Language Development From Birth To Three
Language Development From Birth To Three
FROM BIRTH
TO THREE
This page intentionally left blank
LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT
FROM BIRTH
TO THREE
Moshe Anisfeld
Yeshiva University
Firs! Published 1984 by Lawrence Erlhaum Associa!es, Inc.
All rights rese rve d. No pan ofthis book may be reprinted or reprodueed or
utilised in any form or by any electronic, meehanical , or other means, now
known or hereafter invented, including photoeopying an d recor ding, or in any
informat ion storage or retrieval sys tem, without permission in writing from
the publishers.
Preface xi
1. Introduction 1
vii
viii CONTENTS
Levels of Meaning 66
The Functions of Early Words 91
Processes of Word Learning 94
Lexical Development in Broader Context 100
Glossary 249
References 261
Author Index 275
Subject Index 281
This page intentionally left blank
Preface
xi
xii PREFACE
Ross, Benny Shanon, and Joyce Weil. Two anonymous reviewers also pro-
vided enlightening comments. I am particularly grateful to Richard Steiner
for his elose reading of the chapters concerned with speech and its acquisi-
tion. I am thankful to Elizabeth Anslow for making the initial version of the
sketch that appears on the cover. Special thanks are due to Robert Lummis,
Director of the Scientific Computing Center of the Albert Einstein Col-
lege of Medicine of Yeshiva University, to Associate Director Arthur
Rosenthai, and to their staff for opening for me the rich resources of the
UNIX system for text processing. I also thank Dr. I. Bretherton and Dr. P.
D. Eimas for checking my reading of figures in graphs of their articles.
Finally, I owe the greatest debt of gratitude to my family. My wife,
Elizabeth, read and commented on practically every one of the numerous
versions I brought horne from the Computing Center. My son, Shimon,
gave me the benefit of his meticulous reading of various chapters in
preparation and of the galleys and page proofs. My daughters, Miriam and
Rachel, helped with proofreading and with the checking of references and
quotes. Beyond this direct assistance, my wife and children gave me much
more. They helped me-by being who they are-to develop intuitions ab out
children that guided me throughout this project. This book is dedicated to
them and to my mother, who through the calamitous tragedies of war re-
tained the education of her children as the highest priority.
Moshe Anisfeld
LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT
FROM BIRTH
TO THREE
This page intentionally left blank
1 Introduction
In the course of the first 3 years of life the child becomes transformed from a
squirming, crying, and impulsive creature into a thinking and talking human be-
ing. This book teIls the story of this fundamental transformation, tracing the
small steps and the large leaps that children take in achieving it.
The story of development that unfolds in the following chapters has the qual-
ity of a good drama. In the flow of developmental events, as in the sequencing of
scenes on the stage, there is anticipation and continuity. Every major change in
functioning has been prepared for by antecedent events. The antecedents are of-
ten not appreciated at the time of their occurrence; their full significance emerges
only when they are viewed from a broad developmental perspective. We see, for
instance, that such behaviors as sucking and grasping, which appear intellectu-
ally inconsequential, serve an important function in the development of thinking
and in the preparation for language. We also see that the distorted speech of the
toddler reflects internal patterns and constitutes a step toward the acquisition of
the standard speech of the community.
The material I draw on inc1udes observational and experimental studies, as
weIl as formallinguistic studies. In observational research the investigator ob-
serves the natural behavior of children-be it speech, play, or interaction with
the mother-records the observations, and analyzes them in search for patterns
and trends. The methods of recording and analysis can vary. Some investigators
write down what they see or hear; others use recording devices, such as video
equipment. The analysis may involve impressionistic summaries, quantitative
statistical calculations, or a combination of both. A noteworthy type of observa-
tional study is the diary study, in which the investigator observes a child, often
1
2 1. INTRODUCTION
his or her own, on a regular and continuous basis over aperiod of time, which
may range from a few months to a few years.
Experimental studies, in which specific behaviors are examined under prede-
signed conditions, are harder to conduct with infants and toddlers than with older
subjects. But some ingenious methods have been developed in recent years, and
they have produced results that contribute to our understanding of human
development.
Also used in this book are the studies of linguists conceming the structure of
language. In studying the structure of language, linguists do not typically test
subjects or make observations on them. Rather, they try to develop an account of
the structure of specific languages and of language in general by analyzing com-
monly observed phenomena. We use the work of linguists conceming the struc-
ture of language, because in order to understand how children acquire language,
we need to know something about what it is that they are acquiring.
The most important source material for the present book is the work of the
Swiss psychologist Jean Piaget. Piaget, who died in 1980 at the age of 84, is
generally acknowledged as a great developmental psychologist. I draw particu-
larly on Piaget's theory of infant development, which is based on a study of his
three children. In his work Piaget combines keen observation of naturally
occurring behaviors with creative experimental interventions.
This book tries to synthesize the different strands of research, coming from
different disciplines, using different methodologies, and deriving from different
theoretical orientations, into a cohesive picture of development. My approach is
based on Piaget' s conception of development and is anchored in current thinking
conceming the nature of human information processing. In some respects it is
similar to the approach ofWemer and Kaplan (1963). I make three basic assump-
tions: that similar behaviors can derive from processes at different levels of
mentation, that there are both gradual and abrupt changes in development, and
that children do not absorb passively the language of their community but con-
struct it actively for themselves.
The substance of the book is presented in three parts. Part I, which contains
Chapters 2-4, deals with the formation and socialization of the symbolic capac-
ity . Chapter 2 lays the groundwork for the study of symbolic development. It
c1arifies the concept of symbol and relates it to other concepts. Chapter 2 also
provides a preview of the nature of presymbolic mentation in infancy. Chapter 3
traces the steps infants go through in advancing from presymbolic thinking to
symbolic thinking. In the presymbolic stage infants cannot represent objects and
people intemally and therefore do not realize that they have substance and per-
manence. For instance, young infants have no notion that their mothers continue
to exist when they are not in sensory contact with them. The symbolic capacity ,
which is developed gradually in the course of the first 18 months, makes possible
the construction of reality and the acquisition of language to represent it. Chapter
INTRODUCTION 3
4 describes how words are learned and how their meanings change to approxi-
mate gradually the meanings of adults.
The use of sentences represents a higher level of symbolic capacity than the
use of single words, and sentences appear in children's speech after single-word
utterances. Part 11, which contains Chapters 5-7, deals with the nature of early
syntax and its development. Chapter 5 analyzes the nature of sentences in mature
form and thus provides the necessary background for the study of syntactic acqui-
sition. Chapter 6 describes the characteristics of children's early sentences, and
Chapter 7 examines the developments that foreshadow the emergence of syntax
and the factors that facilitate its growth. It describes how toddlers organize for
themselves the task of learning to form sentences. Some children, for instance,
divide the task of sentence construction into manageable parts and concentrate
their efforts on learning each part separately.
Part III, consisting of Chapters 8-10, is concerned with the development of
speech and morphology. Chapter 8 gives an introduction to the nature of human
speech, providing background information needed for the following two chap-
ters. Chapter 9 discusses children's acquisition of morphology. Morphology
deals with the structure of words and involves such things as the use of inflec-
tions (e.g., the plural s). Chapter 10 discusses how children acquire the sound
system of their language, and the ability to perceive sounds and to articulate
them. The chapters of Part III, like the preceding ones, highlight the child's ac-
tive part in learning language. We see, for instance, that children's pronunciation
errors are creative attempts to construct a sound system on the basis of what they
hear. Similarly, such childish renditions as doed (for did) and mans (for men) are
mistakes only from the adult' s point of view. From the child' s perspective, they
reflect discovery of inflectional generalizations.
The ordering of parts in the book is designed to reflect the chronological order
of development. In this connection, a comment is in order on why I placed the
chapters on speech and morphology after the chapters on syntax. Morphology
emerges after the onset of syntax and therefore belongs after syntax in the exposi-
tion. Speech development begins before syntax, but I put it together with mor-
phology because ofthe affinity of the concepts used in the two areas. Also, from
a pedagogical point of view, it seems advisable to move gradually to increasingly
more technical aspects of language; hence the progression from words, to sen-
tences, to speech and morphology. In addition, it is appropriate to conc1ude the
course of development traced in this book with the chapter on the acquisition of
speech, because this chapter spans the entire age range (0-3 years) covered in the
book.
This page intentionally left blank
References
Table of Contents
Author Index Subject Index 189 203 217 245 249 261
275 281 This page intentionally left blank
References
Belsky, J., & Most, R. K. From exploration to play: A cross-sectional study of infant free play be-
havior. Developmental Psychology, 1981, 17, 630-639.
Benedict, H. Early lexical development: Comprehension and production. Journal of Child Lan-
guage, 1979, 6, 183-200.
Berko, J. The child's learning of English morphology. Word, 1958, 14, 150-177.
Berman, R. A. Regularity vs. anomaly: The aequisition of Hebrew inflectional morphology. Journal
of Child Language, 1981, 8, 265-282.
Blank, M. Mastering the intangible through language. Annals ofthe New York Academy of Sciences,
1975, 263, 44-58.
Bloom, L. Language development: Form andfunction in emerging grammars. Cambridge, Mass.:
MIT Press, 1970.
Bloom, L. One word at a time: The use ofsingle word utterances before syntax. New York: Humani-
ties Press, 1973.
Bloom, L., Hood, L., & Lightbown, P. Imitation in language development: If, when, and why. Cog-
nitive Psychology, 1974, 6, 380-420.
Bloom, L., Lightbown, P., & Hood, L. Strueture and variation in child language. Monographs of the
Society for Research in Child Development, 1975, 40(2, Serial No. 160).
Blount, B. G., & Padgug, E. J. Prosodie, paralinguistic, and interaetional features in parent-child
speech: English and Spanish. Journal of Child Language, 1977, 4, 67-86.
Bolinger, D. Aspects of language (2nd ed.). New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, 1975.
Bower, T. G. R. Development in infancy. San Franeisco, Ca!.: Freeman, 1974.
Bowerman, M. Early syntactic development: A cross-linguistic study with special reference to Finn-
ish. New York: Cambridge University Press, 1973.
Bowerman, M. Semantic factors in the aequisition of rules for word use and sentence construction. In
D. M. Morehead & A. E. Morehead (Eds.), Normal and deficient child language. Baltimore, Md.:
University Park Press, 1976. pp. 99-179.
Bowerman, M. Systematizing semantie knowledge: Changes over time in the ehild's organization of
word meaning. Child Development, 1978,49, 977-987.
Braine, M. D. S. The aequisition of language in infant and child. In C. E. Reed (Ed.), The learning
of language. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1971. pp. 7-95.
Braine, M. D. S. Children's first word combinations. Monographs of the Society for Research in
Child Development, 1976, 410, Serial No. 164).
Branigan, G. Some reasons why sueeessive single word utterances are not. Journal of Child Lan-
guage, 1979, 6, 411-421.
Bretherton, 1., Bates, E., MeNew, S., Shore, C., Williamson, C., & Beeghly-Smith, M. Compre-
hension and production of symbols in infancy: An experimental study. Developmental Psychology,
1981, 17, 728-736.
Bronckart, J. P., & Sinclair, H. Time, tense and aspeet. Cognition, 1973, 2, 107-130.
Bronstein, A. J. The pronunciation of American English: An introduction to phonetics. New York:
Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1960.
Brooks-Gunn, J., & Lewis, M. "Why mama and papa?" The development of sociallabels. Child
Development, 1979, 50, 1203-1206.
Brown, C. J. Reaetions of infants to their parents' voiees. Infant Behavior and Development, 1979,
2, 295-300.
Brown, R. Words and things. Glencoe, Ill: Free Press, 1958.
Brown, R. Afirst language: The early stages. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 1973.
Brown, R., Cazden, c., & Bellugi, U. The child's grammar from I to III. In J. P. Hili (Ed.),
Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology (Vo!. 2). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press,
1969. pp. 28-73.
Bruner, J. S. From communication to language: A psychologieal perspeetive. In I. Markova (Ed.),
The social context of language. New York: Wiley, 1978. pp. 17-48.
REFERENCES 263
Bruner, J. S., Goodnow, J. J., & Austin, G. A. A study of thinking. New York: Wiley, 1956.
Bryant, B., & Anisfeld, M. Feedback versus no-feedback in testing children's knowledge of English
pluralization rules. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1969, 8, 250-255.
Carew, J. V. Experience and the development of intelligence in young children at horne and in day
care. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 1980,45(6-7, Serial No.
187).
Carlson, P., & Anisfeld, M. Some observations on the linguistic competence of a two-year-old child.
Child Development, 1969, 40, 569-575.
Carter, A. L. Prespeech meaning relations: An outline of one infant's sensorimotor morpheme devel-
opment. In P. Fletcher & M. Garman (Eds.), Language acquisition: Studies in first language de-
velopment. New York: Cambridge University Press, 1979. Pp. 71-92.
Cazden, C. B. The acquisition ofnoun and verb infiections. ChildDevelopment, 1968,39,433-448.
Chafe, W. L. Givenness, contrastiveness, definiteness, subjects, topics, and point ofview. In C. N.
Li (Ed.), Subject and topic. New York: Academic Press, 1976. Pp. 25-55.
Chomsky, N. Syntactic structures. The Hague: Mouton, 1957.
Chomsky, N. Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 1965.
Chomsky, N. Language and mind. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World, 1968.
Chomsky, N. Reflections on language. New York: Pantheon Books, 1975.
Chomsky, N. On cognitive structures and their development: A reply to Piaget. In M. Piattelli-
Palmarini (Ed.), Language and learning: The debate between Jean Piaget and Noam Chomsky.
Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 1980. Pp. 35-52. (a)
Chomsky, N. The linguistic approach. In M. Piattelli-Palmarini (Ed.), Language and learning: The
debate between Jean Piaget and Noam Chomsky. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press,
1980. pp. 109-117. (b)
Chomsky, N. Rules and representations. New York: Columbia University Press, 1980. (c)
Chomsky, N., & Halle, M. The sound pattern of English. New York: Harper & Row, 1968.
Chukovsky, K. From two to five. Berkeley, Ca!.: University of Califomia Press, 1963. (Translation
of 16th Russian ed. published, 1959.)
Clark, E. V. What's in a word? On the child's acquisition of semantics in his first language. In T. E.
Moore (Ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press,
1973. Pp. 65-110.
Clark, E. V. Strategies for communicating. Child Development, 1978, 49, 953-959.
Clark, E. V. Building a vocabulary: Words for objects, actions and relations. In P. Fletcher & M.
Garman (Eds.), Language acquisition: Studies in first language development. New York:
Cambridge University Press, 1979. pp. 149-160.
Clark, E. V. Lexical innovations: How children leam to create new words. In W. Deutsch (Ed.), The
chi/d's construction of language. New York: Academic Press, 1981. Pp. 299-328.
Clark, R. A. The transition from action to gesture. In A. Lock (Ed.), Action, gesture and symbol:
The emergence of language. New York: Academic Press, 1978. Pp. 231-257.
Condon, W. S. Method of micro-analysis of sound films of behavior. Behavioral Research Methods
& Instrumentation, 1970, 2, 51-54.
Condon, W. S., & Sander, L. W. Neonate movement is synchronized with adult speech:
Interactional participation and language acquisition. Science, 1974, 183, 99-101. (a)
Condon, W. S., & Sander, L. W. Synchrony demonstrated between movements ofthe neonate and
adult speech. Child Development, 1974,45, 456-462. (b)
Cooper, W. E. The development of speech timing. In S. J. Segalowitz & F. A. Gruber (Eds.), Lan-
guage development and neurological theory. New York: Academic Press, 1977. pp. 357-373.
Corrigan, R. Language development as related to Stage 6 object permanence development. Journal
ofChild Language, 1978,5, 173-189.
Corrigan, R. Cognitive correlates of language: Differential criteria yield differential results. Child
Development, 1979,50, 617-631.
264 REFERENCES
Cross, T. G. Mothers' speech adjustments: The contributions of selected child listener variables. In
C. E. Snow & C. A. Ferguson (Eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition. New
York: Cambridge University Press, 1977. Pp. 151-188.
Cross, T. G. Mothers' speech and its association with rate of linguistic development in young chil-
dren. In N. Waterson & C. Snow (Eds.), The deveiopment of communication. New York: Wiley,
1978. pp. 199-216.
Curtiss, S. Genie: A psycholinguistic study of a modern-day "Wild Child." New York: Academic
Press, 1977.
Curtiss, S. Dissociations between language and cognition: Cases and implications. Journal of Autism
and Developmental Disorders, 1981, 11, 15-30.
Daehler, M. W., & O'Connor, M. P. Recognition memory for objects in very young children: The
effects of shape and label similarity on preference for novel stimuli. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 1980, 29, 306-321.
de Villiers, J. G., & de Villiers, P. A. A cross-sectional study of the acquisition of grammatical
morphemes in child speech. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 1973, 2, 267-278.
Dore, J. Conditions for the acquisition of speech acts. In I. Markova (Ed.), The social context of
language. New York: Wiley, 1978. pp. 87-111.
Dore, J., FrankIin, M. B., Miller, R. T., & Ramer, A. L. H. Transitional phenomena in early lan-
guage acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 1976, 3, 13-28.
Drucker, J. Toddler play: Some comments on its functions in the developmental process. Psychoa-
nalysis and Contemporary Science, 1975,4, 479-527.
Edwards, M. L. Perception and production in chi1d phonology: The testing of four hypotheses. Jour-
nal of Child Language, 1974, 1, 205-219.
Eilers, R. E., Gavin, W. J., & Wi1son, W. R. Effects of early linguistic experience on speech dis-
crimination by infants: A reply. Child Development, 1980,51, 113-117.
Eimas, P. D., Siqueland, E. R., Jusczyk, P., & Vigorito, J. Speech perception in infants. Science,
1971, 171, 303-306.
Eimas, P. D., & Tartter, V. C. On the development of speech perception: Mechanisms and
analogies. In H. W. Reese & L. P. Lipsitt (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior
(Vol. 13). New York: Academic Press, 1979. pp. 155-193.
Epstein, W. The influence of syntactical structure on learning. American Journal of Psychology,
1961, 74, 8~85.
Ervin, S. M. Imitation and structural change in children's language. In E. H. Lenneberg (Ed.), New
directions in the study of language. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 1964. pp. 163-189.
Escalona, S. K. The roots of individuality: Normal patterns of development in infancy. Chicago:
Aldine, 1968.
Fagan, J. F. Memory in the infant. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1970, 9,
217-226.
Fagan, J. F. Infants' delayed recognition memory and forgetting. Journal of Experimental Child Psy-
chology, 1973, 16, 424-450.
Fagan, J. F. An attention model of infant recognition. Child Development, 1977,48, 345-359.
Farah, M. J., & Kosslyn, S. M. Concept development. In H. W. Reese & L. P. Lipsitt (Eds.), Ad-
vances in child development and behavior (Vol. 16). New York: Academic Press, 1982. pp.
125-167.
Fein, G. G. Pretend play in chi1dhood: An integrative review. Child Development, 1981, 52,
1095-1118.
Ferguson, C. A. Baby talk in six languages. In J. J. Gumperz & D. Hymes (Eds.), The ethnography
of communication. (Special publication of the American Anthropologist, 1964, 66, 103-114.)
Ferguson, C. A. Learning to pronounce: The earliest stage of phonological development in the chi1d.
In F. D. Minifie & L. L. Lloyd (Eds.), Communicative and cognitive abilities: Early behavioral
assessment. Baltimore, Md.: University Park Press, 1978. pp. 273-297.
REFERENCES 265
Ferguson, C. A. Phonology as an individual access system: Some data from language acquisition. In
C. J. Fillmore, D. Kempler, & W. S-Y. Wang (Eds.), Individual differences in language ability
and language behavior. New York: Academic Press, 1979. pp. 189-201.
Ferguson, C. A., & Gamica, O. K. Theories of phonological development. In E. H. Lenneberg & E.
Lenneberg (Eds.), Foundations of language development: A multidisciplinary approach (Vol. 1).
New York: Academic Press, 1975. pp. 153-180.
Ferrier, L. J. Some observations of error in context. In N. Waterson & C. Snow (Eds.), The develop-
ment of communication. New York: Wiley, 1978. pp 301-309.
Feuerstein, R. The dynamic assessment of retarded performers: The learning potential assessment
device, theory, instruments, and techniques. Baltimore, Md.: University Park Press, 1979.
Fillmore, C. J. The case for case. In E. Bach & R. T. Harms (Eds.), Universals in linguistic theory.
New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1968.
Flavell, J. H. On cognitive development. Child Development, 1982, 53, 1-10.
Fremgen, A., & Fay, D. Overextensions in production and comprehension: A methodological clarifi-
cation. Journal of Child Language, 1980, 7, 205-211.
Fromkin, V., & Rodman, R. An introduction to language (2nd ed.). New York: Holt, Rinehart &
Winston, 1978.
Fry, D. B. Phonological aspects of language acquisition in the hearing and the deaf. In E. H.
Lenneberg & E. Lenneberg (Eds.), Foundations of language development: A multidisciplinary ap-
proach (Vol. 2). New York: Academic Press, 1975. pp. 137-155.
Frye, D. Stages of development: The Stage IV error. Infant Behavior and Development, 1980, 3,
115-126.
Garvey, C. Play with language and speech. In S. Ervin-Tripp & C. Mitchell-Keman (Eds.), Child
discourse. New York: Academic Press, 1977. pp. 27-47.
Gleason, H. A. An introduction to descriptive linguistics (Rev. ed.). New York: Holt, Rinehart &
Winston, 1961.
Glucksberg, S., & Danks, J. H. Effects of discriminative labels and of nonsense labels upon availa-
bility of novel function. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1968, 7, 72-76.
Goldin-Meadow, S., Seligman, M. E. P., & Gelman, R. Language in the two-year old. Cognition,
1976, 4, 189-202.
Gottfried, A. W., Rose, S. A., & Bridger, W. H. Cross-modal transfer in human infants. Child
Development, 1977,48, 118-123.
Gratch, G. A study of the relative dominance of vision and touch in six-month-old infants. Child
Development, 1972, 43, 615-623.
Gratch, G., Appel, K. J., Evans, W. F., LeCompte, G. K., & Wright, N. A. Piaget's Stage IV
object concept error: Evidence of forgetting or object conception? Child Development, 1974,45,
71-77.
Greenberg, J. H. Some universals of grarnmar with particular reference to the order of meaningful
elements. In J. H. Greenberg (Ed.), Universals of language. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 1963.
pp. 58-90.
Greenfield, P. M., & Smith, J. H. The structure of communication in early language development.
New York: Academic Press, 1976.
Guillaume, P. Imitation in children. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1971. (Originally pub-
lished in French, 1926.)
Guillaume, P. First stages of sentence formation in children's speech. In C. A. Ferguson & D. I.
Siobin (Eds.), Studies of child language development. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston,
1973. pp. 522-541. (Article originally published in French, 1927.)
Halliday, M. A. K. Learning how to mean: Explorations in the development oflanguage. New York:
Elsevier, 1975.
Harding, C. G., & Golinkoff, R. M. The origins of intentional vocalizations in prelinguistic infants.
Child Development, 1979, 50, 33-40.
266 REFERENCES
Harnick, F. S. The relationship between ability level and task difficulty in producing imitation in
infants. Child Development, 1978, 49, 209-212.
HaITis, P. L. Development of search and object pennanence during infancy. Psychological Bulletin,
1975, 82, 332-344.
Hayes, L. A., & Watson, J. S. Neonatal imitation: Fact or artifact. Developmental Psychology,
1981, 17, 655-660.
Hebb, D. O. Textbook of psychology (2nd ed.). Philadelphia: Saunders, 1966.
Hockett, C. F. A course in modern linguistics. New York: Macmillan, 1958.
Horgan, D. Nouns: Love'em or leave'em. Annals ofthe New York Academy ofSciences, 1980,345,
5-25.
Horgan, D. Learning to tell jokes: A case study of metalinguistic abilities. Journal of Child Lan-
guage, 1981,8, 217-224.
Huttenlocher, J. The origins of language comprehension. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Theories of cognitive
psychology: The Loyola Symposium. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1974. pp.
331-368.
Ingram, D. Fronting in child phonology. Journal of Child Language, 1974, 1, 233-241. (a)
Ingram, D. Phonological rules in young children. Journal ofChild Language, 1974,1,49-64. (b)
Jackowitz, E. R., & Watson, M. W. Development of object transfonnations in early pretend play.
Developmental Psychology, 1980, 16, 543-549.
Jackson, E., Campos, J. J., & Fischer, K. W. The question of decalage between object pennanence
and person pennanence. Developmental Psychology, 1978, 14, 1-10.
Jacobson, S. W., & Kagan, J. Interpreting "imitative" responses in early infancy. Science, 1979,
205, 215-217.
Jakobson, R. Child language, aphasia and phonological universals. The Hague: Mouton, 1968.
(Originally published in Gennan, 1941.)
Jakobson, R., Fant, C. G. M., & Halle, M. Preliminaries to speech analysis: The distinctivefeatures
and their correlates. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 1963.
Jakobson, R. Six lectures on sound and meaning. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 1978.
Jespersen, O. Language: Its nature, development, and origin. New York: Norton, 1964. (Originally
published, 1922.)
Kagan, J. The second year: The emergence of self-awareness. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard Univer-
sity Press, 1981.
Kagan, J., Kearsley, R. B., & Zelazo, P. R.Infancy: Its place in human development. Cambridge,
Mass.: Harvard University Press, 1978.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. Afunctional approach to child language: A study of determiners and reference.
New York: Cambridge University Press, 1979.
Kaye, K. Why we don't talk 'baby talk' to babies. Journal of Child Language, 1980, 7, 489-507.
Keenan, E. O. Conversational competence in children. Journal of Child Language, 1974, 1,
163-183.
Keller, H. The story of my life. New York: Doubleday, 1954. (First published, 1902.)
Kessen, W., Levine, J., & Wendrich, K. A. The imitation of pitch in infants. Infant Behavior and
Development, 1979, 2, 93-99.
Klenbort, I., & Anisfeld, M. Markedness and perspective in the interpretation of the active and
passive voice. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1974, 26, 189-195.
Labov, W., & Labov, T. The phonetics of cat and mama. Language, 1978,54, 816-852.
Ladefoged, P. A course in phonetics (2nd ed.). New York: Harcourt Brace Javanovich, 1982.
Langer, S. K. Philosophy in a new key: A study in the symbolism ofreason, rite, and art. New York:
New American Library, 1951.
Largo, R. H., & Howard, J. A. Developmental progression in play behavior of children between nine
and thirty months. I. Spontaneous play and imitation. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurol-
ogy, 1979, 21, 299-310.
REFERENCES 267
Lashley, K. S. The problem of serial order in behavior. In L. A. Jeffress (Ed.), Cerebral mechanisms
in behavior. New York: Wiley, 1951. pp. 112-136.
Latif, I. The physiological basis of linguistic development and of the ontogeny of meaning: Part I.
Psychological Review, 1934, 41, 55-85.
Lenneberg, E. H. Biological foundations of language. New York: Wiley, 1967.
Leonard, L. B. On differentiating syntactic and semantic features in emerging grammars: Evidence
from empty form use. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 1975, 4, 357-364.
Leonard, L. B., Newhoff, M., & Mesalam, L. Individual differences in early child phonology. Ap-
plied Psycholinguistics, 1980, 1, 7-30.
Leonard, L. B., & Schwartz, R. G. Focus characteristics of single-word utterances after syntax.
Journal of Child Language, 1978, 5, 151-158.
Leonard, L. B., Schwartz, R. G., Folger, M. K., & Wilcox, M. J. Some aspects of child phonology
in imitative and spontaneous speech. Journal of Child Language, 1978, 5, 403-415.
Leopold, W. F. Speech development of a bilingual child: A linguist's record (Vol. 1): Vocabulary
growth in the first two years. New York: AMS Press, 1970. (Originally published, 1939.)
Leopold, W. F. Speech development of abilingual child: A linguist' s record (Vol. 2): Sound learning
in the first two years. New York: AMS Press, 1970. (Originally published, 1947.)
Leopold, W. F. Speech development of abilingual child: A linguist' s record (Vol. 3): Grammar and
general problems in the first two years. New York: AMS Press, 1970. (Originally published,
1949.) (a)
Leopold, W. F. Speech development ofa bilingual child: A linguist's record (Vol. 4): Diaryfrom age
2. New York: AMS Press, 1970. (Originally published, 1949.) (b)
Leopold, W. F. Patteming in children's language leaming. In A. Bar-Adon & W. F. Leopold (Eds.),
Child language: A book of readings. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1971. pp. 135-141.
(Article originally published in 1953.)
Levy, Y. Gender in children's language: A study in first language acquisition. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation (in Hebrew), Hebrew University of Jerusalem, 1980.
Lewis, M., & Brooks-Gunn, J. Social cognition and the acquisition of self. New York: Plenum
Press, 1979.
Lewis, M. M. Infant speech: A study of the beginnings of language. New York: Arno Press, 1975.
(Originally published, 1936.)
Lewkowicz, D. J., & Turkewitz, G. Intersensory interaction in newboms: Modification of visual
preferences following exposure to sound. Child Development, 1981, 52, 827-832.
Liberman, A. M. The grammars of speech and language. Cognitive Psychology, 1970, 1,
301-323.
Liberman, A. M. An ethological approach to language through the study of speech perception. In M.
von Granach, K. Foppa, W. Lepenies, & D. Plaag (Eds.), Human ethology: Claims and limits of a
new discipline. New York: Cambridge University Press, 1979. Pp. 682-704.
Liberman, A. M. On finding that speech is special. American Psychologist, 1982, 37, 148-167.
Liberman, A. M., Harris, K. S., Hoffman, H. S. & Griffith, B. C. The discrimination of speech
sounds within and across phoneme boundaries. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1957,54,
358-368.
Lichtenberg, J. D. The development of the sense of self. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic
Association, 1975, 23, 453-484.
Lisker, L., & Abramson, A. S. A cross-Ianguage study ofvoicing in initial stops: Acoustical meas-
urements. Word, 1964, 20, 384-422.
Lisker, L., & Abramson, A. S. Some effects of context on voice onset time in English stops. Lan-
guage and Speech, 1967, 10, 1-28.
Lock, A. (Ed.). Action, gesture and symbol: The emergence of language. New York: Academic
Press, 1978.
Luria, A. R. Language and cognition. New York: Wiley, 1982.
268 REFERENCES
Murphy, c. M., & Messer, D. J. Mothers, infants and pointing: A study of a gesture. In H. R.
Schaffer (Ed.), Studies in mother-irifant interaction. New York: Academic Press, 1977. pp.
325-354.
Neisser, U. Cognitive psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1967.
Nelson, K. Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in
Child Development, 1973, 38(1-2, Serial No. 149).
Nelson, K. The conceptual basis for naming. In J. Macnamara (Ed.), Language learning and
thought. New York: Academic Press, 1977. pp. 117-136.
Nelson, K., Rescorla, L., Gruendel, J., & Benedict, H. Early lexicons: What do they mean? Child
Development, 1978, 49, 960-968.
Newport, E. L., Gleitman, H., & Gleitman, L. R. Mother, I'd rather do it myself: Some effects and
non-effects ofmatemal speech style. In C. E. Snow & C. A. Ferguson (Eds.), Talking to children:
Language input and acquisition. New York: Cambridge University Press, 1977. Pp. 109-149.
Nicolich, L. M. Beyond sensorimotor intelligence: Assessment of symbolic maturity through analy-
sis of pretend play. Merril/-Palmer Quarterly, 1977,23, 89-99.
Ninio, A. Ostensive definition in vocabulary teaching. Journal of Child Language, 1980, 7,
565-573. (a)
Ninio, A. Picture-book reading in mother-infant dyads belonging to two subgroups in Israel. Child
Development, 1980, 51, 587-590. (b)
Ninio, A., & Bruner, J. The achievement and antecedents of labelling. Journal ofChild Language,
1978, 5, 1-15.
Oller, D. K., Wieman, L. A., Doyle, W. J., & Ross, C. Infant babbling and speech. Journal of
Child Language, 1976, 3, 1-11.
Palermo, D. S. Psychology of language. Glenview, Ill.: Scott, Foresman, 1978.
Pavlov, I. P. Conditioned reflexes: An investtgation ofthe physiological activity ofthe cerebral cor-
tex. London: Oxford University Press: Humphrey Milford, 1927. (Translated from Russian.)
Pawlby, S. J. Imitative interaction. In H. R. Schaffer (Ed.), Studies in mother-irifant interaction.
New York: Academic Press, 1977. pp. 203-224.
Piaget, J. The child's conception ofthe world. Totowa, N.J.: Littlefield, Adams, 1979. (Translated
from French. First English ed. published, 1929.)
Piaget, J. Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton, 1962. (Translated from
French. First English ed. published, 1951.)
Piaget, J. The origins ofintelligence in children. New York: Norton, 1963. (Translated from French.
First English ed. published, 1952.)
Piaget, J. The child's conception of number. New York: Norton, 1965. (Translated from French.
First English ed. published, 1952.)
Piaget, J. The construction of reality in the child. New York: Basic Books, 1954. (Translated from
French.)
Piaget, J. Intelligence and affectivity: Their relationship during child development. Pal0 Alto, Cal.:
Annual Reviews, 1981. (Edited translation from original French ed. published, 1954.)
Piaget, J. Schemes of action and language learning. In M. Piattelli-Palmarini (Ed.), Language and
learning: The debate between Jean Piaget and Noam Chomsky. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard Uni-
versity Press, 1980. pp. 164-167.
Piattelli-Palmarini, M. (Ed.). Language and learning: The debate between Jean Piaget and Noam
Chomsky. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 1980.
Popova, M. I. Grammatical elements of language in the speech of pre-preschool children. In C. A.
Ferguson & D.I. Slobin (Eds.), Studies ofchild language development. New York: Holt, Rinehart
& Winston, 1973. pp. 269-280. (Originally published in Russian, 1958.)
Preyer, W. The mind of the child: Observations concerning the mental development of the human
being in the first years of life (Part 2): The development of the intellect. New York: Amo Press,
1973. (Reprint of original English translation from German published, 1889.)
270 REFERENCES