VB Study Guide
VB Study Guide
B . F. S k i n n e r F o u n d a t i o n S t u d y G u i d e S e r i e s
B. F. SKINNER
VERBAL BEHAVIOR
Extended Edition
Study Guide
by Elbert Blakely
B. F. Skinner Foundation
1
ISBN: 978-0-9964539-2-9
www.bfskinner.org
Copyright © 2021, by the B. F. Skinner Foundation. All rights reserved. No part of this publication
may be reproduced in any form, electronic or mechanical, including photocopy, recording, or any
information storage and retrieval system, without permission in writing from the publisher.
B . F. S k i n n e r F o u n d a t i o n S t u d y G u i d e S e r i e s
B. F. SKINNER
VERBAL BEHAVIOR
Extended Edition
Study Guide
by Elbert Blakely, PhD, BCBA-D
Assistant Professor at Florida Institute of Technology
This study guide is designed to be used along with the B. F. Skinner Foundation’s
publication Verbal Behavior: Extended Edition.
Each chapter of the book (except Chapter 18) is featured in a separate section with
questions, followed by sample answers. Some questions do not have answers, because they
are either notes to attract reader's attention to an important point, or assignments, i.e.
"describe in your own words."
Chapter 1
A Functional Analysis of Verbal Behavior
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
2
CHAPTER 1: STUDY GUIDE
11. How is it that the concept of “ideas” has made it tempting to look
inside the organism? What is the problem with this?
12. Skinner also talks about “images” that are said to give rise to
verbal behavior. Taken together, and including other such
explicanda, these constitute explanatory fictions. What is the
problem herein with these?
3
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
17. Check out the important features of this analysis. What are they?
4
CHAPTER 1: STUDY GUIDE
5
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
Speech: it emphasizes vocal behavior. Hard to apply to other forms such as written
or sign language.
Language: Often refers to the practices of a linguistic community, and not the actions
of an individual.
Verbal behavior: It tends to emphasize the behavior of an individual, and lends itself
well to identifying controlling variables. Moreover, it does not have a long history in
traditional explanations.
His work, and the work of others, in Experimental Analysis of Behavior (EAB).
No. If fact, this is an important and essential point of this book. Specifically, we can
apply the techniques of EAB to verbal behavior.
6
CHAPTER 1: STUDY GUIDE
We understand verbal behavior when we denote the contingencies that control what
people say or write.
A) They have not developed the techniques needed for a causal analysis.
B) A formulation appropriate to all fields has not emerged. (i.e., to any field of hu-
man endeavor – see Science and Human Behavior for the same point)
No. There are many kinds and brands, but the approach has failed to yield a unified
system.
A) Easily observed
B) Lots of it!
C) Facts are substantial and can be agreed on
D) Development of writing repertoires has provided a measurement system that is
convenient and precise
11. How is it that the concept of “ideas” has made it tempting to look
inside the organism? What is the problem with this?
Verbal behavior is often considered to be an expression of “ideas.” The ideas gave rise
to the verbal behavior, and were thought to be the cause of it.
7
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
Problem: ideas can not be observed independently. In effect, they are explanatory
fictions, as their evidence of existence is in the behavior they are said to cause.
12. Skinner also talks about “images” that are said to give rise to
verbal behavior. Taken together, and including other such
explicanda, these constitute explanatory fictions. What is the
problem herein with these?
That verbal behavior has an independent existence apart from other behavior. Words
are not the expression of ideas, and we do not “use” them in this way.
When the word refers to a physical object, it is easy to describe its meaning as the
thing referred to. But when there is no physical referent, then it becomes more diffi-
cult. It becomes even more difficult in sentences.
8
CHAPTER 1: STUDY GUIDE
17. Check out the important features of this analysis. What are they?
9
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
10
CHAPTER 2: STUDY GUIDE
Chapter 2
General Problems
11
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
1. Skinner talks about “meaning” in the bottom of page 13, and top of
page 14. Explain in your own words what “meaning” is.
12
CHAPTER 2: STUDY GUIDE
13
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
14
CHAPTER 2: STUDY GUIDE
15
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
1. Skinner talks about “meaning” in the bottom of page 13, and top of
page 14. Explain in your own words what “meaning” is.
Verbal behavior is not just vocal, as other forms can have mediated reinforcement
(signs, notes, pointing, gestures). Yes, behavior can have both characteristics. For ex-
ample, behavior may have reinforcing effects on the physical environment, but also
generate reinforcers produced by others.
In the analysis of behavior, we can describe single instances, but when predicting be-
havior, we are really predicting classes of behavior that have a particular property. For
example, if we predict that a person will open the door, we are less concerned with
any topography, and more concerned with behavior that results in opening the door.
Likewise, in verbal behavior we are interested not in the musculature, but in sound
patterns that produce a given effect on the verbal community.
Speech-sounds are not really the important issue in understanding verbal behavior.
Indeed, if vocal behavior was not present, and we used non-vocal communication, the
problems of verbal behavior would still remain.
16
CHAPTER 2: STUDY GUIDE
If someone asks “What did Shaun say today?” and you respond with “He said he
will be here at 5 pm”, this is a reconstruction of the behavior of interest, and is un-
usual in science. We typically do not report behavior by imitating it. In a sense, the
reconstruction is the “name” of the response (not the response itself, of course). This
reconstruction is hypostatis, and the names of the responses are acoustically similar to
the responses themselves. Moreover, the variables responsible for the reconstruction
are not necessarily the same as those responsible for the original statement.
The former requires only a small repertoire to transcribe speech, but thousands of
different “words.” Moreover, a direct quotation usually involves inferring something
about the causal variables or the effects on a listener.
In linguistics, the unit may be the words and morphemes, or phrases, clauses, sentenc-
es, etc. Any of these may come under functional control and are therefore identifiable
units.
17
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
Over-all frequency is rate of response. But, simply developing “word counts” can be
misleading, as they may be different operant classes.
In the laboratory, we can easily see changes in rate as a function of independent vari-
ables. But, we are often interested in the probability of a single forthcoming event.
It is probably the case that such predictions will rely on rates. Indeed, in multiple
determination of a behavior, we infer the contribution of each variable based on ob-
servations of rates.
Shaping.
18
CHAPTER 2: STUDY GUIDE
When behavior is reinforced with, say, food, then the behavior may be strengthened
by food deprivation.
If a person has learned to get food by 1) going to the refrigerator 2) asking for food
and 3) getting in the car and going to the store, then 1, 2, and 3 will be strengthened
by food deprivation.
It is an account of the speaker and listener behavior. And, to completely account for
verbal behavior, we need both.
19
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
20
CHAPTER 3: STUDY GUIDE
Chapter 3
The Mand
21
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
7. Skinner points out that frequent mands may cause the listener to
“revolt.” How can this effect be mitigated?
22
CHAPTER 3: STUDY GUIDE
11. What is the significance of the example of the two math problems
and the mand “do it on paper.”
12. On page 44, Skinner revisits the “meaning” problem. What is the
traditional view of meaning of mands in this passage? In Skinner’s
view?
14. Explain the point about children learning to cry for different
reasons. What is the advantage of this account?
15. What determines the form of a mand? Are other stimuli relevant?
23
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
18. Can we mand “emotional reactions?” (Cheer up! Dry your tears!)
19. What are magical mands? How does Skinner account for them?
24
CHAPTER 3: STUDY GUIDE
25
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
No. However, some forms are often associated with particular variables such that the
function may be inferred.
He refers to the fact that we must account for both speaker and listener behavior.
The maintaining variables for the listener differ. In a request, the listener complies for
positive reinforcers. In a command, the listener complies to escape from conditioned
aversive stimuli supplied by the speaker.
26
CHAPTER 3: STUDY GUIDE
Traditionally, such classifications often appeal to the “intention” of the speaker. But,
Skinner shows that intention may be translated into contingencies of reinforcement
of the listener behavior.
7. Skinner points out that frequent mands may cause the listener to
“revolt.” How can this effect be mitigated?
A) Softening or concealing the mand character: “I’m thirsty” or “Would you mind
getting me a drink?” instead of “Water!”
B) Flattery or praise: “Get me a drink, my good man.”
Listeners may respond without any particular EO. In a sense, a generalized compli-
ance class.
11. What is the significance of the example of the two math problems
and the mand “do it on paper.”
That listeners may not even be aware that they are responding to mands and other
controlling variables. Indeed, this example shows the effects of multiple control, as
words in the instructions strengthened addition in the first problem, and multiplica-
tion in the second.
27
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
12. On page 44, Skinner revisits the “meaning” problem. What is the
traditional view of meaning of mands in this passage? In Skinner’s
view?
The reinforcer that is specified in the mand. In Skinner’s view, it is the reinforcer, as
well as the EO. Thus, the “meaning” of any mand must specify the entire contingen-
cy: evocative variables and the reinforcers responsible for their effect.
Not in favor, because they do not suggest techniques that would explain the prove-
nance of the verbal behavior.
14. Explain the point about children learning to cry for different
reasons. What is the advantage of this account?
The advantage is that 1) terms can be defined by known experimental processes (con-
ceptually systematic!) 2) it is consistent with explicanda of other verbal and nonver-
bal behavior
15. What determines the form of a mand? Are other stimuli relevant?
It is a mand that occurs in the absence of the usual discriminative stimuli. For exam-
ple, we may demand that our car “stop”, that the door “open”, etc. Or, we may issue
mands to babies, dolls, or untrained animals. They can not possibly provide reinforce-
ment, but they have characteristics that are similar to individuals who do provide
reinforcement for mands. Thus, it is a stimulus induction, or generalization, process.
28
CHAPTER 3: STUDY GUIDE
NOTE: because some of the usual controlling variables are absent, the response may be
unusually weak or “whimsical” with a comment that indicates its lack of strength.
18. Can we mand “emotional reactions?” (Cheer up! Dry your tears!)
In some cases, the mere form of the mand does not have the desired effects. However,
there are other stimuli that may accompany that may have the desired effects. For
example, “Dry your tears” may be accompanied by stroking or soothing tones of voice
that may affect the listener.
19. What are magical mands? How does Skinner account for them?
It is a form of extended mand that has no history of reinforcement to explain its emis-
sion, or the emission of similar behavior.
Skinner accounts for them by appealing to a history of mands that forms a general-
ized response class that is maintained by “control over the environment.” Or, there
may be moments of “great stress” (or strong EOs) that may evoke the response; in a
sense, the speaker may be merely describing the reinforcer appropriate to a given EO.
He discusses “poetic license” in which the writer experiences strong EOs, and emits
mands in the form of lyric poems. The poetic community and readership maintain it,
despite the absence of usual contingencies for mands.
29
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
30
CHAPTER 4: STUDY GUIDE
Chapter 4
Verbal Behavior Under the Control
of Verbal Stimuli
31
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
32
CHAPTER 4: STUDY GUIDE
10. Go back to page 59, and read the passage about the parrot. What is
the point therein? This is an important point that will be found
in many of Skinner’s writings.
33
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
18. If both the stimulus and response products are in the same
dimension (i.e., copying text), is it “self correcting?”
34
CHAPTER 4: STUDY GUIDE
28. What are the dynamic properties of echoics, textuals, and intra-
verbals? Why is this?
35
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
36
CHAPTER 4: STUDY GUIDE
37
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
No. Mands are reinforced only when an audience is present, so discriminative control
by the audience emerges.
A) Reinforce with a variety of reinforcers, and the response will exist in strength
unless there is no EO
B) Use generalized conditioned reinforcement (approval, praise)
A person may deliver a variety of aversive events, and therefore preceding stimuli (i.e.,
threats) may be conditioned. Thus, the elimination of a threat will be a reinforcer.
38
CHAPTER 4: STUDY GUIDE
A) Echoics are not self-praise statements – a verbal stimulus of the same form does
not precede it.
B) Repeating what you heard previously – there is a “special temporal relation” ab-
sent. However, he does not define what this relationship is.
C) Rule-governed behavior that has a formal similarity to the behavior in the rule.
For example, if A says “When he comes in, tell him ‘I am upset.’” B then says “I
am upset” upon arrival of the person. B’s statement is under functional control of
the arrival of the person.
D) Echoics are not “instinct.” There is no similarity between the stimulus (sounds)
and the musculature that produces them. There is, in a sense, a similarity between
the sounds and the effects of the musculature. Echoics are operant classes subject
to contingencies. Moreover, that parents have to teach echoics early in life is ad-
ditional evidence that this class is not innate.
10. Go back to page 59, and read the passage about the parrot. What is
the point therein? This is an important point that will be found
in many of Skinner’s writings.
Parrots do not innately imitate sounds. Sounds do not inexorably evoke imitative
sounds in the parrot. If anything is inherited, it is the capacity to be reinforced by
producing sounds that were previously heard.
39
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
In many cases, the individual sound (or phoneme) is the minimal unit. Children,
upon hearing some stimulus, will reproduce it, sound by sound. However, the mimics
may have even smaller units in their repertoires that include intonations, accents, etc.
These allow him/her to echo novel sound patterns of great variety.
NOTE: Some echoic sounds may combine into larger units, such as the names of
chemical compounds, technical terms, etc.
Echoing one’s own speech. In its pathological form, palilalia: when a person echoes
something heard, and then continues to repeat it.
“Reading”. That is, vocal responding to nonauditory verbal stimuli: text, pictures,
symbols, letters. There is no formal similarity between the stimulus and response;
but, there is a point-to-point correspondence but in different dimensional systems.
(e.g., the written letter A → “a”, letter B → “b”, etc.)
It is maintained by:
A) Educational reinforcers that are conditioned in nature.
B) Collateral effects of textual behavior may also provide automatic reinforcement:
interesting reading, effects that are correlated with other reinforcements.
C) Textual behavior may result in other, more effective verbal operants. For example,
a dictionary may evoke textual behavior, which then sets up the acquisition of in-
traverbals (“The definition of ‘semantics’ is…”) Or, an illustrated dictionary may
present pictures and text, such that the pictures will later be described.
It may be very small, such as at the individual sound corresponding to each letter, or
larger units may emerge that involve word combinations (“positive reinforcement”).
However, if the text is not phonetic (e.g., picture), then no limit is present.
40
CHAPTER 4: STUDY GUIDE
A) Echoics can generally have a finer grained, smaller unit than textuals.
B) Echoics provide for automatic reinforcement of closer and closer approximations
to the correct sounds. Thus, the speaker “knows when he has echoed correctly”,
but not so with textual behavior. Automatic reinforcement of reading interesting
text may strengthen the textual behavior, but it does not differentially reinforce
correct forms at the phonetic level. (there is no automatic reinforcement of cor-
respondence between S and R).
We can create our text that will control our behavior at a later date. It is a relatively
permanent product, unlike the effects of echoic behavior.
Copying written material or taking dictation. The speaker is creating a visual stimulus
from a previous visual or auditory stimulus, respectively.
18. If both the stimulus and response products are in the same
dimension (i.e., copying text), is it “self correcting?”
Yes. The speaker can compare both, and be differentially reinforced for closer and
closer approximations.
Verbal behavior under the control of prior verbal stimuli, but without the point-to-
point correspondence either within a dimension or across two dimensions.
41
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
Not typically. If we interrupt the chain, and then re-present the previous, it is often
hard to get restarted. You often need a “running start” that involves several previous
links.
A haplological error is when two links in a chain are the same, and arrival at the first
link evokes behavior that occurs after the second. It is an “anticipatory” jump.
Sequences of intraverbals.
NOTE: Skinner makes an important point about multiple control and multiple ef-
fects. That is, any given word may be evoked by a variety of variables, and any given
word may in turn evoke a variety of responses, depending on other variables.
A) History of reinforcement of the speaker (e.g., med students will react to “admin-
ister” differently than law students)
B) The size and combination of words. Larger, more complex statements will tend
to evoke a narrower number of responses (e.g., “red” or “white” will evoke many
responses, but “red, white, and ___” will evoke only one: blue!)
42
CHAPTER 4: STUDY GUIDE
No. The unit may be small sounds or larger patterns. However, there is no function-
al unity as in mimicry, and there is no point-to-point correspondence between the
stimulus and response.
28. What are the dynamic properties of echoics, textuals, and intra-
verbals? Why is this?
With a well-trained speaker, the speed or energy tends to be uniform without large
variations. One reason is that the reinforcers are conditioned generalized, and there-
fore strong motivational variations may be absent. However, dynamic properties can
be evoked in echoics (they will have the properties) and to a lesser extent textuals
(using underlines, etc.)
Between the response and the source of the verbal stimulus presented by the original
speaker. Again, meaning is simply the variable (s) responsible for the emission of the
response.
43
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
44
CHAPTER 5: STUDY GUIDE
Chapter 5
The Tact
45
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
4. Check out the figures on pages 84 and 85. Be able to diagram tacts
using a similar layout. What is the evocative stimulus in these
figures?
5. How does Skinner account for the listener who provides the
conditioned generalized reinforcement?
7. What variables will determine whether the listener will take ef-
fective action after hearing the tact?
46
CHAPTER 5: STUDY GUIDE
12. NOTE: On page 92, Skinner talks about behavior coming under
evocative control of a single property. This will be discussed fur-
ther in the section on abstraction, which, you will see, requires
the actions of a verbal community.
47
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
15. Explain the point about the “eye of the needle” on page 94.
18. On page 96, Skinner talks about why metaphors occur. Give a
couple of reasons.
20. Check out the first paragraph on page 97. Explain with an exam-
ple how an extension occurs without a common element between
the stimuli.
21. On the bottom of 97 and top of 98, Skinner makes the case for the
utility of metaphor. What are the main points?
48
CHAPTER 5: STUDY GUIDE
24. How common are metonymical extensions? Why? Can they become
part of standard usage?
28. Explain how names that result from metaphor are more easily
remembered.
29. In the next section, Skinner talks about guessing. Can a response
be classified as a tact if it is not evoked by some environmental
stimulus? What, then, is a guess?
49
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
32. How does the verbal community check the process of tact
extensions in #31? Give an example.
33. At the last paragraph of page 107, Skinner talks about metaphor
and stimulus induction. What is this term? What is the point
here?
50
CHAPTER 5: STUDY GUIDE
37. On page 111, Skinner goes on to talk about the problems in trying
to identify the defining properties of objects, or in trying to
define their “meaning.” He concludes with the solution on page
113. What is this solution?
38. What is the difference between a proper tact and common tact?
51
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
42. Skinner again examines the problem of what a term “refers to.”
Check out the discussion on page 117. What is his point?
43. Check out the table on page 118. It presents combinations of pairs
of tacts. Each cell represents a combination of the kind of stimuli
(same, similar, or different) and kind of response (same, similar, or
different). Some of the cells represent well-known combinations,
such as synonyms and homynyms. Provide examples for each cell.
52
CHAPTER 5: STUDY GUIDE
47. So, how does Skinner conclude his discussion of minimal units of
tacts?
53
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
52. On page 127, Skinner discusses the different kinds of tacts emitted
by scientists and poets to essentially the same situations. Why is
this?
57. The next section deals with the problem of tacting private events.
What is the problem herein?
54
CHAPTER 5: STUDY GUIDE
58. How can the verbal community establish tacts to private events?
59. Check out the passage from Science and Human Behavior. Skinner's
first point has to do with “other variables” that may weaken the
stimulus control of private events. What does this mean?
61. Skinner notes that a given tact of private stimuli may involve a
mixture of stimuli, both public and private. Analyze “My heart is
racing”.
62. On page 136, Skinner talks of tacts that may involve public
properties, but also involve private events. Give an example:
63. Note: On the bottom of 137 and 138, Skinner cautions that private
stimuli are not necessarily involved in many situations. He offers
straightforward, more parsimonious explicanda for his examples.
55
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
65. The next section has to do with tacting one’s own behavior. The
first set of examples involve tacting one’s current behavior. How
does this happen? What are the range of stimuli that evoke the
behavior?
66. Explain the point about the difference between the tact “red”
and “I see red”.
67. Skinner concludes this section with two points. What are they?
70. The next section discusses tacts to prior behavior. How is this
acquired?
56
CHAPTER 5: STUDY GUIDE
72. Skinner also talks about behavior that can not be executed, such
as skiing without snow. Identify the potential evocative stimuli.
This analysis can also be applied to responding to future behavior.
74. The final section deals with tacting the probability of behavior.
What might be the controlling variables?
57
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
58
CHAPTER 5: STUDY GUIDE
59
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
Verbal behavior that is evoked by some feature of the nonverbal physical environ-
ment.
No, it does not refer to anything in a technical sense. Instead, that evocative stimulus
merely strengthens the response because of a reinforcement history.
…the mand permits the listener to infer something about the condition of the speaker
regardless of the external circumstances, while the tact permits him to infer some-
thing about the circumstances regardless of the condition of the speaker. (p. 83)
A tact that presumably is not influenced by any EO, which would result from condi-
tioned generalized reinforcement. Skinner suggests this probably never happens, but
when a tact does occur, its form is definitely determined solely by the environmental
feature.
4. Check out the figures on pages 84 and 85. Be able to diagram tacts
using a similar layout. What is the evocative stimulus in these
figures?
5. How does Skinner account for the listener who provides the
conditioned generalized reinforcement?
60
CHAPTER 5: STUDY GUIDE
Or, in other cases, the speaker may tact events that the listener has no contact with.
Then, the listener may be brought into contact, and gain reinforcers unrelated to the
speaker behavior; then, the listener may reinforce the tact because it has allowed
access to these reinforcers. For example, the speaker may answer the phone, and tell
the listener “phone for you!” The listener may then answer the phone, and obtain
reinforcements from the call, and then say “thank you for getting the phone” to the
original speaker. This would, then, reinforce the tact. In a sense, tacts extend the lis-
tener’s contact with the world, and are therefore maintained for that reason.
Finally, tacts may allow the speaker to be controlled by stimuli in the environment.
For example, those who learn to tact their own private behavior, such as anxiety, may
be able to respond to it more effectively. Same goes for external stimuli. If you are
able to tact an external stimulus, you may be able to react to it more effectively. This
may be related, or the same as, “awareness.”
No. If someone tacts “candy”, he/she can not consume the tact. Moreover, in re-
sponse to Russell’s argument, Skinner also opines that we don’t respond to the tact
in the exact way that we respond to the thing tacted. Thus, we react differently upon
hearing the tact “candy” than to the candy itself.
7. What variables will determine whether the listener will take ef-
fective action after hearing the tact?
61
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
8. On page 89, Skinner talks of the stimulus control of the tact. Start
with the basics: is there audience control in the tact? But do we
always tact in the presence of an audience? What might influence
such tacting?
Yes, there is audience control in tacting, as there is in other forms of verbal behavior.
However, we do not always tact in the presence of an audience, because there are many
environmental characteristics that are not novel, and therefore not reinforced in the
verbal community. Thus, one variable is the novelty of the characteristic. Moreover,
the listener may mand for a particular tact (“what is that?”), and this will strengthen
the tact.
No. Skinner suggests that tacts can vary more in speed, intensity, and clarity, depend-
ing on the stimuli being tacted and the occasion. (a speaker may excitedly say “There
is a shooting star!”)
Step 2 → The defining characteristic is present in a new stimulus, and evokes the tact
A different looking chair is presented, and the person tacts “chair”
Thus, in generic extension, a tact comes under evocative control of a stimulus with a
necessary characteristic. Then, that characteristic is present in a new stimulus, and
the tact is evoked.
No. The stimulus is no longer novel, and the stimulus class has been expanded (e.g.,
62
CHAPTER 5: STUDY GUIDE
there may be several kinds of chairs that evoke “chair”). Thus, the strength of the tact
can be attributed directly to a reinforcement history, not an extension.
12. NOTE: On page 92, Skinner talks about behavior coming under
evocative control of a single property. This will be discussed fur-
ther in the section on abstraction, which, you will see, requires
the actions of a verbal community.
When a response is evoked by a property that was present during previous reinforce-
ment, but was not the defining characteristic (it was not the property that was part of
the contingency arranged by the verbal community)
Skinner’s example involved a child who upon drinking soda water for the first time,
reported that it tasted “like my foot’s asleep.” The child originally learned to say “my
foot’s asleep” in the presence of two stimuli: immobility and pinpoint stimulation.
The VC used immobility as the property for reinforcement, but the pinpoint stim-
ulation was correlated. Then, when it occurred in another situation, it evoked the
response.
No. When the response enters into a three-term contingency, its strength can be
traced to a history of reinforcement. Skinner talks about the metaphor “he is like a
mouse.” Originally, it may have been a metaphor, as the person may have been timid
or quiet, which are typical characteristics of mice but not the defining characteristics.
But, a timid or shy person may now be called “mouse” because that is a simple tact
for such a person. These characteristics are now defining characteristics for calling
someone a mouse.
15. Explain the point about the “eye of the needle” on page 94.
Let us say that a person learns to tact “eye” to the opening of a needle. Interestingly,
the opening has a geometrical shape similar to an animal’s eye. Thus, this similarity
(although not a defining element) may serve to provide supplementary strength to
the response “eye”, thus making the learning process faster and resistant to other tacts.
63
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
Tacts tell us about current conditions, but very little about the speaker. In metaphor,
the responses were acquired under other conditions, and hypotheses may be made
about them. For example, if a speaker says that their friend “hissed like a snake”, we
may infer that the speaker has a history with respect to snakes (esp those that “hiss”).
18. On page 96, Skinner talks about why metaphors occur. Give a
couple of reasons.
In the metaphor “…is bright as night as dark”, a given variable strengthened “bright”
which then evoked “night and dark” as intraverbals.
20. Check out the first paragraph on page 97. Explain with an exam-
ple how an extension occurs without a common element between
the stimuli.
There may be an extension if both stimuli have a common effect on the speaker. Thus,
“Juliet is the sun” may occur because to Romeo, Juliet has an effect on him that is the
same as the effect of the sun (they both glow!). Or, “his mood is black as thunder
clouds” may occur because seeing the person has an effect that is similar to seeing
thunder clouds.
64
CHAPTER 5: STUDY GUIDE
21. On the bottom of 97 and top of 98, Skinner makes the case for the
utility of metaphor. What are the main points?
A) When a tact is available, metaphor may be more effective because it may be more
familiar, and may evoke desired emotional responses in the listener.
B) Allows recombination of properties
C) If no tact is available, it allows some kind of description of current conditions. Skin-
ner makes that point that in psychology, effective ways of describing behavior were,
until recently, unavailable. This is where Literature stepped in and provided colorful,
descriptive metaphors. For example, one writer compared personalities to different
animals. Or, mythology gives us “herculean tasks”.
Science is associated with generic extensions (and tacts), and literature is more readily
associated with metaphors.
An extension in which a tact occurs, but the defining element is absent, and the only
elements that are present are those which may sometimes accompany the defining
element. The relation is often purely accidental, and for that reason, may be confusing
to the listener.
E.g.: “The White House denied the rumor.” In this case, the President denied the
rumor, but the two are often associated.
24. How common are metonymical extensions? Why? Can they become
part of standard usage?
Quite rare. The controlling and contingent (defining) properties are so loosely associ-
ated that the response is of little value.
Yes, they can be used in the standard vernacular. Like metaphors, if they become rein-
forced by the verbal community, they are no longer metonymical extensions.
They are another form of generalization (like metaphor and metonymy), but an even
more extreme form. The evocative properties are distantly related to the defining
65
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
feature, or they are perhaps only similar for irrelevant reasons. These are often called
“malaprops” after Mrs. Malaprop from The Rivals. Such extensions are typically use-
less, and in some cases, Skinner claims, are dangerous.
A) They could be surviving tacts - “The Little Church Around the Corner”
B) Tact extension - e.g., a child is named after another person because the child has
certain features that are similar to the person. This would be metaphor.
C) A name may be chosen to add “prestige” to the child
28. Explain how names that result from metaphor are more easily
remembered.
Names that are metaphor are evoked in part by some feature of the person. Thus,
upon meeting the person, the sight of the person and the evocative characteristics
related to the name may strengthen saying the correct name. Similarly, literature pro-
vides many examples. Skinner cites “Mr. Quiverful” who has a large family, and his
name derives strength from the large number of children that he has.
NOTE: Memory experts may change a proper name to a description of the person to
be named. For example, let us say you are introduced to a guy named “Stanley”, who
has been berating you in a meeting. Upon hearing his name, you may say to yourself
“his name is ‘Slam-me’” as way to remember his name.
29. In the next section, Skinner talks about guessing. Can a response
be classified as a tact if it is not evoked by some environmental
stimulus? What, then, is a guess?
No. A tact is actually a relation between a stimulus and response, and absent such a
relation, there can not be a tact.
66
CHAPTER 5: STUDY GUIDE
However, guesses are sometimes tacts. If you guess who sings a particular song, there
may be a very subtle evocative stimulus in the music that the speaker may not even
realize. Or in other kinds of guesses, the guess may be an echoic or intraverbal. Skin-
ner talks about responding to “heads or tails”. In this situation, the person may simply
echo “tails”.
A) Stimulus clarity
B) Motivational variables (these must be strong in metonymy and guessing)
C) Resemblance of the present situation to the original conditioning situation(s)
32. How does the verbal community check the process of tact
extensions in #31? Give an example.
By reinforcing responses that are evoked only in the presence of a particular proper-
ty. For example, the behavioral community only reinforces the tact “positive rein-
forcement” if a consequence has been presented after a behavior, and the behavior is
strengthened. It does not reinforce the tact “positive reinforcement is a kid smiles or
if a kid asks for it or if he “seems to like it.” (These elements often accompany rein-
forcers, but are not defining features - thus, the tact would be a metaphor).
33. At the last paragraph of page 107, Skinner talks about metaphor
and stimulus induction. What is this term? What is the point
here?
Stimulus induction is another term for generalization. His point is that if metaphor
is attributed to some special mental process or ability, then we will miss the important
behavioral process of generalization. Moreover, even more extravagant mental pro-
cesses will be offered to explain abstraction, and the basic procedures that produce it
will be overlooked.
67
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
Probably not. Skinner suggests that the differential reinforcement process (reinforce
in the presence, extinguish in the absence) can not be implemented with all possible
stimuli. Thus, there may be an occasional metaphor.
No. Skinner suggests that is only possible in a verbal community. He gives the ex-
ample of “red”. We can teach children to tact “red” in the presence of all red objects.
Although we may eat red apples, we don’t eat red books, red pencils, etc.
No. There evocative stimulus may actually be a collection of stimuli. For example,
an “object” may evoke a tact that is controlled by a set of characteristics (“chair” or
“bassett hound”).
37. On page 111, Skinner goes on to talk about the problems in trying
to identify the defining properties of objects, or in trying to
define their “meaning.” He concludes with the solution on page
113. What is this solution?
Systematically manipulate stimuli, and note the presence or absence of the response.
Those stimuli that evoke the response are the defining characteristics. Remember
this, as it answers the question “What is meaning?” In short, the meaning of a word
(e.g., tact) are the variables responsible for its emission.
38. What is the difference between a proper tact and common tact?
Proper tact: response is under the control of a specific person or thing (e.g., “Linda”,
“Stacy”)
Common tact: response is under the control of a property that defines a class of per-
sons or things (e.g., “girl”, “cats”)
68
CHAPTER 5: STUDY GUIDE
The history of reinforcement will determine the strength of an abstract tact. Some
examples of the tact may have been reinforced, but perhaps they have been extin-
guished or punished. This combination will determine current probability.
• Echoics: The speaker can acquire echoics of various sizes. Over time, he/she ac-
quires a minimal repertoire of small echoic units that approximate speech sounds.
This allows the person to echo any new word or phrase.
• Textuals: Textual behavior may show various sizes, and the minimal repertoire
approximates the individual letter such that untrained words will be read.
• Intraverbals: A given stimulus may evoke a variety of responses depending on
the situation. Likewise, a given response will be evoked by a variety of stimuli.
Thus, there is no correspondence between stimulus and response, and a minimal
repertoire is lacking here. There are merely intraverbal tendencies as shown in
word-association experiments.
• Tacts: There is no point-to-point correspondence in tacts, but the contingencies
are more consistent than in intraverbals. Thus, there seems to be some kind of
minimal repertoire.
69
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
42. Skinner again examines the problem of what a term “refers to.”
Check out the discussion on page 117. What is his point?
The referent of a tact is the property or set of properties that evoke the response-again,
the variables responsible for its emission.
43. Check out the table on page 118. It presents combinations of pairs
of tacts. Each cell represents a combination of the kind of stimuli
(same, similar, or different) and kind of response (same, similar, or
different). Some of the cells represent well-known combinations,
such as synonyms and homynyms. Provide examples for each cell.
Stimulus
4. 5. 6.
Body of water → Someone in pain →
Dish → ”Dish”
Response
”Lake” ”Moan”
Similar
Body of water → Someone in trouble → Fish → “Fish”
“Loch” “Groan”
7. 8. 9. Ideal
Someone running →
”Fast” Truck → “Truck” Dog → ”Dog”
Someone running →
Different
”Quick”
Car → “Car” Truck → “Truck”
(Synonyms)
Because these are examples of each property having its own tact.
70
CHAPTER 5: STUDY GUIDE
Words (“cat”), phrases (“under the rug”), and sentences (“How are you?”) may all
have a functional unity controlled by particular stimuli. These may also be combined
in various ways, such as when a child learns “I have candy” and “I have toy”, and then
is able to tact “I have __” depending on what he/she has. This repertoire of words and
phrases allows the child to describe new, complex situations.
An initial “sp” of many words in English have to do with a substance emanating from
a source (spit, speak, spray) or radiating from a point (spoke, spur). Thus, “sp” may be
functional unit evoked by a stimulus that involves emanation or radiation.
NOTE: However, just because a word begins with “sp” does not mean it belongs to
the same operant class (spider, spirochete). Again, we can not reliably infer function
by form, a point Skinner makes in many of his writings.
47. So, how does Skinner conclude his discussion of minimal units of
tacts?
It would be difficult, if not impossible, to define them all, unlike echoics and textuals.
The properties of nature are very numerous, and it would therefore be difficult to list
them all and assign different tacts to each. This fact gives rise to tact combinations.
One in which the same stimuli would be tacted the same, different stimuli would
be tacted differently, and similar stimuli would be tacted similarly. For example, the
71
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
word for “house” would be composed of elements referring to size, color, style, ma-
terials used, etc. In another house, the elements would be re-combined to reflect the
differences. Thus, every tact would be “proper noun.”
An ideal language is probably not possible, but we may approach it by having increas-
ingly separate response units tied to various stimuli.
Onomatopoeia: a tact that has some of the formal (audible) properties of the evoca-
tive stimulus (a bee that “buzzes”).
A) An established echoic repertoire (of the speaker) extended to audible, but non-
verbal stimuli.
B) They may be more effective upon a listener because it resembles an auditory stim-
ulus with which the listener is familiar.
In a sense, if there are 2 tacts available, then the one that will probably be emitted
will have additional evocative strength from the echoic stimulation provided by the
stimulus.
52. On page 127, Skinner discusses the different kinds of tacts emitted
by scientists and poets to essentially the same situations. Why is
this?
The respective repertoires are provided for different audiences, and therefore are un-
der the control of different contingencies provided by the respective verbal commu-
nities.
72
CHAPTER 5: STUDY GUIDE
A) History of reinforcement
B) Response
C) The evocative stimulus
In concept formation, the “formation” occurs in #1, and the “concept” is the relation
between #2 and #3. Note that current views of concept formation is that there is
generalization within classes and discrimination between classes of stimuli. For ex-
ample, if a person knows the concept of dog, then he/she can tact “dog” to any dog
(complete generalization) and will not tact “dog” to a cat (discrimination)
In tacts, there is a continuum beginning with Proper Names and ending with minimal
abstractions (sometimes called “universals”). As we go through the continuum, the
evocative stimuli become more difficult to identify.
73
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
57. The next section deals with the problem of tacting private events.
What is the problem herein?
Tacts are normally reinforced by a verbal community that, along with the speaker, is
exposed to a given set of stimuli (“There is the sun!” → “Yes, you are right”). But in
private events, the verbal community has no direct contact with the evocative stimuli,
and reinforcers can not be directly linked with them.
58. How can the verbal community establish tacts to private events?
D) The verbal community may reinforce tacts to public stimuli, but the magnitude
of the stimulus may decrease to such an extent that it is actually private, thus it
still evokes the response.
E.g.: people may learn to tact their own public behavior. However, the behavior
may decrease in magnitude, but the functional relations may survive.
59. Check out the passage from Science and Human Behavior. Skinner's
first point has to do with “other variables” that may weaken the
stimulus control of private events. What does this mean?
We may behave in ways that suggest private stimuli are evoking behavior, but this may
not be so. E.g.: saying “I don’t feel well” to get out of class.
74
CHAPTER 5: STUDY GUIDE
61. Skinner notes that a given tact of private stimuli may involve a
mixture of stimuli, both public and private. Analyze “My heart is
racing”.
62. On page 136, Skinner talks of tacts that may involve public
properties, but also involve private events. Give an example:
“Familiar”: When the tact “familiar” occurs, the speaker is responding to repeating
stimuli. In other words, “I am being affected by this person in a way that is the same
as I have been affected in the past”. This property of repeating stimuli may then occur
in the presence of other repeating stimuli.
When the tact “beautiful” occurs, it is probably not evoked by a single property of
“beauty.” Instead, the person is probably responding to private stimuli that are evoked
by such objects. Moreover, these particular private stimuli may differ across people
(beauty is in the eye of the beholder!).
Or, in projection, calling someone else “angry” may also be strengthened by your own
private “angry” stimuli.
63. Note: On the bottom of 137 and 138, Skinner cautions that private
stimuli are not necessarily involved in many situations. He offers
straightforward, more parsimonious explicanda for his examples.
75
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
Tacts learned to behavior that are metaphorically extended to inanimate objects (an-
gry seas, angry clouds).
65. The next section has to do with tacting one’s own behavior. The
first set of examples involve tacting one’s current behavior. How
does this happen? What are the range of stimuli that evoke the
behavior?
The verbal community can easily reinforce tacts to current behavior because there are
public stimuli available. However, there is a host of private accompaniments that may
acquire control. For example, “I am driving a car” can be taught by the community
because the behavior is in plain sight. But, the speaker may be able to emit that tact
under the control of the private stimuli involved in driving (the feel of the car, steer-
ing wheel, etc.).
66. Explain the point about the difference between the tact “red”
and “I see red”.
“Red” - an abstract tact under the control of the property of red that is maintained by
the community.
“I see red” - A tact of activity of the speaker. The speaker is responding to events that
accompany, or are similar to, events that are present when the community reinforces
“red”. In other words, the speaker is responding to his/her own private “discriminative
behavior”: the behavior of “seeing red”. This becomes especially important when the
red stimulus is not physically present. In this case, traditional treatments would talk
of an “image of red” (unlike the “sensation of red” when the red stimulus is present.)
67. Skinner concludes this section with two points. What are they?
A) We can not condition abstractions to private stimuli.
B) Discriminative control by private stimuli requires contingencies by the verbal
community. Private stimuli may come to control behavior (learning to swing a
golf club) through natural contingencies, “knowing” about such stimuli (being
able to tact them) requires social reinforcement.
76
CHAPTER 5: STUDY GUIDE
A) Ease of execution
B) Avoids punishment
70. The next section discusses tacts to prior behavior. How is this
acquired?
This is a special ability taught by the community, and is an example of tacting prior
events. Presumably, the question (“What did you have for lunch yesterday?”) evokes
stimuli (seeing yourself eating lunch) that are then tacted.
In many cases, it is merely “weak” behavior resulting from weak stimulation, a poor-
ly conditioned response, the absence of an audience, or appropriate conditions are
lacking.
72. Skinner also talks about behavior that can not be executed, such
as skiing without snow. Identify the potential evocative stimuli.
This analysis can also be applied to responding to future behavior.
77
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
74. The final section deals with tacting the probability of behavior.
What might be the controlling variables?
A) Characteristics of behavior in progress (as you are quickly reaching for a sand-
wich when deprived)
B) Variables controlling the behavior (the lack of food for 12 hours)
Understanding the variables responsible for tacting our own behavior is important.
If we do, then we can design programs to teach better recall, problem-solving, and
thinking repertoires.
78
CHAPTER 6: STUDY GUIDE
Chapter 6
Special Conditions Affecting Stimulus Control
79
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
80
CHAPTER 6: STUDY GUIDE
8. What is an “announcement?”
9. In the middle of page 153, Skinner talks about how a given operant
(tact) may come under the control of other variables, and
therefore become a different operant. Explain this.
13. Skinner makes the point that a “concrete term”, or tact, is more
likely to evoke emotions that abstractions. Why might this be?
81
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
16. What kinds of variables can affect the power of verbal behavior?
17. In the first full paragraph on page 160, Skinner refers to a proce-
dure used by salesmen and therapists to increase “belief.” Please
explain.
18. Talk about the “rhetorical devices” that can intensify the
listener’s reaction.
20. The section, an important one, discusses what happens when the
speaker and listener are the same person. In a sense, a person
talks to him/herself and reinforcing effects are automatically
generated. How can this happen?
82
CHAPTER 6: STUDY GUIDE
23. Note: The final section deals with the behavior of readers. Skin-
ner points out that there are two possible reactions of the read-
er: First, he/she may exhibit textual behavior. Second, there may
be other reactions such as emotional behavior or other operant
behavior that is appropriate to the stimulus. In this second kind
of reaction, the reader may be responding to his/her own textual
behavior as a listener.
83
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
84
CHAPTER 6: STUDY GUIDE
85
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
A) The rate at which it is given may be important (will determine rate of verbal be-
havior emitted)
B) Generalized reinforcement may be given for particular forms or themes of verbal
behavior (see Greenspoon’s experiments)
It will deteriorate, as the listener will terminate reinforcer delivery. However, if the
distortion can be discriminated, as in fictionalized reports, then reinforcers will be
forthcoming and the behavior will be strengthened.
Aversive stimuli may be presented (or the threat thereof ), and then terminated con-
tingent on some form of tacts of events. Thus, there is a risk that precise stimulus
control over the tacts will be distorted, or even absent.
86
CHAPTER 6: STUDY GUIDE
Sight of candy + candy deprivation → ”That is candy!” → Therapist gives praise and
candy
8. What is an “announcement?”
It is a form of tact that occurs because it leads the listener to respond to some stimulus
described in the tact. For example, a teacher may say “I have candy available!” Such
tacts are often preceded by mands “Listen class…”
9. In the middle of page 153, Skinner talks about how a given operant
(tact) may come under the control of other variables, and
therefore become a different operant. Explain this.
“I lost my penny” may originate as a tact, but if the speaker receives a penny, the re-
sponse may become a mand, despite the fact that a penny was not lost. Or Aesop’s
boy who cried “wolf !” started out as a tact, but continued in the absence of the pred-
ator because it caused others to approach the boy.
NOTE: This provides yet another example of defining behavior by its function, not
form, and how the function may change quickly. Think of an example in one of your
client’s behavior.
• Positive: A speaker may tell an amusing story that evokes laughter in the listener.
(laughter may lead to other kinds of reinforcers given by the listener)
87
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
• Negative: A speaker may tell or horrific story that evokes discomfort in the lis-
tener, which may be reinforcing in some cases
NOTE: Skinner, in other writings, talks about “signs of damage to others” as an im-
portant reinforcer in aggression. If you buy that, what variables (EOs) might produce
such a reinforcer value?
Happiness
A) Respondents elicited by current stimuli (increase in BP, heart rate)
B) A predisposition to act in various ways (very likely to smile, laugh, make positive
comments) - presumably the result of some EO
By the pairing of a word and some other event that elicits an emotion (pairing of the
tact “snake” with the actual snake)
NOTE: Skinner makes several points about verbal behavior and emotion. Suffice it
to say that emotional reactions are important in poetry, prose, naming your children,
etc.
13. Skinner makes the point that a “concrete term”, or tact, is more
likely to evoke emotions that abstractions. Why might this be?
Simple tacts are more likely to be correlated with emotional stimuli than abstractions,
which have been conditioned to 1 single property ranging over a variety of condi-
tions, thus breaking ties to any other stimulus.
88
CHAPTER 6: STUDY GUIDE
16. What kinds of variables can affect the power of verbal behavior?
A) Physical characteristics of the stimulus (clarity, speed, volume)
B) Listener’s history with respect to those in a)
C) Listener’s “belief ” - response strength
17. In the first full paragraph on page 160, Skinner refers to a proce-
dure used by salesmen and therapists to increase “belief.” Please
explain.
The salesperson/therapist says several statements which are obviously true, and the
listener reacts appropriately (“Yeah, you are right”). Then, the listener is predisposed
to agree upon hearing other statements. This is perhaps similar to the well-known
procedure of behavior momentum.
18. Talk about the “rhetorical devices” that can intensify the
listener’s reaction.
A) Repeating a statement several times in different ways (“I am very upset with you.
Very angry. Really teed off !”) or presenting a statement + a picture
B) Correspondence of stimulus and response (Long tacts to describe large objects,
onomatopoeia, rare name for a rare bird)
89
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
20. The section, an important one, discusses what happens when the
speaker and listener are the same person. In a sense, a person
talks to him/herself and reinforcing effects are automatically
generated. How can this happen?
NOTE: Presumably, this effect may involve pairing of the sound with other
reinforcers, or it may not.
90
CHAPTER 6: STUDY GUIDE
23. Note: The final section deals with the behavior of readers.
Skinner points out that there are two possible reactions of the
reader: First, he/she may exhibit textual behavior. Second, there
may be other reactions such as emotional behavior or other
operant behavior that is appropriate to the stimulus. In this
second kind of reaction, the reader may be responding to his/her
own textual behavior as a listener.
91
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
92
CHAPTER 7: STUDY GUIDE
Chapter 7
The audience
93
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
94
CHAPTER 7: STUDY GUIDE
10. What is the difference between the effect described in 9b, and
intraverbal stimuli?
11. Examine the point made on page 183 at the bottom: “We have seen
that there are no true synonyms, for when all variables have been
specified there is no choice of remaining terms.” What does this
mean?
95
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
96
CHAPTER 7: STUDY GUIDE
97
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
The audience is, functionally, a discriminative stimulus that strengthens a large group
of responses. Note that different audiences may strengthen various subgroups of the
repertoire. For example, a group of musicians may strengthen tacts and intraverbals
related to music, and a group of psychologists may strengthen tacts and intraverbals
related to psychology.
The presence of some object (ice cream) will evoke a tact of a particular form, de-
pending on the audience. Thus, the audience will strengthen a group of responses,
while the sight of a particular object will have a more specific effect on the form of the
response. Thus, in the ice cream example, the presence of your friends, along with the
sight of the ice cream, may evoke “ice cream” as a tact. However, the presence of some
behavioral people may evoke “positive reinforcer.” In a sense, a behavioral audience
will strengthen a large class of tacts that relate to behavioral terminology.
Some audiences reinforce just mands, others reinforce tacts or intraverbals related to
thematic topics. Still others may reinforce “creative” conversations.
The physical stimuli that control responses are easy to identify in echoics, textual, in-
traverbals, and tacts. Not so in the audience. However, there are some characteristics
that are important:
A) The physical resemblance of the audience to others who have reinforced behavior
B) Uniforms or signs may be important (e.g., “Staff ”)
C) If audience character is correlated with weak (or strong), delayed (or immediate)
reinforcement, then the resulting behavior will be weak (or strong)
D) Receptor orientation (not in the book, but think about this one)
98
CHAPTER 7: STUDY GUIDE
There may be stimuli in the environment that evoke “images” or conditioned seeing.
These same stimuli may also strengthen the writing.
SΔ; SP
Repression results from punishment. In this concept, behavior has been punished se-
verely, and any incipient behavior is reinforced because it terminates aversive stimuli.
This incipient behavior includes the behavior involved in self knowledge (knowing
what you have done or are doing).
Yes. A person can speak, and then react to the verbal behavior as a listener.
E.g.: One may rehearse a speech, and then react to what is said as an audience, and
pose questions that are in turned, answered.
A self audience is functioning when the speaker exhibits verbal behavior even if exter-
nal conditions do not support it. For example, a person is the speaker/listener when
the potential listeners are not “paying attention,” when there is a great deal of noise,
or perhaps they speak a different language.
A) Places: Certain places may be correlated with reinforcement for verbal behav-
ior. Therefore, those places will strengthen the verbal behavior. Thus, we may be
“talkative” at certain locations that have involved reinforcement for such behav-
ior in the past.
99
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
B) Verbal behavior itself: The stimulation arising from talking may be correlated
with reinforcement for continued talking. For example, talking in English is an
SD for continued talking in English. Likewise for speaking in French. This effect
can be seen when an audience changes say, from English to French, and the bilin-
gual speaker’s verbal behavior changes more slowly, as he/she “gets warmed up”
in the second language.
10. What is the difference between the effect described in 9b, and
intraverbal stimuli?
Intraverbal stimuli strengthen particular responses, and the audience effect of lan-
guage described above strengthens a larger group of responses, as all audiences do.
11. Examine the point made on page 183 at the bottom: “We have seen
that there are no true synonyms, for when all variables have been
specified there is no choice of remaining terms.” What does this
mean?
When there are two or more tacts that might be appropriate in a given situation, the
one that is ultimately emitted is not “chosen” by the speaker, but is determined by
other supplementary variables. For example, let us say that “car” and “auto” are equal-
ly strengthened by a variable, and the person tacts “auto”. We would explain this not
by appealing to a “choice” but instead inquiring about other current variables that
added supplementary strength. Perhaps the person had just read something about
“autos” or perhaps he just overheard a conversation in which someone said that an
event was “automatic.”
100
CHAPTER 8: STUDY GUIDE
Chapter 8
The Verbal Operant as a Unit of Analysis
101
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
5. Explain how a child may come to “ask for the word for an item.”
6. Skinner makes that point that mand (or tact) training may
facilitate the acquisition of tacts (or mands). How so?
102
CHAPTER 8: STUDY GUIDE
103
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
13. Why might explain transfers to other media when both are
present in strength?
14. Read the example of a person ordering from a list. This “probe”
concept will be dealt with later, and was addressed in Science and
Human Behavior. Try and explain this.
16. Note: In the next, brief section, Skinner addresses the indepen-
dence of speaking and listening repertoires. Suffice it to say that
they are independent repertoires. In a sense, learning to react to
what you hear does not automatically lead to being able to talk
about it. Skinner again points to the aphasic. I would point to
graduate classes in which students hear about the principles of
behavior, but are not able to talk about them in the same way un-
less they have a chance to “practice” talking about them.
17. In the next section, Skinner discusses how we talk in two or more
languages. Thus, if we read something in English, how is it that
we can explain it in Spanish or French?
104
CHAPTER 8: STUDY GUIDE
23. Skinner points out that verbal behavior often occurs under more
“trivial” aversive conditions. These include finishing a sentence
even though the effect on the listener has already been achieved.
Why is this?
105
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
24. Check out the footnote on page 202. What is happening here?
25. The final section deals with the TAT and the “Verbal Summator.”
The point can actually relate to any projective test. Explain this
in your own words.
26. Note: In the final point of this section, Skinner suggests that
when reinforcement is contingent on speed of response, formal
controls may be weakened.
28.
Skinner goes on to talk about the effects of operant
contingencies on the dynamic properties of behavior. What are
these?
29. Read over the sections on extinction and forgetting. What is the
difference? How could you tell which has occurred?
106
CHAPTER 8: STUDY GUIDE
31.
Skinner presents his interpretation of classic memory
experiments of Ebbinghaus and others. They used “nonsense”
syllables to minimize confounds by everyday usage. What classes
of verbal behavior are involved? What independent variables
are studied?
33. Note: Skinner makes the point that behavior tends to move to-
ward the minimum which satisfies the contingencies. Why might
this be?
34.
Note: The last paragraphs of this section deal with
deterioration of form of verbal behavior. First, Skinner makes
a distinction between changes in the speaker, due to changes
in the requirements of particular listeners, and changes in
speaker behavior due to changes in the reinforcing practices
of the community as a whole. The latter can be seen in the
changes in our verbal behavior related to computers. We have
many new terms and meanings, and these are reinforced by the
community. Others point to the change in what is “acceptable”
language, such as double negatives, the use of “ain’t”, etc.
107
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
38. On the top of page 216, check out his statement regarding “anger”.
What reinforcer maintains such behavior?
108
CHAPTER 8: STUDY GUIDE
40. Note: Check out the last paragraph on 217. Skinner talks about
the possible evocative variables of the tact “I am angry.” They
compose the elements of emotion. He refers to his view of how we
come to tact private events.
42. In the next section on “When does verbal behavior cease”, discuss
some of the variables that bring verbal behavior to an end.
109
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
110
CHAPTER 8: STUDY GUIDE
111
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
No. The functional classes are independent. In fact, there are some mands (and tacts)
that do not exist as tacts (or mands).
5. Explain how a child may come to “ask for the word for an item.”
The behavior of asking for a word (What is that?) is a mand reinforced by the response
of the listener (It is a pencil!), which then serves as a stimulus that when echoed (Give
me the pencil), produces the item. This process is a rather complex chain that ulti-
mately produces the desired item. The response is then an independent mand (Give
me the pencil) which may occur in the future.
6. Skinner makes that point that mand (or tact) training may
facilitate the acquisition of tacts (or mands). How so?
In mand training, the item is often present. Thus, the sight of the object may acquire
evocative control over the tact repertoire. Likewise, in tact training the presence of
the item is a likely situation for delivery of the item. Thus, a mand would be rein-
forced.
112
CHAPTER 8: STUDY GUIDE
Yes. However, if one has a textual repertoire, he/she likely has an echoic; but, an
echoic repertoire does not assume a textual one. In essence, Skinner suggests that the
occurrence of any verbal operant requires us to identify the history responsible for it.
It is the loss of particular functional relations. For example, one may be able to mand
for an item, but not tact it. Thus, the controlling relation between the nonverbal
stimulus and tact is absent.
Transcription may allow us to move from one medium to another. For example, we
write down what someone says, and then react to the textual stimuli. Or, we may
write down our own private speaking, and react to the text. Likewise, we may read
out loud to ourselves to see “how it sounds.”
No. Some media are more likely than others to predominate. For example, writing
may be more common in the deaf community, but not in communities that are illit-
erate.
113
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
13. Why might explain transfers to other media when both are
present in strength?
One medium may occur because the other is weakened by current contingencies. For
example, one might hold up a sign for “Silence” in a quiet library because the vocal
form would be punished.
14. Read the example of a person ordering from a list. This “probe”
concept will be dealt with later, and was addressed in Science and
Human Behavior. Try and explain this.
In ordering food, the customer may read the menu. This behavior will provide stimuli
that strengthen “latent” mands. Latent mands are merely mands that are in strength
(a person may be predisposed to order Mexican food, but the mand is not yet strong
enough to be emitted). The supplementary echoic stimuli provided by the textual be-
havior may permit the mand to be emitted. The same process may be involved when
one person reads the menu to another.
16. Note: In the next, brief section, Skinner addresses the indepen-
dence of speaking and listening repertoires. Suffice it to say that
they are independent repertoires. In a sense, learning to react to
what you hear does not automatically lead to being able to talk
about it. Skinner again points to the aphasic. I would point to
graduate classes in which students hear about the principles of
behavior, but are not able to talk about them in the same way un-
less they have a chance to “practice” talking about them.
114
CHAPTER 8: STUDY GUIDE
17. In the next section, Skinner discusses how we talk in two or more
languages. Thus, if we read something in English, how is it that
we can explain it in Spanish or French?
The first explanation has to do with translation. When we learn a second language,
we are learning intraverbal connections between the first and second. For example,
we may learn that “perro” is the same as “dog.” Then, when we talk about dogs in
Spanish, we may translate dog into perro and then emit “perro.” (e.g., “What is Span-
ish for dog? Hmm. Its perro.”)
After learning a second language (e.g., Spanish), variables that strengthen particu-
lar English operants will also strengthen Spanish operants. For example, if variables
strengthen emission of “dog”, then “perro” will also be strengthened because “dog”
and “perro” are members of the same functional class. Which is emitted will depend
on other ancillary audience or contextual variables that strengthen one of them.
Let us say that variables have strengthened the tact “That dog is a basset hound!”
These same variables may also strengthen the Spanish form of the tact, but the speaker
may emit the Spanish form to himself to see if it “sounds right.” If so, it may be emit-
ted aloud; if not, it may be corrected and then emitted. This appeals to an “editing”
process which will be described later in the book.
Variables may strengthen verbal behavior in English, and this behavior may produce
private events that may be described in Spanish. Imagine you are at a party, and an
English speaking friend asks you “Did you like last night’s movie?” This may strength-
115
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
en Verbal behavior (e.g., “Let’s see. Last night’s movie. Oh yeah. It was about a cast-
away on an island. He talked to a volleyball”) that leads to “seeing” parts of the movie.
You may then describe those sights in Spanish to other friends in the group.
Attention of the listener. This variable will strengthen any behavior that produces it.
EOs have made the speaking of other aversive, and these EOs then evoke a variety of
behavior that is maintained by the absence of others’ speaking. In this case, silence is
reinforcing.
On the other hand, silence may be aversive. EOs may have made silence aversive, so
behavior that avoid/terminate it will be strengthened. This behavior may be varied,
as the only requirement that it avoid/terminate silence. Skinner gives many examples
of this phenomenon, including “stalling.”
23. Skinner points out that verbal behavior often occurs under more
“trivial” aversive conditions. These include finishing a sentence
even though the effect on the listener has already been achieved.
Why is this?
Because in the past, finishing a sentence was important and avoided aversive conse-
quences. This may be shown in using “stock” phrases that finish sentences. It may also
include “trivial” behavior such as small talk or idle chatter.
116
CHAPTER 8: STUDY GUIDE
24. Check out the footnote on page 202. What is happening here?
There was an aversive condition that strengthened the mother’s verbal repertoire.
The sight of the cream pitcher strengthened her remark (it occurred to avoid any
talk about he man’s nose), but talking about his nose was strong, and that part of her
repertoire was injected into the comment. Skinner talks about this as variables that
strengthen a variety of verbal behavior give “free rein” to other variables that may
select out certain forms.
25. The final section deals with the TAT and the “Verbal Summator.”
The point can actually relate to any projective test. Explain this
in your own words.
Mild aversive variables strengthen large portions of the verbal repertoire (e.g., “Ex-
plain what is going on in the picture.” Or “What do you hear?”). Other variables that
are strong in the person’s life will then select out particular forms which will give the
evaluator some “insight” into what is going on in the person’s life.
26. Note: In the final point of this section, Skinner suggests that
when reinforcement is contingent on speed of response, formal
controls may be weakened.
28.
Skinner goes on to talk about the effects of operant
contingencies on the dynamic properties of behavior. What are
these?
A) There is no clear and consistent relation between the energy level of the speaker’s
behavior and the effect on the listener, as well as the ultimate reinforcer supplied
by the listener. The extent of the reinforcement depends more on the energy level
of the listener
B) Verbal behavior is normally very fast, and exceeds the speed of nonverbal behav-
117
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
ior. Thus, we can emit large segments of verbal behavior with reinforcement only
when completed, and this fact can serve to increase effectiveness. (We can make
many important statements in a short period of time, resulting in effective com-
munication for both speaker and listener)
C) The schedule of reinforcement is intermittent. Thus, the behavior may sometimes
be weak in terms of latency and IRTs, but it may also be resistant to extinction
D) Some forms of verbal behavior (e.g., writing) may necessarily involve delayed re-
inforcements. Thus, such behavior may be inherently weak, and fraught with
long latencies, IRTs, etc.
E) Although some verbal behavior may be weak because of delayed reinforcements,
the net effect may be great because of the plethora of listeners (e.g., readers of
novels and poems)
29. Read over the sections on extinction and forgetting. What is the
difference? How could you tell which has occurred?
Extinction involves the weakening of behavior because the behavior has been emitted,
but not reinforced. In forgetting, behavior weakens as a function of passage of time.
To test for extinction vs. forgetting, changes the circumstances. If the behavior was
extinguished, it should re-appear, as extinction effects are tied to the conditions un-
der which it occurred.
A) Infrequent reinforcement
B) Interference from similar names or other similar people having the same name
31.
Skinner presents his interpretation of classic memory
experiments of Ebbinghaus and others. They used “nonsense”
syllables to minimize confounds by everyday usage. What classes
of verbal behavior are involved? What independent variables
are studied?
118
CHAPTER 8: STUDY GUIDE
varying periods of time, or they may examine how many trials are needed to relearn
the pairs after a varying periods of time. They may also have the participant learn sim-
ilar paired associates to study interference with recall of the originals.
33. Note: Skinner makes the point that behavior tends to move to-
ward the minimum which satisfies the contingencies. Why might
this be?
Effort may have aversive properties (see research on FR schedules: animals tend to
terminate schedules the higher the ratio size); thus, there is a net reinforcement effect
that balances effort and payoff. Or, in other cases, more complex forms of behavior
may be punished.
34.
Note: The last paragraphs of this section deal with
deterioration of form of verbal behavior. First, Skinner makes
a distinction between changes in the speaker, due to changes
in the requirements of particular listeners, and changes in
speaker behavior due to changes in the reinforcing practices
of the community as a whole. The latter can be seen in the
changes in our verbal behavior related to computers. We have
many new terms and meanings, and these are reinforced by the
community. Others point to the change in what is “acceptable”
language, such as double negatives, the use of “ain’t”, etc.
E.g.: Children learn to speak correctly and precisely at a young age. As they grow
older, their Verbal behavior may become sloppy, with lack of agreement between sub-
ject and predicate, and inappropriate use of plural nouns, etc. This deterioration may
continue until the contingencies are no longer satisfied, or the community begins to
punish certain deteriorated forms.
119
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
NOTE: Skinner talks about deprivation and satiation, but only as evocative stim-
uli for mands and impure tacts. Control by specific deprivation and satiation is de-
stroyed by generalized reinforcements, as in the tact, echoic, and textual.
A) Responses of the smooth muscles and glands that are respondently conditioned.
These include changes in blood pressure, heart rate, respiration, and gastric mo-
tility
B) Changes in the facial expressions and whole body reactions that can be a result of
operant or respondent conditioning
C) Changes in predispositions to act (the angry person will be more likely to aggress
and attack, the happy person will be more likely to be nice and say positive things)
NOTE: In Science and Human Behavior, Skinner talks about emotion and includes
two components: the respondents and the predisposition to emit operant behavior
of various sorts, depending on controlling variables.
38. On the top of page 216, check out his statement regarding “anger”.
What reinforcer maintains such behavior?
Damage to others.
39. In the remaining part of the Emotion section, Skinner talks about
some of the dynamic properties of verbal behavior that are said
120
CHAPTER 8: STUDY GUIDE
A) To achieve a given effect on the listener – we may say aversive things in an emo-
tional way to make the listener cry
B) Extreme state of strength – we may be joyful as an effect of a high rate of rein-
forcement, or we may act in an extremely angry way when a very aversive stimulus
has been contacted
40. Note: Check out the last paragraph on 217. Skinner talks about
the possible evocative variables of the tact “I am angry.” They
compose the elements of emotion. He refers to his view of how we
come to tact private events.
He points out that damaged repertoires often involve problems with some controlling
variables, but not others. For example, a person may be able to emit an intraverbal,
but not the same word as a mand.
Skinner suggests that the most vulnerable classes are those that rely on generalized
conditioned reinforcement, with the classes having minimal repertoires being the
most robust (echoic and textual). Thus, tacts and intraverbals would be the most
susceptible to disruption. Finally, the extent of conditioning may be important.
42. In the next section on “When does verbal behavior cease”, discuss
some of the variables that bring verbal behavior to an end.
A) When the underlying EO is weakened. For example, mands for food will be
weakened by the receipt of food
B) Skinner suggests that verbal behavior maintained by generalized reinforcement
will not be quickly affected by the process in a)
C) In verbal behavior maintained by generalized reinforcement, the termination of
verbal behavior can involve the audience becoming SΔs or SPs. Thus, after a sin-
gle effective tact of “That is a bird”, the audience may withold reinforcement for
additional tacts. This condition may be brought about by the mere emission of
121
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
the response
D) There may also be SΔ or SP effects presented by the listener. (e.g., “OK, I see your
point”)
NOTE: the contingencies may not apply to synonyms, so these may be emitted
quickly (e.g., “Good”, “OK”, “Excellent”)
E) Verbal behavior in general may be weakened by the “audience character”. For
example, after speaking with someone, it may become clear that the person is deaf
or speaks another language. This is similar to b) in that the audience becomes an
SΔ for further verbal behavior
F) Verbal behavior that is maintained by the effects on the speaker will cease when
that desired effect is achieved. For example, a person may repeat to him/herself
“Don’t be afraid of the dark” until the aversiveness of being in the dark decreases.
E.g.: Child says “Candy” and mom gives it to her. Mom’s behavior has been condi-
tioned by the verbal community to precisely teach and maintain the mand.
Non-e.g.: Mom has candy in her hand, and child approaches her, hits her hand, the
candy flies into the air, and kid grabs it! Mom was involved in the candy procure-
ment, but she was not conditioned by the community to do so.
122
CHAPTER 9: STUDY GUIDE
Chapter 9
Multiple Causation
123
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
3. Check out the end of the first paragraph on page 229. What is
being described therein?
124
CHAPTER 9: STUDY GUIDE
8. Skinner talks about “euphemisms.” What are these, and why might
these be emitted?
125
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
13. On page 242, Skinner talks about “borrowing.” What is this about?
14. In the final paragraph of this section, he talks about ways that
“verbal art” may be emitted in forms that reflect multiple sourc-
es, but avoids punishment. Take the example of “home, home on the
range…” and explain this.
17. Skinner also talks about fragmentary stimuli that can evoke a
full-blown response. Give an example.
126
CHAPTER 9: STUDY GUIDE
18. Note: Skinner points out that some rhyming pairs show echoic
fragmentary sources of strength (wear and tear, Frick and Frack,
high and dry).
20. Skip to the last paragraph on page 251. What is this effect that
Skinner is discussing?
127
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
128
CHAPTER 9: STUDY GUIDE
129
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
NOTE: A given variable may strengthen a whole class of responses that have com-
mon properties or “themes”. Skinner compare this phenomenon to an “attitude.”
Thus, a negative attitude toward taxes means that there is an entire class of verbal
behavior that has one common theme: negative statements about taxes!
Let us say that the question posed to you is “How is your thesis coming?” This ques-
tion may strengthen a host of tacts of various sorts, both positive and negative. In
the presence of your thesis advisor, your verbal behavior may be a simple recounting
of the procedure and tentative results. However, in the presence of one of your class-
mates, your verbal behavior may be a spirited recounting of how much of a “pain in
the butt” the process has been.
3. Check out the end of the first paragraph on page 229. What is
being described therein?
Impure tact.
A) Records of speaking and writing in which the controlling variables can only be
inferred. They are not known.
B) A direct manipulation of variables of which speaking and writing are a function,
and as a result, the resulting behavior may be predicted or even controlled.
130
CHAPTER 9: STUDY GUIDE
A) Two or more audiences that have the same effect: increasing the number of peo-
ple in the audience of an orator will increase the strength of oration.
B) Two audiences that evoke different repertoires: an audience that strengthens
German and another that strengthens English. The available repertoire that is
common to both will be quite limited.
C) Two audiences, one that strengthens a repertoire and another that weakens: peers
who strengthen obscene talk and parents who weaken it. The repertoire may be
suppressed, or emitted at low strength or energy levels.
D) When one positive audience strengthens two forms of response, and one negative
audience weakens just one form, the form that is functionally related only to the
positive audience will be emitted: Bi-lingual parents may speak in the language
that the child does not understand.
E) A single response may have different effects on different audiences: “That is really
great food” may be a compliment to one audience (the cook) and a sarcastic slam
to another (his close friends).
NOTE: In Skinner’s examples of satire, irony, and other kinds of devices, there are
two effects of a passage for two distinct audiences. The satirical effect is achieved
when a person is a member of both audiences. In a sense, he/she is affected in both
ways, and can tact that he/she is so affected: He can “see the irony” in the remark.
A child may tact “That is candy” in the presence of candy, but also under conditions
of candy deprivation.
131
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
8. Skinner talks about “euphemisms.” What are these, and why might
these be emitted?
NOTE: Skinner suggests that which of various responses that are emitted reflect a
kind of “algebraic summation” of the effects of reinforcement and punishment histo-
ries of each of the alternatives.
“Those candy eggs will lay you out flat” The choice of the term “lay you out flat”
received supplementary strength from “eggs” (as in lay eggs). This may have been
emitted instead of “Those candy eggs will make you sick” because of the presence of
“eggs”. This may not have occurred if the statement was “The fried chicken will …”.
In the cartoon Rocky and Bullwinkle, one of the villains is “Snidely Whiplash.” This
is an easy name to remember, first as a proper tact (because of reinforcement history
for emitting the name in his presence on the screen) and as a metaphorical extension
evoked by his evil, hurtful, and devilish ways.
Pun: an amusing use of a word or phrase which has several meanings or which sounds
like another word
E.g.: What is black and white and red (=read) all over? A newspaper!
132
CHAPTER 9: STUDY GUIDE
These are puns when the listener can recognize the sources of multiple control. In the
first example, the listener can tact the intraverbal strength of “black and white and
red” and the intraverbal relation of “read” with newspaper. That the two words sound
the same is amusing.
In the second example, the listener will be amused if he/she recognizes that the word
“sole” has two sources of strength. In the first, it is a tact derived from the fact that
there is only one objectionable fish (sole = only). In the second, it is an intraverbal
strengthened by the previous mention of “fish”. (sole = kind of fish). These two
sources combine to evoke the word “sole” and the listener will be amused if he/she
recognizes this fact.
13. On page 242, Skinner talks about “borrowing.” What is this about?
Borrowing is when a writer uses a similar line to that used by another author. He
makes the point that in many cases, the writer does not realize this supplementary
source of strength. He also points out that such borrowing is most often seen when
someone memorizes a passage, which is then evoked upon contact with one of the
initial words. Thus, it is most often an intraverbal source.
14. In the final paragraph of this section, he talks about ways that
“verbal art” may be emitted in forms that reflect multiple sourc-
es, but avoids punishment. Take the example of “home, home on the
range…” and explain this.
A homesick failure may find that whining and complaining are strong (perhaps some
form of mand with tact properties). But, direct complaining may be punished, so
a “complaining” song with intraverbal strength throughout will avoid punishment.
(refer back to a previous section in which classes of behavior may be strong, and the
punished forms are not emitted: recall the algebraic summation concept).
133
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
Two responses are related “thematically” if they are strengthened by the same vari-
able, but have no point to point correspondence.
These refer to echoic and textual stimuli that strengthen behavior with point to point
correspondence.
E.g.: Let us say that you are trying to remember someone’s name: Linda. You over
hear a conversation in which people are talking about the movie star Linda Hunt.
This talk, especially the stimulus “Linda” strengthen “Linda” in your repertoire. This
is essentially an echoic stimulus.
17. Skinner also talks about fragmentary stimuli that can evoke a
full-blown response. Give an example.
Let us say that you are trying to remember someone’s name: Linda. You over hear
a conversation in which people are talking about the aviator Charles Lindberg. This
talk, especially the fragmentary stimulus “Lin” strengthen “Linda” in your repertoire.
This is essentially an fragmentary echoic stimulus.
18. Note: Skinner points out that some rhyming pairs show echoic
fragmentary sources of strength (wear and tear, Frick and Frack,
high and dry).
134
CHAPTER 9: STUDY GUIDE
20. Skip to the last paragraph on page 251. What is this effect that
Skinner is discussing?
When someone begins to speak, he/she is contacting a host of echoic, textual, and
intraverbal stimuli that may strengthen other behavior. Over time, if the intraverbal
stimuli become strong, the verbal behavior may become a “flight of ideas” uncon-
strained by the original evocative variables (topic of the Verbal behavior), and instead
evoked by “what was just said.” Or if the stimuli contribute formal strength, the re-
sulting Verbal behavior may become sing-song or gibberish.
On the other hand, multiple sources of strength, if appropriate to the topic at hand,
may guarantee a cohesiveness to the verbal behavior that is effective for all concerned.
135
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
136
CHAPTER 10: STUDY GUIDE
Chapter 10
Supplementary Stimulation
137
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
1. Check out the last paragraph on page 253 that continues onto
page 254. He talks about strengthening the behavior of drawing
a cat, and actually strengthening the mand for “pencil” so that
picture can be drawn. Examine this procedure carefully. What
phenomenon is being manipulated to produce the mand “pencil?”
6. Describe how the verbal summator works and how one might use
it.
138
CHAPTER 10: STUDY GUIDE
12. Note: In this entire section, Skinner talks of a speaker and lis-
tener who both possess a strong repertoire of verbal behavior
regarding a particular topic. The speaker then provides supple-
mentary stimulation that evokes a similar set of verbal behavior
in the listener. Thus, both are talking about the same thing, and
for the same reasons. Give an example of this phenomenon.
13.
In the following paragraphs, Skinner takes on another
phenomenon, that of eisegesis. What is this? Give an example.
139
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
15. In the bottom of page 271 and top of page 272, Skinner refers to a
continuum. What is this?
16.
Skinner also points out that the speaker’s supplemental
stimulation may evoke behavior that is not strong in the listener’s
repertoire. Give an example.
140
CHAPTER 10: STUDY GUIDE
20.
Contrast Allport’s view of autobiographies and that of
Skinner’s.
141
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
28. Now, check out the paragraph on 280 where he says “One of the
principle effects…” Explain his point. Do you agree with it?
29. In the next section, Skinner talks about ways to use “Tricks of
strength.” What are some of these?
30. Note: The next section is on “Style” which goes a little far
afield. Suffice it to say that a writer/speaker’s style can involve
techniques of rhyming, alliteration, and rhythms. As in the
current chapter, multiple sources of control are used. For
example, in rhymes, there are both thematic and formal sources
of strength.
142
CHAPTER 10: STUDY GUIDE
31. Go to the bottom of 284 where Skinner talks of the two elements
of literary work. What are they? What is the third that Skinner
adds?
32. On the top of 286, Skinner makes a point about wit and humor.
What is this? Give an example.
33. Check out the top of 288. There is a story about an Englishwoman
and Napoleon III. What is this all about? Why was it considered
witty? Make sure you translate the French and Latin phrases in
this description.
34. Note: The final section is on verbal puzzles and games. He suggests
that they involve arrangements of probes and prompts. Check out
the discussion of crossword puzzles, which involve intraverbal
stimuli in the clues, and formal prompts of intersecting words.
143
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
144
CHAPTER 10: STUDY GUIDE
145
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
1. Check out the last paragraph on page 253 that continues onto
page 254. He talks about strengthening the behavior of drawing
a cat, and actually strengthening the mand for “pencil” so that
picture can be drawn. Examine this procedure carefully. What
phenomenon is being manipulated to produce the mand “pencil?”
NOTE: Make note of Skinner’s point about practical control exerted by supplemen-
tal stimuli. This goal is ubiquitous in his writings.
Prompts: the operator (prompter) can identify the response to be emitted in the
speaker
Probes: the operator (prober) can not identify the response to be emitted in the
speaker
146
CHAPTER 10: STUDY GUIDE
6. Describe how the verbal summator works and how one might use
it.
It is a recording of vague speech sounds at low intensity that provide echoic probes
to a person. The subject will usually “hear something” that, when analyazed, could
reveal the operation of other variables that are currently strengthening certain verbal
behavior. For example, if a person has just lost a loved one, the person may hear words
such as “death”, “dying”, “loss”, “tragedy” etc. Essentially, the stimuli are vague enough
to not strengthen particular responses, but will provide supplementary strength to
already existing verbal operants. Note that when a given operant is emitted, it may
itself provide additional echoic or intraverbal strengthening to additional operants.
Estes invented a way to present patterns of letters either very briefly or out of focus.
The subject was to then emit textual responses to the material.
If a subject knows what the test is about, he/she may change their responses through
an editing process described later.
147
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
12. Note: in this entire section, Skinner talks of a speaker and lis-
tener who both possess a strong repertoire of verbal behavior
regarding a particular topic. The speaker then provides supple-
mentary stimulation that evokes a similar set of verbal behavior
in the listener. Thus, both are talking about the same thing, and
for the same reasons. Give an example of this phenomenon.
In graduate school, the students may be predisposed to talk in certain ways about
behavior. In ours, the preferred way is a behavioral interpretation of events. Thus,
when we talk to each other, our repertoires match very closely, and we only need a
speaker to evoke these “latent” verbal episodes. This is probably seen most often in
second or third year students, who have extensive training in the field. However, in
first year students or undergraduates, this phenomenon is not present, as the speaker
is engaging in “instruction.”
13.
In the following paragraphs, Skinner takes on another
phenomenon, that of eisegesis. What is this? Give an example.
Eisegesis is when supplementary stimulation evokes some verbal behavior in the lis-
tener, which has the same topography as the verbal behavior of the speaker, but has
different controlling variables (i.e., a different meaning).
E.g.. Speaker: “That kid is something again!” (evoked by seeing a child do well on a
math task)
Listener: “That kid is something again!” (evoked by the speaker’s remark, and
after struggling with the same child for an hour before he finally completed the math
task)
148
CHAPTER 10: STUDY GUIDE
15. In the bottom of page 271 and top of page 272, Skinner refers to a
continuum. What is this?
On one end of the continuum, we do not enjoy hearing someone say what is at full
strength in our own repertoire. On the other end, we do not enjoy hearing or find no
utility in hearing someone talk about something that has no parallel in our repertoire.
For example, speaker who makes obvious points, or a speaker who talks about com-
pletely unfamiliar topics, has not much of an interested audience. Note that there is
a parallel with nonverbal behavior. (Being shown how to do something we already
know how to do, or something that we have no interest in doing)
16.
Skinner also points out that the speaker’s supplemental
stimulation may evoke behavior that is not strong in the listener’s
repertoire. Give an example.
When a speaker may say something that evokes a solution by the listener to a difficult
problem.
Readers choose books that supplement verbal behavior that is already at strength.
Thus, our enjoyment comes when the book evokes Verbal behavior that we, as read-
ers, already tend to say.
149
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
20.
Contrast Allport’s view of autobiographies and that of
Skinner’s.
Allport: Autobiographies are interesting because they satisfy the reader’s own self
love.
Skinner: Most people have strong behavior of talking about themselves (which is
reinforcing), and autobiographies supply the effect supplementary stimulation that
evokes such behavior (also applies to stories written in the first person).
In novels, a writer will have lively dialog among the characters such that the reader
will almost “join in.” In a sense, the text provides supplementary stimuli that evoke
behavior that is already in strength in the reader’s repertoire. In this situation, the
reader is said to “identify” with one of the characters. This is not apparent in descrip-
tions of nonverbal events.
150
CHAPTER 10: STUDY GUIDE
NOTE: To end this section, Skinner suggests that speakers may use the above tech-
niques to strengthen useful or important behavior in the listener. It may involve ulti-
mate support for the speaker’s position.
We understand a remark when we can make the same remark for the same reasons.
For example, a student understands “positive reinforcement” if he can not only recite
the definition in echoic form, but also tact the definition under the control of the
essential elements (in a situation where there is a consequence that increases the rate
of the behavior).
In the end, we come to make responses that occur not for textual reasons, but for oth-
er reasons that the author intended. This may occur after several repetitions.
28. Now, check out the paragraph on 280 where he says “One of the
principle effects…” Explain his point. Do you agree with it?
151
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
29. In the next section, Skinner talks about ways to use “Tricks of
strength.” What are some of these?
A) Weakening the listener's response before it is even emitted: the speaker may ad-
dress anticipated objections.
B) Allowing and leading the listener to make a key response on his/her own: using
insinuation, innuendo, or implication such as “you know who just ate the entire
loaf of bread!” (when the now obvious response is the owner’s dog).
C) Writer sets down a passage that is so weak that the reader is likely to emit a stron-
ger form, or so contrary to the evidence that the reader is led to deny or correct:
“This is a real pretty picture” to the aftermath of a child’s tantrum.
D) Sarcasm: Writer/speaker makes a statement that is obviously untrue: “The kid
had a real great day” after a day with many tantrums.
30. Note: The next section is on “Style” which goes a little far
afield. Suffice it to say that a writer/speaker’s style can involve
techniques of rhyming, alliteration, and rhythms. As in the
current chapter, multiple sources of control are used. For
example, in rhymes, there are both thematic and formal sources
of strength.
31. Go to the bottom of 284 where Skinner talks of the two elements
of literary work. What are they? What is the third that Skinner
adds?
152
CHAPTER 10: STUDY GUIDE
32. On the top of 286, Skinner makes a point about wit and humor.
What is this? Give an example.
Skinner opines that stimuli that supplement and evoke a weak response is often fun-
ny. The remark is often “far fetched” and for that reason may be amusing. For exam-
ple, the story about the dentist who is fixing his car and takes the sparkplug by the
pliers, and then remarks “Now this is going to hurt a little.” This remark is weak (we
don’t talk to our cars very often), and is clearly a metaphor evoked by the fact that he
was pulling something out.
NOTE: wit is also experienced if we recognize the multiple sources of strength. See
the example of the “fertile” attorney. This is funny when the reader recognizes that
“fertile” has two sources of strength: his hometown, and the fact that he has fathered
18 kids!
33. Check out the top of 288. There is a story about an Englishwoman
and Napoleon III. What is this all about? Why was it considered
witty? Make sure you translate the French and Latin phrases in
this description.
Napoleon III was exiled in England, and received assistance from an Englishwoman
during that time. Upon return to the throne, he virtually ignored the woman. When
there was a meeting between the two, he asked “Restez-vous longtemps a Paris” which
means “Are you staying long in Paris?”. After her reply, she remarked “Et vous, sire?”
which means “And you, sir?” This closing remark was evoked by the same variables
as if she had said “You will not be on the throne long.” In a sense, it was an aggressive
remark, but witty because it was unpunishable. Skinner suggests that the witty verbal
community exacts a quid pro quo for such remarks. They will be tolerated if amusing.
34. Note: The final section is on verbal puzzles and games. He suggests
that they involve arrangements of probes and prompts. Check out
the discussion of crossword puzzles, which involve intraverbal
stimuli in the clues, and formal prompts of intersecting words.
153
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
154
CHAPTER 11: STUDY GUIDE
Chapter 11
New Combinations of Fragmentary Responses
155
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
2. Note: Skinner makes the point that sometimes the recombined re-
sponse has equal input from each contributing response, or some-
times one of the two dominates.
156
CHAPTER 11: STUDY GUIDE
11. Note: Read over the section on “Witty and Stylistic Effects.” All
of these involve distortions and multiple sources of strength.
The Ogden Nash poetry is amusing. Note the use of distorted
geographical names for conventional expressions on page 305.
157
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
158
CHAPTER 11: STUDY GUIDE
159
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
2. Note: Skinner makes the point that sometimes the recombined re-
sponse has equal input from each contributing response, or some-
times one of the two dominates.
Phrase: you’re probably true from you’re probably right, and it’s probably true
Writing the word “tall” in elongated letters; mathematicians writings that resemble
figures; musicians writings that resemble notes.
Sorry darling being blended into Sarling, slightly fatter into slatter.
NOTE: these blends are not like merely eliminating some of the word elements, as in
nonse for nonsense. This is not a blending, just omissions.
160
CHAPTER 11: STUDY GUIDE
A) One variable may strongly evoke two responses (two or more tacts for the same
item: rone for rock and stone)
B) When a controlling property is vague (ineffective), two responses that have dif-
ferent meanings may be evoked (Teablespoonful evoked when the size of the
measure is unclear between tablespoonful and teaspoonful)
C) Different properties of the same stimulus may evoke two responses (twinfants)
NOTE: the multiple sources may show the interaction of tacts, tacts and intraver-
bals, and intraverbals.
161
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
When a blend occurs, the accompanying fragments still appear. “A crown of thorns”
becomes “A thown of crorns”. Note the reference to W. A. Spooner, who was famous
for these kinds of blends. My mom has a few of these. One relates to her alma mater,
which is Lake Erie College for Women; she sometimes refers to it as Lake College for
Eerie Women.
11. Note: Read over the section on “Witty and Stylistic Effects.” All
of these involve distortions and multiple sources of strength.
The Ogden Nash poetry is amusing. Note the use of distorted
geographical names for conventional expressions on page 305.
162
CHAPTER 12: STUDY GUIDE
Chapter 12
The Autoclitic
163
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
164
CHAPTER 12: STUDY GUIDE
6. Note: The discussion on the bottom of page 316 and top of page 317
underscores an important point about autoclitics. That is, they
modify the effect of the Verbal behavior on the listener’s behav-
ior. See the top of 319 for the same point.
165
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
13. Note: Check the last paragraph on page 326. Here Skinner
explains why assertions are autoclitics in that they modify the
effects of accompanying statements. For example, take the tact
“raining.” Without any other statements, the listener may accept
this as true and act accordingly. However, let us say that prior
to the tact, another person said “I don’t think it is raining.” Thus,
the listener may be predisposed to act as if it were not raining.
Therefore, the speaker needs a way to make the tact “raining” more
effective, and may add the assertion “It is” to the tact “raining”
to produce “It is raining.”
166
CHAPTER 12: STUDY GUIDE
14. Now read the bottom of page 327. Skinner talks about the usage of
the term “wolf.” His explanation is a kind of tour de force of the
entire book. In essence, if a speaker says “wolf”, we do not know
what he “means” unless we know the variables responsible for its
emission. How can autoclitics change this?
17. Note: Skinner also makes a couple final points that bear repeat-
ing. First, quantifying autoclitics may become functional units
(a piece of candy) and are therefore not autoclitic. Second, au-
toclitics may be added to another speaker’s responses. For exam-
ple, you may say “That is very true” when someone says “Dogs are so
much fun!”
167
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
168
CHAPTER 12: STUDY GUIDE
169
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
Autoclitic: Verbal behavior that depends on, or is controlled by, other verbal behav-
ior.
Descriptive autoclitic: Tacts of one’s own verbal behavior that are effective on the
same listener at the same time. It is important to note that the tacts are emitted spe-
cifically to modify the listener’s behavior.
E.g.: “I will tell you about my day in a few minutes.” Or “I just told you what the
answer is!”
E.g.: After explaining about an upcoming event, a speaker may say “I read about this
in the paper”. Thus, he is identifying the controlling stimuli as textual in nature.
170
CHAPTER 12: STUDY GUIDE
6. Note: The discussion on the bottom of page 316 and top of page 317
underscores an important point about autoclitics. That is, they
modify the effect of the Verbal behavior on the listener’s behav-
ior. See the top of 319 for the same point.
They qualify or cancel the effect of the accompanying response, but imply that the
response is strong. “I disagree that the food is a positive reinforcer.”
171
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
Negation: It is not raining. This clearly changes the valence of the tact “It is raining.”
In contrast to “No”, which cancels the effects of a statement (“Vote for Smith? No!”),
“Yes” supports the effects (“Vote for Smith? Yes!”).
13. Note: Check the last paragraph on page 326. Here Skinner
explains why assertions are autoclitics in that they modify the
effects of accompanying statements. For example, take the tact
“raining.” Without any other statements, the listener may accept
this as true and act accordingly. However, let us say that prior
to the tact, another person said “I don’t think it is raining.” Thus,
the listener may be predisposed to act as if it were not raining.
Therefore, the speaker needs a way to make the tact “raining” more
effective, and may add the assertion “It is” to the tact “raining”
to produce “It is raining.”
172
CHAPTER 12: STUDY GUIDE
14. Now read the bottom of page 327. Skinner talks about the usage of
the term “wolf.” His explanation is a kind of tour de force of the
entire book. In essence, if a speaker says “wolf”, we do not know
what he “means” unless we know the variables responsible for its
emission. How can autoclitics change this?
“It is kind of like a wolf.” Or “It is very close to a wolf.” Or “It really does not look
too much like a wolf.”
“All dogs have four legs.” The quantifier “All” modifies the listener’s reaction to “dogs
have four legs.”
The tact “dog” has different effects on a listener when accompanied by “the” or “a”.
They indicate the evocative relation between the tact and stimulus.
17. Skinner also makes a couple final points that bear repeating. First,
quantifying autoclitics may become functional units (a piece of
candy) and are therefore not autoclitic. Second, autoclitics may
be added to another speaker’s responses. For example, you may say
“That is very true” when someone says “Dogs are so much fun!”
173
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
174
CHAPTER 13: STUDY GUIDE
Chapter 13
Grammar and Syntax as Autoclitic Processes
175
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
176
CHAPTER 13: STUDY GUIDE
6. What is an autoclitic “frame?”
177
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
178
CHAPTER 13: STUDY GUIDE
179
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
A) Agreement between subject and predicate (“The girl types” or the “The girls
type”)
B) Possession (“The girl’s doll” or “The girls’ doll”)
180
CHAPTER 13: STUDY GUIDE
An autoclitic frame is a functional unit that can be combined with other responses.
For example, a person may learn to respond to pairs of objects with “the ___ and the
____ which can emitted in the presence of untrained objects. It is example of multi-
ple causation, as the relational features of the situation strengthen the frame, and the
specific stimuli evoke the specific responses inserted into it.
The manipulative autoclitic involves the use of, for example, conjunctions such as
“and” or “but.” But tells the listener to exclude something, and and tells the listener
to add something.
181
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
182
CHAPTER 14: STUDY GUIDE
Chapter 14
Composition and Its Effects
183
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
4. Note: The rest of the material in this section discusses the vari-
ety of sentences that may be emitted to a given state of affairs.
Note the range of autoclitics that are used, and how they may be
used to have slightly different effects on the listener.
184
CHAPTER 14: STUDY GUIDE
6. The passages that Skinner uses are long and complex. Let’s take a
simpler passage to analyze, such as the following:
The boys are really, really good baseball players, have a swimmingly good
time at it, and hit a home run in the classroom!
185
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
186
CHAPTER 14: STUDY GUIDE
16. How can tacts be acquired (as described on the bottom of 359-top
360)? What kind of history may be required for such an effect?
187
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
22. Note: On page 365 (top), Skinner returns to the point about some
statements are merely simple SDs or SΔs, while others show a
function-altering effect.
23. What are some conditions that affect the changes in a listener’s
behavior?
188
CHAPTER 14: STUDY GUIDE
189
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
“Man” and “hungry” are in strength, and the assertive autoclitic “is” and then the
designative autoclitic “the” is also added to refer a particular man. The complete sen-
tence is therefore more effective on the listener’s behavior than “Man hungry.”
No. Some sentences are standard responses to particular situations. Others are
“frames” that are combined with certain other phrases. For example, we may learn
“The ____ is _____” and fill in with nouns and adjectives to form “The bird is grey.”
We may use complete sentences when the verbal community punishes fragments.
4. Note: The rest of the material in this section discusses the vari-
ety of sentences that may be emitted to a given state of affairs.
Note the range of autoclitics that are used, and how they may be
used to have slightly different effects on the listener.
190
CHAPTER 14: STUDY GUIDE
6. The passages that Skinner uses are long and complex. Let’s take a
simpler passage to analyze, such as the following:
The boys are really, really good baseball players, have a swimmingly good time at it,
and hit a home run in the classroom!
They often occur in literature, and do so because of the relaxed contingencies in the
literary world.
The periodic table presents the elements and their atomic weights. They are organized
in such a ways that the reader can readily see the their respective weights, what the
adjacent elements are, and subgroups of elements that are related.
191
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
A speaker may say “On the other hand” which will prepare the reader/listener for a
statement that is contrary to the first statement.
It can be emitted until other primary verbal behavior is available, and it is used be-
cause it modulates the effects of the primary verbal behavior.
The statement “When I say ‘three’, you will receive a shock” changes the evocative
status of ‘three’ such that it will evoke responses similar to those evoked by the shock
and the word “shock.”
NOTE: Hank Schlinger and I coined the term “function-altering” for this effect.
Thus, the statement (or rule) changes the function of the word “three” in that it now
evokes reactions similar to those produced by the shock. In this way, the rule mimics
respondent conditioning by creating a CS.
192
CHAPTER 14: STUDY GUIDE
NOTE: The passage is a function-altering set of stimuli. It alters the function of the
word “eternity.”
NOTE: function-altering effects are not always seen immediately. They are observed
only when the stimulus in question actually occurs. Thus, in the above example, the
FA effect will be observed upon the queen’s arrival, which could be much later.
An autoclitic frame is an “If…..,say….” Or “If….., then….” Statement that can allow sub-
stitutions depending on other variables in a situation. In previous writings, Skinner
discussed situations in which variables combine and evoke various words, and these
combine to form more lengthy statements. This is probably one of those examples.
16. How can tacts be acquired (as described on the bottom of 359-top
360)? What kind of history may be required for such an effect?
We may hear something named, such as “apple” (e.g., “This is an apple”). Then, we
may be able to tact the object when asked “What is that?” Note that the statement
involves an autoclitic of assertion (is).
193
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
The statement “When the light is on, the door will be unlocked” has the following ef-
fect on the listener: behavior evoked by the door being unlocked will then be evoked
by the light coming on. A similar effect is produced by “The telephone is out of
order.” Behavior evoked by “out of order” will now be evoked by the sight of the tele-
phone. Evoked behavior may be verbal or nonverbal.
21.
Skinner points out that “facts” are not transmitted in
instruction. What, then, is made “common?”
What happens is that behavior that the speaker possesses is now in the listener’s rep-
ertoire.
22. Note: On page 365 (top), Skinner returns to the point about some
statements are merely simple SDs or SΔs, while others show a
function-altering effect.
194
CHAPTER 14: STUDY GUIDE
23. What are some conditions that affect the changes in a listener’s
behavior?
195
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
196
CHAPTER 15: STUDY GUIDE
Chapter 15
Self Editing
197
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
3. On pages 372 and 373, check out the discussion about the pigeon
and the use of punishment. What is going on here?
4. Look at the next section, and explain why verbal behavior might
be punished.
198
CHAPTER 15: STUDY GUIDE
199
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
200
CHAPTER 15: STUDY GUIDE
201
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
Written: the writer may read over the passage, and make changes, before delivering
it to a reader.
Speaker: a) Not emitting it b) Not repeating the response upon request c) The re-
sponse may be “taken back” after saying it (e.g., “I meant to say…” or “I did not mean
that the way it sounded…”) d) The speaker may emit the response subvocally to test
it out on him/herself. E) “On the fly” - autoclitics and composition may be done as
the responses are emitted, even without subvocal review.
NOTE: Skinner uses his two-factor theory of punishment in this section. Make sure
that you can articulate this view as it applies to this section. Thus, rejecting a response
reduces conditioned aversive stimuli that are generated by the punished response, or
some incipient form of it.
3. On pages 372 and 373, check out the discussion about the pigeon
and the use of punishment. What is going on here?
Pigeons are conditioned to peck “Red” upon seeing red, “Yellow” upon seeing yellow,
etc. Performance can be improved by using punishment for wrong responses. In this
case, he used timeout. The effect on the pigeon’s behavior is to increase observing
behavior, which in this case is looking at the color before selecting the word. The bird
may also hesitate before selecting the word.
Note that the schedule of reinforcement is a VR 4, and it sounds like the food was not
contingent on color. If this means both corrects and incorrects were reinforced, it is
no surprise that the birds had trouble. If it means that only corrects were reinforced,
then the extra punishment contingency is interesting.
4. Look at the next section, and explain why verbal behavior might
be punished.
A) May be aversive to the listener, as noise! May generate covert Verbal behavior as
a result.
B) Certain features of the Verbal behavior may be aversive (e.g., volume, tone).
202
CHAPTER 15: STUDY GUIDE
C) Deficient stimulus control: any one of a variety of variables may lead to the
“wrong word” being used. Also, lying, hyperbole, etc. may be punished.
D) Poor audience control: Verbal behavior may be aversive to a particular audience.
(e.g., bawdy jokes at a church gathering)
E) It may be a “tit for tat” situation if the listener’s behavior is being punished. For
example, the speaker may be trying to hurt the feelings of the listener, and the
listener may provide punishing consequences back to the speaker.
NOTE: check out the top of 375. He refers to a situation in which the speaker’s
behavior of nagging or criticizing the listener is not aversive to the speaker. In-
stead, Skinner suggests it is a special case of positive reinforcement appropriate to
…aggression. This may not make sense, but consider that for Skinner, aggression
is behavior reinforced by signs of damage to others. Thus, we may be aggressive
because it has been followed by a “hurt victim.” The person may cry, cower, or
beg for mercy. You will not see this operant class discussed in the literature, but
remember this point. You will have cases in which the client’s aggression may
indeed belong to this class.
F) Automatic self-punishing: If the verbal behavior is correlated with other aver-
sive stimuli, and its emission will therefore produce conditioned aversive stimuli,
which can be terminated by stopping the verbal behavior, or emitting an autoclit-
ic (“I take it back!”)
G) When the verbal behavior “gives something away”: as in a joke
H) When the Verbal behavior exposes the speaker to other aversive stimuli: When
confessing a crime, or when the verbal behavior reveals objectionable variables
at work (the person who sees and talks about sexual connotations in everyday
events).
203
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
You are in a situation which your boss treats you unfairly. Behavior such as “You
jerk!” is strengthened, but its incipient form is too aversive. Thus, you say something
like “If you were my brother, I might just throw some water on you and call you a
jerk!”
Autoclitic activity occurs not to physically restrain behavior, but it occurs because it
reduces the conditioned aversive stimuli generated by incipient Verbal behavior that
has been punished. “The behavior is strong because it displaces punishable responses.”
The disadvantage is that the speaker may experience phenomena such as mutism,
emotional confusion, and low rate verbal behavior in many situations.
204
CHAPTER 15: STUDY GUIDE
poorly conditioned, etc, and this reinforcement may be responsible for its ultimate
emission at the vocal level. This process may be operating in “testing for correctness”
with a 2nd language. The speaker may reject errors, but correct responses may be re-
inforced by the speaker/listener, which will contribute to learning the 2nd language.
205
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
206
CHAPTER 16: STUDY GUIDE
Chapter 16
Conditions of Self Editing
207
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
2. Note: The next section discusses what occurs when self editing
has not occurred, but the speaker is confronted with undeniable
evidence that verbal behavior was emitted. In brief, we might
attribute it to another person or to a supernatural power. There
are many literary references as examples.
3. Go to page 393, and check out the first complete paragraph. This
involves what is happening in MPD. Explain his reaction to Miss
Beauchamp’s MPD problem, and why she does not “know” about
Sally.
4. The next section deals with the various kinds of audiences that
will affect the strength of editing repertoires. What are some
of these?
208
CHAPTER 16: STUDY GUIDE
209
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
NOTE: There are several intended and unintended puns on 386. Notice the
presence of supplementary stimuli in terms such as “who followed Hume”, “goril-
la warfare”, and “martial relationships.”
2. Note: The next section discusses what occurs when self editing
has not occurred, but the speaker is confronted with undeniable
evidence that verbal behavior was emitted. In brief, we might
attribute it to another person or to a supernatural power. There
are many literary references as examples.
3. Go to page 393, and check out the first complete paragraph. This
involves what is happening in MPD. Explain his reaction to Miss
Beauchamp’s MPD problem, and why she does not “know” about
Sally.
In essence, MPD individuals do not “notice” or tact their behavior when emitted un-
der other “personalities” because to do so would evoke conditioned aversive stimuli.
Thus, such tacts are effectively punished.
210
CHAPTER 16: STUDY GUIDE
4. The next section deals with the various kinds of audiences that
will affect the strength of editing repertoires. What are some
of these?
A) Speaker as his/her own audience - editing is minimized, but not completely ab-
sent. Punishments by the verbal community will have generated conditioned
aversive stimuli by some Verbal behavior.
B) Talking with a “confidant” (could be a therapist)
C) In the presence of children
D) Lenient audience - king’s jester was free from many forms of punishment
E) Literary audience
1. Some audiences provide for wild extensions, metonymy, etc.
2. Others, such as the scientific audience, provide for only small degrees of
metaphorical extensions
3. Symbols are used, because non-symbols would be punished.
4. Authors can create “characters” and write about things with respect to
that character that would normally produce punishment (in current us-
age, a character could seek revenge for bombings of New York city)
5. Note on page 398 that readers can enjoy reading a book, and engage in
verbal behavior in the act, without experiencing punishment.
A) Incipient behavior that has been punished will generate conditioned aversive
stimuli and correlated emotional effects such as anxiety. The speaker can escape/
avoid these stimuli by “doing something else” or “doing nothing.” The suppressed
behavior will, however, be emitted under other conditions or related forms that
do not produce punishment.
B) A person with suppressed verbal behavior may act to find a suitable audience for
the behavior or another form of behavior that will not be punished.
C) Punished behavior that remains strong may generate chronic anxiety. To decrease
the anxiety, a person’s escape/avoidance behavior may be aversive to him/her-
self or others. Moreover, the chronic anxiety may produce other psychosomatic
symptoms. Therapy may reduce either or both problems.
211
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
212
CHAPTER 17: STUDY GUIDE
Chapter 17
Self Strengthening of Verbal Behavior
213
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
4. Note the use of “abulia” on page 408. This term refers to the
complete absence of behavior in any strength. Definitely an
old-style term.
214
CHAPTER 17: STUDY GUIDE
215
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
216
CHAPTER 17: STUDY GUIDE
217
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
218
CHAPTER 17: STUDY GUIDE
Mechanical production of
verbal behavior
Random striking of keys on Not technically verbal
typewriter; using piglatin behavior, but may result in
verbal behavior.
Change EO
Diets, deprivations Ascetic lifestyles
Generate aversive condi- Accept a royalty for a book,
tions which will create a condi-
tion in which writing will
be negatively reinforced.
Incubation Get away from situation When confronted with a
and interfering variables problem, go for a drive, go
outside and relax.
219
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
4. Note the use of “abulia” on page 408. This term refers to the
complete absence of behavior in any strength. Definitely an
old-style term.
Many writers and speakers study and investigate new topics. In so doing, new tacts
and intraverbals are created.
A) If often asked “What movie did you see last night?”, the person may engage in
“observing responses” before the question is asked, and while you are watching
the movie. Indeed, specific tacts may be memorized during the movie that can
later be emitted (e.g., “Hmmm. This movie is really good. The title is….Oh yes,
‘Invasion of Cleveland’”).
B) Moreover, there may also be “bridging behavior” that involves creating a stimulus
that survives the time delay. For example, you might pick up a flyer at the movie
that will evoke textual behavior in the future when asked “What movie did you
see?”
C) Self-echoics: repeating a phone # after you hear it, while you are getting to the
phone.
220
CHAPTER 17: STUDY GUIDE
When doing a match-to-sample, you may have the kid point and look at the sample
before matching it. Or, in naming numbers, you may have the kid match the number
and name it simultaneously.
221
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
222
CHAPTER 19: STUDY GUIDE
Chapter 19
Thinking
223
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
224
CHAPTER 19: STUDY GUIDE
11. Note: Skinner makes the case that thinking is best thought
of as verbal behavior that has automatically reinforcing
effects on the “thinker.” He talks about verbal fantasy that
is comparable to the writer’s works, the musician’s music, etc.
They all automatically reinforce the producer.
12. The next section deals with the practical advantages of talking
to yourself. Skinner talks of self mands. Why might these occur?
Why might they be effective?
225
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
16. Translate the statement “He should have gone to Chicago.” This
kind of interpretation is classic Skinner. He provides a similar
analysis in Science and Human Behavior.
18. Note the passage of the youngster playing piano, and issuing self
mands and tacts. Skinner is talking about his daughter, Julie.
226
CHAPTER 19: STUDY GUIDE
19. In the bottom of 445, Skinner makes the case for verbal think-
ing having the same properties as verbal behavior with separate
speaker and listener. In the former, who “mediates the reinforc-
er?”
227
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
228
CHAPTER 19: STUDY GUIDE
229
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
Verbal behavior extends the sensory power of the listener, and the action possibilities
of the speaker. The wise-but-weak can combine with uninformed-but-strong to
produce activities that are superior to those of each.
There would be embarrassing gaps in our account of behavior. For example, problem
solving, editing, composition, etc. would be absent from the science of behavior.
No. There are many examples of such behavior that does not involve detectable
movements of musculature.
To try and explain thinking without mental processes. Skinner finds no advantage to
this. First, there is often no detectable musculature; second, there is no important
230
CHAPTER 19: STUDY GUIDE
difference in the audible and subaudible. The latter is in no way the “cause” of the for-
mer. They do not compose a “sequence” of covert → overt verbal behavior. Instead,
both a functions of other external variables.
They are not activities or behavior of the person. Thus, we do not “generalize” from
one situation to another. Instead, they merely describe controlling relations; or, they
describe the effects of contingencies.
Thinking is behavior, and can not be distinguished from other actions of the person.
Verbal behavior that generates its own reinforcement. It does, in a sense, automatical-
ly affect the speaker. Could be overt or covert.
11. Note: Skinner makes the case that thinking is best thought
of as verbal behavior that has automatically reinforcing
effects on the “thinker.” He talks about verbal fantasy that
is comparable to the writer’s works, the musician’s music, etc.
They all automatically reinforce the producer.
231
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
12. The next section deals with the practical advantages of talking
to yourself. Skinner talks of self mands. Why might these occur?
Why might they be effective?
They occur because the speaker (speaker and listener are the same person) has in oth-
er situations issued mands and received reinforcement for doing so. Skinner talks
about “induction.” This is another term for generalization. Such mands could be
effective because the listener has been reinforced for responding to mands issued by
others. Again, there is a process of induction. Moreover, the listener may learn to
respond to self mands because they may involve more effective action.
There are situations in which tacting an object to yourself may strengthen many effec-
tive behaviors with respect to the object. For example, if you emit the tact “positive
reinforcer” to some object, then this will strengthen many important behaviors such
as using the object in a program or contingency contract.
A speaker may probe and prompt his own behavior to tease out other Verbal behavior
that is currently weak. He may do this because of he has been reinforced for such
behavior by other listeners, but may also engage in such behavior because it produces
practical consequences (scientists may, as a result of problem solving, solve many im-
portant problems such as finding a cure for cancer or HIV).
Self rationalization may reduce aversive stimuli produced by emitting behavior that is
normally punished by others. (I hit someone because he was “out of control.”)
16. Translate the statement “He should have gone to Chicago.” This
kind of interpretation is classic Skinner. He provides a similar
analysis in Science and Human Behavior.
If he went to Chicago, there were reinforcers that would have occurred for doing so.
232
CHAPTER 19: STUDY GUIDE
A resolution is a self mand that is emitted when there are strong current contingen-
cies to do so, and that sets up future contingencies to control behavior in the future.
For example, a heavy smoker may, upon contracting pneumonia, resolve to “never
smoke again” and perhaps told all of his friends. Thus, by telling his friends, he sets up
future punishers for smoking in the presence of his friends. (Skinner states that “The
effect is greater if the resolution is publicly announced…”)
18. Note the passage of the youngster playing piano, and issuing self
mands and tacts. Skinner is talking about his daughter, Julie.
19. In the bottom of 445, Skinner makes the case for verbal think-
ing having the same properties as verbal behavior with separate
speaker and listener. In the former, who “mediates the reinforc-
er?”
The speaker and listener are one, so the person is involved in both producing the Ver-
bal behavior and providing the reinforcer.
In many cases, thinking involves verbal behavior. We may emit tacts, mands, etc. that
reinforce our own behavior in doing so. We may “think of 100” by emitting the re-
sponse “100”, we may think of dogs by emitting the response “dog”, etc. But not all of
thinking is verbal. We may think about opening a door by emitting covert behavior
that often precedes the behavior.
233
VERBAL BEHAVIOR: EXTENDED EDITION
234