0% found this document useful (0 votes)
68 views17 pages

Peticidio

Uploaded by

gtanikado
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
68 views17 pages

Peticidio

Uploaded by

gtanikado
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 17

Anthrozoös

A multidisciplinary journal of the interactions between people and


other animals

ISSN: (Print) (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rfan20

Peticide: An Analysis of Online News Media


Articles of Human Suicide Involving Pet Animals

Janette Young, James Andrew Oxley, V. Tamara Montrose & Harold Herzog

To cite this article: Janette Young, James Andrew Oxley, V. Tamara Montrose & Harold Herzog
(2022): Peticide: An Analysis of Online News Media Articles of Human Suicide Involving Pet
Animals, Anthrozoös, DOI: 10.1080/08927936.2022.2109292

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/08927936.2022.2109292

© 2022 The Author(s). Published by Informa


UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis
Group

Published online: 26 Aug 2022.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 1028

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=rfan20
ANTHROZOÖS
https://doi.org/10.1080/08927936.2022.2109292

Peticide: An Analysis of Online News Media Articles of Human


Suicide Involving Pet Animals
a b
Janette Young , James Andrew Oxley , V. Tamara Montrosec, and
Harold Herzogd
a
Allied Health and Human Performance, University of South Australia, South Australia, Australia;
b
Department of Livestock and One Health, University of Liverpool, Neston, UK; cIndependent Researcher,
Manchester, UK; dDepartment of Psychology, Western Carolina University, Cullowhee, NC, USA

ABSTRACT KEYWORDS
While pets may be protective for some people at risk of suicide, Animals; familicide;
they may also become a risk factor or even become co-victims homicide-suicide; human–
when humans end their own lives. It is important to protect animal interaction; peticide;
against simplistic approaches to human–animal relationships, pets
especially where simplification may endanger human and/or
animal lives. Using publicly accessible online media articles
between 2010 and 2020, this research sought to progress our
understanding of suicidal acts involving pet animals. Sixty-one
articles from six countries were identified; a mixed-methods
qualitative descriptive (QD) approach to analysis was undertaken
composed of descriptive statistical mapping followed by thematic
content analysis. Almost 90% of the articles reported the deaths
of multiple humans and 23% reported the deaths of multiple
animals. A total of 116 animals were identified: mainly dogs, but
also 8 cats, 2 rabbits, and 2 non-specified pets. Most animals
died, with only nine surviving. Five key categories of scenarios
were identified: extended suicides, mercy killings, suicide pacts,
family annihilators, and unique. A further level of analysis was
undertaken focused on the family annihilator reports (44/61
articles) using a published homicide-suicide typology. Key points
to emerge from this analysis include the possibly higher
vulnerability of dogs compared with other species. The terms
“extended suicide” and “peticide” are discussed with the
recommendation that the killing of pet animals be linguistically
aligned with that of other killings. A focus on human–animal
relationships reveals commonly unexplored intersections across
criminology, mental health, and domestic violence and suggests
the potential for collaboration across these fields driven by multi-
species awareness. This research adds to arguments for data on
animal presence in scenarios of human violence to be collected
so that responses to protect vulnerable animals, and humans, can
be developed.

CONTACT James Oxley [email protected] Department of Livestock and One Health, University of Liverpool,
Neston, UK
© 2022 The Author(s). Published by Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/
licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly
cited.
2 J. YOUNG ET AL.

Suicide is defined as “Death caused by self-directed injurious behavior with any intent to
die as a result of the behavior” (Crosby et al., 2011). It is estimated that more than 700,000
people die by suicide each year (WHO, 2019), and for every individual who dies by suicide,
an estimated 20 non-fatal suicide attempts occur (Harrison & Henley, 2014). Suicide has a
ripple effect: for every person who dies by suicide, an estimated 135 people are impacted
(Cerel et al., 2019). Identifying protective and preventative sources and approaches to this
human tragedy are needed, and one emerging aspect in this regard is the close relation-
ships that many humans have with companion animals. Companion animals, hereafter
referred to as “pets,” such as dogs, cats, birds, and rabbits, are frequently regarded as
“family members” (Arahori et al., 2017; Cohen, 2002; Howell et al., 2015). They are a
global phenomenon; in 2018 there were an estimated 470 million pet dogs and 370
million pet cats (Statista, 2019).
There is an emerging body of research focusing on the complex intersections between
human suicidality and pets. Three recent papers report the role of human–pet relation-
ships in reducing suicidal behavior for some individuals (Hawkins et al., 2021; Love,
2021; Young et al., 2020). However, Love (2021) found that while pets may be protective
against suicide at some points, for some people they may also become a risk factor.
Indeed, there is a small body of research that explores the topic of pets as co-victims in
human suicide (Herzog et al., 2021; Oxley et al., 2016), including scenarios of suicide invol-
ving more than one person. Cooke (2013) refers to the death of animals in tandem with
humans as “extended suicide,” while Palazzo et al. (2021) call this phenomenon “peticide.”
There is a pressing need to explore both the protective and risk sides of this multi-species
intersection as simplistic approaches to human–animal relationships may endanger
human and/or animal lives: for example, in clinical health settings, one of the authors
has been asked more than once, “should we see if a dog/pet helps?”
Researching suicide and the killing of pets is emotive and complex. Hence, seeking
means of extending our understanding in distanced, non-invasive ways are inherent to
the aim of not re-traumatizing persons impacted by such deaths. This led to the research
described here in which online news media articles reporting the presence (including
deaths) of pet animals in incidents of suicide were sourced, collated, and analyzed
both quantitatively and qualitatively using a qualitative descriptive lens (Kim et al.,
2017; Sullivan-Bolyai et al., 2005) to provide insights into this little-known phenomenon.
This approach continues research investigating the inclusion (or not) of pets in Australian
coronial investigations of suicide (Mattock et al., 2022). The findings of this research were
that pets are treated as insignificant and are mostly invisible in these humancentric cor-
onial analyses of suicide. Even when cross-species relationships had been noted as highly
significant in deceased persons’ lives, human–animal relationships were not presented as
legitimate foci in identifying future preventative approaches.
Three points need to be flagged with readers prior to presenting the data and analysis.
First, the data were obtained through publicly reported stories of suicide. There is a gen-
erally accepted embargo on reporting of suicides based on concerns of copy-cat actions
by vulnerable others (Jordan & McNiel, 2021; Niederkrotenthaler et al., 2020; Stack, 2002).
Hence, the media reports we obtained are exceptions to this rule. Second, the reports
were written by journalists. Other research exploring news media reporting has demon-
strated that the selection of what is newsworthy can be highly biased, with skews to
ANTHROZOÖS 3

dominant social discourses (Young et al., 2017) and the overlooking of true data patterns
(Liem & Koenraadt, 2007). Third, both these preceding factors may be why most articles
described murder-suicides, even though this is a very rare phenomenon (Jordan & McNiel,
2021). However, while recognizing that this research is highly selective, the lack of prior
research positioned the technique of media surveying as one of the very few sources
available for developing understandings of an under-researched and unrecognized
human, and animal, tragedy.

Methods
From Google and Yahoo news, between January 2010 and May 2020, online articles were
collected using the terms “pets,” “animal,” “dog,” “cat,” and “suicide.” To ensure duplicates
were removed, unique identifiers such as date of report and location of incidents and
victims’ names were tracked. Three cases were from a previous research report (Cooke,
2013).
Initially, articles were collected from the USA, the UK, and Australia (the locations of the
authors). However, owing to the low number of articles in the UK and Australia, the search
was extended to include other countries. Articles were collected by three individuals and
crosschecked for duplicates. While the focus was on suicide, many reports were of
murder-suicides. Hence, data on human victims were also collated. For each article, the
following information was gathered: number and species of pets (including deaths, sur-
vivals, pets noted as being present), the sex and age of the person who died by
suicide, number of victims/sex/age (suicides and murders), number of child and adult
victims, and cause of death for both humans and animals. The focus of this report is on
the pet animals.
A mixed-methods, qualitative descriptive (QD) approach to analysis was undertaken
composed of the generation of descriptive statistics followed by a thematic content
analysis of the articles. QD is commonly used in exploring health issues as it aims to gen-
erate clear descriptive summaries of phenomena to provide practical suggestions to
improve care responses (Kim et al., 2017; Sullivan-Bolyai et al., 2005). The overarching
aim was to better understand suicidal acts including pets. Descriptive data were calcu-
lated by the second, third, and fourth authors. The thematic process and analyses were
undertaken by the first and second authors, with themes and coding being independently
identified and then finalized collaboratively. This study did not require ethical approval as
the data were available through the public domain.

Results
We located 66 news articles. Five articles were removed owing to them being outside the
time frame (pre-2010), not being relevant to pets (e.g., man kills wife and then himself as a
result of a prior animal abuse trial), or involving pets but outside the scope of the study
(e.g., a dog following his owners who had jumped into a river). This left us with a total of
61 articles for analysis. The majority of these were from the USA (78.7%; 48/61), with the
remaining being from the UK (9.8%; 6/61), Australia (4.9%; 3/61), India (3.3%; 2/61),
Canada (1.6%; 1/61), and Thailand (1.6%; 1/61).
4 J. YOUNG ET AL.

Animal Victims
Of the 61 reports, the majority (77.0%; 47/61) involved a single animal, with the remaining
involving multiple animals (23.0%; 14/61). A total of 116 animals were identified; of these,
the majority were dogs (89.7%; 104/116), although cats (6.9%; 8/116), rabbits (1.7%; 2/
116), and non-specified “pet(s)” (1.7%; 2/116) were also identified. Of the scenarios invol-
ving multiple animals (n = 14), only one did not involve a dog (cat and rabbit). It is impor-
tant to note that a single scenario involved the death of 31 dogs; however, removing this
report from the data still means that dogs were the most common victims (85.9%; 73/85).
A total of 106 animal deaths were reported. In addition, seven dogs survived and two
were unharmed (one in a suicide pact and one survived unharmed in a household murder
where another dog was killed). Of the surviving dogs, one was suspected to have been
attacked incidentally (perhaps seeking to protect their owner who was the target of
the attack), while the others seem to have been deliberately targeted and only acciden-
tally survived.
The dominant weapon used was a gun (43 animal deaths; 3 survivals), and this was the
weapon most commonly used to take human lives (either victims, their own lives, or both)
as well. Gassing via carbon monoxide (usually automobile exhausts) was the second most
common form of weapon. It is worth noting that if the one instance where 31 dogs were
killed is removed, this makes gassing via carbon monoxide (7 animals) equitable to
reports of poisoning (6 animals) and impacts (e.g., jumping from a height) (5 animals).
In reports of 11 animal deaths, no means of killing was identified (9 reports in comparison
to 3 unknown means reports of humans). Table 1 summarizes the modes of killing used
on animals and those used in the deaths of humans.
In 62.3% (38/61) of the articles, the death of a pet was in the title. The names (e.g.,
Gizmo, Frodo) of 10 pets were reported (9 dogs, 1 cat), the breed of 12 dogs was ident-
ified, and 8 articles included a photo of the pet, both with and without their owner. One-

Table 1. Animal deaths: method of killing, survivals, and comparison with human death methods.
Deaths of animals
Mode of killing of animal(s) by species Animal survivals Mode of human deaths*
Gun 38 dogs 3 dogs survived 41 reports same
3 cats (plus a dog left in garage
1 rabbit separately)
1 pet
Gas (usually carbon 31 + 6 dogs 2 dogs 4 reports same
monoxide) 1 cat
Poisoning 4 dogs 2 reports same
2 cats 1 human survival
Impact (jumping from 5 dogs 3 reports same
height or train)
Strangulation 2 cats 3 different means – knife, car (jumped
in front), hanging
Drowning 1 dog 1 dog (rescued) Human survived attempted drowning
(via car driven into lake)
Knife 1 dog Murder victim same; Perpetrator =
hanging
Unknown 10 dogs 1 dog (left) Humans – strangled, poisoned (3),
1 rabbit jumped off cliff, gun (2)
*Murder-suicides complicate these data as at times multiple means were used by perpetrators.
ANTHROZOÖS 5

third (32.8%; 20/61) of articles used the term “pet” at least once in the text, often combin-
ing this with the species (e.g., “pet rabbit”). The term “family dog” or “family pet” was used
in nine articles (7 articles referred to the “family dog,” with 4 of these references also being
in the articles’ title; and 2 articles referred to the “family pet”). We note that while “com-
panion animal” is preferred in the human–animal studies field, rather than “pet” (Howell
et al., 2022), none of the news media articles used this term, but one-third (20/61) did
include the term “pet” at least once. These articles were from all the countries found
except one: Canada, where only one article was sourced anyway. Arguably, “pet” is the
everyday term recognized and in use.

Human Victims
The majority of the incidents involved murder-suicides, where one (or more) individual(s)
killed others(s) and themselves (82.0%; 50/61). This category includes suspected suicide
pacts, where one individual killed the other individual. The remainder of cases were
suicides where one or more individuals killed themselves (18.0%; 11/61). In the
reports of suicide, 54.5% (6/11) involved an individual female, 18.2% (2/11) involved
an individual male, and 27.3% (3/11) involved multiple individuals. The victim survived
in two reports.
In the murder-suicides, 88% (44/50) of these involved a single perpetuator and 4% (2/
50) involved a pair of perpetuators. In 8% (4/50) of cases, the number of perpetuators was
unknown. In 66% (33/50) of these cases, the perpetuator was male, while in 12% (6/50) the
perpetuator was female. For 4% (2/50) of cases, a male–female couple was reported to
have been responsible for the murder-suicide. In the remainder of the reports, the sex
of the perpetuators was unknown. The perpetuator attempted to kill themselves in all
these incidents and was reported to have done so in the majority of cases (92%; 46/50).
The number of human victims killed by the perpetuator (not including their suicide)
varied, with 56% (28/50) involving single victims and 36% (18/50) involving multiple
victims. In 8% (4/50) of reports, the number of victims was unknown. Forty percent
(20/50) of victims were individual females, 6% (3/50) were individual males, and 42%
(21/50) involved multiple victims. In the remainder of the reports, the number of
victims was unknown. Few victims survived these attacks, with only one individual
male victim survivor (2%; 1/50) being reported.
As identified in Table 1, the most common weapon used in these incidents was a gun.
One of the most obvious features of the news reports was that the majority (86.9%; 53/61)
identified more than one human being as having died, either through suicides involving
multiple individuals (4.9%; 3/61) or via murder-suicide (82.0%; 50/61). Investigating this
phenomenon underpins the qualitative thematic process outlined next.

Human and Animal Locations of Deaths


In 58 of the 61 reports (omitting a case where a dog was left at home with the deceased
wife and two cases where the location of death was unreported), only 7 human and
animal deaths were not in the human and animal’s residence. The non-residence-based
suicides were usually single-person suicides (6 of 7; 1 suicide pact), where the suicidal
6 J. YOUNG ET AL.

human had chosen a death location (cliff, lake, train line, remote location, work – veter-
inary clinic, hotel room).

Thematic Analysis
Thematic content analysis was undertaken, driven by our interest in understanding the
animal experience within the human violence reported and seeking to “organize” the
bulk of reports that involved more than one deceased human. As most articles focused
on the human victims, the first round of theming involved grouping the human scenarios.
In the second approach, we sought to identify patterns of animal involvement. This
included incorporating numerical data on species and survival or fatality of animals
within themed human scenarios, especially family annihilation. Figure 1 summarizes
these results.
Initial theming identified five core scenarios:
Extended Suicides (n = 5/61): Where there was a general indication that the deceased
human had a sense of extended self that had included the pet animal, as per Cooke
(2013).
Mercy Killings (n = 3/61): Where there was an overt statement that the deceased had
been concerned that their pet(s) would not receive adequate/appropriate care once
their human caregiver was gone.
Suicide Pacts (n = 6/61): Where it was reported that there was evidence that a couple
(usually), or multiple family members, had agreed to end their lives together.
Family Annihilators (n = 44/61): In a large majority of cases, a single perpetrator had
taken the life of at least one human family member and attempted or succeeded in
killing a pet(s) and then had ended (or attempted to end) their own life.

Figure 1. Thematic summary: themes, subthemes, numbers of articles, and animal deaths/survivals by
species.
ANTHROZOÖS 7

Unique (n = 3/61): Three reports stood alone. In the first article, another human and a
dog were killed by accident in the process of a person taking their own life. In the
second article, a woman tried to help a friend to end her own life along with her two
dogs. The dogs died but the friend survived. The third article focused on a dog who
was shot in the eye in a domestic murder-suicide; the human victims involved were
not identified.
To understand the mass of family annihilator scenarios (72.1%), we used the homicide-
suicide typology developed by Jordan and McNiel (2021). Their typology was chosen as it
is very recent, builds on previous similar work in this field (McPhedran et al., 2018; Wood
Harper & Voigt, 2007), and has already been cited (Sun et al., 2021). In addition, while
there are critiques of using generic frameworks to understand homicide-suicide that do
not account for regional and cultural contexts (McPhedran et al., 2018), given that only
four of the articles coded “family annihilator” were not from the USA, it was deemed
appropriate to use a USA-based analysis. The proportion of gun deaths in the media
articles and the location of many of these in the USA is in keeping with this country’s
pattern: high gun ownership accompanied by high rates of gun deaths (Eargle &
Esmail, 2016). Jordan and McNiel’s (2021) typology used machine learning to analyze
2447 deaths across the USA via a national reporting system, so is arguably the most objec-
tive approach possible in an emotive field.
The homicide-suicide typology developed by Jordan and McNiel (2021) has eight
categories:

1. Intimate Partner – Relational,


2. Extrafamilial,
3. Intimate Partner-Distress,
4. Other Family,
5. Intimate Partner – Physical Health,
6. Filicide (child),
7. Familicide (multiple victims always including a child), and
8. Indiscriminate/Rage (multiple victims).

Using this framework to screen the family annihilator scenarios, four of these typolo-
gies were seen to be reported (1, 4, 6, 7 – italicized). The theming process was undertaken
independently by the first two authors. Only 7 of the 61 articles were not initially indepen-
dently agreed on. In four of the cases, there was insufficient information in the article.
These articles were less than 200 words long. However, owing to the term “homicide-
suicide” being used in the article, these four, plus two highly indeterminate scenarios,
were categorized as a sub-category of the family annihilator theme.
Given that the first-level theming code identified was “family annihilator,” all second-
level scenarios were familial. Hence, typology 2 (Jordan & McNiel, 2021) was excluded as
only 2 non-familial deaths were reported in the 61 articles. Given the dearth of infor-
mation, neither typology 3 nor 8 were used. In addition, our first-level theme of
“suicide pact” seemed to equate to Jordan and McNiel’s (2021) fifth typology. Once
these human categories were used to categorize the articles, a summative analysis on
the pets’ death was undertaken (see Figure 1).
8 J. YOUNG ET AL.

Our analysis of the family annihilator subthemes did not reveal any exclusionary pat-
terns. However, what this indicates is that pets may be victims of all the forms of
murder-suicide identified. No patterns of exception that could have indicated that
animals may be less vulnerable in some forms of murder-suicide emerged. The human
focus of homicidal intent (e.g., partner, child, both) does not exclude animals as potential
victims. Within this, the dominance of dogs as victims continued to stand out, although
no clear patterns regarding other species emerged. However, as noted, only just over 10%
(12/116) of reports involved non-canine species.

Discussion
This study aimed to better understand suicidal acts including pets. The majority of the
incidents reported involved homicide-suicides, with the remainder being suicides. Most
animals died in these incidents, with only nine surviving. The majority of pets that
were killed were dogs, though the deaths of cats and rabbits were also reported. A
gun was the weapon predominantly used in these incidents. Five core scenarios for
human suicides including animals were identified: extended suicides, mercy killings,
suicide pacts, family annihilators, and unique. Further analysis of the family annihilator
reports highlighted the higher vulnerability of dogs compared with other pets. Given
the lack of previously published research on this topic (pets and suicide) and the even
more limited number of articles focusing on human suicides that include animal
deaths, these stories establish that animal deaths in scenarios of human suicide do
occur and, as indicated by the emotive responses to animal deaths and injuries in
some of the articles, can have traumatic impacts by themselves.
This discussion will focus on the species identified in the reports, suggestions as to the
motives of human caregivers for taking animal lives when suicidal, terminology, and the
societal animal turn. Over all of this discussion sit questions regarding notions of pets as
family: specifically, are these killings of animals grim, dark manifestations of the identifi-
cation of pets as family members?

Species
This collation of articles points to the huge vulnerability of the animals enmeshed in
everyday human lives that we call pets and increasingly identify as family (Arahori
et al., 2017; Dotson & Hyatt, 2008). Even noting the inherent skewing of scenarios
owing to this being an analysis of news media reporting, dogs present as dramatically
at risk in cases of human violence. Yet, as shown by the small number of cat deaths
and (perhaps surprisingly) rabbits as well, no species of pet is necessarily exempt from
the risks humans pose to their lives. A second factor that stands out starkly is the delib-
erateness in many of the animal killings. This is stark: the forms of killing used seem delib-
erately chosen to be lethal to the animals in question, with pets commonly shot or gassed.
Where any details are provided, animals seem to have been killed either prior to the (final)
suicide of the human (in the cases of homicide-suicide) or with the human(s) (e.g., gas-
sings). Only two articles indicate that the animal death was not deliberate, and two
may have been unintentional.
ANTHROZOÖS 9

One of the questions considered at length by the authors was “Why so few cats?” Glob-
ally, it is estimated there are 370 million cats kept as pets, a not distant comparison to the
470 million pet dogs that are kept (Statista, 2019). It may simply be that cats are less likely
to be reported as killed (which raises the question of why their deaths would be over-
looked compared with dogs). However, it may be a reality that reflects species capabilities.
Cats are perhaps far better at escaping and hiding. Compared with dogs, cats can climb
up high, they are less likely to make a noise once hidden, and they may be able to escape
the site of violence more readily, perhaps through a window or cat-flap. It is also possible
that they are less incorporated into killers’ “family” conceptions and may not be con-
sidered as family members in the same way that dogs are.
The two rabbit killings raise similar queries regarding species death reporting and
inclusion in concepts of family. There is some evidence that rabbits are an increasingly
popular pet species (DeMello, 2016), and a survey by Howell et al. (2015) found that
76% of rabbit owners thought of their rabbits as family. However, a unique dynamic is
that rabbits are commonly housed both indoors and outdoors (Rooney et al., 2014),
which may be a factor in their lack of inclusion in such incidents. The reporting of
these rabbit deaths may indicate a general increase in awareness of them as sentient
others more in line with dogs and cats, moving into the limelight as also being
“family.” Paradoxically, this inclusion may also increase their likelihood of becoming
victims in human suicides and homicides.
One (of the many) question to emerge from this research is why animals may be
treated differently? While the species differences were noted earlier, at times even the
treatment of dogs in the same home differed. For example, in one case of familicide,
one dog was killed in the house along with the human family members, but another
dog was found “in the garage.” Why the different treatment? Was the dog someone
else’s? Was it not deemed a family member by the perpetrator? Was it just that this
dog was “overlooked”? Or, was there an element of “mercy” involved in the thinking of
the perpetrator?

Animal Deaths as “Mercy Killing”


“Mercy killing” is the only theme able to directly address the position of the pet animal in
the family. This theme picked up on evidence that someone had taken a pet’s life as part
of their suicide because of concern for the animal’s wellbeing once they were gone. It is
possible that other scenarios included this concern as a driver for animal death(s), but this
was not noted in the article or was not known or perhaps was not revealed by police at
the time of reporting.
A recent study of coroners’ reports of suicides in Australia (Mattock et al., 2022) found
several cases where the person who had died had been very concerned about ensuring
that their pet(s) were cared for after their death. This included asking people to care for
their pets. Similarities can also be drawn to older people who have attempted to kill their
pet before themselves because they were concerned there would be no one to take care
of it after their death (Lynch et al., 2010). These stories stand in contrast to the research
undertaken by Love (2021) and Young et al. (2020), both of whom identified that care for
an animal’s wellbeing after an owner’s demise could be protective against suicide. What
10 J. YOUNG ET AL.

this seemingly contradictory information indicates is that the human–animal connection


regarding human suicide is extremely complex, requiring far more investigation and data
collection to understand and develop a web of informed, and coherent, protective
responses for humans and animals.
This research has focused on suicide involving domestic pets. However, there are
examples of suicide that involve other animals, such as livestock. Akcan et al. (2011)
describe a case in which a child seemingly died by suicide because of the death of a
chicken. Joiner (2014, p. 138) highlights a case of a 59-year-old male farmer killing 51
dairy cows out of the 100 cows he owned and then shooting himself. However, he only
killed the cows that required multiple milkings per day. This story indicates that concerns
for animal welfare after an owner’s suicide are not unique to pet animals. Joiner (2014)
also coined the term “mercy killing” for this human–animal phenomenon, one that he
applies, perhaps even more controversially, to examples of filicide (parents killing a child).
Our research indicates that sometimes the motivations to kill animals within the
context of suicidality may have “caring” roots: roots that can be both protective, and
deadly, for both humans and animals. Again, this is a complexity that needs to be
explored far more in the interests of all the species involved.

Terminology – Extended Suicide: Peticide


Two terms have been used to distinguish the killing of pet animals within the context of
human suicide. Cooke (2013) uses the term “extended suicide with pets” and Palazzo et al.
(2021) uses “peticide.”
Both Cooke (2013) and Oxley et al. (2016) have used and discussed the usage of the
term “extended suicide” as being suicide where human ego/identity is extended to a
pet animal. Along with Oxley et al. (2016), this current research challenges the use of
this term and the notion of extension of self as applying to all suicides that include
pets. Extending the term “suicide” to encompass the animals who have died is inaccurate
as their deaths have been without their (the animal’s) choice. There is no concrete evi-
dence to date that animals purposely suicide, although this is a concept which continues
to be debated (Peña-Guzmán, 2017; Preti, 2007). While numerous news articles have
reported “animal suicides,” generally such cases are explained by other information: for
example, multiple dogs (scent hounds) jumping from a bridge was explained by the pres-
ence of a mink nest underneath the bridge (Bering, 2018, p. 42). While there is evidence of
nonhuman “martyrdom,” such as termites who will fight to the death to protect the
mound within which the colony queen exists (Joiner et al., 2016), evidence that
animals choose to end their lives in the way that humans are demonstrably able to do
is lacking. Hence, we suggest that usage of the term “extended suicide” is erroneous.
Peticide is the other term that has been used (Palazzo et al., 2021). While “extended
suicide” misrepresents the deliberateness of taking another being’s life, “peticide” linguis-
tically positions the deliberate killing of an animal alongside all the other forms of killing
identified in this research: su-icide (self), hom-icide (other), famil-icide (partner and child),
fil-icide (child), pet-icide (animal). Hence, the terminology of including a pet in suicide, as
explored here, would become suicide-peticide, which more clearly delineates the per-
petration of the deaths.
ANTHROZOÖS 11

Currently, documentation and definitions focus on human perpetrators and victims


(Mattock et al., 2022), with the killing of animals by humans not being seen as in any
way equitable to the killing of humans by humans. It is worth noting that similar omis-
sions of animals as victims occur in other human mental health conditions such as Munch-
ausen by Proxy (Oxley & Feldman, 2016), although in recent times animal hoarding has
been included in the DSM-5 as a subtype of hoarding (Ferreira et al., 2017). However,
even within this, the focus is on the human involved – with no descriptive term used
to describe the specific victimhood of the animals hoarded.
The use of the term peticide aligns the deliberate killing of the nonhuman family
member with the deliberate killing of humans. To illustrate the current overlooking of
this issue, one article used the phrase “quadruple murder-suicide” to refer to the
murder of three children and their mother and the suicide of the father. But this
phrase did not encompass the deliberate killing of their pet dog alongside the human
family members. Including the use of the term peticide would give language that recog-
nizes the presence of this other species family member, enabling the phrase “murder-
suicide-peticide” to describe the scenario more fully.
An equitable term for animal deaths also provides a linguistic link to the increasing
conception of pets as family members (Arahori et al., 2017; Cohen, 2002) (and presumably
seen as family members by family annihilators – otherwise, why include them in the
killing?). A number of the articles used the term “family dog” or “family pet” to refer to
the deceased animals, and at times the shared vulnerability of the animal victims, along-
side that of others such as children, was noted. For example, in reporting a filicide where a
man gassed himself, his daughter, and their dog outside his ex-partner’s apartment, an
informant is noted as aligning both victims saying: “If that was what he wanted to do
then he should’ve done it, but the little girl and the dog didn’t need to go with him”
(italics added).
The term “Peticide” enables the killing of an animal family member to be readily incor-
porated into the concept of human killings. Yet it recognizes the dependent, highly vul-
nerable position of these other species who live with humans in their domestic family
lives. This is a terminological approach that links to the work of political scientists and criti-
cal animal theorists Donaldson and Kymlicka (2011). They argue for pets to have a form of
citizenship that recognizes their vulnerability and lack of choice in co-existing within
human societies, gives them rights akin to those given to vulnerable human beings,
and includes them in the political landscape as rightful co-citizens.

Crossing Disciplinary Boundaries


This research highlights how complex human–animal relations and lives challenge aca-
demic and practice siloes. The preponderance of homicide-suicide stories found while
using an animal-aware lens immediately implicates three areas of relatively separate aca-
demic inquiry. Homicide is generally explored by criminologists and those in the justice
and policing fields, while suicide tends to be considered a mental health focus (McPhe-
dran et al., 2018). A third, often quite independent, field of inquiry that our research is
linked to is that of domestic and intimate relationship violence. Auchter (2010) identifies
familial murder-suicide as an extreme form of interpersonal domestic violence. Bridging
12 J. YOUNG ET AL.

to the human–animal studies arena in recent times has also seen mounting evidence of
the role of pets in the domestic violence field (Taylor et al., 2019; Taylor & Fraser, 2019),
including recognition that animal abuse routinely indicates human abuse, and caring
for loved animals is vital in rescuing and protecting victims of intimate violence.
There are links across the human-focused fields identified here. Our research shows not
only the connections across human foci but also the intersections with fields that have a
focus on animals. Those with a concern for animal protection have an interest in the fields
of suicide, homicide-suicide, and domestic violence regarding their concerns for the care
for animals. Concerns for animals enmeshed in human violence by police and the public
were discernible. News reports that focused on animal victims of human violence com-
monly reported large amounts of funds being raised very quickly to support the care
needs of rescued animals. For example, police officers rescued Sophie, a dog who had sur-
vived a gunshot, and took her to an animal rescue service. All are indicators of the way
that human resources can readily be mobilized by stories of pet animals in need. This
aligns with the work of Montrose et al. (2020) on dog bites, who identified that large
amounts of money were raised (e.g., via crowdfunding) along with provision and offers
of in-kind support (e.g., free veterinary services) for dogs who were bitten by other
dogs and required veterinary treatment. While some humans may seek to take the
lives of pets, many others are mobilized personally and financially when they hear of
animals impacted by human violence. This is an intersection that bears exploration by
both human- and animal-focused fields to see how this concern and interest can be
better mobilized and engaged with in the interests of animals, with ways that have impli-
cations for human survival and flourishing as well.

Limitations
It is important to highlight that this research is based on new reports written by journalists
who are more likely to be experts in what is newsworthy, rather than being suicide or
homicide experts. News reports of suicide and homicide-suicide were frequently quite
superficial and rarely gave much insight into why the individuals killed themselves,
their family, or their pets. Their information (as noted in the articles) was generally
from initial police reports, although several reports were of coroners’ investigations.
This, combined with the general journalistic embargo on suicide reporting (Jordan &
McNiel, 2021; Niederkrotenthaler et al., 2020; Stack, 2002), indicates that the reports
present a highly select collection of scenarios, where the presence of multiple human
deaths and/or multiple animal deaths seem to over-ride this embargo. However, while
they may predominately focus on some of the grimmest, but relatively uncommon,
stories of human suicide with animals, they can offer insights of relevance to more
common scenarios.

Future Directions
Several key future directions emerge from this research. First, we recommend that the
term “peticide” be used to distinguish the deliberate killing of a pet animal. Second,
cross-communication between researchers across fields who are interested in homicide,
ANTHROZOÖS 13

suicide, domestic violence, and human–animal relations offers the potential for new
insights, innovative new practices, and findings of benefit to humans and animals.
Third, there need to be processes of routinely collecting data as to species and
numbers of pet animals involved in cases of human violence. While there are robust
sources of data regarding homicide and suicide, there is no consistent collection of
data regarding animals in these.
It is not possible to know if the reports found for this research are exceptions or the tip
of a hidden iceberg of scenarios. Evidence from the field of domestic violence indicates
they are not merely the tip and that actively recognizing and responding to human–
animal relationships are life-saving for many humans and animals (Taylor et al., 2019;
Taylor & Fraser, 2019). There is potential to interrogate some of the data collected on
human violence more deeply for animal connections; this needs to be explored, although
without routine collection of this information at the point of initial investigation by police,
this is challenging as there are no data to investigate. But the noted care and concern for
animals shown by police in several of the articles indicate that it may not be such a hard
task to convince some, perhaps many, officers to do this.

Conclusion
Joiner (2014) notes that “any powerful framework for the understanding of a given
phenomenon should ideally not only illuminate that core phenomenon (i.e., murder-
suicide per se) but should burn brightly enough that it also sheds light on neighboring
phenomena (e.g., incidents involving killing animals and then death by suicide)”
(p. 170). The study of pet animals in scenarios of human suicide (and murder-suicide)
has the potential to throw light onto not only our understandings of animals but also
of the humans involved. These understandings, while at this time more speculative
than concrete, are essential given the increasing discussion about and perception of
pets as being part of the family.

Acknowledgements
We thank our anonymous reviewers, whose feedback, suggestions, and input have strengthened
this paper immeasurably.

Disclosure Statement
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

ORCID
Janette Young http://orcid.org/0000-0002-2284-3485
James Andrew Oxley http://orcid.org/0000-0001-9483-9795
14 J. YOUNG ET AL.

References
Akcan, R., Arslan, M. M., Çekin, N., & Karanfil, R. (2011). Unexpected suicide and irrational thinking in
adolescence: A case report. Journal of Forensic and Legal Medicine, 18(6), 288–290. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.jflm.2011.05.002
Arahori, M., Kuroshima, H., Hori, Y., Takagi, S., Chijiiwa, H., & Fujita, K. (2017). Owners’ view of their
pets’ emotions, intellect, and mutual relationship: Cats and dogs compared. Behavioural
Processes, 141(3), 316–321. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2017.02.007
Auchter, B. (2010). Men who murder their families: What the research tells us. NIJ Journal, 266(June),
10–12. https://www.ojp.gov/pdffiles1/nij/230412.pdf
Australian Institute of Health and Welfare (AIHW): Harrison, J. E., & Henley, G. (2014). Suicide and hos-
pitalised self-harm in Australia: Trends and analysis. Injury Research and Statistics Series No. 93.
Cat. No. INJCAT 169. Australian Institute of Health and Welfare. https://apo.org.au/node/42549
Bering, J. (2018). Suicidal: Why we kill ourselves. University of Chicago Press.
Cerel, J., Brown, M. M., Maple, M., Singleton, M., Van de Venne, J., Moore, M., & Flaherty, C. (2019).
How many people are exposed to suicide? Not six. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 49(2),
529–534. https://doi.org/10.1111/sltb.12450
Cohen, S. P. (2002). Can pets function as family members? Western Journal of Nursing Research, 24(6),
621–638. https://doi.org/10.1177/019394502320555386
Cooke, B. K. (2013). Extended suicide with a pet. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and
the Law Online, 41(3), 437–443. http://jaapl.org/content/41/3/437.long
Crosby, A. E., Ortega, L., & Melanson, C. (2011). Self-directed violence surveillance; Uniform definitions
and recommended data elements. Version 1.0. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention,
National Center for Injury Prevention and Control. https://stacks.cdc.gov/view/cdc/11997
DeMello, M. (2016). Rabbits multiplying like rabbits: The rise in the worldwide popularity of rabbits
as pets. In M. Pręgowski (Ed.), Companion animals in everyday life (pp. 91–107). Palgrave
Macmillan.
Donaldson, S., & Kymlicka, W. (2011). Zoopolis: A political theory of animal rights. Oxford University
Press.
Dotson, M. J., & Hyatt, E. M. (2008). Understanding dog–human companionship. Journal of Business
Research, 61(5), 457–466. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2007.07.019
Eargle, L. A., & Esmail, A. (2016). Gun violence in American society: Crime, justice and public policy.
University Press of America.
Ferreira, E. A., Paloski, L. H., Costa, D. B., Fiametti, V. S., De Oliveira, C. R., de Lima Argimon, I. I.,
Gonzatti, V., & Irigaray, T. Q. (2017). Animal hoarding disorder: A new psychopathology?
Psychiatry Research, 258, 221–225. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2017.08.030
Hawkins, R. D., Hawkins, E. L., & Tip, L. (2021). “I can’t give up when I have them to care for”: People’s
experiences of pets and their mental health. Anthrozoös, 34(4), 543–562. https://doi.org/10.1080/
08927936.2021.1914434
Herzog, H., Montrose, V. T., Young, J., & Oxley, J. A. (2021, June 22–24). Extended suicide involving
pets: An analysis of news reports [Paper presentation]. International Society for Anthrozoology
virtual conference.
Howell, T. J., Mornement, K., & Bennett, P. C. (2015). Companion rabbit and companion bird manage-
ment practices among a representative sample of guardians in Victoria, Australia. Journal of
Applied Animal Welfare Science, 18(3), 287–302. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2015.1017095
Howell, T. J., Nieforth L., Thomas-Pino C., Samet L., Agbonika S., Cuevas-Pavincich F., Fry N. E., Hill K.,
Jegatheesan, B., Kakinuma, M., MacNamara, M., Mattila-Rautiainen, S., Perry, A., Tardif-Williams, C.
Y., Walsh, E. A., Winkle, M., Yamamoto, M., Yerbury, R., Rawat, V., … Bennett, P. (2022). Defining
terms used for animals working in support roles for vulnerable people. Animals, 12(15), 1975.
https://doi.org/10.3390/ani12151975
Joiner, T. E. (2014). The perversion of virtue: Understanding murder-suicide. Oxford University Press.
Joiner, T. E., Hom, M. A., Hagan, C. R., & Silva, C. (2016). Suicide as a derangement of the self-sacrificial
aspect of eusociality. Psychological Review, 123(3), 235–254. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000020
ANTHROZOÖS 15

Jordan J. T., & McNiel D. E. (2021). Homicide-suicide in the United States: Moving toward an empiri-
cally derived typology. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 82(2), 20m13528. https://doi.org/10.
4088/JCP.20m13528
Kim, H., Sefcik, J. S., & Bradway, C. (2017). Characteristics of qualitative descriptive studies: A sys-
tematic review. Research in Nursing & Health, 40(1), 23–42. https://doi.org/10.1002/nur.21768
Liem, M. C. A., & Koenraadt, F. (2007). Homicide-suicide in the Netherlands: A study of newspaper
reports, 1992–2005. The Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology, 18(4), 482–493. https://doi.
org/10.1080/14789940701491370
Love, H. A. (2021). Best friends come in all breeds: The role of pets in suicidality. Anthrozoös, 34(2),
175–186. https://doi.org/10.1080/08927936.2021.1885144
Lynch, C. A., Loane, R., Hally, O., & Wrigley, M. (2010). Older people and their pets: A final farewell.
International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 25(10), 1087–1088. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.2469
Mattock, K., Young, J., & Bould, E. (2022). Discourses and silences: Pets in publicly accessible cor-
oners’ reports of Australian suicides. Anthrozoös. https://doi.org/10.1080/08927936.2022.2042084
McPhedran, S., Eriksson, L., Mazerolle, P., De Leo, D., Johnson, H., & Wortley, R. (2018). Characteristics
of homicide-suicide in Australia: A comparison with homicide-only and suicide-only cases.
Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 33(11), 1805–1829. https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260515619172
Montrose, V. T., Squibb, K., Hazel, S., Kogan, L. R., & Oxley, J. A. (2020). Dog bites dog: The use of news
media articles to investigate dog-on-dog aggression. Journal of Veterinary Behavior, 40, 7–15.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2020.08.002
Niederkrotenthaler, T., Braun, M., Pirkis, J., Till, B., Stack, S., Sinyor, M., Tran, U. S., Voracek, M., Cheng,
Q., Arendt, F., Scherr, S., Yip, P. S. F., & Spittal, M. J. (2020). Association between suicide reporting
in the media and suicide: Systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ, 368, m575. https://doi.org/
10.1136/bmj.m575
Oxley, J. A., & Feldman, M. D. (2016). Complexities of maltreatment: Munchausen by proxy and
animals. Companion Animal, 21(10), 586–589. https://doi.org/10.12968/coan.2016.21.10.586
Oxley, J. A., Montrose, V. T., & Feldman, M. (2016). Pets and human suicide. Journal of the American
Veterinary Medical Association, 249(7), 740–741. https://doi.org/10.2460/javma.249.7.740
Palazzo, C., Pascali, J. P., Pelletti, G., Mazzotti, M. C., Fersini, F., Pelotti, S., & Fais, P. (2021). Integrated
multidisciplinary approach in a case of occupation related planned complex suicide-peticide.
Legal Medicine, 48, 101791. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.legalmed.2020.101791
Peña-Guzmán, D. M. (2017). Can nonhuman animals commit suicide? Animal Sentience, 2(20), 1–25.
https://doi.org/10.51291/2377-7478.1201
Preti, A. (2007). Suicide among animals: A review of evidence. Psychological Reports, 101(3), 831–848.
https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.101.3.831-848
Rooney, N. J., Blackwell, E. J., Mullan, S. M., Saunders, R., Baker, P. E., Hill, J. M., Sealey, C. E., Turner, M.
J., & Held, S. D. (2014). The current state of welfare, housing and husbandry of the English pet
rabbit population. BMC Research Notes, 7(1), 942. https://doi.org/10.1186/1756-0500-7-942
Stack, S. (2002). Media coverage as a risk factor in suicide. Injury Prevention, 8(90004), iv30–iv32.
https://doi.org/10.1136/ip.8.suppl_4.iv30
Statista. (2019). Number of dogs and cats kept as pets worldwide in 2018. https://www.statista.com/
statistics/1044386/dog-and-cat-pet-population-worldwide/
Sullivan-Bolyai, S., Bova, C., & Harper, D. (2005). Developing and refining interventions in persons
with health disparities: The use of qualitative description. Nursing Outlook, 53(3), 127–133.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.outlook.2005.03.005
Sun, Q., Zhou, J., Guo, H., Gou, N., Lin, R., Huang, Y., Guo, W., & Wang, X. (2021). Incomplete homicide-
suicide in Hunan China from 2010 to 2019: Characteristics of surviving perpetrators. BMC
Psychiatry, 21(1), 577. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-021-03574-8
Taylor, N., & Fraser, H. (2019). Companion animals and domestic violence: Rescuing me, rescuing you.
Palgrave Macmillan.
Taylor, N., Riggs, D. W., Donovan, C., Signal, T., & Fraser, H. (2019). People of diverse genders and/or
sexualities caring for and protecting animal companions in the context of domestic violence.
Violence Against Women, 25(9), 1096–1115. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077801218809942
16 J. YOUNG ET AL.

WHO (World Health Organization). (2019). Suicide prevention. https://www.who.int/health-topics/


suicide#tab=tab_1
Wood Harper, D., & Voigt, L. (2007). Homicide followed by suicide: An integrated theoretical per-
spective. Homicide Studies, 11(4), 295–318. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088767907306993
Young, J., Bowen-Salter, H., O’Dwyer, L., Stevens, K., Nottle, C., & Baker, A. (2020). A qualitative analy-
sis of pets as suicide protection for older people. Anthrozoös, 33(2), 191–205. https://doi.org/10.
1080/08927936.2020.1719759
Young, R., Subramanian, R., Miles, S., Hinnant, A., & Andsager, J. L. (2017). Social representation of
cyberbullying and adolescent suicide: A mixed-method analysis of news stories. Health
Communication, 32(9), 1082–1092. https://doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2016.1214214

You might also like