Ling 1 Midterm + Final Study Guide
Ling 1 Midterm + Final Study Guide
Ling 1 Midterm + Final Study Guide
● Teaching grammar = used to learn another language – point of reference from Native language
to new language
● Descriptive grammar has the goal of describing rules people use; it does not want to tell people
which rules to follow
● Linguistics focuses on spoken language, not as much writing (writing is taught, not learned
through exposure)
● Signed languages are the same as spoken languages: they have their own grammar and lexicon
(even in animals, we can see that gestures = signals that have fixed, singular meanings)
● Dichotic listening = different sounds played in different ears simultaneously; subjects report
hearing only one sound (the one in the right ear aka the one presented to the left hemisphere)
● Split-brain patients: severed corpus callosum – L and R hemispheres do not communicate
● Wada test: anesthetizing 1 side of a patient’s brain – without the left hemisphere, language is
not there (they cannot talk/give a linguistic response)
● Language in the left hemisphere: aphasia = a disruption in language abilities (production
and/or comprehension) due to a brain injury
● Broca’s area - Broca discovered that loss of language ability is related to the left frontal lobe
● Broca’s Aphasia: speech is broken and halted (telegraphic speech = words are often dropped
out)
○ Words make some sense, but sentence structure is incorrect
○ Also known as “agrammatic aphasia”
○ Poor comprehension of complex sentences
○ Semantics/meaning = OK, Syntax =/= OK, comprehension = mostly OK
3.2: Brain and Language cont.
● Anomia = difficulty retrieving known words
● Wernicke’s area (farther back than Broca’s area): language deficit associated with damage to an
area in the temporal lobe
● Wernicke’s aphasia:
○ Speech is fluent, but does not make much sense
○ Grammar is usually not affected
○ Problems with word choice and meaning
○ Comprehension is severely impaired
○ This is called semantic aphasia
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
● Few words, but they make sense ● Lots of words, but they don’t make very
● Semantics is okay much sense
● Syntax is not okay ● Semantics is not okay
● Comprehension is mostly okay ● Syntax is okay
● Writing is not okay ● Comprehension is not okay
● Writing is not okay
● Writing = written version of the speaking aphasia → not a speech problem, is a brain problem
● Linguistic deficits are associated with damage to specific areas of the brain
● Language is localized to specific parts of the left hemisphere
● Deaf sign language speakers: – left hemisphere lights up (even though it is not spoken, the
language is still processed in the same area of the brain)
● Language is lateralized to the left hemisphere of the brain
● fMRI allows us to see that signed/spoken languages activate the same areas of the brain
● Severed corpus callosum: can easily name what is seen on the right side, but cannot say what is
on his left side (R brain cannot communicate with the language in the L hemisphere)
● Shows contralateral communication and gives us an idea of how the idea works
● Phonetic inventory = set of sounds in a language (for English, this includes subsets of
consonants and vowels)
● You know how to separate a continuous stream of speech into distinct words and words into
distinct sounds
● Vocal tract: structures that work together to produce speech sounds
○ 1. Alveolar Ridge (Tah, Tah; behind teeth roof of mouth)
○ 2. Hard Palate (Middle roof of mouth)
○ 3, Soft Palate/Velum (Ngah, Ngah, Back roof of mouth)
○ 4. Uvula (Flap of skin in back of throat)
○ 5. Pharynx (Farther back)
○ 6. Glottis (Voice box, Adam’s apple, uh-oh, uh-oh)
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
IPA – CONSONANTS
● Distinguished by 3 features:
○ Place of Articulation: where is the consonant produced?
■ Articulation = part of the mouth (usually the tongue) touches or interacts with
another area of the vocal tract
○ Manner of articulation
○ Voicing
● Places of articulation:
1. Bilabials: sounds produced by bringing both lips together
2. Labiodentals: sounds produced by touching the bottom lip to the upper teeth
[f] Five, fan
3. Interdentals: sounds produced by inserting the tip of the tongue between the
upper teeth and the lower teeth
[θ] Thigh
4. Alveolars (ridge): sounds produced by raising the tip of the tongue to the
alveolar ridge (part of the hard palate directly behind the upper front teeth)
[t] Tie, tip
[s] Sip
[z] Zip
[l] Light
[r] Rock
5. Palatals: sounds produced by raising the front part of the tongue to the hard
palate (bony section of the roof of the mouth behind the alveolar ridge)
[ʃ] Mission
[ʒ] Measure
[dʒ] Judge
[j] You
6. Velars: sounds produced by raising the back of the tongue to the soft palate or
velum
[k] Back
[g] Bag
7. Glottals: [h] produced with the flow of air through the open glottis; [ʔ]
produced if the air is stopped completely at the glottis by tightly closed vocal
cords: glottal stop
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
[h] high
[ʔ] uh-oh
[w] witch
[ʍ] which
● Voicing: are the vocal folds vibrating (voiced) or not (voiceless/open glottal)?
● Voiceless: vocal folds/cords are apart, air flows freely through glottis
○ Super [supər]
○ [p] pear, [f] fine
● Voiced: vocal folds/cords are together, air forces through, causing vibrations
■ Buzz [bʌz]
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
■ [v] vine
○ [s] and [z] differ only in voicing
■ [s] is voiceless
■ [z] is voiced
○ [l], [r]
○ [l] lateral liquid: can feel the air passing by the sides of your tongue if you inhale sharply
○ [r] retroflex liquid: articulated in the alveolar region, and the tip of the tongue is curled
back behind the alveolar ridge
● Glides: very small obstruction of airflow, articulators move closer together (but not by very
much)
○ [j] palatal, [w] labio-velar
○ You -> [ju]
○ What -> [wʌt]
● Trilled [r]
○ Indian English has what is called retroflex trilled [r]
■ Not in Standard American English
○ Trill: One articulator touches another in a very rapid fire, repeating motion
■ Ex: Spanish, Indian English
○ Dialects of the same language differ in their phonetic inventories
● Consonants Review
○ Consonants are distinguished using three features:
■ Place of Articulation
■ Manner of Articulation
■ Voicing
● IPA symbols different from English orthography
○
Wreath [θ] Ring [ŋ] Breathe [ð]
Phonetics Summary
● Your knowledge of language includes knowledge of:
○ Phonetic inventory of your language
○ Places of articulation of sounds in your language
○ Manners of articulation of sounds in your language
○ Voicing of sounds in your language
● Dialects of English differ in their phonetic inventories
● Dialects of English differ in “rules” they apply to sounds
● Consonant review:
○ [p] = voiceless, bilabial, (oral) stop consonant
○ [b] = voiced, bilabial, (oral) stop consonant
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
● Vowel chart:
● Tenseness: Tense vs. Lax
○ Tense Vowels: Muscles tensed/tighter and mouth is relatively narrower
■ [i] [e] [u] [o] [a]
■ These are IPA symbols of the vowels that we were taught in grade school
■ tense vowels are slightly higher than lax vowels and slightly longer than
lax vowels
○ Lax Vowels: Muscles a bit more relaxed
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
■
○ Syllabic consonants are marked with diacritics [ ̩ ]
○ These sounds can also be represented with a schwa [ə] + [l/r/m/n]
dazzle [-əl] [dæzəl]
Week 4.3:
● [j] dropping in American English vs. British English
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
● [r] dropping and social class – Labov 1996 in NYC saw that the local dialect of NYC is an r-less
dialect → [r]’s get dropped in word-final position and when the [r] precedes another
consonant → in NYC the pronunciation of [r] is prestigious, while it is characteristic of the
“lower classes” to drop the [r]
● Labov 1966
○ Saks (expensive)
○ Macy’s (mid-range)
○ Klein’s (low-prices)
○ At each store, he asked for the location of something on the fourth floor (so they
would say that) and then he would say excuse me? And they would repeat themselves
with careful articulation
○ [r] was retained in casual speech a lot more in the expensive stores
○ [r] was retained a similar amount in the clear articulation, and in Macy’s the careful
speech saw a big increase in [r] articulation
● [r] dropping is associated with social class, but it is not any linguistically worse or better
● In received pronunciation, the prestige dialect, [r] is dropped – opposite conclusions in
England and NYC
● Dialect differences in phonetics are not random – specific phonetic environments
● Ling. differences in dialects may be related to social class
● Phonology = how speech sounds are organized in different languages
○ “Pan” [pæn]
○ “Pancake” [pæŋ kek]
○ The alveolar nasal sound changes to a velar sound
○ “Pan bread” [pæm brɛd]
○ The n is pronounced in 3 different ways
● The alternation shows the environment in which pan is surrounded by – consonant
● Blik is a possible word in English, but lbik is not (our phonology shows us what is or is not
possible)
● American English does not permit an alveolar consonant in word-initial position immediately
followed by jod (this is why British English and American English sound different)
● Phonologists want to know:
○ What is the organization of sounds in a given language?
○ Which sounds are predictable and which sounds are unpredictable based on the
environment?
○ What is the phonetic context that allows us to predict appearances of certain sounds?
Sign language phonetics
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
● Bound morphemes: un-, -ish, -tion → these are bound morphemes because they cannot stand
alone
○ Bound morphemes are affixed to a root (therefore they are called affixes)
● 2 types of bound morphemes: inflectional and derivational
○ Inflectional morphemes are grammatical, in that they affect the grammar of the word
they attach to (they do not typically change a word’s category)
■ 8 inflectional morphemes:
Derivational: Inflectional:
rerun runs
*retalk talks
recreate creates
rethink thinks
● Combining morphemes: we can transform words into something else all the time, but we
know what is or is not okay
○ OK: uneaten, not OK: eatenun, OK: unadmired, not OK: admiredun
● This knowledge of combining morphemes allows us to create and understand novel words
● Morphology – creative, yet structured
○ Words have structure and morphemes are the building blocks
○ You know words, but you also know morphemes
○ Morphemes are usually put together in a specific order
● Structure of words – affixes combining
○ Certain affixes (bound morphemes) attach to certain kinds of words
■ -able: loveable, workable, doable = OK, but catable, blueable = not OK
● -able can only combine with verbs
■ -ish: reddish, bookish = OK, but helpish, walkish = not OK
● -ish can only attach to nouns and adjectives
■ Affixes care what they combine with
■ Suffix -ly in English: adjective + ly = adverb
○ Word formation:
■ Rewrite = Re + write = [Re[write]]
■ Boy = Boy = [Boy]
■ Boyish = Boy + ish = [[Boy]ish]
■ Boyishness = Boy + ish + ness = [[[Boy]ish]ness]
○ Word trees: roadmap of how to build a word
■ Morphemes only combine in a particular order and only with specific
categories of other morphemes
■ Word Trees help show these ordered relationships of the morphemes within a
word
Week 5.2
● Kanuri:
● The absence of an affix can be significant:
○ Morpheme for “he” is silent
■ Chaaha = he is tall
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
●
● Sibilant = sounds that have a buzzing or hissing quality: [z], [ʃ], [tʃ], [dʒ], [s], [ʒ]
● Inflection morphemes = purely grammatical, marking tense, #, gender, case, etc.
○ They are often very productive (can attach to almost all roots/morphemes)
○ Typically comes after derivational morphemes in a word
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
○
● Inflection beyond English: IMs can perform a great # of functions; English is an inflection
poor language (we are limited to the set of 8 IM’s)
● Other languages: case marking – nouns have a particular form depending on their category
(subject, object, etc.)
● Case markers in Russian:
○ Viktor + a = “Viktor’s”
○ Viktor + u = “to Victor”
○ Viktor + om = “by Viktor”
○ Viktor + ye = “about Viktor”
● Case markers in English:
○ I love them.
○ They love me.
● Word formation: reduplication – forming new words by duplicating part or all of an existing
word
○ Partial Reduplication (Tagalog)
■ “bili” = buy; “bibili” = will buy
■ “pasok” = enter; “papasok” = will enter
○ Total Reduplication (Indonesian)
■ “rumah” = house; “rumahrumah” = houses
■ “ibu” = mother; “ibuibu” = mothers
○ Contrastive Focus Reduplication (CF Reduplication)
■ Put focus on the most prototypical, stereotypical example of something
■ Ex: I’ll make the tuna salad and you make the salad-salad.
■ Redupe exists in other languages like Spanish and Russian as well
■ Grammatical Constraints on CF Reduplication in English:
● “He is out-of-his-mind-out-of-his-mind”
○ *”He is out-out-of-his-mind”
○ *”He is out-of-his-mind-mind”
● “They’re sleeping together, but they’re not sleeping together-sleeping
together”
○ *”They’re sleeping together, but they’re not sleeping
together-together”
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
■ “Do I like you-like you? No. You’re a little too neurotic for me.”
● “Do I like-like Tom?”
● Can’t do verb + proper name
● *”Do I like Tom-like Tom? No. He’s a little too neurotic for me”
■ “I wouldn’t date-date a linguist”
■ *”I wouldn’t date a-date a linguist”
○ Reduplication in English Varieties: Singlish
■ Singlish: Native English colloquial dialect in Singapore
■ Makes extensive use of reduplication
■ To sound friendlier
● English: Why are you so uptight about this?
● Singlish: Why so serious? I just play play only lah
■ To show intimacy to people (only applicable mono-syllabic words)
● English: How time flies! My son has grown so much!
● Singlish: Time pass really fast hor? My boy-boy grow so big already
(oready) you know?!
■ To emphasize your displeasure
● English: -insert appropriate exclamation- Can you not fiddle around
with the items? They’re all going to be dirty if you do so!
● Singlish: Wa lao! Can don’t touch here touch there or not? Later all
dirty slah!
● Reduplication in English Varieties: Singlish
○ Singlish: Native English colloquial dialect in Singapore
○ Makes extensive use of reduplication
○ To sound friendlier
■ English: Why are you so uptight about this?
■ Singlish: Why so serious? I just play play only lah
○ To show intimacy to people (only applicable mono-syllabic words)
■ English: How time flies! My son has grown so much!
■ Singlish: Time pass really fast hor? My boy-boy grow so big already (oready)
you know?!
○ To emphasize your displeasure
■ English: -insert appropriate exclamation- Can you not fiddle around with the
items? They’re all going to be dirty if you do so!
■ Singlish: Wa lao! Can don’t touch here touch there or not? Later all dirty slah!
● Singlish Morphology
○ Allows for reduplication of nouns, adjectives, and verbs
○ Noun Reduplication: Adds closeness or diminutive sense
■ Examples:
● Where is your boy-boy? (boyfriend, son)
● Say who told you mummy-mummy is a graduate (Dear mom)
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
Week 6: Syntax
Textbook notes
● Grammar = the set of rules that govern all spoken language
● The rules of syntax combine words into phrases and phrases into sentences → rules define the
correct word order for a language
● Grammatical relations of a sentence: subject and direct object
● Tree diagram can be used to represent words as subunits/subtrees – “the child found a puppy”
→ “a puppy” is a constituent
● Lexical ambiguity: sentences can have different interpretations due to the multiple meanings of
words
● Structural ambiguity: sentence has more than one tree structure associated with it
○ Ex: “Sue saw the man with the telescope”
■ The seeing is done with the telescope.
■ The man is holding the telescope.
○ None of the individual words are ambiguous; the ambiguity is structural
● A family of expressions that can substitute for one another without loss of grammaticality = a
syntactic category/part of speech
●
● A tree diagram with syntactic category information = a phrase structure tree/constituent
structure tree
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
Lecture 1
● Syntax = the study of how phrases and sentences are constructed
● Many similarities between word construction in morphology and larger sentence structures in
syntax
● 2 basic approaches
○ Words in a sentence are similar to links in a chain, or beads on a string – they are
ordered linearly, with no internal structure or hierarchy
○ Words in a sentence do have internal structure; some words are more closely connected
than others – some words might “work together” in certain operations, whereas others
would not
● Are words simply beads on a string?
○ Ian ran up the hill.
○ Ian ran up the bill.
○ From a linear perspective, these sentences look nearly identical.
○ The hill and bill seem like they should behave similarly, but they don’t. Did Ian run?
Yes, up the hill. OK Yes, up the bill. NOT OK
○ Words that are related often “move together” in certain constructions: It was up the
hill that Ian ran OK. It was up the bill that Ian ran NOT OK.
○ They behave differently.
○ Ian ran [up the hill] – up the hill is a chunk (you can move it around).
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
○
○ Up the bill does not form a chunk
● Words are NOT simply beads on a string
● Words are grouped into units called constituents
● Sometimes more than one “grouping” is possible
○ City to add twelve foot cops
○ Policemen who are 12 feet tall, or adding 12 cops who are on foot
● Constituents have infinitely extended length
○ Joe bought a car.
○ Joe bought an old car.
○ Joe bought a dirty olf car for $40 from the man down the street.
○ Joe bought a ____ = all appropriate answers to the question, “What did Joe buy?”
● Word order – what do you know when you know the syntax of your language?
○ Dog bites man vs. man bites dog – the word order tells you who did what to who
● You know that word order is constrained
● Basic word order varies greatly across languages
○ English basic word order: Subject-Verb-Object (SVO)
○ Tatar basic word order is different: SOV
○ Signed word order in ASL: VSO (and multiple other word orders)
● Word order matters, even when sentences dont have “real” meaning
○ Colorless green ideas sleep furiously – this is perfectly grammatical, but it is also
meaningless – a sentence can be syntactically well-formed, but have no meaning
○ Grammatical =/= understandable
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
● Similar building blocks across languages: sentences across languages are built using the same
syntactic categories
○ Nouns, verbs (all languages have them)
○ Adjectives, adverbs, conjunctions, articles, prepositions, etc. (not all languages have
them)
● Things we translate as adjectives in English can be expressed as verbs in Wolof
● “Time flies like an arrow; fruit flies like a banana”
○ What is the category of flies? What about like?
● The traditional approach: you determine syntactic categories by looking at the meaning – this
is basically like Schoolhouse Rock
○ Verb = action or state of being
○ Noun = person, place, or thing
○ Adjective = modifier that expresses quality, quantity, or extent
○ What about when meaning is not enough to determine a word’s syntactic category?
○ But what about when meaning isn’t enough to determine a word’s syntactic category?
○ “The assassination of the senator” - the noun expresses an action
○ Annoy vs. Piss off vs. Infuriate – similar verbs, but express different extent
○ The traditional answer does not work
● The Behavioural approach: We determine a word’s syntactic category by looking at how it
behaves, not just what it means
○ A word is what it does
○ A noun is simply a word that does nouny things – Steven Pinker
○ Which words are nouns? Verbs? How do you know? → you know by the distribution
of the word
● Our intuition tells us that “toves” must be a noun given the context. Why do we have this
intuition?
● Word category tests: nouns
○ A word that can follow a definite article, indefinite article, numeral, which phrase, or a
possessor (genitive) is a noun
○ To test if book is a noun:
■ “The book” - definite determiner
■ “A book” - indefinite determiner
■ “Six books” - numbers
■ “Which book” - which phrase
■ “Mary’s books” - possessor/genitive
○ In contrast: cannot say six deviate, a deviate – deviate =/= noun
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
○
○ The triangles show the constituents we have found, but these are not the only
constituents – every sentence and every word is a constituent
○ Failed pronoun replacement: I found the puppy at the park → I found it the park →
the puppy at is not a constituent
● Do so/do too replacement test
○ Great-grandma dared cop to tase her, so he did
○ “Do-so” replacement test: similar to replacement by a pronoun – if a string can be
replaced by do so/did so or do too/did too, then that string forms a constituent
○ The constituency test targets verb phrases
○ The replacement sentence has to retain the original meaning.
○ The old man found a dollar. → Yes, the old man did so. –the string found a dollar can
be replaced by did so (Yes, the old man did so dollar = failure, found a =/= constituent)
○ She went to the West Coast. → Yes, she did so. – grammatical and retains the same
meaning
○ Julie called the governor of Missouri. → Yes, Bill did too. “Called the governor of
Missouri” is a constituent and verb phrase
○ To use do/did too, it’s most natural to change the subject
● Stand alone test: a chunk of words that can stand on its own as an answer to a question = a
constituent
○ The girl ran in the rain. Who ran in the rain? The girl. Where did the girl run? In the
rain. =OK
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
○ What happened in the rain? Girl ran. = Not OK – we need to run all the other
constituency tests we have to conclude that a string is not a constituent
● The movement test: When a group of words can be moved around within a sentence it
indicates that the group of words is a constituent.
○ Fronting and clefting
○ Fronting: moving a group of words to the beginning of a sentence
○ If the resulting sentence is grammatical and has the same basic meaning, then the
moved chunk is a constituent
○ The girl ran in the rain → In the rain, the girl ran. In the rain = a constituent
○ Peter has pickled hot peppers in the kitchen. → Hot peppers Peter has pickled in the
kitchen. Hot peppers = a constituent
● Syntax: summary
○ Words come in syntactic categories
○ Traditional methods of determining these categories does NOT work – meaning alone
is not sufficient to categorize a word
○ There are a # of tests we can do to determine a word’s category
○ Strings of words form chunks that are called constituents
○ There are a # of tests we can do to determine constituency
Lecture 2
● Movement test – continued
● Clefting: breaking up a sentence and feeding it into the following formula:
○ It is/was ___ that ___
○ Ex: the girl ran in the rain. It was in the rain that the girl ran – this is grammatical and
retains the original meaning
○ It was the girl that ran in the rain – both the girl and in the rain are constituents that
can be clefted
○ Cannot be clefted: It was girl ran that the in the rain – not OK
○ Many executives eat at really fancy restaurants. – really fancy restaurants can be
replaced by “there”; “eat at really fancy restaurants” can be replaced by do so – it is a
verb phrase and a constituent
● Constituency tests to trees
○ The young man bought several large crabs → the young man bought them. → several
large crabs = a constituent
○ He bought several large crabs – the young man = a constituent
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
○
○ All words are constituents, but this shows that words can form units that are also
constituents
○
○ Bought several large crabs is also a constituent
○ Every sentence is a constituent
○ All English sentences must contain (minimally) a noun and a verb
○ This generalization can describe the structure of very simple English sentences
■ John Slept = N + V
○ Most sentences, even very simple ones, are more complex
■ The cat slept on the mat = Det + N + V + P + Det + N
■ A + B + C + D + E is linear, it does not reflect constituency
■ A constituent that acts like a noun + a constituent that acts like a verb
■ Noun phrase + verb phrase
■ The cat and john = NP
■ Slept and slept on the mat = VP
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
■
○ All English sentences follow a similar pattern
● Phrase structure rule:
○ S → NP VP
○ This states that a sentence contains 2 distinct parts: a noun phrase (NP) and a verb
phrase (VP)
○ NP → (DET) (adj) N (PP)
○ Noun phrase: optional determiner (ex: the, a), optional adjective, noun, and optional
preposition phrase
○ Ex: the cute dog on the couch.
● Noun phrase rules
○ NPs are interchangeable in sentences. Where an NP appears in a sentence, you can put
in another NP.
○ Ex: John ate a chocolate pie. → John ate Alpo.
○ Pronouns can replace NPs. – He, she, and it are all NPs (If you can replace a
constituent with a pronoun, it is an NP).
○ Pronoun replacement targets NPs (successful pronoun replacement tests reveal a
constituent, and tell you what kind of constituent you are dealing with
● Verb phrase rules
○ VP → V (NP) (PP) (adv)
○ A VP can be inserted into a sentence in a position which requires a verb
○ Examples of VP’s: Ran, fell slowly into the bath, etc/
○ Check using constituency tests– “do/did so” and “do/did too” test target VPs
● Prepositional phrases
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
○ PP → Prep (NP)
○ Examples of PP’s with NP: to the store, from Abby, with any luck
○ Examples of PP’s without NP: looked up
● Phrase-structure trees
○ Syntactic trees allow us to see/encode the constituency of a sentence directly.
○
○ When we use trees to represent sentence structure, we must do two things:
■ Accurately represent word order
■ Identify constituents
■ Ex: up the stairs = PP
■
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
○ She laughed or Mary did so – both are grammatically correct and maintain the
meaning
○ John slept:
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
●
● Phrase structure rules recap:
● S → NP VP
○ The cat slept.
● NP → (DET) (Adj) N (PP)
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
● Starting at one node – can draw a continuous downward path to another node
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
● Sisterhood/sister: two categories that are directly under the same node
●
● Syntax summary:
○ Constituency tests
○ We can make generalizations about sentence structure with phrase structure rules
○ How to go from constituency tests to phrase structure trees – everything found by the
tests can be reflected through the trees
○ Some technical notions (vocab) associated with PS trees
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
Lecture 3
● What do we know about the sentence, “Mary won a prize?” → Yes, Mary did so. (won a prize =
a VP constituent) → Yes, mary won it. (prize = a NP constituent)
●
● A prize = a complement, it gives us more information about winning → the complement to
the verb is the sister to the verb
● In English, the head of the phrase precedes its complement
● In the Japanese sentence of Taro found a dog, the complement precedes the head –
crosslinguistic differences
● Why are Japanese and English different in this syntactic way? Japanese has a different
phrase-structure rule
● VP: found a dog in English → a dog found in Japanese
● VP phrase structure rule in Japanese: VP → NP V
● Japanese PS rules:
○ S → NP VP (Taro-ga inu-o mitsukita).
○ NP → N
○ VP → NP V
○ Taro ate an apple → Taro apple ate.
○ Japanese has a SOV word order, while English has a SVO word order.
● What do PPs look like in Japanese, and where does this particular PP go?
● That boy hit the dog with a stick (English) → Japanese word order: That boy stick with dog
hit.
● S → NP VP and VP → PP NP V and PP → NP P (stick with)
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
●
● Malagasy: spoken in Madagascar – demonstrates VOS word order
○ The student reads the book → reads book the student (verb, object, subject)
○ PS rule for S: S → VP NP
○
○ With our PS rules, we can get a handle on how different languages have different word
orders
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
○
○ Differences in PS rules are one reason that word orders differ across languages
○ Part of a speaker’s knowledge of their native language is knowledge of the PS rules of
their language
● Sentences with multiple meaning: structural ambiguity
○ Sometimes, sentences have multiple meanings
○ The ambiguity may be lexical: big rig carrying fruit crashes on 210 Freeway, creates jam
– jam as in traffic or as in food?
○ Word structure/morphological ambiguity: unlockable (unable to be locked, or capable
to be unlocked)
○ Ambiguity due to different possibilities of reference: If your dog poops, please pick it
up. → it can refer to multiple things, depending on the context
○ Syntactic structure: Sherlock saw the man with binoculars (was Sherlock using the
binoculars, or did the man have binoculars?)
○ “Old men and women” – could mean old men and women of various ages, or old men
and old women
● Creating syntactic trees can be useful for understanding ambiguity
●
● Both structures are possible; each structure is associated with a distinct meaning. Constituency
matters!
● The girl bought the donut with sprinkles.
● The girl bought the donut with cash.
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
○
● [with cash] – info about how the donut is being purchased (with cash is the sister to the VP;
VP → VP PP and VP → V NP) both bought the donut and bought the donut with cash are
verb phrase constituents (can be replaced by did so)
○
● [with Cash] – info about who she was with
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
● “The spy saw the man with a telescope” – the spy used the telescope to see the man, or the spy
saw the man who has a telescope → the different meanings are determined by the placement of
the modifying PP “with a telescope”
● “Using a telescope, the spy saw the man” - the meaning
●
● “The spy saw the man who has a telescope” - the meaning
● “The student texted a friend under the table” - either the friend is under the table, or the
texting is being done under the table
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
●
● Yes, the student did so (under the table).
● The constituency tests are the basis of drawing trees.
○ John tired now = OK, John be tired now = not OK (meaning clashes)
○ The use of the auxiliary habitual be is governed by rules
● “Bobby, what does your mother do everyday?” “She be at home” – she is not at home always; it
is a habitual be.
● Habitual be is systematic
●
● Habitual be does not always have to speak on the current state of the situation
● AAVE auxiliaries:
● Done refers to a completed action, recent or not.
●
● Bin = has been
● BIN = has been for a long time (capitalized = stressed)
● He done ate = he has eaten
● He BIN done ate = he ate a while ago
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
● Finna = going to
● AAVE has auxiliaries that are not found in SAE
● Subject Auxiliary Inversion: I will eat a rutabaga → Will I eat a rutabaga? I and will switch
places
●
● The availability of negative inversion has to do with the kind of subject the sentence has (no
dog vs. the dog) – negative inversion of this kind is not found in SAE, but it is rule-governed
● Malaysian English has wh-questions that look very different from SAE
○ Word exchanges: Switching words – instead of what grammar will a child learn? →
What child will a grammar learn?
○ Phrasal exchange: an end of the sentence occurs at the fall in pitch (I haven’t satten
down and writ it instead of I haven’t sat down and written it)
● Speech errors are not just random reshuffling of words or sounds
● Malapropisms: out of place (a selection/substitution error where the speaker selects an
unintended item from her lexicon or substitutes one lexical item for another)
○ Phonologically based (his immoral soul instead of his immortal soul)
○ Semantically based (my boss’s husband instead of boss’s wife)
○ Syntactic category rule (nationalness instead of naturalness – adj replaces adj)
● Lexicon is a mental dictionary
● Substitutions are oftentimes based on phonetic similarity or semantic/pragmatic
association/similarity
● Words that are semantically associated, not just words that have the same/similar meanings, are
more strongly linked to each other than words that have less semantic association –
semantically associated – the meanings are associated
● Anticipations and perseverations: A tanadian from Toronto (T instead of C because
anticipating the T’s in Toronto) or She can she it (perservernce of sh from she mistakenly
appears instead of see)
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
Lecture 1
● How do children acquire their first language?
● Fact 1: Who is the teacher?
○ A mother tries to instruct the child and the instructions do not work
○ Instructive teaching does not play a primary role in FLA
○ Children acquire their first language effortlessly, without any systematic instruction
● Fact 2: What is the input data?
○ You like to eat eggs with bacon. → What do you like to eat with bacon? OK
○ You like to eat eggs and bacon. → What do you like to eat and bacon? NOT OK
○ How do we know one is OK and one is not OK from a grammatical perspective?
○ Adults, aside from speech errors and jokes, only utter grammatical sentences (positive
evidence)
○ Ambient linguistic data available to children (input) does not include ungrammatical
sentences (negative evidence)
○ The only input available to children is positive evidence
○ Children still end up being able to distinguish between grammatical and
ungrammatical sentences
● Fact 3: How do children interpret novel data?
○ There are an infinite # of grammatical sentences in English
○ Children are exposed to only a finite number of sentences
○ However, they eventually acquire the ability to generate and understand an infinite
number of sentences
● Fact 4: What errors do children make?
○ Children make “errors” in that they sometimes utter sentences adults, as matured
speakers of a language, would not utter
○ But the pattern of their “errors” seems to be selective or limited
○ They would not make certain errors that would be overgeneralized from what they
hear in adult speech
○ Regular verbs and irregular verbs – Children overgeneralize the use of -ed by attaching
it to irregular verbs
○ Do and have (auxiliary verbs) – children do not overgeneralize the -ed ending for these
○ Children’s non-adult-like errors occur in a selective, systematic, limited way
● Facts about language acquisition:
○ No instruction is involved
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
● From one Inuit elder: “You know that a child is acquiring the language when they know what
they are being asked to do”
● Inut mothers typically do not: Engage in vocal play with their children, try to interpret child
vocalization as speech, or respond to these vocalizations (more interested in children’s
comprehension than production)
● Kiche Mayan peoples of Guatemala: parents – address almost no speech to their babies;
however, they engage them in “real” conversation when they are around 1.5 to 2 years old
● Cultural belief of reincarnation = no need to teach the child the language
● Mohawk peoples of NY and canada: mothers and grandmothers interacting with children
spoke at a normal rate of speed
● They didn’t simplify the “elaborate prefixation, suffixation, and noun incorporation” that is
part of normal Mohawk speech
● Mothers interacted with children as though they were true conversation partners
● Caregivers in diff societies interact with young learners in different ways
● Some engage in explicit “teaching”; others don’t
● All that they share in common is the rich verbal environment that the children grow up in
● All these children learn to speak despite cultural differences
● No single method can be “essential” – they all work
○ How sentences are structured in the language using these specific parts
● UG is the seed of a grammar of any language – the seed must be watered with input of a
specific language for it to bloom as the fully developed grammar
○ Ex: if you feed the seed of UG with the input of English, you will become a native
speaker of English
● What is acquired?
○ Differences between languages:
○ Head-initial: head-complement (English) – John ate an apple.
○ Head-final: complement-head (Japanese) – John apple ate.
○ Word order acquisition: children need to determine the value of the head direction
parameter – subconsciously “is my language head-initial?”
○ What are the sounds in the language? What’s their distribution?
● Language acquisition is not driven by imitation, correction, or reinforcement
● Children’s knowledge of language is complex and shows subtle implicit knowledge of rules
● Since we are not overly taught these things, the claim is that this knowledge/system (ie UG) is
hard-wirted into the human minds/genome
● UG therefore facilitates first language acquisition, which explains why children develop
language so effortlessly, rapidly, and uniformly across species
Overview
● The ability to acquire language is innate
● All children everywhere should be able to:
○ Acquire the language of their environment
○ Acquire language rapidly and spontaneously
○ Exhibit linguistic creativity (all children will say things they have never heard)
○ Pass through similar stages of development
● Children are linguistically adults by the time they are 5-7; they are native speakers
Lecture 2
● Is the development of language similar to the development of other cognitive skills?
● The chaffinch is “wired” to learn its specific birdsong – but if it is not exposed to the birdsong
within 10 months, it will never learn its song
● The critical period hypothesis: language learning in children is similar to the acquisition of
birdsong – the ability to acquire language to a native level is innate → both require
environmental input for full development → both are subject to a critical period
● Critical period = the developmental time window where environmental input is necessary for
acquisition (of language) to native-like levels
● Birth to the onset of puberty = the critical period in humans
● Imprinting: within 2 days, goslings become attached to the first moving object that they see →
if there’s no exposure during this critical period, they will shy away from all moving objects
● Critical period for version in cats: 2 week old kittens were assigned to either a vertical condition
(box with vertical lines only) or a horizontal condition for 5 hours each day
● Kittens have no visual placing reaction – kitten does not back away from objects until living in
a normal environment for several days but vision is incomplete (horizontal condition kittens
did not see moving vertical lines/objects – blind to vertically-oriented objects and also vice
versa)
● The absence of environmental inputs = normal development is not possible
● Victor of Averyon: was found in the woods and could not speak:
○ Language progress was generally poor
○ Able to comprehend language, but practically unable to produce it
○ Only 2 pronounced phrases: milk and oh my god
○ Majority of his communication was grunts and howls
● Genie from Arcadia, California – had basically no linguistic input for first 13 years of life
● She learned how to:
○ Communicate a message (verbally or nonverbally)
○ Acquire vocab (by age 17, vocab of a typical 5 year old)
● She could not learn how to use:
○ Grammatical “morphemes” ex: the, a, -ed
○ Complex syntactical structures
● Examples of Genie’s sentences:
○ “Mike paint”
○ “Open door key”
○ “Neal come happy; Neal not come sad”
● Lateralization: where does Genie process language?
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
Childhood aphasia
● Recovery is faster and much better in children than in adults
● If aphasia is early enough, the right hemisphere can step in and take over
● 0-3 month aphasia: no effect on language
● 21-36 months: language accomplishments disappear, later re-acquired with repetition of all
stages
● 3-10 years: aphasic symptoms, but tendency for full recovery
● 11+ years: aphasic symptoms persist throughout the child’s life
● Relationship between earliness of aphasia + ability to recover
● Lateralization is tied to the critical period
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
Research results
● Humans have an innate ability to acquire language, but we require linguistic input from our
environment
● The input must come within a critical period (biologically determined “window of
opportunity”)
● After the critical period, humans are unable to acquire language with native-like proficiency
Summary
● Critical period: time window in which exposure to certain environmental stimuli are necessary
for normal development
● Critical periods exist for cognitive functions in animals (like birdsong in chaffinches, or vision
in cats)
● Critical periods exist for language acquisition in humans (from birth to the onset of puberty)
● After critical period has ended, it becomes nearly impossibly to acquire language to native-like
levels
● Evidence for a critical period for language comes from:
○ Studies of people who had no language input (Genie, Chelsea)
○ Studies of late vs. early acquisition of ASL
○ Studies of second language acquisition
○ Studies of childhood aphasia
● Modern linguistics is concerned with one question: how much of language is built in and how
much is learned from the environment?
● When did the boy say he hurt himself? – 2 answers: he said it in the bathtub, but he hurt
himself this afternoon
● When did the boy say how he hurt himself? – 1 possible answer: in the bathtub
● The “how” blocks one answer
● The imitation theory vs. the innateness theory
● Child has creativity – they do not simply imitate everything from their parents
● We don’t learn language the same way we learn other difficult things – we don’t have training
wheels when we learn a language
● Urinate – I’m a nate
● Children develop language skills regardless of whether mothers correct their children
● Most of language is innate, but surely you don’t inherit all of it
● We do it by analogy – new sentences are like one’s we have heard before, and that’s how we
understand them
● “I pained the red barn” – child can say “I painted the blue barn”
● You hear a sample and extend it to all new cases by similarity
● Concept of analogy DOES Not WORK – under investigation, all the analogies break down →
there must be some kind of mental computation
● John grows does not mean the same thing as it does in the sentence John grows apples.
● When do children/how do they know the difference between a subject and an object?
● Children who are 16 months old and have a limited vocabulary can understand the order of
complex sentences spoken by adults – they can absorb the information
● Word order maps objects and events in the world – this is shared by all languages
● Languages are essentially an organ of the mind/brain
● Learning word meaning is using it in the future to apply to new things
● What’s a concept? Is a meaning of a house different in my head from the meaning of the word
in others’ heads?
● Do concepts change as we grow up?
● Children are biased learners – they assume by expecting object labels to refer to the whole
object (pointing at billboard of rabbit and saying Gavagai! And people assume you are talking
about the whole rabbit, not just the ears, or fur, etc.)
● Children expect objects to have one and only one label/name
● Papua New Guinea – isolated tribes → a very linguistically unique language (700 languages for
3 million people)
● Menya people do not write their language, but their language is just as complicated as any other
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
Lecture 1
● Language contact occurs when 2+ languages or dialects come into extended contact (extended
= over a period of time) with each other
● Languages (and speakers) do not exist in isolation but in social settings
● When we talk about language contact, we are talking about human contact – contact between
people who speak distinct languages
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
● What happens when cultures with multiple languages come into contact?
● Widespread bilingualism/codeswitching is seen in many areas of Africa and Asia
● Codeswitching – switching from one language to another when you are speaking
● Selection of a lingua franca: any language used to enable communication between groups of
people with differing native languages (natural or constructed languages) – in many parts of
the world, English is a lingua franca
● Language creation: pidgins and creoles
● Language shift/endangerment and language death – an unfortunate result of language contact
● Borrowing is extremely common and need not be the result of extended contact between
languages (it happens all the time)
○ Borrowing words: English adopted chimichangas from Spanish
○ Borrowing phonetics: English adopted [ჳ] (the jea sound) from French (only found in
words from Latin or French)
○ Borrowing morphology: English adopted incredible from French, -able/-ible then
became a productive suffix
■ Shm- reduplication in English → ex: Books, shmooks, I don’t care what you’ve
read. Or Flu, shmu, he’s not worried about getting sick. → the Shm-
reduplication has a dismissive or pejorative connotation. It was first attested in
Yiddish in the 1600; borrowed into American English in the 19th century and
became more commonly used in the 1930s. It is the source of “Joe Shmo” aka
the average guy. The Shm- sound does not occur in native English words.
○ Borrowing syntax: English has two ways of asking the reason for something: “Why”
and “how come”
■ “How come” was borrowed from Yiddish into English
■ It has a different syntactic behavior than “why”
■ “Why did John eat the Alpo?” – subject auxiliary inversion
■ “How come John ate the Alpo?” – no subject auxiliary inversion, it would be
incorrect to do so
■ The difference between syntaxes show the difference between origins of the
two phrases, despite meaning the same thing
● Language contact occurs in situations where groups of speakers of different languages come
into contact with one another through:
○ Geography
○ Conquest/war
○ Trade
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
○ It is usually the case that when groups come together, they are not equal in power or
prestige
● Adstrates: languages in contact that have equal prestige
● Superstrate: language of dominant group
● Lexifier language: the input language that provided most of the basic vocabulary or lexicon
(aka “superstrate”)
● Substrate: language of the less dominant or subordinate group. Typically provides most of the
phonological, and usually, grammatical features
○ Ex: superstrate = English, substrate = Native American languages
● Pidgins and creoles are languages that arise in extended contact situations: they are the result of
language creation → we will explore the similarities and differences between pidgins and
creoles
● Cases of natural language creation through groups of people having extended contact with
each other
Pidgins
● Speakers of mutually unintelligible languages are often brought together, perhaps through
economic, political, or social factors
● In order to communicate with each other, they need to overcome the lack of a common
language
● One solution: the creation of a pidgin language
● Pidgin = simplified language used in specific interactions such as business, service, and trade
● Pidgins are found across the globe – they are not rare bc language contact is not rare
● Pidgins have no native speakers – they are second languages for everyone who speaks one
● Pidgins are governed by convention – they have established vocabulary and grammatical
structures
● A person can speak a pidgin well or poorly (or somewhere in between) – they are unlike other
natural languages because they have no native speakers
● Pidgins have grammars that are simpler than the grammars of their source languages – a
simplified code (taking pieces from each language that is coming in contact)
● Simplicity of grammar is one of the characteristics of pidgins – can afford to be simple because
it’s mainly used for trade purposes
● Vocab of pidgins is usually highly restricted and contains few (if any) terms for abstract objects
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
● Pidgin is a second (or third or fourth) language for everyone who speaks it
● Once children begin to acquire a pidgin as a native language, it becomes a creole
Creole
● As children acquire a pidgin as an L1 (first language), they transform its minimal grammar into
a thorough, complex grammar (it becomes a natural, human language)
● There are many creole languages around the world in: Caribbean, West Africa, and islands of
the Pacific (Polynesia, Micronesia, Melanesia, New Guinea)
● Creole language = language that has native speakers (the nativization of a pidgin)
● Many creoles include the word “pidgin” in their name (ex: Hawaiian Pidgin English) - this is
purely historical; HPE is a creole!
● Creoles highlight the difficulty of differentiating between dialects and languages (is it a
different dialect, or is it a completely different language?)
● Creoles are NOT “broken” versions of other languages
● Creoles arose because different groups of speakers needed to communicate (ex: groups of slaves
from West Africa that spoke dozens and dozens of different languages needed to communicate
with each other and with the slave owners)
● There were several possible substrate languages (e.g. Fongbe and other African languages in
Haiti), but no group was large enough or strong enough to push their one language
● In hawaii, there were Japanese, Hawaiian, Chinese, and Portuguese workers on the sugarcane
plantations – they needed to communicate and there were not enough speakers of 1 language
to dominate the group
● Hawaii and its rich blend of immigrants → at the heart is Hawaiian Pidgin English (Creole)
● Hawaiian Creole English:
○ Low status
○ Blamed for poor education test scores
○ Not college level (just casual talk)
○ Associated with lower class (stigmatized as inferior)
○ Considered substandard or broken English
○ 600,000 people speak HCE
○ It is used in the radio and in music
● Jamaican Creole English = also known as Patwa/Patois
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
● It is common for consonant clusters to undergo simplification, but they do not have to
● Fricatives in English becomes stops in Creoles:
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
●
● These properties are quite variable; you will not find them in ALL pidgins and creoles
● Phonetics: pidgins and creoles tend to have sounds that are very common in the world’s
languages
● Usually, cross-linguistically rare sounds are not found in pidgins or creoles
● Implosive sounds (stop sounds blowing air out) – are absent
● Pidgins are not tonal, even when the input languages are tonal
● Tonal creoles are rare but can exist
● Morphology: pidgins lack inflectional morphology
●
● It is not uncommon for creoles to lack inflectional morphology, but some do have it
● Plural formation (Jamaican Creole English): Plural nouns are formed by combining an
unmarked noun with 3rd plural pronoun “dem” (definites only)
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
● Ex:
● Pidgins – no complex sentences
● Creoles – fully developed grammars with complex sentences possible
● Pidgins do not have definite or indefinite articles
● Many creoles lack articles, but some do have them
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
●
● Creoles have fully developed tense systems.
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
●
● Use of different auxiliaries, but the tenses are still there
● You can also combine these auxiliaries – he bin stay walk = he had been walking, or he bin go
stay walk = he will have been walking
Summary
● What are the distinct potential outcomes from extended language contact situations?
● Borrowing: words, grammar, syntax, sounds
● Language creation: pidgins (grammatically simplified systems) and creoles (fully expressive
languages with native speakers)
● All creoles originate as pidgins (children who acquire pidgins as native languages create the
creole, a fully-expressive, natural human language)
● Pidgins and creoles have properties in common:
○ Phonetics/phonology: consonant cluster reduction
○ Little inflection or affixation, but there can be found in creoles, never in pidgins
○ SVO word order, but pidgins, which have no native speakers, tend t have more flexible
word order
○ Creoles have fully developed tense systems
○ Pidgins tend to tolerate much more grammatical variation than creoles, which do have
native speakers
○ Even though similar, pidgins and creoles are distinct (different) results of language
contact
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
Language variation/diversity
● 6912 numbers of languages in the world – but with a completely unequal distribution
● 347 languages (around 5%) have over a million speakers
● 94% of world population speaks one of these (347) languages
● The remaining 95% of languages are spoken by 6% of world population – of this 95%, 497
(around 7%) are nearly extinct (with fewer than 50 speakers left)
● Most of the remaining languages are endangered (exact level of endangerment is hard to
determine)
● Median size of a language in the world: 3000 speakers
●
● 8 languages have over 100 million speakers (altogether, speakers of these languages represent
2.4 billion people)
● Top 20 languages: represent 3.5 billion speakers or more
● Before European Invasion in 1492: 20 million Native Americans who spoke around 300
languages
● Today: 2 million Native Americans, 175 languages – of these 175; 55 have less than 5 speakers
(virtually extinct), 100 are endangered languages, and 20 may survive, since they are spoken by
children
● Last native speaker of Wukchumni, spoken in Northern California – a sense of loss and
urgency
Terminology
● Language death: when the last speaker of a language dies
● Language shift: process by which a language community adopts another language
● Language death is almost always preceeded by language shift
● Language study by Crystal (2000): The study assumes that there are 6,000 languages now, and
there will be a 50% language loss in the next 100 years
● To meet that time frame, at least one language must die, on average, every two weeks or so (this
is happening at the moment; 50% is an optimistic estimate)
●
● Endangered languages compared to endangered species
● Language expresses identity
○ English dialects – would it be better if your dialect didn’t exist or if everyone switched
to Standard English and your dialect was no longer spoken? → feels as though
something is missing or gone
○ People are often proud of their local accents
○ Language is a very powerful group identifier
○ An accent is a clear outward sign of identification with a particular place
● Languages are repositories of history – language is the pedigree of nations; every and any
language is a window on history
● Lexicon can give us an idea of what people they encountered in the past, and who they
borrowed words from, for ex.
● For many languages, histories are only recorded in spoken language
● Beowulf is part of the English linguistic history
● Most likely, the Old English in that clip is completely unintelligible to modern English speakers
● In the case of Beowulf, we have access to the ancient story because of a written record
● Without it, the story would be lost and English speakers would have little or no access to part
of their history
● What if your language is not written down?
● The world’s languages are full of these histories
● As languages are lost, speakers lose access to part of their history
● Languages contribute to the sum total of human knowledge
● Each language is a repository of the shared and accumulated knowledge of speakers over
centuries
● People around the world have a profound awareness of the flora and fauna, rocks, soil, climate
cycles, etc. of their environment
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
● Imagine French had become the dominant language in Britain in 1066 after the Battle of
Hastings — there would be no Shakespeare, Chaucer, etc. → there is definitely human
knowledge out there in different languages; the loss of human language is the loss of knowledge
● Languages are interesting in themselves: they are incredibly complex manifestations of the
human mind
● Although language does not determine thought, the properties of language reflect properties
of the mind
● Loss of a language is a loss of a chance to understand something profoundly and uniquely
human
● Once a language is gone, you cannot get it back
Stages of assimilation:
● Pressure on people to speak dominant language
○ Results in emerging bilingualism in children (children of parents who only speak the
minority language always grow to be fluent in the majority language)
● Younger generation shifts to dominant language
○ Results in shame at using the minority language
○ Leads to self-conscious semilingualism
○ Leads to dominant language monolingualism
● One generation later: discovery of what has been lost (perhaps too late) – language shift has
occurred
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
Summary
● We are facing a world wide language extinction crisis
● The crisis is more severe and immediate than the ecological extinction crises we are facing
● Many of the world’s languages (optimistically 50%) will cease to be spoken in the coming years
● This is a loss for everyone
● Language death is not inevitable
● We can do something about it!
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
Lecture 1
Intro
● Do animals have language?
● Is language unique to humans?
● If not, are animal communication systems fundamentally different from human language?
Popular media
● Animals (especially primates) are presented as having language – are these claims correct?
● We often treat our closest animals (domesticated ones) similar to the ways we treat other people
To figure out whether animals have language, we need to know the properties of human language
Types of communication
● Visual
○ Saguaro cactus flowers (as a form of visual communication) to communicate with bats
that pollenate the cactus
○ Wild blueberries and muscadine grapes have a particular color – this attracts deer who
consume the berries and spread the seeds all over
○ Fireflies: males glow when trying to attract females (light on, light off – rudimentary
system)
● Auditory
○ Birdsong
○ Whales – males repeat and add to each other’s songs to show off their memory and
volume
● Tactile
○ Social grooming: communication has a social function but it has to do with touching
● Chemical/olfactory communication
○ Skunks: some animals can communicate/send a message by scent alone
● These forms are not using language the way humans do – it is possible to have communication
without any use of language
Case studies
Clever Hans
● Horse, early 1900s
● William von Osten believed that horses and other animals were less intelligent than humans
because of a lack of educational opportunities
● Von Osten taught Hans to:
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
○ Tell time
○ Keep track of the calendar
○ Read and spell in German
○ Add, subtract, multiply, and divide
○ Respond correctly to verbal or written questions
● Hands would tap out answers or shake his head a # of times that corresponded to the right
answer
● Board of Education appointed a committee to investigate his claims – they concluded there
was no deception involved
● Oskar Pfungst, psychologist, found that under some conditions, Hands did much worse in
getting the correct answers
○ Ex: horse isolated from audience
○ Blinders on Hans
○ Questioner did not know the answer
● Pfngst concluded that Hans was responding to reactions of his owner and the audience
● He was picking up on unconscious, involuntary movements of the body, changes in facial
expression, and maybe even changes in heartbeat
● Pfungst was able to get Hans to start tapping by stepping in front of him and making slight
movements (without posing a question at all) – tapping would stop when Pfungst straightened
his head slightly
● He could describe a key as a key, regardless of its size or color – he had an abstract idea of what
a key was (and he could determine how the key was different from others)
● Speech sounded remarkably accurate, but it is produced very differently from humans
● Answered simple Qs about objects (ex: size, color, and material)
● Required immense amounts of training (years of direct training)
● Pepperberg does not say that Alex learned a language
● Her research interest is in concept formation in animals, not language
● We have to distinguish being intelligent from the ability to acquire human language
● Alex the parrot and Hans were intelligent, but it does not mean that they were able to acquire
human language
○ 3 types of bee dances: round dance (food source is relatively close), sickle dance
(intermediate distance), and tail-wagging dance (relatively far)
○ Bees have limited displacement abilities
○ Alex the Parrot could also ask for objects that were not present
● Prevarication: language can describe things that are not literally understood as true (imaginary)
○ Ex: we can talk about hobbits even if we don’t believe they exist
○ We can lie with language, but slightly different
○ True deception requires a Theory of Mind (speaker knows what the other person will
believe) – be careful w/ this
● Ambiguity: words in human languages can have several different meanings
○ In animal communication systems, there is no ambiguity; each call and signal has one
meaning
● Productivity: unlimited potential to express novel ideas
○ Humans are capable of creating new expressions for new objects – infinite
○ Animals have limited set of signals to choose from – fixed reference
○ They cannot produce any new signals to describe novel experiences
● Grammar: all human languages have modules of grammar
○ Phonetics
○ Phonology
○ Morphology
○ Syntax
○ Signed languages also have these properties/modules
○ Animal communication systems – if any of these are found, they exist only in very
rudimentary forms
○ Phonetics of alarm calls and dances: there is a finite set of these
Summary:
● Distinguished communication from language
● Looked at case studies of animal communication (ex: Clever Hans and Alex the Parrot)
● Looked at the properties of human language:
○ Interchangeability
○ Cultural transmission
○ Arbitrariness
○ Duality of patterning
○ Displacement
○ Prevarication
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
○ Ambiguity
○ Productivity
○ Grammar
● Various characteristics of human language are present in the animal kingdom – but in animals
it tends to be more rudimentary
● We can appreciate animal communication, even if it is not human language (ex: echolocation
and whale songs)
Lecture 2
○ Arbitrariness: yes, the alarm calls themselves do seem arbitrarily related to their
referents (ex: leopard alarm does not obviously sound like a leopard) – arbitrary
relation between form and meaning
○ Duality of patterning: there is no internal structure ; the alarm calls are NOT
combinable (they are just wholes)
○ Displacement: the ability to talk about things that are not there → not really; these
alarm calls are typically only performed in the presence of appropriate predator
■ Sort of; 2 grups of vervets were fighting and one group was clearly losing – one
member of the losing group gave out the leopard alarm call (the calling monkey
lied)
■ The alarm calls are intentional and this suggests that there is limited
displacement
■ No evidence of vervets “discussing” predators in their absence
○ Prevarication: talking about something the speaker knows is not true
■ The ex above shows prevarication
○ Ambiguity: no; the calls are unambiguous and refer to specific classes of predators
○ Productivity: there are 3 basic alarm calls
■ in forested areas where there are dogs helping human hunters, the vervets have
developed another call (a “dog alarm”) that is short and quiet – when other
vervets hear it, they sneak into the bush where hunters cannot follow
■ massively different than linguistic productivity in humans
○ Cultural transmission: the calls cannot be wholly innate since they seem to be deployed
at will (when vervets are alone, they do not give the alarm call)
■ There’s some learning invovled (ex: dog alarm)
■ Young vervets give the eagle alarm for a wide variety of large birds; not just for
the species that prey on vervets
■ By the time they’re adults, they have refined their “eagle” category – this shows
learning from the enviornment
● Vervets are clearly intelligent/their calls are sophisticated
● There is some overlap in the properties of the vervet alarm call system and human language
● But overall, the two seem quite distinct
○ Emotional reactions
○ Feeding behaviors
● He had very different cognitive development
○ His motor skills evene exceeded the child’s
○ However, he never learned to speak (while the child did)
○ Gua was able to understand 100 words, but he could not produce any intelligible ords
● Viki was also raised in a human home and actively taught to produce words
● By age 6, Viki could say mama, papa, cup, and up (very poorly articulated versions)
● Teaching chimps to speak a language did not work out very well
● Problem: chimps have a vocal tract that makes speech production essentially impossible
● Washoe: started learning language at 11 months; teaching period for about 51 months
● Idea: chimps use gestures as a natural sign in their communication
● Tried to teach her ASL
● The Gardeners used the “molding” technique, in which they would show her an object and
mold her hands into the correct sign configuration
● By 60 months, Washoe had acquired about 160 signs – by 27 years old, she had about 250 signs
● She could extend a sign she learned for a specific object to the entire class of that object - she
had abstracted the concept/category in her mind
● She made multi-word utterances: Washoe sorry, go in, etc.
● There are severe problems with the data from Washoe
● She never acquired ASL since she was never exposed to it (neither the Gardeners nor any of
their assistants were actually fluent in ASL)
● The Gardeners allowed a lot of sloppiness in Washoe’s signs, since her hands were not exactly
shaped like a human’s
● In double blind experiments, Washoe gave signs referring to an object that the observers coding
Washoe’s signs could not see – Washoe’s responses corresponded to the intended object about
60% of the time → it’s difficult to interpret this because we do not know the size of the set of
objects on any particular trial (it could be that Washoe was at chance!)
● Washoe was doing more than imitating, but clearly did not control the grammar of ASL
● Another methodologicla problem: in the 1960s, not much was known about the structure of
ASL (ASL is not just English but using signs)
● Asking what’s that vs who’s that – testing differentiation between person and thing and
location, etc…
● What’s that? If Washoe answered grape (while looking at a picture of a dog) it as counted as
correct – they wanted to know if she knew the category “common noun”
● Doesn’t matter what word Washoe uses, as long as she interprets the word category correctly
● Many of Washoe’s answers were systematically simplified to get rid of repetition
● Washoe did not acquire ASL
● The data is difficult to interpret
● Strongly suggests that it’s possible to teach a chimp a substantial vocabulary of arbitrary signs
with associated meaning
● There’s little to no evidence of any linguistic structure in Washoe’s signs, and certainly no
evidence for even a substantial command of any human language
● Nim Chimpsky (named after Noam Chomsky) → Nim was a 2 week old captive-born chimp
when he started living with a human vamily (for the first 18 months, he lived with a fam that
had a non-native ASL signer) → moved into a mansion in NY
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
● At 9 months: systematic lang training (5 hrs a day/5 days a week) – trainers were not fluent in
ASL (they used the hand molding method)
● After 4 months of training, he produced his first sign; after 3 yrs and 8 months, Nim had about
125 signs
● He signed about 20,000 multi-sign utterances (not unique, this is his total)
● Nim never acquired ASL
● Linguistic structure: in his multi-word signs, there are tendencies but no apparent rules, like in
human language
● Majority of Nim’s (and Washoe’s) multi-signs can be classified into categories like:
○ Agent-action
○ Action-object
○ Modifier-modified
○ And a few others
● These have semantic relation, not necessarily structural or syntactical relation
● In some cases like action-object, the signs occur with equa frequency in either order
● Kick ball occurred at the same frequency as ball kick – this flexibility suggests a lack of
linguistic structure
● Nim did things, like massive repetition, that human children never do (ex: give orange me give
eat orange me eat orange give me eat orange give me you)
● Combos that Nim produced are very different from those of a human child – very repetitive,
no additional complexity
●
● 1979 paper by Terrace:
○ The discourse context of Nim’s utterances directly reflected the teacher’s signing
○ Many multi-sign utterances Nim made were initiated by the teacher, and involved signs
that occurred immediately before in the teacher’s utterance
○ The amount of signs where the structure resulted from Nim’s control of language was
very small, and provides no evidence for linguistic structure or regularities
○ Much of the supposed evidence for Nim’s use of syntax comes from prompted
utterances that were at least partially repetitive
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
○ In Nim’s utterances: 90% were reactions and almost always, the signs referred to the
here and now (almost no displacement)
○ 40% were straight repetition
○ Nim interrupted the signing of the teacher
○ Nim never added new info to the situation in “conversation” with the teacher
● Conclusions of Nim study
○ Nim never acquired ASL or any other human language
○ Discourse context shows that the majority of the data from Nim’s signing was just
Nim repeating the teacher’s signs
● Koko is a lowland gorilla; Patterson has been working with her since Koko was 1 year old
● Koko is portrayed as having “really” learned ASL
● There is not much to say about her linguistically, except that there is no data to support the
claim that she has acquired ASL
● Patterson has not provided anything but summaries, charts of vocab growth, and isolated
anecdotes about Koko’s abilities
● According to Patterson:
○ 3.5 years: 100 signs
○ 5 years: 250 signs
○ Koko’s current vocab is 1000 signs and she understands written English
○ Patterson says she has systematic records: transcripts of Koko’s signing and videotapes,
but nobody has been able to study them
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
○ Info has only been available in Nat’l geo. and stories in the press
○ Koko has not acquired human language – data is maybe being overinterpreted by
Patterson
Conclusions
● Chimps and other non-human primates have:
○ Small vocabularies
○ Little evidence of grammar
○ Litte evidence of productive or innovative language
○ Maybe some evidence of displacement (ability to refer to something that is not in the
here and now)
○ No non-human primate has ever acquired a signed or spoken human language
● Questions
○ Is lang unique to humans? Yes, it appears to be species specific.
○ Are animal communication systems different from human language? Some systems
share a few properties of human language, but NONE shares all of them.
○ Do animals have language? No.
● Many of the signs she learned were iconic – closely related to the object or movement being
referred to
● 18 months: 24 signs acquired by Washoe
● Washoe used an acquired sign, hug, to express an emotional state when she saw Susan
pretending to be crying
● By the second year, her vocab was increasing, she was using signs spontaneously, etc.
● Often she did make mistakes – she signed comb for brush, for ex (showing she obviously
understands the concept) → signing became a part of her life (Washoe looked in the mirror and
signed “me”)
● Observer interpreted Washoe’s signs without seeing the image she was looking at
● Word combinations is a key part of language acquisition: ex – time eat, gimme sweet, out. Go
there.
● Washoe makes use of consistent word order (that is a __, that is a __)
● Something suggests that Washoe knew she could obtain more through the combination of
words – the evidence does not show she had a clear understanding of syntax, but she
understood the concept of it
● Lana lives in a clear plastic box with a computer; she can use symbols to request what she wants
● Lana uses Yerkish, which is designed to have a clear and unambiguous syntax – unlike ASL,
which has a looser syntax
● Question Tim groom Lana? Vs. Question Lana groom Tim? – she understands the difference
and behaves accordingly – this suggests the capacity for understanding syntax
● Washoe always used the correct word category – she had a concept of language and the power
of language (new assistants’ presence would cause Washoe to sign more slowly)
● Washoe would talk to herself in ASL
● Questioning phrases – interrogative phrases (such as who or what) → Washoe used who? And
showed questioning inflection (holding the sign for longer and having an expression on her
face)
● Changes in future procedures: teach chimps from birth, have multiple chimps so they can have
a social relationship and teach each other, and have a # of native ASL speakers on the staff →
use the full richness of the language → they want to go to maturity to see the full impact of
language acquisition
● Will chimps sign to each other? Bowie signs to Bruno to ask him for food
● Will signing mothers teach their children? Koko at the SF Zoo is being taught ASL by Penny
Patterson
● Washoe has split open an entire field of research → Washoe has attacked “the last bastion of
human uniqueness” – she rearranges signs into completely novel utterances (ex: duck = water
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
bird, lucy – watermelon = candy drink or fruit drink; they have their own concepts, ASL just
gives them a form of communication that humans can understand)
Intro
● Does language = thought?
● Is language equivalent to thought? Does language determine thought?
● Socrates thought thinking was an inner form of speech (a discourse the mind carries on with
itself)
Edward Sapir
● Extremely influential linguist and psychologist – he wrote that the real world is built upon the
language habits of the group
● Different languages classify experiences differently, completely, and incommensurably (lacking
a basis of comparison in respect to a quality normally subject to comparison)
● The same experience would be encoded differently in different languages
●
● Your native language determines how you see the world; the structure of a language influences
how speakers perceive the world around them
● Whorf’s linguistic relativity principle: users of markedly different grammars are pointed toward
different types of observations…and hence are not equivalent as observers
● He also said that they had “no general notion or intuition of time as a smooth-flowing
continuum in which everything in the universe proceeds at an equal rate, out of a future,
through present, into a past”
● The following sentence translated from Hopi: “then indeed, the following day, quite early in
the morning at the hour when people pray to the sun, around that time then he woke up the
girl again” → this shows that Whorf was likely incorrect
● Other detailed studies of Hopi showed that Hopi speech has tense markers, metaphors for
time, and units of time
● Ex: days, # of days, weeks, months, lunar phases, seasons, and the year
● The Hopi culture has a horizon-based calendar, exact ceremonial day sequences, knotted
calendar strings, notched calendar sticks, and timekeeping devices similar to sundials
● Whorf’s claims were just factually wrong – all languages have tense and can express concepts
like time and duration
● Hopi provides rich evidence for the opposite of the Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis
● Inuit language
● Whord claimed Inuit language to have several terms for snow:
●
● The idea is that all these different words in Inuit mean that Inuit peoples see the snow in a
more complex way (because of the differentiation the Inuit language provides)
Franz Boas
● Father of American Anthropology
● In 1911 – he wrote the Handbook of American Indian languages
● He said: Just as English has derived forms of water from a single root meaning water in some
other language, Inuit uses the apparently distinct roots
● Boas reported 4 different roots for snow in an Inuit language
Sapir-Whorf H with Inuit Language (cont.)
● In an article in 1940, Whorf claimed Inuit to have 7 roots for snow
● This was widely reprinted and cited in textbooks/popular books about language
● The estimates of the number of “snow” words kept getting inflated with retelling
● However there are many diff Inuit languages, and not all possess the same # of terms
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
● This # of terms to describe snow is probably matched or surpassed by skiers, regardless of their
language
English
● What about English “snow” words?
○ Snow, slush, sleet, avalanche, flurry, powder, dusting, hardpack, snowball, snowdrift,
snowfall, snowflake, etc. – we also have lots of snow words
● Do English speakers experience snowy environments as a winter wonderland of textures of
snow and ice?
● Reports about the Inuit language are false/exaggerated; it is not that different from English
Ambiguity: there are ambiguous words like bat, bug, and bank, but the concepts are clearly distinct
● A single sentence can express clearly distinct concepts
● If language were the same as thought, would it be possible to distinguish the different senses?
Thought in animals
● There are animals that lack language, but clearly they do have thoughts
● Chimpanzees are capable of having thoughts and being smart
● Thirsty crow drops stones into water to raise the water level to get the floating food or drink
the water
● It IS possible to have thoughts, plan things out, and be rational – even without language
○ Language determines thought by seeing if the cognitive system can make distinctions
that are not linguistically represented – can we find cases of this?
● Test the weaker view:
○ Language influences thought – can we find cases of this?
●
● Which bottom square has the same color as the top square? – testing rxn time
● Does having different words for light blue and dark blue have any effect on perception?
● Results:
○ Russian speakers were faster to discriminate two colors when they fell into different
linguistic categories (ex: both dark blue) than when they were from the same linguistic
category
○ English speakers tested on the identical stimuli did not show an advantage in any of the
conditions
○ How much faster were the Russian speakers on average? 124 milliseconds faster (not a
big difference)
● This shows support for a weak version of the Whorfian hypothesis: categories in language affect
performance on simple perceptual color tasks
Conclusions
● The strong view of the SW H, linguistic determinism, is not supported
Empirical basis of it is incorrect:
○ Hopi has time words
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
Lecture 2: Obscenity
Intro
● Profanity is at the interface of language and culture
○ Debates concerning profanity have been at the forefront of issues about censorship
● It also plays a role in how we think about language from a purely linguistic perspective
● Profanity does serve a purpose, although it may not be obvious
●
● They all describe the same thing – it’s not the concept that makes a word/phrase offensive
● Social conventions determine which words are considered profane, obscene, or abusive
● A taboo is a social custom that prohibits certain kinds of behavior
○ Ex: in American culture, there are certain things that you are not supposed to do in
public (sex, defacate, etc.)
● Profane words are those that some people in a culture believe to be unacceptable in certain
circumstances
● Profane or offensive words are taboo words
● It’s usually not the words themselves that are unacceptable; it’s those words when used with a
specific meaning or sense
○ “Ass”, “cock”, “bitch” – can be used to refer to animals OR to people/body parts
● The status of a word as obscene is determined by social custom
● There is actually little data in linguistics on which words are considered offensive/not
● Across English dialects, there are disgareements over which words are obscene/the level of
strength of obscene words
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
●
● There is variation in the rating of profanity across the English-speaking world, but there are
common themes
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
Profanity in English
● English draws its profanity from a few sources
● Profanity is from Latin, which means “outside the temple” (blasphemous; words that desecrate
the holy
● Religious figures: Jesus Christ, Jehovah, Mohammad
● Aspects of religious dogma: holy, hell, God, damn
● Some have fallen out of favor:
○ Zounds - God’s wounds
○ Gadzooks - God’s eyes
● Sex, sex organs, and sexual acts – a great many words in English:
○ Fuck, dick, pussy, skank, etc.
● Body functions:
○ Shit, piss, barf
● Slurs: derogatory reference to a person based on perceived group membership
○ Groups defined by race, ethnicity, sexual orientation, religion, etc.
Profanity across languages
● Cantonese:
○ Most vulgar cantonese words are: (all drawn from the “sex”
category)
● Quebec French:
○ In Quebec French, words like the ones above are considered more vulgar than the
strongest vulgarities in English:
■ Foutre (fuck) or merde (shit)
● German:
○ There are profanities drawn from sex and religion categories, but fewer than English
○ German word ficken “fuck” is not commonly used in swearing
○ Lot of swear words drawn from the excretion category
● Russian:
○ Russian Academy of Language defined the most offensive Russian words:
○
○ They were banned in 2014 by the Russian government from films, concerts, theater
performances, and books
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
● Hausa:
○ Not offensive:
○ Offensive:
not offensive replacement:
Linguistics 1 – Fall 2022
● Similarities between Hausa and English: saying God Damn in Hausa and English can be the
same format
○ Unable to curse, unable to provide correct expletive for a situation described to him,
unable to complete a curse
● His automatic speech was impaired (aka things we just memorize)
● He has the mirror image of Broca’s aphasia (Broca’s = intentional speech impaired; automatic
speech preserved)
● In this patient, the basal ganglia of the right hemisphere were damaged
● Basal ganglia = “subcortical” structures because they are beneath the brain cortex
● The basal ganglia are involved in selecting appropriate motor actions by inhibiting actions that
we don’t want to perform
● They are closely tied to emotional centers of the brain
● In the patient with damage in the basal ganglia, the capacity for blurting out emotionally
charged language was impaired (such as cursing)
● Abusive language has value in that it allows us to attack, without the consequences of an actual
attack
● Profanity is universal to all human languages due to its innate nature
Notes from the reading: 22-25
● Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis: “Human beings do not live in the objective world alone, nor in the
world of social activity as ordinarily understood, but are very much at the mercy of the
particular language which has become the medium of expressio for their society…we see and
hear and otherwise experience very largely as we do because the language habits of our
community predispose certain choices of interpretation.”
● Strong form – linguistic determinism: holds that the language we speak determines how we
perceive and think about the world → language acts like a filter on reality
● Weaker form – linguistic relativism: languages differ in the categories they encode, therefore
speakers of different languages think about the world in different ways (ex: languages break up
the color spectrum at different points)
308-312
● “Situation dialects” = styles or registers (nearly everyone has an informal and a formal style)
● No particular sound is considered to be intrinsically clean or dirty – societal taboos dictate
profanity
● Words relating to sex, sex organs, and natural bodily functions make up a large part of the set of
taboo words of many cultures (often, 2+ words can have the same linguistic meaning)
● Euphemism = a word or phrase that replaces a taboo word or serves to avoid
frightening/unpleasant subjects