Downing and Nauta
Downing and Nauta
Downing and Nauta
Abstract
The purpose of this study was to test the fit of a theoretical model in which
separation-individuation, career exploration, and identity diffusion were specified
as mediators of the relationship between attachment functioning (anxious and avoi-
dant) and career indecision (informational and personal–emotional). Structural
equation modeling (SEM) with latent variables was used to examine the fit of the
model to self-report data provided by college students (N ¼ 285). The model pro-
vided a good fit to the data, with most theorized paths being significant and in the
predicted direction. However, expected paths between separation-individuation
and the career-related variables were not significant, raising questions about this
construct as a mediator of attachment–career indecision relationships. Implications
for future research and for intervention are presented.
Keywords
attachment, separation-individuation, career exploration, identity diffusion, career
indecision
1
Illinois State University, Illinois USA
Corresponding Author:
Haley M. Downing, Department of Counseling, University of Akron, 27 S. Forge Street, Akron, OH 44325
Email: [email protected]
207
208 Journal of Career Development 36(3)
208
Downing and Nauta 209
Attachment Informational
avoidance .37* career
.22* Exploration
indecision
–.12
.45* Separation-
individuation –.08 .24*
–.22*
.00
Personal-
Attachment Identity .23*
emotional
anxiety .10 diffusion
career indecision
.37*
.24*
relationship between attachment and career indecision and to examine its fit among
college students. The model is shown in Figure 1, and the theoretical and empirical
rationale for each of the paths is explained below.
209
210 Journal of Career Development 36(3)
210
Downing and Nauta 211
211
212 Journal of Career Development 36(3)
Method
Participants
212
Downing and Nauta 213
of 19.42 years (SD ¼ 2.16). The racial/ethnic profile of the sample resembled that of
the university from which the participants were recruited, with 230 (81%) identify-
ing as Caucasian, 23 (8%) as Hispanic, 18 (6%) as African American, 6 (2%)
as Asian/Pacific Islander, 1 (<1%) as Native American, and 5 (2%) as ‘‘Other’’;
2 students did not indicate their race/ethnicity. In total, there were 117 (41%)
freshmen, 83 (29%) sophomores, 50 (18%) juniors, and 32 (11%) seniors; 3 students
did not indicate their year in school. In total, 36 (13%) students had undeclared
majors, and the others had a wide variety of majors.
Measures
Participants completed the following five measures. Measures were administered in
their entirety to preserve their psychometric integrity, but as described below for a
few of the measures, scores from only some subscales were used in the analyses.
213
214 Journal of Career Development 36(3)
reactivity and its avoidance scores are associated with emotional cut-off (Wei,
Vogel, Ku, & Zakalik, 2005).
Psychological Separation Inventory (PSI; Hoffman, 1984). The 138-item PSI assessed
four dimensions of separation from one’s parents. Functional independence (FI; 13
items) reflects the ability to manage one’s affairs without parental assistance. Items
from this subscale include ‘‘I wouldn’t make a major purchase without my mother’s/
father’s approval.’’ Emotional independence (EI; 17 items) represents freedom from
excessive need for approval and emotional support; items include ‘‘Being away from
my mother/father makes me feel lonely.’’ Conflictual independence (CI; 25 items)
reflects freedom from guilt, responsibility, and resentment toward one’s parents.
Items include ‘‘I feel like I am constantly at war with my mother/father.’’ Finally,
attitudinal independence (AI; 14 items) reflects an ability to ascribe to beliefs and
values that differ from one’s parents’; a sample item is, ‘‘My attitudes regarding
national defense are similar to my mother’s/father’s’’ (Hoffman, 1984).
Participants rate each item separately regarding their relationship with their
mother and their father, using a 5-point Likert-type scale (1 ¼ not at all true of
me; 5 ¼ very true of me). Although separate maternal and paternal scores for each
subscale can be calculated, in this study, combined scores were used because differ-
ences in relationships with fathers and mothers were not part of the theoretical
model. We also noted that for each of the PSI subscales mother and father scores
were significantly positively correlated in our study, providing additional justifica-
tion for treating them as a single construct in the model. Because previous research
has shown that the FI, EI, and AI subscales correlate fairly highly with each other but
have no or weak relationships with CI (Hoffman, 1984; O’Brien, 1996), CI subscale
scores were not used in this study. The possible score ranges for the FI, EI, and AI
subscales were 26–130, 17–85, and 14–70, respectively, with higher scores reflect-
ing greater separation/individuation from one’s parents.
In previous research, Cronbach’s a reliability coefficients for the PSI subscales
have been shown to range from .84 to .92 (Hoffman, 1984). In this study, a coeffi-
cients ranged from .86 to .93. The PSI scores’ construct validity has been established
by showing that they correlate with personal adjustment, problems in romantic
relationships, and academic problems (Hoffman, 1984).
Career Exploration Survey (CES; Stumpf, Colarelli, & Hartman, 1983). The CES is
intended ‘‘to index career-search behaviors, reactions to exploration, and beliefs
about exploration’’ (Stumpf et al., 1983, p. 192). In this study, two CES subscales
were used. The five-item self-exploration (SE) subscale assessed behaviors involv-
ing self-assessment and retrospection (e.g., ‘‘Understood a new relevance of past
behavior for my future career’’) and the six-item environment exploration (EE) sub-
scale measured behaviors relevant to exploration of organizations and occupations
(e.g., ‘‘Went to various career orientation programs’’). Respondents used a 5-point
Likert-type scale (1 ¼ very little; 5 ¼ very much) to rate how often they engaged
214
Downing and Nauta 215
in relevant exploration behaviors in the last 3 months. Scores for each subscale are
derived by summing responses to its items. The possible range of scores for the SE
scale is 5–25, and the possible range for the EE scale is 6–30. Higher scores are
reflective of greater exploration.
Previous research has revealed Cronbach’s a reliability coefficients ranging from
.83 to .88 for the EE subscale and .88 for the SE subscale (Ketterson & Blustein,
1997). In this study, a coefficients were .84 and .86 for the SE and EE subscales,
respectively. The SE and EE subscales’ convergent validity been supported by
showing that their scores predict career exploration outcomes such as vocational
self-concept crystallization and attitudes toward planning (Hamer & Bruch, 1997).
Career Factors Inventory (CFI; Chartrand & Robbins, 1997). The CFI assessed parti-
cipants’ degree of career indecision. The CFI comprises four subscales: need for
career information (NCI; six items), need for self-knowledge (NSK; four items),
career choice anxiety (CCA; six items), and generalized indecisiveness (GI; five
items). Respondents rate each item using a 5-point Likert-type scale. Together,
scores from the NCI and NSK subscales are believed to represent indecision stem-
ming from an informational deficit, whereas those from the CCA and GI subscales
represent affective or personal–emotional difficulties associated with career deci-
sion making (Chartrand et al., 1990); this two-factor model has been supported
empirically (Dickinson & Tokar, 2004).
Previous research has revealed that Cronbach’s a reliability coefficients for the
CFI subscales range from .87 to .91 (Chartrand & Nutter, 1996). In this study, a for
215
216 Journal of Career Development 36(3)
the NI, NSK, CCA, and GI subscales were .78, .81, .86, and .79, respectively. CFI
subscale scores correlate in expected directions with measures of trait anxiety, goal
instability, vocational identity, and self-esteem (Chartrand et al., 1990).
Procedure
Participants signed up for the study using the Psychology Department’s participant
pool. They attended small-group data collection sessions in which they gave
informed consent and completed the measures in a counter-balanced order. The
researcher then provided a debriefing and gave participants documentation of their
participation, which allowed them to earn extra credit in some psychology classes.
Results
Means, standard deviations, and intercorrelations among all the measured variables
are shown in Table 1.
Structural equation modeling (SEM) with latent variables was used to test the fit
of the theoretical model. The SEM procedure analyzed the covariance matrix and
used the maximum likelihood (ML) estimation method with the MPlus Version 2
statistical software (Muthén & Muthén, 1998). Several fit indices were used to eval-
uate the model fit. A comparative fit index (CFIn) value of .95 or greater, a root
mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) value of .06 or less, and a standar-
dized root mean square residual (SRMR) value of .08 or less are considered to reflect
good model fit (Hu & Bentler, 1999).
The first step in SEM is to test a measurement model to assess how well the mea-
sured variables represent their latent constructs. This measurement model then pro-
vides a baseline estimate of fit to which the theoretical (structural) model is
compared. To create latent variables, it is necessary to have multiple indicators of
each. When possible, we used the measures’ subscales as separate indicators of the
latent construct they represent. In some cases, however, we did not have multiple
subscales representing a given construct, so we created item parcels using proce-
dures described by Russell, Kahn, Spoth, and Altmeier (1998).
In the measurement model, the latent variables attachment avoidance and attach-
ment anxiety were each measured by three item parcels from the ECRS-R-avoidance
and ECRS-R-anxiety subscales, respectively. The FI, EI, and AI subscales of the PSI
were used as indicators of the latent construct separation-individuation. Exploration
was measured by three parcels of items drawn from the EE and SE subscales of the
CES, and identity diffusion was measured by three parcels of items from the EOM-
EIS-R diffusion subscale. Finally, the latent variables informational career indeci-
sion and personal–emotional career indecision were measured by the NI and NSK
subscales and the CCA and GI subscales of the CFI, respectively. All the latent vari-
ables were allowed to covary. The measurement model, w2 (131, N ¼ 285) ¼ 261.44,
p < .0001, provided a good fit to the data (CFIn ¼ .96, RMSEA ¼ .06; SRMR ¼ .05).
216
Downing and Nauta
Table 1. Means, Standard Deviations, and Intercorrelations Among the Measured Variables
Measure 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12
1. Avoidance 1.00 .43** .11 .08 .15** .05 .03 .27** .06 .11 .14* .14*
2. Anxiety 1.00 .12* .10 .04 .02 .07 .16** .10 .17** .30** .29**
3. Functional independence 1.00 .80** .61** .10 .09 .01 .14* .04 .10 .08
4. Emotional independence 1.00 .56** .08 .09 .03 .13* .13* .11 .14*
5. Attitudinal independence 1.00 .02 .05 .17** .16** .16** .13* .19**
6. Environmental exploration 1.00 .33** .08 .21** .17** .08 .12*
7. Self-exploration 1.00 .02 .12* .29** .01 .01
8. Identity Diffusion 1.00 .04 .08 .17** .21*
9. Need for career information 1.00 .52** .25** .11
10. Need for self-knowledge 1.00 .06 .06
11. Career choice anxiety 1.00 .52**
12. Generalized indecisiveness 1.00
M 2.97 3.07 31.46 40.45 26.68 15.36 15.44 47.52 22.13 15.31 16.26 14.51
SD 1.24 1.09 9.52 11.80 10.68 5.68 4.51 9.83 4.44 3.40 5.28 4.14
Note. N ¼ 285.
* p < .05. ** p < .01.
217
217
218 Journal of Career Development 36(3)
All the factor loadings were significant, ps < .01, ranging from .57 to .94, and are
available from the authors.
The next step was to test the theoretical model shown in Figure 1. In this model,
informational career indecision was directly predicted by exploration, and personal–
emotional career indecision was directly predicted by identity diffusion and
attachment anxiety. Identity diffusion was predicted by exploration, separation-
individuation, attachment anxiety, and attachment avoidance, and exploration was
directly predicted by separation-individuation. Finally, separation-individuation was
predicted by attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance. Informational and per-
sonal–emotional career indecision were allowed to correlate with each other, as were
attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance. This model, w2(140, N ¼ 285) ¼
275.95, p < .0001, CFIn ¼ .95, SRMR ¼ .06, RMSEA ¼ .06, also provided a good
fit to the data. In addition, the structural model did not differ significantly from the
measurement model in terms of overall fit, Dw2(9, N ¼ 285) ¼ 14.51, p > .05.
The model accounted for 14% of the variance in informational career indecision,
21% of that in personal–emotional career indecision, 10% of that in identity diffu-
sion, 2% of that in exploration, and 5% of that in separation/individuation.
Figure 1 shows the path estimates for the structural model. Most of the theorized
paths were significant and in the predicted direction, although there were some
exceptions. Specifically, contrary to prediction, separation-individuation was signif-
icantly related to neither exploration nor identity diffusion, and both exploration and
attachment anxiety were unrelated to identity diffusion. Finally, whereas the rela-
tionship between exploration and informational career indecision was expected to
be negative, it was actually positive.
The structural model provided a good fit to the data, but we wanted to determine
whether its fit was superior to a plausible alternate model. A more parsimonious
alternative to the model we tested is one in which attachment anxiety and avoidance
have no direct relationships with the identity and career indecision variables but
instead are related to these variables solely via their association with separation-
individuation. To assess this possibility, we tested the fit of a model that was iden-
tical to the structural model but omitted the direct paths between attachment anxiety
and identity diffusion, between attachment anxiety and personal–emotional career
indecision, and between attachment avoidance and identity diffusion. Based on the
fit indices, this alternate structural model, w2(143, N ¼ 285) ¼ 320.82, p < .0001,
CFIn ¼ .94, SRMR ¼ .10, RMSEA ¼ .07, did not meet the criteria for good fit.
Thus, the direct paths between the attachment variables and the identity and career
indecision variables that are posited in the original structural model appear to be
needed.
Discussion
In this study, we examined a theoretical model predicting two dimensions of career
indecision. Although the relationships in the model were derived from theory and
218
Downing and Nauta 219
many of the bivariate relationships had been supported by earlier studies, no previ-
ous research had investigated separation-individuation, exploration, and identity dif-
fusion simultaneously as mediators of the relationship between attachment and
career indecision.
Several of the hypothesized relationships in our model were supported. Consistent
with earlier research (Tokar et al., 2003), our findings suggest that individuals with
higher attachment anxiety may tend to separate or individuate less from parents. How-
ever, as predicted, those with higher attachment avoidance tended to report greater
separation-individuation from parents. Nevertheless, the role of separation-
individuation in the relationship between attachment and career indecision appeared
to be minimal, as separation-individuation was not significantly related to exploration
or identity diffusion. Although this finding was contrary to expectation given family
systems theory’s (e.g., Lopez & Andrews, 1987) contention that independent function-
ing is essential for healthy ego identity development and the argument of Blustein
et al. (1995) that independent functioning will promote career exploration, our study
is not the first to find a lack of support for the role of separation-individuation in career
development. Several other studies have found either no (Blustein et al., 1991; Lucas,
1997; Santos & Coimbra, 2000) or very weak (Kinnier et al., 1990) relationships
between separation-individuation and variables theoretically related to career indeci-
sion. Thus, the role of separation-individuation as a mediator of the relationship
between attachment and career indecision seems questionable.
More recent theoretical approaches have criticized the implicit assumption that
‘‘individuated is better’’ and have proposed models that conceptualize relational
interdependence as functional and necessary for career development. Qualitative
investigations have found support for the latter notion. Individuals describe using
personal (often parent or close sibling) relationships as sources of support and moti-
vation throughout difficult junctures in the decision-making process, often using
others as a ‘‘sounding board’’ or seeking support for a change in direction
(Schultheiss, Kress, Manzi, & Glasscock, 2001). In the current study, the importance
of support in close relationships was conceptualized as an internal working model,
but it may be the case that ongoing positive interactions would predict healthy out-
comes better than does individuation.
Similar to Tokar et al. (2003), who found that vocational identity crystallization
partially mediated the relationship between attachment and career indecision, we
also obtained support for the role of identity in the relationship between attachment
and career indecision. In this case, attachment avoidance was associated with greater
identity diffusion, and identity diffusion, in turn, was associated with greater per-
sonal–emotional career indecision. Thus, high degrees of attachment avoidance may
be associated with career indecision, in part, because those who avoid close relation-
ships with others and feel others cannot be trusted tend to struggle with forming a
clear and stable sense of themselves and their preferences across a variety of life
realms. This finding is consistent with arguments (Blustein et al., 1989, 1995) that
a ‘‘secure base’’ provided by one’s social support network helps to facilitate the
219
220 Journal of Career Development 36(3)
development of a healthy ego identity. Unlike Tokar et al. (2003), however, we did
not find support for identity diffusion as a mediator of the relationship between
attachment anxiety and career indecision, as attachment anxiety and identity diffu-
sion were not significantly related. It is possible that the relationship between attach-
ment anxiety and identity holds only when identity is conceptualized narrowly, with
respect to a vocational identity, as in the study of Tokar et al., but this speculation
awaits further empirical inquiry.
Although apparently not a function of greater identity diffusion, attachment anxi-
ety, like attachment avoidance, did appear to be a risk factor for greater personal–
emotional career indecision. Therefore, ultimately those high in both attachment
avoidance and anxiety may experience greater degrees of a form of career indecision
that is believed to persist even when individuals have adequate information about
themselves and the world of work (Lucas, 1997). The findings from our study sug-
gest that the link between attachment and career indecision may vary depending on
the nature of one’s relationships with important others. As a result, conceptualizing
attachment dichotomously as secure versus insecure would likely be inadequate for
understanding its association with career-related variables.
An initially surprising finding from our study was the positive relationship between
exploration and informational career indecision. Having explored the self and the
world of work should reduce career indecision, as it provides individuals with a basis
for prioritizing their career goals (Gati & Saka, 2001; Wanberg & Muchinsky, 1992),
so we had expected this relationship to be negative. We believe our unexpected pos-
itive relationship may be a function of our having used the CES exploration subscales
as a measure of exploration. This measure asks respondents about their degree of
recent (the last 3 months) career exploration of the self and the world of work as
opposed to the degree of exploration ever conducted. Those high in informational
career indecision may currently be engaging in greater exploration as a healthy way
of taking steps to reduce their indecision. Accordingly, because of the time frame
assessed by the exploration measure we used, our model may have made more sense
had we considered exploration as a consequence, rather than an antecedent, of infor-
mational career indecision. A measure of exploration that asked participants about
their degree of self and environmental exploration at any time in the past would have
been more consistent with the theoretical order of the variables in our specified model.
We suspect that the recency of exploration assessed by the CES also accounts for the
nonsignificant association between exploration and identity diffusion in our model. It
may simply take time for information gleaned from exploration to result in a clear pic-
ture of one’s preferences and a commitment to an identity, and exploration activities
conducted in the last 3 months may not yet have achieved that outcome.
220
Downing and Nauta 221
221
222 Journal of Career Development 36(3)
model may vary by racial/ethnic group, previous research (e.g., Tokar et al., 2003)
has revealed gender differences in many of the bivariate relationships specified in
our model. Our male sample was not of a sufficient size to allow for an examination
of gender invariance, but doing so in future research would be important. Likewise,
previous research (e.g., O’Brien, 1996; Salami & Aremu, 2007; Tokar et al., 2003;
Wolfe & Betz, 2004) has revealed differences in maternal and paternal attachment
and separation-individuation and their associations with career-related constructs. It
would be useful to attempt to link these differences to theory and, if warranted,
examine maternal and paternal separation-individuation as distinct constructs with
differential relations to other constructs in our model.
Finally, our model, although including several predictors, explained only 14% of
the variance in informational career indecision and 21% of the variance in personal–
emotional career indecision. Thus, a great deal of variance in both remains to be
explained by other variables not included in our model. Other constructs known
to be associated with attachment anxiety or avoidance include decreased self-
esteem, self-efficacy, and internal locus of control (Brennan et al., 1998) and might
be important to include in future examinations of the relationship between attach-
ment and career indecision.
Conclusions
The theoretical model tested in this study helps to explain some of the variance in
career indecision by using collective knowledge of a person’s attachment function-
ing, separation-individuation, exploration behaviors, and ego identity diffusion. This
pan-theoretical approach seems necessary considering the varied perspectives that
offer insight into the difficulties that contribute to career indecision and helps to
illustrate ways in which career development intersects with personal development
and interpersonal relationship functioning. The results seem to support the use of
nontraditional vocational counseling approaches, including the use of interpersonal
relationships to help resolve career decision-making difficulties. Although much
previous research has operationalized attachment as secure/insecure, the current
model shows that development may differ depending on the types of attachment
functioning the client exhibits, whether anxious, avoidant, or even a combination
of both.
Acknowledgment
The authors thank Jeffrey H. Kahn for providing feedback on an earlier draft of this
article.
Authors’ Note
A version of this research was presented at the 2008 International Counseling
Psychology Convention in Chicago, IL. The research was based on the first author’s
master’s thesis under direction of the second author. Haley M. Downing is now
222
Downing and Nauta 223
Funding
The authors received no financial support for their research and/or authorship of
this article.
References
Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1989). Attachments beyond infancy. American Psychologist, 44,
709-716.
Armsden, G. C., & Greenberg, M. T. (1987). The inventory of parent and peer attachment:
Individual differences and their relationship to psychological well-being in adolescence.
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 16, 427-454.
Bennion, L. D., & Adams, G. R. (1986). A revision of the extended version of the objective
measure of ego identity status: An identity instrument for use with late adolescents. Jour-
nal of Adolescent Research, 1, 183-198.
Blustein, D. L. (1992). Applying current theory and research in career exploration to practice.
The Career Development Quarterly, 41, 174-184.
Blustein, D. L., Devenis, L. E., & Kidney, B. A. (1989). Relationship between the
identity formation process and career development. Journal of Counseling Psychology,
36, 196-202.
Blustein, D. L., & Flum, H. (1999). A self-determination perspective of interests and
exploration in career development. In M. L. Savickas & A. R. Spokane (Eds.), Vocational
interests: Meaning, measurement, and counseling use (pp. 345-368). Palo Alto, CA:
Davies-Black.
Blustein, D. L., Prezioso, M. S., & Schultheiss, D. P. (1995). Attachment theory and career
development: Current status and future directions. The Counseling Psychologist, 23,
416-432.
Blustein, D. L., Walbridge, M. M., Friedlander, M. L., & Palladino, D. E. (1991). Contribu-
tions of psychological separation and parental attachment to the career development pro-
cess. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 38, 39-50.
Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Parent-child attachment and healthy human development.
New York: Basic Books.
Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measurement of adult attach-
ment: An integrative overview. In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory
and close relationships (pp. 46-76). New York: Guilford.
223
224 Journal of Career Development 36(3)
Chandler, L. A., & Gallagher, R. P. (1996). Developing a taxonomy for problems seen at a
university counseling center. Measurement & Evaluation in Counseling & Development,
29, 4-12.
Chartrand, J. M., & Nutter, K. J. (1996). The Career Factors Inventory: Theory and applica-
tions. Journal of Career Assessment, 4, 205-218.
Chartrand, J. M., & Robbins, S. B. (1997). Career factors inventory applications and techni-
cal guide. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Chartrand, J. M., Robbins, S. B., Morrill, W. H., & Boggs, K. (1990). Development and
validation of the career factors inventory. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 37, 491-501.
Dickinson, J., & Tokar, D. M. (2004). Structural and discriminant validity of the career factors
inventory. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 65, 239-254.
Fairchild, A. J., & Finney, S. J. (2006). Investigating validity evidence for the experiences in
close relationships-revised questionnaire. Educational and Psychological Measurement,
66, 116-135.
Flum, H. (2001). Relational dimensions in career development. Journal of Vocational
Behavior, 59, 1-16.
Fraley, R. C., & Shaver, P. R. (2000). Adult romantic attachment: Theoretical developments, emer-
ging controversies, and unanswered questions. Review of General Psychology, 4, 132-154.
Gaffner, D. C., & Hazler, R. J. (2002). Factors related to indecisiveness and career indecision
in undecided college students. Journal of College Student Development, 43, 317-326.
Gati, I., & Saka, N. (2001). High school students’ career-related decision-making difficulties.
Journal of Counseling & Development, 79, 331-340.
Guay, F., Ratelle, C. F., Senecal, C., Larose, S., & Deschenes, A. (2006). Distinguishing
developemental from chronic career indecision: Self-efficacy, autonomy, and social sup-
port. Journal of Career Assessment, 14, 235-251.
Hamer, R. J., & Bruch, M. A. (1997). Personality factors and inhibited career development:
Testing the unique contribution of shyness. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 50, 382-400.
Hoffman, J. A. (1984). Psychological separation of late adolescents from their parents.
Journal of Counseling Psychology, 31, 170-178.
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis:
Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1-55.
Ketterson, T. U., & Blustein, D. L. (1997). Attachment relationships and the career explora-
tion process. Career Development Quarterly, 46, 167-178.
Kinnier, R. T., Brigman, S. L., & Noble, F. C. (1990). Career indecision and family enmesh-
ment. Journal of Counseling and Development, 68, 309-312.
Kracke, B. (1997). Parental behaviors and adolescents’ career exploration. The Career
Development Quarterly, 45, 341-350.
Kram, K. E., & Isabella, L. A. (1985). Mentoring alternatives: The role of peer relationships in
career development. Academy of Management Journal, 28, 110-132.
Krumboltz, J. D. (1992). The wisdom of indecision. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 41,
239-244.
224
Downing and Nauta 225
Lee, H., & Hughey, K. F. (2001). The relationship of psychological separation and parental
attachment to the career maturity of college freshmen from intact families. Journal of
Career Development, 27, 279-293.
Lopez, F. G., & Andrews, S. (1987). Career indecision: A family systems perspective. Journal
of Counseling and Development, 65, 304-307.
Lucas, M. (1997). Identity development, career development, and psychological separation
from parents: Similarities and differences between men and women. Journal of Counsel-
ing Psychology, 44, 123-132.
Marcia, J. E. (1966). Development and validation of ego-identity status. Journal of Person-
ality and Social Psychology, 3, 551-558.
Marcia, J. E. (1980). Identity in adolescence. In J. Adelson (Ed.), Handbook of adolescent psy-
chology (pp. 159-187). New York: Wiley.
Mattanah, J. F., Hancock, G. R., & Brand, B. L. (2004). Parental attachment, separation-
individuation, and college student adjustment: A structural equation analysis of media-
tional effects. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 51, 213-225.
Multon, K. D., Heppner, M. J., Gysbers, N. C., Zook, C., & Ellis-Kalton, C. A. (2001). Client
psychological distress: An important factor in career counseling. Career Development
Quarterly, 49, 324-335.
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998). Mplus user’s guide. Los Angeles, CA: Authors.
Nauta, M. M., & Kahn, J. H. (2007). Identity status, consistency and differentiation of inter-
ests, and career decision self-efficacy. Journal of Career Assessment, 15, 55-65.
O’Brien, K. M. (1996). The influence of psychological separation and parental attachment on
the career development of adolescent women. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 48, 257-274.
O’Brien, K. M., Friedman, S. M., Tipton, L. C., & Linn, S. G. (2000). Attachment, separation,
and women’s vocational development: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Counseling
Psychology, 47, 301-315.
Rainey, L. M., & Borders, L. D. (1997). Influential factors in career orientation and career
aspiration of early adolescent girls. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 44, 160-172.
Roisman, G. I., Bahadur, M. A., & Oster, H. (2000). Infant attachment security as a discrimi-
nant predictor of career development in late adolescence. Journal of Adolescent Research,
15, 531-545.
Rosenthal, D. A., Gurney, R. M., & Moore, S. M. (1981). From trust to intimacy: A new
inventory for examining Erikson. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 10, 525-537.
Russell, D. W., Kahn, J. H., Spoth, R., & Altmaier, E. M. (1998). Analyzing data from experi-
mental studies: A latent variable structural equation modeling approach. Journal of Coun-
seling Psychology, 45, 18-29.
Ryan, N. E., Solberg, V. S., & Brown, S. D. (1996). Family dysfunction, parental attachment,
and career search self-efficacy among community college students. Journal of Counseling
Psychology, 43, 84-89.
Salami, S. O., & Aremu, A. O. (2007). Impact of parent-child relationship on the career devel-
opment process of high school students in Ibadan, Nigeria. Career Development Interna-
tional, 12, 596-616.
225
226 Journal of Career Development 36(3)
Santos, P. J., & Coimbra, J. L. (2000). Psychological separation and dimensions of career
indecision in secondary school students. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 56, 346-362.
Schultheiss, D. E., & Blustein, D. L. (1994). Role of adolescent-parent relationships in college
student development and adjustment. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 41, 248-255.
Schultheiss, D. E. P., Kress, H. M., Manzi, A. J., & Glasscock, J. M. J. (2001). Relational
influences in career development: A qualitative inquiry. The Counseling Psychologist,
29, 216-239.
Shaffer, P. A., Vogel, D. L., & Wei, M. (2006). The mediating roles of anticipated risks,
anticipated benefits, and attitudes on the decision to seek professional help: An attachment
perspective. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 53, 442-452.
Sibley, C. G., Fischer, R., & Liu, J. H. (2005). Reliability and validity of the Revised Experi-
ences in Close Relationships (ECR-R) self-report measure of adult romantic attachment.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 1524-1536.
Stumpf, S. A., Colarelli, S. M., & Hartman, K. (1983). Development of the career exploration
survey (CES). Journal of Vocational Behavior, 22, 191-226.
Super, D. E. (1957). The psychology of careers. New York: Harper.
Tokar, D. M., Withrow, J. R., Hall, R. J., & Moradi, B. (2003). Psychological separation,
attachment security, vocational self-concept crystallization, and career indecision: A struc-
tural equation analysis. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 50, 3-19.
Van Ecke, Y. (2007). Attachment style and dysfunctional career thoughts: How attachment style
can affect the career counseling process. Career Development Quarterly, 55, 339-350.
Vogel, D. L., & Wei, M. (2005). Adult attachment and help-seeking intent: The mediating
roles of psychological distress and perceived social support. Journal of Counseling
Psychology, 52, 347-357.
Wanberg, C. R., & Muchinsky, P. M. (1992). A typology of career decision status: Validity
extension of the vocational decision status model. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 27,
71-80.
Wei, M., Vogel, D. L., Ku, T.-Y., & Zakalik, R. A. (2005). Adult attachment, affect
regulation, negative mood, and interpersonal problems: The mediating roles of emotional
reactivity and emotional cutoff. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 52, 14-24.
Wolfe, J. B., & Betz, N. E. (2004). The relationship of attachment variables to career decision-
making self-efficacy and fear of commitment. The Career Development Quarterly, 52,
363-369.
Yalom, I. D., & Leszcz, M. (2005). The theory and practice of group psychotherapy (5th ed.).
New York: Basic Books.
Bios
Haley M. Downing completed her master’s degree in clinical-counseling psychology at Illi-
nois State University and is currently a doctoral student in the Collaborative Program in Coun-
seling Psychology at the University of Akron. Her research interests include career and
identity development and their intersection with issues of social class. In her free time, she
enjoys hiking, reading, and live sports and music.
226
Downing and Nauta 227
Margaret M. Nauta received her PhD in counseling psychology from Iowa State University.
She is a professor in the psychology department at Illinois State University, where she is
affiliated with the clinical-counseling master’s degree program. She conducts research on
social and cognitive factors associated with individuals’ career development and on the rela-
tionships among career interests, self-efficacy, and personality. She enjoys reading, playing
piano, and spending time with her family.
227