The Relative Effects of Metalinguistic Explanation and Direct Written Corrective Feedback On Children's Grammatical Accuracy in New Writing

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 25

The relative effects of metalinguistic

explanation and direct written corrective


feedback on children’s grammatical
accuracy in new writing
Mary Gorman and Rod Ellis
University of Auckland | Curtin University

There has been little research investigating the effects of form-focused


instruction (FFI) on the second language acquisition of children. This article
reports a quasi-experimental study of integrated form-focused instruction
for 33 children aged 9–12 years. They completed four dictogloss tasks
designed to elicit the use of the Present Perfect Tense and received instruc-
tion consisting of either explicit metalinguistic explanation (group 1), direct
written correction (group 2) or no form-focused instruction (the compari-
son group) between performing the tasks. Accuracy in the production of the
target structure across the four tasks was variable and showed no improve-
ment from the first to the last. Nor were there any statistically significant
differences in accuracy among the three groups. The results support some
earlier studies of young children (e.g. Fazio, 2001) that have failed to show
that FFI benefits young children. This may be because children fail to make
use of their metalinguistic knowledge of grammatical features when under-
taking meaning-focused writing tasks.

Keywords: children, form-focused instruction, metalinguistic information,


written corrective feedback, acquisition

1. Introduction

Little research has investigated the effects of form-focused instruction (FFI) on


young second language learners’ acquisition of grammar (Bouffard & Sarkar,
2008). For example, only 21% of the 77 studies of form-focused instruction
included in Norris and Ortega’s (2000) often-cited meta-analysis involved chil-
dren. One reason for the lack of studies of young learners is the belief that they

https://doi.org/10.1075/ltyl.00005.gor
Language Teaching for Young Learners 1:1 (2019), pp. 57–81. issn 2589-2053 | e‑issn 2589-207x
© John Benjamins Publishing Company
58 Mary Gorman and Rod Ellis

are better suited to implicit than explicit learning and thus are not likely to benefit
from form-focused instruction.
In the study reported in this article, we investigated the effects of two ways
of incorporating form-focused instruction (FFI) into meaning-based activities in
what Spada and Lightbown (2008) have called integrated FFI. We asked children
to complete four dictogloss tasks and after the first and the second tasks we pro-
vided them with either metalinguistic information about the target structure (Pre-
sent Perfect Tense) or direct written corrective feedback on their errors in the use
of this tense. The aim was to investigate whether FFI led to increased accuracy in
a specific target structure in new writing, thus challenging Truscott’s (1996) dis-
missal of such a role and, secondly, which type of FFI (metalinguistic explanation
or direct corrective feedback) was the more effective.

1.1 Metalinguistic explanation

There are many differences between children and adults that can influence their
ability to benefit from metalinguistic descriptions of grammatical structures.
Adults have advanced to the stage of formal operations which enables them to
process abstract information and to engage in deductive reasoning (Inhelder &
Piaget, 1958). Older language learners have also developed literacy skills and have
had more experience of engaging with written tasks that require a focus on form.
In contrast, younger children are believed to be better at implicit and inciden-
tal language learning. They are more likely to treat language as a tool for making
meaning than as an object that can be studied and analyzed. They have less devel-
oped literacy skills. The fact that older learners can benefit more easily from met-
alinguistic information may be one reason why they have been shown to learn an
L2 more rapidly than younger learners, at least in the initial stages (Muñoz, 2006).
However, children aged 9–12 (the age of the children in our study) can
develop explicit knowledge of grammatical features and there is some evidence
that instruction can help them to do so. Some studies involving L1-speaking
children have reported positive results for explicit grammar instruction. Bryant,
Devine, Ledward & Nunes (1977), for example, found that the direct teaching
of the apostrophe helped children to develop a clear awareness of its use and
improved their accuracy. Myhill, Jones, Watson and Lines (2013) reported a ben-
eficial effect of explicit instruction directed at enabling young students (aged
10–11) to make informed choices about the grammatical forms they needed to
encode specific meanings in writing. Drawing on the same study, Myhill, Jones
and Wilson (2016) emphasized the importance of teacher guidance and dia-
logic talk in developing metalinguistic knowledge that students could deploy
effectively in writing. In contrast, Andrews et al.’s (2004) review of 10 studies of
Metalinguistic explanation and direct written corrective feedback 59

grammar teaching involving traditional sentence analysis found that it did not
lead to greater accuracy in students’ writing. The differing results have fueled the
long-running debate about the role of grammar in writing instruction for young
learners but as Myhill et al. (2013) pointed out this debate has been conducted
without taking account of how the teaching of grammar is linked to the practice
of writing.
Much of the FFI research involving L2 child learners was carried out within
the context of immersion classrooms. It was aimed at helping the learners to
acquire those grammatical features that were resistant to implicit learning despite
extensive exposure to the L2 – for example, grammatical gender in French
(Harley, 1998), L2 English question formation (White, Spada, Lightbown & Ranta,
1991), and English possessive determiners (White & Ranta, 2002). These studies
all demonstrated that the instruction led to gains in the learners’ ability to use
these grammatical structures more accurately. However, as Hanan (2015) noted,
the children in these studies were not reliant on just explicit instruction as they
also experienced large amounts of input in their immersion classrooms, which
may have helped to reinforce the effects of the instruction.
Hanan’s own study investigated the effects of explicit instruction on the acqui-
sition of two German grammatical structures on 9–11 year old learners in a foreign
language classroom. In accordance with the requirements of Processing Instruc-
tion (VanPatten, 1996), the instruction consisted of explicit information about
the target features and the processing problems these involved followed by struc-
tured input activities. Hanan reported that the children were able to verbalize their
knowledge of the target structures and to make use of this knowledge in both
untimed written tasks and time-pressured oral tasks. Other Processing Instruc-
tion studies (e.g. Laval, 2013) have also shown that Processing Instruction is
effective with children. However, none of these studies included a metalinguistic-
information-only condition so it is not possible to tell to what extent the explicit
information contributed to learning. Studies with adults (e.g. VanPatten & Oiken-
non, 1996), however, have shown that structured input activities alone can be suf-
ficient for learning to take place.
In our study we investigated whether there was any benefit in providing
children just with metalinguistic information in between performing meaning-
focused production tasks.

1.2 Written corrective feedback (WCF)

Arguments against correcting children’s errors are the same as those for avoiding
metalinguistic explanation. Children’s ability to analyse and think about language
in abstract ways only emerges towards the end of primary school (Pinter, 2006),
60 Mary Gorman and Rod Ellis

which suggests that WCF might not be appropriate for younger learners. In
instructional contexts where the primary focus is on meaning (e.g. immersion
classrooms), young learners do not consistently pay attention to oral corrective
feedback (Lyster & Ranta, 1997). Young learners may be unable or disinclined to
engage in the analysis needed to develop an understanding of the errors they have
committed and the corrections they have been given. Even if they do undertake
this analysis, they may fail to use the knowledge they have gained in meaning-
focused writing activities. For this reason WCF may have no effect on the errors
they make in new pieces of writing.
On the other hand, there are grounds for claiming that WCF may be worth-
while for young learners. There is evidence that children do in fact have the ability
to attend to form. In oral corrective feedback studies, where the feedback has been
made explicit (e.g. Ammar & Spada, 2006; Lyster, 2004), a positive effect on young
children’s acquisition of grammar has been reported. Given that WCF is invariably
explicit in nature, it is possible that children will attend to it, at least sometimes,
and be able to utilize what they have learned from it in their subsequent writing.
There is now substantial evidence that WCF can have a positive effect on the
second language acquisition of grammatical features as measured in new writ-
ing. However, the studies reporting this positive effect have almost all investigated
adult learners mainly at university-level. There are virtually no WCF studies of L2
elementary school learners. The most comprehensive account of written correc-
tive feedback to date (Bitchener & Ferris, 2012) makes no reference to research
involving young children. A more recent review of the research (Bitchener &
Storch, 2016) also contains not a single reference to a study of WCF with young
children. A search of articles on WCF in the Journal of Second Language Writ-
ing likewise reveals an almost complete absence of studies investigating young
children. Meta-analytic treatments of research on feedback on writing (e.g. Biber,
Nekrasova & Horn, 2011; Liu & Brown, 2015) similarly point to very few studies
of elementary level L2 students although there are a number of L1 studies at this
level. Somewhat surprisingly even popular books on teaching languages to young
children (e.g. Cameron, 2001; Pinter, 2006) offer no advice about written correc-
tive feedback.
Teachers are often ambivalent about the need to correct young learners’ writ-
ing. Mollestam and Hu’s (2016) interviews with teachers of 9–11 year old Swedish
children studying English revealed that although they all thought it important to
correct their students’ written errors, they did not believe it was always neces-
sary to do so – for example, if the aim was to have the students write a long text.
Mollestam and Hu’s teachers also felt that it was undesirable to correct if a child
was struggling to write and they were generally concerned that WCF could dis-
courage the children.
Metalinguistic explanation and direct written corrective feedback 61

The study of the most direct relevance to the present research is Fazio (2001).
She investigated the effects of three types of written feedback on the journal writ-
ing of 115 Grade 5 students in French language schools in Montreal. The stu-
dents were a mixture of native-speaking French children and minority-language
learners. The feedback was of three kinds – (1) form-focused, (2) content-based
and (3) a combination of form and content-based. The form-focused corrections
were directed at the grammatical spelling errors related to French subject/verb
and noun/adjective agreement. The correction was of the direct kind (i.e. errors
were underlined and the correct form provided immediately above where an error
occurred). The feedback was provided weekly over a three and a half month
period. Thus the feedback was extensive and continuous. The students were also
interviewed to discover their attitudes to the feedback and the extent to which
they attended to it. The results showed no gains in accuracy over time nor any dif-
ference in the effects of the three types of feedback. The minority-language learn-
ers who received the form-focused corrections actually produced more errors as
time passed. The interviews indicated that the students did not consistently attend
to the feedback.
In her discussion of the results, Fazio pointed to a number of reasons for the
ineffectiveness of the feedback. She noted that grammatical spelling is a challeng-
ing aspect of French and takes time to master, that the error rates were relatively
low at the beginning of the study making significant improvement unlikely, that
the general instruction the learners received was very form-oriented which may
have neutralized any effect for feedback, and that most of the students failed to
attend to the corrections even though they had been encouraged to do so. We
would also add that the nature of the writing task – journal writing where the chil-
dren were encouraged to write freely on any topic they chose – was likely to have
accentuated the tendency to focus on meaning at the expense of form. It is pos-
sible, however, that a writing task that encourages greater attention to linguistic
form might encourage children to engage with the WCF.
It was with this in mind that we elected to use dictogloss in our study. Dic-
togloss was introduced by Waynryb (1990) as a way of teaching specific grammat-
ical structures in a contextualized way. It has been mostly used with older learners
but Cameron (2001) suggested that it can be adapted for younger learners. Shak
and Gardner (2008) reported that young learners in Brunei, a similar learning
context to the context of our study, rated dictogloss tasks positively as cognitively
stimulating but not overly demanding. In a dictogloss task the teacher first reads
out a short text two or three times while the students take notes. The students then
use their notes to reconstruct the text either orally in pairs or in writing individ-
ually. Although some studies with adult learners (e.g. Swain & Lapkin, 2001) have
shown that learners may fail to reproduce the target structure, other studies (e.g.
62 Mary Gorman and Rod Ellis

Shintani, Ellis & Suzuki, 2013) have found that dictogloss is successful in elicit-
ing attempted use of the intended structure, especially if this is central to a text’s
meaning as was the case in the present study.
We elected to provide direct correction. Indirect WCF, which forces learners
to draw on their existing L2 knowledge to correct their performance ‘mistakes’, is
often considered preferable because it involves deeper processing. However, indi-
rect WCF is only effective if learners possess the knowledge to make a correction.
It cannot help learners to correct their ‘errors’ (i.e. the gaps in their competence).
In this respect direct WCF that provides learners with the correct forms is needed.
Given that the children in our study were expected to have limited knowledge
of the target structure (Present Perfect Tense), we decided on direct WCF. Direct
WCF has been found effective in a number of studies of adolescent and adult
learners (e.g. Shintani & Ellis, 2013; Van Beuningen, de Jong & Kuiken, 2012).

1.3 FFI and explicit knowledge

One of Truscott’s (1996) critiques of early WCF studies was that they failed to
investigate the effect of feedback on new pieces of writing. Subsequent WCF stud-
ies have remedied this by ensuring that learners are asked to complete a new piece
of writing following correction on earlier writing. However, another of Truscott’s
concerns, namely that WCF only contributes to explicit knowledge, is less easily
addressed. While there is wide agreement that WCF can result in greater accu-
racy, doubts continue as to whether it helps to develop learners’ implicit knowl-
edge (Williams, 2012). Evidence indicating that WCF only affects learners’ explicit
knowledge can be found in Shintani and Ellis (2013). They reported that the posi-
tive effects of WCF wore off over time and suggested this was because it had only
impacted on their learners’ explicit knowledge, which, unlike implicit knowledge,
atrophies easily. What is true for WCF is perhaps even more likely to be the case
for metalinguistic information. Thus, the types of FFI we investigated can only be
expected to contribute to learner’s explicit knowledge.
This raises further doubts about the value of providing metalinguistic infor-
mation and WCF for children. As we pointed out above, children are less well
equipped cognitively to develop metalinguistic understanding of grammar. It may
be, therefore, that while these two types of FFI have a positive contribution to
make to adult learners’ accuracy in writing by expanding their explicit knowledge
of grammar they will be ineffective for children.
Metalinguistic explanation and direct written corrective feedback 63

2. Method

The following research questions were devised to investigate the comparative


effects of two types of FFI – metalinguistic explanation and direct written correc-
tive feedback – on children’s use of the Present Perfect Simple Tense:
1. Does FFI lead to more accurate use of Present Perfect Simple in new pieces of
writing than writing practice alone?
2. Is there any difference in the effect that metalinguistic explanation and direct
written corrective feedback have on the accurate use of Present Perfect Simple
in new pieces of writing?
While we remain open to the possibility that both metalinguistic explanation and
direct WCF may lead to greater accuracy in the children’s use of Present Perfect
Simple Tense, we consider that this is unlikely for the reasons given in the preced-
ing sections. Of the two types of FFI, we considered that direct WCF was the more
likely to result in gains in accuracy as it provides learners with the correct input.

2.1 Participants

The study was carried out at British Council, Kuala Lumpur, with three intact
general English classes that studied once a week for two hours. One of the
researchers was also the class teacher. Forty-two students agreed to participate in
the study but only 33 children completed all the treatments and tests. The chil-
dren were 9–12 years old, with an average age of 10.5. There were 15 boys and
18 girls. Of the 33 subjects, 22 spoke Mandarin or Cantonese at home, 8 spoke
(Malaysian) English and 3 spoke Malay. The majority of the children were multi-
lingual, typically speaking Mandarin and/or Cantonese, Malay, English, and an
additional Chinese dialect. A key feature of these children was that they were
highly functional in using more than one language. They were considered low
intermediate in English, typically displaying intermediate-level receptive skills
but having less control over accuracy in productive tasks. They were fluent
writers who could generally produce Roman script rapidly without conscious
effort. The children were accustomed to formal grammar instruction, which was
favoured in the school environment.

2.2 Design

The three classes were randomly assigned to a treatment: the Metalinguistic


Explanation (ME) group (n = 11), the direct WCF group (n = 11), and the Com-
parison Group (n = 11). The children participated in four sessions as outlined in
64 Mary Gorman and Rod Ellis

Table 1. In Week 1, all participants completed Writing Task 1 (pre-test). In Week 2,


the ME group received eight minutes of teacher-led whole class instruction while
the WCF group had eight minutes to study their direct WCF. Neither treatment
group revised their writing. The Comparison Group had eight minutes to proof-
read their own writing and make their own corrections. The participants then
completed Writing Task 2. The pattern of treatment from Week 2 was repeated for
each group in Week 3 when the students completed Writing Task 3. In Week 5, all
groups completed Writing Task 4 (as a delayed post-test) and a background ques-
tionnaire.

Table 1. Design of the study


ME WCF Comparison
Week 1 Writing Task 1 (pre-test) Writing Task 1 (pre- Writing Task 1 (pre-
8 minutes test) test)
8 minutes 8 minutes
Week 2 Meta-linguistic explanation (based Studying WCF on Self-correction of
on Writing Task 1) Writing Task 1 Writing Task 1
8 minutes 8 minutes 8 minutes
Writing Task 2 Writing Task 2 Writing Task 2
8 minutes 8 minutes 8 minutes
Week 3 Meta-linguistic explanation Studying WCF on Self-correction of
(based on Writing Task 2) Writing Task 2 Writing Task 2
8 minutes 8 minutes 8 minutes
Writing Task 3 Writing Task 3 Writing Task 3
8 minutes 8 minutes 8 minutes
Week 5 Writing Task 4 Writing Task 4 Writing Task 4
8 minutes 8 minutes 8 minutes
Student questionnaire Student questionnaire Student questionnaire

2.3 Target structure

A continuing limitation of WCF studies is the narrow range of grammatical


structures that has been investigated. Study after study has focused on English
definite and/or indefinite articles. A few studies have investigated prepositions
and a few others past simple tense. Shintani, Ellis and Suzuki’s (2013) study
addressed hypothetical conditionals. We chose Present Perfect Simple on the
grounds that this tense causes difficulty for L2 learners of English with regard
Metalinguistic explanation and direct written corrective feedback 65

to its form, meaning and use but also because no study involving the two FFI
options we elected to investigate has investigated this structure to date.1
The Present Perfect Simple was chosen as the grammatical target in part
because it had not been previously taught to the students2 and in part because it is
semantically complex and often causes learners difficulty. Two functional uses of
Present Perfect Simple (McCawley (1971), as cited in Michaelis, 1994, p. 113) were
the target of this study:
Universal/continuative
e.g. He has studied at British Council since 2012.
A state continues throughout a time period whose upper boundary is the time of
speaking.
Existential/experiential
e.g. He has had five teachers so far.
One or more events of a given type occur within a time span that includes the present.
The perfect aspect is problematic for most learners in the Malaysian context. Yong
(2001) and Chang (2001) cite the use of Present Perfect for past events as prob-
lematic for both Malay and Chinese speakers because these languages rely heav-
ily on adverbials instead of morphology to perform the same function. Malaysian
speakers of English typically over use the temporal adverb already without supply-
ing the requisite tense marking. A study of Malaysian high school students’ errors
in written compositions (Talif & Edwin, 1989) identified the Present Perfect as the
most problematic structure.

2.4 Materials

2.4.1 Background questionnaire


A background questionnaire elicited information about the learners’ gender, par-
ent tongue, school, and languages spoken.

2.4.2 Dictogloss tasks (Appendix A)


The materials consisted of four texts describing four children and their life expe-
riences. Each text was approximately 80 words long. The functional distinctions
described by McCawley (1971) (as cited in Michaelis, 1994, p. 113) of universal/
continuative and existential/ experiential events were used to develop the texts.

1. Rummel (2014) investigated the effect of WCF on Present Perfect but this study reported
only the combined results for this tense and Past Simple.
2. The teacher of the class confirmed that the Present Perfect had not been taught in the previ-
ous six months. It is possible, however, that it had been taught at some time prior to this.
66 Mary Gorman and Rod Ellis

The writing tasks also contained temporal adverbials that commonly occur with
the Present Perfect, for example so far, already, since, in order to provide contexts
in which Present Perfect was obligatory and in which Past Simple could not be
substituted. There were five instances of Present Perfect Simple in each text, with
two regular and three irregular verbs to allow for the possibility that WCF might
have a differential effect on regular and irregular forms. Each text also contained
one obligatory use of past simple in order to check for over-generalisation of
Present Perfect Simple after the children received ME or WCF. Reconstruction
tasks rather than free writing tasks were chosen as Malaysian learners were likely
to avoid the Present Perfect Simple in producing their own texts.

2.4.3 Procedure
The procedure outlined here was repeated for each of the four writing tasks. The
teacher/researcher read the text twice while the children watched visual and word
prompts on the interactive whiteboard. Auxiliary have was not contracted. The
children were asked to recall and reconstruct the text individually using a printed
copy of the presentation as a memory aid. This was a modification of the standard
dictogloss procedure described by Wajnryb (1990) that was intended to bring the
texts alive for the children and to reduce the cognitive load involved in processing
them. Each presentation took four minutes and the children had eight minutes to
write the text individually. ME and WCF were provided in Week 2 and Week 3
immediately before the students completed a new writing task.
The ME consisted of an eight-minute mini-lesson in Week 2 (focusing on the
text written in Week 1) and in Week 3 (focusing on the text written in Week 2). It
took place immediately before the students began a new writing task. The teacher
used a gapped version of the interactive whiteboard presentation and elicited the
correct verb forms (Present Perfect Simple and Past Simple) from the whole class.
The teacher asked questions to clarify the difference in meaning and use of the two
structures and to highlight the difference in auxiliary form for singular and plural
subjects. The learners were reminded to refer to their course book to look up the
past participles of irregular verbs.
Students in the WCF group received direct feedback on Writing Task 1 and
Writing Task 2 in Weeks 2 and 3 respectively, immediately prior to completing
a new piece of writing. Errors in verb forms where Present Perfect Simple was
obligatory were crossed out and the correct form was written above them. See
Appendix B for a sample corrected script. Students had eight minutes of super-
vised, silent time to look over their feedback before completing the next writing
task. The students did not have access to the corrected text when they wrote the
new text.
Metalinguistic explanation and direct written corrective feedback 67

The Comparison Group received no feedback but to eliminate the possibility


of extra time contributing to an advantage for the other groups, the learners in
this group were given eight minutes in Weeks 2 and 3 to read over and edit their
own writing from the previous week before completing a new piece of writing.

2.4.4 Coding and scoring


For each occurrence of Present Perfect Simple, five in each text, a maximum of
two points was given as shown in Table 2.

Table 2. Scoring of present perfect simple


Auxiliary ‘have’ Correct form of ‘have’ 1
Incorrect form of ‘have’ (have, has, had) 0.5
Some other auxiliary or verb form 0
Past participle Correct form of past participle 1
Only awarded if preceded by An incorrect form of past participle 0.5
an auxiliary verb
– Over use of ‘ed’ e.g. hitted
– Incorrect spelling
– A past participle is supplied but the choice of verb is
inaccurate e.g. played gymnastics

No past participle / past participle not preceded by an 0


auxiliary verb

Points were only awarded for the past participle if it was preceded by an aux-
iliary verb although not necessarily have. A different main verb was scored if the
meaning was the same (he has seen all his films/he has watched all his films). If the
Present Perfect Continuous was supplied in appropriate contexts, two points were
also possible as shown in Table 3. Where present perfect continuous was used
inappropriately, only one point was possible for auxiliary ‘have’. Examples of how
the scoring was applied can be found in Appendix C.

Table 3. Scoring of present perfect continuous


Auxiliary ‘have’ Correct form of ‘have’ 1
Incorrect form of ‘have’ 0.5
Some other auxiliary 0
Been + verb + ing Correct form of been + verb + ing 1
Incorrect form of been + verb + ing 0
Use in incorrect contexts 0
68 Mary Gorman and Rod Ellis

An accuracy score was calculated for each student for each task, by dividing
the total number of points scored by the total number of possible points,
expressed as a proportion of 1. The scripts were also checked for overuse in obliga-
tory Past Simple Contexts of which there was one possible in each text. If over use
occurred, it was calculated using Pica’s (1994) Target-like Use formula by adding
two to the denominator – two being the number of points awarded for each oblig-
atory occasion of Present Perfect Simple. Scores were calculated for total verbs and
also for regular and irregular verbs separately.

2.4.5 Analysis
To check the reliability of scoring, the pre-tests (25% of the total tests) were re-
scored by the main researcher one month after the original scoring. The two sets
of scores were compared using Pearson Product Moment Correlation (r) with a
correlation of .991.
Three students, from the ME and WCF groups, produced Present Perfect Sim-
ple with 80% accuracy or higher on the pre-test. These students were the excluded
from the data analysis because there was very little room for improvement. The
final sample comprised: ME (n = 10), WCF (n = 9), and Comparison (n = 11).
Because no statistically significant differences were found between the pre-
test scores for each group (F(2, 27) = 1.089, p = 0.351, ηp2 = .075), a repeated mea-
sures ANOVA was chosen to assess the comparative effects of different types of
treatment on accuracy scores for Present Perfect Simple at the four times. Model
diagnostics based on residuals indicated that the normality and homogeneity
assumptions were met.
However, the distributions of the separate scores for regular and irregular
past-participles were found to violate the assumption of normality. The assump-
tion of equal variance, as assessed by a visual inspection of boxplots, was also
not met. As such, non-parametric tests were conducted: Kruskal-Wallis H Tests
with post hoc Whitney-Mann U for group comparisons, and individual Fried-
man tests for time effects.
Between group effect sizes were calculated as partial eta-squared (ηp2), with
.01, .06 and .14 indicating ‘small’, ‘moderate’, and ‘large’ effects respectively (Cohen,
1988). Within group effect sizes were also calculated using Cohen’s d, with d = 0.2
considered ‘small’, d = 0.5 considered ‘medium’, and d = 0.8 considered a ‘large’
effect size.
Metalinguistic explanation and direct written corrective feedback 69

3. Results

Tables showing the mean and standard deviations of all the scores can be found in
the supplementary materials to this article. To obtain a general idea of what effect
the FFI had on accurate use of the Present Perfect Tense a comparison was made
of those children whose accuracy score increased from Task 1 to Task 4 and those
children whose score remained the same or declined. The results of this analy-
sis for the three groups is shown in Table 4. This shows clearly that neither the
ME nor the WCF had any consistent impact on accuracy. While some children’s
accuracy did increase, the same or a greater number of children in the FFI groups
showed no increase. In the Comparison Group a bare majority of the children
improved in accuracy.

Table 4. Number of children whose accuracy showed an increase or no increase


Group # Learners showing increase in accuracy # Learners showing no increase in accuracy
ME 5 5
WCF 3 6
Comparison 6 5

The results for the three groups will now be presented using graphs. To answer
the first research question, whether teacher feedback led to more accurate use
of Present Perfect Simple in new pieces of writing than writing practice alone,
the scores for the combined treatment groups (ME+WCF) were compared with
those of the Comparison Group across the four times. The results are displayed in
Figure 1. A repeated measures ANOVA showed no significant effect for treatment,
(F(1, 28) = 0.290, p = 0.594, ηp2 = .010), time (F(3, 84) = 1.346, p = 0.265, ηp2 = .046) or
any time x treatment interaction (F(3, 84) = 1.300, p = 0.280, ηp2 = .044).
The second research question addressed the comparative effect of ME and
direct CF. The descriptive statistics are shown in Figure 2. A repeated measures
ANOVA revealed no statistically significant differences for group (F(2, 27) = 1.358,
p = 0.274, ηp2 = .091) or changes in accuracy over time (F(3, 81) = 1.575, p = 0.202,
ηp2 = .055). The time-group interaction was also non-significant (F(6, 81) = 0.629,
p = 0.707, ηp2 = .044). A pairwise comparison of means for each group by time/
treatment (see Table 5) showed a small positive effect for each group from Time
1-Time 2, ME (d = 0.33), WCF (d = 0.42), and comparison (d = 0.39). There was also
a small positive gain from Time 3 to Time 4 for the Comparison Group (d = 0.40)
but small negative effects for the ME (d = −0.38) and WCF (d = −0.39).
Given that previous research (e.g. Frear and Chiu, 2015) suggests that the
effects of corrective feedback may vary depending on whether or not the target is
70 Mary Gorman and Rod Ellis

Figure 1. Scores for ME+WCF and the Comparison Group over the four writing tasks

Figure 2. The three groups’ mean scores on the four writing tasks

Table 5. Effect sizes for changes in accuracy over time


Time 1 – Time 2 Time 2 – Time 3 Time 3 – Time 4
Group Cohen’s d Effect size Cohen’s d Effect size Cohen’s d Effect size
ME +0.33 small −0.03 negligible −0.38 small
WCF +0.42 small −0.04 negligible −0.39 small
Comp +0.39 small −0.44 small +0.40 small

rule-based, a further analysis was carried out to see if the FFI benefited just accu-
rate use of regular past-participles. Because these are rule-based, they are arguably
Metalinguistic explanation and direct written corrective feedback 71

more likely to be influenced by FFI.3 Accuracy scores over the four times are
shown in Figure 3. There were statistically significant group differences in the pre-
test scores (Kruskal-Wallis H test, χ2(2) = 7.884, p = 0.019). As a result gain scores
were calculated and used in subsequent analyses. A series of Kruskal-Wallis H
tests found no statistically significant group differences at any time: χ2(2) = 0.926,
p = 0.629 (Gain Score 1), χ2(2) = 1.132, p = 0.568 (Gain Score 2), χ2(2) = 0.978,
p = 0.613 (Gain Score 3). Individual Freidman tests were conducted to see if there
were any significant within group effects over time. These showed no significant
changes for regular verbs (ME (χ2(2) = 0.000, p = 1.00), WCF (χ2(2) = 5.000,
p = 0.082), Comparison (χ2(2) = 1.083, p = 0.582)).

Figure 3. Mean scores for regular past participles by group at each time

To sum up, FFI did not result in increased accuracy of the target structure
(Present Perfect Simple) over time. Also there was no difference in the effects of
the Metalinguistic Explanation and Direct WCF. In both cases, there was an initial
small positive effect but this was also evident in the Comparison Group and it was
not sustained. Nor, when accuracy scores for regular participle forms were con-
sidered separately, was any beneficial effect for either type of FFI found. Within
group comparisons also revealed no significant changes in accuracy in either reg-
ular or irregular participles over time.

3. A limitation of this analysis, however, is that the total possible score for regular past partici-
ple was only 4.
72 Mary Gorman and Rod Ellis

4. Discussion

FFI consisting of metalinguistic explanation (ME) or direct written corrective


feedback (WCF) had no impact on the young learners’ accurate use of Present
Perfect Simple Tense. The initial gain evident in the second writing task dissipated
in the third writing task even though the children had received further FFI on the
writing they produced for the second task. By the final writing task negative effect
sizes were evident for both the ME and the WCF groups while the Comparison
Group had regained all the losses in accuracy in Task 3. In short, there was no evi-
dence that either type of FFI conferred any advantage nor evidence of any differ-
ence in the effect of the two types. Both were equally ineffective.
To investigate further, we also explored whether there was any effect for the
FFI when regular verb forms were considered separately. Previous research (e.g.
Doughty & Varela, 1998; Yang & Lyster, 2010) has suggested that different types of
oral corrective feedback impact differently on regular and irregular verb forms so
it was possible that the same would occur for FFI on writing. However, once again,
no effect for either type of feedback on the accuracy of regular past participle forms
was found and there was no difference in the effect of the two types of FFI.
These results for ME differ from those of other studies (e.g. White et al., 1991;
White & Ranta, 2002), which have shown a positive effect of providing children
with metalinguistic information about grammatical features. However, there is
a fundamental difference between the current study and these other studies, all
of which involved quite extensive metalinguistic explanation coupled with input-
based and output-based practice opportunities. In the current study the ME was
restricted to two sessions of eight minutes and there were no supporting prac-
tice activities other than the follow-up writing tasks. A possible conclusion, then,
is that ME, when unaccompanied with adequate practice, will have no effect on
young learners’ grammatical accuracy in writing tasks. Interestingly, however, ME
has been found to have a positive effect for adult learners even without practice
(Shintani, Ellis, & Suzuki, 2013).
Perhaps, too, the ME was ineffective because it did not sufficiently engage
the children. Myhill et al. (2013) emphasized that “the teaching of grammar and
writing requires metalinguistically aware teaching … involving active discussion
about authorial choices in writing” (p. 110). The approach we adopted was dialogic
in nature but it took as its starting point the form of the Present Perfect Simple
Tense, linking this to the meanings it conveyed in texts that had been contrived
to illustrate its use. This differed from the approach reported in Myhill et al.,
where the primary focus was on writing, not grammar, and the starting point
was making meaning, not grammatical form. Myhill et al. (2016) emphasized
the importance of “teachers skillfully challenging and questioning students about
Metalinguistic explanation and direct written corrective feedback 73

their writing choices” and then “handing back the final decision to the authors”
(p. 39). But the ME in our study was teacher-centred and focused on the structure
itself rather than on the students’ writing.
Like Fazio’s (2001) study, the results of the current study suggest that WCF
is of no value in helping young learners’ to improve grammatical accuracy. The
explanations that Fazio offered for her results also apply here. Present Perfect Sim-
ple is a complex grammatical structure that is typically acquired quite late (see
Bardovi-Harlig, 2000) so the learners may not have been developmentally ready.
The learners may have failed to pay attention to the corrections. Observations of
how they responded to their corrected writing when it was returned to them indi-
cate while some of the older students, especially the girls, engaged with the direct
corrections, the other children were not interested. Possibly, too, the children were
so focused on reconstructing the message content of the texts that they did not
draw on any explicit knowledge they had developed by processing the corrections.
We are still left with why the WCF in this study proved ineffective when
oral corrective feedback directed at learners of a similar age has been found to
contribute to increased accuracy of grammatical structures such as past tense.
Doughty and Varela (1998), for example, reported that 11 to 14 year old children
who received corrective recasts when they made errors in the use of English past
verb forms in an oral report of a science experiment showed gains in accuracy in
both oral and written tests in both the short and long term. Why then, was the
WCF in Fazio’s and our studies so ineffective? The crucial difference between oral
CF and WCF lies in the timing of the feedback. Oral CF occurs immediately and
contiguously to the utterance containing an error. Written CF is delayed. Doughty
(2001) argued that there is a short ‘window of opportunity’ during which learn-
ers can carry out the cognitive comparison of their own erroneous form and the
target language form and that if the opportunity this affords is taken up and the
target form attended to interlanguage modification can occur. In the case of WCF
(and also the ME in our study), however, the act of communication is over and
the window of opportunity long past. If motivation lies within the act of commu-
nication itself, as suggested by McNamara (1972), then there is no inherent need
for learners to attend to a written correction. Adult learners may choose to attend
to the corrections as they are more predisposed to treat language as an object for
analysis and to engage in intentional language learning and thus may benefit from
WCF. The young learners in our study (and perhaps Fazio’s too), however, were
clearly functionally oriented, geared to incidental learning, less equipped cogni-
tively to attend to linguistic objects, and less inclined affectively to do so.
If this explanation is right, it follows that it is the fact that the FFI did not
occur online as learners struggled to produce verbs in the Present Perfect Tense
that made it ineffective. If ME or direct WCF had been available immediately to
74 Mary Gorman and Rod Ellis

learners, as is possible with synchronous computer-mediated feedback (Shintani


and Aubrey, 2016), it is possible that children would have benefitted from it. Per-
haps, then, it is necessary to distinguish two senses of ‘integrated FFI’ (Spada and
Lightbown, 2008) – one where the FFI occurs between practice activities and one
where it occurs within. For children it may be the within-task FFI that is needed.

5. Conclusion

We have examined the effects of two types of FFI on children’s use of a difficult
grammatical structure (Present Perfect Simple) and found it ineffective. The most
likely explanation is that the children were not able or not disposed to use the
information they received to attend to form when focused on meaning in the
writing tasks. Neither abstract information about the grammatical rule nor direct
corrections of their errors helped them to avoid errors in subsequent writing.
However, it would be premature to call for abandoning FFI with young learners.
Within-task FFI or FFI supported by extensive practice opportunities and with
opportunities for young learners to engage dialogically with meaning-to-form
mapping may prove effective. Clearly, though, further research is needed to inves-
tigate the conditions that can make different types of FFI effective for children.
There are a number of other limitations in our study. Working with intact
classes meant it was not possible to balance the groups based on pre-test scores.
The ME group started with notably (but not significantly) greater accuracy in the
use of the Present Perfect Simple although this did not enable them to benefit
any more from the FFI. The mortality that resulted from difficulties in obtaining
parental permission and learners being absent from one of the four testing days
resulted in the relatively small sample size. It could also be said that the dictogloss
writing tasks lacked ecological validity. Van Beuningen (2010) argued that it is
necessary to use writing tasks of the kind typically found in the instructional con-
text under investigation. Perhaps, too, the children may not have been invested
personally in the dictogloss tasks (see Reyes, 1991). There were only two FFI treat-
ment sessions. It is possible that more sessions would have produced an effect
although studies involving adults have shown that even a one-shot treatment can
be effective.

References

Ammar, A., & Spada, N. (2006). One size fits all?: Recasts, prompts, and L2 learning. Studies in
Second Language Acquisition, 28(04), 543–574. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263106060268
Metalinguistic explanation and direct written corrective feedback 75

Andrews, A. et al. (2004). The effect of grammar teaching (sentence combining) in English on 5 to
16 year olds’ accuracy and quality in written composition. Department of Educational
Studies Research Paper, University of York.
Bardovi-Harlig, K. (2000). Tense and aspect in second language acquisition: Form, meaning and
use. Oxford: Blackwell.
Biber, D., Nekrasova, T., & Horn, B. (2011). The effectiveness of feedback for L1-English and
L2-writing development: A meta-analysis. ETS Research Report Series, 1, 1–99.
https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2333‑8504.2011.tb02241.x
Bitchener, J., & Ferris, D. (2012). Written corrective feedback in second language acquisition and
writing. New York, NY: Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203832400
Bitchener, J., & Storch, N. (2016). Written corrective feedback for L2 development. Bristol:
Multilingual Matters. https://doi.org/10.21832/9781783095056
Bouffard, L., & Sakar, M. (2008). Training 8-year-Ood French immersion students in
metalinguistic analysis: An innovation in form-focused pedagogy. Language Awareness,
17, 3–24. https://doi.org/10.2167/la424.0
Bryant, P., Devine, M., Ledward, A. & Nunes, T. (1997). Spelling with apostrophes and
understanding possession. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 67, 91–110.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044‑8279.1997.tb01229.x
Cameron, L. (2001). Teaching languages to young learners. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511733109
Chang, J. (2001). Chinese speakers. In M. Swan & B. Smith (Eds.), Learner English: A teacher’s
guide to interference and other problems. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511667121.022
Doughty, C. (2001). Cognitive underpinnings of focus on form. In P. Robinson (Ed.), Cognition
and second language instruction (pp. 206–257). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139524780.010
Doughty, C., & Varela, E. (1998). Communicative focus on form. In C. Doughty & J. Williams
(Eds.), Focus on form in classroom second language acquisition (pp. 114–138). Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Fazio, L. (2001). The effect of corrections and commentaries on the journal writing accuracy of
minority- and majority-language students. Journal of Second Language Writing, 10(4),
235–249. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1060‑3743(01)00042‑X
Frear, D. & Chiu, Y.-H. (2015). The effect of focused and unfocused indirect written corrective
feedback on EFL learners’ accuracy in new pieces of writing. System, 53, 24–34.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2015.06.006
Hanan, R. (2015). The effectiveness of explicit grammar instruction for the young foreign
language learner: A classroom-based experimental study (Unpublished PhD dissertation).
University of York, UK.
Harley, B. (1998). The role of focus-on-form tasks in promoting child L2 acquisition. In
C. Doughty & J. Williams (Eds.), Focus on form in classroom second language acquisition
(pp. 156–173). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Inhelder, B., Piaget, J. (1958). The growth of logical thinking from childhood to adolescence
(Parsons, A., Milgram, S. Trans.), New York, NY: Basic Books.
https://doi.org/10.1037/10034‑000
76 Mary Gorman and Rod Ellis

Laval, C. (2013). The age factor on the primary, secondary and cumulative transfer-of-training
effects of processing instruction on the acquisition of French as a second language. In
J. Lee & A. Benati (Eds.), Individual differences and processing instruction (pp. 105–130).
London: Equinox.
Liu, Q., & Brown, D. (2015). Methodological synthesis of research on the effectiveness of
corrective feedback in L2 writing. Journal of Second Language Writing, 30, 66–81.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jslw.2015.08.011
Lyster, R. (2004). Differential effects of prompts and recasts in form-focused instruction.
Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26(3), 399–432.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263104263021
Lyster, R., & Ranta, L. (1997). Corrective feedback and learner uptake. Studies in Second
Language Acquisition, 19(1), 37–66. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263197001034
McCawley, J. (1971). Tense and time reference in English. In Fillmore, C. & Langendoen, D. T.
(Eds), Studies in linguistic semantics (pp. 97–113). New York: Holt Rinehart.
McNamara, J. (1972). Cognitive basis of language learning in infants. Psychological Review,
79(1), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0031901
Michaelis, L. (1994). The ambiguity of the English present perfect. Journal of Linguistics, 30(1),
111–157. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022226700016200
Mollestam, E., & Hu, L. (2016, March 18). Corrective feedback on L2 students’ writing. Degree
project; Malmo Hogskola. Retrieved from <https://dspace.mah.se/bitstream/handle/2043
/20687/Corrective%20feedback%20on%20L2%20students%20%20writing.pdf?sequence
=3>
Muñoz, C. (2006). The effects of age on foreign language learning. In C. Muñoz (Ed.), Age and
the rate of foreign language learning (pp. 1–40). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
https://doi.org/10.21832/9781853598937‑003
Myhill, D., Jones, S., Watson, A. & Lines, H. (2013). Playful explicitness with grammar: A
pedagogy for writing. Literacy, 47, 103–111. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1741‑4369.2012.00674.x
Myhill, D., Jones, S. & Wilson, A. (2016). Writing conversations: fostering metalinguistic
discussion about writing. Research Papers in Education, 31, 23–44.
https://doi.org/10.1080/02671522.2016.1106694
Norris, J. M., & Ortega, L. (2000). Effectiveness of L2 instruction: A research synthesis and
quantitative meta-analysis. Language Learning, 50, 417–528.
https://doi.org/10.1111/0023‑8333.00136
Pica, T. (1984). Methods of morpheme quantification: Their effect of the interpretation of
second language data. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 6, 69–78.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263100000309
Pinter, A. (2006). Teaching young language learners. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Reyes, M. (1991). A process approach to literacy using dialogue journals and literature logs
with second language learners. Research in the Teaching of English, 25(3), 291–313.
Rummel, S. (2014). Student and teacher beliefs about written CF and the effect those beliefs
have on uptake: A multiple case study of Laos and Kuwait. (Unpublished PhD
dissertation). Auckland University of Technology, Auckland.
Shak, J., & Gardner, S. (2008). Young learner perspectives on four focus-on-form tasks.
Language Teaching Research, 12, 387–408. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362168808089923
Shintani, N., & Aubrey, S. (2016). The effectiveness of synchronous and asynchronous written
corrective feedback on grammatical accuracy in a computer-mediated environment. The
Modern Language Journal, 100(1), 296–319. https://doi.org/10.1111/modl.12317
Metalinguistic explanation and direct written corrective feedback 77

Shintani, N., & Ellis, R. (2013). The comparative effect of direct written corrective feedback and
metalinguistic explanation on learners’ explicit and implicit knowledge of the English
indefinite article. Journal of Second Language Writing, 22(3), 286–306.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jslw.2013.03.011
Shintani, N., Ellis, R., & Suzuki, W. (2013). Effects of written feedback and revision on learners’
accuracy in using two English grammatical structures. Language Learning, 64(1), 103–131.
https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12029
Spada, N., & Lightbown, P. (2008). Form-focused instruction: isolated or integrated? TESOL
Quarterly, 42(2), 181–207. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1545‑7249.2008.tb00115.x
Swain, M., & Lapkin, S. (2001). Focus on form through collaborative dialogue: Exploring task
effects. In M. Bygate, P. Skehan, & M. Swain (Eds.), Researching pedagogic tasks: Second
language learning, teaching, and testing (pp. 99–118). London: Routledge.
Talif, R., & Edwin, M. (1989). Error analysis of form four English compositions. Retrieved
September 20, 2017, from <http://www.melta.org.my/index.php/11-melta-articles/140-
error-analysis-of-form-four-english-compositions>
Truscott, J. (1996). The case against grammar correction in L2 writing classes. Language
Learning, 46(2), 327–369. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467‑1770.1996.tb01238.x
Van Beuningen, C. (2010). Corrective feedback in L2 writing: theoretical perspectives,
empirical insights, and future directions. International Journal of English Studies, 8(2),
1–27. https://doi.org/10.6018/ijes/2010/2/119171
Van Beuningen, C., de Jong, N., & Kuiken, F. (2012). Evidence on the effectiveness of
comprehensive error correction in second language writing. Language Learning, 62(1),
1–41. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467‑9922.2011.00674.x
VanPatten, B. (1996). Input processing and grammar instruction in second language acquisition.
Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
VanPatten, B. & Oikkenon, S. (1996). Explanation vs. structured input in processing
instruction. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18, 495–510.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263100015394
Wajnryb, R. (1990). Resource books for teachers: grammar dictation. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
White, J., & Ranta, L. (2002). Examining the interface between metalinguistic knowledge and
oral production in a second language. Language Awareness, 11, 259–290.
https://doi.org/10.1080/09658410208667060
White, L., Spada, N., Lightbown, P. M. & Ranta, L. (1991). Input enhancement and L2 question
formation. Applied Linguistics, 12, 416–432. https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/12.4.416
Williams, J. (2012). The potential role(s) of writing in second language development. Journal of
Second Language Writing, 21(4), 321–331. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jslw.2012.09.007
Yang, Y., & Lyster, R. (2010). Effects of form-focused practice and feedback on Chinese EFL
learners’ acquisition of regular and irregular past tense forms. Studies in Second Language
Acquisition, 32(2), 235–263. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263109990519
Yong, J. (2001). Malay/Indonesian speakers. In M. Swan & B. Smith (Eds), Learner English. A
teacher’s guide to interference and other problems (pp. 279–295). Cambridge: University of
Cambridge Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511667121.020
78 Mary Gorman and Rod Ellis

Appendix A. Dictogloss texts

Task 1
Bryan and his family have lived in Ampang for three years. Bryan likes doing exciting things. He
has been to Legoland three times since it opened in 2012, and last year his parents took him to
Hong Kong Disneyland. Bryan is learning English. He has studied at the British Council since
2012. He has had five teachers so far, but Teacher Mary is his favourite. Bryan likes Diary of a
Wimpy Kid. He has read two of the books this week! (81 words)

Task 2
Jessica lives in Damansara with her family. They have known Mary since 2013. Jessica is ten
years’ old and she is very clever. She has already started high school. She has studied hard this
year because the other children are much older than her. Jessica is also very good at sport. She
has done Taekwondo for five years. She got her black belt last year, so don’t fight with her! She
has already won two international competitions so far this year. (81 words)

Task 3
Kylie lives in Penang with her family. Her parents have owned a restaurant there since 2011. Kylie
is a good student and goes to Han Chiang School. She has also studied at the British Council for
two years. Kylie has had a lot of adventures since she moved to Penang. Her parents have taken
her all over the world. Her favourite place is England. She has been there three times. Last year
she met Mr Bean on the London Eye! (80 words)

Task 4
Zac lives with his parents near Universal Studios in Singapore. They have lived there for three
years. Zac wants to be an actor. His parents have taken him to acting classes since 2008. His
hero is Jackie Chan. He first saw a Jackie Chan film when he was eight years’ old and since then
he has seen every Jackie Chan film three times! Zac also wants to be a professional stuntman.
He has done martial arts since he was three years’ old. (82 words)
Metalinguistic explanation and direct written corrective feedback 79

Appendix B. Example of a corrected text

Appendix C. Examples of application of scoring system

Regular past Irregular past Been + verb Total


Example ‘Have’ participle participle + ing (/2)
(Bryan) had lived 0.5 1 1.5
(Bryan and his parents) have been 1 1 2
living
(He) studies at British Council 0 0 0
since 2012
(He) has have five teachers 1 0 1
(He) had five teacher so far 0 0 0
(Kylie) has been 1 1 2
(Her parents) has own 0.5 0 0.5
(She) has been studying 1 1 2
(Her parents) had brought (her all 0.5 0.5 1
over the world)
(She) has been studied at 1 0 1
(He) was seen every film 0 1 1
80 Mary Gorman and Rod Ellis

Supplementary materials

Descriptive statistics for the accuracy scores of present perfect simple comparing
combined treatment scores with the comparison group
Time 2 (immediate Time 3 (immediate Time 4 (delayed
Time 1 (pre-test) post-test) post-test) post-test)
Group N Mean SD Min Max Mean SD Min Max Mean SD Min Max Mean SD Min Max
ME+WCF 19 .31 .22 .00 .75 .40 .29 .00 1.00 .39 .25 .00 .80 .29 .28 .00 .85
Comp 11 .27 .24 .00 .70 .37 .27 .00  .67 .26 .23 .00 .55 .35 .22 .00 .60

Note. ME = metalinguistic explanation, WCF = direct written corrective feedback, Comp = compari-
son group.

Descriptive statistics for the accuracy scores of present perfect simple by treatment group
Time 2 (immediate Time 3 (immediate Time 4 (delayed
Time 1 (pre-test) post-test) post-test) post-test)
Group N Mean SD Min Max Mean SD Min Max Mean SD Min Max Mean SD Min Max
ME 10 .38 .22 .05 .75 .47 .31 .00 1.00 .46 .28 .00 .80 .36 .25 .00 .75
WCF  9 .24 .21 .00 .60 .33 .26 .00  .75 .32 .21 .00 .75 .22 .30 .00 .85
Comp 11 .27 .24 .00 .70 .37 .27 .00  .67 .26 .23 .00 .55 .35 .22 .00 .60

Descriptive statistics for regular and irregular past participle scores


Regular past participles Irregular past participles
Time 1 Time 2 Time 3 Time 4 Time 1 Time 2 Time 3 Time 4
Group N Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD
ME 10 .40 .39 .45 .37 .45 .37 .50 .24 .33 .27 .30 .29 .43 .28 .10 .16
WCF  9 .00 .00 .11 .25 .00 .00 .22 .36 .25 .30 .29 .33 .38 .28 .20 .25
Comp 11 .18 .25 .18 .25 .14 .23 .18 .34 .33 .33 .33 .42 .27 .33 .21 .31
Total 30 .20 .31 .25 .31 .20 .31 .30 .34 .31 .30 .31 .35 .36 .30 .17 .25

Descriptive statistics for gain scores and mean rank scores for accuracy with regular past
participles
Regular past participles
Gain Score 1 Gain Score 2 Gain Score 3
Mean Mean Mean
Group N Mean SD Min Max Rank Mean SD Min Max Rank Mean SD Min Max Rank
ME 10 0.05 0.55 −1.00 0.05 16.65   0.00 0.47 −1.00 0.50 17.30 0.05 0.37 −0.50 0.50 14.65
WCF  9 0.13 0.23 −0.50 0.50 16.39 −0.13 0.23 −0.50 0.00 13.72 0.25 0.38   0.00 1.00 17.67
Comp 11 0.00 0.22 −0.50 0.50 13.73 −0.46 0.27 −0.50 0.50 15.32 0.05 0.35 −0.50 0.50 14.50
Metalinguistic explanation and direct written corrective feedback 81

Table 12. Descriptive statistics for gain scores and mean rank scores for accuracy with
irregular past participles
Irregular past participles
Gain Score 1 Gain Score 2 Gain Score 3
Mean Mean Mean
Group N Mean SD Min Max Rank Mean SD Min Max Rank Mean SD Min Max Rank
ME 10 0.30 0.33 −0.67 0.34 14.15   0.13 0.23 −0.33 0.34 18.05 −0.34 0.27 −0.67 0.00 11.85
WCF  9 0.04 0.12   0.00 0.34 16.94   0.08 0.24 −0.34 0.34 14.56 −0.17 0.40 −0.67 0.34 15.38
Comp 11 0.00 0.54 −1.00 0.67 15.55 −0.06 0.42 −0.67 0.34 13.95 −0.06 0.47 −0.67 0.67 17.59

Address for correspondence

Mary Gorman
University of Auckland
14 Morgan Street
Methven 7730, Canterbury
New Zealand
[email protected]

Biographical notes

Mary Gorman is a recent graduate of the MTESOL programme at Auckland University. She is
currently a freelance teacher trainer based in Christchurch, New Zealand. Her research interests
include teacher response to constructive feedback and second language acquisition.

Rod Ellis is currently a research professor in Curtin University, Perth Australia. He is also a
visiting professor at Shanghai International Studies University and an Emeritus Distinguished
Professor of the University of Auckland. He has recently been elected as a fellow of the Royal
Society of New Zealand. He has written extensively on second language acquisition and task-
based language teaching. His most recent book is Reflections on Task-Based Language Teaching
(2018) published by Multilingual Matters.

You might also like