA, UK, EU,

MA AU

9 Ivan 15 15 10 All but MA CN, CA, AS,

UK, EU, RU, IR

10 Josephine 25 3 0 NE, MW 0

11 Karen 40+ 40+ 40+ All CN, CA, SA,

UK, EU, AF

12 Louis 20 15 10 MW CN

13 Monique 40+ 11 35 All CN, CA, UK,

EU, AU, AF

14 Nicholas 10 0 2 MW AS

15 Ophelia 8 0 3 MA AS

N = 15

Note: NE: Northeast; SE: Southeast; S: South; MW: Midwest; SW: Southwest; NW: Northwest; MA: Mid-Atlantic;

CN: Canada; CA: Central America; SA: South America; AS: Asia; UK: United Kingdom; EU: Europe; AU: Australia;

AF: Africa; RU: Russia; IR: Israel.

144
Interview Protocol and Process

The singers’ interview protocol (see Appendix D) proved to be a valuable tool in

gathering both breath and depth of responses from these singers. The open-ended format

of the questions allowed for probing as was deemed appropriate for each participant. The

singers’ interview guide was sent to study participants prior to the interviews, which

helped the singers to understand imagery terms and be mentally prepared to respond to

the answers during the telephone interviews.

In addition to the changes that were made to the original Nordin and Cumming

(2005) dancers’ interview guide (see Appendix E), a few terms required clarification

during the interviews to maintain a common language in confusion in concepts such as

distinguishing meanings of terms. These were not uncovered during the pilot test. For

example, a misunderstanding relative to the question of times when singers used imagery

occurred in differentiating between breaks and holidays. Singers were perplexed by the

word “holiday” in the fourth probe of question 39 regarding when singers used imagery,

“To what extent does your imagery use differ between rehearsal, performance, and

holiday periods?” Since the height of most vocalists’ performance season was during the

Christmas and New Years holidays, the word “holiday” was changed to “break”, meaning

the period during the year when professionals had time off and were not engaged in

practices, rehearsals, or performances. Most of the participants reported using imagery

during breaks only minimally, if any at all. However, in the sport (Munroe et al., 2000)

and dance (Nordin & Cumming, 2005) studies, breaks meant quiet periods throughout the

day, which contrasted with how this issue was addressed in this study. The meanings of

145
the terms “tactile” or “touch” and “kinesthetic feeling” had to be explained to many of

these participants since they were not accustomed to using the word “kinesthetic” to

describe feelings and movements inside their body. Singers were also somewhat unsure

as to how to answer the sequences and strategies questions (Items 16 and 17). Adding a

single probing question on “planning” seemed to have provided more clarity for these

singers. These changes should be addressed and resolved in future uses of the singers’

interview guide.

Although the first three sections of the singers’ interview guide generated

responses about singers’ background, these responses did not prove to be critical to

answering the research questions of where, when, what, and why singers used imagery.

Upon reviewing the transcripts, it was found that singers’ personal information was not

concise enough to be included in this study. Therefore, in the effort to accurately and

efficiently report the demographics of the participants, the vocal participants’ survey was

designed (see Appendix I) and administered to the participants along with their

transcriptions. This greatly aided in maintaining confidentially and more generally and

accurately reporting the demographics of the participants in this study.

Several different aspects of the interview process itself helped shape this study.

The researcher was an experienced vocal professional, was a student of imagery, and had

training in conducting interviews. As the interviews progressed, the researcher became

more familiar with the process and further refined the interviewing technique with each

subsequent participant. Slight changes were made in terminology of the probes and

singers’ answers more reflected the intentions for which the guide was used. Generally,

the participants were articulate and willing to share their knowledge and experience. A

146
number of them had prior experience being interviewed for radio, television, and

newspaper, and were comfortable with the process. The interviewer generally took a

neutral stance and refrained from offering too much direction so that singers’ responses

could more clearly reflect their individual expertise in imagery.

The nature of the responses of participants to the interview and the specific

questions varied widely. The duration of the interviews lasted between 40 and 150

minutes. Several singers were more verbal and shared a wealth of information, regardless

of my efforts to invite them to be more concise, while other vocalists answered more to

the point and needed continuous prompting. Similarly, participants’ responses ranged

from extensively detailed, insightful, and rich to some that were more perfunctory or less

focused. The quality of responses depended on the degree to which the interviewee

stayed on topic and answered the specific given question. Several of these participants

were teachers and answered in the form of what they would share their responses with

their students. Most of these responses were not included since it was not certain if the

answers comprised that interviewee’s personal experiences of imagery. Additionally,

quotes that were difficult to understand and unclear were also omitted in the analyses, if,

after making a concerted effort through emailing and telephoning the participant, the

researcher failed to gain clarity. Overall, the extensive breadth and depth of these

vocalists’ answers contributed to the growing storehouse of knowledge and

understanding of imagery in professional solo singing performance.

Coding

As described in Chapter 3, transcriptions were initially analyzed using the

predetermined categories and subcategories derived from the four Ws framework, the

147
singers’ interview guide, and the related literature. Data were segmented into units of

meaning, which ranged from a single word to chunks of text containing one item of

information. These were initially coded using the a priori categories and subcategories. In

the case where a meaning unit did not fit into the predetermined codes, a new category or

subcategory was created as appropriate. The final data analyses included six main

themes: (a) where, (b) when, (c) imagery types, (d) sense imagery, (e) imagery ability,

and (f) imagery use (see Appendix J).

Each meaning unit was labeled by theme, category and subcategory. For example,

a thematic code of “SI:KI:VP” indicated “Sense Imagery: Kinesthetic: Vocal

Production.” This represented the imagery type of kinesthetic sensory imagery response

taken from interview question 34, “To what extent do you use imagery relating to

kinesthesia?” and involving images of vocal production. Another example of the coding

was “IU:VS:MO,” indicating “Imagery Use: Vocal Sound: Modeling,” representing the

imagery type of vocal sound from question 26, “Describe any imagery you use based on

your sound?” and the use of modeling. The complete list of themes and codes are

provided in Appendix J.

When it appeared that the codes were beginning to be organized cohesively, four

other researchers trained in coding qualitative data “check-coded” (Miles & Huberman,

1994, p. 64) one of the transcriptions to establish inter-coder agreement. It became

apparent that the first three sections of the singers’ interview guide did provide sufficient

content for understanding the participants’ backgrounds but did not adequately address

the research questions in this study. Initially, the inter-coder agreement was .78 and was

deemed satisfactory but could be improved. The themes and codes were again analyzed

148
and reviewed, revealing several redundancies, which resulted in further consolidation of

the codes. The same transcript, without the responses to the first three sections, was

analyzed by the four researchers, resulting in an inter-coder agreement of .83, which was

found more acceptable for this study.

Themes and codes that had emerged from the first coding were used to analyze

the transcripts a second time and each meaning unit was placed by the participant’s name

and code (by color) into each cell of an Excel document. At the end of the second

analysis, all data were analyzed and redundant categories and codes were collapsed into

codes that represented the findings as efficiently as possible. Another analysis was

employed to scour each transcript for any remaining meaning unit that could correspond

to the individual codes. Transcriptions were saved in Word documents and key words

were entered to find quotes using various descriptive words connected to that code. For

example, in looking for the all the quotes for the theme of “arousal,” specific words such

as “nerves,” “performance anxiety,” “stage fright,” and “afraid” were used to search the

entire document and locate every quote, which had anything to do with “arousal.” This

action was performed in every transcript and all codes (see in Appendix J).

All the coded quotes were transferred into their own Word documents and

analyzed and compared to other coded data in that category for emerging themes and

patterns. These themes and codes were compared with those of the four Ws frameworks

for a final check before confirmation. These data were interpreted and a framework

emerged, which constituted further deductive analyses processes (Strauss & Corbin,

1998). Quotes in all coded categories were analyzed by frequency and general emerging

patterns.

149
Frequency counts were reported according to the number of participants who

responded in these categories rather than the number of times the singer mentioned the

item. This was primarily due to the semi-structured nature of the interview guide and the

repetitive nature of some of the questions. This was exemplified by the researcher’s

statement given at the beginning of the interview, “Also, do not worry if some questions

seem repetitive. This is partly to make sure that I am covering all aspects of your singing

imagery and not leaving anything out, and partly useful to obtain quotes for later

analyses” (see Appendix D). Since so many singers offered answers to questions in

several ways, at times revealing replications, it was deemed that reporting frequency

counts by participants was more appropriate for this study.

In reporting the results of the interviews, it was determined that first knowing

where and when singers engaged in imagery provided a better understanding of what was

imagined and why images were used. An attempt was made to separate what, or the

content of imagery, from why, or the purpose. However, participants’ responses were so

integrated in the content and purposes of their imagery that it presented certain challenges

in reporting these findings. It was determined that the cohesive nature of this information

be maintained as much as possible. Therefore, where, when, and what imagery was

reported first, and the section on why offered a more complete and integrated description

of how these singers incorporated imagery in their profession. What follows is the report

of the analyses of the vocal participants’ responses as organized under the four major

themes of the four Ws of imagery use: where, when, what, and why.

150
Where Do Vocal Professionals Use Imagery to Achieve Optimal Performance?

Results of where singers use imagery are followed by when imagery was used.

Vocalists’ reports of where, or the location of imagery engagement, was separated into

four categories: (a) general locations, (b) home, (c) other places, and (d) in practice or

performance settings. Responses are given here according to the divisions that emerged

which resulted in a number of subcategories within each category as shown in Table 12.

Table 12

Vocal Participants’ Responses to Where They Used Imagery

Category Subcategories Number of Participants

Practice/Rehearsal/ Stage (Auditions & Performances) 15

Performance Settings Studio/Practice Space 6

Rehearsal Space 4

At the piano 2

Dressing Room 2

Home Bed 13

At Home (Bathroom & Kitchen) 9

General Alone 7

Anywhere/Everywhere 6

Quiet/Relaxing Places 5

Other Places Commuting/Driving 8

Exercising/Outside 6

151
Results of the singers’ responses in the where category, from the most frequent to

the fewest, were: (a) practice/rehearsal/performance settings, (b) home, (c) general, and

(d) other places. All of the participants reported using imagery on stage for auditions and

performances, followed by six who imagined in the practice or studio space. Four

vocalists used imagery in the rehearsal space, two at the piano, and two in the dressing

room. Referring to the places she used imagery, Francine said, “I use it in the practice

room. I use it in the rehearsal hall, and I use it most specifically in the wings before a

performance, or the hallways before an audition.” The home was the next most frequently

named space with 13 specifically imagining in bed and nine naming various areas in the

home. For example, Dorine said, “I’m usually in the comfort of my own bed, or on my

couch where I can stretch out and relax. I like to be lying down and I try to get myself as

comfortable as possible.” In the general category, less than half of the participants chose

being alone, anywhere or everywhere, and in quiet or relaxing places. Other places

included eight preferring commuting and driving and six either outside or exercising.

It was significant that all of the vocalists considered using imagery at auditions to

be as important as at performances. For the younger singers, auditioning was mentioned

more frequently than the older, more experienced professionals. Being successful in

auditions allowed the singer to actually perform, since roles were so few and many

performers vie for these in this format. Therefore, with regard to professional vocalists’

use of imagery, auditioning had to be a separate inclusion in the performance category.

These solo singers used imagery in the performance setting where they were

already set to practice or perform. Outside of these settings, vocalists engaged in imagery

wherever it was convenient for them to process their thoughts and feelings in imagery, as

152
evidenced by the high number of responses for being alone, at home, in bed, and

commuting and driving (see Table 12). Their particular performance requirements and

living situations and environments may have influenced these results. Furthermore, some

singers reported imagining in many different places, particularly those who seemed to

enjoy more engagement and experience in this mental practice.

When Do Vocal Professionals Use Imagery to Achieve Optimal Performance?

Responses giving the time periods when imagery was used resulted in six

subcategories: (a) practice, (b) rehearsal, (c) performance, (d) performance season, (e)

breaks, and (f) other times. These are presented in this particular order by when these

singers reported their use of imagery beginning with the primary and specific time

periods of practicing, through performance, and ending with break periods and times not

directly involved in singing. Table 13 presents a summary of vocal participants’

responses to when they used imagery.

With regard to using imagery around practice times, all participants reported

engaging in imagery during practice, two singers used it before, and three singers

reported using imagery to assist in retrospection of what they did and to decide what to

do in the future. Ophelia identified the cyclical nature of what was considered after

practice and the next before practice times. She explained that, “afterwards you are

studying and imaging what happened and you remember what happened and you’re

learning from those things and improving. But if you think about it, the afterward is

really just the beginning of the practice.” Upon further investigation, this distinction held

some weight. Ophelia and Catherine expressed the desire to use more of this type of

153
Table 13

Vocal Participants’ Responses to When They Used Imagery

General Time Specific Time Number of Participants

Practice Before 4

During 15 (6 in the learning process)

(4 after learning music)

After 4

Rehearsal During 9 (2 some)

Performance Before 15

During 9 (5 some)

After 6

Performance Season Before 6

During 15

Breaks During 3 used imagery

5 used less imagery

7 no imagery used

Other Times Night 11 (6 going to sleep)

Quiet Times 8 (2 during tedious tasks)

All the Time 8

Day 5 (3 waking up)

154
imagery during the initial stages of practice. Ophelia mused, “it behooves me to have the

imagery before, because then it transfers naturally to the practice.”

Six singers used imagery during the process of learning the material and four used

it after most of the basic notes and music were secured, as illustrated by Francine’s

response. She said:

The only time I wouldn’t [use imagery] is when I’m being very, very, very

technical and I’m learning a piece or I’m just learning basic plunking of notes . . .

But even in my own private studio work, once a piece has gotten to a certain level

where I need to start making more out of it than just learning the notes and

learning the rhythms, that’s when I start to use imagery.

Louis also engaged in imagery after the rudiments of the piece were learned. He stated:

During the preparation process for musical literature that I’m preparing, during

the learning process, in particular, after a basic knowledge of music is already

there. But I need to hone that knowledge into a more detailed mastery of the

music.

Generally, all vocalists engaged in imagery during practice in relation to the learning

process which considerably outweighed the responses given for before or after practice.

Regarding times these singers used imagery, a distinction between practice and

rehearsal arose as an issue particular to the classical vocal profession. For solo singers,

practice was usually conducted alone, at various times most convenient for the individual.

Rehearsals were more formal events scheduled and organized for the purpose of staging

an opera or recital, usually involving other singers, musicians, and possibly directors and

coaches, depending on the singing genre and performance venue. Singers were required

155
to show up and participate having already memorized their roles and arias. Josephine

explained:

I prepare myself outside of rehearsal and then I come there and just rehearse with

the other musicians. So it’s really a lot of instant music. So it’s not a lot of work.

We’re expected to do a lot of work on our own, and come prepared.

Louis’ response also illustrated this when he said, “I’m much more likely to use imagery

or visualization in advance of a rehearsal than I am to use it during the rehearsal itself.”

Nicholas differentiated his rehearsal and practice efforts when he said, “if I’m in

rehearsal in a show and trying to study a second or third show at the same time then it’s

tricky balance to stay rested enough to accomplish everything.” He was in rehearsals for

one opera while learning and practicing other roles. Differentiating practice, rehearsal,

and performance periods was important when examining professional singers’ imagery

use.

For the purposes of this study, “before performance” referred to periods following

the completion of all practices and rehearsals. All participants used imagery just prior to

performing. Three singers engaged in pre-performance routines of extensive imagery and

quiet time on the day of the performance to mentally prepare for optimal performance

that evening, as Anna aptly described:

The day it all comes together into one thing, and, when I put my makeup and hair

on, I’ll take three hours if I can, if I have it, if I have the time I’ll try to do an hour

to three hours. . . . I’m really just focused on the entirety of the performance.

Nicholas’ value of his extensive pre-performance (or pre-audition) imagery time was

reflected in the following quote, “I feel that giving myself enough time to achieve focus

156
before an audition or performance is almost more important than anything else on a

performance or audition day.” Use of imagery in relation to performances and auditions

represented a great portion of when singers used imagery and both of these types of

performances should be included in future research.

Vocalists’ responses for using imagery during performances varied. Nine singers

used imagery during performance; five reported a significantly diminished amount while

on stage and six after performance. As an example, Catherine said, “During a

performance, I only employ imagery if I fall out of character and need to get back in.

When I'm actually performing, it is distracting to be thinking about performing, it's better

to just do it!” Most of the imagery that singers used in performance was set beforehand

and was intended to render automaticity to their execution and portrayals on stage.

Francine further illustrated, “I’m aware that I’m using it when I’m performing, but I’m so

involved in the performance, that I don’t necessarily remember using it. I just remember

making the conscious decision beforehand to do it.” Conversely, two vocalists reported

using no imagery during performance, including Barry, who said, “I don’t use imagery in

performance. I use imagery in rehearsal so when performance comes, whatever comes

out, comes out.” Harriet explained some of the reasons why she used less imagery in

performance. She said for example, “I use it all beforehand, so that by the time I’m on

stage, I’m only acting in that character.”

Six participants reported using imagery to a lesser extent after performing,

primarily use it as a learning tool for future presentations. Anna stated:

157
[U]sually I give myself at least four or five days before I start to take apart the

performance, and rework in, ‘next time I’m going to do this, it’s gonna feel like

this, and it’s gonna be like this experience.’

Few participants articulated this type of retrospection and intention to change behavior

for the future performances. Imagery engagement after performance was more common

in the responses of singers, such as Monique, who repeatedly performed the same roles

and concerts during the singing season.

Vocalists used imagery differently throughout the year. All participants engaged

in imagery during the performance season whether it was associated with practice,

rehearsal, or performance. The six participants who reported using imagery to prepare for

the next season also reported using some imagery during breaks. Seven singers said they

took a rest from imagery while on extended vacations, five used less imagery, and three

reported incorporating it more. For example, Louis said, “I might utilize it in anticipation

of the next section of the rehearsal.” Still some confusion remained as to whether these

breaks were rest periods throughout the day, between practice and performing, or during

an extended vacation such as the summer.

Singers reported using imagery extensively at times outside of practice, rehearsal,

and performance. Eight participants reported using imagery at anytime or most of the

time, five preferred the day and of those, three imagined upon waking up in the morning.

Night proved to be a more favorable time for 11 participants, including seven who

specified using imagery before going to sleep. One singer had no preference between day

and night. Engaging in imagery required quiet times for seven participants, especially

Eloise who said, “I’m by myself and there’s like nothing going on and I can just do it in

158
my mind, with nothing to interfere.” Karen and Ophelia described using imagery while

engaged in tedious tasks such as cleaning or washing dishes.

To summarize the findings of where and when, all singers used imagery in stage

and practice areas during practice, just prior performance, and during the performance

season. Many participants used imagery more during rehearsal and performance than

before and after. At other times, these participants used imagery at home in bed at night,

and during quiet periods outside, exercising, and during travel. These results have helped

to lay the foundation of what imagery singers use and later why they use it.

What Do Vocal Professionals Use in Their Imagery to Achieve Optimal Performance?

In analyzing these responses, the framework established in dance research of the

four Ws of imagery use (Nordin & Cumming, 2005) was used to differentiate imagery

types from imagery characteristics. Imagery types were all those responses that the singer

qualified as an image. Imagery characteristics encompassed the way in which these

images were experienced, such as through the senses, perspective, and various aspects of

their ability to imagine.

Most often participants’ responses represented specific details of the imagery

type, characteristic, and purpose of imagery as an integrated experience. It was

challenging to extract the singers’ imagery content from its intended purpose.

Furthermore, the same image could be used for a variety of reasons, and many different

images were used for a single purpose. Even during the interviews, participants pointed

this out. Ivan clearly illustrated his difficulty in analyzing his own imagery practices

when he said, “It’s hard to separate it out when I think about it minimally because when I

am visualizing, it’s all these things combined. In other words, they’re all together for

159
me.” Excessively dissecting this information may have reflected too much of an atomistic

approach to analyzing the very natural and holistic human function that is imagery as

experienced by these professional vocalists. Therefore, imagery types and characteristics

are reported as concisely and efficiently as possible in order to provide a clear foundation

of the various qualities of images these singers used. The section in this chapter on why

provides a more comprehensive picture of the intricacies of these participants’ imagery.

Imagery Types

Imagery types are reported in categories that emerged from the singers’ responses

and included the content of the image itself. Their responses were organized into six pre-

determined subcategories that were adapted from dancers’ imagery research (Nordin &

Cumming, 2005). In the original four Ws of imagery use framework for dancers (see

Figure 2) the last category was irrelevant images, which represented an imagery type that

was not found in the singers’ responses. However the category of musical sound was

added to the types of imagery singers described in their interviews. The resulting six

categories of imagery types included: (a) execution, (b) metaphorical, (c) context, (d)

body-related, (e) musical sound, and (f) character/role images. Table 14 provides a

summary of the categories, descriptions, and number of participants.

Execution images. Execution images for singers were divided into three sections:

(a) skill learning and technique, (b) sequences and planning, and (c) goal images.

All singers reported an abundance of images of skill learning and techniques and their

responses resulted in five subcategories: (a) vocal production, (b) text and phrases, (c)

page notation, and (e) breath control. Images of vocal production and technique were

described in all singers’ interviews. For example, in maintaining consistency through her

160
Table 14

Vocal Participants’ Imagery Types, Categories, and Subcategories

Imagery Types Categories Subcategories Number of


Participants
Execution Skill Learning & Technique Vocal Production 15

Text & Phrases 14

Page Notation 10

Breath Control 6

Sequences & Planning Phrasing & Patterns 5

Planning Songs & Arias 8

Song Cycles & Recitals 6

Creating Stories 3

Goal Images Outcome 15

Performance 11

Process 15

Metaphorical Vocal Production Imaginary Actions 13

Objects Not Present 12

Colors (Hue & Timbre) 9

Context Environments Stage Scenarios 15

Song Scenarios 6

Distant Places 1

Home 1

Imaginary People Audition Panelists 4

Other People 3

Non-human 4

Audience Images Favorable images 9

161
Table 14 (continued)

Imagery Categories Subcategories Number of

Types Participants

Body-Related Arousal Breath Control 13

Calm Images 4

Nerves & Tension 4

Appearance Looking Good On Stage 11

Kinesthetic Vocal Production 10

Body Feeling & Movement 6

Posture & Alignment 6

Health 6

Musical Sound Musical Sounds Melody 14

Accompaniment 14

Pitch 10

Vocal Sounds Modeling Expert Singers 10

Ideal Sounds 7

One’s Own Voice 4

Character/Role Emotions Emotions of Characters 15

Past Experiences 6

Behaviors Action & Movement 12

Acting out Roles 4

Appearance Costumes & Character 4

Gender Change 1

162
vocal registers, Monique said, “I see my sound as a steady flowing thing, even from top

to bottom.” Eloise described her images of her efforts to establish continuity through her

registers. She offered, “in order to sing scales that go up and down, I imagine the

opposite direction and the opposite flow of energy.” In securing another type of vocal

technique, Ophelia imagined a “soft palate lift, and maybe flattening of the tongue” to

produce more vocal resonance.

Singing text and words of one language or another have been considered a basic

requirement of vocal performance. Fourteen participants offered responses in which their

imagery included words, text, or phrases, which emerged in the interviews as an imagery

element specific to singers. Francine aptly illustrated this in the following quote:

I see the words. When I study just the language, I see the IPA of the pronunciation

of the words. And most importantly with the languages that I don’t speak fluently,

I see, also in my head, the English translation. So as I’m singing in German, I will

actually see in my head, the English words. It helps me.

Experience of words, text, and phrases in visual imagery emerged naturally from the data

and seemed appropriate since these elements were an integral part of singing training for

performance.

Ten singers reported other uses of visual images with regard to performance,

particularly page notation in relation to memorizing and executing songs. Karen had an

acute ability in visualizing the score. She stated, “I see that all the time. I could tell you

what side of the page something’s on. I don’t have a photographic memory, but it’s

almost like that.” Barry and Francine reported having a photographic memory and page

163
notation images were easy for them to recreate. Regarding his experience of mentally

seeing the score, Nicholas said, “When I’m memorizing, a little bit. I try to get beyond

that soon, as quick as I can, so that I don’t have to think about that and can concentrate on

other things.” Ivan also had remembered his images of notation and described, “there’s a

sense of the music, the sheet, hovering in front in my consciousness. Then that

disappears.” Page notation images often subsided when the music became more

internalized. Mentally reading from a score was sometimes necessary in certain instances.

Ophelia said she used notation imagery “if I do have a difficult passage where this

cadenza for instance has a lot of technically challenging aspects, I would see, yes, the

music notation in front of me.” Monique and Francine mentally imagined the score

particularly while singing modern classical music. Francine said:

[W]ith contemporary music, very specifically when it’s something that may be

atonal, it doesn’t even make sense harmonically, again, I see the score. I see the

score in my head, and I know exactly where that C is compared to the G,

compared to the A.

Page notation images were connected to performing the song and helped singers to

accurately memorize the notes and phrases of the music.

Six singers imagined breathing techniques related to vocal production (e.g.,

Barry: “imagine the path of your breath”). While performing a long phrase, Anna would

imagine inhaling at the same time she was singing. These techniques and breathing

images ranged from specific anatomical replication to individually created metaphors.

Every singer reported mentally rehearsing and silently singing the songs and

roles. Many of their images directly reflected or were related to all the skills they

164
practiced physically. Differences appeared in the degree to which they focused on vocal

production, words, breath, or various combinations of these musical elements and may

have been associated with levels of training and experience.

All participants gave examples of images of sequence and planning involved in

learning or performing music ranging from a simple phrase to an entire recital or role.

These types of images comprised four sections: (a) phrasing and patterns, (b) planning

songs and arias, (c) sequences of song cycles and recitals, and (d) creating stories. Five

vocalists imagined phrases and distinct sections especially while learning repertoire, such

as Josephine who said, “I see the patterns in the music.” Eight participants described

images of planning through the execution of their songs and arias. Catherine illustrated

this by saying, “I imagine myself singing through an entire piece vocally and singing

everything correctly.” For six singers, sequencing imagery was integrated into their

recital arrangement and progression through song cycles. Nicholas created “specific ideas

about how each piece is going to go.” Likewise, Josephine imagined “the shape of the

music and what each song was in the program.” Three singers reported creating stories

that linked the different songs in their recitals together in a cohesive manner. Eloise said

she had “to come up with some kind of a story that makes sense in order to sequence

through them.” Singers used elements naturally intrinsic in music as well as those related

to the compositions to create planning and sequence images in performance preparation.

Imagery of goals reported by these singers primarily consisted of three distinct

kinds: (a) outcome goals, (b) performance goals, and (c) process goals. These images

ranged from vocalists who imagined singing in a role in a famous opera house to

executing the perfect high note.

165
Outcome goal images were reported in 14 participants and ranged from specific,

clearly imagined goals to those more general. Specific outcome goals included images

that were sometimes competitive, the results of being chosen from an audition,

performing a desired role, or singing on a certain level or in a famous venue. For

instance, images involved visualizing the final results of a competition, as when Ivan

imagined himself “with the award in hand.” Catherine and others imagined details of a

specific role and actually being engaged to perform it as a result of the audition.

Catherine stated, “I imagine myself in the costume and in the show I'm auditioning for,

imagining already having gotten the gig and performing it on stage.” Francine organized

a routine for imagining the outcome for performing in different venues. She said:

In order to accomplish successfully singing for them and getting the job, what

I’ve done is go into the house [theatre], see a show there, and imagine myself on

that stage, so putting myself on the other side already before they even give me

the opportunity. Saying ‘I already got the job, I already have gone through

rehearsals, I already accomplished everything.’

Other specific outcome goals included images of performing in prestigious venues such

as the Metropolitan Opera. For example, Nicholas stated, “the MET is one of the places

I’m hoping to sing before I’m done, and so I’ve used that a little bit to imagine myself

singing there.” General outcome goals were less focused and were evident in six quotes.

To illustrate, Gerald had “goals of singing at international performing venues, and being

like other highly sought-after performers,” and Josephine wanted to “sing on the next

level of the business, on a higher level of the business.” These vocalists used outcome

goals from very specific and clear to fairly general images.

166
Performance goals primarily dealt with singers achieving standards of excellence

in their own on stage performances, which was described in 11 participants’ responses.

These goal images included a desire to experience general positive affect as expressed by

Eloise who wanted to “really enjoying what I’m doing,” and the somewhat more specific

intention expressed by Gerald of “making my vocal technique better.” Ophelia

concentrated on the immediate moment with the assumption that her efforts could lead to

better opportunities. She stated, “I try to focus on the task at hand, and the tasks at hand

always lead to the bigger goal.” She also expressed continued interested in “getting

better, and better, and better. It’s something I’ll never be able to attain.” Ophelia’s

performance goals were more general as compared to Louis’ goals which were more

artistic and combined performance and outcome goals as he illustrated:

I want to give the absolute best artistic performance that I can. And when I do

that, regardless of what stage I’m on or what role it is in, that’s when I feel like

I’m the most accomplished vocally that I can be.

Nicholas’ imagined executing his ideal audition and said, “I will imagine it all. I’ll

picture the scene usually ahead of time.” This type of goal work included all three types

of goals, outcome, performance, and process.

Results for process goal images of learning or improving specific performance

skills were so frequent in this study and seemed to be an intrinsic aspect of singers’

imagery content. These ranged from securing techniques in vocal production, achieving a

certain sound quality, embodying the character and emotion of a role, and imagining the

body to have a certain appearance in the effort to achieve automaticity, or habit, in the

desired skill or technique. The specific findings are not reported here since they were

167
found to pervade nearly every aspect of the imagery these professional vocalists used and

are included in many other sections of this chapter.

Metaphorical images. Singers offered a plethora of metaphorical images primarily

relating to vocal production. While most of the participants used some kind of metaphor

in their imagery, all were familiar with a number of metaphors used in traditional vocal

training. Responses of metaphorical images related to vocal production included three

categories: (a) imaginary actions, (b) objects not present, and (c) colors. These

represented categories adapted from dancers’ framework of imagery (Nordin &

Cumming, 2005).

Thirteen participants described metaphorical imaginary actions representing vocal

production and technique, such as regulating the breath and vocal placement. Gerald

controlled his breath by using the image of “very gently, gently, gently, blowing a spec of

dust.” For vocal support, Josephine imagined that her “voice is a shelf. I think of putting

those notes on a shelf.” Francine’s imagery of her vocal production was like “catching

the baseball behind my head.” Related to enlarging the throat for singing, Eloise said,

“Imagine that you’re a seal and you’re going to swallow the fish whole.” Some of these

singers used metaphorical images consistently in their vocal production.

Twelve singers described objects that were not present, constituting the widest

variety of metaphorical images, all of which related to vocal technique and production.

Examples of some object images were, “potato sacks in the back” (Barry), “a conveyor

belt” (Monique), and “a string of spaghetti” (Harriet). Many of these singers reported

using the same images, which may have represented those generally accepted in vocal

pedagogy (e.g., “smelling a rose”; “fireplace bellows”). Many singers created their own

168
metaphors, such as Eloise’s image of a “crescent moon, which is behind you, embracing

you.” Some of these images were used in place of actual mechanical processes of the

voice. More examples of these images and the specific applications were included in the

section on vocal production in this chapter.

For nine of these vocalists, imagery of color represented visual hues and shades

primarily relating to the timbre or the quality of vocal sound. Anna experienced the visual

aspect of color in relation to parts of music. She said, “I’ll see colors, different notes have

different colors and different phrases have different colors, reds or greens or blues.” Ivan

provided the richest color imagery description. He said:

A passionate line will invariably, for me, have reddish tint. A passionate line with

an ascending line, towards the top will be that passionate red that goes to a

brighter color, almost always towards . . . gold, a very bright gold, for the upper

part of the voice. No matter what it is, no matter if it’s blue that I’m envisioning,

it always goes to gold on top, red to gold on top. The top is always the same color.

Unless I using something like a fille voce, sotto voce, pianissini, up top, then it’s a

silver.

This example could arguably be described as chromesthesia in which the visual colors

mix with sound in imagery. Conversely, other singers regarded color images as those

relating to the emotional expression in the voice. Nicholas illustrated this by saying, “I’ll

look to place, or look to work in certain menacing colors in a menacing situation.” In

either case, color images for these singers described a spectrum of vocal qualities they

could apply in their singing.

169
Context images. Vocalists’ imagery encompassed specific contexts and included

three different categorical aspects: (a) environments, (b) imaginary people, and (c)

audiences. All 15 singers imagined various environments, which were classified into four

subcategories: (a) being on stage or in the theatre, (b) the scene of a song, (c) distant

places, and (d) home. Vocalists described various scenarios of themselves performing on

a stage. For example, Catherine said, “I would just imagine being on stage, taking a

curtain call.” Harriet imagined singing to an audience member “in the last row of the

theatre.” Monique was one of six singers who created her own artistic environment in her

mind when she performed. She said, “I try to put myself in the scene of the picture that

that song is painting. So in my mind, I see the sun; I see the water; I see the mountains.

They’re all around me.” Karen imagined being in a far away land. Ivan described images

of his old age with his grandchildren surrounding him at home “in front of a fireplace.”

Several different kinds of imaginary people appeared in seven participants’

images. These included audition panelists and people who were directly related to the

production such as other characters in a scene, the director, or instrumental musicians.

Other singers imagined panelists present in their auditions. Four participants reported

non-humans such as angels and devils. Francine imagined “an angel on one shoulder and

a devil on the other.” At times just prior to performing, Monique reported contending

with images of “Satan” when she was feeling nervous.

Nine vocalists imagined interactions with the audience. Most of these singers

regarded the audience members favorably, as Karen came to realize. She said, “I actually

imagined an audience really not that frightening.” Other images involved sharing

something with the audience. Eloise would “picture the audience and try to imagine what

170
they’re hoping for,” and Josephine imagined her voice was “reaching that last seat, not

just the people in the very front.” Other audience images are reported in the Artistic

Reasons section of this chapter.

Body-related images. Body-related images comprised singers’ concerns with

feeling or seeing their physical body and was classified into four categories: (a) arousal,

(b) appearance, and (c) kinesthetic images. With regard to the body, feelings and images

of arousal were the most frequent, with 13 vocalists sharing their images of breath

control, feelings of calm, and specific areas of physical tension. Breath images in relation

to calming thoughts and feelings were most common in regulating arousal. Gerald

imagined the “feeling of the breath calmly moving through my body,” while Josephine

used “the imagery of seeing the breath coming in and out of my body.” Four singers

experienced calmness throughout their body, including Dorine who imagined “this

feeling of peace, peacefulness and calmness that starts at the top of my head and goes

down my body.” Four singers mentioned images of feeling nervousness and tension that

assisted them in executing their performances.

Eleven participants used appearance images, which was not surprising since much

of their professional lives involved performing in front of an audience. Appearance in this

category primarily included images for stage performance outside of appearance imagery

in performing in character in an opera. Josephine used the “imagery of looking beautiful

and appealing to the audience.” Anna said, “sometimes I’ll look in the mirror to see what

I’m doing right then, and then to create what I want it to look like in my head instead.”

Louis stated, “[I] visualize myself looking the way that I want to.” Generally, appearance

171
imagery was used to create at least the illusion of appropriate visual presentation for

performance.

Singers’ responses varied in the level of detail they imagined their appearance to

be on stage. Several held a more general image in their minds as to what they wanted to

look like on stage (e.g., Josephine, “looking beautiful and appealing to the audience. . . .

looking like a princess,” Ophelia: “poised, trying to be as in control, even though you

completely feel out of control”). Harriet proposed, “I guess there’s a standard that I

expect of myself when on stage. Yes. I expect to have a finished product.” Other singers’

appearance imagery was extensively detailed. For example, Karen illustrated:

I know how I will walk on stage, how I will look, how I will hold my hands, who

am I, how do I want to present myself, how I go on, how I look when I go on,

how I even hold my body when I go on.

Each detail concerning her appearance was fully planned and vividly imagined.

Reports of kinesthetic images included those of (a) vocal production, (b) body

feeling and movement, (c) posture and alignment, and (d) health. Ten responses in vocal

production images involved specific mechanisms in the body. Catherine and Ivan

provided the best examples of these images. Catherine imagined the feeling of “where in

your body the resonance [of the sound] will strike or where it will vibrate.” Ivan used

imagery to experience “the ease, the vibrato, the blood flow, the vibrations of the tone

when my body is supporting the tone, what that feels like in my body.” Along with the

six vocalists who mentioned body movement, Josephine felt it was important to “imagine

yourself as a young child, or as a baby lamb or something to get your body to move a

certain way.” Six singers described images of posture and alignment which either

172
represented optimal qualities or those representing the physical character depicted in a

song or aria. Six participants reported health images. During periods of sickness, Barry

imagined “the times when I was perfectly healthy.” Before even vague symptoms of

illness set in, Gerald would tell himself “that I’m healthy, so that my body functions at its

optimal level.” Images relating to the body seemed to be an intrinsic part of performing

as a singer.

Musical sound images. Singers reported hearing both musical and vocal sound

images in relation to practice and performance. All singers engaged in silent musical

practice and mentally heard various sounds in their head related to vocal and musical

production. Musical auditory images were classified into three subcategories: (a) pitch,

(b) melody, and (c) accompaniment. Ten singers mentally heard the specific pitch just

prior to initiating that sound in their voice. Monique illustrated this perfectly when she

said:

I always breathe in the note I’m about to sing. . . . so that the note is existing

before I actually sing it, so that I’m right in the middle of the note. . . .I can’t tell

you how that helps you mentally. There’s no searching for it, you see, you’ve

pinned it before you even start.

Exact pitches were imagined in the beginning of phrases, in approaching a high note, as

well as notes throughout the aria. Fourteen participants mentioned hearing the melody in

their heads just before singing the line or during mental practice. Josephine imagined

“hearing a passage in my head before I would sing it.” All but one participant reported

hearing the accompaniment, especially the musical intervals that occurred between their

vocal phrases. During silent practice, Dorine said she was “hearing the piano, imagining

173
the accompaniment from start to finish.” Gerald stated, “I do imagine the accompaniment

[and] hear the interludes.” Harriet reported mentally hearing “another instrument’s part

that plays in sort of duet with me while I’m singing my line.” Some singers mentioned

being able to mentally hear several musical elements simultaneously. Nicholas said, “I

will hear basically everything going on in my head. So I’ll hear my line and hear the text,

and hear the orchestra.” It seemed that these participants used sound images to assist

them in silent and vocal practice and performance.

Participants reported using images of vocal sounds which were divided into the

subcategories of (a) modeling sounds of expert singers, (b) ideal sounds, and (c) hearing

one’s own singing voice. Anna was one of 10 vocalists who used sound modeling in her

imagery, which she developed by “[w]atching other singers, and listening to their music,

especially ones that are similar to you.” Likewise, Eloise said, “I have sort of imitated or

imagined performances that I’ve seen other people do.” Several of these participants

included the names of famous vocal professionals they used as inspiration for their

images. Ideal sound images were possibly a culmination of what that particular singer

had seen, heard, and experienced throughout the years as a professional. Seven singers

reported using these images, including Karen who stated, “I know the sound that I want

to make.” Four singers heard their own voice in silent practice and anticipated an ideal

production of that specific sound. These sound images provided an ideal and appropriate

standard that these performers strove to achieve or accomplished successfully.

Character/role images. Another important aspect of imagery for the singing

professional was the element of acting. From the participants’ responses, character and

role images included (a) emotions, (b) behaviors, and (c) appearance.

174
Emotional images emerged as being vital to bringing a character, song, or role to

life for all these professionals. Several of the participants expressed their conviction that

emotional imagery gave them motivation for the behavior of the character. Eliciting the

correct emotional image was critical in providing an appropriate framework for how the

character or story of the song progressed in performance. Ivan said, “the mood changes,

yeah, the moment before the change happens.” Six of the singers reported using their own

memories of past experiences to create the appropriate emotions in a performance. In

recreating a sorrowful feeling for a particular role, Barry said he accomplished this “by

going to that place, and bringing back images of a sadder time.” In order to maintain the

ability to sing in times of great emotion during an aria, Josephine would simply “touch on

memories to be able to bring you to that place that you need to be able to portray the

song.” Specific and appropriate emotional imagery was deemed critical to performance

excellence and served as internal guide and motivation for movement of the character.

Fourteen vocalists reported imagining the behavior of the character, which

included 12 images of motivation for action and four images of physically staging the

specific characters and roles. Nicholas allowed his images to naturally develop by

“setting myself within the text of the plot of the opera and letting the motivation of the

character, and getting ideas that the character would be having, shape the phrases.” Anna

said, “I do some of this visualization where I’ll go through and go, ‘I want this to look

like this,’ especially if you’re dying or you’re angry” as part of the requirements of

staging. Some of these singers employed movement and behavior images in specific

roles. In portraying Mimi in La Boheme, Josephine recalled, “I would be using a lot . . .

constantly having images going through my brain at the same time I was singing, of

175
someone who was wounded, and physically crippled and disabled to portray that type of

character.” For staging Carmen, Francine conjured images of being in Spain many years

ago and described, “Imagine that you are digging through the earth, you’re in Seville . . . .

What does the floor feel like underneath your [sic] feet?” This imagery included sensory

and context details helped recreate the character’s experience in the scene.

Appearance in this category encompassed external aspects of the character, the

costumes, makeup, and historical and cultural settings as dictated by the composer or the

production. Four singers described their imagery of appearance including Eloise, who

best illustrated these details. She said:

I imagine myself in the costume and on the set, and in the castle or whatever it

was, the mansion, or in the country home or wherever, and what was I wearing,

what was my hair, and what were the jewels.

In acting out roles, singers often needed to imagine a change in their physique for

presentation on stage. Being a mezzo, Francine had to appear to change genders, which

she aptly did through her appearance imagery.

Imagery types described here represented the content the participants reported

using in their professional efforts. The following sections describe some of the imagery

qualities these vocal professionals experienced their images.

Imagery Characteristics

Participants’ responses relative to imagery characteristics was coded using five

subcategories from Nordin and Cumming (2005: (a) sense imagery, (b) perspective, (c)

ability, (d) direction and deliberation, and (e) amount and duration.

176
Senses. Six categories of imagery involving the senses emerged in the analysis:

(a) sight or visual, (b) sound or auditory, (c) kinesthetic or feeling, (d) touch or tactile, (e)

smell or olfactory, and (f) taste or gustatory. During the interviews, singers were given

the opportunity to describe the specific extent of their uses of each of the senses. All

singers reported using vision, auditory, and kinesthetic imagery in their artistic

endeavors. The senses of touch, smell and taste were used to a much less extent in

practice and performance. Participants’ specific responses to each of the senses involving

some Imagery Types (addressed in the previous section) are given in Table 15.

All 15 singers reported using visual imagery with six who said they used it a great

deal. Barry and Francine each stated they had a photographic memory and Karen

declared that she was “a visual learner.” The sense of sight was used primarily in

visualizing scenarios, vocal production and technique, and page notation. Auditory

imagery was employed by all the vocalists to imagine hearing various aspects of their

vocal production, the music, and the text involved in their silent practice and

performance. Kinesthetic imagery included feeling the internal technical, physical, and

emotional aspects of singing and acting. Specific images using these three senses were

described in the previously section on imagery types.

Tactile and kinesthetic senses had been confused in seven of the interviewees’

responses, which may have originated from simple terminology differences. For example,

Louis declared, “I do experience the music in a very tactile, visceral kind of way.” Even

though tactile refers to touch and visceral connotes deeper feelings, the assumption could

reasonably be made that he was referring to the internal vocal mechanism and perhaps

not the epidermis of the neck and jaw. This was later confirmed in a subsequent

177
Table 15

Vocal Participants’ Specific Sense Imagery Use

Sense Categories Number of Participants

Sight/Visual Visual 15 (6 a great deal)

Scenario 14 (6 imagined executing the aria)

Vocal Production 13 (8 anatomical, 8 metaphorical)

Page Notation 13

Sound/Auditory Sound 15

Words/Text/Phrase 14

Pitch 10

Melody 14

Accompaniment 14

Silent Singing 15

Vocal Production 15

Kinesthetic Kinesthetic 15

Character/Emotion 14

Vocal Production 15

Body Check 10

Pitch Preparation 3

Automaticity 2

178
Table 15 (continued)

Sense Categories Number of Participants

Touch/Tactile Touch 4 yes, 5 some, 4 not much, 2 none

Character Based 8

Future Performance 2

Communication with Audience 2

Smell/Olfactory Smell 2 yes*, 4 some, 8 not much, 1 none

Character/Emotion 7 (4 emotion in character)

Vocal Production 3

Venue 3

Taste/Gustatory Taste 2 yes*, 5 some, 7 not much, 1 none

Text-based 9

Vocal Production 3

Note: *Both were cooks.

telephone conversation with Louis in which he clarified that he did imagine

kinesthetically in singing and tactile sense imagery was used more in the physical act of

vocalizing and securing technique. Furthermore, when the terms kinesthetic and tactile

were explained as defined in this study, all seven singers agreed they meant the

kinesthetic sense and not touch.

179
The sense of touch was usually employed in imagery when its use was dictated in

the text of a song or the action of a character. For example, Louis was one of two

participants who used the tactile sense in imagining future auditions and performances:

If I know that I’m going to be wearing a certain suit for an audition, I try to feel

. . . what it will be like to be standing up in that suit. Am I going to place my

hands on the piano during a recital and if so, what will that piano feel like? If I’m

going to have a wig on during an operatic performance, how will that constrict my

scalp?

Smell and taste imagery was employed the least with roughly half of the singers

reporting that they did not use it very often. Only two participants seemed to enjoy taste

and smell images and referenced their extensive cooking experience. Otherwise these

vocalists reported using smells and tastes in context of the song or role. Using olfactory

imagery in performing in the opera, La Boheme, Ivan would “imagine what kind of

perfume Mimi would wear. What kind of cologne does Rudolpho put on when he’s

getting ready to go out?” Gerald used the sense of smell to assist him in portraying his

character in specific settings. He said:

In some operas, you find yourself in particular scenes where you’re either in a

dungeon or you’re on the gallows, or some place, and where people are dying

around you, so that imagining the smell of death, or whatever that gives a fear

response, and an honest one.

Singers also used smell imagery in association with the odor of specific venues.

Taste imagery was sometimes connected with text articulation and

characterization. As an illustration, Eloise said, “You’ve got to make this yummy. . . .

180
like felice mi fa, [from Musetta’s Waltz in La Boheme] it’s like, it’s got to be yummier

each time you say it. So I think of darker and darker chocolate as I go up.” These

participants used touch, and particularly smell and taste the least of all the senses, but

these senses remained available for use in imagery to more specifically embody the

character and enhance performance. However, concerning participants reporting using no

specific sense imagery, two participants used no tactile, one singer never used smell, and

another singer did not use taste.

From the above accounts, vocalists used their senses extensively for imagery in

their professional endeavors. In gathering information on rating sense imagery preference

of use, singers were asked the interview question 36, “Which of the above would be the

primary senses you use in your imagery?” Subsequent probes during the interview invited

vocalists to rate the second and third sense they used in imagery. Ivan and Monique felt

the senses of vision, hearing, and kinesthesia held equal and topmost importance. The

final results of the sense imagery rating by the remaining 13 participants revealed vision

to be first with seven votes, auditory second with eight, and kinesthesia third with six (see

Table 16). Several mentioned touch usually as the third choice, but later changed it to

kinesthesia. Taste was used slightly more than smell, and mostly employed by two

participants who enjoyed cooking. Therefore, by a very small margin in the first three,

the rank order of senses used in imagery by these professional soloists from the most to

the least was: (a) vision, (b) auditory, (c) kinesthesia, (d) tactile, (e), taste, and (f) smell.

181
Table 16

Vocal Participants’ Rank Order of Sense Imagery Use

Sense Primary Secondary Third

Vision/Sight 7 3 3

Sound/Auditory 1 8 4

Kinesthesia/Feeling 5 2 6

TOTAL 13 13 13

Note: Two participants choose sound, kinesthetic, and vision together as equally important.

Perspective. Capabilities of visual imagery also involved the use of internal and

external perspectives. Singers in this study favored internal perspective, however several

used external with some employing both as seen in Table 17. Internal perspective,

reported by 14 singers, was seeing things from the first person point of view. Nicholas

stated, “I’m always picturing it from my perspective, because I never see myself in the

scene.” Conversely, five participants used the external, or the third person viewpoint, as

was the preference of Gerald who said, “I’d see me, watching from the audience.” Ivan

was one of three singers, who were able to switch between inner and outer perspective,

but gave more weight to the internal. He said:

It flips from the inner eye to the outer eye, back and forth. . . .[I]f I’m stuck with

just the outer eye, then I’m not as complete as I could be. . . .What’s most

powerful is the inner eye. If I can get into the inner eye of what, what I am initial

seeing, then I know I’m on track. . . . That’s when I know it’s real. If I can do that

transition in my visualization.

182
Anna sometimes used "external to get the perspective the audience.” Again these visual

perspective images were used for various reasons and depended on individual abilities

and skills.

Table 17

Vocal Participants’ Imagery Perspective

Perspective Number of Participants

Internal 14

External 5

Switching 3

Imagery ability. Another pre-determined category of imagery characteristics was

imagery ability, or how well the singers were able to create their images. These data were

classified into three subcategories: (a) ability, accuracy, and level of detail; (b)

deliberation and direction; (c) amount and duration. The frequency of results of the

various characteristics of vocalists’ imagery ability can be seen in Table 18.

When singers were asked about their imagery ability and degree of accuracy

separately, the responds were identical. Twelve singers said it was good while three

others rated theirs as fair. Regarding vividness, 10 vocalists reported having very vivid

images and five felt that theirs were fairly vivid. Four singers described their images as

being very accurate, with Louis adding, “right down to the last level of minutia.” Some

felt that their images could be improved.

183
Table 18

Vocal Participants’ Imagery Ability

Characteristic Number of Participants

Ability 12 Good 3 Fair

Accuracy 12 Very accurate 3 fair

Vividness 10 Very vivid 5 Fair

Manipulate 11 Well 4 Fair

Helpful 10 (5 no answer)

Hurtful 2 Not hurtful 8 at times (5 no answer)

Detail 4 Very vivid (11 no answer)

Need for Improvement 3 Yes (12 no answer)

Deliberation involved whether the image appeared spontaneously or deliberately

and the degree to which it was controlled or manipulated. Singers’ rated their imagery

deliberation as being primarily controlled, with 11 participants reporting that they were

able to easily manipulate their images, and four rating their ability to do this as fair. Some

singers realized that physical practice was a prerequisite to imagining, as exemplified by

Nicholas who said, “I found that probably just because I have a lot of practice on the

stage things now, that I can imagine. Imagery was good.” Some exceptions and problems

in manipulating images included Monique’s “little Satan,” which appeared to her

spontaneously and could have debilitated her performance if she had not exercised

control over it prior to walking out on stage.


184
Direction dealt with hurtful (debilitative) or helpful (facilitative) images. In

response to imagery direction, 10 singers reported that their images were generally more

helpful and facilitative than hurtful. However, eight felt that sometimes their imagery

could be harmful or debilitative. Ophelia illustrated:

It’s hurtful if you don’t control it. . . . If you are discovering technique and you

find the wrong image and it doesn’t help you and you continue to use it over and

over again, then yes it can be hurtful as well.

At times Anna’s images were not helpful, as she stated, “if I don’t do a good job

[imaging], it can be destructive.” Singers reported this occurring especially when their

images were not appropriate to their specific needs. Catherine found that sometimes

“they’re not helpful, because they’re really not the right image.” Francine discovered, “If

it’s hurtful, it’s because I’ve chosen to do something that’s unsuccessful, and then, again,

I have to make an adjustment.” Several participants discovered problems when they did

not take the time to imagine clearly, such as Ophelia discovering her “blind spots” on

stage, where she had inadvertently omitted specific imagery in her performance

preparation.

Regarding amount and duration, the former dealt with the time given to engage in

imagery, while the latter involved how long the image appeared, remained and

proceeded, and faded. Although no question specifically centered on either amount or

duration of the image itself, a few participants offered glimpses of their experiences in

this regard. Amount of time in imagery sessions seemed to vary with the individual from

very quick flashes (Gerald) to consciously practicing imagery all night long in preparing

185
for an upcoming role (Eloise). Regarding duration, Louis described using high-speed

imagery in practice and memorizing:

I go through the recital very quickly and very frantically in my mind in an attempt

to make sure that I can recall the text right to the forefront of my mind very

quickly. It’s a memory drill to make sure that I have the text memorized really,

but I am thinking the notes and the pitches all the way through. Oftentimes what

I’ll do in this type of practice is eliminate any rests or sections where I’m not

singing. It will literally be practicing just my singing responsibility. And I’ll do it

as quickly as I can and I’ll move from one section to the next immediately

without break.

This detailed example of high speed, or compressed mental practice, seemed to help

solidify the song in memory for Louis. Just prior to auditions, Nicholas tried, in his

words, “to visualize pretty much everything that happens for about 10 minutes.” Dorine

spent 15 minutes imagining her performances from start to finish. Information on

imagery amount and duration in vocal performance seemed to depend on the need of the

individual and their facility with imagery skills.

As a result of participating in these interviews, several singers’ understanding of

imagery changed particularly with regard to the uses of their senses and abilities. Gerald

reflected this, when he said, “my first criticism I would make is that I need to make my

visualization more specific, more detailed.” In evaluating her imagery strengths and

weaknesses as a result of answering the questions, Eloise realized that she could shift her

use of sense imagery and include those that were less developed. She said, “I think I’ve

stacked a little bit too much in the kinesthetic and I should be more aware of the

186
emotional. I think I should think about taste, smell, and touch, which I’ve said I don’t

like, and experiment with those.” Catherine’s retrospection also revealed her desire for

improvement when she declared:

I am not a master. I feel a little bit lazy at it all. Like I said, you’re bringing up all

these things that I could be doing. This is so cool. I think I go to a comfort level,

and then don’t push it, because I don’t necessarily take the time. . . . I’m

constantly aware that the imagery is a good way to go, but in practice, it does not

always play out as well as I would hope. But that could be because I’m not very

consistent in my use. . . . So find the accurate and then reinforce it by repetition,

and I don’t think I do enough repetition.

Catherine and some of the other singers reflected a belief that there may be a link

between the specificity and clarity of the image and performance outcome. Furthermore,

it became apparent to many of these participants that imagery ability could be continually

developed and strengthened and that it was a powerful tool in their quest for performance

excellence. Results of singers’ reasons for using imagery content and ability will now be

discussed.

Why Do Professional Vocalists Use Imagery to Achieve Optimal Performance?

Responses regarding why, or the reasons for which imagery was used, were

similar and at times quite different compared to athletic and dance imagery research.

Singers’ imagery functions seemed to generally reflect aspects of the athletic frameworks

of Paivio (1985), Hall and associates (1998), Munroe and others (2000), and perhaps

more specifically those of dance imagery research (Nordin & Cumming, 2005). In

analyzing the data, emerging categories were constantly compared with those found in

187
the sport and dance frameworks. Vocal professionals’ reasons for using imagery were

classified into four categories: (a) cognitive, (b) motivational, (c) artistic, and (d) healing.

Many of these reasons were presented in previous sections to allow the reader to

understand the details of how the content of images interacted intrinsically with the

purposes for which they were being used in these professional vocalists’ experiences.

Additionally, participant responses were interchangeable across different questions,

which created certain challenges in reporting these results.

Cognitive Reasons

The cognitive reasons for using imagery addressed the functions of thinking

through or planning out behaviors. These were divided between cognitive specific (CS),

which included skill learning and execution, and cognitive general (CG), relating to

sequencing and strategies, originally proposed by Paivio (1985). Table 19 included

specific categories and subcategories of singers’ cognitive reasons for using imagery.

Learning vocal techniques and skills (cognitive specific). Cognitive reasons for

singers’ use of imagery were those in which singers reported using images for the

purposes of learning, developing, strengthening, and correcting skills. Results from the

interviews were exceptionally frequent regarding acquiring proficiency in vocal

production and quality and resulted in two distinct subcategories of vocal production and

metaphors in vocal production. These quotes primarily involved changing or securing

vocal technique, and were therefore included in the cognitive category.

188
Table 19

Vocal Participants’ Cognitive Reasons for Imagery Use Categories and Subcategories

Cognitive Reasons Categories Subcategories Number of Participants

Cognitive Specific (CS) Vocal Production Technique 10

Ease & Comfort 9

Breath Support 7

Kinesthetic 6

Audiation 2

Note Duration 2

Projection 2

Metaphors in Breath 11

Vocal Production Throat Opening 6

High Notes 3

Vocal Registers 2

Resonance 2

Changing Quality 2

Vocal Line 4

Memorizing & Planning, Learning & Learning 6

Cognitive General (CG) Memorizing Memorizing 7

Sequencing Songs & Arias 5

Program Planning Recitals 10

Stories 2

Staging Movements 7

Motivation 2

Characterization Clarity of Role 6

189
Vocal production involved specific aspects of using imagery directly involved

with producing, extending, or projecting the sound. Each of the 15 singers offered several

responses in vocal production issues, resulting in seven subcategories: (a) vocal

technique, (b) ease and comfort of producing sound, (c) breath support, (d) kinesthetic

connection, (e) audiation, (f) extending note duration, and (g) projection.

Specific aspects of vocal technique appeared in ten of the responses. Some

singers, such as Catherine used technique images in warm-ups “to help me produce

proper vocal technique.” Eloise illustrated her experience of this, and said that:

in order to sing scales that go up and down, I imagine the opposite direction and

the opposite flow of energy. . . . in order to sing from low to high, I’m gonna sink

down and out through my body.

Imagery to keep the voice grounded in the high notes and lifted in the low ones assisted

this singer in maintaining continuity of sound execution throughout the registers. Other

singers were also concerned with gaining skills in singing in specific registers. For

example, Nicholas was looking to produce “a little more focus and ring in the upper part

of my voice.” In producing a vocal sound particular to mezzos, Karen said, “In order to

do that whistle range, I just see these imaginary folds going almost together. It just pops

right into my mind.” Imagery for vocal placement concerned the muscles of the vocal

apparatus producing tones in a specific way, as Ivan illustrated. He said, “I can anticipate

what the voice is going to feel like in its optimal conditions related to the sequence of

music that I want to sing.” Conversely, several vocalists found that over the years they

tended, as Louis stated, “to rely more on mechanics and more on the science of vocal

technique rather than on sensation.” Instead of kinesthetic feeling, Louis preferred more

190
informed technical imagery for singing. It seemed that as vocal skill developed, imagery

preferences changed. These two elements of vocal skill and imagery interacted and grew

during the course of a vocalist’s career.

In producing vocal sound, nine participants mentioned using imagery to feel ease

or comfort in various areas of the body, and particular parts of the vocal apparatus. In

specifically striving for relaxation in the neck and jaw, Ophelia maintained, “you should

never think about having a voice box, having a neck at all.” To this end, Gerald wanted to

experience “the sound of the passage being easy, the image of freedom and free flow.”

Imagery was particularly useful in creating the feeling of effortlessness of singing. Louis

explained, “I do try for a gentle, easy sound, but oftentimes it’s difficult to interpret

mechanically. Instead you just do it sensorily [sic] and you try to experience a little

gentler, easier style of singing.” These singers believed that creating the image of singing

ease produced the proper vocal sounds and a comfortable feeling in their bodies.

Imagery for skill learning also involved the development and maintenance of

breath support for seven participants. Four singers talked about breath support by

imagining their breath deepening while they sang an ascending vocal line. Gerald

illustrated his use of imagery to gain more breath support. He stated, “I would go back to

breathing and visualization. When I am taking a breath or using a breath, imagining the

air filling places where oxygen really doesn’t go but you want to find expansion.” This

example also incorporated Gerald’s kinesthetic use of imagery in gaining more control in

vocal production.

Kinesthetically imagining in vocal execution arose for six participants who

described a physiological feeling of producing the ideal sound image. Anna exemplified

191
this aptly, and said, “It’s a combination of the feeling and what sounds you’re going for. .

. . You get that exact thing in your head and then you breathe into what you’re already

imagining.” More specifically, Eloise imagined parts of her body in creating a strong

sound, saying:

I think of that through my back, and a weight trainer body, I imagine a very strong

body. . . . I create this huge instrument in my mind and then in my body, just

actually feel the muscles and gauge them with the depths of the voice, which is

what carries.

Nicholas tried to imagine the feeling of sound being “well grounded from deep in my

body.” Francine experienced this deep placement in executing lower notes when she said,

“what I feel is a relaxed letting myself go and letting the sound just drop in the middle of

my body. . . . So I have that image every single time I go into my chest voice.” Vocal

production for these singers was rooted more deeply in their entire body than just the

vocal apparatus of the head, neck, and upper torso.

Regarding integration of both optimal vocal production and body awareness,

Ophelia was one of two who mentioned kinesthetic imagery in relation to gaining

automaticity and forming proper habits for performance. She said:

[A] lot of it comes with doing it again and again and just getting it into the body

and getting that kinesthetic memory, so that when it comes down to performance,

honest to God, you’re not thinking about it, because you have so many other

things to think about, that you want it to just be rote memory. But there are

occasionally those spots that are very difficult and even in performance you need

to think about it, when you have to focus on the kinesthetic memory.

192
Kinesthetic imagery in learning through the body seemed to facilitate the movements and

skills becoming more automatic and habitual.

The last three elements in this section on vocal production were audiation,

extending note durations, and projecting the sound. Eloise heard the sound she wanted to

make before she sang it. She said, “It was to audiate, so to hear it, listen to it, mentally so

to speak, before you try to actually make the sound.” Imagery in audiation often

improved pitch accuracy and proper mental and physical preparation to produce the

sound in the specific context of the music. Extending note durations began with breath

control and mental thought processes in preparing that sound. Ophelia described using

imagery to help learn this technique. She said, “if I have to sing a high C of 16 beats, then

I would just keep thinking of an image that would lengthen the note.” Anna used imagery

slightly differently to achieve the same effect:

[E]ven if you have a note that you need to hold. . . if you imagine while you’re

singing that you’re breathing in at the same time, so you actually are breathing in

at the same time. . . . it restructures your body without out using valuable energy

and voice.

Not only do singers have to find ways to hold extensively long notes, but they have to

make their sound carry through a large theatre or venue without the use of microphone.

Josephine mentioned singing to the “old lady in the back row” as a mental image to help

her voice carry all the way through the theatre. Dorine would “envision a small point on

the wall and I sing to that point.” She imagined the sound touching or going beyond that

chosen point in the back of the theatre. Singers used different kinds of imagery to find

better ways of producing their sound. These accounts included many instances of

193
imagery use in improving various aspects of vocal skill and technique. This next section

examines metaphorical imagery in the technical aspects of singing.

All participants gave accounts of using metaphors in connection with vocal

production. Although several singers preferred not to use metaphors, they were all

familiar with them and how they were used in achieving proper vocal technique.

Metaphors were connected with vocal production in seven distinct areas: (a) support

including breath, (b) throat opening, (c) execution of high notes, d) singing through the

vocal registers, (e) creating resonance, (f) changing sound quality, and (g) connecting the

vocal line.

Supporting vocal sound was dependent on breath control in diaphragmatic

breathing and was vital for classical singing. Metaphors were plentiful in this area and

were reported by 11 singers (e. g., Gerald: “smelling a rose for inhalation,” Francine, “a

steel rod coming out of my feet and going all the way through the core of the earth”). In

order to relieve muscular tension, Anna imagined “making everything really relaxed

[like] a puppet, you’re a dummy on top, where everything would be happening

underneath like the anchor.” Eloise created the image of “a three-dimensional pyramid

that goes out from my body, out behind me, very wide. . . . Your voice goes effortlessly

through the peaks of the pyramid because it has such wide, wonderful support at the

base.” Many singers mentioned that using metaphors greatly aided in establishing support

and controlling the breath, however as they developed as singers these skills became

more automatic and many began to prefer anatomical images of the actual vocal

mechanism.

194
In classical singing training, one of the first techniques most students were taught

was to lift the soft palate in the back of the throat to create more space in singing. It was

most easily accomplished by maintaining the feeling of a yawn during singing. Six

singers in this study mentioned various metaphors to help them achieve that continuous

feeling (e.g., Nicholas: “egg in the back of my throat,” Dorine: “plum in the mouth,”

Louis: “imagine I have a capitol ‘I’ in the back of the throat”).

In order to secure high notes and to keep the sound light and pleasant, three

sopranos used metaphors. Dorine provided a vivid example of this. She stated:

[W]hen I’ve taken a deep enough breath, or if I’m going for higher notes, then I

want my breath to stay strong beneath me. . . . I pretend that I’m sliding down a

banister, moving down physically while the voice is moving up, and it helps to

deepen, it helps my breath to stay deep.

This idea of singing high while thinking low was repeated several times. To achieve a

more delicate sound, Josephine felt that the metaphor of birdsong was the “perfect

imagery for a soprano, high sounds, like something that’s pretty and easy and floating.”

Josephine imagined supported high notes when she spoke of placing the notes on a shelf

as previously quoted. The image of a shelf helped her think of her high notes as having a

place to rest, rather than mentally pushing her sound higher. Singers consistently found

metaphors to create the sound they needed.

Regardless of the vocal category, singers’ voices were considered to have vocal

registers that were delineated by “breaks” in separating the high, middle, and low parts of

their voice. Singers needed to develop and maintain consistency through these areas or

risk “cracking,” or a vocal shift, in passing from one register to another. Two singers

195
mentioned metaphors for contending with this. Gerald felt “the voice narrowing, that sort

of feeling that my voice registers blend there.” In order to successfully deal with this,

Francine described her metaphorical images as if they were:

‘a thread of chest, a thread of head.’ So what we’re constantly trying to

accomplish as singers is making our voice consistent from low to high and all the

way through the middle. . .but it’s never 100% just straight chest, there’s always a

thread of head in that. And then as you go up through the middle, how does that

shift on every note as you go up into the high register. So if you visualize a dial,

you have a high level of chest and a low level of head, and then it slowly and

gradually shifts up until the high registers where you have a high level of head

and a low level of chest.

Consistency through the vocal registers was facilitated with metaphorical imagery as well

as other aspects of the voice production.

After an extensive training was achieved in securing a foundation of vocal

production, these singers also concentrated on shaping the quality of their sound as was

expressed in nine of the transcriptions. Four singers mentioned the connection of human

sound capabilities such as a sigh, a grunt, a groan, or a yawn with providing a variety of

vocal possibilities and extending the quality of their sound. This was best exemplified in

Eloise’s account when she illustrated the following:

The most nutty metaphor has to do with different noises that we naturally make.

But they’re so wacky, like your conscious intellect doesn’t give you permission to

make these noises. . . . See, we have the entire orchestral family of instruments

right in our bodies, in our voices. Wheew, or quack, waaa, [she demonstrated the

196
sounds] because you’re really hot. I mean it’s like there’s a whole bunch of really

crazy people inside of you that need to make all of these different noises and they

live in different parts of your body.

In her interview, Eloise provided a detailed explanation of each of these specific imagery

sounds representing all the families of the orchestra. However, due to space

considerations, these are not given here. Although some of these singers had extensive

familiarity with vocal sound varieties, it did not appear as general knowledge in many of

the singers in this study. This aspect of singing could be further investigated.

Similar to imagining the variety of vocal possibilities, Harriet used metaphors to

create more resonance in her voice. She said, “I imagined that the space that one creates

in the cavity, the head, is sort of shaped like a cone, and that the voice functions within

the resonance of the voice is within that cone.” Harriet also used the image of her ribs

acting like fireplace bellows to increase deeper resonance in her torso. Francine achieved

more vocal resonance in her head when she imagined what “it feels like biting into a big

juicy apple.” These metaphors seemed to connect with kinesthetic feeling to facilitate

specific vocal production.

In performing an aria or song, soloists imagined connecting the entire line of the

music and text in performance. Four singers gave many examples of using metaphors to

help them stay connected throughout the song (e. g., Anna: “like the train is already

running and you just need to get on the train;” Dorine: “hanging each note on the

clothesline;” Monique: “my conveyor belt, a constant flowing line”). More specifically,

Barry used a metaphor taken from nature to help control the voice when the melody line

descended:

197
A lot of times singers crash, and they get lazy when they descend into a line and

the sound will go flat. And what I always say is the metaphor of a bird landing on

a small branch. You can’t just crash on the branch, you have to gently land on it

without cracking it.

Using various metaphors these singers were able to create a smooth and continuous line

throughout the song. These participants were exceptionally adept at using metaphors in

imagery for developing and perfecting extensive vocal technique. Imagery used for the

purpose of sequencing and memorizing musical elements for performance follows.

Memorizing and planning (cognitive general). The cognitive general (CG)

function of imagery use was found in responses and resulted in five sections: (a) learning

and memorizing repertoire, (b) sequence in song or aria, (c) program planning, (d)

sequence for staging, and (e) strategy for characterization.

Learning and memorizing songs, arias, and roles was a skill vocalists developed

and perfected throughout their profession. Six singers described various uses of imagery

in the process of learning the rudiments of a song. Gerald explained that he would

“mentally sing through with all the correct words, correct rhythm, high notes in place,

everything is there, voice tapped in the way I want it to, gestures where they need to be.”

Catherine imagined herself “singing through an entire piece vocally and singing

everything correctly.” Dorine emphasized kinesthetic feeling when learning new music

and said, “if there are passages that are difficult or I’m not doing it correctly, I just keep

doing it until the feeling is there.” Ivan also used repetition by mentally singing the song

“over and over again until it becomes really clear and then as I start to sing. I trace, in

effect, that imagery. I follow it. And I do that for most all my music.” Louis preferred

198
both physical and mental practice to fully memorize and embody the aria. He said, “sub-

vocal or trying to learn literature without really singing it, I find takes a lot more time

and practice to adequately prepare.” Mentally rehearsing the song served to reinforce all

the elements of the song rendering them automatic in the minds and voices of these

singers.

For seven participants, memorization was easier when a song they had to learn

was grouped in sequences and patterns. Two singers visualized these musical patterns on

the page to help them remember the song. Others used movements to assist them in

memory, as Harriet was quoted as saying, “I can imagine myself walking through

staging. I can see myself on stage going through staging outside of me actually doing it. I

use that to memorize things.” Louis ran through the music very quickly to test whether he

could “recall the text right to the forefront of my mind very quickly.” When singers used

imagery in sequences to frame their songs, memorization came more easily.

Elements in songs and arias were imagined in ordered sequences in order to give

optimal performances, as found in five responses. Josephine thought it was necessary to

anticipate “what’s coming up next, and how much energy, and how much vocal power

and breath, and all that it requires” in order to properly perform the song. Singers

specifically planned out how the song was to be executed. Harriet demonstrated this by

saying, “I’ll mark spaces where I’ll give a little less intensity. I’ll give myself a little bit

of a break here.” Ivan found it was important to “anticipate what the voice is going to feel

like in its optimal conditions related to the sequence of music that I want to sing.” Two

singers mentioned sequencing in terms of pacing, especially if the song was long. For

these arias, Ophelia found that “there’s a sequence that’s followed [that allowed her] to

199
go back to previous images or moods or colors [within the song] that . . . draws the

audience in.” In order to keep the song interesting and make it appealing to the listener,

these singers imagined sequential rudiments to deliver a cohesive and interesting aria.

For singers who were involved in giving concerts, one of the most obvious

applications of imagery in sequencing was in planning recital programs. Ten singers

reported mentally preparing these performances and elaborated on the details. In the case

of song cycles, where the composer has provided the connection between each song,

Karen described her treatment of this by saying:

Each piece had its own mood, and so you would do one, and the next one. You

would have to prepare in between. You got in the moment, it was almost like

opera because you went into this next character, and the whole mood and

character, and everything. . . . But that sort of imagery had to happen very quickly

because, of course, you stopped the one piece and then you were in another

characterization for the next one.

In practicing song cycles, Dorine said she “pictured the whole thing from start to finish. .

. . singing each piece through, and then singing in my head and making sure each piece

was correct.” In this case, sequence imagery facilitated memorizing the entire song cycle.

Some recitals required the vocalist to choose the sequence and create some

element to link these songs together in a logical progression. Francine said she had to

“come up with some kind of a story that makes sense in order to sequence through them.”

For Monique, planning her programs was very specifically arranged, as she illustrated:

[Y]ou build your program so that you pace yourself by what you’re singing. . . . I

plan my program so that, especially my solo recitals, that whatever I’m singing

200
first is really pretty easy to sing so that I can get over my nerves. Then the

program builds from there, and the biggest piece being just before the interval

[intermission]. And then you come back after the interval. And I always end my

recitals with something amusing, something light. So I guess it builds to a certain

height and then I go down again.

Connecting one song to another and creating the entire recital to be, as Catherine

described, “an organic whole,” took planning and the ability to imagine the effect of

whatever element the singer chose.

Sequences in staging involved mentally organizing the physical movements that

had to be synchronized precisely with the music. Seven participants reported using

imagery for sequencing performance movement. Ophelia explained that “you have to

prepare ahead of time, where you’re singing on stage, and prepare your singing ahead of

time to get all of those aligned correctly so you get the right effect.” Louis illustrated his

imagery of staging when he said, “it will include everything from blocking, to text, to

pitch, to my particular vocal approach through to the music.” He was able to include

many aspects of his performing experience in imagery. Some singers had to physically

practice the staging prior to imagining it. Louis discovered that “because I have a lot of

practice on the stage things now, that I can imagine, imagery was good.” Harriet also

provided an excellent example of this in the following quote:

I have to see things I physically did for things to physically work in my body

before I can imagine myself walking through staging. [Then] I can see myself on

stage going through staging outside of me actually doing it. I use that to memorize

201
things. That and the sequence of the scene. . . the order of staging for a scene or

the opera itself.

These singers connected these sequences with the physical aspect of performing.

Staging sequencing also was connected with the subtext and motivation of the

character of song. For example, Francine found that “the moment to moment reality is

very much again about ‘Why am I doing this?’ and the subtext, what’s not being said . . .

the motivation . . . every single moment has to be justified.” For Ivan, staging and

memory was very easy:

I rarely have to write my staging in the book because what I do is that when I go

through something, immediately, I am replaying it in my head over and over

again. The directions, where I was going, why I was going, everything, the music

I was going on.

The sequences in movement and music in staging were deeply connected for these

singers and further assisted them in memory of both staging and singing the song or aria.

In performing a role in opera, strategies were used to help the singer embody the

character. Six singers mentioned motivation and clarifying the underlying reasons for

acting out the role. Monique said, “I try to find out who my character is. And therefore

my character is the motivation for virtually everything I do in an opera.” There were

several ways that singers suggested in order to accomplish this. One was through

understanding the strategies of how to feel emotions. Nicholas explained, “once I have

music learned, or a role learned, then I will plot out an emotional path through the aria or

the role, and think of different emotional directions I would like to take it.” Barry

elaborated, “the basic premises for acting is ‘What do I want to achieve?’ or ‘How do I

202
want to make you feel?’ as the other person. . . . Those are strategies that you experiment

with during the rehearsal process.” Eloise eloquently presented reasons subtext was vital

for her acting:

It really does have to be internalized. Like walking for a role. I have to be able to

feel the motivation. Why am I doing this walking? Why is this physical thing

necessary? What is it expressing? And I have to feel it, and then my body is going

to respond. . . . So that it doesn’t interrupt the flow of the story and the emotion. It

actually supports it and contributes to it.

For Eloise and many others, using imagery in understanding and working out the

strategies for the character was essential in feeling or embodying the essence of the

character.

These examples represented the variety of cognitive uses of imagery provided by

the singers’ responses. They included execution of skill, sequencing and strategy and

seemed to be an essential element in vocalists’ efforts to achieve their desired objectives

in performance. The responses of the motivational functions of vocalists’ imagery is now

provided.

Motivational Reasons

Motivation reasons for using imagery were three-fold: (a) motivational specific

(MS) in goal setting and attainment; (b) motivational general-arousal (MG-A) in

regulating arousal, anxiety, and energy; and (c) motivational general-mastery (MG-M)

for self-confidence and mastery (Paivio, 1985; Hall et al., 1998). Categories and

subcategories of findings in motivational reasons can be found in Table 20.

203
Table 20

Vocal Participants’ Motivational Reasons for Imagery Use: Categories and

Subcategories

Motivational Reasons Categories Subcategories Number of

Participants

Motivational Specific (MS) Goal Attainment Long-term 13

Short-term 11

Other Issues 5

Motivational General- Arousal/ Energy Calming 15

Arousal (MG-A) Breathing 14

Body Concerns 6

Focus & Concentration 13

Preparation 10

Self-talk 10

Psyching-Up 13

Motivational General- Confidence Self-Efficacy 8

Mastery (MG-M) Successful Images 9

Self-talk 9

Mastery Mastery 5

Fixing Problems 7

204
Goal attainment (motivational specific). The motivational specific (MS) imagery

reasons included identifying goals in order to inspire the singer to continue to strive to

achieve ideals or specific roles. This purpose for using imagery in participants’ responses

resulted in three categories: (a) long-term, (b) short-term, and (c) other issues regarding

motivation in goal imagery. The discussion of imagery content goals of outcome,

performance, and process has previously been provided. Further concerns involved

distinguishing goal imagery from daydreaming and relative value and success of goal

attainment.

Differences arose between long-term and short-term goals for many participants.

Thirteen singers had long-term goals, mostly replicating those already described in

outcome goals, which inspired them to improve. Louis entertained a general long-term

outline of goals to motivate him in his future plans:

I do tend to try to goal-set for myself, and tell myself where I would like to be. . . .

If I have a five or a ten year accomplishment that I would like, I do make a note of

that goal and I do recognize that that’s something that I want to achieve. . . .

Eleven singers imagined attaining their desired roles in succession and relied more on a

step-by-step process, as in achieving a given series of short-term goals. Ivan would

break down the long-term goals and asked himself, “How did I arrive at this goal?

That’s what I did, okay. How did I arrive at that place? . . . just kind of come like that in

reverse order for me.”

In contrast, several participants had concerns about the usefulness and benefit in

imagining short-term goals to help inspire them. Harriet stated, “the minutia is not

something that I ever imagine. I don’t imagine me in rehearsals or coaching a certain

205
role. Actually, I sort of skip over all of that in my imagery.” Similar to this sentiment,

Catherine said, “all the steps between my one-year goal and that are ridiculously

unimaginable for me.” Eloise also became frustrated with short-term goals, and said, “I

found it more discouraging to see my projected timeline get interrupted by things

beyond my control.” Therefore these singers preferred a broader approach to goals in

imagery.

Other issues and concerns with imagery emerged in goals changing over the

years, and how goals were affected by parenting children. Eloise and other participants

experienced marked changes in their work in goal attainment imagery as they progressed

in their careers and became more accomplished. In describing her experience of finally

being invited to join a renowned professional singing organization after years of

auditioning for them, Monique said:

I came back and sang for them, didn’t care anymore, got straight in . . . because I

didn’t care so much anymore. . . . But it was a wonderful feeling, getting into the

[company] that I’d so desperately wanted to be in. But once I went to the first

rehearsal, I thought, as I listened to them, I thought, ‘I’ve moved beyond this.’

Francine agreed, “goals change as you start to become aware.” The decision to raise

children was a major consideration in how two singers allowed themselves to imagine

achieving their goals. Ivan felt, “It’s difficult as a father and a husband, because you have

to consider your kids, you affect your kids as well.” Josephine, who also had children,

wanted to progress in her career, but had “made the choice not to be away from them for

six weeks or eight.” As a result she was satisfied with regional engagements. Both singers

had to make adjustments in their careers goals and consequently in their imagery.

206
Concerning goal imagery, some of the participants also commented on the

distinction between goal imagery and daydreaming, the effectiveness of imagery, and the

benefits of refining and increasing goal imagery. In describing his experience of

imagining a specific goal, Ivan said, “It’s not so much a wishful sense of ‘Oh, I wish this

might happen.’ But, I allow myself to completely feel it as if it’s real.” In analyzing her

use of imagery to secure a particular role which she finally won after years of effort,

Monique came to realize that “imagining it is like this expression: ‘See it, be it.’ You see

yourself in it. And I think I was longing to be in it, but not seeing myself in it.” Monica

distinguished that goal attainment involved specificity in imagery; longing for something

begets continuing the feeling of longing, while seeing and embodying the goal help

manifest that image. These singers varied as to their efforts and applications in goal

imagery, but generally felt that the more clearly the goals were imagined, the better their

chances were of experiencing that image.

Several singers elaborated on the successes they had experienced with clearly and

elaborately imagining goal outcomes functioning motivationally. Francine secured

several of her roles using this method, wherein she said, “they offered me a contract in

the audition, because I had already gone through the whole process in my brain of

having already performed it.” Francine’s imagery was as though she was actually acting

in that role, convincing herself it was a present reality. Likewise Harriet gave an account

of achieving two different roles. She stated, “maybe a year ago I said . . . I wanted to be

singing Traviata and Lucia. And I imagined myself in that role, and I can imagine

myself on stage, and I had it.” As a result of his years of success with goal imagery,

Barry suggested:

207
[T]he more specific you can be, the more, I use the word, visualization, the more

you can visualize what the actual end product is, and articulate it, write it down,

say it, get really it out of your body on to something concrete, a piece of paper,

then, it becomes, it materializes itself much more easily.

For these vocalists, goal imagery was a vital part of their motivation and their

professional success. As a result of these accomplishments, they increased their goal

imagery, however it was not clear how they initially began and developed this.

This desire to engage in more goal imagery emerged in three of the singers. In

considering using imagery to achieve his goal to sing at the MET, Nicholas said, “I’ve

used that a little bit to imagine myself singing there. . . . I haven’t really gotten into any

detail into it yet, although it could possibly be helpful. It’s more of generalized idea at

this point.” When asked if she imagined achieving her goals, Catherine replied, “I don’t

think I’ve ever actually done it, but it sounds like a good idea, to imagine myself in the

position I want to be in.” Some singers who shared less experience with imagery in

achieving their goals did not feel as strongly about using goal imagery as a tool of

motivation.

All these singers used goal imagery to motivate themselves in their profession but

valued goal work differently. Those who spent a great deal of time engaged in imagining

their desired results did so because they were convinced of its effectiveness. Others

imagined with much less intent and hope for results. In any case, goal imagery was used

as a tool to help these singers move towards optimal performance.

Arousal and energy (motivational general-arousal). Motivational general-arousal

(MG-A) imagery comprised the second reason or purpose for which vocal professionals

208
used imagery. Singers’ responses describing this imagery function resulted in the sole

category of efforts to calm anxiety or tension and energy for psyching up, for which the

same images could achieve optimal arousal levels for performance.

Imagery methods to regulate arousal and energy to maintain appropriate levels for

singing revealed seven subcategories, which were (a) calming efforts, (b) managing the

breath, (c) coping with body concerns, (d) focus and concentration, (e) preparation, (f)

self-talk, and (g) psyching-up. The ability for a singer to calm anxiety and arousal levels

and control nerves was regarded as essential in performing excellence. All participants

described using imagery for calming, which they practiced most particularly before and

during performance and valued highly in performance.

Managing breathing was considered a vital element in calming nerves and

anxiety. High arousal levels often severely diminished a singers’ ability to breath. In

describing how her breath was affected during times of intense performance anxiety,

Monique said, “it’s very hurtful, and it affects my breathing badly.” One of the primary

methods fourteen of these singers suggested to achieve calmness was gaining proper

breath control. Nine singers used their breath to specifically calm down (e.g., Harriet: “a

lot of stress and anxiety and things like that can be harnessed when you just get a hold of

your breath”). Several mentioned the physiological benefits of slow, relaxed breathing.

Nicholas said, “I’m breathing slowly and I’ve found that that helps control or helps me

control my adrenalin and my heart rate a little bit.” Three singers were reluctant to

attribute the calming effects of breathing to imagery. For example, Gerald stated that

“using and moving the air through my body to calm down. I’m not sure if that’s as much

an image as a natural phenomenon.” Imagery might have been instrumental in causing

209
the physical body to breath in a way that calms nerves, especially when that was the

performer’s intention. Learning to control breathing was possibly a result of extensive

practice to make this skill automatic and fully accessible to the professional vocalist.

Adding to the benefits of good breathing, Ophelia said, “If I can’t breathe fully then I

don’t feel like the performance would be as satisfactory for me, or the audience.”

Singers also shared their use of imagery in dealing with physiological concerns

and the effects of nervousness. Over the years, six participants discovered ways to use

their nerves for improving their performances. Eloise reframed her experience of anxiety

and said, “It’s not so much nerves as adrenalin.” Changing the way a singer regarded

nerves seemed to facilitate calmer feelings during performances. Ivan said, “[I] stopped

resisting against the nerves and started embracing them. . . . and just understand that this

is something my body did for whatever reason and I didn’t view it as a negative.” Two

singers identified specific bodily reactions to stress they eventually accepted and found

ways to cope with it. Eloise would locate the area of tension in her body and feel it relax.

She said, “imagine a fist, and that’s the tension, and – tension is not bad. It’s just in the

fist, so that the hand is open and the energy can flow.” Physical manifestations of

nervousness and stage fright were often alleviated or resolved by mentally imagining

their solutions.

Contending with the debilitating effects of performance anxiety was sometimes

overwhelming especially considering the enormous stress singers had to endure. Karen

illustrated this clearly in the following quote:

[I]magery saved my life in stage fright. . . . this particular season, and I think it

was coupled with getting my graduate degree, and I actually had a baby that I was

210
nursing at the same time, and I think I did have a little bit of pressure on me. I

think the combination of all that probably entered into it. . . . I decided that I

would use the imagery to back down the anxiety. . . . it was just a matter of

visualizing the worst and how I could pull it off. And it was just that exit door that

I never had to use.

Often performers were able to regulate arousal when they could identify specific mental

thoughts and feelings that cause fear and those that calmed them.

Maintaining focus and concentration was another method of imagery used by 13

singers to feel the ideal levels of arousal during performance. Specifically, eight singers

mentioned focusing on the task at hand. Some developed skills to deal with their thoughts

wondering during singing by practicing concentration exercises. Staying positive and not

entertaining any negative thoughts was a solution for four singers. Josephine suggested:

[I]f you feel nervous about a performance. . . . focus only on the image of the

music, to focus only on the picture of the music of what you’re singing, not to

allow other thoughts to come into your head. . . . and shut down all the thoughts

that are negative while you’re performing.

Anna and Eloise felt that thoughts of connecting with the audience helped her to more

completely focus on her performance. Eloise described her experience in the following

excerpt:

I picture the audience and try to imagine what they’re hoping for. And when I

remember that they came for something good and to feel good, and that they’re

not there to pick me apart, then I calm down, because then I feel like they’re on

my team.

211
Concentrating and focusing were best practiced in rehearsal so controlling mental

thoughts could be easily achieved on stage.

Obviously, a singer prepared by learning all the aspects of executing songs,

however, they also used imagery to visualize the perfect performance, mentally plan its

execution, and imagine technical excellence. Ten singers cited the importance of

sufficiently preparing roles and arias prior to performing to alleviate performance

anxiety. Louis’ response clearly exemplified this when he said, “I think I am more calm

because of my advanced preparation, mentally.” Five singers expressed the importance of

visualizing the perfect performance to calm their nerves. Ivan illustrated the difference in

preparedness between roles he knew and ones he did not by saying:

If I am not able to visualize clearly what I want, then what happens is that

inevitably my nerves will increase a little bit. I’ll feel a little unsettled. New roles

are this way for me. . . . If, for pieces that I repeat and I know, I just know to the

point that I can just hear it, I can feel it, feel myself singing it, see every

production detail and staging, then the level of stress decreases greatly.

Six singers felt more secure when they mentally planned the details of a performance. For

example, Karen declared, “I don’t like surprises, so I pretty much have in my mind

everything that I’m going to do.” Imagining as many details as possible in the entire

performance calmed performers minds and allowed them to concentrate on executing the

song.

Similar to athletic competitors, 10 vocalists used self-talk in calming nerves and

adjusting their anxiety, or motivational drive. Four singers used self-talk in alleviating

self-doubt. For example, just before stepping on stage, Monique heard:

212
a little Satan inside me. As I’m breathing for the next line, saying, ‘You’re going

to forget this line.’ . . . And as the curtain opened, this devil in me said, ‘Oh,

what’s your first line?’ And at least I had time to say, ‘Go away, just relax. This

line will come.’ And it did.

Louis recognized his anxiety and would mentally respond with self-talk when he said, “I

picture myself on stage, I picture myself nervous, I picture myself ready to give the

performance. But then I follow through and say that even though I am nervous or

apprehensive, I will perform well anyway.” Other singers repeated phrases to strengthen

their motivation, such as Eloise who continually said to herself, “adrenalin is fantastic

energy, it’s flowing through you, you’re a lion, and you’re about to go on stage and

possess the stage and own it.” Similarly, Barry said, “if I’m nervous because I’m not at

100% and I’ll close my eyes and say a mantra, if you will, that allows me to relieve some

of the stress.” Self-talk was another form of imagery these singers used to successfully

calm them when they experienced different forms of performance anxiety.

With regard to imagery use in calming arousal, the other elements emerging from

the interviews involved grounding, meditation, thoughts of family, flow, and balancing

energy. Anna said, “I’ll try to imagine my feet growing into the ground” to relax and

stabilize her nerves. Two performers practiced meditation and one entertained warm

thoughts of family for feeling peaceful and ready to go on stage. Another two singers

imagined being in a flow with the audience (e.g., Anna, “that flow that comes through me

and back out to them, and that infinity thing”). These performers strove to achieve the

proper balance between high and low energy. Eloise explained:

213
[I]f I’m too hyper, again, if I just think about that flow, for some reason that’s a

balancing thing. It kind of works in both directions. If it’s frenetic or if it’s

stopped up, if I just kind of let it, keep it flowing steadily, then it either calms

down what’s too hyper or it energizes what’s comatose.

Singers seemed to find their individual balance for optimal performance, which usually

required calming.

At certain times performers needed to increase their energy and psyche-up as 13

participants reported. Eloise illustrated the need for feeling more motivation and her

remedy in the following quote. She stated:

[W]hen I have to perform and I feel kind of low energy or maybe even down

emotionally . . . you get it flowing and to free up any negative feelings so that

they’ll go away, because you just can’t perform with negativity. . . . [It] is like

waterfalls or rivers flowing, and it’s just this cleaning, this clean flow.

To psyche up, Anna thought about the efforts the audience expended to attend the theatre.

Two singers were motivated by the privilege they felt by being able to sing. Francine

would tell herself, “I get to do this, this is a treat, this is a privilege. Many people work

their entire lives to be able to do this.” Eloise would say, “This is what I love to do. I get

to do what I love to do, which makes me happy.” Gerald created the metaphor of “the

energy of a volcano eruption” to feel more energy for performing. Conversely, three

participants did not think they used imagery to psyche up, using primarily physical

activity to achieve this. As an example, Louis declared:

214
I don’t think that that’s an imagery or visualization issue. I think that’s just a

physical issue. You just have to get to the practice room when you need to,

whether you want to or not. You just do it.

For these singers, psyching up was gained by dedication or rigorous physical activity.

Using imagery to control nerves, relax, and calm down were more critical than

employing those to increase energy to perform on stage. This may have been a distinct

difference from how arousal and energy were regarded in the more physical activities

involved in sport and dance. It is also possible that too much energy especially aggressive

energy could have adversely affected the intricacies of vocal performance for these

singers.

Self-confidence and mastery (motivational general-mastery). The self-confidence

and mastery imagery category included managing difficulties, staying positive, and

mastering situations. All participants reported using images of confidence and mastery,

resulting in five subcategories, (a) self-efficacy feelings, (b) successful image, (c) self-

talk, (d) mastery, and (e) fixing problems.

Imagery to gain and maintain confidence was important to all participants and

was separated between images of self-efficacy (eight singers) and imagining successful

outcomes (nine singers). Confident images of self-efficacy were primarily feelings of

being strong and self-assured. Several mentioned such images as getting “a diva attitude,

like I just know I look good on stage” (Eloise). Josephine preferred to use the modeling

method, and suggested, “imagine yourself as [being someone] . . . you really look up to.

Who do you see as very successful and a very good performer?” This helped her embody

the appearance and feelings of the self-efficacy and confidence she wanted to portray on

215
stage. Other singers combined confident images with successfully imagining a

performance. For example, Karen offered, “I certainly imagine myself as a winner. I

imagine what I could do to make the performance better, I guess, or to make it really,

really good.” Other singers such as Anna believed “If I could imagine through the whole

thing without singing, I felt more secure.” Realizing the importance of directing her own

imagery, Francine reflected:

I think that goes back to definitely imagining a successful outcome as opposed to

an unsuccessful outcome. And that’s a hard thing to do because I’ve become more

and more aware how we do subconsciously project to people our own insecurities.

And if we’re not keeping that stuff in check, you really can convey all of your

fears as opposed to all of your confidences.

Many singers recognized the value in making sure their imagery and thoughts of future

performances reflected excellence.

Another motivational method of achieving confidence and mastery in singers was

self-talk. Nine singers’ responses revealed the use of self-talk in performing songs and

general feelings of confidence. Three singers reported using self-talk to execute songs

successfully. Gerald would mentally say, “This is going to be fine, this is going. I’m

going to be successful. I’m going to sing everything correctly. I’m going to remember my

words, and it’s going to be great.” In singing challenging passages, Monique said:

[W]hen there’s something very difficult coming up, a very high note or difficult

passagio note, or something like that, I would say, ‘I can do all things through

Christ.’ Or I would cut out the Christ part and say ‘I can do all things,’ so that I

216
saw that immediately just before I was going to sing it and it just sort of girded

my loins.

Self-talk, religiously oriented or otherwise, was used to secure notes that were potentially

troublesome for this singer, which also strengthen feelings of self-efficacy in mastering

its execution. Self-talk was also used by six singers to instill general feelings of

confidence. Examples of these included such repetitive affirmations such as, “you are a

wonderful singer. You are a beautiful” (Anna). Just before stepping on stage to perform,

Monique declared to herself, “I AM the Queen!” This helped strengthen her self-

confidence and facilitated performance excellence.

Related to confidence were feelings of mastery in singing performance, especially

when singing was exceptionally challenging or when problems appeared or persisted.

Five singers described using motivational imagery for mastery in more general terms.

Nicholas stated, “I use imagery to . . . master the elements that are within my control, the

ones that I have responsibility for.” Karen expanded this idea by saying:

Being focused, thinking through what I’m going to do and how I’m going to do it,

and all from knowing that I can do it. ‘All right, I have done it. I’m capable of

doing it,’ and the only reason I won’t do it is that I do myself in. So, in order not

to do myself in, then I know I have to be right on top of my game.

Feeling completely competent and clearly practicing the performance helped her to

translate these images to the stage.

Seven singers used mastery images specifically to prepare for difficulties or to fix

problems that arose while performing. Mastery imagery for dealing with difficulties

included vocal execution, managing change, and prior preparation. In classical singing, it

217
was common for singers to switch from one voice type to another during the course of

their career due to physiological changes. Executing higher or lower notes than what the

singer was accustomed to could present problems, as Ivan illustrated:

I had switched from baritone to tenor. . . anything that any note above the staff

freaked me out. . . . I started really visualizing specifically being more confident

and approaching it with confidence and not being so either apologetic, or fearful,

or concerned about it. So as I approached those notes . . . then I was visualizing

specifically just the confidence of the sound. . . [in] my approach to those pitches.

Ivan used imagery to remedy this particular problem and was able to gain mastery.

Singers often had to revise methods of executing the song and craft more

appropriate ways, as Anna related, “it’s easier to go, ‘Okay. I know that I have that

issue,’ and to work through it. It’s almost like a re-knitting of the fabric in your head.”

Part of mastering the voice was recognizing when a technique was no longer appropriate.

Anna realized this and used imagery to re-pattern her skills for the next performance.

Similarly, Louis felt that preparation was vital in mastery and said:

Just the pure advanced repetition of going through the event mentally. I find that

in the moment of performance, if one has not anticipated that moment properly,

it’s very challenging to calm the situation, or to get a mental grasp over what

you’re going through. That work has to be done before you arrive there. At the

moment of performance, it’s almost too late, for me anyway.

Imagery used for mastery assisted these singers in building strong beliefs and self-

efficacy that they were capable and prepared for the task of singing and overcoming any

difficulty that arose in the process.

218
Artistic Reasons

The category of artistic reasons was borrowed from the four Ws framework of

Nordin and Cumming (2005) in dance imagery research and was found to be appropriate

in reporting vocalists’ interview responses. Artistic reasons for using imagery in optimal

performance in this study emerged slightly differently than those of dance. For these

singers, preparing to perform a song or role was considered primarily artistic when their

imagery extended beyond the technical or cognitive and motivational functions. These

resulted in four imagery categories: (a) vocal quality, (b) appearance, (c) character

development, (d) flow, and (e) communication with the audience. Artistic reasons for

singers to use imagery, the categories, details, and frequency of responses are presented

in Table 21.

Vocal quality. Achieving optimum vocal quality was one of the primary artistic

reasons vocalists used imagery. All singers mentioned imagery of some ideal quality of

how they wanted their sound to be heard. Imagery in singers’ vocal quality resulted in

four sections: (a) color, (b) modeling, (c) ideal sound, and (d) sound characteristics.

Eleven singers in this study reported experiencing vocal quality combining the two senses

of seeing visual colors and hearing tones and phrases. These had been divided into two

contexts, visual color and color as timbre or emotion of different vocal sounds. Two

responders spoke primarily of the visual aspect of color as it relates to tones. In striving

to produce the most accurate pitch, Anna reported that for her “different notes have

different colors and different phrases have different colors, reds or greens or blues.”

219
Table 21

Vocal Participants’ Artistic Reasons for Imagery Use Categories

Artistic Reasons Categories Number of Participants

Vocal Quality Color 11

Modeling 11

Ideal Sound 9

Sound Characteristics 6

Appearance Look On Stage 15

Posture 10

Fitness 10

Character Development Modeling 14

Preparation History & Culture 14

Appearance 12

Past Experiences 8

Metaphors 2

Character Development Being in Character 15

Staging Emotion 15

Kinesthetic 8

Sound 6

Zone 4

Communication with Ideas & Emotions 5

Audience Intimate Feelings 3

Flowing Energy 3

Spiritual Interaction 4

220
Eloise described her experience of vocal quality in color imagery when she said,

“you’ve got your chest voice or your low voice is red, and your high voice is yellow, and

they need to mix, so you have all these different beautiful oranges constantly going on.”

The image of yellow or gold in the higher tones was also found in responses from Ivan,

whose quote was included in the previous section on sense imagery. These singers used

visual color imagery to execute the precise vocal quality and tone in the musical context.

Six participants related color in terms of timbre and the emotional aspects of expressing a

song as in Dorine’s experience:

I do use imagery in terms of color, relating color or images to tone. Some songs

require different timbre than other songs, or a different quality to the voice than

other songs, for example with songs that are more melancholy or more sultry or

more passionate or more sad, I’ll envision things like velvet or chocolate or

something rich.

Descriptive terms combining sound and color have often been used in vocal pedagogy to

moderate vocal quality in achieving artistic variety in singing.

Modeling the sound of other more experienced singers was a tool 11 singers said

they used to improve their own sound quality. Several vocalists recalled the sounds of

specific singers that inspired them. For example, Barry said:

[I]f I hear something that totally inspires me to question my technique. . . I

actually try and imagine what their tongue is doing or what their body is doing, or

what the sound that they created and try and recreate that in my own practices.

Anna would imagine replicating the kinesthetic production of an ideal singer’s sound, as

described in the following quote:

221
I can listen to Joan Sutherland and I feel where the vibration is. I’m not even

honestly sure if the vibration really is occurring or we’re just doing imagery for it,

but you have the points on your head where you feel that buzz, and so when I

listen to people, I put that into my body.

Catherine struggled with modeling because she used the mental image of a singer’s voice

that was inappropriate in that it did not match her own instrument. She said, “I have other

people’s voices in my head. I have Jessye Norman’s sound in my head [but] nope, you

have a Barbara Bonney sound, and make sure that you don’t try to sing like Tosca.”

Ophelia recalled, “I’ve run into a lot of problems by trying to copy others.” These singers

may have tried to replicate the vocal quality of expert singers with extensive experience

and characteristics differing from the natural abilities of their own voice. Four singers

mentioned that modeling was something they did more when they were younger, but as

they became more secure and found their own voice, the need for modeling diminished.

Nine of these singers retained an ideal image in their mind of a sound quality they

aimed to recreate in their own voices. Three participants spoke of invoking a wide variety

of human vocal capabilities in their singing, as reflected by Josephine who said, “the

human voice mimics so many different sounds and has such capacity to do so.” Eloise

described an array of sounds that mimicked all the families of the orchestra that she

imagined emanating from various parts of her body. Several singers felt their sound

quality was directly linked to its mental origins. Ophelia endeavored to create “an image

of what you want to come out, how you want it to sound.” Barry’s vocal efforts were

determined or modified more by internal choices than by applying mechanical efforts, as

he aptly illustrated:

222
[You] don’t do a sound, you do the image. . . . You let your mind go to

somewhere, you don’t actually affect the sound directly. There’s something else

in the sound that comes off natural as consequences to that thought.

It is possible that Barry had many of the techniques solidly incorporated in his singing

and now concentrated on executing specific vocal qualities. Singers used their imagery to

affect and change the quality of their vocal sounds in a variety of ways.

Other artistic reasons singers mentioned in their imagery concerned

characteristics of sound and maintaining or changing individual vocal qualities, as

reported in nine responses. Six singers offered illustrations of imagery to achieve greater

depth and richness in their sound. However, Monique felt that a singer’s individual vocal

quality was meant to be consistent. Gerald concurred, “I usually don’t think of the sound

changing from aria to aria. The sound is always the same.” For many singers, vocal

development involved finding your individual sound, which some considered as unique

as your thumbprint.

Notwithstanding, three singers had to cope with changes in their vocal quality as

a result of getting older. Josephine said, “I started out with a very like light, sweet sound.

And it’s grown as I’ve grown older. And it’s become warmer and richer. And I’ve had to

try to figure out how to manage change in my sound.” Not only did the sound quality

change for Nicholas as he matured, but he had to change his vocal quality for certain

songs. He recalled, “I’ve got to rework it just a little bit, so I’m looking for a little more

focus and ring in the upper part of my voice.” Monique shifted her voice quality as

needed for the production. She felt, “it depends on what I’m singing. . . .I have a flexible

voice and I can sing straight or I can sing curly, as I call it, depending on what the

223
conductor wants, I will do it.” There seems to be certain qualities of the voice that are

maintained, others that can be consciously altered, and some that change with age.

Imagery maintained a primary role in the intentions of these singers to manipulate

vocal quality, which may be a consequence of extensive, highly technical experience in

singing, as many of these participants had.

Appearance. All the participants used imagery to change and improve their

appearance, which is hardly surprising given that vocal professionals usually give live

performances on a stage where they are fully visible to the audience. Results from this

category revealed three subcategories of (a) look on stage, (b) posture, and (c) fitness

concerns.

Look on stage comprised how the vocalist imagined the audience viewed him or

her in performance and was cited in all of the responders. This involved the need for a

feedback mechanism, level of imagery detail, and preferred perspective. In order for

singers to know how they appeared on stage, some sort of feedback system was required,

such as a teacher, coach, director, mirror, or video. Anna illustrated this by saying:

[S]ometimes I’ll look in the mirror to see what I’m doing right then, and then to

create what I want it to look like in my head instead. . . . But, if there’s a focus too

much on the external, you will look contrived. . . . or you need to get the feedback

from the director, or you need somebody to be your eyes.

When other people are not available to give feedback, singers had to rely on experience, a

mirror, or video. Ophelia, one of the younger participants, said, “the only way you get

good at this is practice because how you mean to portray yourself may not be how you’re

actually perceived.” In order for some performers to better imagine how the audience

224
viewed them, several participants specifically mentioned using external visual imagery

perspective to gain information on their appearance. Harriet stated, “I can see myself, I

guess from the audience perspective. I’ll imagine certain gestures and things like that.

And I’ll decide what reads better to the audience versus others.” Aligning the ideal

appearance in imagery with the actual look on stage was the goal for singers who wanted

to portray themselves in a certain way.

Singers imagined their posture in how they appeared to hold their body, the

position of their head, shoulders, chest, and arms, as 10 of these singers mentioned. These

participants described being inspired by images of metaphors, stereotypical people, and

elements from special training to achieve their optimal posture. In learning to hold her

body erect but relaxed in singing, Dorine explained, “my teacher used to say, ‘Pretend

you’re on a pole, a posto, the Italians say, that your body is on a pole, it should act

straight.’” Using a different metaphor, Dorine imagined “a string coming out of the top of

my head helping me stand up straight” to imagine her posture more aligned.

Some singers imagined themselves resembling people with excellent posture.

Karen imagined “always looking refined. . . . and it would include head up and confident,

and more in the kind of regal category,” when she performed. Likewise, Monique

envisioned, “’I AM the Queen!’ Now you cannot say that sentence without holding your

head up high, throwing your chest out.” This worked particularly well when she sang

concert and oratorio performances. As a boy, Ivan’s grandmother encouraged him to

stand proudly. She told him, “You have to stand like a bullfighter in the ring,” which was

an enduring image for him even to the present. Even using the image of a stereotypical

person helped these singers stand in the way they felt best for proper vocal production

225
and appearance on stage. Posture was learned in the early stages of life and if developed

incorrectly, singers benefited from retraining to stand properly. One singer found great

assistance in this type of training. Ophelia shared, “I think the most helpful imagery that a

coach gave me recently had to do with alignment of the body. . . in that you need

optimum posture.” Part of achieving excellent posture for these singers was in staying

physically fit.

Fitness for 10 of these singing professionals was an important element in stage

presentation. Monique recognized this and said, “Singing is an extremely athletic pursuit,

because you’re using your stomach, and your back, and your breathing mechanism, and

you can’t have tension in your neck or your jaw.” Several of these responses reflected

their opinion that this was more a physical matter of just exercising than an imagery

issue. Catherine illustrated, “I’d like to lose some girth, but I go exercise. . . . I don’t sit

around and say, ‘I wish I looked better.’ I either make myself look better, or don’t worry

about it.” Others recognized that imagery could be used to shape how they appeared on

stage. Harriet mentally maintained her ideal body image:

If I’m not feeling 100% in shape, or feel like I need to loose five or ten vanity

pounds here or there, I will imagine myself in the physical condition that I want to

be in order to get back in shape next role. . . . that’s my of sort of staying in shape

or preparing that way physically for a role. I’ll use that . . . to manifest how I work

out and what I need to do in order to get to that. I use it as my building block to

get back in shape physically.

226
Harriet used imagery to help her identify what kind of body she wanted personally and

for performances. This imagery also inspired her to achieve it through exercise and

healthy habits.

Singers used appearance imagery to project and maintain an ideal look for stage

presentations. Vocalist’s appearance, attitudes and gestures, posture, fitness, and body

image all comprise the elements of how an audience perceives him or her while

performing. This next section encompasses singers’ use of imagery in developing the

character for performance.

Character development. All of the vocal participants mentioned character

development as a significant component of their imagery to achieve optimal performance.

Character development was often a process that extended from the early stages of

learning the role past the last performance, especially when singers repeated their roles

throughout their career. For this reason, character development was divided between

character preparation and character staging. Character preparation represented gathering

the components for embodying the role or qualities of a song to be performed. Character

staging involved elements of the actual performance of a role.

All singers reported using imagery in various aspects of character preparation.

Singers had to prepare the character or artistic quality of their repertoire whether it was

an entire role in an opera or a single song presentation. Responses for preparing the

character revealed five sub-categories: (a) modeling, (b), appearance, (c) history and

culture, (d) past experiences, and (e) metaphors.

For 14 singers, the use of modeling, or observing exemplary performers who had

successfully performed their roles, facilitated character development and preparation.

227
Modeling use for these participants ranged from catching glimpses “as inspiration”

(Harriet) to pretending to be that model performer entirely (e.g., Anna: “when you’re up

there, you actually picture that you’re that person. . . . you actually embody that person”).

Ophelia also reported, “I’m guilty of watching a movie and liking a character and

identifying with that character and then having it rubbing off on me. Then I’m acting like

that actor or something in certain situations.” She and others cautioned that modeling use

should be limited. Eloise was inspired by another professional’s presentation and “kind of

tried to feel it and internalize it in myself, and then make it my own.” Francine also

illustrated this by saying, “I think of the things that they did successfully and I try and use

them. . . [This] helped me learn and understand stylistically, how to approach things, and

understand what the result is that people are looking for.” Modeling also served singers in

eliciting images and intentions often used by performers of greater experience, as Louis

explained, “I don’t try to mimic that but I certainly respect the artistry and the

performance involvement that they create. I then try to create something that is just as

that significant in my own singing.” Since singing was considered primarily a profession

handed down from generation to generation, modeling continued to be a viable imagery

method to use in grasping characterization in the vocal repertoire as represented in these

singers’ responses.

In the character preparation stage, singers often began by researching the

historical and cultural background of the opera or song. This represented a major part of

creating a foundation for understanding the character for 14 singers. Anna’s account

defined some of the details and reasons she used in imagery of the character, “You do a

lot of history work on where . . . the clothing they wore in the 1800s or the 1600s, and

228
how that affects you as a person. And it all helps create this whole living character.”

Being thorough in her research was key to successful rendering the character according to

Dorine, who added, “I have to do the research before I can create the images to go with

it.” This practice also helped Eloise “to imagine myself there, and in all kinds of different

roles. . . . what motivated them, and to feel the culture.” Details of the history and culture

helped the singer to recreate the character in relation to their environment.

Creating the mental image of the character’s appearance was a natural element

and benefit of researching the history and culture. For 12 singers, differences regarding

stage presentation also arose in relation to the venue in which the singer was performing

(e. g., operatic, concert, or audition). These vocalists imagined their characters’ physical

appearances with varying degrees of detail and in various settings. After doing

preliminary research, Catherine would, in her words:

[I]magine what each character looks like as me, what color hair I have, how long

my hair is, what type of makeup I wear, how much makeup I wear, what kind of

clothes I have, how I walk, how I stand. . . .I imagine myself in the costume.

When Karen performed on the concert stage she had already secured how she wanted to

execute this. She described this process:

I have to imagine first what that character would look like. . . . I sort of decide

who I want to be at that time, how I want to look to the audience. Do I want to be

somebody very regal, or what, and a lot of it is the music that I’m doing.

Karen took time to carefully craft the image and appearance appropriate to each

character.

229
Since mezzos often perform as both male and female, Francine had to use

imagery to quickly change between the genders during auditions, as she described:

If I’m wearing a dress and I’m singing a boy, I have to make a mental image of

that fact that, okay, now I’m a boy. And how does that affect the way that I stand,

in the way that my arms express themselves. . . . imagining that I’m wearing a

suit, or very specifically, imagining that I am in armor [to sing Caesar] . . .

Whereas, Dorabella, if I’m in a dress, that pretty much helps me do Dorabella.

In embodying the character of the song, singers used imagery to recreate the look and

external feeling to enhance the presentation. Appearance imagery also depended on the

type of performance and whether it was operatic, concert, or an audition.

Eight singers reported using their own past experiences to elicit emotions and

feelings in preparing their roles and songs. Gerald used his past experiences to connect

with what “the character has experienced or something that would elicit the response that

I am trying to get, and reliving that, in other words, I’m trying to capture that feeling.”

Dorine concurred, adding that using her own experiences often provided “a much more

believable performance for the audience.” When past experiences were not sufficient,

singers used those of other people they knew or had seen somewhere. The most important

element of employing images of these experiences was the connection with its

accompanying emotion and appropriateness to recreating that character.

Only two participants mentioned using metaphors in creating their character.

These images were specific to some of the roles these two singers had performed in the

past. In performing the role of Mimi in La Boheme, Josephine used “imagery of a

wounded animal, a wounded bird, or . . . someone who was wounded, and physically

230
crippled and disabled” to help her more completely portray this character. Harriet

described, “Gilda in Rigoletto . . . as a trapped bird that wants to fly. . . . But then she’s in

a cage and her house is the cage that her father keeps her in.” Both of these examples

represent simple acting metaphors to help the singer feel the conflicts in the characters.

Ideally, when all the necessary elements of character preparation were

incorporated, the performer was ready for staging rehearsals. Character development in

staging involves aspects of using imagery to bring the character to life while performing

before an audience. The five sub-categories emerging from the interviews were: (a) being

in character, (b) emotion, (c) kinesthetic, (d) sound, and (e) being in the zone.

All singers mentioned that they imagined being in character and performing in

ways that encompassed all dramatic aspects of human behavior. In trying to make the

character more real to him, Nicholas used imagery in, as he said:

[S]etting myself within the text of the plot of the opera and letting the motivation

of the character, and getting ideas that the character would be having. . . .

picturing myself in the scene, not necessarily in the operatic stage, but trying to

imagine if it were real life, how the character would be seeing, and feeling.

Some singers went to great extents to create all the details of the character in their

imagination, as Anna illustrated:

You have to go through and do your whole history of that character. So you’ve

imagined . . . everything that led up to who they are today. You already have that

implanted in your head, and you walk out with all the baggage loaded up on your

shoulders, so that you have, it’s almost like arsenal, ready to use at any minute,

231
based on the kinds of experiences that you’re gonna walk through with this

character now from that point on.

Monique’s characterizations centered more on artistic and scenic images:

I think very deeply about each piece I sing. . . . it’s my acting, I try to put myself

in the scene of the picture that that song is painting. . . . so [I become] that person,

involved in those emotions, the lost love, the whatever. I am that person.

For these singers, imagining being in character included choosing methods that best

suited their personal preferences.

All these vocalists mentioned the importance of recreating the emotion to bring

the character of a song or role to life, particularly in performance. “You always have to

go back to the foundation, which is the emotional, human experience” (Anna). This was

also reflected in a quote by Barry, who said, “your goal as an artist is to live the moment

and . . . the emotion of your character.” Dorine considered emotion to be critical to

performance because “You can’t convey that to an audience unless I think you’ve felt

that emotion.” The inner motivation of a character was depicted in how the performer

recreated the emotions. According to Nicholas, emotions changed throughout the aria or

the entire opera and this helped him more effectively deliver the proper intent to the

audience. He stated, “I think of the emotional changes as I’m maneuvering around the

emotional pathway of the character. The bigger and stronger I can hit each emotional

change the more effective I think it’s going to be.” Emotional imagery in recreating a role

was considered critical to performance and communicating with the audience.

Furthermore, all the participants recognized the natural connection between emotion and

singing, not just in character development in roles for opera. As Josephine put it, “It’s

232
hard to separate emotion and singing because . . . singing is emotion . . . . set to words

and music. . . . it’s just organized emotion.” These singers felt emotional imagery was

inseparable from proper delivery of every song.

Although emotion certainly was considered vital in creating the proper mood,

nine singers had specific words of caution in applying excessive feeling in performance.

Many singers recalled specific experiences that taught them to be careful not to pour too

much passion into their performances. In a recent audition that he described, Barry said

he “allowed it to get too emotional, i.e., [sic] cross the line. You can cross the line where,

especially vocal technique-wise, you get verklempt. If you get verklempt as a singer,

you’re dead in the water.” Francine had to adjust her emotions so she could technically

execute certain songs. She stated, “if your emotions get in the way, you won’t be

successful at it. So I had to learn how to adapt it and make it still functional but not allow

it to overwhelm me.” In illustrating the importance of balancing emotions in singing

performance, Francine said:

I’ll often compare myself with a figure skater. A figure skater has to constantly be

reminding themselves [sic], technically, of what their body needs to be doing in

order to accomplish that triple axel, because if they don’t do that, they will fall

and kill themselves.

Monique recognized the dangers of being overcome with emotion in singing and said:

[Y]ou mustn’t be the emotion, because it will tear your voice apart, but you must

be a spectator of the emotion. Stand aside because if someone is crying out in

agony, if you did that you would loose your voice. . . . Where you cry is where

you sing from.

233
The singers with more experience were able to find the proper balance between

expressing the emotion of the song and maintain technical excellence.

Along with emotion, eight singers connected this to a bodily feeling in order to

ground the experience physically in characterizing a song or role. Many of these

participants felt the need to feel qualities of the character in their body. In discovering the

various aspects of her character, Karen incorporated the kinesthetic feeling when she

said:

I guess it’s visual first, then how that visual fits into the auditory part of it, and

then just movements, of course, any kind of movements. Would they be slow,

would they be fast. If you’re heavy you’re going to be probably slower. I just

have to imagine the body, I guess, and the person, too.

Others mentioned the importance of physically staging the character first in order to more

completely imagine the characters’ movements. Nicholas shared a recent experience of

this when he was performing. He described staging a specific scene:

[In] A Barber of Seville . . . there’s a complicated shaving scene that happens very

quick and . . . I practiced imagery with it, but if I hadn’t done [that] probably the

physical timing of it then actually the physical movements were more important

than the imagery of that particular thing.

Barry also used movements and behaviors of a specific man he had seen on the street. He

said, “I can use that to create my character, and use the body language, the facial

expressions, or the way somebody says something” in creating the details of acting out

the role. Using imagery to understanding the character kinesthetically assisted these

singers to more successfully embody their songs and roles.

234
Singers had the opportunity to choose from a variety of sound qualities to best

represent the individual aria and character. In creating the appropriate sound for a rough

character she portrayed, Karen said, “Her voice would have to project her body. The

body was easy enough to pad, but her voice would have to project that body and the

crudeness, and so forth. I had to just really feel the heaviness.” This practice did not

always display the best qualities of her singing voice but was necessary to better depict

that particular character, as Karen further explained:

[In] these wonderful roles in opera, you’re being judged on just how wonderful

you are in singing. But really, in many cases, you should be in kind of a character

on that. It wouldn’t necessarily be the most refined singing that you would do if

the character was [sic] not that type.

Anna concurred, by saying, “you want a character to come through.” Sound imagery was

used to portray the specific qualities required in each character.

Although the experience of being in the zone or the feeling of flow in

performance only appeared in a four singers’ responses even though it was not included

as a separate question in the interview guide. Flow is worthy of including here since it is

part of what would be considered an optimal performing experience. For these singers,

being in the zone did not emerge as a reason or separate purpose as it had for athletes

(Munroe et al., 2000) but as an experience that culminated after all separate components

of singing the character were incorporated. These were specific times when a singer

performed a song, all the effort faded away and the present experience became all

encompassing, involving different kinds of imagery. Louis experienced this early in his

career. He recounted:

235
I was singing a certain passage that I’d been struggling with maybe in lessons or

weeks prior to and it was really working. And I sang several minutes in sort of a

groove where I was just singing pretty effortlessly and it was working. She [his

teacher] said that she could equate that maybe to a basketball player in the middle

of the game that just got into the zone. And the basket looks wide. She said

sometimes in your singing, the basket will look wide and you know that it’s

working. You can just sort of accomplish things that you might not at other times

be able to do.

This example showed that experiencing flow was possible in a small span of time after

working on a specific skill or technique in a song.

Other singers recounted feeling flow during performances. Nicholas described

this and said, “when I’ve really nailed something, in my mind it will make me laugh, it

might make me cry a little bit, but I may feel in my whole body.” While acting out her

character, Ophelia’s experience of being in the zone was also quite poignant, when she

said, “there’s a magical moment on stage where there’s no audience, there’s no

performer, there’s just the music. And in order to get there I really think you have to

move beyond this whole thing [religious conviction].” Being in the zone to Ophelia was

an experience that transcended any religious beliefs and took place when she was clearly

singing as that character. Harriet also felt this flow feeling when she fully immersed

herself in the character. She recalled, “when I start a performance, my self checks out,

just, I get into the zone, and I’m just that character. . . . cause it’s no longer about me, and

I’m in my character.” It could have been that all the imagery used to secure techniques,

skill, emotion, and the kinesthetic aspects of portraying the character help made these

236
elements so automatic that the singer could just enjoy being that character. This may be

part of what constituted the experience of singers being in the zone or in flow.

Communication with the audience. All but one of the 15 participants shared

thoughts of interacting with the audience in their imagery. This emerged as a separate

category under artistic reasons. Vocalists used imagery to communicate specific ideas to

the audience in four specific areas, which consisted of: (a) communicating ideas and

emotions, (b) sharing intimate feelings, (c) flowing energy, and (d) spiritual interaction.

Some images were used in helping the audience to understand the meaning of

what was being performed. The effort to share specific elements of the composition with

the audience was important to Harriet, who said, “I will take specific moments and

especially with text or something that needs to be communicated. . . . I intend for certain

things to be conveyed to the audience.” Other singers felt it was important for the

audience to understand and appreciate their genuine portrayal of the character. Louis

explained, “I’m looking to communicate authenticity. I’m looking for them to experience

my sincere thought and connection to the music in a way that expresses reality rather than

something that seems contrived or artificial.” Three singers mentioned communicating

emotions and feelings to the audience. Dorine felt that, as a singer, one of her best assets

was her passion. Barry also felt that a major element in singing was conveying emotion

through his singing. He offered, “your goal as an artist is to live the moment and . . . the

emotion of your character.” Sharing emotion with an audience was accomplished using

imagery in a number of ways. Anna illustrated her particular method:

[W]e’re instilling emotion and feeling into other people, and in doing that, they

are getting visual stimulation too, they’re getting imagery. We’re implanting

237
imagery into them by the way that we’re singing. So we need to start with our

own.

Theses singers communicated their ideas and emotional feelings with the audience

through the vehicles of their songs.

For some singers, the idea of touching the audience went beyond the physical

tactile sense and represented the image of connecting with the audience. It is included

here because Josephine and Ophelia directly answered the 33rd question in the interview

guide, “To what extent do you use imagery relating to touch?” using this meaning. While

performing, Josephine intended to mentally contact all the members of the audience. She

said she would “use the image of while you’re standing on stage, to touch or reach the

person in the last row.” She felt she could touch the audience with her vocal sound or an

inner feeling or thought. For Ophelia, touching the audience concerned shaping the voice

in a way that deeply impacted the members of the audience. She illustrated:

[I]t’s about the intimacy and using your voice to touch people both in a feeling,

but also physically getting the voice to reach them in a way. . . . You can’t touch

someone just by singing loud all the time. You have to shape things and in order

to do that, how you touch people through the shaping of the phrase.

Ophelia used a combination of sensory imageries to achieve this intimate and multi-

faceted method of communicating with her audience.

Three participants in this study were sensitive to their personal interaction with

the audience. These singers used imagery to share deep feelings of intimacy with the

audience. Dorine felt keenly aware of her vulnerability in performing as a singer when

she stated:

238
[T]here’s nothing between the audience and the singer. With instrumentalists, it’s

like they’re able to be hiding. They can hide behind an instrument. But when I’m

on stage, there is nothing. I’m completely exposed. My heart is completely

exposed to that audience. And I’m baring my soul to them. My chest is exposed,

because, in actuality, there’s nothing between me and them, and so it’s as if my

heart’s exposed. They’re seeing my heart. So I need to give to them and to let

them feel what I feel, too.

She used her sense of feeling exposed as an opportunity to share her innermost feelings.

Ivan and Josephine had images of good feelings going out into the audience that

helped them achieve greater connection with their listeners. Ivan’s image was intimacy

that involved healing energy he shared with his audience. He described:

[It’s] a sense of, this gets into a little bit of metaphysical thing, but if sound, in

some ways [had] an ability to caress the listener in a way, energetically. And so I

would think it is much like healing, that way. . . just peace, calm, invitation to

participate. Not going at them, but inviting participation. . . . It’s an invitation for

them to come sharing in the embrace. . . . It’s not going out to embrace them but

rather just opening arms to let them meet somewhere in the middle.

This endearing image helped Ivan give him the feeling of drawing his audience more

deeply into his performance. Josephine’s imagery also helped her share good feelings and

a sense of being conveyed to another place together with the audience. She related:

I try to imagine that what I’m doing is giving them so much joy and happiness . . .

I do imagine just like a blanket, I guess, a blanket of joy or bliss reaching out over

the audience while I’m singing. It’s sort of something, I guess, I feel transported

239
to a place, to a different place in between. I’m saying I feel transported that’s

between me and the audience. And I just don’t feel on the stage, I feel somewhere

between me and them.

Dorine, Ivan, and Josephine imagined offering a deep connection and sense of intimacy

to the audience.

Three singers imagined a constant flow of positive energy between themselves

and their audience. During her performances, Dorine imagined the following:

[T]aking all of the good energy that these people are sending me out of the ground

. . . through my feet, and then I’m going to pull it up through my body and give it

back to them. So, it’s just a circle of goodness.

Imagining that the audience gave back energy was important to these singers. Anna also

envisioned being involved in this constant cycle of flowing energy in stating:

[The] infinity symbol is such a precise visualization for me for what happens

between you and the audience . . . because you have to have two whole solid

halves interacting with each other. You can’t have a wimpy side. It just won’t

flow back. . . . I believe that if I’m more open to the audience and to being with

them and the love that goes back and forth. . . . it gets me out of it being about me.

Eloise also was inspired by her metaphorical imagery of continuous cyclical activity with

the audience:

I imagine how ocean waves actually roll under against the ocean floor, and that’s

why then the water peaks on top. . . . it’s the circular thing that keeps, it’s just

rolling. That’s how I see what’s going on between me and the audience. It’s this

rolling wave of energy and mutual love, and we all want each other to have a

240
good time. They’re supporting me, and I’m giving them something in return, or

visa-versa. It’s like you can’t even tell where it starts and ends. That’s what I love.

I just like that, unspoken conversation. And that’s what makes me want to

perform all the time.

During performance, these singers used imagery with this intention of being a part of

flowing goodness with the audience, which inspired them to perform better and more

meaningfully.

Four participants illustrated their communication to the audience as spiritual

sharing. In discussing her motivation for singing, Monique said she imagined herself as:

[A] channel and the energy comes through me and out to the audience. . . . I want

to be a blessing. . . . And that’s your purpose as a singer, too, that that audience

leaves feeling better than when they came in.

Ivan also felt that the spiritual aspect was a vital ingredient in singing. He said, “what we

ultimately get to share is love, and we must be willing to open ourselves to sharing that

love, and taking part in love. And the art, as a whole, is an ultimate dimension of

spirituality.” Along with this idea, Eloise imagined:

picturing God filling up the space. . . between the audience and the stage and all

the space left in the air to the ceiling . . . with love, with a capital L, like an active,

conscious, mindful love, that’s alive, and embracing all of us.

Josephine felt a deep spiritual sense of responsibility to her audience:

I really do feel like I’m pastoring [sic] to people when I’m singing. I really do feel

a spiritual connection. I feel like a lot of times I feel like a real spiritual leader

while I’m singing to people, that I really am feeding their souls with my singing.

241
And that’s actually one of the biggest parts of what’s gratifying what singing I do

right now. . . .And I get that back from them and I feel like I’m giving it to them

as well.

To these performers, spiritual interaction as well as responding to their need to provide

something very precious was intrinsic to their singing and communication with their

audience.

For these singers, interacting with the audience involved the intrinsic reasons why

they became performers, the elements they actually imagined communicating to and with

the audience, feeling a sense of intimacy with each audience member, imagining a flow

of energy with them, and spiritual components. This emerged as a significant and

separate theme of why singers use imagery in their singing.

Healing Reasons

Healing reasons for using imagery were cited in 13 singers’ interviews. These

results included the two main issues of overall health and healing oneself, and more

specifically, vocal health. Table 22 shows singer’s healing reasons for imagery use

including categories, and number of participants.

Overall health. Maintaining optimal health was vital to a singer’s performance,

since the body was the exclusive instrument used in vocal singing. Thirteen participants

used imagery in relation to their health and self-healing. This category included: (a)

simple mental processes in staying positive, (b) body checking systems, and (c) using

alternative healing methods to achieve holistic balance. Francine said, “sometimes when

you’re fatigued or when it’s just not happening, you need to then rely on imagery.”

242
Table 22

Vocal Participants’ Healing Reasons for Imagery Use Categories

Healing Reasons Categories Number of Participants

Overall Health Healing oneself 13

Body Checking 10

Alternative Methods 2

Vocal Health Chronic Illnesses 3

Silent Practice 4

Gerald said:

I don’t wait to get sick to heal myself, feeling what my voice feels like when I am

healthy, before I even get to that place, telling myself that I’m healthy, so that my

body functions at its optimal level.

This also exemplified Gerald’s use of imagery in self-talk and the memory of previous

fitness to maintain good health for singing.

There were ten participants who mentioned the use of kinesthetic imagery to

check physical conditions and alignment in the body for ideal singing and performing.

Barry employed a method of checking his body for functioning excellence in singing, as

he illustrated in the following quote:

[B]y the time you get to the performance level, you go through and identify your

sticky parts, the parts of you that are locking up, or physically not operating at a

243
peak level. So in performance you do have to go back to some of the relaxation

techniques.

Singers spent time and effort to make sure their bodies and its various parts were healthy

and active in facilitating performance excellence. Ophelia spoke about training in this

specific area, and said, “I actually read a book recently and that imagery is focused on, in

that you need your posture optimum. . . . It’s called the Egoscue Method.” This was part

of a training program her vocal coach had provided in using imagery for good posture.

Similar to relaxation and posture techniques were two alternative healing

modalities that appeared in the interviews. Catherine and Ivan mentioned using Reiki, or

energy work, and three other singers used chakrah balancing techniques, or East Indian

healing, to stay healthy, as Ivan illustrated:

I have used a lot of that when I’ve been sick. I connect to Mother Gaia, clear out

the chakrahs, directly to the core of the earth. . . . First it’s always the earth for

me, and then out, reaching to the center of the universe. Always two points

though. Always connecting to the center of something, the center of earth as a

grounding cord, and then to the center of what I would image the center of the

universe looks like.

These meditation techniques were completely immersed in imagery use and these

performers seemed to find value in the results.

Vocal health. Seven singers included responses specific to vocal health in their

imagery. Three singers used imagery to contend with singing during bouts with illnesses.

Four vocalists reported using imagery to practice while their voices rested due to fatigue

244
and sickness. Karen and Ivan had to contend with periodic symptoms of colds or allergies

and found imagery useful in identifying and implementing ease in breathing and singing.

Several singers also used imagery as an alternative to singing when their voices

could not physically sing. Being able to use imagery to practice beyond what the voice

could physically sing was a considered an added asset. For example, Louis used imagery

in this way and said, “if I am not well or the voice is not healthy, then I’ll use imagery as

an alternative to real singing, because the instrument might not be functioning properly so

I can’t physically sing.” Anna also used supplement mental practice and expressed:

And the great thing about it is you could do it without ruining your voice, because

you can’t sing all day. You just can’t. So you have to be able to keep your mouth

shut, and do this work. It’s imperative that you do it, or you’ll wreck your voice.

These vocalists considered silent singing a great asset to continuing practice when it was

not advisable to vocalize.

Fatigue, or lack of rest, mentioned by several participants, was particularly

damaging to vocal quality. After traveling extensively to perform, singers used imagery

to increase feelings of health, as Nicholas illustrated:

[G]etting enough rest. That’s usually the biggest determination of whether I feel

that my voice is healthy or not. . . . I imagined myself what Sherrill Milnes, a

retired baritone [would do] . . . I’m sure he was in situations where he was still

tired and had to perform. . . . to his optimal best, in trying to keep my energy

about me and do the best I could do. . . . Sometimes you’ve just got to make the

most out of what you have and make it through, not push vocally, but endurance-

wise it’s more a matter of keeping yourself focused.

245
Nicholas’ use of modeling imagery inspired him to continue with his performing oversees

engagement. Several other singers cautioned against pushing the voice during critical

times of stress and illness. Preventing and treating sickness and fatigue could be

facilitated by specific applications of imagery as these vocalists attested.

Summary

This chapter included characteristics of the participants, interview protocol issues,

description, categorization, and analyses of the data taken from the 15 vocalists’

interviews. In answering the four Ws questions of where, when, what, and why, a full

description of singers’ use of imagery emerged. These vocal professionals were found to

use imagery around the performance venues, at home, and other places. Times included

primarily during practice, prior to performance, in bed at night, and commuting and

driving. Content of vocalists’ imagery included types and characteristics. Types of

imagery singers used were senses and abilities. Imagery characteristics included vocal

execution, metaphorical, body-related, musical sound, and character images. Reasons

why, or purposes, singers used imagery included cognitive, motivational, artistic, and

healing reasons. Audience interactions emerged as an additional artistic reason for using

imagery in these performers. Other findings revealed specific uses exclusive to solo

singing professionals.

Emergent themes of imagery uses were often consistent with those found in sport

and dance research. Many of the themes reflected more dance imagery uses than those of

sport. Several themes emerged as exclusive to solo singing professionals. In Chapter 5,

these commonalities and differences warrant further discussion in relation to the findings

from sport and dance research and related literature. A singers’ framework for the four

246
Ws of imagery is presented, as well as limitations, implications, and suggestions for

further research.

247
Chapter 5

Summary of Findings, Implications, and Conclusions

This chapter provides a summary of the results of the study, interpretation of the

findings as related to the research questions, and compares the results to the four Ws

framework of imagery use in sport (Munroe et al., 2000), dance (Nordin & Cumming,

2005), and related research. Based on the analyses of the data, a new four Ws framework

of imagery use for singers is proposed. Limitations are reviewed and implications of the

findings examined. Suggestions for further research are also offered.

Summary and Interpretation of Significant Findings

In this study, the data of where and when professional solo singers used imagery,

what they imagined, and why they used imagery were gathered, analyzed, and reported.

In analyzing the results of the study, many similarities and contrasts with sport, dance and

related music imagery research were identified, and new categories and findings

emerged. Subsequently a conceptual framework was created. A discussion of each of

these questions is presented individually and the resulting singers’ imagery framework is

offered.

Where Vocalists Used Imagery

Findings of the results regarding where vocalists’ imagery was carried out,

included practice and stage areas, at home, outside, and anywhere, which replicated

findings in dance (Nordin & Cumming, 2005) and musicians (Carter 1993; Trusheim).

All 15 vocal professionals in this study reported using it in practice and even more in
248
performance venues. This supported reports of athletes using imagery more in

competition for performance enhancement than learning situations in practice (Barr &

Hall, 1992; Hall et al., 1990; Munroe et al., 2000). Outside of their formal training and

stage settings, reportedly engaged in imagery at home and primarily in bed, commuting,

in places where they were alone, exercising, or anywhere. These findings were also

reported studies including dance (Nordin & Cumming, 2005), music (Bellon, 2006;

Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987), and sports (Salmon et al., 1994; Munroe et al., 2000).

When Vocalists Used Imagery

Regarding times, or when, singers reportedly engaged in imagery during practice

and even more prior to performance, which was similar to the findings in athletics (Barr

& Hall, 1992; Rodgers et al. 1991; Munroe et al., 2000) and dance (Nordin & Cumming,

2005). However, the singers’ responses revealed a considerable difference between

practice and rehearsal for musical performers with regard to times of imagery use. Other

performance studies mentioned this distinction (Carter, 1993; Nordin & Cumming, 2000;

Trusheim, 1987), particularly the importance of auditions and rehearsals on future

careers. In this investigation, as well as Carters’ (1993), vocal practice was identified as a

private time of study and singing in a studio or private space, whereas a rehearsal was a

formal session to prepare an entire ensemble for an upcoming performance for which

singers were expected to be prepared. While all vocalists regularly engaged in imagery

during practice, fewer participants reported imagining during rehearsals. This distinction

emerged as a new consideration with regard to both where and when singers used

imagery and should be considered in future studies with regard to performance

preparation.

249
Some confusion regarding breaks and holidays arose during the interviews as a

result of the specific word “holiday” in the singers’ interview guide, which required

immediate clarification. Singers in this study performed more during the holidays (i.e.,

Christmas and New Years) than at any other time of the year. The word “break” was

substituted, meaning an extended period of time (such as days or weeks) away from

work. Participants reported using little if any imagery during this time. Conversely,

Munroe and others (2000) reported break results only with regard to rest periods

throughout the day rather than extended time off work.

Other times when vocalists used imagery were during quiet times when they

could think or when there were few distractions, breaks throughout the day, commuting

and driving, any or all the time, and especially in bed at night just prior to sleeping. This

was similar to what was found in many studies in sport (Munroe et al., 2000; Salmon et

al., 1994) and artistic performance (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Nordin & Cumming,

2005; Trusheim, 1987).

What Imagery Types Vocalists Used

The results of what singers imagined were divided between imagery types, or

content of the image, and imagery characteristics in how they produced the images.

Generally, singers’ use of imagery seemed to reflect more of the elements reported in

dance (Nordin & Cumming, 2005) and somewhat less of those in sport (Munroe et al.,

2000). The six imagery types were categorized as: (a) execution, (b) metaphorical, (c)

context, (d) body-related, (e) musical sound, and (f) character/role, as adapted from the

dance imagery framework of Nordin and Cumming (2005). These represented the

250
significant areas of singer’s imagery content, while the category of musical sounds

emerged from the responses.

Execution images. Execution images involved the technical aspects of singing,

including strategies and planning for practicing and performing, as well as goal

identification and attainment. Skill learning encompassed vocal production, breath

support, text, and diction. Singers extensively engaged in all kinds of images, such as

raising the uvula with a yawn, lifting the palate, and maintaining proper positioning of the

mouth and throat for proper singing. They described many images of expanding the ribs,

back, and lowering the diaphragm for breath control and support. These participants also

imagined text and diction execution as well as phrasing words of the song. These images

were consistent with those of other singers (Bellon, 2006; Gregg, 1998), particularly in

building vocal technique in elite professionals (Carter, 1993). These findings also support

the idea of Vennard (1968) and Arieti (1976) that musical sound and words fused in song

through imagery. The intermingling with skill learning of language and motor behavior

provided some support for Annett’s (1986, 1994) dual coding in motor applications,

action-language-imagination (ALI) imagery model. Furthermore, replicating the exact

motor behavior in imagery reflected the preparation and execution of that task could

support some of the findings of functional equivalence (Decety, 1996b; Halpern &

Zatorre, 1999; Holmes & Collins, 2002; Jeannerod, 1994; Meister et al. 2004).

Participants used sequence and strategy images in songs, entire concerts, recitals,

and operatic roles. Images of strategies and sequences were connected with processes of

learning and executing musical requirements of performance, which were also images of

other vocalists (Carter, 1993). The six participants who had to plan their own recitals

251
used sequence and strategy imagery to develop a cohesive program, which was similar to

findings in sports (Fenker & Lambiotte, 1987; Rushall, 1988; White & Hardy, 1998).

Furthermore, three singers created stories to connect songs they sang in their own

concerts. This use of stories and plans in imagery also appeared in several of the

musicians in the Bellon (2006), Carter (1993) and Trusheim (1987) studies. These

vocalists imagined strategies and plans in organizing their practice and pre-performance

routines, specific performance sequences, methods of handling difficult passages, and

ways of coping with distractions which reflected the results of musicians’ use of imagery

in other studies (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993, Trusheim, 1987). Singers also used sequence

imagery to facilitate memory of songs and phrases, which seems to support Sackett’s

(1934, 1935) “mental blueprints” which were complete concepts stored in the brain.

All singers used goal images, which primarily comprised outcome and process

with less performance types as described in Weinberg and Gould (1995). These results

were similar to other music performers’ imagery (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim,

1987). Participants imagined outcome goals of performing successfully in auditions and

performances, and single songs, arias, and entire roles in opera. This supported the study

by Woolfolk, Murphy, Gottesfeld, and Aitken (1985) who found that athletes who

imagined their desired outcome subsequently influenced that same activity. Several

participants described their discovery that ambiguous daydreams yielded unintended

results while consciously directed and detailed goal imagery more consistently affected

specifically desired outcomes. This issue was addressed in Ungerleider and Golding

(1991) in the dreams and goals of Olympic athletes.

252
The vocalists also imagined various process goals such as accurately executing

sounds, phrases, proper breath support, and other skills, which were also found in

Carter’s (1993) study. It seemed that process goals were used when singers needed to

incorporate various skills into becoming automatic or habitual, as examined in Fields

(1972). The fact that singers discussed how goals evolved over the years further supports

how images change as well. Jackson and Csikszentmihalyi (1999) recommended

employing goal images of whole and parts for achieving performance excellence. While

athletes (Munroe et al., 2000) and dancers (Cumming & Nordin, 2005) were found to use

all these goal images, they used more performance goals than singers in this study.

Metaphorical images. All the participants shared descriptions of metaphorical

images in relation to vocal production, the findings of which were similar to those used

by singers in studies by Bellon (2006) and Carter (1993) and to a lesser extent in brass

instrumentalists (Trusheim, 1987). Just as some athletes used metaphors to help enhance

performance in different sports (Ruiz & Hanin, 2004), singers and dancers used

metaphors to learn and secure vocal technique and enhance performance, particularly in

creating characters and roles (Carter, 1993; Hanrahan & Vergeer, 2000; Nordin &

Cumming, 2005). Specifically, singers reported imagery of objects not present and

imaginary actions, which was similar to dancers’ metaphorical imagery (Hanrahan &

Vergeer, 2000; Nordin & Cumming, 2005; Vergeer & Hanrahan, 1998). Metaphoric

images of actions (e.g., bellows opening and closing; ball balancing on a fountain;

stretching a rubber-band) used to facilitate vocal production in this study were similar to

those in the literature (Fields, 1947; Freed, 2000; Lehman, 1945/1985; Ware, 1992). Just

253
as in Carter’s (1993) study, participants felt that using metaphorical images diminished

the need for elaborate explanations of vocal mechanics.

Singers in this study were divided as to their preference of metaphoric or

technical and anatomic images regarding vocal production. Some singers felt that

metaphorical images helped in achieving comfort and ease in sound production without

requiring extensive technical understanding. Other singers employed more metaphoric

imagery when they were younger and when they gained a better understanding of the

vocal mechanism, they began to also incorporate anatomical imagery. As singers

continued studying the voice over the years, they became more familiar with the

anatomical workings and were able to accurately visualize them, as advocated by various

voice pedagogues (Dayme, 1982, 2005; Reid, 1983; Vennard, 1968). Similarly, singers in

Cleveland’s (1989a) study preferred anatomical to metaphoric images, as a result of

viewing their own voices through laryngeal image biofeedback (LIB) medical imaging

system. Two sopranos and all the men in the study were familiar but indifferent to

metaphors and preferred a more scientific technical approach to imagery in vocal

production. Singers in the study varied in their choices of whether to use metaphorical or

anatomical imagery and their preferences often changed over time, usually in favor of

anatomical imagery. Whether images were metaphorical, technical, or anatomical, they

had to serve the intended function as has been suggested in previous studies (Carter,

1993: Freed, 2000; Moorcroft, 2002; Ware, 1998).

Several participants associated colors with sounds, particularly in vocal

production whereby different colors were attached to different sounds (Lehmann,

1945/1985). While some singers used various colors to represent different timbres of

254
vocal qualities, several vocalists described very specific applications of visual color with

specific sounds. A few singers were very explicit in illustrating the connection of visual

color with tones and phrases, which was a type of multi-sensory imagery known as

chromesthesia (Polzella & Kuna, 1981). Trusheim (1987) also reported brass

professionals who saw different colors associated with the ideal sounds they aimed to

produce on their instruments. Conversely, dancers’ color images were more often

connected with the movement in their performances rather than musical sound (Nordin &

Cumming, 2005). More research could be conducted in this area.

Context images. Singers’ use of context images included scenarios of the

environment or venue, the audience, and imaginary people, including auditioning

panelists and friends for whom they were performing. All participants imagined

performing on stages, for audiences, or specific people, and included details of the venue,

which were reflected other musician’s imagery (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim,

1987). This finding was similar to sport and dance in that the singers were imagining the

venues in which they generally or specifically performed (Munroe et al., 2000; Nordin &

Cumming, 2005). These context images facilitated performance best when they were as

detailed as the actual experience employing multisensory aspects, providing further

support for the functional equivalence theory (Holmes & Collins, 2002). One singer aptly

described this in her account of imagining a previously unfamiliar theatre in which she

had hoped to sing. This imagery method of the Soviet Union’s elite athletic trainers using

photographs of the Montreal Olympic facility to familiarize athletes with this new venue

had been described in Vealey (1986) and Raiport (1988).

255
Furthermore, singers in this and Carter’s (1993) study imagined the details of

mentally created scenarios they devised for the settings of recital songs and arias.

Sometimes singers argued that the images needed to support certain songs were

imbedded in the text or the music, as in Lehmann’s (1945/1985) argument for the

tradition of creating the “right” image for performing a specific song. Conversely, singers

said that the content of the image was immaterial as long as it produced the right effect in

delivering the song, which reflected the views of the singers in Carter’s (1993) study who

supported the use of private images.

Body-related images. Vocalists’ images relating to the body concerned regulating

arousal and kinesthetic feelings particular to performance. Thirteen singers mentioned

arousal images directly dealing with the specific parts of their body most affected by

nerves. This was often related to relaxing the body, focusing attention in areas of tension,

and deepening the breathing, as mentioned by singers in other studies (Bellon, 2005;

Carter, 1993).

Kinesthetic images also included physical posture, alignment, appearance, and

health, which seemed to reflect vocalists’ recognition that their bodies were a significant

concern in singing optimally. This was reported in the imagery of elite singers (Carter,

1993) and dancers (Nordin & Cumming, 2005), and vocal texts (Averino, 1989; Ware

1998). Participants used the mirror and other methods for direct feedback on the look of

their performances so that their ideal appearances, body positions, and movement could

be grounded in their kinesthetic imagery, as found in Carter’s singers (1993). Since

singers performed with their full body visible to the audience, it was understandable that

appearance images were reported in 11 singers’ responses.

256
Singers’ body-related images also encompassed the physically imagined feelings

of how they produced, executed, and memorized vocal sound and in characterizing the

song or role, which was consistent in singers’ interviews in other studies (Carter, 1993;

Hines, 1982; Moyer, 1992). This lends support for Ahsen’s (1984) triple code theory,

image, somatic response, and meaning (ISM) all must be rooted in the body. Participants

in this study were keenly aware of their physical bodies in relation to their vocal

mechanism and its relationship to physical posture and alignment for singing optimally.

Singers in Carter’s (1993) study also reported employing these body-related images.

These findings also support Leyerle’s (1986) five theoretical categories of organic

imagery in singing, which consisted of posture, respiration, phonation, resonation, and

use in vocal or psychological problem areas. The ideal sounds singers intended to make

were directly connected with the physical positions and kinesthetic feelings used to

secure those tones, especially since singers argued that they could not trust their ears in

achieving proper sound. This provides some support for the idea of securing sound in

anatomical feeling rather than hearing it through the ears (Carter, 1993; Geraldine Farrar

cited in Brower & Cooke, 1996; Lamperti cited in Brown, 1931/1973). Several singers

mentioned employing visual anatomical images as well as feelings of the vocal

mechanism in connection with singing production. This strengthens the argument in

support of using anatomical images over metaphorical imagery for vocal production

purposes (Leyerle, 1986; Miller, 1996; Vennard, 1968).

Bodily movement and kinesthetic imagery was also used to facilitate memory,

rhythmic expression, and dynamics of music, which was also found in the literature

(Carter, 1993; Jaques-Dalcroze, 1920/1972; Laban, 1975, 1984). This supported the

257
argument for including simulated movement with motor imagery for increasing

performance execution in athletics (Holmes & Collins, 2002), dance (Hanrahan et al.,

1995) and piano (Highben & Palmer, 2004). Sound production was intrinsically

connected to the kinesthetic feeling of where vocalization originated in the body, as was

also found in Carter (1993). Trusheim (1987) reported that brass instrumentalists

experienced sounds kinesthetically in their embouchure and throughout their body as

well.

Musical sound images. A major category that emerged from the interviews was

images of musical sound. This extended beyond hearing sounds in relation to imagined

venues and those specific to their performance activities reported by athletes (Munroe et

al., 2000) as well as some singers in this study. More specifically, musical sounds were

reported in dancers imagery (Nordin & Cumming, 2005) and were a major imagery

element of all vocalists in this study. All the singers reported hearing internally their

beginning pitches and 14 of the 15 participants said they heard the accompaniment while

practicing either silently or out loud. Auditory images of musical accompaniment were

also found in Carter’s (1993) study, particularly for singers that had keyboard training.

Participants also mentioned mentally seeing the notes on the page in front of them, as did

singers in Carter’s (1993) study. Gordon’s (1976, 1999, 2003a) music learning theory

extensively described the phenomena of hearing musical sounds and elements internally,

or audiating, in musical development.

Other musical sounds in imagery encompassed modeling expert vocalists and

developing an internal ideal sound image. Ten singers used modeling of other

outstanding singers to achieve performance excellence. Perfecting the art of singing by

258
observing experts has been promoted in vocal pedagogy in a number of sources (Fields,

1992; Günter, 1992a, 1992b; Tosi & Galliard, 1723/1968; Ware, 1998). This lent support

to the observational studies of Fadiga and others (1995) in neuroimaging and functional

equivalence.

Seven of the vocalists heard ideal sounds in their head to help them produce

excellence in vocal quality, an element that also appeared in other music studies (Bellon,

2006; Bonpensiere, 1953; Carter, 1993; Hines, 1982; Trusheim, 1987). Miller (1996) and

others (Averino, 1989; DeLay cited in Stockholm, 1975; Fields, 1972; Ware, 1998)

supported the practice of developing a storehouse of strong ideal sounds in which singers

could compare and align with their own personal vocal sounds. During silent practice,

four singers said they heard the ideal sound of their own voices rather than those of other

outstanding professionals. Given their extensive musical training and performance

experience, it was not surprising that all these singers had developed this faculty. This

connection between ideal sound images and vocal execution may provide support for

functional equivalence studies in neuroimaging, in which brain activity of hearing and

imagining music was found to be similar (Kosslyn et al., 2001; Zatorre & Halpern, 2005).

Musical sounds emerged as a distinct category in this study since singers’ imagery

content in this area was so extensive and diverse, and differed markedly from the

environmental and sport specific sound images most athletes reported (Munroe et al.,

2000; Salmon et al., 1994).

Character/role images. Singers’ images of character development included

behavior, emotions, and appearance, which also followed the imagery content category

set by Nordin and Cumming (2005). These images reflected vocal professionals’

259
requirements, movements, and efforts in dramatics from performing a simple aria to an

entire role in opera. All the interviewees expressed the idea that characterization,

emotion, and memories of past experiences were essential elements to expressive singing

(Craig, 1992; Emmons & Sonntag, 2002; Lehman, 1945/1985). Vocalists’ character and

role images were much more detailed and specific to acting than what dancers reported in

this category (Hanrahan & Vergeer, 2000; Nordin & Cumming, 2005). Singers in this

study and those in Carter’s (1993) began methodologically recreating their individual

characters with the words of the song, the melody and rhythm, the context and

background, emotion, and identification of past personal experiences in the effort to

embody the character or song. This supported the methods suggested in a number of

vocal texts (Barten, 1992; Emmons & Thomas, 1998, 2008 Emmons & Sonntag, 2002;

Lotte Lehman, 1945/1985; Ware, 1998). This also concurred with Lang’s (1977, 1979a,

1979b) bio-informational theory of effective imagery, including specific details of

stimulus, response, and meaning propositions as related to both the individual singer and

the character being portrayed.

What Imagery Characteristics Vocalists Used

Imagery characteristics represented qualities of sense, perspective, ability,

deliberation and direction, and amount and duration. These vocalists’ responses reflected

many similarities of athletes and dancers use of sense imagery and general abilities

(Munroe et al., 2000; Nordin & Cumming, 2005; White & Hardy, 1995). Munroe and

colleagues (2000) categorized sense imagery as imagery types. However, the distinction

was made between specific content categories of imagery (as detailed above) and

function in the subsequent dance imagery investigation of Nordin and Cumming (2005).

260
These investigators relegated sense imagery as a characteristic of imagery, and more a

vehicle by which performers experienced certain images, which more closely represented

the findings of this study.

Senses. The results of these singers’ ranking of their sense imagery practices

(from highest to lowest) were visual imagery, auditory imagery, and kinesthetic imagery,

which accurately reflected Betts’ (1909) results gathered exactly one hundred years ago

in his vividness studies of musicians (e.g., 93% vision, 88% sound, and 83% kinesthetic

imagery). Other musical performers were reported to use the same three sense modalities

in their profession (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Holmes, 2005; Moyer, 1992; Ross, 1985;

Trusheim, 1987). However, Carter’s (1993) ranking differed in that elite singers use

kinesthetic, followed by aural, then visual sense imagery. The use of these three main

senses were also discussed in Seashore (1938/1967) and Farnsworth (1958) who

recognized that musicians experienced kinesthetic and visual images interacting with

auditory imagery.

Athletes have acknowledged the reciprocal nature of kinesthetic and visual

imagery interspersed with auditory environmental and sport specific images, including

olfactory, gustatory (Munroe et al., 2000; Salmon et al., 1994), and tactile senses (Vealey

& Greenleaf, 2006). Similar to dancers (Nordin & Cumming, 2005), singers’ auditory

abilities were centered on musical elements rather than environmental sound images as

was found in athletes (Munroe et al., 2000). Vocalists in this study included touch and to

a lesser extent smell and taste, the reports of which appeared primarily with participants

who enjoyed cooking. The use of these three senses was also limited to the dictates of the

text and character of the song. The inclusion of all these varied senses, or what Vealey

261
and Greenleaf (2006) termed, the polysensory experience, served to strengthen the

recommendation that athletes (and performers) use as many sense modalities as possible

in their imagery to increase effectiveness (Moran, 2004; Morris et al., 2005; Murphy et

al. 2008; Vealey & Greenleaf, 2001).

The difference in terminology between the senses of touch or tactile and

kinesthetic feeling was the source of some confusion for seven of these singers. When

asked to rate their three primary senses in imagery, many included touch. However, when

the definitions of both tactile and kinesthetic senses were given, they all changed their

answers to kinesthesia. From subsequent conversations with vocal singers and professors,

the term, kinesthesia, was not as common in voice training as touch or tactile in referring

to the feeling in the body (Ware, 1998). Tactile imagery was not included in the four Ws

framework for athletes (Munroe et al., 2000) but touch did emerge in dancers’ imagery

(Nordin & Cumming, 2005). Brass professionals in Trusheim’s (1987) study did employ

tactile imagery to a moderate degree, which was related to manipulation of their

instruments. This difference in use of tactile and kinesthetic terms should be considered

for any reference to bodily feeling concerning sense imagery use by singers and possibly

in other musicians as well.

Perspective. The use of perspective in imagery connected singers’ employment of

the visual sense with their preference and ability. Vocalists primarily used internal

perspective, believing it to be the most valuable, as had been reported in sport studies

(e.g., Lane, 1980; Mahoney & Avener, 1977; Rotella et al., 1980). Imagery in internal

perspective was helpful for understanding the feeling of actually performing, which

supported findings that internal perspective was more highly correlated with motor

262
movement than external perspective (Hale, 1992; Harris & Robinson, 1986). Singers’ use

of external perspective allowed the performer to comprehend the audience’s viewpoint,

as also reported by dancers (Nordin & Cumming, 2005). However, Ungerleider and

Golding (1991) and Holmes and Collins (2002) found that more successful athletes used

external perspective. Earlier studies had revealed no significant differences between the

effectiveness of internal and external imagery (Barnes, 1982; Highlen & Bennett, 1979;

Mumford & Hall, 1985). More recently sport researchers (Hardy, 1997; Moran, 2004:

White & Hardy, 1995) have argued that imagery perspective preferences relied on the

needs of the individuals and type of sports in which they performed. An internal

viewpoint was found to be more useful for perception in execution and external

perspective was used more for skills involving form (Hardy, 1997). Five singers

visualized externally, and some switched between internal and external, as was preferred

by some athletes (Gould, Weinberg, & Jackson, 1980; Jowdy, Murphy, & Durtschi,

1989). One of the tenors in this study, as well as a singer in Bellon’s (2006) study,

thought that using both perspectives were required in determining the success of the

outcome. Both execution and form were incorporated in vocal performance imagery,

strengthening the argument for using each perspective appropriately for the specific

needs of the performers as previously advocated (e.g. Caldwell & Wall, 2001; Emmons

& Thomas, 1998; Holmes & Collins, 2002).

An unexpected finding that emerged from these singers’ interviews was the issue

of perspective in auditory imagery of musical sounds. Previously, perspective had only

been considered to be a quality of visual imagery in studies of athletes (Hale, 1982;

Hardy & Callow, 1999; Munroe et al., 2000; White & Hardy, 1995) and dancers (Nordin

263
& Cumming, 2005). Singers reported hearing their own vocal sounds internally,

especially when it was associated with kinesthetic feelings. They also described mentally

hearing themselves from an external perspective, as in listening to themselves singing on

stage from the audience’s point of view. A singer imagining another vocalist executing a

song, as in modeling, could be considered external auditory imagery as opposed to when

the singer imagined being in the act of producing vocal sound. Bandura (1977) argued

that when individuals internalized the modeled behavior as their own, performance of the

desired behavior was enhanced. Furthermore, the internal hearing of instrumental

accompaniment, especially during mental rehearsal, implied that it was an external

emanation of musical sounds outside of themselves. This is distinctly an external hearing

perspective since other people played these instruments and these were not vocal sounds

experienced internally by a singer. Vocalists imagining themselves singing arias while

internally hearing instrumental accompaniments could be considered to employ both

internal and external auditory perspective. More investigation is needed to determine the

effects of each perspective on performance outcome and its correlation with ability and

expertise of the performer.

Imagery ability. Imagery abilities encompassed the degree of accuracy, vividness,

deliberation, ability to manipulate and control images, and amount and duration of

imagery engagement. The participants’ assessment of their individual imagery abilities

was generally favorable in accuracy, vividness, and being able to manipulate the images.

Elite athletes were also found to have clear and accurate images (Barr & Hall, 1992;

Isaac, 1992; Orlick & Parrington, 1988; Salmon et al., 1994; Ungerleider & Golding,

1991; Vadocz et al., 1997). Some singers said their images were extremely vivid and

264
accurate while a few others felt they needed improvement (e.g., Nicholas reported that

some of his images were “veiled”). Athletes also reported that their images were

controllable (Weinberg & Gould, 2003; Vealey & Walter, 1993), however Munroe and

others (2000) argued that this was not important in the athletes they interviewed. More

experienced singers in this study said that the images they used were accurate since they

had become accustomed to them. The findings suggested that singers were aware that

they could improve their imagery accuracy, vividness, and learn to control their images

more directly. Athletes had improved their imagery abilities after a 16-week training

program (Rodgers et al., 1991), suggesting that these abilities were modifiable and

trainable. More research regarding imagery ability pertaining to musicians is needed.

It was interesting to note that those singers in the study that reported high self-

assessment in imagery ability, accuracy, vividness, and detail also shared a number of

instances in which their images were associated with successful outcomes such as getting

an award or a role in an opera. Francine and Ivan imagined all the aspects of singing in an

audition and the specific part they intended to perform and were subsequently given the

appointment. Ivan even drew himself “with the award in hand.” Other singers whose

imagery was less accurate or detailed often reflected that they wanted to increase their

practice and get better at the details. Those participants who devoted a significant amount

of time in developing their imagery used it more and reported more successful results, as

was reflected in imagery of athletes (Vadocz et al., 1997). It was previously reported in

sport imagery research that more proficient imagers enjoyed more benefits in

performance and skill learning (Goss et al., 1986; Isaac, 1992). Similarly, athletes

reported that the more experience they gained in imagery, the higher the effectiveness of

265
that imagery became (Hall et al., 1990; Vadocz et al., 1997; Vealey, 1986; Weinberg &

Gould, 2003). Martin and others (1999) determined that these abilities could moderate the

various outcomes to which imagery had been applied.

Imagery direction in assessing whether images facilitated or debilitated

performance, as defined by Short and others (2004), was also addressed in this study.

Generally, singers reported that their images were positive and helpful, as found in sports

investigations (Gould et al., 1980; Hall et al., 1990; Powell, 1973; Short et al., 2004;

Woolfolk, Parrish et al., 1985). Most participants positively imagined the outcomes of

their performances, which they reported yielded beneficial effects. Several singers

grappled with negative imagery prior to performance and felt their confidence and

execution was in danger of deteriorating. This supported the previous findings that

positively imagining the desired outcome improved athletic and artistic performance,

particularly during competition, while negatively imagining the task seemed to be

detrimental (Munroe et al., 2000; Woolfolk, Murphy et al., 1985). Some participants

included the caveat that the degree to which imagery was helpful or hurtful depended on

the appropriateness of the image to the task, which was described in performance studies

of musicians (Emmons & Thomas, 1999; Ristad, 1982) and athletes (Murphy & Martin,

2002). If the image was debilitating in some way, they simply adjusted the image.

Singers’ imagery of their performances was generally controlled and directed to a

positive and desirable outcome. These ranged on a continuum of spontaneously arising

images to those fully controlled, as described in Murphy and others (2008). Participants

who enjoyed greater ability to control their images reported greater positive effects on

266
their performances, which supported the findings of athletes’ imagery use (Clark, 1960;

Isaac, 1992; Mackay, 1981).

Amount, duration, and speed of image engagement varied widely among the

vocalists. Imagery sessions lasted between fleeting seconds and all night mental

rehearsals. Frequency of imagery practice varied by individual mental as well as vocal

experience. Those singers who had learned to use imagery early in life more regularly

employed imagery in their professional pursuits. Professional singers and instrumentalists

reported using a significant portion of their practice time in mental imagery (Bellon,

2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987). Participants reported their images as both

replicating and exceeding the actual execution times especially with regard to securing

memorization of the material. This supported the findings of other musicians’

compressed mental rehearsal (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987), while

dancers engaged in slow, actual, and accelerated imagery speeds to aid memory for

routines and sequences (Nordin & Cumming, 2005). Singers briefly mentioned mentally

slowing down images when they were engaged in the early stages of learning a skill,

piece, or section of music. However, most imagery took place in real time, which could

be attributed to the temporal nature of the musical activity and the importance of

matching the speed of imagery to the actual timing of the motor skill as neural

equivalents (Holmes & Collins, 2002). Sport researchers generally have agreed that real-

time was superior to slow-motion imagery (Holmes & Collins, 2002; Gould et al., 2002;

Moran, 2004; Reed, 2002).

267
Why Vocalists Used imagery

The purposes for which solo professional vocalists used imagery were similar to

those reasons in sport and dance while other themes emerged from the data. The

cognitive and motivational reasons as found in athletics (Paivio, 1985; Hall et al., 1998;

Munroe et al., 2000) were supported, as was the artistic and healing purposes as revealed

in imagery of dancers (Nordin & Cumming, 2005). Within these categories additional

reasons emerged for singers which were specific to their profession.

Cognitive reasons. These vocalists used imagery for cognitive specific (CS)

purposes in many aspects of vocal production including technique, efforts to sing easily

and comfortably, supporting the breath, and using the body and vocal mechanism to

produce the desired sounds, which was also found in other musicians’ experiences

(Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987). Imagery functions of building and strengthening skill

have been recognized in many studies (e.g., Feltz & Landers, 1983; Driskell et al., 1994;

Hall et al., 1990; Hall et al., 1994; Mahoney & Avener, 1977). Purposes of using imagery

for skill learning and technique development differed in vocalists based somewhat on

technical expertise, specific performing requirements, years of singing experience, and

level of imagery ability (Driskell et al., 1994). For example, younger singers reported

using more imagery to gain breath support and lifting the palate than more mature

singers, who, it could be assumed, had already made these techniques a intrinsic part of

their performance behavior. This lent support to the argument of Fields (1972) and Marks

(1977) that conscious use of imagery was used to render motor action automatic. It is

possible that the more experienced singers no longer needed to use imagery for acquiring

268
skills that they had already incorporated into their performance behavior, which supports

the Holmes and Collins (2002) assertion that skill imagery uses changed over time.

All singers in the study reported mentally practicing performances, particularly

after learning the rudiments of the musical composition. Singers combined mental with

physical practice in achieving optimal performance. This supported the finding that

combining mental and physical practice facilitated performance success in studies such as

piano (Coffman, 1987, 1990; Highben & Palmer, 2004; Lim & Lippman, 1991; Lo, 1976;

Rubin-Rabson, 1941), voice Theiler & Lippman, 1995), various instruments (Coffman,

1987; Ross, 1985a, 1985b; Theiler & Lippman, 1995), and dance (Hanrahan, Tetreau, &

Sarrazin, 1995). Similar findings were reported in the sports literature (Driskell et al.,

1994; Feltz & Landers, 1983; Hinshaw, 1991; Richardson, 1967a, 1967b).

Singers in this study used the cognitive general (CG) functions of imagery,

strategy and sequence, in ways that were somewhat different than what had been

described in sports. Those singers who had planned and performed many recitals reported

using imagery to practice strategy and sequence and to carefully project the proper

artistic experience they intended to provide for their audiences. Vocalists in this study

and in Carter (1993) and instrumentalists in Trusheim (1993) reported similar uses of

imagery, particularly to execute conductors’ directions and plan routines. Using imagery

in planning and strategizing was also found in athletes (Fenker & Lambiotte, 1987;

MacIntyre & Moran, 1996; Munroe et al., 2000; Murphy et al., 2008; Rotella et al., 1980;

Rushall, 1988) and dancers (Fish et al., 2004). Vocalists were concerned more with

finding strategies to contend with difficulties in performance, as had been found in other

musicians (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987), gymnasts, and figure skaters

269
(White & Hardy, 1998). Operatic singers also mentioned using more imagery for

sequencing and staging than concert vocalists who sometimes created individualized

stories to connect their recital songs. Singers in Carter (1993) and orchestral brass

professionals in Trusheim (1987) also imagined stories in their performances.

Those singers primarily involved in operatic performance in this study used

imagery of sequencing memorization of a song or role. Dancers, like athletes in artistic

sports, also used imagery in sequencing to assist in memorization (Nordin & Cumming,

2005). These findings supported Sackett’s (1934, 1935) symbolic learning theory of

creating representations for memory task. Singers’ constant interaction with

memorization of text also provided some support for Paivio’s (1986) dual coding since

this type of singing involved both pictorial and verbal imagery.

Motivational reasons. Participants used extensive imagery for the motivation

specific (MS) function of goal identification, setting, and attainment, which reflected

findings in studies in imagery in sport (Hall et al., 1998; Munroe et al., 2000) and dance

(Fish et al., 2004; Monsma & Overby, 2004; Nordin & Cumming, 2005). These singers’

responses reflected goal motivation, which varied by the type of repertoire they were

singing, years of experience as professionals, perceived level of master (Lacaille et al.,

2005), and self-efficacy beliefs (Bandura, 1982). Vocalists combined outcome and

process goals in imagining a variety of desired results from executing the ideal sounds

they intended to produce to aspiring to secure a difficult operatic role. Participants who

had a high degree of success with goal imagery used it more often and in more detail,

providing increased motivation in their professional endeavors. Several singers

mentioned their goals had changed over the years particularly with regard to the

270
development of their vocal technique and as a result of achieving past goals. This seemed

to imply that singers had a fairly high regard for the effects their imagery had on their

professional efforts. Regardless of the singers’ varying levels of use and understanding of

its effects, goal imagery for motivational reasons seemed to be an intrinsic part of

singers’ efforts to achieve optimal performance.

The motivational general-arousal (MG-A) imagery function of modifying arousal

and energy in these singers was closely related to reports of dancers (Hanrahan &

Vergeer, 2000; Nordin & Cumming, 2005; Vergeer & Hanrahan, 1998), athletes (Hall et

al., 1998; Munroe et al., 2000; Paivio, 1985; White & Hardy, 1998), and other musicians

(Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987). Singers’ efforts of using imagery to

maintain appropriate levels of arousal to achieve optimal performance varied by

participants’ individual needs and specific performance requirements.

The majority of the responses focused on efforts to calm states of arousal,

particularly in the younger, less experienced singers. Nine singers adjusted their nervous

feelings by using imagery to focus on breath management, which was similar to other

musicians’ imagery (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987), especially in lowering

their heart rates, as found in athletes (Hecker & Kaczor, 1988). Singers reported using

alternative relaxation techniques such as meditation and creative visualization (Benson,

1987; Gawain, 1978, 2002; Harris, 1986). Other methods of using imagery to calm

nerves included prior preparation, focus, and concentration. Ten participants mentioned

the importance of thorough prior preparation and the knowledge that they were fully

ready to perform, which helped to alleviate their anxiety. Musicians in the studies of

Trusheim (1987), Carter (1993) and Bellon (2006) also mentioned the direct correlation

271
between extensive preparation and level of anxiety in their performances. This

corresponded with the findings in Fish and colleagues (2004) that dancers used cognitive

specific imagery of strengthening skill level to alleviate anxiety. Thirteen singers gave

accounts of focusing and concentrating on the task at hand to calm their nerves, which

also supported Landers (1980) and Murphy, Woolfolk, and Budney (1988), who found

that focusing on task-relevant cues helped athletes achieve the proper arousal levels for

the actions they performed.

Vocalists described accepting their nervous conditions as a natural part of being a

performer and using this energy to benefit them in performance. Singers in Carter’s

(1993) investigation distinguished between the positive feelings of anticipation and

working tension and the negative unwanted tension with which they had to learn to

contend. Participants described spontaneously appearing negative images of “devils” that

sometimes caused distraction, anxiety, and nervous tension. Learning to reframe these

feelings had been previously promoted in various performance texts (e.g., Dayme, 2005;

Emmons & Thomas, 1998; Green, 2002; Green & Gallway, 1986; Leyerle, 1986; Ristad,

1982) as well as studies in dance (Fish et al., 2004) and sport (Munroe et al., 2000;

Vadocz et al., 1997). Several singers reported used a similar ways of calming their fears

by various methods found in general psychology, including dealing with stress with self-

talk (Meichenbaum, 1977, 1985) and response propositions as found in bio-informational

theory of Lang and his colleagues (Lang, 1977, 1979a, 1979b; Lang, Melamed, & Hart,

1970).

Thirteen participants, especially those with more years of experience, described

employing imagery to psyche themselves up for performances. Trusheim (1987) reported

272
similar responses in his older brass professionals. Higher levels of energy were required

when more physical movements were involved as in sport and dance (Harris, 1986;

Landers, 1980; Murphy et al., 2008; Orlick, 1990; Oxendine, 1980; Vadocz et al., 1997).

Schmidt’s (1982) theory of attention-arousal set was supported in that each participant

identified and embodied the optimal arousal state for each specific performance.

Achieving optimal levels in imagery for the given task supported Holmes and

Collins’ (2002) position that imagery should include the specific affective responses and

arousal levels used in the desired performance. Most singers agreed that they required a

certain degree of arousal or calm readiness, which precluded energy levels above

relaxation, in order to successfully perform. The Yerkes-Dodson Law stating that tasks

with more complexity or difficulty required lower levels of drive or arousal was

supported (Oxendine, 1980). This went counter to the assertions of Jacobson (1957) and

Suinn’s VMBR research (1976, 1980a, 1980b, 1980c, 1983, 1992) both of whom

maintained that pre-relaxation imagery facilitated its effectiveness in performance.

Matching arousal levels and emotional affect in imagery with preparation and execution

of performance lent support to the functional equivalence theory (Holmes & Collins,

2002; Jeannerod, 1994).

Motivational general-mastery (MG-M) reasons of imagery use in these singers

included thoughts and feelings of self-confidence and mastery. Specifically the findings

included (a) being mentally tough and positive, (b) focus and concentration, (c) self-

efficacy and confidence, and (d) modeling.

Vocalists’ mental toughness and efforts to be positive was exemplified in the way

many of them handled distractions and resolve to pull through difficult situations. These

273
issues were often connected with maintaining proper arousal levels particularly just prior

to performance. Singers used self-talk to reestablish and maintain feelings of high

confidence and being competent to perform (Gammage, Hardy, & Hall, 2001; Hardy,

Gammage, & Hall, 2001). Negative thought stopping methods, being positive, and

maintaining positive self-images and expectations helped to stifle inner judges and

counteract negative self-criticism that undermined confidence and performance

excellence (Emmons & Thomas, 1998; Kohut, 1992; Ristad, 1982). This grappling with

self-confidence was reported in participants of a wide range of professional experience, a

finding that supported those of other studies (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim

1987).

Many singers used imagery to confidently motivate themselves by practicing

focus and concentration on the task at hand. Several participants even mentioned that

they concentrated solely those elements over which they had control and which could

readily change. Debilitating distractions decreased when performers focused on the

specific tasks of the performance, characterization, and communication with the audience

(Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987). Orlick and Parrington (1988) found that Olympians’ best

focus was achieved by concentrating on factors within their control. Fields (1972) found

that concentration levels affected performers’ involvement in their presentations.

Csikszentmihalyi (1990) observed that sustained task involvement directly correlated

with optimal performance experience.

Participants regularly used imagery to feel high levels of self-efficacy and

confidence as a method of motivation in their profession. It appeared that these vocalists

were well aware that their beliefs and patterns of self-assessment greatly affected the

274
success of their performances, as stated in Bandura (1997). Furthermore, singers who had

enjoyed success over a longer period of time also had increased self-efficacy concerning

their own expectations to continue performing than those with less experience, which was

also reported by other musicians (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; and Trusheim, 1987).

Feelings and images of enjoyment, mastery of technique, and performance excellence

seemed to undergird how these vocalists regarded their professional abilities and efforts,

which reflected the ideas offered by various pedagogues (Fields, 1972; Stedman, 1985;

Vennard, 1971) and sport researchers (Callow & Hardy, 2001; Martin et al., 1999; Mills

et al., 2000-2001; Moritz et al., 1996; Vadocz et al., 1997; Vealey, 1986). High self-

efficacy and strong confidence beliefs and images were factors that distinguished elite

from less successful athletes (Gould et a., 1981; Woolfolk, Murphy et al., 1985), and

allowed for more facility in developing, maintaining, and regaining confidence (Moritz et

al., 1996).

Many of these participants described modeling expert performers as a method of

motivation especially in establishing feelings of self-efficacy (Bandura & Jefferies,

1973). Singers described the development of modeling from observing elite vocalists, to

imagining themselves as though they performed as experts, to finally embodying these

desired skills as an automatic habits as illustrated in Bandura (1977, 1986, 1997) and

Fields (1972). Feltz (1984) had maintained that mental rehearsal of successfully

performing a task served to motivate and strengthen athletes’ beliefs in their abilities.

Artistic reasons. In creating the four Ws imagery framework for dancers, Nordin

and Cumming (2005) included the cognitive and motivational reasons (Munroe et al.,

2000) and identified two new categories of imagery reasons: artistic and healing.

275
Vocalists’ imagery for artistic purposes differed from dancers’ due to the nature of the art

and included vocal quality, appearance, character development, and communication with

the audience. While these categories had been discussed previously in imagery types,

they are addressed here because singers used imagery for other purposes as well as the

specific image itself. As researchers had established, one image could be used for several

reasons as readily as several types of imagery could be used for a single purpose (Callow

& Waters, 2005; Fish et al., 2004; Murphy et al., 2008; Nordin & Cumming, 2005; Short

et al., 2004; Short et al., 2006).

All 15 singers imagined to artistically improve their vocal quality which

encompassed changes in their color or timbre, modeling, hearing an ideal sound, and

increasing depth and richness. Vocalists often imagined certain colors, which were

associated with specific sounds and phrases. This use of imagery was very similar to

those singers who held an ideal sound in their minds to match in vocalizing. Many

mentioned this in trying to achieve a certain depth and richness to their voice. Two

singers felt that each individual had their own sound, however they also admitted it

sometimes took years of training to achieve this. These concerns were also discussed in

the other music imagery studies (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987). Singers

recognized the need to continually develop as a professional, regardless of experience

levels, and relied on imagery more as a result of their ever-developing background.

Eleven singers used modeling to achieve mastery of different aspects of vocal

production. Participants used imagery of techniques, vocal sound qualities, and

performance practices they had observed in expert singers to affect more successful

behaviors and feelings of mastery. These were elements included in Bandura’s self-

276
efficacy theory (1977, 1986, 1997), as well as vocal pedagogues (Tosi & Galliard,

1723/1968; Ware, 1998). Musical performers in other studies also mentioned many

different aspects of modeling use in solidifying, developing, and perfecting their musical

efforts and production (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993; and Trusheim, 1987).

Singers also used imagery to affect their appearance on stage. Ten singers

described images to achieve and maintain appropriate posture and execute staging in

connection with the character or a song. Several singers who performed specific songs in

recitals mentioned using imagery to achieve the right stage movement and gestures,

which more reflected how appearance imagery in dancers was employed (Nordin &

Cumming, 2005). Athletes in synchronized swimming, artistic gymnastics, and figure

skating also reported using imagery to secure their artistic appearance and form in their

particular sports (Hays, 2002; Murphy et al., 2008). Professional vocalists recognized the

importance of their onstage image as perceived by the audience as a vital element in their

success (Lehmann, 1945/1985).

Character development emerged in the interviews in two distinct areas of

preparation and staging. For character preparation, vocalists imagined the backgrounds,

history, culture, and appearance of their characters (Emmons & Sonntag, 2002; Lehmann,

1945/1985). All but one participant recalled being inspired by either live or recorded

images of other professional singers performing in the roles and characters these

participants were engaged to play. Eight singers used images of their own past

experiences to prepare their portrayals.

Character development in staging included imagery purposes to engage in

emotion, the body, vocal sound, and being fully in the character. All singers used various

277
images for feeling the emotions and being in character, which reflected other singers’

accounts (Averino, 1989; Carter, 1993; Hines, 1982). Ahsen (1984) underscored the

importance of including emotion in his triple code theory, ISM, image, somatic response,

and meaning. Similarly, Lang’s (1977. 1979a, 1979b) emphasis on emotion was

incorporated in his bio-informational theory of stimulus, response, and meaning

propositions. Participants also used emotion and imagery in achieving characterization

through vocal production and interpretation as had been discussed in the literature

(Ferrier, 1955; Hines, 1982; Horne & Scorvell, 1984). Addressing and incorporating

these character elements were also consistent with how Craig (1993), Lehmann

(1945/1985), and Emmons and Sonntag (2002) suggested vocalists prepare and imagine

themselves in embodying a song. Brass players also reported creating images of

characterizing elements in the symphonic works they performed (Trusheim, 1987).

Several issues arose for some singers concerning being in the zone, or in flow

(Csikszentmihalyi, 1990). Munroe and others (2000) categorized flow as one of the

motivational functions of imagery in athletes. Flow was not included in imagery for

dancers (Nordin & Cumming, 2005). Similar to Bellon’s (2006) musicians, singers in this

study reported either being in the zone as a result and requirement of their profession or

only rarely achieving the experience of flow. Others described being in a magical moment

where nothing else existed but the music or feeling a responsibility as a performer to be

in the zone as much as possible in order to benefit the audience. Furthermore, the flow

state seemed to be an experience that happened as a result of proper preparation,

reflecting Bellon’s (2006) findings that being in the zone was synonymous with

professionalism. Participants’ responses often reflected that being in the zone was

278
tantamount to embodying the character in the performance. This idea supported Carter’s

(1993) finding that elite vocalists achieved flow as a result of surpassing the experience of

synthesized text, music, and character. More research is needed to find how imagery and

being in flow interact for performers.

Another artistic reason for using imagery was the variety of regard these singers

held for the audience, as initially categorized in dancers’ imagery (Nordin & Cumming,

2005). Nordin and Cumming (2005) identified many of the previously mentioned artistic

reasons as part of dancers’ motivation in communicating with the audience. However,

singers in this study distinguished most of those artistic reasons separately from their

specific intentions to communicate more directly with the audience than had been

identified in dancers’ imagery. Participants further recognized that mastery of technique,

music, and embodying the character was a prerequisite for mentally engaging with the

audience. Therefore the audience communication category was redefined for this study

and included: (a) sharing musical intent, (b) flow of energy, (c) sharing love and joy, (d)

spiritual engagement, and (e) spiritual responsibility. These findings supported and

extended Carter’s (1993) reports of elite vocalists interacting with their audience with

feelings of flow, vibrations, and love.

Several participants focused on the audience members to feel engaged and

supported in their performances, which was analogous to the other vocalists’ mental

regard for the audience (Bellon, 2006; Carter, 1993). Most singers imagined ways of

communicating with the audience, ranging from making sure the audience understood the

intentions of the composer and the text to engaging members of the audience in a flow of

energy, the findings of which were consistent with other professional vocalists (Carter,

279
1993). Several participants imagined the audience feeling their love and joy, while others

shared a more spiritual engagement with those in the theatre. The reasons for specific

audience interaction were often tied to singers’ purpose for which they initially became

professional performers. Several participants spoke of feeling spiritually responsible for

being a beneficial or healing force for their audience. It was possible that presenting

songs directly facing the audience, as has been the tradition in classical singing, could

have inspired certain vocalists to feel this for the people that came to see them perform.

Furthermore, many of these singers were well experienced in liturgical and oratorio

performances, which precluded a spiritual or religious context. These findings were not

addressed as such in instrumentalists or athletes’ imagery and were far more personally

directed than audience communication imagery of dancers (Nordin & Cumming, 2005).

These specific results also elaborated and extended the findings of Carter (1993), and

underscored the importance of audience interactions in these professional singers.

Healing reasons. Healing reasons for using imagery by these vocal professionals

varied in the interviews and reflected some of the findings in dancers (Nordin &

Cumming, 2005). Most singers’ healing images focused on efforts to maintain good

physical and mental health to be able to optimally perform. Ten singers described some

kind of method to check their physical body, the alignment, and status of the vocal

apparatus prior to and during performance, as found in other musicians (Carter, 1993;

Trudheim, 1993). Some participants maintained health by immediately responding to

initial symptoms of sickness or during illness and injury by engaging in healthy images

and feelings, which replicated imagery of other musicians’ (Trusheim, 1987) and athletes

(Calmels et al., 2003; Orlick, 1990). Singers also used imagery in healing methods such

280
as Reiki and other visualization techniques to maintain health (Gawain, 1978, 2002;

Sheikh, 2003; Sheikh & Jordan, 1983; Sheikh & Korn, 1994; Simonton et al, 1971;

Simonton et al., 1978). Singers often employed mental practice when they were sick or

needed to preserve their voices, as was also found in imagery uses by musicians (Bellon,

2006; Carter, 1993; Trusheim, 1987) and dancers (Nordin & Cumming, 2005).

Vocalists in this study reported using imagery to contend with having to sing

while they were excessively tired, during times of illness, and outbreaks of chronic

physical ailments. Nicholas used modeling in recalling how another famous singer would

have dealt with the same stress he experienced. Sickness and fatigue were also times

when these professionals practiced mentally in silence to preserve their voices, which

was also reported in Bellon (2006), Carter, (1993), and Trusheim (1987). These

participants seemed to be consciously connected to how their bodies and voices were

feeling and responded readily with imagery to maintain optimal health for performance

excellence.

Four Ws of Imagery Use Framework for Singers

The singers’ four Ws of imagery use framework was designed as a result of

analyzing the responses of the interviews deductively from imagery research in sport,

dance, music, and inductively from themes and categories that emerged directly from the

participants’ interviews. Figure 3 gives the details of this framework including where,

when, what, and why singers imagine, which was adapted primarily from the frameworks

shown in Figure 1 of athletes (Munroe et al., 2000) and Figure 2 of dancers (Nordin &

Cumming, 2005).

281
Practice/Performance
Settings
General Locations
Where At Home
Other Places

Practice
Rehearsal
When
Performance Execution Images
Performance Season Metaphorical Images
Breaks Context Images
Other Times Body-related Images
Musical Sound Images
Imagery Types Character/Role Images

What Senses
Perspective
Singers’ Ability/Accuracy/Detail
Imagery Imagery Characteristics
Deliberation/Direction
Amount/Duration

Vocal Production
Metaphors in Vocal
Cognitive Reasons Production
Memorizing/Planning

Goal Attainment
Arousal/Energy
Motivational Reasons Mastery/Confidence

Why Vocal Quality


Appearance
Artistic Reasons Character Preparation
Character Staging
Communication with
Audience

Healing Reasons Overall Health


Vocal Health

Figure 3. Four Ws of Imagery Use Framework for Singers

282
This proposed singers’ framework differs from the four Ws of imagery

framework for athletes (Figure 1) and dancers (Figure 2) in a number of important ways.

The landscape of the dance framework has been maintained in favor of the levels

presented in the athletic framework since a fuller description of singers’ imagery use

could be represented. Singers as well as dancers frameworks extended the areas of where

and when imagery was used. Furthermore, in the dancer’s framework content areas were

more fully represented to include specific image types and characteristics that were also

present in singers’ imagery. Additions to the dance framework included vocal production,

musical sounds, and an extended area of communication with the audience.

The four Ws of imagery framework for singers is offered here for several reasons.

It is hoped that the singers’ framework would be used as a guide in future studies of

imagery use in vocalists, instrumentalists, and other musicians. It could also be used to

inform students and professionals in possible ways they could develop and employ their

own imagery in their artistic endeavors. Furthermore, teachers and vocal professors could

use this framework to train singers and possibly musicians to incorporate and develop

imagery applications in their practice and performance. It is further hoped that future

research and applications would serve to improve this framework in any way that would

more perfectly reflect singers’ use of imagery in achieving optimal performance.

Limitations of the Study

Several limitations emerged as a result of the exploratory nature and design of this

investigation and were worthy of consideration. Seven limitations were cause for concern

and included: (a) sample size, (b) sample configuration, (c) instrumentation, (d) pilot

study, (e) interview responses, (f) data analysis, and (g) researcher bias.

283
Sample Size

The first limitation concerned threats due to sample size. The small number of

participants in the sample restricted the ability to generalize to the entire classical solo

vocal professional population in the United States. However, extensive effort was made

to secure the widest range of professional vocalists that qualified for participation and

included soloists residing throughout the United States. Professional vocalists and

teachers and professors of voice were invited to recommend qualified singers to

participate in the study. Generalizabilty was sacrificed in order to gain an in-depth and a

rich data set from highly qualified and experienced professionals. It was determined that

this population provided the most complete responses to the research questions, which

allowed the results of study to contribute to the growing body of research in the use of

imagery in vocal professionals.

Sample Configuration

Another limitation was the sample configuration. The use of snowball sampling

limited the generalizability to those singers who participated in this study. The

exploratory and in-depth design of the study limited the number of participants that could

be included in the interviews. Snowball sampling also was limited to those singers who

were known to the researcher or recommended by other vocalists and singing teachers

and professors who the researcher knew. Due to the researchers’ prior experience in

opera and liturgical singing, many of the vocalists were involved in these performance

areas which possibly limited representation for other types of solo classical vocal

professionals in this study.

284
While generalizability is not possible with this small sample, attempts a broader

representation were made through a sample selection that included (a) male and female

singers, (b) a wide range of ages, (c) a wide range of professional experience, (d) a

variety of vocal types, (e) varied geographical locations across the United States, and (e)

an extensive variety of classical singing genres. Two-thirds of the participants were

female, with sopranos’ average age of 37.4, mezzos’ average age of 58.5. Of the five

men, four were tenors whose average age was 40 and one was a baritone. Sopranos

represented the least experience with mezzos the most, including two who had over 40

years of professional experience each. In spite of many efforts to personally invite bass

singers to participate, none responded. Also missing were singers representing the

Midwest and Northwest regions of the United States. The limiting factors of the sample

populations may have influenced the results.

The wide variety of classical singers’ genre served to represent a broad range of

the general employment options for professional vocalists, however it revealed an even

broader use of imagery and differences between the singers. There was also a high

number of singers who performed primarily in operatic roles, which may not represent

the entire profession of classical solo singers who sang other styles such as concerts,

recitals, and oratorios. Usually the acting requirements of staging and physical movement

in opera were not the same as concert singing. These issues may have influenced the

findings of imagery content and function for these participants.

Instrumentation

The third limitation was the threat of instrumentation (Onwuegbuzie, 2003). The

singers’ interview guide was the initial instrument used to collect data for this study. This

285
interview protocol was borrowed from the dancers’ interview guide of Nordin and

Cumming (2005). Each item in this guide was scrutinized and adapted for the purposes of

this study using related literature and music research pertinent to the use of imagery in

vocal performers. Other efforts to control for this threat included the use of member

checking (Johnson & Christensen, 2004), whereby participants were each sent their

transcribed interviews for editing or including any pertinent information. Furthermore,

several music researchers analyzed the interview protocol for appropriateness in use by

vocal professionals. Although efforts were made to create a guide most appropriate to

gathering data from solo singing professionals, several issues arose during the interview

process. Terminology concerns, including differentiating between such terms as holidays

and breaks, tactile and kinesthesia, and sequencing and planning, caused some confusion

for a few of the participants. In addition, the first three sections of the singers’ interview

guide included inquiries that gathered less relevant data relative to the research questions.

These three sections proved inadequate in acquiring necessary demographic and

descriptive data on the participants. Therefore the participant survey instrument had to be

designed and implemented, which proved adequate for gathering this information.

Pilot Study

The fourth limitation addressed threats in the administration of the pilot study. It

became apparent during the course of the study that the use of only one participant for the

pilot study was not sufficient to identify and clarify several terminology concerns in the

singers’ interview guide. Interviewing more pilot study participants would also have

clarified the inclusion of appropriate probes in the protocol questions. It is possible that

286
including more participants in the pilot study would have prevented these problems and

given the researcher further practice in the interview process.

Interview Responses

The fifth limitation involved the participants’ responses during the interview.

These self-report and personal introspection responses were subjective at best but

represented a viable method in an exploratory study such as this. In order to control for

this threat, the identities of the individual participants were kept confidential.

Interviewees were given pseudonyms for reporting purposes and any personal

information, which could have identified them, was omitted. This helped ensure that the

participants had nothing to lose or gain by falsifying or inflating their answers. It was also

assumed that the responses of the participants were as honest, candid, and forthright as

could be expected in describing their experiences with imagery, since they had no reason

to protect their reputation. Furthermore, in reporting the demographics of the participants,

only enough general information was provided to allow the reader to understand basic

background and experience of each individual but not enough to specifically identify him

or her.

Data Analysis

In the sixth limitation concerning the data analyses, methods were used to reduce

threats of bias. In coding, four researchers with advanced degrees in music research and

training in data analysis were employed in coding one of the transcriptions, which

resulted in an inter-coder agreement of .83. Furthermore, peer debriefing (Lincoln &

Guba, 1985) was conducted in order to expose the researchers’ implicit mental

assumptions so that data coding more adequately reflected the theoretical framework, the

287
related literature, and the responses of the participants. In the effort to allow the reader to

understand the researcher’s coding process, specific descriptive quotes from the

participants were provided in the results section of this study.

Research Bias

The final threat of researcher bias was a major consideration even though the

researcher was considered an instrument in this exploratory study. The researcher’s

familiarity with imagery and the vocal profession offered a unique understanding to these

two fields. This background assisted the investigator in understanding and interpreting

the data in explaining the meaning of their quotes involving the experiences specific to

the vocal profession and imagery. However, the researcher’s prior experience may have

created certain expectations and influenced the direction and interpretation of the

interviews. In the effort to maintain objectivity and reduce bias, the researcher divulged

her training and related experience to another researcher throughout the course of this

study. Furthermore, peer debriefings were conducted throughout all stages of the research

process (Lincoln & Guba, 1985). Additionally, at the end of each interview, the

participants were asked if they were influenced in any way. Only one responded

positively and offered suggestions to alleviate this problem in future interviews. This

participant felt that she was being probed to answer in a way that sometimes confused her

and suggested that I could patiently wait slightly longer for the answers. This change was

immediately implemented with the remaining ten interviews. Subsequently, all other

participants reported that they had not been influenced in their interviews. Many vocalists

recalled that the interviewer helped to clarify concepts and provided ideas to help them

identify and remember what specific imagery they used.

288
Implications of the Study

Since there were no known models of imagery use that have connected sport

psychology and musical performance, particularly in voice, this study represented a

unique and novel approach. Since these performers found imagery to be useful and

effective, it could be assumed that other singers and musical performers may already be

employing many of the same techniques in their artistic endeavors, even if their imagery

use was not specifically identified as those practices identified in sport psychology

research. The results of this study have served to distinguish these vocal professionals’

use of imagery in achieving optimal performance as they relate to the findings of sport

psychology imagery. Implications for vocal and music education on all levels, private and

class music instruction, and general education are discussed. These areas included: (a)

location and times, (b) developing mental capacities, (c) matching imagery content with

function, (d) mental practice, (e) metaphorical and anatomical images, (f) context

imagery, (g) goal attainment, (h) modeling, (i) musical sound imagery, (j) use of the

senses, (k) body-related imagery, (l) healing imagery, (m) arousal modification, (n) self-

efficacy and mastery, (o) character development, (p) communication with the audience,

and (l) using advanced technology to facilitate imagery development.

Location and Times

Participants reported using imagery most prior to performances and during

practice, the finding of which was revealed in athletes (Munroe et al., 2000) and dancers

(Nordin & Cumming, 2005). Other places and times included being at home, traveling

and commuting, exercising, and any time or place. Becoming comfortable with using

imagery in a variety of settings is something that could be trained in student and

289
professional singers as a way to use imagery in augmenting practice and developing

concentration and focus. The more vocal participants could concentrate, the longer they

were motivated to engage in imagery. However, singers varied in their imagery times

from flashes to hours of mental engagement. It would be advisable that in training,

imagery times should begin with short durations and in quiet places then proceeding to

increased time lengths and levels of distractions as indicated by these singers’ responses.

Ultimately, with practice, directed, successful, and effective imagery would take place in

a wide range of times and settings.

Developing Mental Capacities

It is generally acknowledged that imagery ability appears as early as infancy and

is a skill that can be taught and developed (Arieti, 1976). Participants in this study have

described the development of their imagery from memories of playing creatively as very

young children. Valuing and incorporating this important skill could begin in early

childhood musical and preschool programs and possibly provide innumerable benefits in

the academic and creative life of the individual regardless of their profession. Singers and

performing musicians in training programs of all levels including the private studio, the

general music classroom, and undergraduate and graduate private and group classes in

conservatories or universities could benefit from instruction and development of mental

imagery skills. Professional musical performing skills would be incomplete with out

mental training and development. This is a strong implication for a holistic approach to

vocal and musical performance training (Chapman, 2006; Kohut, 1992).

Understanding the actual experiences and development of professional vocalists,

particularly in their use of imagery, could serve to guide vocal pedagogy, just as sport

290
psychology research has positively influenced athletic training programs (Martin et al.,

1999, Morris et al., 2005). Even though singing training is considered to be an oral

tradition, teachers with minimum vocal experience continue to teach vocalists, which

may impact how imagery is incorporated into their educational practices. It is possible

that teachers with or without professional singing experience may have preconceived

opinions concerning imagery applications and could inadvertently omit some aspects of

mental training vital to the performance success of their students. While it may be ideal

for all voice teachers to be successful vocalists, teachers should at least be knowledgeable

in as many aspects of singing performance as possible. Sport psychologists and coaches

may not have been top athletes, but they seek to provide the tools to equip elite sport

competitors with excellent mental skills and techniques. Singing teachers may greatly

benefit not only themselves as educators but the future success of their students if they

learned the various applications of imagery to musical and particularly vocal performance

development and excellence. Even though only a few students may reach professional

status, all students deserve the best training available, including those in mental skills.

For these participants, imagery was used in cognitive functions for learning and

securing vocal technique and execution, motivational reasons of maintaining arousal and

energy balance, and artistic purposes of developing the character and emotional elements

of a song or role. It was also used to augment practice through mental rehearsal, check

and resolve execution problems, generate artistic interpretation, sustain optimal health for

performing, and much more. Given these findings, it is possible that other musical

performers might use similar imagery content and purposes in their artistic processes.

While many music pedagogues have recognized that developing the mental side of

291
performance is intrinsic to training (Caldwell & Wall, 2001; Chapman, 2006; Emmons &

Thomas, 1998; Ware, 1991), current methods of guidance, training, and education in this

area may be insufficient and may not be addressed in ways to achieve optimal

performance.

Matching Imagery Content with Function

Sport psychologists have recognized for years that mental training was vital to

athletic success (Hall, 2001; Morris, 2005). Vocal teachers could be guided by research

such as this to provide all their students with excellent and thorough preparation. The

major components, functions, and purposes of imagery, as identified in the proposed

singer’s imagery framework, could allow vocalists and performers to learn to

systematically connect and apply appropriate imagery to their intended purpose. If

imagery components were brought out during training by the assistance of astute vocal

teachers, singers could learn to consciously direct their use of imagery, which may allow

for more autonomy and natural development in vocal development. Given the extensive

skills and techniques the singer must learn, training in imagery could facilitate building,

securing, and refining these efforts. Furthermore, the teacher could use a variety of

methods in helping the student learn to integrate, mentally and physically, many of the

other skills involved in being a professional singer. The use of software, graphic, video,

and audio media, and the Internet and can all assist in building the repertoire of sensory

and technical imagery upon which the singer could develop a career.

Mental Practice

Many of these singers said that they initially used and developed their imagery in

mental rehearsal primarily during quiet times when there were minimal distractions,

292
which served to augment their physical practice efforts. Implementing a program to

develop mental practice in general and private music programs would require specific

training as well as periods of silence in and outside the classroom or studio to allow

students to imagine recreating and practicing the piece mentally in between their physical

practice sessions. Mixing mental and physical practice was found to be ideal for

memorization and successful practice in a number of music studies (Coffman, 1987,

1990; Highben & Palmer, 2004; Lim & Lippman, 1991; Ross, 1985a, 1985b; Rubin-

Rabson, 1941). More specifically, mental rehearsal was most effective after the rudiments

of the music had been learned, then physical and mental practice was alternated for best

effect. Too often music teachers feel compelled to give their students as much tangible

and active material as possible. Teachers could assist students and children in their own

self-discovery by sensitively calling more attention to their students’ thought processes,

feelings, and mental images throughout the music learning process.

Metaphorical and Anatomical Images

Whether singers use metaphorical or anatomical images in their artistic process

seemed to depend on personal preference and experience, familiarity with the anatomical

features of the vocal mechanism, and the requirements of the specific task. Regarding

vocal production, metaphorical imagery was used to secure technique and execute artistic

interpretation in qualities of singing. These metaphors were also found to facilitate

learning for some singers with less knowledge or familiarity of the vocal anatomy. In her

experience as a vocal performer and a teacher, one participant in this study found that

many young singers regarded their voices as somewhat magical and responded to

metaphorical images in achieving better vocal quality and advancing their technique.

293
However, as vocalists matured and gained more experience and understanding, they often

acquired knowledge of the intricacies of the vocal mechanism in executing their sound.

Anatomical imagery assisted singers to connect the kinesthetic feelings of manipulating

the voice with images of the corresponding muscles. This finding was consistent with

singers’ responses in Carter (1993). If a student was given the mechanical explanations

too soon, it sometimes caused confusion and unnecessary tension possibly resulting in

vocal injury. Several participants in the present study reported that similar experiences

caused them to seek another teacher. Other singers responded to the technical aspects of

the vocal production, preferring to know exactly how their vocal mechanism operated so

they could learn to understand its functions and manipulate it more precisely. For these

participants, especially those with the most experience, this knowledge base grew over

time and became quite extensive, which was reflected in several studies of vocal

pedagogues (Cleveland, 1989a, 1989b, 1989c; Vennard, 1958, 1961, 1968, 1971).

Therefore, vocal teachers should be knowledgeable in a variety of metaphorical

images as well as possess a strong background in the anatomy of vocal mechanics.

Providing graphic and video images of vocal anatomy could facilitate greater

understanding in singers who preferred these images. In taking it a step further, allowing

singers to see their own vocal mechanism by using a flexible fibroscope or a laryngeal

image biofeedback (LIB) medical imaging system (Cleveland, 1998a, 1998b) to see the

vocal folds could greatly enhance their anatomical understanding. Vocalists’ needs and

preferences changed throughout their development, particularly in their imagery use.

Therefore with sensitivity and flexibility, teachers should continue developing their own

knowledge and understanding of the voice and help students learn how imagery can best

294
be employed. These results point to the benefits teachers could gain from a full

knowledge of vocal anatomy as well as an extensive collection of metaphors to share

with their students as needs arise. Teachers’ knowledge of when and how to apply

metaphorical and anatomical images could significantly enhance training of vocal

students. This highlights the necessity of the teacher to put aside personal preference

regarding the uses of imagery in favor of meeting the individual imagery needs of each

singer.

Context Imagery

Several reported hearing environmental sounds of the performance venue. All

these singers imagined themselves in future performance scenarios from upcoming

auditions to their most coveted dream of singing at famous venues, such as the

Metropolitan Opera House. Many participants shared their extensively detailed images of

their future performances, including their affective reactions to the experience and the

uses of many of their senses, which helped them prepare for the actual experiences. This

method of imagery was found to be a vital element in functional equivalence which

linked imagined tasks with the same executed motor activity (Decety 1996a, 1996b,

2002; Holmes & Collins, 2002). Researching the venue by attending, acquiring pictures,

or viewing video of the theatre in which the singer planned to perform allowed personal

imagery to be more precise thus aided performance outcome. Vocalists, as well as

teachers in a private or group singing classes, could easily implement the inclusion of

visual media in imagery of various theatres around the world, especially since

information is so readily available on the Internet.

295
Goal Attainment

An important area of imagery use that these participants stressed was their efforts

to identify, clarify, and attain goals. These outcome goals were also mixed with

performance and process goals (Weinberg & Gould, 1999) and were imagined with as

much detail and accuracy as the individual singer was able. Some participants mentioned

making lists, writing down details, drawing pictures, and clearly identifying the

particulars of what they wanted to accomplish. Several singers continually held in mind

the long-term overall vision of their goals while others were more interested in enjoying

and artistically crafting each performance. Teachers could help students by encouraging

them specifically identify, describe, illustrate, and vividly imagine their immediate and

far-reaching goals. Students could be assisted in developing their own outcome,

performance, and process goals in their musical efforts as well as ways to use imagery to

accomplish them. Allowing students to internalize the music by providing time and

training in imagery, and giving students opportunities to direct their own educational and

personal goals could significantly increase learning and understanding as well as develop

a life-long skill and personal autonomy. This could provide the opportunity for the singer

to share the responsibility for their development and eventually assume the full conscious

direction and progress of their career.

Modeling

Vocal professionals in this study recalled watching, listening to, and being

inspired by outstanding singers. These models provided ideal images in sound,

appearance, movement, and characterization for the singers to emulate in their own

performances, which supported Bandura’s self-efficacy theory (Bandura, 1977, 1986,

296
1997; Bandura & Jefferies, 1973). Providing excellent models in various capacities for

singers, instrumentalists, chorus, general music, and young children could positively

influence their vocal and musical development. Furthermore, the practice of listening to

excellent musical performances serves to build a strong storehouse of musical sounds.

Often music programs have emphasized playing and physical involvement with

performance, however these results indicated that watching and hearing exemplary

models of performances could serve to guide a growing musician in ways that would far

exceed constant physical practice. Employing expert models as teachers and educators

may significantly affect the progress of the student in a variety of fields.

Musical Sound Imagery

Another important finding that emerged in these interviews was that singers

mentally heard a wide variety of sounds in their imagery. All singers mentally practiced

songs and roles, hearing various elements such as the first attack, pitch, tone quality, text,

phrases, entire song, and various parts of the accompaniment. Over the years, many had

developed an every evolving ideal sound image, which served to guide vocal production.

This individualized sound image developed as the vocalist grew as a professional and

incorporated the storehouse of musical sound qualities they had heard and experienced

over the years. Building this personalized vocal or musical sound can be provided

beginning in infancy and even prior to being born (e.g., Woodward, 1992) and continue

throughout childhood, adulthood, and the entire lifetime.

As these singers practiced auditory imagery in music, they became more

accomplished in mentally hearing more musical components. The participants’ musical

experiences represented imitational aspects of audiation (Gordon, 2007), which involved

297
“recalling and performing familiar music from memory” (p. 15). While most singers

reported mentally hearing the melody and accompaniment, especially with regard to

memorization, very few mentioned attending to the musical elements of rhythm, tonality,

or context. Even though it could not be assumed that these were not a part of their

auditory imagery, the fact that these musical images were omitted may be cause for

concern. No singer mentioned hearing any alteration of these phrases or melodies, such

as hearing musical compositions in different tonalities or rhythms, which would have

provided evidence for more developed audiation abilities as defined by Gordon (1999,

2007). However, learning and applying further training in audiation could provide facility

in memorization skills, pitch and vocal production, deeper understanding of musical

context, skill in improvisation as well as composition, and other techniques used to

develop auditory abilities in music. This has been exemplified in a number of texts on

music learning (Azarra, 1992,1999; Grunow, 2001; Liperote, 2006; Reynolds, Valerio,

Bolton, Taggart, & Gordon, 1998).

Another vocal production issue, which emerged from the interviews, was singers’

inability to trust their vocal sounds and therefore their auditory images, preferring to

kinesthetically feel the placement for each tone. Several of them purposefully declared

that they did not hear musical sounds internally as a result of their teachers preventing

them from developing any reliance on their internal auditory images. This dependence on

the kinesthetic feeling of the components to produce specific sounds seemed to supplant

their use and development of internalized musical sound images, a practice which had

been supported in the pedagogic texts (e.g., Farrar cited in Brower & Cooke, 1996;

Lamperti cited in Brown, 1931/1973). With the extensive work done in audiation and

298
music learning theory of Gordon (1976, 1999, 2003z, 2003b, 2007), heavily depending

on bodily feeling in vocal production might seem counterintuitive to the natural

development of a musician, and particularly a professional singer. While the variety of

acoustics in various venues may undermine a vocalists’ confidence in producing correct

intonation, developing musical auditory imagery ability would seem to take precedence.

If teachers of voice would allow their students to connect both aurally and kinesthetically

to the sounds they need to produce, perhaps this would further buttress singers’ skill,

confidence, and self-reliance in their performance experiences, while allowing aural

imagery skills to develop naturally.

Use of the Senses

Singers’ imagery primarily involved sight, auditory, and kinesthetic senses which

were considered to be intrinsic to vocal performance. This has significant implications for

music education programs of all kinds to strengthen use of these senses in imagery.

Traditional music programs have often focused on music literacy, which developed

certain aspects of sight as it related to vocal and instrumental sound. Holmes and Collins

(2002) have acknowledged that learning usually begins with visual or auditory imagery

(in the case of musical training) and then the kinesthetic sense was subsequently

incorporated. Programs that incorporated singing, playing, and movement have helped

incorporate more kinesthetic activity in musical development. Orff (Warner, 1991),

Laban (1975, 1984) and Dalcroze (Jaques-Dalcroze, 1920/1972; Joseph, 1982) had long

recognized the necessity of developing kinesthetic abilities in musical performance.

Elementary music methods such as Orff Schülwerk, Kodály, Dalcroze (see Choksy,

Abramson, Gillespie, Woods, & York, 2001; Landis & Carter, 1972) and music learning

299
theory (Gordon, 2007; Reynolds et al., 1998) have incorporated different aspects of

visual, auditory, and kinesthetic senses in their programs. Developing these senses in

imagery could be incorporated into all general music programs as well as private

instruction. Music education text series would benefit children’s music development if

they incorporated imagery training in developing these three senses in music play and

performance. This would also require the general music teacher to understand how to

implement specific strategies in valuing imagery, directing specific mental tasks, and

providing time to actively engage in imagery.

Body-Related Imagery

A significant finding included participants’ extensive descriptions of their use of

body-related imagery. Their images of the effects of their feelings and experiences had on

their physical body were remarkable. These singers were sensitive to the many intricacies

of the vocal anatomy as well as how they appeared on stage, whether it was how they

wore their costumes, the position and alignment of their body, or how their movements

affected the believability of their character. Several of the singers mentioned requiring

feedback in these areas so they could incorporate the corresponding kinesthetic feelings

in order to reproduce these effects at will. This was accomplished by soliciting the

responses of a sensitive friend or teacher as well as observing video-recordings of their

practices and performances. These skills in correlating physical appearance, behavior,

and movement with kinesthetic images grew over time and with continued experience.

Providing these types of performance experience and opportunities for feedback in a

vocal training program could assist in building a storehouse of kinesthetic memories and

images to be used appropriately for specific performance purposes. This would build

300
autonomy and effective self-governing behavioral and performance techniques so

necessary in a professional singing career.

Healing Imagery

Healing images included both the overall health of the body and, at times more

importantly, the vocal mechanism. Participants revealed using imagery to assist them

during times of illness, fatigue, and exhaustion. Images of optimal health helped these

singers avert illness and take the necessary precautions so that imminent sickness would

not take root in their body. Many used mental rehearsal to augment physical practice

during times when singing was not possible or advisable. Other singers imagined the

release of all unnecessary tension, particularly in the face, neck, and throat, so that injury

to the vocal mechanism could be avoided. These imagery practices could be taught to

teachers as well as developing and professional musicians. Particular imagery

applications could be made so that singers could maintain excellent health and recover

quickly from illness or injury. As recommended in the sport literature (Morris et al.,

2005), the singer or musician would need to have clear and accurate mental pictures of

the healthy state of the specific or affected area of the body. Anatomical pictures or

videos of the actual movement of that part of the body could assist in imagery practices.

Arousal Modification

These vocalists used imagery extensively to help calm, energize, and ultimately

balance their arousal levels. Most of the participants felt alert and ready to perform on

stage and had alleviated most of their problems with stage fright and maintained a

comfortable and enjoyable level of arousal in performing. The various methods these

singers used to achieve the proper energy balance for optimal performance could be

301
incorporated into vocal training. Imagining the ease and control of breath, calming

anxiety, feeling release of unwanted tension in the body, or brushing the devil off a

shoulder were all employed to align the mental and emotional thoughts and feelings for

optimal performance. Music students of all ages have had to cope with adjusting arousal

levels when they perform. These issues could be addressed and resolved with proper and

appropriate applications of imagery, as put forth in Schmidt’s (1982) attention-arousal set

theory. The help of a sensitive teacher knowledgeable in arousal imagery could easily

alleviate these fears. Teachers could offer their students a variety of imagery applications

from which to choose to help every individual they taught learn to embody the optimal

arousal levels for each particular performance.

Self-Efficacy and Mastery

These participants all described feelings of confidence, mastery, and high levels

of self-efficacy. The process usually began with singers focusing, concentrating, and

controlling attention to the specific performance task and diminishing distractions. The

use of thought stopping and positive self-talk also greatly contributed to strengthening

their confidence, especially during auditions, rehearsals, and performances. More

successful experiences seemed to increase feelings of self-efficacy (Bandura, 1997),

which also positively affected future performances. Providing developing singers with

audition and performance simulations could help them develop the skills in

concentration, the use of self-talk to maintain confidence, and build self-efficacious

feelings related to the actual experiences of performance. Teachers could also assist

students in ways to deal with negative self-criticism and use imagery to strengthen their

302
self-image, thoughts, and feelings that facilitate optimal performances early in their

training.

Character Development

Further implications for developing the many aspects of imagery are directly

involved with character development. Participants used imagery to vocally, physically,

and artistically embody the character of the role or song. Precisely imagining the desired

sound quality, appearance, historical and cultural background, and physical gestures and

movement of the character was critical to achieving optimal performance for these

professionals. All singers highly valued recreating the emotion of the song or character

particularly by using memories of past experiences as described in Craig (1993). More

effort in incorporating these imagery elements in vocal training could be made. Learning

proper vocal technique represents only a small part of the required skills to become a

professional. Often these acting essentials are left for singers to learn much later in their

training. Several participants had training in acting, specifically in Method Acting

(Stanislavsky; 1961; Strasberg & Morphos, 1987), and found that their abilities to

embody their characters increased and deepened immensely, resulting in more expressive

vocal performances. Singers as well as voice teachers could greatly benefit from training

and experience in acting and dramatics. The sooner young singers incorporate these

mental, sensory, and emotional components in their performance, the more adept at

embodying them they could become, which could positively influence their work on

stage and possibly the course of their career.

303
Communication with the Audience

Singers in this study were very articulate and somewhat passionate about their

imagery of communicating with the audience. This seemed to be more pronounced in

professionals with more experience on stage than those with less. It appeared that

vocalists were able to engage in this type of imagery when they had achieved a certain

degree of automaticity with vocal technique, the music to be performed, and a level of

personal comfort in being in front of a live audience. These images varied by the degree

of interaction with which the singers were comfortable, their creative images, and the

quality of communication in which they saw themselves engaging. Several mentioned

very personal, intimate, and deeply spiritual images which directly correlated with their

own understanding of why they had become professional singers. This was an interaction

that seemed to develop over time and was extremely individualized. However, it could be

an area of imagery vocalists could develop either on their own or with the assistance of a

caring and sensitive teacher since these images are relatively personal. These images can

evolve and mature to a significant level, which could positively engage the singer in a

richer experience of singing on stage, promoting further optimal performances.

Using Advanced Technology to Facilitate Imagery Development

Singers regularly reported the benefits they had long enjoyed in using imagery in

their profession, employing different kinds of imagery for many purposes. By using

advanced technology, such as the flexible fibroscope or the laryngeal image biofeedback

(LIB) medical imaging system, singers could view their own vocal mechanism, which

could assist them in clearly imagining the healthy state of their voice. It could also

facilitate understanding the mechanisms involved in ideal sound production, as Cleveland

304
(1989a) found in his singers. Furthermore, teachers could incorporate Internet, graphic,

audio, and video images in providing excellent models and examples of elite singers for

students to emulate. The recent addition of websites such as www.youtube.com and

www.expertvillage.com on the Internet has provided a wealth of examples in singing and

musical performance that has heretofore not been available. In building ideal images of

sound, execution, and variety of images for performers, this could be a potential gold

mine and the use of good models from it could be strongly encouraged. With the

guidance of knowledgeable vocal instructors, the student singers would use modeling in

the effort to find the great qualities of their own voices and avoid the adverse effects of

mimicking inappropriate models.

By understanding the specifics of vocal professionals’ use of imagery content and

purpose, singers, teachers, vocal professors, and music educators of all kinds could assist

their students in incorporating the appropriate applications of imagery in their students’

education, training, practice, and performance. These participants’ responses have

provided evidence that imagery use by athletes and dancers compares quite favorably to

how vocal professionals have used imagery, even if the terminology and semantics may

differ. Teachers, students, and professionals could understand and appropriately use the

types, content, and functions of imagery in addressing vocal, musical, and performance

issues and requirement. These findings could be implemented in vocal programs,

instrumental and ensemble groups, general music education, and even early childhood

music. Therefore, the link between imagery use in athletics and vocal performers, as

exemplified in this study and the proposed singers’ imagery framework, provides a basis

upon which to build this training and methods of implementation.

305
Recommendations for Further Research

This study provided evidence that extensive future research in the area of imagery

in performance is warranted. Specific ideas for further research included nine areas: (a)

replication of this study, (b) imagery in the performance process, (c) imagery for vocal

production, (d) applications in imagery training, (e) musical performers’ use of imagery,

(f) sensory imagery, (g) children’s use of imagery, (h) quantitative studies, (i) music

teachers’ imagery, (j) relationship of flow and imagery, (k) functional equivalence of

performers’ imagery, and (l) cross-disciplinary studies.

Replication of the Study

Recommendations for replicating this study would involve shortening the first

three sections of the singers’ interview guide, as well as adding the changes mentioned

previously in this and the previous chapter. It may be advisable to narrow the field of

participants to a perimeter limiting the years of experience, genre of singing, or voice

classification. Future studies could also be designed to compare imagery of one vocal or

instrumental group with another, perhaps older professionals with younger, basses with

tenors, mezzos with sopranos, concert performers with opera singers, or string with

woodwind soloists to identify differences and commonalities in their use of imagery. It

may prove advantageous to limit the interview to a specific area of imagery use by

specific populations. This may allow for a larger sample and more in depth data may

emerge, providing a more complete understanding of imagery uses to emerge. Acquiring

skill in interviewing and psychology of imagery is recommended. Researchers dedicated

to uncovering deep feelings and images that are sometimes only barely conscious are

needed for this type of research.

306
Imagery in the Performance Process

Several singers described imagery use in their learning sequences and processes.

Researchers could examine individual vocalists or instrumentalists in the methods of

imagery they employ in the performance process, beginning with first learning a piece to

formal presentation on stage. Furthermore, vocalists in this study reported using imagery

primarily during practices and just prior to performances. Investigating the differences in

how musical performers use imagery before, during, and after practice sessions, formal

rehearsals, and performances could reveal important findings that assist musicians in

achieving excellence. Singers’ imagery experience could be investigated by allowing

them to journal either in writing or by audio or video recording their thoughts and images

as they progress from initial practice through the last performance, which was similar to

Kosslyn and colleagues’ (1990) study.

Imagery for Vocal Production

The issue of using metaphors and anatomical imagery in vocal production could

be investigated. More details and uses of metaphors could be compiled along with its

intended purpose, extending the work of Barten (1992, 1998), Fields, (1945), and Freed

(2000). Studies investigating reasons and variables for using metaphor and anatomy in

vocal production could provide understanding in matching the type of image with the

needs of the singer. Further investigation is also needed to as how to most appropriately

train singers in vocal mechanics and anatomy (Chapman, 2006; Vennard, 1968) and

apply this knowledge in their imagery through the senses of vision, hearing, and

kinesthetic feeling. Knowledge and imagery resulting from singers’ training in anatomy

and their personal experiences of viewing their own vocal mechanism through a flexible

307
fibroscope and a laryngeal image biofeedback (LIB) medical imaging system could also

be investigated, extending Cleveland’s (1989a, 1989b) studies.

Applications in Imagery Training

Training programs in imagery interventions could be designed, using the singers’

imagery framework, for different musical performers and tested for effectiveness in

performance experiences. Changes in imagery use over time as a result of training have

been investigated in athletics (Martin et al., 1999), and could be applied and researched in

specific populations of musical performers. Specific training programs for imagery

applications could be devised and studied in various musical teachers and performers

such as solo instrumentalists, small and large ensemble groups, chorus and band, and

many more, extending the work of Connolly and Williamon (2004). Studies could also be

planned in order that imagery use match the musical needs of the performers and be

subsequently tested for perceived effectiveness. Results of these investigations could be

compared with those in sport and dance.

Musical Performers’ Use of Imagery

The four Ws of imagery use could be explored in singers and instrumentalists in a

variety of musical performing genres (e.g., blues, popular, hip-hop, and rap artists).

Imagery in musicians in different cultural settings within the United States could be

examined and compared with other performers. Furthermore, imagery of improvising

musicians, especially jazz artists, could be examined and also compared with imagery

used by musicians that memorized or read music in performance. Imagery of musicians

in other cultures could be investigated on their own terms and compared with those that

are common in the United States.

308
Sensory Imagery

A potentially rich source of information on imagery resides in future investigation

of the various senses. Singers in this study reported primarily using vision, hearing, and

kinesthesia in imagery. However, investigations of other musical performers may reveal

differences, as were reported by Trusheim (1987). Furthermore, imagery in specific

senses could be studied. Kinesthetic imagery in a variety of populations could be trained

through movement applications as developed by various experts (e.g., Dalcroze, Laban,

Gordon, and Orff) and compared with other applications or a control group. The sense of

sight through different skills such as visualizing the musical score or internal images of a

keyboard could be studied as to how it is used in development and training in musical

performance. Further studies could include the interaction of imagery perspective in

sense imagery, particularly in auditory imagery. Continued studies in auditory imagery

and audiation research could extend the work of Gordon (1999, 2003b, 2007) and his

colleagues (Azarra, 1992,1999; Grunow, 2001; Liperote, 2006; Reynolds et al., 1998).

The interaction of different sense imagery such as chromesthesia (Polzella & Kuna,

1981) could also be investigated as well as many other sensory interactions.

Children’s Use of Imagery

Investigating children’s use of imagery could include a wide array of variables.

How imagery is developed, nurtured, and used in children in a variety of age groups

could provide a wealth of information. This could extend beyond the music setting, into

imaginary play, creative endeavors such as fine art, dramatics, and movement, extending

Piaget’s (1962) work. Their use of the different senses in imagery could also be studied

longitudinally in these areas. The use of imagery in the general educational setting could

309
inform teachers as to how to value and develop imagery in a variety of academic

applications.

Quantitative Studies

Musicians’ imagery use could be assessed quantitatively in a variety of ways. The

Functions of Imagery in Music Questionnaire (FIMQ) (Gregg et al., 2008) adapted from

the Sport Imagery Questionnaire (SIQ) (Hall et al., 1998; Hall et al., 2005) could

continue to be improved to better represent various performing musician’s imagery

applications. Sheehan’s (1972) shortened adaptation of Betts’ (1909) vividness tests

could be adapted and administered to singers, teachers, and other musicians. Other

imagery surveys and instruments could be designed to gain a more complete

understanding of imagery uses and abilities in singers and instrumentalists. Specific

terminology would have to be carefully chosen by using a variety of terms (e.g.,

visualize, imagine, audiate, feel, and think about), providing definitions and examples so

that music participants would easily grasp each meaning. Manipulation checks to verify

that participants were imagining what they professed to imagine or what they were

directed to imagine would have to be implemented. Imagery treatments and protocol

would need to be clearly described in order to facilitate replication and comparisons to

related studies (Morris et al., 2005). Findings could be compared among musicians, such

as soloists with ensemble members, conductors with orchestra members, and students

with professionals.

Music Teachers’ Imagery

Music teachers’ knowledge and applications of imagery in the different

educational settings could be investigated. Private and general music educators could be

310
surveyed as to their value for imagery and how they specifically use mental practice,

techniques in audiation, and kinesthesia in their programs. Understanding and

implementation of imagery skills could be assessed in vocal professors. Training

programs for teachers and professors in using imagery and its connections to sport and

dance could be devised, implemented, improved, and reported.

Relationship of Flow and Imagery

Extended areas of investigation could include several issues that emerged from

singers’ responses. Flow experiences in musical performance, especially in relation to

imagery use could be examined. More specifically, researchers could interview

performers in their actual daily imagery experiences, the practice of which was employed

by Csikszentmihalyi (1990). This could be done by a written or recorded journal of

imagery during their normal days as a professional or just their experiences relating to

practicing and performing. Thoughts and images of audience interactions could be

analyzed more closely, especially in relation to performance anxiety and specific

performance practices. Vocalists’ personal reasons and purposes in becoming a

professional in relation to their performance imagery could also be a topic for a research

study. Performers’ spiritual imagery and experiences in relation to flow is also worthy of

future investigation.

Functional Equivalence of Performers’ Imagery

The data suggest that singers were generally convinced of the usefulness of

imagery in benefiting many aspects of their work. These vocalists also found imagery to

be intrinsically connected to their physical, technical, and artistic execution. The recent

developments in medical imaging have enormous potential to further the studies of

311
imagery and performance. Functional equivalence studies (Decety, 1996a, 1996b; Finke,

1989; Jeannerod, 1994, 1995, 2006) could be conducted in musical performance, in

furthering previous studies in this area (e.g., Alemen et al., 2000 Meister et al., 2004;

Halpern & Zatorre, 1999). For instance, singers’ images and thoughts could be mapped

using fMRI techniques and compared with those produced prior and during the actual

execution, in various aspects of performance and practice. The results of these studies

could contribute to this growing body of research in ways that would lend even more

credibility to previous imagery studies.

Cross-Disciplinary Studies

The findings in this investigation of vocal professionals’ imagery use seem to

support crossing the lines of musical and artistic performance and sport research. Singers

used imagery in ways similar to those of athletes (Munroe et al., 2000) and dancers

(Nordin & Cumming, 2005). In this study, music research was guided successfully by

findings in dance and sport psychology. Connections to many other areas emerged in the

data as pertinent to singers’ efforts to achieve optimal performance. Several vocalists had

specific training in acting in which they used imagery to create intentions and emotions

of their character. Other participants were trained in visual art and mentioned drawing

and creating specific graphic images that were created internally. Singing, as in other

professions, was not an entity unto itself, especially with regard to imagery use, but

interacted with other skills chosen, developed, or previously experienced by these

participants. Therefore it is reasonable to suggest that different areas could be researched

and compared as to their uses of imagery and effects on performance. For example, sports

research in kinesthetic imagery could assist singers in body-related imagery; artistic

312
applications for healing purposes in the health professions could inform performers

perhaps as effectively as athletic and musical performers’ healing and arousal imagery

could provide information to the medical and psychology community. The possibilities in

correlating these seemingly diverse areas could be quite promising in revealing important

aspects heretofore unidentified in imagery and performance research.

Summary

This chapter has presented the findings of where, when, what, and why 15 solo

vocal professionals used imagery based on the four Ws framework from sport and dance.

The results in each of these areas were summarized and discussed. Limitations included

sampling of the population, generalization, researcher bias, terminology concerns, self-

report responses, and confidentiality. Implications of this study consisted of issues

involving vocal performers, instrumentalists, vocal pedagogy, imagery content and

purposes, goal attainment, arousal modification, character development, imagery training,

and other issues of imagery in music training and performance. Recommendations for

further research were comprised of replicating this study, imagery of singers,

instrumentalists, performers of other genres of music, ways to extend imagery

investigations, and cross-disciplinary studies.

Exploring the imagery used by solo vocalists served to broaden the findings and

the data pool providing further understanding of performance imagery. These singers

were articulate and willing participants, and contributed to the growing link in connecting

sport psychology and musical performance. The similarities are remarkable and serve to

encourage more of such research.

313
The researcher hopes that this study will be a stimulus for further cross-

disciplinary research among music, sport, dance, psychology and more. Sharing the

extensive benefits in this area has potential advantage for all these fields. Further

qualitative and quantitative research in imagery involving musical and other types of

performance would involve applied and theoretical fields of study and continue to add to

the growing body of knowledge in imagery use in performance.

314
References

Abma, C., Fry, M., Li, Y., & Relyea, G. (2002). Differences in imagery content and

imagery ability between high and low confident track and field athletes. Journal

of Applied Sport Psychology, 14, 67-75.

Agnew, M. (1922a). A comparison of the auditory images of musicians, psychologists,

and children. Psychological Monographs, 31, 268-278.

Ahsen, A. (1972). Eidetic parents test and analysis. New York: Brandon House.

Ahsen, A. (1984). The triple code model for imagery and psychophysiology. Journal of

Mental Imagery, 8, 15-42.

Aleman, A., Nieuwenstein, M. R., Böcker, K. B. E., & de Haan, E. H. F. (2000). Music

training and mental imagery ability. Neuropsychologia, 38, 1664-1668.

Anfara, V. A., Brown, K. M., & Mangione, T. L. 2002). Qualitative analysis on stage:

Making the research process more public. Educational Researcher, 31(7), 28-38.

Annett, J. (1986). On knowing how to do things. In H. Heuer, & C. Fromm (Eds.),

Generation and modulation of action pattern (pp. 187-200). Berlin: Springer.

Annett, J. (1994). The learning of motor skills: Sports science and ergonomics

perspectives. Ergonomics, 37(1), 5-16.

Arieti, S. (1976). Creativity: The magic synthesis. New York: Basic Books.

Averino, S. (1989). The principals and art of singing. Aarhus, Denmark: Forlaget

Intention.

315
Azzara, C. D. (1992). Audiation-based improvisation techniques and elementary

instrumental students’ music achievement. Journal of Research in Music

Education, 41(4), 328-342.

Azzara, C. D. (1999). An aural approach to improvisation. Music Educators Journal,

86(3), 21-25.

Bandura, A. (1977). Social Learning Theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Bandura A. (1982). Self efficacy mechanism in human agency. American Psychologist,

37, 122-147.

Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundation of thought and action: A social cognitive theory.

Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: the exercise of control. New York: W. H. Freeman.

Bandura, A., & Jefferies, R. W. (1973). Role of symbolic coding and rehearsal processes

in observational learning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 26, 122-

130.

Barnes, P. R. (1982). Internal vs. external imagery as a mental preparation when applied

by intermediate league bowlers. Ph.D. dissertation, University of North Texas,

United States -- Texas. Retrieved February 11, 2008, from ProQuest Digital

Dissertations database. (Publication No. AAT 8217613).

Barr, K., & Hall, C. (1992). The use of imagery by rowers. International Journal Sport

Psychology, 23, 243-261.

Barten, S. S. (1992). The language of musical instruction. Journal of Aesthetic Education,

26(2), 53-61.

Barten, S. S. (1998). Speaking of music: The use of motor-affective metaphors in music

316
instruction. Journal of Aesthetic Education 32(2), 89-97.

Bellon, D. (2006) Application of sport psychology to music performance: A study based

on a review of sport psychology literature and selected interviews with

professional musicians. D.M.A. dissertation, Arizona State University, United

States -- Arizona. Retrieved February 11, 2008, from ProQuest Digital

Dissertations database. (Publication No. AAT 3220283).

Benson, H. (1987). Your maximum mind. New York: Avon Books.

Betts, G. H. (1909). The distribution and functions of mental imagery. New York:

Teachers College, Columbia University.

Bonpensiere, L. (1953). New pathways to piano technique: A study of the relations

between mind and body with special reference to piano playing. New York:

Philosophical Library.

Brower, H., & Cooke, J. F. (1996). Great singers on the art of singing. Mineola, NY:

Dover. (Originally published by Frederick A. Stokes, 1920).

Brown, W. E. (1973). Vocal wisdom: Maxims of Giovanni Battista Lamperti. Boston:

Crescendo Press. (Originally published 1931).

Budney, A., Murphy, S. M., & Woolfolk, R. L. (1994). Imagery and motor performance:

What do we really know? In A. A. Sheikh & E. R. Korn, (Eds.), Imagery in sports

and physical performance (pp. 97-120). Amnityville, NY: Baywood.

Caldwell, R., & Wall, J. (2001). Excellence in singing: Multilevel learning and multilevel

teaching. Volume 4: Advancing the technique. Redmond, WA: Caldwell.

317
Callow, N., & Hardy, L. (2001). Types of imagery associated with sport confidence in

netball players of varying skill levels. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 13, 1-

17.

Callow, N., Hardy, L., & Hall, C. (2001). The effects of a motivational general-mastery

imagery intervention on the sport confidence of high-level badminton players.

Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 72, 389-400.

Callow, N., & Waters, A. (2005). The effect of kinesthetic imagery on the sport

confidence of flat-race horse jockeys, Psychology of Sport Exercise, 6, 443-459.

Calmels, C., D’Arripe-Longuefille, F., Fournier, J. F., & Soulard, A. (2003). Competitive

strategies among elite female gymnasts: An exploration of the relative influence

of psychological skills training and natural learning experiences. International

Journal of Sport Exercise Psychology, 1, 327-352.

Carpenter, W. B. (1874). Principles of mental physiology. New York: Appleton.

Carter, M. R. (1993). Mental imagery in the science and art of singing: An inquiry into

imagery use by a select group of professional singers. Ed.D. Dissertation, Rutgers

The State University of New Jersey - New Brunswick, United States. (Publication

No. AAT 9328911).

Castellano, R. L. (1983). An anecdotal inquiry into the nature and function of mental

imagery in the artistic process of creative and interpretive artists. Doctoral

dissertation, Rutgers – the State University of New Jersey. (Publication No. AAT

8325866).

Chapman, J. L. (2006). Singing and teaching singing: A holistic approach to classical

voice. San Diego: Plural.

318
Choksy, L., Abramson, R. M., Gillespie, A. E., Woods, D. & York, F. (2001). Teaching

Music in the Twenty-First Century (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice

Hall.

Clark, L V. (1960). Effect of mental practice on the development of a certain motor skill.

Research Quarterly, 30, 560-569.

Cleveland, T. (1989a). Vocal pedagogy in the twenty-first century: Mental imaging and

the teaching of voice. Journal of the National Association of Teachers of Singing,

45(3), 41-42.

Cleveland, T. (1989b). Vocal pedagogy in the twenty-first century: Student observations

on a new teaching device. Journal of the National Association of Teachers of

Singing, 45(4), 28-31.

Cleveland, T. (1989c). Vocal pedagogy in the twenty-first century: Multisensory

feedback systems in the teaching of voice. Journal of the National Association of

Teachers of Singing, 46(1), 26-28.

Corchin, S., Barthelemy, C., Lejeune, B., Roux, S., & Martineau, J. (1998). Perception of

motion and EEG activity in human adults. Electroencephalography and Clinical

Neurophysiology, 107, 287-295.

Corbin, C. (1972). Mental practice. In W. Morgan (Ed.), Ergogenic aids and muscular

performance. New York: Academic Press.

Coffin, B. (1987). Coffin’s sounds of singing: Principals and applications of vocal

techniques with chromatic vowel chart. Metuchen, NJ: Scarecrow Press,

Coffman, D. D. (1987). The effects of mental practice, physical practice, and aural

knowledge of results on improving piano performance. Ph.D. dissertation,

319
University of Kansas, United States -- Kansas. Retrieved February 11, 2008, from

ProQuest Digital Dissertations database. (Publication No. AAT 8813396).

Coffman, D. D. (1988). Rehearsing in your mind: Review of the mental practice literature

with implications for musicians. Update, 6(2), 5-7.

Coffman, D. (1990). Effects of mental practice, physical practice, and knowledge of

results on piano performance. Journal of Research in Music Education, 38, 187-

196.

Connolly, C. (2002). Mental skills to optimize musical performance. In C. Stevens, D.

Burnahm, G. McPherson, E. Schubert, J. Renwick (Eds.), Proceedings of the 7th

International Conference on Musical Perception and Cognition. Sydney,

Australia: Causal Productions.

Connolly, C., & Williamon, A. (2004). Mental skill training. In A. Williamon (Ed.),

Musical excellence (pp. 100-110). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Corbin, C. B. (1972). Mental practice. In W. P. Morgan (Ed.), Erogenic aids and

muscular performance (pp. 93-118). New York: Academic Press.

Corchin, S., Barthelemy, C., Lejeune, B., Roux, S., & Martineau, J. (1998). Perception of

motion and qEEG activity in human adults. Electroencephalography and Clinical

Neurophysiology, 107, 287-295.

Covey, S. (1998). Seven habits of highly effective people. New York: Simon & Schuster.

Craig, D. (1993). The performer prepares: A guide to song preparation for actors,

singers, and dancers. New York: Applause.

320
Crutchfield, W. (1985, September 24). Pavarotti reflects upon the celebrated voice:

[interview]. New York Times (Late Edition (East Coast)). P. C. 13. Retrieved

August 23, 2008 from New York Times database, (Document ID: 954235151).

Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York:

Harper & Row.

Cumming, J., & Hall, C. (2002a). Deliberate practice: The development of imagery skills

in competitive athletes. Journal of Sport Science, 20(1), 137-145.

Cumming, J., & Hall, C. R. (2002b). Athletes’ use of imagery in the off-season. The

Sport Psychologist, 16(2) 160-172.

Cumming, J., Hall, C. R., & Shambrook, C. (2004). The influence of an imagery

workshop on athletes’ use of imagery. Athletic Insight, 6(1), 52-73.

Dayme, M. D. (1982). Dynamics of the singing voice. New York: Springer-Verlag.

Dayme, M. D. (2005). The performer’s voice: Realizing your vocal potential. New York:

Norton.

Dayme, M. B. (2006). An argument for whole body and holistic approaches to research

and singing. The NATS Journal, 63(1), 59-63.

Decety. J. (1996a). The neurophysiological basis of motor imagery. Behavioral Brain

Research, 77, 45-52.

Decety. J. (1996b). Do imagined and executed actions share the same neural substrate?

Cognitive Brain Research, 3, 87-93.

Decety. J. (2002). Is there such a thing as functional equivalence between imagined

observed and executed action? In A. N. Meltzoff, & W. Prinz (Eds.), The

321
Imitative mind: Development, evolution, and brain bases (pp. 291-310). New

York: Cambridge University Press.

Decety, M. (2006). A neural basis of imitation. In S. J. Rogers, & J. H. G. Williams

(Eds.), Imitation of the social mind: Autism and typical development (pp. 251-

278). New York: Guilford Press.

Denis, M. (1985). Visual imagery and education and the use of mental practice in the

development of motor skills. Canadian Journal of Applied Sport Science, 10, 4S-

16S.

Deutsch, D. (Ed.). (1999). The psychology of music (2nd ed.) New York: Academic Press.

Driskell, J. E., Cooper, C., & Moran, A. (1994). “Does mental practice enhance

performance?” Journal of Applied Psychology, 79(4), 481-492.

Emmons, S., & Sonntag, S. (2002). The art of the song recital. Prospect Heights, IL:

Waveland Press.

Emmons, S., & Thomas, A. (1998). Power performance for singers: Transcending the

barriers. New York: Oxford University Press.

Emmons, S. & Thomas, A. (2008). Voice pedagogy: Understanding performance anxiety.

The NATS Journal, 64(4), 461-465.

anEpstein, M. L. (1980). The relationship of mental imagery and mental rehearsal to

performance of a motor task. Journal of Sport Psychology, 2, 211-220.

Fadiga, L., Fogassi. L., Pavesi, G., & Rizzolatti, G. (1995). Motor facilitation during

action observation: A magnetic stimulation study. Journal of Neurophysiology,

73, 2608-2611.

322
Farah, J. (1984). The neurological basis of mental imagery: A componential analysis.

Cognition, 18, 245-272.

Feltz, D. L. (1984). Self-efficacy as a cognitive mediator of athletic performance, In W.

F. Straub & J. M. Williams (Eds.), Cognitive sport psychology (pp. 191-198).

Lansing, NY: Sport Science Associates.

Feltz, D. L. & Landers, D. M. (1983). The effects of mental practice on motor skill

learning and performance: A meta-analysis. Journal of Sport Psychology, 5, 25-

57.

Fenker, R. M., & Lambiotte, J. G. (1987). A performance enhancement program for a

college football team: One incredible season. The Sport Psychologist, 1, 224-236.

Ferrier, W. (1955). The life of Kathleen Ferrier. London: H. Hamilton.

Fields, V. A. (1947). Training the singing voice. Morningside Heights, NY: King’s

Crown Press.

Fields, V. A. (1972). How the mind governs voice. The NATS Bulletin, 22(2), 2-10.

Fish, L., Hall, C., & Cumming, J. (2004). Investigating the use of imagery by elite ballet

dancers. AVANTE, 10(3), 26-39.

Finke, R. A. (1980). Levels of equivalence of mental images and perception.

Psychological Review, 87, 113-132.

Finke, R. A. (1985). Theories relating mental imagery to perception. Psychological

Bulletin, 98, 236-259.

Finke, R. A. (1989). Principles of mental imagery. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

323
Finke, R. A., & Shephard, R. N. (1986) Visual functions of mental imagery. In K. R.

Boff, L. Kaufman, & J. P. Thomas (Eds.), Handbook of perception and human

performance (pp. 37-55). New York: Wiley.

Freed, D. C. (2000). Imagery in early twentieth-century American vocal pedagogy.

Journal of Singing – The Official Journal of the National Association of Teachers

of Singing, 56(4), 5-12.

Freymuth, M. S. (1999). Mental practice and imagery for musicians: A practical guide

for optimizing practice time, enhancing performance and preventing injury.

Boulder, CO: Integrated Musician’s Press.

Gallwey, W. T. (1974). The inner game of tennis. New York: Random House.

Galton, F. (1880). Statistics of mental imagery. Mind, 5, 301-318.

Galton, F. (1883). Inquiries into human faculty and its development. New York:

Macmillan

Galton, F. (1907). Inquiries into human faculty and its development. London: Dent.

Galvan, M. G. (1992) Kinesthetic imagery and mental practice: Teaching strategies for

the piano principal. D.M.A. dissertation, University of Miami, United States --

Florida. Retrieved February 11, 2008, from ProQuest Digital Dissertations

database. (Publication No. AAT 9239657).

Gammage, K. L., Hardy, J., & Hall, C. R. (2001). A description of self-talk in exercise.

Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 2(4), 233-247.

Garcia, M., & Paschke, D. V. (1975). A complete treatise on the art of singing: Complete

and unabridged. New York: Da Capo Press.

Gawain, S. (1987). Creative visualization. San Rafael, CA: New World Library.

324
Gawain, S. (2002). Creative visualization. Navato, CA: New World Library.

Gordon, E. E. (1976). Learning sequences and patterns in music. Chicago: GIA.

Gordon, E. E. (1999). All about audiation and music aptitudes. Music Educators Journal,

86(2), 41-44.

Gordon, E. E. (2003a). Learning sequences in music. Chicago: GIA.

Gordon, E. E. (2003b). A music learning theory for newborn and young children.

Chicago: GIA.

Gordon, R. (1949). An investigation into some of the factors that favour the formation of

stereotyped images. British Journal of Psychology, 39, 156-167.

Goss, S., Hall, C. R., Buckoltz, E., & Fishburne, G. (1986). Imagery ability and the

acquisition and retention of movements. Memory and Cognition, 14, 469-477.

Gould, D., Dieffenbach, K., & Moffett, A. (2002). Psychological characteristics and their

development in Olympic champions. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 14,

172-204.

Gould, D., Eklund, R. C., & Jackson, S. A. (1992). 1988 Olympic wrestling excellence: I.

Mental preparation, precompetitive cognition and affect. The Sport Psychologist,

6, 358-362.

Gould, D., Weinberg, R., & Jackson, A. (1980). Mental preparation strategies, cognitions

and strength performance, Journal of Sport Psychology, 2, 329-339.

Gould, D., Weiss, M. & Weinberg, R. (1981). Psychological Characteristics of

Successful and Non-Successful Big Ten Wrestlers. Journal of Sport Psychology,

3, 69-81.

Green B., & Gallwey, W. T. (1986). The inner game of music. Garden City: Doubleday.

325
Green, D. (2002). Performing success: Performing your best under pressure. New York:

Routledge.

Green, L. (1992). The use of imagery in the rehabilitation of injured athletes. The Sport

Psychologist, 6, 416-428.

Gregg, J. W. (1998). On requirements for a singing career. The NATS Journal, 54(5), 51-

52.

Gregg, M., Clark, T. W., & Hall, C. R. (2008). Seeing the sound: An exploration of the

use of mental imagery by classical musicians. Musicae Scientae, 12(2), 321-247.

Gregg, M., Hall, C., & Hanton, S. (2007). Perceived effectiveness of heptathletes’ mental

imagery. Journal of Sport Behavior, 30(4), 398-414.

Gregg, M., Hall, C., & Nederhoff, E. (2005). The imagery ability, imagery use, and

performance relationship. Sport psychologist, 19(1), 93-99.

Grunow, R. F. (2001). Jump right in: The instrumental series – Teacher’s guide for winds

and percussion. Chicago: GIA.

Günter, H. (1992a). Mental concepts in singing: A psychological approach, Part I. The

NATS Journal, 48(5), 4-10, 46.

Günter, H. (1992b). Mental concepts in singing: A psychological approach, Part II. The

NATS Journal, 48(7), 4-7.

Guralnik, D. B. (Ed.). (1984). Webster’s new world dictionary of the American language

(2nd College Ed.). New York: Simon & Schuster.

Hackfort, D., & Munzert, J. (2005). Mental Simulation. In D. Hackfort, J. L. Duda, & R.

Ludor (Eds), Handbook of research in applied sport and exercise psychology:

326
International perspectives (pp. 3-18). Morgantown, WV: University of West

Virginia.

Hale, B. D. (1982). The effects of internal and external imagery on muscular and ocular

concomitants, Journal of Sports Psychology, 4, 379-387.

Hall, C. R. (1998). Measuring imagery abilities and imagery use. In J. Duda (Ed.),

Advances in sport and exercise psychology measurement (pp. 165-172).

Morgantown, WV: Fitness Information Technology.

Hall, C. R. (2001). Imagery in sport and exercise. In R. N. Singer, H. A. Hausenblas, &

C. M. Janell (Eds.), Handbook of sport psychology (2nd ed., pp. 529-549). New

York: Wiley.

Hall, C. R. & Buckolz, E. (1991). The effect of an imagery training program on imagery

ability, imagery use, and figure skating performance. Journal of Applied Sport

Psychology, 3(2), 109-125.

Hall, C. R., Mack, D., Paivio, A., & Hausenblas, H. (1998). Imagery use by athletes:

Development of the Sport Imagery Questionnaire. International Journal of Sport

Psychology, 29, 73-89.

Hall, C. R., & Martin, K. A. (1997). Measuring movement imagery abilities: A revision

of the movement imagery questionnaire. Journal of Mental Imagery, 21, 143-

154.

Hall, C. R., Moore, J., Annett, J., & Rodgers, W. (1997). Recalling demonstrated and

guided movements using imaginary and verbal rehearsal strategies. Research

Quarterly for Exercise & Sport, 68(2), 136-144.

327
Hall, C. R., Rodgers, W., & Barr, K. (1990). The use of imagery by athletes in selected

sports. The Sport Psychologist, 4, 1-10.

Hall, C. R., Schmidt, D., Durand, M., & Buckoltz, E. (1994). Imagery and motor skills

acquisition. In A. A. Sheikh & E. R. Korn (Eds.), Imagery in sports and physical

performance (pp. 121-134). Amityville, NY: Baywood.

Hall, C. R., Stevens, D. E., & Paivio, A. (2005) Sports Imagery Questionnaire: Test

Manuel. Morgantown, WV: Fitness Information Technology.

Hallam, S. (1997). What do we know about practicing? Toward a model synthesizing the

research literature. In H. Jorgensen, & A. C. Lehmann (Eds.), Does practice make

perfect? Current theory and research on instrumental music practice (pp. 179-

231). Oslo, Norway: Norges Musickkhørgskole.

Halpern, A. R., & Zatorre, R. J. (1999). When that tune runs through your head: A PET

investigation of auditory imagery for familiar melodies. Cerebral Cortex, 9, 697-

704.

Hanin, Y. L., & Stambulova, N. B. (2002). Metaphoric description of performance states:

An application of the IZOF model. The Sport Psychologist, 16(4), 396-415.

Hanrahan, C., & Salmela, J. H. (1990). Dance images: Do they really work or are we just

imagining things? Journal of Physical Education, Recreation, and Dance, 61, 18-

21.

Hanrahan, C., Tertreau, B., & Sarrazin, C. (1995). Use of imagery while performing

dance movement. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 26, 18-30.

Hanrahan, C., & Vergeer, I. (2000). Multiple uses of mental imagery by professional

modern dancers. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 20, 321-255.

328
Hardy, J. (2005). Speaking clearly: A critical review of the self-talk literature.

Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 7, 81-97.

Hardy, L. (1997). Three myths about applied consultancy work. Journal of Applied Sport

Psychology, 9, 277-294.

Hardy, L., & Callow, N. (1999). Efficacy of external and internal visual imagery

perspectives for the enhancement of performance on tasks in which form is

important. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 21, 95-112.

Hardy, L., Gammage, K., & Hall, C. R. (2001). A descriptive study of athlete self-talk.

The Sport Psychologist, 15, 306-318.

Harris, D. V. (1986). Relaxation and energizing techniques for regulation of arousal. In J.

M. Williams (Ed.), Applied sport psychology: Personal growth to peak

performance (pp. 185-207). Mountain View CA: Mayfield.

Harris, D. V., & Robinson, W. J. (1986). The effects of skill level on EMG activity

during internal and external imagery. Journal of Sport Psychology, 8, 105-111.

Hays, K. (2002). The enhancement of performance excellence among performing artists.

Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 14, 299-312.

Hecker, J. E., & Kaczor, L. M. (1988). Applications of imagery theory to sport

psychology: Some preliminary findings. Journal of Sport and Exercise

Psychology, 10, 363-373.

Highben Z., & Palmer, C. (2004). Effects of auditory and motor mental practice in

memorized piano performance. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music

Education, 159, 59-65.

329
Highlen, P. & Bennett, B. (1979). Psychological characteristics of successful and non-

successful elite wrestlers. Journal of Sport Psychology, 1, 123-137.

Hines, J. (1982). Great singers on great singing. Garden City, NJ: Doubleday.

Hinshaw, K. E. (1991). The effects of mental practice on motor skill performance:

Critical evaluation and meta-analysis. Imagination, Cognition and Personality,

11(1), 3-35.

Holmes, P. (2005). Imagination in practice: a study of the integrated roles of

interpretation, imagery and technique in the learning and memorization processes

of two experienced solo performers. British Journal of Music Education, 22, 217-

235.

Holmes, P. S., & Collins, D. (2002). Functional equivalence solutions for problems with

motor imagery. In I. M. Cockerill (Ed.), Solutions in sport psychology (pp. 120-

140). London: Morgan.

Holt, R. R. (1964). Imagery: The return of the ostracized. American Psychologist, 19,

254-264.

Horn, T. S. (2008). Advances in sport psychology. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Horne, M., & Scovell, J. (1983). Marilyn Horne, my life. New York: Atheneum.

Horowitz, M. J. (1978). Image formation and cognition. New York: Appleton-Century-

Crofts.

Ievleva, L., & Orlick, T. (1991). Mental links to enhanced healing. Sport Psychologist,

5(1), 25-40.

Isaac, A. R. (1992). Mental practice: Does it work in the field. The Sport Psychologist, 6,

192-198.

330
Isaac, A. R., Marks, D. F., & Russell, E. (1986). An instrument for assessing imagery of

movements: The Vividness of Movement Imagery Questionnaire (VMIQ).

Journal of Physiology, 96, 23-30.

Jackson, S. A., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1999). Flow in sports. Champaign, IL: Human

Kinetics.

Jacobson, E. (1927). Action currents from muscular contractions during conscious

processes. Science, 66(1713), 403.

Jacobson, E. (1929). Progressive relaxation: A physiological and clinical investigation of

muscular states and their significance in psychology and medical practice.

Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Jacobson, E. (1930a). Electrical measures of neuromuscular states during mental

activities: Part 1. Imagination of movement involving skeletal muscle. American

Journal of Physiology, 91, 567-608.

Jacobson, E. (1930b). Electrical measures of neuromuscular states during mental

activities: Part 2. Imagination and recollection of various muscular acts. American

Journal of Physiology, 94, 27-34.

Jacobson, E. (1930c). Electrical measures of neuromuscular states during mental

activities: Part 3. Visual imagination and recollection. American Journal of

Physiology, 95, 694-702.

Jacobson, E. (1930d). Electrical measures of neuromuscular states during mental

activities: Part 4. Evidence of contraction of muscles during imagination.

American Journal of Physiology, 95, 703-712.

331
Jacobson, E. (1931). Electrical measures of neuromuscular states during mental activities:

Part 5. Variation of specific muscles contracting during imagination. American

Journal of Physiology, 96, 115-121.

Jacobson, E. (1957). You must relax: A practical method of reducing the strains of

modern living. New York: McGraw-Hill.

James, W. (1890). Principles of psychology. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

Jaques-Dalcroze, E. (1972). Eurhythmics, art and education. New York: Benjamin Blom

(originally published in 1920).

Jastrow, J. A. (1892). Study of involuntary movements. American Journal of Psychology,

4(3), 398-407.

Jeannerod, M. (1994). The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and

imagery. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 187-202.

Jeannerod, M. (1995). Mental imagery in the motor context. Neuropsychologia, 33, 1419-

1432.

Jeannerod, M. (1999). Visuomotor channels: Their integration in goal-directed

prehension. Human Movement Science, 18(2), 201-218.

Jeannerod, M. (2006). Motor cognition: What actions tell the self. New York: Oxford.

Johnson, B., & Christensen, L. (2004). Educational research, quantitative, qualitative,

and mixed approaches (2nd ed.). Boston: Pearson.

Jørrgensen, H., & Lehmann, A. C. (Eds.). (1997). Does practice make perfect? Current

theory and research on instrumental music practice. Oslo, Norway: Norges

Musickkhørgskole.

332
Joseph, A. S. (1982). A Dalcroze Eurhythmics approach to music learning in

kindergarten through rhythmic movement, ear-training and improvisation.

Dissertation Abstracts International, 44(02), 230. (AAT No. 8314549).

Jowdy, D. P., Murphy, S. M., & Durtschi, S. (1989). An assessment of the use of imagery

by elite athletes: Athlete, coach and psychologist perspectives [Report]. Colorado

Springs, CO: United States Olympic Committee. IOC.

Karpinski, G. (2000). Aural skills acquisition: The development of listening, reading, and

performing skills in college-level musicians. New York: Oxford University Press.

Klinger, E. (1981). Imagery, concepts, results, and applications. Minneapolis, MN:

Plenum Press.

Klonoski, E. (2003). Connecting the inner ear and the voice. Choral Journal, 44(3), 35-

40.

Kohut, D. L. (1992). Musical performance: Learning theory and pedagogy. Chicago:

Stipes.

Korn, E. R. (1984). Mental imagery in enhancing performance: Theory and practical

exercises. In A. A. Sheikh, & E. R. Korn (Eds.), Imagery in sports and physical

performance (pp. 201-230). Amityville, NY: Baywood.

Kosslyn, S. M. (1994). Image and brain: The resolution of the imagery debate.

Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

Kosslyn, S. M, (2001). Visual consciousness. In P. G. Grossenbacher (Ed.), Finding

consciousness in the brain: A neurocognitive approach (pp. 79-103). Amsterdam,

Netherlands: John Benjamins.

333
Kosslyn, S. M., Ganis, G., & Thompson, W. L. (2001). Neural foundations of imagery,

Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 635-642.

Kosslyn, S. M., & Koenig, O. (1992) Wet mind: The new cognitive neuroscience. New

York: Free Press.

Kosslyn, S. M., Seger, C., Pani, J. R., & Hillger, L. A. (1990). When is imagery used in

everyday life? A diary study. Journal of Mental Imagery, 14, 131-152.

Kvale, S. (1996). Interviews: An introduction to qualitative research interviewing.

Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Kvale, S., & Brinkmann, S. (2009). InterViews: Learning the craft of qualitative research

interviewing. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Laban, R. (1975). Mastery of movement. London: MacDonald and Evans, Limited.

Laban, R. (1984). A vision of dynamic space. Philadelphia, PA: The Falmer Press.

Lacaille, N., Whipple, N., & Koestner, R. (2005). Reevaluating the benefits of

performance goals: The relation of goal type to optimal performance for

musicians and athletes. Medical Problems of Performing Artists, 20(1), 11-16.

LaFosse, L. (1973). Teaching the art of practicing, The Instrumentalist, 23(5), 42-43.

Landers, D. M. (1980). The arousal-performance relationship revisited. Research

Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 51, 77-90.

Landis, B., & Carder, P. (1972). The eclectic curriculum in American music education:

contributions of Dalcroze, Kodaly, and Orff. Washington: Music Educators

National Conference.

334
Lane, J. F. (1980). Improving athletic performance through visuo-motor behavior

rehearsal. In R. M. Suinn (Ed.), Psychology in sports: Methods and applications

(pp. 316-320). Minneapolis, MN: Burgess.

Lang, P. J. (1977) Imagery in therapy: An information-processing analysis of fear.

Behavior Therapy, 8, 862-886.

Lang, P. J. (1979a). A bio-informational theory of emotional imagery. Psychophysiology,

16, 495-512.

Lang, P. J. (1979b). Emotional imagery and visceral control. In R J. Gatchel & K. P.

Price (Eds.), Clinical applications of biofeedback: Appraisal and status (pp. 12-

27). New York: Pergamon.

Lang, P. J., Melamed, B. G., & Hart, J. H. (1970). A psychophysiological analysis of

fear modification using an automated desensitization procedure. Journal of

Abnormal Psychology, 76, 220-234.

Leech, A. L., & Onwuegbuzie, A. J. (2007). An array of qualitative data analysis tools: A

call for data analysis triangulation. School Psychology Quarterly, 22(4), 557-584.

Lehmann, L. (1960). How to sing. New York: MacMillan.

Lehmann, L. (1985). More than singing: The interpretation of songs. New York: Dover

(Originally published: New York: Boosey & Hawkes, 1945).

Leyerle, W. (1986). Vocal development through organic imagery. (2nd ed.). Geneseo,

NY: Leyerle Publications.

Lim, S., & Lippman, L. (1991). Mental practice and memorization of piano music.

Journal of General Psychology, 118, 21-30.

Lincoln, Y. S., & Guba, E. G. (1985). Naturalistic inquiry. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

335
Lindauer, M. S. (1983). Imagery and the arts. In A. A. Sheikh (Ed.), Imagery: Current

theory, research, and application (pp. 468-506). New York: Wiley.

Liperote, K. (2006). Audiation for Beginning Instrumentalists: Listen, Speak, Read,

Write. Music Education Journal, 13(9), 46-52.

Lo, L. N. (1976). The effect of visual memory training on the ability to memorize music

within class piano instruction. (Doctoral dissertation, Indiana University)

Dissertation Abstracts International, 38, 538A.

Mace, R. D., Eastman, C., & Carroll, C. (1987) The effects of stress inoculation training

on gymnastics’ performance on the pommel horse: A case study. Behavioral

Psychotherapy, 15, 272-279.

MacIntyre, T., & Moran, A. (1996). Imagery use among canoeists: A worldwide survey

of novice, intermediate, and elite slalomists. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology,

8, S132.

MacKay, C. G. (1981). The problem of rehearsal or mental practice. Journal of Motor

Behavior, 13, 274-285.

MacPherson, G. E. (1997). Cognitive strategies and skill acquisition in musical

performance. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 133, 64-

71.

Mahoney, M. J., & Avener, M. (1977). Psychology of the elite athlete: An exploratory

study. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 1, 135-141.

Maltz, M. (1960). Psycho-cybernetics: A new way to get more living out of life.

Englewood Cliff, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Marks, D. F. (1973). Visual imagery differences in recall pictures. British Journal of

336
Psychology, 64, 14-24.

Marks, D. F. (1977). Imagery and consciousness: A theoretical review from an individual

differences perspective. Journal of Mental Imagery, 1, 275-290.

Martin, D. W. (1952). Do you auralize? The Journal of the Acoustical Society of

America, 24, 416.

Martin, K. A., Moritz, S. E., & Hall, C. R. (1999). Imagery use in sport: A literature

review and applied model. The Sport Psychologist, 13, 245-268.

Maslow, A. (1954). Motivation and Personality. New York: Harper.

Maslow, A. (1968). Toward a psychology of being (2nd ed.). New York: Van Norstrand.

Maslow, A. (1970). Religion, values and peak experiences. New York: Viking Press.

Maslow, A. (1971). The farther reaches of human nature. New York: Viking Press.

Maxwell, J. A. (1992). Understanding and validity in qualitative research. Harvard

Education Review, 62, 279-300.

McKinney, J. (1994). Diagnosis and correction of vocal faults: A manual for teachers of

singing and for choir directors (Revised ed.). Nashville: Genevox Music Group.

Meichenbaum, D. H. (1977). Cognitive behavior modification: An integrative approach.

New York: Plenum.

Meichenbaum, D. H. (1985). Stress inoculation training. New York: Pergamon Press.

Meister, I. G., Krings, T., Foltys, H., Boroojerdi, B., Müller, M., Tüpper, R., & Thron, A.

(2004). Playing piano in the mind: An fMRI study on music imagery and

performance in pianists. Cognitive Brain Research, 19, 219-228.

Miles, M. B., & Huberman, A. M. (1994). Qualitative data analysis: A sourcebook of

new methods (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

337
Miller, R. (1996). On the art of singing. New York: Oxford University Press.

Mills, K. D., Munroe, K. J., & Hall, C. R. (2000-2001). The relationship between

imagery and self-efficacy in competitive athletes. Imagination, Cognition, and

Personality, 20(1), 33-39.

Monsma, E. V., & Overby, L. Y. (2004). The relationship between imagery and

competitive anxiety in ballet auditions. Journal of Dance Medicine & Science,

8(1), 11-18.

Moorcroft, L. (2002). Embracing alternative methodologies: Science and imagery in the

teaching and performance of singing. In Stevens, C., Burnham, McPherson, G.,

Schubert, E., & Renwick (Eds.). Proceedings of the 7th International Conference

on Music Perception and Cognition, Sydney, 2002. Adelaid: Causal Productions.

Moran, A. (2004). Sport and exercise psychology: A critical introduction. Hove, East

Sussex, UK: Routledge.

Moritz, S. E., Hall, C. R., Martin, K. A., & Vadocz, E. (1996). What are confident

athletes imaging?: An examination of image content. The Sport Psychologist, 10,

171-179.

Morris, T. (1997). Psychological skills training in sport: An overview. (2nd ed.). Leeds,

UK: National Coaching Foundation.

Morris, T., Spittle, M., & Watt, A. (2005). Imagery in sport. Champaign, IL: Human

Kinetics.

Moyer, K. E. (1992). A survey of singers: Is mental imagery used in the

conceptualization of pitch and vowel? M.M. dissertation, University of North

338
Texas, United States -- Texas. Retrieved February 11, 2008, from ProQuest

Digital Dissertations database. (Publication No. AAT 1350991).

Mumford, B. & Hall, C. R. (1985). The effects of internal and external imagery on

performing figures in figure skating. Canadian Journal of Applied Sport Sciences,

10(4), 171-177.

Munroe, K. J., Giacobbi, P. R., Jr., Hall, C., & Weinberg, P. (2000). The four Ws of

imagery use: Where, when, why, and what. The Sport Psychologists, 14, 119-137.

Munroe, K. J., Hall, C. R., Simms, S., & Weinberg, R. (1998). The influence of type of

sport and time of season on athletes’ use of imagery. The Sport Psychologist, 12,

440-449.

Munroe-Chandler, K. J., & Hall, C. R. (2004). Enhancing the collective efficacy of a

soccer team through motivational general-mastery imagery. Imagination,

Cognition, and Personality, 24(1), 51-67.

Murphy, S. M. (1985). Emotional imagery and its effects on strength and fine motor skill

performance. Ph.D. dissertation, Rutgers The State University of New Jersey -

New Brunswick, United States. Retrieved February 11, 2008, from ProQuest

Digital Dissertations database. (Publication No. AAT 8604844).

Murphy, S. M., & Jowdy, D. P. (1992). Imagery and mental practice. In T. S. Horn (Ed.),

Advances in sport psychology (pp, 221-250). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Murphy, S. M., & Martin, K. A. (2002). The use of imagery in sport. In T. Horn (Ed.),

Advances in sport psychology (2nd ed., pp. 405-439). Champaign, IL: Human

Kinetics.

339
Murphy, S. M., Nordin, S. M., & Cumming, J. (2008). Imagery in sport, exercise, and

dance. In T. Horn (Ed.), Advances in Sport Psychology (3rd ed., pp. 297-324).

Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Murphy, S. M., Woolfolk, R. L., & Budney, A. J. (1988). The effects of emotive imagery

on strength performance. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 10(3), 334-

345.

Neisser, U. (1972). Changing conceptions of imagery. In P. W. Sheehan (Ed.), The

function and nature of imagery. New York: Academic Press.

Neisser, U. (1976). Cognition and reality: Principles and implications of cognitive

psychology. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.

Nicklaus, J., & Bowden, K. (1974). Golf my way. New York: Simon & Schuster.

Nordin, S. M., & Cumming, J. (2005). Professional dancers describe their imagery:

Where, when, what, why, and how. The Sport Psychologist, 19, 395-416.

Onwuegbuzie, A. J. (2003). Expanding the framework of internal and external validity in

quantitative research. Research in the Schools, 10(1), 71-89.

Orlick, T. (1990). In pursuit of excellence (2nd ed.). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Orlick, T., & Parrington, J. (1988). Mental links to excellence. Sport Psychology, 2, 105-

130.

Overby, L. Y. (1990). The use of imagery by dance teachers: development and

implementation of two research instruments. Journal of Physical Education,

Recreation, and Dance, 61, 24-27.

340
Overby, L. Y., Hall, C., & Haslam, I. (1998). A comparison of imagery used by dance

teachers, figure skating coaches, and soccer coaches. Imagination, Cognition and

Personality, 17, 323-237.

Oxendine, J. B. (1969). Effect of mental and physical practice on the learning of three

motor skills. Research Quarterly, 40, 755-763.

Oxendine, J. B. (1980). Emotional arousal and motor performance. In R. Suinn (Ed.),

Psychology in sports: Methods and applications (pp. 103-111). Minneapolis:

Burgess.

Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. New York: Holt.

Paivio, A. (1985). Cognitive and motivational functions of imagery in human

performance. Canadian Journal of Applied Sports Science, 10, 22S-28S.

Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: A dual coding approach. Oxford: Oxford

University Press.

Patton, M. Q. (2002). Qualitative evaluation and research methods. (3rd ed.). Thousand

Oaks, CA: Sage.

Perry, C., & Morris, T. M. (1995). Mental imagery in sport. In T. M., Morris & J.

Summers (Eds.), Sport psychology: Theory, applications and issues (pp. 339-

385). Brisbane, Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.

Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton.

Polzella, D. J., & Kuna, A. H. (1981). Chromesthetic responses to the music of G. F.

Handel. In E. Klinger (Ed.), Imagery: Concepts, Results, and Applications (pp.

165-173). New York: Plenum.

341
Powell, G. (1973). Negative and positive mental practice in motor skill acquisition.

Perceptual and Motor Skills, 37, 312.

Raiport, G. (1988). Red gold: Peak performance techniques of the Russian and East

German Olympic victors. Los Angeles: J. P. Tarcher.

Reed, C. L. (2002). Chronometric comparisons of imagery to action: Visualizing versus

physically performing springboard dives. Memory and Cognition, 30(8), 1169-

1178.

Reid, C. L. (1983). A dictionary of vocal terminology: An analysis. New York: Joseph

Patelson Music House.

Reynolds, A. M. (1995). An investigation of the movement responses performed by

children 18 months to three years of age and their caregivers to rhythm chants in

duple and triple meters. Dissertation Abstracts International, 56(04), 186. (AAT

No. 9527531)

Reynolds, A. M., Valerio, W. H., Bolton, B. M., Taggart, C. C., & Gordon, E. E. (1998).

Music play. Chicago: GIA.

Richardson, A. (1967a). Mental practice: A review and discussion. Part 1. Research

Quarterly, 38, 95-107.

Richardson, A. (1967b). Mental practice: A review and discussion. Part 2. Research

Quarterly, 38, 264-273.

Richardson, A. (1969). Mental Imagery. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Richardson, A. (1983). Imagery: Definition and types. In A. A. Sheikh (Ed.), Imagery:

Current theory, research, and application (pp. 3-42). New York: John Wiley &

Sons.

342
Richardson, A. (1994). Individual differences in imaging: Their measurement, origins

and consequences. Amityville, NY: Baywood.

Ristad, E. (1982). Soprano on her head. Moab, UT: Real People Press.

Rodgers, W. M., Hall, C. R., & Buckolz. E. (1991). The effect of an imagery training

program on imagery ability, imagery use, and figure skating performance. Journal

of Applied Sport Psychology, 3, 109-125.

Rosenberg, H. S. (1987). Creative drama and imagination. New York: Holt, Reinhart and

Winston.

Rosenberg, H. S., & Trusheim, W. (1989). Creative Transformations: How visual artists,

musicians, and dancers use mental imagery in their work. In J. E. Shore, P. Robin,

J. A. Connella & M. Wolpin (Eds.), Imagery: Current perspectives (pp. 55-76).

New York: Plenum Press.

Ross, S. L. (1985a). The effectiveness of mental practice in improving the performance

of college trombonists. Ph.D. dissertation, Northwestern University, United States

-- Illinois. Retrieved February 11, 2008, from ProQuest Digital Dissertations

database. (Publication No. AAT 8511853).

Ross, S. L. (1985b). The effectiveness of mental practice in improving the performance

of college trombonists. Journal of Research in Music Education, 33, 221-230.

Rotella, R. J., Gansneder, B., Ojala, D., & Billing, J. (1980). Cognitions and coping

strategies of elite skiers: An exploratory study of young developing athletes.

Journal of Sport Psychology, 2, 350-354.

343
Rubin-Rabson, R. K. (1941). Studies in the psychology of memorizing piano music, VI:

A comparison of two forms of mental rehearsal and keyboard overlearning.

Journal of Educational Psychology, 32, 593-602.

Ruiz, M. C., & Hanin, Y. L. (2004). Metaphoric description and individualized emotion

profiling of performance states in top karate athletes. Journal of Applied Sport

Psychology, 16, 258-273.

Rushall, B. S. (1988) Covert modeling as a procedure for altering an elite athlete’s

psychological state. The Sport Psychologist, 2, 131-140.

Ryan, E. D., & Simons, J. (1982). Efficacy of mental imagery in enhancing mental

rehearsal of motor skills. Journal of Sport Psychology, 4, 41-51.

Sackett R. S. (1934). The influence of symbolic rehearsal upon the retention of a maze

habit. Journal of General Psychology, 10, 376-395.

Sackett, R. S. (1935). The relationship between the amount of symbolic rehearsal and

retention of a maze habit. Journal of General Psychology, 13, 113-128.

Salmon, J., Hall, C., & Haslam, I. (1994). The use of imagery by soccer players, Journal

of Applied Sport Psychology, 6, I116-133.

Schmidt, R. A. (1982). Motor control and learning. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Schmidt, R. A. (1987). Motor control and learning (2nd ed.). Champaign, IL: Human

Kinetics.

Schwartz, G. (1984). Psychophysiology of imagery and healing: A systems perspective.

In A. Sheikh (Ed.), Imagination and healing (pp. 38-50). Farmingdale, NY:

Baywood.

Schwartz, G. & Russek, L. G. (2003). Imagery, love, and health: How dynamic energy

344
systems theory can integrate conventional and complementary medicine. In A.

Sheikh (Ed.), Healing images: The role of imagination in health (pp. 91-14).

Amityville, NY: Baywood.

Seashore, C. E. (1919). The psychology of musical talent. Boston: Silver, Burdett and

Company.

Seashore, C. E. (1939). New approaches to the science of voice. Scientific Monthy, 49,

340.

Seashore, C. E. (1967). Psychology of music. New York: Dover. (Originally published by

McGraw-Hill, 1938)

Shaw, W. A. (1938). The distribution of muscular action potentials during imagining.

Psychological Recreation, 2, 195-216.

Sheehan, P. W. (1972). The function and nature of imagery. New York: Academic Press.

Sheikh, A. A. (Ed.) (1983). Imagery: Current theory, research, and application. New

York: Wiley.

Sheikh, A. (Ed.). (2003). Healing images: The role of imagination in health. Amityville,

NY: Baywood.

Sheikh, A. A., & Jordan, S. C. (1983). Clinical uses of mental imagery. In A. A. Sheikh

(Ed.), Imagery: Current theory, research, and application. New York: Wiley.

Sheikh, A. A., & Korn, E. R. (1994). Imagery in sports and physical performance.

Amityville, NY: Baywood.

Shepard, R. N. (1978). The mental image. American Psychologist, 33, 125-137.

Shepard, R. N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects.

Science, 171, 701-703.

345
Short, S., Monsma, E., & Short, M. (2004). Is what you see really what you get?

Athletes’ perception of imagery’s functions. The Sport Psychologist, 18, 341-349.

Short, S., Ross-Stewart, L., Monsma, E. (2006). Onwards with the evolution of imagery

research in sport psychology. Athletic Insight, 8(3), 47-63.

Simons, J. (2000). Doing imagery in the field. In M. B. Anderson (Ed.), Doing sport

psychology (pp. 77-92). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Simonton, O. C., Matthews-Simonton, S., & Sparks, T. F. (1971). Psychological

intervention in the treatment of cancer. Psychosomatics, 8, 483-490.

Simonton, O. C., Simonton, S., & Creighton, J. L. (1978). Getting well again: A step-by-

step, self-help guide to overcoming cancer for patients and their families. New

York: St. Martin’s Press.

Singer, J. (1966). Daydreaming: An introduction to the experimental study of inner

experience. New York: Random House.

Sisterhen, L. A. (2005). The use of imagery, mental practice, and relaxation techniques

for musical performance enhancement. D.M.A. dissertation, The University of

Oklahoma, United States. Retrieved January 16, 2008, from ProQuest Digital

Dissertations database. (Publication No. AAT 3174357).

Skeat, W. W. (1943). An etymological Dictionary of the English language (2nd ed.). New

York: Macmillan.

Sommer, R. (1980). A practical guide to behavioral research: Tools and techniques. New

York: Oxford University Press.

Sordoni, C., Hall, C. R., & Forwell, L. (2000). The use of imagery by athletes during

injury rehabilitation. Journal of Sport Rehabilitation, 9(4), 329-338.

346
Stanislavsky, K. (1961). Creating a role. New York: Theatre Arts Books.

Start, K. B., & Richardson, A. (1964). Imagery and mental practice. British Journal of

Educational Psychology, 34, 280-284.

Stedman, S. E. (1985). Singing and self: The psychology of performing. Doctoral

dissertation. Northwestern University –United States – Illinois. Retrieved

February 11, 2008, from ProQuest Digital Dissertations database. (Publication

No. AAT 8703785).

Stockholm, G. (1975). Dorothy DeLay discusses teaching. The Instrumentalist, 30(5), 40-

43.

Strasberg, L., & Morphos, E. (1987). A dream of passion: The development of the

method. Boston: Little, Brown.

Strauss, A., & Corbin, J. (1998). Basics of qualitative research: Techniques and

procedures for developing Grounded Theory (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Suinn, R. M. (1972). Behavior rehearsal training for ski racers: Brief report. Behavior

Therapy, 2, 210-212.

Suinn, R. M. (1976). Visual motor behavior rehearsal for adaptive behavior. In J.

Krumboltz & C. Thoresen (Eds.), Counseling Methods (pp. 360-366). New York:

Holt.

Suinn, R. M. (1980a). Body thinking: Psychology for Olympic champions. In R. M.

Suinn (Ed.), Psychology in sports: Methods and applications (pp. 306-315).

Minneapolis: Burgess.

347
Suinn, R. M. (1980b). Psychology and sports performance: Principles and applications.

Research of Imagery in Sports Psychology. In R. M. Suinn (Ed.), Psychology in

sports: Methods and applications (pp. 25-26). Minneapolis: Burgess.

Suinn, R. M. (1980c). Psychology in sports: Methods and applications. New York:

Macmillan.

Suinn, R. M. (1983). Imagery in sports. In A. A. Sheikh (Ed.), Imagery: Current theory,

research, and application (pp. 507-534). New York: Wiley.

Suinn, R. M. (1986). Seven steps to peak performance: The mental training manual for

athletes. Toronto: Huber.

Suinn, R. M. (1992). Visualization in sports. In A. A. Sheikh & E. R. Korn (Eds.),

Imagery in sports and physical performance (pp. 507-534). Amnityville, NY:

Baywood.

Suinn, R. M. (1996). Imagery rehearsal: A tool for clinical practice. Psychotherapy in

Private Practice, 15(3), 27-31.

Suinn, R. M. (1997). Mental practice in sport psychology: Where have we been, where

do we go? Clinical Psychology, 4, 189-207.

Suinn, R. M. (2001). Imagery. In R. Singer, M. Murphy, & L. K. Tennant (Eds.),

Handbook of research on sport psychology (2nd ed., pp. 496-510). New York:

Macmillan.

Taggart, C. C., Bolton, B. M., Reynolds, A. M., Valerio, W. H., & Gordon, E. E. (2000).

Jump right in: The music Curriculum. Chicago: GIA.

Taylor, R. (1993). Imagery: Its role in mental rehearsal to improve sports performance:

Research Report No. 27. Edinburgh, EH: The Scottish Sports Council, Caledonia

348
House.

Theiler, A. M., & Lippman, L. G. (1995). Effects of mental practice and modeling on

guitar and vocal performance. Journal of General Psychology, 122(4), 329-343.

Titze, I. (1986). Standardization in voice research. The NATS Journal, 42(2), 30-31.

Tosi, P. F., & Galliard, J. E. (1968). Observations on the florid song. New York: Johnson

Reprint (Originally published in Bologna, 1723).

Trusheim, W. H. (1987). Mental imagery and musical performance: An inquiry into

imagery use by eminent orchestral brass players in the United States. Ed.D.

dissertation, Rutgers The State University of New Jersey - New Brunswick,

United States -- New Jersey. Retrieved February 11, 2008, from ProQuest Digital

Dissertations database. (Publication No. AAT 8808237).

Ungerleider, S., & Golding, J. M. (1991). Mental practice among Olympic athletes.

Perceptual and Motor Skills, 72, 1007-1017.

Ungerleider, S., & Golding, J. M. (1992). Beyond strength: Psychological profiles of

Olympic athletes. Dubuque, IA: Wm. C. Brown

Vadocz, E., Hall, C., & Moritz, S. (1997). The relationship between competitive anxiety

and imagery use. Journal of Applied Sports Psychology, 9, 241-253.

Vealey, R. S. (1986). Imagery training for performance enhancement. In J. M. Williams

(Ed.), Applied sport psychology: Personal growth for peak performance (pp. 209-

232). Mountain View, CA: Mayfield.

Vealey, R. S., & Greenleaf, C. A. (2001). Seeing is believing: Understanding and using

imagery in sport. In J. M. Williams (Ed.), Applied sport psychology: Personal

349
growth to peak performance (4th ed., pp. 247-272). Mountain View, CA:

Mayfield.

Vealey, R. S., & Greenleaf, C. A. (2006). Seeing is believing: Understanding and using

imagery in sport. In J. M. Williams (Ed.), Applied sport psychology: Personal

growth to peak performance (5th ed., pp. 306-343). Mountain View, CA:

Mayfield.

Vealey, R. S., Walter, S. M. (1993). Imagery training for performance enhancement and

personal development. In J. M Williams (Ed.), Applied sport psychology:

Personal growth to peak performance (2nd ed., pp. 200-224). Mountain View,

CA: Mayfield.

Vella-Brodrick, D. A., & MacRae, K. (2004). A review of mental imagery scales

commonly used in sporting contexts. Journal of Mental Imagery, 28(3), 121-147.

Vennard, W. (1958). Philosophies of vocal pedagogy. American Music Teacher. May-

June. May/June, 4-5.

Vennard, W. (1961). Singers and science. Selected Articles from the writings of William

Vennard. USA: NATS.

Vennard, W. (1968). Singing: The mechanism and the technic. New York: Carl Fisher.

Vennard, W. (1971). Selected Articles from the writings of William Vennard (Gurnee, R.

T. (Ed.). USA: NATS.

Vergeer, I., & Hanrahan, C. (1998). What modern dancers do to prepare: Content and

objectives of preperformance routines. AVANTE, 4(2), 47-71.

Ware, C. (1998). Basics of vocal pedagogy: The foundations and process of singing.

Boston: McGraw Hill.

350
Warner, B. (1991). Orff-Schulwerk: Applications for the classroom. Englewood Cliffs,

NJ: Prentice Hall.

Washburn, M. F. (1916). Movement and mental imagery. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Watson, J. B. (1913). Psychology as the behaviorist sees it. Lancaster, PA: Psychological

Review Company.

Weerts, T. C., & Lang, P. J. (1978). Psychophysiology of fear imagery: Differences

between focal phobia and social performance anxiety. Journal of Consulting and

Clinical Psychology, 46(5), 1157-1159.

Weinberg, R., Butt, J., Knight, B., Burke, K. L., & Jackson, A. (2003). The relationship

between the use and effectiveness of imagery: an exploratory investigation.

Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 15, 26-40.

Weinberg, R. S., & Gould, D. G. (1995). Foundations of sport and exercise psychology.

Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Weinberg, R. S., & Gould, D. G. (1999). Foundations of sport and exercise psychology

(2nd ed.). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Weinberg, R. S., & Gould, D. G. (2003). Foundations of sport and exercise psychology

(3rd ed.). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

White, A. & Hardy, L. (1995). Use of different imagery perspectives on the learning and

performance of different motor skills. British Journal of Psychology, 86, 169-180.

White, A. & Hardy, L. (1998). An in-depth analysis of the uses of imagery by high-level

slalom canoeists and artistic gymnasts. The Sport Psychologist, 12, 387-403.

Williamon, A. (2004). Musical excellence. New York: Oxford.

351
Woodward, S. (1992). Intrauterine Rhythm and Blues? British Journal of Obstetrics and

Gynaecology, 99, 787-790.

Woody, R. H. (1999). The relationship between advanced musicians' explicit planning

and their expressive performance of dynamic variations in an aural modeling task.

Journal of Research in Music Education, 47, 331-342.

Woolfolk, R. L., Murphy, S. M., Gottesfeld, D., & Aitken, D. (1985). Mental rehearsal of

motor task activity and mental depiction of task outcome on motor skill

performance. Journal of Sport Psychology, 7, 191-197.

Woolfolk, R. L., Parrish, M. W., & Murphy, S. M. (1985). The effects of positive and

negative imagery on motor skill performance. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 9

(3), 335-341.

Yuille, J. C. (1985). A laboratory-based experimental methodology is inappropriate for

the study of mental imagery. Journal of Mental Imagery, 9(2), 137-150.

Zatorre, R. J., & Halpern, A. R. (2005). Mental concerts: Musical imagery and auditory

cortex. Neuron, Volume 47, Issue 1(7), 9-12.

352
Appendices

353
Appendix A

Permission to Publish Imagery Frameworks

354
Appendix B

Introductory Letter and Scripts: Vocal Professionals

Patricia L. Bowes
6712 Sandscape Lane
Temple Terrace, FL 33617
813 985-1199, cell 561 523-2223
[email protected]

December 1, 2008

Dear Vocal Professional

I am a candidate for the Ph.D. degree in music education at the University of South
Florida, Tampa. I am currently beginning field work on my dissertation, titled: An
Exploratory Study of the Use of Imagery by Vocal Professionals: Applications of a Sport
Psychology Framework.

You were highly recommended by ______ as having these qualifications

I am looking for male and female participants in all vocal registers from major cities
throughout the United States who
(a) hold a four year degree in vocal performance, and
(b) have been working for at least four years making at least half their income as a solo
vocal professional
or
(c) if they have no performance degree, they have eight years making at least half their
income as a solo vocal professional.
In order to identify vocal professionals who might wish to participate in my study, I am
respectfully requesting your assistance. I have contacted you because you are a
recognized vocal professional currently working in the field.

If you decide to participate, we will set up an initial telephone conversation to get to


know a little about each other, the purpose of the study, imagery definitions, expectations
of the participants, anonymity and pseudonyms, interview procedures, intention to
digitally tape and transcribe verbatim the interview, procedures to check the written
transcriptions, and uses of the results and data. The actual telephone interview itself will
last between 30 to 75 minutes at a time and telephone number convenient to you.

355
Appendix B (continued)

There are a few potential benefits to you and members of the vocal profession. You may
learn more about your use of imagery in achieving optimal performance. Your responses
may also be of possible benefit to other singers, voice teachers, and music education in
general. Unfortunately, there is no pay for your time.

The telephone call for the interview should be free for you since I will call you. All
names, locations and identifying references will remain anonymous in order to ensure
your most candid and honest responses.

I sincerely hope you decide to participate in this research study, and that it will be a
rewarding experience for you. As soon as you have a chance, please send me an email
([email protected]) and let me know whether you decide to participate.
Furthermore, if you are able to suggest possible participants for my dissertation study
who meet the above criteria, please send an email to me that includes the names of
singers you recommend, and either their email and/or telephone numbers where they can
be reached.

If you would like more information about my study, please take a look at the attached
information sheet. If you have additionally questions or concerns, please feel free to
email me at the above address. Thank you for your kind consideration. I look forward to
hearing from you soon.

Sincerely,

Patricia L. Bowes

356
Appendix C

Introductory Letter and Scripts: Participants: Vocal Professors

Patricia L. Bowes
6712 Sandscape Lane
Temple Terrace, FL 33617
813 985-1199, cell 561 523-2223
[email protected]

December 1, 2008

Dear Vocal Professor,

I am a candidate for the Ph.D. degree in music education at the University of South
Florida, Tampa. I am currently beginning field work on my dissertation, titled An
Exploratory Study of the Use of Imagery by Vocal Professionals: Applications of a Sport
Psychology Framework.

In order to identify vocal professionals who might wish to participate in my study. I am


respectfully requesting your assistance. I have contacted you because you are a
recognized teacher of successful vocal professionals who are currently working in the
field.

I am looking for male and female participants in all vocal registers from major cities
throughout the United States who
(a) hold a four year degree in vocal performance, and
(b) have been working for at least four years making at least half their income as a solo
vocal professional
or
(c) if they have no performance degree, they have eight years making at least half their
income as a solo vocal professional.

I sincerely hope that you are willing and able to assist me in identifying possible
participants for my dissertation study. I am asking you to take a few moments to think of
singers who meet the above criteria. Then, please send an email to me
([email protected]) that includes the names of singers you recommend, and
either their email and/or telephone numbers where they can be reached.

357
Appendix C (continued)

If you would like more information about my study, please take a look at the attached
information sheet. If you have additionally questions or concerns, please feel free to
email me at the above address. Thank you for your kind consideration. I look forward to
hearing from you soon.

Sincerely,

Patricia L. Bowes
Ph.D. Candidate
School of Music
College of Visual and Performing Arts
University of South Florida
Tampa, FL 33620

358
Appendix D

Singers’ Interview Guide


An Exploratory Study of the Use of Imagery by Vocal Professionals:
Applications of a Sport Psychology Framework
The Four Ws of Singing Imagery

Subject number: _______ Name:_______________________________


Age: ________________Date:________________________________
Time began:__________ Time ended:__________________________

Section 1: Introduction (not recorded)


Hello, I am Patricia Bowes from the School of Music at the University of South Florida.
Thank you for agreeing to participate in this interview! Your anonymity is secured as we
have already agreed. In this project I am talking to singers about where, when singers use
imagery, of what their imagery consists, and why they use imagery to achieve optimal
performance. Optimal performance is also referred to as “the flow state,” “being in the
zone,” or “in the groove.” It is a state that you, as an experienced professional, work
toward and can control, in which all elements fall into place and it just feels right. It is
different from peak performance, which is an outstanding, unforgettable, often a
penultimate or once in a lifetime experience. Do you have any questions about optimal
performance?
For the sake of clarity, I am going to define imagery to you:
“Imagery is an experience that mimics real experience. We can be aware of [hearing
music inside our head],‘seeing’ an image, feeling movements as an image, or
experiencing an image of smell, taste or sounds without experiencing the real thing. It
differs from dreams in that we are awake and conscious when we form an image.”
(White and Hardy, 1998).
Imagery is not simply talking to yourself, and it is not just watching demonstrations by
other people. A lot of the time it is seeing, hearing, and feeling as if you were doing
something else. Imagery can be done while performing an aria or during a concert and in
an appropriate setting, but just as well while being in a completely different environment,
and being completely still.
Do you have any questions about what I mean with imagery? Is this similar to how you
would define imagery?

359
Appendix D (continued)

As we go through the interview, I have definitions and examples of the various kinds of
imagery available so please ask if you are not quite clear about what I am asking for.
Also, do not worry if some questions seem repetitive. This is partly to make sure that I
am covering all aspects of your singing imagery and not leaving anything out, and partly
useful to obtain quotes for later analyses.

Section 2: Imagery experience


This section of the interview focuses on some of your experience with imagery.
1) Do you ever engage in this type of imagery to achieve optimal performance? (if
yes, go on to #2, If no, ask: Is there ever a time when you see or hear yourself or
somebody else singing a song or aria in your mind? If yes, continue to # 2. If no,
say thank you and terminate the interview.)
2) What kind of imagery do you remember using when you first began?
i. Visual, sound, feeling?

3) Why did you first begin using imagery?


i. i.e., for what purpose?

4) How did you first learn to use imagery?


i. How did you develop this skill?
ii. Who taught you?
iii. Did it come naturally?

5) Describe how your use of imagery has developed over your singing career?
i. i.e., is it different now in any way from what it was to start with?

Section 3: Singing experience


This section of the interview focuses on your background in singing.
6) How old were you when you first began singing?
i. types of singing
ii. what attracted you to singing?

7) How many times per week did you sing then?


i. types of song

8) Describe what type(s) of singing you are involved in now?

9) How many times per week do you sing now?


i. if several kinds, how often for each?
360
Appendix D (continued)

10) Did your teacher ever use imagery with you in connection with your training in
achieving optimal performance? (If yes, go on to #11. If no go on to Section 4, #
15.)

11) Describe what kinds of imagery your singing teacher involved in the lessons when
you were younger?

i. Elaboration probe:
1. e.g. suggesting you could imagine an aria in your head?
2. When was this? / How old were you then?
3. What did this imagery involve?

12) What kinds of imagery do your singing teachers/coaches involve in your


lessons/rehearsals now?
i. What does this imagery involve (content)?
ii. How often does this occur?

13) What kinds of imagery do your teachers/coaches encourage you to use yourself,
outside of lessons/rehearsals?
i. Describe some of that imagery that they encourage?
ii. To what extent do you use what they recommend?

14) Where else did you learn to use imagery in vocal performance?
i. Did you read books, collaborate with peers, or suggested by a
teacher?

Section 4: Why
Imagery use for optimal performance can serve different functions for singers, and in the
following sections, I will be asking you to elaborate on the reasons why you use imagery.

15) Describe any imagery you use that is based on skill learning and execution?
i. i.e. imagery based on technical instruction and doing things
correctly?
ii. e.g. imagining a certain aria performed perfectly.
iii. elaboration probes:
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)

16) Describe any imagery you use relating to sequences?


i. i.e. imagery based on sections of a programme, or a whole
performance?

361
Appendix D (continued)

ii. e.g. imagining a stage entry going exactly according to plan and
practice.
iii. elaboration probes:
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)

17) Describe any imagery you use relating to strategies?


i. i.e. imagery based on how to do things, order, and planning? (like a
game plan in sport, entrances, blocking, exits?)
ii. e.g. imagining focusing on technique for the first half of a stage
entry, and then focusing on form?
iii. elaboration probes:
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)

18) Describe any imagery you use relating to arousal and anxiety?
i. i.e. imagery based on stress, nervousness, stage fright etc.?
ii. e.g. imagining the excitement associated with performing,
imagining handling your performance anxiety or stage fright?
iii. elaboration probes:
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)

19) Describe any imagery you use relating to self-confidence and mastery?
i. i.e. imagery based on being in control, being focused, and/or
working successfully through difficult situations?
ii. e.g. imagining feeling really confident when walking onto the
stage?
iii. elaboration probes:
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)

20) Describe any imagery you use relating to goals?


i. i.e. imagery based on what it will be like to work toward your
goals?
ii. How do you break down large and small goals?
iii. e.g. imagining working hard and the process that will take you to
where you want to go?
iv. elaboration probes:

362
Appendix D (continued)

1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)

i. i.e. imagery based on what it will be like to reach your goals?


ii. e.g. imagining great applause from the audience for your solo
performance, and/or how it would feel to get accepted to a certain
company/orchestra, or likewise?
iii. elaboration probes:
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)

21) Describe any imagery you use based on metaphors?


i. i.e. imagery based on symbolism and imagining being something
else?
ii. e.g. imagining being filled with a color, rising like a bird, singing
in an old cathedral, sounding like a certain musical instrument?
iii. elaboration probes:
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)

22) Describe any imagery you use for character development?


i. i.e. imagery based on being somebody else, and identifying
yourself with your role?
ii. e.g. living in the era or place (time, style, or country) in which a
performance is set?
iii. elaboration probes:
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)

23) Describe any imagery you use based on emotions?


i. i.e. imagery based on emotions such as happiness, sadness,
melancholy, exhilaration?
ii. e.g. imagining being really excited?
iii. elaboration probes:
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)

363
Appendix D (continued)

24) Describe any imagery you use based on energy?


i. i.e. imagery based on feeling energized and psyched up?
ii. e.g. imagining being really energetic or calming down?
iii. elaboration probes:
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)

25) Describe any imagery you use based on appearance?


i. i.e. imagery based on how you look to others, your body image,
posture, fitness, health etc.?
ii. e.g. imagining looking how you would ideally like to look?
iii. elaboration probes:
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)

26) Describe any imagery you use based on your sound?


i. i.e. imagery based on how you sound to others, your vocal image,
fitness, health, quality etc.?
ii. e.g. imagining sounding how you would ideally like to sound?
iii. elaboration probes:
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)

27) Describe any other kinds of imagery you use to achieve optimal performance,
which are different from the ones we have mentioned?
i. e.g. healing, spiritual imagery?
ii. elaboration probes:
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content

28) Describe any other reasons that you have for using imagery to achieve optimal
performance, that are different from the ones we have mentioned?
i. e.g. motivating yourself to sing?
ii. elaboration probes:
1. What does it consist of? (content)

364
Appendix D (continued)

Section 5: What
This section of the interview focuses on what senses you involve in your imagery for
optimal performance. As you know, you have the five senses of vision, hearing, smell,
taste, and touch. Additionally, you can experience kinaesthetic imagery, and the emotion-
based imagery we talked about earlier.
29) To what extent do you use imagery relating to vision?
i. What perspective does that imagery take?
a. i.e. is it Internal/External/Switching?

30) To what extent do you use imagery relating to sound and hearing?

31) To what extent do you use imagery relating to smell?

32) To what extent do you use imagery relating to taste?

33) To what extent do you use imagery relating to touch?


i. i.e. imagining what it feels like to be touched, e.g. when a partner
touches you or the teacher corrects you.

34) To what extent do you use imagery relating to kinaesthesia?


i. i.e. awareness of your body position in space
ii. e.g. knowing how your body feels when singing a high note.

35) Describe any other kinds of sensations that you feel in your imagery?
i. e.g. heat or other temperature changes? Pain? Body Image?

36) Which of the above would be the primary senses you use in your imagery?
i. Which would be not so important/by-products?

Section 6: What continued: Imagery details


This section of the interview relates to some of the details of your imagery use.
37) Describe your ability to use imagery to achieve optimal performance?
i. i.e. how good do you think you are at imaging? Different sense
imagery abilities.
ii. Elaboration probes:
1. how accurate are your images?
2. how vivid are they?
3. to what extent are you able to manipulate your images as
you wish?

365
Appendix D (continued)

38) To what extent is your imagery facilitative, debilitative, or both?


i. i.e. is your imagery helpful or hurtful to you? Examples?
ii. Elaboration probe: is this always the case or does it vary?

Section 7: When and Where


This section of the interview relates to places and times you use imagery.
39) When do you use imagery?
i. Elaboration probes:
1. Time of day or night
2. In relation to practice (before/during/after)
3. In relation to performance (before/during/after)
4. In relation to performance season (before/during/after)
5. To what extent does your imagery use differ between
rehearsal, performance, and holiday periods?

40) Where do you use imagery?


i. i.e. where are you normally when you are imaging?
ii. Where do you usually practice imagery, where is your favorite
place?

Section 8: Conclusions
Ok, this just about wraps up the interview. However, before we finish, let me ask you a
few final questions:
41) How did you think the interview went?

42) Did I lead you or influence your responses in any way?

43) Do you think we failed to discuss any important factors?


i. What are they?

44) Do you have any comments or suggestions about the interview itself?

THANK YOU so much for helping out by participating in this interview!

366
Appendix E

Dancers’ Interview Guide (Nordin & Cumming, 2005)


The Four Ws of Dance Imagery

Subject number: _______ Name:_______________________________


Age: ________________Date:________________________________
Time began:__________ Time ended:__________________________

Section 1: Introduction (not recorded)

Hello, I am Sanna Nordin from the School of Sport and Exercise Sciences at The
University of Birmingham. Thank you for agreeing to participate in this interview! In this
project I am talking to dancers about why, when, and where dancers use imagery, and
what their imagery consists of. First of all, I am going to define imagery to you:
“Imagery is an experience that mimics real experience. We can be aware of ‘seeing’ an
image, feeling movements as an image, or experiencing an image of smell, taste or
sounds without experiencing the real thing. It differs from dreams in that we are awake
and conscious when we form an image.”
(White and Hardy, 1998).
Imagery is not simply talking to yourself, and it is not just watching demonstrations by
other people. A lot of the time it is seeing and feeling as if you were doing something
else. Imagery can be done whilst performing a movement and in an appropriate setting,
but just as well whilst being in a completely different environment, and being completely
still.
Do you have any questions about what I mean with imagery? Is this similar to how your
would define imagery?

As we go through the interview, I have definitions and examples of the various kinds of
imagery available so please ask if you are not quite clear about what I am asking for.
Also, do not worry if some questions seem repetitive. This is partly to make sure that I
am covering all aspects of your dance imagery and not leaving anything out, and partly
useful to obtain quotes for later analyses.

367
Appendix E (continued)

Section 2: Imagery experience


This section of the interview focuses on some of your experience with imagery.
1) Why did you first begin using imagery?
i. i.e., for what purpose?

2) How did you first learn to use imagery?


i. How did you develop this skill?
ii. Who taught you?
iii. Did it come naturally?

3) Describe how your use of imagery has developed over your dancing career?
i. i.e., is it different in any way from what it was to start with?

Section 3: Dance experience


This section of the interview focuses on your background in dance.
4) How old were you when you first began dancing?
i. types of dance

5) How many times per week did you dance then?


i. types of dance

6) Describe what type(s) of dance you are involved in now?

7) How many times per week do you dance now?


i. if several kinds, how often for each?

8) Describe what kinds of imagery your dance teacher involved in the lessons when
you were younger?
i. Elaboration probe: e.g. suggesting you could imagine a dance
routine in your head?
i. When was this? / How old were you then?
ii. What did this imagery involve?
iii. Was this a regular occurrence?

9) What kinds of imagery do your dance teachers/choreographers involve in your


classes/rehearsals now?
i. What does this imagery involve (content)?
ii. How often does this occur?

10) What kinds of imagery do your teachers/choreographers encourage you to use


yourself, outside of classes/rehearsals?

368
Appendix E (continued)

i. Describe some of that imagery that they encourage?


ii. To what extent do you use what they recommend?
Section 4: Why
Imagery can serve different functions for dancers, and in the following sections, I will be
asking you to elaborate on the reasons why you use imagery.

11) Describe any imagery you use that is based on skill learning and execution?
i. i.e. imagery based on technical instruction and doing things
correctly?
ii. e.g. imagining a double pirouette performed perfectly.
iii. elaboration probes:
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)

12) Describe any imagery you use relating to sequences?


i. i.e. imagery based on sections of a programme, or a whole
performance?
ii. e.g. imagining a stage entry going exactly according to plan and
practice.
iii. elaboration probes:
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)

13) Describe any imagery you use relating to strategies?


iv. i.e. imagery based on how to do things, order, and planning? (like a
game plan in sport?)
v. e.g. imagining focusing on technique for the first half of a stage
entry, and then focusing on form?
vi. elaboration probes:
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)

14) Describe any imagery you use relating to arousal and anxiety?
i. i.e. imagery based on stress, nervousness, stagefright etc.?
ii. e.g. imagining the excitement associated with performing,
imagining handling your stagefright?
iii. elaboration probes:
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)
369
Appendix E (continued)

15) Describe any imagery you use relating to self-confidence and mastery?
i. i.e. imagery based on being in control, being focused, and/or
working successfully through difficult situations?
ii. e.g. imagining feeling really confident when walking onto the
stage?
iii. elaboration probes:
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)

16) Describe any imagery you use relating to goals?


i. i.e. imagery based on what it will be like to work toward your
goals?
ii. e.g. imagining working hard and the process that will take you to
where you want to get?
iii. elaboration probes:
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)

vii. i.e. imagery based on what it will be like to reach your goals?
viii. e.g. imagining great applause from the audience for your solo
performance, and/or how it would feel to get accepted to a certain
academy/school, or likewise?
ix. elaboration probes:
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)

17) Describe any imagery you use based on metaphors?


i. i.e. imagery based on symbolism and imagining being something
else?
ii. e.g. imagining being filled with a colour, moving through water,
holding a beach ball, stretching like elastic, jumping over a hurdle?
iii. elaboration probes:
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)
18) Describe any imagery you use for character development?
i. i.e. imagery based on being somebody else, and identifying
yourself with your role?
ii. e.g. living in the era or place a performance is set?
iii. elaboration probes:

370
Appendix E (continued)

3. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
4. What does it consist of? (content)

19) Describe any imagery you use based on emotions?


i. i.e. imagery based on emotions such as happiness, sadness,
melancholy, exhilaration?
ii. e.g. imagining being really excited?
iii. elaboration probes:

1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)

20) Describe any imagery you use based on energy?


i. i.e. imagery based on feeling energized and psyched up?
ii. e.g. imagining being really energetic?
iii. elaboration probes:
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)

21) Describe any imagery you use based on appearance?


i. i.e. imagery based on how you look to others, your body image,
fitness, health etc.?
ii. e.g. imagining looking how you would ideally like to look?
iii. elaboration probes:
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)

22) Describe any other kinds of imagery you use, that is different from the ones we
have mentioned?
i. e.g. healing imagery?
ii. elaboration probes:
1. Describe your reasons for using it? (i.e., what are you
trying to achieve)? (function)
2. What does it consist of? (content)

23) Describe any other reasons that you have for using imagery, that are different
from the ones we have mentioned?
i. e.g. motivating yourself to go dancing?
ii. elaboration probes:
1. What does it consist of? (content)
371
Appendix E (continued)

Section 5: What
This section of the interview focuses on what senses you involve in your imagery. As you
know, you have the five senses of vision, hearing, smell, taste and touch. Additionally,
you can experience kinaesthetic imagery, and the emotion-based imagery we talked about
earlier.
24) To what extent do you use imagery relating to vision?
ii. What perspective does that imagery take?
1. i.e. is it Internal/External/Switching?

31) To what extent do you use imagery relating to sound and hearing?

32) To what extent do you use imagery relating to smell?

33) To what extent do you use imagery relating to taste?

34) To what extent do you use imagery relating to touch?


i. i.e. imagining what it feels like to be touched, e.g. when a partner
lifts you or the teacher corrects you.

35) To what extent do you use imagery relating to kinaesthesia?


i. i.e. awareness of your body position in space,
ii. e.g. knowing how your body feels when standing in an arabesque.

36) Describe any other kinds of sensations that you feel in your imagery?
i. e.g. heat or other temperature changes? Pain?

37) Which of the above would be your primary senses in your imagery?
i. Which would be not so important/by-products?
ii.
Section 6: What continued: Imagery details
This section of the interview relates to some of the details of your imagery use.
38) Describe your ability to use imagery?
i. i.e. how good do you think you are at imaging?
ii. Elaboration probes:

1. how accurate are your images?


2. how vivid are they?
3. to what extent are you able to manipulate your images as
you wish?
39) To what extent is your imagery facilitative, debilitative, or both?
i. i.e. is your imagery helpful or hurtful to you?
Elaboration probe: is this always the case or does it vary?
372
Appendix E (continued)

Section 7: Where and When


This section of the interview relates to places and times you use imagery.
40) Where do you use imagery?
i. i.e. where are you normally when you are imaging?

41) When do you use imagery?


i. Elaboration probes:
1. Time of day
2. In relation to practice (before/during/after)
3. In relation to performance (before/during/after)
4. To what extent does your imagery use differ between
rehearsal, performance, and holiday periods?

Section 8: Conclusions
Ok, this just about wraps up the interview. However, before we finish, let me ask you a
couple of final questions:
42) How did you think the interview went?

43) Did I lead you or influence your responses in any way?

44) Do you think we failed to discuss any important factors?


i. What are they?

45) Do you have any comments or suggestions about the interview itself?

THANK YOU so much for helping out by participating in this interview!

373
Appendix F

Permission to Use the Dancers’ Interview Guide

374
Appendix G

Research Participant Request

RESEARCH STUDY PARTICIPANTS NEEDED!

Study Title:
An Exploratory Study of Vocal Professionals Use of Imagery: Applications of a Sport
Psychology Framework

Researcher:
Patricia L. Bowes
Ph.D. Candidate in Music Education, University of South Florida
813 985-1199, Cell: 561 523-2223
[email protected]

Researcher Qualifications:
Ph.D. Candidate in Music Education, University of South Florida
Bachelors and Masters Degrees in Music in Voice
Bachelors and Master Degrees in Art and Drama
Professional experience as solo vocalists
10 years teaching university classes in arts and music.
11 years experience as arts educator in public and private K-12 schools
Florida Teacher Certification: Music, Art, and Drama, to 2013

Purpose of the Study:


The purpose of this study is to investigate and explain the four Ws: where, when, what,
and why of imagery use to achieve optimal performances in vocal professionals from a
sport psychology framework.

Participants:
The participants must:
(a) hold a four year degree in vocal performance, and
(b) have been working for at least four years making at least half their income as a solo
vocal professional
or
(c) if they have no performance degree, they have eight years making at least half their
income as a solo vocal professional.

375
Appendix G (continued)

Responsibilities of Participants:
Study participants will complete and sign the agreement to participate in this study. An
initial telephone discussion will clarify the purpose of the study, intention of participant’s
anonymity, intention to digitally record and transcribe verbatim the interviews, and the
use of their responses. Participants will participate in a telephone interview lasting from
30 to 75 minutes. After the interviews are transcribed using a pseudonym, they will be
sent to the participant for clarification or any changes they would like to make within five
business days. Upon receiving the responses, the results will be categorized according to
the framework of the four Ws of imagery use. Participants can request a copy of the final
copy of the results upon completion of the study.

376
Appendix H

Consent Script

An Exploratory Study of the Use of Imagery by Vocal Professionals:


Applications of a Sport Psychology Framework

Patricia Bowes

Consent Script to be read over the telephone to participants

Invitation to Participate
Hello, my name is Patricia Bowes and I am a Ph.D. candidate in Music Education at the
University of South Florida, conducting a dissertation study on the use of imagery by
vocal professionals. You are invited to participate because you have been working as a
professional vocalist for at least 4 years with a musical performance degree or 8 years
without the degree and you make at least half your income in this manner.

Purpose
The purpose of this study is
1. To better understand the questions where, when, what, and why of imagery use by
professional vocalists
2. To link the use of imagery in sport psychology research to the use of imagery by
vocal professionals.

Description of Procedures
You will be interviewed by telephone in an open-ended interview that will take place
between 30 and 75 minutes, and with your permission, it will be audio-taped. The
interview will take place at an agreed upon time and date and the interviewer will call
you so as to incur no telephone charges to you. It is up to the participant to cover any
charges for their own incoming calls, if any. Please make sure that you set up a time
when you can concentrate and will not be interrupted for the duration of this interview.
Upon completion of transcribing the interview verbatim, you will be emailed a copy for
your revision and approval. You will be given 5 business days to return this document.
Upon your agreement, the document will be included in the data for the study.

Risks and Inconveniences


There are minimal risks attached to this study. Your interview will be kept confidential:
available only to the research team for analysis purposes. If the length of the interview is
inconvenient for you, you may stop the interview at any time without any consequence to
you.
377
Appendix H (continued)

Benefits
Although there is no direct benefit to you for participating in this study, I feel your
participation will likely benefit vocal teachers, students, and professionals. You may even
learn something about your own use of imagery that you didn’t know before. Upon
completion of the study, each participant will be emailed a link to a website where they
can read and/or download the study for your use.

Confidentiality
Interview tapes will be locked in a safe place. Only the research team will listen and
transcribe the information you give to us.
Interview responses will not be linked to your name, address, email, or telephone
number, and therefore there will be no follow-up sessions. You should know that the
University of South Florida Institutional Review Board (IRB) may inspect study records
as part of its auditing program, but these reviews only focus on the researchers and the
study, not on your responses or involvement. The IRB is a committee that reviews
research studies to make sure that they are safe and that the rights of the participants are
protected.

Voluntary Participation
Participation is voluntary. You do not have to participate in this study if you do not want
to. If you agree to be in this study, but later change your mind, you may withdraw at any
time. There are no consequences of any kind if you decide you do not want to
participate.

Questions
If you have any questions about this study I will be happy to answer them now. If you
have any questions in the future, please contact me Patricia Bowes, at 561 523-2223. If
you have any questions about your rights as a research participant, you may contact our
Institutional Review Board at 813 974-5638.

Consent
Now that you have been given the details of this study, I need to ask you for your verbal
agreement to participate in this study.

Do you understand what is required of you?


Do you understand your rights?
Do you understand that you will be audio-taped and your identity will be kept
confidential?
Do you understand that your responses may be used in the study and that direct quotes
may be given anonymously?
Do you understand that you will be emailed the transcribed interview for your review?
Do you agree to return the revisions in 5 business days from date sent?
Do you agree to participate in this interview study?
378
Appendix H (continued)

Great! Thank you so much.

When would be a good time for the telephone interview, which should last between 30
and 75 minutes? _____ time ______ date
What number would you like me to call? _____________________________________
And may I have your email address, please? __________________________________
That’s great, so I will call you at ______ (time) on _______ (date) at
__________________________ (phone number).

I will email you this information so you can verify it. If there are any questions or
conflicts you will be able to tell me and we will adjust interview time accordingly.
I just want to thank you for taking the time to participate in this study.

379
Appendix I

Vocal Participant Survey

VOCAL PARTICIPANT SURVEY NAME: ___________________

Please answer the following questions by checking, or completing the appropriate spaces.

Personal Information

1. Gender: ___ Male ___Female

2. Voice part (range): ___Soprano, ___Mezzo, ___Counter-Tenor, ___Tenor,

___Baritone, ___Bass, other (please specify) __________________

3. Age (check one): ___25-30, ___ 30-35, ___35-40, ___40-45, ___45-50, ___50-55,

___55-60, ___65-70, ___70-75, ___75-80, ___80+

Educational Information

4. Undergraduate degree in musical performance: ___yes, ___no

5. Undergraduate degree in music other than performance: ___yes, ___no

a. Graduate degree: ___no, __ yes: __ in Music, __ in another area.

6. Highest degree earned? __4 year degree __graduate degree _post graduate _doctorate.

Professional Solo Singing Experience

7. Number of years of regional experience in Classical solo singing: ___

a. Region: ___Northeast, ___Southeast, ___South, ___Midwest, ___Southwest,

___Northwest, ___Mid-Atlantic, other __________________________

8. Number of years of national experience in Classical solo singing: ______

380
Appendix I (continued)

9. Number of years of international experience in Classical solo singing: ______

a. Nations: __Canada, __Central America, __South America, __Asia, __England,

__Europe, __Indonesia, __Australia, __Africa, __Russia, ______________other.

10. Total number of years of experience in Classical solo singing: ______

11. Types of music primarily (most often) performed: __Opera, __Oratorio, __Recital,

__Concert, __Art Song, __Early Music, ___Liturgical, _____other (please specify).

12. Types of music secondarily performed: __Opera, __Oratorio, __Recital, __Concert,

__Art Song, __Early Music, ___Liturgical, _____other (please specify).

13. Types of music performed thirdly: __Opera, __Oratorio, __Recital, __Concert,

__Art Song, __Early Music, ___Liturgical, _____other (please specify).

14. I am now retired: ___yes, ___no

381
Appendix J
Key of Emergent Codes and Categories/Themes

Code Categories/Themes

WR Where

WR:GR:AL Where: General: Alone

WR: GR:QP Where: General: Quiet/Relaxed Place

WR: GR:EV Where: General: Everywhere/Anywhere

WR:HO Where: Home

WR:HO:BD Where: Bed

WR:OH:EC Where: Outside Home: Exercising/Outside

WR:OH:CM Where: Outside Home: Commuting & Driving

WR:PP:SJ Where: Studio

WR:PP: DR Where: Dressing Room

WR:PP:RS Where: Rehearsal Space

WN When

WN:PR:BF When: Practice: Before

WN:PR:DU When: Practice: During

WN:PR:AF When: Practice: After

WN:PR:RH When: Rehearsal: During

WN:PF: BF When: Performance: Before

382
Appendix J (continued)

Code Categories/Themes

WN:PF:DU When: Performance: During

WN:PF:AF When: Performance: After

WN:PS:BF When: Performance Season: Before

WN:PS:DU When: Performance Season: During

WN:BK When: Breaks

WN:DF When: Differences

WN:AT When: All the Time

WN:DA When: Day

WN:DA:WU When: Day: Waking Up

WN:NT When: Night

WN:NT:GS When: Night: Going to Sleep

WN:QT When: Quiet Times

WN:TT When: Tedious Tasks

WT What

IT Imagery Type

IT:EX Imagery Type: Execution

IT:EX:SK Imagery Type: Execution: Skills and Techniques

IT:EX:SQ Imagery Type: Execution: Sequences and Planning

IT:EX:SC Imagery Type: Execution: Scenarios

IT:EX:GL Imagery Type: Execution: Goals

383
Appendix J (continued)

Code Categories/Themes

IT:MT Imagery Type: Metaphor

IT:MT:CO Imagery Type: Metaphor: Colors

IT:MT:OP Imagery Type: Metaphor: Objects not Present

IT:MT:IM Imagery Type: Metaphor: Imaginary Actions

IT:CX Imagery Type: Context

IT:CX:AD Imagery Type: Context: Audience

IT:CX:OP Imagery Type: Context: Imaginary People

IT:CX:ET Imagery Type: Context: Environments

IT:BD Imagery Type: Body/Related

IT:BD:AA Imagery Type: Body/Related: Arousal

IT:BD:AP Imagery Type: Body/Related: Appearance

IT:BD:KN Imagery Type: Body/Related: Kinesthetic Feelings

IT:BD:VP Imagery Type: Body/Related: Vocal Production

IT:VS Imagery Type: Musical Sound

IT:MS:MO Imagery Type: Musical Sound: Modeling

IT:MS:IS Imagery Type: Musical Sound: Ideal Sound

IT:MS:OV Imagery Type: Musical Sound: Own Voice

IT:CR Imagery Type: Character/Role

IT:CR:AP Imagery Type: Character/Role: Appearance

IT:CR:BH Imagery Type: Character/Role: Behaviors

384
Appendix J (continued)

Code Categories/Themes

IT:CR:EM Imagery Type: Character/Role: Emotions

IT:EX Imagery Type: Execution

IT:EX:SK Imagery Type: Execution: Skills and Techniques

IT:EX:SQ Imagery Type: Execution: Sequences and Planning

IT:EX:SC Imagery Type: Execution: Scenarios

IT:EX:GL Imagery Type: Execution: Goals

IT:MT Imagery Type: Metaphor

IT:MT:CO Imagery Type: Metaphor: Colors

IT:MT:OP Imagery Type: Metaphor: Objects not Present

IT:MT:IM Imagery Type: Metaphor: Imaginary Actions

Imagery Characteristics

SI Sense Imagery

SI:VI Sense Imagery: Vision/Sight

SI:VI:SE Sense Imagery: Vision: Scenario

SI:VI:VP Sense Imagery: Vision: Vocal Production

SI:VI:IN Sense Imagery: Vision: Perspective: Internal

SI:VI:EX Sense Imagery: Vision: Perspective: External

SI:VI:PN Sense Imagery: Vision: Page Notation

SI:SO Sense Imagery: Sound/Auditory

SI:SO:WD Sense Imagery: Sound: Words/Text/ Phrase

385
Appendix J (continued)

Code Categories/Themes

SI:SO:PI Sense Imagery: Sound: Pitch

SI:SO:ML Sense Imagery: Sound: Melody

SI:SO:AM Sense Imagery: Sound: Accompaniment

SI:SO:SS Sense Imagery: Sound: Silent Singing

SI:SO:KN Sense Imagery: Sound: Vocal Production/Kinesthetic

SI:KN Sense Imagery: Kinesthesia

SI:KN:EM Sense Imagery: Kinesthesia: Character/Emotion

SI:KN:VP Sense Imagery: Kinesthesia: Vocal Production

SI:KN:BO Sense Imagery: Kinesthesia: Body Check

SI:KN:AU Sense Imagery: Kinesthesia: Automaticity

SI:TC Sense Imagery: Touch/Tactile

SI:TC:CB Sense Imagery: Touch: Character Based

SI:TC:FP Sense Imagery: Touch: Future Performance

SI:TC:CA Sense Imagery: Touch: Communication with Audience

SI:SM Sense Imagery: Smell/Olfactory

SI:SM:CB Sense Imagery: Smell: Character Based

SI:SM:VP Sense Imagery: Smell: Vocal Production

SI:SM:VE Sense Imagery: Smell: Venue

SI:TA Sense Imagery: Taste/Gustatory

SI:TA:TB Sense Imagery: Taste: Text Based

386
Appendix J (continued)

Code Categories/Themes

SI:TA:VP Sense Imagery: Taste: Vocal Production

SI:PM Sense Imagery: Primary

SI:SN Sense Imagery: Secondary

SI:TR Sense Imagery: Thirdly

SI:TE Sense Imagery: Three Equally (SO, VI, KN)

IA Imagery Ability

IA:AY Imagery Ability: Accuracy

IA:VV Imagery Ability: Vividness

IA:MN Imagery Ability: Manipulate

IA:HP Imagery Ability: Helpful

IA:HT Imagery Ability: Hurtful

IA:DN Imagery Ability: Duration

IA:DT Imagery Ability: Detail

IA:NI Imagery Ability: Need for Improvement

WY Why

IU Imagery Use

IU:SL Imagery Use: Skill Learning/Execution

IU:SL:VP Imagery Use: Skill Learning/Execution: Vocal Production

IU:SL:VP:MT Imagery Use: Skill Learning/Execution: Vocal Production: Metaphor

IU:YS Imagery Use: Your Sound

387
Appendix J (continued)

Code Categories/Themes

IU:YS:CO Imagery Use: Your Sound: Color

IU:YS:MO Imagery Use: Your Sound: Modeling

IU:YS:VP Imagery Use: Your Sound: Vocal Production

IU:YS:VQ Imagery Use: Your Sound: Vocal Quality

IU:SQ Imagery Use: Sequences

IU:SQ:LP Imagery Use: Sequences: Learning Process

IU:SQ:PF Imagery Use: Sequences: Performance

IU:SQ:SY Imagery Use: Sequences: Story

IU:SG Imagery Use: Strategies

IU:SG:PF Imagery Use: Strategies: Performance

IU:GL Imagery Use: Goals

IU:GL:DD Imagery Use: Goals: Different than Dreaming

IU:GL:LT Imagery Use: Goals: Long Term

IU:GL:SH Imagery Use: Goals: Short Term

IU:GL:OT Imagery Use: Goals: Outcome

IU:GL:PR Imagery Use: Goals: Performance

IU:GL:MO Imagery Use: Goals: Modeling

IU:GL:PV Imagery Use: Goals: Perspective

IU:GL:FR Imagery Use: Goals: Frustration

IU:GL:GN Imagery Use: Goals: General

388
Appendix J (continued)

Code Categories/Themes

IU:AA Imagery Use: Arousal/Anxiety

IU:AA:BR Imagery Use: Arousal/Anxiety: Breathing

IU:AA:FC Imagery Use: Arousal/Anxiety: Focus/Concentration

IU:AA:PA Imagery Use: Arousal/Anxiety: Performance Anxiety

IU:AA:PP Imagery Use: Arousal/Anxiety: Practice Preparation

IU:AA: ST Imagery Use: Arousal/Anxiety: Self Talk

IU:EN Imagery Use: Energy

IU:EN:CL Imagery Use: Energy: Calming Down

IU:EN:PU Imagery Use: Energy: Psyching Up

IU:MA Imagery Use: Mastery

IU:MA:FP Imagery Use: Mastery: Fixing Problems

IU:MT Imagery Use: Metaphors

IU:MT:VP Imagery Use: Metaphors: Vocal Production

IU:MT:NH Imagery Use: Metaphors: Not Helpful

IU:CD Imagery Use: Character Development

IU:CD:AP Imagery Use: Character Development: Appearance

IU:CD:BC Imagery Use: Character Development: Being in Character

IU:CD:EM Imagery Use: Character Development: Emotion

IU:CD:HC Imagery Use: Character Development: History/Culture

IU:CD:KN Imagery Use: Character Development: Kinesthetic

389
Appendix J (continued)

Code Categories/Themes

IU:CD:SO Imagery Use: Character Development: Sound

IU:CD:MT Imagery Use: Character Development: Metaphor

IU:CD:MO Imagery Use: Character Development: Modeling

IU:CD:PE Imagery Use: Character Development: Past Experiences

IU:EM Imagery Use: Emotion

IU:EM:NH Imagery Use: Emotion: Not Helpful/Fine Line

IU:AP Imagery Use: Appearance

IU:AP:LO Imagery Use: Appearance: Look On stage

IU:AP:PO Imagery Use: Appearance: Posture

IU:AP:FI Imagery Use: Appearance: Fitness

IU:HE Imagery Use: Health

IU:VH Imagery Use: Vocal Health

IU:EP Imagery Use: Extra Practice

IU:OB Imagery Use: Optimize Best Performance

IU:QC Imagery Use: Quality Check

IU:CA Imagery Use: Communicating w/Audience

IU:SP Imagery Use: Singing Purpose

IU:SR Imagery Use: Spiritual

IU:UF Imagery Use: Usefulness

IU:TM Imagery Use: Terminology Conflict

390
Appendix J (continued)

Code Categories/Themes

RI Review of Interview

RI:HW Review of Interview: How it Went

RI:HW:LS Review of Interview: How it Went: Learned Something

RI:IF Review of Interview: Influence

RI:IF:WE Review of Interview: Influence: With Exceptions

RI:FD Review of Interview: Factors not Discussed

RI:CM Review of Interview: Comments or Suggestions

RI:LA:CI Review of Interview: Last Additions: Continued Interest

391
About the Author

Patricia Bowes has taught music and art on the college level for over 10 years in

universities around South Florida. Dr. Bowes has trained pre-service music teachers,

instructed Elementary Music Methods and World Music Ensemble classes at the

University of South Florida and the University of Tampa. In addition, Dr. Bowes has

taught general and choral music, fine arts, and drama from K-12 for over ten years in

Florida public and private schools. Dr. Bowes holds a BA in Theatre from Indiana

University, a BFA in Fine Art, a BM in Music, an MAT in Music from Florida Atlantic

University, and an MFA in Theatre from the University of Arizona. Dr. Bowes has

presented lectures and workshops regionally as well as internationally. She has sung

professionally in New York City and South Florida in Western Classical and Jazz genres

in a variety of venues for the past twenty years.

You might also like